Skip to main content

Full text of "Ciba Foundation colloquia on ageing"

See other formats


m 


CffiA  FOUNDATION 
COLLOQUIA  ON  AGEING 

Vol.  5.    The  Lifespan  of  Animals 


Leaflets  giving  details  of  available  earlier  volumes  in  this  series^ 
and  also  of  the  Ciba  Foundation  General  Symposia,  Colloquia 
on  Endocrinology t  and  Study  Groups,  are  available  from  the 

Publishers. 


CIBA  FOUNDATION 
COLLOQUIA  ON  AGEING 

VOLUME  5 
The  Lifespan  of  Animals 

Editors  for  the  Ctha  "Foundation 

G.  E.  W.  WOLSTENHOLME,  O.B.E.,  M.A.,  M.B.,  M.R.C.P. 

and 
MAEVE  O'CONNOR,  BjV. 


■<(. 

C^ 


With  58  niustratioiis 
and  Cumulative  Index  to  Volumes  1-5 


LITTLE,  BROWN  AND  COMPANY 

BOSTON 


This  book  is  protected  under  the  Berne  Convention. 
It  may  not  be  reproduced  by  any  means,  in  whole 
or  in  part,  without  permission.  Application  with 
regard  to  reproduction  should  be  addressed  to  the 
Publishers. 


J.  &  A.  CHURCHILL  LTD.  1959 


The  Ciba  Foundation,  a  unique  international  institution, 
owes  its  inception  to  the  generosity  of  CIBA  Limited,  Basle. 
However,  being  established  under  British  trust  law,  it  enjoys 
complete  independence  in  practice  and  policy. 

Under  the  guidance  of  its  distinguished  Trustees,  the 
Foundation  offers  accommodation  to  scientists  from  all  over 
the  world  at  its  home  in  Portland  Place.  Foremost  in  its  acti- 
vities is  the  organization  of  small  conferences,  the  proceedings 
of  which  are  published  in  book  form  in  the  manner  of  the 
present  volume.  The  Foundation  convenes  many  other  in- 
formal discussions  between  research  workers  of  different  dis- 
ciplines and  different  nationalities  and  each  year  invites  an 
outstanding  authority  to  deliver  a  special  lecture.  An  exchange 
programme  between  French  and  British  postgraduates  is  con- 
ducted and  a  library  service  is  available.  Furthermore,  the 
Ciba  Foundation  attempts  in  every  other  way  possible  to  aid 
scientists,  whether  they  be  Nobel  Laureates  or  young  grad- 
uates making  their  first  original  contribution  to  research. 

The  purpose  of  the  Ciba  Foundation,  which  is  to  promote 
international  co-operation  in  medical  and  chemical  research, 
is  symbolized  in  the  armorial  bearings  by  five  interlaced  rings 
representing  the  continents,  a  black  sacrificial  cock  (emblem 
of  Aesculapius)  holding  a  medical  caduceus,  and  three  regular 
hexagons  for  chemistry.  Its  domicile  in  London  is  indicated  by 
the  red  sword  of  St.  Paul  and  the  British  lion;  the  wyvern 
and  the  crozier,  symbols  associated  with  Basle,  refer  to  the 
sponsoring  firm  located  in  this  ancient  Swiss  town. 


THE  CIBA  FOUNDATION 

Jor  the  Promotion  of  International  Co-operation  in  Medical  and  Chemical  Research 

41  Portland  Place,  London,  W.l. 

Trustees 

The  Right  Hon.  Lord  Adrian,  O.M.,  F.R.S. 

The  Rt.  Hon.  Lord  Beveridge,  K.C.B.,  F.B.A. 

Sir  Russell  Brain,  Bt. 

The  Hon.  Sir  George  Lloyd-Jacob 

Sir  Raymond  Needham,  Q.C,  F.S.A. 

Executive  Council 

Sir  Russell  Brain,  Bt.,  Chairman  Professor  Dr.  Dr.  h.c.  R.  Meier 

Professor  A.  Haddow,  F.R.S.  Sir  George  Lloyd-Jacob 

Sir  Arthur  Vere  Harvey,  C.B.E.,  M.P.  Professor  F.  G.  Young,  F.R.S. 

Director  and  Secretary  to  the  Executive  Council 
Dr.  G.  E.  W.  Wolstenholme,  O.B.E. 

Deputy  Director 
Dr.  H.  N.  H.  Genese 

Assistant  Secretary 
Miss  N.  Bland 

Scientific  Assistant  Editorial  Assistant 

Miss  Cecilia  M.  O'Connor,  B.Sc.  Miss  Maeve  O'Connor,  B.A. 

Librarian 
Miss  Lyliane  A.  R.  Treuil 

vi 


PREFACE 

Early  in  1954  the  Trustees  of  the  Ciba  Foundation  desig- 
nated funds  over  a  period  of  five  years  for  the  encouragement  of 
basic  research  relevant  to  the  problems  of  Ageing.  One  feature 
of  this  special  programme  was  to  be  the  organization  of  smaU 
international  conferences,  on  lines  familiarly  known  at  the 
Foundation  in  regard  to  other  subjects  in  medical  research,  to 
assemble  and  consider  such  information  as  could  be  gathered  in 
the  youthful  work  of  ageing  research. 

This  volume  contains  the  proceedings  of  the  fifth  Colloquium 
on  Ageing,  and  also  a  combined  index  to  this  and  the  previous 
four  volumes.  The  Trustees  now  suspend  the  Foundation's 
special  stimulation  of  work  in  this  field.  The  Director  believes 
that  considerable  attention  has  been  drawn  to  the  many  large 
gaps  in  knowledge  which  must  be  filled  before  the  postpone- 
ment and  amelioration  of  senescence  can  be  realized.  Research 
workers  should  now  be  left  to  their  task,  the  size  and  importance 
of  which  cannot  be  exaggerated.  When  exciting  progress  has 
been  made,  the  Director  will,  no  doubt,  be  anxious  to  arrange 
for  further  conferences  and  discussions  as  part  of  the  general 
programme  of  the  Foundation. 

The  Director  and  his  co-Editor  wish  to  place  on  record  with 
these  proceedings  the  indebtedness  of  the  Foundation  and 
themselves  to  Professor  Danielli  and  Dr.  Comfort  for  giving  life 
to  this  meeting;  to  Dr.  Genese  and  Miss  Chater  for  its  adminis- 
tration; to  Mr.  William  Hill  for  his  skill  and  speed  in  indexing; 
and  to  Messrs.  J.  &  A.  Churchill  Ltd.  and  Messrs.  Spottiswoode, 
Ballantyne  &  Co.  Ltd.  for  minimizing,  without  skimping  or 
harassment,  the  delay  in  publication  in  this  difficult  year. 


vu 


CONTENTS 


PAGE 


Chairman's  opening  remarks 
J.  F.  Danielli    . 


Actuarial  aspects  of  human  lifespans 

by  B.  Benjamin  .......  2      )/ 

Discussion:  Benjamin,  Berg,  Comfort,  Danielli,  Gerking, 
Gruneberg,  Jalavisto,  Rockstein,  Rotblat,  Sacher, 
Smith  .........  15 

Parental  age  effects  on  man  . 

by  Eeva  Jalavisto      .......         21     ./ 

Discussion :  Benjamin,  Berg,  Comfort,  Danielli,  Hinton, 

Jalavisto,  Muhlbock,  Rockstein,  Sacher,  Smith   .  .         31 

Studies  on  the  longevity  and  mortality  of  English  thorough- 
bred horses 

by  A.  Comfort    ........         35     ^ 

Discussion:    Comfort,     Hartwig,     Kershaw,     Rockstein, 

Smith  .........         55 

Lifespan  of  cattle  and  horses  under  various  climatic  con- 
ditions and  the  reasons  for  premature  culling 

by  W.  Hartwig  .......         57 

Discussion:    Benjamin,    Bourliere,     Comfort,    Danielli, 

Hartwig,  Kershaw,  Muhlbock,  Wolstenholme      .  .         70 

Onset  of  disease  and  the  longevity  of  rat  and  man 

by  H.  S.  Simms,  B.  N.  Berg,  and  D.  F.  Davies  .  .  72 

Discussion:  Berg,  Bourliere,  Comfort,  Gerking,  Grune- 
berg Holt,  Jalavisto,  Muhlbock,  Perks,  Rockstein, 
Rotblat,  Sacher,  Smith,  Tanner,  Verzar,  Wigglesworth         79 

Lifespans  of  mammalian  and  bird  populations  in  nature 

by  F.  Bourliere  .......         90 

Discussion:  Bourliere,  Comfort,  Chitty,  Danielli,  Ker- 
shaw, Sacher,  Scheidegger,  Smith,  Rotblat  .  .  .        103 

Arteriosclerosis  in  birds 

by  S.  Scheidegger       .......       106 

Discussion:  Berg,  Bourliere,  Comfort,  Danielli,  Hinton, 
Kershaw,    Jalavisto,    Lindop,    Nigrelli,     Rockstein, 
Scheidegger,  Verzar     .  .  .  .  .  .  .112 

ix 


76551 


X  Contents 

PAGE 

Relation  of  lifespan  to  brain  weight  and  body  weight  in 
mammals  / 

by  G.  A.  Sacher 115   ^ 

Discussion :  Berg,  Bourliere,  Comfort,  Danielli,  Holt,  Lin- 

DOP,  NiGRELLI,  ROTBLAT,  SaCHER,  SmITH,  VeRZAR,  WiGGLES- 
WORTH  .........  133 

A  review  of  the  lifespans  and  mortality  rates  offish  in  nature 
and  their  relation  to  growth  and  other  physiological  charac- 
teristics 

by  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt    ....       142 

Discussion:  Beverton,  Comfort,  Danielli,  Gerking,  Holt, 

NiGRELLI,  ROCKSTEIN,  ROTBLAT  .....  177 

Physiological  changes  accompanying  ageing  in  fishes  / 

by  S.  D.  Gerking 181  ^ 

Discussion:  Comfort,  Danielli,  Gerking,  Holt,  Nigrelli, 

RocKSTEiN,  Rotblat      .......       208 

Longevity  of  fishes  in  captivity  with  special  reference  to 
those  kept  in  the  New  York  Aquarium 

by  R.  F.  NiGRELLI 212 

Discussion:    Beverton,    Bourliere,    Comfort,    Danielli, 

Gerking,  Holt,  Nigrelli,  Rockstein,  Scheidegger         .       226 

Factors  influencing  the  lifespan  of  bees 

by  Anna  Maurizio       .......       231 

Discussion :  Bourliere,  Comfort,  Hinton,  Holt,  Kershaw, 

Maurizio,  Nigrelli,  Rockstein,  Sacher,  Wigglesworth        243 

The  biology  of  ageing  in  insects 

by  M.  Rockstein         .......       247 

Discussion :  Berg,  Comfort,  Gerking,  Jalavisto,  Kershaw, 

Rockstein,  Sacher,  Smith,  Tanner,  Wigglesworth         .       265 

The  rate  of  ageing  in  Drosophila  subobscura  / 

by  J.  Maynard  Smith  ......       269  ^ 

Discussion :  Berg,  Danielli,  Gerking,  Gruneberg,  Hinton, 
Kershaw,  Rockstein,  Rotblat,  Sacher,  Smith,  Wiggles- 
worth .........       281 

Group  Discussion 

Benjamin,  Chitty,  Comfort,  Gerking,  Holt,  Muhl- 
BOCK,  Perks,  Rockstein,  Rotblat,  Sacher,  Smith, 
Tanner,  VerzAr  .......       286 

Chairman's  closing  remarks 

J.  F.  Danielli 298 

Appendix 

Notes  on  some  mathematical  mortality  models 

by  R.  E.  Beard 302 


List  of  those  participating  in  or  attending  the  Colloquium  on 
"  The  Lifespan  of  Animals  ", 

14th-16th  April,  1959 


B.  Benjamin 
B.N.  Berg 

R.  J.  H.  Beverton 

F.  BOURLIERE 

D.  H.  Chitty 

A.  Comfort 

J.  F.  Danielu     . 

S.  D.  Gerking     . 

H.  Gruneberg    , 

W.  Hartwig 
H.  E.  Hinton 
S.  J.  Holt  . 

Eeva  Jalavisto 
W.  E.  Kershaw 

Patricia  J.  Lindop 

Anna  Maurizio 


O.  MUHLBOCK 
R.  F.  NlGREUil 

W.  Perks 
M.  Rockstein 

J.  Rotblat 


Dept.  of  Statistics,  General  Register  OflBce, 

London 
Dept.    of    Pathology,    Columbia    University 

College  of  Physicians  and  Surgeons,  New 

York 

Min.  of  Agriculture,  Fisheries  and  Food,  Fisher- 
ies Laboratory,  Lowestoft,  Suffolk 

Dept.  of  Physiology,  Faculty  de  M^decine, 
Paris 

Bureau  of  Animal  Population,  University 
Oxford 

Dept.  of  Zoology,  University  College  London 

Dept.  of  Zoology,  King's  College,  London 

Dept.  of  Zoology,  Indiana  University,  Bloom- 
ington,  Indiana 

Medical  Research  Council  Group  for  Experi- 
mental Research  in  Inherited  Diseases,  Uni- 
versity College,  London 

Stade  Herdbuchgesellschaft,  Stade,  Germany 

Dept.  of  Zoology,  University  of  Bristol 

Research  Programmes  Section,  Fisheries  Bio- 
logy Branch,  F.A.O.U.N.,  Rome 

Inst,  of  Physiology,  Helsinki 

Dept.  of  Parasitology  and  Entomology,  Uni- 
versity of  Liverpool 

Dept.  of  Physiology,  St.  Bartholomew's  Hospi- 
tal Medical  College,  London 

Bienenabteilung,  Eidg.  Milchwirtschaftliche 
Versuchsanstalt,  Liebefeld-Bem 

Netherlands  Cancer  Inst.,  Amsterdam 

New  York  Aquarium,  New  York 

Pearl  Assurance  Co.  Ltd.,  London 

Dept.  of  Physiology,  Bellevue  Medical  Center, 
New  York  University 

Dept.  of  Physics,  St.  Bartholomew's  Hospital 
Medical  College,  London 

xi 


Xll 


List  of  Participants 


G.  A.  Sacher 

S.  SCHEIDEGGER  . 

J.  Maynard  Smith 
J.  M.  Tanner 

F.  Verzar 

V.  B.  WiGGLESWORTH 


Division  of  Biological  and  Medical  Research, 
Argonne  National  Laboratory,  Lemont,  Il- 
linois 

Inst,  of  Pathology,  University  of  Basle 

Dept.  of  Zoology,  University  College  London 

Inst,  of  Child  Health,  Hospital  for  Sick 
Children,  London 

Gerontology  Laboratory,  Inst,  of  Anatomy, 
University  of  Basle 

Dept.  of  Zoology,  University  of  Cambridge 


CHAIRMAN^S  OPENING  REMARKS 

J.  F.  Danielli 

The  investigation  of  problems  of  ageing  is  still  in  its  early 
stages.  One  major  reason  for  this  is  that  observations  must 
be  made  on  old  animals,  which  are  not  readily  come  by.  A 
research  worker  in  this  field  must  have  patience  above  average, 
or  he  will  not  wait  until  his  animals  are  sufficiently  old.  And 
he  must  have  money  above  average,  or  he  will  not  be  able  to 
afford  to  keep  his  animals  sufficiently  long.  Even  where 
patience  is  available,  the  money  is  usually  not. 

In  view  of  this,  we  must  remember  that,  where  data  pre- 
sented in  this  colloquium  seem  inadequate,  it  is  usually  the 
cost  of  getting  better  information  which  is  the  main  restricting 
factor. 

This  colloquium  arose  largely  as  a  result  of  the  enthusiasm 
and  initiative  of  Alex  Comfort,  and  despite  the  seeming 
paucity  of  data  the  contributors  appear  to  have  succeeded  in 
producing  a  fascinating  group  of  papers. 


AGEING — ^V — 1 


ACTUARIAL  ASPECTS  OF  HUMAN 
LIFESPANS 

B.  Benjamin 

General  Register  Office^  London 

Actuaries  have  always  been  profoundly  interested  in  the 
variation  in  the  incidence  of  mortality  with  age  since  many  of 
their  calculations  of  contingencies  depend  upon  this  variation. 
The  study  of  this  age  variation  in  mortality  has  given  rise  to 
a  number  of  hypothetical  "laws"  of  mortality  based  on 
theories  about  the  exertion  on  the  human  body  of  deleterious 
influences  or  about  the  wearing  out  of  components  of  the  body 
and  the  exhaustion  of  living  resources.  These  theories  date 
from  Gompertz  (1825)  who  argued  on  physiological  grounds 
that  the  intensity  of  mortality  (in  his  terms  the  average 
exhaustion  of  man's  power  to  avoid  death)  gained  equal  pro- 
portions in  equal  intervals  of  age  and  Makeham  (1867)  who 
introduced  a  constant  component  as  well  as  a  logarithmically 
increasing  component  of  the  force*  of  mortality  as  a  reflection 

*  It  is  necessary  to  define  certain  functions  of  the  life-table : 

(1)  Ix,  the  number  still  living  at  exact  age  out  of  an  original  generation 
of  Iq  births  (Iq  is  called  the  radix  of  the  table). 

(2)  dx,  the  number  dying  between  exact  ages  x  and  x  +  1  {=  Ix  —  Iz+i) 

(3)  px,   the   chance   of  surviving  from   exact   age  x  to   exact  x  +  1 

(4)  qx,  the  chance  of  dying  between  exact  age  x  and  exact  x  +  1 

d-i 


i-t) 


Note:  p^  +  q^  =  1 

(5)  [ix,  the  force  of  mortality.  The  concept  is  of  an  "instantaneous" 
rate.  It  is  approached  by  expressing  the  average  rate  of  mortality 
at  age  x  (nix)  over  a  finite  interval  of  time  as  the  ratio  of  (deaths  at 
age  X  in  the  interval)  to  (average  population  at  age  x  in  the  interval) 

2 


Actuarial  Aspects  of  Human  Lifespans  3 

of  the  division  of  causes  of  death  into  two  kinds,  those  due  to 
chance  and  those  due  to  deterioration. 

It  was  soon  evident  that  such  a  relatively  simple  law  would 
not  represent  mortality  experience  throughout  life  and  sub- 
sequent developments  led  to  the  proposal  of  more  complex 
mathematical  relationships  between  age  and  the  force  of 
mortality  and  even  of  different  relationships  over  different 
parts  of  the  age  range.   Thiele  (1871)  for  example  proposed 

pL^  =  a^  e-^»^  -f  a^  ^-^^(^-^>'  +  ^3  e^^'^ 

in  which  the  last  term  is  a  Gompertz  curve  to  represent  old- 
age  mortality,  the  first  a  decreasing  Gompertz  curve  to 
represent  the  mortality  in  childhood,  and  the  middle  term  a 
normal  curve.  Perks  (1932)  introduced  a  new  family  of 
curves  in  the  general  form 

A +5c^ 


Kc"^  +  1  +  Dc' 


and  rationalized  this  procedure  with  some  interesting  specu- 
lations on  the  theory  of  mortality.  He  found  an  analogy 
between  the  "inability  to  withstand  destruction"  of  Gom- 
pertz and  the  then  current  physical  concept  of  entropy  change 
— the  measure  of  the  time  progression  of  a  statistical  group 
from  organization  to  disorganization.  Perks  also  referred  to 
the  previous  work  of  Karl  Pearson  who  fitted  overlapping 
curves  not  to  the  force  of  mortality  but  to  the  curve  of 
deaths,  the  curves  being  intended  to  represent  the  mortality 
of  old  age,  middle  life,  youth,  childhood,  and  infancy,  the 
causes  of  death  being  different  in  these  different  periods  of 
life.  Perks  pointed  out  that  this  search  for  homogeneity  in  the 
pattern  of  causes  of  death  might  lead  to  endless  subdivision 

and  then  considering  what  happens  to  rrix  when  the  interval  of  time 
becomes  infinitely  small.   Clearly  then  the  deaths  between  ages  x^ 

Xi  x+1 

and  X2  in  the  life-table  =  J   y.x.lx-dx-  and  dx  =  j     [ix-lx-dx- 

Xi  X 

The  continuous  curve  of  {[ix-lx)  is  called  the  curve  of  deaths. 


4  B.  Benjamin 

of  the  curve  of  deaths  into  component  curves  and  that  it  was 
questionable  whether  such  subdivision  could  be  theoretically 
validated.  Nevertheless  he  agreed  that  one  important  case  of 
sudden  change  of  composition  was  the  rapid  transition  between 
the  period  of  physical  growth  and  the  adult  period.  "  In  the 
adult  period  we  appear  to  be  subject  to  a  continually  increas- 
ing disorganisation  or  'inability  to  withstand  destruction' 
while  in  childhood  we  appear  on  balance  to  gain  organisation 
or  'ability  to  withstand  destruction'." 

While  further  work  continued  on  the  fitting  of  mathematical 
functions  to  the  force  of  mortality,  [i^.,  attention  was  focused 
on  the  curve  of  deaths  (i.e.  of  {i^-h)  ^s  an  alternative  operand 
by  a  paper  by  Phillips  (1935),  though  this  was  not  yet  to  be 
taken  up.  Meanwhile,  taking  a  closer  look  at  the  concept  of 
deterioration.  Rich  (1940)  evolved  a  theory  of  mortality 
based  on  an  analogy  between  degrees  of  health  and  degrees  of 
temperature.  This  gave  rise  to  a  "health  frequency  distri- 
bution" (a  concept  close  to  the  present  view  of  biological 
normality  as  a  dispersion  of  characteristics  within  broadly 
separated  limits),  and  changes  of  rating  within  this  distribu- 
tion could  be  regarded  as  forces  of  deterioration  or  of  recupera- 
tion. A  "natural  law  of  mortality"  emerged  from  which 
Makeham's  and  one  of  Perks's  functions  could  be  derived  as 
special  cases. 

Clarke  (1950)  took  up  again  the  analysis  of  the  curve  of 
deaths.  He  argued  that  mortality  improvements  had  not 
extended  the  natural  lifespan  but  had  only  allowed  more  to 
achieve  it.  He  distinguished  between  "anticipated"  and 
"senescent"  deaths;  the  ages  at  death  in  the  latter  group 
were  measures  of  natural  lifespans  and  had  a  frequency  dis- 
tribution like  other  animal  characteristics.  His  paper  formed 
the  basis  of  the  present  work  and  his  hypothesis  will  be  re- 
ferred to  again  presently. 

Beard  (1950)  also  paid  attention  to  the  curve  of  deaths, 
using  the  incomplete  gamma  function  as  the  basic  analytical 
function. 


Actuarial  Aspects  of  Human  Lifespans  5 

Later,  in  a  discussion  of  another  paper  on  fitting  a  mathe- 
matical law  to  l^  of  the  mortality  table  (Ogborn,  1953),  Perks 
suggested  that  reference  to  the  probability  models  of  the 
biological  field  and  the  data  of  simple  populations  was  the 
only  way  in  which  an  advance  would  be  made  in  the  develop- 
ment of  a  satisfactory  theory  of  a  life-table.  It  is  a  pity  that 
this  suggestion  has  not  yet  been  exploited  as,  if  linked  with 
the  idea  of  loss  of  biological  organization,  it  seems  to  the 
present  author  to  open  up  an  important  line  of  approach.  But 
we  are  straying  from  the  objective  of  this  historical  introduc- 
tion. 

In  1954  Phillips  returned  to  consideration  of  the  curve  of 
deaths  and  hypothesized  the  existence  of  a  basic  curve  of 
deaths  "to  which  all  curves  of  deaths  are,  as  it  were,  striving 
to  attain". 

Clarke's  division  of  deaths  into  "anticipated"  and  "sene- 
scent" has  been  further  developed  by  Barnett  (1955  and 
1958)  but  applied  to  the  force  of  mortality,  not  the  curve  of 
deaths.  On  the  basis  of  cause  of  death  grouping  and  the 
actual  shape  of  the  curve  of  observed  age  rates  of  mortality 
Barnett  distinguished  several  different  groups  of  anticipated 
deaths. 

We  may  now  return  to  Clarke's  paper. 

The  earlier  approach 

The  objective  of  Clarke's  approach  was  the  forecasting  of 
the  rates  of  mortality  which  would  operate  in  the  future.  His 
hypothesis  was  that  every  individual  carried  with  him  from 
birth  a  genetically  endowed  term  of  life  beyond  which  it  was 
impossible  for  him  to  survive,  and  that  if  we  knew  these 
terms  for  every  member  of  the  population  we  could  form  a 
frequency  distribution  similar  to  that  of  any  other  biometric 
quantity.  This  distribution  would  represent  a  limiting  form 
of  the  curve  of  deaths.  Clarke  went  further  and  supposed 
that  this  distribution  would  not  shift  as  a  whole  toward  later 


6  B.  Benjamin 

ages,  i.e.  that  the  modal  span  of  hfe  was  invariant;  he  re- 
jected the  possibihty  as  "intangible  speculation"  outside  the 
practical  framework  of  his  study,  namely  the  relatively  short- 
term  trend  of  mortality.  Deaths  were  then  divided  into  two 
categories,  namely  those  which  happened  because  the  term 
of  the  lifespan  had  run  out — senescent  deaths — ,  and  all  others, 
whether  from  accident  or  disease,  which  were  in  fact  a  cutting 
short  of  the  lifespan — anticipated  deaths.  He  first  obtained, 
therefore,  a  limiting  curve  of  deaths  by  constructing  a  table 
of  mortality  in  which  the  q^  (probability  of  death  between  age 
X  and  age  x  -[-  1)  column  would  consist  of  values  (g|)  appro- 
priate only  to  causes  of  death  which  could  be  regarded  as 
senescent.  The  next  stage  was  to  predict  the  pace  at  which 
actual  rates  of  mortality  would  in  a  given  future  period 
approach  those  of  the  limiting  table.  It  is  not  necessary  to 
discuss  this  aspect  here. 

Clarke  originally  intended  to  define  "senescent"  deaths  by 
choosing  certain  degenerative  diseases  (e.g.  cerebral  vascular 
lesions,  myocardial  diseases,  angina  pectoris,  arteriosclerosis, 
other  diseases  of  the  circulatory  system,  bronchitis,  nephritis), 
but  he  naturally  found  it  difficult  to  select  disease  groups  with 
sufficiently  specific  reference  to  degeneration.  Ultimately 
therefore  he  arbitrarily  assumed  ratios  of  qljq^  rising  from 
0-05  at  age  20  to  0-10  at  age  40,  0-20  at  age  50,  0-70  at  age 
70,  1-00  at  ages  80  and  above. 

His  limiting  curve  of  deaths  was  not  symmetrical.  There 
was  a  sharp  peak  at  age  80  with  a  tailing  off  rapidly  on  one 
side  to  age  100  or  so  and  on  the  other  side  a  rapid  decline  to 
about  age  60  and  then  a  much  slower  tailing  off  to  age  20. 

The  present  approach 

The  present  author's  approach  has  been  even  more  arbitrary 
and  pragmatic.  In  a  particular  life-table  the  values  of  d^ 
(deaths  between  age  x  and  x  -\-  1)  have  been  plotted  for 
every  value  of  x  in  the  table  (Figs.  1-3),  thus  producing  an 


Actuarial  Aspects  of  Human  Lifespans 


approximation  to  the  curve  of  deaths  {d^  is  of  course  discon- 
tinuous while  the  "curve  of  deaths"  is  continuous.  It  has  been 
assumed  that  d^  =  [i^^:^.!^^^).  The  curve  has  then  been  treated 
from  its  later  mode  (e.g.  the  peak  at  age  76  in  Fig.  3)  to  the 
upper  limit  of  age  as  the  right-hand  side  of  the  distribution  of 
"senescent"  deaths,  i.e.  of  normal  lifespans,  and  the  left-hand 


Fig.  1.   Curve  of  deaths.  English  Life-Table  No.  1.   1841.  Males. 

total  deaths 

senescent  deaths 

—  —  —  —  —  —  .  —  .  anticipated  deaths 


side  of  this  distribution  has  been  drawn  in  (broken  line  in  the 
figures)  to  exactly  mirror  the  right-hand  side.  It  is  thus 
assumed  (unlike  Clarke)  that  the  biometric  distribution  of 
lifespans  is  symmetrical.  When  the  deaths  of  this  left-hand 
side  of  the  distribution  are  subtracted  from  the  main  curve  of 
deaths  the  residual  (of  "anticipated"  deaths)  tails  off  (by  a 
broken  line  in  Fig.  3)  to  zero  at  the  peak  of  the  senescent 
deaths.  In  effect  it  is  assumed  in  Fig.  3  that  no  deaths  before 


8 


B.  Benjamin 


age  55  and  all  deaths  after  age  76  are  senescent.  It  is  hoped 
that  this  brash  sweeping  aside  of  the  honest  testimonies  of 
millions  of  medical  practitioners  which  are  recorded  in  the 
vaults  of  Somerset  House  wdll  not  give  offence.  It  is  of  course 
a  moot  point  as  to  whether  any  death  after  age  76,  even 


Fig.  2.   Curve  of  deaths.  English  Life-Table  No.  8.   1910-1912.   Males. 

total  deaths 

senescent  deaths 

_  —  ._._  —  —  ._.  anticipated  deaths 


allegedly  due  to  some  accident,  is  other  than  of  senile  origin, 
but  some  deaths  before  age  55  are  of  degenerative  origin  and 
at  age  55  Clarke  assumed  that  20  per  cent  of  them  were 
"senescent".  Whether  this  degeneration  is  senescent  in  the 
sense  of  the  completion  of  a  genetically  endowed  lifespan  or 
whether  it  is  the  cutting  short  of  the  span  by  departure  from 
optimum  environmental  conditions  and  behaviour  is  at  least 


Actuarial  Aspects  of  Human  Lifespans  9 

arguable  and  it  is  proposed  to  adopt  the  latter  hypothesis 
here. 


The  data  used 

The  analysis  already  described  was  applied  to  three  national 
life-tables  (for  both  males  and  females,  though  Figs.  1-3 
relate  to  males  only): 


Fig.  3.   Curve  of  deaths.  English  Life-Table  No.  11.    1950-1952.  Males. 

— — total  deaths 

senescent  deaths 

—  —  . —  —  .  —  anticipated  deaths 

English  Life-Table  No.  1.  The  first  to  be  compiled  by  Dr. 
William  Farr  and  based  on  the  deaths  of  1841.  It  differs  from 
the  other  two  tables  which  in  the  case  of  both  males  and 
females  start  with  a  radix  of  100,000  births,  in  that  Farr  to 
facilitate  the  product  of  a  "persons"  table  chose  radices  of 
51,274  male  births  and  48,726  female  births  (the  proportions 


10 


B.  Benjamin 


of  males  and  females  in  the  births  of  that  period).  All  the 
ordinates  of  Fig.  1  should  therefore  be  almost  doubled  to 
render  it  comparable  with  Figs.  2  and  3,  though  the  general 
shape  is  correct. 

English  Life-Table  No.  8.  The  first  short  period  table 
prepared  by  an  actuary,  George  King,  for  the  Registrar- 
General  and  based  on  the  deaths  of  1910-12. 

English  Life-Table  No.  11.  Based  on  the  deaths  of  1950-52 
and  prepared  by  the  Government  Actuary. 


The  results 

The  basic  results  of  this  analysis  are : 

Senescent  deaths 


Period  of 
deaths 

Males 

Females 

Peak 
age 

Standard 
deviation 
of  distri- 
bution 

Propor- 
tion of 
total 
deaths 

Peak 
age 

Standard 
deviation 
of  distri- 
bution 

Propor- 
tion of 
total 
deaths 

1841 

1910-12 

1950-52 

years 
72  0 
73-5 
75-7 

years 
9-38 

8-70 
7-89 

% 
39-9 
51-5 
69-4 

years 
73-5 
760 
80-3 

years 
919 
8-51 
712 

/o 

41   0 
55-3 
70-3 

It  is  clear  that  from  a  practical  point  of  view  Clarke  was 
right.  The  main  change  in  the  hundred  years  or  so  has  been 
the  increase  in  the  proportion  of  people  attaining  their 
allotted  term  of  lifespan,  from  about  40  per  cent  to  70  per 
cent,  while  the  shift  in  the  peak  has  been  a  mere  three  or  four 
years  for  men  and  seven  years  for  women.  Judged  by  the 
standard  deviation  the  spread  of  the  distribution  has  narrowed 
a  little  but  this  is  not  a  substantial  change.  If — and  it  is  a 
large  question  mark — this  arbitrary  distribution  of  "sene- 
scent" deaths  can  be  used  as  an  indicator  of  the  natural 


Actuarial  Aspects  of  Human  Lifespans  11 

distribution  of  lifespan  in  humans  then  it  appears  possible 
that  over  and  above  the  large  increase  in  ability  to  attain  the 
allotted  span,  the  modal  span  itself  is  slowly  increasing.  Even 
in  1841  there  had  been  a  "break  through"  of  the  barrier  of 
three  score  and  ten.  Women  can  now  talk  modestly  of  "four 
scores". 

Comparison  between  men  and  women 

If  the  proportion  of  all  deaths  which  are  senescent  can  be 
used  as  a  measure  of  ability  to  survive  the  allotted  term  of 
life,  then  the  improvement  in  the  mortality  of  women  as 
compared  with  that  of  men  appears  more  as  a  greater  shift  in 
the  optimum  than  as  a  greater  improvement  in  attainment. 
The  advantage  of  women  over  men  (or  conversely  the  dis- 
advantage of  men  as  compared  with  women)  is  thus  a  very 
general  one  and  calls  for  intense  examination. 

Other  aspects  for  examination 

It  would  be  possible  to  forecast  mortality  not  only  by 
examining  the  medical  and  environmental  possibilities  for  the 
reduction  of  anticipated  deaths  as  Clarke  suggested  but  also 
by  predicting  the  further  change  in  the  parameters  shown  in 
the  table.  It  is  tempting  for  example  to  suggest  that  the 
national  life-table  in  1970-72  will  show  for  men  a  modal  span 
of  almost  77  and  an  attainment  of  natural  senescence  by  78 
per  cent,  but  this  kind  of  temptation  will  be  pursued  else- 
where. 

Another  possibility  is  that  the  "attainment"  proportion 
(i.e.  ratio  of  senescent  deaths  to  all  deaths)  might  be  used  as  a 
mortality  index  for  international  comparison.  This  also  is 
outside  the  present  discussion. 

The  limitations  of  the  life -table 

The  life-table  as  normally  computed  is  based  on  the  rates  of 
mortality  experienced  by  the  population  of  all  ages  as  they 


12  B.  Benjamin 

pass  through  those  ages  within  a  short  period  of  term.  The 
population  observed  is  therefore  a  combination  of  a  large 
number  of  generations  born  over  a  very  long  period,  as  long 
indeed  as  the  extent  of  the  table.  It  does  not  follow  that  those 
who  passed  through  age  40  in  1950-52  will  experience  at  60 
(in  1970-72)  the  mortality  rates  given  at  age  60  by  English 
Life-Table  No.  11. 

For  actuaries  the  Life-Table  is  an  experience  which  they 
know  will  never  be  reproduced  but  is  nevertheless  a  model 
which  serves  to  guide  estimation  of  the  future  expectations. 

It  should  therefore  be  borne  in  mind  that  the  senescent 
deaths  are  of  people  born  long  before  those  whose  deaths  fall 
in  the  "anticipated"  category,  so  that  it  is  not  strictly  correct 
to  compute  the  ratio  which  one  group  bears  to  the  other  as  in 
the  "attainment"  proportions  referred  to  above.  Similarly 
the  peak  age  of  senescent  deaths  refers  to  generations  born  70 
years  or  so  ago  and  does  not  indicate  the  natural  lifespan  of 
those  who  are  now  in  their  infancy.  However,  the  indications 
of  secular  trend  are  acceptable. 

Strictly  we  should  calculate  "generation  life-tables",  i.e. 
tables  each  based  on  the  observed  mortality  of  a  single  genera- 
tion of  births  (for  practical  purposes  those  born  in  a  narrow 
interval  of  years,  say  five)  but  the  recorded  mortality  of  this 
country  does  not  permit  the  calculation  of  complete  tables  for 
generations  separated  by  more  than  ten  to  20  years. 

Accuracy  of  age  at  death 

In  all  the  kinds  of  analysis  of  death  rates  or  of  the  curve  of 
deaths  which  are  undertaken  by  actuaries  and  have  been 
considered  above,  use  is  made  of  a  life-table  model  rather  than 
the  actual  deaths  of  a  particular  year  in  order  to  base  the 
analysis  on  a  population  with  a  fixed  birth  entry,  thereby 
avoiding  the  irregularities  in  the  run  of  deaths  from  one  age 
to  another  which  affect  the  published  deaths  statistics  of 
any  one  period  and  arise  from  birth  fluctuations  or  other 


Actuarial  Aspects  of  Human  Lifespans  13 

population  disturbances.  It  is  sometimes  thought  that  the 
hfe-table  may  be  inaccurate  at  very  advanced  ages  because  of 
errors  in  the  stated  age  of  the  population  and  deaths  involved. 
In  order  to  settle  this  issue  a  check  was  made  of  the  ages  of 
alleged  centenarians.  The  Home  Office  supplied  a  list  of 
persons  (males  and  spinsters)  reaching  the  age  of  100  years  to 
whom  a  message  of  congratulation  had  been  sent  from 
Buckingham  Palace  between  April  1956  and  June  1958 
(married  women  were  excluded  because  a  maiden  name  would 
be  needed  for  checking  the  age  and  would  not  be  known). 
This  group  is  somewhat  selected  but  their  age  accuracy  is 
probably  not  seriously  affected.  For  the  114  persons  (53  males, 
61  females)  on  the  list,  my  colleagues  at  Somerset  House 
searched  the  birth  registers  for  the  relevant  entries  of  a  100 
years  or  so  earlier  (no  small  undertaking).  Of  these  seven 
could  not  be  found  and  identification  was  doubtful  in  four 
other  cases.  This  left  103  identified  and  of  these  there  were 
92  cases  where  the  birth  entry  agreed  exactly  with  the 
alleged  date  of  birth  while  11  showed  errors  as  follows: 

1  day  younger  than  stated  4 

1  day  older  ,,  ,,  2         ^ 

2  days  older  ,,  ,,  1 
2  years  older  ,,  ,,  2 
5  years  older        ,,           ,,  2 

11 

Serious  errors  amounted  therefore  to  only  4  per  cent  of  the 
total.  If  anything  these  figures  suggest  a  slight  understate- 
ment of  lifespan. 

Discussion 

For  the  purpose  of  indicating  the  tendency  for  natural 
lifespans  to  become  longer,  or  for  there  to  be  a  more  general 


14  B.  Benjamin 

approach  to  some  as  yet  unknown  natural  lifespan  (and  it  is 
difficult  to  separate  the  two  concepts)  the  present  over- 
simplified analysis  may  serve  well  enough.  It  is  indeed  argu- 
able whether  the  present  knowledge  of  ageing  processes 
justifies  a  more  recondite  approach. 

We  may,  however,  discern  the  possible  lines  of  future 
development.  If  it  be  accepted  that  ageing  is  a  process  of 
disorganization — the  introduction  of  the  random  element — 
then  we  may  apply,  as  Perks  has  suggested  (1932),  stochastic 
processes  to  study  first  the  distribution  of  ages  at  which 
organization  gives  place  to  disorganization  and,  second,  the 
distribution  of  subsequent  lengths  of  life  of  the  group  of  lives, 
w^hich  at  each  age  are  subject  to  progressive  disorganization. 
A  prerequisite  is  co-operative  study  by  biologists  and  statisti- 
cians of  available  evidence  of  the  age  incidence  of  the  dis- 
continuity between  organization  and  disorganization  in 
animals.  This  means  shifting  attention  from  death  to  early 
signs  of  degeneration  in  healthy  lives  under  continuous 
observation. 


Summary 

In  the  past  hundred  years  or  so  the  peak  in  the  age  distri- 
bution of  deaths  in  the  general  population  has  moved  to  a 
more  advanced  age  (for  men  from  72  to  76  years,  and  for 
women  from  73  to  80),  and  the  proportion  of  deaths  which, 
on  simple  assumptions,  might  be  regarded  as  "senescent" 
(i.e.  of  those  who  attain  a  predetermined  lifespan)  has  in- 
creased (for  both  sexes  from  about  40  per  cent  to  about  70  per 
cent).  Previous  analysis  of  the  so-called  "curve  of  deaths"  is 
reviewed  and  some  suggestions  are  made  for  future  analysis. 


REFERENCES 

Barnett,  H.  a.  R.  (1955).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  81,  105. 
Barnett,  H.  a.  R.  (1958).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  84,  212. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (1950).  Proc.  Centen.  Assembl.  Inst.  Actu.,  2,  12. 
Clarke,  R.  D.  (1950).  Proc.  Centen.  Assembl.  Inst.  Actu.,  2,  12. 


Actuarial  Aspects  of  Human  Lifespans  15 

GoMPERTZ,  B.  (1825).  Phil.  Trans.,  115,  513. 

Makeham,  W.  M.  (1867).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  13,  325. 

Ogborn,  M.  E.  (1953).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  79,  170. 

Pearson,  K.    Karl  Pearson's  Early  Statistical  Papers.  Cambridge 

University  Press   (1948). 
Perks,  W.  (1932).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  63,  12. 
Phillips,  E.  W.  (1935).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  66,  17. 
Phillips,  E.  W.  (1954).   J.  Inst.  Actu.,  80,  289. 
Rich,  C.  O.  (1940).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  70,  314. 
Thiele,  T.  N.  (1871).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  16,  313. 


DISCUSSION 

Danielli:  What  is  the  basic  mechanism  underlying  this  shift  of  the 
death  curve  to  the  right — that  is  to  say,  why  was  there  an  improve- 
ment in  mortality? 

Benjamin:  I  cannot  hazard  a  guess.  All  I  could  deal  with  was  the 
observed  deaths,  and  it  is  a  fact  that  the  peak  has  shifted  to  the 
right.  This  might  be  because,  as  Phillips  has  suggested,  successive 
generations  are  tending  to  attain  a  sort  of  ideal  curve  of  deaths — 
that  is,  a  very  sharp  peak,  even  further  over  to  the  right  than  has  yet 
been  observed.  Or  it  may  simply  be  that  more  people  are  in  fact 
surviving  to  a  constant  natural  lifespan,  which  means  that  the  curve, 
instead  of  being  pulled  to  the  left  by  what  Clarke  called  anticipated 
deaths,  is  allowed  to  grow  up  more  on  the  right-hand  side. 

Rockstein:  Does  the  initial  portion  of  your  curve  correspond  closely 
with  that  for  data  in  other  countries  ?  There  seems  to  be  a  rather 
high  mortality  rate  for  males  in  Britain  during  the  early  years. 

Benjamin:  I  have  not  yet  made  much  comparison  with  the  life- 
tables  of  other  countries.  I  should  not  have  thought  it  was  unusual 
for  the  Western  countries,  at  least  for  Western  Europe. 

Rotblat:  The  peak  for  the  anticipated  deaths  seems  to  become 
sharper  in  the  1950-52  curve.  Why  should  this  happen  ?  I  would 
have  expected  this  peak  to  become  flatter  and  spread  over  the  whole 
span  of  life,  rather  than  sharper.  If  you  used  a  skewed  distribution 
rather  than  a  symmetrical  one  perhaps  you  would  not  get  this 
sharp  peak. 

Benjamin:  I  am  very  grateful  that  you  have  made  that  point 
because  this  sharpening  of  the  peak  in  the  anticipated  deaths  is  in 
fact  phoney;  it  is  simply  an  accident  of  the  simplified  type  of 
analysis  and  has  no  meaning  so  far  as  I  can  see.  One  can  do  as 
Clarke  did:  make  an  arbitrary  assumption  about  the  proportion  of 
deaths  which  are  senescent  and  so  avoid  this  assumption  of  a  sym- 
metrical distribution.  But  my  difficulty  is  that  I  do  not  know  where 


16  Discussion 

to  draw  this  line.  At  the  end  of  this  meeting  I  may  know  a  Uttle  bit 
more  about  where  it  should  go. 

Jalavisto:  One  should  perhaps  think  about  three  curves — one 
independent  of  age,  running  horizontally,  a  second  one  beginning  at 
middle  age  and  rising  with  advancing  age  as  a  result  of  inadequate 
living  habits,  and  a  third  one  representing  the  random  distribution 
of  species-specific  lifespan. 

Have  you  studied  the  difference  between  the  mortality  curves  for 
males  and  females  ?  In  your  symmetrical  component  curve  I  would 
expect  the  base  to  be  narrower  for  the  females.  In  the  female  and 
male  mortality  rates  the  Gompertz  rule  holds  fairly  well  for  males 
but  in  the  females  the  assumption  of  a  random  distribution  around 
the  age  of  75  years  would  fit  the  facts  better. 

Benjamin:  The  width  of  the  symmetrical  distribution  for  women 
is  only  slightly  narrower  than  that  for  men.  You  would  like  to  take 
a  slice  off  the  bottom  right  across  ? 

Jalavisto :  Yes,  but  furthermore  there  would  be  one  group  which  is 
quite  clearly  a  result  of  externally  induced  pathological  changes,  i.e. 
through  accumulation  during  decades  of  carcinogenic  substances,  of 
cholesterol  deposits  following  high  fat  diet,  slowly  developing 
deficiency  diseases,  etc.  Of  course,  they  tend  to  increase  the  mortality 
with  advancing  age  because  they  are  just  a  function  of  the  chrono- 
logical age.  These  three  groups,  as  far  as  I  can  see,  can  result  in  any 
form  of  mortality  curve  according  to  their  mutual  relationships. 
But  in  any  case  the  end-point  as  part  of  a  normal  distribution  curve 
comes  out  very  clearly,  I  think. 

Berg:  There  is  no  difference  in  the  nature  of  the  diseases  that 
cause  death  at  age  50  and  those  that  cause  death  at  age  80.  The 
diseases  of  senescence  also  occur  at  age  50.  In  animals,  as  well  as  in 
man,  the  so-called  diseases  of  senescence  occur  in  early  life. 

Benjamin:  That  is  precisely  what  I  want  to  learn. 

Maynard  Smith:  The  anticipatory  deaths  you  mentioned  were 
presumably  deaths  due  to  causes  which  would  kill  a  person  at  any 
age,  and  not  merely  if  they  were  old.  What  worries  me  is  that, 
whatever  those  causes  are,  it  is  assumed  in  your  analysis  that  no- 
body dies  from  them  after  the  ages  of  about  60  to  70.  If  there  are 
causes  which  will  kill  people  at  any  age  they  presumably  will  kill 
old  people.  If  you  allowed  for  the  fact  that  old  people  are  dying  from 
accidental  causes,  as  well  as  young  people,  there  might  be  no  in- 
crease in  the  modal  age  at  death  in  the  later  life-tables.  In  other 
words  if  you  continued  the  anticipatory  death  curve  throughout  the 
whole  period  of  life,  the  peak  might  stay  at  about  70  to  72  instead  of 
shifting  to  the  right. 


Discussion  17 

Benjamin:  Two  different  kinds  of  error  may  be  introduced  by  this 
over-simplified  analysis.  I  am  more  worried  about  my  first  assump- 
tion that  no  one  under  the  age  of  55  can  die  of  senescence.  I  am  not 
so  much  worried  about  this  second  assumption  that  no  one  over  the 
age  of  76  can  die  otherwise  than  by  senescence,  because  if  an  old 
person  dies  of  an  accident,  it  is  very  difficult  to  know  whether  they 
would  have  had  the  accident  if  they  had  not  been  old.  In  other 
words,  although  a  deaJth  appears  as  accidental  in  the  statistics  it 
may  still  be  a  death  of  senile  origin. 

Danielli:  To  what  extent  can  you  correct  your  curves  for  acci- 
dents ?  There  must  be  some  proportion  of  accidents  occurring  to 
which  the  person  who  was  killed  makes  no  contribution;  for  example 
there  are  people  who  get  killed  in  railway  accidents,  and  as  passengers 
in  cars.  Then  there  are  other  accidents,  such  as  those  to  car  drivers 
and  motor  cyclists,  to  which  the  individual  concerned  does  make  a 
contribution.  These  two  groups  may  show  some  variation  with  age 
so  that  the  non- contributory  accident,  so  to  speak,  would  be  more 
independent  of  age. 

Benjamin:  You  could  calculate  the  deaths  which  are  due  to 
accidents  to  which  the  individual  may  have  made  no  contribution 
so  far  as  these  are  shown  by  the  certified  causes  of  death.  You  would 
of  course  get  rid  of  some  part  of  that  peak  of  "anticipated"  deaths. 

Comfort:  In  the  curve  for  deaths  of  pedestrians  in  road  accidents 
by  age  the  mode  is  a  very  close  fit  with  the  curve  of  general  deaths  if 
allowances  are  made  for  the  different  risks  to  infants.  Pedestrian 
deaths  are  an  excellent  measure  of  general  vigour — the  power  to  see 
a  vehicle  coming,  jump  fast  enough  to  avoid  it,  and  recover  if  it  hits 
you  (see  Comfort,  A.  (1957).  Ciba Found.  Coll.  Ageing,  3, 7.  London: 
Churchill). 

Benjamin:  Isn't  it  true  to  say  that  pedestrians  do  make  a  contri- 
bution to  the  accident  ? 

Comfort:  They  make  a  large  contribution  and  that  is  the  point. 
An  inestimable  amount  of  this  pedestrian  mortality  is  of  an  age- 
distributed,  or  age-conditioned  kind.  The  same  point  arises  over  my 
horses,  when  I  have  to  decide  which  I  am  going  to  call  natural  deaths 
and  which  I  am  going  to  call  non-natural  deaths.  It  is  a  point  where 
the  scoring  convention  becomes  very  difficult  to  determine. 

Jalavisto:  Death  from  appendicitis  might  almost  be  described  as 
an  accident.  Mortality  in  appendicitis  was  formerly  a  nearly 
horizontal  line,  especially  in  males,  and  it  did  not  rise  much  with 
age.  Later  on  when  conditions  improved,  it  can  be  seen  that  it  is 
especially  young  people  who  escape  death  from  appendicitis.  The 
result  is  that  the  curve  rises  and  begins  to  resemble  the  form  of  the 


18  Discussion 

usual  curves  of  mortality  seen  in  nearly  any  disease.  Obviously 
therefore,  even  in  these  age-independent  causes  of  death,  the  aged 
do  not  profit  from  improved  conditions. 

Sacher:  Your  analysis  of  the  curve  of  deaths  into  three  components. 
Dr.  Benjamin,  implies  the  hypothesis  that  the  population  is  divided 
into  three  mutually  exclusive  sub-groups,  each  of  which  is  subject 
to  just  one  of  the  three  modes  of  death.  An  alternative  hypothesis 
is  that  each  individual  in  the  population  is  subject  to  all  three  risks. 
On  this  basis  one  should  consider,  for  each  individual  at  each  age,  the 
joint  probability  of  dying  of  these  diseases.  The  basic  actuarial  func- 
tion for  the  discussion  of  mortality  on  this  probability  model  is  there- 
fore the  rate  of  mortality  function  rather  than  the  curve  of  deaths, 
for  in  this  model  it  is  the  contingent  probabilities,  the  mortality 
rates,  that  combine  additively.  The  multiple  risk  model  seems  to  me 
to  conform  better  to  our  intuitive  judgments  about  the  nature  of  the 
mortality  process.  The  same  data  that  you  analysed  in  terms  of  the 
rate  of  the  curve  of  deaths  can  be  analysed  in  terms  of  the  rate  of 
mortality  function.  The  conclusion  reached  is  that  one  component, 
identifiable  with  mortality  from  infectious  disease,  has  decreased 
markedly,  whereas  the  component  identifiable  with  mortality  from 
degenerative  disease  (your  senescent  mortality)  has  changed  very 
little.  The  changes  in  mortality  over  a  period  of  a  century  were 
assignable  primarily  to  the  progressive  change  in  numerical  value  of 
two  parameters  (Sacher,  unpublished). 

Gerking:  What  did  you  really  mean  by  saying  theoretical  life- 
tables  are  never  actually  reproduced.  Dr.  Benjamin  ? 

Benjamin:  A  life-table  is  made  up  of  a  large  number  of  genera- 
tions, and  people  who  are  dying  at  the  older  ages  have  been  born  a 
long  while  earlier,  so  that  it  is  only  a  model.  You  could  construct  a 
generation  life-table  in  which  you  had  only  the  mortality  appro- 
priate to  people  born  at  one  particular  time,  but  of  course  it  would 
take  a  long  while  to  accumulate  this  data  because  you  would  need  to 
follow  a  generation  right  through.  So  the  life-table,  which  is  based 
on  a  short  period  of  observation,  is  actually  never  reproduced 
because  the  environmental  conditions  are  changing  all  the  time.  The 
actuary  in  normal  practice  merely  uses  the  life-table  as  a  model  to 
indicate  the  variation  of  mortality  from  age  to  age,  to  obtain  a  basis 
for  his  calculations ;  he  makes  no  claim  that  it  will  be  reproduced  in 
the  future. 

I  agree  with  Sacher  that  you  would  have  to  use  rates  if  you  were 
applying  probabilistic  theory  because  that  is  more  fundamental, 
but  it  did  seem  to  me  to  be  easier  to  look  at  this  from  the  point  of 
view  of  lifespans  by  stretching  out  the  curve  of  deaths.   If  you  did 


Discussion  19 

what  I  suggested  that  you  should  do  here  and  took  a  look  at  the 
application  of  probability  theory  to  people  who,  as  it  were,  made  the 
change  from  organization  to  disorganization,  when  degeneration 
begins,  you  would  have  to  revert  to  operation  on  the  force  of 
mortality. 

Danielli:  It  has  been  alleged  that  people  exposed  to  increased 
radiation  have  a  decreased  expectation  of  life.  What  would  happen 
to  the  right-hand  peak  in  your  death  curves  with  this  particular 
group  ? 

Benjamin:  I  don't  think  there  is  sufficient  evidence  yet. 

Rotblat:  The  general  population  has  had  such  a  small  increase  in 
radiation  compared  with  natural  background  radiation  that  one 
would  not  expect  to  see  any  effect  on  the  death  curves.  On  the  other 
hand  we  should  consider  various  theories  of  ageing,  for  example  the 
recent  theory  of  Szilard,  who  suggests  that  we  all  start  with  a  certain 
load  of  faults,  or  mutants  as  he  calls  them,  and  that  these  determine 
the  lifespan  of  a  population.  If  we  accept  the  fact  that  radiations 
cause  mutations  which  may  influence  the  lifespan,  then  one  might 
have  expected  that  in  the  course  of  time  there  would  be  an  increase 
of  these  faults  within  us,  because  we  accumulate  the  radiation 
from  generation  to  generation.  I  would  expect,  therefore,  that 
the  whole  curve  would  gradually  shift  to  the  left  rather  than  to  the 
right. 

Prof.  Griineberg,  is  there  an  equilibrimn  which  we  may  expect  to 
reach  and  which  would  take  care  of  this  ?  How  would  such  an 
equilibrium  be  affected  by  the  fact  that  nowadays  people  who 
formerly  would  have  died  younger,  now  live  longer  because  of  con- 
ditions in  a  welfare  state  ? 

Griineberg:  I  think  the  effects  of  radiation  fall  into  two  categories. 
We  may  expect  the  ultimate  appearance  of  more  or  less  deleterious 
mutations  in  the  homozygous  condition ;  however,  as  close  inbreeding 
is  restricted  in  man,  the  appearance  of  recessive  mutations  in  homo- 
zygous condition  will  take  a  long  time.  On  the  other  hand,  right 
from  the  beginning,  we  may  experience  the  effect  of  these  same 
mutations  in  heterozygous  condition.  It  is  now  becoming  increas- 
ingly clear  that  at  least  some  of  these  mutations  in  heterozygous 
condition  can  be  advantageous  even  if  they  are  disadvantageous  in 
homozygous  condition.  One  might  thus  expect  an  advantageous 
result  in  the  early  stages  of  radiation  when  we  are  mainly  concerned 
with  heterozygotes ;  this  would  ultimately  be  counteracted  by  dis- 
advantageous effects  when  the  mutations  become  homozygous.  It 
is  between  these  opposite  effects  that  equilibrium  is  likely  to  estab- 
lish itself  in  the  end. 


20  Discussion 

Rotblat:  What  are  the  advantageous  effects  which  one  may  expect 
from  radiation  in  heterozygous  conditions  ? 

Griineberg:  It  appears  that  these  are  improvements  of  general 
viability.  In  Drosophila  the  chronic  irradiation  experiments  of 
Bruce  Wallace  (1957.  Proc.  not.  Acad.  Sci.  (Wash.),  43,  404)  indicate 
that  the  exposed  population  on  the  whole  increased  in  viability 
rather  than  the  reverse.  The  effects  on  general  viability  are  ap- 
parently due  to  hybrid  vigour,  due  to  the  presence  of  freshly  arisen 
mutations  in  heterozygous  condition. 


PARENTAL  AGE  EFFECTS  ON  MAN 

Eeva  Jalavisto 

Institute  of  Physiology,  Helsinki 

The  life  expectancy  of  offspring  in  relation  to  parental  age 
has  received  very  little  attention.  This  is  easily  understood 
because  of  the  difficulties  in  the  collection  of  material  suitable 
for  such  a  study.  In  official  vital  statistics  the  dates  of  birth 
of  the  parents  are  not  recorded,  and  the  only  feasible  method 
is  to  compile  data  from  family  records.  Holmes  and  Wilson 
(1925)  and  Holmes  (1928)  have  collected  material  from 
European  royal  lineage,  but  although  it  extends  over  about 
eight  or  nine  centuries  the  data  are  nevertheless  rather  scanty. 
Holmes  and  Wilson  showed  that  the  life  expectancy  at  20 
years  is  not  dependent  upon  maternal  age  if  families  of  the 
same  size  only  are  compared.  However,  on  the  whole  the 
older  brothers  tend  to  live  longer  than  their  younger  brothers. 
The  same  relationship  was  shown  by  Beeton  and  Pearson 
(1901).  Ansell  (1874),  Yerushalmy  (1938,  1939),  Gardiner  and 
Yerushalmy  (1939),  Burns  (1942),  Tabah  and  Sutter  (1948) 
and  Hoogendoorn  (1953),  amongst  many  others,  clearly 
demonstrated  that  the  age  of  the  mother  influences  the  rate 
of  stillbirths  and  early  postnatal  mortahty.  With  increasing 
age  of  the  mother  the  death  rate  of  children  under  one  year  of 
age  increases  steadily  even  in  children  of  the  same  birth  order 
(Yerushalmy,  1938,  1945),  but  the  question  of  the  postnatal 
mortality  is  controversial,  the  opposite  relationship  also  being 
found  (Heady  et  a/.,  1955;  Knox  and  Mackintosh,  1958).  For 
references  MacMahon  and  Gordon  (1953)  may  be  consulted. 

Material  and  Methods 

The  material  to  be  presented  was  collected  from  Swedish 
and  Finnish  published  family  records  (Jalavisto,  1950,  1951). 

21 


22 


Eeva  Jalavisto 


It  comprises  17,986  cases  in  which  the  years  of  birth  and 
death  of  the  children  as  well  as  those  of  their  parents  were 
known.  The  families  were  therefore  completed  when  recorded. 
All  cases  of  violent  death  were  excluded.  The  material  goes 
back  to  the  16th  century,  but  most  of  it  is  from  the  18th  and 
the  first  half  of  the  19th  century.    Most  cases  come  from 


35 


30 

- 

25 

Xr^ 

Ov 

0f     / 

\ 

s 

-20 

0) 

/ 
/ 

1 

\ 

\  \ 

o 

/ 

\  \ 

«  15 

.  (/ 

\  \ 

a. 

\  \ 

10 

.     •Born  before  1830 
O    ..      after  1830 

\ 

5 

— 1 1 i_ 

%1U 


240 


25-29        30-34        35-39 
Maternal  age,  years 

Fig.  1.   Percentage  of  eases  in  various  maternal 

age  groups  of  children  born  before  and  after 

1830,  respectively. 

Finnish  middle-class  and  noble  families,  the  rest  from  Swedish 
middle-class  and  noble  families.  The  social  level  is  thus  rather 
high.  The  life  expectancy  is  calculated  as  mean  age  at  death 
minus  the  years  lived. 

The  time  of  observation  must  be  at  least  100  years  if  correct 
values  for  the  entire  expectation  of  life  are  to  be  recorded. 
However,  this  necessarily  implies  inconsistency  in  the  material, 
caused  by  changing  environmental  conditions  during  such  a 


Parental  Age  Effects  on  Man 


23 


long  time  period.  In  this  study,  therefore,  no  attention  has 
been  paid  to  observance  of  the  100-year  span.  The  expecta- 
tion of  Hfe  was  calculated  for  different  maternal  ages  at  the 
birth  of  the  children,  notwithstanding  the  excess  of  short- 
lived among  those  born  in  the  latter  half  of  the  19th  century. 
It  was  assumed  that  this  would  give  erroneous  absolute 
values  for  the  total  expectation  of  life  but  that  it  would  not 
affect  the  parental  age  effects.   In  order  to  determine  whether 


50 

40 
30- 
920 


UJ 

t 


10 


Total  material 


50 
40 
30 


o 

II 

■X 

LU 

T 


Born_  before  1830 

Total  material 
cT 


20h 
j^All  parities 
2  Second  and  subsequent  children 


S2A     25-29   30-36  35-39    S40  ^24     25-29   30-34  35-39   ^40 

Maternal   age,  years 

Fig.  2.    Mean  length  of  life  {Ex=o)  in  various  maternal  age  groups  of 
boys  and  girls  born  before  1830  (12,786)  and  in  the  whole  series  (17,980). 


this  assumption  was  justified  the  relative  number  of  the 
mothers  in  various  maternal  age  groups,  in  those  born  before 
and  after  1830,  respectively,  was  calculated.  The  result  is 
shown  in  Fig.  1.  As  may  be  seen,  the  distribution  is  not 
uniform:  among  tnose  born  after  1830  the  number  of  young 
mothers  is  smaller  than  in  those  born  before  1830.  The 
deficit  is  evenly  compensated  for  by  the  other  maternal  age 
groups.  This  means  that  there  is  a  bias  in  the  expectation  of 
life  in  favour  of  the  progeny  of  the  youngest  mothers.  Fig.  2 
records  the  expectation  of  life  calculated  for  those  born  before 
1830  and  for  the  whole  material.    It  seems  evident  that  most 


24 


Eeva  Jalavisto 


of  the  decrease  in  mean  length  of  Hfe  is  due  to  inconsistency 
in  the  distribution  of  young  and  old  mothers.  Whether  other 
factors  were  operative,  possibly  in  the  reverse  direction, 
during  the  earlier  centuries  is  difficult  to  assess.  It  seems 
essential,  therefore,  to  avoid  calculation  of  the  entire  expecta- 
tion of  life  and  to  search  for  some  other  method  of  study. 


Maternal  age: 
cTlI-^" <  25  years 


Expectation  of  life  as 
function  of  age  (years) 


10       15       20 
■►AGEjx) 


30 


^0 


50 


60 


Fig.  3.    Expectation  of  life  (Ex)  as  function  of  age  in  the  male 
progeny  of  young  (<25  years)  and  old  (>40)  mothers;  first- 
born excluded. 


However  in  one  respect  the  expectation  of  life  calculated  for 
each  age  and  for  different  maternal  age  groups  is  instructive. 
Fig.  3  shows  the  expectation  of  life  as  a  function  of  age  for  the 
progeny  of  young  and  old  mothers  (first-born  excluded).  It 
may  be  noted  that  from  6  years  of  age  onwards  the  difference 
in  expectation  of  life  between  the  progeny  of  young  and  old 
mothers  begins  to  diminish  and  between  the  ages  of  approxi- 
mately 15  to  40  years  the  difference  is  constant.  This  can  be 
interpreted  as  evidence  that  at  least  in  the  middle-aged  the 


Parental  Age  Effects  on  Man  25 

maternal  age  does  not  affect  the  mortality.  Therefore,  it 
should  be  possible  to  use  a  group  between  the  ages  of  15  and 
40  years  as  a  basis  of  reference  when  the  maternal  age  effects 
have  to  be  elucidated.  The  frequency  of  births  in  different 
age  groups  cannot  be  determined  directly  because  of  the 
fictitious  nature  of  the  population  to  be  studied.  Since  only 
cases  of  death  are  recorded,  variations  in  death  rate  affect  the 
age  structure  of  such  a  population.  Furthermore,  since  the 
material  comprises  data  from  individuals  whose  birth  may  be 
several  centuries  apart  it  would  not  be  possible  to  use  data 
given  by  vital  statistics.  The  distribution  of  births  varies  in 
different  historical  periods  and  for  Finland  the  births  grouped 
according  to  the  age  of  the  mother  are  given  only  from  1871 
onwards.  The  present  author  therefore  thought  that  since  the 
mortality  in  the  age  group  20-29  years  is  probably  not 
affected  by  maternal  age  the  percentage  distribution  of  cases 
of  this  age  would  reflect  the  frequency  of  births  in  the  dif- 
ferent maternal  age  groups.  If  similar  percentage  distribution 
curves  are  constructed  for  ages  at  death  below  6  years  of  age  a 
possible  parental  age  effect  would  manifest  itself  in  an  excess 
of  cases  in  one  of  the  maternal  age  groups,  and  this  is  what  is 
actually  seen.  Fig.  4  gives  the  distribution  of  cases  in  various 
maternal  age  groups.  As  can  be  seen,  the  curves  representing 
deaths  during  the  first  and  the  second  years  of  life  have  a  dif- 
ferent course  from  all  the  other  groups  studied.  The  maternal 
age  group  of  25-29  years  seems  to  be  particularly  favourable 
for  the  infants  whereas  the  maternal  ages  over  35  years  seem 
to  have  an  excess  mortality  of  infants  less  than  2  years  of  age. 
If  the  distribution  of  cases  is  studied  by  the  x^  method  it 
shows  that  the  distribution  of  deaths  of  children  under  2 
years,  from  2  to  19,  and  between  20  and  29  years,  of  these 
maternal  age  groups,  differs  from  a  random  distribution  quite 
significantly:  x^  =25-7  (degrees  of  freedom  =  6)  P< 0-001. 
If,  however,  deaths  at  2-4  years  are  examined  it  may  be  noted 
that  the  distribution  of  deaths  in  this  age  resembles  more 
closely  the  distribution  of  deaths  in  the  20-29  year  group  than 


26 


Eeva  Jalavisto 


that  in  the  group  under  2  years  old.  Furthermore,  if  a  group 
is  formed  containing  the  cases  of  2-19  years  it  differs  from  the 
20-29  years  of  age  group  only  slightly,  the  probability  for 
random  occurrence  being  0-05  >  P  >  0-02.  Since  the  age 
group  20-29  is  small,  the  age  group  2-29  years  may  be  used 
as  the  basis  of  reference. 


35- 


30 


25 


20 


C  15 

« 

a 
10 


X  Dead  during  first  year  of  life 

•    ■•         ••     second 

O    .,         „     3rd  &  4th 

D    ••      between  ages  2-29  years 
^ 20-29     '• 


i2U 


25-29 


25-29       30-34       235 


30-34        235  i2L 

Maternal  age,  years 

Fig.  4.  Distribution  in  various  maternal  age  groups  of  deaths 

of  children  under  30  years  of  age.    Total  series,  5,590  cases ; 

2,735  were  born  before  1800. 


It  may  be  asked  whether  the  difference  found  is  a  real  one, 
or  could  be  due  to  differences  in  recording,  i.e.  due  to  the  fact 
that  in  the  latter  part  of  the  19th  century  the  number  of 
recorded  infant  deaths  is  relatively  greater  than  in  earlier 
years.  In  order  to  exclude  this  fallacy,  the  same  comparison 
has  been  made  with  cases  born  before  the  year  1800.  The 
result  is  exactly  the  same  except  that  the  number  of  cases  in 
the  20-29-year  group  is  rather  small  and  gives  a  somewhat 
abnormal  distribution  of  deaths.    The  2-29-year   group   is 


Parental  Age  Effects  on  Man  27 

therefore  to  be  preferred  as  an  indication  for  the  distribution 
of  births  in  this  population.  Since  on  an  average  only  29  years 
separate  the  deaths  occurring  during  early  infancy  from  this 
"standard  distribution"  it  is  not  conceivable  that  differences 
in  external  conditions  could  affect  the  result.  The  conclusion 
would  therefore  seem  to  be  that  a  maternal  age  of  over  35 
years  increases  the  mortality  of  the  progeny  during  the  first 
two  years  of  life.  The  optimurn  age  with  the  least  deaths  is 
that  of  25-29  years  of  age.  Beyond  early  infancy,  namely 
between  the  ages  of  2  and  4  years,  the  effect  of  advanced 
maternal  age  is  already  quite  small  or  non-existent,  and  it 
cannot  be  demonstrated  in  older  groups,  either  because  there 
is  no  influence  or  because  of  methodological  difficulties 
arising  out  of  incompatibility  of  life-expectancy  data  col- 
lected from  an  over-long  (at  least  100  years)  time  period. 

No  mention  has  so  far  been  made  of  the  possible  role  of 
paternal  age  effects.  Since  maternal  and  paternal  ages  tend 
to  be  correlated  the  question  is  rather  intricate.  If  the 
material  is  divided  into  paternal  age  groups  and  a  dependence 
of  life  expectancy  or  infant  mortality  on  paternal  age  is 
recorded,  this  may  simply  reflect  the  effect  of  maternal  age. 
If  on  the  other  hand  no  difference  is  noted,  then  the  paternal 
effect  possibly  acts  in  the  opposite  sense  to  the  maternal  age, 
high  paternal  age  being  favourable  for  the  progeny.  Curiously 
enough,  in  the  total  material,  in  spite  of  the  bias  shown  to 
arise  from  the  different  distribution  of  young  and  old  mothers 
in  the  material  collected  before  and  after  1830,  no  paternal 
age  effect  upon  the  total  expectation  of  life  could  be  demon- 
strated. The  most  probable  explanation  would  be  that 
maternal  and  paternal  ages  may  after  all  not  be  strictly 
correlated.  The  rather  large  mean  difference  found  between 
the  age  of  spouses  (7-10  years)  obviously  allows  fairly  wide 
discordant  variations  in  the  material  considered  here.  The 
same  objections  can,  however,  be  made  against  calculation  of 
the  total  expectation  of  life  in  the  paternal  age  series  as  in  the 
maternal  series.  Therefore  the  same  procedure  for  elimination 


28 


Eeva  Jalavisto 


of  external  bias  arising  from  the  100-year  time  span  necessary 
for  studies  of  total  life  expectancy  has  also  to  be  applied  in  the 
paternal  age  series. 

The  percentage  distribution  of  deaths  at  various  ages  (0,  1, 
2-4,  20-29  and  2-29  years)  in  different  age  groups  of  fathers 
is  calculated.  The  paternal  age  groups  are  formed  so  that  the 
limits  are  five  years  higher  than  in  the  maternal  series,  and  the 


30r 


25 


20 


^  15 


10 


Total 


30 


c25 


X  Dead  during  first   year  of  life 

•  ■■     second 

O    •■         ••     3rd  & 4th 

D    ••      between  ages  2-29  years 
^ 20-29     •• 


<30       30-34     35-39      UO-UU     245  <30       30-34     35-39    40-U      245 

Paternal  age,  years 

Fig.  5.    Distribution  in  various  paternal  age  groups  of  deaths  of 

children  under  30  years  of  age.    Total  series,  5,590  cases;  2,735 

were  born  before  1800. 


last  group  extends  ten  years  beyond  the  highest  maternal  age 
group. 

The  result  is  seen  in  Fig.  5.  It  may  be  seen  at  a  glance  that 
differences  in  paternal  age  do  not  affect  the  distribution  of 
cases  at  any  age :  there  is  hardly  any  excess  of  infant  deaths 
with  fathers  of  advanced  ages.  The  difference  between  the 
maternal  and  paternal  age  distribution  of  infant  deaths 
strengthens  the  impression  that  the  maternal  age  effects  are 
not  artifacts,  and  that  this  method  of  study  is  practicable. 


Parental  Age  Effects  on  Man  29 

Discussion 

One-third  of  the  total  material  is  made  up  of  deaths  under 
the  age  of  30  years  (5,590  cases).  These  are,  however,  divided 
into  several  parental  age  groups  and  groups  with  different 
ages  at  death.  The  cases  in  a  group  are  thereby  reduced  to 
quite  a  small  number,  always  less  than  a  thousand  and  some- 
times as  low  as  200.  A  further  reduction,  e.g.  resulting  from 
grouping  according  to  birth  order,  would  not  give  relevant 
information  because  of  the  smallness  of  the  material.  Since 
parity  and  maternal  age  tend  to  correlate  it  is  not  possible  to 
study  the  effects  of  birth  order  on  infant  mortality  in  this 
series.  However,  the  main  result  of  this  investigation  suggests 
that  the  maternal  age  effect  is  restricted  to  the  first  two  years 
of  life.  It  is  therefore  not  necessary  to  use  genealogical  material 
for  the  elucidation  of  questions  on  the  effects  of  parity,  etc., 
since  recent  statistics  are  available  and  more  appropriate  for 
this  purpose. 

The  difficulties  when  dealing  with  family  histories  are  many, 
and  lie  mostly  in  their  heterogeneity.  The  only  uniform 
feature  is  the  rather  high  social  rank  of  the  families.  Family 
records  of  labourers'  or  peasants'  families,  for  example,  are 
rarely  available.  The  advantage  of  the  uniformity  is,  however, 
lost  by  the  necessarily  long  time  period,  usually  covering 
several  centuries,  needed  to  compile  a  population  sample  of  a 
sufficient  size.  During  such  a  long  period  of  time  famines, 
epidemics,  general  hygiene  and  the  standard  of  living  have 
changed  and  influenced  mortality  rates  and  causes  of  death. 

The  results  concerning  maternal  age  effects  are  rather  con- 
troversial. Whereas  there  is  agreement  concerning  perinatal 
mortality,  which  increases  with  age  of  the  mother,  infant 
mortality  is  found  sometimes  to  increase  and  sometimes  to 
decrease  with  maternal  age.  When  family  histories  have  been 
used  for  investigation  of  effects  on  longevity  a  slight  de- 
leterious effect  of  advanced  maternal  age  has  usually  been 
found.  Gibson  and  McKeown  (1950)  have  pointed  out  that 


30  Eeva  Jalavisto 

favourable  economic  circumstances  in  the  population  studied 
tend  to  eliminate  the  maternal  age  effects,  which  may  explain 
the  discrepancy  between  earlier  and  more  recent  data.  In 
spite  of  the  high  social  level  of  the  family  histories  on  which 
this  study  is  based  they  represent  a  population  with  a  very 
high  mortality.  It  is  quite  obvious  that  if  a  maternal  age 
effect  exists,  it  is  never  very  pronounced.  Therefore  it  is 
easily  understood  that  low  mortality  in  favourable  conditions 
does  not  allow  such  small  effects  to  manifest  themselves. 
Consequently  recent  population  statistics  in  western  "Welfare 
States"  can  no  longer  be  used  for  detection  of  maternal  age 
effects.  However,  there  still  exist  countries  with  sufficiently 
low  standards  of  living  for  such  studies.  It  would  be  interest- 
ing to  compare  records  from  these  countries  with  data  from 
countries  with  a  high  standard  of  living. 

Summary 

Parental  age  effects  were  studied  in  material  comprising 
17,986  cases  collected  from  Scandinavian  noble  and  middle- 
class  family  histories.  It  was  concluded  that  total  expectation 
of  life  is  not  suited  for  elucidation  of  such  effects,  because  of 
the  necessarily  extended  period  of  observation  with  resulting 
inconsistency  of  the  data.  Since,  however,  such  calculation 
made  it  probable  that  advanced  maternal  age  had  no  effect  on 
deaths  after  the  age  of  15  years,  the  group  of  dead  between 
the  ages  of  20  and  29  could  be  used  as  indicative  of  the 
frequency  of  births  in  different  parental  ages.  When  the  dis- 
tribution of  dead  during  the  first  and  second  years  was  com- 
pared to  that  in  the  20-29  year  group,  a  slight  excess  of  infant 
deaths  during  the  first  and  second  years  of  life  was  noted  in 
the  highest  maternal  age  group  (  ^  35  years).  In  the  maternal 
age  group  25-29  years  the  relative  number  of  infant  deaths 
was  remarkably  low,  this  maternal  age  being  the  most 
favourable  for  the  survival  of  the  offspring.  A  similar  com- 
parison in  the  paternal  series  did  not  show  any  significant 
paternal  age  effect  on  the  survival  of  the  progeny. 


Parental  Age  Effects  on  Man  31 


REFERENCES 

Ansell,  S.  (1874.).  Quoted  by  MacMahon  and  Gordon  (1953). 
Beeton,  M.,  and  Pearson,  K.  (1901).  Biometrika,  1,  50. 
Burns,  C.  M.  (1942).   Quoted  by  MacMahon  and  Gordon  (1953). 
Gardiner,  E.  M.,  and  Yerushalmy,  J.  (1939).  Amer.  J.  Hyg.,  30,  11. 
Gibson,  J.  R.,  and  McK:eown,  T.  (1950).  Brit.  J.  soc.  Med.,  4,  221. 
Heady,  J.  A.,  Daly,  C,  and  Morris,  J.  N.  (1955).  Lancet,  1,  395. 
Heady,  J.  A.,  Stevens,  C.  F.,  Daly,  C,  and  Morris,  J.  N.  (1955). 

Lancet,  1,  499. 
Holmes,  S.  J.  (1928).    Univ.  Calif.  Publ.  ZooL,  31,  359. 
Holmes,  S.  J.,  and  Wilson,  I.  (1925).  J.  Hered.,  16,  47. 
HooGENDOORN,  D.  (1953).   Ned.  T.  Geneesk.,  97,  2130. 
Jalavisto,  E.  (1950).  Rev.  med.  Liege,  5,  719. 
Jalavisto,  E.  (1951).   Ann.  Med.  intern.  Fenn.,  40,  263. 
Knox,  G.,  and  Mackintosh,  J.  (1958).   Brit.  J.prev.  soc.  Med.,  12, 131. 
MacMahon,  B.,  and  Gordon,  J.  E.  (1953).  Amer.  J.  med.  Sci.,  226,  326. 
Tabah,  L.,  and  Sutter,  J.  (1948).  Population  (Paris),  3,  63. 
Yerushalmy,  J.  (1938).   Amer.  J.  Hyg.,  28,  244. 
Yerushalmy,  J.  (1939).   Hum.  Biol,  11,  342. 
Yerushalmy,  J.  (1945).   Hum.  Biol.,  17,  65. 


DISCUSSION 

S acker:  Aren't  many  of  the  early  deaths  at  present  due  to  con- 
genital defects  and  could  that  be  strongly  related  to  parity  ? 

Jalavisto:  In  young  mothers  the  mortality  rate  increases  according 
to  their  parity,  but  in  older  mothers  that  does  not  matter  so  much. 

Sacher:  So  you  do  not  think  that  parity  is  a  major  factor  ? 

Jalavisto:  It  might  be. 

Rockstein :  There  are  two  distinct  ways  in  which  parental  age  may 
affect  the  offspring.  There  are  the  direct  effects  upon  the  foetus, 
which  might  possibly  be  environmental,  and  there  are  those  upon 
the  overall  longevity  of  the  successful  offspring,  which  might  be 
hereditary.  In  the  housefly,  for  example,  the  effect  of  the  advanced 
age  of  the  parent  upon  the  effective  potential  of  the  total  population 
is  deleterious,  primarily  through  fewer  eggs,  of  lesser  viability, 
being  produced  by  older  parents.  On  the  other  hand,  where  we  do 
have  surviving  offspring,  the  longevity  of  the  male  housefly  is 
extended  considerably  (by  about  20  per  cent).  We  may  likewise 
have  tw^o  distinct  effects  in  human  populations :  where  an  offspring 
of  an  older  parent  survives,  i.e.  where  an  old  mother  successfully 
produces  young,  the  young  will  be  longer-lived.  The  mother  in  this 
way  may  be  selecting  (or  producing  a  selective  effect  upon  the 


32  Discussion 

population)  to  the  extent  that  she  is  herself  a  long-lived  individual. 
That  is,  she  may  be  still  fertile  at  the  age  of  40  or  45  and  producing 
a  large  family.  These  offspring  will  then  be  long-lived  by  virtue  of 
this  and  the  net  effect  will  be  not  to  have  an  adverse  effect  upon  the 
successful  offspring.  In  other  cases,  offspring  from  older  parents  will 
be  stillborn  or  malformed  at  birth.  However,  Sonneborn  has 
analysed  the  statistics  for  a  large  population  of  offspring  from  the 
New  York  City  vital  statistics  records.  He  found  that  there  was  a 
stronger  correlation,  in  a  positive  sense,  between  'paternal  age  and  the 
number  of  stillbirths,  than  with  maternal  age.  This  is  usually 
ignored  in  the  statistics,  because  normally  the  age  of  the  mother  is 
known  rather  than  the  age  of  the  father. 

Berg:  Was  the  normal  lifespan  about  45  years  in  1800  ? 

Jalavisto:  Yes. 

Berg:  That  would  be  before  the  onset  of  diseases  of  senescence. 
Causes  of  death  were  chiefly  physical  or  infectious  in  nature. 

Danielli:  There  are  three  distinct  factors  which  might  affect  the 
mortality  of  the  children.  First,  there  may  be  changes  in  the  Mendel- 
ian  characteristics  transmitted  through  the  chromosome  material. 
If  it  were  true  that  there  were  no  parental  effects,  one  would  think 
that  no  type  of  deterioration  of  the  chromosome  genes  was  contri- 
buting to  your  results.  Prof.  Jalavisto.  Second,  there  is  the  possi- 
bility that  you  may  get  cytoplasmic  inheritance  effects  coming  in. 
Then,  of  course,  the  actual  environment  to  which  the  embryo  is 
exposed  is  also  changing  as  the  age  of  the  mother  changes,  but  not  as 
the  age  of  the  father  changes.  So  the  mother  has  two  chances  (or 
three,  if  cytoplasmic  inheritance  effects  are  included)  of  altering  the 
expectation  of  life  of  the  child,  whereas  the  father  has  only  one 
chance. 

Maynard  Smith:  Even  in  organisms  without  placentae,  e.g. 
Drosophila,  there  is  plenty  of  evidence  that  the  age  of  the  female 
laying  an  egg  will  influence  the  probability  that  this  egg  will  hatch. 
In  human  data  one  might  suppose  that  the  age  of  the  mother,  through 
the  cytoplasm  of  the  egg  she  produces,  influences  the  rate  of  still- 
births. There  is  no  difficulty  in  supposing  that  either  egg  cytoplasm 
or  uterine  environment  might  influence  the  survival  during  the  first 
two  years  of  life,  but  they  are  less  likely  to  influence  long-term 
survival. 

Danielli:  To  what  extent  are  more  children  surviving  in  recent 
years  from  older  mothers  than  was  the  case  100  years  ago  ?  If  more 
are  indeed  surviving,  the  shift  to  the  right  of  the  final  curve  in  Dr. 
Benjamin's  death  curves  might  be  due  to  such  an  effect. 

Comfort :  There  are  several  social  factors  which  affect  the  parental 


Discussion  33 

age  at  first  birth,  and  there  must  be  considerable  differences  in  its 
distribution  now,  compared  with  populations  where  parenthood 
was  not  so  optional.  Children  born  to  very  young  mothers  now  are 
often  also  socially  underprivileged  or  illegitimate.  But  in  spite  of 
this,  they  have  a  statistically  better  performance  than  first  children 
born  to  predominantly  prosperous  mothers  between  35  and  40 
(Baird,  D.,  Hytten,  F.  E.,  and  Thomson,  A.  M.  (1958).  J.  Ohstet. 
Gynaec.  Brit.  Ejnp.,  65,  865).  There  is  also  the  point  in  regard  to 
parental  age  effects  that  when  there  are  very  large  disparities  in  age 
between  husband  and  wife,  the  proportion  of  cases  where  the  husband 
is  not  the  father  increases  very  considerably. 

Maynard  Smith:  There  is  another  point  on  this  genetic  question  of 
whether,  if  more  old  mothers  are  having  children  which  survive,  this 
could  have  a  genetic  consequence  on  the  expected  longevity  of  a 
population.  It  would  be  very  dangerous  to  assume  without  evidence 
that  there  will  be  a  positive  correlation  between  the  longevity  of 
parents  and  offspring.  Beeton  and  Pearson  (1901)  found  a  correla- 
tion, but  it  was  very  small  compared  with,  for  example,  the  cor- 
relations for  stature  or  for  other  metrical  characters.  There  are 
genetic  reasons  why  one  might  expect,  for  a  character  closely 
associated  with  fitness,  to  get  small  parent-offspring  correlations. 
Certainly  Dr.  Comfort  and  I  in  independent  work  have  found  low 
parent-offspring  correlations  for  longevity  in  Drosophila.  Yet  Prof. 
Rockstein  has  implied  that  he  gets  quite  a  considerable  correlation 
in  the  housefly,  for  the  male  offspring  only.  We  require  better 
evidence  in  the  human  population  as  to  whether  there  is  or  is  not  a 
high  correlation  between  the  longevity  of  parents  and  their  children. 

Danielli:  We  do  not  really  know  whether  there  are  significantly 
more  children  of  older  parents  surviving,  because  once  there  is  a 
probability  through  advancement  in  social  techniques  that  a  woman 
of  potentially  childbearing  capacity  will  survive  longer,  then  her 
opportunities  for  not  having  children,  so  to  speak,  also  increase  and 
this  effect  may  be  working  in  the  opposite  direction.  So  unless  we 
have  actual  evidence  on  this  point,  we  have  not  even  got  the  raw 
material  to  find  out  whether  there  is  any  genetic  effect  at  all. 

Muhlbock:  In  our  inbred  mice  some  of  these  factors  are  not 
apparent.  We  can  control  the  genetic  constitution  equally  well  in  all 
the  animals.  We  observed  first  that  there  are  more  deaths  in  utero 
with  a  higher  age  and  that  the  litter  size  is  also  much  smaller.  This 
is  not  just  the  effect  of  the  number  of  eggs,  but  it  is  also  a  uterus 
effect.  Maybe  the  hormonal  stimulation  in  the  endometrium  of  the 
uterus  is  not  good  enough  in  old  age.  Then  we  tried  to  find  out 
whether  the  lifespan  of  these  inbred  mice  is  influenced  by  the  age  of 

AGEING — V — 2 


34  Discussion 

the  mother.  We  did  that  with  the  C57  black  strain.  We  considered 
the  Hfespan  only  of  female  animals  living  longer  than  one  year,  i.e. 
half  the  usual  lifetime  of  a  mouse.  The  fertility  age  of  this  strain  is 
up  to  twelve  months.  We  divided  the  offspring  according  to  the  age 
of  the  mother,  first  three  months,  then  four  to  six  months,  and  then 
six  to  twelve  months.  There  was  no  difference  in  the  lifespan  of 
these  offspring.  The  environmental  factors  were  the  same  and  there 
is  no  difference  in  genetic  make-up. 

Berg:  We  have  been  able  to  extend  the  age  of  fertility  considerably 
in  the  rat,  by  regulating  the  food  intake.  For  example,  700-day-old 
female  rats  fed  ad  libitum  have  a  fertility  rate  of  12  per  cent.  On  a 
controlled  food  intake  fertility  is  increased  to  67  per  cent. 

Jalavisto :  One  feature  of  these  family  records  is  that  the  families 
are  very  big.  There  are  great  age  differences  between  the  members 
of  one  family,  although  the  environment  is  fairly  consistent. 

Hinton:  Your  main  maternal  effect  may  be  restricted  to  the  first 
two  years  of  the  life  of  the  offspring  partly  because  the  older  mothers 
have  more  experience.  For  example,  we  have  children  17  years 
apart,  and  we  took  care  of  our  later  children  much  better  than  we 
did  of  the  first  ones. 

Comfort:  Even  so  I  think  there  is  a  higher  early  death  rate  among 
first  babies  born  to  prosperous  mothers  over  40  than  among  those 
of  underprivileged  mothers  of  18.  I  doubt  if  this  is  entirely  a  matter 
of  maternal  experience. 

Jalavisto:  I  have  divided  the  material  into  first  and  second 
children  and  there  is  no  difference.  The  second  and  subsequent 
children  show  the  same  effect  as  the  first  children. 

Danielli:  It  may  just  be  that  the  mothers  do  not  bother  so  much 
with  the  later  children. 

Benjamin:  The  present  discussion  does  seem  to  tie  in  with  what 
Dr.  Berg  said  earlier  about  deaths  of  children  being  due  to  acute 
infections  or  other  endogenous  diseases.  This  factor  of  the  experience 
of  the  mother  helping  her  to  deal  with  infections  would  seem  to  be 
important.  You  would  find  great  difficulty  in  making  such  researches 
in  this  country  now,  because  there  seems  to  be  a  strong  tendency  for 
women  to  have  their  children  very  young  and  very  early  in  married 
life. 

Jalavisto:  In  Finland  we  have  on  record  what  is  probably  the 
highest  maternal  age — a  woman  nearly  60  years  old ! 


STUDIES  ON  THE  LONGEVITY  AND 
MORTALITY  OF  ENGLISH  THOROUGHBRED 

HORSES* 

A.  Comfort 

Department  of  Zoology,  University  College,  London 

The  General  Stud  Book  records  the  year  of  foaling,  and  in 
many  cases  the  year  of  death  or  disposal,  of  the  thoroughbred 
racehorses  foaled  in  Britain  since  the  end  of  the  18th  century. 
This  record  has  been  compiled  with  careful  attention  to 
identity,  and  is  greatly  superior  in  quality  and  quantity  to  the 
other  non-human  mammalian  vital  statistics  so  far  examined. 
The  possibility  of  using  it  as  a  source  of  biological  data  has 
been  recognized  before  (e.g.  Vitt,  1949)  but  never  fully  ex- 
ploited, chiefly,  no  doubt,  because  for  most  purposes  each 
life  history  must  be  individually  extracted,  and  statistical 
treatment  of  a  large  sample  is  therefore  very  slow  work. 

Since  actuarial  figures  for  large  mammals  are  scarce  and 
theoretically  important,  the  Stud  Book  has  been  examined 
to  see  how  far  it  could  be  expected  to  yield  useful  material  for 
comparative  age  studies,  especially  in  relation  to  parental  age 
effects  and  the  inheritance  of  longevity.  Study  of  parental 
age  effects  on  lifespan  in  man  is  complicated  by  the  high  cor- 
relation between  ages  of  spouses  (Sonneborn,  1957);  there  is 
no  such  correlation  between  ages  of  sire  and  dam  in  horse- 
breeding,  and  both  mares  and  stallions  commonly  remain 
at  stud  to  advanced  ages.  One  special  object  of  the  study  was 
to  examine  Vitt's  (1949)  claim  that  the  Stud  Book  records 
indicate  a  large  parental  age  effect  on  the  vigour  and  longevity 

*  The  work  described  in  this  paper  was  carried  out  during  the  tenure  of  a 
Nuffield  Research  Fellowship  in  Gerontology.  Part  of  it  received  a  Ciba 
Foundation  Ageing  Award  in  1958. 

85 


36  A.  Comfort 

of  thoroughbreds.  Another  was  to  see  whether  the  records 
could  be  used  to  determine  the  extent  of  the  parent-offspring 
and  sib-sib  correlations  for  lifespans. 

The  following  account  combines  and  summarizes  the  results 
already  described  elsewhere  (Comfort,  1958a,  b,  1959a). 

Materials  and  Method 

The  Stud  Book  consists  to  date  of  34  volumes,  published  at 
four-yearly  intervals  since  1808.  It  is  essentially  a  nominal 
list  of  brood  mares,  giving  details  of  the  serving,  foaling  and 
ownership  of  each  since  the  last  entry.  Arabian  mares  are 
listed  separately. 

From  this  record  can  be  obtained  (1)  the  year  of  birth  of 
every  thoroughbred  foal  under  the  name  of  its  dam,  (2)  the 
life  history  of  every  filly  which  returns  to  stud  as  a  brood 
mare,  from  her  first  covering  by  a  thoroughbred  stallion  until 
death  or  disposal  from  the  stud. 

The  life  histories  were  extracted  by  following  each  in- 
dividual animal  by  name  from  its  first  appearance  as  a  brood 
mare  until  its  last ;  in  all,  including  those  required  to  establish 
parental  longevity,  and  additional  lives  scored  in  the  course  of 
coat-colour  studies,  about  10,000  histories  were  extracted  in 
this  way.  Initial  samples  taken  were  ( 1 )  all  the  thoroughbred 
fillies  foaled  in  Britain,  excluding  Ireland,  in  the  years  1875-80 
(Sample  A)  and  1860-64  (Sample  B)  which  subsequently  re- 
enter the  record  as  brood  mares,  (2)  all  the  Arabian  mares 
foaled  in  1880  and  the  35  subsequent  years — a  compact  group 
of  manageable  size,  where  over  half  the  fillies  returned  to 
stud.  The  original  six  annual  cohorts  of  thoroughbreds 
(Sample  A)  were  chosen  so  that  their  survival  period  avoided 
the  World  Wars.  After  this  sample  had  been  analysed.  Sample 
B  was  taken  to  obtain  more  data  upon  the  relationship  of 
longevity  to  parental  age.  The  thoroughbred  cohorts  of  1900 
and  1901  were  scored  later,  to  see  whether  secular  changes  had 
occurred  in  the  course  of  the  record. 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      37 

Each  life  history  normally  ends  in  an  entry  that  the  animal 
was  shot,  died,  was  exported,  or  was  disposed  of  (put  out  of 
stud,  sold,  given  away  as  barren).  Some  end  in  unexplained 
disappearance  from  the  record  or  in  "no  further  return". 
Each  unaccounted  absentee  was  sought  by  name  in  each 
volume  to  the  end  of  a  32-year  period  from  its  year  of  foaling, 
and  for  two  volumes  thereafter  to  allow  for  corrections. 

The  forms  used  in  the  terminal  entries  are  "died",  "shot", 
"sold",  "sent  abroad",  and  qualifications  of  these,  e.g.  "broke 
leg  and  shot",  "died  after  foaling"  (the  last  of  these  implies 
only  that  the  mare  had  foaled,  not  that  foaling  was  the  cause 
of  death).  The  entries  therefore  discriminate  between  losses 
to  the  record,  deaths  due  to  accident,  animals  destroyed,  and 
deaths  presumed  to  be  due  to  natural  causes ;  but  of  animals 
entered  as  "died"  only  those  dying  at  or  after  foaling,  and  of 
animals  shot  only  those  which  met  with  an  accident,  are 
usually  distinguished.  Deaths  not  attributed  to  accident  or 
shooting  account  for  1,009  out  of  the  total  2,742  lives  in  the 
cohort  samples.  Only  these,  which  include  deaths  at  or  after 
foaling,  were  scored  as  "natural"  (d^)  in  computing  the  age- 
dependent  mortality,  all  other  losses,  and  all  animals  dying  at 
imprecise  ages,  being  scored  as  lost  to  the  record  (a^).  Where 
the  last  surviving  individual  is  lost  to  the  record,  it  is  assumed 
to  have  died  in  that  year. 

Life-tables  from  4  years  of  age  were  then  prepared  by  cal- 
culating the  yearly  mortality  under  this  convention,  with  the 
usual  correction  for  losses.  Details  of  the  calculation  have 
been  given  elsewhere  (Comfort,  1958a).  Standard  errors  of  the 
expectation  of  life  were  obtained  by  Irwin's  (1949)  approxima- 
tion. 

Results  and  Discussion 

General  form  of  the  survival  curve 

Survival  curves  drawn  for  the  2,742  thoroughbred  mares  in 
samples  A  and  B,  and  the  tabulated  figures  from  which  they 


38 


A.  Comfort 


are  derived,  are  given  in  Fig.  1  and  Table  la  and  b.  The 
curves  for  the  samples  coincide  closely;  they  are  of  typical 
Gompertzian  form,  with  a  high  early  survival  and  a  steadily 
increasing  force  of  mortality. 


The  modal  age  of  adult  death 


Table  la 

Life-table  for  1,492  thoroughbred  mares,  foaled 

1875-80 


Year 
interval 

dx 

«x 

qx 

Lx 

Corrected 

deaths 

(10,000) 

ex 

4- 

4 

11 

0-0027 

10000 

27 

17  043 

5- 

7 

49 

0048 

9973 

48 

16 

04 

6- 

11 

48 

0079 

9925 

78 

15 

05 

7- 

16 

56 

0120 

9847 

118 

14 

06 

8- 

20 

69 

0159 

9729 

155 

13 

08 

9- 

15 

48 

0127 

9574 

122 

12 

12 

10- 

18 

50 

0162 

9452 

153 

11 

17 

11- 

17 

45 

0162 

9299 

151 

10 

23 

12- 

19 

42 

0194 

9148 

178 

9 

31 

13- 

22 

35 

0237 

8970 

213 

8 

40 

14- 

23 

43 

0265 

8757 

233 

7 

51 

15- 

36 

40 

0448 

8524 

382 

6 

65 

16- 

33 

37 

0452 

8142 

369 

5 

82 

17- 

37 

34 

0560 

7773 

436 

5 

02 

18- 

41 

41 

0699 

7337 

513 

4 

•26 

19- 

32 

49 

0639 

6824 

436 

3 

•56 

20- 

31 

52 

0742 

6388 

474 

2 

•90 

21- 

40 

40 

1173 

5914 

694 

2 

•28 

22- 

61 

38 

2328 

5220 

1216 

1 

•72 

23- 

34 

33 

2054 

4004 

822 

1 

•26 

24- 

24 

25 

2342 

3182 

744 

0 

•90 

25- 

12 

24 

2222 

2438 

542 

0 

•62 

26- 

10 

11 

4082 

1896 

774 

— 

27- 

2 

2 

2500 

1122 

280 

— 

28- 

— 

1 

00 

0842 

0 

— 

29- 

1 

1 

2857 

0824 

241 

— 

30- 

— 

2 

(10) 

0601 

601 

— 

31- 

— 

— 

00 

— ■ 

— 

n  = 

566 

926 

10,000 

Ve  =  0-046 
(Je  =  0-214 
Standard  deviation  =  5-5  yrs. 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      39 

is  22  years.  The  proportions  are  very  like  those  of  prosperous 
human  survival  curves;  the  time  equivalence,  obtained  by 
fitting  the  median  and  second  quartile  to  the  U.S.  Census 
curve  for  white  males,  1941,  beginning  at  the  age  of  10,  is 


Table  lb 

Life-table  for  1,250  thoroughbred  mares,  foaled 

1860-64 


Year 
interval 

Corrected 

dx 

Ox 

Ix 

Lx 

deaths 
(10,000) 

ex 

4r- 

1 

00 

1-0000 

17-314 

5- 

3 

15 

0024 

10000 

24 

16 

31 

6- 

4 

30 

0033 

9976 

33 

15 

32 

7- 

11 

55 

0094 

9943 

93 

14 

23 

8- 

16 

48 

0145 

9850 

143 

13- 

33 

9- 

14 

80 

0136 

9707 

122 

12 

35 

10- 

16 

59 

0170 

9585 

163 

11 

41 

11- 

18 

45 

0206 

9422 

194 

10 

44 

12- 

8 

50 

0099 

9228 

91 

9 

50 

13- 

23 

37 

0303 

9137 

277 

8 

57 

14- 

25 

41 

0359 

8860 

318 

7 

69 

15- 

23 

25 

0360 

8542 

308 

6 

82 

16- 

29 

30 

0493 

8234 

406 

5 

98 

17- 

25 

31 

0473 

7828 

370 

5 

•17 

18- 

33 

41 

0706 

7458 

527 

4 

41 

19- 

32 

36 

0808 

6931 

560 

3 

69 

20- 

30 

33 

0910 

6271 

580 

3 

•03 

21- 

33 

38 

1250 

5791 

724 

2 

•42 

22- 

35 

31 

1718 

5067 

902 

1 

•87 

23- 

19 

22 

1407 

4165 

586 

1 

41 

24r- 

22 

24 

2366 

3579 

847 

1 

03 

25- 

15 

18 

3000 

2732 

820 

0 

•71 

26- 

6 

9 

2791 

1912 

534 

— 

27- 

1 

5 

1176 

1378 

162 

— 

28- 

1 

1 

2222 

1216 

270 

— 

29- 

1 

1 

4000 

0946 

378 

— 

30- 

— 

1 

10 

— 

568 

— 

n  — 

443 

807 

10,000 

Ve  =  0  044 

Ge  =  0-214 

Median  =  22-07  yrs. 


40 


A.  Comfort 


roughly  X  3-2.  The  last  part  of  the  curve  calculated  from  q^ 
values  at  ages  of  25  and  over  is,  of  course,  largely  arbitrary, 
and  in  the  late  intervals  losses,  many  of  them  from  age- 
dependent  causes,  equal  or  exceed  deaths. 

The  survival  curves  and  expectations  of  the  separate  cohorts 
are  roughly  similar,  but  there  is  substantial  scatter  (Table  II), 


lOOi- 


SURVIVAL    CURVES    OF   THOROUGHBRED 
MARES  -GENERAL   STUD-BOOK 


AGE    IN     YEARS 


Fig.  1.    Survival  curves  of  mares  foaled  in  1860-64  (- 


-),  in  1875-80 


( ),  of  brood  mares  by  three  selected  long-lived  stallions  (Hampton, 

Galopin  and  Hermit)  ( ),  and  of  grey  mares  ( ). 


the  last  three  years  of  the  1875-80  sample  having  a  shorter 
expectation  of  life  than  any  of  the  others  (0-05  <  P  <  0-02 
for  the  largest  difference).  The  proportion  of  scoreable  lives 
to  total  fillies  foaled  is  rather  higher  in  sample  B  (33-0  against 
26-8  per  cent)  but  there  is  no  immediately  obvious  reason 
for  the  differences  in  cohort  performance.  There  may  be  bias 
in  sampling,  since  the  two  lowest-scoring  cohorts  of  sample  A 
contain  an  unusually  low  proportion  of  late  entrants  to  the 
record.  Mares  are  normally  entered  when  first  covered,  but 
animals  among  the  unaccounted  majority  which  have  not 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      41 

Table  II 
Longevity  of  thoroughbred  mares  by  years  of  birth 


Total 

Number     yeafs 

scored      fillies 


( Years) 


Median 


1860 

242 

726 

33-33 

17-44 

0-21 

22-70 

1861 

255 

763 

33-42 

17-28 

0-18 

21-86 

1862 

244 

757 

32-23 

17-33 

0-35 

21-82 

1863 

241 

783 

30-78 

16-58 

0-21 

21-62 

1864 

268 

756 

35-45 

17-39 

0-17 

22-03 

1860-64 

1250 

3785 

33-03 

17-31 

0-044 

22-07 

1875 

285 

910 

31-32 

17-26 

0-34 

22-43 

1876 

245 

863 

28-39 

17-64 

0-21 

22-43 

1877 

233 

915 

25-46 

17-44 

0-21 

22-49 

1878 

248 

969 

25-59 

17-49 

0-30 

22-15 

1879 

261 

952 

27-42 

16-13 

0-16 

20-81 

1880 

221 

954 

23  17 

16-01 

0-18 

21-81 

1875-80       1492 


5565 


26-82 


17-04 


0-046 


22-18 


1900 
1910 

501 
650 

1674 
1566 

33-41 
41-50 

15-20 
1511 

<0-l 
<0-l 

19-55 
20-65 

Arabians 
1880-1915 

183 

314 

58-26 

18-81 

0-43 

23-66 

Thoroughbred  mares — longevity  and  survival  by 
coat  colour 


Group 


^Z  =  4 


V, 


Median 


Bays  (1875-79) 
Blacks (1854-1900) 
Chestnuts  (1875-79) 
Greys  (1845-1920) 


568 

16-68 

0  08 

22-03 

358 

16-53 

0-15 

21-95 

262 

17-23 

0-20 

22-50 

200 

15-57 

0-28 

20-43 

42  A.  Comfort 

been  regularly  at  stud  may  enter  the  record  at  any  age  if  they 
produce  a  thoroughbred  foal;  some  of  these  have  probably 
been  missed.  But  on  the  basis  of  mortality  rates  calculated 
for  all  the  cohorts  at  4  years  of  age,  omitting  subsequent 
entrants,  it  appears  that  this  bias  is  only  enough  to  account 
for  a  small  part  of  the  difference  observed.  The  1900  and  1910 
cohorts  gave  much  lower  figures  for  mean  and  median  ex- 
pectation of  life  than  either  of  the  earlier  samples.  This 
apparent  fall  in  performance  might  be  influenced  (1)  by  the 
1914-18  war:  the  increased  losses  are  concentrated  early  in  the 
1910  table  and  about  10  years  later  in  the  1900  table,  (2)  by  a 
change  in  breeding  policy — mares  over  23  are  rare  in  recent 
volumes,  being  now  apparently  put  out  of  stud  at  earlier  ages. 
A  secular  trend  in  lifespan  cannot  be  assumed  without  scoring 
further  cohorts,  but  there  is  some  prima  facie  evidence  of  it 
here. 

The  mean  expectation  of  life  of  Arabians  was  significantly 
higher  than  that  for  any  cohort  of  thoroughbreds  {e^^^  = 
18-81  ±0-66  years)  and  the  rate  of  decline  slower.  These 
lives  are  spread  too  thinly  over  too  long  a  period  for  any 
secular  trend  to  be  made  out. 


Maximum  age  records 

The  highest  ages  in  the  series  were  reached  by  Arabians, 
three  mares  reaching  31  years,  and  one  dying  in  its  33rd  year 
(born  1911,  died  1943;  last  covered,  but  barren,  1942);  these 
ages  may  only  indicate  more  conscientious  returns  for  Arabians 
past  breeding  age,  compared  with  thoroughbreds.  The  two 
oldest  thoroughbred  mares  in  the  sample  were  alive  at  30 
years.  The  Stud  Book  has  not  been  searched  in  detail  for 
higher  records — the  oldest  mare  so  far  encountered  (Blue  Bell, 
by  Heron  out  of  Jessie)  was  foaled  in  1851  and  died  in  1885  at 
the  age  of  34.  Pocahontas  (Stockwell's,  Rataplan's  and  King 
Tom's  dam)  was  foaled  in  1837,  died  in  1870,  and  bore  her 
last  foal  in  1862.    The  frequency  with  which  such  ages  are 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      43 

actually  reached  by  thoroughbred  mares  is  largely  determined 
by  human  intervention,  since  many  which  disappeared  from 
the  record  at  ages  of  25  or  over  were  probably  capable  of  living 
longer — some  no  doubt  did  so,  dying  unrecorded.  The  stallion 
Matchem  (1749-1781)  reached  a  reputed  age  of  33;  in  the 
obituary  lists  of  the  Stud  Book  one  other  stallion  reached  32, 
and  four  reached  31. 

These  ages  agree  with  maximum  authenticated  records  in 
other  breeds  (Hokkaido  ponies  32 -f,  Matsumoto,  1935; 
Hafling  mares,  over  32,  Schotterer,  1939;  Lipitsa  horses 
(J31,  533,  Kadic,  1949).  Claims  of  higher  ages  have  been 
reviewed  elsewhere  (Comfort,  1956).  Many  of  these  refer  to 
ponies,  and  none  is  supported  by  Stud  Book  records.  Thirty- 
eight  years  is  recorded  in  a  captive  zebra  (Weber,  1942). 

Effects  of  parental  age  on  the  longevity  of  progeny 

Vitt  (1949)  has  claimed  that  the  longevity  and  racing  per- 
formance of  thoroughbred  horses  are  substantially  influenced 
by  the  age  of  both  dam  and  sire,  and  that  impairment  of 
vigour  by  the  use  of  old  breeding  stock  is  cumulative.  He 
found  that  in  a  sample  of  100  mares  from  the  early  years  of 
the  General  Stud  Book,  the  progeny  of  dams  twelve  years  old 
or  less  developed  more  slowly,  judged  by  the  age  at  first 
foaling,  and  lived  longer  {6^=4^  =19-5  years)  than  the  progeny 
of  dams  aged  13  or  more  {e^^4^  =  16-4  years).  Absolute 
figures  and  standard  errors  are  not  given,  and  it  is  not  clear 
whether  the  estimates  are  corrected  for  losses  or  based  on  the 
distribution  of  recorded  deaths  alone.  Vitt  also  compared  the 
fertility  and  racing  form  of  foals  by  old  and  young  stallions, 
and  concluded  that  there  was  an  equally  marked  paternal  age 
effect,  the  optimal  performance  being  reached  by  the  foals  of 
stallions  8-16  years  old  out  of  mares  6-13  years  old. 

To  test  this  the  lives  in  sample  A  were  distributed  (a)  by  age 
of  dam  at  foaling,  (b)  by  age  of  sire  at  covering,  one  year 
earlier,  (c)  by  age  of  dam  at  foaling  and  sire  at  covering,  where 


44 


A.  Comfort 


these  fell  in  the  same  grouping  interval.  Of  1,492  lives,  1,342 
were  scored  and  grouped  by  age  of  dam,  1,355  by  age  of  sire, 
and  719  by  both,  the  missing  lives  among  these  being  scored 
for  one  parent  only — chiefly  the  progeny  of  imported  horses, 
of  stallions  whose  dates  of  birth  were  not  easily  ascertainable 
from  the  record,  or  of  mares  covered  by  more  than  one  stallion 
in  the  season.  The  distribution  of  parental  ages  is  shown  in 
Fig.  2,  and  the  results  of  the  calculation  in  Table  III. 


DISTRIBUTION  OF  PARENTAL  AGES 
THOROUGHBRED  MARES    1875-1880 


DAM    • 


SIRE    O 


AGE:YRS  2 


Fig.  2.    Mares  foaled  in  1875-80:  distribution  of  ages   of  sires   at 

covering  (O)  and  dams  at  foaling  (O)  (Comfort,  1958a). 

Reproduced  by  courtesy  of  the  Editor,  Journal  of  Gerontology. 

There  was  no  significant  difference  in  expectation  of  life 
between  foals  of  mares  under  and  over  13  years  of  age  (  ^  12, 
e^^^  =  16-89;  ^  13,  e^^^  =  16-86  years).  With  further  sub- 
division the  progeny  of  the  oldest  mares  had  the  shortest 
lifespans,  but  the  largest  difference  was  less  than  twice  its 
standard  error.  Still  smaller  differences  were  obtained  for  the 
same  lives  grouped  by  paternal  age  alone.  Of  the  719  lives 
grouped  by  age  of  both  parents,  those  whose  dam  and  sire 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      45 


Table  III 

Longevity  of  thoroughbred  mares  by  parental  ages 
(sire  at  covering,  dam  at  foaling) 


n 


€z  =  i 


( Years) 
Ve  Median 


1492 

17  04 

0-046 

22-18 

1250 

17-31 

0-044 

22  07 

Whole  sample 

Foaled  1875-80  (A) 
„      1860-64  (B) 


Mares  foaled  1875-80  {Sample  A)  and  1860-64  {Sample  B) 
Dam  (Sample  A) : 

under  8  yrs. 
8-12 
13-16 
^  17 


Sire  (Sample  A) : 

under  8  yrs. 

8-12 

13-16 

17-19 

^  20 

Dam  and  sire : 

^  12*  Sample: A 

B 

A  +  B 

^  13*  A 

B 


<  9 
^  16 


A  +  B 
A 
B 
A 
B 


Progeny  of  Hermit,  Galopin  and  Hampton 

All  mares : 

Got  in  or  after  sire's  20th 

year : 

Got  in  or  after  sire's  16th 

year,  dam  ^  16  at  foaling : 

All  mares  {A,  B  and  selected  sires) 
by  parents  $5 16  yrs  154 


297 

16-90 

0-12 

21-07 

537 

16-66 

0  09 

21-06 

303 

17-29 

0-17 

22-21 

248 

16-15 

0-23 

22-83 

250 

16-73 

0-16 

22-10 

537 

1711 

0-10 

22-35 

352 

17-26 

0-20 

21-83 

151 

16-87 

0-28 

22-08 

65 

16-87 

0-73 

21-03 

449 

17-39 

0-22 

22-28 

531 

17-07 

0-11 

21-90 

980 

17-33 

0-07 

22-10 

270 

16-45 

0-23 

21-61 

150 

17-24 

0-28 

22-64 

420 

17-03 

0-16 

22-05 

128 

17-91 

0-22 

22-28 

220 

16-51 

0-27 

21-50 

70 

15-71 

0-69 

20-41 

44 

17-26 

1-10 

23-17 

ampton 

412 

16-60 

015 

21-73 

124 

16-65 

0-33 

21-83 

41 

16-29 

0-89 

2119 

16-45 

*  Include  extreme  groups  ( <9,  >  16). 


0-38 


2112 


46  A.  Comfort 

were  under  13  years  old  lived  slightly  longer  (17-39  ±0-36) 
than  those  whose  parents  were  over  13  (16-45  ±  0*48;  t  ^ 
1-4,  0-2  >  P  >  0-1),  and  the  difference  was  greater  in  the 
extreme  segments  of  these  groups  (dam  and  sire  ^  9, 17  •  91  ± 
0-47;  ^  16,  15-71  iO-83;  t  ^  2-3,  002>P  >  0-01).  This 
difference  is  much  smaller  than  that  described  by  Vitt  from 
maternal  age  alone,  and  is  of  the  order  of  the  difference 
between  cohorts. 

In  view  of  this  result,  the  five  additional  cohorts  (sample  B) 
were  extracted  and  scored  for  parental  age,  with  the  results 
shown  in  Table  III.  The  differences  found  in  the  1875-80 
sample  were  not  repeated  here.  The  longest-lived  group  were 
the  progeny  of  parents  of  16  years  and  over,  but  the  standard 
error  was  very  large  (17-26  i  1-90);  the  220  animals  which 
were  the  progeny  of  two  young  parents  had  numerically  the 
shortest  lifespans  (16-51  i  0  •  52) ;  none  of  the  differences  was 
significant,  and  all  were  in  the  reverse  direction  to  those  in  the 
1875-80  sample. 

The  mean  expectations  of  life  were  also  calculated  for  the 
foals  sired  early  and  late  in  life  by  three  selected  stallions: 
Hermit,  by  Newminster  (1864-1890);  Galopin,  by  Vedette 
(1872-1899);  and  Hampton,  by  Lord  Clifden  (1872-1897),  for 
comparison  with  Vitt's  analysis  of  the  progeny  of  Swynford. 
These  three  stallions  produced  in  their  lifetime  141,  119  and 
159  fillies  which  returned  to  stud.  Forty-six,  13  and  six  of 
these  came  from  the  cohorts  already  scored,  the  remaining  347 
being  new  lives.  The  combined  curve  of  survival  for  all 
Hermit,  Galopin  and  Hampton  mares  coincided  closely  with 
that  for  the  original  six  cohorts ;  their  mean  expectation  of  life 
at  4  years  was  16-50  ±0-39  years.  The  121  mares  got  during 
or  after  their  sire's  20th  year  had  a  slightly,  but  not  a  signi- 
ficantly, higher  expectation  than  the  global  mean  (16-65  db 
0-57)  (Table  III;  Fig.  3).  Only  41  mares  were  got  by  the 
three  selected  stallions  in  or  after  their  16th  year  upon  dams 
16  years  old  or  more;  these  had  a  mean  expectation  of  life  of 
16-29  ±0-94  years.    By  combining  these  mares  with  the 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      47 

progeny  in  samples  A  and  B  of  parents  16  years  old  or  over, 
154  lives  were  obtained,  with  e^^^  =  16-45  ±0-62  years, 
which  is  less  than  any  of  the  three  global  means,  but  not  sig- 
nificantly so.  These  results,  taken  as  a  whole,  seem  to  afford 
no  good  evidence  of  any  consistent  effect  of  parental  age  on 
the  longevity  of  mares. 


lOO 


9    PROGENY  OF  STALLIONS     HERMIT. 
GALORN     &  HAMPTON 

ALL  (412) 


0  0  0     SIRE$20  YRS 
(121) 


Fig.  3.    Survival  curves  of  lifetime  brood  mare  progeny  of  Hampton, 

Galopin  and  Hermit  (O O)  and  of  mares  got  in  or  after  their  sire's 

20th  year  (Comfort,  1958a). 
Reproduced  by  courtesy  of  the  Editor,  Journal  of  Gerontology. 

Correlation  betw^een  lifespans  of  parents  and  offspring 

Since  age  of  death  depends  in  part  upon  heritable  factors 
there  should  be  a  measurable  difference  in  longevity  between 
the  foals  of  long-lived  and  short-lived  parents,  though  Beeton 
and  Pearson's  (1901)  results  in  man  suggest  that  it  would  not 
be  large.  All  the  mares  in  sample  A  were  scored  for  the  longev- 
ity of  their  dam,  and  as  many  as  possible  for  the  longevity  of 
their  sire;  the  date  of  death  could  be  ascertained  only  for 
stallions  appearing  in  the  obituary  lists,  or  rather  less  than  half 


48  A.  Comfort 

the  sires  contributing  to  the  sample.  Life-tables  were  made  (a) 
for  all  the  mares  in  sample  A  whose  dams  were  known  to  have 
reached  the  age  of  25,  or  died  before  the  age  of  14,  (b)  for  mares 
in  sample  A  whose  sires  reached  25  or  died  before  15,  (c)  for  the 
female  progeny  of  those  mares  in  sample  A  which  reached  25 
or  died  before  14,  (d)  for  113  mares  in  the  sample  whose  dam 
and  sire  both  reached  23  years.    The  grouping  limits  in  all 

Table  IV 

Longevity  of  thoroughbred  mares  by  longevity  of  parents 


( Years) 
n  ex=t  Ve  Median 


Mares  foaled  in  1875-80 


Sire  reached  25  yrs.*  132  17-54  0-50  22-63 

Sire  died  ^  14  113  16-27  0-60  22-53 

Dam  reached  25  yrs.  238  16-96  0-16  22-25 

Dam  died  ^  13  53  16-35  0-59  21-18 

Dam  and  sire  reached  23  yrs.  113  18-07  0-34  23-74 

Progeny  of  mares  foaled  in  1875-80 

Dam  reached  25  yrs.  168  16-33  0-31  22-54 

Dam  died  ^  13  58  16-67  1-25  20-02 

Progeny  of  Hermit,  Galopin  and  Hampton 

Dam  reached  24  yrs.  100  17-11  0-82  21-52 

Dam  died  ^  18  yrs.  58  15-39  0-83  21-51 

•  Stallions  whose  date  of  death  appears  in  the  obituary  lists ;  mares  by  Hermit,  Galopin  and 
Hampton  are  excluded  from  this  figure,  but  included  in  the  figure  for  dam  and  sire  >  23  years. 

these  cases  were  fixed  to  secure  enough  lives  for  the  calcula- 
tion; the  relative  contribution  of  short-lived  mares  to  the 
sample  was  so  small  that  it  was  not  possible  to  prepare  a 
table  for  the  survival  of  their  fillies  by  short-lived  stallions. 
The  mares  by  the  three  long-lived  stallions,  Hermit,  Galopin 
and  Hampton  were  also  grouped  by  longevity  of  dam. 

The  calculated  means  (Table  IV)  show  differences  of  less 
than  twice  the  standard  error  in  favour  of  all  the  groups  with 
one  long-lived  parent,  except  the  progeny  of  long-lived  mares 
in  the  1875-80  sample;  the  113  mares  with  two  long-lived 
parents  had  a  mean  expectation  of  life  at  4  years  of  18-07  i 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      49 

0  •  58  years,  which  is  significantly  more  than  the  global  mean, 
or  the  mean  for  any  other  group.  The  true  difference  is  more- 
over likely  to  be  minimized,  since  nearly  half  the  dams  and 
more  than  half  the  sires  contributing  to  the  global  total  died 
at  unkno^vTi  ages  or  from  accidental  causes,  and  these  losses 
must  include  some  potentially  or  actually  long-lived  pairs. 
The  difference  in  lifespan  of  fillies  by  the  three  selected 
stallions  out  of  long-  and  short-lived  dams  was  about  1  •  3 
times  its  standard  error,  but  the  short-lived  mothers  contri- 
buted only  53  lives,  even  when  the  grouping  limit  was  raised 
to  18  years,  and  the  comparison  means  little. 

In  view  of  Vitt's  opinions,  a  table  of  the  early  progeny  of 
long-lived  parents  was  also  made,  taking  all  the  available 
mares  from  all  the  scored  samples  w^hose  dam  and  sire  were 
aged  15  or  less  at  the  time  of  foaling  or  conception,  but  lived 
eventually  to  an  age  of  23  or  more,  thus  avoiding  competition 
between  any  age  effect  and  inheritance  of  longevity.  The 
performance  of  these  mares  was  in  fact  numerically  but  not 
significantly  poorer  than  that  of  the  groups  scored  without 
regard  to  parental  age  (e^^^  =  17-09  ±0-89  years). 

Goat  colour 

Five  cohorts  (1875 — 79)  were  scored  for  coat  colour — of 
1,271  mares  composing  them,  588  were  bays,  181  browns,  262 
chestnuts,  27  blacks,  5  greys,  3  roans  and  2  grey-roans,  the 
balance  being  of  doubtful  or  unstated  colour.  In  order  to 
compare  the  less  common  coat  colours,  further  records  of 
greys  and  blacks  were  collected  from  other  volumes  of  the 
Stud  Book.  Brown  mares  were  excluded  because  of  the 
heterogeneity  of  coat  colours  included  under  this  description; 
so  were  all  individuals  of  doubtful  colour,  e.g.  "  black  or  grey", 
at  first  registration. 

Of  the  colours  examined  (Table  II),  only  greys  appear  to 
diverge  significantly  from  the  means  calculated  for  all  mares 
(P  <  0-01)   (Fig.    1).     There   was   no   significant   difference 


50  A.  Comfort 

between  the  longevity  of  greys  in  the  early  and  late  years  of 
the  sample.  Large  factors  of  selection  may  well  operate — at 
many  periods  grey  horses  appear  to  have  been  selectively 
exported  and  they  show  a  high  proportion  of  losses  to  the 
record.  Most  of  the  apparent  reduction  in  their  expectation  of 
life  is  due  to  early  deaths,  the  expectations  at  10  and  15  years 
being  10-96  and  3-85  years  in  greys,  as  against  11-17  and 
2  •  93  years  in  the  whole  of  sample  A.  Causes  of  death  are  not 
given,  and  there  is  consequently  no  information  about  the 
incidence  of  melanomas,  to  which  grey  horses  are  sometimes 
subject  (McFadyean,  1933). 

Longevity  of  stallions 

The  Stud  Book  does  not  contain  records  of  stallions  com- 
parable to  those  of  mares.  Three  different  estimates  of 
thoroughbred  stallion  longevity  have  been  obtained,  with  the 
help  of  other  records — two  are  based  (unlike  the  mare  studies) 
on  cross-sectional  samples,  and  the  third  is  a  longitudinal 
study  of  the  earliest  age  group  for  which  a  list  of  names  could 
be  had.  All  three  leave  a  good  deal  to  be  desired,  but  they 
give  some  provisional  indications  of  the  rate  at  which  the 
expectation  of  life  declines  with  age  in  entire  males. 

Cross-sectional  samples  were  taken  of  (a)  all  the  animals 
listed  in  volumes  1  and  2  (1910  and  1913)  and  (b)  all  the 
animals  listed  in  volume  5  (1921)  of  the  Register  of  Thorough- 
bred Stallions  (excluding  the  appendix).  Thirty-three  animals 
from  sample  (a)  were  still  alive  in  1921  and  figure  in  both 
samples.  The  cohort  sample  was  obtained  by  taking,  from  the 
lists  of  sires  of  brood  mares  in  volumes  17  and  18  of  the 
General  Stud  Book,  all  the  stalhons  (180)  foaled  in  1880-84 
inclusive.  Life-tables  were  prepared  by  calculating  age  speci- 
fic death  rates  from  death  and  disposal  records  in  the  obituary 
and  export  lists  of  the  Register,  the  General  Stud  Book,  and 
the  lists  of  premium  stallions  of  the  National  Hunters  and 
Light  Horse  Society. 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      51 

Table  V  gives  the  full  life-table  for  the  two  cross-sectional 
samples  (correcting  an  error  in  the  L^  column  of  the  1921 
sample  as  originally  published — Comfort,  1959a);  and  Table 


Table  V 

Abbreviated  life-tables  for  thoroughbred  stallions 


Listed  1910-1913 

Listed  1921 

Age 

n 

Qx 

Lx 

ex       oe 

n 

qx 

Lx 

ex        oe 

4 

9       0 

0         1 

0000 

17-71  ±0-84 

2 

00          1 

0000  19  - 

502  ±0-75 

5 

38 

0526  1 

0000 

12 

0 

0         1 

0000 

6 

73       0 

0 

9474 

30 

0 

0         1 

0000 

7 

126-5 

0079 

9474 

38-5 

0260  1 

0000 

8 

162       0 

0 

9399 

56 

0357 

9740 

9 

195-5 

0102 

9399 

63 

0 

0 

9392 

10 

220 

0227 

9303 

12-87±0-58 

81-5 

0 

0 

9392  15 

-6     ±0-53 

11 

233-5 

0385 

9092 

93 

0 

0 

9392 

12 

240 

0208 

8742 

111 

0 

0180 

9392 

13 

244-5 

0123 

8560 

112 

0268 

9223 

14 

237-5 

0337 

8455 

110-5 

0090 

8976 

15 

217-5 

0276 

8170 

9-31  ±0-59 

115-5 

0087 

8895  10 

-13  ±0-50 

16 

201 

0199 

7945 

110-5 

0 

0 

8817 

17 

190-5 

0210 

7787 

104 

0288 

8817 

18 

176-5 

0453 

7623 

97 

0619 

8563 

19 

159-5 

0376 

7278 

89 

0562 

8033 

20 

146-5 

0956 

7004 

5 -41  ±0-64 

74-5 

0402 

7582     6 

-28  ±0-47 

21 

122-5 

1061 

6334 

58-5 

1026 

7277 

22 

100 

1100 

5662 

44 

0682 

6530 

23 

71-5 

1399 

5039 

35-5 

0282 

6085 

24 

57 

1404 

4334 

28-5 

1404 

5913 

25 

43 

2093 

3726 

3-01±l-ll 

18-5 

2162 

5083     3 

-05  ±0-52 

26 

27 

2222 

2946 

10 

1000 

3984 

27 

16-5 

2424 

2291 

8 

2500 

3586 

28 

11 

3636 

1736 

6 

3333 

2690 

29 

4 

2500 

1105 

3 

0 

0 

1793 

30 

2       0 

0 

0829 

2 

1 

0 

1793 

31 

1        1 

0 

0415 

0 

0 

VI  gives  the  mean  further  expectation  of  life  at  4,  10,  15,  20 
and  25  years  for  all  three  samples  of  stallions,  compared  with 
the  two  main  samples  of  mares.  There  is  no  significant  dif- 
ference at  any  age  between  the  expectation  of  mares  and  of  the 


52 


A.  Comfort 


stallions  in  the  cohort  sample;  the  two  cross-sections  differ 
significantly  from  one  another,  from  the  mares,  and  from  the 
cohort. 


Table  VI 

Expectation  of  further  life  (years)  for  thoroughbred 

MARES    AND    STALLIONS    AT    DIFFERENT    AGES.     (MeANS    AND 
STANDARD    ERRORS.) 


Stallions 

Mares 

Age 

Listed 
1910  and  1913 

Listed 
1921 

Foaled 
1880-84 

Foaled               Foaled 
1860-64              1875-80 

4 
10 
15 
20 
25 

17-71  (0-84) 

12-87  (0-58) 

9-31  (0-59) 

5-41  (0-64) 

3-01  (1-11) 

19-50  (0-75) 

15-60  (0-53) 

10-13  (0-50) 

6-28  (0-47) 

3-05  (0-52) 

17-29  (0-72) 

11-95  (0-73) 

8-08  (0-69) 

5  01 (0-67) 

2-83  (0-43) 

17-04  (0-21)   17-31  (0-21) 

11-82  (0-21)  11-90  (0-20) 

7-80(0-20)     7-98(0-20) 

4-54(0-22)     4-76(0-21) 

2-54(0-45)     2-60(0-35) 

We  can  take  our  choice  among  these  findings.  The  cohort 
sample  is  closest  in  date  and  method  of  treatment  to  the 
samples  of  mares,  but  it  is  small,  and  depends  on  only  38 
conventionally  "natural"  deaths,  none  of  them  under  8  years 
of  age,  while  of  the  two  cross-sections,  that  for  1910-13  is 
probably  the  better,  on  grounds  of  size  and  absence  of  intervals 
in  the  middle  of  the  table  where  q^  =  0. 

Over  most  of  the  lifespan  the  plot  of  log  qjt  for  both  samples 
of  mares  is  a  presentable  straight  line  with  a  doubling  time  of 
3|-4  years  (see  Sacher,  this  colloquium,  p.  115).  The  present 
data  are  too  poor  for  inference  about  its  shape  in  stallions. 
Most  of  the  apparent  gain  in  male  survival  occurs  over  the 
years  when  mares  may  die  of  causes  connected  with  foaling. 
The  only  valid  conclusion  from  the  figures  is  that  contrary  to 
the  impression  given  by  the  uncorrected  table  (Comfort, 
19596),  stallions  are  not  shorter-lived  than  mares  under  these 
conditions  of  performance. 


Longevity  of  English  Thoroughbred  Horses      53 

Conclusions 

The  main  use  of  the  study  has  been  in  providing  a  survival 
curve  for  a  large  mammal,  sufficiently  detailed  to  be  used  in 
criticizing  hypotheses  about  lifespan  determinants,  for  which 
maximum  age-records  are  unsuitable.  More  such  curves  are 
badly  needed.  By  the  criteria  needed  in  dealing  with  a  matter 
as  theoretically  important  as  the  supposed  paternal  age  effect 
the  study  was  too  small  and  the  chance  of  systematic  biases, 
especially  in  losses,  too  large  for  convincing  subdivision  by 
parental  ages,  though  the  data  were  better  than  those  on 
which  such  effects  have  sometimes  been  claimed.  The 
literature  of  mammalian  parental  effects  on  longevity  is  con- 
tradictory, and  has  been  reviewed  elsewhere  (Miner,  1954; 
Comfort,  1956).  Yerushalmy  (1939)  found  an  increased  still- 
birth rate  in  babies  with  very  old  or  very  young  fathers,  but 
the  difficulty  arising  from  correlation  between  ages  of  spouses 
(Sonneborn,  1957)  affects  the  value  of  such  data.  Our  study 
was  confined  to  parental  age  effects  on  longevity;  paternal 
age  has  been  held  to  affect  other  characters  of  stock  perform- 
ance, particularly  by  Russian  breeders  (e.g.  Zamyatin  et  at., 
1946;  Isupov,  1949;  Ponomareva  and  Spitskaya,  1953; 
Pospelov,  1952;  Eidrigevits  and  Polyakov,  1953;  Barton,  1951 ; 
Frankland,  1955).  Man  and  the  horse,  since  they  continue  to 
breed  into  old  age,  are  clearly  the  mammals  in  which  such  an 
effect  on  longevity  is  most  likely  to  be  demonstrable;  our 
figures  do  not  bear  out  the  suggestion  that  old  stallions  have 
short-lived  offspring,  but  it  might  still  be  desirable  to  examine 
the  stillbirth  rate  in  the  mares  which  they  covered.  This, 
unfortunately,  could  not  be  done  from  stud-book  records. 

The  small  but  positive  correlation  of  filial  with  parental 
longevity  is  in  accord  with  Beeton  and  Pearson's  (1901)  work, 
and  with  Haldane's  (1949)  interpretation  of  it — if  heterozygos- 
ity is  an  important  correlate  of  vigour,  the  sib-sib  correlation 
of  lifespan  should  be  larger  than  that  between  generations. 
Perhaps  the  most  striking  feature  of  the  study  is  the  smallness 


54  A.  Comfort 

of  the  contribution,  under  these  breeding  conditions,  made  by 
short-hved  parents  to  the  next  generation.  There  is,  effect- 
ively, intensive  spontaneous  selection  for  longevity,  which  is 
not  a  deliberately-sought  character  in  racehorses.  Murie's 
work  on  sheep  (1944)  suggests  that  for  some  large  mammals 
the  wild  adult  survival  curve  is  not  very  different  from  that 
in  domestication.  If  this  were  true  of  wild  horse  populations, 
longevity  of  the  dam,  and  probably,  in  the  presence  of  com- 
petition between  males,  longevity  of  the  stallion,  would  have 
a  large  selective  advantage;  the  upper  limit  of  the  lifespan 
would  have  in  this  case  to  be  fixed  by  something  other  than 
decline  of  selection  pressure. 

REFERENCES 

Barton,  R.  A.  (1951).   Nature  (Lond.),  168,  37. 

Beeton,  M.,  and  Pearson,  K.  (1901).   Biometrika,  1,  50. 

Comfort,  A.  (1956).   The  Biology  of  Senescence.   London:  Routledge. 

Comfort,  A.  (1958a).  J.  GeronL,  13,  342. 

Comfort,  A.  (19585).   Nature  (Lond.),  182,  1531. 

Comfort,  A.  (1959a).  J.  Geront.,  14,  9. 

Comfort,  A.  (19596).   IV  Int.  Congr.  Geront.,  1,  133. 

EiDRiGEViTS,  E.  v.,  and  Polyakov,  E.  V.  (1953).  J.  gen.  Biol.,  Moscow, 

14,  435. 
Frankland,  H.  M.  T.  (1955).  J.  agric.  Sci.,  46,  180. 
Haldane,  J.  B.  S.  (1949).  Ann.  hum.  Genet.,  14,  288. 
Irwin,  J.  O.  (1949).  J.  Hyg.  {Lond.),  47,  188. 
Isupov,  A.  P.  (1949).   Horsehreeding,  Mosk.,  No.  4,  29. 
KADid,  M.  (1949).   Jugosl.  vet.  Glasn.,  2,  602. 
McFadyean,  J.  (1933).  J.  comp.  Path.,  46,  186. 
Matsumoto,  K.  (1935).   Z.  Gestutk.  Pferdez.,  30,  127,  165. 
Miner,  R.  W.  (ed.).  (1954).   Ann.  N.Y.  Acad.  Sci.,  57,  451. 
MuRiE,  A.  (1944).    The  Wolves  of  Mt.  McKinley.    Washington:  U.S. 

Dept.  Int.  Nat.  Parks  Service. 
Ponomareva,  L.  I.,  and  Spitskaya,  T.  D.  (1953).  Horsehreeding,  Mosk., 

No.  23,  28. 
PosPELOV,  S.  P.  (1952).  J.  gen.  Biol,  Moscow,  13,  445. 
ScHOTTERER,  A.  (1939).   Dtsch.  landw.  Tierz.,  43,  228. 
Sonneborn,   T.   M.    (1957).     Proc.    Ageing    Conference,    Gatlinburg. 

Washington,  D.C. :  A.I.B.S.,  in  press. 
ViTT,  V.  O.  (1949).  J.  gen.  Biol.,  Moscow,  10,  161. 
Weber,  R.  (1942).   Zool.  Gart.  (Dusseldorf),  14,  208. 
Yerushalmy,  J.  (1939).   Hum.  Biol.,  11,  342. 
Zamyatin,  N.,  Stolbova,  A.,  Chugaeva,  M.,  and  Kuznetsova,  G. 

(1946).   Trud.  Novosibirsk  selskh.  Inst.,  7,  107. 


Discussion  55 


DISCUSSION 

Rockstein:  Perhaps  the  small  but  significant  difference  obtained 
for  longer-lived  animals  from  the  older  parents  might  have  resulted 
because  you  started  with  a  fairly  long-lived  strain,  and  you  were 
dealing  with  inbred  animals. 

Comfort:  This  may  be  so;  it  has  been  said  of  at  least  one  other 
strain  of  horses  that  the  longevity  depends  on  the  proportion  of 
English  thoroughbred  blood  they  have. 

Rockstein:  You  said  that  among  thoroughbreds  there  is  a  tendency 
to  breed  for  longevity  as  well  as  for  racing  ability. 

Comfort:  This  is  not  deliberate.  It  happens  because  short-lived 
animals  contribute  surprisingly  little  progeny  to  the  total.  Mares  are 
not  usually  covered  until  they  are  taken  out  of  training.  As  you 
know,  horses  in  flat  racing  very  rarely  run  over  the  age  of  four, 
whereas  horses  which  are  raced  under  National  Hunt  rules  may  go 
on  being  ridden  up  to  quite  high  ages.  Most  of  these  animals  here, 
if  they  had  been  raced,  would  have  been  raced  before  they  were  used 
as  brood  mares.  The  successful  ones  would  then  have  been  valued  as 
brood  mares;  likewise  the  stallions — as  soon  as  a  stallion  has  made 
its  name  as  a  promising  racehorse  its  value  goes  up  enormously  and 
it  will  be  used  to  sire  just  as  many  foals  as  can  be  got  out  of  it  during 
the  rest  of  its  life. 

Maynard  Smith:  I  should  like  to  make  a  few  comments  about  the 
genetics  of  longevity.  The  consideration  that  was  at  the  back  of  my 
mind  in  suggesting  earlier  that  there  might  not  be  very  much  cor- 
relation between  parents  and  offspring  was  as  follows :  if  a  character 
has  been  influenced  by  natural  selection  for  a  long  time — if  there 
has  been  natural  selection  tending  to  move  it  in  one  direction — then 
most  of  the  genetic  variability  that  is  left  will  not  be  additive  in  the 
genetic  sense,  and  will  not  give  a  positive  correlation  between  father 
and  child  or  mother  and  child,  though  it  will,  of  course,  do  so  between 
brother  and  sister.  Resemblances  of  the  kind  that  Prof.  Jalavisto 
showed  are  very  similar  to  the  ones  which  I  shall  show  you  later  on  in 
Drosophila;  that  is,  there  are  resemblances  between  parents  and 
ofl'spring  of  the  same  sex,  but  not  of  different  sexes.  This  pattern  is 
what  one  would  call  sex-limited,  and  would  be  expected  if  the  causes 
of  death  were  to  some  extent  different  in  the  two  sexes.  On  the 
other  hand,  if  you  get  a  resemblance  between  mother  and  son,  and 
father  and  daughter,  as  you  might  in  some  cases,  then  this  is  what 
would  be  expected  in  sex-linked  inheritance. 


56  Discussion 

Comfort:  I  do  not  think  one  can  get  much  along  those  Hnes  out  of 
my  data.  The  differences  for  the  two  sexes  scored  separately  was 
very  small.  The  subjects  were  all  fillies,  but  you  have  to  score  both 
parents  to  get  a  significant  difference. 

Hartwig:  Have  you  also  studied  the  influence  of  parental  age  on 
the  fertility  of  the  offspring  ? 

Comfort:  No.  I  could  probably  do  so  now,  by  going  through  the 
data  again.  But  it  would  mean  following  each  life  from  start  to  finjsh 
and  counting  the  number  of  foals.  In  many  instances  there  is  some 
doubt  whether  the  animal  missed  or  whether  it  miscarried.  The 
Stud  Book  usually  distinguishes  cases  where  the  foal  was  born  dead, 
or  where  there  was  a  miscarriage,  from  those  where  the  mare  failed 
to  conceive ;  but  one  would  have  to  be  sure  of  differentiating  between 
unsuccessful  pregnancies,  and  pregnancies  which  did  not  take  place 
at  all.  The  other  trouble  is  that  as  the  animals  are  not  crossed 
twice  with  the  same  stallion  in  succession,  one  would  have  to  allow  for 
the  fertility  of  the  stallion,  which  varies  a  great  deal.  In  these 
thoroughbreds  there  is  a  surprisingly  high  rate  of  infertility. 

Kershaw:  You  showed  a  death  curve  [not  printed]  which  starts 
with  a  slow  rise  on  the  left.  The  figures  which  we  had  on  industrial 
horses,  that  is  draught  horses  and  police  horses,  show  that  while  the 
general  survival  curve  is  the  same,  the  death  curve  may  have  its 
slow  fall  on  the  right  (Chalmers,  T.  A.,  Kershaw,  W.  E.,  and  King, 
J.  O.  L.  (1956).  Nature  {Lond.),178y4<8).  I  assume  that  the  arbitrary 
end-point  in  the  racehorses  is  in  part  economic;  in  those  figures  of 
ours  for  the  draught  horses  and  the  police  horses  it  was  certainly 
economic.  This  suggests  that  the  curve  of  death  is  arbitrarily 
determined  by  the  index  that  one  uses.  We  had  assumed  that  the 
working  life  had  some  relation  to  natural  longevity.  It  does  seem 
now,  in  animals  for  which  one  can  get  the  same  data  for  different 
indices,  that  the  different  indices  may  produce  curves  which  are 
made  up  differently. 

Comfort:  What  was  the  maximum  age  for  the  police  horses  ? 

Kershaw:  Fifteen  to  twenty  years. 

Comfort:  These  thoroughbred  mares  live  a  good  deal  longer.  My 
figures  refer  to  breeding  practice  during  the  last  century.  The  maxi- 
mum age  is  lower  today,  particularly  in  mares  from  commercial 
studs,  which  are  put  out  at  23-24  years,  but  rather  less  so  in  stallions. 
The  number  of  stallions  which  "fall  dead"  and  the  number  of  them 
recorded  as  having  died  of  old  age  is  large,  even  now.  They  are  kept, 
if  they  were  famous  animals,  very  much  as  pets  of  the  establishment. 
One  of  the  common,  and  I  suppose  the  most  enviable,  terminal 
entries,  is  "fell  dead  after  serving  a  mare". 


LIFESPAN  OF  CATTLE  AND  HORSES  UNDER 

VARIOUS  CLIMATIC  CONDITIONS  AND  THE 

REASONS  FOR  PREMATURE  CULLING 

W.  Hartwig 

Institutfiir  Tierzucht,  Martin-Luther-Universitdtf  Halle 

In  cattle  breeding  performance  can  be  separated  into  two 
major  components :  (1)  the  special  productivity  (of  milk,  meat, 
wool),  (2)  the  general  performance.  General  performance 
means:  fecundity,  longevity,  power  of  resistance  to  disease 
(particularly  to  those  hereditary  diseases  caused  by  failure  to 
adapt  to  environmental  changes),  food  utilization  and  so  on. 
Certain  factors  of  the  general  performance,  including  longevity, 
also  come  under  our  definition  of  constitution.  Besides  the 
increase  in  special  performance,  the  improvement  of  the 
general  performance  of  agricultural  domestic  animals  is  a 
primary  object  of  cattle  breeding;  in  particular  increased 
lifespan  must  be  considered. 

In  the  determination  of  the  real  duration  of  life  of  large 
agricultural  domestic  animals  there  are,  however,  considerable 
difficulties.  First  of  all  domestic  animals  are  kept  for  their 
economic  productivity;  their  lifespan  is  therefore  affected  by 
economic  considerations  and  is  more  or  less  variable.  Factors 
such  as  decreased  performance,  hard  milking,  price  relations 
between  milk  and  meat,  shortage  of  space,  period  of  feeding, 
bad  fodder  in  some  years,  technical  developments,  etc.,  may 
lead  to  the  sale  for  slaughter  of  completely  healthy  animals 
that  could  have  lived  longer.  That  is  why,  in  considering  the 
average  age  of  living  animals,  we  have  to  deal  not  with  a  real 
biological  parameter,  as  in  human  vital  statistics,  but  with 
arbitrarily  biased  values.    Other  conditions  are  to  be  found, 

57 


58  W.  Hart  WIG 

however,  where  exterior  circumstances  are  not  controlled 
directly  by  man  (for  instance,  diseases,  epidemics,  inferior 
constitution  and  its  consequences,  all  influence  the  lifespan  of 
animals).  Here  we  get  approximate  real  values  of  the  life- 
span, man  and  animals  in  their  struggle  with  nature  being 
exposed  to  these  influences  everywhere. 

The  major  task  in  cattle  breeding  and  veterinary  science  is 
to  analyse  the  struggle  against  the  various  factors  which 
shorten  the  age  of  productive  animals  and  contribute  to  their 
premature  culling.  In  the  following  discussion  of  the  age  of 
cattle  and  horses  first  of  all  the  average  age  of  living  animals 
and  then  the  recorded  average  age  of  herd-book  animals  of 
various  species  will  be  given.  Finally  the  most  important 
reasons  influencing  the  premature  culling  of  animals  will  be 
considered.  The  lifespan  of  cattle  has  been  examined  in  detail 
in  recent  years,  but  there  are  still  only  a  few  results  regarding 
horses.  This  fact  is  easily  comprehensible  because  cattle 
breeding  is  of  great  economic  importance  in  many  countries, 
while  the  importance  of  the  horse  has  diminished  considerably 
as  a  result  of  increased  mechanization. 

Milch  cows 

The  research  listed  in  Table  I  covers  about  25  years,  from 
1932  to  1957.  It  will  be  noted  that  cattle  of  highland  breeds 
show  a  longer  lifespan,  1-1  •  7  years  on  an  average,  than  low- 
land cattle.  It  is  not  possible  to  explain  clearly  how  far  this 
difference  is  based  on  breed-conditioned,  genetic  factors  and 
how  far  it  may  be  attributed  to  climatic  circumstances, 
quality  of  soil  or  economic  reasons.  The  average  lifespan  of 
the  different  breeds  is  between  4-7  and  9-35  years  and  the 
general  average  age  is  7-1  years.  Three  years  may  be  sub- 
tracted for  breeding  and  the  productive  period  therefore 
amounts  to  1-7  to  6-3  years.  It  is  a  fact  proved  by  many 
authors  that  the  productivity  of  the  animals  rises  according 
to  the  number  of  lactations  after  the  fifth  to  the  seventh  calf; 


Lifespan  of  Cattle  and  Horses 


59 


that  is  to  say  the  highest  productivity  is  reached  between  7 
and  9  years,  depending  on  the  breed  and  the  individual  animal, 
and  then  it  decreases  again  gradually.  This  optimal  age  of 
productivity  is  not  attained  by  the  average  animal. 


Table  I 

Average  age  of  living  cows  of  various  breeds 


Breed 

Lowland  cattle 
Scottish  cows 
English  breeds 

American  breeds  and  Iowa 

Black-pied  Schleswig-Holstein 
Black-pied,  East  Prussia 
Black-pied     and     red-pied 

(Rhineland) 
Black-pied  (Rhineland) 
Various  breeds  (North-western 

Germany) 
Unicoloured  red  cattle 
Shorthorn 
Anglia  cattle 
Red-pied  lowland  cattle 

(Minsterland) 


Black-pied     coloured 
(Saxony-Anhalt) 


cattle 


Highland  cattle 

Swiss  brown  cattle 
Spotted  cattle  (Simmental) 
Spotted  cattle  (Bavaria) 
Brown  cattle  (Wurtemberg) 


Number  Average 

of  cows  age 

Author      examined  (y^s.) 


Wright  (1933)  — 

Smith,  Buchanan  and  — 

Robinson  (1932) 

Cannon    and    Hansen  — 

(1940) 

Ripke  (1938)  4,000 

Bauer  (1940)  — 

Mannes  (1947)  4,000 


6-4 

(milk-     5-5- 
fattened)  6  0 

4-7 


Schieren  (1948) 
Winnigstedt  (1949) 

Winnigstedt  (1949) 
Winnigstedt  (1949) 
Ziegenhagen  (1951) 
Rottgermann  (1953) 
1910-1950  controlled 

cows 
Hartmann  (1953) 


Engeler  (1947) 
Stockklausner  (1937) 
Hagel  (1939) 
Piel  and  Rumbaur 
(1948) 

Spotted  cattle  (Upper  Bavaria)  Martin  (1950) 
Spotted  cattle  (Northern  Wurzel  (1952) 

Baden) 


4,000 


1,455 


4,361 

30,000 

3,000 


2,150 
166 


4 
2 

81 


81 
■4^71 

7-2 
6-6 

6-7 
•6-70 


7-4 


8-5 

6-5-7-6 

90 

9-35 

9-23 
711 


It  is  easy  to  see  that  the  average  productivity  of  many  cows 
would  be  considerably  higher  if  they  lived  to  a  greater  age. 
This  alarming  circumstance  has  been  pointed  out  repeatedly 
in  recent  years.    We  cannot,  however,  fully  agree  with  the 


60  W.  Hartwig 

opinion  sometimes  expressed,  that  the  age  of  the  animals 
would  decrease  as  a  consequence  of  higher  productivity. 
Mannes  (1947)  and  Dietrich  (1956)  succeeded  in  proving,  in  the 
course  of  their  researches  on  red-pied  coloured  cows  in  the 
Rhineland  and  on  black-pied  cows  in  Saxony,  that  long-lived 
cows  produce  more  milk,  even  during  their  earlier  years,  than 
those  having  a  short  period  of  produce,  and  that  highly  pro- 
ductive cows  live  to  the  greatest  age.  From  the  results  shown 
below  it  can  be  seen  that  there  has  been  a  general  rise  in  the 
average  lifespan  of  herd-book  animals,  even  though  the  pro- 
ductivity has  also  risen  considerably. 

Table  II 

Average  age  of  black-pied  cattle  of  the  Middle-Weser 
association  of  cattle  breeders  (from  bottcher,  1952) 

No.  of  Average  lifespan 

Date  cows  iv^s.) 

1930  420  5-5 

1935  752  6  0 

1940  1,094  5-9 

1945  1,618  6-5 

1949  1,722  6-9 

The  rise  in  the  average  age  in  20  years  was  1  •  4  years.  Ziegen- 
hagen  undertook  the  same  examination  of  the  Anglia  breed 
(Table  III). 

Table  III 

Average  age  of  Anglia  cattle  (from  Ziegenhagen,  1951) 


No.  of                      Average 

lifespan 

Date 

cows                               Yrs. 

Mos. 

A:  Herd-book  cows 

1924 

9,851                                   5 

10 

1929 

9,458                                   6 

10 

1932 

7,797                                    7 

2 

1939 

8,492                                    7 

10 

1947 

9,831                                    7 
B :  Herd-book  and  not  herd-book 

11 

1930 

15,830                                   6 

1 

1939 

14,991                                   6 

1 

1947 

29,350                                    6 

7 

Average  age 
Yrs.  Mos. 

5 
6 

7 

8 

11 

3 

Lifespan  of  Cattle  and  Horses  61 

The  rise  in  average  age  of  herd-book  cows  of  this  breed  there- 
fore amounts  to  2  years,  1  month,  and  cattle  not  registered  in 
herd-books  showed  the  same  tendency. 

Finally,  let  us  mention  the  values  for  grey-brown  highland 
cattle  (Table  IV). 

Table  IV 
Average  age  of  grey-brown  highland  cattle  (herd-book) 

Date 

1900 
1925 
1949 

Like  the  lowland  cattle  they  show  an  increase  in  age,  amount- 
ing in  this  case  to  1  year,  5  months.  This  generally  observed 
tendency  to  an  increase  in  age  is  due  to  improved  feeding,  to 
improved  methods  of  keeping  and  breeding  and  to  the  struggle 
against  epidemics  and  disease. 

Besides  the  age  of  living  cattle,  the  age  of  cattle  at  death  is 
of  great  interest,  for  this  gives  a  clear  idea  of  the  average 
longest  lifespan  of  the  animals.  In  this  case  it  does  not  matter 
w^hether  the  animals  were  killed  because  of  insufficient  pro- 
ductivity or  whether  they  died  as  a  result  of  epidemics,  disease 
or  accident.  Bottcher  (1952)  gives  the  figures  shown  in  Table 
V. 

Table  V 

Average  age  at  death  of  black-pied  cattle  of  the  Middle- 
Weser  breed  (Bottcher,  1952) 


No.  of 

Age 

Date 

cows 

{yrs.) 

1930 

559 

6-4 

1935 

1,482 

7-2 

1940 

2,205 

72 

1945 

3,171 

7-9 

1949 

2,665 

8-2 

Table  VI  gives  results  found  by  Konig  (1951)  for  another 
breed. 


62  W.  Hartwig 

In  both  these  studies  on  the  age  of  cows  at  death  we  find  the 
same  trend  as  in  the  ages  of  living  animals  already  quoted. 
Although  there  is  a  rise  in  age  at  death,  it  still  remains  true 
that  the  majority  are  culled  before  they  reach  the  years  of 
greatest  productivity. 

Table  VI 

Average  age  at  death  of  grey-brown  highland  cattle 
(herd-book)  (Konig,  1951) 

Date  Yrs.  Mos. 

1901  6   9 

1925  8   4 

1949  9   4 

Rottgermann  (1953)  has  carried  out  research  on  the  age 
structure  of  red-pied  cows  in  Westphalia  and  the  Rhineland. 
From  1944  to  1951  an  average  of  36  per  cent  of  cows  were  aged 
up  to  5  years,  51  per  cent  were  aged  from  5  to  10  years,  and 
only  about  13  per  cent  were  older  than  10  years.  Thus  87  per 
cent  of  cattle  died  before  attaining  their  tenth  year. 

Freudenberg  and  Francke  (1956)  found  that  in  black-pied 
cattle  of  the  Central-German  arid  region  the  highest  number 
of  deaths  occurred  between  the  fourth  and  the  eighth  years. 
At  the  age  of  3,  14-7  per  cent  of  the  cattle  died;  at  the  age  of 
5,  21-3  per  cent;  at  6,  22-6  per  cent;  at  7,  13-2  per  cent;  and 
at  the  age  of  8,  9-8  per  cent.  Thus  81  per  cent  of  all  the  cows 
died  in  these  five  years,  and  the  average  age  at  death  was  6  •  43 
years.  This  ratio  of  losses  is  high,  considering  that  these 
figures  are  for  herd-book  cattle  which  are  valuable  for  breeding 
and  might  be  expected  to  be  kept  alive  longer.  The  figures  for 
cattle  not  registered  in  herd-books  reveal  still  worse  results. 

In  this  connexion  we  need  to  ask  what  are  the  causes  that 
have  a  decisive  influence  on  the  age  of  the  animals  and  lead  to 
their  premature  culling  or  death.  Freudenberg  and  Francke 
(1956)  performed  special  researches  along  this  line  on  469 
cows  from  12  big  farms  of  the  arid  Central-German  district. 
They  found  the  following  reasons  for  suppression.  First,  54*8 


Lifespan  of  Cattle  and  Horses  63 

per  cent  of  the  animals  were  sterile ;  the  authors  pointed  out 
that  this  is  due  to  genital  tuberculosis  as  well  as  to  Bang's 
disease,  which  causes  most  of  the  sterility.  Secondly,  12-79 
per  cent  of  them  had  tuberculosis  (udder  tuberculosis  included). 
Thirdly,  inflammation  of  the  udder  accounted  for  7-89  per 
cent.  The  remaining  115  animals  died  from  various  other 
diseases,  such  as  cancer  of  the  lungs  and  cardiac  weakness. 

Dinkhauser's  (1940)  investigations  in  Lower  Saxony  showed 
that  23  per  cent  of  all  cattle  had  to  be  culled  because  of  steril- 
ity. From  research  in  the  Central-German  dry  region  Marlow 
(1951)  succeeded  in  showing  that  softening  of  the  bone  was  the 
most  important  cause  of  death.  In  his  researches  on  cattle 
from  smaller  farms  in  Baden,  Gerner  (1952)  obtained  the 
following  results: 


1. 

Sterility 

32  •  0  per  cent 

2. 

Tuberculosis 

14-2     „      „ 

3. 

Swallowing  foreign  bodies 

13-4     „      „ 

4. 

Other  diseases 

130     „      „ 

5. 

Age 

21-0     „      „ 

6. 

Insufficient  productivity 

6-1     „       „ 

Other  diseases  here  means  those  that  are  specially  connected 
with  the  sexual  organs,  such  as  dropping  of  the  womb  and 
emergency  slaughtering  at  calving.  Piel  and  Rumbaur  (1948) 
studied  the  causes  of  death  in  first-class  brown  cattle  in 
Wurtemberg.  They  obtained  the  following  results : 


1. 

Sterility 

24-3 

per 

cent 

2. 

Tuberculosis 

4-6 

59 

?> 

3. 

Brucellosis 

1-7 

5> 

5> 

4. 

Age 

23-4 

>> 

5J 

5. 

Slaughtering  without  indication 

of  cause 

26-0 

?J 

J5 

6. 

Garget 

11-7 

>J 

J> 

7. 

Foot-and-mouth  disease 

2-9 

J5 

>5 

8. 

Emergency  slaughtering 

5-4 

JJ 

JJ 

64  W.  Hartwig 

From  research  into  the  family  history  tuberculosis  was 
found  in  41  families,  six  of  which  had  two  or  more  cases. 
The  authors  therefore  suppose  that  there  is  a  hereditary 
inclination  to  tuberculosis,  as  the  same  phenomenon  is  to  be 
found  in  human  medicine. 

Martin  (1950)  performed  research  on  this  line  on  2,160  cows 
in  Baden.  Piel  (1951)  examined  the  reasons  for  culling  2,507 
brown  Wurtemberg  cattle  from  1939  to  1944,  while  Ziegen- 
hagen  (1951)  examined  the  causes  of  death  of  5,244  Anglia 
cows.  These  authors  found  that  the  causes  were  more  or  less 
the  same  as  described  above.  All  these  statements  clearly 
demonstrate  that  sterility,  tuberculosis,  garget,  brucellosis 
and  softening  of  the  bones  are  the  most  important  diseases  and 
deficiencies  that  lead  to  culling  of  breeding  animals  and  shorten 
their  lifespans  and  utilization  as  brood  cattle. 

Using  black-pied  cows  of  various  ages  from  the  Central- 
German  dry  region,  Spohde  (1948)  undertook  research  on 
death  from  the  three  most  important  diseases  (sterility, 
tuberculosis  and  garget).  Of  all  the  cows  culled  because  of 
sterility,  78  per  cent  were  aged  from  4  to  8  years.  Sixty-eight 
per  cent  of  tuberculous  animals  were  culled  between  the  ages 
of  5  and  7  years;  78  per  cent  of  the  animals  infected  with 
garget  were  also  culled  at  these  ages.  These  three  principal 
diseases  cause  the  relatively  premature  suppression  of  animals 
in  the  second  to  fifth  years  of  life,  that  is  to  say  at  an  age  when 
their  maximum  capacity  has  not  yet  been  reached. 

The  different  measures  that  might  be  taken  in  order  to  raise 
the  average  age  of  the  animals  include  breeding,  by  scrupulous 
selection,  for  fecundity,  longevity,  and  intensified  resistance 
to  diseases,  with  simultaneous  stress  on  optimal  productivity, 
as  well  as  improvements  in  breeding,  keeping  and  feeding. 

Bulls 

In  his  studies  on  the  ages  of  bulls  in  Brunswick  slaughter- 
houses Hogreve  (1955)  found  the  following  average  ages:  bulls 
from  the  South  Hanover-Brunswick  region,  3  years,  5  months ; 


Lifespan  of  Cattle  and  Horses  65 

from  Luneberg,  3  years,  4  months;  from  East  Friesland,  3 
years,  4  months.  He  concludes  from  this  low  age  of  death 
that  the  premature  culling  of  bulls  is  due  more  to  private  and 
economic  reasons  than  to  the  proper  physiological  limit  of 
productivity  of  the  individual  animals.  Hartwig  (1959,  un- 
published) examined  the  average  age  at  death  of  2,000  herd- 
book  bulls  from  Saxony-Anhalt.  An  average  age  of  4  years,  9 
months  was  noted;  83-5  per  cent  of  the  animals  had  been 
killed  because  they  were  no  longer  used  for  covering.  The 
reasons  for  this  were  principally  economic,  as  Hogreve  had 
also  found.  Schroder  (1958)  found  a  higher  average  age  of  5 
years,  4  months  among  suppressed  bulls  kept  for  fertilization. 
In  insemination  stations  we  find  that  the  bulls  are  kept  by 
means  of  better  utilization  of  breed  and  better  conditions 
so  that  the  animals  in  general  grow  older.  Schroder  found  19 
bulls  which  were  older  than  10  years.  The  most  important 
causes  of  death  of  the  bulls  were : 

culled  for  genetic  reasons  60  bulls  =  20-27  per  cent 

diseases  of  the  genital  system  59      ,,      =19*9      ,,     ,, 

tuberculosis  58      ,,      =19-5      ,,     ,, 

diseases  of  the  legs  33      ,,      =11*1      ,,     ,, 

Five  per  cent  of  the  bulls  were  slaughtered  after  swallowing 
foreign  bodies  and  2  per  cent  because  of  viciousness.  Here 
again  economic  aspects  play  the  leading  part,  so  that  these 
factors  make  the  determination  of  a  real  average  age  impos- 
sible. Cows  and  bulls  can  both  reach  18  or  20  years  of  age, 
though  these  figures  are  exceptional. 

Horses 

It  is  very  difficult  to  make  corresponding  observations  on 
the  regional  breeding  of  horses,  therefore  these  studies  were 
carried  out  on  stallions  that  were  kept  as  long  as  they  con- 
tinued to  breed.  For  that  reason  a  better  estimate  of  their 
real  age  at  death  can  be  made.  Research  along  this  line  has 
been  undertaken  on  stallions  in  the  district  stud  of  Kreuz  by 

AGEING— V — 3 


66  W.  Hartwig 

Wussow  and  Hartwig  (1956-57).  All  of  the  635  stallions  were 
examined  carefully  and  classified  according  to  breed  in  order 
to  find  the  average  age  of  each  breed.  The  following  values 
were  noted: 

Average  age  {yrs.) 
20  East  Prussian  stallions  15-3 

24  Hanoverian  13-6 

52  Oldenburg  12-9 

The  average  lifespan  of  warm-blooded*  stallions  was  13-8 
years. 

Average  age  {yrs.) 
150  original  Belgian  and  Dutch  stallions  13-2 

53  stallions  of  the  Rhineland  12*6 
42  imported  English  cold-blooded  stallions  10  •  5 
284  Belgian  stallions,  born  in  Saxony  10-4 

The  average  lifespan  of  cold-blooded*  stallions  was  11-5 
years. 

Table  VII  gives  a  summary  of  the  ages  at  death  of  different 
breeds.  This  table  shows  that  well-bred  or  thoroughbred  warm- 
blooded horses  have  the  longest  lifespans.  They  also  have  the 
highest  percentage  of  stallions  reaching  an  age  of  more  than 
15  or  20  years,  whereas  by  the  age  of  15,  81  per  cent  of  the 
cold-blooded  horses  have  died  and  only  0  •  7  per  cent  reach  an 
age  over  20  years.  Oldenburg  stallions,  being  heavy  but  warm- 
blooded, show  a  lifespan  that  is  between  the  well-bred  warm- 
blooded and  the  cold-blooded  horses.  From  the  above  it  seems 
that  thoroughbred  and  well-bred  warm-blooded  horses  have 
a  better  constitution  and  live  to  a  greater  age  than  do  cold- 
blooded ones.  The  constitution  of  the  animals  seems  less 
good  the  less  thoroughbred  blood  they  have.  This  tendency 
can  also  be  found  in  examining  the  lifespan  of  mares.  Flade 
(1958),  for  instance,  while  doing  research  on  64  Arab  thorough- 
bred mares  born  between  1921  and  1945  in  Poland,  found  an 

*  "Warm-blooded"  horses  are  thoroughbreds;  "cold-blooded"  horses  are 
heavy  ones. 


Lifespan  of  Cattle  and  Horses  67 

Table  VII 

Ages  of  stallions  at  death 

53  thoroughbred  and  well-bred  warm-blooded  horses : 

Age  group  No.  of  stallions  Percentage 

3-5  5  9-5 

3-10  22  40-6 

3-15  31  58-5 

3-20  42  79  0 

10  stallions  attained  an  age  over  20  years  =  19-0 

52  heavy  warm-blooded  horses  (Oldenburg  and  Friesian) : 

Age  group  No.  of  stallions  Percentage 

3-5  9  160 

3-10  21  40-3 

3-15  84  63-3 

3-20  50  96  0 

2  staUions  reached  an  age  of  more  than  20  years  =    4*0 

534  cold-blooded  horses : 

Age  group  No.  o'  stallions  Percentage 

3-5  63  11-8 

3-10  229  42-9 

3-15  434  81-0 

3-20  530  99-3 

4  stallions  reached  an  age  of  more  than  20  years  =    0-7 

average  age  of  16-7  years.  Konopinski  and  Detkens  (quoted 
by  Flade)  found  an  average  age  of  11  •  5  years  for  598  half-bred 
Poznan  mares  in  Poland. 

The  most  important  causes  of  the  premature  culling  of 
stallions  are,  according  to  Wussow  and  Hartwig  (1956-57),  the 
diseases  and  constitutional  faults  shown  in  the  table  on  p.  68, 
which  were  observed  in  390  stallions  whose  causes  of  death 
were  evident. 

In  summary  we  can  state  the  following  points  concerning 
the  average  lifespan  of  cattle  and  horses : 

1.  Research  on  living  cows  of  19  different  breeds  revealed 
an  average  lifespan  of  7-1  years  (4 -7-9 -35  years).  This 
average  is  undesirably  low,  because  the  majority  of  the 
animals  are  culled  before  attaining  the  years  of  highest  pro- 
ductivity. 


68  W.  Hartwig 

2.  Research  on  various  breeds  shows  a  small  rise  (1-5  to  2 
years)  in  the  average  lifespan  both  of  living  cows  and  of  cows 
which  died  during  the  past  two  or  three  decades.  We  cannot 
agree  with  the  often  expressed  opinion  that  increased  pro- 
ductivity will  cause  the  premature  death  of  the  animals. 

Stallions  Percentage 


1. 

Colic 

135 

34-0 

2. 

Dermatitis  verrucosa  (malanders) 

74 

19-5 

3. 

Disease  of  the  heart, 

lungs 

and 

chest 

58 

15-1 

4. 

Bad  covering 

52 

13-5 

5. 

Weakness  of  the  legs 

30 

8-0 

6. 

Sepsis 

12 

31 

7. 

Viciousness 

11 

2-8 

8. 

Sleepy  staggers  (Borna) 

1 

9 

2-3 

9. 

Cancer 

Total 

8 
390 

1-8 

100-0 

3.  The  lifespan  of  cows  is  considerably  influenced  by 
economic  factors,  which  frequently  lead  to  the  premature 
killing  of  healthy  animals. 

4.  The  most  important  causes  of  death  that  have  a  great 
influence  on  the  lifespan  of  cows  are  sterility,  tuberculosis  and 
garget  (mastitis). 

5.  The  average  lifespan  of  bulls  is  still  lower.  Economic 
factors  act  even  more  intensively  here,  so  that  the  animals 
only  attain  an  average  lifespan  of  between  3  years,  5  months 
and  5  years,  4  months. 

6.  The  average  lifespan  of  Arab  mares  in  Poland  was 
stated  to  be  16*7  years  and  that  of  Poznan  half-bred  mares 
11-5  years. 

7.  Warm-blooded  stallions  at  the  Kreuz  stud  attained  an 
age  of  13-5  years  and  cold-blooded  stallions  of  Belgian  origin 
11*5  years. 


Lifespan  of  Cattle  and  Horses  69 

8.  The  lifespan  of  horses  seems  to  decrease  the  less  they  are 
related  to  English  and  Arab  thoi'oughbreds. 

9.  The  most  important  causes  of  loss  of  the  stallions  were: 
colic,  dermatitis  verrucosa  (malanders),  infirmity  of  the  heart, 
chest  and  lungs,  and  bad  covering. 


REFERENCES 

Bauer,  K.  (1940).   Z.  Tierz.  ZuchtBiol,  47,  261. 

BoTTCHER,  T.  (1952).   Volkerode,  Sonderdruck,  18. 

Cannon,  C,  and  Hansen,  E.  (1940).   Zuchtungskimde,  15,  193. 

Dietrich,  H.  (1956).    Dissertation,  Halle. 

DiNKHAUSER,  F.  (1940).   Dtscli.  landw.  Tierz.,  44,  153. 

Engeler,  W.  (1947).   Das  schweizerische  Braunvieh. 

Flade,  J.  E.  (1958).   Arch.  Tierz.,  1,  354. 

Freudenberg,  F.,  and  Franke,  G.   (1956).    Dtsch.  Landw.,  Berl.,  7, 

301. 
Gerner,  K.  (1952).   Tierzuchter,  4,  589. 
Hagel,  L.  (1939).   Dissertation,  Munich. 
Hartmann,  W.  (1953).   Dissertation,  Halle. 
Hartwig,  W.  (1959).   Unpublished  results. 
HoGREVE,  F.  (1955).   Tierzuchter,  7,  60. 
Konig,  K.  (1951).   AUgau.  Bauernblatt. 
KoNOPiNSKi  and  Detkens  (quoted  by  Flade,  1958). 
Mannes,  a.  (1947).   Dissertation,  Hannover. 
Marlow,  H.  (1951).   Dissertation,  Halle. 
Martin,  W.  (1950).   Ziichtungskunde,  22,  97. 
PiEL,  H.  (1951).   Ziichtungskunde,  23,  79. 
PiEL,  H.,  and  Rumbaur.  (1948).    Dissertation,  Hohnheim. 
RiPKE,  G.  (1938).   Dissertation,  Munich. 
RoTTGERMANN,  W.  (1953).   Z.  Ticrz.  ZiichtBioL,  62,  1. 
ScHiEREN,  J.  (1948).   Dtsch.  tierdrztl.  Wschr.,  55,  49. 
Schroder,  G.  (1958).   Thesis,  Halle. 
Smith,  Buchanan,  A.  D.,   and   Robinson,    O.   J.    (1932).    Z.   Tierz. 

ZuchtBiol,  25,  307. 
Spohde,  H.  (1948).   Thesis,  Halle. 

Stockklausner,  F.  (1937).   Dtsch.  landw.  Tierz.,  41,  570. 
Winnigstedt,  R.  (1949).   Ziichtungskunde,  20,  193. 
Wright,  N.  C.  (1933).   Ziichtungskunde,  8,  424. 
WuRZEL,  W.  (1952).   Zuchtungskunde,  23,  239. 
Wussow,  W.,  and  Hartwig,  (1956/57).    Wiss.  Z.  Martin-Luther -Univ., 

6,  13. 
Ziegenhagen,  G.  (1951).   Z.  Tierz.  ZuchtBiol,  59,  331. 


70  Discussion 

DISCUSSION 

Benjamin:  The  improvement  in  average  lifespan  in  the  more 
recent  groups  you  mentioned  was  very  much  smaller  than  in  the 
cattle  in  earlier  years.  Is  there  some  sort  of  resistance  to  increasing 
the  lifespan  or  is  there  some  other  factor  involved  ? 

Hartwig:  The  overall  tendency  was  towards  an  increased  lifespan, 
and  the  smaller  increment  there  is  not  significant. 

Bourliere :  Is  there  any  correlation  between  the  average  lifespan  of 
the  different  breeds  of  cows  and  their  weight  ? 

Hartwig:  The  lighter  cattle  live  longer  than  the  heavy  ones. 

Danielli:  Have  you  any  data  for  highland  cattle  living  in  the 
lowlands,  and  vice  versa  ? 

Hartwig:  There  is  the  example  that  when  cows  were  exported  to 
Africa  the  highland  cows  adapted  themselves  better  to  those  condi- 
tions than  the  lowland  ones. 

Kershaw:  When  were  these  cattle  exported  ? 

Hartwig:  At  the  end  of  the  1920's. 

Kershaw:  In  that  case  there  is  a  complicating  factor,  because  I 
have  seen  their  progeny  in  the  Cameroons  and  they  are  remarkably 
resistant  to  sleeping  sickness. 

Danielli:  To  what  extent  is  this  increase  in  average  age  due  to 
better  veterinary  services? 

Hartwig:  That  is  too  complex  to  answer  and  I  am  unable  to 
decide  the  cause  of  it. 

Danielli:  Unless  one  has  some  idea  of  the  extent  to  which  some 
specific  disorders  have  been  suppressed  by  veterinary  work,  it  is 
very  difficult  to  evaluate  the  data  at  all. 

Hartwig:  The  veterinarians  claim  that  they  helped  towards  this 
increase,  but  on  the  other  hand  the  breeders  say  it  is  due  to  their 
work. 

Comfort:  Has  it  been  possible,  in  your  data  on  horses,  to  compute 
lifespans  or  life-tables  in  the  same  sort  of  way  as  I  have  done,  on  the 
basis  of  age-specific  natural  death  rates  allowing  for  the  animals 
lost  or  culled  ? 

Hartwig:  Yes,  that  has  been  done. 

Comfort:  The  agricultural  lifespans  are  a  very  different  matter 
indeed  from  those  of  horses,  which  are  kept  to  advanced  ages.  I  am 
sure  these  cows  would  live  a  great  deal  longer  if  they  were  not 
culled. 

Hartwig:  I  agree  with  you. 

Comfort:  It  would  be  very  useful  if  we  could  get  a  good  life-table 
for  one  of  the  heavy  breeds  for  comparison  with  thoroughbreds. 


Discussion  71 

Hartwig:  The  great  difficulty  is  that  if  these  animals  are  not  kept 
for  breeding  purposes  they  are  mostly  sold  and  records  are  very 
difficult  to  obtain. 

MiXhlhock:  Cancer  research  workers  are  very  interested  in  keeping 
cattle  to  the  end  of  their  lives,  because  no  one  has  ever  seen  a  mam- 
mary tumour  in  cattle.  The  question  is  are  they  naturally  resistant, 
or  are  they  slaughtered  before  they  reach  the  age  at  which  tumours 
would  develop  ?  A  considerable  amount  of  money  is  now  being 
collected  just  to  keep  cattle  to  the  end  of  their  natural  lives  to  see 
what  happens. 

There  is  another  cancer  in  cattle  which  is  very  peculiar,  and  that 
is  the  so-called  "cancer  eye",  or  cancer  of  the  conjunctiva.  It  is  a 
very  interesting  condition,  hardly  ever  seen  in  Europe,  but  more 
often  found  in  countries  with  plenty  of  sunshine.  It  is  thought  to  be 
a  virus  infection,  and  studies  are  being  carried  out  in  Texas  where  a 
great  number  of  cattle  are  kept  just  for  this  purpose.  Gerontologists 
could  therefore  find  out  from  this  material  what  the  normal  lifespan 
is. 

Wolstenholme :  Could  one  obtain  any  figures  from  the  cattle  in 
India,  which  are  allowed  to  live  their  normal  lives  to  the  full  ? 

ComfoH:  There  is  an  institute  in  India  which  studies  these  cattle, 
but  their  ages  are  not  known,  and  unless  one  has  reliable  stud-book 
records  and  the  actual  date  of  the  calving  it  is  no  good.  If  I  was  un- 
justifiably sceptical  about  centenarians  who  have  birth  certificates, 
I  would  be  ten  times  more  sceptical  about  a  sacred  cow  which  has 
not. 

Wolstenholme:  Have  any  sacred  cows  been  seen  to  have  mammary 
tumours  ? 

MiXhlhock :  They  are  in  such  a  poor  condition  that  it  is  no  wonder 
that  they  do  not  get  any. 


ONSET  OF  DISEASE  AND  THE 
LONGEVITY  OF  RAT  AND  MAN 

Henry  S.  Simms,  Benjamin  N.  Berg, 
and  Dean  F.  Davies 

Department  of  Pathology,  College  of  Physicians 
and  Surgeons,  Columbia  University,  New  York 

It  is  self-evident  that  the  longevity  of  any  species,  such  as 
the  rat  or  man,  depends  upon  the  age  range  at  which  the 
major  diseases  of  that  species  result  in  death.  This,  in  turn, 
depends  upon  three  factors : 

First,  the  ages  at  which  early  lesions  of  the  major  diseases 
are  most  likely  to  appear  in  individuals  of  the  species.  (This 
we  call  the  "probable  age  of  onset.") 

Second,  the  time  required  for  lesions  to  develop  from  the 
early  stages  to  the  severe  lesions  that  cause  death. 

Third,  the  effect  of  advancing  age  on  the  onset  of  lesions  in 
that  species. 

The  combined  action  of  these  three  factors  results  in  the 
mortality  curves  with  which  we  are  familiar  (Fig.  1). 

One  hundred  and  thirty-four  years  ago  Gompertz  (1825) 
published  his  law  relating  mortality  with  age.  More  than  one 
hundred  years  later  this  law  was  rediscovered  by  one  of  the 
present  authors — and  this  started  him  on  a  career  in  gerontol- 
ogy.  The  law  may  be  expressed  in  the  following  form : 

Log  Pm  -  Log  Pmo  =  ^W 
where  P^  is  the  probability  of  death  (mortality  rate)  at  age  t, 
P^Q  is  the  (extrapolated)  probability  of  death  at  the  age  of 
birth,  and  k^  is  a  constant  having  a  positive  value. 

Fig.  1  shows  a  plot  of  the  logarithm  of  mortality  rate  against 
age.  This  is  seen  to  be  approximately  a  straight  line  through- 
out adult  life.   The  equation  of  this  line  is  the  one  just  given. 

72 


Onset  of  Disease  and  Longevity  of  Rat  and  Man  73 

For  nearly  15  years  we  have  been  studying  the  onset  of 
lesions  in  rats  in  relation  to  age  and  longevity.  For  the 
purpose  of  these  studies  we  established  a  special  rat  colony 
with  conditions  that  were  unusually  favourable  and  uniform. 
The  temperature  was  kept  constant  (76°  f).  The  humidity 
Avas  kept  at  about  60  per  cent.   The  lighting  was  indirect  and 


.o^s^ 


V 


Fig.    1.     Logarithm   of  human  mortahty,   plotted 
against  age  (From  Simms,  1946). 

Figs.  1-5  reproduced  by  courtesy  of  the  Editor,  Journal  of  Gerontology. 


was  the  same  in  all  cages,  with  12  hours  of  light  and  12  hours 
darkness  each  day  throughout  the  year.  The  diet  was  uniform 
and  the  quarters  were  quiet  and  clean.  Data  on  growth  and 
disease  in  rats  kept  under  these  conditions  have  been  reported 
by  Berg  and  Harmison  (1957). 

Finally,  we  were  able  to  reduce  respiratory  infection  (and 
other  infections)  to  such  a  low  level  that  they  were  practically 
non-existent.   Under  these  conditions  it  was  possible  to  study 


74 


H.  S.  SiMMS,  B.  N.  Berg,  and  D.  F.  Davies 


the  onset  of  lesions  (of  a  non-infectious  nature)  in  relation  to 
age  and  in  relation  to  mortality  and  longevity. 

Fig.  2  shows  the  percentage  of  male  rats  that  showed  detect- 
able lesions  of  five  selected  diseases  (Simms  and  Berg,  1957). 
Each  curve  is  an  S -shaped  curve  approaching  a  maximum 
level  of  incidence.  Two  of  these  curves  approach  100  per  cent, 
whereas  the  other  three  approach  lower  levels  of  incidence. 

It  will  be  noted  that  the  curve  for  chronic  nephrosis  and 
glomerulonephritis  and  also  the  curve  for  myocardial  degener- 
ation are  spread  over  a  wide  age  range.   This  means  that  onset 


100 


PERCENT 
80 

(,0 

"""■^ 

ADENOMA   OF 

- 

40 

20 

PITUITARY 

^::^:^^ 

0  100        200 

AGE   IN  DAYS 


300       400        500       600        700        B05        900      iooo" 


1100 


Fig.  2.    Percentage  of  rats  having  detectable  lesions 

of  five  major  diseases,  plotted  against  age  at  autopsy 

(From  Simms  and  Berg,  1957). 


of  these  lesions  was  observed  in  some  rats  at  an  early  age  but 
in  other  rats  it  was  not  seen  until  they  were  much  older.  On 
the  other  hand,  onset  of  muscular  degeneration  occurred  in  all 
rats  within  a  narrow  age  range  (lasting  only  about  500  days). 
In  the  majority  of  the  rats  the  onset  of  detectable  lesions  of 
this  disease  occurred  between  700  and  900  days  of  age. 
Hence  our  rat  colony  was  quite  homogeneous  in  regard  to  this 
disease — but  much  less  homogeneous  in  regard  to  other 
diseases. 

Fig.  3  shows  the  slope  of  these  curves  at  various  ages.  Each 
curve  in  this  chart  represents  the  age  distribution  of  onset  of  a 
disease.  For  example,  muscular  degeneration  (Berg,  1956)  had 


Onset  of  Disease  and  Longevity  of  Rat  and  Man  75 


50 

PERCENT         i             1 
PER  100   DAY 
INTERVAL 
40     -100/il 

1       1       1 

1  ^1       1 

ImuscI 
Ioegen\ 

! 

30 

— 

20 

CHR   NEPH. 

/jPER!            \ 

10 

j^W^ 

)WA    \ 

^ 

0     1       r<^ 

"^^^l^S 

"t ^N" 

1 

AGE 


100        200 
IN  DAYS 


300       400        500       600 


700 


800       900       1000     1100 


Fig.  3.  Age  distribution  of  onset  of  rat  lesions.  These 

curves  show  the  probabihty  of  onset  at  various  ages 

(among  the  total  number  living  at  each  age)  (From 

Simms  and  Berg,  1957). 


its  greatest  probability  of  onset  at  750  days  of  age,  although  a 
few  rats  acquired  lesions  at  500  days — and  some  not  until 
1,000  days.  Similarly,  the  peak  for  chronic  nephrosis  and 
glomerulonephritis  was  580  days  and  the  peak  for  periarteritis 
was  710  days. 


0  300        200 

AGE  IN  DAYS 


900    1000 


Fig.  4.  Probability  of  onset  of  new  lesions  among  the 

surviving  rats  having  no  lesions  (From  Simms  and 

Berg,  1957). 


76 


H.  S.  SiMMS,  B.  N.  Berg,  and  D.  F.  Davies 


The  reason  why  the  curves  fall  off  after  reaching  a  maximum 
is  simply  that  there  are  fewer  remaining  animals  without 
lesions  of  a  given  disease  and  which  consequently  can  acquire 
lesions  for  the  first  time.  There  is  no  decrease  in  the  tendency 
to  form  lesions  among  those  individuals  that  have  survived  to 
advanced  age  without  them.  This  is  shown  in  Fig.  4,  where  it 
is  seen  that  the  tendency  to  form  lesions  increases  progressively 
with  age. 


— .        AGE  IN  DAYS 
0-    100        200 

— 1 r- 


^iL5 


3di 


^>- 


^20. 


Fig.  5.  Logarithm  of  the  probabihty  of  onset  of  new 
lesions  (among  surviving  rats  having  no  lesions).  Also 
a  curve  for  rat  mortality   (From  Simms  and  Berg, 

1957). 

That  this  increase  is  a  logarithmic  function  of  age  is  shown 
in  Fig.  5,  where  the  logarithm  of  the  probability  of  onset 
(among  rats  not  having  lesions)  is  plotted  against  age.  These 
curves  approximate  to  straight  lines.  Note  that  they  parallel 
the  bottom  curve  for  mortality  of  rats. 

We  may  now  compare  rat  and  man.  Fig.  6  contains  two 
charts  each  having  data  from  both  rat  and  man.  At  the  left 
end  of  the  top  chart  are  curves  for  occurrence  of  lesions  in 
rats.  These  are  identical  with  the  curves  in  Fig.  2,  except 
that  here  the  time  scale  is  very  much  condensed,  making  the 
curves  much  steeper.  Similarly,  at  the  left  end  of  the  bottom 
chart  are  curves  for  the  distribution  of  onset  of  lesions  in 


Onset  of  Disease  and  Longevity  of  Rat  and  Man  77 

rats.  These  are  identical  with  those  in  Fig.  3,  except  that  the 
time  scale  is  here  very  much  condensed. 

At  the  right-hand  end  of  these  charts  are  data  on  man — 
plotted  on  the  same  time  scale  as  the  rat  data.  These  data  were 
obtained  by  one  of  us  (Dr.  Davies)  in  1945-46  while  working 
in   this   department.   Examination   was   made   of  over   500 


100 
80 

o   40 

o 

"  20 
0 


RAT 


MAN 


,\hr.  neph. 
•^myo.  degen. 

^^periarteritis 


PERCENT    OF    INDIVIDUALS 
HAVING     LESIONS 


10 


20 


30 


80         90 


AGE    IN    YEARS 


RAT    At  r  100  DAYS 


MAN   At  =10  YEARS 


40  - 


20 


^MUSC.    DEGEN. 


XHR.    NEPH. 
PERIARTERITIS 
rMYO.    DEGEN. 


DISTRIBUTION    OF 
AGE   OF  ONSET 


HYPER.    PR05T 


0  10 

AGE 


20  30 

YEARS 


COR.  THROM. 


90 


Fig.  6.  Data  of  rat  and  man  plotted  on  the  same  age  scale. 

Top  Chart :  Percentage  of  individuals  having  detectable  lesions  of  diseases 

of  their  species. 
Bottom  chart :  Age  distribution  of  onset  of  lesions  (probabiUty  of  onset). 


autopsy  records  of  the  Presbyterian  Hospital.  The  data  were 
tabulated  and  some  of  the  findings  on  males  are  reported  here. 
We  recognize  that  such  data  are  open  to  the  criticism  that  the 
autopsies  were  performed  by  numerous  pathologists  and  that 
the  cases  came  from  a  very  heterogeneous  population.  How- 
ever, these  objections  apply  equally  to  all  the  age  groups 
reported.    Unfortunately,  the  number  of  cases  in  the  upper 


78        H.  S.  SiMMS,  B.  N.  Berg,  and  D.  F.  Davies 

age  groups  was  small  (56  males  in  the  70-79  group  and  12 
males  in  the  80-89  group). 

It  will  be  seen  from  Fig.  6  that  when  the  data  of  rat  and 
man  are  plotted  on  the  same  time  scale,  the  curves  of  the  two 
species  are  identical  in  form,  except  that  those  of  the  rat 
occur  much  sooner,  and  are  compressed  into  a  much  shorter 
time  range,  than  those  of  man.  This  applies  not  only  to  one  or 
two  diseases  of  each  species,  but  also  to  other  diseases  not 
reported  here. 

It  should  be  pointed  out  that  the  rat  data  were  obtained 
from  a  fairly  homogeneous  colony  of  animals  raised  under 
uniform  conditions.  On  the  other  hand,  the  human  data  were 
obtained  from  a  heterogeneous  collection  of  individuals  who 
had  lived  under  differing  conditions.  Had  the  humans  been 
as  homogeneous  as  the  rats,  their  data  on  these  two  charts 
would  be  characterized  by  much  steeper  curves  within  a  much 
narrower  time  range — perhaps  approximating  to  the  curves 
of  the  rats  in  shape,  but  not  in  their  location  on  these  charts. 

Hence,  we  may  conclude  that  the  factors  which  determine 
the  longevity  of  these  two  species  (rat  and  man)  appear  to  be 
identical,  except  that  in  one  species  the  lesions  of  the  major 
diseases  have  their  onset  much  sooner  and  over  a  much 
shorter  range  of  time,  than  do  those  of  the  other  species,  when 
compared  on  the  same  time  scale. 

That  it  is  possible  to  alter  the  age  of  onset  of  lesions  has 
been  shown  by  dietary  experiments  in  which  rats  on  a  re- 
stricted food  intake  were  found  to  have  a  considerable  delay 
in  the  onset  of  their  major  diseases,  as  compared  with  rats 
receiving  as  much  food  as  they  wanted  to  eat.  This  suggests 
that  there  may  be  other  methods  for  modifying  lifespan  which 
may  result  in  altering  the  age  of  onset  of  lesions  of  major 
diseases. 

Summary 

Observations  on  the  accumulation  of  lesions  in  rats  in 
relation  to  age  have  shown  that  the  lifespan  of  these  animals 


Onset  of  Disease  and  Longevity  of  Rat  and  Man  79 

is  directly  related  to  the  age  of  onset  of  lesions  of  the  major 
diseases  of  this  species.  The  age  of  onset  of  these  lesions,  when 
plotted  against  age,  gives  smooth  curves  which  are  character- 
istic of  the  diseases  in  question. 

Data  on  human  lesions,  found  from  autopsy  data  at  dif- 
ferent ages,  give  curves  similar  to  those  of  rats,  except  that 
the  onset  of  the  human  diseases  does  not  occur  until  many 
years  later  than  is  the  case  with  the  rats.  This  indicates  that 
there  is  a  mechanism  for  the  deferment  of  onset  of  lesions, 
which  accounts  for  the  difference  in  lifespan  between  the 
species. 

REFERENCES 

Berg,  B.  N.  (1956).  J.  Geront,  11,  134. 

Berg,  B.  N.,  and  Harmison,  C.  R.  (1957).  J.  Geront.y  12,  370. 

GoMPERTZ,  E.  (1825).  Phil.  Trans.,  115,  513. 

SIMMS,  H.  S.  (1946).  J.  Geront,  1,  13. 

SIMMS,  H.  S.,  and  Berg,  B.  N.  (1957).  J.  Geront.y  12,  244. 

DISCUSSION 

Gerking:  The  curve  showing  the  age  distribution  of  onset  of  dis- 
ease in  your  rats  was  a  normal,  or  nearly  normal  distribution.  The 
right-hand  portion  of  the  curve  of  deaths  that  Dr.  Benjamin  showed 
us,  indicating  senescence  in  the  human,  was  also  the  normal  distri- 
bution. These  both  occurred  late  in  life  and  they  may  actually  reflect 
the  same  phenomenon. 

Berg:  That  is  true.  We  find  the  same  type  of  distribution  curve  for 
the  onset  of  lesions  as  for  mortality — in  both  rat  and  man.  This  is 
because  the  period  for  the  full  development  of  the  disease,  from  the 
time  of  the  early  lesion  to  the  late  lesion,  does  not  change  with  age. 

Gerking:  The  correspondence  between  the  shape  of  these  ciu'ves, 
over  a  wide  range  of  ages,  is  very  remarkable. 

GriXneherg:  I  have  some  confirmatory  evidence  on  what  you  said 
about  the  relation  between  the  age  of  onset  of  the  disease  and  the 
environment.  In  mice  of  the  A  strain,  in  animals  above  a  certain  age, 
nearly  all  individuals  suffer  from  the  deposition  of  a  substance  called 
amyloid  in  the  kidneys  and  various  other  organs.  A  group  of  in- 
vestigators in  the  National  Institutes  of  Health,  Bethesda,  has  suc- 
ceeded in  influencing  this  condition  by  a  change  of  diet.  If  these 
animals  are  fed  on  a  protein-poor  diet  the  onset  of  the  condition  is 
delayed  and  the  total  incidence  is  greatly  reduced.    Still  more  in 


80  Discussion 

keeping  with  your  results  is  the  fact  that  if,  in  addition  to  the 
reduction  in  proteins,  the  total  intake  of  food  is  reduced,  amyloidosis 
in  this  strain  can  be  made  to  disappear  almost  completely.  In  the 
A  strain  of  mice  there  are  three  different  entities  which  tend  to  kill 
these  animals:  (1)  high  incidence  of  mammary  tumours  (but  only 
in  breeding  females),  (2)  high  incidence  of  lung  tumours,  and  (3)  the 
deposition  of  amyloid.  So  far  as  I  know  the  appearance  of  the  other 
two  conditions  cannot  be  easily  retarded. 

To  what  extent  were  your  rats  inbred  in  these  experiments,  Dr. 
Berg  ?  Were  they  genetically  homogeneous  or  were  you  dealing 
with  a  mixed  colony  ? 

Berg:  They  were  not  homogeneous  but  were  closely  related.  We 
have  two  lines  of  rats  that  have  been  randomly  inbred  for  65 
generations.  These  derive  from  a  small  group  of  Sprague-Dawley 
rats  acquired  in  1945.  Obviously,  if  the  rats  were  more  homogeneous 
the  curves  would  be  steeper  than  those  we  have  shown. 

Qruneherg:  That  being  so,  additional  information  might  be  ob- 
tained by  using  highly  inbred  strains  of  mice,  which  have  very  dif- 
ferent lifespans.  Some  strains  die  early  because  they  have  an  early 
onset  of  manunary  tumours,  such  as  the  C3H  strain;  and  the  I 
strain  has  a  short  lifespan  because  it  develops  lesions  in  the  stomach. 
Other  strains  differ  in  lifespan  for  reasons  which  are  not  yet  fully 
understood.  In  addition  one  can  use  homogeneous  but  not  inbred 
material  (first  generation  hybrids)  and  segregating  material  (Fg 
generations)  and  so  on.  It  should  also  be  possible  to  subdivide  the 
causes  of  mortality  further  by  systematic  changes  in  the  environ- 
ment, particularly  change  in  diet,  as  you  discussed,  but  also  by  dif- 
ferences in  crowding,  temperature,  etc.  If  this  were  done  it  would 
probably  turn  out  that  there  is  no  unique  life  expectation  of  the 
species ;  presumably  each  genotype  and  each  type  of  environmental 
situation  has  its  own  expectation  of  life,  and  in  man  the  survival 
curve  is  presumably  a  superimposition  of  a  whole  family  of  curves. 

Muhlbock:  Amyloidosis  is  one  of  the  major  diseases  in  mice.  In 
different  inbred  strains  there  are  differences  in  incidence  of  amyloido- 
sis. In  some  strains  there  is  a  very  high  incidence  at  18  months  of 
age,  whereas  in  other  strains  it  is  nil,  or  very  low.  Therefore  one 
should  investigate  a  number  of  different  strains  before  generalizing 
and  saying  that  for  that  species  this  is  the  age  of  onset  of  the  disease. 

Berg:  You  are  quite  right.  Our  results  apply  only  to  our  particular 
Sprague-Dawley  strain,  maintained  under  our  conditions. 

Sacher:  Amyloidosis  has  been  under  investigation  in  our  laboratory 
by  Dr.  S.  Lesher  (1957.  J.  nat.  Cancer  Inst,  19,  1119).  He  finds  a 
high  incidence  in  the  A  strain  and  in  F^  hybrids  with  an  A-strain 


Discussion  81 

parent.  Amyloidosis  in  mice  is  a  disease  with  maximum  incidence  in 
middle  age.  Incidence  falls  to  zero  in  later  life.  Thus  it  resembles 
some  diseases  in  man,  particularly  some  liver  diseases.  If  a  single 
X-ray  dose  is  given  to  the  young  animal,  this  whole  sequence  is 
moved  to  the  left  on  the  time  axis  so  that  a  given  incidence  of 
amyloidosis  is  seen  at  an  earlier  age,  and  about  the  same  total 
incidence  is  seen. 

Perks:  I  was  also  struck  by  the  symmetry  of  the  curves  of  distri- 
bution of  onset  of  the  different  lesions  in  your  rats,  Dr.  Berg.  We 
should  bear  in  mind  that  when  you  combine  symmetrical  curves  of 
this  kind  and  get  a  curve  of  onset  of  lesions  of  all  kinds,  you  do  not 
necessarily  reproduce  symmetry.  Further,  as  the  mean  delay 
between  onset  and  death  is  significantly  different  for  the  different 
lesions,  the  final  death  curve  could  well  be  highly  skew  as  compared 
with  the  component  symmetrical  curves  of  onset. 

The  other  point  I  was  particularly  interested  in  was  the  delay  in 
onset  for  females.  My  mind  always  goes  to  the  question  of  the  useful- 
ness of  statistics,  and  in  the  life  assurance  world  we  are  seriously 
concerned  with  the  differential  mortality  between  males  and  females, 
particularly  in  view  of  the  growing  volume  of  pension  business.  In 
this  country  there  is  a  difference  of  about  five  years  in  effective  age 
between  males  and  females  at  the  older  ages.  In  some  of  the  Scandi- 
navian countries  there  is  a  considerably  narrower  difference  and  the 
mortahty  for  males  is  much  lower  than  in  this  country.  We  actuaries 
do  not  really  know  why  male  and  female  mortalities  differ,  nor  do  we 
know  why  the  Scandinavian  male  mortality  is  much  more  favourable 
than  in  this  country,  although  some  of  us  have  thought  in  terms  of 
environmental  factors.  It  does  seem  to  me  that  there  may  be  some 
clue  in  the  figures  given  here  today.  If,  in  fact,  there  is  delay  in  the 
onset  of  some  of  these  degenerative  diseases  in  females  as  compared 
with  males,  maybe  the  answer  is  essentially  constitutional. 

Jalavisto:  In  Finland  the  excess  of  male  over  female  mortality 
is  about  the  highest  in  the  world,  especially  in  the  50-year-old  group. 
This  seems  to  be  because  coronary  death  is  so  common  in  Finland. 
I  have  the  impression  from  Finnish  data  that  at  that  age  or  in  that 
sex  which  has  a  greater  disposition  for  a  certain  disease,  it  is  very 
difficult  to  lower  the  mortality  in  that  disease  by  improving  external 
conditions.  For  example,  in  cholecystic  diseases  in  which  females 
have  a  higher  mortality  than  males,  the  reduction  which  has  taken 
place  in  recent  years  is  greater  in  the  male — although  in  most 
diseases  the  reduction  is  much  greater  in  females. 

Perks:  When  I  mentioned  the  Scandinavian  countries  I  was  not 
thinking  so  much  of  Finland  as  of  the  Netherlands,  Denmark, 


82 


Discussion 


Norway  and  Sweden — countries  which  have  an  exceptionally  low 
mortality  amongst  the  males. 

While  your  rats  were  in  captivity  and  being  observed,  Dr.  Berg, 
were  they  allowed  their  normal  reproductive  functions? 

Berg:  No.  The  experimental  animals  were  kept  separate  from  the 
breeders  and  w^re  never  mated. 

Verzdr:  The  rat  colony  kept  in  our  laboratory  has  a  50  per  cent 
survival  rate  at  23-5  months,  even  under  very  varying  conditions. 
(This  is,  of  course,  not  counting  early  deaths,  since  each  mother  is 
allowed  to  feed  only  four  or  five  young  ones.)  Spontaneous  death 
appears  more  frequently  after  the  tenth  month  of  age.   The  ages  of 


0       2      4      6       8       lb     12      14      16     18     20    22    24    26    28     30    32     34    36 

Age  in  months   (876  cf) 

Fig.  1  (Verzar). 

death  of  1,602  rats  (876  ^  and  726  ?)  are  shown  in  Figs.  1  and  2, 
which  also  show  the  number  of  tumours  present  in  these  animals. 
Tumours  first  appeared  in  8-month-old  animals,  in  both  males  and 
females.  The  number  is  much  higher  in  females  than  in  males  but  the 
occurrence  is  fairly  constant  during  life.  It  decreases  only  between 
the  29th  and  37th  months,  because  so  few  animals  survive.  This 
means  that  the  relative  quantity  of  tumours  in  all  deaths  is  smallest 
when  the  death  rate  is  highest,  i.e.  between  the  18th  and  24th 
months. 

The  main  cause  of  death  was  lung  disease  (bronchiectasis),  in 
which  the  lung  becomes  infected  and  has  large  amounts  of  white  pus. 
This  is  found  in  24  per  cent  of  females  and  35  •  5  per  cent  of  males. 
There  are  more  tumours  in  females  than  in  males  (21  per  cent  against 
11  per  cent)  and  this  difference  thus  counterbalances  the  greater 


Discussion 


83 


number  of  deaths  due  to  lung  disease  in  the  male.  Unexplained 
deaths  were  due  to  vertigo  as  signs  of  destruction  of  one  labyrinth 
were  found  (9-5  per  cent  in  females,  8-5  per  cent  in  males).  Other 
causes  of  death  which  were  diagnosed  account  for  about  16  per  cent. 
We  have  no  obvious  cases  of  nephrosis  leading  to  death,  but  all  rats 
over  the  age  of  10  months  have  proteinuria  and  this  becomes  very 
high  in  some  very  old  animals.  Very  old  animals,  of  about  36  months, 
generally  show  no  pathological  findings  which  could  explain  their 
death.    The  possibility  exists,  therefore,  that  natural  death  may 


20  n 


0      2      4      6      8      10      12     14     16     18     20     22    24    26     28    30    32    34    36 

Age  in  months   (7269) 

Fig.  2  (Verzar). 

occur  without  obvious  illness  being  suffered.  (About  15  per  cent  of 
our  rats  were  not  autopsied.) 

The  survival  curves  show  that  after  the  10th  month  "ageing" 
occurs  at  the  same  rate  as  resistance  to  external  damage  (infections) 
diminishes.  This  leads  to  maximal  mortality  at  about  23-5  months, 
which  is  also  the  time  of  survival  of  50  per  cent  of  all  individuals. 

Another  point  is  that  creatinuria  in  our  rats  begins  at  500  days,  at 
exactly  the  same  time  as  the  muscular  dystrophy  in  Dr.  Berg's  rats. 
Creatinuria  is  also  present  with  old  age  in  man. 

Rockstein :  How  much,  if  at  all,  did  you  extend  the  life  of  those  rats 
by  restricted  diets.  Dr.  Berg  ? 

Berg:  Lifespan  studies  have  not  been  completed.  At  800  days  the 
survival  rate  of  ad  libitum-ied  males  was  48  per  cent  as  compared 


84  Discussion 

with  87  per  cent  for  restricted  rats.  Incomplete  data  indicate  that 
the  onset  of  lesions  was  delayed  nine  to  twelve  months. 

Rockstein:  What  was  the  maximum  weight  attained  ? 

Berg:  The  body  weight  of  restricted  rats  was  25  per  cent  lower 
than  maximum  weight  of  ad  libitum-fed  animals. 

Rockstein:  Is  the  protein  content  restricted  to  the  same  extent  as 
the  caloric  value  of  this  diet  ? 

Berg:  The  protein  content  of  the  diet  was  20  per  cent  and  was  the 
same  for  the  restricted  diet. 

BourUere:  Have  you  measured  the  basal  metabolic  rate  in  both 
restricted  and  unrestricted  animals  of  the  same  age  ? 

Berg:  No,  I  have  not. 

Comfort:  In  view  of  the  interesting  similarities  between  the  life- 
span curves  in  man  and  in  rats,  it  pays  to  remember,  when  consider- 
ing dietary  restrictions,  that  there  are  differences  in  growth  patterns 
between  them.  Your  rats  show  virtually  determinate  growth,  but 
under  some  conditions  the  rat  grows  in  weight  and  in  bone  length  for 
most  of  its  life.  This  is  a  very  different  situation  from  that  in  man. 

Berg:  After  170  days,  skeletal  growth  practically  ceases  in  the  rat 
though  some  of  the  epiphyses  remain  open  for  the  entire  lifespan. 
There  is  no  evidence  of  osteogenesis  in  the  cartilage  plate  of  the 
tibial  epiphysis  of  old  rats.  Increments  in  body  weight  of  ageing  rats 
are  due  largely  to  fat  accumulation  associated  with  prolonged 
inactivity. 

Comfort :  But  my  point  was  that  in  man  you  could  not,  I  imagine, 
restrict  growth.  The  effect  of  underfeeding  on  the  growth  pattern 
and  on  the  appearance  of  sexual  maturity  in  man  may  be  different 
from  the  effects  you  can  produce  in  rats.  McCay  kept  his  rats 
infantile  for  over  1,000  days;  I  doubt  whether  a  comparable  effect 
could  be  produced  in  man. 

Berg:  I  think  that  if  we  had  a  comparable  inbred  strain  of  glutton- 
ous men,  and  could  perform  a  similar  experiment,  we  might  obtain 
results  corresponding  to  those  in  the  rat. 

Tanner:  I  am  accustomed  to  dealing  with  growth  data  rather  than 
with  data  dealing  with  the  other  end  of  the  lifespan.  But  methodo- 
logically there  are  very  great  similarities.  We  must  consider,  for 
example,  the  implications  of  the  use  of  chronological  age  in  all  these 
data.  One  possible  interpretation  of  your  data  could  be  as  follows. 
We  think  of  children  or  animals  growing  in  the  same  way  as  we 
think  of  trains  moving  along  a  railway  line.  You  can  think  of  the 
various  diseases  as  trapdoors  on  the  railway  line.  You  can  either 
consider  that  those  trapdoors  have  been  moved  nearer  the  start  so 
that  the  train  gets  to  them  earlier,  or  you  can  consider  that  the 


Discussion  85 

train  has  been  slowed  down  and  therefore  does  not  reach  the  trap- 
doors, which  have  not  altered  their  positions,  until  later.  Your 
graph  of  the  numbers  dying,  such  as  these  that  died  before  800  days, 
somewhat  supports  the  latter  interpretation.  You  have  got  straight 
lines  against  age,  but  the  slope  of  the  line  for  the  restricted  rats  is 
less  than  it  is  for  the  ad  libitum-ied  rats.  This  is  the  equivalent  of 
the  velocity  of  the  train — to  follow  my  analogy — being  less.  If  you 
altered  the  time  scale  you  could  superimpose  those  two  lines.  This 
is  a  possible  explanation,  though  not  necessarily  a  correct  one. 

The  specific  question  following  that  is  how  much  was  puberty,  the 
opening  of  the  vagina,  actually  delayed  in  your  rats  ? 

Berg:  There  is  a  delay  of  about  three  to  four  weeks. 

Tanner:  That  is  relatively  small.  We  do  not  really  know  how  an 
animal  measures  the  passage  of  time — except  that  it  is  not  by  a 
calendar  !  We  see  this  particularly  in  children,  and  we  have  various 
measures  of  what  we  call  developmental  age :  the  stage  of  ossification 
of  the  bones  of  the  wrist,  the  number  of  teeth  erupted,  the  menarche 
(the  first  menstrual  period  in  girls)  and  so  on.  These  stages  are  similar 
to  the  developmental  horizons  of  the  embryologists.  It  seems  to  me 
that  in  gerontology  we  are  all  badly  in  need  of  some  equivalent  of 
this. 

Berg:  We  all  recognize  that  various  functions  (such  as  sexual 
development,  bone  growth,  and  onset  of  lesions)  take  place  more 
rapidly  in  lower  animals  than  in  man.  This  difference  between 
species  in  rate  of  growth,  development  and  ageing  is  a  problem  in 
itself.  What  we  have  attempted  to  do  in  our  paper  is  to  show  the 
similarity  between  the  species  in  that  they  follow  the  same  laws  in 
regard  to  onset  of  lesions  and  death. 

Tanner:  The  growth  curves  of  man  and  rodents  (and  your  rodents, 
of  course,  are  living  in  dreadful  conditions)  are  really  considerably 
different.  But  the  growth  curves  of  primates  as  a  whole  are  very 
similar.  This  points  out  the  tremendous  importance  of  doing  this 
sort  of  work  with  the  cheapest  monkeys  you  can  get  hold  of. 

Berg:  The  rats  in  our  colony  really  live  under  very  fine  conditions. 
Except  for  being  in  captivity,  they  live  under  better  conditions  than 
most  humans.  However,  the  cost  of  these  conditions  is  very  high. 
A  similar  colony  of  monkeys  would  cost  millions  of  dollars. 

Comfort:  Monkeys  survive  extremely  poorly  in  captivity.  Another 
trouble  is  the  time  factor.  A  baboon  can  live  for  over  thirty  years 
(Duetz,  G.  H.  (1938).  Lab.  Rep.  zool.  Soc.,  Philadelphia,  66,  31). 

Verzdr:  You  have  just  underlined  one  of  the  main  points  of 
gerontological  research.  Dr.  Tanner — that  we  need  tests  of  biological 
age.   We  speak  far  too  little  about  tests,  and  all  our  work  should 


86  Discussion 

depend  on  them.  You  can  all  judge  the  age  of  a  man,  but  how  do  you 
do  it  ?  One  form  of  tests  in  rats  is  adaptation  methods :  with  ageing 
the  power  of  adaptation,  such  as  to  cold,  or  lack  of  oxygen,  decreases. 
You  can  also  measure  the  age  of  the  collagen  in  the  rat's  tail  tendon 
(see  Verzar,  F.  (1957).  Ciba  Found.  Coll.  Ageing,  3,  60.  London: 
Churchill).  But  then  it  turns  out  that  everything  ages  differently, 
and  rats  age  differently  in  their  brains  than  in  their  tendons.  We 
irradiated  rats  with  700  r.  and  they  died  quickly,  but  their  collagen 
had  not  aged. 

Holt:  I  was  going  to  raise  the  same  point,  because  my  experience 
is  also  in  the  comparison  of  growth  curves  which  are  completed  at 
relatively  different  rates  in  different  species.  The  dispersions  of  the 
age  of  incidence  curves  which  you  showed.  Dr.  Berg,  in  the  compari- 
son between  man  and  rat,  seemed  proportional  to  their  means.  You 
thought  that  the  curve  for  man  had  a  higher  dispersion  because  you 
were  dealing  in  that  case  with  a  heterogeneous  group ;  my  interpreta- 
tion was  that  both  distributions  were  equally  dispersed,  because  I 
mentally  converted  them  to  equivalent  relative  time  scales. 

Rotblat:  Have  you  ever  drawn  graphs  on  which  you  plot  age  not  in 
years  but  in  the  fraction  of  the  span  of  life,  so  that  you  can  compare 
the  spans  of  life  directly  ?  Otherwise  how  do  you  know  that  the 
onset  of  disease  is  the  same  in  the  rat  as  in  man  ? 

Berg:  Prof.  Simms  and  I  have  discussed  this  extensively.  He  feels 
that  such  a  plot  would  be  meaningless.  The  lifespan  is  determined 
by  the  age  of  onset  of  lesions.  Hence,  to  use  lifespan  as  a  standard 
for  comparing  age  of  onset  would  have  no  significance.  It  would  be 
like  comparing  the  speed  of  two  racehorses — not  in  terms  of  minutes 
per  mile — but  in  terms  of  minutes  per  mile  multiplied  by  miles  per 
minute. 

Rotblat:  From  the  change  in  the  slope  of  the  Gompertz  curve 
which  you  showed  us  I  would  expect  a  large  extension  of  the  time 
scale. 

Tanner:  This  works  out  as  the  equivalent  in  the  human  of  around 
17  to  18  years.  In  other  words  if  you  multiply  the  scale  30  :  1, 
which  is  roughly  right,  the  curves  you  showed  for  the  rat  would  be 
almost  superimposable  on  those  for  man. 

Rotblat:  This  is  what  I  wanted  to  know:  whether  they  are  really 
the  same  if  they  are  superimposed. 

Maynard  Smith:  To  me  the  most  surprising  thing  you  told  us,  Dr. 
Berg,  was  that  the  ages  of  onset  for  a  whole  variety  of  at  first  sight 
causally  unrelated  lesions  were  all  shifted  in  the  same  direction  by  the 
same  environmental  treatment,  i.e.  restriction  of  the  diet.  I  do  not 
believe  that  one  can  tell  very  much  about  the  causes  of  ageing  in  any 


Discussion  87 

organism  by  just  looking  at  curves.  But  if  you  experimentally 
interfere  with  the  conditions  and  find  that  those  curves  move,  then 
you  do  know  something,  and  that  seems  to  me  very  exciting.  People 
working  on  mice  have  mentioned  cases  where  specific  diseases  have 
been  shifted  to  a  younger  or  older  age  by  specific  environmental 
causes.  What  they  have  not  discussed  is  whether  either  dietary 
restrictions  or  irradiation,  or  any  other  environmental  treatment, 
have  a  common  effect  on  a  number  of  apparently  unrelated  diseases. 
If  you  delay  one  disease  in  the  mouse  by  restricting  diet,  do  you 
expect  to  delay  the  others  or  not  ?  This  is  of  enormous  theoretical 
importance  and  it  may  one  day  be  of  great  practical  importance. 

Rothlat:  It  has  been  dealt  with  to  a  certain  extent  by  Curtis,  who 
has  tried  six  different  environmental  effects  (2nd  International  Con- 
ference on  the  Peaceful  Uses  of  Atomic  Energy,  September  19.58). 

Sacher:  Curtis  reported  only  on  the  after-expectations,  and  not  on 
the  kinds  of  pathology  present. 

Maynard  Smith:  I  want  to  know  whether  the  ages  of  the  onset  of 
tumours,  of  kidney  diseases,  etc.,  are  shifted  in  the  same  direction 
by  the  same  environmental  causes. 

Berg:  Yes.  The  delay  in  onset  of  all  lesions  including  tumours 
produced  by  dietary  restriction  points  to  a  single  factor  that  controls 
the  time  of  onset  of  disease. 

Before  the  discovery  of  the  tubercle  bacillus  the  various  forms  of 
tuberculosis  involving  different  organs  were  considered  to  be 
different  diseases.  With  the  discovery  of  the  tubercle  bacillus  these 
conditions  were  found  to  be  various  consequences  of  a  single  cause. 
Although  this  analogy  is  not  exact  it  is  possible  that  a  single  mechan- 
ism may  be  involved  in  the  onset  of  many  widely  different  diseases. 

Wigglesworth :  An  even  better  analogy  is  that  of  malaria.  If  you 
reduce  malaria  in  a  region  the  mortality  from  many  other  diseases  is 
reduced. 

Have  you  had  the  opportunity  yet  to  switch  over  the  diet  at  some 
stage  of  life  in  your  experiments  ?  In  other  words,  is  it  indulgence  in 
youth  or  indulgence  in  age  which  is  significant  in  these  effects  ? 

Berg:  We  are  planning  such  experiments. 

Comfort:  I  can  think  of  two  factors  which  could  produce  exactly 
such  a  non-specific  effect  on  many  diseases.  One  is  the  so-called 
stress  response.  I  do  not  know  whether  you  measured  the  adrenal 
weight  in  these  creatures.  The  other  is  immunological ;  I  am  thinking 
of  auto-immunization  processes  taking  place  in  the  body,  and 
depending  on  the  escape  of  cell  antigens  with  the  passage  of  time. 
Either  of  those  could  quite  readily  produce  marked  changes  in  many 
apparently  unrelated  diseases. 


11 


88  Discussion 

Berg:  We  do  have  adrenal  weights  but  we  have  not  studied  them 
in  relation  to  time  of  onset  or  incidence  of  lesions. 

Comfort:  Do  these  rats  appear  to  produce  more  cortisone  than  the 
ordinary  animal  of  that  size  ? 

Berg:  I  do  not  look  upon  these  restricted  animals  as  being  under 
stress. 

Comfort:  Even  if  you  spend  your  life  in  a  deck  chair,  dietary 
retardation  is  still  a  physiological  stress.  These  animals  have  less  to 
eat  than  they  would  normally  have — although  wild  rats  do  not 
get  all  they  want  to  eat. 

Berg:  Within  certain  limits  a  state  of  hunger  in  the  restricted 
animal  appears  to  be  nearer  normal  for  the  rat  than  the  satiety  of 
the  ad  libitum-fed  rat. 

Comfort :  It  is  still  possibly  a  stress.  The  domestic  rat  is  the  result 
of  selection  for  equanimity  and  low  adrenal  weight.  Wild-caught 
rats  are  quite  unmanageable.  We  have  got  laboratory  animals 
which,  whether  we  like  it  or  not,  have  been  adapted  by  covert 
selection  to  living  under  conditions  of  captivity. 

Berg:  We  have  in  progress  stress  experiments  based  on  variations 
in  light,  noise,  and  other  unfavourable  conditions.  The  adrenal 
weights  of  these  rats  will  be  compared  with  the  adrenals  of  rats 
under  standard  conditions. 

Tanner:  Maynard  Smith  asked  about  the  possible  common 
mechanism  whereby  the  times  of  incidence  of  these  various  diseases 
could  all  be  brought  forward  together.  I  think  that  is  rather  a  dif- 
ferent situation  from  the  amyloid  disease  one.  As  Comfort  said,  this 
somewhat  nebulous  concept  of  stress  does  provide  a  basis.  I  have 
recently  been  to  the  Mental  Health  Research  Fund  conference  on 
"Stress  in  relation  to  mental  health  and  disorder"  at  Oxford  (1959. 
BlackweU's  Scientific  Publications,  in  press).  Prof.  Hans  Selye  was 
there  and  talked  about  stress  as  almost  equivalent  to  ageing.  The 
two  concepts  were  being  pushed  very  close  together.  Selye  dis- 
cussed some  very  interesting  data  on  the  effect  of  myocardial 
degeneration  of  various  balances  of  deoxycorticosterone-type 
hormones  and  cortisol-type  hormones.  He  evidently  regarded  the 
ordinary  circulation  of  the  blood  as  constituting  a  stress,  while  we 
would  regard  it  as  something  which  perhaps  produces  ageing.  It  is 
particularly  in  such  endocrinological  regulations  that  the  general 
mechanism  Maynard  Smith  is  querying  probably  lies. 

Sacher:  Ionizing  radiations,  which  are  normally  deleterious  and 
shorten  life,  can  in  some  circumstances  increase  life  expectation, 
although  without  increasing  the  maximum  lifespan.  When  this 
occurs  in  mice  and  rats,  it  is  observed  that  the  infectious  diseases 


Discussion 


89 


that  are  enzootic  in  the  particular  populations  (such  as  pneumonia 
in  the  rat,  various  kinds  of  enteric  infections  in  the  mouse)  have  a 
lower  incidence  in  the  irradiated  populations  than  in  the  controls. 
What  one  sees  then  is  a  much  more  nearly  rectangular  life-table  (less 
mortality  in  early  and  middle  life)  with  no  actual  increase  in  the 
maximum  span.  L.  D.  Carlson,  W.  J.  Scheyer  and  B.  H.  Jackson 
(1957.  Radiat.  Res,  7,  190)  at  the  University  of  Washington,  Seattle, 
found  evidence  of  this  sort,  as  did  E.  Lorenz  and  co-workers  (1954. 
In  Biological  Effects  of  External  X  and  Gamma  Radiation,  ed. 
Zirkle,  R.  E,  p.  24.  New  York:  McGraw-Hill).  They  irradiated  rats 
and  mice,  respectively,  with  small  daily  doses  of  gamma  rays.  We 
have  obtained  similar  results  at  Argonne  Laboratory  (unpublished). 


LIFESPANS  OF  MAMMALIAN  AND  BIRD 
POPULATIONS  IN  NATURE 

F.    BOURLIERE 

Centre  de  Gerontologie  Claude  Bernard  and 
Ldboratoire  de  Physiologie,  Faculte  de  Medecine,  Paris 

The  various  marking  and  banding  techniques  devised  by 
mammalogists  and  ornithologists  during  the  last  25  years 
have  provided  us  with  a  good  many  data  on  the  maximum 
lifespan  of  numerous  species  of  mammals  and  birds  belonging 
to  a  large  number  of  families  of  these  two  classes  of  verte- 
brates. There  are  still  too  many  gaps,  especially  for  some 
groups  peculiar  to  certain  geographical  areas  (such  as  tropical 
species  in  general,  small  Australian  marsupials),  or  for 
families  with  specialized  ecology  (as  cetaceans  among  mammals 
and  humming-birds  or  swifts  among  birds).  Nevertheless  we 
already  have  a  preliminary  idea  of  the  potential  lifespan  of 
most  families  living  in  temperate  countries. 

When  we  turn  from  individuals  to  populations,  the  situa- 
tion is  far  less  satisfactory.  Very  few  species  of  mammals  and 
birds  can  indeed  be  aged  accurately  or  have  been  marked  in 
sufficient  numbers  and  followed  long  enough  to  provide  us 
with  data  which  can  be  used  to  construct  adequate  life-tables 
of  natural  populations.  There  are  none  the  less  a  few  figures 
available  and  the  purpose  of  this  review  is  to  bring  together 
this  scattered  information,  bearing  in  mind  that  most  of  the 
data  at  hand  have  been  gathered  haphazardly  during  eco- 
logical studies  made  for  other  purposes  and  that  none  of  them 
are  quite  satisfactory  from  our  present  point  of  view. 

The  more  accurate  observations  we  have  for  mammals  con- 
cern some  of  the  larger  ungulates  which  are  of  interest  in  game 
management  in  Western  Europe  and  North  America.    All 

90 


Mammals  and  Birds  :  Lifespans  of  Wild  Populations  91 

these  ungulates  breed  once  a  year,  have  a  herbivorous  diet  and 
a  long  maximum  lifespan.  For  such  species  sources  of  eco- 
logical data  for  the  construction  of  life-tables  are  of  three 
kinds:  (1)  knowledge  of  age  at  death  for  an  adequate  and 
reasonably  random  sample  of  the  population;  (2)  knowledge 


1000 


500 


100' 


MALES 


OALL  SHEEP 


Fig.  1.  Survival  curves  for  males  of  five  ungulate 
populations. 


of  the  fate  of  individuals  of  a  single  cohort,  at  frequent 
intervals;  and  (3)  knowledge  of  the  age  structure  among  the 
living.  As  Deevey  (1947)  pointed  out,  the  first  and  third  types 
of  information  can  be  used  only  if  one  is  prepared  to  assume 
that  the  population  is  stable  in  time. 

The  survival  curves  of  males  and  females  of  four  species 
(roe  deer,  red  deer,  black-tailed  deer  and  dall  sheep),  based  on 


92 


F.    BOURLIERE 


the  ly.  columns  in  their  Hfe-tables,  are  shown  in  Figs.  1  and  2. 
Survival  curves  of  both  sexes  taken  together  are  given  in 
Fig.  3  for  barren-ground  caribou.  Original  figures  on  which 
these  calculations  were  based  have  been  published  by  Evans 
(1891)  for  Cervus  elaphus  of  the  island  of  Jura  off  Scotland,  by 


FEMALES 


black-tailed  oeer 
(chaparral) 


OALL  SMEEP 


I 

Fig 


2,    Survival  curves  for  females  of  five  un- 
gulate populations. 


Murie  (1944)  for  Ovis  dalli  of  Mount  McKinley  National  Park 
in  Alaska,  by  Andersen  (1953)  for  Danish  Capreolus  capreolus 
of  the  Game  Research  farm  of  Kalo  and  by  Taber  and  Das- 
mann  (1957)  for  Odocoileus  hemionus  of  California.  Data  for 
Canadian  Rangifer  arcticus  are  from  Banfield  (1955).  All 
these  populations,  except  that  of  dall  sheep,  were  hunted, 
either  by  sportsmen  or  natives. 


Mammals  and  Birds  :  Lifespans  of  Wild  Populations  93 

When  we  compare  these  survival  curves,  we  can  quite 
clearly  distinguish  some  interesting  similarities  and  differences. 

(1)  A  very  steep  initial  slope,  indicating  a  very  high  mortal- 
ity during  the  first  year  of  hfe,  is  found  in  both  sexes  of  all  the 
species.  The  actual  mortality  rate  for  calves  is  probably  even 
greater,  since  their  skulls  ar^  more  easily  overlooked  and  more 


Fig.  3.   Survival  curve  graduated  on  a  logarithmic  scale, 
for  a  series  of  292  barren-ground  caribou. 


quickly  destroyed  than  those  of  the  adults.   This  age  class  is 
therefore  quite  probably  under-represented  in  most  samples. 

(2)  During  the  second  year  of  life  there  is  a  small  loss  in  both 
sexes  of  all  species,  with  but  two  exceptions,  that  of  the  male 
roe  deer  where  there  is  emigration  and  that  of  the  male  black- 
tailed  deer  of  the  Californian  chaparral  where  some  yearlings 
are  killed. 

(3)  During  the  third  year  of  life  there  is  a  heavy  loss  among 
male  and  female  roe  deer  and  male  black-tailed  deer  from  both 


94  F.    BOURLIERE 

range  types.  The  roe  deer  loss  is  due  to  emigration  (Andersen, 
1953)  and  that  of  the  black-tailed  deer  to  hunting  (Taber  and 
Dasmann,  1957).  Among  the  male  dall  sheep  and  the  male 
red  deer  there  is  little  loss  during  the  third  year. 

(4)  From  the  fourth  year  onward,  to  old  age,  the  hunted 
populations  (roe  deer,  black-tailed  deer  and  red  deer)  show 
fairly  steep  losses  in  both  sexes.  The  rate  of  loss  tends  to 
lessen  in  full  adulthood  in  the  male  black-tailed  deer,  pre- 
sumably because  learning  and  behaviour  make  these  individ- 
uals less  vulnerable  to  hunting  (Taber  and  Dasmann,  1957). 
In  the  red  deer,  on  the  contrary,  the  rate  of  loss  becomes 
heavier  in  full  adulthood  because  of  the  selection  of  prime 
stags  by  sportsmen. 

(5)  The  dall  sheep,  which  is  not  hunted,  shows  very  little 
loss  from  adulthood  to  9  years  old.  If  it  were  not  for  hunting, 
the  other  ungulate  populations  would  probably  exhibit 
survival  curves  rather  more  similar  to  those  of  Ovis  dalli.  As 
Taber  and  Dasmann  (1957)  pointed  out,  it  is  nevertheless 
doubtful  that  they  could  ever  attain  as  high  a  survival  as  long 
as  their  ranges  were  fully  stocked  and  starvation  was  a  com- 
mon cause  of  death.  In  that  connexion,  it  is  interesting  to 
note  that  the  survival  curve  of  the  barren -ground  caribou, 
which  is  hunted  mainly  by  natives,  is  closer  to  that  of  the  dall 
sheep  than  to  that  of  hunted  deer. 

(6)  In  old  age  there  tends  to  be  in  most  cases  a  steepening 
of  the  survival  curve ;  this  accelerated  loss  may  be  due  directly 
or  indirectly  to  senescence.  In  dall  sheep  we  know,  for  instance, 
that  both  the  very  young  and  very  old  animals  were  preferably 
killed  by  wolves.  Heavily  hunted  species  do  not  display  such 
a  pattern  because  the  high  kill  permits  few  individuals  to  grow 
old. 

On  the  whole,  it  seems  that  most  of  the  differences  between 
these  various  populations  of  wild  ungulates  are  not  inherent 
in  the  species,  but  rather  imposed  by  environmental  condi- 
tions. If  a  roe  deer  population  is  fenced,  emigration  is  pre- 
vented but  winter  mortality  due  to  starvation  becomes  high. 


Mammals  and  Birds  :  Lifespans  of  Wild  Populations  95 

In  such  a  case,  it  would  be  expected  that  the  population 
dynamics  would  be  different  from  those  found  by  Andersen 
(1953)  in  Kalo;  the  older  animals  would  be  competing  with 
younger,  physiologically  more  efficient  animals.  Similarly,  if  a 
dall  sheep  herd  were  not  culled  by  predators,  the  population 
would  presumably  be  limited  by  food  supplies,  and  the 
mortality  among  prime  adults  would  increase  accordingly. 
Taber  and  Dasmann  are  thus  certainly  right  in  pointing  out 
the  danger  of  considering  the  population  dynamics  of  a  given 
animal  under  given  circumstances  as  typical  of  that  species  in 
general. 

All  the  species  of  long-lived  and  slow-breeding  ungulates 
considered  above,  belonging  either  to  hunted  or  unhunted 
populations,  show  in  most  cases  age-specific  mortality  rates. 
The  situation  seems  very  different  in  small  mammals,  which 
are  both  short-lived  and  fast-breeding.  All  the  species  investi- 
gated so  far  appear  to  have  age-constant  mortality  rates 
(after  very  early  life). 

In  his  study  of  the  survival  of  wild  brown  rats  on  a  Mary- 
land farm,  Davis  (1948)  shows,  for  instance,  that  no  more  than 
about  5  per  cent  of  the  rats  live  for  a  year.  In  the  tropical 
environment  of  Malayan  jungles,  the  situation  looks  very 
much  the  same  for  the  12  species  or  subspecies  of  Murids 
studied  by  Harrison  (1956).  Table  I  indicates  the  estimate  of 
mean  survival  rates  per  month  for  marked  animals,  together 
with  the  mean  and  maximum  length  of  life  (in  months)  and 
the  age  at  which  only  5  per  cent  of  the  population  can  be 
expected  to  survive  (effective  maximum,  95  per  cent). 

The  white-footed  mice  (Peromyscus  leucopus)  of  the  George 
Reserve  in  south-eastern  ^lichigan  have  likewise  a  very  low 
survival  rate  (Snyder,  1956).  With  the  high  rate  of  mortality 
of  the  winter  1950-1951,  only  one  mouse  in  a  thousand  could 
be  expected  to  reach  an  age  of  93  weeks;  with  the  lower 
rate  of  the  previous  winter,  34  mice  could  be  expected  to  reach 
93  weeks,  and  at  least  one  would  probably  reach  197  weeks. 
The  mean  length  of  life  from  birth  for  such  individuals  would 


96 


F.    BOURLIERE 


be  17-4  and  31-5  weeks  respectively.  Such  figures  contrast 
sharply  with  a  potential  longevity  of  six  to  eight  years, 
recorded  in  captivity. 

The  same  situation  occurs  in  the  Tulare  kangaroo  rat. 
Fitch  (1948)  reported  that  35-2  per  cent  of  the  Dipodomys 
heermani  taken  during  a  four-year  live-trapping  programme 

Table  I 

Summary  of  survival  figures  for  12  species  and 

SUBSPECIES    OF    RATS    IN    MaLAYA    (AFTER    HARRISON,    1956) 


Species 

Survival 

rate 

per 

month 

Length  of  life  in  months 

Mean 

Maximum 
recorded 

Effective 

maximum 

95% 

Chiropodomys  gliroides 

Rattus  rattus  diardii    $ 

R.  rattus  diardii  ^ 

R.  rattus  jalorensis,  sheltered 

R.  rattus  jalorensis,  scrub 

R.  rattus  argentiventer 

R.  rattus  jarak 

R.  exulans 

R.  miilleri 

R.  bowersi 

R.  whiteheadi 

R.  rajah 

R.  sabanus 

R.  canus 

0-88 
0-75 
0-72 
0-88 
0-76 
0-85 
0-90 
0-73 
0-84 
0-85 
0-75 
0-86 
0-78 
0-82 

7-8 
3-5 
30 
7-8 
3-6 
6-2 
90 
3-2 
5-6 
6-2 
3-5 
6-5 
41 
50 

26 

14 

4 

9 
10  + 

9  + 
10  + 

23 
10-5 

9 
22 
11 
18 
28 
10 
18 
18 
10 
20 
12 
15 

had  records  that  extended  over  not  more  than  one  month, 
34  •  2  per  cent  had  records  of  one  to  six  months  on  the  study 
area,  and  only  4-7  per  cent  had  records  extending  for  more 
than  a  year.  The  longest  record  was  33  months  for  an  in- 
dividual marked  as  a  partly-grown  juvenile.  Other  instances 
of  short  expectation  of  life  at  birth  in  wild  rodents  are  reported 
by  Blair  (1953)  and  Bourhere  (1954). 

Larger  rodents  have  a  shghtly  longer  mean  longevity  in 
natural  conditions.   Kalabouchov  (1933)  found  that  31-5  per 


Mammals  and  Birds  :  Lifespans  of  Wild  Populations  97 

cent  of  a  population  of  little  souslik  {Citellus  pygmaeus) 
reached  the  age  of  one  year  in  the  Caucasus  and  Fitch  (1947) 
reported  9  to  38  per  cent  of  cottontail  rabbits  reaching  one 
year  in  central  California. 

Shrews  appear  even  more  short-lived  than  rodents.  In 
England,  Crowcroft  (1956)  estimated  that  the  common  shrew 
{Sorex  araneus)  lives  for  only  18  months  at  the  longest  and 
that  most  individuals  die  before  reaching  one  year  of  age. 

Bats,  on  the  contrary,  seem  to  have  a  higher  survival  rate 
than  other  mammals  of  similar  size.  The  expectation  of 
life  in  the  colony  of  Myotis  mystacinus  ringed  by  Sluiter,  Van 
Heerdt  and  Bezem  (1956)  in  Holland,  was  4*4  years,  with  a 
maximum  lifespan  exceeding  20  years!  Such  longevity  is  quite 
unexpected  in  such  small  mammals  and  probably  has  some- 
thing to  do  with  the  very  peculiar  metabolic  pattern  of  these 
animals. 

To  sum  up  the  available  data  on  mammals,  it  seems  certain 
that,  in  natural  conditions,  small  and  fast-breeding  species, 
with  a  low  ratio  of  mean  to  potential  duration  of  life,  show 
very  high  and  age-constant  mortality  rates.  The  only 
exception,  that  of  bats,  is  probably  due  to  the  rather  peculiar 
physiology  of  these  animals.  On  the  other  hand,  large  and 
slow-breeding  species,  with  a  high  ratio  of  mean  to  potential 
duration  of  life,  tend  to  have  age-specific  mortality  rates; 
this  pattern  is  nevertheless  strongly  influenced  by  ecological 
conditions. 

In  birds,  we  find  the  same  difference  as  in  mammals  between 
small  and  fast-breeding  species  on  the  one  hand  and  relatively 
large  and  slow-breeding  ones  on  the  other.  Most  of  the 
available  data  concerning  this  group  have  already  been  sum- 
marized by  Lack  (1954),  Farner  (1955)  and  Hickey  (1955)  and 
need  not  be  mentioned  again  here.  We  will  therefore  limit 
ourselves  to  a  few  examples. 

The  best  life-table  we  have  for  any  bird  of  relatively  large 
size  is  that  of  the  common  terns  {Sterna  hirundo)  studied  by 

AGEING — ^V — 4 


98 


F.    BOURLIERE 


2^9S.   ,    ■    I    I    I    .    ,    ,    ,    I 


I    I    I    »    » 


<     1    T     T    I     I    f 


Years  of  age 

Fig.  4.    Survival  curve  of  a  cohort  of  common 

terns  banded  as  chicks  in  1934  in  Cape  Cod 

colonies  (After  Austin  and  Austin,  1956). 


Austin  and  Austin  (1956).  Adults  (6,965)  banded  as  chicks  in 
the  Cape  Cod  colonies  were  subsequently  trapped  in  the  same 
places  by  these  ornithologists.  To  overcome  the  usual  bias 
caused  by  band  loss  in  long-lived  sea  birds,  the  Austins  took 
the  precaution  of  adding  new  bands  to  every  bird  wearing  a 
band  it  had  carried  eight  or  more  years.  Their  results  are 
therefore  more  reliable  than  those  of  other  observers.   Fig.  4 


Mammals  and  Birds  :  Lifespans  of  Wild  Populations  99 

shows  the  survival  curve  of  a  cohort  of  these  terns  which  were 
banded  as  chicks  in  1934. 

In  this  sample,  the  first  year  mortality  was  over  94  per  cent 
and  the  mean  annual  mortality  rate  from  the  fourth  to  the 
18th  year  averaged  26  per  cent.  Beyond  the  18th  year  the 
curve  continues  downwards,  showing  a  steady  increase  in  a 
mortality  rate  that  had  remained  a  straight  line  during  the 
previous  14  years.  The  continuous  rebanding  of  all  the  older 
birds  handled  during  this  study  reduces  the  possibility  that 
this  sudden  increase  in  the  death  rate  can  be  explained  by 
band  loss ;  the  Austins  therefore  consider  that  it  might  be  due 
to  senility.  The  composite  life-table  based  on  all  the  returns 
of  common  terns  banded  as  chicks  and  trapped  in  nests  by 
these  observers,  1940  through  1955,  shows  the  same  increase 
in  the  death  rate  after  the  18th  year. 

This  tendency  towards  an  increased  mortality  rate  in  old 
long-lived  birds  may  be  counteracted  by  a  progressive  im- 
provement in  the  survival  rate  as  the  birds  get  older,  at  least 
in  the  species  which  are  heavily  hunted  by  man.  Inexperienced 
immature  or  young  adults  appear  to  be  shot  much  more 
frequently  than  older  ones.  Such  an  improvement  in  the 
survival  rate  of  older  individuals  is  quite  apparent  in  the 
survival  curve  of  Scandinavian  buzzards  (Buteo  huteo)^  com- 
mon herons  (Ardea  cinerea)  and  tawny  owls  {Strix  aluco) 
drawn  by  Olsson  (1958)  and  shown  in  Fig.  5. 

In  small  and  short-lived  passerines,  the  situation  is  very 
similar  to  that  of  the  small  rodents  and  shrews,  and  the 
population  turnover  is  very  rapid.  The  mortality  rate  is 
always  very  high,  especially  at  the  nestling  stage  and  in  the 
first  four  months  of  life.  Summers-Smith  (1959)  has  found,  for 
instance,  a  mortality  rate  of  about  87  per  cent  for  the  juveniles 
(one  to  four  months  old)  and  about  40  per  cent  for  the  adults 
in  the  urban  populations  of  house  sparrows  {Passer  domesticus) 
he  has  studied.  We  have  found  even  higher  figures  in  a  tropical 
population  of  the  red-billed  fire  finch  {Lagonosticta  senegala) 
now  under  study  in  the  lower  Senegal  valley.  Kluijver  (1951) 


100 


F.    BOURLIERE 


has  found  an  average  annual  adult  mortality  of  49  per  cent 
for  great  tits  (Parus  major)  in  Holland. 

The  lowest  adult  mortality  rate  for  small  birds  is  found 
among  swifts,  where  it  averages  only  18  to  20  per  cent  per 


1000 


500- 


100  - 


5  10 

AGE   IN  YEARS 

Fig.  5.  The  number  {Ix)  of  surviving  buzzards, 
herons  and  tawny  owls  at  beginning  of  age  group 
{x).  Abscissa :  Age  in  years.  Ordinate :  Numbers 
of  surviving  individuals,  on  a  logarithmic 
scale. 


year.   It  should  be  remembered  that  swifts,  like  bats,  have  a 
very  poor  temperature  regulation. 

The  lowest  figures  in  the  whole  class  of  birds  are  those  of 
two  sub-antarctic  birds.  In  the  yellow-eyed  penguin  (Mega- 
dyptes  antipodes)  population  studied  by  Richdale  (1957),  the 
adult  mortality  rate  was  only  12-9  per  cent  per  year;  in  the 


Mammals  and  Birds  :  Lifespans  of  Wild  Populations  101 

royal  albatross  {Diomedea  epomophora)  this  figure  even  goes 
down  to  3  per  cent.  A  summary  of  investigations  on  mortality 
rates  in  non-passerine  birds  is  given  in  Table  II  (based  on 
Farner,    1955   and  completed  after  Bendell,   1955;   Hickey, 

Table  II 

Summary  of  ustvestigations  ox  mortality  rates 
IX  xox-passerixe  birds 


Approximate  range  of 

Approximate  range  of 

Order 

juvenile  mortality 

adult  annual  mortality 

rates 

rates 

/o 

o/ 

/O 

Sphenisciformes 

13-30 

Procellariiforines 

(Diomedeidae) 

about  3 

Pelecaniformes 

( Phalacrocoracidae ) 

35-80 

12-30 

Ciconiiformes 

(Ardeidae) 

about  60 

about  30 

Anseriformes 

(Anatidae) 

50-85 

17-65 

Falconiformes 

(Accipitridae) 

about  60 

about  30 

Galliformes 

20-50 

50-83 

Charadriiformes 

(Charadrii) 

15-50 

(Lari) 

40-60 

18-30 

Columbiformes 

(Columbidae) 

about  80 

55-58 

Strigiformes 

(Tytonidae) 

50-79 

28-57 

(Strigidae) 

about  50 

about  30 

Apodiformes 

(Apodidae) 

about  30 

18-20 

1955;  Lack,  1956;  Richdale,  1957;  Boyd,  1957, 1959;  Summers- 
Smith,  1959;  and  Southern,  1959).  The  adult  mortality  rates 
given  there  apply  after  the  first  0-5  to  1-5  years,  according 
to  the  species  or  groups  concerned.  The  juvenile  mortality 
rates  are  calculated  for  a  year  beginning  with  the  fledgling 
leaving  the  colony,  or  sometime  later  during  the  summer  of 
hatching. 


102  F.    BOURLIERE 

We  therefore  find  very  much  the  same  dichotomy  in  birds 
as  in  mammals,  between  small  and  fast-breeding  species  on  the 
one  hand  and  large  and  slow-reproducing  ones  on  the  other. 
The  highest  ratio  of  mean  to  potential  lifespan  is  indeed 
found  in  a  bird,  the  royal  albatross,  which  reproduces  only 
every  second  year  and  may  reach  an  age  of  at  least  25  years  in 
the  wild. 

Reproductive  and  mortality  rates  are  thus  closely  adjusted. 
Both  vary  mainly  with  ecological  conditions  and  an  increase 
in  mortality  rate  in  older  individuals,  which  may  be  due  to  the 
onset  of  old  age,  is  apparent  only  in  large,  slow-reproducing 
and  long-lived  birds  and  mammals. 

REFERENCES 

Andersen,  J.  (1953).   Dan.  Rev.  Game  Biol.,  2,  127. 

Austin,  O.  L.,  and  Austin,  O.  L.,  Jr.  (1956).   Bird  Banding,  27,  55. 

Banfield,  a.  W.  F.  (1955).   Canad.  J.  Zool.,  33,  143. 

Bendell,  J.  F.  (1955).   Canad.  J.  Zool.,  33,  195. 

Blair,  W.  F.  (1953).   Advanc.  Genet.,  5,  1. 

BouRLiERE,  F.  (1954).   The  Natural  History  of  Mammals.   New  York: 

Knopf. 
Boyd,  H.  (1957).   Bird  Study,  4,  80. 
Boyd,  H.  (1959).   Ibis,  101,  in  press. 
Crowcroft,  p.  (1956).  Proc.  zool.  Soc.  Lond.,  127,  285. 
Davis,  D.  E.  (1948).   Ecology,  29,  437. 
Deevey,  E.  S.  (1947).    Quart.  Rev.  Biol,  22,  283. 
Evans,   H.   (1891).     Some  Account  of  Jura  Deer.   Derby,   privately 

printed. 
Earner,  D.  S.  (1955).    In  Recent  Studies  in  Avian  Biology,  p.  397. 

Urbana :  University  of  Illinois  Press. 
Fitch,  H.  S.  (1947).   Calif.  Fish  Game,  33,  159. 
Fitch,  H.  S.  (1948).  J.  Mammal,  29,  5. 
Harrison,  J.  L.  (1956).   Bull.  Raffles  Mus.,  27,  5. 
Hickey,  J.  J.  (1955).    In  Recent  Studies  in  Avian  Biology,  p.  326. 

Urbana:  University  of  Illinois  Press. 
Kalabouchov,  N.  I.  (1933).   Rec.  Trav.  Sci.  Univ.  Moscou,  1,  29. 
Kluijver,  H.  N.  (1951).   Ardea,  39,  1. 
Lack,  D.  (1954).  The  Natural  Regulation  of  Animal  Numbers.  Oxford: 

Clarendon  Press. 
Lack,  D,  (1956).   Swifts  in  a  Tower.   London:  Methuen. 
MuRiE,  A.  (1944).   The  Wolves  of  Mount  McKinley.  Washington:  U.S. 

Dept.  Int.  Nat.  Parks  Service. 


Mammals  and  Birds:  Lifespans  of  Wild  Populations  103 

Olsson,  V.  (1958).  Acta  Vertebratica,  1,  86. 

RiCHDALE,  L.  E.  (1957).    A  Population  Study  of  Penguins.    Oxford: 

Clarendon  Press. 
Sluiter,  J.  W.,  Van  Heerdt,  P.  F.,  and  Bezem,  J.  J.  (1956).   Arch. 

neerl.  ZooL,  12,  63. 
Snyder,  D.  P.  (1956).   Misc.  PubL,  Mus.  ZooL  Univ.  Mich.,  95,  1. 
Southern,  H.  N.  (1959).   Ibis,  101,  in  press. 
Summers-Smith,  D.  (1959).   Ibis,  101,  in  press. 
Taber,  R.  D.,  and  Dasmann,  R.  F.  (1957).   Ecology,  38,  233. 

DISCUSSION 

Rotblat :  Is  anything  known  about  the  lifespans  of  the  same  species 
in  captivity  ? 

Bourliere:  Not  for  the  swift,  because  it  is  at  present  impossible  to 
keep  them  caged.  What  we  have  are  good  figures  on  the  maximum 
lifespan  of  some  individuals.  We  also  know  that  in  both  large  and 
small  mammals  and  birds  the  maximum  lifespan  in  captivity  is 
always  far  greater  than  in  the  wild.  Nevertheless,  in  wild  popula- 
tions, at  least  in  those  species  for  which  we  have  data,  a  very  small 
percentage  of  very  old  individuals  is  found ;  but  in  order  to  find  these 
very  scarce  old  animals,  you  need  to  study  a  very  large  population 
for  a  very  long  time. 

Rotblat:  I  understand  that  bats  kept  in  zoos  live  much  longer  than 
the  20  years  which  you  mentioned  for  the  albatrosses. 

Bourliere:  Twenty-five  years  is  the  longest  recorded  lifespan  for 
the  royal  albatross  in  the  wild  in  New  Zealand,  but  as  far  as  I  know, 
nobody  has  ever  kept  an  albatross  in  captivity  for  more  than  a  few 
years  because  it  is  very  difficult  to  feed  them. 

Comfort:  A  chaffinch  has  been  kept  for  29  years  (Moltoni,  E. 
(1947).  Riv.  ital.  Orn.,  17,  139),  and  even  an  inbred  budgerigar  is 
reported  to  have  reached  nearly  20. 

Scheidegger:  One  swift  in  a  big  colony  near  Basle  lived  for  18  years, 
but  the  rest  had  an  average  age  of  about  5  to  6  years. 

Danielli:  Your  remarks  about  birds  which  survive  longest  being 
large  does  not  fit  in  with  data  for  the  swift,  as  you  pointed  out.  Prof. 
Bourliere.  Do  you  attribute  the  advantage  the  swift  seems  to  possess 
to  the  fact  that  it  more  or  less  hibernates  ? 

Bourliere:  That  explanation  was  advanced  by  Farner  (1955)  and 
it  may  be  true  because  we  have  the  very  same  phenomenon  in 
mammals.  We  may  compare  rodents  and  bats  of  similar  size  and 
weight.  Small  mice,  for  instance,  never  live  in  captivity  or  in  the 
wild  for  more  than  four  years,  whereas  bats  of  the  same  weight  will 
live  for  20  years ;  so  there  is  certainly  some  correlation  between  a  long 


104  Discussion 

maximum  lifespan  and  the  ability  to  lower  the  body  temperature 
and  the  basal  metabolic  rate  for  more  or  less  prolonged  periods. 

Danielli:  Have  experiments  been  done  in  which  groups  of  animals 
which  normally  hibernate  have  been  prevented  from  doing  so  ? 

Bourliere:  At  the  present  time  I  know  of  no  good  observations 

*    which  have  been  made  on  mammals.   The  difficulty  is  not  only  to 

house  a  large  number  of  animals  during  30  or  40  years,  but  also  to 

secure  an  investigator  who  could  study  such  a  problem  for  three 

decades. 

Danielli:  Even  if  one  did,  of  course,  you  would  still  be  up  against 
some  difficulty  in  interpreting  the  facts,  because  it  seems  to  me  that 
the  advantage  which  is  gained  may  either  be  that  the  "biological 
time-scale"  is  altered  by  hibernating,  or  alternatively  the  hibernat- 
ing animal  may  be  protected  from  all  accidents  and  so  forth.  One 
does  not  know  which  of  these  two  alternatives  is  involved. 

Comfort:  Many  small  birds  presumably  die  in  winter.  If  a  species 
hibernates  it  has  not  got  to  search  for  food,  and  so  it  is  not  so  liable 
to  die  from  lack  of  it. 

Bourliere:  In  swifts  there  is  no  true  hibernation  during  winter, 
because  they  migrate  to  tropical  Africa  at  that  time  of  the  year,  but 
their  temperature  control  is  nevertheless  very  different  from  that  of 
passerines.  The  studies  on  swifts  which  have  been  made  in  Oxford 
and  Switzerland  by  D.  Lack  and  E.  Weitnauer  have  shown  (see  Lack, 
1956)  that  during  bad  weather,  especially  in  early  spring  when  the 
parent  swifts  are  unable  to  obtain  enough  flying  insects  to  feed  their 
young,  the  young  then  undergo  a  kind  of  pseudo-hibernation  or 
torpid  stage  instead  of  dying  as  other  species  do.  J.  Huxley,  C.  S. 
Webb  and  A.  T.  Best  (1939.  Nature  (Lond.),  143,  683)  have  des- 
cribed the  same  feature  in  adult  humming-birds  at  night,  and  they 
are  also  long-lived  animals.  One  humming-bird  lived  in  captivity  in 
the  Copenhagen  Zoo  for  more  than  eight  years,  which  is  quite  a 
record  for  a  bird  so  difficult  to  keep  in  captivity. 

Danielli:  Humming-birds  might  be  the  right  material,  as  they  have 
this  diurnal  "hibernation". 

Maynard Smith:  In  the  terns  there  was  a  very  low  mortality  in  the 
second  and  third  years  of  life.  Is  this  associated  with  the  fact  that 
this  species  does  not  breed  until  its  fourth  year  ? 

Bourliere:  Yes,  common  terns  do  not  return  to  breed  in  quantity 
until  their  fourth  summer. 

Sacher:  Have  the  life-tables  of  animal  populations  in  wild-life 
preserves,  such  as  the  European  and  American  bison,  been  studied  ? 

Bourliere :  As  far  as  I  know,  no  such  studies  have  ever  been  made 
on  bison  in  America  or  in  Europe.    The  first  good  study  on  the 


Discussion  105 

behaviour  and  ecology  of  the  American  bison  was  pubhshed  a  year 
ago  (McHugh,  T.  (1958).  Zoologica,  N.Y.,  43,  1),  and  we  are  still 
waiting  for  a  similar  work  on  the  European  form.  The  so-called  wild 
European  bison  are  in  fact  so  domesticated  that  they  are  not  a  good 
example  to  study.  If  you  want  to  have  samples  large  enough  to  be 
studied,  you  need  to  choose  rather  common  species  which  can  live 
in  national  parks  or  some  place  where  human  distui'bance  is  very 
rare.  That  is  why  most  of  the  available  information  has  been  drawn 
from  the  field  of  game  management  or  rodent  control. 

Chitty:  Did  you  say  there  was  no  age-specific  mortality  rate  for 
small  rodents  in  the  wild  ? 

Bourliere:  I  do  not  know  of  any  study  showing  such  an  age- 
specific  mortality  rate  in  wild  rodents. 

Chitty:  I  do  not  really  see  how  this  kind  of  information  could  be 
obtained  very  easily  for  wild  populations.  Such  evidence  as  I  have 
published  (1952.  Phil.  Trans.  B.,  236,  505)  shows  that  there  is  a 
higher  mortality  rate  with  increasing  age  but  of  course  it  is  exceed- 
ingly difficult  to  separate  the  environmental  components  from  it. 
There  is  an  increase  in  mortality  rate  as  the  winter  goes  along,  but  it 
is  not  known  whether  that  is  because  of  changing  ecological  condi- 
tions, or  because  of  an  increase  in  average  age. 

Kershaw:  This  may  be  compared  with  observations  on  insects  in 
the  wild.  There  seems  to  be  evidence  now  that  the  survival  of 
insects  with  a  rapid  population  turnover  is  modified  by  predators 
and  natural  hazards,  whereas  those  with  a  long  and  slow  population 
turnover  maintain  their  own  intrinsic  survival.  In  mosquitoes  it 
seems  likely  that  the  intrinsic  survival,  having  a  Gompertz  function 
with  a  sloping  straight  line,  is  altered  completely  by  field  conditions, 
and  has  a  flat  Gompertz  function.  For  the  last  ten  years  we  have 
been  studying  the  life-cycle  of  one  of  the  West  African  flies,  Chrysops, 
which  turns  over  once  a  year.  In  the  laboratory  that  fly  has  a 
normal  rectangular  survival,  both  in  those  bred  from  the  pupae  and 
in  wild-caught  flies.  We  have  been  following  through  natural 
populations  of  flies  coming  in  to  bite  man,  which  of  course  is  a 
selective,  but  functionally  selective,  population.  We  have  found  that 
throughout  the  year  the  population  is  made  up  of  separated  succes- 
sive cohorts,  each  behaving  with  its  own  particular  rectangular 
survival,  so  that  in  this  particular  fly  the  intrinsic  survival  is  the 
natural  one.  I  think  one  has  to  go  back  to  mosquitoes  now  that 
one  has  biological  markers  for  them,  based  on  parity  and  so  forth,  to 
see  whether  what  is  true  of  Chrysops  is  also  true  of  mosquitoes.  This, 
of  course,  is  of  importance  in  producing  mathematical  models. 


ARTERIOSCLEROSIS  IN  BIRDS 

S.  SCHEIDEGGER 
Institute  of  Pathology,  University  of  Basle 

Arteriosclerosis  ranks  first  among  diseases  affecting  the 
arteries,  and  it  is  of  much  more  importance  in  human  beings 
than  in  animals.  All  experiments  designed  to  produce  such  a 
form  of  degeneration  in  the  vessels  of  animals  are  doubtful, 
since  a  severe  form  of  this  disease  can  only  be  found  in  man. 
In  animals  we  never  find  such  severe  forms  as  regards  the 
spreading  of  the  disease  or  transformation  of  the  tissue,  and  it_ 
can  never  be  described  as  a  disease  producing  clinical  symp- 
toms. In  other  words,  whereas  arteriosclerosis  is  one  of  the 
principal  diseases  in  man  and  a  frequent  cause  of  death,  in 
animals  it  is  only  of  secondary  significance,  showing  only 
slight  malformations,  possibly  with  transformation  of  the^ 
vessel  wall.  The  term  arteriosclerosis,  which  etymologically 
means  a  hardening  of  the  vessels,  in  fact  stands  for  a  combina- 
tion of  different  lesions.  At  the  onset  of  the  disease,  we  only 
find  some  inclusions  in  the  wall.  In  the  final  stage  there  is 
accumulation  of  degenerations,  malformations,  and  trans- 
formations ;  all  layers  of  the  vessel  walls  are  now  affected  and 
we  can  see  calcifications,  ossifications,  and  often  an  occlusion 
of  the  lumen.  In  other  cases  the  same  calcification  and  ossi- 
fication process  can  produce  an  enlargement  of  the  vessel.  At 
the  beginning  of  the  illness  we  can  see  a  series  of  various  re- 
actions of  unknown  and  doubtful  origin.  Some  fox^ms  of 
arteriosclerosis  constitute  pure  inflammations,  whereas  some 
can  be  classified  as  pure  degenerations.  In  some  cases  this 
disease  of  the  vessels  is  restricted  to  a  single  organ  or  to  a 
specific  system,  i.e.  it  may  be  found  only  in  the  vessels  of  the 
heart,  or  the  vessels  of  the  brain,  or  perhaps  the  finer  vessels 

106 


Arteriosclerosis  in  Birds  107 

of  the  extremities.  It  is  a  known  fact  that  the  vessel  can  be 
affected  in  various  ways.  The  disease  can  be  a  diffuse  one, 
and  then  the  whole  vessel  is  in  an  arteriosclerotic  state. 
Alternatively,  the  severe  degeneration  of  the  vessel — often  the 
arteria  coronaria — is  restricted  to  small  defined  areas.  With 
this  form,  we  speak  of  a  so-called  arteriosclerosis  in  plaques. 
Furthermore,  we  know — and  this  is  another  important  factor 
— that  the  organ  has  an  influence  on  the  development  and 
type  of  arteriosclerosis.  In  the  brain,  for  example,  ^ve  never 
find  the  same  forms  of  vessel  degenerations  as  in  the  kidneys 
or  in  the  heart.  In  the  brain  vessels,  especially  in  the  finer 
vessels  of  the  brain  substance,  we  never  find  atherosclerosis 
with  deposits  of  fat  and  cholesterol  esters,  etc.  In  this  organ 
we  have  a  more  diffuse  hyaline  transformation,  in  others  fine 
fibrillar  changes,  a  so-called  fibrillar  vessel  degeneration, 
similar  to  that  which  can  be  seen  in  the  neuroglial  tissue  (of 
the  neurofibrillar  changes  and  in  the  senile  plaques  of  the 
Alzheimer  disease).  This  form,  which  Scholz  (1938;  Scholz 
and  Nieto,  1938)  was  the  first  to  describe,  is  a  typical  mal- 
formation of  the  brain  vessel.  The  final  stage  is  always  the 
same :  the  occlusion  of  the  vessel.  This  occlusion  is  the  only 
pathological  symptom  which  can  be  found.  The  pathological 
process  and  the  way  of  evolution  can,  however,  vary. 

In  most  observations  of  arteriosclerosis  it  is  not  possible  to 
give  a  key  to  its  origin  and  development.  We  know  that 
arteriosclerosis  of  the  vessels  of  the  heart,  which  develops 
only  in  restricted  areas,  is  often  the  result  of  an  inflammation 
whereas  the  diffuse  forms  mostly  result  from  a  primary  pure 
degeneration.  Experimental  studies  of  this  disease  are  not 
possible.  It  is  possible  that  some  types  of  arteriosclerosis  are 
the  cause  of  either  an  acute  or  a  chronic  arteritis.  The  acute 
arteritis  can  in  turn  be  the  result  of  an  infection  with  bacteria 
or  with  toxins.  In  the  place  of  the  inflammation  of  the  vessel 
wall,  parietal  thrombi  develop.  The  bacterial  infections  may 
disappear.  After  the  inflammation  a  degenerative  form 
of  arteriosclerosis  may  occur.    An  injured  vessel  wall,   or 


108  S.    SCHEIDEGGER 

infiltration  by  parasites,  can  often  be  the  causative  factor  in 
chronic  arteritis.  Domestic  animals,  especially  cattle,  dogs 
and  pigs,  show  a  slight  thickening  of  the  arterial  walls  through 
proliferations  of  the  intimal  tissue.  Scherer  (1944)  describes  a 
case  of  a  20-year-old  chimpanzee  with  sclerosis  of  the  main 
vessel  of  the  brain  without  degeneration  of  the  tissue.  In  four 
Macacus  rhesus  monkeys  the  same  author  could  observe  fine 
degenerations  of  the  cortex  of  the  brain  with  multiple  small 
areas  showing  a  softening  of  the  tissue.  In  the  cerebellum  the 
granular  layer  was  in  a  state  of  degeneration  with  multiple 
areas  in  which  the  cells  had  disappeared.  In  birds  more  severe 
forms  of  such  degenerations  can  be  seen.  Two  different 
phenomena  may  be  the  cause :  some  species  of  birds  reach  an 
advanced  age,  and  some  are  (and  this  is  another  important 
fact)  purely  carnivorous.  The  distribution  and  the  anatomy 
of  arteriosclerosis  in  birds  present  some  difficulties.  In  most 
cases  the  aorta  is  the  seat  of  the  malformation.  The  aorta 
valves  are  free  of  lesions.  In  the  intima  layers  we  often  find 
hyperplasia  with  an  increasing  of  fibres.  The  media  shows  a 
muscular  granularity  in  this  part  of  the  vessel  wall.  Micro- 
scopical examination  of  such  vessels  shows  a  media  with 
ruptured  muscle  fibres  and  and  a  split  or  broken  elastica.  The 
intima  is  often  covered  with  a  fibrocellular  exudate. 

One  of  the  best  and  most  complete  reports  on  the  problem  of 
arteriosclerosis  in  birds  has  been  published  by  Fox  (1923),  who 
gives  a  survey  of  the  different  types  of  diseases  affecting  the 
animal  in  captivity  and  in  the  wild  state.  To  illustrate  the 
problem  the  author  has  collected  some  thousands  of  post- 
mortem examinations  in  the  Washington  Zoo.  Psittaci  have  a 
high  percentage  of  arterial  disease.  Some  findings  are  of 
interest.  Often  the  central  vessels  are  not  the  principal  seat  of 
an  atheromatosis  as  in  other  classes,  and  the  lesion  shows 
reactions  with  tissue  proliferation.  Accipitres  have  the  great- 
est percentage  of  any  order.  The  arterial  lesions  are  frequently 
accompanied  by  renal,  myocardial,  and  valvular  disease. 
Degenerations  are  equally  severe  in  the  media  and  intima. 


Arteriosclerosis  in  Birds  109 

Calcifications  are  not  seldom  found  in  this  group  of  birds.  In 
parrots  the  arterial  damage  is  caused  in  the  arteria  carotis  or 
in  the  small  wing  arteries,  but  the  most  common  seat  of  the 
process  is  the  lower  thoracic  region.  Galli  often  demonstrate  a 
vascular  disease  combined  with  myocardial  disease.  Cormo- 
rants, pelicans  and  gannets  often  have  arteriosclerotic  mal- 
formations and  intimal  proliferations.  Ducks  and  geese 
present  a  considerable  number  of  cases  which  demonstrate 
arteriosclerotic  degenerations  accompanied  by  cardiac  and 
general  pathology.  The  character  of  the  lesions  is  similar  to 
that  found  in  the  Accipitres.  Arteriosclerosis  is  common  to 
many  zoological  orders.  Fox  gives  a  good  general  review  of 
this  problem.  The  order  of  percentage  incidence  is  Accipitres 
6-6;  Anseres  3-4;  Psittaci  1-8;  GalK  1-6;  Passeres  0-22. 
Carnivorous  birds  have^the  highest  incidence  of  chronic^ 
arterial  disease.  Next  in  order  are  the~ungulates,  the  anserine 
birds  and  the  carnivorous  mammals.  Fox  remarks  on  the 
interesting  fact  that  the  orders  with  great  activity,  such  as  the 
primates  and  the  passeres,  are  at  the  end  of  the  list.  This  is 
perhaps  due  to  the  fact  that  their  food  consists  chiefly  of 
carbohydrates. 

Arteriosclerosis  in  mammals  and  birds  in  captivity  is  often 
accompanied  by  nephritis,  chronic  infectious  disease  or  chronic 
enteritis. 

In  some  cases  of  this  disease,  aneurysm  in  the  vessels  can 
occur.  This  develops  above  the  valves  and  arises  from  a 
degenerative  arteritis.  The  present  author  has  examined 
cases  of  birds  with  slight,  medium,  and  severe  arteriosclerosis. 
These  observations  are  all  autopsies  in  the  Basle  Zoo.  The 
animals  were  vultures,  storks,  cranes,  flamingoes  and  geese. 

Milder  forms  of  this  disease  are  quite  common.  In  these 
cases  the  aorta  showed  a  thickening  of  the  intima  and  plaques 
could  be  seen,  especially  where  the  main  vessels  branch 
off.  In  these  parts  we  found  small  deposits  of  fat  or  a 
pure  fibrosis  without  cholesterol  deposits  or  atheromatous 
ulcerations.    (See  Figs.  1-10.) 


110  S.    SCHEIDEGGER 

In  all  the  cases  observed  the  elastic  membranes  were  split 
off  or  interrupted.  In  the  medium  forms  of  atherosclerosis 
yellow  areas  could  be  seen  in  the  vessel  wall  and  with  increas- 
ing age  the  disease  became  more  severe. 

Wolkoff  (1925)  described  a  typical  arteriosclerosis  in  a  40- 
year-old  parrot.  Observations  of  arteriosclerosis  in  a  85-  and 
a  42-year-old  parrot  with  severe  atheromatosis  are  reported 
by  Nieberle  (1931),  Nieberle  and  Cohrs  (1931),  by  Pallaske 
(1930),  and  by  Beneke  (1931).  In  his  thesis  Fahr  (1935)  dis- 
cussed arteriosclerosis  in  chickens. 

The  comprehensive  article  written  by  Krause  (1939)  about 
the  pathology  of  animals  states  that  the  beginning  of  the  aorta 
is  not  the  place  of  the  primary  degeneration.  He  found  that  in 
most  cases  the  principal  seat  of  this  disease  was  the  part  of  the 
aorta  between  the  kidneys.  He  reported  that  in  his  observa- 
tions atheromatous  ulcerations  could  never  be  seen.  The 
intima  was  always  intact,  hardened  and  thickened  by  a 
fibrosis.  Inflammatory  processes  are  not  of  importance.  In 
his  opinion  lipoidosis  and  sclerosis  are  two  diff'erent  processes. 
He  found  the  primary  infiltration  of  fat  in  the  aorta  between 
the  kidneys,  never  in  the  aorta  ascendens  or  in  the  arcus 
aortae.  Such  forms  of  degeneration  could  be  observed  in  5  to 
6-year-old  poultry.  A  calcification  could  very  seldom  be 
found.  He  suggests  that  only  in  some  cases  with  avitaminosis 
or  hypervitaminosis  can  a  primary  necrosis  produce  small 
calcareous  deposits. 

Fahr  (1935)  suggests  that  no  connexion  exists  between  the 
degree  of  the  infiltration  of  fat  and  age.  Sclerosis  is,  however, 
a  process  that  can  only  be  found  in  advanced  age.  He  was 
never  able  to  see  a  coronary  sclerosis.  In  these  studies  of 
arteriosclerosis  in  birds  only  one  animal  developed  an  arterio- 
sclerotic disease  of  sufficient  clinical  significance  to  cause 
severe  functional  disturbances.  A  vulture  nearly  60  years 
old  developed  an  arteriosclerotic  disease  with  such  clinical 
symptoms  as  can  be  found  in  man.  The  cause  of  death  was  a 
diffuse  vascular  sclerosis.    All  the  different  stages  could  be 


lit.  ' 


'<^'"'^ 


Fig.  1.   Diffuse  pure  mucoid  degeneration  with  a  fine  lipoidosis 
in  the  arteria  carotis  {Sarcorhamphus  gryphus  L.). 


K|?jaCv«% 


«    .  -  -*  w. 


i-*^  •    ^-^' 


■!^^ 


Fig.  2.   Diffuse  thickening  of  tlie  aorta  by  infiltration  of  mucoid 

and  fat  substances  with  degeneration  of  the  elastic  membranes 

{Balaeniceps  rex  Gould). 

facing  page  110 


Fig.  3.    Arteriosclerosis  of  the  basilar  artery.    Thickening  of  the 
intima  with  degeneration  of  the  media  {Sarcorhamplms  gryphus  L.). 


Fig.  4.    Coronary  vessels  with  a  fine  thickening  of  the  intima  and 
deposits  of  cholesterol  esters  and  fat  (Sarcorhamphus  gryphus  L.), 


^i*w«;if^"''^^i;^pft^BB^ 


•  ~  ^  #■  T.ir-^'*'' 


Fig.  5.    Diffuse  infiltration  of  mucoid  substances  in  all  the  dif- 
ferent layers  of  the  vessel  wall,  with  degeneration  of  the  media 
(Phoenicopterus). 


Fig.  6.    xVorta  of  the  red-breasted  goose  with  degeneration  of 

all  the  different  layers  of  the  vessel  wall  and  a  splitting  off  of  the 

elastic  membranes  in  the  media  {Branta  ruficollis  Pall.). 


Fig.  7.    Thickening  of  the  media  and  intima  with  a  fine  calci- 
fication   and    multiple    small    deposits    of    cholesterol    esters 
{Balaeniceps  rex  Gould). 


X 


0^. 


tf 


Fig.  8.    Severe  thickening  of  the  arterial  wall  by  large  deposits 
of  fat  and  cholesterol  esters  {Balaeniceps  rex  Gould). 


Fig.  9.      Vascular     hyalinosis     with     perivascular     softening 
{Sarcorhamphus  gryphus  L.). 


Fig.  10.     Sections  throuoh  the  brain  of  a  vulture  showing  post- 
malacic  cyst  secondary  to  cerebral  arteriosclerosis,  with  enlarge- 
ment of  the  lateral  ventricle  {Sarcorhampfms  gryphus  L.) 


Arteriosclerosis  in  Birds  111 

seen  in  this  case:  proliferations  of  the  intimal  cells,  with  con- 
centric lamellations  which  completely  filled  the  lumen,  hyaline 
transformation,  swelling  and  necrosis  of  the  media,  com- 
pression of  the  lumen,  medial  calcification  and  ossification. 
Atheromatous  plaques  were  found  in  the  main  vessels.  In  the 
observation  under  review  the  concentric  lamellations  and  the 
necrosis  of  the  media  produced  a  compression  of  the  lumen  of 
the  vessel  which  was  accompanied  by  a  softening  of  the  brain 
with  subsequent  formation  of  cysts.  The  coronary  vessels 
showed  a  high  degree  of  arteriosclerosis.  The  lumen  was  often 
nearly  obstructed  by  intima  proliferations.  Several  infarctions 
of  the  myocardium  could  be  seen. 

The  present  author's  suggestion  is  that  only  in  captivity 
can  such  severe  forms  of  arteriosclerosis  occur.  In  nature  the 
animal  concerned  would  have  died  earlier  of  hunger  since  the 
higher  functions  of  life  would  have  been  handicapped.  The 
atherosclerosis  induced  by  several  conditions  in  animals  may 
be  reversed  by  removing  those  conditions  but  there  is  no 
evidence  that  the  same  possibility  exists  as  far  as  the  disease  in 
human  beings  is  concerned.  Atheromatosis  in  the  human  adult 
must  be  considered  as  irreversible.  Atherosclerosis  is  induced 
in  rabbits  and  chickens  by  adding  cholesterol  in  large  quan- 
tities to  the  food.  But  animals  species  differ  greatly.  The 
concentration  of  cholesterol  in  the  blood  is  a  very  important 
factor  in  atherosclerosis  both  in  the  human  being  and  in 
animal  species.  Atherosclerosis  can  be  seen  in  any  artery  but 
the  major  concern  is  the  coronary  artery.  It  is  common  in 
young  persons,  and  quite  common  m  persons  over  30,  but  this 
type  of  degeneration  cannot  be  seen,  or  extremely  rarely,  in 
animals. 

Arteriosclerosis  is  of  much  more  importance  in  human  beings 
than  in  animals.  This  fact  is  important.  Unfortunately  there 
is  no  possibility  of  studying  this  disease  which  so  frequently 
affects  the  human  being,  in  animal  experiments. 


112  S.    SCHEIDEGGER 

REFERENCES 

Beneke,  R.  (1931).   Beitr.  path.  Anat.,  87,  285. 

Fahr,  H.  O.  (1935).   Dissertation:  Giessen. 

Fox,  H.  (1923).  Disease  in  Captive  Wild  Mammals  and  Birds.  Phil- 
adelphia: Lippincott. 

Krause,  C.  (1939).  Ergebn.  allg.  Path.  path.  Anat.,  34,  226,  367. 

NiEBERLE,  K.  (1931).    Verh.  dtsch.  path.  Ges.,  26,  239. 

NiEBERLE,  K.,  and  Cohrs,  P.  (1931).  Lehrbuch  der  spez.  path.  Anatomie 
der  Haustiere.   Jena:  Fischer. 

Pallaske,  G.  (1930).  Frankfurt.  Z.  Path.,  40,  64. 

Scherer,  H.  J.  (1944).  Vergleichende  Pathologic  des  Nervensystem  der 
Saugetiere.  Liepzig :  Georg  Thicme. 

ScHOLZ,  W.  (1938).    Z.  ges.  Neurol.  Psychiat.,  162,  694. 

ScHOLZ,  W.,  and  Nieto,  D.  (1938).   Z.  ges.  Neurol.  Psychiat.,  162,  675. 

WoLKOFF,  K.  (1925).   Virchows  Arch.  path.  Anat.,  256,  751. 

DISCUSSION 

Verzdr:  Was  there  any  difference  in  the  diet  of  the  birds  which 
had  arteriosclerosis  and  those  which  had  not  ?  Diet  is  important  in 
view  of  the  present  theories  that  arteriosclerosis  is  connected  with 
fatty  acids. 

Scheidegger:  All  these  birds  are  carnivores,  and  it  seems  to  me  that 
this  is  one  of  the  most  important  factors  in  arteriosclerotic  disease. 
The  birds  all  came  from  the  Basle  Zoo,  and  this  form  of  arterio- 
sclerotic disease  is  seen  especially  in  vultures,  flamingoes,  cranes  and 
so  on.  I  have  never  seen  severe  forms  of  arteriosclerosis  in  other 
birds  than  the  kinds  mentioned  here. 

BourUere:  Can  it  really  be  said  that  arteriosclerosis  in  birds  is 
always  related  to  a  carnivorous  diet  ?  European  flamingoes  are 
"mud-eaters",  feeding  mainly  on  small  crustaceans  and  inverte- 
brates, and  these  kinds  of  food  should  contain  a  lot  of  unsaturated 
fatty  acids.  Geese  also  feed  as  much  on  plants  as  on  invertebrates. 
In  some  zoos  flamingoes  may  receive  a  different  diet  than  they  do  in 
the  wild. 

Scheidegger:  In  Basle  Zoo  the  flamingoes  ate  crustaceans  and  so 
on,  and  they  also  received  small  pieces  of  meat  in  this  food.  I  do  not 
know  about  geese. 

Hinton:  I  know  there  are  a  lot  of  differences  between  arthropods 
and  the  rest  of  the  animal  kingdom,  but  small  crustaceans  are  bits 
of  meat,  aren't  they  ? 

Rockstein:  Arteriosclerosis  has  been  observed  in  the  common  fowl, 
of  course,  and  it  is  primarily  a  herbivore,  so  I  do  not  think  one  can 
generalize  so  easily  on  this  point. 


Discussion  113 

Lindop :  There  are  two  studies  going  on  in  this  country  on  athero- 
sclerosis in  poultry  (Saxl,  H.,  in  press;  Hall,  D.  A.,  in  press).  One  is 
done  by  the  Ovaltine  workers  who  are  keeping  two  groups  of  animals 
on  identical  diets,  but  one  group  has  limited  exercise.  These  groups 
are  being  followed  for  the  development  of  atherosclerosis.  The 
other  study,  at  Leeds,  is  more  on  the  cytological  and  the  biochemical 
side.  There  they  have  been  able  to  produce  atherosclerosis  in  poultry 
which  have  limited  exercise,  and  they  have  also  been  able  to  reverse 
atherosclerosis  in  poultry  which  have  been  allowed  to  exercise  after 
they  had  been  inhibited.  Atherosclerosis  developing  in  zoo  animals 
might  therefore  be  caused  by  the  comparatively  limited  exercise 
they  get. 

Scheidegger :  The  problem  really  concerns  the  age  of  these  birds. 
This  old  vulture  lived  in  the  zoo  for  about  5Q  years.  It  came  as  a 
young  bird,  at  the  age  of  3  or  4  years,  and  the  food  was  always  the 
same.  In  its  last  year  the  bird  had  severe  arteriosclerosis.  The  ages 
of  the  other  birds  are  not  known,  and  I  cannot  tell  you  what  the 
birds  had  to  eat  in  captivity. 

Jalavisto :  Orma  has  made  observations  on  cholesterol-fed  poultry 
and  it  seems  that  even  a  little  exercise  may  be  enough  to  retard 
development  of  atherosclerosis.  He  put  the  food  on  one  side  of  a 
fence,  and  the  water  on  the  other  side.  The  poultry  drink  after  they 
have  eaten  and  therefore  have  to  go  over  the  fence  and  back  again  all 
the  time.  That  could  be  very  easily  arranged  in  a  zoo,  and  it  would 
be  one  means  of  checking  easily  whether  it  is  exercise  or  something 
else  which  is  operative  in  this  case. 

Nigrelli:  Arteriosclerosis  and  atherosclerosis  can  be  induced 
experimentally  by  dietary  methods  in  poultry,  as  Dr.  Lindop  said, 
and  I  think  there  is  some  relationship  with  the  pantothenic  acid  in 
the  diet. 

Comfort:  The  blood  pressure  of  these  birds  in  relation  to  their  con- 
figuration may  have  some  effect.  It  is  just  conceivable  that  birds  of 
the  flamingo  type,  which  have  both  a  long  neck  and  a  considerable 
change  of  posture  between  their  head-erect  and  head-down  positions, 
have  larger  fluctuations  in  blood  pressure  than  other  animals.  It  is  a 
point  which  needs  investigation. 

The  other  point  is  that  I  am  quite  sure  all  zoo  birds  and  animals 
are  better  fed,  or  more  consistently  fed,  than  they  are  in  the  wild. 
I  rather  suspect  that,  like  Dr.  Berg's  rats,  these  birds  would  be  less 
likely  to  get  atherosclerosis  if  they  were  only  fed  intermittently.  I 
suspect  that  in  the  wild  the  vulture  does  not  feed  every  day,  or  even 
every  week,  unless  he  is  lucky. 

Bourliere:  I  understand  that  a  study  is  already  under  way  in 


114  S.    SCHEIDEGGER 

South  Africa  concerning  the  blood  pressure  of  the  giraffe.  It  was 
found  to  be  extremely  high,  but  as  far  as  I  know  no  increase  in 
arteriosclerosis  was  observed  in  that  species,  at  least  in  the  wild. 

Comfort:  There  is  the  slight  drawback  that  the  giraffe  is  such  an 
extremely  timid  animal  that  even  in  a  zoo  it  is  a  nightmare  to  file 
its  hooves,  or  anything  like  that — you  have  to  give  it  an  anaesthetic. 
It  must  be  rather  a  difficult  animal  on  which  to  estimate  the  blood 
pressure,  unless  you  could  telemeter  it. 

Rockstein:  The  birds  which  show  this  atherosclerotic  condition 
seem  to  be  among  the  longest-lived  birds.  For  example  I  have  here 
records  of  a  common  crane  which  lived  to  more  than  42  years,  a 
flamingo  to  more  than  22,  a  stork  to  30,  and  a  vulture  to  close  to  60 
years  of  age.  The  question  is  whether  atherosclerosis  is  indeed  a 
factor  in  longevity  as  such  ? 

Berg:  Prof.  Scheidegger's  presentation  underlines  again  the 
necessity  of  knowing  causes  of  death  in  lifespan  studies.  Important, 
too,  is  the  question  of  whether  degenerative  diseases  are  inevitable 
with  ageing. 

Kershaw:  The  Royal  Air  Force  has  been  doing  routine  post- 
mortems on  its  fittest  people — those  who  fly  jet  planes.  Most  of 
these  are  aged  around  19,  20,  and  21.  In  the  coronary  arteries  of 
these  men  a  high  and  astonishing  degree  of  coronary  atheroma  was 
found.  Whether  that  means  that  most  of  us  have  vessels  which  have 
atheroma  in  them  which  is  of  no  significance,  or  whether  that 
particular  selected  group  is  more  likely  to  have  atheroma,  is  not 
clear. 

Danielli:  It  should  be  perfectly  easy,  shouldn't  it,  to  get  post- 
mortems on  the  average  young  adult  killed  in  an  accident  ? 

Kershaw:  The  difficulty  lies  in  getting  comparable  objective 
reports. 


RELATION  OF  LIFESPAN  TO  BRAIN  WEIGHT 
AND  BODY  WEIGHT  IN  MAMMALS* 

George  A.  Sacher 

Division  of  Biological  and  Medical  Research, 
Argonne  Natioiial  Laboratory,  Lemont,  Illinois 

My  interest  in  the  comparative  study  of  lifespans  stemmed 
from  the  reahzation  that  the  zoological  literature  contains  a 
wealth  of  data  that  can  be  used  to  analyse  the  factors  govern- 
ing ageing  and  longevity  in  mammalian  species.  In  this  paper  I 
shall  first  describe  a  statistical  analysis  of  the  relations  of  life- 
span to  brain  and  body  weight,  and  then  enter  into  a  discus- 
sion of  the  theoretical  issues.  These  considerations  have  been 
alluded  to  briefly  in  previous  communications  (Sacher,  1957, 
1958). 

The  objective  of  the  empirical  analysis  is  to  establish  the 
quantitative  dependence  of  the  lifespans  of  mammalian 
species  on  the  body  weights,  brain  weights,  and  metabolic 
rates  of  adult  representatives  of  these  species.  This  is  in  effect 
a  study  of  allometric  relationships,  in  which  lifespan  is  regarded 
as  a  physical  dimension  of  a  species  on  the  same  footing  as  the 
linear  or  mass  dimensions.  In  this  paper  the  species  lifespan 
is  defined  as  the  maximum  documented  longevity  for  that 
species.  With  a  few  exceptions  the  lifespan  data  come  from 
domesticated  animals  or  from  zoo  animals. 

There  were  two  reasons  for  using  the  lifespan  rather  than 
the  life  expectation.  The  first  is  that  satisfactory  estimates  of 
the  life  expectations  even  now  are  available  for  only  a  few 
domesticated   species  and  for  an   even  smaller  number  of 

*  This  work  was  performed  under  the  auspices  of  the  U.S.  Atomic  Energy 
Commission, 

115 


116 


George  A.  Sacher 


captive  species  (Comfort,  1956,  1958).  Second,  when  data 
come  from  animals  kept  under  very  different  environmental 
conditions  the  lifespan  is  a  more  stable  longevity  parameter 
than  is  life  expectation.   This  is  clearly  seen  in  the  life-tables 


1 0,000  c — r 


o 
o 
o 

o" 


q: 
ui 

Q. 


1000 


100 


10 


Thoroughbre,d    MarftS 
A.  Comfort,    J.  Ge^otitol 
13:342-350,    1958 


X 


12 


16 
AGE, 


20 
years 


24 


28 


32 


Fig,  la.  Logarithm  of  age-specific  rate  of  mortality 
plotted  versus  age  (Gompertz  plot).  Data  on  thorough- 
bred mares  from  Comfort  (1958).  A  life-table  for 
which  the  Gompertz  plot  has  a  straight-line  relation 
to  age  conforms  to  the  Gompertz  equation  qx  =  Ae'^^. 


of  human  populations  in  different  countries  or  in  the  same 
country  in  different  historical  periods.  Instances  can  be  found 
in  which  life  expectations  vary  by  more  than  a  factor  of  two, 
but  even  in  these  extreme  cases  the  lifespans  do  not  differ  by 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    117 

as  much  as  20  per  cent.  There  is  a  good  reason  for  this,  as  will 
be  discussed  below. 

One  objection  that  has  been  raised  to  the  use  of  lifespans  is 
that  the  estimated  lifespan  will  increase  as  the  sample  on 
which  the  estimate  is  based  increases.  Until  recently  there 
was  no  comprehensive  statistical  treatment  of  this  question. 
With  the  publication  of  Gumbel's  (1958)  treatise  on  the 
Statistics  of  Extremes,  we  now  have  a  statistical  theory  that 
is  adequate  to  deal  with  most  questions  that  arise.  The 
characteristic  oldest  age  (the  age  attained  when  one  survivor 
remains  of  the  initial  cohort)  is  an  easily  computed  statistic  in 
terms  of  which  we  can  discuss  the  dependence  of  lifespan  on 
cohort  size.  If  the  survivorship  curve  is  of  the  Gompertz  type, 
in  which  the  age-specific  rate  of  mortality  increases  exponenti- 
ally with  age,  the  characteristic  oldest  age  increases  as  a  double 
logarithmic  function  of  the  cohort  size.  This  is  illustrated  in 
Fig.  1,  where  the  characteristic  oldest  age  is  shown  in  relation 
to  cohort  size  for  an  actual  life-table  of  the  Gompertz  type, 
drawn  from  Comfort's  analysis  (1958)  of  the  life-table  of 
thoroughbred  mares.  It  can  be  seen  that  increasing  the  cohort 
size  by  a  factor  of  10^  would  increase  the  characteristic  oldest 
age  by  only  one-third.  The  lifespan  itself,  i.e.  the  age  at  death 
of  the  oldest  survivor,  would  vary  more  slowly  than  this. 

The  typical  mammalian  life-table  can  be  adequately  des- 
cribed by  the  Gompertz-Makeham  equation,  in  which  the 
relation  of  rate  of  mortality  to  age  contains  a  constant  term 
in  addition  to  the  Gompertz  term: 

q^  =Ae^  +  B  (1) 

The  Makeham  term,  B,  is  markedly  influenced  by  environ- 
mental conditions,  whereas  the  Gompertz  term  is  influenced 
to  only  a  small  degree.  However,  the  Gompertz  term  will 
always  dominate  at  advanced  ages,  and  the  lifespan  therefore 
tends  to  behave  like  an  extremum  statistic  of  a  Gompertz  life- 
table.  Since  the  inherent  ageing  parameters  characteristic  of 
the  species  are  embodied  in  the  parameters  A  and  a  of  the 


118 


George  A.  Sacher 


40 


12     35 

o 


lU 

o 

< 


to 

UJ 

o 
o 

o 

I- 


o 

< 
cr 

< 

X 

o 


30  - 


25  - 


20   - 


15   - 


10   - 


5   - 


1        1 

I    ■  ■       !              1              1 

^^..-^"""^"^ 

- 

^ 

yE 

— 

X 

- 

/ 

- 

X 

- 

. 

/ 

/ 

G- Gompertzion,    Bq  =22yr.. 

/ 

/e 

1        1 

E-  Exponential,    Sq  =   2  yr. 
1              1              1              1 

10 


10' 


I0> 


10^ 


10= 


10^ 


SIZE    OF    SAMPLE 


Fig.  16.  Relation  of  characteristic  oldest  age  to  size  of 
initial  cohort.  The  curves  drawn  are  for  a  Gompertzian 
life-table  (see  text)  and  for  an  exponential  life-table. 
The  curve  for  the  Gompertzian  populations  is  repre- 
sentative of  the  amount  of  variation  of  lifespan  with 
sample  size  that  might  be  expected  in  populations  of 
domestic  or  zoo  animals  kept  under  good  conditions. 
The  line  for  the  exponential  population  is  illustrative 
of  the  relation  of  lifespan  to  sample  size  that  would  hold 
for  populations  under  very  heavy  environmental 
pressure,  such  as  small  birds  or  rodents  in  the  wild. 


Gompertz  term*  it  follows  that  the  lifespan  is  the  preferred 
statistic  for  the  characterization  of  species  longevity. 

*  It  can  be  argued  that  the  life  expectation  should  be  preferred  precisely 
because  it  measures  the  response  to  environmental  as  well  as  intrinsic  factors. 
There  can  be  valid  reasons  for  preferring  the  life  expectation  in  certain  contexts, 
and  especially  in  the  discussion  of  evolutionary  or  ecological  adaptations. 
However,  the  present  paper  is  addressed  to  the  question  of  intrinsic  limitations 
on  length  of  life,  and  these  questions  are  appropriately  discussed  in  terms  of 
measures  that  are  as  nearly  as  possible  invariants  for  the  species,  independent 
of  environmental  influence. 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    119 

The  theory  of  extremes  can  be  used  to  compensate  for  the 
bias  introduced  by  very  great  differences  in  cohort  size.  This 
has  not  been  attempted  in  the  present  study. 

The  data  on  brain  weight  and  body  weight  were  taken  from 
the  extensive  compilations  of  these  values  by  Count  (1947), 

BODY     WEIGHT -GRAMS 
10301^3X10^     lo'    3XI0'     10*    3X10*     lo'    3Xlo'     10*     3X10* 


2.0  - 


-.        1.0 


LOG|o    BODY    WEIGHT 

Fig,  2.  Relation  of  lifespan  to  body  weight  for  63  species  of 
mammals.  Data  plotted  on  double  logarithmic  grid.  The 
symbols  denote  groups  of  species  as  follows :  open  circles — 
primates  and  lemurs ;  solid  circles — rodents  and  insectivores; 
crosses — carnivores;  solid  triangles — ungulates  and  ele- 
phants ;  star  in  circular  field — man. 

von  Bonin  (1937),  and  Quiring  (1950).  The  body  weights  and 
brain  weights  are  in  some  instances  averages  of  values  re- 
ported by  two  or  more  investigators.  Lifespans  are  the  maxi- 
mum records  encountered  in  the  sources  examined,  and  were 
obtained  principally  from  the  compilations  by  Bourliere 
(1946),  Comfort  (1956),  Flower  (1931),  Walker  (1954),  and 
Yerkes  and  Yerkes  (1929). 


120 


George  A.  Sacher 


There  are  63  species  for  which  body  weight,  brain  weight 
and  Hfespan  values  were  tabulated.  Most  orders  of  placental 
mammals  are  represented,  the  most  important  omissions 
being  the  bats  and  marine  mammals.  Common  logarithms  of 
the  numbers  were  used  in  the  statistical  analysis.  Simple  and 
multiple  linear  regressions  were  computed.    The  statistical 


1.0 


BRAIN     WEIGHT  -  GRAMS 
10  30         100       300       1000     3000 


LOG,, 


2  0 
BRAIN     WEIGHT 


3.0 


Fig.  3.  Relation  of  lifespan  to  brain  weight  for  the  same  63 
species  shown  in  Fig.  2.    Symbols  as  defined  in  legend  to 

Fig.  2. 


formulae  are  presented  in  lucid  fashion  by  Hald  (1952). 
Appendix  1  contains  the  means  and  standard  errors  of  the 
variables,  the  regression  coefficients  and  their  standard  errors, 
the  total  variances  of  the  variables,  and  their  residual  vari- 
ances after  removing  the  variance  in  regression.  With  the 
information  provided  the  total,  partial  and  multiple  correla- 
tion coefficients  can  also  be  computed. 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    121 

The  relation  of  lifespan  to  body  weight  is  exhibited  in  Fig.  2, 
with  the  points  plotted  on  a  log-log  grid.  The  species  are 
divided  into  four  groups,  each  comprising  one  or  more  orders, 
and  each  group  has  a  distinguishing  symbol.  No  use  is  made 
of  this  subdivision  in  the  statistical  analysis  since  the  data 
were  not  sufficient  to  justify  a  separate  analysis  by  orders; 
such  analysis  will  be  undertaken  when  more  extensive  data 
are  collected. 

It  can  be  seen  that  there  is  a  highly  significant  relation 
between  lifespan  and  body  weight.  The  relation  of  log  lifespan 
in  years  (x)  to  log  body  weight  in  grams  (y)  is  found  by  least 
squares  to  be  (see  also  Appendix  lb) 

X  =  0-198?/  +  0-471  (2) 

This  regression  accounts  for  60  per  cent  of  the  variance  of 
lifespans.  It  can  also  be  seen  that  the  different  groups  are 
stratified  in  the  diagram,  with  the  rodent  lifespans  lying  below 
the  regression  line,  those  for  carnivores  and  ungulates  more  or 
less  evenly  distributed  around  the  line,  and  those  for  primates 
lying  almost  entirely  above. 

A  similar  diagram  showing  the  relation  of  lifespan  to  brain 
weight  is  displayed  in  Fig.  3.  The  least  squares  regression  of 
log  lifespan  (x)  on  log  brain  weight  in  grams  (z)  (Appendix  Ic) 

X  =  0-3252  +  0-684  (3) 

accounts  for  79  per  cent  of  the  lifespan  variance.  This  is 
significantly  greater  than  the  variance  reduction  brought 
about  by  regression  on  body  weight,  so  it  can  be  concluded 
that  brain  weight  by  itself  is  a  better  predictor  of  lifespan 
than  is  body  weight.  The  superiority  of  brain  weight  over 
body  weight  as  a  predictor  is  manifested  by  a  reduced  scatter 
between  the  groups  (shown  by  their  clustering  closer  to  the 
regression  line)  and  also  by  a  decreased  scatter  within  groups 
(shown  by  the  smaller  dispersion  of  the  individual  deviations 
from  the  mean  deviation  for  the  group).  There  is  still  evidence 
of  stratification,  however,  so  that  brain  weight  does  not  by 
itself  account  for  all  the  extractable  lifespan  variance. 


122 


George  A.  Sacher 


Next  we  may  ask  whether  Ufespan  can  be  predicted  more 
accurately  by  a  combination  of  brain  and  body  weight  than 
by  either  of  them  alone.  The  answer  to  this  question  is  given 
by  the  multiple  regression  of  lifespan  on  body  weight  and 
brain  weight  (Appendix  Id), 


X 


0'6S6z  -  0-2252/  +  1-035 


(4) 


it!      1.0- 


.5  1.0  1.5 

MULTIPLE     REGRESSION     VARIABLE 


2.0 


Fig.  4.   Relation  of  lifespan  to  multiple  regression  variable 

defined  by  Equation  (4)   or  Equation  (8).     Symbols  as 

defined  in  legend  to  Fig.  2. 


This  regression  removes  84  •  4  per  cent  of  the  lifespan  variance. 
This  is  not  a  significant  increase  over  the  79  per  cent  removed 
by  regression  on  brain  weight  alone.  However,  the  scatter 
diagram  (Fig.  4)  suggests  that  the  multiple  regression  has 
further  reduced  the  differences  between  groups.  It  is  possible 
that  a  more  detailed  statistical  analysis,  in  which  a  separate 
allometric  relation  is  obtained  for  each  of  the  major  taxonomic 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    123 

subdivisions  of  mammals,  will  further  increase  the  goodness 
of  prediction,  since  Figs.  2,  3  and  4  give  evidence  that  the 
relation  of  brain  weight  to  body  weight  for  the  different  groups 
cannot  be  described  by  a  single  allometric  relation.  Count 
(1947)  is  only  the  last  of  a  number  of  authors  to  point  this  out. 
Discussion  of  the  independent  roles  of  brain  and  body 
weights  will  be  facilitated  by  the  use  of  a  transformed  variable. 
Brain  weight  and  body  weight  are  closely  related  variables,  for 
the  regression  of  brain  weight  on  body  weight  (Appendix  le) 

z=  0-666?/  -  0-888  (5) 

accounts  for  91  •  7  per  cent  of  the  brain  weight  variance.  The 
deviation  of  an  individual  brain  weight  value  from  the  regres- 
sion line  is  the  logarithm  of  that  fraction  of  the  brain  weight 
of  the  species  that  is  independent  of  the  overall  regression 
of  brain  weight  on  body  weight.  This  deviation  is  defined 
to  be  a  new  variable,  w,  which  is  given  by  the  equation 

w  =  z  -  0-6662/  +  0-888  (6) 

This  quantity  is  called  the  index  of  cephalization.  It  should  be 
understood  that  this  is  by  definition  a  measure  of  brain  de- 
velopment that  is  orthogonal  to  body  weight.  The  definition 
contains  none  of  the  a  priori  considerations  that  have  fre- 
quently entered  into  the  definition  of  this  quantity  since  the 
time  of  Dubois  (1924).  Von  Bonin  (1937)  has  urged  that  the 
index  of  cephalization  be  defined  in  this  objective  fashion. 

The  regression  of  lifespan  on  index  of  cephalization  (w)  is 
found  to  be  (Appendix  If) 

X  =  0-636r£;  -i-  1-283  (7) 

By  the  definition  of  w,  the  regression  coefficient  for  x  on  w  is 
numerically  equal  to  the  partial  regression  oi  x  on  z  in  Equa- 
tion (3).  However,  the  variance  of  w  is  but  8  -  3  per  cent  of  the 
variance  of  z.  In  consequence  the  sampling  error  of  ft^.^  is 
larger  than  that  for  b^^y  (Appendix  If),  and  the  variance 
removed  by  the  regression  of  a;  on  w  is  23  per  cent  rather  than 


124  George  A.  Sacher 

84  per  cent  (Appendix  1).  However,  the  coefficient  of  regres- 
sion of  X  on  w  is  more  than  four  times  larger  than  its  standard 
error,  so  the  regression  is  highly  significant. 

The  multiple  regression  of  lifespan  on  body  weight  and  index 
of  cephalization  is  found  to  be  (Appendix  Ig) 

X  =  O'QSQw  +  0-198Z/  +  0-471  (8) 

It  will  be  noted  that  the  partial  regression  ofxony  in  Equation 
(8)  is  numerically  equal  to  the  coefficient  of  total  regression  of 
cconyin  Equation  (2).  This  follows  from  the  fact  that  y  and  w 
are  orthogonal  variables,  so  that  the  regression  of  cC  on  i/  is 
completely  independent  of,  and  unaffected  by,  the  regression 
of  X  on  w. 

One  further  dimension  of  mammalian  constitution  that  has 
been  measured  for  a  large  number  of  species  is  that  of  meta- 
bolic rate.  The  great  amoimt  of  data  accumulated  by  many 
investigators,  and  especially  by  Rubner,  Benedict  and  Brody, 
has  been  masterfully  organized  in  Brody's  treatise  on  Bio- 
energetics  and  Growth  (1945).  Brody  has  shown  that  the 
relation  between  basal  or  resting  metabolic  rate  and  body 
weight  for  warm-blooded  vertebrates  (including  birds)  follows 
a  power  law  relation  with  great  precision.  The  regression  of 
logarithm  of  specific  metabolic  rate,  m  (in  calories  per  gram 
per  day),  on  log  body  weight  is  (Appendix  Ih) 

m=  -  0'2Q6y  +  1-047  (9) 

The  correlation  coefficient  is  over  0-99  (Brody,  1945).  In 
view  of  this  high  correlation,  the  partial  regression  of  specific 
metabolic  rate  on  index  of  cephalization  must  necessarily  be 
small.  We  can  therefore  assume  tentatively  that  this  cor- 
relation is  zero  and  substitute  m  (given  by  Equation  (9) )  for  ?/ 
in  Equation  (8).  The  resulting  equation  for  the  regression  of 
lifespan  on  metabolic  rate  and  index  of  cephalization  is 

X  =  0-636i£;  -  0-744m  +  1-252  (10) 

Sampling  errors  and  residual  variance  for  this  relation  cannot 
be  given. 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    125 

It  can  be  concluded  that  two  independent  factors  are  re- 
quired to  account  for  the  observations.  One  of  these  may  be 
identified  by  the  highly  correlated  pair  of  variables — metabolic 
rate  and  body  weight.  The  other  factor  may  be  identified  by 
the  index  of  cephalization,  which  is  orthogonal  to  body  weight 
and  must  also  be  nearly  orthogonal  to  metabolic  rate  (see 
above).  Both  of  these  factors  are  represented  in  the  brain 
weight,  so  that  brain  weight  alone  is  almost  as  good  a  predictor 
of  lifespan  as  brain  weight  and  body  weight  combined. 

Although  the  existence  of  two  independent  factors  is  very 
reliably  established,  their  resolution  into  the  two  orthogonal 
terms  specified  above  is  not  unique.  The  extension  of  these 
procedures  of  multivariate  analysis  to  larger  sets  of  anatomical 
and  physiological  variables  may  make  it  possible  in  future  to 
identify  these  factors  more  confidently,  and  perhaps  also  to 
establish  the  existence  of  one  or  more  additional  factors 
governing  length  of  life. 

In  spite  of  the  coarseness  of  the  measures  used,  this  first 
statistical  investigation  of  the  allometry  of  lifespan  has  been 
rewarding.  Much  more  remains  to  be  learned  from  a  more 
detailed  examination  of  these  relations  within  individual 
orders  of  mammals,  and  also  in  other  vertebrate  classes,  birds 
in  particular.  It  is  to  be  expected  that  the  numerical  values 
of  the  coefficients  will  differ  in  these  different  groups,  for  brain 
function  can  be  specified  by  a  single  number  such  as  total  mass 
only  to  the  degree  that  brain  structure  is  describable  in  all  its 
anatomical  details  by  a  single  allometric  coefficient.  These 
same  considerations  apply  to  the  somatic  dimensions. 

This  completes  the  discussion  of  the  statistical  analysis  and 
the  implications  of  these  findings  for  the  theory  of  ageing  in 
mammals  will  now  be  considered. 

First,  let  us  consider  the  above  findings  in  terms  of  a  theory 
of  ageing  put  forth  by  Rubner  almost  exactly  a  half- century 
ago  (1908).  He  took  note  of  the  fact  that  several  species  of 
domestic  animals  with  markedly  different  body  sizes  and  life- 
spans  all  had  lifetime  basal  energy  expenditure   of  about 


126  George  A.  Sacher 

200  kilocalories  per  gram.  The  evidence  adduced  by 
Rubner  was  essentially  that  in  Table  I.  The  lifespans  he 
attributed  to  some  species  in  the  table  are  out  of  line  with 
currently  accepted  values,  and  introduction  of  the  more 
accurate  values  would  considerably  weaken  his  evidence. 

Although  Rubner' s  own  evidence  is  hardly  adequate  to 
support  his  thesis,  the  results  of  the  present  analysis  indicate 
that  his  conjecture  nevertheless  has  considerable  merit.  His 
hypothesis  may  be  reformulated  to  say  that  the  lifespan  of  a 
species  varies  inversely  as  its  basal  metabolic  rate  or,  in  the 
notation  employed  above, 

X  =  —  1-OOm  +  constant  (11) 

Equation  (10)  shows,  however,  that  the  empirical  relation 
between  these  variables  is  numerically 

X  =  —  0'75m  +  constant  (12) 

The  significance  of  the  difference  between  the  theoretical  and 
observed  coefficients  cannot  be  tested  rigorously,  so  we  cannot 
say  whether  this  difference  is  in  fact  significant.  However, 
Rubner's  hypothesis  that  lifespan  varies  inversely  as  the  first 
power  of  the  metabolic  rate  is  in  any  event  subject  to  quantita- 
tive revision,  for  he  considered  only  the  basal  metabolic 
energy.  There  is  no  good  reason  to  distinguish  between  the 
resting  and  active  energy  expenditure  with  respect  to  their 
effects  on  length  of  life.  Furthermore,  the  relation  of  active 
energy  expenditure  to  body  size  is  not  accurately  known. 
Hence,  it  can  only  be  concluded  at  present  that  the  empirical 
findings  are  in  accord  with  the  general  hypothesis  that  the 
attainable  length  of  life  of  a  mammalian  species  is  dependent 
in  part  on  its  rate  of  energy  dissipation. 

Rubner's  original  discussion  of  the  energetic  theory  called 
attention  to  the  fact  that  the  lifetime  energy  expenditure  for 
man  is  seriously  out  of  line  with  the  values  calculated  for 
domestic  animals  (Table  I).  This  discrepancy  was  in  fact  one 
of  the  considerations  that  inspired  me  to  undertake  this 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    127 


Table  I 

Lifetime  energy  expenditure  (cal./g.)  for  several,  species 

OF  DOMESTIC  ANIMALS  AND  MAN  (FROM  RUBNER,  1908) 


Species 


Horse 

Cow 

Dog 

Cat 

Guinea  pig 

Man 


Body  Wt. 


kg. 

450 

450 

22 

3 

0-6 
70 


Length 

of 
life 

(years) 

30 
26 

9 

8 

6 
100 


Lifetime 

energy 

expenditure 

(cal./g.) 

170,000 
141,000 
164,000 
224,000 
266,000 
800,000 


allometric  analysis.  I  had  been  working  on  a  theory  of 
mortaHty  and  ageing  which  took  particular  cognizance  of  the 
role  played  by  physiological  fluctuations  (Sacher,  1956,  1958). 
The  essential  points  of  this  theory  are : 

(a)  mortality  is  essentially  a  random  process  in  that  the 
circumstances  leading  to  death  in  an  individual  case  cannot  be 
predicted  with  certainty ; 

(b)  the  physical  basis  for  this  uncertainty  is  to  be  found  in 
the  fluctuations  of  physiological  state  that  are  inevitably 
present  in  living  systems;  and 

(c)  the  magnitude  of  the  physiological  fluctuations  is 
determined  by  the  interplay  between  the  random  impinging 
disturbances  (of  external  and  internal  origin)  and  the  regula- 
tory mechanisms  that  act  to  limit  the  magnitude  and  duration 
of  their  effects  on  the  organism. 

A  central  problem  in  the  mathematical  development  of  the 
theory  is  that  of  deriving  the  quantitative  dependence  of  the 
probability  of  mortality  in  given  circumstances  on  the  para- 
meters of  the  physiological  fluctuation  process.  The  important 
implication  of  the  theory  in  the  present  context  is  that  the 
attainable  length  of  life  for  a  species  depends  on  the  precision 
of  physiological  regulation  that  the  species  is  capable  of. 
The  logical  or  mathematical  developments  that  lead  to  this 


128  George  A.  Sacher 

inference  will  not  be  discussed,  because  in  qualitative  terms  it 
is  readily  evident,  and  a  quantitative  mathematical  statement 
is  not  testable  at  present  owing  to  lack  of  appropriate  data 
on  precision  of  physiological  regulations  in  different  species. 

In  the  absence  of  data  that  would  permit  a  direct  compari- 
son of  theory  with  experiment  it  was  finally  resolved  to 
approach  the  problem  indirectly,  by  introducing  the  mediating 
assumption  that  the  precision  of  physiological  regulation  is 
directly  related  to  the  degree  of  encephalization.  Several 
lines  of  published  evidence  suggested  that  such  an  approach 
might  be  fruitful.  First,  there  was  the  great  discrepancy 
between  the  human  lifespan  and  that  of  other  species,  and  also 
Rubner's  statement  about  the  discrepancy  between  man  and 
other  mammals  in  lifetime  energy  expenditure  (see  above). 
Second,  Flower's  tabulation  of  lifespans  of  mammals  brought 
out  the  important  point  that  man's  long  lifespan  relative  to 
that  of  other  mammals  is  not  unique,  for  primate  species  in 
general  live  longer  than  non-primates  (Flower,  1931).  Finally, 
there  was  the  massive  literature  on  the  index  of  cephalization 
which  indicated  that  this  varies  over  wide  limits  (von  Bonin, 
1937),  thus  making  possible  an  evaluation  of  the  contribution 
of  brain  weight  independent  of  body  weight. 

The  statistical  analysis  presented  above  strongly  supports 
the  hypothesis  that  lifespan  is  specifically  related  to  brain 
development.  To  establish  my  thesis  completely  I  should 
next  justify  the  mediating  assumption  that  the  overall  pre- 
cision of  physiological  regulations  is  governed  by  the  central 
nervous  system.  To  do  so  would  take  me  far  beyond  the 
bounds  of  this  paper,  for  it  would  entail  a  review  of  the  vast 
and  rapidly  increasing  literature  on  the  control  of  vital  physio- 
logical functions  by  the  central  nervous  system,  and  by  the 
cerebral  cortex  in  particular  {Ciba  Found.  Symp.  Neurological 
Basis  of  Behaviour,  1958).  The  developments  of  the  last  few 
years  have  done  much  to  dispel  the  dichotomy  between 
"higher"  and  "lower"  forms  of  behaviour  in  so  far  as  their 
representation  in  the  nervous  system  is  concerned,  and  we 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    129 

tend  more  and  more  to  the  view  that  the  central  nervous 
system  participates  in  the  vegetative  processes  continually. 
I  therefore  conclude  that  my  assumption  about  the  close 
relation  between  brain  development  and  overall  precision  of 
physiological  regulation  is  justified  by  our  present  knowledge. 
Moreover,  I  am  confident  that  this  relation  will  find  concrete 
support  when  a  quantitative  comparative  physiology  comes 
into  being  which  will  make  possible  direct  tests  of  more  specific 
forms  of  this  general  assumption. 

Identification  of  the  factors  which  determine  the  character- 
istic ageing  rates,  and  hence  the  lifespans  of  mammalian  spec- 
ies, is  one  of  the  central  problems  of  gerontology.  In  recent 
years  several  authors  have  proposed  theories  of  ageing  which 
posit  a  relation  of  ageing  to  the  spontaneous  mutation  rate 
(Szilard,  1959)  or  to  loss  of  information  content  (Yockey, 
1958).  Whatever  other  utility  these  theories  may  have,  they 
contribute  little  to  our  understanding  of  ageing,  for  their 
authors  fail  to  grasp  the  essential  point  that  the  spontaneous 
mutation  rate  and  the  ageing  rate  are  concomitant  species 
characters,  so  that  to  account  for  one  in  terms  of  the  other  is 
merely  to  restate  an  observed  fact.  It  would  undoubtedly  be 
widely  acknowledged  that  ageing  must  ultimately  be  accounted 
for  in  terms  of  irreversible  alterations  in  the  molecular  make- 
up of  organisms,  and  that  gene  mutations  are  an  especially 
important  class  of  such  irreversible  molecular  changes.  Given 
this  consensus,  the  basic  question  is:  why  does  the  species 
mutation  rate  tend  to  be  proportional  to  the  mean  death  rate, 
or  inversely  proportional  to  the  lifespan?  I  shall  next  discuss 
this  question  in  the  light  of  the  energy  dissipation  and  fluctua- 
tion hypotheses. 

The  occurrence  of  irreversible  molecular  changes  in  bio- 
logical systems  can  be  exhaustively  discussed  under  four 
headings,  as  follows: 

(a)  thermal  denaturation — alterations  of  molecular  struc- 
ture that  are  essentially  due  to  thermal  energy,  and  occur  at  a 
rate  that  is  primarily  dependent  on  the  temperature; 

AGEING — ^V — 5 


130  George  A.  Sacher 

(b)  errors  of  synthesis  and  denaturations  that  occur  in  the 
steady  state  of  metabohc  activity — the  probabihty  of  produc- 
ing incorrect  molecules  per  unit  time  arising  from  inherent 
imperfections  of  the  metabolic  process  (inadequate  specificity 
of  enzymes,  presence  of  by-product  metabolic  poisons,  etc.) 
in  the  ideal  steady  state  for  the  species  as  this  is  established 
by  natural  selection  operating  on  the  genetically  controllable 
thermodynamic  properties  of  enzymes ; 

(c)  errors  in  synthesis  and  denaturations  occurring  as  a 
result  of  deviations  from  the  steady  state ; 

(d)  adventitious  poisoning  of  environmental  origin — radi- 
ation, poisons,  etc.  (this  class  will  not  be  discussed  here). 

These  classes  of  determiners  are  distinguishable  by  the  way 
in  which  the  observed  ageing  rate  (mutation  rate,  etc.) 
depends  on  certain  environmental  and  system  variables. 
Thermal  inactivation  would  operate  equally  on  almost  all 
mammalian  species  since  they  have  (except  for  bats  and  some 
other  forms)  essentially  the  same  constant  body  temperature. 
Thus  thermal  denaturation  can  be  ruled  out  as  a  factor 
determining  the  different  rates  of  ageing  in  different  species. 
The  error  rate  in  the  metabolic  steady  state  should  have  a 
direct,  essentially  proportional  relation  to  metabolic  rate  and 
in  consequence  a  determinate  relation  to  body  mass.  Hence, 
if  this  is  an  important  factor  in  determining  the  rate  of 
accumulation  of  ageing  events  one  would  expect  lifespan  to 
vary  as  an  inverse  function  of  metabolic  rate.  This  is  the 
Rubner  hypothesis,  which  was  seen  above  to  be  in  general 
accord  with  the  data. 

The  error  rate  due  to  fluctuations  away  from  the  steady 
state  would  tend  to  be  smaller  in  species  possessing  superior 
physiological  regulatory  mechanisms,  for  in  these  species  the 
mean  square  deviation  from  the  steady  state  is  smaller.  The 
evidence  on  this  point,  deriving  from  the  relation  of  lifespan 
to  index  of  cephalization,  is  indirect  but  nevertheless  persua- 
sive, as  was  discussed  earlier. 

Thus  the  comparative,  allometric  approach  has  shown  us 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    131 

that  the  rate  of  accretion  of  ageing  injury  in  mammahan 
species  increases  as  the  metabohc  rate  increases  and  decreases 
as  precision  of  physiological  regulation  increases.  Therefore 
the  irreversible  changes  that  underlie  ageing  are  not  necessarily 
and  inevitably  determined  by  the  rate  of  dissipation  of 
energy.  The  intervention  of  improved  physiological  regula- 
tions, by  maintaining  the  average  value  of  the  milieu  interieur 
nearer  to  the  biochemically  ideal,  and  by  reducing  the 
magnitude  of  the  fluctuations  away  from  this  average,  acts 
to  reduce  the  probability  per  unit  time  of  irreversible  changes, 
and  thus  to  reduce  the  rate  of  ageing.  This  has  been  a  highly 
significant  feature  in  the  evolution  of  mammals,  and  it 
remains  to  be  seen  whether  man  can  take  conscious  advantage 
of  this  principle  to  bring  about  a  real  decrease  in  his  rate  of 
ageing. 


Appendix  1.  Relation  of  log  lifespan  to  log  body  weight,  log  brain 
weight,  index  of  cephalization  and  log  specific  metabolic  rate.  Also 
mean  values,  variances,  and  standard  errors  of  the  coefficients*. 

a.  Mean  values  and  standard  errors  of  the  variables,  and  their 
variances. 

Log  lifespan  (years)  ^  =  1  -283  ±  -042  SI  =     -10900 

Log  body  weight  (grams)  ^  =  4-099  ±  -163  SI  =  1  -67158 

Log  brain  weight  (grams)  2   =1-841  ±-113  SI  =     -80666 

Index  of  cephalization  w=  0+ -032  S^  =     -06570 

b.  Log  lifespan  (x)  on  log  body  weight  (y).  See  Equation  (2). 

b^  =  -198  ±   -021  Slj,   =  -04402 

*  The  total  regression  of  variable  x  on  variable  y  is  denoted  by  bxj,.   The 
corresponding  regression  equation  is 

X  —  X  =  bxy(y  —  y) 

The  partial  regression  of  x  on  y,  independent  of  the  regressions  of  x  and  y  on  a, 
third  variable,  z,  is  denoted  by  bxp.z.  The  multiple  regression  equation  is  of  the 
form 

X  -  X  =  hxy.z{y  -y)  +  h^y.ziz  -  z) 

The  total  variance  of  variable  x  is  denoted  by  S^.  The  variance  of  x  that  is  not 
accounted  for  by  regression  on  the  other  variable  or  variables  in  the  regression 
equation  is  denoted  by  S^y,  S^^y  etc. 


132  George  A.  Sacher 

c.  Log  lifespan  {x)  on  log  brain  weight  (z).   See  Equation  (3). 

b„  =  -325  ±   -022  SI,   =  -02403 

d.  Log  lifespan  (x)  on  log  body  weight  {y)  and  log  brain  weight  (z). 
See  Equation  (4). 

b^^^  =  --225  +    -045 

Sl^  =  -01730 
b^^^  =   -636  ±   -065 

e.  Log  brain  weight  (z)  on  log  body  weight  {y) ;  definition  of  index  of 
cephalization  {w).   See  Equations  (5)  and  (6). 

h^  =      -666  ±   -025  Sly  =  -06678 

w   =  z-  -666?/  +  0-888 

f.  Log  lifespan  {x)  on  index  of  cephalization  (w).   See  Equation  (7). 

b^,^  =       -636  ±   -143         Sl„  =   -08381 

g.  Log  lifespan  (x)  on  log  body  weight  (y)  and  index  of  cephalization 
(w).   See  Equation  (8). 

K^o  =  -198  ±   -013 

Sl^  =   -01730 
K..,  =   -636  ±   -065 
h.  Relation  of  log  specific  metabolic  rate  (m)  to  log  body  weight  {y) 
as  given  by  Brody  (1945);  regression  of  log  lifespan  (x)  on  index  of 
cephalization  (w)  and  log  specific  metabolic  rate  (m).  See  Equations 
(9)  and  (10). 

m  =  -  -2661/  +  1-047 
h,„„  =  -636  6^.„  =  --745 


'xw.m 


REFERENCES 

BoNiN,  G.  VON  (1937).  J.  gen.  Psychol,  16,  379. 

BouRLiERE,  F.  (1946).   Annee  biol.,  22,  249. 

Brody,  S.  (1945).    Bioenergeties  and  Growth.    New  York:  Reinhold 

Publishing  Co. 
Ciba  Found.  Symp.  Neurological  Basis  of  Behaviour  (1958).    London: 

Churchill. 
Comfort,  A.  (1956).    The  Biology  of  Senescence.    New  York:  Rine- 

hart. 
Comfort,  A.  (1958).  J.  Geront,  13,  342. 
Count,  E.  W.  (1947).   Ann.  N.Y.  Acad.  Sci.,  46,  993. 
Dubois,  E.  (1924).   Proc.  kon.  med.  Akad.  Wet.,  27,  430. 
Flower,  S.  (1931).  Proc.  zool.  Soc.  Lond.,  145. 
GuMBEL,  E.  J.  (1958).    Statistics  of  Extremes.    New  York:  Columbia 

University  Press. 


Relation  of  Lifespan  to  Brain  and  Body  Weight    133 

Hald,  a.  (1952).    Statistical  Theory  with  Engineering  AppUeations. 

New  York :  John  Wiley  and  Sons. 
Quiring,  D.   P.   (1950).    Functional  Anatomy  of  Vertebrates.    New 

York:  McGraw-Hill. 
RuBXER,  M.  (1908).    Das  Problem  der  Lebensdauer  und  seine  Bezie- 

hungen  zurn  Wachstum  und  Ernahrung.    Munich:  Oldenbourg. 
Sacher,  G.  a.  (1956).   Radiology,  67,  250. 
Sacher,  G.  a.  (1957).   Anat.  Rec,  128,  616. 
Sacher,  G.  A.  (1958).  In  Symposium  on  Information  Theory  in  Biology, 

ed.  Yockey,  H.  P.,  et  ah,  p.  317.  London  and  New  York:  Pergamon 

Press. 
Szilard,  L.  (1959).  Proc.  nat.  Acad.  Sci.  {Wash.),  45,  30. 
Walker,  E.  P.  (1954).   The  Monkey  Book.   New  York:  Macmillan. 
Yerkes,  R.,  and  Yerkes,  A.  (1929).    The  Great  Apes.    New  Haven: 

Yale  University  Press. 
Yockey,   H.   P.   (1958).     In   Symposium   on   Information  Theory  in 

Biology,  ed.    Yockey,  H.    P.,  et    al.,  p.  297.     London  and  New 

York:  Pergamon  Press. 


DISCUSSION 

Danielli:  The  simplest  physical  interpretation  of  your  regression 
equation,  Mr.  Sacher,  is  that  it  is  an  advantage  to  have  a  brain,  and 
a  disadvantage  to  have  a  body  ! 

Wigglesworth:  Barcroft's  thesis  (Barcroft,  J.  (1934).  Features  of 
the  Architecture  of  Physiological  Function.  Cambridge  University 
Press)  was  that  a  constant  internal  environment  was  primarily 
essential  for  the  functioning  of  the  brain,  and  that  for  higher 
development  of  the  brain  you  needed  a  more  constantly  regulated 
internal  environment.  As  I  understand  your  theory,  that  same 
constancy  will  also  be  favourable  to  longevity,  so  that  cephalization 
and  longevity  should  go  hand  in  hand. 

Verzdr:  Do  you  not  think  that  lifespan  as  a  measurement  of  ageing 
trends  is  a  bad  measurement,  Mr.  Sacher  ?  Lifespan  is  the  most  un- 
certain of  all  our  records.  The  mortality  curves  are  not  straight  ; 
they  are  always  an  "S  "  shape,  and  the  right  side  of  that  "S  "  shape, 
especially,  is  extremely  long,  so  that  exceptionally  long  lives  are 
particularly  noticed.  Wouldn't  it  be  much  better  to  relate  all  our 
age  theories  not  to  the  maximal  lifespan,  but  to  something  like  a  50 
per  cent  survival  of  a  certain  population  ?  That  would  make  the 
whole  thing  experimentally  much  more  certain. 

Sacher:  I  agree  that  maximum  lifespan  is  a  gross  measure.  That  it 
is  a  bad  measure  I  would  not  agree.  Lifespan  is  an  extremum  statis- 
tic, and  can  be  handled  in  a  perfectly  rigorous  fashion  by  statistical 


134  Discussion 

reasoning,  as  is  shown  in  Gumbel's  treatise  (1958).  Moreover,  life 
expectations  or  the  median  survival  times  are  available  for  only 
about  six  of  the  100  or  so  species  that  ideally  we  would  like  to  use  in 
this  sort  of  comparative  analysis.  Finally,  as  I  pointed  out  in  my 
paper,  maximum  lifespan  is  a  better  estimator  of  the  intrinsic 
ageing  characteristics  of  the  population  than  is  the  median  or 
average  survival.  However,  I  do  not  advocate  its  use  in  the  analysis 
of  laboratory  data,  or  in  any  situation  where  life-tables  are  available. 

Incidentally,  I  have  been  concerned  about  the  problem  of  what  is 
proper  to  use  as  a  lifespan  for  man.  Zoo  animals  are  not  kept  under 
ideal  conditions,  as  they  get  little  individual  attention  or  medical 
care.  Therefore  perhaps  some  earlier  state  of  human  culture  would 
be  more  comparable.  It  is  interesting  that  you  can  get  data  on  the 
ages  of  fossil  skulls  back  to  the  Palaeolithic  (Vallois,  H.  V.  (1937). 
Anthropologic,  Paris,  47,  499;  Weidenreich,  F.  (1939).  Chin.  med.  J., 
55,  34).  Peking  man,  in  a  sample  of  only  six  skulls,  yielded  one  that 
probably  had  an  age  of  over  50  years.  They  lived  under  far  poorer 
conditions  than  our  zoo  animals  today.  Neolithic  man  lived  to  more 
than  70  years,  even  according  to  rather  small  samples. 

Comfort:  I  think  you  have  been  taking  much  more  plausible 
maximum  ages  than  Rubner  did,  Mr.  Sacher.  Rubner  gave  the  life- 
span of  a  dog  as  nine  years  and  of  a  cat  as  eight  years,  which  is 
quite  arbitrary.  Cats  can  quite  possibly  live  for  30  years  and  the 
extreme  credible  record  for  a  dog  is  between  20  and  25.  There  are 
much  greater  discrepancies  between  the  modal  and  maximum  records 
for  animals,  and  the  acceptable  mode  and  maximum  for  man.  I 
think  most  people  would  agree  that  110  to  120  is  the  extreme  limit 
for  which  there  is  any  good  evidence  in  man,  in  spite  of  the  140- 
year-old  Russians.  Most  cats  die  before  they  are  16,  but  a  few  have 
lived  very  nearly  twice  that  time.  One  has  also  to  beware  among 
animals  of  the  possibility  of  very  long-lived  genotypes  and  of  the 
differences  between  hybrid  and  inbred  strains. 

Sacher:  The  extreme  error  in  the  individual  lifespan  records  is 
probably  a  factor  of  2  or  so,  but  if  the  errors  are  random,  their  only 
effect  is  to  weaken  the  degree  of  order  observed. 

Holt:  Have  you  looked  at  data  for  aquatic  mammals  ?  They  have 
some  exceptionally  large  body  sizes,  without  corresponding  increases 
in  brain  size,  and  there  are  many  published  age  determinations  for 
them,  but  perhaps  not  maximum  lifespans. 

Sacher:  When  I  did  this  work  I  did  not  have  enough  data  on 
aquatic  mammals,  but  I  want  to  study  them  in  future. 

Maynard  Smith:  There  is  a  possibility  of  bias  in  estimates  of  this 
kind  since  most  of  the  small  mammals  in  your  sample  are  rodents, 


Discussion  135 

and  most  of  the  large  ones  are  ungulates.  It  would  be  interesting 
to  know  whether,  if  you  calculate  your  coefficients  just  on  rodents, 
just  on  primates,  and  just  on  ungulates,  you  would  get  results  which 
are  at  least  approximately  consistent  with  those  you  get  on  the 
whole  sample  combined. 

Sacher:  I  only  know  this  qualitatively  and  graphically.  The 
values  of  the  allometric  coefficients  vary  considerably  from  order  to 
order.  The  goodness  of  fit  would  have  been  greatly  improved  if  I 
had  omitted  the  ungulates,  because  they  have  quite  different 
allometric  relations  of  body  weight  to  brain  weight,  and  in  effect 
they  made  the  overall  relation  poorer. 

Rotblat:  I  was  very  glad  to  see  that  you  describe  life  processes  in 
terms  of  numbers.  I  was  a  little  bit  disturbed,  however,  when  you 
ended  up  by  introducing  a  term  which  cannot  be  expressed  in 
numbers,  namely  stability  or  adaptability  of  physiological  function. 
Can  you  put  this  in  some  kind  of  quantitative  relation  to  the  index 
of  cephalization,  or  the  mitotic  activity  ?  Have  you  any  indication 
that  there  may  be  some  ways  of  increasing  lifespan  by  increasing 
stability  ? 

Sacher:  The  term  "  stability  "  in  my  thinking  is  a  general  term  that  <^ 
subsumes  all  the  properties  that  have  to  do  with  mortality,  stress 
resistance,  length  of  life,  etc.  In  this  sense,  an  animal  that  lives 
longer  has  greater  stability,  and  a  species  that  has  a  lower  mortality 
rate  for  a  given  disease  or  stress  has  greater  stability.  In  other 
words,  stability  is  a  general  physical  character  of  organisms.  If  one 
holds  the  point  of  view  that  all  these  things  are  fated,  determined  in 
advance  by  the  genotype,  then  there  is  no  reason  for  talking  about 
stability.  But  if  one  thinks  of  organisms  as  dynamic  functioning 
systems  whose  probabilities  of  failure  arise  from  their  function 
(Sacher,  1956,  1958),  then  stability  is  a  natural  term  and  it  becomes 
reasonable  to  think  of  improving  the  stability  characteristic  of 
organisms.  We  cannot  replace  any  of  our  bodily  elements  with 
better  ones,  as  an  engineer  can  replace  vacuum  tubes,  but  it  might 
be  possible  (remembering  that  the  central  nervous  system  is  im- 
plicated in  every  physiological  activity,  and  that  these  responses 
are  conditionable)  eventually  to  evolve  a  kind  of  ontogeny  and  con- 
ditioning that  would  make  for  more  stable  physiological  functioning 
in  the  given  environment. 

Rotblat:  This  seems  to  be  going  around  in  circles.  You  have  ex- 
plained the  span  of  life  in  terms  of  stability  and  vice  versa.  It  seems 
to  me  that  mutation  rate  is  a  quantity  outside  the  circle,  because  you 
can  say  that  there  is  something  which  goes  on  all  the  time  inde- 
pendently of  us. 


136  Discussion 

Sacher:  If  mutations  are  considered  to  be  molecular  events,  then 
one  has  to  ask  why  the  molecules  in  the  mouse  mutate  some  thirty 
times  faster  per  unit  time  than  they  do  in  man.  I  have  shown  that 
two  physical  characteristics  of  mammals,  namely  the  metabolic 
rate  and  the  goodness  of  physiological  regulation,  account  for  most 
of  the  lifespan  variance.  Some  fraction  of  the  remaining  variance  is 
undoubtedly  under  specific  genotypic  control,  but  this  is  a  small 
part  of  the  total.  Thus  it  would  appear  to  be  true  that  species  tend 
to  have  the  maximum  attainable  lifetimes  permitted  by  their  body 
size  and  complexity  of  organization. 

Danielli :  You  suggested  that  there  might  be  a  standard  amount  of 
metabolism  which  was  permissible  per  gram  of  tissue  before  it 
deteriorated  beyond  hope.  Could  any  information  about  this  be 
obtained,  perhaps  in  fish,  by  using  a  poison  such  as  dinitrophenol, 
which  causes  a  good  deal  of  useless  metabolism  to  go  on  ?  This  might 
enable  one  either  to  discover  that  the  life  expectancy  was  a  function 
of  the  amount  of  oxygen  utilized  in  respiratory  processes,  or  else  to 
distinguish  between  one  type  of  metabolism  which  has  an  ageing 
effect,  and  other  types  of  metabolism  which  have  not.  It  is  probably 
easier  to  keep  up  a  constant  concentration  of  dinitrophenol  in  fish 
than  it  is  in  many  other  animals. 

Sacher:  I  have  not  yet  had  the  opportunity  to  do  such  experi- 
ments ;  it  would  certainly  be  extremely  productive  to  use  metabolic 
poisons.  In  warm-blooded  mammals  one  also  could  replace  part  of 
the  basal  energy  production  by  producing  heat  internally  with 
radiofrequency  heating.  Anything  that  would  produce  a  dissocia- 
tion between  the  amount  of  metabolism  and  the  other  physical 
characteristics  of  the  organism  would  be  extremely  valuable. 

I  would  also  like  to  determine  whether  one  could  systematically 
yet  diffusely  decrease  the  general  regulatory  ability,  perhaps  by 
destroying  the  brain  to  a  certain  degree,  with  sonic  radiation  or 
diathermy.  Can  anyone  suggest  how  a  uniform  controllable  deterior- 
ation could  be  produced  which  could  be  followed  in  terms  of  its 
effect  on  survival  ? 

Lindop :  Could  one  use  colchicine  as  a  mitotic  inhibitor  in  different 
doses  ? 

Danielli:  It  might  have  some  effect,  but  I  should  have  thought  it 
might  be  better  if  you  could  use  something  of  the  nature  of  a 
cholinesterase  inhibitor,  or  strychnine. 

Comfort:  An  experiment  with  dinitrophenol  was  done  upon  mice 
by  M.  L.  Tainter  (1936.  Proc.  Soc.  exp.  Biol.  (N.Y.),  31,  1161).  Mice 
treated  over  a  period  of  time  did  not  seem  to  have  their  lifespan  very 
much  affected — certainly  not  in  proportion  to  their  metabolic  rate. 


Discussion  137 

Danielli:  That  would  definitely  mean  that  it  was  not  just  a 
respiration  effect. 

Comfort:  With  many  of  these  animals  you  may  increase  their 
metabolism  and  at  the  same  time  decrease  their  appetite,  or  some- 
thing like  that.   One  has  to  be  careful. 

Sacher:  If  it  did  not  have  any  effect  on  body  temperature  it  would 
not  greatly  increase  the  overall  metabolism  would  it  ? 

Comfort :  I  do  not  know  whether  there  was  that  effect  or  not — the 
paper  may  say. 

Danielli:  Dinitrophenol  would  normally  increase  the  metabolism 
a  lot,  unless  there  were  some  compensatory  mechanisms,  and  one 
would  expect  appetite  to  increase  rather  than  diminish. 

Bourliere:  Long  ago  T.  B.  Robertson  (1928.  Aust.  J.  exp.  Biol, 
med.  Sci.,  5,  69)  found  that  continuous  treatment  of  the  mouse  with 
desiccated  thyroid,  in  quantities  that  stimulated  growth,  also 
shortened  the  lifespan. 

Berg:  We  are  studying  the  effect  of  thyroidectomy  on  the  lifespan 
of  the  rat. 

Maynard  Smith :  Another  possible  way  of  studying  the  effects  of 
metabolic  rate  on  longevity  is  to  use  different  genetic  strains  of  the 
same  species.  The  most  striking  differences  in  longevity  you  can  get 
in  flies  are  between  inbred  and  outbred ;  outbred  flies  will  live  about 
twice  as  long  as  inbred  ones.  This  certainly  is  not  associated  with  a 
lower  rate  of  metabolism  in  the  outbred  flies.  They  are  not  animals 
in  which  it  is  easy  to  measure  the  basal  metabolic  rate,  but  if  one 
judges  it  by  rate  of  eg^g  production,  for  example,  the  hybrids  are 
laying  eggs  at  about  twice  the  rate  of  the  inbreds,  as  well  as  living 
twice  as  long.  Also  the  hybrids  are  much  more  active.  Their  greater 
longevity  is  much  more  easily  explained  in  terms  of  the  other  con- 
cept that  Sacher  used,  namely  that  the  hybrids  in  many  respects, 
both  physiologically  and  developmentally,  seem  to  have  far  better 
stabilizing  mechanisms  than  do  inbreds,  and  that  what  is  wrong  with 
inbred  animals  is  that  they  are  just  not  good  at  regulating  against 
anything. 

Sacher:  Yes,  that  is  a  view  which  is  put  forward  systematically  in 
the  concept  of  genetic  homeostasis. 

Maynard  Smith:  I  think  it  is  a  true  one. 

Sacher:  In  general  terms  I  think  it  is  true  also.  We  have  the  same 
phenomenon  in  mice  but  not  to  the  same  degree. 

Verzdr:  Thyroxine  treatment  of  the  tadpole  and  axolotl,  which — 
as  you  know — leads  to  transformation  from  larval  to  adult  forms, 
always  leads  to  a  shortening  of  life.  The  transformed  animals  never 
survive  long. 


138  Discussion 

Nigrelli:  Is  that  true  of  well-fed  and  of  starved  tadpoles  ?  Well- 
fed  tadpoles  should  not  metamorphose  as  rapidly  as  starved  ones. 

Verzdr:  In  the  cases  which  I  know  of  the  tadpoles  were  well  fed. 

Lindop:  Mr.  Sacher,  you  were  trying  to  put  something  forward 
mathematically,  using  your  concept  of  lifespan.  I  tried  to  relate 
your  criterion  to  what  we  were  doing,  and  I  found  that  I  could  not 
apply  it.  In  a  discussion  group  like  this,  where  there  are  so  many 
different  disciplines,  and  we  are  each  putting  forward  our  own 
specialist  information,  a  short  discussion  on  what  is  the  most  useful 
concept  of  lifespan  for  us  to  be  able  to  compare  our  different  groups 
might  be  helpful.  People  who  have  experiments  which  are  half-way 
through  would  then  know  what  others  want  them  to  do  for  the  rest 
of  them. 

Can  you  use  your  mathematical  interpretations  to  get  a  mathe- 
matical correlation  between  lifespan  in  naturally  occurring  popula- 
tions and  in  populations  where  we  ha^^e  altered  the  lifespan  ?  By 
irradiation  we  have  altered  lifespans  both  by  your  definition  and 
ours,  but  we  have  not  changed  the  index  of  cephalization. 

Sacher:  There  are  certainly  many  factors  affecting  length  of  life. 
The  thing  has  to  be  put  into  perspective.  I  have  used  the  lifespan  I 
defined  above — the  maximum  attained  life — only  in  the  present 
context  of  doing  a  comparative  study  on  a  very  broad  scale.  When 
I  am  working  on  laboratory  data  I  usually  use  the  life  expectation, 
and  specifically  the  after-expectation  of  life  from  the  beginning  of 
exposure.  In  order  to  characterize  the  effects  of  radiations  on  popul- 
ations, we  have  found  that  it  is  particularly  convenient  to  discuss 
these  in  terms  of  the  log  rate  of  mortality  (Gompertz)  curve  (Sacher, 
1956;  Brues  A.  M.,  and  Sacher,  G.  A.  (1952).  In  Symposium  on 
Radiobiology,  ed.  Nickson,  J.  J.,  p.  441.  New  York:  John  Wiley). 
Before  discussing  this  I  wish  to  point  out  that  any  one  of  the  life- 
table  functions  contains  the  same  amount  of  statistical  information 
as  any  other,  as  long  as  you  have  not  lost  information  by  rounding, 
setting  up  large  class  intervals,  etc.  The  reason  for  preferring  some 
particular  analytical  function  of  the  basic  data  is  that  it  seems  to  give 
the  clearest  insight  into  underlying  mechanisms. 

Cohorts  of  mice  kept  under  laboratory  conditions  have  life-tables 
such  that  the  plot  of  logarithm  of  rate  of  mortality  (Gompertz 
transform)  either  is  a  straight  line  or  shows  a  moderate  amount  of 
curvature.  In  various  mouse  strains,  the  slope  of  the  best-fitting 
straight  line  at  advanced  ages  (omitting  the  mortality  primarily  due 
to  infectious  disease  in  young  mice)  does  not  vary  significantly.  In 
hybrids  showing  marked  hybrid  vigour  the  slope  again  remains 
unchanged,  and  the  increased  survival  is  due  to  a  decrease  in  the 


Discussion 


139 


intercept  values.  A  single  dose  of  X-rays  given  in  early  adult  life  is 
followed,  after  a  latent  period  of  100-200  days,  by  a  displacement  of 
the  Gompertz  transform  parallel  to  itself  (Fig.  1).  In  cases  where  the 
Gompertz  transform  shows  some  curvature,  it  is  possible  to  infer  that 
this  displacement  is  a  translation  to  the  left  on  the  time  axis.  The 
interpretation  is  that  the  Gompertz  transform  is  a  linear  measure  of 
the  amount  of  ageing  injury  present.  A  single  X-ray  dose  produces  a 
residue  of  permanent  injury,  and  this  is  manifested  by  a  displacement 
of  the  Gompertz  transform  by  a  constant  amount. 


1000- 


o 
o 
o 
o* 

IT. 
UJ 
Q. 


o 


UJ 
O 

o 


200  400  600  800 

TIME    FROM    BEGINNING    OF    EXPOSURE 

Fig.  1  (Sacher).  Schematic  representation  of  the  long-term 
effect  of  single  or  repeated  exposure  to  ionizing  radiations 
on  the  Gompertz  function  (logarithm  of  the  age- specific 
rate  of  mortality)  for  mammals.  The  age-trend  of  mortality 
is  considered  to  conform  to  the  Gompertz  equation  (compare 
with  equation  1  and  text) 


'°s(x-f)  =  "'g^  + 


OCX 


where  A'  is  the  number  living  at  age  x. 
O — unirradiated  population;  S^,  Sg, — populations  given 
single  exposures  at  time  zero;  R^,  Rg — populations  given 
repeated  or  continuous  exposure  beginning  at  time  zero. 
Early  portion  of  lines  S^  and  Sg  dotted  to  indicate  time 
needed  for  displacement  to  attain  its  steady  value. 


140 


Discussion 


If  each  X-ray  dose  produced  a  constant  displacement  of  the 
Gompertz  function,  and  if  they  added  with  one  another  as  well  as 
with  the  basic  ageing  trend,  then  a  cohort  exposed  daily  throughout 
adult  life  should  show  a  constant  divergence  from  controls,  and  a  set 
of  different  daily  doses  should  produce  a  fan  of  Gompertz  curves. 
This  is  in  fact  seen  (Sacher,  1956).  The  parameters  of  the  daily  dose 
response  are  consistent  with  those  of  the  single  dose  response. 


o 


> 

cc 

If) 


I- 

o 
< 
a: 


.00 

— 

=^ 

■\ 

N 

s 

^ 

- 

.80 

\ 

\ 

.60 

" 

\\ 
\  \ 
\  \ 

\  \ 

V         \ 

V                  \ 

.40 

- 

\    \ 
\    \ 
\     \ 

V       \ 

\  \             \ 
\  \              \ 

\     \'       \ 

"■ 

.20 
n 

1 

r\ 

\     \^        \ 

xS^rX^S,         \p 

200 


400 


600 


800 


TIME  FROM  BEGINNING  OF  EXPOSURE 
Fig.  2  (Sacher).  Survivorship  curves  corresponding  to  the 
schematic  Gompertz  functions  in  Fig.  1.  The  "single  dose" 
curves  can  be  superimposed  on  the  "control"  curve  by 
sliding  them  to  the  right.  The  "repeated  exposure"  curves 
can  be  superimposed  on  the  "control"  by  multiplications 
of  their  time  scales  by  scaling  factors. 


This  mode  of  analysis  therefore  gives  a  parsimonious  description  of 
radiation  mortality,  and  relates  it  to  the  ageing  process  in  terms  of  a 
life-table  function,  the  Gompertz  function,  where  properties  are 
consistent  with  the  hypothesis  that  the  function  is  a  linear  measure 
of  the  amount  of  ageing  injury  present  in  the  population.  A  theoreti- 
cal justification  of  this  hypothesis  can  be  given  in  terms  of  the  con- 
cepts of  physiological  fluctuations  and  probability  of  failure  that 
were  introduced  in  the  text  (Sacher,  1956). 

In  view  of  the  earlier  point,  that  any  one  of  the  life-table  functions 
contains  the  same  information  as  the  others,  it  follows  that  any 
other  desired  quantities,  such  as  expectations,  medians,  deciles, 


Discussion  141 

etc.,  can  be  computed  in  turn.  My  chief  concern  was  to  validate  a 
theory  of  radiation  mortahty  and  ageing.  However,  empirical 
analysis  and  theoretical  analysis  should  have  the  same  goals  of 
parsimonious  description.  Thus  the  Gompertz  function,  which  is  a 
theoretically  meaningful  one,  should  also  be  best  for  empirical 
analysis. 

Verzd?':  Could  you  describe  the  same  thing  with  survival  curves, 
Mr.  Sacher  ? 

Sacher:  An  animal  following  a  single  radiation  dose  acts  at  a  given 
age  like  a  control  animal  at  somewhat  greater  age.  The  irradiated 
population  tends  to  show  shallower  survivorship  curves  which  can 
be  translated  and  scaled  so  that  they  can  be  superimposed  on  a  con- 
trol population  of  a  later  starting  age  (Fig.  2).  This  can  be  accom- 
plished without  changing  the  time  scale,  and  corresponds  to  the  fact 
that  single  X-ray  doses  displace  the  Gompertz  function  parallel  to 
itself  without  change  of  slope.  If  we  give  daily  doses  of  irradiation, 
the  effect  is  not  as  if  we  had  set  the  clock  forward  but  rather  as  if  we 
had  changed  the  regulator,  so  that  the  clock  runs  faster.  Thus,  con- 
comitant with  the  decrease  in  survival  time  there  is  a  steepening  of 
the  survival  curve  in  the  daily  dose  condition.  That  corresponds  to 
the  fan  of  lines  on  the  log  rate  of  mortality  plot. 

Maynard  Smith:  In  comparing  life-tables  based  on  wild  popula- 
tions and  on  laboratory  populations,  I  think  what  both  Dr.  Comfort 
and  Mr.  Sacher  have  had  in  mind  here  is  that  what  such  distributions 
are  most  likely  to  have  in  common  is  the  maximum  lifespan;  the 
oldest  individuals  in  wild  populations  may  correspond  roughly  in 
age  to  the  oldest  individuals  in  laboratory  populations,  but  the  two 
distributions  have  little  else  in  common. 

Sacher:  I  can  agree  with  that.  It  is  not  yet  possible  to  reach  a 
meaningful  correlation  between  life-tables  in  the  field,  and  life- 
tables  in  controlled  environments.  These  conditions  are  so  far  apart 
that  we  cannot  discuss  the  respective  life-tables  in  terms  of  common 
parameters.  It  would  seem  that  there  have  to  be  intermediate 
grades  of  environment  between  the  wild  and  the  laboratory. 

Lindop:  Is  there  any  one  method  of  investigation  in  which, 
instead  of  going  through  the  changes  gradually,  we  could  correlate 
them  more  rapidly  ?  For  instance,  one  might  take  the  causes  of 
death  in  wild  animals  and  the  causes  of  death  in  laboratory  animals, 
exclude  from  each  group  the  causes  which  are  not  in  common,  and 
see  how  the  survival  curves  fitted  for  the  causes  of  death  which  are 
in  common. 

Sacher:  That  certainly  could  be  done  if  they  had  enough  causes  of 
death  in  common. 


A  REVIEW  OF  THE  LIFESPANS  AND 

MORTALITY  RATES  OF  FISH  IN  NATURE, 

AND  THEIR  RELATION  TO  GROWTH  AND 

OTHER  PHYSIOLOGICAL  CHARACTERISTICS 

R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

Ministry  of  Agriculture,  Fisheries  and  Food,  Fisheries  Laboratory, 
Lowestoft;  and  Fisheries  Biology  Branch,  F.A.O.,  Rome 

Studies  on  the  dynamics  of  fish  populations  have  received 
a  major  impetus  in  recent  years  owing  to  the  need  to  provide 
an  adequate  scientific  basis  for  conservation.  One  aspect  of 
these  studies  is  the  measurement  of  fish  longevity  and  the 
force  of  natural  mortality  in  fish  populations.  In  this  contri- 
bution we  attempt  to  review  the  present  state  of  knowledge 
on  these  questions. 

In  so  doing  we  have  two  objectives  in  mind.  One  is  to 
present  the  data  on  longevity  and  mortality  in  fish  for  com- 
parison with  what  is  known  for  other  animals  and  presented 
at  this  symposium;  the  other  is  to  see  to  what  extent  these 
characteristics  are,  in  fish,  associated  with  size,  growth, 
maturation  and  certain  other  physiological  factors  for  which 
data  are  available. 

It  has  not  been  possible  for  us  to  search  through  the  widely 
scattered  literature  as  thoroughly  as  we  would  have  wished. 
The  paper  is  therefore  perhaps  best  regarded  as  a  progress 
report  from  which  certain  tentative  conclusions  can  be  drawn 
at  this  stage. 

Natural  mortality  and  lifespan 

Many  of  the  fish  populations  which  have  been  intensively 
studied  are  those  supporting  a  major  commercial  fishery  and 
are  therefore  ones  in  which  the  effect  of  fishing  has  profoundly 

142 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  143 

influenced  the  shape  of  the  survival  curves  and  the  maximum 
age  recorded  in  samples.  There  are,  nevertheless,  a  certain 
number  of  instances  in  which  the  natural  age  composition,  or 


■    t         V**^  CLUPEOIDS 

\                \\         \        ^arenqus 

\                \         '"        \ 

I 
'1 

•  "l 

I sprattus 
\ 
1  1            1 

pallasi              \  "^ 
1              1       \ 

2- 


O  5  lO  15  20 


T         1          1 

■ 

% 

? 

''1 

\ 

\ 

Collionymus  \      \ 

lyra 

,  .\ 

Fig.   1.    Some  examples  of  survival  curves  in  relatively  unexploited  fish 

populations. 


something  fairly  close  to  it,  has  been  determined,  and  some 
examples  of  these  are  shown  in  Fig.  1.  It  will  be  noted  that  in 
no  case  do  the  data  cover  the  whole  lifespan  from  birth  on- 
wards ;  this  is  because  representative  sampling  of  the  fry  and 


144  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

juveniles  is  seldom  possible.  It  is  known,  however,  that  in 
many  species,  and  especially  in  those  which  lay  large  numbers 
of  eggs,  there  is  a  very  high  mortality  during  the  first  weeks 
of  life ;  in  North  Sea  plaice,  for  example,  only  about  one  in  ten 
thousand  survive  the  first  few  months.  Thus  the  survival 
curve  for  fish  characteristically  descends  very  rapidly  at  first 
and  then  flattens  out,  though  in  viviparous  species,  and 
species  which  lay  a  small  number  of  eggs  but  afford  the  newly- 
hatched  fry  some  degree  of  parental  care,  this  initial  descent 
is  probably  less  marked. 

Even  after  the  early  phase  of  heavy  mortality  some  con- 
siderable time  may  elapse  before  the  fish  have  grown  large 
enough  to  be  retained  by  the  fishing  gear,  so  that  a  representa- 
tive survival  curve  has  to  begin  at  some  later  age  when  the 
individuals  are  first  properly  represented  in  the  samples.  The 
survival  curves  shown  in  Fig.  1  therefore  start  at  the  age 
group  which  is  most  abundant  in  catches,  and  for  comparison 
all  the  data  have  been  adjusted  to  a  peak  number  of  1,000. 
The  numbers  are  plotted  on  a  logarithmic  scale,  so  that  a 
linear  survival  curve  indicates  a  constant  natural  mortality 
rate  independent  of  age,  whereas  a  downward  curve  shows 
that  the  mortality  rate  is  increasing  with  age.  The  broken 
lines  are  drawn  purely  to  assist  the  eye  in  detecting  linearity 
or  departures  from  it,  and  where  the  survival  curve  is  not  a 
straight  line  the  broken  lines  are  drawn  through  the  first  and 
last  points. 

In  the  long-lived  species,  of  which  the  examples  shown  in 
Fig.  1  are  sturgeon  (Acipenser  fulvescens ;  Probst  and  Cooper, 
1954),  whitefish  (Coregonus  clupeaformis ;  Hart,  1931)  and 
perch  {Perca  fluviatilis ;  Aim,  1952),  the  raortality  ra,te-  seems 
to  be  effectively  constant  over  a  considerable  span  of  age  at 
about  5  to  10  per  cent  per  year,  although  in  the  age  groups  of 
sturgeon  beyond  about  30  years  the  mortality  rate  appears 
to  increase.  The  fluctuations  in  the  data  for  sturgeon  and 
whitefish  are  partly  due  to  the  fact  that  sampling  was  possible 
for  a  limited  period  only  and  that  the  age  groups  refer  to 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  145 

different  year  classes  of  fish  whose  initial  abundance  varies 
considerably.  The  perch  data  are  unique  because  they  show 
the  survival_of  a  known  number  of  fish  introduced  into 
experimental  ponds — in  the  one  case  as  fry  {a)  and  in  the 
tJtheF'asrfive-year-old  fish  (b) — and  then  sampled  regularly, 
for  15^  years  and  17  years  respectively;  within  these  spans 
of  age  there  is  ho  evideiice  of  a  varying  age-specific  mortality 
rate. 

Of  the  shorter-lived  species  shown  in  Fig.  1,  nearly  all  have 
a  survival  curve  with  some  degree  of  downward  curvature 
over  nearly  the  whole  range.  This  is  seen  particularly  clearly 
Tnjthe  herring  data  (Clupea  spp.),  of  which  those  for  the 
Norwegian  herring  (C.  harengus;  Lea,  1930)  are  the  combined 
data  for  a  period  of  twenty  years  in  which  year-class  fluctu- 
ation has  been  largely  eliminated.  The  data  for  the  bullhead 
(Cottus  gobio;  Smyly,  1957)  and  the  dragonet  (Callionymus 
lyra;  Chang,  1951)  are  included  to  show  the  difference  be- 
tween the  survival  curves  for  males  and  females.  In  both  of 
these  the  males  have  a  higher  mortality  rate  and  a  shorter 
lifespan,  and  this  is  indeed  what  is  usually  found  where 
there  is  any  difference  between  the  sexes;  we  have,  however, 
come  across  one  or  two  exceptions  which  are  referred  to  later. 

The  tendency  for  the  natural  mortality  rate  to  increase 
with  age,  which  is  noticeable  in  several  of  the  examples  shown 
in  Fig.  1,  is,  indeed,  found  more  often  than  not,  and  for  other 
instances  the  reader  is  referred  to  papers  by,  for  example, 
Kennedy  (1954)  on  the  Lake  trout  {Cristivomer  namaycush), 
by  Wohlschlag  (1954a)  on  the  Alaskan  whitefish  (Leucichthys 
sardinella),  and  by  Ricker  (1949)  on  several  species.  This 
variation  of  the  mortality  rate  with  age  reaches  an  extreme 
form  in  species  where  all  or  nearly  all  individuals  die  at,  or 
soon  after,  spawning  for  the  first  time.  The  best  known 
instance  of  this  is  in  the  Pacific  salmon  (Oncorhynchus  spp.), 
which  migrates  up-river  from  the  sea  at  between  three  and 
five  years  of  age,  spawns  and  then  dies.  The  immature  phase 
of  the  life-cycle  spent  in  the  sea  has  only  recently  been  studied, 


146  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

but  there  is  no  reason  to  believe  that  the  mortahty  rate 
during  that  time  is  abnormally  high.  Other  examples  of  a 
catastrophic  mortality  at,  or  shortly  after  spawning  include 
the  Tasmanian  whitebait  {Lovettia  seali;  Blackburn,  1950); 
the  capelin  {Mallotus  villosus;  Templeman,  1948);  the  small 
freshwater  atherinid  Labidesihes  sicculus  (Hubbs,  1921) 
which  spawns  at  about  one  year  of  age  and  then  dies  off 
within  a  further  two  or  three  months;  and,  probably,  the 
dragonet  {Callionymus  lyra;  Chang,  1951).  In  most  of  these 
it  is  the  males  which  suffer  the  most  severe  mortality,  the 
evidence  being  that  a  proportion  of  the  females  spawn  more 
than  once,  even  though  that  proportion  may  be  small. 

Survival  curves  in  fish  thus  range  from  effective  linearity 
over  the  whole  of  the  observed  range  of  age  to  sharp  dis- 
continuity at  the  onset  of  maturity j^  with  a  wide  range  of 
intermediates  in  which  the  mortality  rate  increases  steadily 
with  age  without  obvious  discontinuity.  This  makes  it  dif- 
ficult to  adopt  any  single  numerical  index  as  an  index  of  life::, 
span,  or  of  force  of  mortality,  for  all  species.  Thus  the  maxi- 
mum age  recorded  in  samples  is  satisfactory  for  the  species  in 
which  the  mortality  rate  increases  fairly  sharply  with  age,  but 
is  less  so  in  the  long-lived  species,  where  it  becomes  rather 
critically  dependent  on  the  size  of  the  samples  and  on  the 
accuracy  of  the  age-determination  technique.  Conversely, 
the  average  mortality  rate  is  not  a  particularly  useful  measure 
where  the  mortality  rate  is  highly  age-specific,  but  is  satis- 
factory in  the  long-lived  species  with  nearly  linear  survival 
curves. 

For  the  time  being  we  have  therefore  tabulated  wherever 
possible  both  the  maximum  age  recorded  in  the  sample  (T^^ax) 
and  the  average  instantaneous  coefficient  of  natural  mortality 
(M)  over  the  range  of  age  groups  which,  as  far  as  we  could 
judge,  were  fully  represented  in  the  same  samples.  These  are 
given  in  Table  I,  from  which  it  will  be  seen  that  the  lifespan  of 
fish  can  range  from  little  more  than  a  year  in  several  quite 
unrelated  species  including  Labidesihes,  of  the  mullet  family, 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  147 

and  Lovettia  seali  of  the  salmonoid  family  mentioned  above, 
Hypomesus  olidus,  the  pond  smelt,  another  of  the  salmonoids, 
and  the  dwarf  sea-horse  {Hippocampus  hudsonius),  to  over  80 
years  in  the  Lake  sturgeon  {Acipenser  fulvescens).  The  maxi- 
mum recorded  age  we  have  found  is,  in  fact,  for  this  latter 
species  (Anonymous,  1954),  a  specimen  206  cm.  long  taken  in 
an  Ontario  lake  having  been  assigned  an  age  of  152  years  by 
examining  the  structure  of  the  pectoral  fin-ray.  While  it  is 
quite  possible  that  the  precise  age  of  such  a  fish  cannot  reliably 
be  determined  in  this  way,  the  work  of  a  number  of  authors 
on  the  longevity  of  this  and  related  species  of  sturgeon  is  con- 
sistent in  showing  that  they  can  live  to  a  great  age,  and  it  is 
indeed  not  unlikely  that  the  occasional  truly  centenarian 
sturgeon  is  still  to  be  found  in  the  more  remote  water  basins 
as  yet  untouched  by  man.  Apart  from  sturgeon  and  the 
whitefish  mentioned  above,  other  long-lived  species  include 
the  Arctic  char  {Salvelinus  alpinus;  Grainger,  1953)  and  the 
halibut  (Hippoglossus  spp.);  whitefish  and  char  are  both 
salmonoids,  but  sturgeon  and  halibut  are  of  different  sub- 
orders, so  that  neither  of  the  extremes  of  lifespan  in  fish  are 
confined  to  a  particular  taxonomic  group. 

In  compiling  the  data  on  maximum  age  in  Table  I  we  have 
not  used  the  records  for  fish  in  captivity,  of  which  a  recent 
summary  is  included  in  the  longevity  data  given  by  Brown 
(1957)  and  further  data  are  presented  to  this  colloquium  by 
Nigrelli.  There  is  nevertheless  a  broad  agreement  between 
the  records  from  the  two  sources,  and  the  few  instances  in 
which  there  is  reason  to  believe  that  the  entry  in  Table  I  may 
be  substantially  below  the  true  maximum  age  of  the  species 
are  noted  in  the  legend  to  that  Table. 

Size  and  growth 

Longevity  and  body  sizes  are  known  to  be  associated  in 
higher  vertebrates,  especially  in  mammals  (e.g.  Sacher,  this 
volume),  so  that  it  is  of  interest  to  see  to  what  extent  the 
same  is  true  of  fish.  The  growth  cycle  in  fish  is,  however,  more 


148 


R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 


Table  I 


COLLECTED  DATA  ON  GROWTH,  MORTALITY,  LONGEVITY  AND 

Notes:  (i)  Loo  and  K  are  the  parameters  of  the  growth  equation  (1)  given  on  p.  157, 
Lm  =  mean  length  at  maturity. 

(ii)  In  a  few  instances  the  value  of  Tmax  obtained  from  the  age  composition  samples 
longevity  of  the  same  or  closely  related  species  in  captivity,  of  which  a  recent  sum- 
ages  recorded  by  Brown  are  as  follows:  Gadus  virens,  14  years;  Melanogrammus 
Dasyatis  pastinaca,  21  years. 


Species 

Common  Name 

Locality 

Clupeoidei 

Clupea  harengus 

Atlantic  herring 

North  Sea 

C.  harengus 

Atlantic  herring 

Norwegian  Sea 

C.  harengus 
C.  harengus 

Atlantic  herring 
Atlantic  herring 

Lusterfjord  (Norway) 
New  Brunswick 

C.  pallasii 

Pacific  herring 

Canada  (west  coast) 

C.  sprattus 

Sprat 

North  Sea 

Sardinops  caerulea 

California  sardine 

California 

S.  neopilchardus 

Australian  sardine 

Australasia 

Gadiformes 

Boreogadus  saida 

— 

Arctic  Ocean 

Gadus  callarias 

Cod 

North  Sea 

G.  callarias 

Cod 

Barents  Sea 

G.  minutus 

Poor  cod 

English  Channel 

G.  minutus 

Poor  cod 

Mediterranean 

G.  virens 

Coalfish 

Norwegian  Sea 

Melanogrammus  aeglefinus 

Haddock 

North  Sea 

Merluccius  merluccius 

Hake 

Marmora  Sea 

Pleuronectoidei 

Citharichthys  sordidus 

Sand  dab 

California 

Hippoglossus  vulgaris 

Halibut 

N.  Atlantic 

Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  149 

SIZE  AT  MATURITY  OF  FISH  IN  NATURAL  POPULATIONS 

M  =  natural   mortality   coefficient,    Tmax  =  maximum   age   recorded   in   samples, 

(indicated  by  an  asterisk)  is  somewhat  below  that  recorded  from  records  of  the 
mary  has  been  compiled  by  Brown  (1957).  For  the  fish  in  question,  the  maximum 
aeglefimis,  14^15  years;  Salmo  trutta,  18  years  and  more;  Anguilla  anguilla,  55  years; 


Author 

Sex 

(cm.) 

K 

M 

-t^max 
(sample) 

(cm..) 

LmlLoo 

Burd  (unpub.) 

30 

0-38 

0-25 

12 

24 

0-80 

/Lea  (1930) 
\Runnstrom  (1936) 

34 

0-27 

<0-2 

22 

28 

0-82 

Aasen  (1952) 

21 

0-65 

0-78 

10 

— 

— 

Tibbo  (1957) 

34 

0-36 

— 

19 

25 

0-74 

/Tester  (1937) 
\Ricker  (1958) 

23 

0-29 

0-56 

11 

— 

— 

Robertson  (1938) 

13 

0-70 

<l-2 

5  '5 

10 

0-77 

^  Clark  (1940) 

Silliman  (1943) 
^  Phillips  (1948) 

26 

0-39 

015 

13 

18-5 

0-71 

Beverton  &  Holt 

(1957) 

Blackburn  (1950) 

20-5 

0-22 

— 

6-5 

9(?) 

0-44(?) 

VNIRO  (1949) 

22 

0-67 

5 

'Beverton  &  Holt 

<       (1957) 

132 

0-2 

~0-2 

>11 

— 

— 

(^Beverton  (unpub.) 

/Rollefsen  (1954) 
\  Taylor  (1958) 

134 

01 

— 

23 

85 

0-64 

Menon  (1950) 

{T 

20 
24 

0-42 
0-40 

11 

0-9 

5 
5 

11 
13 

0-55 
0-54 

Vives  &  Suau 

21 

0-97 

>2-3 

2 

— 

— 

(1956) 

Gottlieb  (1957) 

107 

019 

015 

10* 

71 

0-66 

r  Beverton  &  Holt 

^       (1957) 

53 

0-20 

>0-2 

10* 

26 

0-49 

tRaitt  (1939) 

Akyuz  (1959) 

{T 

44 
60 

0-13 
0   10 

0-6 
0-5 

10 
10 

23 
27 

0-52 
0-45 

Arora  (1951) 

/m 
\f 

30 
>30 

0-3\ 
<0-3/ 

<0-3 

7 
8 

19 

<0-63 

Devoid  (1938) 

/m 
\f 

170 
250 

004 
0  02 

— 

30 
30 

95 
132 

0-56 
0-53 

150 


R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

Table  I — continved 


Species 

Common  Natne 

Locality 

H.  stenolepis 

Hahbut 

N.  Pacific 

Isopsetta  isolepis 

Butter  sole 

Canada  (west  coast) 

Pleuronectes  platessa 

Plaice 

North  Sea 

Pseudopleuronectes 
americanus 

Winter  flounder 

Canada  (east  coast) 

Solea  vulgaris 

Sole 

North  Sea 

Salmonoidei 

Argentina  semifaxiata 
Coregonus  clupeaformis 
C.  clupeaformis 

C.  clupeaformis 
C.  clupeaformis 

Argentine 
Whitefish 
Whitefish 

Whitefish 
Whitefish 

Japan 

L.  Nipigon,  Canada 

Shakespeare  Is.  Lake, 

Canada 
L.  Opeongo,  Canada 
Trout  Lake,  Wisconsin 

C.  clupeaformis 

Dwarf  Whitefish 

L.  Opeongo,  Canada 

Cristivomer  namaycush 

Lake  Trout 

Gt.  Slave  L.,  Canada 

Hypomesus  olidus 

Pond  smelt 

L.  Suwa,  Japan 

Leucichthys  artedi 

Cisco 

Wisconsin,  Trout  Lake 

,,         Muskel- 
lenge  L. 

,,         Silver  L. 

- 

,,         Clear  L. 

L.  kiyi 

Chub 

U.S.A. 

L.  sardinella 

Whitefish 

L.  Ikroavik,  Alaska 
Tasmania 

Lovettia  seali 

Tasmanian  whitebait 

Mallotus  villosus 

Capelin 

Labrador 

Oncorhynchus  nerka 

Sockeye  salmon 

Cultus  L.,  Canada 

Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  151 


Author 

Sex 

L(X) 

(cm.) 

K 

M 

^max 
[sample 

(cm.) 

LmlLoo 

'Thompson  & 

Herrington  (1930) 

Similar  to 

<0-3 

^  Thompson  &  Bell 
(1934) 

//.  5 

mlgaris 

Hart  (1948) 

{T 

38 
42 

0-36 
0-26 

<1-41 
<102 

10 

18 
21 

0-47 
0-50 

Beverton  (unpub.) 

{T 

45 

70 

015 
0-08 

0-22 
012 

13 
22 

25 

28 

0-56 
0-40 

Dickie  & 

McCracken  (1955) 

44 

0-4 

0-3 

>10 

32 

0-73 

f  Beverton  &  Holt 

<       (1957) 
(^Margetts  (mipub.) 

39 

0-4 

:^0-25 

>8 

— ■ 

— 

Hanyu  (1956) 

19 

1-2 



2 

— 

— 

Hart  (1931) 

50 

013 

0  17 

24 

~27 

^0-55 

Hart  (1931) 

49 

009 

015 

27 

^27 

::^0-55 

Kennedy  (1943) 

70 

0-06 

<0-5 

12 

— 

— 

Hile  &  Deason 

44 

009 

— 

14 

>23 

>0-52 

(1934) 

/Kennedy  (1943) 
\Rieker  (1949) 

14 

0-43 

1-3 

5 

— 

— 

Kennedy  (1954) 

56 

007 

0-6 

25 

18-4 

0-33 

rShiraishi  (1957) 
\Sato  (1950) 

11-12 

1-5-1 -8 

11-3 -8 

1-3 

10 

2::0-9 

Cm 

V 

19 

0-65 

1  -1 
11 

6 
11 

12-5 

0-66 

J  m 
If 

1     TTi 

21 

0-36 

1-2 
1-2 
1  •  1 

3 

4, 

150 

0-72 

>Hile  (1936) 

J 

If 

32 

006 

J.       X 

0-9 

7 

14-0 

0-44 

Jm 
\f 

39 

0-27 

0-4 
0-3 

9 
11 

130(?) 

0-33(?) 

Deason  &  Hile 
(1947) 

{T 

28 

0-51 

<0-9 

<0-8 

7 
10 

<18 

<0-64 

'  Wohlschlag 

(1954a,  b)  (and 

38 

0-40 

0-6 

11 





1      personal  comm.) 

Cohen  (1954) 

Blackburn  (1950) 

6-7 

— 

— 

2 

5(?) 

0-8(?) 

Templeman  (1948) 

{T 

20 
19 

0-48 
0-48 

1-3 

5 
5 

18 
17 

0-90 
0-90 

Foerster  (1929) 

69 

0-58 

— 

6 

60 

0-87 

152 


R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

Table  I — continued 


Species 

Common  Name 

Locality 

Columbia  R.,  Canada 

0.  keta 

Chum  salmon 

4-year  spawners 
3-year  spawners 

Salmo  salar 

Atlantic  salmon 

Scotland 

S.  trutta 

Trout 

L.  Windermere, 
England 

Salvelinus  alpinus 

Char 

Baffin  I.,  Canada 

ACIPENSERIFORMES 

Acipenser  fulvescens 

Lake  sturgeon 

Wisconsin 

J  f  medirostris 

'\transmontanus 
A.  nudiventris 

^\liite  sturgeon 
Sturgeon 

California 
Europe 

Anguilloidei 
Anguilla  anguilla 

Eel 

Windermere 

Blennioidei 
Blennius  pholis 

Blenny 

Welsh  coast 

Callionymoidei 

Callionymus  lyra 

Dragonet 

English  Channel 

COTTOIDEI 

Coitus  gobio 

Bullhead 

Windermere 
R.  Brathay 

Cyprinodontiformes 

Gambusia  holbrookii 

Top  minnow 

Portugal 

Cyprinoidei 
Phoxinus  phoxinus 

Minnow 

Windermere 

Gasterosteiformes 
Gasteroteus  aculeatus 

3-spined  stickleback 
10-spined  stickleback 

Cheshire 
>> 

Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  153 


Author 


Marr  (1943) 

Nail  (1927) 

Frost  &  Smyly 
(1952) 

Grainger  (1953) 

Probst  &  Cooper 
(1954) 

Pycha  (1956) 

Paeeagnella  (1948) 

Frost  (1945) 

Qasim  (1957) 

Chang  (1951) 

■Smyly  (1957) 

Da  Franca  (1953) 
Frost  (1943) 


Jones  &  Hynes 
(1950) 


Sex 


{ 


(Mostly  f ) 


{ 


/m 


{ 


ni 
f 


Lao 

(cm.) 


120 
105 
106 
102 

125 

30 

150 
140 


178 

300 
250 

165 

17 


250 
17-5 


7-2 
7-3 
6-5 
6-5 


0-36 

002 
003 


005 

0-06 
0  04 

0  02 

0-30 


0-43 
0-55 


0-7 
0-4 
0-9 
0-5 


1-2 

0-8 


0-55 


0-64 
1-6 


M 


(3-0) 

(1-2) 

l-l(f) 

0-94 

0-24 
0-24 

001 
003 


^max 
(sample) 


6(f) 
5(m) 

8* 

24  + 

24  + 


82 

30 
30 


V.  small  (17)* 


0-9 


0-96 
0-86 


11 

0-9 
0-9 

0-8 


<l-6 
<0-8 


11 


0-9 
11 


{cm.) 


\70 


24 

60 

100-125 

100-125 
119-141 


Lm/Lo 


0-68 
0-72 
0-72 
0-69 


0-75 
0-43 

~0-6 

~0'4 
^0-5 


60 

0-37 

8 

0-47 

17-4 

0-70 

4-6 

0-64 

4-2 

0-58 

^5 

^0-77 

~5 

:^0-77 

3-5-4 

:^0-4 

3-6 

0-54 

3-7 

0-86 

154 


R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

Table  I — continued 


Species 


MUGILOIDEI 

Leuresthes  tenuis 
Labidesthes  sicculus 

Percoidei 

Cynoscion  macdonaldi 

Perca  fluviatilis 

P.  fluviatilis 
Sillago  sihama 

Stizostedion  canadensis 

Rajiformes 
Dasyatis  akajei 

Scombroidei 

Rastrelliger  neglectus 
Pneumatophorus  diego 

P.  japonicus 

SiLUROIDEI 

Ictalurus  lacustris  punctatus 


Syngnathiformes 

Hippocampus  hudsonius 

H,  hudsonius 

Thunniformes 

Neothunnus  macropterus 
Thunnus  thynnus 
Istiophorus  americanus 


Common  Name 


California  grunion 
Brook  silverside 

Totoaba 

Perch 

Perch 

Indian  sand  whiting 

Sanger 


Ray 


Chub  mackerel 
Pacific  mackerel 

Japanese  mackerel 


Locality 


California 
U.S.A. 

Mexico 

Sweden  (a) 

Sweden  (b) 
S.  India 

L.  Nipigon,  Canada 


Japan 


Gulf  of  Thailand 
California 


Channel  catfish 

Mississippi  R 

Sea  horse 

Florida 

Pigmy  sea  horse 

Florida 

Yellowfin  tuna 
Bluefin  tuna 
Sailfish 

Hawaii 
North  Sea 
Atlantic 

Japan 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  155 


Author 

Sex 

Leo 

(cm.) 

K 

M 

Tma.x 
(sample) 

(cm.) 

LmjLco 

Clark  (1925) 

{T 

17-8 
18-4 

0-33 
0-39 

<l-3 

3 
3 

110 
11-9 

0-62 
0-65 

Hubbs  (1921) 

9-2 

3-7 

— 

1-3 

70 

0-76 

Berdegue  (1955) 
Aim (1952) 

{T 

128 
30 

0-3 
0-20 

0-3 
0-29 

15 
>16 

8-12 
13-19 

^0-33 
2^0-53 

Aim  (1952) 
Radhakrishnan 

34 
37 

0  13 
0-4 

016 

>22 
4 

>13 

>0-35 

(1957) 
/Hart  (1928) 
\Ricker  (1949) 

40 

0  14 

0-44 

13 

>  32(f) 

>0-8 

Yokota  (1951) 

{T 

105 
150 

01 
01 

l-8(?) 
0-4r-0-5 

4(?)* 
7(?)* 

40 
44 

0-38 
0-29 

Holt  (1959a) 
Fitch  (1951  and 

1956) 
Holt  (19596) 

22 

40 

46 

0-7 
0-4 

0-25-0'4 

<2 

0-8-1   0 

2 
9 

4^5 

17 
32 

28-33 

0-77 
0-80 

C^0  67 

Appelget  &  Smith 
(1951) 

119 

006 

<0-8 

12 

36 

0-30 

Herald  &  Rakowicz 

14 

2-5 

^1 

1 

7 

0-50 

(1951) 
Strawn  (1958) 

2 

12 

2-3 

1 

2 

10 

Moore  (1951) 
Tiews  (1957) 
de  Sylva  (1957) 

190 
270 
236 

0-5 
0-6 
11 

0-8 

5 
13 
3-5 

— 

— 

156  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

protracted  than  in  most  higher  vertebrates,  and  as  a  conse- 
quence the  maximum  size  is  often  not  reached  within  the 
range  of  age  covered  by  the  data.  This  makes  it  necessary 
to  examine  in  more  detail  the  actual  pattern  of  growth  in 
fish  in  order  to  arrive  at  suitable  indices  to  correlate  with 
longevity. 

Fig.  2  gives  a  few  examples  of  the  growth  in  length  of  fish. 
We  use  length  as  the  measure  of  body  size  rather  than  weight 
because,  as  can  be  seen  from  Fig.  2,  growth  in  length  nearly 
always  follows  a  simple  curve  without  an  inflection.*  This 
is  true  whether  the  species  is  one  which  can  grow  to  a  large 
or  to  a  small  size,  and  whether  it  completes  its  growth 
pattern  rapidly  or  slowly.  Examples  of  all  these  are  included 
in  Fig.  2. 

In  the  upper  part  of  the  diagram  is  shown  the  growth  of 
sturgeon  {A.  nudiventris),  which  both  grows  slowly  (i.e.  com- 
pletes its  growth  pattern  slowly)  and  also  attains  a  large  size, 
that  of  sockeye  salmon  (0.  nerka;  Foerster,  1929)  which  grows 
to  a  fairly  large  size  but  does  so  rapidly,  and  that  of  whitefish 
{Coregonus  clupeaformis)  which  grows  slowly  to  a  rather  smaller 
size.  In  the  lower  part  of  Fig.  2  are  some  examples  of  the  smaller 
species,  and  for  these  the  scales  of  both  length  and  age  are 
increased  roughly  fivefold;  to  aid  comparison,  the  growth  of 
Lusterfjord  herring  {Clupea  harengus;  Aasen,  1952)  is  shown 
in  both  parts  of  the  diagram.  It  will  be  seen  that  although 
the  smaller  species  usually  develop  their  growth  pattern  more 
rapidly  than  do  the  larger  species,  there  is  still  quite  a  range 
of  variation.  Thus,  for  its  size,  the  blenny  {Blennius  pholis; 
Qasim,  1957)  is  relatively  slow-growing,  whereas  Labidesthes 
has  virtually  reached  its  maximum  size  in  little  more  than  a 
year;  and  the  10-spined  stickleback  (Pygosteus  pungitius; 
Jones  and  Hynes,  1950),  although  growing  to  little  more  than 
half  the  size  of  Labidesthes,  takes  several  years  to  do  so. 

*  Since  the  growth  of  most  fish  is  closely  isometric  after  the  juvenile  phase, 
the  curve  of  growth  in  weight  is  approximated  to  by  cubing  that  of  growth  in 
length.  This  produces  a  weight-growth  curve  which  has  an  inflection  at  about 
one-third  of  the  asymptotic  weight. 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  157 


It  is  found  that  all  the  growth  curves  shown  in  Fig.  2  and, 
indeed,  those  for  a  great  many  other  species  of  fish,  can  be 
adequately  represented  by  the  mono-molecular  or  inverse 
exponential  equation  which,  in  its  simplest  form,  is 


i,  =  L„(l-e-«) 


(1) 


ISO 


lOO 


L(cm.) 


Acipenser  nudiventris 


L(cm) 


Oncorhynchus  y 

nerka 


.oreqonus 
T  clupeaformis 

Clupea   harenqus 


25  30 


4  5b 

ACE     (yr.)- 

Fig.  2.   Some  examples  of  curves  of  growth  in  length  of  fish. 


where  l^  is  the  length  at  age  t,  L^  is  the  maximum  or  asympto- 
tic length  and  ^  is  a  constant  which  determines  the  curva- 
ture of  the  growth  curve,  that  is,  the  rate  at  which  the  asymp- 
totic length  L^  is  approached  (see,  e.g.  von  Bertalanffy,  1938; 


158 


R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 


Beverton  and  Holt,  1957).  There  is  evidence  that  both  the 
constants  L^  and  K  have  a  physiological  significance,  as  will 
be  mentioned  later;  at  this  stage  we  need  only  regard  equation 


200  r 


I50- 


100 


Fig.  3.  Growth  data  of  Fig.  2  plotted  as  length  at  age  t  against  length  at  age 
<  +  !.  The  slope  of  the  line  drawn  through  the  points  is  e-^  and  the  inter- 
section with  the  bisector  (shown  as  a  broken  line)  gives  an  estimate  of  L. 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  159 

(1)  as  a  means  of  representing  mathematically  the  general 
growth  pattern  of  fish  in  terms  of  two  parameters  to  provide 
a  simple  means  of  relating  size  and  growth  to  mortality  and 
lifespan. 

It  is  a  property  of  equation  (1)  that  it  can  be  transformed 
to  a  linear  function  relating  length  at  age  t  to  length  at  age 
^  +  1,  namely: 

/,^i  =  L„(l-e-^)  +  /,e-^  (2) 

Fig.  3  shows  the  growth  curves  of  Fig.  2  plotted  in  this  way. 
From  equation  (2)  it  will  be  seen  that  the  slope  of  the  line 
drawn  through  the  points  provides  an  estimate  of  e~^,  and 
hence  of  K;  and  that  the  intersection  of  the  line  with  the 
bisector  drawn  through  the  origin  (shown  by  broken  lines  in 
Fig.  3)  gives  an  estimate  of  the  asymptotic  length  L^.  Esti- 
mates of  L^  and  K  for  all  the  species  under  consideration  are 
listed  in  Table  I. 

Apart  from  providing  a  means  of  estimating  the  two  para- 
meters of  the  growth  equation  (1),  plotting  Z^  against  Z^^^  in 
this  way  is  a  valuable  technique  for  the  comparative  analysis 
of  growth  curves  (Walford,  1946).  For  example,  it  can  be  seen 
from  Fig.  3  that  male  plaice  not  only  have  a  lower  L^  than  do 
females,  but  also  grow  towards  it  rather  more  quickly,  i.e. 
they  have  a  higher  K.  In  the  case  of  Lahidesihes  sicculus 
(insert  in  lower  part  of  Fig.  3)  the  lengths  are  at  monthly 
instead  of  yearly  intervals,  but  when  plotted  one  against  the 
next  they  nevertheless  give  a  close  approximation  to  a  straight 
line;  in  this  case,  however,  the  slope  is  e"^^^,  and  so  in  reality 
is  very  much  flatter  than  the  other  graphs  of  Fig.  3.  The 
method  is  also  useful  for  detecting  departures  from  the  simple 
growth  pattern  which  sometimes  arise  because  of  special  en- 
vironmental conditions,  of  which  lack  of  uniformity  in  the 
supply  of  food  to  fish  of  difiPerent  sizes  is  usually  the  most 
important  (see  below  and  also  papers  by  Aim,  1946,  and 
Decider,  1951). 


160 


R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 


Interspecific  relations  between  maximum  age  (Tj^ax)* 

mortality  rate  (M),  asymptotic  length  (L^)  and 

growth  rate  (K) 

Table  I  lists,  for  each  species,  values  of  a  pair  of  parameters 
defining  lifespan  and  the  force  of  mortality  {T^^^  and  M)  and 
a  pair  defining  the  asymptotic  size  of  the  organism  and  the 
rate  at  which  that  size  is  attained  {L^  and  K).  The  para- 
meters T^iax  ^^d  ^  ^r^'  of  course,  closely  linked  on  purely 


50  lOO 


mathematical  grounds ;  there  is  no  a  priori  reason  why  L^and_ 
K  should  be,  but  it  appears  from  the  data  that  they  ar'e  fairly, 
closely  correlated  (inversely),  although  there  are  some  im- 
portant exceptions.  In  this  paper  we  therefore  consider  only 
two  of  the  possible  relationships,  that  between  T^^ax  ^^^  -^oo 
concerning  the  extremes  of  age  and  size,  and  that  bfijtw£^n 
M  and  K  which,  in  effect,  refer  to  the  course  of  events  within 
the  lifespan.  Other  possible  relationships  which  might  give  a 
better  interpretation  of  the  available  data  are  under  investiga- 
tion. 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  161 


30 


25 


20- 


100                    I50 
L^(cm) 


200 


•    • 


SALMONOIDEI 


/     true   salmon 


ISO 


200 


250 


Fig.  4.  Relation  between  maximum  age  (Tmax)  and  asymptotic  length  (Loo) 
in  the  Clupeoidei,  Gadiformes,  Pleuronectoidei  and  Salmonoidei  (from  Table 
I).  Because  the  correlation  between  Tmax  and  M,  and  Loo  and  K,  is  inverse 
the  species  appearing  in  the  bottom  left-hand  corner  of  Figs.  4  and  5  tend  to 
occur  in  the  top  right-hand  corner  of  Figs.  6  and  7 ;  this  point  should  be  borne 
in  mind  when  comparing  the  two  sets  of  diagrams. 


AGEING — V — 6 


162  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

In  four  of  the  main  groups  of  fish  there  is  a  sufficient  range 
of  values  to  enable  each  group  to  be  examined  separately. 
These  are  the  herring  and  related  species  (Clupeoidei),  the  cod 
family  (Gadiformes),  the  salmonoids  (Salmonoidei)  and  the 
flatfishes  (Pleuronectoidei).  Fig.  4  shows  the  relations  between 
L^  and  T^^^ax  i^  these  groups.  In  each  case  there  is  a  well- 
defined  trend,  especially  so  in  the  Clupeoidei  which  are  per- 
haps a  more  homogeneous  group  than  the  others.  The  lines 
drawn  through  the  points  have  no  statistical  significance, 
since  the  precise  accuracy  of  the  individual  points  is  largely 
unknown,  and  in  some  cases  the  values  recorded  are  certainly 
over-  or  under-estimates  of  the  true  values ;  this  is  particularly 
so  with  the  parameters  Tj^^ax  ^^^  ^'>  which  are  more  difficult 
to  determine  accurately  than  are  the  growth  parameters  L^ 
and  K,  and  the  lines  have  been  drawn  with  these  considera- 
tions in  mind. 

Despite  these  qualifications,  it  does  seem  that  the  line  for 
the  Clupeoidei  differs  from  that  for  the  other  groups,  the  larger 
members  of  the  herring  family  appearing  to  live  to  a  greater 
age  than  do  fish  of  the  other  groups  of  a  comparable  size,  the 
contrast  being  most  noticeable  with  the  gadoids.  The  scatter 
of  the  points  is  most  marked  in  the  salmonoids,  which  may  be 
a  reflection  of  the  heterogeneity  of  this  group  and  of  the  varied 
environments  in  which  members  of  it  are  found,  since  they 
include  marine,  freshwater  and  anadromous  species.  The 
true  salmon,  ringed  by  a  broken  line,  fall  outside  even  the 
considerable  variation  of  the  rest  of  the  salmonoids,  since  for 
their  size  they  have  a  very  short  lifespan  indeed.  The  pleuro- 
nectoids  form  a  compact  group,  with  a  closely  linear  relation 
between  L^  and  T^^^^  with  the  exception  of  the  halibut ;  the 
maximum  age  recorded  for  this  species  (30  years)  may,  how- 
ever, be  somewhat  below  the  real  maximum  owing  to  diffi- 
culties of  determining  the  true  age  of  the  oldest  fish,  and  the 
fact  that  there  was  some  fishing  on  the  populations  in  question. 

The  lines  drawn  for  the  four  groups  shown  in  Fig.  4  have 
been  reproduced  in  Fig.  5,  together  with  the  data  for  all  other 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature    163 


species.  Most  of  these  fall  somewhere  near  the  lines  for  one  or 
other  of  the  first  four  groups,  with  the  sturgeon  in  the  top 
right-hand  section  of  the  diagram  having  the  highest  values 
of  both  Tjj^ax  ^^d  ^ooj  ^^^  ^  cluster  of  the  small  and  short- 
lived species  near  the  origin  (see  enlarged  panel).    The  only 


50  - 


40 


O' '-^ ' ' ' '  /SALMONOIDEI 

O       lO      20      30      40      bO     / 


PLtURONECTOIDEI 


3CO 


Fig.  5.   Relation  between  maximum  age  (Tmax)  and  asymptotic  length  (Loo)  in 
various  species  not  included  in  Fig.  4  (from  Table  I).  The  lines  are  those  for  the 

four  groups  shown  in  Fig.  4. 


species  which,  from  the  data  we  have  examined  so  far,  appear 
to  be  exceptional  are  the  Thunniformes — with  their  large  size 
and  relatively  short  life  they  occupy  a  position  similar  to  that 
of  the  true  salmon — and  possibly  the  Rays  (e.g.  Dasyatis 
akajei;  Yokota,  1951),  but  age  determination  is  difficult  in  the 


164 


R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 


cartilaginous  fish  and  it  may  well  be  that  the  values  of  T^^^ 
recorded  for  this  species  in  Table  I  are  too  low  (see  legend  to 
Table  I). 

Figs.  6  and  7  show  the  relations  between  M  and  K  in  the 


Fig.  6.   Relation  between  natural  mortality  coefficient  (M)  and  rate  of  curva- 
ture of  growth  curve  (K)  in  the  Clupeoidei,  Gadiformes,  Pleuronectoidei  and 

Salmonoidei  (from  Table  I). 


same  way  as  do  Figs.  4  and  5  for  Tj^^x  ^^^  ^oo-  Again  there  is 
a  fairly  definite  trend  within  most  groups,  although  the  scatter 
is  rather  greater  than  before  and  a  trend  in  the  case  of  the 
pleuronectoids  is  hardly  detectable.  Part,  at  least,  of  this 
greater  variation  is  due  to  inaccuracies  or  uncertainties  in  the 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  165 

values  of  ikf ,  and  in  several  instances  it  has  been  necessary  to 
draw  an  arrow  indicating  the  direction  in  which  the  true  value 
of  M  is  thought  to  lie.  However,  when  the  remaining  values 
are  superimposed  on  those  for  the  four  main  groups  (Fig.  7), 
a  rather  more  ordered  picture  is  produced  than  that  of  Fig.  5. 
Of  the  previously  aberrant  species,  the  Thunniformes  now  fall 


2-5 


20 


SALMONOIDEI 


PLEURONECTOIDEI 


175 


Fig.  7.  Relation  between  natural  mortality  coefficient  (M)  and  rate  of  curva- 
ture of  growth  curve  {K)  in  various  species  not  included  in  Fig.  6  (from  Table  I). 
The  lines  are  those  for  the  four  groups  shown  in  Fig.  6. 


into  line,  because  although  they  grow  to  a  relatively  large  size 
they  approach  it  rapidly,  i.e.  they  have  a  high  K,  The  same 
is  true  for  the  Atlantic  salmon  (Salmo  solar  \  Nail,  1927)  since 
this  also  has  a  relatively  high  value  of  ^;  so  indeed  have  the 
Pacific  salmons  {Oncorhynchus  spp.),  although  the  trend  of 
mortality  with  age  is  so  abruptly  discontinuous  in  these 
species  that  a  single  value  of  M  cannot  be  assigned  to  all  of 
them. 


166  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

Because  both  T^^^  and  M,  and  L^  and  K,  are  themselves 
fairly  closely  correlated  (inversely),  it  is  not  unexpected  that 
if  a  relationship  is  found  between  Tj^ax  ^^^  -^oo  ^^^  should 
also  appear  between  M  and  K.  What  is  perhaps  significant 
is  that  the  clearly  established  exceptions  to  the  general  positive 
correlation  between  longevity  and  size  (which  undoubtedly 
emerges  from  the  above  analysis)  are  those  species  which  have 
a  much  higher  value  of  K  than  would  be  expected  from  their 
L^.  This  suggests  that  the  "rate  of  completion  of  the  growth 
pattern"  is  more  closely  related  to  longevity  than  is  size  and 
some  further  evidence  on  this  point  is  discussed  below.  Be 
that  as  it  may,  we  have  not  yet  come  across  a  slow-growing 
species  which  dies  from  natural  causes  when  only  a  small 
fraction  of  its  growth  pattern  has  been  covered,  or  the  con- 
verse— a  species  whose  natural  lifespan  extends  far  beyond 
the  point  at  which  the  limiting  size  is  reached  (as  in  man);  and 
it  seems  that  for  a  wide  range  of  fish  species  the  natural  span 
of  life  is  nicely  adjusted  to  the  time  needed  to  complete,  or 
nearly  to  complete,  the  growth  pattern. 

Some  intra -specific  comparisons 

We  have  so  far  looked  at  the  relations  between  growth  and 
mortality  in  different  species,  but  intra-specific  comparisons 
also  throw  some  light  on  this  question.  If,  firstly,  we  consider 
differences  between  sexes  we  notice  that,  when  the  growth 
rates  are  clearly  different,  L^  for  males  is  usually  less  than  for 
females  in  the  same  population  {Dasyatis  akajei,  Gadus 
minutus,  Ganibusia,  Coitus,  Isopsetta,  Pleuronectes).  In  these 
cases  K  for  males  is  greater  than  for  females,  and  the  male 
mortality  rate  is  higher.  When  the  growth  rates  do  not  differ, 
or  differ  only  slightly,  the  natural  mortality  rates  are  also 
similar  (Leucichthys  artedi,  L.  kiyi,  Salvelinus,  Mallotus, 
Leuresthes,  Citharichthys).  The  chum  salmon  (Oncorhynchus 
keta)  of  the  Columbia  River  is  exceptional :  the  male  natural 
mortality  is  greater  than  that  of  the  female,  and  the  male  has 
a  higher  L^.    In  Callionymus,  also,  the   males  approach  a 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  167 

greater  size;  they  have  a  lower  K  than  the  females,  but  a 
higher  mortality  rate.  In^loAce  {Pleuronectes  platessa)  of  the  /f 
North  Sea,  and  perhaps  in  other  species,  the  sexual  difference 
in  mortality  rates  is  not  simple;  thus  in  both  sexes  the  rnortalr 
ity  rate  tends  to  vary  with  the  age  of  fish,  but  whereas  in  i 
"rnales  it  increases  with  increasing  age — at  least  from  the  fifth 
To  about  the  fourteenth  year — in  the  females  the  evidence  is 
that  it  may  even  decrease.  A  species  of  mosquito  fish, 
Gambusia  holbrooki,  gives  evidence  that  males  are  more 
susceptible  than  females  to  adverse  conditions  of  temperature, 
oxygen,  ion  concentration  and  presence  of  cyanide.  The 
females  also  overwinter  more  successfully  and  are  less  severely 
affected  by  catastrophies  due  to  unknown  causes  (Geiser, 
1924).  It  would  be  interesting  to  know  whether  similar  sexual 
differences  have  been  noticed  in  the  many  tests  which  have 
been  made  with  several  fish  species  of  the  toxicity  of  various 
substances,  especially  those  found  in  polluted  water;  we  have 
not,  however,  found  information  of  this  kind  in  the  published 
reports  of  such  experiments. 

Analysis  of  growth  rates  within  populations  of  the  same  or 
closely  related  species  living  in  different  areas  suggests  that 
two  factors  account  for  most  of  the  variation  found :  food  and 
temperature.  The  asymptotic  size  is  greatly  modified  by  the 
supply  of  food  available,  but  this  does  not  affect  the  para- 
meter K.  Differences  in  environmental  temperature,  however,  /  / 
aftect  both  K  and  L^;  thus  w^th  an  increase  in  water  tempera- 
ture K  increases  roughly  proportionally  with  the  logarithm 
of  temperature  and  L^  decreases,  but  to  a  lesser  extent  (see 
Taylor,  1958;  Holt,  1959a). 

This  temperature  relation  at  least  partly  explains  the 
statement  often  repeated  in  fisheries  literature  that  in  warmer 
waters  the  fish  tend  to  be  smaller  than  in  cooler  waters  but, 
equally,  that  they  grow  faster  in  the  former  (see  e.g.  d'Ancona, 
1937;  Gunter,  1950);  the  size  distribution  that  is  actually 
observed  at  any  time  depends,  however,  on  the  mortality 
rate  as  well  as  the  growth  pattern.   There  are  rather  few  data 


168  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

which  can  be  used  to  examine  this  question,  but  those  we 
have  seen  suggest  that  in  this  case  also  a  high  value  of  K  is 
associated  with  both  a  low  L^  and  a  high  mortality.  This  can 
be  seen  for  Gadus  minutus  in  Table  I,  and  there  is  other 
scattered — but  usually  incomplete — evidence  pointing  in  the 
same  direction.  Thus  the  grayling  {Thymallus  signifer)  has  a 
higher  K  and  lower  L^  in  Michigan  lakes  (warmer)  than  in  the 
Great  Bear  Lake  (colder)  and  it  apparently  lives  about  twice 
as  long  in  the  latter  locality  as  in  the  former  (Brown,  1943; 
Miller,  1946).  It  is  said  that  in  France,  where  it  grows  fast, 
the  stickleback  (Gasterosteus  aculeatus)  lives  only  14-18 
months,  whereas  in  northern  Europe  it  lives  much  longer,  and 
indeed  does  not  mature  until  it  is  several  years  old  (Bertin, 
1925) ;  according  to  Flower  (1935),  sardines  (Sardinapilchardus) 
grow  more  slowly  and  live  longer  in  the  English  Channel  than 
in  the  south  of  the  Bay  of  Biscay;  and  so  on.  Jenkins,  Elkin 
and  Finnell  (1955)  studied  the  growth  of  six  species  of  sunfish 
(Lepomis  spp.  and  Chaenohryttus)  in  over  one  hundred  water 
bodies  in  Oklahoma  and  noted  for  each  species  that  the  oldest 
individuals  were  always  in  the  populations  having  the  slowest 
growth  rates.  We  have  to  be  careful  in  interpreting  data  of 
this  kind,  however,  because  a  general  observation  that  the 
maximum  age  attained  is  lowest  in  areas  where  growth  is 
fastest  may  sometimes  be  due  to  effects  of  fishing  coupled 
with  a  density-dependent  growth  rate,  the  fishing  causing  a 
reduced  survival  and  population  density  and  so  permitting  a 
better  supply  of  food  per  fish  with  a  consequent  increase  in 
the  growth  rate  (see,  for  example  Fry,  1936,  for  populations 
of  Hesperoleiicus  venustus  in  Calif ornian  streams). 

It  is  interesting  to  note  that  the  same  associations  we  have 
recorded  above  between  growth  and  longevity  in  related 
species,  or  even  in  populations  of  the  same  species  which 
have  become  established  as  independent  units  in  different 
water  basins,  do  not  necessarily  hold  when  growth  is  modified 
experimentally.  There  is  not  much  information  on  this,  but 
the  studies  of  Aim  (1946)  on  perch  populations  with  stunted 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  169 

growth  did  not  indicate  any  marked  difference  in  longevity 
compared  with  those  in  which  growth  was  normal.  The  ex- 
perimental studies  being  carried  out  by  Comfort  (personal 
communication)  on  growth  and  longevity  in  guppies  {Lebistes 
reticulatus)  appear  so  far  to  be  giving  the  same  result,  although 
severe  underfeeding  during  the  early  life  of  trout  kept  in 
tanks  has  been  shown  to  delay  maturity  and  actually  prolong 
life  (McCay,  Dilley  and  Crowell,  1929).  Again,  the  association 
between  high  values  of  M  and  of  K  noted  above  may  not  hold 
for  comparisons  between  populations  of  the  same  species  in 
closely  adjacent  waters,  as  in  the  case  of  the  bullhead  {Coitus 
gobio;  Smyly,  1957)  in  Lake  Windermere  and  the  River 
Brathay  (see  Table  I). 

Mortality,  growth  and  metabolic  rate 

To  understand  the  relations  tentatively  identified  above, 
it  is  necessary  to  extend  our  studies  to  include  comparative 
physiology  and  behaviour,  and  at  this  stage  w^e  can  do  little 
more  than  indicate  the  lines  of  comparison  that  might  profit- 
ably be  pursued.  One  of  these  follows  from  the  fact  that  the 
growth  parameter  K  is  predictable  from  the  rate  of  endogen- 
ous nitrogen  excretion  by  a  starved  animal  (von  Bertalanffy, 
1938),  and  it  would  be  expected  that  this  is  also  closely 
related  to  metabolic  rate  and  to  activity,  as  Edmonds  (1957) 
has  shown  in  a  comparative  study  of  some  invertebrate 
groups.  For  fish,  the  available  data  seem  to  confirm  the 
relation  of  K  to  metabolic  rate.  Thus  the  oxygen  consumption 
of  Gadus  callarias  at  7-11°  is  0-33-0-35  O2  ml./g.^^^/hr.  and 
that  of  Gadus  virens  at  the  same  temperature  is  similar, 
0-36-0 -47  O2  ml./g.2/3yhr.  (Sundnes,  1957).  These  two  species 
have  the  same  K  values  (0-2)  though  the  natural  mortality  of 
G.  callarias  is  possibly  rather  higher  than  that  of  G.  virens. 
On  the  other  hand,  Leucichthys  sardinella  has,  at  about  the 
same  temperature  (7-9-4°),  an  oxygen  consumption  of  0-55- 
0-75  O2  ml./g.2/3yhr.,  corresponding  with  a  higher  K  value 
(0-4)  and  much  higher  M  (0-6).   The  cyprinids  Labeo  rohita, 


170  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

Catla  catla,  and  Carassius  carassius,  all  of  which  have  rather 
low  values  of  K,  have  a  low  oxygen  consumption  of  about 
0-2  ml./g.^^^/hr.  (Blazka,  1958).  Over  the  temperature  range 
5-35°,  oxygen  uptake  by  another  cyprinid,  the  goldfish, 
Carassius  auratus,  ranges  from  0-05-0-46  ml./g.^^^/hr.  (Fry 
and  Hart,  1948);  this  species  has  a  K  value  of  about  0-3. 

Metabolic  rate  has  been  estimated,  in  connexion  with  ex- 
periments on  the  transport  of  live  fish  in  closed  containers, 
from  the  rate  of  carbon  dioxide  accumulation;  in  one  such 
case  Tilapia  mossambica,  which  has  a  higher  K  value  than 
Cyprinus  carpio,  respired  faster  than  the  latter,  though  kept 
at  the  same  temperature  (Vaas,  1952).  Further  evidence  of 
relative  metabolic  rates  comes  from  studies  of  the  rate  of 
uptake  and  loss  of  radioactive  substances  by  fishes.  Thus,  in  a 
review  of  this  subject.  Boroughs,  Chipman  and  Rice  (1957) 
quote  results  indicating  that  the  exponential  loss  coefficient 
of  orally  administered  strontium  89  from  the  body  of  Tilapia 
mossambica  is  two  and  a  half  times  that  of  skipjack  {Euthynnus 
yaito),  yellow-fin  {Neothunnus  macropterus)  and  "dolphin" 
(Coryphaena  hippurus).  These  latter  fishes  are  more  active 
species  than  Tilapia  and,  from  the  scanty  data  available, 
appear  to  have  higher  K  values.  We  have  not  found  any 
published  data  to  indicate  whether,  in  fish  for  which  K  is 
higher  in  males  than  in  females,  the  respiratory  rate  of  males 
is  also  higher,  as  might  be  expected. 

Natural  death  and  reproduction 

The  last  line  of  evidence  we  shall  mention  is  that  concerning 
the  connexion,  in  fish,  between  natural  death  and  reproduc- 
tion. We  have  previously  mentioned  that  in  the  short-lived 
species  where  there  is  an  abrupt  end  to  the  lifespan,  death 
usually  occurs  at  or  soon  after  spawning.  What  has  been 
called  "reproductive  drain"  may  also  become  apparent  in 
other  ways.  For  example,  the  ratio  of  the  weight  of  a  fish  to 
the  cube  of  its  length  (called  the  "condition  factor"  or 
"ponderal  index"  in  fisheries  literature)  varies  seasonally, 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  171 

being  highest  just  before  spawning.  In  the  plaice  it  is  apparent 
that  this  variation  is  very  much  greater  in  older  than  in 
younger  individuals,  and  it  seems  that  as  the  fish  gets  older 
(or,  perhaps,  merely  bigger)  the  strain  of  meeting  the  repro- 
ductive demand  increases  to  a  point  at  which  recovery  is  not 
possible.  It  seems  that  this  kind  of  effect  is  most  evident  in 
species  with  high  K,  which  mature  at  an  early  age  but  at  a 
size  which  is  rather  large  in  relation  to  the  asymptotic  length, 
L^.  Fish,  such  as  sturgeon,  with  a  low  K,  which  mature  when 
relatively  rather  small,  do  not  show  a  decline  in  their  repro- 
ductive capacities;  Gamhusia  affinis,  on  the  other  hand, 
exhibits  an  absolute  decrease  in  brood  size  w^ith  increasing 
age  (Krumholz,  1948)  and  indeed  this  species  seems  to  have  a 
true  post-reproductive  phase,  which  is  rather  rare  in  fish. 
Further  evidence  on  the  decline  in  reproductive  powers  with 
age  in  fish  is  presented  in  the  contribution  by  Gerking  to  this 
colloquium. 

The  complexity  of  the  relations  between  the  growth  and 
reproduction  parameters  and  mortality  rates  is  illustrated  by 
Svardson's  (1943)  review  of  data  for  the  guppy,  Lehistes 
reticulatus.  Male  guppies  mature  before  females,  and  die 
younger.  Under  experimental  conditions  of  low  food  supply 
they  grow  slowly  to  a  low  asymptotic  size,  and  mature  late; 
with  a  medium  food  supply  the  final  size  is  greater  and 
maturity  somewhat  earlier,  but  with  an  abundant  food  supply 
the  final  body  size  is  again  lowered  although  the  onset  of 
maturity  is  still  further  accelerated. 

Other  observations  concerning  the  relations  between 
growth,  reproduction  and  death  have  to  be  fitted  into  the 
picture  outlined  in  this  paper,  but  pending  a  detailed  survey 
of  the  known  facts,  can  only  briefly  be  mentioned  here.  It 
has  been  thought  for  many  years  that  the  onset  of  sexual 
maturity  in  fish  is  a  function  of  their  size  rather  than  of  their 
age  but  like  most  such  generalizations  this  is  only  partly  true 
since,  within  a  species  population,  individuals  reach  maturity 
over  a  considerable  range  of  both  age  and  size.    There  is,  in 


172 


R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 


fact,  a  considerable  amount  of  data  on  the  size  at  which  fish 
first  reach  maturity,  and  we  have  investigated  whether  the 
average  length  at  which  maturation  occurs  (L„J  in  relation  to 
the  asymptotic  length  (L^)  has  any  bearing  on  longevity. 
Thus,  in  the  last  column  of  Table  I  are  given  the  ratios  LJL^ 
and  in  Fig.  8  these  are  plotted  against  maximum  age,  T 
The  points  are  very  much  scattered,  although  there  is  perhaps 


00> 


max* 


Lm 

L    CO 


lOO 

-o 

•  • 
o    • 

•  ( 

3          X 

X 

07S 

oO    o  Xo    ^ 

X 

•  •• 

o» 

X 

°<^ 

o°*J 

o« 

0 

0*' 

o 

08 

0 

°1 

•      • 

0 
0 

-0 

0 

0 

i 

i 
0 

§ 

0 

0 

0 

1 

0 

0 
0 

1 

1 

10 


20  30 

—     T  max  (yr )     — 


40 


50 


Fig,  8.    Size  at  maturity  (L^)  and  longevity.    Plot  of  ratio  LmjLco 
against    Tmax-      •   =  Salmonoidei,    x  =  Clupeoidei;    other    species 

shown  as  Q  • 


just  a  hint  that  the  shortest-lived  species  are  those  which  have, 
on  average,  the  highest  value  of  LJL^,  that  is,  mature  at  a 
relatively  late  stage  in  their  growth  cycle.  This  tendency  is  a 
little  clearer  within  the  salmonoid  group  (solid  circles),  but  in 
the  clupeoids  (crosses),  with  one  doubtful  exception,  the 
values  of  LJL^  are  consistently  high  and  have  no  trend  at  all. 
It  may,  of  course,  be  that  this  way  of  relating  maturity  and 
longevity  is  too  crude;  maturation  size  might  better  be  ex- 
pressed, for  example,  as  a  function  of  both  K  and  L^,  but  this 
needs  further  examination. 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  173 

The  undoubted  association  between  reproduction  and 
death  in  many  species  of  fish  makes  it  difficult,  merely  on  the 
kind  of  evidence  we  have  considered  in  this  paper,  to  detect 
whether  true  senescent  processes  play  a  part  in  determining 
longevity  in  fish,  as  they  do,  for  example,  in  mammals.  The 
mathematical  representation  of  the  characteristic  growth 
pattern  in  fish  which  we  have  adopted  here  does,  of  course, 
imply  that  growth  proceeds  towards  a  finite  limiting  size,  and 
so  is  not  "indeterminate"  in  the  sense  used  by  Bidder  (1932). 
When  the  growth  of  fish  is  plotted  as  in  Fig.  3  the  impression 
gained  is  not  only  that  this  is  a  valid  interpretation  but  also 
that  the  growth  pattern  of  the  long-lived  species  (including 
plaice)  does  not  differ  qualitatively  from  that  of  the  short- 
lived ones  in  which  a  limiting  size  is  efPectively  reached  within 
the  observed  range  of  age.  As  Comfort  (1956)  has  pointed  out, 
however.  Bidder's  hypothesis  of  immortality  in  fish  does  not 
necessarily  depend  on  whether  there  is  a  finite  limit  to  their 
growth  or  not,  and  can  equally  well  be  maintained  if  that  limit 
can  be  shown  to  be  approached  asymptotically — and  hence 
reached  only  after  an  infinite  span  of  time — as  opposed  to 
abruptly,  with  growth  ceasing  beyond  a  certain  specific  age. 
To  test  this  latter  alternative  directly,  at  least  in  the  slow- 
growing  species,  is  as  difficult  as  it  is  to  prove  whether  or  not 
there  is  a  finite  Umit  to  the  lifespan  of  a  species  which  has  a 
constant  mortality  rate  within  the  observed  range  of  age.  In 
this  connexion,  it  is  perhaps  worth  noting  that  although  the 
growth  equation  we  have  adopted  requires  an  asymptotic 
approach  to  a  limiting  size,  high  enough  values  of  ^  (as  are 
found,  for  example,  for  Labidesthes  sicculus)  can  produce  a 
theoretical  growth  curve  in  which  the  approach  to  L^  is  so 
rapid  that  it  would  be  indistinguishable  in  practice  from  an 
abrupt  approach,  especially  when  it  is  remembered  that  there 
is  usually  a  seasonal  periodicity  of  growth  superimposed  on 
the  general  pattern. 

Thus  we  are  inclined  to  the  view  that  further  speculation 
along  these  lines  is  unlikely  to  contribute  much  to  the  solution 


174  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

of  the  question  as  to  whether  ageing  in  fish  differs  funda- 
mentally from  that  in  higher  vertebrates.  A  more  profitable 
approach  would  seem  to  lie  in  a  better  understanding  of  the 
intrinsic  causes  of  natural  death  in  fish,  about  which  relatively 
little  is  yet  known.  A  recent  study  of  the  European  eel 
{Anguilla  anguilla)  by  Tucker  (1959)  suggests  that  the  debility 
of  these  fish  at  the  inception  of  gonadal  and  other  hormone 
activity,  which  causes  them  to  drift  passively  downstream,  is 
due  to  demineralization  of  the  starving  body.  That  this 
process  is  reversible,  at  least  in  the  early  stages,  is  shown  by 
the  fact  that  silver  eels  imprisoned  in  fresh  water  can  survive 
by  regression  of  the  gonads  and  consequent  remineralization 
of  the  body  fluids ;  and  it  is  also  known  that  recovery  of  the 
Atlantic  salmon  (Salmo  solar)  after  spawning  can  be  hastened 
by  placing  them  in  salt  water.  It  is  true  that  both  these 
species,  and  more  especially  the  eel,  have  a  highly  atypical 
life  history,  but  this  kind  of  explanation  of  certain  behavioural 
patterns  in  physiological  terms  would  appear  to  be  an  essen- 
tial step  in  the  solution  of  at  least  some  aspects  of  the  problem 
of  longevity  in  fish.  The  other  line  of  investigation  that  would 
seem  to  be  of  special  significance  is  a  comparative  study  of  the 
physiology  of  growth  and  reproduction  in  species  which  have 
a  post-reproductive  phase.  We  would  hope  that  an  under- 
standing of  the  beginning  of  the  reproductive  phase  of  the 
life  history  in  relation  to  growth  processes  would  help  inter- 
pretation of  those  events  at  the  end  of  the  reproductive  life- 
span that  lead  to  death. 

REFERENCES 

Aasen,  O.  (1952).  Fiskeridir.  Skr.  Havundersok.,  10,  (2). 

Akyuz,  E.  (1959).    Unpublished  data,  filed  at  FAO,  Rome,  and  Et  ve 

Balik  Kurumu,  Istanbul. 
Alm,  G.  (1946).   Rep.  Inst.  Freshw.  Res.  Drottning.,  25. 
Alm,  G.  (1952).   Rep.  Inst.  Freshw.  Res.  Drottning.,  33,  17. 
d'ANCONA,  U.  (1937).   Rapp.  Comm.  int.  Mer  Medit.,  10,  162. 
Anonymous  (1954).    Comm.  Fish.  Rev.,  16,  28. 

Appelget,  J.,  and  Smith,  L.  L.  (1951).  Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc.,  80, 119. 
Arora,  H.  L.  (1951).   Calif.  Fish  Game,  37,  3. 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  175 

Berdegue,  J.  (1955).  Rev.  Soc.  mex.  Hist,  nat.,  16,  45. 

Bertalaxffy,  L.  von  (1938).  Human  Biol,  10,  181. 

Bertin,  L.  (1925).   Ann.  Inst.  Oceanogr.,  2,  1. 

Bevertox,  R.  J.  H.   Unpublished. 

Beverton,  R.  J.  H.,  and  Holt,  S.  J.  (1957).  Fish.  Invest.,  Lond.y 

Ser.  2,  19. 
Bidder,  G.  P.  (1932).   Brit.  med.  J.,  2,  583. 
Blackburn,  M.  (1950).   Aust.  J.  Mar.  Freshiv.  Res.,  1,  155. 
Blazka,  p.  (1958).  Physiol.  ZooL,  31,  117. 
Boroughs,  H.,  Chipman,  W.  A.,  and  Rice,  T.  R.  (1957).    U.S.  Nat. 

Acad.  Sci.JNat.  Res.  Counc.,  Publ.  551,  80-87. 
Brown,  C.  J.  D.  (1943).  J.  Wildlife  Mgmt,  7,  353. 
Brown,  M.  E.  (Editor).   (1957).  The  Physiology  of  Fishes.  New  York: 

Academic  Press. 
Burd,  a.  C.   Unpublished. 

Chang,  Hsiao-Wei.   (1951).  J.  Mar.  hiol.  Ass.  U.K.,  30,  281. 
Clark,  F.  N.  (1925).  Fish  Bull.,  Sacramento,  No.  10. 
Clark,  F.  N.  (1940).    Calif.  Fish  Game,  26,  39. 
Cohen,  D.  M.  (1954).   Staiif.  ichthijol.  Bull,  4,  167. 
Comfort,  A.  (1956).    The  Biology  of  Senescence.    London:  Routledge 

and  Kegan  Paul. 
Deason,  H.  J.,  and  Hile,  R.  (1947).   Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  74,  88. 
Deelder,  C.  L.  (1951).   Hydrobiologia,  3,  357. 
DE  Sylva  D.  (1957).     Bull  Mar.  Sci.  Gulf  Caribb.,  7,  1. 
Devold,  F.  (1938).  Fiskeridir.  Skr.  Havundersok,  5,  (6). 
Dickie,  L.  M.,  and  McCracken,  F.  D.  (1955).  J.  Fish.  Res.  Bd  Can.,  12, 

187. 
Dymond,  J.  R.  (1933).   Contr.  Canad.  Biol,  8,  1. 
Edmonds,  S.  J.  (1957).   Aust.  J.  Mar.  Freshw.  Res.,  8,  131. 
Fitch,  J.  E.  (1951).  Fish.  Bull.,  Sacramento,  No.  83. 
Fitch,  J.  E.  (1956).   Calif.  Fish  Game,  42,  143. 
Flower,  S.  S.  (1935).  Proc.  zool.  Soc.  Lond.,  265. 
Foerster,  R.  E.  (1929).   Contr.  Canad.  Biol.,  5,  1. 
Franca,  P.  da  (1953).   Bull.  Soc.  portug.  Sci.  nat.,  4,  198. 
Frost,  W.  E.  (1943).  J.  Anim.  EcoL,  12,  139. 
Frost,  W.  E.  (1945).  J.  Anim.  EcoL,  14,  106. 

Frost,  W.  E.,  and  Smyly,  W.  J.  P.  (1952).  J.  Anim.  EcoL,  21,  62. 
Fry,  D.  H.  (1936).    Calif.  Fish  Game,  22,  64. 

Fry,  F.  E.  J.,  and  Hart,  J.  S.  (1948).  Biol.  Bull.,  Wood's  Hole,  94,  66. 
Geiser,  S.  W.  (1924).   Biol.  Bull.,  Wood's  Hole,  47,  175. 
Gerking,  S.  D.  (1959).   This  volume,  p.  181. 
Gottlieb,  E.  (1957).   "On  the  dynamics  of  the  population  of  coal-fish, 

Gadus  virens  L.,  in  Norwegian  waters."  MS. 
Grainger,  E.  H.  (1953).  J.  Fish.  Res.  Bd  Can.,  10,  326. 
GuNTER,  G.  (1950).    Copeia,  298. 
Hanyu,  I.  (1956).   Bull.  Jap.  Soc.  sci.  Fish.,  21,  991. 
Hart,  J.  L.  (1928).  Publ.  Ont.  Fish.  Res.  Lab.,  No.  84. 


176  R.  J.  H.  Beverton  and  S.  J.  Holt 

Hart,  J.  L.  (1931).   Contr.  Canad.  Biol,  6,  427. 

Hart,  J.  L.  (1948).   Trans,  roy.  Soc.  Can.,  42,  (Ser.  Ill),  65. 

Herald,  E.  S.,  and  Rakowicz,  M.  (1951).   Aquarium  J.,  22,  234. 

HiLE,  R.  (1936).   Bull.  U.S.  Bur.  Fish.,  48,  211. 

HiLE,  R.,  and  Deason,  H.  J.  (1934).   Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  64,  231. 

Holt,  S.  J.  (1959a).  J.  Cons.  int.  Explor.  Mer,  24,  374. 

Holt,  S.  J.  (19596).   Draft  Report  of  International  Training  Centre 

on   the  Methodology   and  Techniques   of  Research   on  Mackerel 

(Rastrelliger).   Rome:  FAO,  Mimeo. 
HuBBS,  C.  L.  (1921).   Ecology,  2,  262. 
Jenkins,  R.,  Elkin,  R.,  and  Finnell,  J.  (1955).    Oklahoma  Fish  res. 

Lab.  Rep.,  49. 
Jones,  J.  W.,  and  Hynes,  H.  B.  N.  (1950).  J.  Anim.  EcoL,  19,  59. 
Kennedy,  W.  A.  (1943).  Publ.  Ont.  Fish.  Res.  Lab.  No.  62. 
Kennedy,  W.  A.  (1954).  J.  Fish.  Res.  Bd  Can.,  11,  827. 
Krumholz,  L.  a.  (1948).   Ecol.  Monogr.,  18,  1. 
Lea,  E.  (1930).   Rapp.  Cons.  Explor.  Mer,  65,  100. 
McCay,  C.  M.,  Dilley,  W.  E.,  and  Crowell,  M.  F.  (1929).   J.  Nutr., 

1,  233. 
Margetts,  a.  R.   Unpublished. 
Marr,  J.  C.  (1943).  Stanf.  ichthyol.  Bull,  2,  157. 
Menon,  M.  D.  (1950).  J.  Mar.  biol.  Ass.  U.K.,  29,  185. 
Miller,  R.  B.  (1946).   Copeia,  Til. 
Moore,  H.  L.  (1951).  Fish.  Bull.,  U.S.,  52,  132. 
Nall,  G.  H.  (1927).   Fisheries,  Scotland,  Salmon  Fish.,  II. 
NiGRELLi,  R.  F.  (1959).   This  volume,  p.  212. 

Paccagnella,  B.  (1948).   Arch.  Oceanogr.  LimnoL,  Venezia,  5,  141. 
Phillips,  J.  B.  (1948).  Fish.  Bull,  Sacramento,  No.  71,  5. 
Probst,  R.  T.,  and  Cooper,  E.  L.  (1954).   Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  84, 

207. 
Pycha,  R.  L.  (1956).   Calif.  Fish.  Game,  42,  23. 
Qasim,  S.  Z.  (1957).  Proc.  zool.  Soc  Lond.,  128,  161. 
Radhakrishnan,  N.  (1957).   Indian  J.  Fish.,  4,  254. 
Raitt,  D.  S.  (1939).   Rapp.  Cons.  Explor.  Mer,  110,  65. 
RiCKER,  W.  E.  (1949).   Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  77,  114.  SL 
RiCKER,  W.  E.  (1958).   Bull.  Fish.  Res.  Bd  Can.,  119.      ^ 
Robertson,  J.  A.  (1938).  Fish.  Invest.  Lond.,  Ser.  2,  16,  (2). 
RoLLEFSEN,  G.  (1954).  Rapp.  Cons.  Explor.  Mer,  136,  40. 
RuNNSTROM,  S.  (1936).  Fiskeridir.  Skr.  Havundersok.,  5,  (2). 
Sacher,  G.  (1959).   This  volume,  p.  115. 
Sato,  R.  (1950).   TohokuJ.  agric  Res.,  1,  87. 

Shiraishi,  Y.  (1957).   Bull.  Freshw.  Fish.  Res.  Lab.,  Tokyo,  7,  33. 
SiLLiMAN,  R.  P.  (1943).  Spec  sci.  Rep.  U.S.  Fish  Wildl.  Serv.,  No.  24. 
Smyly,  W.  J.  P.  (1957).  Proc.  zool.  Soc  Lond.,  128,  431. 
Strawn,  K.  (1958).    Copeia,  16. 

Sundnes,  G.  (1957).  Fiskeridir.  Skr.  Havundersok.,  11  (9). 
SvARDSON,  G.  (1943).   Rep.  Inst.  Freshw.  Res.  Drottning.,  21. 


Longevity  and  Mortality  Rates  of  Fish  in  Nature  177 

Taylor,  C.  C.  (1958).  J.  Cons.  int.  Explor.  Mer,  23,  366. 
Templeman,  W.  (1948).   Bull.  Newf.  Govn.  Lab.,  17. 
Tester,  A.  L.  (1937).  J.  biol.  Bd  Can.,  3,  108.  * 

Thompson,  W.  F.,  and  Bell,  F.  H.  (1934).   Rep.  int.  Fish.  Comm.,  8. 
Thompson,  W.  F.,  and  Herrington,  W.  C.  (1930).    Rep.  int.  Fish. 

Comm.,  2. 
TiBBO,  S.  N.  (1957).   Bull.  Fish.  Res.  Bd  Can.,  Ill,  85. 
TiEWS,  K.  (1957).   Ber.  Dtsch.  Komm.  Meeresforsch.,  14,  192. 
Tucker,  D.  W.  (1959).  Nature  (Loud.),  183,  495. 
Vaas,  K.  F.  (1952).  Proc.  Indo-Pacif.  Fish.  Coun.,  3,  119. 
VivES,  F.,  and  Suau,  P.  (1956).   Invest.  Pesq.,  5,  17. 
VNIRO  (1949).     Ministry  of  Industrial  Fisheries,  Moscow. 
Walford,  L.  a.  (1946).   Biol.  Bull,  Wood's  Hole,  90,  141. 
WoHLSCHLAG,  D.  E.  (1954a).   Ecology,  35,  388.      • 
WoHLSCHLAG,  D.  E.  (19546).  Stanf.  ichthyol.  Bull.,  4,  189. 
YoKOTA,  T.  (1951).   Bull.  Jap.  Soc.  sci.  Fish.,  16,  188. 


DISCUSSION 

Rotblat:  Have  you  also  plotted  1/jK"  against  T^jax?  IjKhas  the  dimen- 
sions of  time  and  is  proportional  to  the  time  it  takes  to  grow  to  half 
size.  This  may  also  be  a  linear  function  of  the  span  of  life. 

Beverton:  No,  we  have  not  yet  done  that;  so  far  we  have  examined 
only  the  relationships  between  K  and  M  and  between  Loo  and  T,^^. 

Rotblat:  The  combination  of  these  two  factors  is  of  special  interest 
because  you  relate,  then,  time  to  time.  This  also  agrees  with  what 
Sacher  said  about  low  metabolic  rate. 

BeveHon:  In  fish  the  natural  mortality  coefficient  and  the  maxi- 
mum age  are  closely  correlated,  because  most  of  the  logarithmic 
survival  curves  tend  to  be  rather  straight.  We  do  not  have  curves  of 
nearly  the  same  slope  which  suddenly  dip  to  very  diff'erent  maximum 
ages. 

Danielli:  These  growth-rate  limitations  may  be  entirely  due  to 
differences  in  natural  conditions.  Have  you  any  data  from  fish 
which  have  been  exposed  to  toxic  substances  in  the  water  ? 

Beverton :  The  only  paper  on  exposure  of  fish  to  toxic  substances 
that  I  can  recall  offhand  is  on  Gambusia  (Geiser,  S.  W.  (1924).  Biol. 
Bull.  {Wood's  Hole),  47,  175).  There  the  investigation  was  to  show 
that  males  not  only  died  off  more  quickly  than  the  females  after 
reproduction  but  were  also  more  susceptible  to  a  range  of  toxic  sub- 
stances. In  other  words,  their  balance  with  the  environment  seemed 
more  precarious  than  that  of  the  females,  with  respect  to  artificially 
induced  hazards  as  well  as  natural  ones  such  as  reproduction.  On  the 
first  point,  growth  is  undoubtedly  very  flexible  in  fish.  Nevertheless, 


178  Discussion 

our  impression  is  that  looking  over  the  whole  range  of  growth 
data  for  fish  it  is  possible  to  see  fairly  characteristic  sorts  of  growth. 
However  much  food  you  give  a  small  fish  it  never  grows  very  much 
bigger  than  its  characteristic  size  in  nature.  This  is  a  pretty  general 
statement,  but  despite  the  extent  to  which  growth  can  be  varied 
experimentally,  there  does  seem  to  be  an  overall  pattern  in  nature 
which  is  fairly  consistent. 

Holt:  This  sexual  difference  in  susceptibilities  is  found  rather 
commonly  in  experiments  on  the  effects  of  water  pollution  and  in 
studies  to  improve  the  transport  of  live  fish  in  closed  containers ;  the 
males  are  usually  more  sensitive.  Unfortunately  I  have  no  good  data 
with  which  to  compare  respiration  rates  of  the  sexes.  Spawning 
plays  a  more  important  part  as  a  factor  associated  with  death  in 
fish,  than,  for  example,  food  supply.  One  can  vary  the  growth  rate 
tremendously  by  adjusting  the  food  supply,  without  changing  the 
mortality  rate  at  all.  But  where  death  is  caused  through  spawning 
or  is  associated  with  it  and  maturity,  it  seems  that  the  males  are 
more  drastically  affected  than  the  females.  Thus  in  one  salmon 
species  all  the  males  and  most  of  the  females  die  after  spawning,  but 
some  of  the  females  return  to  the  sea. 

Nigrelli:  In  aquarium  management  we  constantly  find,  when  we 
autopsy  fish,  that  there  are  about  three  females  to  one  male,  which 
means  that  there  is  some  sort  of  selection  in  the  collection. 

Danielli :  If  the  fish  are  dying  as  a  result  of  spawning  perhaps  one 
can  change  the  situation.  Death  following  spawning  in  salmon  is 
said  to  be  due  to  some  syndrome  involving  the  pituitary.  This, 
surely,  could  be  modified  by  appropriate  hormone  treatment. 

Nigrelli:  Are  there  any  comparative  figures  on  population  or 
growth  studies  on  salmon  or  trout  in  hatcheries  and  those  under 
natural  conditions  ? 

Holt:  I  do  not  know. 

Nigrelli:  I  think  that  in  large  hatcheries  there  is  a  lower  infant 
mortality  rate  than  is  found  under  natural  conditions. 

Comfort:  In  these  natural  populations,  is  there  always  a  tendency 
for  the  growth  to  be  smooth  in  outline  ?  Or  does  the  same  effect 
occur  in  wild  fish  as  I  have  produced  by  keeping  them  small  arti- 
ficially and  then  increasing  the  food  supply  ? 

Beverton:  Yes;  perch  is  the  most  notable  example.  You  get  that  in 
the  wild,  particularly  where  there  is  a  marked  change  in  feeding 
habits  as  the  fish  grow  bigger.  For  example,  perch  up  to  about  15  cm. 
feed  primarily  on  animal  plankton  such  as  freshwater  shrimps,  etc. 
Above  that  size  they  change  to  exclusively  carnivorous  habits.  If 
the  pond  or  lake  has  no  supply  of  small  fishjthey^  just  stop  growing. 


Discussion  179 

But  the  data  also  show  that  this  stunted  growth  does  not  seem  to  be 
Associated  with  any  marked  increase  in  mortality  rate.  ~    '■ 

■^  Comfort:  I  think  that  is  what  I  am  going  to  find. 

Another  point  is  that  H.  J.  Van  Cleave  (1934;  1935.  Ecology,  15, 
17;  16,  101)  suggested  that  the  apparent  senescence  of  some  mol- 
luscan  species  which  are  more  or  less  indeterminate  in  growth  is 
actually  a  size  effect,  because  there  is  no  accumulation  of  animals  in 
the  older  age  groups.  In  one  of  the  freshwater  limpets  the  question 
arose  whether,  when  they  got  beyond  a  certain  size,  their  holding-on 
mechanisms  became  inefficient  or  whether  they  were  taken  selectively 
by  predators  (Hunter,  W.  R.  (1953).  Proc.  zool.  Soc.  Lond.,  123,  623). 
Have  you  any  instance  of  an  adverse  size  effect  in  fish  ? 

Beverton:  I  should  have  thought  it  usually  worked  the  other  way 
in  fish.  For  instance,  they  escape  predation  as  they  get  bigger, 
rather  than  the  reverse,  I  would  say. 

Comfort:  What  about  catching  them  ? 

Beverton:  That  depends  on  the  gear.  Usually  fishermen  are  after 
the  bigger  fish  and  take  steps  to  catch  them,  but  not  always. 

Holt:  It  is  usually  in  the  smaller  fish,  having  rather  high  K  values, 
that  the  effect  of  reproduction  on  lifespan  seems  to  be  greatest,  as 
they  die  off  quickly  after  reproduction.  In  fish  like  sturgeons  which 
grow  to  large  sizes  rather  slowly  there  is  no  noticeable  effect  of  re- 
production on  their  mortality,  even  though  they  mature  at  a  re- 
latively small  size.  In  middle-sized  fish,  such  as  the  plaice,  there  is  a 
certain  effect  but  not  such  a  drastic  one.  Thus  in  large  spawning 
plaice  there  is  a  relatively  great  seasonal  variation  in  the  relation  of 
length  to  weight  (what  we  call  the  "condition  factor"),  suggesting 
that  attainment  of  spawning  condition  becomes  an  increasing  strain 
on  the  fish  as  they  grow  older. 

Rockstein:  Do  land-locked  salmon  reproduce  year  after  year  ? 

Holt:  Some  land-locked  populations  do. 

Beverton:  There  are  the  "residual"  and  the  "Ko  Kanee"  salmon 
{Oncorhynchus  nerka).  Both  are  non-migratory,  but  whereas  the 
"residuals"  are  the  progeny  of  anadromous  parents,  the  "Ko 
Kanee"  is  a  self-maintaining  stock  which  has  no  connexion  with 
either  of  the  other  two. 

Gerking:  There  are  some  land-locked  Atlantic  salmon  that  repro- 
duce year  after  year. 

Rockstein:  In  a  large  reservoir  in  New  York  State  we  have  brown 
trout,  also  called  salmon  trout,  and  these  can  be  caught  in  all  sizes 
depending  on  how  successful  the  first  year  stock  is  in  eluding  the 
angler.  It  appears  from  their  annual  movements  into  the  lake  in 
spring  and  out  again  in  the  fall  that  they  are  spawning  each  year. 


180  Discussion 

something  like  the  salmon.  If  they  do  spawn  each  year,  however, 
then  spawning  may  have  no  appreciable  effect  on  their  longevity, 
as  it  is  said  to  have  in  the  case  of  the  salmon. 

Gerking:  There  has  been  a  very  good  study  on  those  lines  in 
Scotland,  but  it  does  not  concern  the  question  of  ageing.  It  is  a 
migration  pattern. 


PHYSIOLOGICAL  CHANGES 
ACCOMPANYING  AGEING  IN  FISHES* 

Shelby  D.  Gerking 

Department  of  Zoology,  Indiana  University ^ 
Bloomiyigton,  Indiana 

This  paper  is  a  review  of  efforts  that  have  been  made  to 
show  the  relation  between  age  and  two  important  Ufe  func- 
tions of  fishes,  nutrition  and  reproduction.  Only  a  few  studies 
have  been  made  on  the  effects  of  age  on  food  conversion,  but 
feeding  experiments  demonstrate  clearly  that  the  ability  to 
convert  protein  to  body  flesh  declines  as  size  and  age  increase. 
Other  vertebrates  also  conform  to  this  pattern,  although  they 
achieve  a  specific  size  relatively  early  in  life  in  contrast  to  the 
prolonged  period  of  growth  in  fishes.  The  rate  of  decline  in  the 
ability  to  utilize  food  for  growth  seems  to  be  a  matter  of 
degree,  rather  than  a  basic  difference  between  animals  of 
specific  and  non-specific  size. 

Studies  of  the  effect  of  age  on  reproduction  in  fishes  have 
produced  no  general  conclusions.  Nevertheless,  the  subject 
deserves  attention  because  changes  in  reproductive  capacity 
with  age  are  commonly  used  as  a  criterion  of  senescence  in 
other  vertebrates.  The  reproductive  capacity  of  live-bearing 
fishes  of  the  family  Poeciliidae  declines  with  age  and  there 
may  be  a  period  of  sterility  before  death.  Neither  of  these  two 
facts  apply  unequivocally  to  egg-laying  fishes,  however.  There 
is  no  period  of  reproductive  senility,  and  it  is  an  open  question 
whether  or  not  age  has  an  effect  on  reproductive  capacity. 
Individual  variation  in  fecundity  is  so  great  that  age  effects 
cannot  be  detected  by  refined  statistical  techniques.   A  more 

*  Contribution  No.  668  from  the  Department  of  Zoology,  Indiana  Univer- 
sity, Bloomington,  Indiana. 

181 


182  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

subtle  change  in  the  ovary  has  been  discovered  that  has  not 
been  appreciated  before,  however.  Egg  number  does  not 
increase  in  proportion  to  ovary  weight.  Either  eggs  become 
larger  and  fewer  or  the  relative  amount  of  connective  tissue 
increases  as  the  ovary  grows.  No  critical  evidence  is  available 
to  support  either  contention.  If  the  latter  is  true,  ageing 
changes  in  the  gonads  of  fishes  would  be  similar  to  those  in 
higher  vertebrates.  The  lack  of  conclusive  proof  of  the  effect 
of  age  on  reproduction  is  disturbing  because  it  has  not  been 
possible  to  accept  or  refute  without  question  that  portion  of 
Bidder's  argument  (1925a)  that  fish  are  immortal  because  old 
individuals  show  no  decline  in  reproductive  capacity. 

Knowledge  about  fishes  has  not  progressed  to  the  point 
w^here  the  relative  importance  of  the  influence  of  rate  of 
growth  and  chronological  age  on  physiological  function  can 
be  distinguished.  This  constitutes  a  great  w^eakness  in  the 
analysis  of  differences  in  nutrition  and  reproduction  related 
to  size  and  age.  Svardson  (1951)  has  expressed  the  opinion 
that  age  of  fishes  should  be  expressed  as  "physiological  age" 
based  on  nutrition  and  rate  of  growth.  Larkin,  Terpenning 
and  Parker  (1957)  have  demonstrated  that  growth  of  rainbow 
trout  {Salmo  gairdneri)  in  different  British  Columbia  lakes  can 
be  dealt  with  more  effectively  by  comparing  growth  rates  of 
fish  of  the  same  size  rather  than  of  the  same  age.  This  point 
of  view  reflects  a  tendency  to  depart  from  traditional  age  and 
growth  studies  because  of  dissatisfaction  wdth  chronological 
age  as  an  adequate  unit  on  which  to  base  physiological  change. 
Critical  experiments  are  clearly  needed  to  separate  the  effects 
of  rate  of  growth  from  those  of  chronological  age.  In  view  of 
the  lack  of  information  it  has  been  necessary  to  refer  in  the 
ensuing  discussion  to  age  and  size  indiscriminately  without 
distinguishing  which  of  the  tw^o  factors  is  the  more  important. 

Efficiency  of  Protein  Utilization  for  Growth 

A  series  of  studies  on  the  protein  metabolism  of  sunfish, 
family  Centrarchidae,  has  been  done  in  order  to  learn  whether 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish        183 

or  not  size  and  age  have  an  effect  on  the  abihty  to  convert 
protein  to  body  flesh  (Gerking,  1952,  1954,  1955a,  b).  Longear 
sunfish  (Lepomis  megalotis),  green  sunfish  {Lepomis  cyanelhis), 
and  bluegill  (Lepomis  macrochirus)  gave  similar  results.  The 
methods  used  in  these  experiments  were  essentially  the  same 
as  those  used  to  study  food  conversion  in  other  animals  (May- 
nard,  1951).  A  weighed  quantity  of  food  was  fed  each  day  to  a 
group  of  fish  of  various  sizes  kept  in  separate  aquaria  at 
temperatures  of  about  25°  for  a  period  of  30  to  50  days.  They 
were  fed  at  maximum  or  near-maximum  rates.  At  the  end  of 
the  period  the  fish  were  killed,  weighed,  and  analysed  for 
protein.  At  the  beginning  of  the  period  their  weight  was 
known,  and  their  protein  composition  was  estimated  by 
averaging  protein  determinations  by  a  micro-Kjeldahl  method 
on  several  fish  collected  at  the  same  time  and  place  as  the  ones 
used  in  the  experiments.  The  food  was  mealworms,  Tenehrio 
molitor  larvae,  which  had  also  been  analysed  for  their  protein 
content.  Thus  the  efficiency  of  protein  utilization  for  growth 
could  be  determined  for  fish  of  various  sizes  by  comparing  the 
amount  retained  with  the  amount  absorbed  by  the  gut. 
Absorption  of  protein  was  measured  by  subtracting  the 
amount  of  nitrogen  in  the  faeces  from  that  consumed.  Ab- 
sorption was  practically  complete  in  every  fish. 

Menzel  (1957)  has  duplicated  these  experiments  on  two 
Bermuda  reef  fishes,  angelfish  (Holocanthus  hermudensis)  and 
red  hind  (Epinephalus  guttatus).  The  former  species  is  a 
herbivore  and  the  latter  a  carnivore.  They  were  fed  as  much 
as  they  would  eat  at  three  temperatures,  19,  23,  and  28°.  The 
angelfish  were  fed  algae  (Enteromorpha  salina  and  Monostroma 
oxysperma)  which  were  kept  in  the  tanks  with  them,  and  the 
red  hinds  wxre  fed  three  species  of  small  fish,  Harengula 
callolepis,  Sardinella  anchovia,  and  Anchoa  choerostoma. 
Efficiency  of  protein  utilization  for  growth  was  determined 
over  a  21 -day  period  in  the  manner  described  above. 

The  weight  of  Menzel's  fish  varied  from  50  to  763  g.  and  the 
sunfish  from  7  to  184  g.   Protein  accumulation  was  used  as  an 


184 


Shelby  D.  Gerking 


index  of  the  efficiency  of  the  growth  process  because  protein 
synthesis  is  the  most  characteristic  feature  of  growth  in 
animals.  The  fat  content  of  fishes  varies  considerably  from 
one  individual  to  another  and  during  the  seasons  of  the  year. 
Such  large  variations  in  fat  complicate  precise  growth  measure- 
ments based  on  body  weight,  dry  weight,  or  caloric  content. 


LONGEAR     SUNFISH 
olO-DAY  EXPTS. 
•  50-OAY   EXPTS. 


BLUEGfLL    SUNFISH 


DRY    WEIGHT    IN    GRAMS 


Fig.   1.    Relationship  between  efficiency  of  protein  utilization  and  dry 
weight  of  four  species  of  fish.    Sources  of  information  given  in  text. 


With  the  exception  of  the  angelfish,  the  other  four  species 
fit  a  common  pattern.  The  efficiency  of  protein  utilization  for 
growth  decreases  as  the  fish  increase  in  size  (Fig.  1),  indicating 
that  either  size,  age,  or  both  affect  the  ability  of  the  fish  to 
utilize  their  food  for  the  synthesis  of  new  protoplasm.  The 
angelfish  is  a  special  case  because  of  its  herbivorous  feeding 
habits.  They  were  unable  to  grow  on  a  diet  of  algae  and 
generally  had  a  negative  nitrogen  balance.  Menzel  concluded 
that  angelfish  cannot  grow  on  a  diet  of  plants  alone  unless  they 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish        185 

eat  extraordinary  quantities  or  algae  with  a  much  higher 
protein  content  than  he  used.  Probably  the  "herbivorous" 
fish  depends  to  a  large  extent  on  the  invertebrates  living  in 
association  with  plants  for  the  protein  required  for  growth. 
The  results  of  the  experiments  on  angelfish  are.  therefore, 
not  comparable  with  the  others. 

The  experimental  results  on  the  four  carnivorous  species 
are  similar.  In  every  case  the  efficiency  of  protein  utilization 
decreased  as  the  size  of  the  fish  increased.  Efficiency  was  very 
high  in  the  smallest  fishes.  For  example,  green  sunfish  (body 
weight  =  7'1  g.)  were  39*7  per  cent  efficient  in  using 
protein  for  growth;  longear  sunfish  (9-1  g.)  were  33-3  per 
cent  efficient;  bluegills  (7-7  g.)  38-0  per  cent;  and  the  red 
hind  (about  227  g.)  32-1  per  cent.  In  contrast,  utilization 
among  the  largest  specimens  was  20-0  per  cent  (48-5  g.),  4-7 
per  cent  (103-3  g.),  23  •  6  per  cent  (184-0  g.),  and  22  •  7  per  cent 
(612  g.),  respectively,  for  the  four  species.  The  value  for  the 
largest  longear  sunfish  departs  considerably  from  the  others. 
This  specimen  was  as  large  as  any  ever  observed  in  the  creek 
where  it  was  captured  near  Bloomington,  Indiana,  and  it  was 
probably  over  six  years  old.  The  other  three  species  are  not 
represented  by  individuals  of  maximum  size  or  age.  This  may 
indicate  that  extremely  large  individuals  have  a  very  low 
efficiency  of  protein  utilization  for  growth.  More  evidence  is 
required  to  establish  this  point,  however. 

The  shape  of  the  graphs  is  not  consistent.  A  linear  relation- 
ship expresses  the  relation  between  protein  efficiency  and  dry 
weight  in  the  longear  sunfish,  but  it  is  curvilinear  in  the  other 
species.  The  inconsistency  is  due  to  individual  variation  and 
the  difficulty  in  establishing  the  initial  protein  content  of  the 
experimental  fish  from  analyses  of  sample  fish. 

Menzel  answered  an  important  question  with  respect  to  the 
effect  of  temperature  on  protein  utilization.  The  red  hind  ate 
only  slightly  more  at  23°  than  at  19°  but  ate  about  twice  as 
much  at  28°.  This  great  difference  in  feeding  rate  did  not 
alter  the  efficiencies  of  protein  utilization,  however.   Thus  all 


186  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

protein  utilization  experiments  in  this  9°  temperature  range 
are  comparable. 

A  large  amount  of  work  has  been  done  on  food  conversion 
by  fishes,  but  most  of  it  relates  weight  gain  to  the  amount  of 
food  consumed.  Large  variations  have  been  encountered  and 
are  due  to  variable  fat  deposition,  unknown  organic  composi- 
tion of  the  food,  differences  in  the  size  of  experimental  fish, 
and  other  factors.  Most  of  the  research  has  been  done  on  a 
variety  of  foods  fed  to  young  trout  in  order  to  obtain  maxi- 
mum growth  rates  in  hatcheries.  This  material  is  not  applic- 
able to  the  present  discussion  since  the  fish  used  in  the  experi- 
ments were  nearly  uniform  in  size  and  age.  Ivlev  (1939a,  h,  c), 
Karzinkin  (1939)  and  Morgulis  (1919)  have  studied  fish  nutri- 
tion by  detailed  analyses  of  the  organic  constituents  of  the 
fish  and  their  food,  but  none  of  these  workers  compared  the 
efficiency  of  food  utilization  by  fish  of  different  sizes. 

Although  the  effect  of  age  and  size  on  protein  utilization  for 
growth  cannot  be  separated,  it  can  be  stated  definitely  that 
larger  fish  are  less  efficient  in  this  respect  than  smaller  ones. 
Chronological  age  may  play  some  part  in  this  phenomenon. 
It  is  universally  true  that  the  rate  of  growth  declines  as  age 
and  size  increase.  In  this  respect  fishes  conform  to  the  com- 
mon vertebrate  pattern.  The  protein  metabolism  studies 
demonstrate  that  this  decline  is  due  to  a  decreasing  ability  of 
the  fish  to  utilize  its  food  for  growth  as  it  grows  larger  and 
older.  The  growth  of  most  vertebrates  stops  relatively  early 
in  life  while  that  of  fishes  is  prolonged.  Although  this  dif- 
ference cannot  be  explained  at  the  present  time,  the  loss  of 
growth  efficiency  with  age  is  clearly  exhibited  by  both. 

Fecundity 

Live -bearing  fishes 

There  are  several  groups  of  fish  which  give  birth  to  well- 
developed  young,  but  reproduction  in  relation  to  age  has  been 
studied  in  only  one  family,  the  Poeciliidae.   In  this  family  the 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish        187 

male  is  much  smaller  than  the  female.  His  growth  stops  soon 
after  sexual  maturity  is  reached,  but  the  female  continues  to 
grow  until  death.  Fertilization  is  internal  and  the  sperm 
transfer  is  accomplished  by  a  greatly  modified  anal  fin,  the 
gonopodium,  which  acts  as  the  male  copulatory  organ.  The 
sperm  penetrate  the  ovarian  wall  and  fertilize  the  eggs  while 
they  are  within  the  ovarian  follicle.  Development  proceeds, 
and  the  young  are  born  in  various  stages  of  development 
according  to  the  species. 

Reproduction  and  senescence  have  been  studied  only  in  the 
western  mosquitofish,  Gambusia  a.  affinis.  Krumholz  (1948) 
has  provided  an  unusually  good  series  of  observations  on  the 
number  of  young  in  successive  broods  of  females  from  the 
onset  of  sexual  maturity  until  death.  Females  were  collected 
from  ponds  in  southern  Michigan  and  in  the  vicinity  of 
Chicago  during  the  summers  of  1939  to  1944,  and  the  number 
of  embryos  in  the  ovary  was  counted.  The  number  of  young 
produced  by  a  single  female  depends  on:  (1)  the  number  of 
broods  liberated  during  a  season,  (2)  the  length  of  the  mother 
fish,  (3)  the  time  of  season  at  which  the  individual  broods  are 
cast,  and  (4)  the  locality  in  which  the  mother  fish  lives.  The 
first  two  factors  are  pertinent  to  the  present  discussion. 

The  age  and  size  at  first  maturity  are  correlated  with  the 
time  of  year  when  the  female  offspring  are  born  and  their 
rate  of  growth.  Faster-growing  individuals  generally  reach 
maturity  at  an  earlier  date  than  slower-growing  individuals. 
Mosquitofish  born  late  in  the  summer  do  not  reach  maturity 
until  the  following  spring  while  those  born  early  in  the 
summer  may  reproduce  within  a  month  or  two.  The  number 
of  young  in  a  brood  increase^  as  the  length  of  the  female 
increases,  but  at  large  sizes  the  rate  of  increase  is  drastically 
reduced.  The  rate  of  increase  in  fecundity  in  relation  to  length 
of  the  female  is  greater  in  the  second  brood  than  in  the  first  or 
subsequent  ones.  In  one  case  the  number  of  embryos  in  the 
first  brood  increased  as  the  1  •  3  power  of  the  length ;  the  second 
brood  as  the  1  •  4  power,  and  the  third  brood  as  the  0  •  8  power. 


188 


Shelby  D.  Gerking 


Numbers  of  embryos  in  the  fourth  brood  were  so  variable  that 
no  relationship  with  length  of  the  female  could  be  demon- 
strated. 

The  influence  of  age  on  fecundity  was  proved  by  both  field 
and  laboratory  observations.  Females  of  similar  size  col- 
lected at  an  earlier  date  in  the  summer  contained  a  greater 

Table  I 

The  effect  of  age  on  fecundity  of  female  mosquitofish 

{Gambusia  affinis)  of  the  same  size.   Modified  from 

Krumholz  (1948) 


Size  of  female 

Date  of  collection     No.  of  females        Average  no.  of 

in  mm. 

embryos 

Argonne  Woods  Pond 

46-55 

June  9 

63                          210-4 

July  13 

194                           152-9 

38-44 

July  13 

179                           153-3 

Aug.  14 

3                             42-7 
Sanitary — District  Lake 

35-44 

June  9 

67                            30-7 

July  13 

236                             28-4 

50-55 

July  13 

228                             28-8 

Aug.  14 

449                               8-8 
Parr's  Lake 

27-45 

July  31 

330                            47-4 

Aug.  14 

324                             34-3 

35-46 

Aug.  14 

167                              49-9 

Sept.  19 

25                              17-0 

number  of  embryos  than  those  collected  later  (Table  I).  The 
decrease  in  embryo  production  was  greatest  during  late 
summer,  near  the  end  of  life.  Among  fish  born  at  approxi- 
mately the  same  time  there  was  a  decrease  in  fecundity  with 
age  despite  an  increase  in  size.  One  group  of  females  averaged 
49  mm.  in  length  and  yielded  a  mean  of  205-4  embryos  on 
June  9,  1939;  on  July  13  the  females  were  52-6  mm.  long  and 
gave  155  •  7  embryos;  and  a  few  remaining  fish  of  the  same  age 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish        189 

group  measured  52-3  mm.  on  August  14  and  contained  42-7 
embryos.  Under  laboratory  conditions  four  fish  gave  birth  to 
a  decreasing  number  of  young.  They  averaged  30  •  8  young  in 
the  first  two  broods,  10-0  in  the  third,  and  7-0  in  the  fourth. 

There  may  be  a  period  of  reproductive  sterihty  in  Gamhusia 
late  in  life.  In  one  collection  the  largest  female  was  not  gravid 
while  other  sexually  mature  individuals  were.  The  ovaries  of 
this  female  were  examined  microscopically,  but  no  ova  were 
found. 

The  only  other  pertinent  observation  in  this  connexion  was 
made  by  Fraser  and  Renton  (1940)  on  Heterandria  formosa, 
the  dwarf  top-minnow.  Successive  broods  were  followed  in  a 
single  female  from  April  1934  until  death  in  May  1936.  She 
grew  to  a  remarkably  large  size  (40  mm.),  and  produced  a 
total  of  170  young,  150  of  which  were  born  during  the  first 
eight  months.  She  showed  diminishing  fertility  during  the 
last  few  months  of  life.  At  the  time  of  death  the  ovary  was 
examined  microscopically  and  found  to  be  a  "mass  of  de- 
generating tissue". 

Egg-laying  fishes 

Bidder  (1925a,  19256,  and  1932)  raised  an  extremely  provo- 
cative question  when  he  proposed  that  fish  and  certain  other 
aquatic  animals  are  potentially  immortal.  He  based  his 
argument  on  reproduction  and  growth  in  the  plaice  (Pleuro- 
nectes  platessa).  He  noted  that  these  fish  continue  to  grow 
throughout  their  lives  and  that  the  oldest  individuals  retain 
their  reproductive  capacity.  His  idea  was  immediately 
challenged  by  Wallace  (1925)  who  pointed  out  that  male 
plaice  had  a  higher  mortality  rate  than  females  and  that  "this 
apparently  implies  natural  death".  Bidder  answered  by 
making  a  distinction  between  "senile  death"  and  "parental 
death".  Parental  death  refers  to  that  which  occurs  as  a  result 
of  the  reproductive  act.  He  supported  this  definition  by  draw- 
ing upon  the  same  plaice  data  which  Wallace  had  used  to 
show  that  six-sevenths  of  the  males  die  after  the  first  spawning 


190  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

Senile  death  in  Bidder's  opinion  should  be  reserved  for  those 
animals  which  exhibit  "negative  growth"  after  full  sexual 
maturity  and  specific  growth  have  been  achieved.  In  his 
last  paper  Bidder  took  his  stand  on  the  basis  of  correlating 
ageing  with  specific  growth  and  ceased  to  make  an  issue  of 
reproductive  performance. 

The  issue  of  reproduction  was  soon  raised  again  by  Orton 
(1929)  in  a  somewhat  different  manner.  He  asked  the  question 
whether  or  not  fishes  might  die  as  a  direct  or  indirect  result  of 
expending  themselves  in  reproduction.  Russell's  (1914)  data 
were  cited,  which  suggested  that  the  reproductive  organs  of 
large  haddock  (Melanogr animus  aeglefinus)  make  up  a  larger 
proportion  of  the  body  weight  than  they  do  in  smaller  speci- 
mens. Metabolism  is  concentrated  overwhelmingly  on  repro- 
duction, and  although  the  expenditure  of  energy  may  not  kill, 
the  animal  may  become  so  unstable  that  otherwise  sublethal 
factors  might  be  brought  into  play  and  cause  death.  Orton 
dramatized  this  effect  by  terming  it  "over-reproduction". 

Orton's  viewpoint  turns  up  in  another  connexion.  Svardson 
(1949)  considered  the  effect  of  natural  selection  on  the  egg 
number  of  fishes  and  concluded  that  there  must  be  opposing 
selection  pressures  for  decrease  as  well  as  an  increase  in  egg 
number.  Clearly,  a  mutation  causing  an  increase  in  egg 
number  would  have  a  selective  advantage  and  spread  through 
a  population  unless  there  were  factors  opposing  such  a  change. 
In  his  words:  "There  would'  be  an  anatomical  and  physio- 
logical limit  for  the  females'  capacity  of  producing  more  eggs. 
When  the  egg  number  has  been  brought  up  to  this  limit,  only 
those  individuals  not  exceeding  the  limit  could  spawn,  while 
the  others  died."  Svardson  later  rejects  this  as  a  major 
factor  in  evolution  and  concludes  that  egg  number  would  be 
limited  by  the  ability  of  fish  to  protect  the  young  among 
those  fishes  which  behave  in  this  way.  More  importantly,  he 
thinks,  egg  number  is  limited  by  the  premium  on  large  eggs 
which  produce  large  fry.  He  postulates  that  the  large  fry  are 
in  a  better  competitive  position  in  the  population. 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish        191 

Bidder's  theory  that  fish  are  potentially  immortal  has  never 
attracted  much  attention  among  fishery  workers.  No  doubt 
its  lack  of  popularity  is  due  to  the  fact  that  those  who  work 
with  fish  know  that  they  die  and  that  the  lifespan  of  most 
species  is  short  compared  with  our  own.  Among  the  host  of 
workers  who  have  aged  fish  by  inspecting  the  growth  rings  on 
the  scales  or  other  hard  parts,  none  has  observed  a  specimen 
which  spanned  the  centuries.  Direct  observation  by  aquarium 
curators  throughout  the  world  has  led  to  the  same  conclusion 
(S.  Hinton,  unpubHshed).  Only  21  of  328  species  reported 
lived  longer  than  20  years.  The  oldest  fish  was  a  sturgeon 
(Acipenser  ruihenus)  in  the  Royal  Zoological  Society  Aquarium 
in  Amsterdam,  Holland,  which  lived  for  69  years  and  8  months. 
The  cause  of  death  was  not  reported  for  any  species  in  the  list. 

On  the  other  hand,  Orton's  views  have  led  to  some  hard 
thinking.  There  have  been  several  attempts  to  learn  whether 
or  not  fishes  "over-reproduce"  to  the  point  where  the  effort 
interferes  with  other  life  functions.  As  we  shall  see,  some 
studies  affirm  while  others  negate  this  point  of  view. 

Variability.  There  is  a  tremendous  variation  in  fecundity, 
not  only  in  different  species  but  within  the  same  species  from 
place  to  place  and  among  individuals  of  the  same  size.  This 
variation  immeasurably  complicates  studies  on  the  effect  of 
age  on  fecundity,  and  as  a  result  the  critical  studies  are  of  a 
statistical  nature.  The  following  sources  of  variation  should 
be  kept  in  mind  while  reviewing  the  evidence. 

1.  Size  variation.  Fecundity  increases  as  the  length  and 
weight  of  the  fish  increases. 

2.  Individual  variation.  Every  fecundity  study  has 
demonstrated  a  great  individual  variation  in  fecundity  for 
fish  of  the  same  length  and  of  the  same  weight. 

3.  Geographical  variation.  The  fecundity  of  individuals  of 
comparable  size  varies  considerably  from  one  locality  to 
another.  Maar's  (1949)  work  on  the  char  (Salmo  alpinus)  of  the 
Faxalven  Water  System,  Sweden,  has  shown  that  the  egg 


192  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

number  per  female  may  vary  as  much  as  fivefold  in  different 
lakes  of  the  same  watershed.  In  fact,  geographical  variation 
has  been  so  prominent  that  "races"  of  some  species  have  been 
delineated  partly  on  the  basis  of  fecundity.  Davis  (1957)  has 
shown  that  the  ova  production  of  the  American  shad  {Alosa 
sapidissima)  varies  considerably  from  one  river  system  of 
the  Atlantic  Coast  drainage  to  another,  and  McGregor  (1923) 
could  distinguish  certain  river  races  of  the  king  salmon 
(Oncorhynchus  tshawytscha)  on  the  Pacific  Coast  on  the  basis 
of  egg  counts. 

4.  Year-to-year  variations  in  the  same  locality.  Individuals 
of  the  same  size  may  bear  significantly  different  numbers  of 
eggs  from  one  year  to  the  next.  This  is  true  for  the  long 
rough  dab  {Hippoglossoides  platessoides)  off  the  coast  of 
Scotland  (Bagenal,  19576)  and  for  the  lake  trout  {Salvelinus 
namayciish)  of  Lake  Opeongo,  Ontario,  Canada  (Fry,  1949). 

These  wide  variations  in  fecundity  are  usually  explained  on 
the  basis  of  genetic  and  dietary  differences.  The  racial 
studies  imply  that  heredity  is  very  important  in  determining 
fecundity.  Year-to-year  variation  has  been  attributed  to  dif- 
ferences in  the  availability  of  food.  Both  sources  undoubtedly 
account  for  the  fluctuations  in  the  egg  number  of  individuals 
of  the  same  size. 

Because  of  the  complex  nature  of  the  information  on 
fecundity,  evidence  of  the  ageing  process  from  a  few  species 
in  which  fecundity  has  received  considerable  study  will  be 
reviewed  here. 

Methods  of  counting  eggs.  The  usual  methods  of  deter- 
mining fecundity  vary  from  counting  the  entire  number  of 
eggs  from  both  ovaries  to  counting  samples  from  various 
sections  of  one  or  both  ovaries  and  computing  the  total 
number  of  eggs  on  the  basis  of  the  sample.  Three  sampling 
methods  have  been  used:  volumetric,  dry  weight,  and  wet 
weight.  In  the  first  method  the  total  volume  of  eggs  is 
determined  in  a  graduated  cylinder  and  the  number  in  a 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish        193 

known  sample  volume  is  counted.  The  fecundity  is  deter- 
mined by  direct  proportion.  The  dry- weight  and  wet- weight 
methods  are  essentially  the  same  except  that  the  counts  are 
made  either  on  eggs  which  have  been  dried  to  constant  weight 
or  on  eggs  taken  directly  from  the  preserving  fluid. 

Like  those  of  other  animals,  the  ovaries  of  fishes  contain 
eggs  in  various  stages  of  development.  Early  workers  debated 
the  issue  as  to  which  eggs  would  be  spawned.  By  measuring 
the  diameter  of  the  ova  in  fish  just  ready  to  spawn,  by  des- 
cribing their  external  appearance,  and  by  examining  histo- 
logical sections  of  the  ovary,  various  categories  of  eggs  were 
established.  A  comparison  of  these  observations  with  those 
on  the  ovaries  of  spent  fish  was  the  basis  for  deciding  which 
eggs  to  count  for  an  estimate  of  the  fecundity  of  an  individual. 
These  observations  have  been  made  in  great  detail,  and  they 
have  resulted  in  learning  that  not  all  ripe  eggs  are  shed  dur- 
ing the  spawning  season.  Usually  the  numbers  retained  are 
insignificant  compared  to  those  that  are  liberated.  Ripe  ova 
which  are  retained  in  the  ovary  quickly  degenerate  and  are 
resorbed.  Such  observations  have  also  been  responsible  for 
discovering  that  some  species,  like  the  long  rough  dab,  do  not 
spawn  every  year  (Bagenal,  1957a)  while  in  others,  like  the 
yellowfin  tuna  {Neothunnus  macropterus),  several  batches  of 
eggs  may  mature  during  a  single,  long  spawning  season  (June, 
1953). 

Herring.  Wynne-Edwards  (1929)  immediately  set  about 
testing  Orton's  hypothesis  about  a  possible  disproportionate 
growth  of  the  gonad  in  relation  to  body  weight  in  the  herring 
{Clupea  harengus).  Both  ovary  and  testes  reach  maximum 
weight  during  the  spawning  season  and  decrease  to  an 
insignificant  size  immediately  after  the  sexual  products  have 
been  shed.  The  cycle  of  gonad  growth  then  begins  again  in 
preparation  for  the  next  spawning.  Since  the  reproductive 
tissue  makes  up  about  20  per  cent  of  the  weight  of  the  body 
at  maturity,  a  considerable  amount  of  the  anabolic  process  is 
concentrated  on  the  maturation  of  the  sex  organs. 

AGEING — ^V — 7 


194  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

Wynne-Edwards  argued  that  if  the  gonads  of  mature  fish 
increase  in  weight  each  year  at  a  rate  greater  than  that  of  the 
body,  then  the  increasing  tax  of  spawning  may  bring  about 
the  animal's  death.  Otherwise,  death  must  be  due  to  other 
factors  if  the  development  of  the  reproductive  organs  is  in 
harmony  with  the  rest  of  the  body.  The  main  study  was  made 
on  a  sample  of  herrings,  called  "calf  herrings",  from  the  Irish 
Sea.  Each  individual  was  measured,  the  fish  and  gonads 
were  weighed  separately,  and  the  age  was  determined  by 
examining  the  number  of  annual  rings  on  the  scales.*  A  linear 
relationship  was  found  between  body  weight  and  gonad 
weight.  The  testes  were  somewhat  heavier  than  the  ovaries  in 
herrings  of  comparable  size,  but  the  rate  of  growth  of  the 
gonads  was  practically  the  same.  The  relative  size  of  the 
gonads  in  relation  to  the  body  increased  during  the  first  four 
spawnings  but  remained  constant  thereafter.  Wynne-Edwards 
also  compared  the  ratio  of  gonad  weight  to  body  weight  for 
fish  of  the  same  weight  but  of  varying  ages  and  learned  that 
the  ratio  remained  constant.  He  concluded  that  age  was  not 
responsible  for  any  significant  change  in  the  weights  of  the 
gonad.  He  states,  "There  is  no  indication  of  an  increasing 
tax  which  the  fish  cannot  make  up,  the  effects  of  which  con- 
tinue to  pile  up  until  ultimately  they  cause  its  death,  in  a  way 
that  has  sometimes  been  suggested".  The  growth  of  the 
gonads  was  in  harmony  with  that  of  the  other  organs. 

The  question  appeared  to  be  settled  until  Farran  (1938) 
undertook  a  further  study  of  the  Irish  herring.  He  was  pri- 
marily concerned  with  the  difference  in  ova  diameter  between 
autumn  and  spring  spawners,  the  latter  having  larger  and  less 
numerous  eggs  than  the  former.  During  the  course  of  the 
analysis,  he  related  the  weight  of  the  ovary  and  the  number 
of  ova  in  435  herring  to  the  4-5  power  of  the  length  of  the 
fish,  a  value  greater  than  the  relation  of  total  body  weight  to 

*  An  age  group  consists  of  all  fish  in  a  population  sample  which  have  the 
same  number  of  annual  rings.  A  Roman  numeral  is  conventionally  used  to 
indicate  the  number  of  annual  rings. 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish       195 

length.  No  ages  were  reported.  This  relationship  was  des- 
cribed without  rigid  statistical  treatment,  but  it  is  apparent 
from  his  diagrams  that  more  refined  methods  would  have 
produced  little  change  from  his  value.  Farran's  formula 
described  the  situation  except  for  the  largest  fish,  and  it  is 
these  that  interest  us  most.  The  ovary  weights  and  number  of 
ova  for  these  individuals  fall  below  the  values  predicted  by 
Farran's  equation.  He  recognized  this  departure  and  con- 
ceded that  very  large  fish  show  a  smaller  rate  of  increase  in  the 
size  of  the  ovaries  than  smaller  fish.  He  interprets  this  growth 
pattern  in  the  following  way:  "...  that  the  rate  of  increase  in 
number  (of  ova)  in  fish  over  32  cm.  in  length  ceases  to  cor- 
respond to  the  increase  in  length  and  either  falls  off  consider- 
ably or  ceases  altogether."  Contrary  to  Wynne-Edward's 
conclusions,  Farran's  results  might  indicate  that  reproduction 
places  an  increasing  strain  on  the  larger  female  herring,  leading 
to  a  reduction  in  number  of  eggs.  On  the  other  hand,  Farran 
may  have  simply  described  the  normal  course  of  ovary 
growth  in  relation  to  the  body. 

Sensitive  to  both  of  the  above  workers'  findings,  Hickling 
(1940)  took  up  the  question,  this  time  using  the  herring  of 
East  Anglia.  Both  of  the  previous  workers  had  used  only 
weight  of  gonads  in  their  analyses.  Farran  had  counted  the 
eggs  of  only  three  specimens  and  used  the  ratio  of  number  of 
eggs  to  ovary  weight  to  calculate  egg  numbers  in  the  remainder 
of  his  sample.  Hickling  counted  the  eggs  of  136  herring  of 
known  age  and  length,  and  observed  the  sex,  length,  weight, 
and  age  of  475  additional  individuals. 

Hickling  agreed  with  Farran  that  the  rate  of  gonad  growth 
was  greater  than  the  rate  of  body  weight  gain  in  relation  to 
length,  and  concluded  that  reproduction  became  an  increas- 
ingly greater  burden  to  both  male  and  female  as  they  grew 
larger  and  older  (Table  II).  Even  more  interesting  is  the  fact 
that  the  weight  of  the  ovaries  increased  at  a  more  rapid  rate 
in  relation  to  length  (L^^^)  than  the  rate  of  increase  in  the 
number  of  eggs  (L^'*^).   This  was  consistent  with  his  analysis 


196 


Shelby  D.  Gerking 


of  the  weight  of  the  gonad  and  egg  number  as  related  to  age. 
The  rate  of  increase  in  gonad  weight  with  respect  to  age  was 
greater  than  the  rate  of  increase  in  egg  number.  Therefore 
Hickhng  was  forced  to  conclude  that  the  permanent  tissue  of 
the  ovary  increases  disproportionately  as  the  herring  grows 
larger  and  older.  This  suggests  a  gradual  degeneration  of  the 
ovary,  the  reproductive  tissues  being  replaced  by  connective 
tissue  in  much  the  same  manner  as  the  testis  changes  in 
Astyanax  mexicanus  in  relation  to  age  (Rasquin  and  Hafter, 
1951).  Unfortunately,  there  have  been  no  observations  of  this 
sort  on  the  histology  of  the  fish  ovary. 

Table  II 

Regressions  of  body  weight  in  grams  (TF),  gonad  weight  in  grams 

(GW),  AND  fecundity  {F)  on  body  length  in  centimetres  (L)  in  the 

ENGLISH  HERRING  {Clupca  harengus).  Taken  from  Hickling  (1940) 


Males 

Females 

Body  weight 
Gonad  weight 
Egg  number 

W  =  0-0661  L2.312 
GW  =  2-41  X  10-5L4-237 

W  =  1-1471  Li-^se 
GW  =  5  94>  X  10-5L3-9'3 
F  =  0-2954  L3*  465 

Research  on  the  fecundity  of  the  Pacific  herring  (Clupea 
pallasii)  agrees  with  the  conclusions  of  Hickling  and  Farran. 
Katz  and  Erickson  (1950)  analysed  the  relationship  of 
fecundity  and  length  in  different  age  groups.  This  is  a  log-log 
relation  described  by  the  formula:  F  =  CL^,  where  F  = 
fecundity,  L  =  length,  and  C  and  n  are  empirically  deter- 
mined constants.  The  values  of  the  exponent  differed  con- 
siderably among  separate  age  groups.  Those  herring  that 
were  spawning  for  the  first  time  (age  II)  were  the  least  effec- 
tive egg  producers  {n  =  3-46).  Age  groups  III  and  IV  were 
most  efficient  (n  =3-89  and  3-87,  respectively).  The  rate  of 
increase  in  fecundity  was  considerably  less  among  ages  V  to 
VIII  (n  =3-52).    This  result  suggests  that  the  relation  of 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish        197 

fecundity  to  length  is  not  a  simple  one  and  can  be  broken 
down  into  three  parts,  possibly  a  sigmoid  curve.  Eschmeyer 
(1950,  1955)  has  described  a  sigmoid  fecundity /length  re- 
lationship in  the  walleye  {Stizostedion  vitreum)  and  lake  trout. 
The  correlation  of  a  decrease  in  the  rate  of  egg  production 
with  age  was  regarded  by  Katz  and  Erickson  as  a  criterion  of 
ageing  in  the  herring. 

A  ballot  on  whether  or  not  the  strain  of  reproduction  in  the 
Atlantic  and  Pacific  species  of  herrings  upsets  the  homeostatic 
mechanism  to  the  point  where  the  fish  dies  as  a  direct  or 
indirect  effect  of  its  reproductive  efforts  yields  three  votes 
affirmative,  one  vote  negative.  The  affirmative  votes  should 
be  scrutinized  carefully,  however,  because  none  of  them  were 
cast  after  having  taken  individual  variation  into  account. 

Salmon.  Some  interesting  information  has  been  accumu- 
lated which  indicates  that  age  influences  egg  production  in  two 
species  of  salmon.  The  Atlantic  salmon  (Salmo  solar)  spawns 
in  rivers  after  spending  either  two  or  three  years  in  the  sea  and 
some  may  spawn  more  than  once.  Belding  (1940)  studied  the 
fecundity  of  this  species  from  the  Gulf  of  St.  Lawrence  and 
learned  that  the  youngest  spawners,  those  which  had  lived 
two  years  in  the  sea,  produced  a  greater  number  of  eggs  in 
relation  to  their  weight  (834  eggs  per  lb.)  than  either  three- 
year  sea-life  individuals  (723  eggs  per  lb.)  or  those  which  had 
previously  spawned  (738  eggs  per  lb.).  In  actual  numbers  the 
two-year  salmon  produced  8,850  eggs  per  female  and  the 
three-year  salmon  produced  about  14,000.  He  attributed  this 
decline  in  relative  egg  production  in  part  to  the  fact  that  large 
salmon  usually  have  larger  eggs  than  small  salmon,  but  he  did 
not  discount  age  as  a  factor  influencing  egg  production. 

Rounsefell  (1957)  has  made  a  more  detailed  study  of  fecund- 
ity of  the  sockeye  salmon  {Oncorhynchus  nerka)  in  the  Karluk 
River,  Alaska.  In  this  part  of  the  world  the  young  sockeye 
spend  either  three  or  four  years  in  freshwater  lakes  before 
migrating  to  the  sea  and  remain  there  either  two  or  three 
years    before   returning   to    freshwater    streams    to    spawn. 


198  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

Classifying  the  salmon  on  the  basis  of  the  same  freshwater  age 
but  different  ocean  age,  Rounsefell  pointed  out  that  the  older 
salmon  produced  a  significantly  smaller  number  of  eggs  than 
younger  ones.  The  average  for  the  former  was  2,987  per 
female  and  the  average  for  the  latter  was  3,285,  based  on 
about  150  specimens  in  each  group.  The  difference  in  fecund- 
ity for  sockeye  with  identical  ocean  histories  but  different 
freshwater  ages  also  favoured  the  idea  that  age  has  an  effect 
on  salmon  fecundity  since  the  older  females  produced  118 
fewer  eggs  than  the  younger  ones.  The  difference  was  not 
statistically  significant  and  the  data  were  more  variable.  The 
increased  variation  was  explained  by  the  variable  freshwater 
environment  as  opposed  to  more  stable  ocean  conditions. 

Rounsefell  reviews  the  literature  of  the  fecundity  of  the 
family  Salmonidae,  and  of  primary  interest  here  is  his  demon- 
stration that  generally  the  rate  of  increase  in  number  of  eggs 
declines  as  the  fish  increase  in  size.  This  may  indicate  that  age 
influences  egg  production,  or  it  may  be  simply  a  description  of 
the  way  in  which  the  ovary  grows  in  relation  to  the  rest  of  the 
body.  It  does  explain,  however,  why  trout  hatcheries  discard 
their  old  brood  stock.  The  number  of  eggs  in  relation  to  the 
weight  of  the  fish  declines  as  the  fish  increase  in  size,  and  it  is  to 
the  hatchery's  advantage  to  have  on  hand  a  greater  number  of 
smaller  breeders  than  an  equal  weight  of  older  ones. 

Haddock.  One  of  the  most  penetrating  analyses  on  the 
effect  of  age  on  the  fecundity  of  egg-laying  fishes  has  been 
done  by  Raitt  (1933).  He  was  also  influenced  by  Orton's 
writings.  The  mature  eggs  of  169  haddock  {Melanogrammus 
aeglefinis)  of  Scottish  waters  were  counted  and  fecundity  was 
related  to  length,  weight,  and  age.  Four  main  comparisons 
were  made:  (1)  rate  of  increase  in  fecundity  with  length, 
(2)  rate  of  increase  in  ovary  weight  with  length,  (3)  rate  of 
decrease  in  body  weight  in  relation  to  length  during  ovarian 
development,  and  (4)  rate  of  increase  in  body  weight  with 
length.  Comparison  of  these  relationships  indicated  that 
fecundity  increases  with  age  up  to  age  V,  but  at  older  ages 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish       199 

egg  production  declined  (Table  III).  The  values  were  based 
on  regressions  calculated  separately  for  the  different  age 
groups.  At  similar  lengths  regular  increases  in  fecundity  with 
age  occur  among  ages  II,  III,  IV,  and  V.  Ages  VI  and  VII 
were   combined  in  the   calculations   because  individuals   of 

Table  III 

Fecundity  of  haddock  {Melanogrammns  aeglefinis)  of  the 

SAJME    SIZE    AT    VARIOUS    AGES.     MODIFIED    FROM    RaITT    (1933) 


Length 

Age  II 

Age  III 

Age  IV 

AgeV 

Ages  VI 

in  cm. 

and  VII 

20 

11,495 

25 

34,255 

58,185 

30 

88,305 

105,150 

35 

172,950 

181,500 

189,410 

178,650 

40 

266,450 

283,850 

290,900 

276,950 

45 

415,150 

424,250 

50 

591,600 

595,550 

55 

809,850 

796,600 

these  ages  were  scarce  in  the  population.  Their  fecundity  was 
about  five  per  cent  lower  than  that  of  age  V.  At  extreme 
lengths  the  latter  group  has  a  slightly  lower  fecundity  than 
age  IV  of  the  same  length.  The  same  result  was  found  when 
the  fecundity  of  haddock  of  the  same  weight  were  compared 
at  different  ages. 

The  effect  of  age  was  shown  to  be  due  to  the  relationship 
between  fecundity/length  and  body  weight/length.  Raitt 
calculated  the  first  of  these  comparisons  separately  for  the 
different  age  groups.  Fecundity  increased  very  rapidly  in  the 
youngest  spawners  of  age  group  II  {F  =  0-005187  X  L*'^^), 
but  was  considerably  lower  in  the  remainder  of  the  popula- 
tions, as  the  remaining  equations  show :  age  III  {F  =  1-  788  X 
L^-^%  age  IV  (i^  =  1-527  X  L^'^^),  age  V  (i^  =  2-069  X 
L3-21),  and  ages  VI  and  VII  combined  (2^  =  1-546  X  L^^^). 
Raitt  used  Russell's  extensive  data  (1914)  on  the  haddock  to 


200  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

compute  the  rate  of  increase  in  body  weight  with  length. 
Here  he  found  the  relationship  to  he  W  =  0-0044  L^'^^  based 
on  the  mean  yearly  weights  as  recorded  by  Russell.  At  all 
ages  fecundity  increased  more  rapidly  than  body  weight. 

The  relationship  between  fecundity  and  body  weight  was: 
F  =  196  X  PF^'^*  (all  ages).  Since  the  exponent  is  greater 
than  one,  the  equation  confirmed  the  above  interpretation 
that  fecundity  increases  at  a  rate  greater  than  body  weight. 
The  rate  of  increase  in  ovary  weight  with  length  was  consistent 
with  this  result.  The  rate  of  decline  in  body  weight  with 
length  during  ovarian  development  showed  that  somatic 
tissue  was  being  converted  to  gonad  from  November  to  June, 
the  spawning  season,  and  that  somatic  tissue  increased  from 
July  to  November. 

The  consistency  of  Raitt's  results  arouses  serious  suspicion 
that  reproduction  is  a  drain  on  the  individual  in  later  life.  In 
his  words,  "One  cannot  but  regard  the  above  evidence  as 
hinting  at  an  end  point  to  reproduction,  and  inviting  postula- 
tion  of  stress  of  egg  production,  ultimately  overbalancing 
ability  to  recover  within  the  annual  cycle.  It  would  seem  that 
an  affirmative  answer  is  suggested  to  Orton's  question  of 
whether  'over-reproduction'  may  be  regarded  as  a  general 
predisposing  cause  of  death  in  fishes." 

Long  rough  dab.  Bagenal  (1957a)  has  recently  studied 
the  fecundity  of  the  long  rough  dab  of  Scotland  by  detailed 
statistical  procedures.  His  fish  were  caught  in  one  locality  by 
a  small  mesh  cotton  trawl.  Length,  sex,  gutted  weight,  gonad 
weight,  and  age  were  determined  for  a  large  series  of  specimens 
taken  from  October  1933  to  May  1955.  Egg  counts  and  the 
foregoing  measurements  were  made  on  two  samples,  totalling 
119  females,  one  caught  in  February  and  the  other  in  March 
1954,  just  before  spawning.  By  an  analysis  of  covariance  he 
was  able  to  show  that  there  was  no  effect  of  age  on  the  length/ 
weight  regression  so  all  ages  were  pooled  for  the  calculation  of 
the  regression  coefficient  describing  this  relationship.  Simil- 
arly, age  had  no  significant  effect  on  the  ovary  weight/body 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish        201 

weight  regression.  There  was  a  significant  difference  due  to 
age  on  the  ovary  weight/length  relationship  in  the  March 
sample,  but  on  good  grounds  Bagenal  considers  this  to  be  an 
anomalous  result.  Weight  increases  at  a  power  3-11  of  length 
in  maturing  females  while  ovary  weight  increases  at  about  the 
3  •  5  power  of  length.  The  latter  figure  is  an  estimate  because 
the  entire  data  would  have  to  be  recalculated  in  order  to 
obtain  an  overall  coefficient  for  the  two  samples.  Since  the 
ovary  increases  at  a  more  rapid  rate  than  body  weight,  atten- 
tion is  again  directed  to  Orton's  theory  of"  over-reproduction". 
Whether  or  not  there  is  a  statistical  difference  between  these 
coefficients  would  require  a  separate  analysis. 

There  was  no  effect  of  age  on  fecundity /length  or  fecundity/ 
weight  relationships  according  to  a  covariance  analysis.  Thus 
differences  in  these  relations  could  not  be  attributed  to  age 
but  to  the  individual  variations  that  occur  in  fecundity  at 
any  given  length  or  weight.  This  result  makes  us  more  cautious 
in  accepting  the  rather  small  decline  in  fecundity  of  six-  and 
seven-year-old  haddock  which  Raitt  found.  He  presented  no 
measure  of  deviations  from  the  regressions,  and  since  the 
individual  measurements  were  not  given  the  computation 
cannot  be  performed. 

Even  though  Bagenal  was  satisfied  with  statistical  proof 
that  age  played  no  significant  role  in  determining  ovary  weight 
or  fecundity,  he  was  disturbed  about  two  features  in  his  data 
which  did  not  coincide  with  this  interpretation.  Milinsky's 
(1944)  very  large  dabs  from  the  Barents  Sea  did  not  produce 
the  egg  numbers  expected  of  their  size,  based  on  the  Scottish 
population.  The  difference  might  be  explained  by  geographical 
or  by  racial  variation,  which  is  often  very  great.  Also,  the 
number  of  eggs  did  not  increase  in  proportion  to  the  weight  of 
the  gonad.  The  regression  coefficients  between  these  two 
variables  are  0-6907  and  0-8117  in  his  two  samples  computed 
from  a  regression  of  the  logarithm  of  fecundity  on  the  loga- 
rithm of  ovary  weight.  The  latter  result  was,  as  Bagenal 
says,  "...  unexpected  since  the  larger  gonads  will  have  a 


202  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

proportionally  smaller  surface  area  and  so,  not  only  should 
carry  less  surface  moisture  when  they  are  weighed,  but  also 
less  ovarian  tissue  should  be  found  surrounding  the  eggs  in  the 
larger  gonads.  We  can  only  suppose  that  the  heavier  gonads 
produce  fewer  eggs  per  gram  than  do  the  lighter  ones,  so  the 
eggs  are  presumably  larger  and  heavier."  It  is  also  possible 
that  the  amount  of  connective  tissue  of  the  ovary  may  in- 
crease disproportionately  as  it  grows  larger. 

After  comparing  the  information  on  the  haddock  and  long 
rough  dab,  we  have  mixed  feelings.  Raitt's  data  were  con- 
sistent throughout  and  left  the  impression  that  there  was  a 
slight  but  definite  effect  of  age  on  fecundity  although  statis- 
tical tests  were  lacking.  Bagenal,  on  the  other  hand,  offers 
statistical  proof  to  the  contrary  but  cannot  explain  satis- 
factorily at  least  one  important  feature  of  his  data  from  a 
purely  biological  point  of  view. 

Plaice.  A  long  history  is  associated  with  studies  on  the 
fecundity  of  the  plaice.  Just  before  the  turn  of  the  century 
Reibisch  (1899)  completed  a  detailed  study  of  the  histology 
of  the  ovary  and  performed  many  egg  counts  on  plaice  from 
the  Baltic  Sea.  He  was  acquainted  with  the  fact  that  egg 
production  declines  with  age  in  higher  vertebrates  and  was 
puzzled  to  find  that  this  was  apparently  not  true  in  fishes. 
One  case  was  pointed  out  where  an  older  and  larger  individual 
produced  the  same  number  of  eggs  as  a  younger  one.  This 
observation  was  by  no  means  consistent,  and  Reibisch  was 
unable  to  reach  a  definite  conclusion  about  the  effect  of  age  on 
fecundity. 

Soon  after,  Franz  (1909)  duplicated  Reibisch's  work  and 
again  was  unable  to  answer  the  question.  Franz  admitted 
that  he  had  insufficient  material  from  older  age  groups  to 
judge  whether  older  plaice  had  a  greater  or  lesser  egg  number 
than  younger  ones.  Individual  variation  was  very  great  in  the 
specimens  above  age  X  in  his  sample.  The  lack  of  sufficient 
old  specimens  has  plagued  all  the  studies  to  date  and  con- 
stitutes the  chief  source  of  difficulty  in  settling  the  problem. 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish       203 

The  older  age  groups  are  represented  by  so  few  individuals  in 
the  population  that  it  is  virtually  impossible  to  collect 
enough  material  on  which  to  base  critical  judgments. 

A  thorough  review  of  plaice  fecundity  has  been  done 
recently  by  Simpson  (1951),  who  added  a  considerable  number 
of  egg  counts  from  the  Southern  Bight  and  Flamborough 
regions  of  the  North  Sea.  Ovaries  were  gathered  from  a  total 
of  256  females  taken  just  before  spawning,  from  mid-October 
to  mid-February,  in  1948  and  1949.  By  inspecting  graphs  of 
the  fecundity  of  plaice  of  similar  lengths  against  age  and 
graphs  of  fecundity  of  fish  of  the  same  age  against  length, 
Simpson  was  convinced  that  age,  apart  from  its  relation  to 
size,  plays  an  insignificant  part  in  determining  fecundity. 

On  the  chance  that  statistical  analysis  might  show  up  dif- 
ferences that  a  graphical  inspection  would  not,  a  covariance 
analysis  of  Simpson's  1948  sample  from  Southern  Bight 
(Table  IV)  was  performed.  Ages  ranging  from  II  to  XVI  are 
represented,  although  it  was  necessary  to  lump  together 
ages  II  and  III  and  ages  XII  through  XVI  in  order  to  have 
sufficient  numbers  of  observations  in  the  younger  and  older 
categories.  Regressions  of  body  weight  on  fecundity  at  each 
age  were  calculated  and  compared.  Simpson  had  found  that 
fecundity  bore  a  linear  relation  to  weight  in  plaice,  thereby 
simplifying  the  computations.  The  regression  coefficients 
ranged  from  0-054  to  0-273  and  showed  no  trends  with  age. 
No  significant  diff'erences  were  found  between  the  regression 
coefficients  or  between  the  adjusted  means.  Thus  we  conclude, 
as  Simpson  did,  that  age  has  no  detectable  eff'ect  on  the 
fecundity  of  the  plaice. 

Simpson  also  measured  ovary  weights  and  found  a  rather 
high  correlation  between  those  and  egg  number  (r  =  0-890). 
He  provided  a  complete  tabulation  of  his  data,  and  it  was 
possible  to  make  a  more  detailed  study  of  this  relationship. 
The  Southern  Bight  information  was  again  used,  consist- 
ing of  163  pairs  of  observations.  Egg  number  and  ovary 
weights  were  converted  to  logarithms  and  the  regression  was 


204 


Shelby  D.  Gerking 


calculated  to  solve  the  formula :  F  =  COJ^ ;  where  F  =  fecun- 
dity in  thousands  of  eggs;  0^^  =  ovary  weight  in  grams.  C  and 
n  are  empirically  determined  constants. 

The  advantage  of  this  computation  is  that  it  is  possible  to 
judge  whether  fecundity  is  increasing  in  a  linear  fashion  in 


Table  IV 

COVARIANCE    ANALYSIS     OF    REGRESSION     OF    BODY    WEIGHT    IN    GRAMS     AND 
FECUNDITY  IN  THOUSANDS  OF  EGGS  IN  PLAICE  (PleurOliecteS  platCSSO)  OF  VARIOUS 

AGES.   Calculated  from  data  of  Simpson  (1951) 


Source  of 

Degrees  of 

Regression 

Mean  square 

variation 

freedom 

coefficient 

from  regression 

Age  II-III 

9 

0-099 

68-06 

IV 

35 

0-183 

238-09 

V 

8 

0-273 

146-99 

VI 

5 

0  054 

102-19 

VII 

17 

0-117 

116-81 

VIII 

25 

0-113 

470-47 

IX 

17 

0-148 

13648-19 

X 

9 

0  138 

1017-94 

XI 

11 

0-098 

828-72 

XII-XVI 

11 

0-164 

453-74 

Within  age  groups 

147 

481-98 

Due  to  regression 

9 

899-11 

Common  to  all  age 

groups 

156 

0-140 

506-04 

Due  to  adjusted 

means 

9 

605-99 

Total 

165 

511-50 

F  for  regression  coeff 

icients:  899-11  = 
481-98 

1  •  87  (not  significant) 

F  for  adjusted  mean 

3:             605-99  = 
506-04 

1  •  20  (not  significant) 

relation  to  ovary  weight  or  whether  it  is  increasing  more 
rapidly  or  less  rapidly.  The  exponent,  n,  would  not  deviate 
significantly  from  1  in  the  first  case,  would  be  <  1  in  the 
second,  and  >  1  in  the  third.  The  solution  of  the  equation 
was:  F  =  7'14  OJ^'^'^^^  with  95  per  cent  confidence  limits  of 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish 


205 


the  exponent  lying  between  0-5553  and  0-6035.  Thus,  the 
number  of  eggs  does  not  increase  in  proportion  to  the  weight 
of  the  ovary  (Fig.  2). 

Plaice  is  the  fourth  species  to  show  this  peculiar  relationship. 
It  had  also  been  reported  by  Hickling  in  the  English  herring, 
by  Raitt  in  haddock  and  by  Bagenal  in  the  long  rough  dab. 


SOOr 


240 


180 


120 


60 


•  ^  ••  • 

•  •• 


PLAICE 


60  120  180 

OVARY    WEIGHT    IN    GRAMS 


300 


Fig.  2.  Scatter  diagram  of  the  relation  between  fecundity 

and  ovary  weight  in  the  plaice  (Pleuronectes  platessa). 

From  Simpson  (1951). 


The  explanation  for  it  is  still  in  doubt,  but  a  clear-cut  problem 
has  emerged.  A  choice  can  be  made  between  Bagenal's  inter- 
pretation that  larger  ovaries  may  produce  larger  but  fewer 
eggs,  and  Hickling' s  tentative  conclusion  that  connective 
tissue  may  increase  disproportionately  in  the  ovaries  of  older 
fish.  The  decision  can  be  made  by  studying  the  histology  of 
the  ovary,  provided  a  method  can  be  devised  for  measuring 


206  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

the  relative  amounts  of  connective  tissue.  This  would  have  to 
be  done  in  maturing  ovaries  where  maximum  egg  diameters 
could  be  measured  at  the  same  time. 

There  seems  to  be  no  reason  to  continue  studying  fecundity 
of  egg-laying  fishes  with  respect  to  age  for  gathering  evidence 
for  or  against  the  ageing  process.  Individual  variation  masks 
any  effect  that  age  may  have.  Great  variation  plus  the 
difficulty  in  collecting  a  sufficiently  large  number  of  old 
individuals  makes  it  very  improbable  that  this  line  of  research 
will  ever  become  profitable. 

Summary 

Nutrition  and  reproduction  of  fishes  are  reviewed  in  relation 
to  age.  A  decline  in  the  ability  to  utilize  protein  for  growth 
is  exhibited  as  fish  grow  larger  and  older.  This  conclusion 
is  based  on  laboratory  feeding  experiments  on  the  longear 
sunfish  (Lepomis  megalotis),  green  sunfish  {Lepomis  cyanellus), 
bluegill  sunfish  {Lepomis  macrochirus),  and  red  hind  [Epine- 
phalus  guttatus).  Other  vertebrates  also  conform  to  this 
pattern,  although  they  achieve  a  specific  size  relatively  early 
in  life  in  contrast  to  the  prolonged  period  of  growth  in 
fishes. 

The  reproductive  capacity  of  live-bearing  fishes  of  the  family 
Poeciliidae  declines  with  age  and  there  may  be  a  period  of 
sterility  before  death.  These  results  were  obtained  by  field 
observations  and  laboratory  experiments  on  the  fecundity  of 
the  western  mosquitofish,  Gambusia  affinis.  Scattered  ob- 
servations among  other  species  in  the  family  agree  with  this 
viewpoint. 

The  effect  of  age  on  fecundity  in  egg-laying  fishes  is  not 
yet  clear.  The  number  of  eggs  in  three-year  sea-life  sockeye 
salmon  (Oncorhynchus  nerka)  is  significantly  lower  than  in 
two-year  individuals.  The  same  general  phenomenon  has  been 
said  to  be  true  of  haddock  {Melanogr animus  aeglefinis),  but 
there  is  some  doubt  about  this  conclusion  since  statistical 


Physiological  Changes  with  Age  in  Fish       207 

procedures  fail  to  show  any  effect  of  age  on  the  fecundity  of 
either  the  long  rough  dab  (Hippoglossoides  platessoides)  or 
plaice  {Pleuronectes  platessa).  Individual  variation  in  fecundity 
is  very  great  and  masks  any  effect  that  age  may  have. 

Ovary  weight  and  fecundity  increase  more  rapidly  in  rela- 
tion to  length  than  does  body  weight  in  the  long  rough  dab, 
haddock,  and  herring  (Clupea  harengus  and  Clupea  pallasii). 
This  result  strengthens  Orton's  hypothesis  that  reproduction 
becomes  an  increasing  strain  on  the  metabolism  of  fish  as  they 
grow  larger  and  older,  thereby  causing  death  either  directly  or 
indirectly. 

The  number  of  eggs  does  not  increase  in  proportion  to  the 
weight  of  the  ovary  in  the  haddock,  dab,  herring,  or  plaice. 
Either  larger  ovaries  produce  larger  and  fewer  eggs  or  con- 
nective tissue  increases  disproportionately  in  the  ovaries  of 
larger  fish.  No  critical  evidence  is  available  to  support  either 
contention.  If  the  latter  is  true,  ageing  changes  in  the  gonads 
of  fishes  would  be  similar  to  those  in  higher  vertebrates. 


REFERENCES 

Bagenal,  T.  B.  (1957a).  J.  Mar.  hiol.  Ass.  U.K.,  36,  339. 

Bagenal,  T.  B.  (19576).  J.  Mar.  hiol.  Ass.  U.K.,  36,  377. 

Belding,  D.  L.  (1940).   Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  67,  285. 

Bidder,  G.  P.  (1925a).   Nature  (Lond.),  115,  155. 

Bidder,  G.  P.  (19256).   Nature  (Lond.),  115,  495. 

Bidder,  G.  P.  (1932).   Brit.  med.  J.,  2,  583. 

Davis,  W.  S.  (1957).   Res.  Rep.  U.S.  Fish  Serv.,  49,  1. 

Eschmeyer,  p.  H.  (1950).   Bull.  Inst.  Fish.  Res.  Univ.  Mich.y  3,  7. 

EscHMEYER,  P.  H.  (1955).   Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  84,  47. 

Farran,  G.  p.  (1938).  J.  Cons.  int.  Explor.  Mer,  13,  91. 

Franz,  V.  (1909).    Wiss.  Meeresuntersuch.  (AM.  Helgoland),  9,  59. 

Eraser,  E.  A.,  and  Kenton,  R.  M.  (1940).   Quart.  J.  micr.Sci.,  81,  479 

Fry,  F.  E.  J.  (1949).   Biometrics,  5,  27. 

Gerking,  S.  D.  (1952).  Physiol.  Zool.,  25,  358. 

Gerking,  S.  D.  (1954).  Ecology,  35,  490. 

Gerking,  S.  D.  (1955fl).  Physiol.  Zool,  28,  267. 

Gerking,  S.  D.  (19556).  Phijsiol.  Zool.,  28,  283. 

HiCKLiNG,  C.  F.  (1940).  J.  Mar.  hiol.  Ass.  U.K.,  24,  619. 

IvLEV,  V.  S.  (1939a).   C.R.  Acad.  Sci.  U.R.S.S.y  25,  87. 


208  Shelby  D.  Gerking 

IvLEV,  V.  S.  (19396).   Bull.  Soc.  Nat.  Moscou,  48,  70. 

IvLEV,  V.  S.  (1939c).   C.  R.  Acad.  Sci.  U.R.S.S.,  25,  90. 

June,  F.  C.  (1953).  Fish.  Bull.,  U.S.,  77,  47. 

Karzinkin,  G.  S.  (1939).   Arb.  limnol.  Sta.  Kossino,  22,  219. 

Katz,  M.,  and  Erickson,  D.  W.  (1950).   Copeia,  p.  176. 

Krumiiolz,  L.  (1948).   Ecol.  Monogr.,  18,  1. 

Larkin,  p.  a.,  Terpenning,  J.  G.,  and  Parker,  R.  R.  (1957).    Trans. 

Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  86,  84. 
McGregor,  E.  A.  (1923).   Calif.  Fish  Game,  8,  138. 
Maar,  a.  (1949).   Rep.  Inst.  Freshw.  Res.  Drottning.,  29,  57. 
Maynard,  L.  a.  (1951).  Animal  Nutrition,  3rd  ed.  New  York :  McGraw- 

HiU. 
Menzel,  D.  W.  (1957).   Ph.D.  Thesis,  University  of  Michigan. 
MiLiNSKY,  G.  I.  (1944).   Trans.  Knipovich  polyar.  sci.  Inst.,  8,  388. 
MoRGULis,  S.  (1919).   Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  48,  34. 
Orton,  J.  H.  (1929).   Nature  (Lond.),  123,  3088. 
Raitt,  D.  S.  (1933).   Rep.  Fish.  Bd.  Scot.,  p.  1. 
Rasquin,  p.,  and  Hafter,  E.  (1951).   J.  Morph.,  89,  397. 
Reibisch,  J.  (1899).    Wiss.  Meeresuntersuch.  {Abt.  Kiel),  4,  233. 
Rounsefell,  G.  a.  (1957).  Fish.  Bull.,  U.S.,  122,  451. 
Russell,  E.  S.  (1914).  Fish.  Invest.,  Lond.,  Ser.  2,  1. 
Simpson,  A.  C.  (1951).  Fish.  Invest.,  Lond.,  Ser.  2,  17,  3. 
SvARDSON,  G.  (1949).   Rep.  Inst.  Freshw.  Res.  Drottning.,  29,  115. 
SvARDSON,  G.  (1951).  Rep.  Inst.  Freshw.  Res.  Drottning.,  32,  79. 
Wallace,  W.  (1925).   Nature  (Lond.),  115,  337. 
Wynne-Edwards,  V.  C.  (1929).  J.  Mar.  biol.  Ass.  U.K.,  16,  49. 


DISCUSSION 

Comfort:  We  have  done  some  work  on  regeneration  in  guppies 
(Comfort,  A.,  and  Doljanski,  F.  (1958).  Gerontologia  (Basel),  2,  266) 
which  might  have  some  relevance  to  what  you  said  about  protein 
utilization,  Dr.  Gerking.  We  cut  off  the  tips  of  the  tails  and  measured 
the  percentage  restoration  in  length  at  various  times.  In  female 
guppies  up  to  three  years  of  age  the  growth  curves  were  typically 
asymptotic,  and  the  corresponding  curve  for  percentage  restoration 
of  an  excised  regenerate  was  roughly  a  mirror  image  of  them.  In 
fish  that  had  been  kept  without  much  to  eat,  the  growth  curve 
flattened  out  and  the  regeneration  rate  fell  exactly  as  in  freely 
growing  fish  approaching  full  size.  Full-size  and  retarded  brood- 
mates  were  therefore  behaving  in  almost  the  same  way  as  regards 
regeneration.  If  retarded  fish  are  then  allowed  to  grow,  the  rate  of 
regeneration  rises  until  growth  declines  again.  In  other  words,  as 
the  asymptote  for  size  under  given  conditions  is  approached,  the 


Discussion  209 

regeneration  rate  falls  with  the  growth  rate.  Growth  eventually 
declines  to  zero,  and  the  regeneration  rate  to  its  basal  level,  which 
persists  even  in  starving  fish. 

Danielli:  Are  these  measurements  in  terms  of  percentage  re- 
generation of  what  was  removed  ? 

Comfort:  Yes.  They  are  not  absolute  measurements.  We  had  to 
adjust  the  size  of  the  amputate  to  the  size  of  the  fish.  The  general 
finding  seems  to  be  that  as  somatic  growth  flattens  out,  the  regenera- 
tion rate  comes  down  to  its  basic  level;  as  growth  is  restarted,  so 
regeneration  is  restarted.  I  would  predict  that  this  occurs  also  with 
nitrogen  utilization,  as  in  Dr.  Gerking's  experiments. 

As  G.  V.  Samokhvalova  has  shown  (1952.  J.  gen.  Biol.,  Moscow, 
12,  153),  in  guppies  during  the  first  part  of  life  the  number  of  young 
per  brood  is  a  function  of  the  size  of  the  female.  I  have  kept  them 
up  to  four  and  a  half  years  of  age.  Brood  size  declines  fairly  rapidly 
even  though  body  growth  continues,  and  there  is  quite  a  long  post- 
reproductive  period  during  which  females  may  produce  one  or  two 
broods  if  remated,  but  generally  they  do  not. 

Gerking:  It  seems  that  my  paper  would  have  been  much  more  com- 
plete if  your  experiments  had  been  published  a  little  earlier. 

Holt:  Some  light  could  probably  be  thrown  on  the  regeneration 
question  if  the  growth  could  be  modified  not  just  by  changing  the 
food  supply,  but  also  by  changing  the  temperature.  You  could 
then  see  whether  the  regeneration  curves  behaved  in  the  same  way. 
In  our  terminology  your  food  supply  is  changing  the  Loo  (the 
asymptotic  size),  whereas  the  temperature  would  change  K  (the 
rate  of  approach  to  the  asymptote). 

Comfort:  I  am  now  engaged  in  temperature  experiments.  If  two 
batches  of  fish  are  allowed  to  regenerate  at  different  temperatures 
the  final  percentage  restoration  is  identical,  but  it  is  reached  at  a 
different  rate.  It  is  striking  that,  as  far  as  I  have  got,  quite  large 
shifts  in  temperature  do  not  alter  the  final  percentage  of  restoration; 
they  only  alter  the  rate  at  which  it  is  approached,  as  you  suggested, 
Mr.  Holt. 

Rotblat:  One  of  your  graphs  [not  printed]  appears  to  be  the 
differential  of  the  other. 

Comfort:  In  general  but  not  invariably.  If  very  old  retarded  fish 
are  kept  very  long,  as  is  often  the  case,  some  of  them  will  not  restart 
growth.  They  grow  only  very  sluggishly  or  very  little.  Nevertheless 
the  regeneration  rate  still  rises. 

Rotblat:  Even  when  there  is  a  small  change  in  weight,  the  initial 
slope  appears  to  increase. 

Comfort:  Some  of  them  do  not  increase  at  all  or  show  barely 


210  Discussion 

measurable  growth.   But  even  in  these  the  regeneration  rate  rises, 
and  I  would  suggest  that  the  nitrogen  uptake  rises  too. 

Roiblat:  Does  this  mean  that  the  rate  of  restoration  equals  the 
rate  of  growth? 

Comfort:  The  response  of  regeneration  to  a  growth-promoting 
stimulus  is  more  sensitive  than  that  of  body  growth.  That  is  why  I 
would  not  like  to  say  that  regeneration  rate  is  a  direct  function  of 
growth  rate. 

Gerking:  Do  your  findings  apply  to  the  male  ? 

Comfort:  No;  the  male  guppy  not  only  stops  growing  rather 
suddenly  and  early,  but  also  it  has  tail  shapes  of  different  kinds. 
As  far  as  we  have  got,  in  males  with  small,  wild-type  tails  the  per- 
centage regeneration  falls  in  relation  to  growth  cessation,  as  in 
females,  but  the  basic  restoration  rate  always  stays  higher  than  in 
the  female.  You  sometimes  find  male  guppies  of  all  ages  which  for 
some  reason  have  not  quite  completed  their  growth  and  which  have 
a  very  high  restoration  rate.  In  strains  with  big  tails,  it  appears 
that  the  rate  of  restoration  is  based,  as  it  were,  on  the  wild-type  tail, 
but  that  anything  after  that  is  extra. 

Holt:  Dr.  Gerking,  you  have  looked  at  particularly  good  sets  of 
data  and  put  aside  the  incomplete  oddments.  I  have  had  a  look  at 
the  oddments  and  have  the  impression  that  although  no  single 
species  shows  a  statistically  significant  decline  in  fecundity  (eggs 
per  gram)  with  increase  in  size,  yet  in  many  cases  the  points  for  the 
larger  fish  fall  below  the  proportional  line. 

Gerking:  That  is  very  true.  I  am  not  convinced  whether  the 
decrease  is  significant  or  not. 

Holt:  In  this  kind  of  study  you  may  be  seeing  the  effects  of  less 
fecund  fish  surviving  longer.  What  we  cannot  do  is  to  follow  a  cohort. 
We  need  a  method  of  determining  the  fecundity  of  live  individuals 
and  marking  them. 

Rockstein:  Was  this  large  longear  sunfish  that  utilized  very  little 
protein  caught  at  a  great  depth,  Dr.  Gerking  ? 

Gerking:  No,  it  was  caught  in  a  shallow  stream.  This  observation 
needs  to  be  confirmed,  and  I  do  not  wish  to  over-emphasize  that 
portion  of  Fig.  1.  It  may  indicate  that  extremely  old  fish  utilize  very 
little  protein  for  growth. 

Nigrelli:  In  the  experiments  carried  out  in  Bermuda  on  the  angel- 
fish  you  characterize  these  fish  as  herbivores. 

Gerking:  We  have  thought  up  to  this  time  that  they  were  herbi- 
vores; it  is  known  that  they  feed  upon  algae  because  algae  can  be 
found  in  the  gut.  Menzel's  experiments  (1957),  however,  indicate 
that  if  they  are  fed  exclusively  on  algae  they  will  not  grow,  even  if 


Discussion  211 

they  eat  fairly  large  quantities.  I  would  conclude  that  these  angel- 
fish  are  not  strict  herbivores  but  that  they  must  have  some  animal 
food. 

Nigrelli:  That  is  true,  because  we  keep  small  angelfish  in  our 
aquarium  and  feed  them  with  clams,  etc.,  and  no  algae  at  all,  and 
they  grow  to  a  good  size.  However,  the  algae  may  prevent  growth 
by  producing  antibiotics. 

Gerking:  We  wonder  now  whether  there  is  a  strictly  herbivorous 
fish. 


V 


LONGEVITY  OF  FISHES  IN  CAPTIVITY, 

WITH  SPECIAL  REFERENCE  TO  THOSE 

KEPT  IN  THE  NEW  YORK  AQUARIUM 

Ross    F.    NiGRELLI 
New  York  Aquarium 

The  New  York  Aquarium,  at  one  time  or  another  during  its 
history,  has  exhibited  for  varying  periods  of  time  fishes 
representing  33  of  the  57  known  orders.  This  figure  is  more 
spectacular  when  it  is  reaUzed  that  10  of  the  57  orders  contain 
species  restricted  to  fife  in  the  benthic  areas  of  the  seas,  which 
have  never  been  brought  to  the  surface  ahve  long  enough  to 
exhibit.  The  orders,  as  listed  by  Berg  (1947),  contain  425 
families,  at  least  50  of  them  deep-sea  forms.  The  New  York 
Aquarium  has  exhibited  species  representative  of  152  families. 
Other  aquaria  have  probably  kept  fishes  of  another  five  or  six 
orders,  involving  an  additional  25  families.  Therefore,  it  is 
apparently  possible  to  maintain  in  captivity  fishes  represent- 
ing 38  of  the  57  orders  and  from  175-250  of  the  425  families. 
However,  only  relatively  few  of  the  estimated  25,000  species 
have  ever  been  captured  and  kept  alive  in  aquaria;  the  exact 
number  has  never  been  determined.  Jordan,  Evermann  and 
Clark  (1930)  list  4,137  species  in  the  North  and  Middle  Ameri- 
can waters,  and  probably  only  a  little  more  than  one-fourth 
of  these  have  ever  been  exhibited.  Breder  (19366)  summarizes 
some  of  the  environmental  and  physiological  barriers  that 
may  be  limiting  factors  for  the  successful  maintenance  of 
many  species.  He  concedes,  however,  that  these  barriers 
may  be  overcome  and  that  some  day  it  may  be  possible  to 
exhibit  species  not  heretofore  shown,  including  the  exotic  life 
in  the  great  depths  of  the  oceans. 

212 


Longevity  of  Fishes  in  Captivity  213 

It  is  generally  known  that  fishes  represent  a  physiologically 
highly  diversified  group  of  vertebrates,  and  attempts  to 
maintain  them  in  so-called  standard  aquarium  conditions 
regardless  of  environmental  origin  are  fraught  with  danger  and 
will  invariably  be  reflected  in  a  short  lifespan  in  captivity.  As 
environmental  origin  is  recognized  as  important  in  aquarium 
management,  it  is  the  practice  in  the  New  York  Aquarium, 
in  so  far  as  it  is  possible,  to  diversify  physical  and  chemical 
conditions  such  as  temperature,  pH,  surface-volume  ratios, 
conditioning  factors,  w^ater  movements,  salinity  and  illumina- 
tion. As  a  result  of  such  management  we  have  been  able  to 
increase  our  survival  rates  significantly.  Thus,  in  1940,  our 
last  full  year  of  operation  at  the  New  York  Aquarium  at  the 
Battery,  average  longevities  were  as  follows:  marine  fishes 
9*58  months,  temperate  freshwater  species  24*50  months, 
and  tropical  freshwater  fishes  11-22  months.  The  mortality 
rate  for  the  year  was  169  per  1,000. 

Infectious  diseases  are  the  primary  causes  of  death  in 
aquaria  (Nigrelli,  1940,  1943).  It  is  generally  known  that 
fishes  are  susceptible  to  a  large  variety  of  metazoan  parasites, 
but  what  is  not  common  knowledge  is  that  they  are  also 
prone  to  infections  by  pathogenic  micro-organisms  that  are 
similar  in  many  respects  to  those  responsible  for  diseases  in 
man  and  other  mammals.  For  example,  to  mention  a  few, 
fishes  are  susceptible  to  infections  by  viruses,  Rickettsia, 
PseudomonaSf  Proteus,  diphtheroids,  tubercle  bacilli  (Myco- 
bacterium), Monilia  and  other  mycotic  organisms.  They  are 
also  susceptible  to  such  protozoans  as  trypanosomes,  haemo- 
gregarines,  coccidians,  babesioids,  and  Toxoplasma,  in  addition 
to  such  ubiquitous  parasites  as  flukes,  tapeworms,  nematodes 
and  acanthocephalans.  The  most  important  aetiological 
agents  of  fish  diseases,  however,  belong  to  a  subclass  of 
Sporozoa  called  cnidosporidians.  These  are  truly  spore- 
producing  parasites  in  which  transmission  is  direct,  i.e.  by 
ingestion  of  the  spore.  During  the  course  of  routine  autopsies 
at  the  New  York  Aquarium  in  the  last  25  years,  the  present 


214  Ross    F.    NiGRELLI 

author  has  observed  more  than  150  species  of  enidosporidians 
(myxo-  and  microsporidians)  in  more  than  1,000  species  of 
fishes  (see  Walford,  1958).  These  parasites  are  often  tissue- 
and  cell-specific  and  have  been  found  in  all  the  tissues  and 
organs,  including  the  eyes,  brain  and  heart.  The  parasites 
produce  a  variety  of  lesions,  the  extent  of  which  varies  with 
the  species  of  parasite  and  degree  of  infection.  They  may 
cause  no  more  damage  than  the  development  of  a  simple 
cyst,  or  they  may  cause  acute  and  chronic  diseases.  For 
example,  some  may  produce  cellulitis,  cystitis,  nephritis, 
hepatitis,  enteritis,  pericarditis  and  endocarditis;  others  may 
induce  tumours  of  the  infected  organs  and/or  the  surrounding 
tissues,  many  of  them  bordering  on  true  neoplasia;  still  others 
may  cause  hyaline  degeneration  of  muscle  and  other  tissues. 

It  is  safe  to  say  that  all  fish  harbour  one  or  more  kinds  of 
parasites.  The  resistance  of  fish,  or  the  rate  at  which  they  can 
acclimatize  to  changes  in  the  environment,  appears  to  be 
related  to  their  parasitic  load.  Experience  has  shown  that  as 
a  rule  a  10  per  cent  mortality  can  be  expected  when  fish  are 
first  netted  or  trapped  randomly,  another  10  per  cent  as  the 
result  of  handling  and  shipment,  and  10  per  cent  more  will 
succumb  in  the  first  few  weeks  of  life  in  the  tanks.  Such  fish 
tend  to  show  a  relatively  high  degree  of  parasitaemia,  and 
their  ability  to  withstand  shock  is  related  to  the  intensity  and 
the  site  of  infection  or  infestation,  the  rates  being  highest  for 
those  fish  in  which  infections  are  localized  in  the  kidneys, 
gills  and  skin,  which  are  important  organs  of  osmoregulation. 
Selective  methods  of  trapping,  handling  and  shipping  usually 
result  in  higher  survival  values.  For  example,  fish  caught  in 
traps  rather  than  in  nets  are  less  subject  to  trauma  and  can 
be  transferred  to  holding-pens  with  very  little  injury.  Survival 
rates  are  further  increased  if  the  fishes  are  starved  for  a  period 
of  time  and  before  shipping  are  transferred  to  waters  with 
slightly  altered  densities.  Relatively  young  fish,  as  related  to 
potential  age,  are  better  risks  than  yearling  or  older  (larger) 
fish. 


Longevity  of  Fishes  in  Captivity  215 

Conditions  in  aquaria  at  best  are  still  artificial  since  move- 
ments of  fishes  are  restricted,  and  for  this  reason  there  cannot 
be  any  escape  from  environmental  stress.  It  is  apparent,  then, 
that  fishes  that  survive  aquarium  conditions  are  those  that 
can  withstand  shock  stresses  and  can  acclimatize  to  a  variety 
of  exaggerated  environmental  factors.  But  even  such  fish  as 
these  are  often  at  the  limits  of  their  tolerance,  and  any  sudden 
change  in  one  or  more  of  the  physical,  chemical  and  bio- 
logical factors  often  results  in  death  or  increased  suscepti- 
bility to  infections.  Invariably  these  infective  agents  are  ex- 
ternal (gill  and  skin)  protozoan  and  helminthic  parasites;  only 
rarely  are  they  bacterial  or  mycotic  organisms.  This  would 
indicate  that  once  the  fish  is  acclimatized  to  its  new  environ- 
ment (captivity),  its  resistance  is  increased  and  the  parasite  load 
diminished  to  a  point  where  immunity  is  maintained  by  pre- 
munition.  Diseases  caused  by  internal  parasites  are  often  self- 
limiting  and  in  some  instances  may  spontaneously  disappear. 

Once  a  balance  has  been  established  between  fish,  parasites 
and  environment,  other  diseases  of  a  non-infectious  nature 
may  develop.  It  may  not  be  surprising  that  the  greatest 
single  cause  of  mortality  is  associated  with  nutrition.  Fishes, 
like  other  animals,  are  herbivorous,  carnivorous  and  omnivor- 
ous and  all  need  an  exogenous  source  of  vitamins  and  other 
nutriments.  The  main  food  source  in  the  New  York  Aquarium 
consists  chiefly  of  commercial-grade  fresh  and  frozen  fish, 
clams  and  Crustacea.  The  kind  of  fish  used  for  feeding 
depends  entirely  on  their  availability  on  the  market  and  in 
collecting  areas.  Feeding  oily  fish  (mackerel,  herring,  etc.)  over 
relatively  long  periods  to  species  that  normally  eat  invert- 
ebrates and  non-oily  fish  frequently  results  in  liver  damage, 
commonly  referred  to  as  fatty  degeneration  but  properly 
called  fatty  "metamorphosis"  or  "fatty  change".  The 
pancreas,  kidneys  and  other  organs  may  also  be  involved  in 
this  type  of  damage.  Fatty  changes  may  be  due  to  a  relative 
anoxia,  the  result  of  prolonged  passive  congestion.  Other 
diseases  of  fishes  indicative  of  disturbances  in  carbohydrate 


216  Ross    F.    NiGRELLI 

and  protein  metabolism  are  glycogen  storage  comparable  to 
von  Girke's  disease  in  man,  cloudy  swelling  (albuminous 
degeneration),  hydropic  degeneration  and  amyloidosis  of  the 
liver  and  kidney,  to  mention  a  few.  The  development  of 
melanosis,  a  common  disturbance  in  fishes  generally,  certainly 
indicates  profound  changes  in  metabolism  involving  the 
amino  acids  phenylalanine  and  tyrosine,  and  the  excessive 
accumulation  of  guanine  crystals  in  the  tissues  of  certain 
marine  species  in  captivity  is  indicative  of  disturbance  in 
purine  metabolism. 

Further,  it  should  also  be  emphasized  that  fishes  are  sus- 
ceptible to  neoplasia  (Nigrelli,  1952,  1954a).  With  the  possible 
exception  of  typical  leukaemias,  all  types  of  tumours  and 
cancers,  benign  and  malignant,  occur  in  fish.  The  same 
basic  tissues  as  in  mammals  are  involved,  with  tumours  of 
mesenchymal  origin  predominating.  Although  we  do  not  have 
definite  age  statistics,  there  is  ample  evidence  that  such 
abnormal  growths  occur  more  frequently  in  older  fish.  This  is 
especially  true  for  sarcomas  and  lymphomas,  basic  tumour 
types  that  appear  more  frequently  in  young  persons.  This 
finding,  together  with  the  fact  that  tumours  in  fish  are  usually 
slow-growing,  may  have  some  physiological  and  phylogenetic 
significance. 

It  serves  no  purpose  here  to  extend  the  list  of  metabolic 
diseases  that  we  have  found  in  fishes  in  captivity.  The 
vertebrate  fish  is  no  different  from  vertebrate  man  in  these 
respects.  It  is  sufficient  to  say  that  we  have  ample  evidence 
that  some  of  the  metabolic  diseases  may  be  hereditary  or 
congenital  in  origin,  or  that,  in  older  fishes  at  least,  they  may 
result  from  nutritional  disturbances  and  hormonal  imbalances. 
The  pathological  changes  are  generally  similar  to  those  found 
in  warm-blooded  vertebrates;  in  extreme  cases  they  include 
hyperaemia,  anaemia,  haemorrhage,  inflammation,  sclerosis, 
atrophy,  hypertrophy,  hyperplasia,  oedema,  ascites  and 
necrosis.  Basically,  such  conditions  result  in  disturbances  in 
the  electrolyte  balance,  thus  affecting  homeostasis. 


Longevity  of  Fishes  in  Captivity  217 

The  literature  concerning  growth  and  senescence  in  fishes 
has  been  reviewed  by  Comfort  (1956)  and  Brown  (1957)  and 
it  is  generally  agreed  that  many  of  the  larger  teleosts,  and 
perhaps  certain  sharks,  continue  to  grow  throughout  life. 
Most  fishes,  however,  reach  their  maximum  growth,  often 
with  sexual  maturity,  within  a  limited  time.  Nevertheless, 
the  growth  pattern  and  lifespan  of  only  a  relatively  few  but 
well-known  species  have  been  established  by  fishery  biologists. 
The  lifespan  of  the  more  exotic  forms  is  based  mainly  on 
longevity  records  kept  by  various  aquaria.  These,  and  others, 
have  been  summarized  by  Flower  (1925,  1935),  Bourliere 
(1946),  and  more  recently  by  Hinton  (1959,  personal  com- 
munication). In  1956  Hinton  canvassed  twenty  institutions 
in  Europe  and  the  United  States  for  information  concerning 
longevity  of  fishes  in  attempts  to  bring  these  records  up-to- 
date.  The  New  York  Aquarium's  longevity  lists  for  fishes 
4  years  or  over  are  included  in  this  report  as  Tables  II,  III  and 
IV.  The  data  were  obtained  from  published  papers  by 
Townsend  (1904,  1913,  1928a,  b),  Mellen  (1919,  1925),  Breder 
(1936a),  Nigrelli  (1940,  19546)  and  from  our  own  mortality 
records  not  previously  published.  In  the  most  recent  listing 
by  Hinton,  325  species  had  lifespans  of  5  years  or  more  and 
these  belong  to  88  families.  A  further  analysis  of  his  data 
shows  the  following:  64  families  contained  93  species  with 
lifespans  from  5  to  9  years,  11  months;  43  families  contained 
108  species  that  lived  from  10  to  19  years,  11  months;  and  18 
families  contained  28  species  that  lived  more  than  20  years. 
The  latter  group  is  listed  in  Table  I.  Families  with  the  largest 
number  of  species  that  lived  for  5  years  or  more  are  Charac- 
idae,  37  species;  Cyprinidae,  39  species;  Serranidae,  28 
species;  Sparidae,  12  species  and  Cichlidae,  17  species.  The 
two  orders  represented  by  these  families  are  Cypriniformes 
and  Perciformes.  Forty-four  of  the  88  families  are  represented 
by  single  species  and  the  other  families  by  two  to  nine  species. 

A  casual  examination  of  all  published  lists  of  long-lived 
fishes  in  captivity  shows,  with  few^  exceptions,  the  following 


218  Ross   F.   NiGRELLI 

general  characteristics :  (1)  they  are  phylogenetically  primitive, 
(2)  they  are  sluggish  in  their  movements,  (3)  they  are  bottom 
inhabitants  or  live  in  fairly  shallow  waters,  (4)  they  have 
accessory  respiratory  devices,  (5)  they  can  aestivate  or 
hibernate,  (6)  they  live  in  regions  of  extreme  sunlight,  (7)  they 
are  adapted  to  live  in  environments  with  low  oxygen  concen- 
tration (less  than  5  p.p.m.),  and  (8)  they  are  adapted  to 
environments  with  extreme  fluctuations  in  temperatures  and 
salinities.  The  significance  of  some  of  these  factors  may  not 
be  apparent.  For  example,  there  seems  to  be  some  correlation 
between  longer  daylight  and  decreased  respiratory  rates, 
especially  in  fishes  kept  above  15°.  Also,  fishes  that  live 
on  some  coral  reefs  and  banks  no  doubt  are  acclimatized  to 
great  changes  in  temperature  and  salinities.  Certain  "reef" 
fishes  can  even  survive  in  fresh  water,  provided  the  calcium 
content  is  high.  Thus,  Breder  (1934)  reports  12  typically 
marine  species  that  he  found  living  in  a  freshwater  lake  (Lake 
Forsyth)  on  Andros  Island,  British  West  Indies.  Further, 
experience  has  shown  that  many  marine  tropical  species 
survive  longer  in  captivity  if  the  sea  water  is  reduced  in 
salinity  to  around  30  parts  per  thousand. 

Although  there  is  much  evidence  that  fishes  (teleosts) 
undergo  reproductive  and  actuarial  senescence,  there  is  very 
little  information  in  the  literature  relative  to  pathological 
changes  associated  with  ageing.  Rasquin  and  Hafter  (1951) 
and  Hafter  (1952)  reported  age  changes  in  the  testes  and 
thymus  of  the  teleost,  Astyanax  mexicanus.  From  the 
clinical  records  of  the  New  York  Aquarium,  in  addition  to 
gonad  atrophy  we  have  found  the  following  pathological 
manifestations  to  be  most  frequently  associated  with  ageing: 
cirrhosis  and  fatty  changes  in  the  liver  and  kidneys,  haemo- 
chromatosis,  hypochromic  anaemia,  degeneration  of  the 
mucus-producing  glands  of  the  skin,  hyperostosis  of  the 
haemal  arch  bones  (see  Breder,  1952)  and  other  vertebral 
abnormalities.  The  haemochromatosis  and  anaemia  are  the 
direct  result  of  changes  in  the  kidney  and  spleen,  important 


2  3 

3   3 
o"  cr 

o  c 

tJD  o 

2  o 

Oh-; 


3H 


•?  ^  T3  .;i 


o  >  a 


§  s  s 

■c  .2 .2 
E3  fc-  ti 

3   cS  as 

„  era  3 

c«<    CO" 
.2  g  ^   ^ 


.2  g^ 

3  *u  C5 
O"  C3  rH 


C  j^ 


o  o 

C3  fe 


£3        333  .323        3g 

3  J3  e  'C  ^  t-  c  Ja  'S  .2  "C  c  'C  3 

rt  2..2  ss.='.2r'^=''^=333rt 
§  o"  E  cr  g"  g-'C  U  cr  3  cr-c  cr  3 
cr<  ^  <  <  <  ^    r^  5'<  <s  <  cr 

<s|ss2||g^e|e^ 


'H  o  „ 

!»     O     02 

£22 


TJ  'd  '^ 


'2  oT' 


£  2  t5  S  -« 


v 


O'S   3 

03 

o  g-b 


CO  00 


t*  CO  «5  rH  00  Tft 


CO 


(N  ifl 


o 
o 

o 

H 
H 

o 

X 


?     >-'^'-'(Me0'^C00iOO©M00i-(rHQ0r-(O«0b-OOOO'^CiC5OC0 
k^     WCl<NC^C0(N«OC0C0(N<MC^<NC0C0(N^(M(NC0W<N(M(NC0(N5i(N 


K    C  ^ 


•«*    5J 


« 


S    OS    sc 
O    «    S 


=0 


S  «o  &5 

<ii  S  g 

?j  f*  ^ 

*»  ^  ^ 

s  2  a 

'^^  S^  =^ 

~  •*  ^  ^ 

V  ?*  cc    ^    ^ 


-§ 


00   =0        "^ 


«  2 


o  (=^  ^  ;^  (§  ^  ^* 


50 


S   CO 


Si,   50 


-     ^2 

K^  K^  R^  Co  ^  ^  *«^ 


e  *5 


09 


tt* 


^ 


j»   eo   K   ^ 


0)  (DO) 

■^  =«  ^  -c  :2  -c 


'§:S:2 


i;    tH    OJ    iN    O   O 


03 


cc 


M  s 

H   C 


a 

•3^ 

'5  a 


^'S  -2  3 

2  2^1 


^:2 

(h  a 
0.3 


-<  _:  hH   HH   O  O 


o  2 

fa   Q 


Sp 


0) 

C8 

'2 

'2 

03 

3 


CO 

H 

o 
fa 


a; 

03 

'2 
'2 

1/2 


o 
fa 

M 
O 

PS 
04 


,0 


o 

•a 

a 
a 

a 
*n 

3 


o 
o 


W  ^ 

K  (u 

O  ^ 

t3  • 


1^ 


00 


«0  94 


(N 


M 

PS 
O 


^ 

o 

o 

05 

'9. 

4J 

<U                    u 

u 

£? 

43cC              5 

§ 

^5 

s£ 

."^  C« 

C/2  S   ^         ti 

8 
o 

p 

irse  Shark 
nd  Shark 
w-nosed  B 
ort-nosed 

o 

0. 

ur-spined 
mmon  Kil 
uirrel  Fish 
riped  Bass 
;llow-fin  G 

^^    C3    O^    «    QJ    O   O    O-^,*^ 

^    .5 


0) 

(-4 

03    O 

u 


:3 
o 


O     Co 


•r*     CO 
•<-»   '.Si 

-Si    «•> 


CO 


05 

S 
so  '^ 


IS     ^  ?     Co 


CO 

O 

CO   w 


3  o 


o 


w^ 


.£fJ  OJ  c3  (U 

ehPh;z;ph 


O   C3 
t-i    D 

c  o 


I 

2    O    0) 

•a  S  »- 


CO 
•<s>  ^ 


«0 


O 

c  s  a 

O   o   c3 


■c* 


CJ     i^i 


O    W    VJ    »i  -^  '^ 


S2  ^  -  ^ 


S5. 


=0  Coo 


CO     ^ 
CO     ^^    >     g 


;S§^ 


P  S  o 


^  ^  ^ 


^^^c4&qd;a^ 


CO 
o 

.1 

«     CO 

?^  § 

«    . 


CO     CO 


O 


555 


cS  c5  4^  eg  0) 


CO  •,-- 
ex- 
es 


D 


Six!    >^  O    1|    S 


0) 

c5 

D  .— 

VI    OJ 

05   O 


0) 

05 

•  p-l    fl) 


I 

es 


tf<QOOU 


o 


o 


y,^^^^^i^j,^i;5Ot>w:'*O"^O"^O'^o-^a0'^"^ifl«Tf(TjiTfi'ij(<r}i»fl 


)  .2  a  ^  9'_ 


c 

d 

O 
a 


X2 
=«     w    r- 


CO 


»h3 


^^-^ 


.2       '^ 

fe  -^  j:-   _ 


s  «j  1; 


s:  s  s  .2  o 
^  55   5^   r*  N 


5j 
o 

2  S 


4^ 


^     CO 

i- 

I- 

co^    =^ 
^^    I 


eo 
S 

•2  e 
s.  o 

•S  2 

C    y 


CO 

e 

Oh 


'S  fc  c  e 

<ii   o   .-    ^ 


fees   C   ^   ^   ^K^S   J^l 

u  M  Pu  m  Q  U  O  ^  H  PW  Pn  H 


Co     'iJ 


-    o    « 

60     S2    W     CO 


CO 

■w   »»    — . 


t 


^  «J  CO    ^ 

*~  CO  S   «^ 

g  S  ?l}«o 

.2  g  ^   S 

•r*  *.  «   05 

JJ  O  CO    o 


CO   o   CO 


Q    O    5~ 
O    Q    O 


CO  13   »»  piO 

-O  .<  "??     CO 

i  i  ""^ 

Co   "Ci    ^   -ci 

^  "?*  CO  ■:? 
.<  ly  C  Q 


2.  A 


CO 


"^  5;  « 

.  ^    !£    C 
■^    5S5> 


ao 

s:  s  i 

(^  o  ?• 
«"«  § 


C3 

•a 

3 


0) 

eSTS 

it 


0) 

35  •=;  ^  TS 


w 


03 

C     CO 


0) 

03 


c8 


d       +j  73  .5 
—        C  .5  O 

5  '^  - 

c3 

CO 

o 


hocq 


OQ 
O 

o 

Q 
O 

i 


I 


eo*-  eOCOlN  riri  r-( 


M 

t 

< 
o 


o 

g 

o 


c 
o 

ti  o 

^  ^  5 

<?  §^ 

j3    O    C8 


C 

o 

be 


« 

^^  2 
i^  S 

CO    fci    .^ 

IS  i 

B 


C3     r-     O 


-6 


tn 
0 

0 

bD 

!4 

C 

0 

0 

r^ 

h5c/2 

^  '^  >  cZ  .«-  -^ 
o  a>  O  -     ,  ^  ta 


2  Q 


O  3 

2  «^  rt  £  Sr;  o 


cj  c  ^  o.-S  5 
•^  -J  o  o  ^  i 


0) 

3  o  3  S 


e«9 

O 

S  s  05 

a  S   55 
w     •     • 


60    c 


65 


60 


«  I  S 

65  ?:  .< 

■S^  ^  ^ 

a,  ^  ^ 

•       ;j       r»       ^  •       S 


^  J* 

S  o 

•I:  i      *5 

«-«      s 

^  vj  C<i  vj 


^ 


o 

i     O     S"     '^ 

^.   O  p 


I'- 
ll 


c 

'« 
< 


cs 

rs 

"53 

12 

1 

<h4 

o 

aJ 
-d 

'S 
o 

s 

72 


c3 

'2 
*C 

O 

W) 

0; 

o 


a 

12 
'3 
o 

CO 


CO 

ce  >» 


n 

QQ 

fi 

H 

^ 

S 

Pi 

PS 

V 

0 

p 

^ 

g 

09    ^ 

1 

S 
H 

OQ 

Ph  22 

Siq 

0  2 

H 

U*  Q 

< 

45 

03 

Pi 

O 
Ph 


t6 


M 


ei 


O»0t>l>O'*OO>0OOOTjiNrHt>>^'»J>»J(00Tfixcqoi0W5 


4) 

•2   a> 

C/3   O 


-S  -«  J?  -^  .S  ^ 


CC 

:1  ^  1 1  H 
o 


<5  ^  -5  .2 


55  'C  C/5   5   3   a:  .2 


C2  TJ  C/2 


3  +->  T3 


5c/2cn« 


C/2  '^ 


£  e 


1^ 


w    3    3 


O   3   fc-   1> 


1/   c3 
rt  «  o  _«J  — 


S  to 
c3 


o  Q 


s  "  ? 

<y  ^  tN 

• « ,2  S 

^  "oJ  ^ 


^'  ^  k5  ^ 


-d 

•c 

03 

•s 


0) 


3    OJ    M    0) 


«^  '3 


ceo   03   t;   g 

C    C€    Cj    4,)    4J 


(S 

!2 

0) 

eS 

'S 

;0 

c3 

(h 

43 

t4 

•M 

(U 

O 

w 

U 

09 

H 

S 

CC 

(4 

o 

S 

b 

CC 

CO 

2 

S 

BS 

O 

^ 

3 

o 

tf 
H 

H 

u 

o 

S 

M 

Ph 

^ 

< 

«!! 

a 

<OOCLi 


o  o 


03 


< 
o 

w 

H 


PC4 

O 

H 
> 
O 

o 


e  -^ 
s 


a 


«3  o 

o 

H 


c 
a 

o 


Co 


c3 


o 


CO  2  ^ 

Sj    S    53 


o 

^  ^  S 
of  ^ 


1 

< 

ifefi 

Ra 

yin 
tfisl 

om: 
our 

na 

Coates'  Kni 
Electric  Ee; 
Three-lined 
Slender  Ra; 
Malayan  Fl 
Electric  Cal 
Pike-killie 
Snake-head 
Orange  Chr 
Croaking  G 

4) 

"S 

CO 

1 

c 
O 

CO 

-i 

CO  "Si 


o 


Is 


v3    i^j 


5  e 


00   CO 

■£  o  05  -o 

s.  C  o  5: 


4?     S 

K  o 


o  ?: 

•     CO 


■»~>     CO 


CO 

«  ^  « 

CO     c  '^ 

e  7  » 


.^ 


o 

05 

rs 

i» 

*c 

03 

a;  t3 

•t-j 

a* 

O 

o 

S 

4J 

t-* 

o 

c8 

t, 

J2 

P^^ 

V 
03 

rt  o  <u 

O   O  'S 

d  ^  .5 
2  4->  •r' 


03 
13 


OJ 


OKu 


t-l 

3 

c8 

T3 

+J 

i^ 

a 

^ 

^ 

o 

'V 

'3 

a 

i^p; 

11 


o 


h^u 


o 
o 

.  .   PS 

Oh    W 


UOPk 


Longevity  of  Fishes  in  Captivity  225 

organs  of  haemopoiesis.  Fishes  with  these  abnormahties  may 
Hve  for  an  exceptionally  long  time,  even  when  the  kidneys  are 
extensively  damaged,  since  the  osmoregulation  function  of 
this  organ  can  be  taken  over  in  part  by  the  gills.  Finally,  arterio- 
or  atherosclerosis  has  not  been  found  in  fishes. 


Summary 

Fishes  are  susceptible  to  a  variety  of  parasitic  and  metabolic 
diseases,  many  of  which  are  counterparts  of  those  that  occur 
in  mammals.  These  diseases,  together  with  changes  in  the 
physical,  chemical  and  biological  characteristics  of  the  environ- 
ment, are  responsible  for  mortality. 

Athero-  or  arteriosclerosis  has  not  been  found  in  fishes  but 
pathological  changes  indicative  of  ageing  do  occur  in  other 
organs.  These  are  cirrhosis  and  fatty  changes  in  the  liver  and 
kidneys,  haemochromatosis,  hypochromic  anaemia,  degenera- 
tion of  mucus-producing  glands  of  the  skin,  hyperostosis  of  the 
haemal  arch  bones  and  other  vertebral  abnormalities. 

Some  lifespans  of  fishes  in  the  New  York  Aquarium  and  in 
other  institutions  are  listed.  The  long-lived  fishes  kept  in 
captivity  have  certain  general  characteristics,  and  the  signifi- 
cance of  some  of  these  is  discussed. 

REFERENCES 

Berg,  L.  S.  (1947).    Classification  of  Fishes,  both  Recent  and  Fossil. 

Ann  Arbor :  J.  W^.  Edwards. 
BouRLiERE,  F.  (1946).   Annee  bioL  22,  249. 
Breder,  C.  M.,  Jr.  (1934).   Zoologica,  N.Y.,  18,  57. 
Breder,  C.  M.,  Jr.  (1936a).   Bull  N.Y.  zool.  Soc,  39,  116. 
Breder,  C.  M.,  Jr.  (19366).   Bull.  N.Y.  zool.  Soc,  39,  149. 
Breder,  C.  M.,  Jr.  (1952).   Growth,  16,  189. 
Brown,  M.  E.  (1957).    In  Physiology  of  Fishes,  1,  861.    New  York: 

Academic  Press. 
Comfort,  A.  (1956).  The  Biology  of  Senescence.  New  York:  Rinehart. 
Flower,  S.  S.  (1925).  Proc.  zool.  Soc,  Lond.,  247. 
Flower,  S.  S.  (1935).  Proc  zool.  Soc,  Lond.,  265. 
Hafter,  E.  (1952).  J.  Morph.,  90,  55. 
HiNTON,  S.  (1959).  Personal  Communication. 

AGEINQ — ^v— 8 


226  Ross    F.    NiGRELLI 

Jordan,  D.  S.,  Evermann,  B.  W.,  and  Clark,  H.  W.  (1930).  Rep.  U.S, 

Comm.  Fish.,  No.  1055,  1. 
Mellen,  I.  (1919).   Rep.  N.Y.  zool.  Soc,  No.  23,  101. 
Mellen,  I.  (1925).   Bull.  N.Y.  zool.  Soc,  27,  61. 
NiGRELLI,  R.  F.  (1940).   Zoologica,  N.Y.,  25,  525. 
NiGRELLI,  R.  F.  (1943).   Zoologica,  N.Y.,  28,  203. 
NiGRELLI,  R.  F.  (1952).   Ann.  N.Y.  Acad.  Sci.,  53,  1076. 
NiGRELLI,  R.  F.  (1954a).   Trans.  Amer.  Fish.  Soc,  83,  262. 
NiGRELLI,  R.  F.  (19546).   Trans.  N.Y.  Acad.  Sci.,  16,  296. 
Rasquin,  p.,  and  Hafter,  E.  (1951).  J.  Morph.,  89,  397. 
TowNSEND,  C.  H.  (1904).   Bull.  N.Y.  zool.  Soc,  No.  14,  163.^ 
TowNSEND,  C.  H.  (1913).   Bull.  N.Y.  zool.  Soc,  16,  1049. 
TowNSEND,  C.  H.  (1928a).   Bull.  N.Y.  zool.  Soc,  31,  11. 
TOWNSEND,  C.  H.  (19286).   Rep.  U.S.  Comm.  Fish.,  No.  1045,  249. 
Walford,  L.  a.  (1958).    Living  Resources  of  the  Sea.,  New  York: 

Ronald  Press. 


DISCUSSION 

Rockstein:  Which  is  the  longest-Uved  freshwater  fish  known? 

Nigrelli:  In  captivity  it  is  this  sturgeon  which  hved  69  years. 

Beverton:  Sturgeons  are  also  the  longest-lived  according  to  field 
observations;  the  longest  we  have  ever  seen  recorded  was  151  years, 
an  estimate  based  on  the  number  of  rings  in  the  rays  of  the  pectoral 
fin.  Are  any  age  determinations  of  the  conventional  kinds  done  on 
these  fish  at  death? 

Nigrelli:  We  have  attempted  to  make  age  determinations  but  we 
have  given  it  up  as  hopeless  because  at  these  steady  temperatures 
they  lose  the  markings.  We  do  take  measurements  of  the  fish  though, 
to  give  us  some  idea  of  growth. 

Scheidegger :  In  which  organs  have  you  found  cancer? 

Nigrelli:  In  practically  every  organ.  We  have  also  seen  virus 
tumours,  and  I  have  published  a  paper  (Nigrelli,  1952)  on  virus  in 
relation  to  cancer.  There  are  two  or  three  interesting  "tumours", 
however,  that  are  not  of  the  neoplastic  type,  and  one  is  well  known  in 
Europe — it  is  called  lymphocystis  disease  (Nigrelli,  1954a).  It  was 
first  described  in  English  plaice  but  it  is  widely  distributed  both  in 
freshwater  and  marine  species.  This  is  a  hypertrophy  of  the  con- 
nective tissue  cells  in  which  diseased  cells  blow  up  from  about  10  fx  to 
0  •  5  mm.  or  more.  This  disease  was  definitely  shown  to  be  of  virus 
origin. 

Holt:  Most  of  the  long-lived  species  are  rather  large,  but  there  are 
clear  exceptions,  just  as  there  are  for  mammals,  where  rats  and  bats 
of  roughly  the  same  size,  as  Mr.  Sacher  pointed  out,  have  quite 


Discussion  227 

different  lifespans.  We  find  the  exceptions  among  the  large  fish  in 
that  sturgeons  and  tunas  of  about  the  same  size  have  quite  different 
growth  rates  and  lifespans,  but  I  do  not  know  any  examples  of  very 
small  fish  which  are  long-lived.  None  of  those  indicated  as  reaching 
20  years  or  more  were  small  species.  By  small  fish  I  mean  those  with 
low  upper  limits  to  Lqq.  Are  there  some  such  fish  which  commonly 
live  for  ten  years  or  more? 

Comfort:  There  is  a  reported  instance  of  a  goldfish  being  kept  till 
40,  but  it  was  small  and  had  been  kept  in  a  barrel.  (Hervey,  G.  F., 
and  Hems,  J.  (1948).   The  Goldfish.   London:  Batchworth  Press.) 

NigrelU:  That  is  an  important  factor  in  aquarium  management. 
Fish  are  restricted  in  size  by  the  size  of  the  container,  though  not 
necessarily  by  the  volume  of  water.  Tarpon  have  been  kept,  for 
instance,  for  seven  or  eight  years,  and  some  have  never  grown  larger 
than  about  18  inches.  An  8-year-old  tarpon  in  the  wild  might  be 
about  4  feet  long  or  more. 

Holt:  By  small  fish,  I  did  not  mean  fish  which  were  artificially 
kept  small,  but  ones  which  cannot  grow  big  no  matter  how  much 
food  or  space  they  are  given. 

NigrelU:  There  are  several  examples  of  small  fish,  hke  goldfish, 
that  live  for  about  ten  years  (see  Table  III). 

Comfort:  The  matter  of  fatty  degeneration  is  one  which  has 
always  puzzled  me  when  looking  at  pathological  sections.  The 
normal  liver,  in  the  guppy  at  any  rate,  looks  very  fatty  but  I  do  not 
know  if  it  is  degenerate.  In  underfed  fish  the  liver  is  much  more 
cellular. 

NigrelU:  Glycogen  infiltration  is  also  evident  in  stained  sections. 

Comfort:  I  suspect  that  a  lot  of  what  appears  to  be  degeneration 
is  reversible,  and  is  actually  fat  storage. 

NigrelU:  We  call  it  fatty  change  and  it  does  indicate  a  disturb- 
ance of  lipid  metabolism  that  is  not  reversible. 

Comfort:  I  believe  you  showed  that  fatty  degeneration  occurs  in 
small  aquarium  fish  fed  only  with  enchytrae  and  nothing  else? 

NigrelU:  That  is  correct. 

Bourliere:  It  is  interesting  to  notice  that  tuna  are  said  to  have  a 
higher  internal  temperature  than  most  of  the  other  marine  fish  of 
similar  size.  Maybe  there  is  here  again  a  correlation  between 
metabolic  rate,  growth  rate  and  ageing  rate. 

Has  any  study  ever  been  made  on  the  relative  incidence  of  the 
various  pathological  causes  of  death  in  wild  fish? 

Beverton:  I  do  not  know  of  any  comprehensive  study.  A  member 
of  the  Aberdeen  Fisheries  Laboratory  has  taken  it  up  as  his  special 
subject  in  the  last  year  or  so. 


228  Discussion 

Nigrelli:  No  critical  work  has  ever  been  done  to  determine  the 
exact  meaning  of  natural  mortality.  Fishery  biologists  lump  many 
things  under  this  item.  For  example,  epidemics  of  myxosporidiosis 
are  quite  common  but  not  recognized,  and  the  disease  affects  young 
populations  much  more  than  the  old.  Such  an  infection  may  be 
diffuse.  All  it  takes  to  kill  off  a  population  after  an  epidemic  is  a 
slight  change  in  environment,  as  the  fish  are  already  weakened  by 
the  disease.  These  diseases  are  often  self-limiting,  and  if  the  fish  can 
survive  to,  say,  a  year  old,  then  they  apparently  can  withstand 
changes  in  temperature,  or  other  factors  including  pollution.  If  the 
fish  die  and  are  not  examined  for  disease  the  death  is  then  attributed 
to  natural  mortality. 

Holt:  There  is  a  current  work  which  is  relevant  to  this  question  but 
it  is  being  done  for  another  reason.  Quite  intensive  population  stud- 
ies of  parasitization  are  being  carried  out  on  cod  and  salmon  in  an 
attempt  to  identify  races  or  sub-populations.  The  results  may  well 
help  us  to  interpret  the  mortality  rates  of,  say,  different  sub- 
populations  of  the  same  species. 

Bourliere:  Are  there  any  indications  about  the  rate  of  infestation 
by  the  various  parasites  in  wild  fish?  Is  it  as  great  as  in  captive  ones? 

Gerking:  There  have  been  some  studies  made  in  Canada  of  Tri- 
aenophorus  (a  tapeworm)  infestation  in  whitefish,  because  it  reduces 
their  market  value.  There  have  been  attempts  made  to  reduce  the 
incidence  of  this  parasite  by  eliminating,  as  much  as  possible,  the 
intermediate  hosts,  and  there  has  been  partial  success. 

Rockstein:  What  is  the  rate  of  growth  of  the  common  carp,  and 
its  longevity?  It  is  a  fairly  large  fish,  and  it  must  therefore  grow 
rather  rapidly. 

Nigrelli:  I  cannot  tell  you  about  that. 

Gerking:  One  of  the  most  recent  developments  in  the  study  of  large 
fish  with  rapid  growth  is  some  work  on  the  sailfish  in  Florida  waters 
(de  Sylva,  D.  (1957).  Bull  Mar.  Sci.  Gulf  and  Carib.,  7,  1).  It  is  a 
well-known  sport  fish  because  it  is  large  and  can  be  caught  on  hook 
and  line.  It  was  amazing  to  learn  that  these  fish  grow  very  rapidly, 
living  only  four  or  five  years,  yet  reaching  100  lb. 

Holt:  Carp  grow  rapidly,  but  they  have  a  low  K;  that  is,  their 
increments  in  length  tend  to  be  constant,  but  they  have  a  particul- 
arly plastic  growth.  I  think  it  is  one  of  the  few  larger  species  in. 
which  there  is  some  evidence  of  a  post-reproductivx  phase. 

Gerking:  Is  that  well  documented? 

Holt:  No — except  that  I  believe  in  pond  culture  it  is  the  practice 
not  to  keep  large  carp  because  their  reproductive  capacities  are 
beginning  to  fall  off. 


Discussion  229 

Comfort:  Goldfish  breeders,  who  are  not  scientists,  say  that  very 
often  when  a  goldfish  gets  to  about  16  years  of  age  and  is  post- 
reproductive,  it  improves  in  condition  and  size  for  show  purposes, 
and  that  a  goldfish  between  the  ages  of  16  and  20  will  often  be  a  very 
large  and  particularly  glossy  specimen  for  show  purposes. 

Gerking:  What  was  said  about  the  carp  is  true  of  trout  also. 
Breeders  do  not  keep  old  trout  around  because  they  are  not  efficient 
reproductively.  This  is  not  due  to  a  sterility  factor,  however,  but  it 
reflects  the  growth  of  the  gonad  in  relation  to  the  weight  of  the  trout. 
This  is  a  special  case  that  I  did  not  have  time  to  mention.  The 
growth  of  the  gonad  is  not  proportional  to  the  growth  of  the  fish. 
Therefore,  older  trout  do  not  produce  as  many  eggs  as  a  younger  one 
does  in  proportion  to  the  weight  of  the  fish.  It  is  more  efficient  for 
the  hatchery  to  keep  younger  fish  for  breeding  than  it  is  to  keep  the 
same  weight  of  older  fish. 

Nigrelli:  There  are  many  parasites  that  affect  the  gonads.  If 
there  is  an  epidemic  of  gonadal  strigeidiasis  in  any  population,  it  will 
affect  the  reproductivity  for  that  particular  time. 

Holt:  Would  you  suggest,  then,  that  the  older  fish  has  had  a  longer 
time  to  be  exposed  to  infestation,  and  therefore  might  be  more 
likely  to  have  a  low  rate  of  reproduction? 

Nigrelli:  No,  it  would  have  to  be  an  acute  infection  of  some  kind, 
which  could  spread  around  by  contact,  particularly  in  close  schools 
of  fish.  Tuberculosis  of  the  ovaries  and  testes  will  be  found  in 
relatively  older  fish. 

Beverton:  You  gave  us  examples  of  fish  living  quite  happily  under 
extreme  environmental  conditions.  The  opposite  occurs  as  well. 
We  have  done  some  work  on  the  Arctic  cod  over  the  last  few  years, 
and  that  is  an  interesting  example  of  a  fish  which  is  living  on  the 
borderline  of  its  environmental  tolerance.  When  it  goes  across  that 
border,  in  effect  into  the  cold  water  from  the  Polar  basin,  the  cold 
water  seems  to  cause  quite  a  substantial  mortality.  The  immediate 
cause  of  death  seems  to  be  a  breakdown  in  the  osmoregulation,  and 
the  blood  saline  of  these  fish  that  are  caught  in  cold  water  is  up  to 
four  times  the  normal  level.  It  is  difficult  to  measure  mortality  in 
that  case,  but  the  inference  is  that  the  cold  had  quite  a  substantial 
effect. 

Danielli:  If  fish  of  the  same  age  are  put  in  different-sized  con- 
tainers, they  grow  to  different  sizes.  What  happens  if  they  are  put 
into  a  larger  container  which  is  separated  into  two  compartments, 
one  large  and  one  small,  with  a  perforated  screen  which  allows  the 
passage  of  water  but  not  of  fish? 

Comfort:  I  have  tried  this  with  guppies.   If  you  confine  a  fish  in  a 


230  Discussion 

perforated  netting  cage  inside  a  two-litre  jar,  it  grows  at  a  rate  not 
much  less  than  if  it  had  the  full  swimming  space.  I  do  not  think 
restriction  of  movement  affects  the  growth  of  these  fish.  There  is  a 
very  different  effect  when  you  put  more  than  one  fish  into  a  tank. 
There  is  a  social  effect  on  size,  which  is  partly  behavioural  and  partly 
chemical.  Where  the  fish  have  been  kept  in  separate  perspex  com- 
partments and  water  comes  rapidly  through  from  a  large  tank,  they 
seem  to  grow  at  a  not  much  lower  rate  than  they  would  have  grown 
if  they  had  been  loose  in  the  tank. 

Gerking:  Many  fish  are  very  aggressive  to  others  around  them. 
A  fish  in  company  with  others  will  fight  or  nip  in  an  attempt  to 
maintain  territory.  The  social  hierarchy  is  similar  to  that  which  has 
been  described  for  the  chicken  and  many  other  animals.  These 
social  factors  play  an  important  role  in  the  rate  of  growth  offish  kept 
together  in  aquaria. 

Danielli:  Has  the  experiment  been  tried  of  taking  fish  out  of  a 
tank  in  which  a  "pecking"  order  has  been  established,  and  putting 
them  into  a  mirrored  tank? 

Comfort:  No,  but  if  you  remove  the  largest  fish  from  a  tank  in 
which  a  size  hierarchy  has  been  established,  for  no  good  reason 
everybody  "moves  up"  one  place. 

Danielli:  The  fish  in  the  mirrored  tank  would  see  one  exactly  the 
same  size  as  itself. 

Comfort:  You  might  try  an  enlarging  mirror!  This  should  be  done 
with  other  fish  and  not  with  guppies,  because  guppies  are  extremely 
unaggressive.  I  have  been  very  fortunate  in  having  guppies  which 
have  never  shown  any  sign  of  eating  their  young,  and  the  counts 
from  trap  tanks  are  no  higher  than  without  traps.  This  is  not  true 
of  all  breeds  of  guppy,  and  it  would  be  a  perpetual  reservation  on  all 
this  work  if  the  fish  fight  and  eat  each  other's  young. 


FACTORS  INFLUENCING  THE 
LIFESPAN  OF  BEES 

Anna  Maurizio 
Bienendbieilung,  Liebefeld-Bern 

The  lifespan  of  the  honey-bee  and  its  dependence  on 
internal  and  external  influences  have  been  the  subject  of 
numerous  investigations  during  the  last  ten  years.  The 
problem  is  a  complex  one  because  the  honey-bee  does  not 
exist  as  an  individual  insect  but  as  a  member  of  a  colony. 
The  characteristics  of  the  honey-bee  must  therefore  be  con- 
sidered not  only  from  the  point  of  view  of  the  solitary  insect, 
but  also  from  that  of  the  colony.  The  lifespan  of  the  colony 
is  theoretically  unlimited,  but  that  of  the  individual  bee  is 
quite  short.  The  lifespan  is  also  very  closely  related  to  the 
physiological  condition  of  the  individual,  which  in  its  turn 
depends  on  the  environment,  nutrition  and  general  condition 
of  the  colony.  The  author  would  therefore  like  to  present 
this  communication  in  two  parts : 

(1)  the  lifespan  of  the  bee  in  a  free-flying  colony,  and 

(2)  the  lifespan  of  the  bee  when  separated  from  the  colony 
and  caged. 

Lifespan  and  Physiological  Condition  of 
Bees  in  a  Free -Flying  Colony 

In  those  regions  of  the  Temperate  Zone  marked  by  clearly 
defined  seasons,  the  colony  is  inhabited  by  two  types  of  bees : 
summer  bees  and  winter  bees.  These  represent  two  entirely 
distinct  generations  of  bees  with  regard  to  their  lifespan,  their 
physiological  condition  and  their  mode  of  life.  Here  we  are 
confronted  with  one  of  the  most  important  and  interesting 

231 


232  Anna  Maurizio 

phenomena  in  the  hfe  of  a  bee  colony,  namely,  overwintering, 
or  the  bridging  of  an  unfavourable  season  during  which  the 
bees  cannot  forage  for  food. 

More  primitive  social  insects  like  bumble-bees  and  wasps 
solve  this  problem  by  dispersing  the  colony  in  the  autumn, 
leaving  only  the  inseminated  females  to  overwinter.  In  the 
case  of  the  honey-bee,  however,  the  entire  colony  overwinters 
with  the  queen,  and  the  rearing  of  the  first  spring  brood 
takes  place  before  the  active  season  begins.  The  difficulties  of 
overwintering  are  overcome  by  producing  in  the  autumn  a 
long-lived  generation  with  big  body  reserves;  the  colony  is 
thus  enabled  to  live  through  the  winter  and  rear  the  first 
spring  generation. 

To  understand  this,  one  has  to  consider  in  greater  detail  the 
characteristic  habits  of  the  summer  and  winter  bees,  and  the 
ageing  and  seasonal  changes  which  certain  organs  undergo. 
In  particular,  the  pharyngeal  glands  and  the  fat  body  are 
affected;  the  wax,  mandibular,  and  salivary  glands,  and  the 
ovaries  of  the  working  bees,  are  also  affected,  though  to  a 
lesser  extent  (Anderson,  1931;  Armbruster,  1931;  El-Deeb, 
1952;  Evenius,  1937;  Farrar,  1936,  1949Z?,  1952;  Freudenstein, 
1924;  Gontarski,  1950,  1954;  Haydak,  1934,  1937a,  h,  1939, 
1940a,  h;  Haydak  and  Tanquary,  1943;  Koehler,  1921; 
Kratky,  1931;  Levin  and  Haydak,  1951;  Lotmar,  1939; 
Mauermayer,  1954;  Maurizio,  1946,  1954;  Nickel  and  Arm- 
bruster, 1937,  1938;  Phillips,  1922,  1928;  Ribbands,  1953; 
Rockstein,  1950a;  Zander,  1947,  1951). 

Let  us  begin  with  the  summer  bee.  In  Central  Europe  it  is 
to  be  found  in  the  colonies  between  May  and  August.  Its 
lifespan  varies  from  a  minimum  of  25-35  days  to  a  maximum 
of  60-70  days,  and  it  emerges  at  the  height  of  full  colony 
development.  It  will  find  in  the  colony  large  patches  of  open 
brood,  and  very  often  the  number  of  nurses  is  scarcely  suf- 
ficient to  feed  the  young  larvae.  During  the  first  10-14  days 
of  its  life  the  young  bee  consumes  a  large  amount  of  pollen, 
thus  stimulating  full  development  and  functioning  of  the 


Factors  Influencing  the  Lifespan  of  Bees      233 

pharyngeal  glands.  The  newly  emerged  bee  has  undeveloped 
hyaline  pharyngeal  glands  of  irregularly  angular  shape;  its 
fat  body  consists  of  a  very  delicate  transparent  cell  layer  with- 
out inclusions.  In  the  approximately  ten-day-old  summer  bee 
performing  nursery  duties,  the  pharyngeal  glands  are  rounded, 
milky-white  to  yellow,  with  well-filled  lobes  completely 
covering  the  secretory  ducts.  In  its  third  week  of  life,  when 
the  bee  assumes  different  duties,  the  pharyngeal  glands 
slowly  degenerate  but  the  wax  glands  attain  full  develop- 
ment. In  the  four-week-old  summer  flying-bee  the  pharyngeal 
and  wax  glands  have  already  degenerated,  and  under  normal 
conditions  the  fat  body  of  the  summer  bee  remains  un- 
developed throughout  its  life.  This  form  of  presentation  is 
somewhat  schematic,  because  we  know  from  studies  carried 
out  by  Lindauer  (1953),  Ribbands  (1952),  Sakagami  (1953), 
and  others  that  the  division  of  labour  within  the  colony  varies 
greatly;  thus  it  is  possible  that  the  length  of  any  of  the  above- 
mentioned  duties  can  be  prolonged,  shortened  or  even  omitted, 
and  the  degree  of  development  of  the  pharyngeal  and  wax 
glands  shows  parallel  fluctuations. 

The  life  of  the  bee  emerging  in  autumn  follows  quite  a  dif- 
ferent course.  The  bee  makes  its  appearance  in  the  colony 
at  a  time  when  the  brood  is  greatly  reduced,  the  stores  pre- 
pared and  the  bees  ready  for  overwintering.  The  young  bee 
feeds  for  several  weeks  on  pollen  without  having  to  perform 
nursery  duties  (maximal  pollen  consumption  is  between 
September  and  October — Lotmar,  1939).  Thus  the  pharyngeal 
glands  and  the  fat  body  attain  full  development  and  remain 
in  this  state  until  spring,  when  the  autumn  bee  takes  up  the 
nursing  of  the  newly  emerging  brood.  The  pharyngeal  glands 
of  the  winter  bee  therefore  remain  in  the  nursing  state  for  six 
to  seven  months,  and  the  fat  body  consists  of  several  densely 
folded  layers,  the  cells  of  which  are  milky- white,  rounded,  and 
packed  full  with  inclusions.  At  the  same  time  the  bee's 
expectation  of  life  increases,  and  it  becomes  a  long-lived 
winter  bee,   remaining  in  the   colony  six  to  eight  months 


234  Anna  Maurizio 

(maximum  expectation  of  life  300-400  days — Anderson,  1931; 
Farrar,  1949a;  Lotmar,  1939;  Maurizio,  1954). 

Many  attempts  have  been  made  to  explain  these  striking 
differences  between  the  lifespan  of  summer  and  winter  bees. 
The  customary  conception  is  that  the  lifespan  is  regulated,  in 
particular,  by  the  collecting  activity  of  the  bee  outside  the 
hive.  According  to  this  conception  the  bee  works  itself  "to 
death"  on  its  collecting  flights,  when  death  is  often  caused  by 
accidents  (Phillips,  1922,  1928).  However,  exacting  experi- 
ments have  proved  that  where  death  is  due  to  foraging  the 
shortening  of  life  is  only  a  matter  of  between  four  and  eight 
days,  and  thus  is  not  long  enough  to  explain  the  difference  in 
lifespan  between  summer  and  winter  bees  (El-Deeb,  1952; 
Mauermayer,  1954).  The  genetically  conditioned  differences 
in  lifespan  between  bees  of  different  races  and  cultivated 
strains  are  also  few.  The  bee  is  subject — like  any  other  living 
creature — to  the  process  of  physiological  ageing,  which  is 
based  on  the  destruction  of  certain  nerve  cells.  Rockstein 
(1950a,  6,  1953)  and  Weyer  (1931)  estabhshed  that  in  the 
ageing  bee  the  number  of  nerve  cells  in  the  olfactory  lobe  and 
in  the  lower  pharyngeal  ganglion  decreases  by  35  per  cent. 

Factors  influencing  the  bees'  lifespan  become  clear  only 
when  one  considers  the  behaviour  of  bees  under  experimental 
conditions.  For  example,  if  one  forces  a  young  summer  bee 
in  the  nursing  state  to  undertake  continuous  brood-rearing, 
the  pharyngeal  glands  will  remain  at  their  full  development 
for  several  weeks  and  thus  increase  its  lifespan  (Moskovljevic, 
1939).  This  increase,  as  well  as  an  enhancement  of  physio- 
logical condition,  accompanies  the  limitation  of  brood  nests 
in  a  colony  ready  to  swarm.  Particularly  striking  is  the 
behaviour  of  bees  when  put  into  a  queenless  or  broodless 
colony  during  the  summer.  Such  bees  not  only  possess  for 
several  weeks  fully  developed  pharyngeal  glands,  but  also 
form  a  winter  fat  body  and  thus  become  long-lived  (maximum 
life-span  166  days,  as  against  38  days  in  a  colony  with  brood — 
Maurizio,    1954).     Therefore   it   is   possible   to   transform   a 


Factors  Influencing  the  Lifespan  of  Bees      235 

summer  bee  during  the  active  season  into  a  physiological 
winter  bee  by  preventing  it  from  brood-rearing. 

Further  indications  of  the  interdependent  effects  of  food, 
care  of  brood,  physiological  condition  and  lifespan  were 
obtained  from  experiments  carried  out  with  bees  separated 
from  their  colony,  and  kept  in  cages  or  in  small  experimental 
colonies. 

Lifespan  and  Physiological  Condition  of 
Caged  Bees 

In  the  experiments  with  caged  bees  attempts  were  made  to 
clarify  the  following  questions: 

(a)  What  effect  has  pollen-feeding  on  caged  bees  and  are 
there  any  differences  in  the  effects  of  pollen  from  various 
plant  species? 

(b)  Which  organs  of  the  bee  depend  directly  on  the  food- 
composition? 

(c)  Does  a  direct  relationship  exist  between  physiological 
condition  and  lifespan? 

(d)  Which  substances  in  the  pollen  are  decisive  in  the 
activation  of  the  physiological  condition,  and  in  the  lifespan? 

The  first  investigations  in  this  direction  go  back  to  the  time 
when  Czech  workers  (Soudek,  1927;  Hejtmanek,  1933,  1943; 
Svoboda,  1940)  observed  that  the  pharyngeal  glands  of  newly 
emerged  bees  could  be  brought  to  full  development  by  feeding 
with  pollen,  soya  flour  and  albumin.  Subsequently,  the  present 
author  proved  (1946,  1954)  that  pollen-feeding  of  caged  bees 
not  only  brought  about  an  activation  of  the  pharyngeal  glands, 
but  also  caused  a  building-up  of  the  fat  body,  the  development 
of  the  ovaries,  and  a  statistically  proved  longer  lifespan.  These 
findings  were  confirmed  by  other  workers  (Back,  1956,  1959; 
Bertholf,  1942;  Beutler  and  Opfinger,  1949;  Freudenstein, 
1958;  de  Groot,  1953;  Hess,  1942;  Mussbichler,  1952;  Pain, 
1951a,  b;  Wahl,  1956),  and  it  was  also  shown  that  pollen- 
feeding  produces  development  of  the  wax  glands  (Freuden- 
stein, 1958). 


236  Anna  Maurizio 

These  experiments  further  confirmed  that  pollens  of  dif- 
ferent plants  can  vary  greatly  in  their  effect  on  the  bees. 
Thirty-five  pollen  samples  were  examined  and  these  can  be 
divided  into  many  different  groups.  Depending  on  their 
biological  effectiveness,  they  range  from  the  very  effective 
(e.g.  Salix  spp.,  fruit  trees,  Papaver  spp.,  Zea  mays,  Crocus 
alhiflorus,  Trifolium  spp.,  Castanea  sativa)  to  the  almost 
ineffective,  e.g.  all  seven  species  of  conifer  examined  (Pinus 
cembra,  P.  silvestris,  P,  montana,  Picea  excelsa,  Abies  alba, 
Cedrus  libani,  Pseudotsuga  taxifolia).  No  relationship  could 
be  established  between  the  type  of  floral  biology  of  any 
particular  plant  and  the  effectiveness  of  its  pollen  on  bees ;  on 
the  other  hand,  pollen  collected  by  bees  w^as  more  effective 
than  hand-collected  pollen  from  the  same  plant  species.  It 
remains  to  be  mentioned  that  bees  are  very  selective  in  their 
choice  of  pollen  sources,  and  this,  according  to  Louveaux 
(1958),  is  connected  with  the  nitrogen  content  of  the  pollen. 
These  observations  on  bees  were  recently  confirmed  by  ex- 
periments carried  out  with  other  insects  (Tribolium  larvae, 
Koch,  1952;  Osmia  lignaria,  Levin  and  Haydak,  1958). 
These  authors  also  established  differences  in  effectiveness 
between  pollen  from  different  plant  species  and  between 
hand  and  insect-collected  pollen. 

Further  evidence  shows  that  the  effect  of  pollen  is  time- 
conditioned,  i.e.  consumption  must  take  place  within  the 
first  10-14  days  of  the  bee's  life  in  order  to  activate  the 
pharyngeal  and  wax  glands  as  well  as  the  fat  body.  Pollen- 
feeding  at  a  later  stage  has  little  or  no  effect  (Beutler  and 
Opfinger,  1949;  Freudenstein,  1958;  de  Groot,  1953;  Kratky, 
1931;  Maurizio,  1954). 

Data  obtained  from  the  author's  experiments  were  statistic- 
ally interpreted  in  order  to  examine  in  greater  detail  the 
relationship  between  lifespan  and  physiological  condition.  On 
the  basis  of  a  multiple  correlation  (part-regression)  it  was 
established  that  there  is  a  positive  relationship  between 
physiological  condition  and  lifespan,  i.e.  that  56  per  cent  of 


Factors  Influencing  the  Lifespan  of  Bees      237 

the  variations  in  lifespan  can  be  traced  to  the  physiological 
condition.  The  closest  correlation  was  found  between  the 
development  of  the  fat  body  and  lifespan ;  the  least  close  was 
that  between  ovary  development  and  lifespan.  The  result  of 
this  statistical  evaluation  confirmed  the  biological  investiga- 
tions which  showed  that  the  main  function  of  the  fat  body  is 
the  storing  of  protein,  glycogen  and  fat,  whereas  the  pharyn- 
geal glands  not  only  serve  as  a  store,  but  perform  other 
important  functions  such  as  the  secretion  of  larva  food  and 
the  enzymes  necessary  for  honey  production.  The  functioning 
of  the  ovaries  of  the  workers  is  mainly  controlled  by  hormones 
and  in  the  queenright  colony  the  ovaries  remain  undeveloped 
(Butler,  1954,  1955,  1956;  Pain,  1954,  1958;  and  Voogd, 
1955).  Thus,  of  all  the  organs  examined,  it  appears  that  the 
fat  body  plays  the  most  important  role  in  the  regulation  of 
lifespan  of  the  summer  and  winter  bee. 

A  further  problem  is  posed  in  deciding  which  substances  in 
the  pollen  are  responsible  for  the  activation  of  the  physio- 
logical condition  and  lengthening  of  the  bees'  lifespan. 
Pollen  is  rich  in  carbohydrates,  fats,  proteins,  vitamins  and 
minerals,  but  the  concentration  of  any  one  of  these  substances 
varies  greatly  from  one  plant  species  to  another  (Hay dak  and 
Palmer,  1938,  1940,  1941,  1942;  Haydak  and  Vivino,  1943, 
1950;  Hejtmanek,  1943;  John,  1958;  Kocher,  1942;  Pearson, 
1942;  Sarkar  et  ah,  1949;  Todd  and  Bretherick,  1942;  Vivino 
and  Palmer,  1944;  Weaver  and  Kuiken,  1951;  Weygand  and 
Hofmann,  1950).  For  instance,  the  protein  content  of  dif- 
ferent types  of  pollen  can  vary  as  much  as  5  to  35  per  cent, 
and  the  spectra  of  amino  acids  present  may  be  variously 
composed.  It  was  found  that  pollen  contains  the  following 
vitamins:  B^  (thiamine),  Bg  (riboflavin),  Bg  (pyridoxine), 
nicotinic  acid  and  biotin.  Moreover  vitamin  K,  w^hich  is  not 
normally  present  in  fresh  pollen,  was  found  in  pollen  obtained 
from  comb  cells.  Bees  have  sufficient  carbohydrates  at  their 
disposal  in  honey,  thus  pollen  is  their  main  protein  and  vit- 
amin source.    Over  the  last  few  years  many  lively  discussions 


238  Anna  Maurizio 

have  taken  place  on  the  relationship  between  lifespan  and 
physiological  condition  in  the  adult  bee,  and  to  what  ex- 
tent it  is  influenced  by  the  proteins  and  vitamins  of  pollen. 
It  must  be  pointed  out  that  this  concerns  only  the  vitamin 
requirements  of  the  adult  bee,  as  there  are  no  doubts  about  the 
vitamin  requirements  of  the  brood. 

The  results  of  the  experiments  carried  out  so  far  can  be 
summarized  as  follows  (Back,  1956,  1959;  Freudenstein, 
1958;  de  Groot,  1953;  Haydak  and  Vivino,  1950,  Koch  and 
Schwarz,  1956;  Maurizio,  1954;  Miissbichler,  1952;  Pain, 
1951a,  b).  In  the  caged  young  bee  lifespan  and  physiological 
condition  are  closely  related  to  the  protein  content  of  food. 
Bees  fed  exclusively  on  devitaminized  casein  develop  pharyn- 
geal glands,  fat  bodies  and  ovaries,  and  a  statistically  proved 
increase  of  lifespan  is  observed  by  comparison  with  bees  of  the 
same  age  fed  on  a  pure  sugar  diet.  An  increase  in  nitrogen  and 
in  dry  weight  occurs  in  the  young  bee  if  it  is  fed  from  its 
emergence  onwards  on  synthetic  food — a  mixture  of  sugar  and 
pure  amino  acids  (de  Groot,  1953).  Addition  of  any  one  of  the 
vitamins  (B^,  Bg,  Bg,  nictotinic  and  pantothenic  acids) 
resulted  in  no  statistically  proved  increase  in  lifespan,  but 
addition  of  vitamins  to  a  protein-containing  diet  influenced 
the  development  of  the  pharyngeal  and  wax  glands.  Although 
young  bees  can  develop  their  pharyngeal  glands  on  vitamin- 
free  casein  food,  the  degree  of  development  is  enhanced  if 
vitamins  are  added.  Back's  latest  investigations  contribute 
greatly  towards  a  solution  of  this  question.  According  to  her, 
large  doses  of  vitamins  added  to  the  casein  food  have  the 
effect  of  shortening  the  lifespan.  A  very  slight  prolongation 
was  observed  when  very  weak  mixtures  of  vitamins  were 
added  (mixture:  Bj,  Bg,  Bg,  A,  C,  E,  K,  nicotinic,  pantothenic 
and  folic  acids,  biotin  and  inosite).  Young  bees  fed  with 
vitamin-free  casein  food  reared  one  generation  of  normal 
brood;  bad  rearing  occurred  only  with  the  second  generation 
of  larvae.  Young  bees  reared  by  nurses  fed  on  a  vitamin-free 
diet  are  unable  to  utilize  the  protein  in  their  food  and  thus 


Factors  Influencing  the  Lifespan  of  Bees      239 

cannot  rear  a  brood  unless  vitamins  are  added  to  their  diet. 
In  such  vitamin-starved  bees  addition  of  pantothenic  acid 
or  a  mixture  of  vitamins  to  the  diet  will  restore  normal  brood- 
rearing  within  24  hours. 

Apparently  the  newly  emerged  young  bee  is  left  with  large 
body  reserves  of  vitamins,  thus  making  it  possible  to  develop 
its  pharyngeal  glands,  lengthen  its  lifespan  and  rear  one 
brood-generation  in  spite  of  consuming  a  protein-rich  but 
vitamin-free  diet.  It  seems,  however,  that  this  vitamin 
reserve  is  not  sufficient  to  pass  on  to  the  brood  an  adequate 
vitamin  surplus;  so  that  the  second  generation  of  vitamin- 
starved  bees  shows  signs  of  vitamin  deficiency,  i.e.  the 
development  of  the  pharyngeal  glands  and  the  lifespan  are 
affected.  Similarly,  a  colony  of  bees  kept  on  a  protein-free 
diet  are  able  to  raise  one  generation  of  normal  brood  by  using 
up  their  own  protein  reserves  (Haydak,  1935). 

Discussion 

Present-day  knowledge  of  the  inter-efFects  of  nutrition, 
physiological  condition  and  lifespan  of  the  bee,  and  of  the 
processes  of  life  in  the  colony  throughout  the  year,  can  be  sum- 
marized as  follows: 

(1)  Pollen  is  the  main  source  for  protein,  vitamins,  and 
minerals,  and  therefore  forms,  with  the  sugar-containing 
juices,  the  basis  of  nutriment  and  development  of  the  bee 
colony.  Pollen-feeding  of  the  young  bee  promotes  the  develop- 
ment of  the  pharyngeal  and  wax  glands,  the  formation  of  a 
many-layered  fat  body  and  an  increase  in  lifespan.  Of  all  the 
pollen  substances  proteins  are  the  most  decisive  in  influencing 
the  lifespan  and  physiological  condition  of  the  bee.  For  the 
adult  bee  vitamins  in  food  are  less  essential  as  it  still  has  a 
large  store  of  vitamins  from  the  larval  phase.  Absolutely 
essential  is  the  extra  vitamin  supply  at  the  time  of  brood- 
rearing.  There  is  a  statistically  comprehensible  correlation 
between  physiological  condition  and  lifespan. 

(2)  The  yearly  cycle  of  life  in  the  bee  colony  may  be 


240  Anna  Maurizio 

described  as  follows.  During  the  warm  season  the  young  bee 
feeds  for  the  first  few  days  of  its  life  on  pollen,  developing  its 
pharyngeal  glands.  With  the  commencement  of  brood- 
rearing  the  reserves  are  used  up  and  the  bee  ages  physio- 
logically, thus  becoming  a  short-lived  summer  flying-bee. 
However,  if  for  any  cause  brood-rearing  is  limited  (e.g.  due  to 
a  queenless  colony,  swarming,  prolonged  rain,  etc.),  or  com- 
pletely omitted,  the  pharyngeal  glands  will  remain  fully 
developed,  the  fat  body  will  be  formed  and  the  lifespan  thus 
increased.  In  the  free-flying  colony  during  the  active  season 
it  is  possible  to  produce  experimentally  such  a  generation  of 
physiologically  young,  long-lived  bees. 

When  brood-rearing  is  naturally  limited  and  at  the  same 
time  intensive  pollen-feeding  takes  place,  a  long-lived  winter 
bee  with  large  body  reserves,  fully  developed  pharyngeal 
glands  and  a  many-layered  fat  body  is  produced  in  late 
summer  or  autumn ;  the  bee  is  thus  enabled  to  overwinter  and 
to  rear  the  first  brood  in  spring.  During  the  winter  the  bee 
feeds  almost  entirely  on  sugar  and  therefore  it  seems  that  the 
substances  (protein,  glycogen  and  vitamins)  necessary  to 
survive  winter  are  drawn  from  the  reserves  built  up  in  the  fat 
body.  A  similar  kind  of  overwintering  is  known  of  other  solitary 
insects  such  as  Anopheles,  Culex,  Lasiopticus,  Epistrophe,  Ips 
typographus,  etc.  (Buxton,  1935;  Kuhn,  1949;  Schneider,  1947, 
1948;  Wigglesworth,  1950). 

(3)  It  appears  that  the  lifespan  and  physiological  condition 
of  bees  in  free-flying  colonies  depend  greatly  on  nutrition  and 
brood-rearing.  Long-lived  bees  always  appear  in  a  colony 
where  a  rich  supply  of  pollen  is  available  and  where  little  or 
no  brood  is  in  need  of  nursing.  In  our  climate  long-lived  bees 
appear  in  autumn  and  winter,  but  under  diff'erent  climatic 
conditions  this  may  occur  at  different  times  or  fail  to  take 
place.  Thus  the  physiological  condition  of  the  "summer"  and 
"winter"  bee  does  not  depend  on  a  certain  season,  but  can 
be  promoted  at  any  time  as  a  reaction  to  external  conditions 
essential  to  life. 


Factors  Influencing  the  Lifespan  of  Bees      241 

The  continuity  of  life  in  a  bee  colony  depends  upon  an 
unbroken  succession  of  generations.  This  succession  is  main- 
tained by  the  fluctuations  between  nutrition,  brood-rearing, 
physiological  condition  and  lifespan,  giving  the  colony 
adaptability  to  the  prevailing  external  conditions. 

The  greater  the  brood  nest  is  in  proportion  to  the  number 
of  nursing  bees,  the  larger  is  the  new  batch  of  young  bees,  but 
the  shorter  the  lifespan  of  the  individual  insect.  Restricted 
brood  areas  resulting  in  small  numbers  of  young  bees,  on  the 
other  hand,  mean  long-lived  bees.  Both  types  of  colony  are 
well  known  to  the  practising  bee-keeper.  These  types  are 
partly  genetically  conditioned,  it  being  probably  less  the 
longevity  of  the  bees  which  is  hereditary  than  the  tendency 
to  the  laying  down  of  large  or  small  brood  nests. 

[The  editors  would  like  to  thank  Mrs.  M.  Silverman  for  her  work  in 
translating  this  paper,  and  Mr.  H.  K.  Airy  Shaw  for  his  expert  advice.] 

REFERENCES 

Anderson,  J.  (1931).   Bee  World,  12,  25. 

Armbruster,  L.  (1931).   Handb.  Erndhr.  Nutztiere,  3,  478. 

Back,  E.  (1956).   Insect.  Soc.,  3,  285. 

Back,  E.  (1959).   Personal  communication. 

Bertholf,  L.  M.  (1942).   J.  econ.  EnL,  35,  887. 

Beutler,  R.,  and  Opfinger,  E.  (1949).   Z.  vergl.  Physiol.,  32,  383. 

Butler,  C.  G.  (1954).   Bee  World,  35,  169. 

Butler,  C.  G.  (1955).   Amer.  Bee  J.,  95,  275. 

Butler,  C.  G.  (1956).  Proc.  roy.  Soc.,  A.,  31,  12. 

Buxton,  P.  A.  (1935).  Parasitology,  27,  263. 

El-Deeb,  a.  L.  a.  (1952).   Amer.  Bee  J.,  92,  517. 

EvENius,  C.  (1937).   D.  Imkerfuhrer,  11,  128. 

Farrar,  C.  L.  (1936).   Amer.  Bee  J.,  76,  452. 

Farrar,  C.  L.  (1949«).   Bee  World,  30,  51. 

Farrar,  C.  L.  (19496).  In  The  Hive  and  the  Honeybee,  ed.  Grout,  R.A. 

Hamilton,  Illinois:  Dadant. 
Farrar,  C.  L.  (1952).  J.  econ.  Ent.,  45,  445. 
Freudenstein,  K.  (1924).   Arch.  Bienenk.,  6,  49. 
Freudenstein,  H.  (1958).   In  XVII  Congr.  int.  Apia. 
Gontarski,  H.  (1950).   Imkerfreund,  11,  183. 
Gontarski,  H.  (1954).   Z.  Bienenforsch.,  2,  161. 
Groot,  a.  p.  de  (1953).   Dissertation,  Utrecht. 
Haydak,  M.  H.  (1934).  J.  agric.  Res.,  49,  21. 


242  Anna  Maurizio 

Haydak,  M.  H.  (1935).  J.  econ.  Ent,  28,  657. 

Haydak,  M.  H.  (1937a).  J.  agric.  Res.,  54,  791. 

Haydak,  M.  H.  (19376).   Ann.  ent.  Soc.  Amer.,  30,  258. 

Haydak,  M.  H.  (1939).  J.  econ.  Ent,  32,  663. 

Haydak,  M.  H.  (1940a).  J.  econ.  Ent.,  33,  397. 

Haydak,  M.  H.  (19406).   Glean.  Bee  Cult.,  68,  615. 

Haydak,  M.  H.,  and  Palmer,  L.  S.  (1938).  J.  econ.  Ent.,  31,  576. 

Haydak,  M.  H.,  and  Palmer,  L.  S.  (1940).  J.  econ.  Ent.,  33,  396. 

Haydak,  M.  H.,  and  Palmer,  L.  S.  (1941).  J.  econ.  Ent.,  34,  37. 

Haydak,  M.  H.,  and  Palmer,  L.  S.  (1942).  J.  econ.  Ent.,  35,  319. 

Haydak,  M.  H.,  and  Tanquary,  M.  C.  (1943).   Bull.  Minn,  agric.  Exp. 

Sta.y  160,  1. 
Haydak,  M.  H.,  and  Vivino,  E.  (1943).   Arch.  Biochem.,  2,  201. 
Haydak,  M.  H.,  and  Vivino,  E.  (1950).  Ann.  ent.  Soc.  Amer.,  43,  361. 
Hejtmanek,  J.  (1933).    des.  VMaf,  10. 
Hejtmanek,  J.  (1943).    Die  PoUenverdauung  der  Honigbiene,    Ustr. 

slov.  vcel.  spolkov,  Prievidza. 
Hess,  G.  (1942).   Beth.  Schweiz.  Bienenztg.,  1,  33. 
John,  M.  (1958).   Z.  vergl.  Physiol,  41,  204. 
Koch,  A.  (1952).   Tijdschr.  Ent.,  95,  166. 
Koch,  A.,  and  Schwarz,  J.  (1956).   Insect.  Soc,  3,  213. 
KocHER,  V.  (1942).  Beih.  Schweiz.  Bienenztg.,  1,  155. 
KoEHLER,  A.  (1921).  Schweiz.  Bienenztg.,  57,  424. 
Kratky,  E.  (1931).   Z.  wiss.  ZooL,  139,  120. 

KuHN,  W.  (1949).   Mitt.  Schweiz.  ZentAnst.  Forstl.  Versuchsw.,  26,  245. 
Levin,  M.  D.,  and  Haydak,  M.  H.  (1951).  J.  econ.  Ent.,  44,  54. 
Levin,  M.  D.,  and  Haydak,  M.  H.  (1958).  X  Int.  Congr.  Ent.,  4, 1079. 
Lindauer,  M.  (1953).   Bee  World,  34,  63,  85. 
Lotmar,  R.  (1939).   Landw.  Jb.  Schweiz.,  53,  34. 
LouvEAUx,  J.  (1958).   These,  Paris. 
Mauermayer,  G.  (1954).   Arch.  Bienenk.,  31,  31. 
Maurizio,  A.  (1946).   Beih.  Schweiz.  Bienenztg.,  2,  1. 
Maurizio,  A.  (1954).   Landw.  Jb.  Schweiz.,  68,  115. 
Moskovljevic,  V.  (1939).  Schweiz.  Bienenztg.,  75,  689,  and  Bee  World, 

21,  39. 
Mussbichler,  a.  (1952).   Z.  vergl.  Physiol,  34,  207. 
Nickel,  H.  K.,  and  Armbruster,  L.  (1937).  Arch.  Bienenk.,  18,  257. 
Nickel,  H.  K.,  and  Armbruster,  L.  (1938).   Arch.  Bienenk.,  19,  134. 
Pain,  J.  (1951a).   Arch.  inU  Physiol,  59,  203. 
Pain,  J.  (19516).   C.  R.  Soc.  Biol  (Paris),  165,  1505. 
Pain,  J.  (1954).   Insect  Soc,  1,  59. 
Pain,  J.  (1958).   Apiculteur,  103,  1. 

Pearson,  P.  B.  (1942).  Proc  Soc.  exp.  Biol  (N.Y.),  51,  291. 
Phillips,  E.  F.  (1922).  J.  econ.  Ent,  15,  368. 
Phillips,  E.  F.  (1928).   Beekeeping.   New  York:  Macmillan  Co. 
Ribbands,  C.  R.  (1952).  Proc.  roy.  Soc  B.,  140,  32. 
Ribbands,  C.  R.  (1953).   The  Behaviour  and  Social  Life  of  Honeybees. 

London :  Bee  Research  Association  Ltd. 


Factors  Influencing  the  Lifespan  of  Bees      243 

RocKSTEiN,  M.  (1950a).  Ann.  ent.  Soc.  Amer.,  43,  152. 

RocKSTEiN,  M.  (19506).  J.  cell  comp.  Physiol,  35,  11. 

RocKSTEiN,  M.  (1953).   Biol.  Bull.,  Wood's  Hole,  105,  154. 

RoscH,  G.  A.  (1925).   Z.  vergl.  Physiol,  2,  571. 

SakagAxMI,  S.  F.  (1953).  Jap.  J.  Zool,  11,  117. 

Sarkar,  B.  C.  R.,  Wittwer,  S.  H.,  Luecke,  R.  W.,  and  Shell,  H.  M. 

(1949).  Arch.  Biochem.,  22,  352. 
Schneider,  F.  (1947).   Mitt,  schweiz.  enl  Ges.,  20,  306. 
Schneider,  F.  (1948).  Mitt,  schweiz.  enl  Ges.,  21,  249. 
SouDEK,  S.  (1927).   Bull  Fac.  Agric.  Brunn,  10,  1. 
SvoBODA,  J.  (1940).  Schweiz.  Bienenztg.,  76,  206. 
Todd,  F.  E.,  and  Bretherick,  O.  (1942).  J.  econ.  Enl,  35,  129. 
ViviNO,  E.,  and  Palmer,  L.  S.  (1944).   Arch.  Biochem.,  4,  129. 
VooGD,  S.  (1955).   Experientia,  40,  181. 
Wahl,  O.  (1956).  Sudwestdtsch.  Imker,  8,  348. 
Weaver,  N.,  and  Kuiken,  A.  (1951).  J.  econ.  Enl,  44,  635. 
Weyer,  F.  (1931).   Z.  Zellforsch.,  14,  1. 

Weygand,  F.,  and  HofxMann,  H.  (1950).   Chem.  Ber.,  83,  405. 
Wigglesworth,  V.  B.  (1950).  The  Principles  of  Insect  Physiology,  4th 

ed.  London:  Methuen. 
Zantder,  E.  (1947).   Das  Leben  der  Biene,  5th  ed.  Stuttgart:  Ulmer. 
Zander,  E.  (1951).   Der  Bau  der  Biene,  4th  ed.  Stuttgart:  Ulmer. 

DISCUSSION 

Wigglesworth:  Have  you  considered  the  further  complication  that 
building  up  of  these  reserves  may  not  be  a  direct  effect  of  the  various 
nutrients,  but  an  effect  through  the  endocrine  system?  For  example, 
it  has  long  been  known  that  in  many  adult  insects  deprived  of  the 
corpus  allatum  the  eggs  do  not  develop  and  do  not  build  up  yolk. 
There  is  the  same  effect  in  protein  starvation,  where  the  same 
histological  eff'ects  appear.  A.  S.  Johansson  (1958.  Nature  (Loud.), 
181,  198),  working  on  the  milkweed  bug,  showed  that  if  an  active 
corpus  allatimi  is  implanted  in  the  starved  insect  there  is  normal  egg 
development.  In  other  words,  the  immediate  effect  of  protein  feeding 
seems  to  be  to  activate  the  corpus  allatum.  The  prime  deficiency  in 
a  protein-starved  insect  is  inactivity  of  the  gland.  In  your  experi- 
ments it  would  not  necessarily  be  the  corpus  allatum;  it  might  be  the 
neurosecretory  cells  or  some  other  component  in  the  endocrine 
system. 

Hinton:  In  bees  doesn't  the  queen  substance  inhibit  the  develop- 
ment of  the  corpora  allata  and  keep  their  volume  small? 

Maurizio:  The  degree  of  development  of  the  ovaries  of  the  honey- 
bee worker  is  directly  connected  with  the  endocrine  system  and  the 
queen  substance  (see  references  to  Butler,  Pain  and  Voogd).   It  is 


244  Discussion 

possible  that  the  development  of  the  pharyngeal  glands  and  the  fat 
body  also  come  under  endocrine  influence  but,  as  far  as  I  know,  this 
has  not  yet  been  examined  closely  in  the  honey-bee. 

Rockstein:  Dr.  Maurizio  did  imply  in  her  paper  that  it  was  a 
matter  of  endocrine  control.  I  personally  think  that,  generally,  the 
ultimate  controlling  influence  will  be  found  to  lie  in  the  endocrine 
system.  I  do  not  think  that  Dr.  Maurizio  said  that  vitamins  or 
proteins  were  involved  in  this  physiological  senescence.  She  did 
indicate  that  there  was  a  depressed  development  of  the  pharyngeal 
gland,  which  suggests  hormone  control. 

Comfort:  Is  the  rate  of  loss  of  nerve  cells  different  in  summer  and 
winter  bees? 

Rockstein:  At  the  two  levels  of  the  brain  which  were  counted  there 
were  about  500  cells  in  the  young  bees  and  only  about  325  in  the  old 
bees,  regardless  of  whether  these  were  indoor  (summer)  bees  without 
a  queen,  or  outdoor  (summer)  bees  which  had  been  living  a  normal 
life  in  the  hive. 

Comfort:  In  other  words  the  difference  in  lifespan  does  not  depend 
on  the  difference  in  the  rate  of  loss  of  cells? 

Rockstein:  Yes,  it  does,  because  the  old  bees  in  both  conditions 
showed  precisely  the  same  degree  of  loss.  The  outdoor  bees  I  had 
were  only  seven  weeks  old,  whereas,  indoors,  I  had  bees  still  living 
at  ten  weeks  of  age.  For  one  thing,  my  bees  were  exposed  to  light 
24  hours  a  day.  According  to  Dr.  Maurizio  the  activity  may  have 
nothing  to  do  with  age,  but  nevertheless  I  could  not  keep  them  alive 
any  longer  under  these  conditions  even  on  a  completely  unlimited 
diet.  I  do  think  that  the  similarity  in  development  of  the  overwinter- 
ing or  the  summer-broodless  bees  is  very  significant.  It  points  to  an 
entirely  different  phenomenon  from  that  in  the  caged  bees — namely 
that  these  summer,  "queenright"  animals  normally  are  not  able  to 
attain  their  potential  longevity  owing  to  the  demands  of  brood- 
rearing  upon  their  nutrition.  As  soon  as  you  eliminate  this  really 
exorbitant  nutritional  dernand,  by  removing  the  queen,  the  bees  are 
able  to  attain  a  lifespan  of  something  like  six  to  eight  months.  As 
Prof.  Wigglesworth  suggested  and  Dr.  Maurizio  implied,  the  diet 
does  this,  but  where  it  produces  its  primary  effect  is  not  known. 
Most  probably  it  is  through  the  endocrine  system ;  certainly,  that  is 
evident  in  the  effect  upon  the  development  of  the  pharyngeal  glands 
and  of  the  fat  body,  which  must  be  controlled  at  a  biochemical  level. 
The  most  plausible  explanation  at  a  biochemical  level  is  that  of 
altering  hormone  control. 

Nigrelli:  What  would  happen  if  you  did  not  feed  the  caged  bees? 

Maurizio:  They  would  be  dead  of  starvation  in  two  or  three  days. 


Discussion  245 

Nigrelli:  Have  they  no  capacity  for  food  storage? 

Maurizio:  They  have  proteins  but  no  sugar. 

Wigglesworth:  As  I  understand  it,  the  queenless  worker  develops 
ovaries  but  the  winter  bee  does  not.  That  would  suggest  that  there 
is  a  more  subtle  difference  in  addition  to  the  direct  nutritional  effect. 

Maurizio:  The  queen  substance  is  there  in  the  overwintering 
colony. 

Wigglesowrth :  So  there  is  a  dual  effect. 

Hinton :  Do  the  workers  develop  ovaries  whatever  time  of  the  year 
you  remove  the  queen? 

Maurizio:  Yes. 

Kershaw:  There  is  evidence  in  blood-sucking  flies  that  they  take 
blood  under  hormonal  influence,  and  that  there  is  an  antibiting 
hormone  (Lavoipierre,  M.  M.  J.  (1958).  Nature  (Lond.),  182,  1567). 
This  may  be  a  similar  phenomenon. 

Maurizio :  We  will  probably  ask  Dr.  Butler  to  do  this  experiment 
with  the  queen  substance. 

Bourliere:  Have  you  made  any  determination  of  oxygen  consump- 
tion in  winter  bees  as  compared  with  summer  bees? 

Maurizio:  That  was  done  by  Corkins  and  Gilbert  (Corkins,  C.  L. 
and  Gilbert,  C.  S.  (1932).  Bull.  Wyo.  agric.  Exp.  Sta.,  No.  187,  1). 
They  found  that  carbon  dioxide  output  at  a  hive  temperature  of 
4°  was  52-62  per  cent  of  the  output  at  16°. 

Sacher:  Is  the  difference  between  summer  bees  and  the  over- 
wintering bees  in  the  colony  determined  directly  by  nutrition  and 
activity,  or  is  there  a  change  in  the  behaviour  of  the  colony  which  is 
caused  by  the  environment  and  which  in  turn  leads  to  these  differ- 
ences? 

Maurizio:  The  winter  bees  are  inactive;  there  is  no  brood,  they 
cannot  fly  and  they  stay  in  the  colony  in  a  cluster.  The  only  function 
is  to  keep  warm  in  the  cluster. 

Sacher:  Then  the  fact  that  it  gets  cold  and  they  cluster  in  this 
fashion  and  cease  their  brood  rearing  is  what  leads  to  this  change  in 
survival. 

Maurizio:  But  the  bees  begin  to  brood  in  February  in  Switzerland. 
It  depends  on  the  climatic  conditions. 

Hinton:  There  must  be  some  indirect  effects  here  because  there 
are  many  long  periods  in  the  winter  which  are  just  as  warm. 

Maurizio:  In  Northern  Europe  bees  are  unable  to  forage  for 
four  or  five  months  in  winter,  and  the  brood-interval  is  much  longer 
than  in  England  and  Central  Europe. 

Rockstein:  There  may,  however,  very  well  be  a  light-dependent 
factor,  such  as  a  diurnal  or  photoperiod  effect.    Thirty  minutes 


246  Discussion 

before  sundown,  for  example,  most  foraging  bees  will  start  to  come 
back  to  the  hive,  even  on  a  hot  summer  day,  so  it  is  not  a  matter  of 
temperature. 

Holt:  What  is  the  mode  of  life  of  tropical  bees? 

Maurizio:  I  think  that  in  the  tropics  there  are  no  winter  bees, 
because  they  can  fly  all  the  year.  They  have  pollen  and  nectar, 
they  seem  to  have  brood  the  whole  year  and  there  are  no  long-lived 
bees  in  the  colony.  But  a  proper  study  of  bees  in  the  tropics  has 
yet  to  be  made. 

Hinton:  That  would  be  primarily  an  adjustment  to  food  supplies 
and  not  necessarily  to  temperature. 

Maurizio:  It  is  complex.  In  the  tropics  there  may  be  no  over- 
wintering, but  during  the  two  or  three  months  of  the  dry  season 
there  may  be  long-lived  bees  in  the  colony. 

Rocksiein:  There  is  probably  more  than  one  factor,  such  as  light 
or  temperature,  that  would  produce  the  end-result  of  conserving  the 
colony.  In  Minnesota,  bees  begin  brood-rearing  in  January  when 
temperatures  are  sometimes  at  —30°  f.  These  animals  are  being 
continually  decimated  during  the  winter  months  because  some  of 
them  have  emerged  in  August,  some  in  September  and  some  in 
October,  and  we  are  going  to  end  up  with  a  very  small  nucleus  from 
the  last-laid  eggs  by  the  end  of  winter.  There  appears  then  to  be  a 
social  pressure  upon  the  hive  which  stimulates  brood-rearing  late  in 
the  winter.  Somehow,  the  queen  is  aware  of  this,  even  in  the  dead  of 
winter.  As  far  as  light  is  concerned,  the  shortest  day  is  in  mid- 
January  in  Minnesota,  but  the  queen  begins  to  lay  eggs  again  at  that 
time. 

Hinton:  What  is  the  temperature  in  the  cluster  in  winter  in 
Minnesota? 

Rockstein:  The  centre  of  the  cluster  is  always  maintained  at  a 
temperature  of  about  33°  c.  This  has  been  established  by  thermo- 
couple measurements.  The  bees  on  the  outside  of  the  cluster  change 
places  with  bees  from  the  inside;  otherwise  those  on  the  outside 
would  fall  torpid  from  cold.  These  outside  bees  must  feed,  generate 
heat,  enter  and  allow  the  inner  bees  to  move  out.  If  the  cluster  gets 
too  far  away  from  the  food  stores,  the  whole  cluster  will  die,  with  the 
queen  the  last  one  to  go. 

Hinton:  Thus  social  insects  can  be  said  to  be  homoio thermic. 

Rockstein:  I  would  rather  say  that  the  colony  as  a  whole  can 
be  said  to  be  homoiothermic. 


THE  BIOLOGY  OF  AGEING  IN  INSECTS 

Morris  Rockstein 

Department  of  Physiology,  New  York  University  College  of 
Medicine t  New  York 

For  well  over  a  decade  the  present  author's  interest  in 
ageing  has  been  that  of  establishing  firm,  quantitative  bio- 
chemical criteria  for  physiological  ageing,  particularly  in 
structures  manifesting  senescence  in  the  form  of  declining 
function  as  well  as  degenerative  anatomical  alterations  with 
advancing  age.  As  a  direct  consequence  of  these  physiological 
studies,  longevity  data  have  been  obtained  for  large  numbers 
of  individuals  for  each  of  two  species  of  insects  and  for  strains 
of  similar  genetic  constitution  in  each  case.  This  discussion 
will  present  details  of  the  lifespan  of  the  common  housefly, 
Musca  domestica  L.,  and  the  honey-bee,  Apis  mellifera  L., 
with  special  emphasis  upon  recent  studies  directly  concerned 
with  the  factors  affecting  the  lifespan  of  male  and  female 
houseflies. 

In  an  earlier  series  of  experiments,  involving  a  comparison 
of  change  in  cell  number  with  alteration  in  cholinesterase 
activity  with  age,  in  the  adult  worker  honey-bee  (Rockstein, 
1950a),  the  time  of  final  samplings  of  living  animals  was 
determined  by  the  availability  of  sufficient  numbers  of  bees 
for  enzymological  and  histological  evaluation.  In  all  cases 
this  occurred  when  one  per  cent  or  less  of  the  original  number 
of  bees  employed  were  still  living,  and  represented  values  very 
close  to  the  maximum  lifespan  for  this  strain  of  Italian  golden 
honey-bees.  Thus,  for  3,750  queenless  worker  bees,  maintained 
indoors  in  large  cages  supplied  with  honey,  pollen  and  water 
in  excess  and  exposed  to  artificial  lighting  day  and  night,  the 
final  sampling  was  made  at  68  days.   A  similar  value  of  ten 

247 


248  Morris  Rockstein 

weeks  was  obtained  for  approximately  4,000  of  the  same  strain 
of  honey-bee  maintained  indoors,  in  a  more  recent  study 
involving  changes  in  alkaline  and  acid  phosphatase  in  ageing 
bees  (Rockstein,  1953).  For  a  similar  study  of  2,700  "queen- 
right"  bees,  marked  with  coloured  lacquer  immediately  after 
emergence  and  returned  to  the  hive  to  engage  in  normal  hive 
activities  (during  the  summer  months),  the  last  sample  of 
bees  had  to  be  taken  at  51  days  of  age.  These  values  compare 
well  with  observations  of  apiculturists  and  other  students  of 
the  biology  of  the  honey-bee  (see  Rockstein,  1950fc).  Indeed, 
Dr.  Maurizio's  own  studies  (1954)  include  data  for  two  strains 
of  Italian  bees  of  maximum  lifespans  of  54  and  62  days, 
respectively,  maintained  in  the  hive  during  summer  months. 

In  the  earlier  studies  (Rockstein,  1950a),  the  number  of 
neurones  at  two  distinct  levels  of  the  honey-bee  brain  was  em- 
ployed as  an  anatomical  criterion  for  biological  old  age;  the 
absolute  number  of  cells  remaining  in  the  brains  of  old  bees 
(as  well  as  the  percentage  loss  from  emergence  to  old  age) 
was  remarkably  similar  (325  ;  350)  for  both  kinds  of  old  bees, 
whether  they  were  living  the  normal  lives  of  the  hive  bee  or 
were  maintained  indoors  in  a  small  queenless  colony  under  the 
conditions  described.  This  loss  of  about  35  per  cent  of  the 
original  number  of  brain  cells  in  the  adult  worker  bee  is 
singularly  similar  to  that  of  a  35  to  40  per  cent  loss  in  mam- 
malian brain  cell  number  reported  for  humans  by  Hodge 
(1894),  Ellis  (1919,  1920),  Andrew  (1938)  and  Gardner  (1940) 
and  for  the  white  rat  by  Hatai  (1902)  and  by  Inukai  (1928). 

From  a  recent  study  of  the  decline  with  age  in  the  activity 
of  enzymes  concerned  with  the  energizing  of  flight  activity  in 
the  common  housefly,  from  emergence  to  senility,  longevity 
data  have  been  obtained  for  thousands  of  male  and  female 
flies  of  the  NAIDM  standard  laboratory  strain  of  houseflies, 
which  had  been  intensively  inbred  for  more  than  one  hundred 
generations.  In  our  laboratory  these  animals  are  reared  and 
maintained  on  a  standardized  laboratory  diet  in  an  air- 
conditioned  room  kept  at  80°  f  and  45   per  cent  relative 


The  Biology  of  Ageing  in  Insects  249 

humidity.  Except  for  specific  experiments  designed  to  test 
the  role  of  parental  age  in  determining  the  longevity  of  the 
offspring,  all  flies  were  reared  from  eggs  laid  by  parents  as 
soon  as  they  were  capable  of  oviposition,  i.e.  between  the 
fourth  and  fifth  days  of  adult  age.  A  well-regimented  strain 
under  the  conditions  of  rearing  and  maintenance  described, 
these  flies  emerge  as  adults  exactly  two  weeks  following  the 
emergence  of  adults  of  the  previous  generation  and  young 
females  begin  laying  eggs  on  exactly  the  fourth  day  after  they 
have  reached  the  imaginal  state. 

Longevity  and  diet 

During  the  course  of  collecting  adult  male  and  female  house- 
flies  for  biochemical  study  (Rockstein,  1956),  it  was  observed 
that  there  were  relatively  fewer  and  fewer  males  available  for 
enzyme  determinations,  especially  by  the  end  of  the  second 
week.  Thus,  from  a  sex  ratio  of  one  to  one,  the  male  to  female 
population  composition  fell  to  a  one  to  two  ratio  by  the  end 
of  two  weeks  and  to  a  less  than  one  to  three  ratio  by  the  end 
of  the  third  week.  A  pilot  follow-up  study  was  made  of  six 
cages  of  about  125  flies  each  in  which  mortality  (rather  than 
survival)  counts  were  made;  the  results  (Rockstein,  1957) 
clearly  confirmed  the  fact  that  male  longevity  was  consider- 
ably smaller  than  that  of  the  female  housefly.  However,  in 
both  of  these  cases,  although  the  larvae  were  reared  on  a 
standard  laboratory  medium  of  powdered  whole  bovine  milk 
(KLIM,  Borden's)  dried  brewer's  yeast  and  agar,  the  adults 
had  been  maintained  on  sucrose  and  water  alone  (in  order  to 
eliminate  extraneous  factors  of  diet  and  egg-production  in  the 
females  particularly,  in  the  age-dependent  enzyme  study). 
Under  such  conditions  of  restricted  diet,  it  was  thought  that 
the  observed  sex-related  diff'erential  in  longevity,  favouring 
the  female  of  this  species,  might  have  resulted  from  the  pos- 
sible availability  of  nutrient  reserves  (such  as  oocytes  or  fully 
developed  ova  in  the  ovarioles  of  the  young  female  at  emerg- 
ence) to  individuals  of  that  sex  exclusively.   Figs,  la  and  lb 


250 


Morris  Rockstein 


graphically  illustrate  the  data  obtained  for  two  sets  of  ex- 
periments involving  a  total  of  600  males  and  600  females 
maintained  as  adults  on  sucrose  and  water,  as  before,  and  600 
males  and  600  females  maintained  on  a  diet  of  sucrose,  water 
and  powdered  whole  milk.  (The  latter  is  the  normal  diet  for 
stock  breeding  adult  houseflies.)   As  is  clearly  seen  in  Fig.  la, 


00 

— 

90 

- 

°    • 

80 

• 

70 

— 

60 

c 

• 

50 

.  <« 

40 

30 

- 

•  KUM-CASEI 

^ 

•% 

•  KUM-  CASe  a 

20 

~           ,m 

oSUGAR-CASei 

o 

'SUGAR' CASE n 

10 

• 

•o 

1               1               1           _L 

o 

o 
q: 


0        10      20      30      40      50 
AGE  IN  DAYS 

Fig.  la.  The  effect  of  diet  on 
male  houseflies. 

Reprinted  from  Rockstein  and  Liebennan  (1959),  by  courtesy  the  Editor,  Gerontologia. 

no  appreciable  advantages  accrued  to  the  longevity  of  the 
male  houseflies  from  inclusion  of  this  (high  protein,  lactose, 
butter-fat  and  mineral-containing)  adult  dietary  component. 
On  the  other  hand,  female  flies,  with  a  greater  longevity  than 
males  even  under  conditions  of  restricted  diet  (sucrose  and 
water  alone),  showed  a  considerable  prolongation  of  lifespan 
as  a  result  of  including  KLIM  in  the  adult  food,  beginning  with 


The  Biology  of  Ageing  in  Insects 


251 


the  second  week  of  life  (Fig.  lb).  As  can  be  seen  from  Table  I 
(reprinted  from  Rockstein,  1957),  the  mean  longevity  for  a 
total  of  600  female  flies  maintained  on  the  enriched  diet  was 
about  31  days,  in  contrast  to  a  mean  value  of  about  19-5  days 
for  females  maintained  on  sugar  and  water  alone.  Moreover, 
the  maximum  longevity  for  females  on  the  KLIM-enriched 


CO 
Uj 

—I 
< 

Uj 
U. 

>■ 

o 

Uj 
U 

q: 


00 

Q 

o** 

.•••• 

90 

o 

O   ° 

• 

•• 

80 

~                                 o 

,    • 

o  ° 

• 

70 

o 

• 

• 

60 

_                             •                      •• 

*• 

50 

-                                          o                               *    • 
O                                    * 

40 

—                                                                        • 
*• 

30 

•    • 
—                                                       •         • 

•    • 

•KLIM-CASE  I 

,.' 

'  KLIM-CASEU 

20 

"^  y 

°  SUGAR-CASE  I 

°  v» 

•  SUGAR-CASE  IC 

'•°. 

10 
0 

10 


20       30      40      50 
AGE    IN    DAYS 


60       70 


Fig.  lb.  The  effect  of  diet  on  female  house- 
flies. 

Reprinted  from  Rockstein  and  Liebennan  (1959),  by  courtesy  the  Editor,  Gerontologia. 


diet  was  about  30  per  cent  higher  than  for  females  on  the 
restricted  diet. 

These  results  are  in  direct  contrast  to  the  well-known 
findings  of  McCay  and  his  co-workers  (1935,  1939,  1941)  that 
restricting  the  dietary  intake  immediately  or  soon  after  weaning 
retarded  the  growth  of  male  rats  and  concomitantly  extended 
the  average  lifespan  from  483  to  894  days  (and  maximum 


252 


Morris  Rockstein 


longevities  from  927  to  1,306  days).  Female  longevities,  on  the 
contrary,  were  relatively  unaffected  by  such  alterations  in 
diet.  It  would  appear  from  their  results  that  retardation  of 
growth  in  the  male  rat  by  such  a  dietary  restriction  eliminates 
the  sex  differential  in  longevity,  normally  favouring  the 
female  rat.  However,  McCay  emphasized  the  low  calorie 
aspect  of  his  restricted  diet,  which  was  in  reality  a  high 
protein,  salt  and  vitamin  diet.    It  is  therefore  likely  that  the 

Table  I 

Effect  of  diet  on  longevity  of  male 
and  female  house  flies 


WUh  KLIM 

Sugar  and 
water  only 

With  KLIM 

Sugar  and 
water  only 

Males 

Fe- 
males 

Males 

Fe- 
males 

Males 

Fe- 
males 

Males 

Fe- 
males 

Average 
longevity 
(days) 

17-5 

32-7 

15-6 

19-5 

14-4 

30-8 

14-6 

19-7 

Maximum 
longevity 
(days) 

44 

64 

40 

50 

40 

56 

29 

35 

critical  factor  involved  in  such  a  restricted  diet  might  well  have 
been  the  effective  protein  content  of  the  diet.  Indeed,  the 
ingestion  of  such  a  "low  calorie"  diet  might  mean  the  utiliza- 
tion of  the  protein  in  this  diet  to  meet  the  basal  energy 
requirements  (in  the  absence  of  carbohydrates  and  fats 
customarily  utilized  by  the  body  in  energizing  processes). 
There  would  result  an  unavailability  of  protein  required  for 
normal  cell  growth,  replacement  and  addition  and,  therefore, 
retardation  of  normal  body  growth  and  maturation.  This  (and 
evidence  to  follow)  suggests  that  there  may  exist  for  each 
species  a  specific  protein-calorie  optimum  for  normal  growth 


The  Biology  of  Ageing  in  Insects 


253 


and  maturation.  Indeed,  an  early  paper  by  McCay  and  Crowell 
(1934)  reported  that  reduction  in  the  protein  content  of  the  diet 
fed  to  trout  doubled  their  longevity. 

In  insects  too,  there  appears  to  be  an  optimal  dietary  level 
of  protein  for  growth  and  development  and  indirectly  for 
lifespan.  Thus  in  three  species  of  cockroaches  (all  of  which  are 
not  fully  grown  or  mature  at  the  onset  of  the  final  adult 
stage),  Haydak  (1953)  reported  specific  optimum  dietary 
protein  requirements  both  for  development  and  survival  of 

Table  II 

Effect  of  diet  on  the  development  and  longevity  of  three 
species  of  cockroaches  (after  haydak,  1953) 


Effect  of  diet 

upon : 

Optimum  protein  content  of  diet 

P.  americana 

B.  orientalis 

B.  germanica 

Minimal  nymphal  mortality : 
Shortest  development  period 

49%-79% 

22%-24% 

22%-24% 

Greatest  average  adult 
longevity 

22% 

11% 

11% 

the  nymphal  instars  as  well  as  for  longevity  of  the  adult. 
Table  II,  prepared  from  Haydak's  data,  shows  that  for  the 
American  cockroach  the  optimum  protein  for  shortest  nymphal 
development  and  associated  lowest  nymphal  mortality  was  a 
broad  range  of  49-79  per  cent;  for  either  the  Oriental  or 
German  cockroach,  this  was  a  much  lower  22-24  per  cent. 
For  maximum  adult  longevity,  on  the  other  hand,  the 
optimum  protein  content  of  the  diet  was  22-24  per  cent  for 
the  adult  diet  of  the  American  cockroach  and  11  per  cent  for 
both  the  Oriental  and  German  cockroaches.  Haydak  con- 
cluded from  his  data  that  for  adults  the  heaviest  protein 


254  Morris  Rockstein 

eaters  had  the  shortest  lifespans  and  that  the  total  lifespan 
was  inversely  proportional  to  the  protein  intake  from  eclosion 
to  death. 

Maurizio's  work  has  also  demonstrated  the  importance 
of  protein  (and  possibly  vitamins)  in  the  longevity  of  adult 
worker  honey-bees  (1954).  She  has  shown  that  captive 
worker  honey-bees  which  are  fed  pollen  early  in  their  adult- 
hood resemble  overwintering  bees  in  two  major  respects; 
they  are  longer-lived  than  similar  bees  rearing  brood  and, 
secondly,  their  "physiological  state",  i.e.  well-developed  fat 
body  and  pharyngeal  glands  maintained  for  a  longer  time,  is 
typical  of  overwintering  bees  (which  may  live  as  long  as  six 
to  eight  months  in  contrast  to  six  to  eight  weeks  for  maximum 
summertime  longevities).  Thus,  retardation  of  ageing  in  the 
worker  bee  is  associated  with  adequate  pollen  (protein  and 
vitamin)  intake  at  an  early  adult  age,  in  the  face  of  reduced 
demands  upon  such  food  reserves,  such  as  occurs  in  over- 
wintering or  non-brooding  "summer  bees".  This  in  turn 
results  in  slow  build-up  and  extended  maintenance  of  those 
structures  upon  which  the  prolongation  of  adult  life  is 
significantly  dependent. 

In  all  the  cases  mentioned  above  it  appears  that,  where 
growth  or  maturation  has  not  yet  been  completed,  an  optimum 
level  of  food  (perhaps  protein,  specifically)  intake  is  necessary 
to  complete  that  development.  Restricting  the  diet,  as  in  the 
case  of  the  young  male  white  rat,  trout  and  at  least  three  species 
of  cockroaches,  delays  the  attainment  of  adult  form  and  con- 
comitantly defers  the  cessation  of  growth  and  therefore 
senescence.  In  the  case  of  holometabolous  insects,  like  the 
housefly  or  the  honey-bee,  the  situation  is  quite  different. 
These  animals  are  essentially  fully  matured  within  a  few 
hours  to  several  days,  respectively.  Heavy  demands  upon  the 
food  (protein)  reserves  of  the  female  such  as  oviposition  in 
the  case  of  the  housefly  (Rockstein,  1958)  and  brood-rearing 
in  the  case  of  the  worker  bee  shortens  the  lifespan  in  these 
animals.     One   can  perhaps  speak   of  a  "negative   protein 


The  Biology  of  Ageing  in  Insects  255 

balance"  operating  against  the  attainment  of  the  longevity 
potential  of  a  particular  sex  for  a  particular  species,  when  the 
diet  is  inadequate  for  the  physiological  needs  of  such  animals. 
Rockstein  (1958)  suggested  that  food  reserve  in  the  ovaries 
might  indeed  be  a  factor  important  in  higher  female  longevity 
even  during  starvation.  In  this  connexion,  a  paper  by  Grosch 
(1950)  has  shown  that  starving  female  wasps,  Habrohracon 
juglandis  (Ashmead),  draw  upon  the  reserves  of  the  ovarioles, 
as  evidenced  by  the  gradual  resorption  of  ova  from  the  ovari- 
oles which  is  especially  marked  during  the  latter  days  of  their 
lives.  Woke,  Ally  and  Rosenberger  (1956)  further  support 
this  idea  of  the  ovaries  as  a  source  of  nutrition  in  starvation 
for  the  female  mosquito,  Aedes  aegypti  L.,  in  the  observations 
that  delaying  the  first  blood  meal  or  decreasing  the  size 
thereof  lowers  total  egg  production  markedly.  In  the  Levant 
housefly,  Musca  vicina  Macq.,  Ascher  and  Levinson  (1956) 
have  also  found  protein  essential  to  the  adult  diet  for  ovi- 
position  and  cited  similar  evidence  for  other  species  of  muscoid 
adults.  However,  the  common  housefly  does  lay  eggs,  albeit 
much  more  spottily,  even  on  a  protein-free  diet,  but  no 
information  is  available  as  to  the  number  and  viability  of  such 
eggs. 

V 

Longevity  differences  and  sex 

In  order  to  obtain  further  insight  into  the  differences 
between  male  and  female  longevities  in  the  housefly,  a  study 
on  a  much  larger  scale  was  undertaken.  Fig.  2  (taken  from 
Rockstein  and  Lieberman,  1958)  shows  the  survival  curves 
for  about  8,500  flies  of  both  sexes  of  the  same  NAIDM 
strain  extending  over  nine  generations,  reared  and  maintained 
on  a  complete  diet  under  the  controlled  laboratory  conditions 
described  earlier.  Both  survival  curves  show  a  rectangular 
character  typical  of  animal  populations  manifesting  sene- 
scence, namely,  a  very  low  mortality  rate  during  the  early 
days  of  the  cohort's  existence,  and  a  middle  period  of  rapid 
dying  off.  However,  during  the  final  five  to  ten  per  cent  of  the 


256 


Morris  Rockstein 


population's  existence  the  survival  curves  for  both  sexes 
show  a  logarithmic-like  character  (in  the  extremely  retarded 
rate  of  dying  off  of  this  small  proportion  of  long-lived  members 
of  the  population).  In  actual  fact,  for  the  males  about  90  per 
cent  die  off  during  the  period  of  ten  to  thirty  days  after 


100 

90 

.    80 

70 

I      60 

a. 

^     50 

I- 

a  40 

a. 

UJ 

^     30 
20 


r\. 


•    MALES 
o   FEMALES 


AGE  IN  DAYS 
Fig.   2.     Survival   curves   of  male   and  female 
houseflies. 

Reprinted  from  Rockstein  and  Lieberman  (1958),  by  courtesy  of  the  Editor,  Nature, 


"senescence" 


emergence  as  adults;  for  females  this  period  of 

occurs  during  the  longer  period  of  ten  to  fifty  days  of  adult 

Ufe. 

One  striking  feature  of  the  particular  colony  studied  is 
the  remarkable  homogeneity  of  the  population,  as  regards 
longevity  at  least.   Thus,  both  for  the  total  600  flies  per  sex, 


The  Biology  of  Ageing  in  Insects 


257 


from  the  earlier  study  on  diet  (Rockstein,  1957),  and  the  more 
recent  total  of  over  4,000  flies  per  sex,  fifty  per  cent  mortality 
occurred  at  16  days  for  the  males  and  at  30  days  for  the 
females,  respectively.  The  mean  longevity  values  for  males  of 
about  17  days  and  for  females  of  about  29  days,  for  this  large 


0.01  I — 


0.05 
0.1 
02 

0.5 
I 

2 


95 

98 
99 

99.5 

99.8 
99.9 


•  MALES 
o  FEMALES 


/ 


o» 


J I I  I  I  1 1 II 


J L 


2  3       4     5    6  78910  20        30    40  50  6070    80 

LOG    TIME 

Fig.  3.  Log-probit  plot  for  male  and  female  housefly 
survival. 

Reprinted  from  Rockstein  and  Lieberman  (1959),  by  courtesy  of  the  Editor,  Nature. 


number  of  flies,  also  compare  favourably  with  those  reported 
in  1957.  The  close  approximation  of  mean  and  medium 
longevity  values  is  also  a  strong  indication  of  a  good  natural 
distribution  in  so  far  as  the  populations  of  the  two  sexes  in 
this  strain  are  concerned. 

A  log-probit  plot  of  these  data  (probability  of  survival 

AGEING — V — 9 


258  Morris  Rockstein 

plotted  against  time),  shown  in  Fig.  3  (Rockstein  and  Lieber- 
man,  1959),  portrays  in  more  dramatic  fashion  the  actual 
course  of  the  survival  (or,  conversely,  the  mortality)  trends 
of  Fig.  2,  above.  From  the  similar  flat  slopes  of  the  log-probit 
curves  of  both  males  and  females  during  the  first  ten  days,  a 
similar  low  mortality  rate  is  apparent.  After  the  tenth  day, 
however,  a  marked  shift  upward  of  the  male  curve,  which  con- 
tinues as  a  straight  line  through  the  fortieth  day,  represents  a 
high  and  uniform  mortality  rate  during  93  per  cent  of  the  male 
population's  existence.  Thus  a  single  (or  at  least  relatively 
simple)  mortality  factor  appears  to  be  operating  in  the  great 
majority  of  the  male  houseflies  of  this  strain.  In  the  females, 
however,  one  sees  that  the  same  log-probit  curve  follows  a 
more  irregular  course,  with  a  slight  upward  trend  at  the  tenth 
to  21st  days,  a  steep  shift  upward  at  the  21st  day,  another  more 
gentle  upward  rise  in  slope  at  the  30th,  and  a  final  shift  upward 
at  the  40th  day,  which  persists  unchanged  as  a  steep,  straight 
line  from  the  fortieth  to  final  day  of  the  female  population's 
existence.  One  might  infer  from  this  kind  of  plot  that,  for  the 
female,  mortality  (and  therefore  survival)  is  a  much  more 
complex  phenomenon,  with  more  factors  for  mortality  becom- 
ing effective  w4th  increasing  age.  These  inferences  have  been 
further  substantiated  in  the  life-tables  which  have  been  recently 
completed  by  Rockstein  and  Lieberman  (1959)  from  mortality 
data  for  this  population  of  male  and  female  flies.  Despite  the 
wide  distribution  of  the  ubiquitous  housefly,  only  a  few  other 
studies  have  been  made  on  its  longevity.  The  study  of  Wilkes 
and  co-workers  (1948)  employed  the  Peet-Grady  method  (in 
which  some  of  the  dietary  components  are  not  standard)  and 
obtained  average  longevities  of  12  days  for  males  and  20 
days  for  females  of  a  laboratory  strain  and  about  12  days 
and  24  days,  respectively,  for  male  and  female  flies  of  a 
wild  strain,  kept  at  80°  f  and  50  per  cent  relative  humidity. 
Aside  from  dietary  variation,  the  authors  also  describe  major 
difficulties  of  overcrowding,  especially  in  the  larval  stage  and 
also  for  adults.   In  a  recent  letter,  Rollins  (1959)  has  supplied 


The  Biology  of  Ageing  in  Insects  259 

some  interesting  data  on  differences  in  longevity  between  the 
sexes  for  over  2,000  males  and  females  of  a  15-year  inbred 
strain  of  normal  houseflies,  originating  from  a  wild  strain 
collected  in  the  Sacramento,  California,  area,  and  reared  and 
maintained  on  a  Peet-Grady  medium  at  80°  f.  For  male 
houseflies,  he  obtained  a  50  per  cent  mortality  at  11  days  (as 
opposed  to  our  values  of  16  days)  and  for  females  at  29  •  5  days 
(very  much  like  our  own  data  for  females).  Thirty-day 
mortalities  of  98  per  cent  for  males  and  52  per  cent  for  females 
corresponded  very  closely  to  those  obtained  for  our  own 
NAIDM  strain.  For  the  Levant  house-fly,  Musca  vicina 
Macq.,  however,  Feldman-Muhsam  (1944)  and  Ascher  and 
Levinson  (1956)  found  no  significant  difference  in  longevity 
between  the  two  sexes. 

Parental  age 

In  an  attempt  to  establish  the  possible  effect  of  parental 
age  of  houseflies  at  oviposition  upon  the  longevity  of  the 
offspring,  a  preliminary  investigation  was  made  according  to 
the  procedure  of  Lansing  (1947,  1948,  1954).  For  standard 
breeding  and  in  all  previous  studies,  eggs  were  collected  from 
parents  at  the  youngest  possible  age  (at  the  fourth  to  fifth 
day  after  emergence).  In  this  series  of  experiments,  however, 
225  eggs  each  were  collected  at  the  fourth,  sixth,  ninth,  15th, 
23rd  and  27th  days  and  the  offspring  otherwise  reared  and 
maintained  as  before  on  complete  diets.  Table  III  shows  that 
the  female  offspring  longevity  is  progressively  diminished 
with  advancing  age  of  the  parents  at  the  time  of  oviposition ; 
e.g.  survival,  as  expressed  as  30-day  mortality  for  female 
offspring,  falls  from  50  per  cent  mortality  for  eggs  from  young 
parents,  to  92  per  cent  mortality  for  eggs  laid  at  27  days  of 
parental  life.  The  30-day  mortality  data  for  male  offspring, 
on  the  contrary,  were  interpreted  as  meaning  that  male 
offspring  longevity  was  unaffected  by  increasing  parental  age. 
Values  also  included  in  Table  III  show  corresponding  declines 
in  average  longevities  for  females  from  about  32  days  for 


260 


Morris  Rockstein 


young  parents  to  22  days  for  females  from  very  old  parent 
flies.  In  the  case  of  Lansing's  parthenogenetically  reproducing 
rotifers,  his  "cumulative  reversible  aging  factor"  could  be 
directly  attributable  to  cessation  of  growth  in  the  old  female 
parent.  In  the  present  study,  however,  several  interpreta- 
tions were  possible  for  the  sexually  reproducing  housefly; 

Table  III 

Effect  of  parental  age  of  houseflies  on 
longevity  of  offspring 


Parental 
age  (daifs) 

Males 

Females 

%  Mortality 

%  Mortality 

Average  longevity 

4 

95 

50 

32  days 

6 

93 

52 

29  days 

9 

97 

70 

28  days 

15 

92 

82 

25  days 

23 

97 

80 

24  days 

27 

90 

92 

22  days 

either  or  both  of  the  old  parents  might  be  contributing  to  the 
reduced  female  offspring  longevity  which  is  associated  with 
the  advancing  age  of  the  parents  at  oviposition.  A  second 
possibility  is  that  the  observed  data  represent  the  adverse 
effect  of  long-term  storage  of  spermatozoa  in  the  female 
spermatheca.  With  Dr.  Lieberman,  an  expanded  study  of  the 
possible  r61e  of  parental  age  in  the  longevity  of  the  housefly 


The  Biology  of  Ageing  in  Insects  261 

was  undertaken  last  year,  with  four  types  of  matings  as 
follows:  young  males  by  old  (about  29  days)  virgin  females, 
old  (about  23  days)  males  by  young  virgin  females,  old  males 
by  old  virgin  females,  and  "modified  old-old  crosses"  (in 
which,  as  in  the  original  study  of  parental  age,  males  and 
females  were  allowed  to  mate  freely  from  emergence,  but 
where  eggs  were  collected  only  from  parents  at  an  advanced 
age).  A  fifth  set  of  cages  involved  the  usual  young-by-young 
matings  employed  in  routine  stock  breeding  and  in  other 
studies  in  which  parental  age  was  not  a  variable ;  this  was  the 
control  series.  Offspring  longevity  data  were  obtained  for 
over  1,000  flies  of  each  sex  for  each  type  of  cross,  for  three  to 
four  generations  in  each  case,  in  order  to  test  the  possible 
presence  of  a  Lansing-like,  cumulative  ageing  factor  related  to 
parental  age,  in  the  housefly.  The  results  obtained  indicate 
that  the  slightly  lowered  30-day  mortality  for  males  from 
oldest  parents,  seen  in  our  earlier  study  (Table  III),  might 
indeed  have  been  significant.  Statistical  analysis  of  data  from 
our  current  study  indicates  that,  for  two  generations,  the 
average  male  longevity  (20-6  days)  for  offspring  from  "  modified 
old-old  parents"  is  higher,  by  three  days,  than  the  mean 
longevity  for  young-by-young  crosses  (P  is  less  than  0-01). 
That  this  result  may  be  due  to  the  effect  of  the  female  parent 
upon  male  offspring  is  supported  by  the  observation  that  a 
similar  increase  in  male  offspring  longevity  was  obtained  for 
crosses  between  young  males  and  old  virgin  feynales  and  for 
two  cases  of  crosses  between  old  males  and  old  virgin  females. 
Thus,  the  mechanism  involved  may  be  one  of  selection  of  a 
long-lived  strain  of  male  offspring  from  the  longer-lived 
female  parents  surviving  to  almost  30  days  of  age  and  still 
sexually  functional  at  that  advanced  age.  As  for  the  effects  of 
parental  age  on  female  offspring  longevity,  data  obtained  in  this 
more  recent  and  extensive  study  suggest  that  the  parental 
ageing  effect  on  this  sex  is  more  complex:  it  is  considered 
essential  at  this  point  to  continue  these  studies  with  single- 
pair  crosses  in  order  to  follow  the  longevity  of  the  adults  as 


262  Morris  Rockstein 

well  as  of  their  offspring  on  an  individual,  rather  than  on  the 
group  population  basis  employed  in  all  studies  to  date. 

In  Drosophila  suhohscura  Comfort  (1953)  found  no  apprecia- 
able  change  in  longevity  of  the  population  resulting  from 
selection  of  eggs  from  old  parents,  over  eight  generations  of 
breeding.  Tracey  (1958),  on  the  other  hand,  found  that  larvae 
of  eggs  from  week-old  adult  beetles  of  the  mealworm,  Tenebrio 
molitor,  had  longer  developmental  periods  than  those  from 
eggs  laid  by  five-week-old  (middle-aged)  beetles  and  consider- 
ably longer  than  those  from  eggs  laid  by  very  old  (nine  weeks) 
parents.  The  adult  lifespan  was  likewise  shortened  for  beetles 
reared  from  eggs  laid  by  oldest  parents.  Thus,  as  the  parental 
age  lengthened,  the  entire  lifespan  of  offspring  was  shortened 
in  this  species.  Although  the  above  held  true  for  animals  reared 
and  maintained  at  25°c,  at  30°  c  some  larvae  from  oldest 
parents  had  much  longer  larval  development  periods  than  those 
from  younger  parents  ( ! ).  However,  increasing  the  temperature 
to  30°  c  did  not  alter  the  shorter  adult  lifespan  observed  for 
offspring  from  oldest  parents. 

What  role  the  age  of  parents  plays  in  the  lifespan  of  children 
in  humans  is  not  statistically  clear,  although  Murphy  (1954) 
reported  that  known  abnormalities  like  congenital  cardiac  im- 
pairment, Mongolian  idiocy,  spina  bifida  and  stillbirth,  and 
possibly  hairlip  and  cleft  palate,  increase  in  frequency  as  the 
age  of  the  mother  at  the  time  of  birth  increases,  after  30  years 
of  age.  However,  Sonneborn  (1957)  has  pointed  out  that  the 
age  of  the  mother  is  frequently  positively  correlated  with  the 
paternal  age,  for  foetal  deaths.  Indeed,  for  data  covering 
foetal  deaths  and  a  10  per  cent  randomized  sample  of  330,000 
live  births  from  1954-55  New  York  City  Department  of 
Statistics,  he  found  there  was  a  consistently  higher  foetal  death 
rate  for  older  fathers,  when  single  rather  than  five-year  age 
classes  were  employed  for  females  over  30  years  of  age. 


The  Biology  of  Ageing  in  Insects  263 

Summary 

Queenless,  Italian  golden  worker  honey-bees,  Apis  mellifera, 
maintained  indoors  on  an  excess  of  honey,  pollen  and  water, 
lived  to  a  maximum  of  about  9  •  5  weeks ;  in  a  second  study  of 
over  4,000  bees  a  similar  maximum  value  of  ten  weeks  was 
obtained.  A  maximum  longevity  of  7  •  5  weeks  was  found  for 
over  2,700  bees,  properly  marked  and  returned  to  the  hive  to 
perform  normal  hive  activities.  As  a  criterion  of  old  age,  the 
number  of  brain  cells  at  two  distinct  levels  of  the  adult  honey- 
bee brain  was  found  to  be  virtually  identical  for  both  indoor  and 
outdoor  (hive)  senescent  bees. 

In  a  study  of  over  8,500  houseflies,  Musca  domestica,  of  the 
NAIDM  strain,  females  were  found  to  have  a  mean  longevity 
of  29  days  and  males  a  mean  longevity  of  17  days.  Curves  for 
probit-log  time  plots  indicated  a  relatively  simple  mortality 
factor  for  the  male  population  but  a  complex  of  several 
mortality  factors  for  the  female  cohort.  Female  longevity 
was  enhanced  by  the  inclusion  of  powdered  whole  milk  in  the 
adult  diet  of  sugar  and  water.  No  such  beneficial  effect  was 
obtained  for  male  houseflies.  A  study  of  the  role  of  parental 
age  at  the  time  of  oviposition  indicated  a  possible  adverse 
effect  on  the  longevity  of  female  offspring  from  oldest  parents ; 
for  males  reared  from  eggs  from  oldest  parents  there  appears 
to  be  an  enhancement  of  the  mean  longevity,  probably  by  a 
selection  of  long-livedness  through  the  old  surviving  female 
parents. 

Acknowledgements 

Figs,  la  and  lb  were  drawn  by  Miss  Mary  Lorenc. 

Fig.  3  was  drawn  by  Mr.  Rudolph  Cavalcante. 

The  author  is  deeply  appreciative  of  the  assistance  of  Dr.  Albert  S. 
Perry  of  the  USPHS  Technical  Development  Laboratories,  Savannah, 
Georgia,  for  generously  supplying  the  pupae  of  the  NAIDM  strain  from 
which  this  colony  has  been  developed. 

The  kind  assistance  of  Miss  Gertrude  Uhr,  Secretary  of  the  Depart- 
ment of  Physiology,  and  Mrs.  Elaine  S.  Rockstein,  in  the  preparation  of 
the  manuscript,  is  acknowledged. 


264  Morris  Rockstein 

The  research  described  in  this  report  was  supported  in  part  by  the 
United  States  Army  Surgeon  General's  Office  and  the  United  States 
PubHc  Health  Service  Division  of  Research  Grants. 


REFERENCES 

Andrew,  W.  (1938).   Z.  Zellforsch.,  28,  294. 

AsciiER,  K.  R.  S.,  and  Levinson,  Z.  H.  (1956).   Riv.  Parassit,  17,  217. 

Comfort,  A.  (1953).   Nature  (Lond.),  172,  83. 

Ellis,  R.  S.  (1919).  J.  comp.  Neurol,  30,  329. 

Ellis,  R.  S.  (1920).   J.  comp.  Neurol,  32,  1. 

Feldman-Muhsam,  B.  (1944).   Bull  ent.  Res.,  35,  53. 

Gardner,  E.  (1940).   Anal  Rec.,  77,  529. 

Grosch,  D.  S.  (1950).   Biol  Bull,  Wood's  Hole,  99,  65. 

Hatai,  S.  (1902).   J.  comp.  Neurol,  12,  107. 

Haydak,  M.  H.  (1953).   Anti.  ent.  iSoc.  Amer.,  46,  547. 

Hodge,  C.  F.  (1894).  J.  Physiol,  17,  129. 

Inukai,  T.  (1928).  J.  comp.  Neurol,  45,  1. 

Lansing,  A.  I.  (1947).  J.  Geronl,  2,  228. 

Lansing,  A.  I.  (1948).  Proc.  nat.  Acad.  Sci.  {Wash.),  34,  304. 

Lansing,  A.  I.  (1954).   Ann.  N.Y.  Acad.  Sci.,  57,  455. 

Maurizio,  a.  (1954).   Landw.  Jh.  Schweiz.,  68,  115. 

McCay,  C.  M.,  and  Crowell,  M.  F.  (1934).   Sci.  Mon.,  N.Y.,  39,  405. 

McCay,  C.  M.,  Crowell,  M.  F.,  and  Maynard,  L.  A.  (1935).    J.  Nutr., 

10,  63. 
McCay,  C.  M.,  Maynard,  L.  A.,  Sperling,  G.,  and  Asgood,  H.  S. 

(1941).  J.  Nutr.,  21,  45. 
McCay,  C.  M.,  Maynard,  L.  A.,  Sperling,  G.,  and  Barnes,  L.  L.  (1939). 

J.  Nutr.,  18,  1. 
Murphy,  D.  P.  (1954).   Ann.  N.Y.  Acad.  Sci.,  57,  451. 
Rockstein,  M.  (1950a).  J.  cell.  comp.  Physiol,  35,  11. 
Rockstein,  M.  (19506).   Ann.  ent.  Soc.  Amer.,  43,  152. 
Rockstein,  M.  (1953).   Biol  Bull,  Wood's  Hole,  105,  154. 
Rockstein,  M.  (1956).  J.  Geronl,  11,  282. 
Rockstein,  M.  (1957).  J.  Geronl,  12,  253. 
Rockstein,  M.  (1958).  J.  Geronl,  13,  7. 

Rockstein,  M.,  and  Lieberman,  H.  M.  (1958).  Nature  {Lond.),  181,  787. 
Rockstein,  M.,  and  Lieberman,  H.  M.  (1959).    Gerontologia  {Basel), 

3,  23. 
Rollins,  R.  Z.  (1959).   Personal  Communication. 

SoNNEBORN,  T.  M.  (1957).  Proc.  Ageing  Conference,  Gatlinburg.    Wash- 
ington, D.C. :  A.I.B.S.,  in  press. 
Tracey,  K.  M.  (1958).   Ann.  enl  Soc.  Amer.,  51,  429. 
Wilkes,  A.,  Buciier,  G.  E.,  Cameron,  J.  W.  McB.,  and  West,  A.  S.,  Jr. 

(1948).   Canad.  J.  Res.,  D.,  26,  8. 
Woke,  P.  A.,  Ally,  M.  S.,  and  Rosenberger,  C.  R.,  Jr.  (1956).   Ann. 

ent.  Soc.  Amer.,  49,  435. 


Discussion  265 


DISCUSSION 


Gerking:  Did  McCay  keep  these  trout  you  mentioned  until  they 
died?  You  referred  to  an  increased  Hfespan  for  these  fish. 

Rockstein:  My  recollection  is  that  he  was  able  to  double  the  life- 
span of  the  trout. 

Comfort :  The  total  duration  of  the  experiment  was  only  a  matter  of 
months,  while  they  were  in  the  hatchery.  The  lifespan  of  trout  is  at 
least  10  years. 

Berg:  The  advantage  of  using  rats  for  lifespan  studies  is  that  we 
can  determine  causes  of  death  in  this  species,  whereas  the  pathology 
of  other  species  has  not  been  studied  as  thoroughly. 

Rockstein :  We  are  not  as  fortunate  as  you  in  having  a  pathologist. 
There  are  insect  pathologists,  but  they  are  very  rare  and  very 
costly. 

Berg:  McCay  employed  drastic  underfeeding  so  that  his  animals 
were  severely  retarded  in  growth  and  did  not  attain  sexual  maturity. 
The  greater  longevity  of  McCay's  rats  has  been  attributed  in  part 
to  the  retardation  of  sexual  development.  In  our  experiments, 
sexual  maturity  of  the  females  was  delayed  only  three  to  four  weeks, 
and  skeletal  measurements  were  only  5  to  7  per  cent  less  than  in  ad 
libitum-fed  rats.  These  findings  indicate  that  longevity  of  the  rat 
can  be  increased  by  dietary  restriction  without  seriously  affecting 
skeletal  growth  or  sexual  maturity. 

Rockstein:  We  are  all  eagerly  awaiting  the  results  of  your  experi- 
ments on  longevity. 

Comfort:  1  am  a  little  disturbed,  Prof.  Rockstein,  about  drawing 
analogies  between  the  nutritional  requirements  of  rats  and  those  of 
insect  imagos.  For  example,  I  take  it  there  is  very  little  extragonadal 
mitosis  in  your  flies.  Any  change  you  get  is  not  due  to  altering  the 
number  of  cell  generations  or  the  stage  of  development. 

Rockstein:  No,  I  did  not  mean  to  draw  that  analogy.  I  should 
have  mentioned  that  adult  flies  are  essentially  fully-grown  animals, 
much  like  the  honey-bee,  but  even  more  so  because  the  honey-bee 
takes  about  ten  days  to  attain  full  maturity.  The  housefly  on  the 
other  hand  is  completely  mature  within  a  few  hours  except  for  the 
ability  to  lay  fertile  eggs,  under  standard  laboratory  conditions.  But 
I  did  mean  very  definitely  to  compare  the  human  with  the  rat,  and 
to  emphasize  that  starvation  would  hardly  be  of  any  use  in  the 
human  if  one  wanted  to  prolong  life.  I  do  not  know  that  it  has  ever 
been  shown  to  do  so;  if  anything,  it  would  shorten  the  lifespan  in 
man.  When  McCay  and  his  workers  came  out  with  the  pronounce- 
ment that  a  low  protein,  low  calorie  diet  was  what  we  needed  for  a 


266  Discussion 

long  life,  I  thought  that  was  rather  a  broad  inference  from  their 
particular  study  on  white  rats. 

Comfort:  I  do  not  think  any  human  population  has  ever  been  sub- 
jected to  the  sort  of  controlled  and  selective  restriction  of  diet  which 
Dr.  Berg  and  Prof.  Simms  have  been  using.  Starved  populations  are 
deficient  in  all  foods,  and  do  not  receive  adequate  vitamin  supple- 
ments, as  McCay's  rats  did.  There  is  a  difference  there.  I  agree  with 
you  entirely  about  the  general  principle. 

Sacher:  Restrictions  in  diet  during  and  after  the  past  war  in 
several  countries  may  have  had  a  relation  to  the  observed  decrease 
in  mortality  from  heart  disease. 

Tanner:  In  some  of  the  degenerative  diseases,  for  example  diabetes, 
the  incidence  and  the  death  rates  went  down.  But  this  is  different 
from  the  notion  that  at  the  same  time  children  are  being  starved  and 
therefore  they  might  live  longer  later  on.  The  starvation  during  the 
war  lasted  a  sufficiently  short  time,  so  that  those  children  who  were 
starved  probably  picked  up  on  to  their  natural  growth  curves  a  little 
later  on.  It  was  acute  or  sub-acute  starvation,  which  is  probably 
compensated  for  pretty  rapidly.  We  know  that  the  human,  like  the 
rat,  gets  back  to  the  normal  growth  curve  fairly  rapidly,  even  after 
severe  disease  or  severe  starvation.  It  is  for  this  reason  that  I  do  not 
think  this  data  is  particularly  relevant. 

Sacher:  Nevertheless,  such  children  constitute  a  cohort  which  can 
be  followed  in  successive  decades.  Even  though  normal  growth  is 
resumed,  there  'may  still  be  permanent  after-effects  detectable  in 
later  susceptibility  to  disease. 

Jalavisto:  Did  you  measure  the  death  rate  at  earlier  dates  in  this 
parental  age  series.  Prof.  Rockstein? 

Rockstein:  Yes,  I  have  curves,  but  this  was  a  very  limited  study, 
involving  about  150  flies  in  each  case,  and  so  the  data  are  not  really 
adequate  for  preparing  such  curves. 

Maynard Smith:  My  colleague  Miss  Clarke  has  been  doing  experi- 
ments on  the  effect  of  larval  nutrition  on  the  longevity  of  Drosophila, 
The  animals  are  kept  as  adults  in  the  same  environment  on  the  same 
food,  but  are  fed  as  larvae  on  diets  varying  from  0  •  03  per  cent  up  to 
about  16  per  cent  of  dead  yeast.  I  do  not  think  she  would  want  to 
commit  herself  very  much  on  the  results,  because  she  has  not 
finished  doing  the  sums.  However,  it  is  quite  clear  that  the  effect, 
if  any,  of  larval  nutrition  on  adult  longevity  is  very  small.  It  has  an 
effect  on  the  time  it  takes  the  animals  to  develop,  and  on  how  big 
they  are  when  they  emerge  from  the  pupae,  but  it  has  only  a  very 
tiny  effect  on  their  adult  survival  in  either  sex.  I  confess  I  find  that 
surprising. 


Discussion  267 

Rockstein:  Do  they  lay  eggs? 

Maynard  Smith :  Yes.  The  more  protein  you  give  them  as  larvae, 
the  higher  the  rate  of  egg-laying  when  they  are  adults.  We  therefore 
suspected  that  the  ones  which  had  a  lot  of  protein  might  not  live  as 
long  as  the  others,  but  there  is  no  overall  effect  of  any  great  magni- 
tude. If  there  is  an  effect  it  is  of  the  order  of  10  or  20  per  cent — not 
more  than  that. 

Kershaw:  There  is  an  analogous  situation  in  parasitism.  The 
ability  of  the  tsetses  to  act  as  vectors  of  sleeping  sickness  is  largely 
determined  by  the  temperatures  at  which  the  pupae  are  maintained 
before  the  adults  come  out.  What  effect  that  has  on  the  longevity 
of  the  adult  is  not  known,  but  it  will  be  a  sort  of  parallel  viability,  or 
parallel  parameter  that  we  put  against  longevity. 

It  is  the  middle-aged  insect  which  is  the  important  survivor  for 
parasites,  because  the  ones  which  die  young  do  not  hve  long  enough 
to  transfer  the  parasite.  Secondly,  a  very  complex  pattern  of 
mortality  is  evident  in  the  development  of  a  parasite  in  different 
selected  organs  of  an  insect.  A  third  point  is  the  ability  of  the  insect 
to  support  the  parasite,  so  that  those  at  the  tag  end  of  their  life 
cannot  act  as  vectors.  That  has  an  obvious  application  in  the  field 
but  what  this  means  biologically  one  does  not  know.  Unfortunately 
there  are  no  means  at  the  moment  of  quantitatively  assessing  the 
capacity  of  insects  to  support  some  parasites. 

Wigglesworth :  Could  you  describe  the  sexual  difference  in  the 
effect  of  protein  feeding  by  saying,  rather  as  Prof.  Kershaw  is  im- 
plying, that  the  adult  male  is  not  protein- starved,  but  that  the  adult 
female  is  starved  of  protein  by  its  reproductive  activities — therefore 
in  the  absence  of  the  extra  protein  feeding  it  succumbs  early? 

Rockstein:  Yes,  that  inference  would  be  very  appropriate. 

Sacher:  I  found  a  survivorship  curve  for  male  Drosophila  almost 
identical  with  what  you  found  for  the  male  housefly.  Prof.  Rockstein. 
Unfortunately  I  did  not  get  data  for  female  Drosophila.  In  regard  to 
your  remarks  about  the  complexity  of  the  survivorship  curve  for  the 
female  (and  I  think  we  should  say  for  the  male  too),  perhaps  we 
should  recall  the  controversy  between  Crozier  and  Pearl  (Pearl, 
R.,  White,  P.,  and  Miner,  J.  R.  (1929).  Proc.  nat.  Acad.  Sci.  (Wash.) 
15,  425).  Pearl  had  studied  the  resistance  of  Drosophila  to  alcohol 
as  a  function  of  age  and  he  got  a  curve  which  he  graduated  smoothly. 
Crozier  (Crozier,  W.  J.,  Pincus,  G.,  and  Zahl,  P.  A.  (1936).  J.  gen. 
Physiol.,  19,  523)  objected  that  such  smoothing  was  not  proper  and 
he  did  a  far  more  extensive  experiment.  He  established  that  the 
resistance  of  Drosophila  to  alcohol  as  a  function  of  age  went  through 
many  stages  and  was  an  exceedingly  complex  curve.    This  comes 


268  Discussion 

back  to  the  fact  that  we  must  eventually  dissect  the  life-table  into 
several  components,  as  Dr.  Benjamin  has  pointed  out,  and  as  H.  S. 
Simms  (1940.  Science,  91,  7)  has  shown  previously. 

Brauer  (personal  communication)  at  the  Naval  Radiological 
Defence  Laboratory  in  San  Francisco  is  using  McCay's  technique 
now.  He  re-feeds  rats  at  various  ages  and  finds  that  the  rate  of 
growth  when  full  feeding  is  re-established  carefully  is  preserved  up  to 
quite  late  ages,  and  then  diminishes.  This  is  somewhat  like  Comfort's 
regeneration  and  re-feeding  experiments.  Brauer  is  also  examining 
the  effect  of  previous  X-irradiation  on  the  ability  to  resume  growth. 

Rockstein:  McCay  could  accelerate  senescence  in  an  old  animal 
which  appeared  to  be  young  because  of  starvation.  By  re-establish- 
ing the  full  diet,  the  animal's  appearance  rapidly  shifted  to  that  of  an 
old  animal  and  mortality  was  accelerated. 


THE  RATE  OF  AGEING  IN 
DROSOPHILA  SUBOBSCURA 


J.  IVIaynard  Smith 

Department  of  Zoology,  University  College  London 

To  a  geneticist,  the  oddest  feature  of  gerontology  is  the 
absence  of  a  coherent  and  generally  accepted  theory  of  age- 
ing, comparable  to  the  chromosome  theory  of  heredity.  In 
case  this  remark  should  cause  any  misgivings,  it  should  be 
said  that  no  attempt  will  be  made  to  remedy  this  defect. 
Instead,  two  kinds  of  theory  which  seem  to  be  possible  will 
be  indicated,  since  this  will  help  to  interpret  some  experiments 
to  be  described  later. 

We  have  to  accept  that  a  theory  of  ageing  may  be  valid 
only  for  a  single  species  or  group  of  related  species.  It  may 
be  that  we  shall  find  a  theory  which  proves  to  have  the  same 
universality  in  the  study  of  ageing  as  does  the  chromosome 
theory  in  genetics,  but  this  does  not  at  present  seem  very 
likely.  What  kinds  of  theory,  then,  can  be  put  forward  to 
explain  ageing  in  a  single  species,  say  in  men  or  in  mice  or  in 
fruitflies?  A  distinction  should  be  made  between  two  types  of 
theory,  which  may  be  called  "single"  and  "multiple"  theories 
of  ageing. 

A  multiple  theory  would  postulate  that  there  are  a  number 
of  partially  independent  processes  occurring  in  every  in- 
dividual, any  one  of  which  may  ultimately  cause  death.  It 
is  not  intended  to  imply  that  two  processes  in  a  single  in- 
dividual can  ever  be  wholly  independent;  by  "partially 
independent"  is  meant  only  that  each  process  would  continue, 
perhaps  at  an  altered  rate,  in  the  absence  of  the  others.  Now 
some  ageing  processes  are  fairly  certainly  independent  in  this 
sense.  For  example,  the  mechanical  wearing  away  of  the 
teeth  of  herbivorous  mammals  would  occur  even  if  other 

269 


270  J.  Maynard  Smith 

ageing  processes  were  arrested,  and,  unless  there  is  contin- 
uous tooth  growth,  would  ultimately  lead  to  death.  Similarly, 
in  so  far  as  cancer  is  the  result  of  cumulative  environmental 
insult,  it  is  partially  independent  of  other  ageing  processes, 
though  it  would  be  rash  to  assume  that  it  is  wholly  so.  But  it 
is  always  possible  that  apparently  unrelated  symptoms  of 
ageing  may  be  due  to  a  single  cause,  just  as  apparently  un- 
related abnormalities  of  development  may  be  the  pleiotropic 
effects  of  a  single  gene.  A  "single"  theory  would  postulate 
that  all  or  most  of  the  symptoms  of  ageing  are  the  consequence 
of  a  single  process  (or  of  a  single  series  of  processes),  either  at 
a  cellular  or  organism  level. 

There  is  one  observation  which  at  first  sight  appears  to 
support  such  a  single  theory.  In  a  given  species,  the  deter- 
ioration of  different  organ  systems  proceeds  at  roughly  the 
same  rate;  if  this  were  not  so,  individuals  dying  of  "old 
age"  would  always  die  of  the  same  immediate  cause.  This 
synchrony  might  suggest  a  high  degree  of  physiological  inter- 
dependence, with  some  one  particular  process  acting  as  a 
timekeeper.  But  the  synchrony  can  be  explained  in  another 
way.  Suppose  that  ageing  in  mammals  is  in  fact  multiple  in 
character.  Then  if  in  any  species  one  ageing  process,  say  the 
deterioration  of  the  central  nervous  system,  proceeded  at  a 
much  higher  rate  than  did  other  ageing  processes,  there 
would  be  strong  natural  selection  tending  to  slow  down  the 
rate  of  ageing  in  this  system,  if  necessary  at  the  expense  of 
accelerating  other  ageing  processes.  In  other  words,  natural 
selection  will  tend  to  synchronize  different  ageing  processes, 
even  if  these  are  physiologically  independent  of  one  another. 
The  example  of  tooth  wear  already  mentioned  demonstrates 
that  selection  can  in  fact  act  in  this  way.  The  volume  of  tooth 
worn  away  in  unit  time  is  proportional  to  the  volume  of  food 
eaten,  which  in  turn  is  roughly  proportional  to  the  surface 
area  of  the  animal.  Consequently,  the  expectation  of  life  of 
the  teeth  of  small  mammals  is  less  than  that  of  large  mam- 
mals.    However,    many    small    herbivores    (rodents)    have 


Rate  of  ageing  in  Drosophila  suhohscura  271 

evolved  molar  teeth  which  grow  throughout  life,  whereas 
large  herbivores  have  not.  Similarly,  the  age  of  onset  of 
cancers  in  species  with  different  life  expectancies  (say  in  mice 
and  men)  is  roughly  proportional  to  those  expectancies,  and 
this  proportionality  seems  more  likely  to  be  a  consequence  of 
synchronizing  selection  than  of  a  direct  physiological  con- 
nexion between  ageing  generally  and  cancer. 

It  follows  that  a  decision  between  a  single  and  a  multiple 
theory  in   any   species   is   impossible   without   experimental 

Table  I 

Mean  survival  times  in  days  of  adult  flies  at  various 

temperatures 


Temperature 

°c. 

Males 

Females 

No. 

of 
flies 

Survival  time 
in  days 

No. 

of 
flies 

Survival  time 
in  days 

20 

„(, /raised  at  15^ 
^\  raised  at  25 

30-5 
33 

50 

25 
25 

50 
10 

67-4  ±  2-46 

29-5±107\^ 
24-6  ±  110/" 

7-58  ±  0-28 
0-79  ±  008 

50 

25 
25 

50 
10 

55-9  ±  2-58 

40-5  ±  l-68\„.    .. 
30-6  ±  l-65/'^^"^'' 

17-60  ±  0-65 
0-82  ±  005 

interference  with  the  process.  The  grafting  of  organs  from 
young  individuals  into  old  and  vice  versa,  or  between  individ- 
uals with  different  genetically  determined  rates  of  ageing,  is 
perhaps  the  most  promising  experimental  approach  (Jones  and 
Krohn,  1959;  Medawar,  1957).  Some  experiments  w411  now  be 
described  on  ageing  in  Drosophila  suhohscura,  using  a  different 
approach,  but  leading  to  the  conclusion  that  the  ageing  pro- 
cess is  a  multiple  one  (Maynard  Smith,  1957,  1958). 

It  has  been  known  for  a  long  time  that  in  poikilothermous 
animals  the  expectation  of  life  decreases  with  increasing 
temperature.    It  was  the  purpose  of  the  investigation  now  to 


272 


J.  Maynard  Smith 


be  described  to  discover  how  far  the  processes  responsible  for 
death  in  D.  subobscura  are  the  same  at  different  temperatures, 
differing  only  in  the  rate  at  which  they  proceed,  and  how  far 
different  processes  are  concerned  at  different  temperatures. 


100-1 


c  4 

4) 


O     ^ 


10- 


1- 


J I I L 


J I I I I L 


-1000 


\-  100    ^ 

c 


20 


35 


10 


25  30 

Temperature  (°C.) 

Fig.  1.    Survival  time  of  flies  at  different  temperatures.    A,  in  food 

vials;  B,  in  saturated  air;  C,  in  dry  air;   A»    ^j   females;    0>    •> 

males;  □,  ||,  sexes  combined. 

Figs.  1-3  and  Table  II  reproduced  by  courtesy  of  the  Editor, 
Journal  of  Experimental  Biology. 

The  mean  ages  at  death  (measured  from  adult  emergence) 
of  adults  kept  continuously  at  various  temperatures  are 
shown  in  Table  I.  In  Fig.  1  these  values  are  plotted  on  a 
logarithmic  scale,  together  with  the  survival  times  of  flies 
exposed  to  higher  temperatures  without  food  or  water  in  dry 
and  in  saturated  air.  The  rather  sudden  change  in  the  slope 
of  the  curve  in  Fig.  1  suggests  that  the  causes  of  death  at  high 


Rate  of  Ageing  in  Drosophila  suhohscura  273 

temperatures  may  be  different  from  those  acting  below  about 
31°.  This  suggestion  is  confirmed  by  the  finding  that  the 
changes  which  occur  at  high  temperatures  are,  wholly  or  in 
part,  reversible,  whereas  the  changes  which  occur  at  30-5°  are 
irreversible. 

The  reversibility  of  a  change  is  judged  by  exposing  in- 
dividuals to  high  temperatures  intermittently,  with  interven- 
ing periods  at  a  lower  temperature.  Thus  if  flies  are  exposed 
to  a  high  temperature  (33-5°  in  dry  air  or  34-3°  in  saturated 
air)  for  50  minutes  (i.e.  for  about  half  their  expectation  of  life 
at  that  temperature)  and  are  then  kept  for  three  hours  at 
20°,  their  survival  times  when  they  are  again  exposed  to  the 
high  temperature  are  as  great  or  greater  than  the  survival 
times  of  flies  not  previously  exposed.  Thus  death  in  these 
conditions  is  due  to  changes  which  can  be  reversed  at  a 
lower  temperature;  therefore  the  changes  responsible  for 
death  at  high  temperatures  are  not  regarded  as  processes  of 
senescence.  Experiments  in  which  flies  were  kept  in  food  vials 
alternately  for  eight  hours  at  33°  and  for  16  hours  at  20° 
showed  that  the  changes  responsible  for  death  at  33°  in  food 
vials  are  also  in  part  reversible. 

In  contrast,  as  is  shown  in  Table  II,  the  changes  responsible 
for  death  at  30-5°  are  not  to  any  appreciable  extent  reversed 
at  lower  temperatures.  There  is  evidence  for  a  small  degree  of 
recovery  in  males,  since  the  first  eight-day  interruption  at 
20°  did  slightly  increase  the  further  expectation  of  life  at 
30-5°,  although  the  second  interruption  did  not.  Females 
which  were  exposed  intermittently  had  total  survival  times 
which  were  if  anything  slightly  shorter  than  those  of  flies 
exposed  continuously. 

Since  the  changes  responsible  for  death  at  30-5°  are,  at 
least  in  the  females,  irreversible,  and  since  they  take  an 
appreciable  time  to  reach  completion  (mean  of  17-6  days  for 
females),  it  seems  reasonable  to  regard  them  as  processes  of 
ageing.  The  question  then  arises,  are  they  the  same  processes 
as  are  responsible  for  ageing  at  20°?  If  the  processes  of  ageing 


274 


J.  Maynard  Smith 


at  the  two  temperatures  were  in  fact  identical,  it  would  be 
possible  to  predict  the  total  lifespan  of  flies  kept  for  varying 
periods  at  the  two  temperatures.  For  example,  a  female 
exposed  for  eight  days  to  30-5°  soon  after  emergence  would 


Table  II 

Expectation  of  life  at  30-5°  c. 


No.  of 
flies 

Further 
expectation  of  life 

at  30  •  5°  (days) 

Females 

(1)  Exposed  continuously  to  30-5° 

50 

17-60  ±  0-65 

(2)  After  5  days  at  30-5° 

(a)  Exposed  continuously 

50 

12-60  ±  0-65 

(b)  8-day  interruption  at  20°  after  5 

25 

1102  ±  0-28 

days  at  30  •  5° 

(3)  After  13  days  at  30-5° 

(a)  Exposed  continuously 

44 

5-82  ±  0-49 

(b)  8-day  interruption  at  20°  after  5 

25 

302  ±  0-28 

days  at  30-5° 

(c)  Two  8 -day  interruptions  at  20° 

22 

5-23  ±  0-32 

after  5  and  13  days  at  30-5° 

Males 

(1)  Exposed  continuously  to  30-5° 

50 

7-58  ±  0-28 

(2)  After  5  days  at  30-5° 

(a)  Exposed  continuously 

49 

2-64  ±  0-27 

(b)  8-day  interruption  at  20°  after  5 

44 

5-23  ±  0-38 

days  at  30  •  5° 

(3)  After  8  days  at  30  •  5° 

(a)  Exposed  continuously 

21 

1-40  ±  0-35 

(b)  8-day  interruption  at  20°  after  5 

36 

2-97  ±  0-36 

days  at  30-5° 

(c)  Two  8-day  interruptions  at  20° 

25 

2-42  ±  0-23 

after  4  and  8  days  at  30  •  5° 

be  expected  to  have  completed  about  half  its  expected  life- 
span, and  therefore  to  have  a  further  expectation  of  life  at 
20°  of  about  28  days.  Experiments  do  not  confirm  this 
simple  additive  hypothesis. 

Fig.  2  and  Table  III  show  the  results  of  exposing  young 
adult  females  to  30-5°  for  varying  periods,  and  then  keeping 


Rate  of  Ageing  in  Drosophila  subobscura         275 

them  at  20°  until  they  died.  The  exposure,  so  far  from 
decreasing  their  expectation  of  Hfe,  in  fact  increased  it,  by  as 
much  as  50  per  cent  in  females  exposed  for  eight  days.  In  a 
similar  experiment,  a  group  of  males  were  exposed  to  30-5° 
for  five  days,  or  two-thirds  of  their  expectation  of  life  at  that 
temperature.  The  further  expectation  of  life  of  these  males 
at  20°  did  not  differ  from  that  of  a  group  of  unexposed  con- 
trols. 


56   64   72   80 

Age  in  days 


96  104  112  120  128  136 


Fig.  2.   Survival  time  at  20°  of  females  previously  exposed  to  30-5°. 
A,  unexposed;  B,  exposed  for  5  days;  C,  exposed  for  8  days;  D,  ex- 
posed for  12  days. 


It  follows  that,  both  for  males  and  females,  different  pro- 
cesses are  responsible  for  death  at  the  two  temperatures;  we 
are  therefore  obliged  to  accept  a  multiple  theory  of  ageing  for 
this  species.  The  situation  is  further  complicated  by  the 
different  response  of  males  and  females  to  exposure  to  30  •  5°, 
w^hich  prolonged  the  life  of  females  but  left  that  of  males 
unaltered.  The  clue  to  this  difference  was  found  when  it  was 
observed  that  exposure  to  a  high  temperature  caused  a 
partial  regression  of  the  ovaries  of  females,  which  sub- 
sequently laid  eggs  at  only  about  half  the  rate  of  unexposed 
females.  This  suggested  that  the  process  of  egg-laying  might 
accelerate  ageing  in  females,  and  that  the  exposure  to  a  high 


276  J.  Maynard  Smith 

Table  III 

Expectation  of  life  in  days  of  females  kept  at  20°  c. 


No.  of  flies 

Further  expectation 
of  life  in  days 
at  age  17  days 

Kept  continuously  at  20° 
Exposed  to  30  •  5°  for  5  days 
(6th  to  10th  day  after  emergence) 
Exposed  to  30  •  5°  for  8  days 
(6th  to  13th  day  after  emergence) 
Exposed  to  30-5°  for  12  days 
(6th  to  17th  day  after  emergence) 

50 
47 
18 
15 

38-9  ±  2-6 
57-2  ±  30 
67-8  ±  4-9 
500  ±  6-6 

temperature  prolongs  life  because  it  slows  down  the  rate  of 
egg-laying. 

This  suggestion  has  been  confirmed  by  experiments  using 
virgin  females  (which  lay  eggs  at  a  reduced  rate),  and  females 
lacking  ovaries.  The  latter  were  obtained  by  using  females 
homozygous  for  the  mutant  "  grandchildless "  (Spurway, 
1948),  whose  offspring  appear  to  be  normal  in  all  respects 


Table  IV 

Expectation  of  life  of  femai.es  kept  at  20°  c. 


No.  oj 
flies 

Further  expectation 
of  life  in  days 
at  age  10  days 

Mated  females 

kept  continuously  at  20° 
exposed  to  31°  for  5  days 

Virgin  females 

kept  continuously  at  20° 

OVARILESS  FEMALES 

kept  continuously  at  20° 
exposed  to  31°  for  3  days 

48 
23 

89 

28 
22 

331  ±  1-6 
61-2  ±  5-7 

58-7  ±  2-7 

67-6  ±  4-7 
64-2  ±  5-6 

Rate  of  Ageing  in  Drosophila  suhohscura         277 

except  for  the  absence  of  gonads.  The  results  of  these  experi- 
ments are  shown  in  Fig.  3  and  Table  lY.  As  before,  the 
exposure  of  normal  mated  females  to  a  high  temperature 
increased  their  expectation  of  life  at  20°.  As  would  be  ex- 
pected from  the  hypothesis  being  tested,  both  virgin  and 
ovariless  females  lived  for  longer  than  did  the  controls,  and 
closely  resembled  the  females  exposed  to  a  high  temperature. 
The  final  confirmation  of  the  hypothesis  comes  from  the  fact 


16      24      32      40      48       56      64      72      80      88      96      104    112    120 
Age  in  days 

Fig.  3.    Survival  time  of  females  at  20°.    A,  normal  mated 

females;    B,    ovariless    females;    C,    normal   virgin   females; 

D,  normal  mated  females  exposed  for  5  or  6  days  to  31°. 


that  the  expectation  of  life  of  ovariless  females,  as  of  males, 
is  not  increased  by  exposure  to  a  high  temperature ;  since  such 
females  will  not  lay  eggs  in  any  case,  exposure  cannot  further 
prolong  their  life.  The  greater  longevity  of  virgin  as  com- 
pared to  mated  females  has  previously  been  demonstrated  by 
Bilewicz  (1953)  in  Drosophila  melanogaster,  and  by  Griffiths 
and  Tauber  (1942)  in  Periplaneta  americana. 

It  is  natural  to  suppose  that  the  causes  of  ageing  of  ovariless 
or  virgin  females  at  20°  are  the  same  as  the  causes  of  ageing  of 
males  at  the  same  temperature.  But  these  experiments  leave 
one  question  unanswered.    Does  egg-laying  shorten  the  life 


278 


J.  Maynard  Smith 


of  females  because  it  accelerates  processes  which  occur  in  any 
case  in  ovariless  females,  or  is  it  a  process  which  would  by 
itself  ultimately  result  in  death,  even  if  other  ageing  processes 
could  be  arrested?  We  are  again  faced  by  a  choice  between  a 
single  and  a  multiple  theory.  As  yet  we  have  not  been  able 
to  find  a  way  of  deciding  between  them,  but  we  hope  we  may 
be  able  to  do  so  by  studying  ageing  in  genetically  different 
strains  on  varying  diets,  since  in  this  way  we  have  other 
means  of  altering  both  the  rate  of  ageing  and  of  egg-laying. 

Table  V 

The  longevities  of  inbred  and  of  outbred  flies 
IN  days  at  20°  c. 


Mean  lifespan 

Coefficient  of 
variation 

Females 

Males 

Females 

Males 

Nine  inbred    /range 
lines              \mean 

Four  outbred  r  range 
populations'^  mean 

17 -2-53 -8 
36-4 

55  -  9-64  1 
GOO 

17-1-69-2 
40-0 

44-7-67-4 
56-8 

0-35-0-69 
0-51 

0-29-0-35 
0-32 

0-35-0-66 
0-55 

0-23-0-50 
0-33 

We  may  now  turn  to  the  genetics  of  ageing  in  D.  subobscura. 
Our  interest  in  ageing  originated  with  the  discovery  (Clarke 
and  Maynard  Smith,  1955)  that  the  hybrids  between  inbred 
lines  live  for  longer,  and  are  less  variable  in  lifespan,  than  their 
inbred  parents.  These  findings  have  been  confirmed  by  later 
work  (Table  V),  although  we  were  perhaps  fortunate  that  the 
particular  pair  of  inbred  lines  originally  available  for  study 
showed  the  effect  in  a  particularly  striking  manner.  But  later 
work  has  shown  that,  in  addition  to  genetic  variance  due  to 
"heterosis"  or  "overdominance",  much  of  the  genetic 
variance  of  longevity  is  due  to  genes  with  sex-limited  effects, 
i.e.  to  genes  with  different  effects  on  the  longevity  of  males  and 


Rate  of  Ageing  in  Drosophila  suhohscura  279 

of  females  (Maynard  Smith,  1959).  This  can  be  shown  in  two 
ways.  Table  VI  shows  the  lifespans  of  males  and  of  females 
of  nine  inbred  lines,  two  kinds  of  F^  hybrids  between  inbred 
lines,  and  the  offspring  of  two  groups  of  wild-caught  females, 
one  from  Kent  and  one  from  Galilee.  In  eight  of  these  13 
populations  there  was  a  significant  difference  between  the 

Tables  VI 
Relative  longevities  of  males  and  females,  in  days,  at  20°  c. 


Lifespan 

Females 

Males 

Ratio 

P 

Inbred  lines 

K 

17-2 

31-2 

0-55 

+  +  . 

M 

35-3 

51-8 

0-68 

+ 

F 

53-8 

69-2 

0-77 

+ 

O 

48-7 

52  o 

0-93 

NFS 

40-7 

42-4 

0-98 

D 

50-2 

47-5 

106 

B 

33-3 

25-8 

1-29 

+ 

G 

30  0 

22-6 

1-33 

+ 

E 

36-2 

171 

212 

+  + 

Fj  Hybrids 

K/NFS 

55-9 

67-4 

0-83 

+  + 

B/K 

61-5* 

61-6 

100 

Offspring  of 

WILD  flies 

Kent 

58 -C 

53-4 

110 

Galilee 

64  1 

44-7 

1-43 

+  + 

+  +,  significant  at  0-001  level;  +,  significant  at  0  10  level. 

longevities  of  the  two  sexes,  but  in  four  cases  it  was  the  males 
and  in  four  cases  the  females  which  lived  for  longer.  This  can 
only  be  explained  by  the  presence  of  genes  which  affect  the 
longevity  of  the  two  sexes  differently.  The  same  conclusion 
emerges  from  a  study  of  the  correlations  between  the  longevi- 
ties of  relatives  in  a  population  derived  from  females  caught  in 
Galilee  (Table  VII).  All  the  correlations  are  rather  low;  this 
means  only  that  many  differences  between  members  of  the 


280 


J.  Maynard  Smith 


population  were  due  to  uncontrolled  variations  in  environ- 
mental conditions.  But  all  the  correlations  between  relatives 
of  like  sex  were  significant  and  positive,  whereas  only  one  of 
the  four  correlations  between  relatives  of  unlike  sex  was 
significantly  different  from  zero. 

The  presence  of  sex-limited  genetic  variance  of  longevity  is 
understandable  in  view  of  the  physiological  findings  described 
earlier.   Since  the  causes  of  ageing  in  males  and  ki  females  are 

Table  VII 

CORREL/V-TIONS    BETWEEN    RELATIVES    AMONG    THE    DESCENDANTS    OF 
FEMALES    CAUGHT    IN    GaLILEE 


Like 
sex 

Unlike 
sex 

Brother-brother  <  p,^ 

Sister-sister         <  ^^ 

Father- son 
Mother-daughter 

013 
0-19 

012 
0-20 
0-29 
015 

Brother-sister  <  p,^ 

Father-daughter 
Mother-son 

0  04 
004 

0  19 
-004 

at  least  in  part  different,  it  is  to  be  expected  that  gene  dif- 
ferences should  exist  with  different  effects  on  the  longevity  of 
the  two  sexes.  The  moral  seems  to  be  that  the  genetics  of  a 
character  can  often  be  better  understood  if  something  is 
known  of  its  physiology. 

To  sum  up,  the  most  important  conclusion  which  has 
emerged  from  this  work  seems  to  be  that  ageing  in  Drosophila 
subobscura  is  "multiple"  in  character.  The  processes  re- 
sponsible for  death  at  30-5°  are  reasonably  regarded  as  pro- 
cesses of  "ageing"  or  "senescence",  since  they  are  not 
reversed  or  repaired  at  20°,  and  since  they  take  an  appreciable 
time  to  reach  completion.  Yet  they  are  not  the  same  as  the 
processes  responsible  for  ageing  and  death  at  20°.  Further, 
the    process    of    egg-laying    either    accelerates    the    normal 


Rate  of  Ageing  in  Drosophila  subobscura  281 

ageing  processes  of  females  at  20°,  or  is  itself  an  age  processing 
capable  independently  of  causing  the  death  of  females. 

REFERENCES 

BiLEWicz,  S.  (1953).  Folia  biol.  {Krakow),  1,  177. 

Clarke,  J.  M.,  and  Maynard  Smith,  J.  (1955).  J.  Genet.,  53,  172. 

Griffiths,  J.  T.,  and  Tauber,  O.  E.  (1942).   Physiol.  ZooL,  15,  196. 

Jones,  E.  C,  and  Krohx,  P.  L.  (1959).   Nature  {Lond.),  183,  1155. 

IVIaynard  Smith,  J.  (1957).  J.  exp.  Biol.,  34,  85. 

Maynard  Smith,  J.  (1958).  J.  exp.  Biol.,  35,  832. 

Maynard  Smith,  J.  (1959).  J.  Genet.,  in  press. 

Medawar,  p.  B.  (1957).    The  Uniqueness  of  the  Individual.  London: 

Methuen. 
Spurway,  H.  (1948).  J.  Genet.,  49,  126. 

DISCUSSION 

Danielli:  There  may  be  an  alternative  explanation  for  your 
experiments  to  the  one  you  suggest,  namely  that  the  causes  of 
ageing  are  multiple.  In  the  study  of  cell  division  it  is  now  common 
practice  to  synchronize  cells  by  giving  them  a  cycle  of  temperature 
changes.  The  logic  behind  this  is  that  the  synchronization  is  due  to 
the  breaking  of  a  cycle  or  to  interference  with  some  phase  of  the 
cycle  of  metabolic  activity,  so  that  when  the  constraint  due  to 
temperature  change  is  removed,  all  the  cells  take  up  a  new  cycle  at 
the  same  point.  In  your  animals  the  variance  decreased  in  some  of 
the  experiments,  which  would  suggest  that  some  measure  of  syn- 
chronization was  occurring.  You  may  fail  to  get  the  additive  effect 
one  expects  in  ageing,  not  because  the  cause  of  death  is  different  at 
different  temperatures,  but  because  you  break  the  initial  ageing 
cycle  by  moving  from  one  temperature  to  another  and  then  later 
the  animals  begin  somewhat  closer  to  the  origin  of  a  cycle  than  they 
would  have  done  had  you  kept  them  constantly  at  one  temperature. 
This  would  mean  that  there  is  possibly  only  one  cause  of  death, 
although  they  are  behaving  as  if  there  were  two  causes. 

Maynard  Smith:  I  would  accept  that  as  a  very  possible  explanation 
of  the  reduction  in  variance  of  the  population  exposed  to  30  •  5°  inter- 
mittently, compared  to  that  exposed  continuously.  We  shall  have 
to  repeat  the  experiment  which  showed  this  striking  reduction  in 
variance  to  see  whether  it  was  just  one  of  those  things  that  happen 
once,  or  whether  it  will  happen  every  time.  But  I  would  not  accept 
your  suggestion  as  an  alternative  to  the  existence  of  the  two  processes 
of  ageing.  After  eight  days  at  30  •  5°  one  knows  that,  although  all  the 
animals  are  alive,  they  are  all  actually  "half  dead".   They  are  all 


282  Discussion 

halfway  through  a  programme  towards  death,  and  if  left  at  that 
high  temperature  most  of  them  would  be  dead  in  another  eight  days. 
If  it  was  the  same  programme  which  is  responsible  for  death  at 
20°,  then  one  would  expect  that  flies  exposed  for  eight  days  to  30  •  5° 
and  then  kept  at  20°  until  they  died  would  behave  as  if  they  were 
halfway  through  the  programme,  and  not,  as  actually  happens, 
back  at  the  beginning. 

Danielli:  I  was  actually  suggesting  that  the  abrupt  change  of 
temperature  swung  the  animals  back  to  the  beginning  of  the  cycle  in 
each  instance.  This  would  mean  that  you  could  take  the  animals 
halfway  through  their  expectation  of  life  at  30  •  5°  and  then  the  actual 
change  in  temperature  swings  them  back  to  the  beginning  of  their 
cycle  again,  or  somewhere  closer  to  it. 

Maynard  Smith:  If  that  were  true,  one  could  presumably  make 
them  almost  immortal.  You  are  suggesting  that  flies  kept  at  a 
constant  temperature  die  of  physiological  boredom. 

Sacher:  I  do  not  know  whether  your  evidence  clearly  establishes 
the  hypothesis  of  multiple  as  against  a  single  cause  of  ageing.  We 
had  a  diff'erent  experimental  situation  which  leads  to  results  similar 
to  yours.  Fruit-flies  were  given  daily  doses  of  X-rays  throughout 
their  lives  from  emergence  onwards.  Under  these  circumstances 
flies  that  received  about  1  •  5  to  3  kilorontgens  per  day  throughout 
life  lived  more  than  30  per  cent  longer  than  their  controls  and  at  the 
same  time  manifested  a  markedly  decreased  variance.  Subsequently 
I  discovered  that  W.  P.  Davey  (1917,  1919.  J.  exp.  Zool,  22,  573;  28, 
447)  had  also  done  this  with  flour-beetles.  My  interpretation  is  that 
X-irradiation  is  a  stress,  and  that  a  moderate  degree  of  stress  invokes 
adaptive  responses  that  are  not  invoked  in  the  animal's  natural 
environment.  This  leads  me  to  ask  whether  you  could  do  an  experi- 
ment in  which  temperature  shocks  are  given  daily  or  at  frequent 
intervals? 

Maynard  Smith:  I  shall  have  to  do  such  an  experiment.  Whether 
you  do  or  do  not  accept  my  conclusion  that  we  have  a  multiple 
process  here  hinges  largely  on  whether  you  accept  my  argument  that 
what  ultimately  kills  them  at  the  high  temperature  is  properly  re- 
garded as  a  process  of  ageing.  If  you  just  starve  a  population  of 
Drosophila  they  will  all  die  in  about  three  days,  and  you  will  get  a 
survival  curve  with  an  increasing  force  of  mortality,  looking  just 
like  a  life-table.  But  I  do  not  think  any  of  us  regard  this  as  a 
proper  process  of  senescence  because  it  is  fully  reversible ;  if  you  give 
the  flies  food  after  two  days'  starvation  they  recover  completely.  I 
am  arguing  that  the  process  at  30-5°,  which  takes  as  long  as  18  days 
to  reach  completion  in  a  fly  whose  normal  life-expectation  is  only 


Discussion  283 

about  50  days,  and  which  does  not  seem  to  be  reversible,  can  properly 
be  regarded  as  a  process  of  ageing  in  its  own  right.  Therefore  I  am 
rather  unhappy  about  thinking  of  exposure  to  30  •  5°  simply  as  the 
application  of  a  stress. 

Rotblat:  Our  own  results  could  be  explained  by  either  single  or 
multiple  processes,  but  like  Sacher  I  think  your  results  do  not 
necessarily  contradict  the  single  theory.  You  assume  that  high 
temperature  produces  only  ageing  processes.  But  it  may  cause 
something  else ;  it  may  cause  trauma  or  some  other  process  which  is 
not  normally  present,  and  consequently  it  may  not  be  just  an  ageing 
process. 

Griineberg:  The  question  of  whether  we  are  dealing  with  single  or 
multiple  processes  of  ageing  could  probably  be  tackled  by  investigat- 
ing the  effects  of  individual  genes  on  the  ageing  process.  In  the  ex- 
periments you  described,  considerable  differences  were  found 
between  different  inbred  strains.  These,  of  course,  differ  in  a  multi- 
tude of  genes  and  in  practice  it  is  impossible  to  sort  out  the  effects  of 
individual  genes  following  a  cross  between  two  inbred  strains,  as  I 
have  repeatedly  found  in  my  mouse  crosses.  It  would  probably  be  a 
better  plan  to  start  with  single-gene  differences.  We  are  about  to  do 
that  in  mice,  to  see  whether  genes  without  obvious  pathological 
effects  in  some  way  affect  the  longevity  of  the  animal.  In  the  mouse 
this  will  take  about  three  years,  whereas  if  you  are  so  inclined,  you 
could  probably  produce  significant  results  in  Drosophila  by  Christ- 
mas. 

Maynard  Smith:  I  certainly  could  produce  results  by  Christmas. 
R.  Pearl  (1928.  The  Rate  of  Living.  University  of  London  Press) 
found  a  long  time  ago  that  the  gene  vestigial  in  Drosophila  melano- 
gaster  halves  the  expectation  of  life.  I  have  no  doubt  that  a  number 
of  other  genes  would  alter  the  lifespan.  But  I  am  not  a  good  enough 
insect  pathologist,  and  to  learn  anything  from  such  an  experiment 
one  should  analyse  the  causes  of  death. 

Griineberg :  The  gene  vestigial  is  not  a  good  gene  to  use  because  it  is 
itself  obviously  pathological:  because  of  their  reduced  wings  the 
vestigial  flies  tend  to  get  stuck  in  the  food.  One  should  use  genes 
that  have  no  obvious  pathological  effect. 

Maynard  Smith:  Suppose  that  you  find  a  particular  gene,  which  is 
not  obviously  pathological,  but  which  reduces  or  extends  the  life. 
It  would  not  tell  you  much  unless  you  could  then  show  that  animals 
with  the  gene  did  not  die  of  a  specific  cause  that  the  others  are  dying 
of,  or  vice  versa. 

Griineberg:  That  is  exactly  what  I  mean.  Once  you  have  shown 
that  a  gene  affects  the  lifespan,  whether  it  has  an  effect  one  way  or 


284  Discussion 

the  other,  it  may  then  be  possible  to  identify  the  physiological 
channels  through  which  the  gene  affects  the  lifespan.  But  this  is 
difficult  if  not  impossible  if  you  are  dealing  with  the  joint  results  of  a 
multitude  of  genes  which  differentiate  two  different  inbred  strains  or 
populations. 

Maynard  Smith:  It  will  be  easier  in  the  mouse,  although  it  will 
take  longer,  because  you  have  more  idea  of  what  mice  die  of  than  I 
have  of  what  flies  die  of. 

Rockstein:  Your  results  do  not  really  agree  with  Dr.  Maurizio's  or 
mine.  The  honey-bee  lives  longer  when  it  has  a  functioning  ovary, 
certainly  in  the  queenless  colony.  The  housefly  lives  longer  when  it 
is  well  fed  and  is  laying  lots  of  eggs. 

On  the  other  hand,  you  implied  that  the  effect  of  temperature  in 
prolonging  life  was  through  the  destruction  of  the  ovaries.  Of 
course,  these  occur  together  and  may  not  necessarily  involve  cause 
and  effect.  Instead,  this  may  be  the  net  effect  of  temperature 
through  a  more  important  higher  level  of  control,  which  affects  ovary 
development  (as  well  as  other  processes)  so  as  to  result  ultimately  in 
the  rapid  dying  off  of  the  population. 

Sacher:  There  is  also  the  opposite  interpretation,  that  since  the 
ovary  is  regenerated  it  is  not  the  destruction,  but  rather  the  regener- 
ation, that  extends  life. 

Maynard  Smith:  You  can  get  the  same  results  with  animals  which 
never  had  and  never  will  have  ovaries — these  animals  will  live  much 
longer  than  their  double  first  cousins  who  have  got  ovaries.  It  is 
reasonable  to  take  the  simple  hypothesis  that  it  is  the  egg-laying  that 
matters.  Since,  as  Prof.  Wigglesworth  commented  earlier,  females 
are  liable  to  suffer  from  protein-shortage,  my  results  are  quite  con- 
sistent with  yours.  On  sugar  and  water  your  females  may  have  died 
young  because  they  were  suffering  from  serious  protein  shortage. 
My  experiment  suggests  that  if  you  deprive  a  female  of  her  ovary  or 
cause  it  to  regress,  then  she  does  not  suffer  from  protein  shortage  as 
much  as  she  would  if  she  were  laying  eggs. 

Gerki7ig:  Can  you  give  an  estimate  of  the  size  of  the  ovary  in 
relation  to  the  body?   Is  it  as  much  as  50  per  cent? 

Maynard  Smith:  I  do  not  know  exactly,  but  it  is  certainly  not  as 
much  as  that. 

Gerking:  In  the  fish  I  talked  about  the  ovary  may  weigh  as  much 
as  20  or  30  per  cent  of  body  weight  at  maturity.  Once  the  eggs  are 
shed  then  you  can  hardly  find  the  o^"ary.  Its  restoration  to  this  20 
or  30  per  cent  level  requires  a  great  amount  of  energy.  I  wanted  to 
point  out  that  in  both  the  fish  and  Drosophila  a  very  large  proportion 
of  metabolism  is  devoted  to  egg  production.   You  have  concluded 


Discussion  285 

that  egg  production  is  at  least  one  factor  responsible  for  ageing  in 
Drosophila,  but  I  have  been  unable  to  find  any  relation  between 
fecundity  and  age  in  egg-laying  fishes. 

Hinton:  In  most  flies  when  the  ovaries  ripen  the  abdomen  swells 
greatly,  and  the  ripened  ovaries  occupy  a  considerable  percentage 
of  the  total  volume  of  the  abdomen.  The  other  organ  systems  are 
frequently  much  displaced  by  the  ripened  ovaries,which  may  account 
for  a  high  percentage  of  the  total  weight  of  the  female. 

Maynard  Smith:  They  are  not  so  big  in  Drosophila.  Eggs  are 
pumped  through  at  a  rate  of  about  30  or  40  per  day,  but  the  ovary 
itself  is  never  very  large. 

Berg:  Is  the  ovary  a  self-regulatory  mechanism  in  the  fly?  A 
selective  effect  on  the  ovary  without  aff'ecting  hormonal  regulatory 
mechanisms  would  be  unusual. 

Maynard  Smith:  What  do  3^ou  think  the  regulatory  mechanism 
might  be  here?  The  ovary  is  not  itself  a  hormone  producer,  is  it? 

Wigglesworth :  One  of  the  main  detectable  abnormal  effects  on  the 
insect  of  raising  the  temperature  is  the  effect  on  hormone  action. 
You  can  get  an  insect  which  is  apparently  metabolizing  normally, 
but  you  knock  out  the  action  of  the  growth-promoting  hormone. 
The  ovary  certainly  appears  to  have  a  hormonal  influence,  a  sort  of 
feedback  influence,  upon  the  endocrine  system.  So  that  even  in  your 
ovariless  insects  produced  genetically  you  might  be  impairing  their 
endocrine  system  through  lack  of  this  feedback  mechanism. 

Maynard  Smith:  The  ovariless  flies  are  the  off'spring  of  females 
homozygous  for  the  mutant  grandchildless,  and  the  suspicion,  which 
is  not  adequately  demonstrated,  is  that  females  carrying  this  gene 
produce  eggs  without  pole  plasm.  This  would  explain  the  fact  that 
the  female  off'spring  have  no  ovaries  and  the  male  off'spring  no  testes. 
If  the  ovariless  females  had  lived  for  a  shorter  time  than  the  controls, 
I  would  have  said  something  else  was  wrong  too,  but  they  lived  50 
per  cent  longer.  I  was  very  reluctant  to  accept  this  simple  mechani- 
cal explanation,  that  they  live  longer  because  they  do  not  lay  eggs, 
but  everything  seemed  to  fit  in  so  well  that  until  something  does  not 
fit,  I  have  to  accept  it. 

Kershaw :  The  overwhelming  effect  of  ovaries  and  this  relation  to 
nutrition  has  been  shown  in  some  experiments  that  we  have  done. 
We  have  exactly  the  opposite  results  from  those  in  your  Drosophila. 
In  Aedes  aegypti,  which  depends  largely  on  blood  meals,  the  virgin 
females  live  for  a  much  shorter  time  than  the  normal  egg-laying 
females  (Lavoipierre,  M.  M.  J.  (1958).  Nature  (Lond.),  181,  1781). 
This  disparity  would  fit  in  with  the  complete  dislocation  of  nutrition 
invoked  by  the  disturbance  of  normal  ovarian  function. 


GROUP  DISCUSSION 

Comfort:  I  would  like  to  emphasize  the  variety  of  the  material  to 
which  we  have  been  obliged  to  apply  the  term  lifespan.  I  am 
inclined  to  say  that  if  a  single  parameter  must  be  used  to  designate 
a  curve — it  is  better  it  should  not  be,  but  if  it  must  be — for  most  of 
the  purposes  we  have  been  talking  about  I  would  favour  the  modal 
age  of  adult  death  which  Dr.  Benjamin  showed  us. 

Unfortunately  there  are  many  curves  for  which  you  cannot  use  the 
modal  age  of  death.  In  zoo  animals  there  is  effectively  no  mode, 
since  the  survival  curve  is  almost  an  arithmetic  straight  line  (Com- 
fort, A.  (1957).  Proc.  zool.  Soc.  Lond.,  128,  349;  Ciba  Found.  Coll. 
Ageing,  3,  14.) 

Another  possible  parameter  that  has  been  mentioned  is  the  median. 
It  has  the  advantage  for  experimental  purposes  that  you  need  not 
wait  till  the  animals  studied  are  all  dead — you  can  rush  into  print 
when  half  of  them  are  dead.  But  I  think  its  standard  error  is  a  little 
difficult  to  handle.  It  also  has  the  drawback  that  it  is  very  sensitive 
to  the  effects  of  environment  on  the  survival  curve.  The  last  decile 
is  far  more  stable  in  this  respect. 

You  could  also  use  the  limit.  The  limit  has  the  advantage  that 
even  in  small  populations  of  animals  one  or  two  commonly  survive 
much  longer  than  their  fellows — their  performance  is  a  better  index 
of  "physiological"  performance  than  the  crude  mean  or  median.  Its 
drawback  is  the  existence  of  a  large  number  of  doubtful  records  of 
very  old  age  in  man  and  animals. 

In  Bourliere's  curves  for  birds,  and  also  many  of  Beverton's 
curves  for  fish,  standing  mortality  at  low  ages  is  so  high  that  it  is 
effectively  independent  of  age;  the  most  obvious  parameter  is  the 
half-life,  but  since  these  populations  contain  some  long-lived 
individuals  the  limit  is  also  possible.  But  they  cannot  be  made  to 
indicate  what  happens  to  the  mortality  at  ages  which  are  so  rarely 
reached. 

To  compare  lifespans  we  might  fit  a  set  of  curves  and  compare 
their  time  scales.  Without  using  any  equations  I  superimposed 
those  for  the  K/B  Drosophila,  for  a  human  population  (1941  United 
States  males),  for  Murie's  wild  sheep,  and  for  my  thoroughbred 
mares,  on  different  time  scales,  by  fitting  the  last  three  quartiles  of 
each  unsmoothed  curve.  That  is  another  way  of  defining  and  com- 
paring lifespans ;  but  if  you  do  that  you  must  allow  for  the  fact  that 
man  has  a  uniquely  long  developmental  period,  whereas  sheep  have 

286 


Group  Discussion  287 

not,  while  in  Drosophila  I  used  only  the  imaginal  lifespan  and 
ignored  the  whole  of  its  previous  larval  career. 

I  have  not  included  among  these  examples  the  agricultural  type 
of  lifespan,  which  Dr.  Hartwig  mentioned  for  his  cattle  and  horses ; 
that  is  yet  another  question.  And  I  should  add  that  in  fish  kept  in 
the  laboratory  we  obtain  a  series  of  curves  under  different  degrees 
of  environmental  comfort  which  are  very  like  those  for  mammals, 
including  man.  In  all  cases  the  force  of  mortality  rises  with  in- 
creasing age. 

Muhlbock:  The  mouse  has  the  advantage  that  there  are  inbred 
strains  available,  as  you  know,  and  we  use  them  in  our  work  on 
cancer  research.  These  inbred  strains  come  from  brother-sister 
matings,  made  for  at  least  20  generations,  and  all  the  strains  I  shall 
refer  to  here  have  been  through  more  than  100  inbred  generations. 
Survival  curves  for  the  females  of  the  DBA  and  020  strains  show  that 
hybrids  from  the  two  strains  live  longer  than  the  pure  inbred  lines. 
An  analysis  of  the  DBA  strain  shows  that  the  males  die  first  and  the 
virgin  females  live  longest;  in  between  come  the  breeding  females. 
By  breeders  we  mean  females  which  are  allowed  to  rear  their  young. 
For  special  purposes  in  cancer  research  there  is  another  group  of 
females  which  is  described  as  force-bred.  That  means  that  the  young 
are  discarded  after  birth,  so  that  no  lactation  occurs.  The  lifespans 
of  the  force-bred  females  are  plotted  from  the  12th  month,  after 
the  fertility  period  has  ended,  so  their  shorter  lifespan  cannot  be  due 
to  accidents  in  pregnancy  and  there  must  be  some  influences  which 
affect  lifespan  in  the  second  half  of  life  when  the  fertility  period  has 
ended.  The  CBA  strain  is  one  of  the  longest-lived  strains  we  have; 
some  of  the  virgin  females  live  to  an  age  of  35  months.  Here  again 
the  virgin  females  have  the  longest  survival.  In  this  strain  the  males 
are  not  so  different,  but  the  force-bred  females,  with  rapid  pregnan- 
cies and  without  lactation,  have  a  shorter  survival.  But  that  is  not 
the  case  in  all  the  different  strains  we  have.  In  the  O20  strain  the 
differences  between  the  various  states  are  not  so  great  as  in  the  other 
ones.  There  are  therefore  differences  in  these  different  strains  but  I 
do  not  know  what  is  the  reason  for  them. 

Rockstein:  In  the  life-tables  of  the  male  houseflies  I  was  interested 
particularly  in  the  d^  values  because  from  about  the  10th  to  the  24th 
day  of  life  this  represents  a  fairly  large  part  of  the  population.  We 
are  really  at  the  peak  of  the  mortality  during  this  period  of  cohort 
existence.  In  the  females  there  is  a  grouping  in  the  d^  values,  so  that 
they  reach  a  peak,  then  fall  a  little,  and  then  reach  another  peak,  and 
so  on.  This  illustrates  the  idea  which  the  probit  curve  seems  to  sug- 
gest, namely  that  the  female  is  involved  in  a  more  complicated  type 


288  Group  Discussion 

of  mortality  for  the  populations,  so  that  at  a  different  age  an  addi- 
tional factor  seems  to  be  interjected  into  the  mortality  picture. 
Perhaps  Mr.  Perks  would  comment  on  these  tables  [not  printed]. 

Perks:  First,  I  find  it  rather  strange  that  you  have  such  a  large 
number  of  "ages"  in  your  life-tables.  We  actuaries,  of  course,  string 
out  the  rates  of  mortality  for  each  year  of  age,  but  that  is  for  the 
practical  purposes  of  life  assurance.  For  understanding  the  mortality 
that  underlies  the  life-table  we  would  certainly  compress  it,  and  we 
would  not  have  67  values  of  the  independent  variable.  We  would 
probably  group  these  in  fives  and  show  the  values  of  qx  for  five 
intervals  at  a  time.  That  is  a  general  question  of  presentation,  and 
of  trying  not  to  confuse  the  reader  with  too  many  figures. 

The  next  point  is  that  the  distribution  of  dx  for  male  houseflies 
gave  me  the  impression  of  a  curve  very  much  like  the  Karl  Pearson 
type  3  frequency  curve,  that  is  the  gamma  distribution,  that  comes 
up  to  a  peak  fairly  quickly  and  has  a  long  tail  away  to  the  right.  The 
mortality  curves  for  electric  light  bulbs  that  E.  G.  Pearson  published 
25  to  30  years  ago  had  very  much  of  that  characteristic ;  they  were 
fitted  fairly  well  by  the  type  3  distribution.  If  we  are  actually  to 
understand  anything  about  the  underlying  mathematical  processes 
of  mortality  curves,  we  should  start  with  the  simpler  organisms,  or 
simple  physical  objects,  and  electric  light  bulbs  are  particularly 
suitable  for  this  purpose.  You  can  get  them  fairly  homogeneous, 
and  put  them  on  a  uniform  circuit,  so  cutting  down  much  of  the 
extraneous  variation.  It  certainly  is  interesting  to  see  a  death 
curve  with  a  long  tail  to  the  right.  The  human  death  curve  tends  to 
have  the  tail  to  the  left — coming  up  slowly  to  the  peak,  and  then 
coming  down  very  sharply.  Beard  has  fitted  incomplete  gamma 
functions  to  a  number  of  human  life-tables,  but  he  had  to  do  a  bit  of 
manipulation  with  the  data  first,  to  remove  the  accident  and 
infectious  diseases  mortality,  otherwise  the  tail  on  the  left-hand  side 
would  not  asymptote  to  zero.  There  is  a  mathematical  model  that 
provides  some  analogy  with  the  death  process.  Imagine  that  a 
population  of  objects  are  put  on  a  wall,  and  shot  at  at  regular 
intervals  so  that  each  is  equally  likely  to  be  hit.  Then  suppose  you 
define  death  as  when  a  particular  object  has  been  hit  n  times;  then 
the  death  distribution  is  in  fact  the  type  3  distribution.  Thinking 
along  those  lines  may  help  us  to  get  mathematical  representations  of 
the  death  curves  of  more  complicated  objects.  I  am  particularly 
interested  to  see  that  the  housefly  appears  to  give  a  relatively  simple 
distribution. 

My  impression  of  the  female  table  is  that  it  is  rather  similar  to  the 
male,  except  that  the  peak  is  much  flatter.   So  far  as  the  so-called 


Group  Discussion  289 

subsidiary  peaks  are  concerned,  I  would  be  astonished  if  they  were 
not  just  the  result  of  random  error,  q^  is  irregular,  and  so  obviously 
no  graduation  process  has  been  applied.  I  assume  that  the  life-table 
was  obtained  by  following  the  history  of  a  cohort,  and  that  the  Ix 
figures  are  in  effect  the  numbers  actually  surviving  to  each  age,  the 
numbers  being  reduced  to  a  radix  of  a  thousand. 

Rockstein:  That  is  right.  There  were  about  4,000  animals  of  each 
sex  there,  3,875  females  and  slightly  more  males. 

Perks:  That  is  a  technique  that  with  humans  we  have  not  found 
very  helpful,  because  it  takes  100  years  to  follow  a  cohort  through. 
But  even  though  you  get  your  rates  of  mortaUty  that  way,  before  I 
would  draw  any  conclusions  whatever  from  bumps  on  the  dx  curve 
I  should  want  to  put  a  light  graduation  through  the  q^  values,  and 
then  recompute  the  l^  and  dx  columns.  You  would  have  to  apply  a 
goodness  of  fit  test.  But  a  quite  elementary  graphical  graduation 
would  probably  be  sufficient  to  get  rid  of  the  accidental  bumps  in  the 
dx  column. 

Gerking:  What  do  you  mean  by  a  light  graduation? 

Perks:  I  mean  putting  a  smooth  curve  through  the  points  repre- 
sented by  qxy  so  that  you  get  rid  of,  or  greatly  reduce,  the  random 
errors — on  the  hypothesis  that  if  you  had  a  much  larger  number  in 
your  sample  the  departures  from  the  smooth  curve  would  largely 
disappear.  I  agree  that  the  assumption  of  the  smooth  curve  for  the 
qx  is  only  a  hypothesis,  but  there  is  a  great  deal  of  observational 
evidence  for  that  assumption,  provided  you  keep  your  condition 
reasonably  constant. 

S acker:  When  you  say  smooth,  you  do  not  necessarily  mean 
simple? 

Perks:  No.  There  is  no  satisfactory  mathematical  definition  of 
smoothness.  There  has  been  some  controversy  in  the  Institute  of 
Actuaries  on  what  we  mean  by  smoothness,  and  some  people  suggest 
it  should  be  absence  of  roughness ! 

Rotblat:  How  sensitive  is  the  gamma  distribution  to  the  value  of  n? 

Perks:  You  can  get  a  wide  range  of  different  curves  with  different 
values  for  n. 

Comfort:  I  am  impressed  with  that  remark,  because  this  is  almost 
what  Failla  or  Szilard  have  suggested  is  in  fact  happening,  isn't  it? 
(Failla,  A.  (1958).  Proc.  Ageing  Conf.,  Gatlinburg.  Washington, 
D.C.:  A.I.B.S.,  in  press.  Szilard,  L.  (1959).  Proc.  nat.  Acad.  Set. 
(Wash.),  45,  30).  The  objects  are  actually  being  shot  at  by  radiation, 
and  this  may  be  one  of  the  causes  of  chromosome  deterioration. 

Perks:  If  you  are  interested  in  that,  R.  E.  Beard  developed  the 
subject  further  some  years  ago  (see  Appendix,  p.  302).  If  you  increase 

AGEING — ^V — 10 


290  Group  Discussion 

the  probability  of  being  hit  according  to  the  number  of  times  the 
object  has  been  hit  before,  and  if  you  proceed  further  and  increase 
the  speed  of  the  shooting,  the  mathematics  develop  in  the  direction 
of  the  Makeham  curve  and  certain  modifications  of  it. 

Maynard  Smith:  I  do  not  believe  for  one  moment  that  the  shape 
of  these  curves  has  anything  to  do  with  the  organism  that  has  been 
studied.  I  think  it  has  something  to  do  with  the  environment  in 
which  it  was  studied. 

Perks:  I  have  thought  about  this  problem  for  a  long  time  and  I 
believe  the  shape  has  something  to  do  with  time. 

Tanner:  You  would  expect  a  different-shaped  curve  in  a  less  long- 
lived  organism? 

Perks:  No,  I  think  it  is  the  cumulative  factor  in  life;  injuries  and 
so  on  are  additive,  or  cumulative  in  almost  a  geometrical  sense,  and 
you  must  expect  to  have  exponentials  coming  into  the  form  of  the 
mortality  rates. 

Tanner:  Does  this  make  the  change,  then,  from  the  ganmia  func- 
tion into  the  Makeham  type  of  curve?  You  think  that  the  reception 
state  changes  according  to  the  number  of  shots  impinging? 

Perks:  Yes. 

Holt:  In  the  models  for  fitting  the  depreciation  of  motor  cars 
death  is  assumed  to  result  from  either  single  big  accidents,  or  an 
accumulation  of  small  ones,  and  a  complex  death  curve  follows;  it 
seems  to  have  some  possible  application  to  animal  mortality. 

Rotblat:  I  am  concerned  to  know  what  lifespan  is  because  we  have 
to  express  some  of  our  findings  in  terms  of  lifespan.  For  example, 
people  often  speak  about  the  effect  of  radiation  in  causing  a  shorten- 
ing of  the  span  of  life  and  they  put  down  figures  of  the  percentage 
of  fife- shortening  per  rontgen.  But  everyone  I  have  asked  what  he 
meant  by  the  term  lifespan  gave  a  different  answer.  I  was  hoping 
that  perhaps  in  this  meeting  we  might  come  to  some  agreement  on 
that.  I  was  impressed  by  your  remarks  on  the  first  day.  Dr.  Benja- 
min, when  you  brought  in  what  you  called  the  senescence  peak. 
This  peak  seems  to  me  to  be  the  quantity  to  put  down  as  the  *'  normal 
lifespan".  I  was  particularly  interested  to  see  data  presented  by 
various  people  which  showed  that  although  the  shapes  of  the 
mortality  curves  may  differ  enormously,  nevertheless  for  a  given 
species  they  all  reach  nearly  the  same  end-point.  If  we  could  draw 
the  ideal  curve,  i.e.  if  we  could  eliminate  all  deaths  due  to  accidents, 
infections,  etc.,  then  the  "normal  lifespan"  would  be  obtained  im- 
mediately from  the  senescence  peak.  But  in  practice  this  will  be 
very  difficult,  because  most  mortality  curves  do  not  approximate  to 
this  ideal.  We  are,  therefore,  still  left  with  the  question  of  what  we 


Group  Discussion  291 

mean  by  lifespan.  If  one  could  establish,  once  and  for  all,  the  normal 
lifespan  for  a  given  species,  then  the  observed  mortality  curve  would 
tell  us  the  deviation  due  to  environmental  conditions,  such  as  being 
kept  in  a  zoo,  or  hunted.  Then  we  might  introduce  another  index  to 
take  these  conditions  into  account.  We  could  take  the  50  per  cent 
survival  time  from  the  actual  mortality  curve  and  the  ratio  of  this 
to  the  normal  lifespan  might  serve  as  an  index  of,  say,  longevity  or 
senescence.  These  two  quantities,  the  normal  lifespan  and  the  50 
per  cent  survival  time,  might  be  the  proper  parameters  to  use. 

Comfort:  The  limit,  as  I  say,  is  not  always  advantageous,  because 
of  the  very  great  divergence  in  the  reports  of  maximum  age  in 
animals.  My  own  feeling  is  that  once  you  can  get  a  family  of  curves 
like  this,  or  a  rough  indication  of  the  family,  you  could  perhaps  take 
a  point  10  per  cent  back  from  the  limit. 

Roiblat:  If  you  take  the  mortality  curve,  then  depending  on  the 
condition  in  which  the  animals  are  kept  we  would  obtain  different 
times  for  the  50  per  cent  survival  but  the  same  limit. 

Comfort :  I  am  thinking  of  cases  where  you  have  not  got  curves  as 
good  as  that.  If  you  do  have  such  a  family  of  curves,  there  is  no 
difficulty.  The  trouble  is  to  know  what  to  do  with  the  sort  of  thing 
that  Sacher  was  describing,  where  he  wants  to  compare  a  whole 
range  of  mammals ;  merely  to  get  a  rough  correlation  it  is  necessary 
to  give  some  sort  of  figure  which  one  can  compare. 

Perks:  The  difficulty  with  the  modal  value  is  that  it  is  influenced 
to  a  certain  extent  by  the  infantile  mortality,  and  by  mortality  from 
accidents  and  infections.  If  you  reduced  those  mortalities,  then,  as  a 
mere  piece  of  arithmetic,  the  mode  is  advanced  and  the  curve  of 
death  becomes  much  steeper,  by  the  mere  fact  that  more  lives 
survive  to  the  ages  at  which  the  rates  of  mortality  are  high.  I  have 
been  thinking  about  this  question  of  what  would  be  a  useful  measure 
of  lifespan.  Some  sort  of  technique  such  as  actuaries  use,  which  we 
call  multiple  decrement  technique,  might  be  used,  at  least  in  theory. 
It  is  rather  laborious,  but  if  you  could  separate  from  the  death  curve 
all  those  deaths  which  have  nothing  to  do  with  lifespan — accidental, 
predatory,  infantile,  and  anticipatory  deaths, — then  there  is  a 
technique  for  getting  a  residual  life-table,  a  hypothetical  life-table, 
that  is  concerned  only  with  those  causes  of  death  which  affect 
biological  lifespan.  This  would  have  all  the  disturbances  taken  out  of 
it,  and  of  course  you  would  get  a  much  later  and  taller  death  curve. 

Comfort:  The  trouble  with  that  is  to  know  which  causes  are  age- 
dependent. 

Rockstein:  I  think  we  are  being  unrealistic  about  this.  I  would 
suggest  that  mean  longevity  seems  to  be  a  thing  that  you  could 

10* 


292  Group  Discussion 

obtain  all  the  time,  regardless  of  the  cause  of  death.  If  you  are  con- 
cerned with  a  standard  value  or  reference  against  which  you  can  com- 
pare, why  not  use  an  animal,  as  you  do  in  the  laboratory,  from 
which  you  can  get  such  data?  I  have  been  amazed  that  for  over  ten 
generations  in  the  housefly,  for  example,  one  can  continue  to  get  the 
same  mean  value.  I  say  this  value  is  a  good  basis  for  comparison  in 
an  experimental  study.  With  the  white  rat,  under  specific  laboratory 
conditions,  for  example,  the  only  thing  that  will  vary  in  irradiation 
tests  will  be  the  extent  or  dosage  of  radiation.  With  humans  we  run 
into  another  problem  because  we  always  deal  with  life  expectation 
based  on  a  population  that  is  not  always  an  identical  cohort,  but  for 
experimental  studies  we  can  get  cohorts  of  an  identical  nature  for 
inbred  lines  of  a  number  of  species  of  lower  animals.  I  do  not  see 
why  the  mean  longevity  is  not  as  good  a  criterion  as  anything  else ; 
it  considers  the  accidental  deaths,  it  considers  the  possible  disturb- 
ance that  the  mode  would  have  from  having  had  early  deaths  or 
accidental  deaths,  and  even  a  tail  at  the  end  resulting  from  the 
extended  longevity  of  the  few  long-lived  cohort  members. 

Perks:  I  am  sure  you  are  right  for  your  problem,  but  diff'erent 
problems  require  different  solutions. 

Rotblat:  If  we  say,  for  example,  that  radiation  causes  a  contraction 
of  the  scale  of  life,  and  suppose  we  are  dealing  with  the  ideal  case  in 
which  all  animals  die  of  old  age,  then  for  the  irradiated  animals  we 
would  obtain  the  same  curve  but  bodily  shifted  to  the  left.  This 
would  be  very  nice,  but  in  practice  it  may  not  be  so.  It  may  be  that 
radiation  has  caused  earlier  deaths  without  changing  the  end-point. 
The  curve  would  then  change  completely.  Which  of  these  will 
actually  happen  depends  upon  the  effect  that  radiations  have  on  the 
lifespan. 

Sacher:  The  average  is  certainly  the  first  quantity  to  use  in  the 
experimental  situation,  but  you  cannot  characterize  all  the  effects 
of  radiations,  or  of  any  other  environmental  influence,  in  terms  of  a 
single  parameter.  Empirically  you  can  then  proceed  to  the  succeed- 
ing central  moments.  The  question  is  to  find  out  what  parameters 
of  the  survival  curve  are  being  influenced  by  the  particular  environ- 
mental factors  under  investigation.  I  have  pointed  out  how,  in 
studies  of  radiation  effects  on  mice,  you  could  characterize  the  eff'ects 
of  radiations  in  terms  of  the  A  and  the  a  parameter  of  the  Gompertz 
equation  [q^  =  Ae°^].  A  single  dose  of  radiation — to  restate  what  I 
said  yesterday — causes  a  change  in  A,  without  a  change  in  a.  Con- 
tinuous exposure  causes  a  change  in  the  a  coefficient.  It  is  perfectly 
true  that  this  Gompertz  equation  is  not  an  entirely  adequate  des- 
cription of  the  life-tables  of  natural  populations,  but  it  should  be 


Group  Discussion  293 

borne  in  mind  that  this  is  an  oversimpHfied  form,  suitable  for  dis- 
cussing general  principles.  In  application  to  data,  more  complicated 
expressions  are  used.  Each  major  disease  category  needs  a  separate 
Gompertz  term,  as  has  been  shown  by  Simms  (1940.  Science,  91,  7). 
In  addition  the  experiment  need  not  be  simply  proportional  to  age, 
a?,  but  may  be  a  function  of  x.  Thus  the  general  expression  for  the 
description  of  mortality  in  terms  of  a  summation  of  Gompertz  terms 
is 

gx  =  2  A^efi^^) 

i  =  l 

Perks :  If  you  are  fitting  mathematical  expressions  to  your  data  of 
statistical  distribution,  then  clearly  you  estimate  the  parameters, 
and  your  estimated  values  for  the  parameters  sum  up  the  statistics. 
I  thought  the  problem  we  were  really  talking  about  was  how  to 
characterize  statistics  for  which  you  have  not  got  a  mathematical 
expression.  For  a  single  measure  to  be  sufficient  the  distribution 
would  have  to  be  a  very  simple  one,  such  as  l^  =  an  exponential,  in 
which  case  you  have  got  a  single  parameter  and  the  measure  might 
be  the  constant  rate  of  mortality  or  the  half-life.  In  any  other  case, 
you  cannot  sum  up  the  distribution  by  a  single  measure,  and  you 
cannot  even  say  that  any  particular  measure  is  the  best  one.  All  you 
can  say  is  that  for  some  purposes  one  measure  may  be  better  than 
another.  You  have  to  accept  that  the  expectation  of  life  or  the  half- 
life  or  mode  or  whatever  you  may  regard  as  the  lifespan,  gives  you 
only  part  of  the  information  contained  in  the  statistics. 

When  the  life-tables  of  different  animals  are  compared,  it  may  be 
that  the  lifespan  is  good  enough,  and  I  think  that  view  was  expressed 
yesterday.  When  the  mortalities  of  the  same  species  in  different 
environments  are  compared,  I  would  agree  that  expectation  of  life 
is  probably  as  good  as  any.  In  general  terms,  if  you  are  going  to  have 
more  than  one  figure  as  a  measure  of  a  death  curve,  probably  the 
1st,  2nd,  and  3rd  moments  of  the  death  curve  would  be  as  good  as 
any.  It  is  not  until  you  get  a  mathematical  expression  for  the  death 
curve  that  you  can  really  say  that  any  parameters  are  better 
estimators  than  any  others. 

Benjamin:  All  this  discussion  of  lifespan  seems  to  be  only  a  means 
to  an  end ;  we  really  want  to  get  away  from  lifespans  to  considering 
the  ageing  effects,  for  example,  of  changes  in  environment.  For  that 
we  really  want  two  things.  First,  we  need  some  kind  of  function 
which  is  as  discriminating  as  possible  of  the  effects  of  ageing,  so  that 
it  is  very  sensitive.  That  suggests  that  what  is  wanted  is  the  middle 
part  of  the  survival  curve  where  a  small  change  in  the  survival  risk 


294  Group  Discussion 

may  make  a  difference  between  the  curve  bulging  up  one  way,  or 
bulging  down  the  other.  The  largest  possible  dispersion  of  effects  is 
found  here,  which  brings  us  to  the  idea  of  the  50  per  cent  survival 
age  point.  To  get  a  time  scale  which  enables  comparisons  to  be 
made  between  different  kinds  of  animals,  on  the  other  hand,  you 
need  something  which  is  not  sensitive  to  that  kind  of  change.  This 
suggests  that  you  should  use  the  peak  of  the  curve  of  deaths,  because 
although  the  height  of  the  peak  is  ^'e^y  much  correlated  with  antici- 
pated deaths  and  so  on,  the  actual  movement  of  the  peak  is  not 
particularly  sensitive.  So  while  it  is  unprofitable  to  talk  about  an 
ideal  lifespan,  it  is  quite  practical  and  profitable  to  look  at  large 
families  of  curves  of  death  for  the  same  species,  and  see  what  kind 
of  shape  they  tend  to  in  general,  so  that  you  can  get  for  the  different 
species  a  typical  modal  attained  length  of  life,  which  you  could  use 
as  the  time  scale. 

Comfort:  This  is  Bodenheimer's  "physiological  longevity" 
(Bodenheimer,  F.  S.  (1938).  Problems  of  animal  ecology.  Oxford 
University  Press). 

Perks :  I  would  like  to  plead  that  you  should  all  take  an  interest  in 
the  international  actuarial  notation.  I  think  it  is  a  very  good  thing 
that  all  scientists  should  use  the  same  notation  if  there  is  one  which 
is  generally  accepted.  Dr.  Benjamin  has  included  some  of  it  in  his 
paper  and  I  think  it  probably  could  be  extended  to  cover  all  your 
needs. 

We  have  heard  the  phrases  lifespan  and  ageing  over  and  over 
again  but  I  do  not  think  you  will  ever  succeed  in  rigorously  defining 
them.  I  think  they  are  best  left  as  rather  vague  concepts,  as  we 
know  in  general  what  we  are  talking  about. 

I  have  been  a  little  puzzled  by  the  extent  to  which  logs  have  been 
taken  of  various  observed  figures.  To  me  it  only  confuses  the  issues, 
particularly  when  you  have  a  graph  on  a  logarithmic  scale,  although 
I  do  understand  that  sometimes  it  is  necessary  to  do  that  to  com- 
press the  graph  to  reasonable  dimensions.  But  there  is  no  excuse  for 
taking  logs  in  arithmetic  merely  to  get  rid  of  some  of  the  variations, 
and  apparently  to  produce  a  correlation  or  regression  which  possibly 
is  not  there  if  you  do  not  take  logs.  What  does  the  logarithm  of  a 
residual  mean?  When  you  take  a  logarithm  of  the  cephalization 
index  and  then  associate  it  with  a  logarithm  of  body  weight,  and 
finish  up  with  a  logarithm  of  a  lifespan,  what  does  it  all  mean? 

There  may  be  a  very  simple  technique  that  might  be  useful  for 
those  of  you  who  study  lifespans  or  mortality  of  animals  in  their 
natural  state.  I  understand  that  it  is  very  difficult  to  get  their  ages, 
but  often  I  imagine  you  will  find  stationary  populations  in  the  natural 


Group  Discussion  295 

state.  So  if  you  take  the  crude  death  rate  over  an  interval  of  time, 
then  a  fair  measure  of  the  expectation  of  hfe,  in  terms  of  that  inter- 
val, is  the  reciprocal  of  the  crude  death  rate,  provided  that  popula- 
tion is  stationary,  or  nearly  stationary.  If  it  is  not  nearly  stationary, 
you  can  probably  make  an  approximate  adjustment. 

The  only  other  thing  I  want  to  say  is  that  the  actuary's  use  of  life- 
tables  is  very  different  from  yours.  The  life-table  is  not  an  end 
in  itself  for  actuaries ;  it  is  merely  a  step  on  the  way  from  a  set  of 
mortality  rates  to  the  calculation  of  premium  rates,  reserves, 
bonuses  and  surrender  values. 

Sacher:  Logarithms  are  not  introduced  to  mystify.  They  are 
actually  a  great  convenience  for  computation.  The  classical  law  of 
allometry  is  that  one  dimension  of  an  organism  is  related  to  another 
as  [F  =  AX'^']  so  that  one  of  them  varies  as  a  power  of  the  other; 
these  allometric  relations  are  almost  always  presented  graphically 
on  a  double  logarithmic  scale.  When  you  take  the  log  of  Y  and  the 
log  of  X  there  is  then  a  linear  relationship  between  these  values. 
There  are  great  advantages  in  using  logarithms  to  fit  a  power  func- 
tion by  least  squares.  All  of  these  considerations  apply  to  brain 
weight,  body  weight,  and  lifespan  as  I  have  analysed  them  here.  The 
index  of  cephalization  is  a  pure  number.  It  is  the  logarithm  of  the 
ratio  of  the  actual  brain  weight  of  a  species  to  the  brain  weight  that 
is  predicted  by  the  overall  regression  of  log  brain  weight  on  log  body 
weight. 

Logarithms  are  also  convenient  in  the  present  application  because 
they  introduce  the  property  that  all  of  the  observations  have 
approximately  the  same  statistical  weight  in  terms  of  the  logarithmic 
transform.  The  lifespan,  brain  weight  and  body  weight  measurement 
all  have  about  the  same  percentage  error  from  mice  to  elephants  and 
therefore  the  error  in  logarithmic  units  is  roughly  constant,  even 
though  the  original  absolute  values  have  a  million-fold  range  of 
variation — from  5  grams  or  so  to  5,000  kilograms. 

Perks:  I  agree  that  if  you  have  reason  for  a  relationship  of  that 
form,  then  logarithms  may  ease  the  arithmetical  processes. 

Sacher:  There  is  no  reason,  in  the  sense  of  a  general  theory  of 
growth  and  of  the  relationship  between  parts  of  an  organism,  that  is 
capable  of  explaining  why  the  allometric  relationships  should  be  of 
this  form.  It  is,  however,  a  fact  of  observation  that  the  power 
function  does  describe  these  relations,  and  no  other  function  does  it 
as  well. 

Comfort:  It  is  also  true  that  in  drawing  the  survival  curves  of  birds 
and  small  mammals,  where  over  a  large  part  of  their  lifespan  their 
mortality  is  so  high  that  it  is  almost  age-independent,  most  people 


296  Group  Discussion 

use  an  arith./log  scale,  arith.  for  time  and  log  for  survival,  so  that 
constant  mortality  gives  a  straight  line. 

Tanner:  I  have  been  sitting  here  for  the  last  half-hour  with  a  very 
strong  feeling  of  dejd  vu.  The  people  who  are  interested  in  growth 
have  been  fitting  growth  curves  with  decreasing  enthusiasm  for  about 
35  or  40  years.  It  seems  to  me  that  you  are  pursuing  a  vertiginous 
and  descending  pathway ! 

I  do  not  think  there  is  anything  in  the  general  aspects  of  growth 
which  leads  one  to  suppose  that  the  allometric  relationship  is  very 
useful  in  general.  There  may  be  instances  where  it  is  necessary,  not 
from  any  theoretical  considerations  but  because  using  logs  produces 
a  straight  line;  I  do  not  think  that  there  can  be  any  other  justifica- 
tion. 

Sacher:  A  transformation,  such  as  the  logarithmic,  cannot  increase 
the  amount  of  information  contained  in  a  set  of  data,  so  if  the  cor- 
relation of  the  transformed  variables  is  0  •  99  +  this  expresses  a  fact 
about  the  data,  i.e.  that  only  a  fraction  of  a  percentage  of  the  total 
variance  is  error  variance  when  the  proper  functional  relation  be- 
tween the  variables  is  found.  No  a  priori  justification  is  needed  for 
the  use  of  the  power  function.  In  my  own  and  Brody's  data  there 
is  no  question  about  its  appropriateness.  The  fact  that  some  other 
data  are  adequately  rectified  by  a  linear  plot  is  interesting  but  it 
cannot  contravene  the  allometric  relations  as  they  have  been  estab- 
lished in  many  other  cases. 

Chitty:  My  particular  problem  is  to  find  out  why  animal  popu- 
lations in  nature  do  not  go  on  increasing  indefinitely,  and  what  it  is 
that  they  die  of.  ^lost  people  up  to  the  present  have  considered  that 
deaths  in  nature  could  be  almost  entirely  accounted  for  through 
predation  or  epidemic  disease,  heavy  infestation  with  parasites,  or 
food  shortage,  but  it  is  now  clear  that  this  is  a  wholly  inadequate 
explanation,  particularly  for  the  huge  mortalities  which  occur  in  the 
young  stages.  The  problem  arises — what  exactly  is  it  that  they  die 
of?  The  suggestion  was  first  made  by  P.  H.  Leslie  and  R.  M.  Ranson 
in  1940  (J.  Anim.  EcoL,  9,  27),  for  the  field  mouse,  that  the  life-table 
type  of  explanation  might  be  applied  to  field  populations.  In  the 
laboratory  you  recognize  that,  with  age,  there  is  an  increasing  prob- 
ability of  death  from  a  variety  of  causes  which  are  peculiar  to  the 
particular  environments — that  is  to  say  a  group  of  mice  in  one 
laboratory  would  not  have  the  same  final  causes  of  death  as  they 
would  in  another — but  in  each  case  there  would  be  the  common  fact 
that  as  they  grew  older  they  became  increasingly  liable  to  die  of 
whatever  it  was  that  was  peculiar  to  those  environments.  The 
field  evidence  strongly  suggests  that  this  may  be  a  profitable  way  of 


Group  Discussion  297 

looking  at  natural  populations,  too.  The  problem  now  becomes  to 
try  and  find  some  general  law  which  is  applicable  in  spite  of  the 
fantastic  variety  of  conditions  in  the  field.  I  think  one  would  never 
hope  to  find  any  common  causes  of  death  associated  with  the  actual 
time  of  death  in  nature.  Every  environment  differs  in  its  hazards 
frona  every  other  one.  The  point  is,  can  we  find  anything  in  the 
properties  of  the  animals  which  does  obey  some  sort  of  general  law 
under  all  these  varied  circumstances?  In  other  words,  what  is  it  that 
makes  an  animal  increasingly  likely  to  die  regardless  of  what 
actually  kills  it  in  the  end?  We  might  divide  the  problem  into  both 
multiple  and  single  processes.  The  final  causes  of  death  would  be  the 
multiple  processes,  and  there  we  have  very  little  hope  of  introducing 
much  unity;  but  by  concentrating  on  susceptibility  there  may  be 
some  hope  of  finding  a  common  process  which  can  equally  well  be 
studied  in  those  animals  which  live  to  a  great  age  in  the  laboratory, 
and  those  which  die  at  a  much  younger  age  in  nature.  Exactly  how 
one  goes  about  this  I  do  not  know,  and  that  is  the  point  at  which  my 
work  is  hung  up.  I  have  to  account  for  very  violent  changes  in  the 
probability  of  survival  at  different  times  of  the  population  cycle,  and 
exactly  where  does  one  go  to  look  in  the  organism  for  something 
which  may  be  an  index  of  this  change  in  properties?  That  seems  to 
me  to  be  very  similar  to  the  problems  with  which  one  is  faced  in 
trying  to  account  for  the  increasing  probability  of  death  with  age. 
Animals  in  the  field  very  seldom  live  to  an  age  at  which  you  can  say 
they  are  senescent.  Nevertheless,  it  is  a  fact  that  even  at  a  much 
younger  age  than  they  die  at  in  captivity,  some  species  periodically 
show  this  very  great  increase  in  probability  of  dying  (see  Green,  R.  G. 
and  Evans,  C.  A.  (1940).  J.  Wildlife  Mgmt,  4,  220,  267,  347).  The 
question  arises  of  whether  or  not  we  should  regard  these  as  problems 
of  senescence  and  ageing,  or  as  much  more  analogous  to  the  high 
probability  of  human  beings  dying  young,  or  whether  age  is  irrelevant 
and  some  index  of  physiological  condition  is  the  only  thing  worth 
trying  to  find  in  any  species. 

Tanner:  There  does  not  seem  to  me  to  be  any  connexion  between 
the  situation  in  non-domesticated  animals  and  the  situation  in  the 
human.  All  the  other  mammals  and  birds  would  have  died  early  in 
terms  of  human  growth.  The  human  was  the  only  animal  which 
seemed  to  be  surviving  long  enough  to  experience  senescent  processes. 
Cellular  ageing  might  paradoxically  haA^e  been  closer  to  the  situation 
in  man,  than  is  the  situation  in  the  passerine,  for  example.  After 
what  Dr.  Chitty  has  said  I  feel  like  withdrawing  this  comment, 
because  if  in  other  species  the  probability  of  their  dying  in  the  field 
increases  with  age,  this  is  the  fundamental  thing.   It  comes  back  to 


298  Group  Discussion 

what  I  was  trying  to  say  before,  that  just  as  in  growth  we  talk  about 
developmental  age,  so  we  can  talk  about  the  probability  of  death  in 
any  given  situation  as  developmental  age  further  down  the  scale. 
If  that  is  really  true,  if  these  analogies  can  be  made  the  same  in 
animals  and  man,  then  we  can  use  the  results  of  both  field  workers 
and  the  experimentalists  much  more  usefully  to  shed  light  on  the 
human  situation. 

Comfort :  The  figures  we  get  for  small  mammals  have  often  misled 
people  into  thinking  that  all  mammals  behave  in  this  way.  We  have 
only  very  few  figures  for  the  larger  ones  but  there  is  some  evidence 
that  many  larger  mammals  in  the  wild  achieve  high  ages  quite 
often.  I  would  be  very  surprised  if  some  of  the  larger  wild  ungulates 
didn't  live  long  enough  to  senesce.  Wild  horses,  if  one  had  the  chance 
of  observing  them,  may  perhaps  live  to  a  fair  proportion  of  the  age 
reached  by  tame  horses. 

Sacher:  Porcupines  can  survive  until  they  are  so  arthritic  they  can 
hardly  climb  trees — nobody  comes  up  against  them  except  mountain 
lions. 

Verzdr:  Dr.  Chitty,  did  you  imply  that  the  time  of  survival  for  a 
certain  species  depends  on  the  number  of  individuals  living  at  the 
time?  If  you  put  a  pair  of  fish  in  a  pond,  they  will  have  certain 
survival  characteristics  for  their  age.  When  the  pond  becomes  full 
with  fish  the  population  will  become  constant.  Then  the  individual 
survival  curve  will  probably  be  different,  otherwise  the  number  of 
fish  would  continue  to  increase,  which  is  impossible  because  there  is 
not  enough  food. 

Chitty:  The  number  alive  has  a  very  great  influence  on  the  survival 
rate  but  the  survival  time  can  certainly  not  be  predicted  from  the 
numbers  of  the  animals  alone.  One  must  also  take  into  account  the 
behaviour  of  the  population ;  experimentally  you  can  have  a  family 
group  of  50  animals  which  is  perfectly  amicable  and  has  a  good 
survival  rate,  yet  with  two  animals  in  a  similar  space  who  are 
strangers,  the  survival  time  of  one  of  them  will  be  about  half  a  day. 
The  presence  of  other  hostile  animals  of  the  same  sort  is,  I  think,  one 
of  the  strongest  environmental  factors  which  does  affect  survi^^al, 
regardless  of  the  amount  of  food  available. 


Danielli:  I  shall  not  attempt  in  any  way  to  summarize  this  meet- 
ing. I  have  been  considerably  refreshed  by  it  in  many  ways.  We 
have  learned,  for  example,  that  man,  skunks  and  porcupines  are 
amongst  the  few  animals  who  know  how  to  survive  into  the  period  of 


Group  Discussion  299 

senescence.    The  nature  of  this  grouping  probably  has  some  moral 
significance. 

A  point  that  impressed  me  very  much  was  the  difficulty  of  evaluat- 
ing the  significance  of  data  obtained  from  animals  in  captivity,  which 
are  living  in  conditions  of  constant  diet,  no  exercise,  constant  illumin- 
ation, very  little  in  the  way  of  seasonal  change,  and  so  on,  to  which 
even  animals  which  have  been  selected  for  laboratory  purposes  are 
not  really  100  per  cent  adapted.  After  all,  the  period  of  adaptation 
to  laboratory  life  is  comparatively  short  compared  with  the  period  of 
evolution.  I  do  not  really  know  what  can  be  done  about  that.  What 
is  outstandingly  important  is  that,  wherever  possible,  a  pathologist 
should  look  at  the  animals  when  dead,  and  this  should  be  done  for 
insects  as  much  as  for  any  other  form  of  animal  life. 

The  concept  of  a  biological  time  scale  continues  to  interest  us, 
though  it  is  obviously  even  vaguer  than  some  of  the  other  concepts, 
which,  as  ]\Ir.  Perks  remarked,  are  better  kept  vague.  However,  this 
particular  one  is  of  no  use  to  us  unless  we  can  measure  it.  It  is  useless 
to  continue  to  use  it  without  exact  definition. 

Whatever  the  source,  if  we  exclude  accidents  to  which  the  individ- 
ual concerned  does  not  contribute,  in  most  instances  susceptibility 
to  mortality  in  a  species  or  a  strain  does  develop  in  a  typical  manner. 
It  is  perfectly  clear  from  what  has  been  said  that  if  one  cause  of 
mortality  is  removed,  for  most  of  the  animals  for  which  we  have  had 
data  analysed  in  sufficient  detail  here,  some  other  cause  of  mortality 
would  rapidly  cause  life  to  come  to  an  end  in  the  individuals  which 
have  survived.  The  total  gain  of  lifespan  which  would  result  from 
eliminating  one  cause  of  death  is  not  great.  In  other  words,  opera- 
tionally we  appear  to  be  dealing  with  a  unitary  process.  Whether 
ageing  is  in  fact  unitary,  however,  cannot  be  determined  from  the 
data  we  have  been  presented  with  so  far.  I  am  not  at  all  clear  to 
what  extent  Maynard  Smith's  theory  of  synchronization  of  inde- 
pendent lethal  processes  is  a  valid  one,  but  it  is,  I  am  sure,  a  very 
important  matter  indeed  to  have  had  raised,  and  one  which  necessi- 
tates a  good  deal  of  further  thought  and  investigation. 

The  concept  of  a  limit  to  the  life  of  a  tissue  which  is  set  in  terms  of 
the  energy  conversion  per  unit  mass  per  life  cycle  is  a  very  attractive 
one,  but  possibly  dangerously  attractive.  If  we  accept  it  at  its  face 
value,  it  presumably  means  that  living  matter  commits  accidents 
at  a  rate  which  is  proportional  to  the  rate  of  energy  conversion  in  it  ; 
this  is  quite  a  tenable  view  physically,  and  one  which  must  surely  be 
open  to  investigation.  We  need  more  data,  for  example  from  the 
study  of  hibernating  animals,  and  from  the  use  of  metabolic  poisons. 
We  ought  to  use  metabolic  poisons  and  radiation  in  attempts  to 


300  Group  Discussion 

desynchronize  some  of  the  processes  which  may  be  synchronized.  It 
may  be  in  this  connexion  that  we  could  make  some  progress  by 
studying  systems  in  which  one  is  normally  dealing  with  symbiosis. 
Here  the  breaking-up  of  the  symbiotic  relationship  might  reveal 
phenomena  which  would  be  difficult  to  reveal  by  any  other  method. 

A  cellular  approach  to  ageing  would  under  certain  circumstances 
facilitate  examination  of  the  actual  process  of  ageing,  if  this  is  in 
fact  to  some  degree  unitary,  but  I  am  not  convinced  that  this  is  so 
with  higher  animals.  The  ageing  process  in  higher  animals  may  be 
fundamentally  a  function  of  the  complex  of  cells  and  operate  at  a 
higher  level  of  organization  than  is  present  in  cells. 

Another  point  which  we  have  not  discussed  yet,  but  which  we  are 
probably  all  agreed  upon,  is  that  it  is  quite  possible  that  lifespan  in 
man,  as  we  see  it  now,  is  an  entirely  accidental  by-product  of 
selection  for  breeding  efficiently  at  a  much  earlier  age,  and  does  not 
in  any  sense  correspond  to  social  needs  of  the  moment.  An  accumu- 
lation of  experience  has  become  much  more  important,  or  at  any 
rate  equally  as  important  as  physical  vigour;  therefore  socially 
speaking  there  is  a  very  good  case  for  anything  which  will  enable  us 
to  modify  the  expectation  of  life  in  a  radical  manner.  Looking  at 
this  from  the  long-term  point  of  view,  I  think  the  possibilities  for  so 
doing  would  in  fact  be  good  by  chemical  means,  provided  ageing 
does  in  fact  occur  by  a  unitary  process.  If  on  the  other  hand  it  is 
due  to  a  very  large  number  of  non-unitary  processes  which  do  not 
have  any  common  mechanism,  then  I  think  the  chemical  approach  to 
extension  of  lifespan  is  fraught  with  so  many  difficulties  that  it  is 
hardly  worth  considering.  To  illustrate  the  order  of  magnitude  which 
I  would  expect  if  there  is  a  unitary  process  involved,  I  would  like  to 
refer  briefly  to  the  rate  of  mutation,  or  rather  the  rate  at  which 
damage  is  caused  by  radiation.  It  is  of  course  a  tenable  hypothesis 
that  somatic  mutation  is,  one  way  or  another,  directly  concerned 
with  the  ageing  process.  There  are  cells  which  are  killed  by  a  dosage 
of  the  order  of  100  r. ;  there  are  other  cells  which  require  a  dosage  of 
1,000,000  r.  to  kill  them.  This  difference  of  four  orders  of  magnitude 
is  not  at  present,  as  far  as  I  know,  accountable  for  in  any  cytological 
or  physiological  terms  whatever.  Therefore,  if  ageing  were  associ- 
ated primarily  with  somatic  mutation,  or  some  other  generalized 
mechanism  of  this  type,  we  might  expect  that  under  suitable  con- 
ditions lifespans  could  be  varied  by  that  order  of  magnitude.  This 
sounds  a  little  like  science  fiction,  but  all  of  us  have  seen  so  much  of 
what  we  regarded  as  science  fiction  20  years  ago  turning  up  as 
reality,  that  I  think  we  must  envisage  the  possibility  that  we  may  be 
able  to  make  radical  changes  in  lifespan. 


Group  Discussion  301 

My  final  comment  is  that  I  regard  it  as  singularly  unfortunate  that 
so  much  of  the  research  on  cancer  is  conducted  essentially  without 
reference  to  the  phenomena  of  ageing,  of  which  I  think  it  is  a  part. 
I  believe  that  not  only  would  a  study  of  ageing  benefit  from  more 
knowledge  of  what  is  going  on  in  older  animals,  but  the  whole  field 
of  investigation  of  cancer  might  benefit  equally,  and  perhaps  far 
more.  This  for  the  simple  reason  that  to  a  very  considerable  degree, 
at  the  moment,  cancer  experiments  are  conducted  by  putting  trans- 
plantable tumours  into  young,  often  rapidly  growing  animals,  and 
then  seeing  what  could  be  done  in  the  way  of  chemotherapy  in  these 
conditions — a  thing  which  does  not  approximate  very  closely  to 
what  happens  in  the  average  human  patient.  Prof.  Miihlbock's 
laboratory  is  a  shining  example  of  an  institute  where  this  isolation 
of  cancer  research  from  ageing  research  does  not  occur,  but  I  hope 
it  will  not  long  continue  to  be  one  of  the  very  few  shining  examples. 


V 


APPENDIX 

NOTE  ON  SOME  MATHEMATICAL  MORTALITY 

MODELS 

R.  E.  Beard 

Pearl  Assurance  Co.  Ltd.,  London 

1.  A  satisfying  basis  for  a  law  of  mortality  would  be  a  formula 
that,  starting  from  some  fundamental  concepts  about  the  biological 
ageing  process,  led  to  a  distribution  of  deaths  by  age  which  was 
comparable  with  observational  data.  Such  comparison  would  not  be 
simple  and  straightforward  because  environmental  and  secular 
factors  would  introduce  distortions  as  compared  with  the  theoretical 
underlying  distribution. 

2.  In  the  course  of  numerical  work,  extending  over  a  number  of 
years,  on  the  expression  of  human  mortality  functions  by  mathe- 
matical formulae,  various  attempts  have  been  made  by  the  writer 
to  develop  an  approach  on  this  basis.  The  results  obtained  have  not 
led  to  any  satisfying  formulae,  but  they  are  suggestive  of  different 
lines  of  approach  and  have  been  summarized  below  in  the  hope  they 
may  be  of  value  to  others  interested  in  the  subject.  The  note  follows 
the  sequence  in  which  the  ideas  have  developed  in  the  mind  of  the 
writer  and  leads  from  considerations  based  on  the  force  of  mortality, 
/x^,  to  those  based  on  the  curve  of  deaths,  ^xjl^. 

3.  The  first  mathematical  expression  which  provided  a  reasonable 
representation  of  the  observed  force  of  mortality  in  human  data  was 
that  first  proposed  by  Gompertz  (1825)  and  later  modified  by  Make- 
ham  (1867).  Basically  the  "law"  was  derived  by  postulating  a 
relationship  between  the  rate  of  change  of  the  force  of  mortality  at 
any  age  and  its  value  at  that  age.  The  next  significant  modification 
to  the  Makeham  law  was  the  system  of  curves  devised  by  Perks  in 
1932  and  of  which  the  important  formula  was  the  logistic.  Many 
human  life-tables  have  been  graduated  by  this  basic  cur^'e,  modified 
in  some  instances  to  allow  for  special  features  of  the  data,  particu- 
larly at  the  younger  and  early  middle  ages,  and  the  clear  fact  emerges 
that  adult  human  mortality  can  be  very  well  represented  by  a 
logistic  curve  of  the  form 

ix^-  A  =  Be^^'Kl+De^')  (1) 

302 


Appendix  303 

which  will  be  referred  to  as  a  Perks  curve  since  this  is  the  name  by 
which  it  is  generally  known  by  actuaries  (Perks,  1932;  Beard,  1936, 
1939a,  1951a,  1952a;  Registrar  General,  1951;  Mortality  of  Assured 
Lives,  1956). 

4.  Now  [Xg  is  the  ratio  of  the  ordinate  at  age  x  of  the  curve  of 
deaths  to  the  area  under  the  curve  above  age  x.  We  may  look  upon 
the  curve  of  deaths  as  a  frequency  distribution  of  deaths  by  age  at 
death  and  for  many  types  of  frequency  curves  it  will  be  found  that 
this  ratio  has  a  sigmoid  form.  It  is  not  apparent  whether  the  satis- 
factory representation  of  /x^  by  a  Perks  curve  is  because  the  formula 
has  a  theoretical  significance  or  because  the  formula  does  provide  a 
good  approximation  to  the  particular  function  of  a  family  of  fre- 
quency curves  which  can  be  used  to  represent  the  distribution  of 
deaths  by  age  (Perks,  1953). 

5.  What  evidence  is  available  tends  to  support  the  idea  that  the 
force  of  mortality  does  not  continue  to  increase  indefinitely  with  age. 
The  concept  of  a  limiting  age  by  which  all  individuals  must  be 
dead  (i.e.  a  maximum  lifespan)  does  not  seem  to  be  in  accordance 
with  the  facts — the  use  of  a  limiting  age  as  a  mathematical  device 
to  cut  off"  a  long  slender  tail  has  nothing  to  do  with  the  present  dis- 
cussion. Formula  (1)  leads  to  an  upper  limit  of  BID  for  fx^  and  it  is 
not  without  interest  to  note  that  the  numerical  values  of  B/D 
obtained  from  the  graduation  of  human  mortality  data  are  of  the 
same  order  as  the  force  of  mortality  which  can  be  deduced  from  select 
mortality  tables  as  being  appropriate  to  "damaged  lives",  i.e.  about 
0-57  (Beard,  19516). 

6.  If  the  rapidly  decreasing  mortality  associated  with  the  infantile 
and  growth  period  be  ignored  the  pattern  of  human  mortality  then 
exhibits  a  basic  sigmoid  form  on  which  are  superimposed  waves  and 
other  disturbances.  The  waves  appear  to  be  due  largely  to  secular 
effects  (e.g.  selective  effect  of  war  deaths);  the  main  disturbances 
are  those  arising  from  accidental  deaths  and  the  (rapidly  disappear- 
ing) hump  at  the  early  adult  ages  from  deaths  from  tuberculosis. 

7.  For  a  broad  mathematical  approach  we  will  be  concerned  with 

(a)  accidental  deaths  (assumed  to  be  at  a  constant  rate  at  all  ages), 

(b)  an  upper  limit  to  the  rate  of  mortality,  and  (c)  a  progression  in 
time. 

Gompertz'  law  arises  by  using  condition  (c)  only, 

i.e.     djjijdx  =  A/x^        whence    fi^  =  B  e^'  (2) 

Makeham's  law  arises  by  using  conditions  (a)  and  (c), 

i.e.     dfxjdx  =  X{ix^—A)        whence    ii^  =  A+Be^         (3) 


304  Appendix 

Perks'  law  arises  by  using  conditions  (a),  (b)  and  (c), 
i.e.     dfijdx  =  X(yi.-A)  {E-tJi,)l{E-A) 


whence    /x^,  =  ^  +     ^  .  -n  .x.  (^) 


The  Perks  (logistic)  relation  can  be  expressed  as  stating  that  the 
rate  of  change  of  yi^  is  proportional  to  the  product  of  its  value  and  the 
amount  by  which  it  falls  short  of  its  upper  limiting  value. 

8.  If  the  requirement  of  a  constant  upper  limit  for  the  rate  of 
mortality  is  relaxed  other  formulae  can  be  developed  on  similar  lines 
to  those  of  the  preceding  paragraph.   For  example, 

d^  _     X{fi^-A)  _ 

dx  -  l+B(fji^-A)    ^'^^'    "^'^     ~^^ 

where     Wj,  =  B{jx^—A)  (5) 


and 


^  X{fM,-A)(l+^fi,-A 

dx  ~  I       2D 


(l+-g/x,-^j 


gives    /Lt,  =  ^  +  ^(-l  +  \/l+4Z)e^^)  (6) 

Formula  (6)  is  equivalent  to  a  continued  fraction  form  for  /x^,  i.e. 

^■^^^ D?^ 


1  +  ... 


and  the  relationship  between  formulae  (2)  to  (6)  is  clearly  seen  by 
expanding  the  expressions  for  /^^  in  terms  of  powers  of  e^'^  i.e. 

formula  (2)  gives        B  e^' 
„       (3)     „    A+Be^' 

(4)  „    A-\-B  e^^'-BD  e'^'^  +  BD''  e^^  _.  .  . 

(5)  „    A+Be^'-BDe''^  +  lBD''e^^'^  —^'     • 

(6)  „    A+Be^-BDe''^  +  2BD^e^^'  _.  .  . 


Appendix  305 

9.  The  differences  between  formulae  (4),  (5)  and  (6)  will  become 
apparent  only  at  the  old  or  very  old  ages  and  unless  the  data  were 
extensive  the  differences  would  be  unlikely  to  be  significant  for  many 
numerical  processes.  From  a  scientific  point  of  view  the  models  are, 
of  course,  quite  different. 

10.  An  alternative  approach  to  the  question,  but  still  based  upon 
rates  of  mortality,  is  to  determine  the  conditions  necessary  for  /x^  to 
be  a  Perks  (logistic)  curve,  given  that  the  population  can  be  stratified 
according  to  a  longevity  factor  and  that  the  basic  mortality  law  is 
Makeham  in  form  (Beard,  19526).  Thus  letfx'f,  be  the  force  of  mortality 
at  time  ( =  age)  k  for  the  group  having  longevity  factor  s  and  let  (f)(s)  ds 
be  the  proportion  of  the  initial  population  having  factor  s.  Then  the 
survivors  of  (j){s)  ds  at  time  k  are 

<l>{s)  ds .  exp  ( -  r  fit dt\  (7) 

and  the  total  survivors  at  time  k 

h  =  j  <l>{s)  exp  (- J*  fJLtdt^ds  (8) 

where  the  integral  is  taken  over  the  whole  range  of  s. 
The  force  of  mortality  at  time  k  {=  —d  log  Ikjdk)  is  then 

(j>{s)  jLtfc  exp  (  —      jLt*  dt)  ds 
<^(5)  exp (  —      yi,ldt\  ds 

11.  From  formula  (9)  it  will  be  noted  that  fx^  is  a  weighted  mean 
oi  ix\  (=  /x^  say).  Since  the  number  of  lives  with  heavier  mortality 
will  diminish  more  rapidly  than  those  with  lighter  mortality,  s  will 
decrease  with  increasing  k.  If  the  basic  mortality  is  Makeham  in 
form,  then  dfij^/dk  will  show  a  slackening  off  at  the  higher  ages,  i.e. 
the  sigmoid  feature  shown  by  a  logistic  curve.  In  order  to  meet 
practical  conditions  some  limitations  are  necessary  on  the  form  of 
<j){s) ;  the  lower  limit  must  be  ^  0,  but  the  upper  limit  can  be  oo. 

12.  If  it  be  assumed  that  </>(5)  is  a  gamma  function  such  that 
(f){s)  ds  =  ks^  ery'  ds  (0  ^  s  <oo)  and  that  the  mortality  function  for 
<l>(s)  is  fil  =  (x  +  ps  e^^,  we  have 

f    ks^  €ry\<y.  +  ^s  e^^)  exp  {-[   (a  +  jS*  e^')  dt)  ds 
Jo                                                 \      J  0                             /  (ifW 

H-k  =  1^ 7 THc ^^ \^^) 

ks"  e-y  exp  {-\    (ol+^s  e^')  dt)  ds 


306  Appendix 

which  reduces  to 

which  is  a  Perks  (logistic)  form. 

13.  The  results  of  the  immediately  preceding  paragraphs  are 
interesting  in  that  the  limiting  value  of  [jl,,  arises  from  the  manner  in 
which  the  "mixed"  population  runs  off.  They  have  a  certain  appeal 
in  that  they  are  based  on  the  assumption  that  the  population  is  not 
homogeneous  in  regard  to  a  mortality  (or  longevity)  factor  and  that 
the  mortality  for  an  individual  group  continues  to  increase  indefi- 
nitely. The  Hmiting  value  oi  ^jlj^  —  ol  as  k^co  from  formula  (11)  is 
(2?  +  l)A  =  4A//S1  where  ft  is  the  Pearson  moment  function  of  (^(5). 
For  human  lives  fx,.  '^  0  •  6  at  the  limit,  according  to  one  fairly  recent 
mortality  table,  and  A  -^  0-1  so  that  ft  ~  0-67,  i.e.  a  skew  distribu- 
tion with  a  tail  towards  the  higher  values  of  s.  If  5  is  a  heredity 
factor,  then  stability  of  </.(5)  over  generations  would  imply  fertility 
rates  negatively  correlated  with  longevity,  otherwise  the  shorter 
reproductive  period  of  those  with  higher  values  of  s  would  lead  to  a 
falling  average  value  of  s  in  the  population.  It  is  an  interesting  co- 
incidence that  the  distribution  of  married  women  according  to 
number  of  children  born  has  a  ft  coefficient  of  the  order  of  0*7 
(Papers  of  Royal  Commission  on  Population,  1950). 

14.  The  assumption  of  other  forms  for  (f){s)  in  formula  (9)  leads  to 
other  forms  for  ^^  which  can  have  the  appropriate  shape  but  which 
are  not  convenient  mathematically,  and  no  experiments  have  been 
made  in  this  direction. 

15.  From  the  point  of  view  put  forward  in  paragraph  1  formula 
(10)  suffers  from  the  objection  that  it  is  based  on  the  assumption  of 
a  Makeham  law,  and  is  thus  basically  empirical.  A  further  approach 
to  the  question  is  to  build  up  models  based  on  the  so-called  "shot 
hypothesis"  in  which  individuals  are  assumed  to  be  subject  to 
random  firings  and  are  assumed  to  die  when  they  have  been  "hit" 
a  specified  number  of  times.  Two  main  types  of  model  have  been 
investigated,  which  are  referred  to  below  as  the  "forward"  and 
"backward"  models  respectively.  In  the  forward  model  hits  are 
assumed  to  accumulate  and  death  to  occur  when  the  total  reaches  a 
certain  figure.  In  the  backward  model  the  individual  is  assumed  to 
start  with  a  quota  of  units  which  are  progressively  lost  in  time, 
death  occurring  when  the  total  remaining  falls  below  a  certain 
figure. 

16.  The  simplest  forward  model  is  derived  by  assuming  that  the 


Appendix  307 

chance  that  an  individual  is  hit  in  an  interval  dt  is  p;  this  leads  to  a 
difference-differential  equation 

^l  =  -pl'^+plf-'  (12) 

where  Zf  represents  the  number  at  time  t  who  have  been  "hit"  a 
times.  If  l„  is  the  number  of  individuals  at  time  o  then  a  solution  of 
equation  (12)  is 

/«  =  le-^'(ptfloc\  (13) 

If  the  number  of  hits  causing  death  is  r,  then  the  survivors  at  time  t 
are 

I,  =  l^e--*{l-]-{pt)ll\+  .  .  .  +(pty-^l{r-l)\} 

and  the  deaths  in  the  interval  t  to  t  +  dt 

lji,l,  =  ke-^'pH^-'l(r-\)\  (14) 

The  force  of  mortality  at  time  t  is 

(ptr' 


-Si/i-f;- 


+ 


(r-1)! 
=  pe-^\pty-^l  r  e^x'-^  dx  (15) 

Formula  (15)  shows  that  the  curve  of  deaths  is  an  incomplete  gamma 
function,  or  a  Pearson  type  III  curve,  ju,  has  the  value  0  for  ^  =  0 
and  asymptotes  to  a  value  p  at  ^  =  oo  (Beard,  19396). 

17.  A  more  natural  function  than  ju,,.  in  the  present  context  is  to 
use  the  function  which  bears  the  same  relationship  to  /x^/^  as  /z,  does  to 
h,     i.e. 

dt  fjif  dt 

and  from  formula  (14)  we  find  this  to  be 

'^<'°f-'->  =  -p+'-^  (16) 

18.  Attempts  to  use  the  formula  of  paragraph  16  on  human 
mortality  data  have  been  unsuccessful,  the  shape  of  c^(log  /x^y/^^  not 


308  Appendix 

fitting  well  to  observed  values  which  show  a  negative  second  dif- 
ferential coefficient  over  the  adult  ages. 

19.  As  an  extension  of  formula  (12)  a  model  can  be  set  up  in 
which  the  "hits"  in  an  interval  can  be  single,  double,  etc.,  in  known 
proportions.   The  basic  relation  then  takes  the  form 

J"  =  -plT+p^^f{r)ir  (17) 

This  can  be  integrated  to 

If  =  e-^'  J  pe^'  2  f{r)  Vf^  dt  (18) 

and  by  noting  that  ll  =  e-'"  /„  values  of  If  can  be  obtained  by  succes- 
sive integration.  No  experiments  have  been  made  using  this  form, 
mainly  because  the  form  of  c/(log  fJLtQIdt  seems  to  be  unsuitable  for 
human  data.   The  form  of /(r)  is  also  speculative. 

20.  A  different  forward  model  can  be  devised  in  which  the  proba- 
bility of  a  "hit"  is  dependent  on  the  number  of  "hits"  recorded 
already.  We  then  have  the  following 

/77a  

^  =   -(^4-pa)  Zf +  (^+^.a-l)  ir  (19) 

This  can  be  integrated  to  give 


with 


'te^)  =  -(^  +  0^^)  +^^^^^-  (21) 


Here  again  the  form  of  equation  (21)  does  not  accord  with  observa- 
tions from  human  data. 

21.  In  the  attempts  to  fit  these  forward  type  formulae  to  human 
data  it  was  found  (Beard,  1950,  1952c)  that  satisfactory  numerical 
results  could  be  obtained  by  expressing  /x,  /^  in  gamma  function  form 
subject  to  a  terminal  age  cu,  i.e.  the  infinite  tail  of  the  curve  is  the 
opposite  way  round  to  what  would  be  considered  natural.  This 
formula,  after  elimination  of  a  constant  element  representing 
accidental  mortality,  can  be  derived  from  the  difference-differential 
equation 

^^=plf-pf-'  (22) 


Appendix  309 

the  solution  of  which  leads  to 

If  =  l^  ^pe^-*)  {p{oj  - 1)  Yjoi !  (23) 


from  which 


dt  ^     co-t 


if  the  deaths  occur  at  the  ath  hit.  In  this  formula  p'~0-3,a^ll 
and  oj  --^  110  for  human  mortality. 

22.  No  obvious  physical  model  applies  to  equation  (22),  but  the 
relationship  can  be  written  in  the  backward  form 

f  =  -  ^  Z,«  +  «-±l  r^  (24) 

at  to  — I        co  —  t 

in  which  the  rate  at  which  a  unit  is  lost  is  proportional  to  the  number 
of  units  remaining  divided  by  the  years  of  life  remaining  to  the  final 
age  CO.  From  a  biological  point  of  view  the  concept  of  a  final  age  by 
which  the  organism  must  be  dead  is  unsatisfactory,  but  the  fact  that 
satisfactory  numerical  results  arise  only  from  a  backward  formula 
suggests  that  a  closer  study  of  this  type  of  model  might  be  more 
profitable. 

23.  The  simplest  backward  model  arises  from  the  relationship 

^=  -plf+pir  (25) 

where  the  organism  is  assumed  to  lose  a  unit  at  rate  p. 
This  has  a  solution 

If  =  l^  e-^*  {pt)^°'l(n  -  a) !  (26) 

where  n  is  a  maximum  number  of  units.  If  death  is  assumed  to  occur 
when  the  number  of  units  faUs  below  ?•,  we  have 

dCioQ  11.1)  n  —  r  ,„^. 

This  is  of  similar  form  to  equation  (16)  and  is  not  suitable  for  human 
data. 

24.  By  assuming  that  the  rate  of  loss  of  a  imit  is  proportional  to 
the  number  of  units  remaining  the  relation 

^  =  -piP  +  c.)  lf+piP  +  o^  +  1)  r^  (28) 


310  Appendix 

may  be  set  up.   This  has  the  solution 

If  =  k  e^'l(l  +De^'y+°'+'-  (29) 

If  death  occurs  when  the  units  fall  below  a,  we  have 


We  also  have 


=  I  D{\  +Z))^+«/(l  -{-De^y^''  (30) 


dt  ^  \+De^'  ^     ' 

and 

I"'  -  ^^Td^  (^^) 

We  have  now  found  a  difference  equation  model  which  leads  to  a 
Perks  (logistic)  formula  for /x^.  In  formula  (31)  the  upper  limit  of  /x,  is 
_p(/S  +  a);  p  ^  0-1  and  the  limit  ^  0-7  so  that  (jS  +  a)  ~  7. 

25.  The  distribution  of  a  in  the  population  at  age  0  implied  by 
equation  (29)  is  a  decreasing  geometrical  progression,  i.e. 

D  D  D 


1+D'  {l+Df"'(l-{-D)'^ 

For  human  mortality  D  is  small  (of  the  order  of  10~^)  so  that  the  dis- 
tribution is  very  slowly  decreasing  with  increasing  a. 

26.  The  significant  result  which  emerges  from  the  experiments 
made  along  these  lines  is  that  to  provide  results  which  have  some 
reasonable  semblance  to  observed  human  mortality  the  backward 
type  of  model  has  to  be  adopted.  This  is  consistent  with  death 
being  regarded  as  the  culmination  of  a  degenerative  process  such 
that  death  occurs  when  the  organism  reaches  a  certain  level  of 
degeneration.  The  mathematical  models  are  based  on  numerical 
results  for  adult  ages  and  interpolation  back  to  birth  is  possibly  a 
questionable  process,  a  more  suitable  approach  being  to  regard  the 
life  and  death  process  as  a  period  during  which  the  organism  is 
building  up  to  a  complex  situation  with  a  subsequent  degeneration. 
This  would  lead  to  models  in  which  the  whole  of  life  process  would 
be  looked  upon  as  the  resultant  effect  of  two  opposing  forces. 


Appendix  311 

27.  Calculation  of  the  moments  of  the  distribution  of  deaths  by 
age  for  a  population  of  mice  (Greenwood,  1928)  shows  that  a  Pearson 
type  III  (gamma  function)  would  give  a  fair  representation,  but,  as 
with  the  himian  data,  the  curve  is  the  "opposite  way  round",  i.e. 
subject  to  a  terminal  age.  By  inference  the  Perks  (logistic)  curve 
would  give  a  fair  representation  of  this  data.  No  calculations  have 
been  made  on  animal  data  or  on  physical  objects  such  as  electric 
light  bulbs  and  motor  cars  (e.g.  Cramer,  1958)  but  it  would  seem 
worth  while  trying  to  find  out  if  observed  data  of  this  latter  type 
would  distinguish  between  the  two  types  of  processes. 

REFERENCES 

Beard,  R.  E.  (1936).  J.  Inst.  Acta.,  67,  53. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (1939a).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  70,  53. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (19396).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  70,  373. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (1950).  Proc.  Centen.  Assembl.  Inst.  Acta.,  2,  89. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (1951«).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  77,  382. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (1951b).   J.  Inst.  Actu.,  77,  394. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (1952«).   J.  Inst.  Actu.,  78,  82. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (19526).   J.  Inst.  Actu.,  78,  201. 
Beard,  R.  E.  (1952c).   J.  Inst.  Actu.,  78,  341. 
Cramer,  J.  S.  (1958).  J.  R.  statist.  Soc,  121,  18. 
Gompertz,  B.  (1825).   Phil.  Trans.,  115,  513. 
Greenwood,  M.  (1928).   J.  Hyg.  (Lond.),  28,  282. 
Makeham,  W.  M.  (1867).   J.  Inst.  Actu.,  13,  325. 
Mortality  of  Assured  Lives  (1956).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  82,  3. 
Papers  of  Royal  Commission  on  Population  (1950).   2,  154. 
Perks,  W.  (1932).   J.  Inst.  Actu.,  63,  12. 
Perks,  W.  (1953).  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  79,  199. 

Registrar  General's  Decennial  Supplement,  England  and  Wales  1951, 
Review  in  J.  Inst.  Actu.,  83,  168  (1957). 


AUTHOR  INDEX 


Plain  numbers  indicate  a  contribution  either  in  the  form  of  an  article  or  as  a  contribution 
to  the  discussions.     Italic  numbers  indicate  a  reference  to  an  author's  work. 


Aasen,  O.   . 
Akyuz,  E. 
Ally,  M.  S. 
Aim,  G.      . 

d'Ancoma,  U. 
Andersen,  J. 
Anderson,  J. 
Andrew,  W. 
Ansell,  S.    . 
Appelget,  J. 
Armbruster,  L. 
Arora,  H.  L. 
Ascher,  K.  R.  S. 
Asgood,  H.  S, 
Austin,  O.  L. 
Austin,  O.  L.  Jr. 


149,  156,  174 

.       149,  174 

.       255,  264 

144,  155,  159, 

168,  174 

.       167,  174 

92,  94,  95,  102 

232,  234,  241 

.       248,  264 

21,  31 

.       155,  174 

232,  241,  242 

149,  174 

255,  259,  264 

.       251,  264 

98,  102 

98,  102 


Back,  E. 
Bagenal,  T.  B. 

Baird,  D. 
Banfield,  A.  W.  F. 
Barcroft,  J. 
Barnes,  L,  L. 
Barnet,  H.  A.  R. 
Barton,  R.  A, 
Bauer,  K.    . 
Beard,  R.  E. 


Beeton,  M. 
Belding,  D.  L. 
Bell,  F.  H. 
Bendell,  J.  F. 
Benedict 
Beneke,  R. 
Benjamin,  B. 

Berdegue,  J. 
Berg,  B.  N. 


Berg,  L.  S. 
Bertalanffy,  L.  von 
Bertholf,  L.  M.    . 
Bertin,  L.    . 
Best,  A.  T. 
Beutler,  R. 


235,  238,  241 
192,  193,  200,  201, 
202,  205,  207 
.  33 
92,  102 
.  133 
251,  264 
.  5,  15 
53,54 
59,  69 
4,  14,  288,  289,  302, 
303,  305,  307,  308, 
311 
21,  31,  33,  53,  54 
197,  207 
151,  177 
101,  102 
.  124 
110,  112 
2,  15,  16,  17,  18,  19, 
32,  34,  70,  293 
.  755,  175 
16,  32,  33,  72,  73, 
74,  75,  76,  79,  79,  80,  82, 
83,  84,  85,  86,  87,  88,  114, 
137,  265,  266,  285 
.  212,  225 
157,  169,  175 
.  235,  241 
.  168,  175 
.  104 
235,  236,  241 


Beverton,  R.  J.  H. 

.       142,  149,  151, 

158, 

175,    177,    178,    179, 

226,  227,  229 

Bezem,  J.  J. 

97,  103 

Bidder,  G.  P. 

173,  175,  182,  189, 

190,  191,  207 

Bilewicz,  S. 

.       277,  281 

Blackburn,  M.     . 

146,  149,  151,  175 

Blair,  W.  F. 

96,  102 

Blazka,  P. 

.       170,  175 

Bodenheimer,  F.  S. 

.     294 

Bottcher,  T. 

.    60,  61,  69 

Bonin,  G.  von 

119,  123,  128,  132 

Boroughs,  H. 

.       170,  175 

Bourliere,  F. 

70,  84,  91,  96,  102, 

103, 

104,    105,    112,    113, 

119, 

132,    137,   217,   225, 

227,  228,  245 

Boyd,  H.     . 

.       101,  102 

Breder,  C.  M.      . 

212,  217,  218,  225 

Bretherick,  O.      . 

.       237,  243 

Brody,  S. 

.       124,  132 

Brown,  C.  J.  D.  . 

.       168,  175 

Brown,  M.  E. 

147,  148,  149,  175, 

217,  225 

Brues,  A.  M. 

.     138 

Buchanan,  A.  D. 

59,  69 

Bucher,  G.  E.      . 

.      258,  264 

Burd,  A.  C. 

.       149,  175 

Burns,  C.  M. 

21,  31 

Butler,  C.  G. 

237,  241, 243 

Buxton,  P.  A. 

.      240,  241 

Cameron,  J.  W.  McB.           .      258,  264 

Cannon,  C. 

59,  69 

Carlson,  L.  D.     . 

.       89 

Chalmers,  T.  A.  . 

.       56 

Chang,  H-W.       . 

145,  146,  153,  175 

Chipman,  W.  A. 

.       170,  175 

Chitty,  D.  H.       . 

105,  296,  298 

Chugaeva,  M. 

53,  54 

Ciba  Foundation 

.       128,  132 

Clark,  F.  N. 

149,  155,  175 

Clark,  H.  W.        . 

.       212,  226 

Clarke 

.     266 

Clarke,  J.  M.       . 

.       278,  281 

Clarke,  R.  D.      . 

4,  5,  6,  7,  8,  10,  11, 

14,  15 

Cohen,  D.  M.      . 

.      151,  175 

Cohrs,  P. 

.      110,  112 

313 


314 


Author  Index 


Comfort,  A. 

17,  17,  32,  34,  35,    1 

Fitch,  H.  S. 

96,  97,  102 

36, 

37,  43,  44,  47,  52,  53, 

Fitch,  J.  E. 

.       155,  175 

54, 

55,  56,  70,  71,  84,  85, 

Flade,  J.  E. 

66,  69 

87, 

88,  103,  104,  113,  114, 

Flower,  S.  . 

119,  128,  132,  168,  175, 

116,   117,   119,   132,    134,    | 

217,  225 

m 

\,    137,   169,   173,   175, 

Foerster,  R.  E.    . 

151,  156,  175 

\n 

5,    179,   208,   208,   209, 

Fox,  H.       . 

108,  109,  112 

210,   217,   225,   111,   11^,    1 

Franca,  P.  da 

.       153,  175 

23C 

),   244,   262,   264,   265, 

Franke,  F. 

62,  69 

26e 

i,   286,   286,   289,   291, 

Frankland,  H.  M 

.  T.    .          .           53,  54 

294,  295,  298 

Franz,  V.    . 

.       202,  207 

Cooper,  E.  L. 

144,  153,  176 

Eraser,  E.  A. 

.       189,  207 

Corkins,  C.  L. 

.     245 

Freudenberg,  F.  . 

62,  69 

Count,  E.  W. 

119,  123,  132 

Freudenstein,  H. 

235,  236,  237,  241 

Crowell,  M.  F. 

169,  176,  251,  253, 

Freudenstein,  K. 

.       232,  241 

264 

Frost,  W.  E. 

.       153,  175 

Crozier,  W.  J.      . 

.     267 

Fry,  D.  H. 

168,  170,  175 

Curtis 

.       87 

Fry,  F.  E.  J. 

.       192,  207 

Daly,  C.      . 

21,  31 

Gardiner,  E.  M. 

21,  31 

Danielli,  J.  F. 

1,  15,  17,  19,  32,  33, 

Gardner,  E. 

.       248,  264 

34, 

70,  103,  104,  114,  133, 

Geiser,  S.  W, 

167,  175,  177 

13( 

),    137,    177,    178,   209, 

Gerking,  S.  D. 

18,  79,  171,  175, 

229,  230,  281,  282,  298 

17S 

),    180,    181,    183,   207, 

Dasmann,  R.  F, 

92,  94,  103 

209,   210,   211,   228,   229, 

Davey,  W.  P. 

.     282 

230,  265,  284,  289 

Davies,  D.  F. 

72 

Gerner,  K. 

63,  69 

Davis,  D.  E. 

95,  102 

Gibson,  J.  R. 

29,  31 

Davis,  W.  S. 

.       192,  207 

Gilbert,  C,  S. 

.     245 

Deason,  H.  J. 

.       151,  176 

Gompertz,  B. 

?,  15,  72,  79,  302,  311 

Decider,  C.  L. 

.       159,  175 

Gontarski,  H, 

.       232,  241 

Deevey,  E.  S. 

91,  102 

Gordon,  J.  E. 

21,  31 

Detkens 

67,  69 

Gottlieb,  E. 

.       149,  175 

Devoid,  F. 

.       149,  175 

Grainger,  E.  H. 

147,  153,  175 

Dickie,  L.  M. 

.       151,  175 

Green,  R.  G. 

.     297 

Dietrich,  H. 

60,  69 

Greenwood,  M. 

.     311 

Dilley,  W.  E. 

.      169,  176 

Griffiths,  J.  T. 

.       277,  281 

Dinkhauser,  F. 

63,  69 

Groot,  A.  P.  de 

235,  236,  237,  238, 

Doljanski,  F. 

.     208 

241 

Dubois,  E. 

.       123,  132 

Grosch,  D.  S. 

.       255,  264 

Duetz,  G.  H. 

.       85 

Griineberg,  H. 

19,  20,  79,  80,  283 

Dymond,  J.  R. 

.     175 

Gumbel,  E.  J. 
Gunter,  G. 

117,  132,  134 
.       167,  175 

Edmonds,  S.  J. 

.       169,  175 

Eidrigevits,  E.  V. 

53,  54 

Hafter,  E. 

196,  208,  218,  225, 

El-Deeb,  A.  L.  A 

232,  234,  241 

226 

Elkin,  R.     . 

.       168,  176 

Hagel,  L. 

59,  69 

Ellis,  R.  S. 

.       248,  264 

Hald,  A.     . 

.       120,  133 

Engeler,  W. 

59,  69 

Haldane,  J.  B.  S. 

53,  54 

Erickson,  D.  W. 

196,  197,  208 

Hall,  D.  A. 

.     113 

Eschmeyer,  P.  H 

.       197,  207 

Hansen,  E. 

59,  69 

Evans,  C.  A, 

.     297 

Hanyu,  I.    . 

.       151,  175 

Evans,  H.   . 

92,  102 

Harmison,  C.  R. 

73,  79 

Evenius,  C. 

.       232,  241 

Harrison,  J.  L. 

.  95,  96,  102 

Evermann,  B.  W 

.       212, 226 

Hart,  J.  L.. 
Hart,  J.  S.  . 

144,  151,  155,  175,  176 
.       170,  175 

Fahr,  H.  O. 

.       110,  112 

Hartmann,  W. 

59,  69 

Failla,  A.    . 

. 

.     289 

Hartwig,  W. 

56,  57,  65,  66,  67, 

Earner,  D.  S. 

.  97, 101,  102,  103 

69,  70,  71 

Farr,  W.     . 

. 

9 

Hatai,  S.     . 

.       248,  264 

Farran,  G.  P. 

.       194,  207 

Haydak,  M.  H. 

232,  236,  237,  238, 

Farrar,  C.  L. 

232,  234,  241 

239,  241,  242,  253, 

Feldman-Muhsai 

Tl,  B. 

.       259,  264 

264 

Finnell,  J. 

. 

.       168,  176 

Heady,  J.  A. 

21,31 

Author  Index 


315 


Hejtmanek,  J. 
Hems,  J. 
Herald,  E.  S. 
Herrington,  W.  C 
Hervey,  G.  F. 
Hess,  G. 
Hickey,  J.  J. 
Hickling,  C.  F 
Hile,  R.       . 
Hinton,  H.  E. 

Hinton,  S.  . 

Hodge,  C.  F. 

Hoffman,  H. 

Hogreve,  F. 

Holmes,  S.  J. 

Holt,  S.  J.  .  86,  1 
158, 
179, 

Hoogendoorn,  D 
Hubbs,  C.  L. 
Hunter,  W.  R. 
Huxley,  J. 
Hynes,  H.  B.  N. 
Hytten,  F.  E. 

Inukai,  T. 
Irwin,  J.  O. 
Isupov,  A.  P. 
Ivlev,  V.  S. 

Jackson,  B.  H. 
Jalavisto,  Eeva, 

Jenkins,  R. 
Johansson,  A.  S. 
John,  M.     . 
Jones,  E.  C. 
Jones,  J.  W. 
Jordan,  D.  S. 
June,  F.  C. 

Kadic,  M.  . 
Kalabouchov,  N. 
Karzinkin,  G.  S. 
Katz,  M.     . 
Kennedy,  W.  A. 
Kershaw,  W.  E, 

King,  G.     . 
King,  J.  O.  L. 
Kluijver,  H.  N. 
Knox,  G.    . 
Koch,  A.     . 
Kocher,  V. 
Koehler,  A. 
Konig,  K. 
Konopinski 
Kratky,  E. 
Krause,  C. 
Krohn,  P.  L. 
Krumholz,  L.  A. 


235,  237,  242 

.     227 

155,  176 

151,  177 

.     227 

235,  242 

97,  101,  102 

195,  196,  205,  207 

151,  176 

34,  112,  243,  245, 

246,  284 

191,  217,  219,  225 

.       248,  264 

.       237,  243 

.    64,  65,  69 

21,  31 

34,  142,  149,  151,  155, 

167,   175,   176,    178, 

209,   210,   226,   227, 

228,  229,  246,  290 

21,  31 

146,  155,  176 

.     179 

.     104 

153,  156,  176 

.       33 

.       248,  264 

37,  54 

53,  54 

186,  207,  208 


.       89 

16,  17,  21,  21,  31,  31, 

32,  33,  81,  113,  266 

.       168,  176 

.     243 

.       237,  242 

.       261,  281 

153,  156,  176 

.       212,  226 

.       193,  208 

43,  54 
96,  102 
.       186,  208 
196,  197,  208 
145,  151,  176 
56,  56,  70,  105,  114, 
245,  267,  285 
10 
.       56 
99,  102 
21,  31 
236,  238,  242 
.       237,  242 
.       232,  242 
.    61,  62,  69 
67,  69 
232,  236,  242 
.       110,  112 
.       271,  281 
171,  176,  187,  188, 
208 


Kuhn,  W. 

, 

, 

240,  242 

Kuiken,  A. 

.       237,  243 

Kuznetsova,  G.   . 

53,  54 

Lack,  D.     . 

.  97,  101,  102,  104 

Lansing,  A.  L 

.       259,  264 

Larkin,  P.  A. 

.       182,  208 

Lavoipierre,  M.  M. 

J.  ". 

.       245,  285 

Lea,  E. 

145,  149,  176 

Lesher,  S.   . 

.       80 

Leslie,  P.  H. 

.     296 

Levin,  M,  D. 

232,  236,  242 

Levinson,  Z.  H. 

255,  259,  264 

Lieberman,  H.   M. 

250,  251,  255,  256, 

257,  258,  260,  264 

Lindauer,  M. 

.       233,  242 

Lindop,  Patricia  J. 

113,  136,  138,  141 

Lorenz,  E. 

.       89 

Lotmar,  M. 

232,  233,  234,  242 

Louveaux,  J. 

.       236,  242 

Luecke,  R.  W.     . 

.       237,  243 

McCay,  C.  M.     . 

169,  176,  251,  252, 

253,  264,  265,  268 

McCracken,  F.  D. 

.       151,  175 

McFadyean,  J.     . 

50,54 

McGregor,  E.  A. 

192,  208 

McHugh,  T. 

.     105 

McKeown,  T. 

29,  31 

MacMahon,  L.    . 

21,  31 

Maar,  A.     . 

191,  208 

Mackintosh,  J.     . 

21,  31 

Makeham,  W.  M. 

2,4,15,302,311 

Mannes,  A. 

59,  60,  69 

Margetts,  A.  R. 

.       151,  176 

Marlow,  H. 

63,  69 

Marr,  J.  C. 

153,  176 

Martin,  W. 

59,  64,  69 

Matsumoto,  K.    . 

43,54 

Mauermayer,  G. 

232,  234,  242 

Maurizio,  Anna  . 

231,  232,  234,  235, 

236, 

238,   242,   243,  244, 

245, 

246,    248,   254,   264 

Maynard,  L.  A. 

183,  208,  251,  264 

Medawar,  P.  B. 

.       271,  281 

Mellen,  I.    . 

.       217,  226 

Menon,  M.  D.     . 

.       149,  176 

Menzel,  D.  W.     . 

183,  184,  185,  208, 

210 

Milinsky,  G.  L    . 

.       207,  208 

Miller,  R.  B. 

168,  176 

Miner,  J.  R. 

.     267 

Miner,  R.  W. 

53,54 

Moltoni,  E. 

.     103 

Moore,  H.  L. 

.       155,  176 

Morgulis,  S. 

.       186,  208 

Morris,  J.  N. 

21,  31 

Moskovljevic,  V. 

.       234,  242 

Miihlbock,  O.      . 

.      33,  71,  80,  287 

Murie,  A.   . 

54,  92,  102 

Murphy,  D.  P.     . 

^.       262, 264 

Mussbichler,  A.  . 

2 

35,  238,  242 

316 


Author  Index 


Nail,  G.  H. 
Nickel,  H.  K. 
Nieberle,  K. 
Nieto,  D.    . 
Nigrelli,  R.  F 


Ogborn,  M. 
Olsson,  V.  . 
Opfinger,  E. 
Orton,  J.  H. 


J53,  165,  176 
.  232,  242 
.  no,  112 
.  107,  112 
113,  138,  147,  176, 
178,  210,  211,  212,  213, 
216,  217,  226,  216,  111, 
228,  229,  244,  245 


.  5,  15 

99,  103 

235,  236,  241 

190,  191,  193,  198, 

200,  201,  208 


Paccagnella,  B 
Pain,  J. 
Pallaske,  G. 
Palmer,  L.  S. 
Parker,  R.  R. 
Pearl,  R.     . 
Pearson,  E.  G. 
Pearson,  K. 

Pearson,  P.  B. 
Perks,  W. 


Phillips,  E.  F. 
Phillips,  E.  W. 
Phillips,  J.  B. 
Piel,  H. 
Pincus,  G.  . 
Polyakov,  E.  V. 
Ponomareva,  L.  I. 
Pospelov,  S.  P.    . 
Probst,  R.  T. 
Pycha,  R.  L. 


.  153,  176 

235,  237,  238,  242,  243 

.       110,  112 

237,  242,  243 

.       182,  208 

.       267,  283 

.     288 

3,  15,  21,  31,  33, 

53,  54,  288 

.       237,  242 

3,4,5,14,  75,81,288,289, 

290,  291,  292,  293,  294, 

302,  303,  311 

232,  234,  242 

4,  5,  15 

.  149,  176 

59,  63,  64,  69 

.     267 

53,  54 

53,  54 

53,  54 

144,  153,  176 

.       153,  176 


Qasim,  S.  Z, 
Quirling,  D.  P. 


Radhakrishnan, 
Raitt,  D.  S. 

Rakowicz,  M. 
Ranson,  R.  M. 
Rasquin,  P. 
Reibisch,  J. 
Renton,  R.  M. 
Ribbands,  C.  R. 
Rice,  T.  R. 
Rich,  C.  O. 
Richdale,  L.  E. 
Ricker,  W.  E. 

Ripke,  G.   . 
Robertson,  J.  A. 
Robertson,  T.  B. 
Robinson,  O.  J. 


153,  156,  176 
.       119,  133 


N.   .    .   755,  776 

149,  176,  198,  199, 

200,  201,  202,  205,  208 

.       155,  176 

.     296 

196,  208,  218,  226 

.       202,  208 

.      189,  207 

232,  233,  242 

.       170,  175 

.  4,  15 

100,  101,  103 

145,  149,  151,  155, 

176 

59,  69 

.       149,  176 

.     137 

59,  69 


Rockstein,  M.      .  15,  31,  55,  83,  84, 

112,  114,  179,  210,  226, 
228,  232,  234,  243,  144, 
245,  246,  247,  247,  248, 
249,  250,  251,  254,  255, 
256,  257,  258,  264,  265, 
267,  268,  284,  287,  289, 
291 

Rosch,  G.  A 243 


Rottgermann,  W. 
RoUefsen,  G. 
Rollins,  R.  Z.       . 
Rosenberger,  C.  R. 
Rotblat,  J. 


59,  62,  69 
149,  176 
258,  264 
255,  264 
15,  19,  20,  86,  87, 


Rounsefell,  G. 
Rubner,  M. 

Rumbauer 
Runnstrom,  S. 
Russell,  E.  S. 


Sacher,  G.  A. 


103,  135,  177,  209,  210, 
283,  289,  290,  291,  292 
.   797,  208 
124,  125,  126,  127, 
128,  130,  133,  134 
.    59,  63,  69 
.       149,  176 
190,  199,  208 


87,  { 
133, 
136, 
140, 
266, 

Sakagami,  S.  F. 
Samokhvalova,  G. 
Sarkar,  B.  C.  R. 
Sato,  R.      . 
Saxl,  H.      . 
Scheidegger,  S. 

Scherer,  H.  J. 
Scheyer,  W.  J. 
Schieren,  J. 
Schneider,  F. 
Scholz,  W. 
Schotterer,  A. 
Schroder,  G. 
Schwarz,  J. 
Selye,  H.     . 
Shell,  H.  M. 
Shiraishi,  Y 
Silliman,  R.  P. 
Simms,  H,  S. 

Simpson,  A.  C. 

Sluiter,  J.  W. 

Smith 

Smith,  L.  L. 

Smith,  J.  M. 

87, 
266, 
279, 
284, 

Smyly,  W.  J.  P.   . 

Snyder,  D.  P.       . 


75,  18,  31,  52,54,  80, 
18,  104,  115,  775,  727, 
133,  134,  135,  755, 
137,  138,  138,  139, 
141,  147,  176,  245, 
282,  284,  289,  292, 
295,  296,  298 
233,  243 
.     209 

237,  243 
151,  176 

.     113 

103,  106,  112,  113, 

226 

108, 112 

89 

59,  69 

240,  243 

107,  112 

43,54 

65,  69 

238,  242 
.       88 

237,  243 

151,  176 

.      149,  176 

11,  73,  74,  75,  76, 

79,  266,  268,  293 

203,  204,  205,  208 

97,  103 

59,  69 

.       155,  174 

16,  32,  33,  55,  86, 

104,    134,    137,    141, 

267,   268,   277,   278, 

281,    281,    282   283, 

285,  290 

7-^5,  153,  169,  175, 

176 

95, 103 


Author  Index 


317 


Sonneborn,  T.  M. 

Soudek,  S. 
Southern,  H.  N. 
Sperling,  G. 
Spitskaya,  T.  D. 
Spohde,  H. 
Spurway,  H. 
Stevens,  C.  F. 
Stockklausner,  F 
Stolbova,  A. 
Strawn,  K. 
Suau,  P.      . 
Summers-Smith,  D. 
Sundnes,  G. 
Sutter,  J.     . 
Svardson,  G. 

Svoboda,  J. 
de  Sylva,  D. 
Szilard,  L.  . 


Tabah,  L.   . 
Taber,  R.  D, 
Tainter,  M.  L. 
Tanner,  J.  M. 

Tanquary,  M.  C. 
Tauber,  O.  E. 
Taylor,  C.  C. 
Templeman,  W. 
Terpenning,  J.  G 
Tester,  A.  L. 
Thiele,  T.  N. 
Thompson,  W.  F 
Thomson,  A.  M. 
Tibbo,  S.  N. 
Tiews,  K.    . 
Todd,  F.  E. 
Townsend,  C.  H 
Tracey,  K.  M. 
Tucker,  D.  W. 

Vaas,  K.  F.  '  . 
Vallois,  H.  V.  . 
Van  Cleave,  H.  J. 
Van  Heerdt,  P.  F. 


32,  35,  53,  54, 

262,  264 

235,  243 

101,  103 

251,  264 

53,  54 

64,  69 

276,  281 

21,31 

59,69 

53,54 

155,  176 

149,  177 

99,  101,  103 

169,  176 
21,31 

171,  176,  182,  190, 

208 

.       235,  243 

155,  175,  228 

.  19,  129,  133,  289 

21,  31 

92,  94,  103 

.     136 

84,  85,  86,  88,  266, 

290,  296,  297 

.       232,  242 

.       277,  281 

149,  167,  177 

146,  151,  177 

182,  208 

149,  177 

.  3,  15 

151,  177 

.       33 

149,  177 

155,  177 

237,  243 

217,  226 

262,  264 

174,  177 

170,  177 
.  134 
.     179 

97,  103 


Verzar,  F. 

Vitt,  V.  O. 
Vives,  F.     . 
Vivino,  E. 
VNIRO       . 
Voogd,  S.   . 

Wahl,  O.     . 
Walford,  L.  A. 
Walker,  E.  P. 
Wallace,  B. 
Wallace,  W. 
Weaver,  N. 
Webb,  C.  S. 
Weber,  R.  . 
Weidenreich,  F 
Weitnauer,  E. 
West,  A.  S. 
Weyer,  F.   . 
Weygand,  F. 
White,  P. 
Wigglesworth,  V. 


82,85,55,  112,  133, 

137,  138,  141,298 

35,  43,  46,  49,  54 

.       149,  177 

237,  238,  242,  243 

.       149,  177 

.       237, 243 


159, 


B. 

243, 


Wilkes,  A. 
Wilson,  I.   . 
Winnigstedt,  R 
Wittwer,  S.  H. 
Wohlschlag,  D. 
Woke,  P.  A. 
Wolkoff,  K. 
Wolstenholme,  G.  E.  W 
Wright,  N.  C 
Wurzel,  W. 
Wussow,  W. 
Wynne-Edwards,  V.  C 


Yerkes,  A. 
Yerkes,  R. 
Yerushalmy,  J. 
Yockey,  H.  P. 
Yokota,  T. 

Zahl,  P.  A. 
Zamyatin,  N. 
Zander,  E. 
Ziegenhagen,  G. 


243, 


.       235,  243 

177,  214,  226 

.       119,  133 

.       20 

.       189, 208 

.       237,  243 

.     104 

43,  54 

.     134 

.     104 

.       258,  264 

.       234,  243 

.       237,  243 

.     267 

87,  133,  240, 

245,  267,  285 

.       258,  264 

21,  31 

59,  69 

.       237,  243 

145,  151,  177 

.       255,  264 

.       110,  112 

.       71 

59,  69 

59,  69 

.    66,  67,  69 

.      193,  194, 

195,  208 

.  119,  133 
.       119,  133 

21,  31,  53,  54 
.       129,  133 

155,  163,  177 

.     267 

53,  54 

.      232, 243 

59,  60,  64,  69 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


Accipitres,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 
Acipenseriformes,  lifespan  and  size  of,  152, 

220, 222 
Acipenser  fiihescens,  survival  curves  of, 

143,  144 
Acipenser  ruthenus,  oldest  age  of,  191 
Actuarial  aspects  of  human  lifespan,  2-20 
Aedes  aegypti,  effect  of  diet  on,  255 
Age,  changes  due  to  in  fish,  218 

effect  on  fecundity  in  fish,  198-200,  201 

effects  of  in  insects,  247-268 

effect  on  reproduction  in  fish,  181-182, 
186-200 

parental,  and  lifespan,  21-34 
Ageing,  genetics  of  in  Drosophila,  278- 
280,  283 

in  Drosophila,  269-285 

molecular  changes  in,  121-131 

physiological  changes  in  fish  due  to, 
181-211 

theory  of,  129-131,  169-111,  297,  281- 
282 
Albatross,  lifespan  of,  100,  103 
Albumin,  effect  on  bees,  235 
Allometry  of  lifespan,  125 
Alosa  sopidissima,  fecundity  of,  191-192 
Amiiformes,  lifespan  of,  222 
Amino  acids,  effect  on  bees,  238 

in  pollen,  237 
Amyloid,  effect  of  diet,  79 
Amyloidosis,  in  mice,  80-81 
Angelfish,  lifespan  of,  221 

protein  metabolism  in,  183-185 
Anglia  cattle,  lifespan  of,  60 
Anguilliformes,  lifespan  of,  223 
Anguilloidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  152 
Animal  populations,  control  of,  296-297 
Anseres,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 
Anseriformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Apis  mellifera,  247 
Apodiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Arctic  char,  lifespan  of,  147 
Ardea  cinerea,  lifespan  of,  99 
Argentine,  lifespan  and  size  of,  150 
Arteriosclerosis,  aetiology  of,  107-108 

brain  in,  107 

cholesterol  in,  110-111,  113 

in  birds,  106-114 

in  fish,  225 

in  mammals,  109 

in  monkeys,  108 

site  of,  1 10 

Bass,  lifespan  of,  220,  221,  223 
Batrachoidjfonnes,  lifespan  of,  221 


Bats,  lifespan  of,  97,  103 
Bees,  ageing  in,  248 

brain  cells  in,  248 

effect  of  diet,  254 

effect  of  protein  on,  254 

in  tropics,  246 

lifespan  of, 

caged,  235-239 

factors  influencing,  23 1-246 

in  free-flying  colony,  231-235 

physiological  condition  of, 
free-flying,  231-235 
caged,  235-239 

yearly  life  cycle  in,  239-240 
Beryciformes,  lifespan  of,  220 
Birds,  arteriosclerosis  in,  106-114 

lifespan  of  in  captivity,  103 

lifespan  of  in  nature,  90-105 
Bison,  lifespan  of,  105 
Blackfish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Black-pied  cattle,  lifespan  of,  60 
Biennioidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 52 
Blennius  pholis,  growth  of,  156,  157,  158 
Blenny,  growth  of,  156,  157,  158 

lifespan  and  size  of,  152 
Blue-head,  lifespan  of,  221 
Blue  striped  grunt,  lifespan  of,  221 
Body  weight,  relationship  to  lifespan,  115- 

139 
Bowfin,  lifespan  of,  222 
Brain,  in  arteriosclerosis,  107 

internal  environment  of,  133 

weight  of, 

relationship  with  lifespan,  115-139 
Brain  cells,  ageing  in,  248 
Breast  cancer,  in  cattle,  71 
Bullhead,  effect  of  diet  on,  169 

lifespan  and  size  of,  152,  223 

survival  curves  of,  143,  145 
Bulls,  causes  of  death  in,  65 

lifespan  of,  64-65 
Burbot,  lifespan  of,  223 
Biiteo  buteo,  lifespan  of,  99 
Butterfly  fish,  lifespan  of,  121 
Buzzards,  Kfespan  of,  99 

Callionymoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  152 
Callionymus  lyra,  mortality  rate  of,  146 

survival  curves  of,  143,  145 
Cancer,  in  fish,  216,  226 
Cancer  eye,  in  cattle,  71 
Capelin,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 50 

mortality  rate  of,  146 
Capreolus  capreolus,  lifespan  of,  92-94 
Carbohydrate,  in  pollen,  237,  238 


318 


Subject  Index 


319 


Caribou,  lifespan  of,  92-94 
Cat,  lifespan  of,  134 

lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  127 
Catfish,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154,  220,  223 
Cattle,  breeding  of,  57-58 

lifespan  of,  57-65,  70-71 
Cell,  effect  of  temperature  on,  281 
Centenarians,  accuracy  of  age  of,  1 3 
Central  nervous  svstem,  control  of  vital 

functions  by,  128-129 
Centrarchidae,    protein    metabolism    in, 

182-183 
Cervus  elaphus,  lifespan  of,  92-94 
Char,  fecundity  of,  191-192 

lifespan  and  size  of,  152 
Characin,  lifespan  of,  224 
Charadriiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Chickens,  arteriosclerosis  in,  110,  113 
Chiropodomys  gliroides,  lifespan  of,  96 
Cholesterol,  in  aetiology  of  arteriosclerosis 

110-111,  113 
Cholinesterases,  age  activity  of,  247 
Chrysops,  lifespan  of,  105 
Chub,  lifespan  and  size  of,  150,  221 
Ciconiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Cisco,  lifespan  and  size  of,  150 
Citellus  pygmaeus,  lifespan  of,  97 
Clupea  harengus,  growth  of,  156,  157,  158 
Clupea  pallasii,  fecundity  of,  196-197 
Clupeiformes,  lifespan  of,  220,  222 
Clupeoidei,  lifespan  and  growth  in,  160- 
166 

lifespan  and  size  of,  148 
Clupeoids,  survival  curves  of,  143,  145 
Coalfish,  lifespan  and  size  of,  148 
Coate's  knifefish,  224 
Cockroaches,  effect  of  diet  on,  253-254 

lifespan  of,  253-254 
Cod,  hfespan  and  growth  of,  160-166 

lifespan  and  size  of,  148 
Columbiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Cormorants,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 
Corregonus  clupeaformis,  survival  curves 

of,  143,  144 
Cottoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  152 
Cottus  gobio,  survival  curves  of,  143,  145 
Cow(s),  average  age  of  different  breeds,  59 

cause  of  death,  63-64 

lifespan  of,  58-64,  70 

lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  127 
Cowfish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Creatinuria,  in  rats,  83 
Cristivomer  namaycush,  mortality  rates  of, 

145 
Croaking  gourami,  224 
Cunner,  lifespan  of,  221 
Cyprinids,  metabohsm  of,  169 
Cypriniformes,  lifespan  of,  220,  222,  224 
Cyprinodontiformes,  lifespan  and  size  of, 

152,  224 
Cyprinoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 52 

Dab,  fecundity  in,  192,  200-202 
Dace,  lifespan  of,  223 


Dasyatis  akajei,  lifespan  and  growth  of, 

163 
Death,  accuracy  of  age  at,  12-13 
Death  curves,  6-9,  15,  17,  286-296 

of  horses,  56 
Death  rate,  anticipated,  4,  5,  7,  12,  15 

laws  governing.  2-4 

senescent,  4,  5,  6,  7,  10,  11,  12 
Deaths,  accidental,  17 
Deer,  lifespan  of,  91-94 
Diet,  effect  on  amyloid,  79 

effect  on  bees,  254 

effect  on  cockroach,  253,  254 

effect  on  fertility,  34 

effect  on  fish,  178,215-216 

effect  on  flies,  249-255 

effect  on  growth,  84-85,  177,  178,  254- 
255 

effect  on  lifespan,  169,  177-178,  249- 
255,  265-267,  268,  282 

effect  on  hfespan  of  rats,  78,  83-85,  87, 
88,251-252,254 

effect  on  mice,  79-80 

effect  on  mosquito,  255 

effect  on  sexual  maturity,  84-85 

effect  on  trout,  253,  254 

effect  on  wasps,  255 

protein  in,  252-254 
Dipodomys  heermani,  lifespan  of,  96 
Disease,  onset  of  and  longevity,  72-89 
Doctor  fish,  hfespan  of,  221 
Dog,  hfespan  of,  134 

hfetime  energy  expenditure  of,  127 
Dog  snapper,  lifespan  of,  221 
Dragonet,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 52 

mortality  rate  of,  146 

survival  curves  of,  143,  145 
Drosophila  subobscura,  lifespan  of,  262, 
266 

rate  of  ageing  in,  269-285 
Drum,  lifespan  of,  221 
Ducks,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 
Dwarf    top-minnow,    reproduction    and 

senescence  in,  189 

Eel,  lifespan  and  size  of,  152,  223 

natural  death  in,  174 
Electric  catfish,  hfespan  of,  224 
Electric  eel,  lifespan  of,  224 
Environment,  effect  on  hfespan,  167-168, 
229 

effect  on  onset  of  disease,  86-87 
Epinephalus  guttatus,  protein  metabolism 

in, 183-184 

Falconiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Fat  body  in  bees,  233,  235,  237,  238,  239, 

244 
Fat(s),  in  pollen,  237 

storage  in  bees,  237,  238,  239 
Fecundity,    variation    of   in    fish,    191- 

192 
Fertility,  effect  of  parental  age  on,  56 
Finch,  lifespan  of,  99,  103 


320 


Subject  Index 


Fish  (see  also  under  common  names) 
changes  in  due  to  ageing,  218 
effect  of  diet  on,  167 
effect  of  fat  diet  on,  215-216 
effect  of  growth  and  size  on  lifespan, 

147-159 
effect  of  toxic  substances  on,  177 
egg  counting  in,  192-193 
fecundity  in  relation  to  age  in,   186- 

206 
growth  and  senescence  in,  217 
growth  of, 

utilization  of  protein  in,  182-186 
growth  rate  and  lifespan,  227,  229 
infectious  disease  in,  213-214,  228 
lifespan  of, 

characteristics  of  long,  217-218 

in  different  species,  219-224 

in  captivity,  212-230 

in  Nature,  142-180 
lipid  metabolism  in,  227 
metabolic  disease  in,  216 
metabolism  of,  169-170 
natural  death  and  reproduction  in,  170- 

174 
natural  mortality  of,  142-147 
neoplasia  in,  216 
nutrition  in,  215 
ovaries  of,  193 
parasites  in,  213-216 
physiological  changes  due  to  ageing, 

181-211 
protein  utilization  in,  210 
relationship    of    age,    mortality    and 

growth  in,  160-166 
survival  curves  of,  143-147 
variation  in  fecundity  of,  191-192 
Flamingoes,  effect  of  diet  on,  112 
Flounder,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 50 
Fly  (see  Housefly) 
Food,  conversion  of  in  fish,  186 

Gadiformes,  lifespan  of,  223 

lifespan  and  growth  of,  160-166 
lifespan  and  size  of,  148 
metabolism  of,  169 

Galli,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 

Galliformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 

Gambusia   a.    affinis,    reproduction    and 
senescence  in,  187-189 

Gannets,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 

Gar,  lifespan  of,  222 

Gasterosteiformes,  lifespan  of,  152,  223 

Gasterosteus  aculeatiis,  effect  of  environ- 
ment of,  168 

Geese,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 

Genetics,  and  lifespan,  137 

of  ageing,  in  Drosophila,  278-280,  283 

Glomerulonephritis,  in  man,  77 
in  rats,  74-76 

Glycogen,  storage  in  bees,  237,  238,  239 

Golden  Shiner,  223 

Goldfish,  lifespan  of,  223 
metabolism  of,  170 


Gompertz'  Law,  2,  72,  302 
Gompertz-Makeham  equations,  1 1 7 
Grayling,  effect  of  environment  of,  168 
Grouper,  lifespan  of,  220 
Growth,  296,  298 

effect  of  diet,  84-85,  254-255 

effect  on  lifespan,  147-159,  160-166 
Grunion,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154 
Guinea  pig,  lifetime  energy  expenditure  of, 

127 
Guppies,  effect  of  diet  on,  169 

growth  of,  229,  230 

regeneration  in,  208-210 

reproduction  in,  171,  209 


Habrobracon  juglandis,  255 
Haddock,  effect  of  age  on  fecundity,  198- 
200 
Hfespan  and  size  of,  148 
relationship    of  fecundity   and    body 

weight,  200 
sex  organs  in,  190 
Hake,  lifespan  and  size  of,  148 
Halibut,  hfespan  and  size  of,  148,  150 
Heart  disease,  in  man  and  rat,  74-77 
Herons,  lifespan  of,  99 
Herring,  fecundity  of,  196-197 
growth  of,  156,  157,  158 
hfespan  and  growth  of,  160-166 
lifespan  and  size  of,  148 
relationship  of  gonad  growth  to  body 

weight,  193 
relation  of  size  and  maturity,  172 
survival  curves  of,  143,  145 
Hesperoleucus  venustus,  effect  of  environ- 
ment on,  168 
Heterandria  formosa,  reproduction  and 

senescence  in,  189 
Hibernation,  effect  on  lifespan,  103-104 
Highland  cattle,  lifespan  of,  59,  61,  62 
Hippocampus  hudsonius,  lifespan  of,  147 
Hippoglossoides  platessoides,  fecundity  in, 

192 
Hippoglossus  spp,,  lifespan  of,  147 
Holocanthus  bermudensis,  protein  meta- 
bolism in,  183-185 
Horses,  breeding  of,  55 
causes  of  death  in,  68 
coat  colour,  and  longevity,  49-50 
death  curves  of,  56 

lifespan  of,  under  various  climatic  con- 
ditions, 65-69 
lifespan  of  English  thoroughbred,  35- 

56 
lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  127 
survival  curves  of,  35-42 
Housefly,  ageing  in,  247-262 
effect  of  diet  on,  249-255 
lifespan  of,  249-262,  287-288 
effect  of  paternal  age,  259-262 
sex  differences  in,  255-259 
sex  ratio  in,  249 
Humming-birds,  lifespan  of,  105 


Subject  Index 


321 


Ide,  lifespan  of,  223 
Index  of  cephalization,  123,  135 
Infectious  disease,  in  fish,  213-214 
Insects  (see  also  under  names  of  species) 

ageing  in,  247-268 

lifespan  of,  105 

overwintering  in,  232-234,  240 
Irradiation,  effect  on  lifespan,  19-20,  138- 
141,282,290,292 

effect  on  onset  of  disease,  87,  88 

Jack,  lifespan  of,  220 

Kidney  disease,  in  man  and  rat,  74-77 
Killifish,  lifespan  of,  220 

Labidesthes,  growth  of,  156,  157,  158 
Labidesthes   sicculus,    mortality  rate  of, 

146 
Lagonostica  senegala,  lifespan  of,  99 
Lamniformes,  lifespan  of,  220 
Laws  of  mortality,  2-5,  302-311 
Lebistes  reticulatus,  effect  of  diet  on,  169 

fecundity  in,  209 

regeneration  in,  208-210 
Lepidosteiformes,  lifespan  of,  223,  224 
Leucichthys  kiyi,  survival  curves  of,  143, 

145 
Leucichthys  sardinella,  metabolism  of,  169 

mortality  rates  of,  145 
Leuresthes  tenuis,  survival  curves  of,  143 
Lifespan,  allometry  of,  125 

and  parental  age,  21-34 

effect  of  brain  and  body  weight  on,  1 1 5- 
139 

effect  of  diet  on,  169,  249-255,  282 

effect  of  disease  on,  72-89 

effect  of  egg  laying  on,  in  Drosophila, 
275-278 

effect  of  environment,  167-168 

effect  of  growth  and  size  on,  147-159 

effect  of  hibernation,  103-104 

effect  of  metabolism,  103-104, 124-126, 
129, 136-137,  169-170 

effect  of  mitotic  inhibitors,  136 

effect  of  parental  age,  43-47,  53,  259- 
262 

effect  of  radiation  on,  19-20,  138-141, 
282,  290,  292 

effect  of  reproduction,   179,  275-279, 
284-285 

effect  of  temperature  on,  in  Drosophila, 
271-279,  283, 284 

effect  of  thyroid  gland  on,  137 

genetic  aspects  of,  33,  55,  137 

in  nineteenth  century,  23,  32 

mathematical  basis  of,  286-296 

measurement  of,  133-134,  138-141 

methods  of  study,  21-26 

of  albatross,  100,  103 

of  Arabian  horses,  42 

of  Ardea  cinerea,  99 

of  bats,  103 

of  bees,  factors  influencing,  231-246 


Lifespan 

of  bison,  105 

of  birds  in  captivity,  103 

of  birds  in  Nature,  90-105 

of  bulls,  64-65 

of  Buteo  buteo,  99 

of  buzzards,  99 

of  Capreolus  capreolus,  92-94 

of  caribou,  92-94 

of  cat,  134 

ofcattle,  57-65,  70-71 

of  Cervus  elaphus,  92-94 

of  Chiropodomys  gliroides,  96 

of  Citellus  pygmaeus,  97 

of  cockroaches,  253-254 

of  deer,  91-94 

of  Dipodomys  heermani,  96 

of  dog,  134 

of  Drosophila  subobscura,  161,  266 

of  English  thoroughbred  horses,  35-56 

of  finch,  99,  103 

offish,  characteristics  of  long,  217-218 

offish  in  captivity,  212-230 

offish  in  Nature,  142-180 

offish,  of  different  species,  219-224 

of  flies,  105,  249-262,  287-288 

of  Hafling  mares,  43 

of  herons,  99 

of  Hokkaido  ponies,  43 

of  horses,  35-56,  65-69 

of  houseflies,  249-262,  287-288 

sex  differences  in,  255-259 
of  human  beings,  actuarial  aspects  of, 

2-20 
of  humming-birds,  104 
of  insects,  105 
of  kangaroo  rats,  96 
of  Lagonostica  senegala,  99 
of  Lapitsa  horses,  43 
of  mammals  in  Nature,  90-105 
of  man  and  woman  compared,  10-11, 

16 
of  Megadyptes  antipodes,  100 
of  mosquitoes,  105 
ofmouse,  95-96,  287 
of  Myotis  mystacinus,  97 
of  Odocoileus  hemionus,  92-94 
of  Ovisdalli,  91-94 
of  owls,  99 
of  parent  and  progeny  correlated,  47- 

49 
of  Par  us  major,  100 
of  Passer  domesticus,  99 
of  Passerines,  99-100 
of  Peromyscus  leucopus,  95-96 
of  rabbits,  97 
of  Rangifer  articus,  92-94 
ofrats,  95-96,  251-252 

effect  of  diet,  78 

effect  of  disease,  72-89 
of  sheep,  91-94 
of  shrews,  97 
of  sousUk,  97 
of  Sorex  araneus,  97 


322 


Subject  Index 


Lifespan 

of  sparrows,  99 

of  Sterna  hirundo,  97-99 

of  Strix  aluco,  99 

of  swifts,  100,  103,  104 

of  terns,  97-99,  104 

of  tits,  100 

of  trout,  265 

of  ungulates,  91-95 

relationship  to  index  of  cephalization, 
123-125 

relationship  with  growth,  160-166 
Life  tables,  9-11 

limitations  of,  11-12,  18 
Liver,  in  fish,  215-216,  227 
Longevity  {see  also  Lifespan) 

onset  of  disease  and,  72-89 
Look-down,  lifespan  of,  221 
Lovettia  seali,  mortahty  rate  of,  146 
Lung  disease,  in  rats,  82 
Lungfish,  lifespan  of,  224 

Mackerel,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 54 
Makeham's  Law,  2,  302 
Malayan  flying  barb,  lifespan  of,  224 
Mallotus  villosiis,  mortality  rate  of,  146 
Mammals,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 

lifespan  of  in  Nature,  90-105 

life  tables  of,  117-118 

relationship  of  brain  and  body  weight  to 
lifespan,  115-139 
Man,  disease  in,  effect  on  lifespan,  72- 
89 

lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  127 
Mandibular  glands,  in  bees,  233 
Maternal  age,  effect  on  lifespan,  23-27, 

31,32 
Meal  worm,  effect  of  paternal  age  on  life- 
span, 262 
Megadyptes  antipodes,  lifespan  of,  100 
Melanogrammus  aeglefinis,  fecundity  of, 
198-200 

sex  organs  in,  190 
Metabolic  disease,  in  fish,  216 
Metabolism,  effect  on  hfespan,  103-104, 

124-126,  129,  136-137,  169-170 
Mice,  amyloidosis  in,  80-81 

effect  of  diet  on,  79-80 

lifespan  of,  287 

white-footed,  lifespan  of,  95-96 
Miller's  thumb,  lifespan  of,  223 
Minerals,  in  pollen,  237 
Minnow,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 52 
Mitotic  inhibitors,  136 
Molecular  chanjges  in  ageing,  129-131 
Monkeys,  arteriosclerosis  in,  108 
Moonfish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Mortality,  relation  with  age  and  growth, 

160-166 

theory  of,  127-129 
Mortality  rates,  286-296 

laws  governing,  2-5 

mathematical  models  for,  302-311 

sex  difi"erences  in,  81 


Mosquito,  effect  of  diet  on,  255 

lifespan  of,  105 
Mosquito  fish,  mortality  rates  of,  167 

reproduction  and  senescence  in,  187- 
189 
Mothers,  age  of,  effect  on  lifespan,  23-27, 

31,32 
Mugiloidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154 
Musca  domestica  {see  Housefly) 
Muscular  degeneration,  77 

in  rats,  74-76 
Muskallunge,  lifespan  of,  222 
Myocardial  degeneration,  77 

in  rats,  74-76 
Myotis  mystacinus,  lifespan  of,  97 

Neoplasia,  in  fish,  216,  226 

Neothunnas  macropterus,  fecundity  of,  193 

Nephrosis,  in  man,  77 

in  rats,  74-76,  83 
Nerve  cells,  in  bees,  234,  244 
Nitrogen,  in  pollen,  236 

Odocoileus  hemionus,  lifespan  of,  92-94 
Oncorhynchus  nerka,  fecundity  in,  197- 
198 

growth  of,  156,  157,  158 
Oncorhynchus  spp.,  mortality  rates   of, 

145 
Ophiocephaliformes,  lifespan  of,  224 
Orange  chromide,  Hfespan  of,  224 
Ovaries,  effect  of  age  on  in  fish,  201 

growth  of  in  herring,  195 

in  bees,  233,  235,  237,  238,  239,  243 

of  Drosophila,  275-277,  285 

in  fish,  193 

changes  due  to  age,  182 
Overwintering,  232-234,  240 
Ovis  dalli,  lifespan  of,  92-94 
Owls,  lifespan  of,  99 

Palometa,  lifespan  of,  220 
Parasites,  in  fish,  213-216,  228 
Parental  age,  effect  on  fertility  of  off- 
spring, 56 

effect  on  lifespan,  21-34,  43-47,  53, 
259-262 
Parental  death,  in  fish,  189 
Parrots,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109,  110 
Par  us  major,  lifespan  of,  100 
Passer  domesticus,  lifespan  of,  99 
Passeres,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 
Passerines,  lifespan  of,  99-100 
Pelecaniformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Pelicans,  arteriosclerosis  in,  109 
Penguin,  mortality  rate  of,  100 
Perca  fluviatilis,  survival  curves  of,  143, 

144 
Perch,  effect  of  diet,  178 

lifespan  and  size  of,  154,  220,  223 

survival  curves  of,  143,  144,  145 
Perciformes,  lifespan  of,  220,  223,  224 
Percoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 54 
Peromyscus  leucopus,  lifespan  of,  95-96 


Subject  Index 


323 


Pharyngeal  glands,  in  bees,  233,  235,  237, 

238,  239,  244 
Physiological  function,  stability  of,  135 
Pike,  lifespan  of,  222 
Pike-killie,  lifespan  of,  224 
Pilotfish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Plaice,  fecundity  of,  202-206 

growth  of,  157,  158,  159 

lifespan  and  size  of,  1 50 

mortality  rates  of,  167 

reproduction  and  growth  in,  189-190 
Pleuronectes  platessa,  fecundity  in,  202- 
206 

growth  of,  157,  158 

reproduction  and  growth  in,  189-190 
Pleuronectoidei,  lifespan  and  growth  of, 
161-166 

lifespan  and  size  of,  148 
Poeciliidae,  reproduction  and  age  in,  181, 

186-189 
Pollen,  content  of,  237 

eflfect  on  bees,  236-240 
Pompano,  lifespan  of,  220 
Porcupine  fish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Porgy,  hfespan  of,  221 
Poultry,  arteriosclerosis  in,  llO,  113 
Procellariiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Protein,  effect  on  bees,  234,  254 

effect  on  Drosophila,  266-267 

in  diet,  252-254 

in  pollen,  237.  238 

storage  in  bees,  237,  238,  239 

utilization  of  in  fish,  210 
Psittaci,  arteriosclerosis  in,  108-109,  110 
Pudding  wife,  lifespan  of,  221 
Puffer,  lifespan  of,  221 
Pygosteus  pungUius,  growth  of,  156,  157, 

158 

Quillback,  lifespan  of,  222 

Rabbits,  cottontail,  lifespan  of,  97 
Rajiformes,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154,  220 
Rangifer  articus,  lifespan  of,  92-94 
Rasbora,  lifespan  of,  224 
Rats,  brown,  lifespan  of,  95-96 

causes  of  death  of,  82-83 

disease  in,  effect  on  lifespan,  72-89 

efiFect  of  diet  on,  78,  251-252,  254,  265, 
268 

kangaroo,  lifespan  of,  96 

lifespan  of,  95-96,  251-252 

lung  disease  in,  82 

nephrosis  in,  83 
Rattus  rattus,  lifespan  of,  95-96 
Ray,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154,  163,  221 
Red  hind,  lifespan  of,  220 

protein  metabolism  in,  183-185 
Regeneration,  in  guppies,  208-210 
Reproduction,  effects  of  age  on  in  fish, 
181-182,  186-206 

effect  on  Ufespan,  179,  275-279,  284- 
285 

in  fish,  170-174 


Roach,  lifespan  of,  222 

Rockfish,  lifespan  of,  220 

Rosy  tetra,  lifespan  of,  224 

Rubner's  theory  of  ageing,  125-126,  128 

Rudder  fish,  hfespan  of,  221 

Sailfish,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154 
Salivary  glands,  in  bees,  233, 235, 237, 238, 

239 
Salmo  gairdneri,  growth  rates  of,  182 
Salmon,  fecundity  of,  192,  197-198 

growth  of,  156,  157,  158 

lifespan  of,  222 

hfespan  and  growth  of,  160-166 

lifespan  and  size  of,  150,  152 

mortality  rates  of,  145,  166 

natural  death  in,  174 

relation  of  size  and  maturity,  172 

reproduction  in,  179 
Salmonoidei,  lifespan  and  growth  of,  161- 
166 

hfespan  and  size  of,  150 
Salmo  salar,  fecundity  in,  197-198 
Salve!  in  us  alpinus,  hfespan  of,  147 
Sand  dab,  lifespan  and  size  of,  148 
Sardina  pilchardis,  eflfect  of  environment 

on,  168 
Sardines,  effect  of  environment  on,  168 
Sanger,  hfespan  and  size  of,  154 
Scandinavians,  lifespan  of,  21-26 
Scat,  lifespan  of,  221 
Scombroidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  1 54 
Sea  horse,  lifespan  of,  147,  154 
"Senile  "death,  in  fish,  189 
Sex  differences,  in  growth  of  fish,  1 59 

in  mortahty  among  fish,  145,  166-167 

in  reactions  to  environment  in  fish, 
178 
Sex  organs,  changes  in  due  to  age  in  fish, 
218 

in  fish,  229 

relationship  to  body  weight  in  fish, 
193-196 
Sexual  maturity,  eflfect  of  diet,  84-85 

in  fish,  170-171 
Shad,  fecundity  of,  191-192 
Shark,  lifespan  of,  220 
Sheep,  hfespan  of,  91-94 
Sheepshead,  lifespan  of,  221 
Shrews,  lifespan  of,  97 
Siluroidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154 
Size,  eflfect  on  hfespan,  147-159 
Skipjack,  metabolism  of,  170 
Smelt,  lifespan  of,  147,  150 
Snake-head,  lifespan  of,  224 
Snapper,  lifespan  of,  221 
Sole,  hfespan  and  size  of,  150 
Sorex  araneus,  hfespan  of,  97 
Souslik,  lifespan  of,  97 
Soya  flour,  effect  on  bees,  235 
Spadefish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Sparrows,  lifespan  of,  99 
Sphenisciformes,  mortahty  rate  of,  101 
Sprat,  lifespan  and  size  of,  148 


324 


Subject  Index 


Squirrel  fish,  lifespan  of,  220 
Stallions,  causes  of  death  in,  68 

lifespan  of,  65-67 
Sterility,  in  cows,  63,  64 
Sterna  hirundo,  lifespan  of,  97-99 
Stickleback,  effect  of  temperature  on,  168 

growth  of,  156,  157,  158 

lifespan  of,  152,  220,  223 
Stress,  88 

Strigiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  101 
Strix  aluco,  lifespan  of,  99 
Sturgeon,  growth  of,  156,  157,  158 

lifespan  of,  147,  220,  222,  226 

lifespan  and  growth  of,  163 

lifespan  and  size  of,  152 

oldest  age  of,  191 

reproduction  in,  171 

survival  curves  of,  143,  144 
Sugar,  effect  on  bees,  238 
Sunfish,  effect  of  environment  of,  168 

lifespan  of,  223 

protein  metabolism  in,  182-183 
Swifts,  lifespan  of,  100,  103,  104 
Syngnathiformes,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154 

Tarpon,  lifespan  of,  220 
Temperature,  effect  on  bees,  245,  246 

effect  on  cell,  281 

effect  on  lifespan,  167-168 

effect  on  lifespan  in  Drosophila,  271- 
279,  283,  284 

effect  on  protein  metabolism,  185-186 
Tench,  lifespan  of,  223 
Tenebrio  molitor,  effect  of  paternal  age  on 

lifespan,  262 
Terns,  lifespan  of,  97-99,  104 
Tetrodontlformes,  lifespan  of,  221 
Thunniformes,  lifespan  and  growth  of,  163 

lifespan  and  size  of,  154 


Thymallus  signifer,  effect  of  environment 

on,  168 
Tits,  lifespan  of,  100 
Toadfish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Totoaba,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154 
Trigger  fish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Trout,  effect  of  diet  on,  169,  253,  254 

effect  of  reproduction  on,  179 

fecundity  in,  192,  197 

food  conversion  in,  186 

growth  rates  of,  1 82 

lifespan  of,  222,  265 

lifespan  and  size  of,  1 50,  1 52 

mortahty  rates  of,  145 
Tuberculosis,  in  cows,  63,  64 
Tuna,  fecundity  in,  193 

lifespan  and  size  of,  1 54 

Ungulates,  lifespan  of,  91-95 

Vitamins,  effect  on  bees,  238 

in  pollen,  237,  238,  239 
Vultures,  arteriosclerosis  in,  110 

Walleye,  fecundity/length  relationship  in, 

197 
Wasps,  232,  255 
Wax  glands  in  bees,  233,  239 
Weakfish,  lifespan  of,  221 
Whitebait,  lifespan  and  size  of,  150 

mortality  rate  of,  146 
Whitefish,  growth  of,  156,  157,  158 

lifespan  of,  222 

lifespan  and  size  of,  1 50 

survival  curves  of,  143,  144 
Whiting,  lifespan  and  size  of,  154 

Yellow  tail,  lifespan  of,  221 


Printed  by  Spottiswoode,  Ballantyne  db  Co.  Ltd.^  London  and  Colchester 


CUMULATIVE  INDEXES  TO  VOLUMES  1-5 

AUTHOR  INDEX 

Numbers  in  bold  type  indicate  volume  number.    Plain  numbers  indicate  a  contribution 

either  in  the  form  of  an  article  or  as  a  contribution  to  the  discussions.     Italic  numbers 

indicate  a  reference  to  an  author's  work. 


Aas,  K 
Aasen,  O.   . 
Abbot,  W.  E.       . 
Abderhalden,  E. 
Abercrombie,  M. 
Abess,  A.  T. 
Abraham,  K. 
Ackermann,  P.  G 

Adair,  F.    . 

Adair,  F.  L. 
Adams,  C.  E. 
Adamsons,  K. 
Addis,  T. 
Adlersberg,  G.  D 
Adolph,  E.  F.     . 


Aebi,  H.     . 
Ahlman  K.  L. 
Akyuz,  E. 
Albertini,  A.  von 
Albrectsen,  S.  R. 
Albright,  F. 
Alex,  M.      . 


Alexander,  F. 
Alexander,  J.  D. 
Alexander,  L.  C. 
Alexander,  M.  O. 
Alexejev-Berkmann,  I. 
Allee,  W.  C. 
Allen,  E.     . 
Allison,  A.  C. 
Allison,  J.  B. 
Allott,  E.  N. 
Ally,  M.  S. 
Aim,  G.      . 

Alpatov,  W.  W. 
Altman,  K.  I. 

Alving,  A.  S. 

Amatruda,  T.  T. 


.  4,  275,  294 
5,  149,  156,  174 

.  4,  109,  113 

4,  201,  202,  205 

.  3,  70 

.  4,  727,  134 

.      1,  33,  48 

1,  110,  121,  122; 
4,  241,  246 

2,  120,  121,  125, 
127 

.  1,  151,  158,  160 
.  1,  143,  159 
4,  92,  93 
4,  254,  257,  259,  261 
.  3,  136,  142 
4,  3,  4,  5,  6,  7,  8,  9, 
10,  11,  12,  13,  14,  34,  34, 
60,  75,  94,  95,  96,  97,  152, 
162,  195,  196,  221,  228, 
225,311,575 
.  1,  77 
.  4,  74,  76 
.  5,  149,  174 
1,  53,  55,  108 
4,  67,  68,  69,  72 
1,  109,  122 
1,  91,  99,  103; 
3,  97,  100 


.  3,  757,  142 

.  4,  275,  295 

.  4,  289,  296 

4,  40,  41,  57 

A.      .  4,  287,  295 

1,  10,  14 

2,  38,  50,  53 

.  2,  235,  238 

.  4,  757,  134 

4,  38,  39,  41,  43,  56 

.  5,  255,  264 

5,  144.  155,  159,  168, 

174 

.      3,  24,  29 

2,  277,  257,  234,  238, 

239 

4,  243,  244,  246, 

248 

4,  75 


Amoroso,  E.  C.  1,  7^2,  159;  2,  1, 

16,  27,  66,  67,  68,  85,  705, 
106,   113,    115,    117,    125, 

127,    128,    145,   755, 

160, 


126, 

755, 
241, 


American  Academy  of  Pediatrics  .        4, 

160,  161 
Ames,  R.  G.        .         .         .4,6,11,97 


d'Ancona,  U 
Andersen,  J. 
Anderson,  A.  R. 
Anderson,  J. 
Anderson,  J.  L. 
Anderson,  W.  E. 

Andersson,  B. 
Andjus,  R.  K. 
Ando,  S.     . 
Andrew 
Andrew,  W. 
Andrews,  M.  C. 
Anitschkow,  N. 
Anner,  G. 
Ansell,  S. 
Anslow,  W.  P. 
Anthonisen,  P. 
Anthony,  H.  E. 
Appelboom,  J.  W 
Appelget,  J. 
Arai,  H.      . 
Araki,  Y.    . 
Arataki,  M. 
Aristophanes 
Armbruster,  L. 
Ariiey,  S.  F. 
Aron,  C.     . 

Aron,  M.    . 
Arons,  W,  L. 
Arora,  H.  L. 
Artunkal     . 
Arvidsson,  V.  B. 
Ascher,  B.  M. 
Ascher,  K.  R.  S. 
Asdell 

Asgood,  H.  S. 
Ashton,  N. 
Assheton,  R. 
Astbury,  W.  T. 
Astrup,  P.  . 
Aub,  J.  C.  . 


175,   200,   240. 

243,   246,   247,    253 

.  5,  7<57,  174 

5,  92,  94,  95  102 

.  4,  301,  308 

5,  232,  234,  241 

.  3,  178,  181 

4,  720,  727, 

128,  131,  135 

4,  37,  42,  56 

.  1,  765,  169 

4,  68,  69,  72 

4,  225 

.  5,  248,  264 

2,  129,  132,  143 

1,  89;  3,  707 
4,  757,  795 

5,  27,  57 

4,  277,  298 
4,  41,  56 

2,  184,  187 
4,  24,  31 

5,  755,  77^ 

2,  32,  34,  53 
4,  68,  69,  72 
.  4,  257,  259 

.  1,  34 
5,  232,  241,  242 
.  2,206,209 
2,  36,  37,  38,  39,  41,  42, 
46,  47,  53,  60,  63,  66 
2,  60,  63,  66 
.  4,  109,  112 
.  5,  149,  174 
.  2,  84 
3,  759 
.  3,  752,  143 
5,  255,  259,  264 
.  1,  30 
.  5,  257,  264 
.  2,  30 
.  2,  755,  755 
.    3,  96,  100 
.      4,  41,  56 
1,  52,  54,  77,  104,  105, 
124,  125,  205:2,775, 
779,  750,  755 


11 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Aumonier,  F.  J. 
Austin,  C.  R. 
Austin,  O.  L. 
Austin,  O.  L.  Jr. 
Axelrad,  B.  J. 


Bailey,  M.  . 
Baillie,  W.  H.  ' 
Bainey,  J.  D. 
Baird,  D. 
Baker,  B.  L. 
Bale,  W.  F. 
Balfour,  W.  M 
Balinsky,  B. 

Ball,  M.  R. 
Ball,  Z.  B, 
Ballon,  J. 

Balo,  J. 


.  3,  56 

.      2,  86,  96 

.    5,  98,  102 

.    5,  98,  102 

4,  292,  294,  297 


Babcock,  H.         .         .         .3,  179,  181 
Babkin,  B.  P.       .  .4,  62,  72 

Back,  E.      .  .  .5,  235,  238,  241 

Baclesse,  M.         .  .  .2, 168, 172 

Bacon,  F 3,  9,  18 

Bacsich,  P.  .1,  142,  159;  2,  41,  53 

Bagenal,  T.  B.      .    5,  192,  193,  200,  201, 

202,  205,  207 
4,  146,  147,  149 
3,  24,  29 
4,  109,  112 
.  5,33 
4,260 
2,  263,  238 
2,  236,  238 
1,  37,  38,  48;  3,  178, 
181,  186 
.    4,  114,  291,  297 
.  1,  180,  185 
1,  219,  223,  224,  225,  227, 
232,  233,  236 
1,  98,  99,  103,  104,  107; 
3,  32,  44,  70,  97,  98,  100, 
101,  101,  103 
Banfield,  A.  W.  F.        .         .    5,  92,  102 


Banga,  I. 


Banta,  A.  M. 
Barber,  J.  K. 

Barbour,  A. 
Barcroft,  Sir  J. 

Barker,  J.  P. 
Barker,  S.  B. 
Barlow,  J.  S. 
Barnes,  B.  A. 
Barnes,  L.  L. 

Barnes,  R.  H. 
Barnet,  H.  A.  R, 
Barr 

Barrett,  G.  R. 
Barrett,  M.  K. 
Barron,  D.  H. 
Barrows,  C.  H. 
Bassett,  D.  L. 
Bates,  D.  V. 
Bauer,  K.    . 
Bauer,  L.  S. 
Bartholomew,  R 
Bartlett,  Sir  F.  C 


Barton,  R.  A. 
Bartter,  F.  C. 

Beams,  H.  W. 


1,  98,  99,  103,  104,  107; 
3,  65,  66,  67,  97,  98,  100, 
101,  103 
3,  24,  29 
4,  89,  92,  292,  293, 
294,  297 
.  4,  265,  268 
2,  126,  151,  158; 
5,  133 
4,  3,  4,  11 
.  2,  130,  143 
.  4,  106,  112 
.  4,  49,  56 
1,  188,  193,  197,  200; 
3,  8,  18;  5,  251,  264 
.  1,  180,  185 
5,  5,  15 
.  1,89 
.  1,  149,  160 
.  3,  123,  124 
.  2,  118,  125 
.  4,  245,  246 
.  2,  60,  66 
1,  58,  62,  64,  65 
.  5,  59,  69 
.  4,  133,  134 
.  2,  112,  113 
1,  41,  202,  209, 
214,215,216,217,218,2^5 
.  5,  53,  54 
4,  46,  57,  89,  92, 196, 
280,  292,  293,  294,  297 
.      2,  86,  96 


Bean,  H.  W. 
Bean,  W.  B. 

Beard,  R.  E. 


Beck,  J,  C. 
Becker,  W.  H. 


4, 120,  121,  131,  135 

1,  78,  79,  80,  87, 

105,  171 

5,  4,  14,  288,  289, 

302,  303, 305,  307,  308, 

311 

.  4,  293,  294 

.  4,  275,  295 

Beeton,  M.   1,  238;   5,21,  31,  33,  53,  54 


Belding,  D.  L 
Bell,  F.  H. 
Bell,  R.  G. 
Bendell,  J.  F. 
Benedict,  F.  G. 
Beneke,  R. 
Benjamin,  B. 


Bennett,  H.  S. 

Bensley 
Bercu,  B.  A. 
Berdegue.  J. 
Berg,  B.  N. 


Berg,  L.  S. 
Bergerard,  Y. 
Bergmann,  K. 
Bergstrom,  J. 
Bergstrom,  W.  H. 
Berkeley,  A. 

Berman,  C. 
Bernard,  C. 

Bernhard,  K. 
Bernstein,  F. 
Bertalanffy,  L.  von 
Berthet,  J.  . 
Bertholf,  L.  M.    . 
Bertin,  L.    . 
Bertucio,  M. 
Best,  A.  T. 
Best,  C.  H 


Beutler,  R. 
Bevan,  W.  . 


Bezem,  J.  J. 
Biale,  J.  B. 
Bichat,  X. 
Bidder,  G.  P. 


Bienvenu,  B. 
Bilewicz,  S. 
Billeter,  J-R. 
Billingham,  R.  E 


5,  197,  207 
.  5,  151,  177 
.  1,  196,  200 
.  5,  101,  102 
3,9,  18;4,138;5,124 
.  5,  110,  112 
.     1,  198,  199;  5,  2,  15, 
16,  17,  18,  19,  32,  34,  70, 
293 
.     2,  106,  112,  114,  129, 
144 
.2,103 
.4,  280,  294 
.  5,  755,  175 
.      5,  16,  32,  33,  72,  73, 
74,  75,  76,  79,  79,  80,  82, 
83,  84,  85,  86,  87,  88,  114, 
137,  265,  266,  285 
.  5,  212,  225 
.      2,  11,  13 
.     4,  284,  286,  291,  296 
4,298 
.  4,  120,  134 
1,  219,  223,  224,  225, 
226,  227,  232,  233,  236 
.  3,  106,  114 
1,  205;  2,23,27, 
108,  113,  118,  137 
.  2,  177,  183 
3,  195 
5,  157,  169,  175 
.  2,  136,  143 
.  5,  235,  241 
.  5,  168,  175 
.     4,  140,  141,  146,  149 
5,  104 
3,  1,  36,  46,  49,  101, 
103,    126,    127,    128,    146, 
147,    182,    188,    192,    195 
.    5,235,236,241 
.  4,  127,  134 
Beverton,  R.  J.  H.        5,   142,   149,   151, 
158,   175,    111,    178,    179, 
226,  227,  229 
.    5,  97,  103 
.  2,  208,  209 
1,  245 
1,28;  3,  9,  18;  5,  173, 
175, 182, 189, 190, 191, 207 
.  1,  198,  200 
.  5,  277,  281 
.  4,  181,  195 
1,  170; 
2,229,231; 
3,  116,  124 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Billis,  L.  de 
Binet,  A.     . 
Birch-Andersen,  A. 
Birchard,  W.  H. 
Birren,  J.  E. 
Bischoff,  F. 
Bishop,  L.  R. 
Biskind,  G.  S. 
Biskind,  M.  S. 
Bittner,  J.  J. 
Bjorkman,  N. 
Bjorksten,  J. 
Black,  A. 
Black,  D.  A.  K. 


.  4,  242,  246 

.  3,  176,  182 

.  4,  126,  136 

.      4,  51,  56 

.      1,  38,  48 

4,  262 

.  2,  206,  209 

.  1,  153,  160 

.  1,  153,  160 

.   1,  155,  160 

.    2,  155,  156,  158 

.   3,  66,  67 

.  4,  127,  135 

4,  13,  38,40,41,50, 


Blackburn,  M. 
Blackman,  F.  F. 
Blair,  W.  F. 
Blake,  W.  D. 
Blalock,  A. 
Bland,  J.  H. 
Blandau,  R.  J. 
Blaxter,  K.  L. 
Blazka,  P. 
Blegen,  E. 
Bloch,  E.    . 
Bloom,  W.  L. 
Blum,  H.  F. 
Boas,  E.  P. 
Bodenheimer,  F 
Bottcher,  T. 
Bogdonoff,  M.  D 


56,57,59,59,15,  113,  150, 

150,    163,   208,   224,   226, 

247,   248,   264,   267,   268, 

269,  310,  317 

5,  146,  149,  151,  175 

2,  205 

.    5,  96,  102 

4,  275,  287,  294 

.  4,  287,  296 

4,  294 

1,  149;  2,  81,  83,  85 

4,  301,  307,  308,  309 

5,  170,  175 

4,  273,  294 


4,  90,  93 
4,  127,  134 
2,  228,  231 
3,  136,  142;  4,  136,  142 


5,294 
5,  60,  61,  69 

1,  110,  120,  121,  122; 
3,  75,  90;  4,  225 

Bogomolets,  A.  A.        .         .  3,  133,  142 
Boling,  E.  A.        .     4,  102,  106,  108,  113 
.  .2,  191,  198 

.  4,  109,  113 
1,  42,  48;  3,  137,  143, 
173,  182 
.  4,  257,  259 
M.      .  .4,  80,  92 

5,  119,  123,  128,  132 
.  2,  208,  209 
4,  89,  92,  93 
.  4,  132,  135 
3,  117,  118,  120,  123, 
124,  125 
.  5,  170,  175 
4,  60,  101,  114,  196, 
248,260,  261,  269,298 
.  4,  200,  205 
.  3,  151,  168 
.  2,  56 

2,  183,  210,  211,247, 
247,24S,249;3, 10, 17,18, 
20,  28,  2P,  31,  129;  5,  70, 
84,  91,  96,  102,  103,  104, 
105,  112,  113,  119,  132, 
137,217,225,221,228,245 

3,  34,  35,  47,  50,  68, 
72,  130,  144,  188,  189 


Bommer,  S 
Bondurant,  J. 
Bondy,  E. 

Bendy,  P.  K. 
Bongiovanni,  A. 
Bonin,  G.  von 
Bonner,  J.  . 
Boog,  J.  M. 
Booher,  L.  E, 
Boot,  L.  M. 

Boroughs,  H. 
Borst,  J.  G.  G. 

Borum,  E.  R. 
Botwinick,  J. 
Bounoure   . 
Bourliere,  F. 


Bourne,  G.  H. 


Boyd,  E.  M. 
Boyd,  H.     . 
Boyd,  J.  D. 
Boyle,  A.  J. 
Boyne,  A.  W. 
Bozic,  B.     . 
Brachet,  J. 
Bradley,  G.  P. 
Bradley,  S.  E. 
Brambell,  F.  W.  R. 

Brand,  F.  C. 


.  2,  122,  125 
.  5,  101,  102 
.  2,  185,  186 
.  4,  289,  296 
.  1,  196,  199 
2,  11,  13,  17 
.  1,  192,  193 
.  4,  274,  294 
4,  274,  275,  294 
2,  34,  50,  53, 
57,  60,  66,  81,  83 
.      4,  23,  32 
Brandfonbrener,  M.  1,  237;  3,  77,  84,  88, 
89,  90,  91 ;  4,  234,  236,  246 


Brandt,  F.  A. 
Brannon,  E.  S. 
Breder,  C.  M. 
Breed,  E.  S. 
Bretherick,  O. 
Breward,  M.  M 
Brewer,  W.  D. 
Bricker,  N.  S. 
Brien,  P.     . 
Briggs,  A.  P. 
Briscoe,  W.  A 
Brod,  J. 


Brodsky,  W.  A. 

Brody,  M.  B. 
Brody,  S. 

Bronks,  D. 
Brooks,  L. 
Bro-Rasmussen,  F 
Brown,  A.  N. 
Brown,  C.  J.  D. 
Brown,  I.  W. 
Brown,  L.  M. 
Brown,  M.  E. 

Brown,  R.  A. 
Brozek,  J. 
Brubacher,  Z. 
Bruce,  R.  A. 
Brues,  A.  M. 
Bruhin,  H. 
Brull,  L.      . 

Bryden,  J.  G. 
Bryuzgin,  V.  L. 
Buchanan,  A.  D. 
Buchborn,  E, 
Bucher,  G.  E. 
Bucht,  H.    . 


3,  102 

4,  279,  297 

5,  272,  217,  218  225 

4,  274,  297 

5,  237,  243 
2,  33,  53 

1,  722,  725 
4,  45,  56 
1,244 
4,  275,  277,  295 
.  4,  264,  268 
4,  248,  271,  272,  273,  279, 
281,  284,  286,  288,  295,  296 


Buchwald,  N.  A. 
Buehl,  C.  C. 
Buehler,  C. 
Buettner,  K. 
Bujard,  E.  . 
Bull,  G.  M. 
Bull,  J.  P.   . 


4,  24,  31,  40, 

46,57 

.  3,  775,  182 

1,  188,  189,  190,  193, 

197,  199;  5,  124,  132 

3,  70,  96,  100 

.  4,  109,  113 

.    4,  63,  65,  68,  73 

.  4,  131,  135 

3,  26,  29;  5,  168,  175 
.  2,  234,  238 
.      4,  46,  57 

5,  147,  148,  149,  175, 

217,  225 

3,  757 

4,  757,  757,  752 

.  2,  777,  183 

.      4,  46,  56 

5,  138 

2,  777,  181,  183 

1,  27,  68,  123,  124,  202, 

207,  238,  241 

.  3,  707,  114 

.      3,  77,  18 

5,  59,  69 

.      4,  280, 292,  293,  295 

.  5,  258,  264 

4,  273,  279,  280,  283, 
295,  298 

3,  755,  7^2 
1,  198,  201 

.  1,49 

4,100 

2,  42,  53 

4,  265,  268 
4,98,95,  115,  150,  151, 

163,207,228,249,269 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Bullough,  w.  s. 
Bulmer,  M.  G. 
Bunge,  R.  G. 
Bunting,  H. 
Burch,  H.   . 
Burd,  A.  C. 
Burgen,  A.  S.  V. 
Burger,  M.. 
Burgess,  E.  W. 
Burgos,  M.  H. 
Burkl,  W.   . 
Burn,  G.  B. 
Burnet,  F.  M. 
Burns,  C.  M. 
Burrill,  M.  W. 
Burrows,  H. 
Burton,  D. 
Bush,  I.  E. 
Butler,  A.  M. 
Butler,  C.  G. 
Butt,  W.  R. 
Buxton,  P.  A. 
Byers,  S.  O. 
Bykov,  K.  M. 
Byrom,  F.  B. 


2, 


4, 


2,  38,  53,  64,  66 

4,  65,  69,  73,  74 

1,  171 

2,  191,  192,  198 

1,  197,  199 

5,  149,  175 

4,  64,  73 

3,  6,  18 

1,  43,  48 

2,  86,  87,  96 

36,  42,  43,  44,  53 

.  2,  235,  238 

.  2,  228,  231 

.      5,  21,  31 

.  2,  12 

2,  49,  51,  53 

.    3,  97,  100 

.  4,  181,  194 

139,  141,  147,  149 

5,  237,  241,  243 
.  1,  130,  137 
.  5,  240,  241 
.  3,  137,  142 
.  4,  287,  295 
.  4,  258,  259 


Caldwell,  M.  J.    . 
Caligaris,  L.  C.  S. 
Calloway,  D.  H. 
Calvin,  M. 
Cameron,  G.  R. 


Cameron,  J.  W.  McB 
Cameron,  W. 
Campbell,  H.  A, 
Campbell,  R.  M. 
Canivenc 
Cannon,  C. 
Cannon,  P.  R 
Capek,  K.  . 
Card,  W.  I. 
Carlo,  J.     . 


Carlsen,  E. 
Carlson,  A.  J. 
Carlson,  L.  D.     . 
Carter,  S.  B. 
Castle,  W.  E. 
Cathcart,  E.  S.     . 
Caton,  J.  D. 
Caton,  W.  L. 
Cavan,  R.  S. 
Cavanaugh,  M.  W. 
Cavazzana,  P. 
Challice,  C.  E.     . 
Chalmers,  T.  A.  . 
Chaloupka,  M.    . 
Chang,  H-W. 
Chaplin,  H. 


.  4,  200,  205 

.      4,  89,  93 

4,  131,  134,  135 

.  2,  223,  232 

1,  16,  26,  54, 

55,  66;  3,  93,  100 

5,  258,  264 

.  1,57 

1,  197,  200 

2,  166,  172 
2,175 

5,  59,  69 
4,  133,  134 
4,  165,  179 
4,  301,  309,  310,  317 
1,  126,  137,  219,  223,  224, 
225,  226,  227,  232,  233, 
236;  4,  90,  93 
4,  46,  56,  264,  268 
1,  197,  199,  206 
5,89 
3,  51,  59 


Chaplin,  J.  P. 
Chapman,  R.  N. 
Chart,  J.  J. 


1,  155,  160 
4,  283,  295 

2,  779,  183 
2,  166,  172 

1,  43,  48 

1,3,  10,  II,  13 

2,  7P7,  196,  198 

.      2,  86,  96 

.  5,  56 

.  I,  122,  123 

5,  145,  146,  153,  175 

1,  164,  169;  2,  216, 

226,  231,  232,  236,  238 

.  4,  755,  179 

.  1,  7P5,  199 

4,  193,  194,  195,  296 


Chase,  H.  B. 
Chasis,  H.  . 
Cheek,  D.  B. 

Chesley,  L.  C.      . 
Chevremont 
Chibnall,  A.  C.    . 
Chipman,  W.  A. 
Chitty,  H. 

Chow,  B.  F. 

Christensen,  P,  R 
Christie,  R.  V 

Chugaeva,  M. 
Ciba  Foundation 
Claesson,  L. 
Clara,  M.    . 
Clark,  F.  N. 
Clark,  H.  W. 
Clark,  J.     . 
Clarke 

Clarke,  J.  M. 
Clarke,  R.  D. 

Clay,  H.  M. 
Clermont,  Y. 
Cloette 
Cochran,  W. 
Cockrum,  E.  L. 
Cohen,  A.  . 
Cohen,  B.  L. 
Cohen,  D.  M. 
Cohen,  H.  W. 
Cohen,  J.    . 
Cohn,  J.  E. 
Cohrs,  P. 
Cole,  D.  F. 
CoUazos,  Ch.  J.  I. 
Colonge,  R.  M.  A 
Comfort,  A 


Comroe,  J.  H 
Conn,  J.  W. 
Conrad,  H.  S. 
Conway,  E.  J. 

Cook,  C.  D. 
Cooke,  R.  E. 


2,  191,  192,  194,  199 

.  4,  230,  246 

4,  105,  109,  113,  120, 

121,  132,  133,  134 

4,  82,  84,  89,  92,  93 

2,  103 

.  2,  204,  209 

.  5,  170,  175 

3,  10,  18;  5,  105, 

296,  298 

1,  198,  199;  3,  79, 

91 ;  4,  245,  246 

.      1,  40,  48 

1,  58,  57,  62,  65,  65, 

66,  67,  68,  246 

.      5,  53,  54 

.  5,  128,  132 

.      2,  60,  66 

.  1,54 

5,  149,  155,  175 

.  5,  272,  225 

.  1,  239,  240 

5,  266 

.  5,  278,  281 

5,  4,  5,  6,  7,  8,  10, 

11,  14,  15 

.    3,  155,  162,  168 

.      2,  86,  96 

.  2,  159,  160 

4,  139,  141,  147,  149 

.      3,  27,  29 

2,  22,  23,  26 

.    4,202,203,205 

.  5,  757,  775 

.      4,  57,  57 

4,  132,  133,  135,  227 

.    4,243,244,246 

.  5,  770,  772 

.  4,  193,  194 

.    1,  109,  121,  122 

2,  3,  13,  84,  168,  172 

.      1,  28,  29,  30,  51,  52, 

107,   138,    171,  204,  238, 

241,   242,  243,  244,  245; 

2,  215,  231;  3,  2,  7,11,12, 

18,  29,  30,  36,  37,  38,  46, 

68,  118,  124;  5,  17,  17,  32, 

34,  35,  36,  37,  43,  44,  47, 

52,  53,  54,  55,  56,  70,  71, 

84,  85,  87,  88,   103,   104, 

113,    114,   775,   777,   779, 

752,    134,    136,    137,   759, 

77i,   775,    178,    179,   208, 

208,   209,   210,   277,   225, 

111,   129,   230,   244,   262, 

264,   265,   266,   286,   286, 

289,  291,  294,  295,  298 

.  4,  264,  268 

.  4,  44,  56,  76,  280,  295 

.    3,  777,  182,  186 

4,  22,  23,  24,  31,  35,  99, 

203,  205,  207,  226 

.     4,  705,  775,  143,  149 

.  4,  133,  134 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Cooke,  W.  T. 
Cooper,  A.  R. 
Cooper,  E.  L. 
Cooper,  I.  S. 
Cooper,  K.  E. 
Cope,  O.     . 
Copenhaver,  J.  H. 
Coquoin-Carnot,  M 
Corbo,  S.    . 
Corey,  E.  L. 
Corkins,  C.  L. 
Cornaro,  L. 
Combleet,  T. 
Comer,  G.  W. 


Corsa,  L. 
Cort,  J.  H. 


Cotes,  P.  M. 
Cotlove,  E. 
Count,  E.  W. 
Court,  D.  M. 
Coumot,  L. 
Courrier,  R. 
Coville,  F.  E. 
Cowan,  G.  A 
Cowdry,  E.  V. 


.  4,  109,  112 

4,  120,  121,  135 

5,  144,  153,  176 
4,  39,  40,  41,  43,  56 

2,  120,  121,  125 

4,  49,  56 

4,  209,  219 

4,  217,  219 

1,  199,  200 

2,  109,  113 
5,245 

1,  194,  200 

2,  197,  198 
2,  15,  55,  56,64,  66, 

67,  246,  252 

4,  105,  113,  114,  143,  149 

4,  33,  45,  56,  214,  219, 

222,  278,  283,  287,  288, 

289,  291,  292,  295 

.  2,  162,  172 

.  4,  129,  135 

5,  119,  123,  132 

.    3,  95,  100 

4,  262 

2,  126,  168,  172 

.  4, 133,  134 

.      3,  55,  56 

1,  24,  26,  27,  50,  53, 

55,  88,  103,  108,  171,  172, 


204,  205,  207,  242,  243 
Cowie,  D.  B.        .         .    2,  111,  112,  113 

Crabb6,  J 4,  280,  298 

Cramer,  W.  .         2,  23,  27,  122,  125 

Crane,  W.  A.  J.  .  .         .  4,  260 

Cravioto  Munoz,  J.  .  .1, 198,  200 
Crawford,  B.  .  .  .4,51,56 
Crawford,  H.  2,  216,  226,  231,  237,  238 
Crawford,  J.  D.  .  4,  139,  140,  141, 

146,  147,  149 
Cresseri,  A.  .         .         .3,  137,  142 


Crevier,  P.  H. 
Crosley,  A.  P. 
Crowell,  M.  F 


.   4,39,40,43,56 
.  4,  289,  297 
1,  197,  200;  3,  25, 
29;  5,  169,  176,  251,  253, 
264 


Crozier,  W.  J. 
Cruickshank,  D.  H. 
Grumpier,  H.  R. 
Cruz,  W.  O. 
Csapo,  J.    . 
Cummins,  J. 
Cuny,  G.    . 
Currie,  C. 
Curtis 

Curtis,  R.  H. 
Curzon,  E.  G. 
Cutbush,  M. 

Cuthbertson,  D.  P. 

Dacie,  J.  V. 
Dack,  S.     . 
Daines,  M.  C. 
Dalton,  K. 


5,267 

2,  203,  210 

2,  112,  113 

2,  236,  238 

4,  155,  161 

4,  46,  56 

4,  242,  246 

1,  198,  201 

.  5,87 

4,  292,  297 

4,  131,  135 

2,  216,  226, 

231,  237,  238 

.  4,  130,  135 

.  4,  199,  205 
.  3,  75,  90 
.  4,  266,  268 
.      4,  84,  93 


Dalton,  N.  N. 
Daly,  C.      . 
Danielli,  J.  F. 


Danowski,  T.  S. 
Darrow,  D.  C.     , 
Dasmann,  R.  F. 
Davey,  W.  P. 
Davidson,  F.  J. 
Davidson,  J.  N. 
Davies,  B.  M.  A. 


Davies,  C. 
Davies,  D. 


Davies,  H,  E. 
Davis,  D.  E. 
Davis,  G.  K. 
Davis,  J.  E. 
Davis,  J.  M. 
Davis,  J.  O. 
Davis,  W.  S. 
Davson,  H. 


Dawes,  G.  S. 
Dawson,  A.  B. 
Dean,  R.  F.  A 
Deane,  H.  W. 


Deason,  H.  J. 
De  Duve,  C. 
Decider,  C.  L. 
Deevey,  E.  S. 
Dejdar,  R. 
De  Jongh,  S.  E. 
De  la  Blaze,  F.  A 
Del  Castillo,  E.  B. 


.  2,  228,  232 
.  5,21,31 
.  3,  39,  43,  45,  46,  47, 
48,49,50,68,95,129,130, 
144,  148,  185;  5,  1,  15,  17, 
19,  32,  33,  34,  70,  103,  104, 
114,  133,  136,  137,  177, 
178,  209,  229,  230,  281, 
282,  298 
.  4,  289,  295 
4,  106,  113,  133,  134 
5,  92,  94,  103 
5,  282 
3,  107 
.  1,  191,  193 
1,  192,  193, 
209,  219 
.  4,  266,  268 
1,  220,  236;  3,  88, 
90;  5,  72 
.  4,  150,  266,  *268 
.  5,  95,  102 
.  2,  148,  159 
1,  206 
4,  102,  106,  108,  113 
.  4,  277,  295 
.  5,  192,  207 
4,  15,  20,  28,  29,  31, 
32,  33,  34,  35,  58,  59,  73, 
96,  99,  100,  101,  114,  162, 
206,  207,  208,  269,  309, 
312,  315,  316 
2,  104,  115,  125,  126 
.  2,  81,  83 
.  1,  198,  200 
2,  23,  26,  37,  53, 
60,66 
.  .5,  151,  176 
.  2,  136,  143 
.  5,  159,  175 
1, 10,  15;  5,  91,  102 
.  4,  291,  296 
.  2,  64,  66 
.  2,  86,  96 
1,  151,  160 


Delea,  C.       4,  89,  92,  292,  293,  294,  297 

Deming,  Q.  B.     .         .    4,  280,  293,  297 

Dempsey,  E.  W.  .        1,  103;  2,  41, 

54,  54,  57,  60,  66,  67,  68, 

100,   103,   104,   106,   107, 

109,   110,   111,   113,   114, 

129,   144,   146,    147,    186, 

191,   192,   198,  241,  243, 

246,  248,  250,  251,  253 

Dennis,  W.  H.     .         .         .4,  24,  31 

Dent,  C.  E.  .         .         .2,  112,  113 

Deringer,  M.  K.  .    3,  121,  123,  124 

Desaulles,  P.  A.  .  4,  59,  60,  76,  94, 

180,   181,   187,   188,   194, 

195,    196,    197,    198,   206, 

227,  228,  262,  316 

Detkens  .         .         .5,67,  69 

Dettmer      .         .         .         .3,  102,  103 

Devoid,  F.  ...  5,  149,  175 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Dewar,  M.  M. 
Deyrup,  I.  . 
Diamond,  I. 
Dice,  L.  R. 
Dickerson,  J.  W.  T 
Dickie,  L.  M. 
Dieckmann,  W.  J. 
Dietrich,  H. 
Dilley,  W.  E. 


Dinkhauser,  F. 
Dlouhd,  H. 
Dobriner,  K. 
Dobson,  E.  L. 
Dodson 
Doisy,  E.  A. 
Dole,  V.  P. 
Doljanski,  F. 
Donohue,  D. 
Donohue,  D.  M. 
Dorfman,  R.  I. 

Dornhorst,  A.  C. 
Dorst,  J. 
Douglass,  P.  M. 
Downing,  D.  F.  . 
Doyle,  A.  E. 
Dreizen,  S. 
Drescher,  A.  N. 
Droop,  M.  R. 
Drummond,  J.  K 
D'Silva,  J.  L. 

Dublin,  L.  I. 

Dubois,  A.  B. 
Dubois,  E. 
Duckworth,  J. 
Duesberg,  J. 
Duetz,  G.  H. 
Duncan,  L.  E 


Dunewitz,  A. 
Dunham,  L.  J. 
Dunn,  Th.  B. 
Dunning,  M.  F. 
Dupret,  L. 
Dyke,  H.  B.  van 
Dymond,  J.  R. 
Dyrenfurth,  I. 


4,  10,  11 
4,  24,  31 

4,  24,  31 
3,  10,  18 

4,  217,  219 

5,  151,  175 
2,  112,  113 

5,  60,  69 

1,  197,  200;  5,  169, 
176 

.      5,  63,  69 

4,  9,  10,  11,  165,  779 

.  1,  133,  137 

.  4,  289,  295 

3,  185 

2,  50,  53 

4,  67,  73,  77 

5,  208 
.  4,  46,  56 
.  2,  237,  238 
1, 126, 130, 134, 
137;  4,  90,  93,  187,  195 
.  2,  234,  238 
.  3,  27,  29 
.  1,  156,  161 
.  1,  198,  200 
.  4,  278,  295 
.  1,  198,  200 
.  4,  145,  149 
.  1,  187,  193 

3,  107 

2,  148,  148,  149, 
150,  158 

1,195,200,  203;  3,  85, 
90,  137,  142 
.  4,  264,  268 
.  5,  123,  132 

2,  184,  187;  4,  309 
2,  86,  93,  96 

.  5,  85 
1,  139;  3,  78,  90;  4, 
89,  92,  226,  280,  292,  293, 
294, 297 
.  1,  198,  200 
1,  151,  158,  160 

3,  121,  122,  124 
.  4,  46,  56 
.  2,  136,  143 
.      4,  92,  93 

5,  175 
4,  89,  93,  293,  294 


Eadie,  G.  S. 
Earle,  D.  P. 
Eberlein,  W.  R. 
Economou-Mavrou, 
Eddy,  M.    . 
Edelman,  I.  S. 

Edmonds,  S.  J,  . 
Edmunds,  T.  R.  . 
Eeg-Larson,  N.    . 

Eichelberger,  L.  . 


2,  234,  238 

4,  275,  295 

4,  80,  92 

4,  218,  219 

1,  160 

4,  36,  47,  56,  109, 

113,  280,  295 

.  5,  169,  175 

.  1,  196,  200 

.  1,  111,  114,  115, 

120,  122,  123 

.  4,  119,  135 


Eichna,  L.  W. 
Eidrigevits,  E.  V. 
Einstein,  A. 
Eisenberg,  S. 
Ek,  J. 

El-Deeb,  A.  L.  A. 
Eliasch,  H, 

Elkin,  R.     . 
Elkinton,  J.  R.     . 

Elliott,  R.  H. 
EUis,  R.  S. 
Elmadjian,  F. 

225, 


Elrick,  H.   . 
Elsdon-Dew,  R. 
Elvehjem,  C.  A. 

Ely,  R.  S.  . 
Emerson,  A.  E. 
Engel,  S.  L. 
Engeler,  W. 
Engfeldt,  B. 
Engle,  E.  T. 

Engstrom,  W.  W 
Enzmann,  E.  V. 
Epstein,  F.  H. 
Erickson,  D.  W. 
Ershoff,  B.  A. 
Escher,  D.  J.  W. 

Eschmeyer,  P.  H. 
Essen-Moller,  E. 
Etheridge,  J. 
Evan,  J.  V. 

Evans,  B.  M. 
Evans,  C.  A. 
Evans,  H.   . 
Evans,  H.  M. 
Evans,  W.  A. 
Evenius,  C. 
Everett,  N.  B. 
Evermann,  B.  W 
Ewing,  G. 

Fahmy 
Fahr,  H.  O. 
Failla,  A.    . 
Fainstat,  T.  D. 


Falbriard,  A. 
Falek,  A. 
Falk,  G.      . 

Falzone,  J.  A. 
Farber,  S.  J. 
Fargo,  W.  C. 
Earner,  D.  S. 
Farr,  W.     . 


4,5, 


.  4,  275,  295 

.      5,  53,  54 

.  2,  227,  231 

.  4,  283,  298 

4,  273,  279,  280,  283, 

295,  298 

.    5,  232,  234,  241 

4,  273,  279,  280,  283, 

295,  298 

.  5,  168,  176 

4,  38,  46,  56,  289, 

295,  297 

.  2,  120,  125 

.  5,  248,  264 

I,  219,  219,  223,  224, 
226,   227,   230,  232, 

233,  234,  236 

.  1,  199,  200 

3,  188 

1,  197,  200;  4,  133, 

136 

.  4,  105,  113 

.      1,  10,  14 

4,  92,  93,  257,  259 

.      5,  59,  69 

.  2,  162,  172 

1,  151,  160;  2,  60, 

66 

.      4,  42,  56 

2,  36,  37,  54 

4,  46,  57 

5,  196,  197,  208 

.  1,  197,  201 

4,  277,  280, 

295,  297 

.  5,  197,  207 

.  3,  134,  142 

.  4,  109,  113 

4,  201,  202,  203, 

205,  221 

4,  265,  268 

5,297 

5,  92,  102 

1,  29:  2,  41,  53 

4,  243,  246 

5,  232,  241 
2,  34,  53 

5,  212,  226 

4,  288,  296 

2,128 

5,  110,  112 
5,289 

2,  19,  26,  168, 

172 

4,  292,  293,  295 

3,  132,  139,  143 

II,  12,  13,  167,  169, 

172,  179 

.      3,  77,  90 

.  4,  275,  295 

.  1,  198,  201 

5,  97,  101,  102,  103 

.    5,9 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Farran,  G.  P.       .  .         .5,  194,  207 

Farrar,  C.  L.        .  .    5,  222,  234,  241 

Fawcett,  D.  W.    .      2,  86,  87,  96,  97,  98, 

99,  103,  104, 109,  114,  129, 

144, 177,  183,  250 

Fazekas      ....  3,  147 

Feaster,  J.  P.        .         .         .2,  148,  159 

Feingold,  L.         .     1,  42,  48;  3,  137,  143 

Feingold-Jarvik   ...  3,  139 

Fejfar,  Z.    .     4,  32,  33,   115,  163,  248, 

261,   271,   271,   272,  273, 

279,   281,   282,   284,   286, 

291,   295,   296,   298,   299, 

300,  317 

Fejfarovd,  M.      .    4,  281,  284,  286,  291, 

295,  296 
Fekete,  E.  .     1,  146,  150,  156,  160 

Feldman-Muhsam,  B.  .         .5,  259,  264 


Fell,  H.  B, 
Fencl,  V.    . 
Femgold,  L. 
Fielding,  U. 
Figuerson,  W.  G. 
Filer,  L.  J. 
Finch,  C.  A. 

Finck,  M.  A.  von 
Findlay,  G.  H. 
Finkel,  M.  P. 
Finnell,  J. 
Firket,  H. 
Fischer,  A. 
Fischer-Piette,  E. 
Fisher,  A.  J. 
Fiske,  C.  H. 
Fister,  H.  J. 
Fitch,  H.  S. 
Fitch,  J.  E. 
Fitzgerald,  M.  G 
Flade,  J.  E. 
Fletcher,  H.  M. 
Flexner,  L.  B 


Flink,  E.  B. 
Fhnt,  M.  H. 
Flipse,  R.  J. 
Flower,  S.  . 

Floyer,  M.  A. 
Fliickiger,  E. 
Foa,  C. 
Foldi,  M.    . 
Foerster,  R.  E. 
Fogg,  G.  E. 
Folin 

Folkes,  B.  F. 
Folkes,  J.  P. 
FoUis,  R.  H. 
Forbes,  G.  B. 

Forbes,  R.  M. 
Forster,  R.  E. 
Forwell,  G.  D. 


2,  4,  12 

4,  278,  289,  295 

.  3,  173,  182 

.      2,  50,  53 

.  4,  200,  205 

.  4,  133,  134 

2,  228,  229,  231,  232, 

237,  238 

4,  211,  212,  219 

3,  102 

.  2,  148,  158 

.  5,  168,  176 

.      2,  62,  66 

.  3,  32 

.      3,  25,  29 

.  4,  98 

.  4,  120,  135 

.  4,  120,  135 

5,  96,  97,  102 

.  5,  755,  775 

4,  301,  308,  309 
.  5,  66,  69 
.      4,  63,  73 

2,  109,  111,  113, 

118,  125,  148,  158,  159; 

4,  26,  31 

.    4,  301,  308,  309 

.  3,  70 

.  4,  200,  205 

5,  77P,  128,  132,  168,  175, 

217,  225 

.  4,  258,  259 

.      3,  64,  68 

1,  757,  755,  160 

4,  45,  57,  284,  286,  296 

5,  757,  755,  775 
.  1,  186,  193 

1,  184 

.  2,  208,  209 

1,  757,  192,  193 

4,  257,  259 

4,  709,  775,  727, 

755,  143,  149 

4,  720,  727,  755 

.  4,  264,  268 

.      4,  65,  73 


Foulds,  G.  ...  3,  750,  168 

Fountaine,  M.  E.  .         .      3,  25,  29 

Fouracre  Barns,  H.  H.  .  2,  774 

Fourraan,  P.  .  4,  36,  44,  57,  59,  60, 
95,  97,  115,  138,  151,  163, 
164,  196,  795,  197,  206, 
221,  224,  225,  262,  263, 
507,    308,    309,    309,    575 


Fowell,  D.  M 
Foy,  H.       . 
Fox,  H.       . 
Frahck,  R.  L.      . 
Franca,  P.  da 
Franke,  F. 
Frankland,  H.  M 
FrankUn,  K.  J 


Eraser,  D.  . 
Eraser,  E.  A. 
Eraser,  F,  C. 
Fraser,  R. 
Franz,  V.    . 
Frederic 
Freeman,  H. 


236 
Freudenberg,  F.  . 
Freudenstein,  H. . 
Freudenstein,  K. 
Freydberg,  V. 
Friedemann,  T.  E 
Friedman,  M. 
Friedman,  R. 
Friedman,  S.  M. 


4,  275,  277,  295 
3,  189 
1,  88;  5,  705,  709,  772 
2,  5,  70,  75 
.  5,  755,  775 
.  5,  62,  69 
T.  .  .5,  53,  54 
1,  31,  65,  76,  77,  79, 
202,  205,  214,  242,  245; 
3,  51,  52,  59,  71,  72,  126 
2,  174 
.  5,  759,  207 
2,  19,  26,  168,  172 
.  4,  507,  308 
.  5,  202,  207 
2,  103 
1,  36,  50,  51,  52, 
137,  138,  139,  219,  279, 
223,  224,  225,  226,  227, 
230,  232,  233,  234,  236, 
237,  238;  4,  90,  93 
.  5,  62,  69 
5,  235,  236,  237,  241 
5,  232,  241 
3,57 


Friis-Hansen,  B.  J. 


Friley,  M. 
Froesch,  E.  R 
Frontali,  G. 
Frost,  W.  E. 
Funaro,  R. 
Fry,  D.  H. 
Fry,  F.  E.  J. 


2,  750,  143 

3,  755,  142 
3,  75,  90 

3,  45,  128,  146, 
184,  192 
4,  102,  705, 
705,  709,  775,  114,   143, 
149 
2,  757,  772;  3,  77,  75 
.  4,  280,  298 
.  1,  799,  200 
.  5,  755,  775 
.  1,  799,  200 
5,  755,  770,  775 
.  5,  792,  207 


Gaarenstroom,  J 
Gabrio,  B.  W. 


Gakkel,  L.  B. 
Gale,  E.  F. 
Galvan,  R.  R. 
Gamble,  J.  L. 
Gans,  B.     . 
Gardiner,  E 
Gardner,  E. 
Gardner,  G. 
Gardner,  W.  U 
Garrod,  O. 
Gatenby,  J.  B. 
Gauer,  O.  H. 


4, 


M. 


H.  .    .2,  64,  66 

2,  228,  229,  231,  232, 

237,  238 

.  3,  755,  142 

1,  757,  792,  795 

.  1,  795,  200 

138,  144,  149,  216,  219 

4,  79,  93,  97,  98 

.   5,  27,  57 

.  5,  248,  264 

.   3,  70,  75 

1,  755,  154,  160,  161 

4,  292,  293,  296 

.      2,  55,  96 

4,  255,  284,  296,  297 


8 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Gaunt,  R. 
Geiser,  S.  W. 
Gele,  P.      . 

Gellerstedt,  N. 
Gellhorn,  A. 

Genest,  J.  . 
Geoghegan,  H 
G6rard,  P. 
Gerking,  S.  D. 


Gemer,  K. 
Gerritsen,  T. 
Gianferrari,  L. 
Giardini,  A. 
Gibson,  J.  G. 
Gibson,  J.  R. 
Gilbert,  C. 
Gilbert,  C.  S. 
Gilbert,  J.  C. 
Gilligan,  D.  R 
Gilman,  A. 
Gillman,  J 


Gillman,  T. 


Ginsburg,  J. 
Ginsburg,  M. 
Girard,  A. 
Giroud,  A. 
Giroud,  C.  J. 
Glass,  B.     . 
Godden,  W. 
Godet,  R. 
Godwin,  H. 
Gomori,  P. 
Gofman 
Goldblatt,  H. 
Golden,  J.  B. 
Goldfarb,  N. 
Goldhamer,  H 
Goldner,  F. 
Goldring,  W. 
Goldstein,  K. 
G6mez,  F. 
Gompertz,  B. 

Gonse,  P.   . 
Gontarski,  H. 
Goodbody,  M, 
Goodman,  L. 
Gordon 
Gordon,  E.  B. 
Gordon,  E.  E. 
Gordon,  E.  S. 
Gordon,  G.  L. 

Gordon,  J. 


4,  193,  194,  195,  296 

.    5,  167,  175,  177 

.    4,  273,  278,  296 

.  1,  35,  48;  3,  147 

2,  111,  113,  118,  125, 

148,  158 

.  4,  277,  296 

4,  24,  31,  35 

2,  39,  40,  42,  53,  56 

5,  18,  79,  171,  175, 

179,  180,  181,  183,  207, 

209,210,211,228,229, 

230,  265,  284,  289 

.      5,  63,  69 

.  3,  110,  114 

.  3,  137,  142 

.  4,  75 

.  4,  243,  246 

.      5,  29,  31 

3,  126 
5,  245 

.  3,  160,  168 
.  1,90,  103 
.  4,  37,  57 
3,  20,  26,  104,  105, 
106,  107,  108,  109,  114, 
126,  189 
.  3,  33,  36,  48,  49,  70, 
70,  72,  96,  100,  104,  104, 
105,  106,  107,  108,  109, 
114,  126,  145,  146,  188, 
189,  190 
.  2,  148,  158 
.  4,  46,  57 
.  1,  129,  137 
.  2,  765,  172 
.  4,  293,  294 
.  3,  136,  142 

4,  309 
.  2,  16,  17 
.  2,  206,  209 

4,  284,  286,  296 

1,89 

4,  253,  258,  259 

1,  143,  153,  160 
.  3,  173,  182 
.      1,  43,  48 

4,  38,  40.  43,  44,  57 
.  4,  230,  246 
.  3,  180,  182 
.  1,  198,  200 

5,  2,  15,  72,  79,  302, 
311 
.  2,  21,  26 
.  5,  232,  241 
.  4,  245,  246 
.  1,  151,  160 
1,  138 
.      4,  49,  56 

2,  142,  143,  144 
.  4,  292,  296 

4,  38,  40,  43,  44, 

57 

4,  226,  257,  259 


Gordon,  J.  E. 
Gottlieb,  E. 
Gotzche,  H. 
Gould,  R.  G. 
Gowenlock,  A.  H. 
Graber 
Graham,  I. 
Grainger,  E.  H.  . 
Gram,  M.  R. 
Granick,  S. 
Gray,  M.  J. 
Greaves,  M.  S.    . 
Green,  C.  V. 
Green,  H.  H. 
Green,  R.  G. 
Green,  S.  H. 
Greenberg,  D.  M. 

Greene,  R. 
Greene,  R.  R. 
Greenfield,  A.  D.  M. 
Greenwood,  M. 
Greep,  R.  O. 
Gregory,  F.  G. 
Gresson,  R.  A.  R. 
Gribetz,  D, 
Griew,  S. 
Griffin,  G.  E. 
Griffith,  L. 
Griffiths,  J.  T.      . 
Groot,  A.  P.  de 


Grosch,  D.  S. 
Gross,  F. 
Gross,  J.     . 
Grossman,  M.  I. 
Grossman,  R. 
Grosz,  S.    . 
Gruber,  G.  B. 
Griineberg,  H. 


Grueninger,  R.  M 
Gruenwald,  P. 
Guild,  W.  R. 
Guilford,  J.  P. 
Gumbel,  E.  J. 
Gunter,  G. 
Guzmdn,  M.  A. 

Haddow,  A. 
Hagel,  L. 
Hagerman,  D.  D 
Hahn,  P.     . 
Hahn,  P.  F. 
Hafter,  E. 

Haigh,  L.  D. 
Hain,  A.  M. 
Halberstaedter,  L 
Hald,  A.     . 
Haldane,  J.  B.  S 

Haldane,  J.  S. 
Haley,  H.  B. 


.  5,  21,  31 
.  5,  149,  175 
.  A,  41,  56 
.  2,  217,  232 
4,  60,  193,  195 

4,  151 
.    3,  98,  100 

5,  147,  153,  175 
.  1,  122,  123 
2,  238 
.  4,  89,  93 
.  4,  265,  268 
3,  9,  18 
.  4,  200,  205 

5,  297 
2,  32,  33,  53 

4,  301,  307, 
308,  309 
.      4,  84,  93 
2,  3,  12,  17 
2,  120,  121,  125 
5,  311 
.      2,  63,  66 
2,  204,  207,  209 
.      2,  86,  96 
.  4,  143,  149 
.  3,  166,  169 
.  4,  109,  113 
.  4,  109,  113 
.  5,  277,  281 
5,  235,  236,  237, 
238,  241 
5,  255,  264 
1,242 
.  3,48 
1,191,  193 
4,  280,  295 
2,  179,  183 
2,  181,  183 
238;  3,  118,  124;  5, 
19,  20,  79,  80,  283 
.  1,  198,  201 
.      2,  16,  17 
4,  45,  56,  57 
.      1,  40,  48 
5,117,  132,  134 
.  5,  167,  175 
.  1,  198,  200 


.  3,  68 

.      5,  59,  69 

.  2,  142,  144 

.  4,  165,  179 

.  2,  236,  238 

5,  196,  208,  218,  225, 

226 

.  4,  131,  135 

.  2,  162,  172 

.      2,  46,  53 

.  5,  120,  133 

1,10,  15,  238;  3,  5, 

12,  18;  5,53,  54 

.      4,  63,  73 

.  4,  109,  113 


h 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


9 


Hall,  D.  A.         .         .         .        1,  104: 

3,  65,  66,  67,  96,  97, 
98,  100,  102,  103;  5,  113 

.  2,  120,  125 

.  4,  256,  259 

4,  274,  277,  297 

4,  201,  202,  203,  205 
4,  279,  296 

4,262 


Hall,  F.  G 
Hall,  K.      . 
Hall,  P.  E. 
Hallman,  N. 
Halperin,  M.  H. 
Hamburger,  J. 
Hamilton,  B, 
Hamilton,  H.  B. 
Hamilton,  J.  A. 
Hamilton,  J.  B. 
Hamilton,  T.  S. 
Hamilton,  W.  F, 
Hamilton,  W.  J. 


Hamlett,  G.  W.  D. 
Hammond,  J. 
Hancock,  W. 
Hanley,  T.  . 
Hanon,  F.  . 
Hansen,  E. 
Hansen,  J.  D.  L. 
Hansen,  W.  H. 
Hanyu,  I.    . 
Harmison,  C.  R. 
Harris,  B.  A. 
Harris,  E.  J. 
Harris,  G.  W. 
Harris,  H.  . 
Harrison,  J.  H. 
Harrison,  J.  L. 
Harrison,  M.  F. 
Harrison,  R,  J. 


Harrison,  T.  R. 

Hartman,  C.  G 
Hartmann,  W. 
Harvey,  G.  F. 
Hartwig,  W. 

Hart,  J.  L. 

Hart,  J.  S.  . 
Hass,  G.  M. 

Hastings 
Hastings,  A.  B 


Hatai,  S.     . 
Hatcher,  J.  D.     , 
Hatey,  J.     . 
Hathom,  M. 
Hatton,  H. 
Haugen,  G.  E.     . 
Havighurst,  R.  J. 
Hawkins,  D.  F.  , 
Haydak,  M.  H.   , 

Hayman,  J.  M.    , 


4,  10,  11 

1,  227,  236 

4,  242,  246 

1,  227,  236 

4,  120,  121,  131,  135 

4,  275,  277,  295 

2,  114,  115, 

116,    117,   755,   156,   158, 

159,  160 

2,  38,  40,  53,  56 

.  1,  189,  193 

.      4,  63,  73 

4,  269 

.  4,  217,  219 

.      5,  59,  69 

.  4,  133,  135 

.  3,  123,  124 

.  5,  757,  775 

.      5,  73,  79 

2,  729,  132,  143 

.      4,  20,  31 

.  1,  752,  760 

4,  202,  203,  205 

.      4,  45,  57 

5,  95,  96,  102 

.  1,  7P7,  193 

2,  37,  54,  68, 

81,  83,  83,  84,  85,   115,   148, 

148,   149,   150,   155,   156, 

158,  159,  160,  199 

4,  283,  287,  288,  296, 

298 

.  1,  146,  160 

.      5,  59,  69 

3,  9,  18 

.     5,  56,  57,  65,  66,  67, 

69,  70,  71 

.    5,  144,  151,  155,  175, 

176 

.  5,  770,  775 

.    1,  88,  103 

1,207 

.    .       2,  141,  143,  144;  3, 

792;  4,  779,  720,  7i7,  135 


5,  248,  264 

.  4,  279,  296 

.      2,  24,  27 

3,  70^,  705,  77^ 

.      1,  70,  75 

.  2,  7iO,  143 

.      1,  43,  48 

.      4,  90,  93 

5,  232,  236,  237,  238, 

239,  241,  242,  253,  264 

.    A,  271,  288,  297 


Heady,  J.  A. 
Hebb,  D.  O. 
Hecht,  H.  H. 
Hechter,  O. 
Hediger,  H. 
Hegsted,  D.  M. 
Heidenhain,  R. 
Heilbrunn,  L.  V. 
Heim,  A.  W. 
Heinbecker,  P. 
Hejtmanek,  J. 
Helikson,  J. 
Heller,  H. 


.      5,  27,  31 

.  3,  77^,  182 

.  4,  266,  268 

1,139;  4,  262 

.  2,  777,  183 

1,  709,  727,  722 

.      4,  63,  73 

.  1,55 

.  3,  163,  168 

4,  256,  257,  259 

5,  235,  237,  242 
.  1,  198,  201 

4,  6,  7,  11,  12,  72, 

13,   13,  97,  98,   114,   137, 

163,   755,   757,   755,   759, 

178,  179,  195,  196,  247,  575 

Hellman,  L.  M.       2,  777,  772,  775,  729, 

752,  144;  4,  82,  85,  93,  251, 

259 


Hems,  J. 
Henly,  A.  A. 
Henry,  J.  P. 


Henschel,  A. 
Herald,  E.  S. 
Herbeuval,  R. 
Herbst,  E.  J. 
Herkel,  W. 
Herlant,  M. 
Herlitzka,  A. 
Herrington,  W.  C. 
Hers,  H.  G. 
Herscheimer,  A 
Hertig,  A.  T. 
Hertz,  R. 
Hervey,  G.  F, 
Hervey,  G.  R. 
Hess,  A. 
Hess,  G. 
Hess,  J.  H. 
Hesselberg,  C. 
Heston,  W.  E. 
Heusler,  K. 
Hevesy,  G. . 
Hickey,  J.  J. 
Hickling,  C.  F. 


.     3,  9,  18;  5,  227 

.  1,  750,  757 

4,  38,  57,  283,  284, 

296,  297 

4,  757,  757,  752 

.  5,  755,  775 

.  4,  242,  246 

.  1,  797,  200 

.  2,  757,  158 

2,  39,  40,  42, 53 

.      2,  33,  53 

.  5,  757,  777 

.  2,  755,  7^5 

4,  750 

1,745,  161;  2,  110,  113 

1,  143,  151,  160 

5,227 

4,  40,  57 


Higgins,  G. 
Higginson,  J. 
Hilden,  T.  . 
Hile,  R.      . 
Hill, 

Hill,  A.  V. 
Hill,  S. 
Hillarp,  N.  A 
Hiller,  A.    . 
Hillman,  D. 
Hilton,  J.  G. 
Himbert,  J. 
Himwich     . 
Hines,  B.  E. 
Hingerty,  D.  . 


3,745 

5,  235,  242 

4,  750,  755 

2,  80,  83,  84 

3,  775,  727,  124 
.  4,  181,  195 
.  2,  204,  209 

5,  97,  101,  102 

5,  795,  795, 

205,  207 

4,  36,  41,  43,  57 
.  3.  770,  114 
.  4,  41,  56 
.  5,  757,  775 

2,  727 
1,  77,  76,  77,  78 
.  2,  757,  158 
.      2,  60,  66 
.  4,  272,  298 
4,  740,  747,  745,  749 
4,  245,  246 
4,  273,  278,  279,  296 
.  1,57 
.  4,  274,  279 
4,  35,  99,  99, 
101,    113,   207,   207,  226, 
226,  247,  310,  570 


10 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Hinshelwood,  C. 
Hinton,  H.  E. 


Hinton,  S.  . 
Hippocrates 
Hisaw,  F.  L. 

Hitchcock,  M.  W 
Hoagland,  H 


Hobbs,  G.  E. 
Hobson,  W. 
Hodge,  C.  F. 
Hoelzel,  F. 
Hoffman,  F.  G 
Hoffman,  H. 
Hoffman,  J. 
Hoffman,  K. 
Hogreve,  F. 
Holland,  B.  C. 
Hollander 
Hollander,  W. 
Holliday,  M.  A 
Holmes,  E.  G. 
Holmes,  S.  J. 
Holmgren,  A. 
Holt,  S.  J.  . 


Honmia,  H. 
Honsova,  H. 
Hoogendoom,  D. 
Hooker,  C.  W.    . 
Hope,  J.  M. 


3,  5,  18 

5,  34,  112,  243,  245, 

246,  284 

5,  191,  217,  219,  225 

2,  129 

1,  143,  151,  160', 

2,  81,  83 

S.    .  .2,  122,  125 

.    Iy219,223,224,225, 

226,  227,  230,  232,  233, 
234,  236 

1,  223,  232,  236 
.    3,  94,  100 
.  5,  248,  264 
1,  197,  199,  206 
.      2,  39,  54 
.  5,  237,  243 
.  1,  153,  160 
.  1,  153,  161 
5,  64,  65,  69 
.      4,  51,  57 
2,  251 
.  4,  279,  296 
.  4,  129,  135 
.  4,  130,  135 
.      5,  21,  31 
4,  279,  280,  295 
5,  86,  134,  142,  149,  151, 
155,   158,   167,   175,   176, 
178,    179,   209,   210,   226, 

227,  228,  229,  246,  290 
.     2,  188,  191,  192,  199 

.  4,  291,  296 

.      5,  21,  31 

.      2,  91,  96 

1,  219,  223,  224,  225, 


226,  227,  232,  233,  236 
Hopwood,  H.  H.  .         .  1,  198,  201 


Horn,  G. 
Howell,  T.  H. 

Hoy,  P.  A. 
Huant,  E.   . 
Hubble,  D. 
Hubbs,  C.  L. 
Hudson,  P. 
Huggett,  A.  St.  G. 


Hughes,  A. 
Hughes,  J.  S. 
Hughes  Jones,  N. 

Hugin,  F.    . 
Hull,  T.  Z. 
Hultquist,  G.  T. 
Hunmiel,  K.  P.    . 
Humphries,  E.  M 
Hungerland,  H. 
Hunt,  J.  N. 


2,  189,  199 
.  1,18,23; 
3,  93,  100 
4,  3,  4,  5,  6,  9, 11 
.  2,  167,  172 
.  4,  78,  93 
5,  146,  155,  176 
.  1,  136,  137 
2,  27,  29,  56, 
58,  84,  116,  117,  118,  119, 
120,  121,  123,  125,  126, 
127,  144,  145,  146,  159, 
160,  174,  175,  186,  200, 
201,  210,  214,  240,  242, 
243,  246,  247,  251,  252 
2,  251 
.  4,  200,  205 
2,  234,  235, 
238;  4,  265,  268 
3,  63,  64,  67 
.  4,  131,  135 
.  2,  162,  172 
.  1,  156,  160 
2,  34,  51,  53 
.  4,  212,  219 
.  4,  210,  219 


Hunter,  W.  R. 
Hurley,  T.  H. 
Hurst,  H.    . 
Hurst,  J.  G. 
Huseby,  R.  A. 
Huxley,  J. 
Hyett,  A.  R. 
Hynes,  N.  H.  B. 
Hytten,  F.  E. 

Ickowicz,  M. 
Ikkos,  D.    . 
Ingle,  D.  J. 
Ingle,  L. 
Ingram,  D.  L. 
Innes,  L.  R. 
Inukai,  T. 
lob,  L.  V. 
Irving,  J.  T. 
Irwin,  J.  O. 
Iseri,  L,  T. 
Iske,  B. 
Isupov,  A.  P. 
Ito,  T. 
Itoh,  S. 
Ivlev,  V.  S. 
Ivy,  A.  C. 
Iwashige,  K. 
Izzo,  M.  J. 

Jackson,  B.  H. 
Jackson,  H. 
Jackson,  L. 
Jackson,  W.  P.  U 
Jacob,  H.    . 
Jacob,  M. 
Jacobsohn,  D. 
Jacquot,  R. 
Jaczewski,  Z. 
Jaeger,  L.  M. 
Jalavisto,  Eeva 


James,  A.  H. 
James,  D,  W. 
James,  W.  . 
James,  W.  O. 
Jelinek,  J.  . 
Jenkins,  R. 
Jennings 
Jenson,  R.  L. 
Jepson,  R.  P. 
Jeter,  M.  A. 
Jewell,  P.  A. 
Johansson,  A.  S. 
John,  M.     . 
Johnsen,  S.  G. 
Johnson,  B.  B. 

Johnson,  L.  C. 
Johnston,  M.  E. 
Jolliffe,  N. 
Jolly,  W.  A. 
Jones,  A.  R. 


5,  179 
.  2,  235,  238 
.  3,  10,  18 
.  1,  155,  161 
.  1,  755,  160 

5,  104 

.  2,  755,  759 

5,  755,  756,  775 

.  5,  33 

.  2,  46,  53 
.  4,  709,  775 

4,  257,  259,  260 
1,  797,  200;  3,  24,  29 

2,  35,  44,  53 
.  2,  755,  772 
.  5,  284,  264 
.  4,  750,  755 
.  1,  770,  725 
.  5,  37,  54 
.  4,  289,  296 
.  1,  775,  185 
.  5,  53,  54 
.  2,  189,  199 
.      4,  24,  32 

5,  186,  207,  208 
.   2,  12 

2,  759,  797,  799 
.  2,  236,  239 

.   5,  89 

.  1,  34 

.  1,  722,  725 

.  2,  757,  772 

.  3,  75^,  142 

.  4,  252,  259 

.  1,  752,  750 

.   2,  24,  27 

2,  181,  182,  183 

.      3,  28,  29 

3,  757,  142;  5,  16, 

17,21,27,57,31,32, 

33,  81,  113,266 

4,  709,  775 
.  3,  70 
.  1,43 

2,  205,  209 
4,  165 

5,  168,  176 
2,252 
4,222 
4,  750 

2,  148,  159 
4,  37,  57 
5,243 
5,  237,  242 
1,  69,  75,  76 
4,  187,  195,  280,  293, 
294,  297 
1,  145,  161 
4,  289,  298 
1,  799,  200 
2,  33,  54 
2,225 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


11 


Jones,  E.  C. 
Jones,  E.  R. 
Jones,  H.  E. 

Jones,  H,  S. 
Jones,  J.  W. 
Jones,  R.  V.  H. 
Jordan,  D.  S. 
Josephson,  B. 
Josimovich,  J.  I 
Jost,  A. 


Judson,  W. 
June,  F.  C. 


.  5,  277,  281 

.  4,  130,  135 

3,  160,  168,  171,  182, 

186 

.      4,  64,  73 

5,  153,  156,  176 

.      4,  54,  57 

.  5,  212,  226 

4,  279,  280,  295 

.      2,  23,  26 

2,  3,  11,  12,  13,  13,  14,  15, 

16,  17,  18,  18,  19,  20,  21, 

23,  24,  26,  27,  27,  28,  29, 

30,  84,  116,  126,  127,  146, 

167,   172,    173,    174,    175, 

252 

.  4,  279,  296 

.  5,  193,  208 


Kadid,  M.  . 
Kagan,  B.  M. 
Kahlenberg,  O.  J. 
Kalabouchov,  N.  I. 
Kallas,  H.  . 
Kallmann,  F.  J. 


.   5,  43,  54 
.  4,  130,  135 
.  4,  127,  135 
.    5,  96,  102 
.  1,  144,  160 
1,42,  48;  3,  37,101, 
131,  132,    133,   137,   139, 
142,   143,    143,  144,  145, 
146,  147,  148,  173,   182 

Kalter,  H 2,  168,  172 

Kaplan,  S.  A.      .  .         .4,  287,  296 

Karnovsky,  M.  L.         .  .  2,  141,  143 

Karvonen,  M.  J.  4,  74,  76,  199,  201, 

202,  203,  205,  206,  207, 

208,  221,  316 


Karzinkin,  G.  S. 
Kassila,  E. 
Katersky,  E.  M. 
Kattus,  A. 
Katz,  J. 
Katz,  M.     . 
Kaufman,  N. 
Kauzman,  E.  F.  . 
Kay,  H.      .  1,  213;  3, 
Keech,  M.  K.      . 
Kelley,  R.  B. 
Kellner,  G. 
Kellogg,  V.  L.     . 
Kennedy,  G.  C. 


5,  186,  208 

.  4,  74 

.    1,90,  103 

.  4,  277,  296 

4,  257,  259,  261 

5,  196,  197,  208 
3,  189 

.  2,  228,  231 

162,  169,  182,  183 

3,  97,  100,  103 

.      3,  10,  18 

2,  36,  42,  43,  53 

1,  196,  200 


1,  189,  193; 

4,  136,  136,  137,  150,  164, 

197,   226,   227,   250,   251, 

253,  254,  257,  259,   260, 

261,  262,  263,   313,  314, 

317 

Kennedy,  W.  A.  .    5,  145,  151,  176 

Kerpel-Fronius,  E.       .        4,  39,  57,  96, 

105,  113,  154,  155,  157, 

161,  162 

Kerr,  S.  E.  .  .  .4, 202,  205 

Kerrigan,  G.  A.  .    4,  139,  140,  141,  146, 

147,  149 

Kershaw,  W.  E.  .  5,  56,  56,  70,  105, 

114,  245,  267,285 

Keston,  A.  S.       .         .         .2,  206,  209 

Keszler,  H.  .         .         .4, 291,  296 


Kety,  S.  S.  .         3,  147;  4,  286,  296 

Keys,  A.     .     1,  89,  207;  3,  75,  91;  4, 

157,  161,  162 

.  2,  206,  209 

4,  63,  65,  68,  73 

.  4,  266,  268 

4,  253,  257,  259 

.  5,  10 

.      3,  20,  29 

.  5,  56 

4,  202,  203,  205 

3,  189 

.  I,  227,  236 

,  69,  75, 76, 76, 77,  78, 79 

4,  63,  73 

2,  197,  198,  199,  200 

3,  139 

.      3,  21,  29 

3,  189 

.      4,  46,  57 

2,  127 

.  1,  154,  160 

.  4,  133,  135 

,  33,  287,  288,  295 

3,  134,  136,  143 

4,  133,  135 

5,  99,  102 


Kidd,  F.     . 
Killmann,  S-A. 
Kilpatrick,  J.  A, 
Kimball,  G.  C. 
King,  G.     . 
King,  H.  D. 
King,  J.  O.  L. 
King,  J.  W.  B. 
Kinney,  T.  D. 
Kirk,  E.      . 
Kirk,  J.  E.     1, 69 
Kittsteiner,  C. 
Klaar,  J. 
Klaber 

Klauber,  L.  M. 
Klavins,  J. 
Kleeman,  C,  R. 
Klein 

Klein,  M.   . 
Kleinman,  A. 
Kleinzeller,  A. 
Klopfer,  H.  W. 
Klug,  H.  L. 
Kluijver,  H.  N. 
Knobil,  E. 
Knowles,  H.  C. 
Knox,  G.    . 
Koch,  A.    . 
Kocher,  v.. 
Koczorek,  K.  R 


Koehler,  A. 
Kohler,  M. 
Konig,  K.  . 
Koltay,  E.  . 
Kondi,  A.  . 
Konopinski 
Koonig,  T. 
Kopec,  S.    . 
Korenchevsky,  V 
Kosterlitz,  N. 
Kovach,  A. 
Kovach,  A.  G.  B 
Kovach,  I. 
Kowalewski,  K. 
Kracke,  R.  R. 
Kratky,  E. 
Krause,  C. 
Kfecek,  J. 


Kfeckova,  J. 
Krogh,  A.  . 
Krohn,  P.  L. 


Kruesi,  O.  R. 
Krumholz,  L.  A. 


2,  166,  172 

4,  36,  57 

5,  21,  31 
5,  236,  238,  242 

.  5,  237,  242 

4,  280,  292, 

293,  298 

.  5,  232,  242 

.  4,  292,  298 

5,  61,  62,  69 

4,  284,  286,  296 

3,  189 

.      5,  67,  69 

.  4,  301,  308 

1,  197,  200;  3,  25,  29 

.  4,  256,  259 

.  2,  166,  172 

4,  284,  286,  296 
4,  45,  57,  284,  286,  296 

.  4,  157,  161 
.  1,  227,  236 
.2,  112,  113 

5,  232,  236,  242 
.  5,  110,  112 

4,9,  10,  11,  113, 

165,    179,    195,    196,    316 

.     4,  9  10,  11,  165,  179 

1,  77;  4,  75 

.      1,  30,  139,  141,  156, 

160,   162,    111,   203,   242, 

245;2,  57,  55,  67,  84,  211, 

239,   240,   243,   249,   250, 

252;  3,  123,  124,  125,  126, 

127;  5,271,281 

4,  245,  246 


5,  171,  176, 
187,  188,  208 


12 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Kruse,  H.  D. 
Kuechle,  M.  E. 
Kuiken,  A. 
Kuhn,  W.  . 
Kun,  K.      . 
Kuno,  Y.    . 
Kurzrok,  R. 
Kuznetsova,  G. 

Lacassagne,  A. 
Lack,  D.     . 

Ladman,  A.  J. 
Lambdin,  E. 
Lament,  N. 
Lanciano,  G. 
Landau,  B.  R. 
Landowne,  M. 


4,  301,  308 
1,  198,  201 

5,  237,  243 
5,  240,  242 
4,  161,  162 

4,  64,  73 
1,  145,  160 

5,  53,  54 


2,  34,  46,  53 

1,  4,  15;  5,  97,  101,  102, 

104 

2,  23,  26 

4,  46,  57 

3,  105,  188 

1,  199,  200 

2,  141,  143 
1,  66;  3,  33,  35,  36, 

46,  47,  69,  69,  73,  75,  80, 

84,88,89,90,91,94,  129; 

4,  235,  236,  246 

Lane,  C.  E.         .    1,  152,  160;  2,  63,  66 

Lane-Claypon,  J.  E.     .    2,  36,  37,  39, 53 


Lang,  C.  A. 
Langley,  J.  N. 
Langmaid,  C. 
Langstroth,  G.  O. 
Lansing,  A.  L 


3,  79,  91 

4,  63,  73 
.  4,  44 

4,  64,  65,  73 
1,  25,  26,  27,  30,  50, 


Laragh,  J.  H. 
Lardy,  H.  A. 
Larkin,  P.  A. 
Larsen,  C.  D. 
Larsen,  E.  E. 
Lasch,  W.   . 
Laser,  H.    . 
Laszio,  D.  . 
Laursen,  T.  J.  S. 
Lavoipierre,  M.  M.  J 
Law,  L.  W. 
Lawson,  H.  D. 
Lea,  E. 
Leaf,  A. 

Leavenworth,  C. 
Leblond,  C.  P. 
Leduc,  E.  H. 
Lee,  S.  van  der 
Leeson,  P.  M. 
Lehman,  H. 
Leitch,  L     . 
Leiter,  L.    . 
Lell,  W.  A. 
Le  Magnen,  J. 
Le  Marquand,  H 
Len^gre,  J. 
Lenhossek,  M. 
Leonard,  J.  A. 
Leppanen,  V. 
Lesher,  S.   . 


51,  55,  78,  88,  91,  99,  103, 
104,  105,  106,  107,  108, 
148,  160,  203,  206,  241, 
242,  243,  244;  2,  109,  113, 
247;  3,  97,  98,  100,  102;  5, 
259, 264 
4,  53,  57,  292,  293,  296 
.  2,  208,  209 
.  5,  182,  208 
.  3,  121,  124 
.  4,  95 
.  4,  97 
.  4,  218,  219 
1,  110,  122,  123 
.  1,69 
.  5,  245,  285 
.  1,  147,  160 
.  1,  143,  160 
5,  145,  149,  176 
4,  42,  46,  57,  145 
.  2,  203,  210 
.      2,  86,  96 

2,  251 
.  3,  117,  125 

.  4,  36 

1,  39,  40,  48, 51 

.  1,  196,  199 

277,  280,  295,  297 

.  2,  109,  113 

3,  129 
.  1,  197,  200 

4,  273,  278,  296 

.      2,  86,  96 

.  1,  211,  213 

4,  200,  202,  203,  205 

.  5,  80 


4, 


Leslie,  P.  H.         .  3,  10,  18;  5,  296 

Leutscher,  J.  A.  .         .         .  4, 294 

Lever,  J.  D.         .         .         .  2,  102 

Leverton,  R.  M.  .         .  1,  122,  123 

Levin,  M.  D.  .  .  5,  232,  236,  242 
Levine,  A.  D.       .  .  .  4, 228 

Levine,  J 4,  228 

Levinson,  Z.  H.  .  .    5,  255,  259,  264 

Lewin,  W.  .  .4,  36,  41,  43,  57 

Lewis  ....  2,  103 

Lewis,  A.  J.  .      1,  32,  49,  50,  51,  52, 

138,  214,  218,  238,  246; 

3,  143,  144,  146,  147,  185 
Lewis,  C.  S.  .  .  .4, 266, 268 
Lewis,  G.  T.  .  .  .4,  127,  134 
Lewis,  H.  B.        .         .         .1, 94,  103 

Lewis,  K 1,  33,  48 

Lewis,  W.  H.       .     4,  242,  244,  246,  248 

Li 2,174 

Li,  C.  H 1,  29 

Li,  M.  H 1, 154,  160 

Liber,  K.  E.  .         .2,  109,  113 

Liddle,  G.  W.      .        4,  89,  92,  280,  292, 

293,  294,  297 

Lieb,  C.  W.  .         .         .  4, 262 

Lieberman,  H.  M.        .  5, 250,  251, 

255, 256,  257, 258, 260, 

264 

Liebman,  A.        .         .         .4, 42,  56 


Liener,  I.  E. 
Light,  A.  E. 


Lightbody,  H.  D 
Lilly,  D.  M. 
Lindan,  O. 
Lindauer,  M. 
Linderstr0m-Lang,  K 
Lindgren,  A. 
Lindop,  Patricia 


J. 


Lippi,  B.     . 
Lipschutz,  A. 

Little,  C.  G. 
Liu,  C.  H.  . 
Ljunggren,  H. 
Llaurado,  J.  G 
Lloyd,  C.  W. 
Lobitz,  W.  C. 
Locke,  W.  . 
Lochhead,  J. 
Loeb,  J. 
Loeb,  L. 


Loeschcke,  H. 
Loeser,  H. 
Logan,  M.  A. 
Logethetopoulos,  J. 
Lombardo,  A.  J. 
Lombardo,  T.  A. 
Long,  C.  N.  H.   . 
Longley,  L.  P. 
Lorenz,  E. 


1,  192,  193 

4,  120,  ni!  128^  131, 

135 

.  4,  133,  135 

.  1,  187,  193 

.  2,  112,  113 

.  5,  233,  242 

.  2,  204,  209 

.  3,  134,  143 

3,  52,  59;  5, 

113,  136,  138,  141 

.  4,  242,  246 

1,  151,  160;  2,  34, 
53 

.  1,  156,  160 
.  1,  110,  122 
.  4,  109,  113 
.  4,  293,  297 
.  4,  193,  195 

2,  191,  192,  194,  199 
.      4,  64,  73 

2,  23,  27,  122,  125 

3,  24,  29 

1, 146, 157, 158,  160;  1,41, 

53,  54,  55,  69,  70,  73,  75, 

80,  83,  84,  161,  172 


2,  196,  197,  199 
2,  129,  143 
1,  91,  103 
4,  265,  268 
1,  198,  200 
4,  283,  298 
4,  82,  93 
4,  271,  288,  297 
.   5,  89 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


13 


Lorge,  I. 


Lotka,  A.  . 
Lotmar,  M. 
Loutit,  J.  F. 
Louveaux,  J 
Lovelock,  J.  E. 


Lowry,  O.  H. 

Luall,  A.     . 
Luck,  J.  M. 
Ludemann,  H. 
Luecke,  R.  W, 
Luetscher,  J,  A 
Luft,  R.      . 
Lundberg,  A. 
Lundeen,  E. 
Lutwak-Mann,  C 
Lutz,  R.      . 
Lwoff,  A.    . 


3,  32,  33,  36,  37,  72,  147, 
148,  170,  170,  182,  183, 
184,  185,  186,  189 
.  3,  137,  142 
5,  232,  233,  234,  242 
2,  221,  232,  236,  238 
.  5,  236,  242 
.  1,  163,  164,  165, 
168,  169;  2,  215,  217,  218, 
221,  224,  232,  240,  241, 
242,  243 
1,90,  103;  3,  192; 
4,  120,  131,  135 
.  1,  110,  123 
.  4,  132,  135 
.  4,  57,  56 
.  5,  237,  243 
4,  280,  292,  293,  297 
.  4,  109,  113 
.  4,  69,  70,  73,  76 
.  4,  130,  135 
.  4,  218,  219 
.  1,  122,  123 
.  3,  46 


MacArthur,  J.  W.  3,  24,  29;  4, 

141,  149 

McCall,  J.  T.       .  .2,  148,  159 

McCance,  R.  A.  .         1,  67,  76,  77, 

103,    186,   186,   193,   197, 

200,  203,  205,  207;  3,792; 

4,  1,  6,  11,  13,  38,  40,  41, 
44,  56,  57,  59,  76,  95,  96, 
96,  97,  100,  115,  120,  121, 
128,  136,  137,  138,  150, 
157,  160,  162,  162,  180, 
195,  197,  799,  203,  205, 
207,  208,  209,  277,  272, 
27^,  275,  277,  218,  219, 
222,  222,  223,  257,  259, 
262,   263,   269,  298,  305, 

306,308,  309,  310,  315 

MacCarty,  C.  S.  4,  41,  56 

Macaskill,  E.  H.  .  .  4,  200,  205 

Macaulay,  D.      .  .  .4,  46,  57 

McCay,  C.  M.     .  1,  27,  29,  30,  722, 

72J,    124,    125,    173,   174, 

181,   185,   188,   189,   193, 

194, 197, 200, 204, 206, 245 ; 

2,  248;  3,  8,   18,  25,  29; 

5,  759,  775,  257,  252,  253, 
264,  265,  268 


McCoIlum,  E.  V. 
McCormack,  J.  L 
McCracken,  F.  D. 
McCrory,  W.  W. 
MacDonald,  A.  M. 

McDonald,  R.  K. 


4,22^ 

4,  24,  31,  35 

.  5,  757,  775 

.      4,  46,  57 

4,  301,  307, 

308,  309 

1,  220,  236;  4, 

2J7,  233,  235,  237,  246, 

247 

McFadyean,  J.    .         .         -5,  50,  54 

McFarland,  R.  A.         .         .3,  133,  143 

McGaughey,  R.  S.        .         .4,  289,  296 


McGee,  L.  E. 
McGregor,  E.  A. 
McGregor,  J.  H. 
McHugh,  T. 
McIIroy,  M.  B. 
Mcintosh,  B.  J. 
Maclntyre,  L 
McKeown,  T. 
Mackintosh,  J. 
Maclaren,  A. 
MacMahon,  L. 
McMaster,  P.  D. 
McMurrey,  J. 
McMurrey,  J.  D. 

MacPhee,  G. 
McRae,  D.  R. 
Maar,  A.    . 
Mach,  R.  S. 

Mackler,  B. 
Macy,  L  G. 
Madden,  S.  C. 
Maddock,  W.  G. 
Magendie   . 
Maggioni,  G. 
Magnus,  I.  C. 
Mahler,  R.  F. 
Mahoney,  J.  J. 
Maizels,  M. 
Makeham,  W.  M 

Malamud,  N. 
Malerba,  G. 
Malm,  O.  J. 

Mamby,  A.  R. 
Manca,  P.  V. 
Manciaux,  M. 
Mancini,  R.  E. 
Mandel,  L. 
Mandel,  P. 
Mandelstam,  J. 
Mandl,  A.  M. 


Manery,  J.  F. 
Mangel,  M. 
Mannes,  A. 
Mantz,  F.  R. 
Marcus,  S. 
Marden,  W.  G.  R 
Marescaux,  J. 

Margetts,  A.  R. 
Markee,  J.  E. 
Marks,  H.  . 
Marks,  L  N. 
Mario w,  H. 
Marmer,  L  L. 
Marmorston,  J. 
Marr,  J.  C. 
Marsden,  J.  L. 


3,  52,  59 
5,  192,  208 

2,  87,  96 

5,  705 

1,  57,  65 

4,  24,  31 
A,  115 

5,  29,  31 
5,  27,  31 

3,  7,  18 

5,  27,  31 

4,  29,  32 

4,  102 

4,  702,  705, 

108,  113 

.      4,  90,  93 

4,  64,  65,  73 

.  5,  797,  208 

4,  292,  293, 

295,  297 

.      4,  40,  57 

.  4,  143,  149 

4,  7i2,  133,  135 

.      4,  39,  57 

1,  174 

.  1,  799,  200 

4,  702,  705,  108,  113 

.  4,  193,  195 

2,  14,  15,  17 

.  4,  799,  205 

5,  2,  4,  15, 

302,  311 

.  3,  134,  143 

.  4,  242,  246 

1,  109,  777,  775,  720, 

722,  725 

4,  42,  46,  57 
2,  797,  792,  799 

.  4,  242,  246 

.      2,  86,  96 

.  4,  252,  259 

.  4,  252,  259 

.  3,  108,  114 

1,  144,  160;  2,  32,  33, 

34,  35,  49,  50,  51,  53, 

54,  60,  66;   3,  727;  4, 

257,  259 

.  4,  705,  772 

.  1,  722,  725 

5,  59,  60,  69 
.  4,  709,  775 
.  4,  187,  195 
.  1,  7^5,  750 

2,  36,  37,  38,  39,  41, 
42,  46,  47,  53,  62,  66 
.  5,  757,  775 
.  1,  752,  760 
.  3,  85,  90 
4,  301 
.  5,  63,  69 
.  4,  725,  755 
4,  257,  259,  261 
.  5,  755,  775 
.  3,  56 


14 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Marshall,  F.  H.  A. 


2,  33,  54,  148, 

159 

1,  67,  65 

3,  171,  182 

4,  301,  308 
5,  59,  64,  69 

4,  165,  179 

.  4,  75 

4,  280,  294 

3,  75,  85,  90 

2,  113 

5,  43,  54 

4,  289,  292,  295 

2,  37,  54,  56, 

81,  83,  85,  186,  187 

.    5,  232,  234,  242 

5,  231,  232,234,235, 

236,  238,   242,   243,   244, 

245,  246,  248,  254,  264 

May,  R.  M.         .         .         .1,  151,  160 

Mayer         ....  2,  775 

Maynard,  L.  A.  .  1,  188,  193; 

3,8, 18,  25,  29;  5, 183, 

208,  251,  264 

Maynard-Smith,  J.       .    1,  239,  240,  241 


Marshall,  R. 
Marston,  M. 
Martin,  H.  E. 
Martin,  W. 
Martinek,  J. 
Massart 
Massie,  E.  . 
Master,  A.  M. 
Masters,  W.  H. 
Matsumoto,  K. 
Matthews,  H.  L. 
Matthews,  L.  H. 

Mauermayer,  G. 
Maurizio,  Anna 


Maxwell,  E.  L. 
Maxwell,  M.  H. 
Meara,  P.   . 
Medawar,  P.  B. 


Meduna 

Meggendorfer,  F 
Megyesi,  K. 
Mehl,  J.      . 
Meier,  R.    . 
Mellen,  I.    . 
Mendel,  G. 
Menon,  M.  D. 
Menzel,  D.  W.  5 
Mercier-Parot,  L 

Merendino,  K.  A 
Merkel,  F. 
Merrill,  A.  J. 
Merrill,  J.  P. 
Metcalf,  E.  V. 
Metchnikoff,  E. 
Meves,  F.   . 
Meyer,  H.  . 
Michael,  G. 
Michie,  D. 
Mickelsen,  O. 
Middlesworth,  L 
Miescher,  K. 

Miley,  J.  F. 
Milinsky,  G.  I. 
Millard,  A. 
Miller,  F.  J.  W. 
Miller,  G.  E. 
Miller,  J.  F. 


2,  3,  10,  11,  13 
.  4,  274,  297 
.  4,  145,  149 

1,  4,  5,  12,  15, 


24,  54,  78,  105,  139,  169, 

170,  171,  201,  245;  2,  199, 

229, 23 1;3, 5,  6, 18;  5, 271, 

281 

1,  237 

.  3,  137,  143 

.  4,  286,  296 

.  4,  301,  308 

4,  181,  187,  194,  195 

5,  277,  226 

4,208 

5,  149,  176 

183,  184,  185,  208,  210 

2,  161,  163, 

168,  170,  172 

.   4,  46,  56 

.   4,  62,  73 

4,  271,  277,  279,  297 

4,  45,  56,  57 

1,  223,  232,  236 

1,175,  181,  185 

.      2,  86,  96 

.  4,  130,  135 

.  2,  202,  209 

3,  7,  18 

4,  757,  757,  762 

van  .    .   3,  55,  59 

1,  56,  57,  138,  202; 

3,  194 

4,  24,  31 

5,  201,  208 

2,  208,  209 

3,  95,  100 

4,  288,  297 

4,  301,  308 


Miller,  J.  H. 

Miller,  L.  L. 
Miller,  R.  B. 
Milne,  M.  D. 


257, 


206, 

227, 


Miner,  C.  S. 
Miner,  J.  R. 
Miner,  R.  W. 
Minot,  C.  S. 
Mitchell,  H.  H. 
Mitchison,  M.  J. 
Moffitt,  G.  R. 
Moise,  T.  S. 
Mokotoff,  R. 

MolUson,  P.  L. 

276, 

233, 
238, 

Moltoni,  E. 

Monakow,  von    . 

Montagna,  W. 

103, 
792, 
201, 


Moon,  H.  D, 
Moore,  C.  R. 

Moore,  F.  D. 

Moore,  H.  L. 

Moore,  R.  A. 

Moore,  W.  W. 

Morant,  G.  M. 

Moreschi,  C. 

Morgulis,  S. 

Morris,  C.  J.  O.  R 

Morris,  J.  N. 

Morrison,  A. 

Morrison,  J.  L. 

Morrison,  S. 

Moscoso,  I. 

Moskovljevic,  V. 

Moskowitz,  M. 

Mossman,  H.  W, 

Mothes,  K. 

Moulton,  C.  R. 

Mounib,  M.  S. 

Muehrcke,  R.  C. 

Miihlbock,  O.      . 
118, 
125, 

Mussbichler,  A. 
Muggleton,  A. 
Muir,  H.  M. 
Muller,  A.  F. 
Munro,  H.  N. 
Munson,  P.  L. 


1,  220,  236;  4,  12, 

233,238,240,241,246 

.  4,  133,  135 

3,  26,  29;  5,  168,  176 
4,  93,  96,  138,  197, 

221,   222,    224,   225, 

248,   262,   268,   280, 

297,  299,  300 

.  2,  228,  232 

5,  267 

.      5,  53,  54 

1,6;  4,  250,  259 

4,  120,  121,  131,  135 

.  2,  223,  232 

.  4,  289,  297 

.  4,  254,  259 

.  4,  227,  280,  295, 

297 

.     1,  164,  169;  2,  215, 

221,  226,  231,   232, 

234,  235,   236,  237, 

239,   239,   240,   241, 

242,  243,  244,  245 

.     3,  9,  18;  5,  103 

.  1,  50 

2,  86,  91,  96,  96,  99, 

160,    175,    188,   797, 

79^,   799,    199,   200, 

211,   240,   247,  247, 

248,  249,  251 

1,  29 

2,  3,  13,  18,  '27! 34, 

54 

4,  102,  108,  110,  113, 
114,  291,  297 

5,  755,  776 
4, 


238,  246,  251,  259 

.      2,  81,  83 

1,  195,  196,  200,  201 

.  1,  777,  185 

.  5,  186,  208 

.  1,  130,  137 

.      5,  27,  31 

4,  226,  257,  259 

.  4,  279,  297 

2,  159 

1,  109,  121,  122 

.  5,  234,  242 

.  2,  223,  232 

.  2,  105,  114 

.  2,  204,  209 

.  4,  131,  135 

.  4,  202,  205 

.  4,  280,  297 

3,99,  106,  115,  777, 

779,   720,   72-^,   725, 

126,    127,    128,    129, 

130;  5,  33,  71,  80,  287 

.    5,235,238,242 

.  3,  42 

.  2,  217,  232 

4,  292,  293,  295,  297 

.  4,  130,  135 

.  .2,  762,  772 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


15 


Muntwyler,  E.     . 

.  4,  109,  113    1 

Olbrich,  O. 

1,  30,  51,  52,  138, 

Murie,  A.   . 

3,15,18:5,54,92, 

201,   203,   223,  236,  237; 

102 

3,  31,  32,  35,  46,  125,  144, 

Murie,  O.  J. 

.  2,  184,  187 

147,    148,    183,    190,    191, 

Murphy,  D.  P.    . 

.  5,  262,  264 

192;  4,  777,  772,  77i,  180, 

Murray,  J.  E. 

.      4,  45,  57 

195, 248 

Murrell,  K.  F.  H 

.  3,  166,  169 

Olesen,  K.  H. 

.    4,  102,  110,  113,113, 

Myers,  G.  B. 

.  4,  289,  296 

114,299,  313,315 

Myers,  H.  L. 

.      2,  41,  54 

Oliver,  B.  B. 
Oliver,  J.     . 

.  4,  283,  298 
4,  224,  250,  253,  254,  259 

Nabarro,  J.  D.  N 

. 

4,  308 

Oliver,  J.  A. 

.      3,26,29 

Nadal,  J.  W. 

.      4,  39,  57 

Olney,  J.  M. 

4,  109,  113,  114 

Nadell,  J.    . 

.  4,  265,  268 

Olsen,  A.  G. 

.      2,  81,  83 

Naeslund,  J. 

.  1,  148,  159 

Olsen,  C.    . 

.  1,  204,  209 

Nagorniy,  O.  V. 

.  3,  31 

Olsson,  V.  . 

.    5,  99,  103 

Nagy,  Z.     . 

4,  284,  286,  296 

O'Neil,  G.  C. 

.  1,  198,  200 

Nalbandov,  A.  V 

.      2,  81,  83 

Opfinger,  E. 

5,  235,  236,  241 

Nail,  G.  H. 

5,  153,  165,  176 

Ophuls,  W. 

.   1,89 

Natelson,  S. 

4,  40,  41,  57 

Opie,  E.  L. 

4,  23,  24,  32 

Neher,  R.   . 

4, 

60,  292,  293,  295 

Oppenheimer, 

E.T. 

.      3,  75,  90 

Neil,  N.  W. 

1,  148,  149,  158 

Orent,  E.  R. 

.  4,  301,  308 

Nelson,  J.  B. 

.  1,  179,  185 

Orent-Keiles, 

E.  '. 

4,  224 

Nelson,  K.  R.      . 

.      4,  82,  93 

Orr,  R.  T.  . 

.      3,  10,  18 

Nelson,  N. 

1,  130,  133,  143 

Ortiz,  E.     . 

.  1,  143,  160 

Nelson,  W.  P.      . 

.      4,  45,  57 

Orton,  J.  H. 

'.    5,  190,  191,  193,  198, 

Nesbett,  F.  B.      . 

.  2,  141,  143 

200,  201,  208 

Neuberger,  A. 

1,  100,103,217, 

Osterlund,  K. 

.  4,  203,  205 

232 

Outler,  J.  C. 

.  2,  148,  159 

Neukirch,  F. 

.      4,  41,  56 

Owen,  E.  C. 

.  1,  770,  123 

Neuman,  R.  E. 

.    1,91,  103 

Owens,  W.  A 

3,  163,  169,  172,  174, 182 

Newman,  E.  A. 

.  1,  191,  193 

Newman,  E.  V. 

4,  277,  296,  297 

Pacaud,  S. 

.         .3,  160,  169 

Neyzi,  O.    . 

.    4,  146,  147,  149 

Paccagnella,  B.    . 

.  5,  153,  176 

Nichols,  M.  P. 

1,  110,  120,  121,  122, 

Pace,  N.      . 

, 

.  4,  289,  298 

139;  3,  78,  90 

Padykula,  H. 

A. 

1,  106,  107, 

Nickel,  H.  K. 

.  5,  232,  242 

108,  109,  111,  114 

Nicolaysen,  R. 

1,  109,  111,  114, 115, 

Page,  E.      . 

.  1,  112,  114 

12C 

),   122,   123,   123,    124, 

Page,  E.  W. 

.  1,  129,  143 

12t 

),  205,  236,  237,  243; 

Pain,  J. 

5',235, 

237,  238,  242,  243 

3,  70,  147,  193 

Palade,  G.  E. 

.      1,  93,  96 

Nieberle,  K. 

.  5,  110,  112 

Pallaske,  G. 

.  5,  110,  112 

Nielson,  P.  E. 

.  2,  148,  159 

Palmer,  L.  S. 

.    5,237,242,243 

Nieto,  D.    . 

.  5,  107,  112 

Pannabecker, 

R.F. 

1,  3,  3,  11,  13 

Nieuwkoop 

.  2,  56 

Papanicolaou 

,  G.N. 

.      1,  41,  54 

Nigrelli,  R.  F. 

.    5,  113,  138,  147,  176, 

Papper,  S.  . 

.      4,  57,  57 

175 

i,  210,  211,   212,  213, 

Parets,  A.  D. 

.  3,  136,  142 

21( 

5,   217,   226,   226,   227, 

Park,  O.      . 

.      1,  10,  14 

228,  229,  244,  245 

Parker,  F.  . 

.  1,  112,  113 

Nitsche,  H. 

.  2,  779,  183 

Parker,  H.  V. 

'.    4,  102,  102,  106,  108, 

Nixon,  W.  C.  W. 

2,  162,  172, 

110,  113 

173;  4,  90,  93 

Parker,  J.  B. 

.  4,  130,  135 

Nolazco,  J. 

.      2,  86,  96 

Parker,  R.  L. 

.  1,  112,  113 

Nordmann,  R. 

4,220 

Parker,  R.  R. 

.  5,  182,  208 

Norris,  A.  H. 

1,  66;  3,  77,  90,  91 

Parkes,  A.  S. 

.    1,  139,  143,  154,  160, 

Northrop,  J.  H. 

1,  196,  200;  3,  24,  29, 

162,   163,  164,   165,   167, 

101 

168,  169,    170,    171,   172, 

Norton,  H.  W. 

.      1,81,83 

203,   215,   216,   217,  245; 

Norton,  R. 

.  I,  198,  200 

1,  16,  34,  49,  50,  53,  54, 

Nunes,  J.  P. 

.      1,36,54 

55,  56,  57,  58,  60,  65,  66, 
66, 127,  215, 216,  232,  239, 

O'Brien,  J.  R.  P. 

4,  36,  41,  43,  57 

240,   241,  242,  243,   244, 

O'Connor 

1,  138 

245,246,  248,  249;  3,30, 

Ogbom,  M.  E. 

.          .        5,5,75 

99,116,124,126,  127,130, 

Ohlson,  M.  A. 

.  1,  122,  123 

185;  4,  755 

16 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Parmington,  S.  L.        .         .  4, 228 

Parrish,  D.  B.      .  .  .4,  200,  205 

Parrott,  D.  M.  V.  .  4,  257,  259 

Patterson,  H.  D.  .      2,  35,  54 

Pearce,  J.  W.        .  .4,  28,  57 

Pearcy,  M.  .         .         .         .  4,  75 

Pearl,  R.     .     \,  243\3,  24,  29,  137,  143; 

5,  267,  283 
Pearl,  R.  de  Witt  .  3,  137,  143 

Pearson,  E.  G.     .         .         .  5, 288 

Pearson,  J.  A.  .  .  2,  206,  208,  209 
Pearson,  K.  1,  52,  238;  3,  8,  18;  5,  3, 
15,   21,    31,   53,   54,   288 


Pearson,  O.  P 
Pearson,  P.  B. 
Pedersen,  S. 
Pemberton,  J. 
Penkow 
Penn,  J. 
Pepler,  W.  J. 
Perks,  W.   . 


Perley,  A,   . 
Perrone,  J.  C. 

Perry,  S.  M. 
Persike,  E.  C. 
Peters,  J.  P. 
Petrovitch,  A. 

Petter,  J.  J. 
Petter-Rousseaux,  A. 


Pettinari,  V, 
Pfeiffer,  C.  A. 
Phillips,  E.  F. 
Phillips,  E.  W. 
PhiUips,  J.  B. 
PhiUips,  J.  C. 
Phillipson,  A.  T. 
Piaget 
Pickens,  M. 
Pictet,  A.    . 
Piel,  H. 

Pienaar,  U.  de  V 
Pierce,  E.  C. 
Piggot,  A.  P. 
Pignard,  P. 
Pilcher,  C. 
Pincus,  G. 


von  Pirquet 
Pirre,  G.  D. 
Pitt,  F. 
Pitts,  R.  F. 
Piatt,  R.      . 
Platts,  M.  M. 
Plentl,  A.  A. 


3,  27,  28,  29 
.  5,  237,  242 
.  4,  39,  57 
.    3,  94,  100 

1,  145 

3,  70,  96,  100,  106,  114 

3,  102 

5,  3,  4,  5,  14,  15,  SI,  288, 

289,  290,  291,  292,  293, 

294,  302,  303,  311 

4,  143,  149 

1,  100,  103;  2,  217, 

232 

,  4,  200,  205 

.  4,  257,  259 

4,  38,  40,  57 
.      2,  36,  37,  38,  39,  41, 

42,  46,  47,  53,  62,  66 
.  3,  28,  29 
3,  17,  18,  28, 
29 
.  2,  33,  54 
.  1,  151,  152,  160 
.  5,  232,  234,  242 
5,  4,  5,  15 
.  5,  149,  176 
.  1,  755,  160 
.  4,  202,  205 
.  1,47 
.  4,  131,  135 
.  1,  196,  200 
5,  59,  63,  64,  69 
.  3,  34,  36 
.  1,  76 
1,  18,  23;  3,  93,  100 
.4,217,219 
.  4,  287,  288,  296 
1,  36,  48,  126,  126,  128, 
130,  134,  136,  137,  219, 
219,  223,  224,  225,  226, 
227,  229,  230,  232,  233, 
234,  236;  2,  36,  37,  54;  4, 
90,  93,  187,  195,  262;  5, 
267 
1,201 
1,  198,  199 

3,  10,  18 
4,  265,  268 

4,  50,  56 
4,  265,  268 

4,  89,  93 


Pohl,  H.  A. 
Polge,  C.    . 

Pollock,  W.  F. 
Polyakov,  E.  V. 
Ponder,  E. 
Ponomareva,  L. 
Pooler,  N.  R. 
Popjak,  G. 
Pospelov,  S.  P. 
Potter,  V.  R, 
Power,  M.  H. 
Prader,  A.  . 
Prankerd,  T.  A. 
Price,  D.     .      1 

Primrose,  T. 
Pritchard,  J.  A. 
Pritchard,  J.  J. 
Pritchard,  W.  H 
Probst,  R.  T. 
Pucher,  G.  W. 
Puetter 
Pycha,  R.  L. 
Pyrkosch,  W. 

Qasim,  S.  Z. 
Quinton,  A. 
Quiring,  D.  P. 


.     2,  109,  113,  148,  158 
1,  163,  164,  169;  2,  215, 
232 
.  2,  98 
.      5,  53,  54 
2,  217,  223,  232 
I.         .  .5, 53,  54 

3,  707 
.  2,  277,  232 
.      5,  53,  54 
.  4,  133,  135 
4,  41,  56 
.  4,  60 
J.        2,  238,  239;  4,  208 
,  143,  160;  2,  3,  3,  4,  13, 
14,  15,  17,  28 
.      4,  89,  93 
.  2,  235,  239 
.  2,  123,  125 

4,  277,  288,  297 

5,  144,  153,  176 
2,  203,  209,  210 

.  1,  56,  57 
.  5,  75i,  775 
.  3,  134,  142 

5,  755,  755.  775 
.  4,  709,  772 
.  5,  779,  133 


Radhakrishnan,  N 
Ragan,  M.  S. 
Rahm,  M.  P.  G. 
Rahn,  O.    . 
Raiha,  N.   . 
Raitt,  D.  S. 


Rakowicz,  M. 
Ramasarma,  G.  B 

Rand,  R.  W. 
Ranson,  R.  M. 

Rao,  C.  R.  N. 
Rapoport,  S. 


Rascoff,  H. 
Rasmussen,  A 
Rasquin,  P. 
Raven,  J.  C. 
Ray,  L.  A. 
Raynaud,  A. 
Reardan,  J.  B. 
Rector,  F.  C. 
Reed,  R.     . 

Regan 

Rehm,  W.  S. 
Reibisch,  J. 
Reid,  A.  F. 
Reifenstein,  E, 
Reinke,  F. 
Relman,  A.  S. 


5,  755,  176 
1,  797,  200 

3,  25,  29 
1,  775,  185 

4,  204,  205 
.  3,  22,  30;  5,  7^9,  775, 

198,   199,   200,   201,  202, 

205,  208 

.  5,  755,  775 

.      1,  97,  94,  103; 

3,  97,  100 

.      2,  81,  83 

3,  70,  18; 

5,295 

.   1,39,40,42,54 

2,  237,  239;  4,  40, 

46,  57,  287,  296 

.  1,  198,  200 

.      2,  95,  96 

5,  196,  208,  218,  226 

.  3,  160,  168 

.  1,  777,  185 

2,  3,  11,  13,  167,  172 

4,  45,  56 
.  4,  209,  219 

3,  65,  66,  67,  96,  97,  100, 
102,  103 
.  2,56 
4,  24,  31 

5,  202,  208 
4,  109,  113 
1,  109,  122 

2,  92,  96 
4,222 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


17 


Remington,  J.  W 
Renold,  A.  E. 
Renton,  R.  M. 
Renzi,  A.  A. 
R6v,  J. 
Reynolds,  S.  R.  M 


Ribbands,  C.  R. 
Ribot 
Rice,  T.  R. 
Rich,  C.  O. 
Richards,  D.  W. 
Richards,  F.  J. 
Richardson 
Richardson,  I.  M 
Richdale,  L.  E. 
Richet,  G.  . 
Richie,  R. 
Richmond,  J. 
Richter,  C.  P. 
Richter,  W. 
Ricker,  W.  E. 


Rickham,  P. 
Riesen,  W.  H. 
Riesman,  D. 
Riess,  B.  F. 
Rigby,  B.    . 
Rijssel,  Th.  G.  van 
Rinsler,  M.  G. 
Riondel,  A.  M. 
Ripke,  G.   . 
Rittenberg,  D. 
Roberts,  E,         1 
Roberts,  J.  M. 
Roberts,  J.  R.  E. 
Roberts,  P.  H. 
Roberts,  R.  B. 
Robertson,  J.  A. 
Robertson,  J,  J. 
Robertson,  O.  H, 
Robertson,  R.  N, 
Robertson,  T.  B. 
Robinson,  C.  V. 
Robinson,  J.  R. 
Robinson,  O.  J. 
Robinson,  S. 
Robson,  J.  M. 
Rockstein,  M. 


4,  275,  277,  295 
.  4,  280,  298 
.  5,  189,  207 

4,  296 

.  4,  286,  296 

2,  80,  83,  129, 

143,  148,  159 

5,  232,  233,  242 
.  1,  ii 

5,  170,  175 

5,  4,  15 

3,  79,  91 

2,  207,  209 
2,200 

3,  156,  169 
5,  100,  101,  103 

4,  99,  228,  262 

4,  139,  141,  149 

4  34 

\  4,  166,' 179 

.  2,  197,  199 

3,  11,  19;  5,  145, 

149,  151,  155,  176 

.  4,  98 

.  1,  197,  200 

1,  47,  48,  49 

.  3,  171,  182 

4,  89,  90,  93 

3,  120,  121,  125 

4,  89,  90,  93 

4,  293,  297 
5,  59,  69 

2,  204,  209 
91,  94,  103;  3,  97,  100 


Rodgers,  C. 
Rodolfo,  A. 
Rosch,  G.  A. 
Roessle,  R. 
Rottgerman,  W. 
Rogers,  J.  B. 
Roguski 
Rokaw,  S.  N. 
Rolf,  D.      . 


2,  81,  83 
.  4,  75 

1,  122,  123 

2,  148,  158 
5,  149,  176 

1,  755,  166 

2,  234,  239 
2,  206,  208,  209 

1,  777,  185;  5,  7i7 
2,  130,  143 
4,  23,  24,  32,  214,  219 
5,  59,  69 
.  4,  76 
2,  168,  172 
.  5,  15,  31,  55,  83,  84, 
112,  114,  179,  210,  226, 
228,  232,  234,  243,  244, 
245,  246,  247,  247,  248, 
249,  250,  251,  254,  255, 
256,  257,  258,  264,  265, 
267,268,284,287,289,291 


4,  289,  298 

2,  109,  114,  118,  125 

5,  243 

.  4,  257,  259 

5,  59,  62,  69 

3,  77,  19,  20,  30 

.  4,  33 

.  4,  280,  294 

.    4,256,257,259 


Rollason,  H.  D.  . 
Rollefsen,  G. 
Rollins,  R.  Z. 
Romanoff,  A.  J. 
Romanoff,  A.  L. 
Romanoff,  L.  P. 


.  4,  252,  259 

.  5,  149,  176 
.  5,  258,  264 
3,  20,  21,  30 
3,  20,  21,  30 
1,725,  136,  137,219, 


Rominger,  E, 
Rook,  J.  A.  F.     . 
Rorig,  A.    . 

Rorschach,  H. 
Rosenbaum,  J.  D. 
Rosenberg,  E.  K. 
Rosenberger,  C.  R. 
Rosenman,  R.  H 


27P,  223,  224,  225,  226, 
227,  230,  232,  233,  234, 
236;   4,  90,  93,   187,  195 


4,  130,  135 

4,  301,  307,  30,  3098 

2,  179,  183,  184,  185, 

187 

.  3,  181,  182 

.   4,  45,  51,  56,  57 

.   1,  139;   3,  78,  90 

.  5,  255,  264 

.  3,  137,  142 

Rosenthal,  T.  B.  1, 91, 99, 103;  3,  97,  100 


Ross,  G. 
Ross,  M.  A. 
Rotblat,  J. 


Roth,  C.  B. 
Rothman 
Rothschild,  P. 
Roulet,  F.  . 
Roulet,  F.  C. 
Rounsefell,  G.  A 
Rous,  P.     . 
Roux,  M.   . 
Rowlands,  I.  W 


Rowntree,  L. 
Rowson,  L.  E.  A. 
Rubin,  B.  L. 

7i7, 

Rubini,  M.  E. 
Rubner,  M. 


Rudzinska,  M.  A 


Ruger,  H.  A. 
Rumbauer 
Runnstrom,  S. 
Rupple,  B. 
Russell,  E.  S. 
Russell,  W.  I. 
Russell,  W.  L. 
Ryberg,  C. 
Rynearson,  E.  H 


4,  277,  297 

.  4,  256,  259 

.      5,  15,  19,  20,  86,  87, 

103,    135,    177,   209,   210, 

283,  289,  290,  291,  292 

2,  765,  772 

2,207 

2,  757,  755 

4,  257,  259 

1,  16,23;  3,93,  100 

.  5,  797,  208 

2,  234,  236,  239 

3,  70,  96,  100 

1,  142,  159;  2, 

29,  66,  69,  81,  83,  83,  84, 

85,  214,  260 

4,  95 

'.  1,  164,  'l69 

1,  54,  126,  130,  134, 

138,    139,    140,   237; 

4,  90,  93 

.      4,^5,57 

3,  23,  30;  5,  124, 

125,   126,   127,   129,   130, 

133,  134 

1,  187,  193, 

197.  200;  3,  4,  19 

'  .  3,  777,  752 

5,  59,  63,  69 

.  5,  149,  176 

1,  160 

5,  190,  199,  208 

1,  155,  156,  161 

.  3,  775,  725 

.  4,  209,  219 

.      4,  41,  56 


Sabine,  J.  C. 
Sacher,  G.  A. 


.  2,  235,  239 
5,  75,  18,  31,  52,  54, 
80,  87,  88,  104,  115,  775, 
727,  133,  133,  134,  135, 
755,  136,  137,  138,  138, 
139,  140,  141,  147,  176, 
245,  266,  282,  284,  289, 
292,  295,  296,  29a 


18 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Sagild,  U. 
Sakagami,  S.  F 
Salazar 
Samokhvalova,  G 
Samuels,  A.  J. 
Sander,  G. 
Sandulesco,  G. 
Santos,  R.  F. 
Sapsford,  C.  S. 
Sarkar,  B.  C.  R 
Sato,  R.      . 
Sawyer,  W.  H. 
Saxl,  H.      . 

Saxon,  L.    . 
Saxton,  J.  A. 
Sc6bat,  L. 
Schaffer,  J. 
Schaumann,  O. 
Scheerer,  M. 
Scheidegger,  S. 

Scheinberg,  P. 
Scherer,  H.  J. 
Scheyer,  W.  J. 
Schiefferdecker,  P 
Schieren,  J. 
Schilling,  A. 
Schloerb 
Schloerb,  P.  R. 
Schmidlin,  J. 
Schmidt,  C.  F. 
Schmidt,  L  G. 
Schmidt,  K.  P. 
Schneider,  C.  L. 
Schneider,  F. 
Schoenheimer 
Schoenheimer,  R 
Scholz,  W. 
Schotterer,  A. 
Schroder,  G. 
Schroeder,  H.  A. 
Schuler,  W. 
Schultze,  M.  O. 
Schultze,  W. 
Schuster,  D. 
Schutzman,  F.  L 
Schwartz,  I. 
Schwartz,  I.  L. 
Schwartz,  R. 

Schwartz,  W.  B. 
Schwarz 
Schwarz,  J. 
Schwarz,  W. 
Scott,  J.  P. 
Scott,  W.  C. 
Scribner,  B.  H. 
96, 

Scrimshaw,  N.  S. 
Segar,  W.  E. 
Seldin,  D.  W.      . 
Selkurt,  E.  W.     . 


4,  109,  HI,  113 

5,  233,  243 

2,  64,  66 

5,209 

4,  266,  268 

3,  137,  143 
1,  130,  137 

4,  46,  57 
2,  86,  96 

5,  237,  243 
5,  151,  176 

4,  167,  179 
3,  97,  98,  100,  103; 

5,  113 

.      4,  51,  57 

.    4,  253,  257,  259 

4,  273,  278,  279,  296 

.  2,  196,  199 

.  4,  167,  179 

.  3,  180,  182 

5,  103,  106,  112,  113, 

226 

3,  147',  4,  286,  297 

.  5,  108,  112 

.  5,  89 

.  2,  188,  199 

.      5,  59,  69 

1,  110,  122,  123 

4,  269 

.  4,  109,  113 

.  4,  181,  195 

.  4,  286,  296 

.      2,  39,  54 

.      1,  10,  14 

.  2,  112,  114 

.  5,  240,  243 

.      3,  48,  49 

.  2,  204,  209 

.  5,  707,  112 

.      5,  43,  54 

.      5,  65,  69 

.  4,  288,  297 

.  4,  187,  195 

.  1,  192,  193 

1,  28,  67,  217 

.      1,  42,  48 

.  4,  301,  308 

.  4,  274,  297 

4,  24,  32,  67,  73,  77 

4,  129,  132,  133, 

135 

4,222 

3,  702,  70i 

5,  238,  242 
4,  29,  32 

2,171,  182 

4,  709,  772 
4,  46,  56,  93, 

100,101,114,222,249, 

269 

.  1,  198,  200 

.         .4,  133,  134 

.  4,  209,  219 

.    4,274,277,297 


Sellers,  A.  L. 
Selwyn,  J.  G. 
Selye,  H.     . 
Sen,  P.  K.  . 
Serane,  J.    . 
Serpen, G. 
Severinghaus,  E. 
Seville,  R. 
Seymour,  W.  M. 
Shaeflfer,  K. 
Shannon,  C. 
Sharaf,  A,  A. 
Sheldon,  D.  S. 
Sheldon,  J.  H. 


4,  257,  259,  261 

.  4,  199,  205 

4,  256,  259;  5,  88 

2,  204,  207,  209 
4,  262 
.  2,  30 

1,  143,  153,  160 

.    3,  96,  100 

B.      .    4,271,288,297 

.  4,  130,  135 

.  2,  230,  232 

.  2,  168,  172 

.  4,  109,  113 

3,  94,  100,  112,  114; 

4,  264,  268 

Shell,  H.  M.         .  .  .5, 237,  243 

Sherman,  H.  C.   .         .  1,  197,  200; 

4,  132,  133,  135 

Shiraishi,  Y.         .         .  .5,  757,  775 

Shirley,  R.  J.        .  .  .2,  148,  159 

Shock,  N.  W.      .      1,  52,  53,  55,  66,  66, 

110,   120,   121,   122,   139, 

139,   206,   207,   207,   213, 

214,   217,   220,  223,  224, 

229,   230,   231,   232,  236, 

237,  242,  244;  3,  77,  78, 
79,  84,  88,  89,  90,  91,  151, 
168;  4,  11,  72,  777,  772, 
77i,  114,  115,  164,  207, 
225,  226,  229,  230,  231, 
232,  233,   235,   236,   237, 

238,  240,  241,  242,  243, 
244,  245,  246,  247,  247, 
248,    249,  250,    264,  268, 

277,  295,  314,  315 
Shohl,  A.  T.         .  .  .4,  143,  149 

Shorr,  E 4,  280,  295 

Sieker,  H.  O.  .  4,  283,  284,  296,  297 
Silberberg,  M.  .  .  .1,  189,  193 
Silberberg,  R.  .  .  .1,  189,  193 
SilUman,  R.  P.  .  .  .5,  149,  176 
Silver,  H.  M.        .  .  3,  75,  80,  90,  91 

Silversten,  I.  .  .  .1,  180,  185 
Silverstone,  F.  A.         .         .         1,  237; 

3,  77,  80,  90,  91 
Simms,  H.  S.       .      5,  72,  73,  74,  75,  76, 

79,  266,  268,  293 
Simon         ....  3,  775 

Simon,  E.  R.  .  .  .4,  200,  205 
Simon,  E.  W.       .  .  .2,  208,  209 

Simonson,  E,       .  .  .3,  75,  91 

Simpson,  A.  C.  .  5,  203,  204,  205,  208 
Simpson,  M.  E.  .  .  1,  29;  2,  33,  54 
Simpson,  S.  A.    .  .    4,  292,  293,  296 

Simpson,  T.  .  .  .4, 267,  268 

Sinclair,  H.  M.  .  1,  194,  195, 

197,  200,   201,   202,   203, 

205,  207 
Sinclair,  Sir  J.      .  .  .1,  194,  201 

Sinclair-Smith,  B.  .  .4,  277,  296 

Singer,  B.    .  .  .4,  181,  195,  297 

Singer,  M.  .    2,  775,  178,  183,  185, 

186,  187 
Singer,  R.  B.        .         .         .4,  289,  297 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


19 


Sirota,  J.  H. 
Sjogren,  H 
Sjogren,  T. 
Skjelkvale,  L. 
Slack,  H.  G.  B. 
Slater,  P.     . 
Sloviter,  H.  A. 
Sloviter,  J.  A. 
Sluiter,  J.  W. 
Smith 

Smith,  A.  D. 
Smith,  A.  H. 

Smith,  A.  U. 

Smith,  A.  V. 
Smith,  G.  S. 
Smith,  G.  van  S 
Smith,  G.  V. 
Smith,  H.   . 
Smith,  H.  L. 
Smith,  H.  W. 
Smith,  I.  H. 
Smith,  J.  Maynard 


Smith,  K.  R. 
Smith,  L.  E. 
Smith,  L.  L. 
Smith,  M. 
Smith,  O.  C. 
Smith,  P.  E. 
Smith,  P.  K. 
Smyly,  W.  J. 
Sniffen,  R.  C. 
Snyder,  D.  P. 
Snyder,  F.  F. 
Sobel,  E.  H. 
Soberman,  J. 
S0bye,  P.    . 
Solomon,  A.  K 
Solomon,  D.  H. 


Solti,  F.      . 
Sommerson,  W.  H 
Somerville,  I.  F.  . 
Sonneborn,  T.  M. 


4,  288,  295 
3,  134,  143 
3,  134,  143 

1,  109 
1,  100,  103 

1,  37,  48 

1,  164,  169 

2,  215,  232 
5,  97,  103 

5,  59,  69 

2,  215,  216,  232 
4,  120,  121,  128,  131, 

135,  254,  259 
1,  163,  164,  165,  167, 
168,  169;  2,  215,  232 

2,  33,  53 
.  1,  18 

1,  145,  161 

2,  112,  114 
4,227 

3,  74,  75,  76,  91 
4,  230,  246,  277,  298 

.  1,  198,  201 
5,  16,  32,  33, 
55,  86,  87,  104,  134,  137, 
141,  266,  267,  268,  271, 
278,  279,  281,  281,  282, 
283,  284,  285,  290 

3,  151,  169 

1,  230,  236 

5,  755,  174 
3,  9,  19 

2,  112,  114 
1,  145,  160 

4,  120,  121,  128,  131,  135 
P.  5,  145,153,169,175,176 
2,  91,  96 
5,  95,  103 
2,  109,  112,  113,  114 
.  4,  141,  149 
4,  295 
.  3,  137,  143 
.  4,  109,  112 
1,  139,  223,  224,  229, 
232,  236;  3,  78,  79,90,91; 
4,  235,  237,  246 

4,  286,  296 


Sontag,  L.  W. 
Soudek,  S. 
Southern,  H.  N 
Spacek,  B. 
Spahr,  A.    . 
Spector,  H. 
Spence,  J.   . 
Spencer,  A.  G. 
Spencer,  M.  P. 
Sperhng,  G 


.  2,  130,  143 
.  2,  112,  114 
2,  252;  3,  16; 
5,  32,  35,  53, 
54,  262,  264 
2,  162,  172 
5,  235,  243 
5,  101,  103 
4,  291,  296 
.  4,60 
4,  131,  135 
3,  95,  100 
4,308 
4,  274,  277,  297 
1,  188,  193,  197,  200; 
2,189,199;3,8,18;S,251 
264 


Sperry,  W.  M. 
Spiegelman,  M. 
Spielmeyer,  W. 
Spies,  T.  D. 
Spitskaya,  T.  D. 
Spohde,  H. 
Sprague,  P.  H. 
Spray,  C.  M. 

Sproul,  E.  E. 
Spurway,  H. 
Squires,  R.  D. 
Stanbury,  S.  W. 

Stanier,  M.  W. 
Stark,  W.    . 
Stavraky,  G.  W. 
Stead,  E.  A. 

Stearns,  G. 
Steenburg,  R.  W 
Steggerda,  F.  R. 


Steinbach,  H.  B. 
Stephenson,  R.  P 
Stevens,  A.  R. 
Stevens,  C.  F. 
Stevenson,  J.  A.  F 
Steward,  F.  C. 
Stewart,  J.  M. 
Stewart,  W.  B. 
Stigall,  C.   . 
Stockard,  C.  R. 
Stockklausner,  F 
Stoerk,  H.  C. 
Stoesigger,  B. 
Stolbova,  A. 
StoU,  G.      . 
Stowers,  J.  M. 
Strangeways,  W. 
Strauss,  F. 


Strauss,  M.  B. 
Strawn,  K. 
Strong,  L.  C. 
Strube,  H.  . 
Stuart-Harris,  C.  H 
Suau,  P.      . 
Subbarow,  Y. 
Sumner,  F.  B. 
Sumner,  J.  B. 
Summers-Smith,  D 
Sundnes,  G. 
Sutow,  W.  W. 
Sutter,  J.     . 
Svardson,  G. 

Svoboda,  J. 
Swanson,  M.  A. 
Swanson,  P,  P. 
Swanson,  W.  W, 
Sward,  K. 
Swezy,  O.   . 
Swift,  R.  W. 


4,  23,  32 

3,  137,  142 
1,  33,  48 

1,  198,  201 

5,  53,  54 
5,  64,  69 

3,  74,  75,  76,  91 
4,  120,  121,  128, 

136,  306,  308 

.  4,  253,  259 

.  5,  276,  281 

.  4,  289,  297 

4,  50,  56,  193,  195, 

267,  268 

4,  128,  130,  135,  216 
.  1,  194,  201 
4,  64,  65,  73 

4,  51,  57,  271,  277, 
298 

4,  130,  135 
.4,114 

4,  120,  121, 
131,  135 

4,  22,  32 

4,  167,  179 
2,  228,  232,  237,  238 

.  5,  21,  31 
1,  223,  232,  236 
.  2,  207,  209 
.  2,  236,  239 
.  2,  236,  239 
.  4,  130,  135 
.  2,  41,  54 
.      5,  59,  69 

4,  292,  293,  296 
.3,  171,  182 
.  5,  53,  54 
.  4,  109,  lis 

4,  308 

M.  B.  .  4,  211,  219 

2,  56,  66,  67,  68,  160, 

174,  185,  200 

4,  45,  51,  56,  57 

5,  755,  775 
1,  747,  767 

3,  134,  142 
4,269 

5,  149,  177 

4,  720,  7i5 
3,  9,  19 

1,177 
5,  99,  101,  103 

5,  169,  176 
4,  105,  113 

5,  21,  31 

5,  777,  775,  182,  190, 
208 

.  5,  235,  243 
.  2,  136,  143 
.  1,  722,  72i- 
.  4,  130,  135 
1,48;  3,  159,  169 
2,  35,  41,  53,  54,  55 
.  4,  127,  135 


20 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Swyer,  G,  I.  M. 


de  Sylva,  D. 
Szabo 

Szabo,  G.   . 
Szafran,  J. 

Szasz,  J. 
Szilard,  L.  . 


2,  774,  7 75;  4,13,78,93, 

94,95,  97,  98,  115,  197, 

198,227,  260,  313,  316 

.    5,  755,  775,  228 

3,  103 

.    4,  284,  286,  296 

1,211,213;  3,  760, 

162,  169 

.  4,  286,  296 

5,  19,  129,  133,  289 


Tabah,  L.   . 
Taber,  R.  D. 
Taffel,  M. 
Tainter,  M.  L 
Tait,  J.  F. 
Takacs,  L. 

Talbot,  F.  G. 
Talbot,  N.  B. 


Talke,  L.     . 
Tallqvist,  H. 
Tanaka,  T. 
Tandler,  J, 
Tang,  C-T. 
Tannenbaum,  A 
Tanner,  J.  M 


.  5,  21,  31 
5,  92,  94,  103 
.  4,  38,  56 
5,  136 
4,  292,  293,  296 
4,  45,  57,  284,  286, 
296 
3,  9,  18 
4,  13,  32,  64,  73,  76, 
94,  97,  100,  705,  77i,  137, 
139,  139,  140,  141,  143, 
146,  147,  149,  150,  151, 
152,  162,  163,  226,  260, 
261,  i77 
2,  7P7,  799 
4,227 


3,  24,  30 

2,  779,  183 

2,  747,  143 

1,  797,  207 

4,80,  93;  5,  84,  85, 

86,  88,  266,  290,  296,  297 

Tanquary,  M.  C.  .         .5,  232,  242 


Tattersall,  R.  N 
Tauber,  O.  E. 
Tayler,  R.  Q.  C. 
Taylor,  A.  . 
Taylor,  C.  C. 
Taylor,  D.  J. 
Taylor,  H.  L. 
Taylor,  W.  H.      . 
Ted,  H.  M. 
Templeman,  W.  . 
Templeman,  W.  G. 
Templeton,  H.  A. 
Tener,  J.  S. 
Tengbergen,  W.  van  E 
Tenney,  B. 
Terao,  A.    . 
Terman,  L.  M. 


Terpenning,  J,  G 
Terry,  M.    , 

Tester,  A.  L. 
Thaysen,  J.  H. 


Theard,  A. 

Thelander,  H. 
Thiele,  T.  N. 
Thomas,  W.  A 


3,  96,  100 

5,  277,  281 

4,  40,  57 

4,  116 

5,  149,  167,  177 

3,  188 

4,  757,  161,  162 

4,  36,  41,  43,  57 
.  2,  762,  772 

5,  146,  151,  177 
.  2,  207,  209 
.  1,  797,  207 
.  3,  70,  18 
.  3,  720,  725 

2,  770,  772,  114 
.      3,  24,  30 
1,  40,  48;  3,  772,  174, 
182 
.  5,  182,  208 
4,  7i9,  140,  141,  145, 
146,  147,  149 
.  5,  149,  177 
4,  62.  63,  64,  65,  67, 
68,  69,  72.73,  73,  74,  74, 
75,  76,  77,  77,  94,  99 
4,  273,  278, 
279,  296 
2,  720,  727,  725,  727 
5,  3,  15 
4,  81,  93,  262 


Thomasson,  B.    . 
Thompson,  J.  C. 
Thompson,  J.  F. 
Thompson,  W.  F. 
Thomsen,  A.  C.  . 
Thomsen,  K. 
Thomson,  A.  M. 
Thorn,  D.  W.       . 
Thorn,  G.  W.      . 
Thorn,  N.  A. 

Thorndike,  E.  L. 


Thorndike,  R.  L. 
Thung,  P.  J. 
Thurstone,  L.  L. 
Tibbo,  S.  N. 
Tiews,  K.    . 
Timoner,  J. 
Tizard, 
Todd,  F.  E. 
Topper,  Y.  J. 
Toro,  G.     . 
Tosteson,    . 
Townsend,  C,  H. 
Tracey,  K.  M. 
Trowbridge,  P.  F 
Tschertok   . 
Tsuchiya,  K. 
Tuchmann-Duplessis,  H. 


4,  273,  283,  298 

4,  79,  93,  97,  98 

.  2,  207,  209 

.  5,  757,  777 

.      4,  41,  56 

2,  110,  112,  114 
.  5,  33 

.      4,  82,  93 

4,  82,  93,  280,  298 

4,  63,  65,  67,  68, 

73,  76 

3,  762,  769, 

772,  182 

.  3,  178,  182 

3,  779,  727,  725 
.  3,  181,  182 
.  5,  149,  177 
.  5,  755,  777 
.  4,  266,  268 

1,  138 
.  5,  237,  243 
.  4,  200,  205 
1,  770,  727,  722 
4,  206 
.  5,  277,  226 
.  5,  262,  264 
.  4,  757,  755 

1,  145 
.  2,  189,  199 

2,  84, 
127,    161,   765,   168,   172, 

173,  174,  175,  245 

Tucker,  D.  W.     .  .5,  174,  177 

Tucker,  W.  A.     .  .         .3,  766,  769 

Tudvad,  F.  .         .         .4,  726,  756 

Tufts,  E.  V.  .     4,  507,  307,  308,  309 

Tunbridge,  R.  E.        1,  1,  23,  48,  50,  52, 

53,  54,  57,  67,  68,  104,  105, 

107,    108,    125,    137,    138, 

205,  206,  238,  241,  245; 

3,  46,  65,  66,  67,  69,  71, 

72,  92,  96,  97,  100,  102, 

103,  128,  182,  183 

Turner,  CD..  .         .2,  18,  27 


Turner,  J.  R. 

Ueberwasser,  H. 
Ullmann,  E.  A.    . 
Unna,  P.  G. 

Vaas,  K.  F. 
Vacek,  Z.    . 
Valach,  A. 
Vallois,  H.  V.      . 
Van  Cleave,  H.  J. 
Vanderlinde,  R.  J. 
Van-Eck,  G.  J.  V. 

Van  Heerdt,  P.  F. 
Van  Slyke,  D,  D. 
Van  Wagenen,  G. 
Vara,  P.      . 
Varga,  F.    . 
Varnauskas,  E.    . 


.  2,  236,  239 

.  4,  757,  795 
.  3,  52,  59 
.    3,  96,  100 

.  5,  770,  777 

4,  165,  755,  187 

.  4,  291,  296 

5,  134 

5,  179 

.  4,  709,  772 

2,  36,  44,  45, 

46,  54,  55 

.    5,  97,  103 

4,  248,  272,  298 

.  1,  755,  767 

.  4,  203,  205 

.  4,  767,  762 

4,  273,  283,  298 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


21 


Velardo,  J.  T.       .         .         .2,  81,  83 

Verney,  E.  B.       .         .         .4,  37,  57 

Venning,  E.  H.    .         4,  89,  93,  293,  294 

Verschuer,  O.  v.  .         .  3,  137,  431 

Verzdr,  F.        1,  24,  57,  67,  68,  108,  171, 

204,  238;  3,  30,  60,  63, 

64,  66,  67,  68,  68,  69,  70, 

94,  128,  194;  5,  82,  85,  86, 

112,133,137,138,141,298 

Verzar,  J 1,  215,  216 

Verzdr-McDougall,  J.  3,  64,  68,  187 

Vair,  W.  N.  .  .  .4,  283,  298 

Vickery,  H.  B.     .         .    2,  203,  209,  210 

Videbaeck,  A.      .         .         .4,  241,  246 

Villee,  C.  A.        2,  17,  28,  107,  112,  114, 

117,    129,   136,   141,   142, 

143,   144,   144,    145,    146, 

147,   212,   213,   242,   243, 

245,  249,  250,  253 

F.     .         .         .      1,37,48 

L.      .     1,  16.  23,  25,  49,  66; 

3,  93,  100 

B.   .         .    hl79,  180,  185 

5,  35,  43,  46,  49,  54 

.  5,  149,  177 

5,  237,  238,  242,  243 

.      3,  10,  18 

.  5,  149,  177 

1,  35,  48,  49 

.  2,  184,  187 

1,  35,  48,  49;  3,  147 

.  4,  161,  162 

.  5,  237,  243 

2,  111,  112,  113, 

148,  159 

.      2,  34,  53 


Vincent,  D. 
Vischer,  A. 


Visscher,  M. 
Vitt,  V.  o. 
Vives,  F.     . 
Vivino,  E. 
Vizoso,  M. 
VNIRO 
Vogt.  C.      . 
Vogt,  F.      . 
Vogt,  O.     . 
Vonoczky,  J. 
Voogd,  S.   . 
Vosburgh,  G. 

Voss,  H.  E.  V, 


Wackwitz,  J.  D 
Waelsch,  L. 
Wagenen,  G.  van 
Waggoner,  R.  W 
Wagman,  I.  H. 
Wahl,  O.     . 
Wakman,  A.  J. 
Walaas,  E. 
Walaas,  O. 
Waldeyer,  L. 
Waldo,  C.  M. 


Walford,  L.  A. 
Walker,  A.  R.  P. 
Walker,  E.  P. 
Walker,  W.  G. 
Wallace,  B. 
Wallace,  J.  G. 
Wallace,  W. 
Wallace,  W.  M 


Wallart,  J. 
Walliker,  C. 
Walton,  W.  S, 


.  3,  165,  169 

.  2,  197,  199 

2,  33,  54 

.  3,  134,  143 

.      3,  77,  91 

.  5,  235,  243 

.  2,  203,  210 

.  2,  133,  144 

.  2,  133,  144 

.  1,  145,  161 

2,  777,  178,  179, 

180,  182,  183,  184,  187 

5,  159,  177,  214,  226 

3,  110,  114,  189 

.  5,  119,  133 

.  2,  148.  159 

.  5,  20 

.  3,  163,  168 

.  5,  189,  208 

4,  34,  58,  59, 

74,  75,  98,  116,  120,  129, 

132,   133,   134,   135,    136, 

137,    138,    152,    164,   224, 

225,  226,  227,  316 

.  1,  145,  161 

.1,  197,200 

.    3,  95,  100 


Wang,  H.   . 
Wang,  H.  W. 
de  Wardener,  H. 
Ward,  B.     . 
Warkany,  J. 
Warming-Larsen 
Warner,  F.  G. 
Warnock,  W.  M. 
Warren,  F.  L. 
Warren,  J.  V. 
Watchorn,  E. 


.      2,  34,  54 

.  2,  129,  144 

4,  54,  57,  150 

.  2,  30 
.  2,  165,  172 

.  4,  75 
.  4,  289,  298 

4,  309 

4,  202,  203,  205 

.  4,  271,  298 

4,100 


Watkin,  D.  M.  1,  206,  207;  3,  79,  88,  91; 
4,  226,  231,  233,  244,  246 


Watson,  M. 
Watts,  R.  M. 

Waymouth,  C. 
Weatherford,  H 
Weaver,  N. 
Weaver,  W. 
Webb,  C.  S. 
Weber,  R.  . 
Weidenreich,  F. 
Weil,  W.  B. 
Weir,  J.  F. 
Weisman,  R. 
Weismann  . 
Weiss,  J.  M. 
Weitnauer,  E. 
Weizel,  N.  C. 
Welford,  A.  T 


Wells,  H.  G. 
Wells,  L.  J. 
Welt,  L.  G. 
Wendt,  W.  E. 
Wener,  J.  . 
Wenner,  R. 
Werko,  L.  . 

Werther,  M. 
Wertman,  M. 
Wesman^  A.  G. 
Wesson,  L.  G. 
West,  A.  S. 
West,  C.     . 
West,  C.  D. 


Westman,  A. 
Weston,  H.  C. 
Weston,  R.  E. 
Wettstein,  A. 
Wetzel 
Weyer,  F.  . 
Weygand,  F. 
Wheeler,  N.  C. 
Whipple,  G.  H. 
White,  A.  G. 
White,  H.  L. 
White,  P. 
White,  V.  K. 
Whitehouse,  A. 


2,  86,  96 

1,  151,  158,  160; 

2,  162,  172 

1,191,  193 

L.       .  .2,  176,  183 

5,  237,  243 

2,  230,  232 

5,  104 

5,  43,  54 

5,  134 

4,  116 
.  4,95 

2,  235,  238 

.     3,4 

2,  101,  103 

5,  104 
1,  198,  201 

1,41,  48,  209,  210, 
213,  215;  3,  71,  149,  160, 
162,   169,   182,   183,   184, 
186,  187 
.  1,89 
2,  3,  10,  11,  12,  13 
.  4,  75 
.  4,  283,  296 

4,  297 
.  1,  151,  161 

4,  273,  279,  280,  283,  295, 

298 

.      4,  62,  73 

.  4,  301,  308 

.  3,  174,  182 

.  4,  277,  298 

.  5,  258,  264 

.  2,  206,  209 

4,  40,  46,  57,  120, 

121,  132,  134 

2,  49,  54,  145,  161 

3,  162,  164,  169 
.  4,  280,  295 

4,  94,  181,  195 

.3,184 

.  5,  234,  243 

.  5,  237,  243 

4,  275,  277,  295 
4, 132,  133,  135 

.  4,  99 
4,  256,  257,  259 

5,  267 
.  2,  141,  144 

G.  R.  .      4,  63,  73 


22 


Cumulative  Author  Index 


Whitney,  L.  F.     .  .  .1,  755,  161 

Widdas,  W.  F.  .  .  2,  118,  119,  120, 
125;4,  201,203,  205 

Widdowson,  E.  M.       .  1,  186,  195, 

198,  201;  4,  13,  96,  97, 
113,  114,  120,  121,  128, 
136,  136,  137,  160,  162, 
163,  164,  180,  195,  199, 
203,  205,  208,  209,  215, 
217,  219,  221,  222,  305, 
306,  308,  309 

Wieland,  P.  .         .         .4,  181,  195 


Wiesner,  B.  P.      . 
Wigglesworth,  V.  B. 

243, 
Wilde,  E. 
Wilde,  W.  S. 


Wilens,  S.  L. 
Wilkes,  A. 
Wilkins,  L. 
Wilkinson,  C.  F. 
Wilkinson,  E. 
Williams,  I.  T.  D. 
Williams,  J.  N.    . 
WilUams,  P.  C. 


Williams,  R.  G. 
Willis,  K.    . 
Willius,  F.  A. 
Wilson,  C. 
Wilson,  C.  P. 
Wilson,  I.   . 
Wilson,  J.  W. 
Wilson,  R.  C. 
Wiltner,  W. 
Wiltshire,  G.  H 
Wimsatt,  W.  A. 
Winiwarter,  H.  de 
Winnigstedt,  R. 
Winsatt,  W.  A. 
Winslow,  J.  A. 
Wise,  G.  H. 
Wislocki,  G.  B 


Witschi,  E. 
Wittwer,  S.  H.     . 
Wohlschlag,  D.  E. 
Woke.  P.  A. 
Wolfe,  J.  M. 
Wolff 

Wolff,  H.  P. 
Wolkoff,  K. 
Woll,  E.      . 


1,  152,  161 

5,  87,  133,  240, 

243,  245,  267,  285 

.  1,  198,  201 

2,  111,  112, 

113,  148,  159 

4,  253,  259 

5,  258,  264 
4,  79,  93 

3,  137,  143 
4,  6,  11 

4,  283,  295 

4,  133,  136 
2,  58,  59,  60,  63,  66, 

66,  67,  68,  145,  174,  175, 
213,   242,   243,   247,   249, 
252 
2,  91,  95,  96 
.  4,  283,  298 

3,  74,  75,  76,  91 
.  4,  258,  259 
.  4,  287,  296 
.      5,  21,  31 

2,  251 
.      1,  40,  48 

4,  284,  286,  296 
.  2,  204,  209 

2,  155,  158,  159 
.  2,  42,  54 
.  5,  59,  69 
.  2,  106,  114 

4,  275,  277,  295 
.  4,  200,  205 

2,  27,  98,  103,  104, 
105,  106,  107,  108,  109, 
110,  112,  113,  114,  115, 
116,  129,  144,  147,  159, 
176,  176,  177,  178,  179, 
180,  181,  182,  183,  184, 
184,  185,  185,  186,  187, 
191,  192,  198,  241,  243, 
246,  248,  249,  251;  4, 
217,  219 
2,  14,  15,  17 
.  5,  237,  243 

5,  145,  151, 177 
.  5,  255,  264 
.  1,  146,  161 

.  2,56 
4,  280,  292,  293,  298 

5,  110,  112 
1,  145,  161 


Wolstenholme,  G.  E. 
Wood,  G.  C. 
Wood,  J.  G. 
Wood,  M.  J. 
Wood,  T.  R. 
Woodford- Williams, 

Woodger,  J.  H.   . 
Woollard,  H.  H. 
Worcester,  J. 
Wright,  A.  W. 
Wright,  N.  C. 
Wright,  P.  L. 
Wrong,  O. 
Wurzel,  W. 
Wussow,  W. 
Wyburn,  G.  M. 
Wynn,  V.    . 
Wynne-Edwards,  V. 


W.       .         .5,  71 

3,  66,  68,  98,  100 

.  1,  203,  210 

3,  97,  100,  103 

.      3,  24,  29 

4,  111, 
112,  113,  180,  195 

.  2,  86,  96 
.  2,  189,  199 
.  4,  64,  73 
.  1,  146,  161 
.  5,  59,  69 
.  2,  81,  83 
4,  46,  57,  266,  268 
.  5,  59,  69 
5,  66,  67,  69 
.  2,  41,  53 
.  4,  145,  149 

5,  193, 
194,  195,  208 


Yannet,  H. 
Yemm,  E.  W. 


Yerkes,  A. 
Yerkes,  J.   . 
Yerkes,  R. 
Yerushalmy,  J 
Yiengst,  M.  J. 


Yockey,  H.  P. 
Yokota,  T. 
Young,  F.  G. 
Young,  I.  M. 
Young,  L.  E. 
Young,  P.  T. 
Young,  W.  C. 
Young,  W.  F. 

Yudkin,  J. 


.  4,  106,  113 

.    2,  145,  202,  203,  204, 

208,  209,  210,  210,   211, 

212,  213,  214,  241,   242, 

249 

.  5,  119,  133 

.    3,  179,  182,  185 

.  5,  119,  133 

5,  21,31,  53,  54 

1,  206,  207;  3,  77, 

88,91;4,242,244,245, 

246,  247 

.  5,  129,  133 

.    5,  155,  163,  177 

162,  166,  167,  172,  174 

2,  221,  232,  236,  238 

.    2,236,238,239 

.  4,  166,  179 

1,  149,  161;  2,  41,  54 

4,  38,  40,  41,  56,  57,  59, 

59,  96,  162 

1,  192,  193;  4,  209,  219 


5,  267 

.      5,  53,  54 

.  5,  232,  243 

M.    .  .2,  179,  183 

3,  147 

5,  59,  60,  64,  69 

3,  190 

.  3,  133,  142 

.      2,  86,  96 

.    3,  95,  100 

2,  191,  192,  199 

1,  144,  145,  149,  160, 

161;   2,    15,    28,    31,    31, 

32,  33,  34,  35,  38,  49,  50, 

51,  53,  54,  55,  56,  57,  58, 

60,  65,  66,  66,  67,  98,  126, 

184,   187,  201;  3,  127;  4, 

82,  93,  251,  259 

Zweifach,  B.  W.  .         .  4,  280,  295 

Zweymiiller,  E.    .         .4,  220,  221,  246 


Zahl,  P.  A. 
Zamyatin,  N 
Zander,  E. 
Zawadowsky,  M. 
Zeman,  F.  D. 
Ziegenhagen,  G 
Zilh,  A.       . 
Zinina,  N.  V. 
Zlotnik,  J.  . 
Zonneveld,  R.  J 
Zorzoli,  G, 
Zuckerman,  S 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


Figures  in  heavy  type  indicate  volume   number.     Figures   in   ordinary  type  indicate 

page  number. 


Acid-base  balance,  changes  in  due  to  age, 
4, 224-245 

development  of,  4,  209-223 

during  menstrual  cycle,  4,  93 

in  foetal  life,  4,  217-219 

in  old  age,  4,  242-243 
Accipitres,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Achievements  in  later  life,  1,  39,  40,  50,  51 
Achilles'  tendon,  age  changes  in  structure 

of,  3,  68,  69 
Acid  phosphatase,  in  placenta,  2,  107,  108 
Acipenser  fulvescens,  survival  curves  of,  5, 

143,  144 
Acipenser  ruthenus,  oldest  age  of,  5,  191 
Acipenserifonnes,  lifespan  of,  5,  220,  222 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  152 
ACTH,  administration  of  in  aged  schizo- 
phrenics, 1,  220-236,  237 

effect  on  adrenals,  4,  175 

effect  on  glycogen  storage,  2,  24-25,  29 

effect  on  hair,  2,  175 

effect  on  potassium  excretion,  4,  176, 
177,  178 

effect  on  schizophrenic  patients,  1,  229 

effect  on  sodium  excretion,  4,  176,  177, 
178 

effect  on  water  loss,  3,  176,  177,  178 

response  to  in  old  age,  1,  139 
Actuarial  aspects  of  human  lifespan,  5, 2-20 
Adaptation,  decHne  of,  3,  62,  63 

definition  of,  3,  61 

in  study  of  ageing,  3,  60-67,  71 
Adenosine  triphosphate,  in  placenta,  2, 
107,  108 

in  red  cells,  2,  238 
Adjustment  in  old  people,  1,  41,  42,  43,  46, 

47 
Adolescence,  water  and  electrolyte  changes 

during,  4,  80-81 
Adrenal  cortex,   activity   of,   in  elderly 
schizophrenic  patients,  1,  219-238 

mitochondria  in,  2,  101-102,  103 

relationship  with  pituitary  gland,  2,  22 
Adrenal    corticosteroids,     excretion    of, 

changes  due  to  age,  4,  91 
Adrenal  glands,  control  of  sodium  intake 
by,  4,  166 

effects  of  ACTH  and  cortisone,  4,  175- 
176 

effect  of  castration,  4,  197-198 

effect  on  diuresis,  4,  1 3 

mitochondria  in,  2,  101,  102 


Adrenal  hyperplasia,  effect  on  water  and 

electrolytes,  4,  79-80 
Adrenal  steroids,  biosynthesis  of,  1,  126- 
128,  129, 133 
effect  of  age  on  influence  of,  4,  192-194 
effect  on  kidney,  4,  257,  262 
effect  on  water  and  electrolyte  excre- 
tion, 4,  180-194,  196-198 
excretion  of,  age  changes  in,  1,  126-140 
relationship  with  urinary  ketosteroids, 
1,  128-130,  133-139 
Adrenalectomy,  effect  on  rats,  2,  58 
Adrenaline,  effect  on  water  diuresis,  4,  9, 
14 
in  emphysema,  1,  67 
Adrenosterone,  administration  of  in  old 

age,  1,  134-136 
Adults,  water  in  body  of,  4,  106-110 
Aedes  aegypti,  effect  of  diet  on,  5,  255 
Africans,  ageing  in,  3,  34,  104-114 
carcinoma  in,  3,  104,  106,  113 
liver  disease  in,  3,  188-190 
malnutrition  in,  3,  104-114,  145,  188- 

190 
mental  disease  in,  3,  145 
nutrition  in,  3,  188-190 
Age,  body  water  changes  due  to,  4,  IIQ- 
112,  114,  115 
causing  changes  in  acid-base  balance, 

4,  224-225 
causing  changes  in  effect  of  aldosterone 

on  urine,  4,  182-187 
causing  changes  in  effect  of  pitressin,  4, 

239-240 
causing  changes  in  extracellular  water, 

4,31,  110-112,  114-115 
causing  changes  in  glomerular  filtration 

rate,4,  231,238,  246 
causing  changes  in  haemoglobin,  4, 203, 

206,  207 
causing  changes  in  hormonal  control  of 

homeostasis,  4,  168-179 
causing  changes  in  intracellular  water, 

4,110-112,114,115 
causing  changes  in  nitrogen  excretion, 

4,  243 
causing  changes  in  oestrogen  excretion, 

4,91 
causing  changes  in  steroid  metabolism, 

4,  90-92 
cellular  changes  due  to,  4,  199-205 
changes  due  to  in  fish,  5,  218 


23 


24 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Age 

changes  in  ketosteroid  excretion  due  to, 

4,91 
effect  of  fecundity  in  fish,  5,  198-200, 

201 
effect  on  blood  volume,  4,  243 
effect  on  cells,  4,  199-205 
effect  on  deer  antler  growth,  2,  183-184 
effect  on  diuresis,  4,  6-10 
effect  on  fecundity  in  fish,  5,  201 
effect  on  homeostasis,  4,  139-153 
effect  on  influence  of  adrenal  steroids, 

4, 192-194 
effects  on  insects,  5,  247-268 
effect  on  kidney,  4,   11-12,  227-228, 

229-249,  253-254 
effect  on  oestrogen  excretion,  4,  91 
effect  on  renal  disease,  4,  250-263 
effect  on  reproduction,  5,  181-182 
effects  on  reproduction  in  fish,  5,  186- 

206 
effect  on  starvation,  4,  226 
effect  on  water  diuresis,  4,  238-240 
electrolyte  changes  due  to,  4,  241,  311- 

312 
erythrocyte  changes  due  to,  4, 199-205, 

207 
haemoglobin  changes  due  to,  4,  203, 

207 
parental,  and  lifespan,  5,  21-34 
pulmonary  effects  of,  4,  264 
renal  effects  of,  4, 11-12,  227-228,  229- 

249,  253-254 
Age  factor  in  prenatal  endocrine  events,  2, 

18-30 
Ageing,  actuarial  measurement  of,  1,  7-14 
adaptation  in,  3,  60-67,  71 
and  endocrine  glands,  2,  161-162 
biological  approach  to,  3,  2-17 
clinicopathological  tests  of,  3,  92-100 
definition  of,  1,  4-14,  55,  56,  57,  242, 

243,  244,  245;  2,  246-248,  249;  3,  73, 

93,  131-132,  191 
due  to  rapid  maturation,  1,  194-199 
effect  of  environment,  3,  35,  37,  45,  48, 

171,  183-184 
effect  on  skill,  1,  209-218 
emotional  changes  during,  3,  170-182, 

184 
genetic  factors  in,  1,  26,  27,  238-241 ; 

3,131,  132-133,  144;  5,  137 
genetics  of, 

in  Drosophila,  5,  278-280,  283 

twin  data  in,  3,  131-148 
horizontal  studies  of,  3,  95 
in  Africans,  3,  104-114 
in  Drosophila,  5,  269-285 
in  leaves,  2,  202-214 
in  red  cells,  2,  233-238 
intelligence  changes  in,  3,  170-187 
longitudinal  studies  of,  3,  95 
measurement  of,  1,  4-15;  3,  6-8 
mental  aspects  of,  1,  32-52 
molecular  changes  in,  5,  129-131 


Ageing 

neglected  areas  in,  research  on,  1,  173- 
185 

nutritional  aspects  of,  1,  186-193 

pathological  basis  of,  1,  16-31 

performance  changes  in,  3,  149-169 

personal  measurement  of,  1,  5-7 

psychological  aspects  of,  1,  209-218 

physiological  approach  to,  3,  20-29 

physiological  changes  due  to  in  fish,  5, 
181-211 

theory  of,  5, 1 29-1 3 1 ,  269-272, 28 1 ,  282, 
297 

use  of  inbred  strains  in  research  into,  3, 
115-130 

use  of  various  animals  in  research  into, 
1, 177-181 

variability  in,  3,  32,  33 
Alanine,  in  elastin,  1,  95,  96 
Albatross,  lifespan  of,  5,  100,  103 
Albumin,  effect  on  bees,  5,  235 
Aldosterone,  4,  59,  60 

effect  on  potassium  excretion,  4,  183- 
184,  186,  192-194,  196-197 

effect  on  sodium  excretion,  4,  183,  185, 
192-194,  196 

effect  on  sodium/potassium  ratio,  4, 
184-185,  186-187,  192-194,  196 

effect  on  urinary  output,  4,  182,  192- 
194,  196 

excretion,  in  congestive  heart  failure,  4, 
280,  292-293,  298,  300 
in  pregnancy,  4,  89-90 
Algyria,  2,  100,  101 
Allantoic  fluid,  4,  217,  218 
Allometry  of  lifespan,  5,  125 
Alosa  sapidissima,  fecundity  of,  5, 191-192 
Alveolar   air,    contact   with    pulmonary 

blood,  measurement  of,  1,  64-65,  66 
Alzheimer's  disease,  3, 134,  135-136,  143- 

144,  146 
Amide  N,  in  elastin,  1,  96 
Amiiformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  222 
Amino  acids,  effect  on  bees,  5,  238 

in  aorta,  3,  97 

in  cells,  3,  47,  49 

in  elastic  tissue,  1,  91,  94-96,  104 

in  leaves,  2,  203,  206,  207,  212 

in  plants,  2,  212 

in  pollen,  5,  237 
Amoeba,  growth  and  development  of,  3, 

41-42 
Ammonia,  excretion,  4,  209-210,  213-215 

in  respiratory  acidosis,  4,  266 
Ammonium  salts,  in  metabolism,  4,  209- 

210 
Ammotragus  lervia,  lifespan  of,  3,  12,  13, 

14,  15 
Amphibia,  lifespan  of,  3,  9 
Amyloid,  effect  of  diet,  5,  79 
Amyloidosis,  in  mice,  3, 121, 122, 125, 126; 

5,80-81 
Anaemia,  erythrocytes  in,  4,  199 

in  animals,  3,  33,  34,  36 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


25 


Androgens,  effect  on  development  of  re- 
productive tract,  2,  10,  11 

effect  on  foetal  growth,  2,  173 

produced  by  ovary,  2,  15-16 
Androsterone,!,  128, 129, 130, 131, 133, 134 

excretion  of  in  old  age,  1,  134 
Aneurysm,  in  old  people,  1,  22 
Angelfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 

protein  metabolism  in,  5,  183-185 
Angiomata,  1,  82-84,  86 
Anglia  cattle,  lifespan  of,  5,  60 
Anguilliformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  223 
Anguilloidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  152 
Animal  populations,  control  of,  5,  296-297 
Animals,  ageing  process  in,  3,  20 

lifespan  of  {see  also  under  names  of 
animals),  3,  3,  8-11 
cause  of  decrease  in,  3,  4—6 
methods  of  obtaining  data,  3,  11-17 
Anions,  excretion  of,  4,  209,  210 

in  infancy,  4,  211-213 
Anseres,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Anseriformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Antibiotics,  effect  on  lifespan,  3,  36 
Antidiuretic  hormone,  4,  12,  37,  46,  47,  53, 
55,  92, 238-240 

in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  280 
Antlers,  deer  {see  Deer,  antlers  of) 
Anuria,  due  to  respiratory  infection,  4,  268 
Aorta,  amino  acids  in,  3,  97 

calcification  of  elastic  tissue  in,  1,  97-98 

calcium  in,  3,  97,  98 

degenerative  changes  in,  3,  97 

effect  of  elastase,  3,  98,  102 

elastin  content  of,  1,  92-93,  94,  101 

glycolysis  in,  1,  76 
Aortic  disease,  in  cancer  cases,  1,21 
Aortic  sclerosis,  age  incidence  of,  3,  76 
Aortic  tissue,  diffusion  of  solutes  through, 

1,  69,  72-74,  76-78 
Apis  mellifera,  5,  247 
Apocrine  sweat  glands,  ageing  of,  2,  188- 
201 

control  of  by  endocrine  glands,  2,  198 

distribution  of,  2,  188 

effect  of  age  on,  2,  194-198,  199 

effect  of  menstruation  on,  2,  196-198, 
199-200 

effect  of  pregnancy,  2, 197-198, 199-200 

glycogen  in,  2,  195 

in  children,  2,  198 

in  young  adults,  2,  190-194 

iron  content  of,  2,  191-193,  195 

lipid  content  of,  2,  191,  195 

mitosis  in,  2,  196,  199 

phylogeny  of,  2,  188 

pigment  in,  2,  190-194,  195 

ribonucleic  acid  in,  2,  194 

structure  of,  2,  189 
Apodiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Apples,  ageing  in,  2,  206,  208,  210 
Aqueous  humour,  concentration  of  ions  in, 

4,  25-26,  28,  29 
Arctic  char,  lifespan  of,  5,  147 


Arcus  senilis,  3,  109 

Ardea  cinerea,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 
Argentine,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 50 
Arginine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 
Armadillo,  oogenesis  in,  2,  39,  40 
Arterial  spiders,  1,  81,  82 
Arteries,  age  changes  in,  1,  53 

cholesterol  in,  3,  101 

degenerative  changes  in,  3,  97 

pressure  waves  in,  3,  69 
Arteriosclerosis,  1,  56;  3, 74-77,  80, 97-99, 
102,  134,  136,  137,  191,  193-194 

aetiology  of,  5,  107-108 

biochemistry  of,  1,  28 

brain  in,  5,  107 

calcification  of  elastic  tissue  in,  1,  97, 
98,  102 

cholesterol  in,  1,  78-79,  89 ;  5,  1 10-1 1 1, 
113 

elastase  in,  1,  99 

elastic  tissue  in,  1,  88,  89 

in  animals,  3,  23,  31,  32,  33 

in  birds,  5,  106-114 

in  cases  of  cancer,  1,  21 

in  centenarians,  1,  16 

in  dogs,  3,  32,  33 

in  fish,  5,  225 

in  mammals,  5,  109 

in  monkeys,  5,  108 

incidence  of,  in  old  people,  1, 18,  19,  20, 
21,  24,  25,  26 

site  of,  5,  110 
Ash,  in  rat  body,  4,  120-124 

relation  to  body  composition,  4,  118, 
122 
Aspartic  acid,  in  elastin,  1,  91,  94,  95,  96 

in  plants,  2,  212 
Asthma,  effect  on  lung,  1,  65,  66 

Barley  leaves,  protein  in,  2,  203-204,  205 

respiration  in,  2,  206 
Basal  metabolic  rate,  variations  with  age, 

1,206 
Bass,  hfespan  of,  5,  220,  221,  223 
Batrachoidiformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Bats,  Hfespan  of,  5,  97,  103 

metabolism  and  lifespan  in,  3,  27 
Bees,  ageing  in,  5,  248 

brain  cells  in,  5,  248 

effect  of  diet,  5,  254 

effect  of  protein  on,  5,  254 

in  tropics,  5,  246 

lifespan  of,  caged,  5,  235-239 
factors  influencing,  5,  231-246 
in  free-flying  colony,  5,  231-235 

physiological  condition  of,  free-flying, 
5,231-235 
caged,  5,  235-239 

yearly  life  cycle  in,  5,  239-240 
Beryciformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Bicarbonate,  excretion  of  in  respiratory 
acidosis,  4,  265-266 

in  pancreatic  juice,  4,  64 

in  parotid  saliva,  4,  64 


26 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Birds,  ageing  in,  3,  9 

arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  106-114 

expectation  of  life  in,  1,4 

in  research  on  ageing,  1,  177 

lifespan  of,  3,  9,  17,  37 
in  captivity,  5,  103 
in  Nature,  5,  90-105 

metabolism  and  lifespan  in,  3,  28 
Bison,  lifespan  of,  5,  105 
Blackfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Black-pied  cattle,  lifespan  of,  5,  60 
Bladder,  cancer  of  in  old  people,  1,  21 
Blatella  germanica,  in  research  on  ageing, 

1,  174 
Blennioidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 52 
Blermius  pholis,  growth  of,  5, 156, 157, 158 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  152 
Blenny  (see  Blennius  pholis) 
Blood,  ventilation  of,  1,  62,  63,  66,  68 
Blood-brain  barrier,  4,  26 
Blood  pressure,  age  changes  in,  3, 32, 33, 64 

changes  in,  3,  6 
Blood  supply  to  brain,  in  old  people,  1,  51 
Blood  volume,  effect  of  age,  4,  243 
Blue-head,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Blue-striped  grunt,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Body  composition,  effect  of  protein  and 

mineral  intake  on,  4,  116-138 
Body  weight,  relationship  to  lifespan,  5, 

115-139 
Bone,  magnesium  in,  4,  309 

molecular  changes  in,  3,  47,  48 
Boselaphus  tragocamelus,  lifespan  of,  3, 

12,  13 
Bowfin,  lifespan  of,  5,  222 
Brain,  blood  supply  to  in  old  people,  1,  51 

changes  in,  in  Alzheimer's  disease,  3, 
135-136.  143-144,  146 
in  old  age,  1,  34,  35,  36, 49,  51 ;  3, 145, 

146,  147 
in  Pick's  disease,  3, 135-136, 143-144, 
146 

in  arteriosclerosis,  5,  107 

internal  environment  of,  5,  133 

weight  of,  relationship  with  lifespan,  5, 
115-139 
Brain  cells,  in  bees,  5,  248 
Breast  cancer,  in  cattle,  5,  71 

in  centenarians,  1,  16 

in  mice,  3,  119,  120 
Bronchiectasis,  in  aged  rats,  1,  178-179 
Bronchitis,  and  emphysema,  1,  65,  66 
Bull  frog,  metabolism  and  lifespan,  3,  26 
Bullhead,  effect  of  diet  on,  5,  169 

lifespan  of,  5,  152,  223 

size  of,  5,  152 

survival  curves  of,  5,  143,  145 
Bulls,  causes  of  death  in,  5,  65 

lifespan  of,  5,  64-65 
Burbot,  lifespan  of,  5,  223 
Buteo  buteo,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 
Butterflies,  3,  25 
Butterfly  fish,  lifespan  of,  5,  121 
Buzzards,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 


Calcium,  absorption  of,  1,  110,  111-114, 

119-120,  124-125 

effect  of  diet  on,  4,  120,  121,  127-128, 
132,  138 

in  aorta,  3,  97,  98 

in  body  of  rat,  4, 120, 121, 127-128, 132, 
138 

in  elastin,  1,  97-98,  104,  105 

in  foetal  urine,  4,  218 

in  placenta,  2,  110 

requirements  of,  1,  109,  120-122 
Calcium  balance  studies,  1,  114-118 
Calcium  binding,  age  changes  in,  3,  67 
Calcium  metabolism,  in  old  age,  1, 109-125 

in  osteoporosis,  1,  109,  110,  116-118, 
121,  122,  123,  125 

in  rats,  1,  111-114,  124-125 
Callionymoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5, 1 52 
Callionymus  lyra,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  146 

survival  curves  of,  5,  143,  145 
Cancer,  {see  Carcinoma) 
Cancer  eye,  in  cattle,  5,  71 
Canis,  lifespan  of,  3,  12-13,  14,  17;  5,  134 
Capelin,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  150 

mortality  rate  of,  5,  146 
Capreolus  capreolus,  lifespan  of,  5,  92-94 
Carbohydrate,  in  pollen,  5,  237,  238 
Carbohydrate  metabolism,  endocrine  fac- 
tors in,  2,  24-25 
Carbon    dioxide,    diffusion    of   through 
aortic  tissue,  1,  72-74,  77 

diffusion  through  tentorium  cerebelli, 
1,  74-75 
Carbon  dioxide  production,  age  changes  in, 

3,  77,  84-90 
Carbonic  anhydrase,  4,  218,  223 

control  of  urinary  pH,  4,  210 
Carcinoma,  arteriosclerosis  in  cases  of,  1, 
21 

in  Africans,  3,  104,  106,  113 

in  animals,  3,  3 1 

in  fish,  5,  226 

in  inbred  strains,  3,  117,  118-119,  120, 
121 

incidence  of,  in  old  people,  1, 18, 19,  20, 
21,  24,  25-26 

sites  of  in  old  people,  1,  21-22 
Cardiac  output,  3,  84-86,  87,  191 

effect  on  kidneys,  4,  234,  248,  267 

in  congestive  failure,  4,  272,  276 
Cardiac  stroke  index,  age  changes  in,  3, 

87,88 
Caribou,  lifespan  of,  5,  92-94 
Castration,  4,  227 

effects  of,  2,  10 

effect  on  adrenals,  4,  197-198 
Cat,  hfetime  energy  expenditure  of,  5, 127 

lifespan  of,  5,  1 34 
Catfish,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154,  220, 

223 
Cattle,  breeding  of,  5,  57,  58 

lifespan  of,  5,  57-65,  70-71 
Caviar  lesions,  1,  84,  86 
Cavy,  lifespan  of,  3,  12,  13 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


27 


Cell(s),  age  changes  in,  1,  52-53,  54,  55; 
2,  250;  3,  39-50;  4,  199-205 

changes  in,  3,  43-44 

cold  storage  of,  2,  215,  216 

eflfect  of  environment  on,  3,  45 

effect  of  nutrition  on,  1,  191 

effect  of  temperature  on,  5,  281 

electrolytes  and  water  in,  4,  15-35 

electrolyte  transfer  in,  4,  19 

enzymes  in,  3,  47,  48 

function  of,  3,  41 

in  testis,  2,  86-99 

lifespan  of,  2,  216,  227;  3,  5 

metabohsm,  2,  216,  217 

mutation  in,  3,  43,  45 

nuclear  transplantation  in,  3,  46 

nucleic  acids  in,  3,  47,  48 

nucleus  of,  2,  239-240,  241 

osmotic  equilibrium  of,  4,  1 8 

oxygen  in  metabolism,  2,  243 

proliferation  of,  3,  41-42,  46 

proteins  in,  3,  48,  49 

replacement  of,  3,  4,  5,  6,  8 
Cell  membrane,  permeability  of,  4,  16-31 
Cell  respiration,  relationship  with  protein 

metabolism,  2,  207-208 
Centenarians,  3,  93 

accuracy  of  age  of,  5,  13 

emphysema  in,  1,  16 

memory  in,  1,  25 

mental  state  of,  1,  25 

pathological  lesions  in,  1,  16-17 
Central  nervous  system,  control  of  vital 

functions  by,  5,  128-129 
Centrarchidae,  protein  metabolism  in,  5, 

182-183 
Cerebral  blood  supply,  in  old  people,  1,  51 
Cerebral   hypoxia,    in   congestive   heart 

failure,  4,  286 
Cerebral  vascular   disease,   in   cases   of 
cancer,  1,  21 

in  old  people,  1,  22 
Cerebrospinal  fluid,  concentration  of  ions 

in,  4,  25-26 
Cervus  elaphus,  lifespan  of,  5,  92-94 
ChaflSnch,  lifespan  of,  3,  9 
Char,  fecundity  of,  5,  191-192 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 52 
Characin,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Charadriiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Cherry  angiomata,  1,  82-84,  86 
Chickens,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  110,  113 
Children,  diet  for  spastic,  1,  183 

heights  and  weights  of,  1,  195,  201 

malnutrition  in,  1,  195 

water  in  body  of,  4,  103-106 
Chiropodomys  gliroides,  lifespan  of,  5,  96 
Chlorides,  effect  of  diet  on,  4,  120,  121, 
126,  132 

excretion  of,  in  congestive  heart  failure, 
4,  276,  277,  278,  284-285 

in  babies'  urine,  4,  21 1 

in  body  of  rat,  4,  120,  121,  126,  132 

in  erythrocytes,  4,  203 


Chlorides 

in  foetal  urine,  4,  217 

in  sweat,  4,  64,  74 

in  tears,  4,  64,  71 

loss  of  during  labour,  4,  90 
Chlorophyll,  in  nasturtium,  2,  203 
Cholesterol,  1,  126,  127 

in  aetiology  of  arteriosclerosis,  5,  110- 
111,  113 

in  arteries,  3,  101 

in  arteriosclerosis,  1,  78-79,  89;  5, 110- 
111,  113 

in  elastin,  1,  97-98 

in  placenta,  2,  122 

loss  of  from  red  cells,  2,  218,  219,  220 
Cholesterol  metabolism,  3,  191,  193-194 

and  ageing,  3,  133 

fatty  acids  in,  3,  193-194 
Cholinesterases,  age  activity  of,  5,  247 
Chorioallantoic  membrane,  4,  218 
Chorion,  membranous,  in  goat,  2,  151, 

153, 154,  156-158 
Chrysops,  lifespan  of,  5,  105 
Chub,  lifespan  of,  5,  150,  221 

size  of,  5,  1 50 
Chymotrypsin,  3,  101 
Ciconiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Circulation,  age  changes  in,  3,  77,  84-90 

effects  ofdeficiency  of  water,  4, 160, 163 

nervous  control  of  in  foetus,  2,  29 
Cirrhosis  of  liver,  3,  107,  108,  113 
Cisco,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  150 
Citellus  pygmaeus,  lifespan  of,  5,  97 
Citric  acid,  excretion  of,  4,  217,  218,  221, 

222 
Cladocera,  1,  30 
Cleft  palates,  produced  by  cortisone,  2, 

18-19 
Clinicopathological  tests  of  ageing,  3,  92- 

100 
Clupea  harengus,  growth  of,  5,  156,  157, 

158 
Clupea  pallasii,  fecundity  of,  5, 196-197 
Clupea  pilchardis,  3,  26 
Clupeiformes,  lifespan  of  5,  220,  222 
Clupeoidei,  lifespan  and  growth  in,  5, 160- 
166 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  148 
Clupeoids,  survival  curves  of,  5,  143,  145 
Coalfish,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  148 
Coates'  knifefish,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Cobalt  deficiency,  3,  188-190 
Cockroaches,  effect  of  diet  on,  5,  253-254 

in  research  on  ageing,  1,  174,  175 

hfespan  of,  5,  253-254 
Cod,  hfespan  and  growth  of,  5,  160-166 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  148 
Cold,  effect  on  cell,  4,  24 

effect  on  red  cells,  2,  224-229,  236-237, 
244 
Collagen,  3,  96,  97,  98 

age  changes  in,  3,  48,  65,  66,  68,  71 

contraction  of,  3,  66,  70 

in  elastin,  1,  91 


28 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Collagen 

in  placenta,  2,  146 

in  scar  tissue,  3,  70 
Collagen  fibres,  1,  88,  106,  107,  108 
Colon,  cancer  of  in  old  people,  1,  21 
Columbiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Comparative  age  studies,  3,  1-38 

clinicopathological  tests,  3,  92-100 
Compensatory  adjustment  in  ageing,  1,  25, 

27,41,42,43,46,47 
Congestive  heart  failure,  aldosterone  ex- 
cretion in,  4,  280,  292-293,  298,  300 

cerebral  hypoxia  in,  4,  286 

humoral  factors,  4,  279-288 

neural  factors,  4,  279-288 

renal  changes  in,  4,  275-279 

renal  function  in,  4,  271-275 

salt  and  water  retention  in,  4,  288-293 

water  and  electrolyte  metabolism  in,  4, 
271-300 
Connective  tissue,  age  changes  in,  3,  65 

water  and  electrolytes  in,  4,  27 
Coregonus  clupeaformis,  survival  curves 

of,  5,  143,  144 
Cormorants,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Coronary  disease,  in  cancer  cases,  1, 21, 22 
Coronary  sclerosis,  age  incidence  of,  3,  74, 

75 
Coronella  laevis,  fertility  of,  3,  21 
Cor  pulmonale,  renal  function  in,  4,  266- 

267 
Corpus  luteum,  effect  on  parturition,  2,  79, 
85 

effect  on  placenta,  2,  84 

effect  on  pregnancy,  2,  78,  79,  84 

effect  on  uterus,  2,  78-79 

growth  rate  of,  2,  79,  80 

of  guinea  pig,  2,  69-85 

in  guinea  pigs,  growth  rate  of,  2,  73,  75 
in  pregnancy,  2,  74,  75,  76,  84 
ovulation  changes  in,  2,  71-72,  83,  84 

lifespan  of,  2,  80-82 
Cortexone,  effect  on  potassium  excretion, 
4,  174-175 

effect  on  sodium  excretion,  4,  174,  175, 
177 

effect  on  water  loss,  4,  174,  175 
Corticosterone,  administration  of  in  old 

age,  1,  134-136 
Cortin,  excretion  of,  in  elderly  and  schizo- 
phrenics, 1,  221-222,  223-236 
Cortisol,  1,  127,  128 

administration  of  in  old  age,  1, 134-136 

effect  on  potassium  excretion,  4,  188 
189,  192-194,  196 

effect  on  sodium  excretion,  4,  187-188 
189, 192-194,  196 

effect  on  sodium/potassium  ratio,  4 
190-192,  193-194,  196 

effect  on  urinary  output,  4,  187,  188 
192-194,  196 
Cortisone,  effect  on  adrenal  glands,  4, 
175-176 

effect  on  diuresis,  4,  1 3 


Cortisone 

effect  on  hair,  2,  175 

effect  on  placenta,  2,  141 

effect  on  postnatal  growth,  2,  168-171 

effect  on  potassium  excretion,  4,  171, 

172,  176,  178 

effect  on  sodium  excretion,  4,  171,  172, 

173,  176,  178 

effect  on  water  loss,  4, 171,  172 
Cottoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  152 
Cottus  gobio,  survival  curves  of,  5,  143, 

145 
Cough,  effect  on  lung,  1,  65 
Cows,  average  age  of  different  breeds,  5, 59 

cause  of  death,  5,  63-64 

lifespan  of,  5,  58-64,  70 

lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  5,  127 
Cowfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Creatinine  excretion,  4,  249 

in  elderly  and  schizophrenics,  1,  221- 
222, 223-236 
Creatinuria,  in  rats,  5,  83 
Cristivomer  namaycush,  mortality  rates  of, 

5,145 
Croaking  gourami,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Cross  linking,  3,  66,  68,  69 

in  carcinogenesis,  3,  68 
Cross-sectional  studies,  3,  158-159,  171- 

172,  173,  174,  179,  186 
Crystalloids  of  Reinke,  2,  92,  94,  95 
Cunner,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Cyprinids,  metabolism  of,  5,  169 
Cypriniformes,  lifespan  of,  5, 220, 222, 224 
Cyprinodontiformes,  lifespan  of,  5, 1 52, 224 

size  of,  5,  1 52 
Cyprinoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 52 
Cystine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 

Dab,  fecundity  in,  5,  192,  200-202 
Dace,  lifespan  of,  5,  223 
Daphnia,  effect  of  metabolic  rate  on  life- 
span, 3,  24 
effect  of  nutrition  on  lifespan,  3,  24-25 
Dasyatis  akajei,  lifespan  and  growth  of, 

5,163 
Dasypus,  oogenesis  in,  2,  56 
Deaths,  accidental,  5,  17 
accuracy  of  age  at,  5, 12-13 
age  at,  3,  7 
Death  curves,  5,  6-9,  15,  17,  286-296 

of  horses,  5,  56 
Death  rate,  anticipated,  5,  4,  5,  7,  12,  15 
formula  for,  1,  8 
laws  governing,  5,  2-4 
senescent,  5,  4,  5,  6,  7,  10,  11,  12 
Decarboxylic  amino  acids,  in  elastin,  1, 94, 

95,  105 
Deer,  antlers  of,  absence  of,  2,  186 
blood  supply  of,  2,  177,  185 
effect  of  age  on,  2,  183-184 
effect  of  nutrition  on,  2,  184 
effect  of  testosterone  on,  2,  179-180, 

186 
enervation  of,  2,  178,  185-186 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


29 


Deer 

antlers  of  {continued) 

endocrine  factors  in  growth  of,  2, 

179-181 
growth  cycle  of,  2,  176-187 
mechanism  of  growth,  2,  176-178 
radio-phosphorus  uptake  in,  2,  177- 

178 
shedding  of,  2,  179,  181,  184,  211 

cyclic  gonadal  changes  in,  2,  179 

growth  in,  1,  205-206 

lifespan  of,  3,  12;  5,  91-94 
Dehydration,  effect  on  water  intake,  4,  4 

in  elderly,  1,  207 

in  labour,  4,  94,  95 
Dehydration  reaction,  4,  38-39,  47 
Dehydroe7J/androsterone,  1,  127,  128 
Dementia,  genetics  of,  3,  137,  147 

senile,  1,  35,  36,  44,  45,  46,  214 
de  Morgan's  spots,  1,  82-84,  86 
11-Deoxycortisol,  1,  127,  128 
Deoxypentose  nucleic  acid,  3,  47,  48,  49 
Diabetes  insipidus,  causing  loss  of  water, 

4,  39,  42-43 
Diarrhoea,  causing  hypematraemia,  4,  58 
Dibenamine,  effect  on  kidney,  4,  281,  282 
Diet,  effect  on  amyloid,  5,  79 

effect  on  bees,  5,  254 

effect  on  body  composition,  4,  117-138 

effect  on  cockroach,  5,  253-254 

effect  on  electrolytes,  4,  116-138 

effect  on  fertiUty,  5,  34 

effect  on  fish,  5,  178,  215-216 

effect  on  flies,  5,  249-255 

effect  on  growth,  4,  116-138;  5,  84-85, 
177,  178,  254-255 

effect  on  homeostasis,  4,  143-144 

effect  on  lifespan,  1, 30;  5, 78,  83-85,  87, 
88,  169,  177,  178,  249-255,  265-267, 
268,  282 

effect  on  mice,  5,  79-80 

effect  on  mosquito,  5,  255 

effect  on  rat,  5,  78,  83-85,  87,  88,  251- 
252,  254 

effect  on  sexual  maturity,  5,  84-85 

effect  on  trout,  5,  253,  254 

effect  on  wasps,  5,  255 

protein  in,  5,  252-254 
Diffusion  coefficients  of  solutes  for  mem- 
branes, 1,  69-79 
Digestive  disease,   incidence   of  in   old 

people,  1,  20 
Dingoes,  lifespan  of,  3,  12 
Dipodomys  heermani,  lifespan  of,  5,  96 
Disease,  effect  on  lifespan,  3,  31,  32;  5, 

72-89 
Diuresis,  effect  of  adrenaline,  4,  9,  14 

effect  of  age,  4,  6-10,  238-240 

effect  of  cortisone,  4,  1 3 

effect  of  hypoxia,  4,  8 

effect  of  pitressin,  4,  7-8,  1 1 

effect  on  adrenal  glands,  4,  1 3 

in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  272-273, 
275 


DOCA  (deoxycorticosterone  acetate), 

effect  on  foetus,  2,  24 
Doctor  fish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Dog(s),  in  research  on  ageing,  1,  182 
lifespan  of,  3,  12,  13,  14,  17;  5,  134 
lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  5,  127 
oogenesis  in,  2,  43 
Dog  snapper,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Dolichotis  patogona,  lifespan  of,  3,  12,  13 
Domestication,  effects  of,  3,  15,  37 
Dragonet,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 52 
mortahty  rate  of,  5,  146 
survival  curves  of,  5,  143,  145 
Drosophila,  effect  of  heredity  on  lifespan, 

1,  239-241 
Drosophila  melanogaster,  effect  of  tem- 
perature on  lifespan,  3,  24 
Drosophila  subobscura,  lifespan  of,  5,  262, 
266 
rate  of  ageing  in,  5,  269-285 
Drum,  hfespan  of,  5,  221 
Ducks,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Dwarf    top-minnow,    reproduction    and 
senescence  in,  5,  189 

Education,  effect  on  performance,  3,  1 59- 

160 
Eel,  lifespan  of,  5,  152,  223 

size  of,  5,  1 52 

natural  death  in,  5,  174 
Eggs,  effect  on  nutrition,  1,  205 
Egg  production,  variation  with   age,   3, 

21 
Elastase,  1,  98-101,  107;  3,  97-98,  101- 
103,  136 

in  arteriosclerosis,  1,  99 

in  fish,  1,  99 

in  pancreas,  1,  98,  99,  100 

preparation  of,  3,  101 

production  of,  3,  45 

tracer  studies  with,  1,  100-101 
Elastic  tissue,  ageing  of,  1,  88-108;  3,  65, 
66 

amino  acids  in,  1,  91,  94-96,  104 

in  atherosclerosis,  1,  88,  89 

in  skin,  1,  100-102,  105,  106,  107,  108 

preparation  of,  1,  90-91 
Elastin,  calcium  content  of,  1,  97-98,  104, 
105 

cholesterol  content  of,  1,  97-98 

in  aorta,  1,  92-93,  94,  101 

in  pulmonary  artery,  1,  93-94 

polysaccharides  in,  1,  104,  107 

preparation  of,  1,  90-91,  104 
Elastosis,  3,  96,  97 

senile,  1,  101-103,  106-108 
Electric  catfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Electric  eel,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Electrolytes    (see    also    under    Sodium, 
potassium,  etc.) 

cellular  aspects  of,  4,  15-35 

changes  in  due  to  age,  4,  241,  311-312 

deprivation  of,  4,  144 

effect  of  diet,  4,  116-138 


30 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Electrolytes 

effect  of  hormones,  4,  313-314 

effect  of  hypercapnia,  4,  265 

effect  on  mineral  content  of  body,  4, 

125 
effect  on  protein  body  content,  4,  125 
excretion  of,  in  elderly  schizophrenics, 
1,  223-237 
response  to  adrenal  steroids,  4,  180- 
194, 196-198 
glandular  secretion  of,  4,  62-77 
hormonal  aspects  of,  4,  78-98 
in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  271-300 
in  muscle,  4,  164 

in  parenteral  fluid  therapy,  4,  146-148 
in  pregnancy,  4,  88-90 
metabolism,  in  infancy,  4,  154-164 
regulation  of  by  kidney,  4,  229-249 
total  exchangeable  in  body,  4,  108 
Elephants,  lifespan  of,  1,  242 
Emotion,  changes  in  with  age,  3,  170-182, 

184 
Emphysema,  aetiology  of,  1,  65-66 
antispasmodics  in,  1,  67 
bronchitis  and,  1,  65,  66 
diffusing  capacity  in,  1,  64-65,  66 
in  centenarians,  1,  16 
pulmonary  ventilation  in,  1,  62,  63 
sex  distribution  in,  1,  68 
vital  capacity  in,  1,  58,  59,  60 
Endocrine  organs,  age  changes  in,  2,  161- 
162; 3, 128-129 
control  of  apocrine  sweat  glands  by,  2, 
198 
Endometrium,  pregnancy  changes  in,  2, 

115 
Environment,  effects  of,  3,  171 

effect  on  ageing,  3,  35,  37,  45,  48,  145, 

148,  171,  183-184 
effect  of  lifespan,  5,  167-168,  229 
effect  on  onset  of  disease,  5,  86-87 
Enzymes,  in  cells,  3,  47,  48 
in  rat  placenta,  2,  107-108 
in  red  cells,  2,  235,  241 
Enzyme  production,  3,  44,  45 
Eosinophils,  levels  of,  variations  with  age, 

1,223,231,232 
Ephedrine,  in  emphysema,  1,  67 
Epinephalus  guttatus,  protein  metabolism 

in,  5, 183-184 
Erythema,  palmar,  1,  82 
Erythrocytes,  electrolytes  and  water  in,  4, 
17-21,  199-208 
in  foetus,  4,  204,  205,  206 
in  sheep,  4,  200-203,  204,  206 
Ewes,  effect  of  nutrition  on,  2,  184 
Experience,  effect  of  on  performance,  3, 

156-158 
Extracellular     fluid,     equilibrium     with 
plasma,  4,  15-16 
volume  of,  changes  due  to  age,  4,  244 
Eyes,  changes  with  age,  1,  214;  3,  109,  195 

water  content  of,  4,  28,  29 
Eyesight,  3,  182-183 


Falconiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Fats,  in  body,  4,  113,  114,  115,  129,  132 

in  pollen,  5,  237 

in  rat  body,  4,  119 

storage  in  bees,  5,  237,  238,  239 
Fat  body  in  bees,  5, 233, 235, 237, 238,  239, 

244 
Fatigue,  in  old  people,  5,  150,  151 
Fatty  acids,  in  cholesterol  metabolism,  3, 

193-194 
Fecundity,  variation  of  in  fish,  5,  191- 

192 
Female,  longevity  in,  3,  30 
Fertility,  effect  of  age  on,  3,  20-22 

effect  of  parental  age  on,  5,  56 
Fibrin,  in  placenta,  2,  110 
Fibrinoid,  in  placenta,  2, 110 
Finch,  lifespan  of,  5,  99,  103 
Fish  {see  also  under  common  names) 

changes  in  due  to  ageing,  5,  218 

effect  of  diet  on,  5,  1 67 

effect  of  fat  diet  on,  5,  215-216 

effect  of  growth  and  size  on  lifespan,  5, 
147-159 

effect  of  toxic  substances  on,  5,  177 

egg  counting  in,  5,  192-193 

elastase  in,  1,  99 

fecundity  in  relation  to  age  in,  5,  186- 
206 

fertility  of,  3,  22 

growth  of,  utilization  of  protein  in,  5, 
182-186 

growth  and  senescence  in,  5,  217 

growth  rate  and  lifespan,  5,  227,  229 

in  nutritional  research,  1,  174,  175 

infectious  disease  in,  5,  213-214,  228 

lifespan  of,  1,  28,  29;  3,  9,  15-16 
characteristics  of  long,  5,  217-218 
in  captivity,  5,  212-230 
in  different  species,  5,  219-224 
in  Nature,  5,  142-180 

lipid  metabolism  in,  5,  227 

metabolic  disease  in,  5,  216 

metabolism  of,  5,  169-170 

metabolism  and  lifespan  in,  3,  25-26 

natural  death  and  reproduction  in,  5, 
170-174 

natural  mortality  of,  5,  142-147 

neoplasia  in,  5,  216 

nutrition  in,  5,  215 

ovaries  of,  5,  193 

parasites  in,  5,  213-216 

physiological  changes  due  to  ageing,  5, 
181-211 

protein  utilization  in,  5,  210 

relationship    of    age,    mortality    and 
growth  in,  5,  160-166 

survival  curves  of,  5,  143-147 

variation  in  fecundity  of,  5,  191-192 
Flamingoes,  effect  of  diet  on,  5,  112 
Flexure  lines,  1,  12-13 
Flies  {see  also  Houseflies) 

lifespan  of,  5,  105 
Flounder,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  150 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


31 


Fluids,  metabolic  disturbances,  reasons 

for,  4,  154-155 
Foetal  gigantism,  2,  112,  162,  164,  166, 

167,  171 
Foetus,  acid-base  balance  in,  4,  217-219 

blood  vessels  of,  radio-potassium  in,  2, 
160 

decapitation  of,  effects  of,  2,  12,  20-30 

effect  of  cortisone  on,  2,  167-175 

effect  of  DOCA  on,  2,  24 

effect  of  gonadotrophic  hormone  on,  2, 
28 

effect  of  growth  hormone  on,  2,  161- 
167,  171,  173-175 

effect  of  testosterone  on,  2,  173 

endocrine  events  in,  2,  18-30 

haemoglobin  in,  4,  203 

modification  of  growth  of,  by  growth 
hormone  and  cortisone,  2,  161-175 

urine  in,  4,  217 

weight  of,  2,  124,  125,  127 

effect  of  growth  hormone,  2,  164-165 
Follicles,  atresia  of,  2,  59-65 

changes  of  in  guinea  pigs,  2,  69 

effect  of  irradiation  on,  2,  67,  68 

effect  of  oestrogens  on,  2,  60-65 

inhibition  of  in  guinea  pigs,  2,  78 

redundant,  history  and  fate  of,  2,  59-68 

ruptured,  in  guinea  pigs,  2,  75-78 
Food,  conversion  of  in  fish,  5,  186 
Fowls,  fertility  of,  3,  20,  21 
Freezing,  resistance  of  tissue  to,  1,  163- 

165 
Fructose,  in  placenta,  2,  120-122 

production  in  placenta,  2,  120,  121 
Fruit,  ageing  in,  2,  206 
Function,  studies  in  human,  3,  73-9 1 
Functional  residual  capacity,   variations 

with  age,  1,  60 

Gadiformes,  growth  of,  5,  160-166 

lifespan  of,  5,  148,  160-166,  223 

metabolism  of,  5,  169 

size  of,  5,  148 
Galago,  oogenesis  in,  2,  56 
Galli,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Galliformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Gambusia   a.    affinis,    reproduction    and 

senescence  in,  5,  187-189 
Gannets,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Gar,  hfespan  of,  5,  222 
Gasterosteiformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  1 52,  223 
Gasterosteus  aculeatus,  3,  26 

effect  of  environment  of,  5,  168 
Geese,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Genes,  relation  with  macromolecules,  3, 

47 
Genetical  aspects  of  ageing,  1,  238-241 ; 
3,  131,  132-133,  144;  5,  137 

in  Drosophila,  5,  278-280,  283 
Germinal  epithelium,  proliferative  powers 

of,  2,  36,  37,  38 
Gibbs-Donnan  equilibrium,  4,  15-18,  27, 

28,30 


Gigantism  of  foetus,  2,  112,  162,  164,  166, 

167,  171 
Gingival  epithelium,  effect  of  brushing  on, 

3,  55-57,  71-72 
Glomerular  filtration  rate,  changes  in  with 

age,4,  231,238,  246 

effects  of  pyrogen,  4,  237 

in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  275,  277 
Glomerulonephritis,  in  man,  5,  77 

in  rats,  5,  74-76 
Glucose,    administration    of   in    elderly 
schizophrenics,  1,  220-236,  237 

diffusion  of  through  aortic  tissue,  1, 
72-74 

diffusion  through  tentorium  cerebelli, 
1,  74-75 

in  foetal  liver,  2,  136,  145 

in  placenta,  2,  135-137,  144,  145 

production  in  placenta,  2,  120,  121 
Glutamic  acid,  in  elastin,  1,  91,  94,  95,  96 

in  plants,  2,  212 
Glutamine,  in  senescent  leaves,  2,  203, 

212 
Glycerophosphatase,  in  placenta,  2,  107, 

108 
Glycine,  in  elastin,  1,  91,  94,  95,  96 
Glycogen,  in  apocrine  sweat  glands,  2,  195 

in  placenta,  2, 27-28, 107,  108, 122, 123, 
135-137,  144,  145,  159 
Glycogen  storage,  effect  of  ACTH  on,  2, 
24-25,  29 

effect  of  hormones  on,  2,  24 

inbees,  5,  237  238  239 

in  liver,  age  effects  of,  2,  23-25,  27,  29 
Glycolysis,  in  dog  aorta,  1,  76 
Goats,  lifespan  of,  3,  13,  14,  36 

placenta  of,  2,  120,  127,  154-158,  159 

placentome  of,  development  of,  2,  155- 
158,  159 

radio-potassium  uptake  during  preg- 
nancy, 2,  148-160 
Golden  stiver,  lifespan  of,  5,  223 
Goldfish,  lifespan  of,  3,  9 ;  5,  223 

metabolism  of,  5,  170 
Golgi  complex,  2,  88,  89 
Gompertz'  Law,  5,  2,  72,  302 
Gompertz-Makeham  equations,  5,117 
Gonadotrophic  hormone,  effect  on  foetus, 
2,28 

effect  on  reproductive  tract,  2,  1 5 

produced  by  pituitary  gland,  2,  21-22, 
28 
Gonadotrophin,  effect  on  ovary,  1,  150, 
151,  152,  153;2,  31,35,  55 

effect  on  sterilized  ovary,  2,  49,  51,  57 

ovulation  induced  by,  2,  75,  79,  82 
Grass,  protein  in,  2,  203 

respiration  in,  2,  206 
Grayling,  effect  of  environment  of,  5,  168 

metabolism  and  lifespan  in,  3,  26 
Grouper,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Growth,  2,  251;  5,  296,  298 

body  water  changes  due  to,  4,  103-106 

effect  of  nutrition  on,  1,  186,  188 


32 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Growth 

effect  of  diet,  4,  116-138;  5,  84-85,  177, 
178,254-255 

effect  on  electrolytes,  4,  160 

effect  on  lifespan,  1,  29;  5,  147-159, 
160-166 

in  mice,  4,  136 

in  rats,  4,  136 

relationship  with  lifespan,  5,  160-166 

utilization  of  protein  in,  5,  182-186 
Growth  hormone,  action  of,  1,  29;  2,  161- 
167, 171,  173-175 

effect  on  lifespan,  1,  29 
Growth  rate,  1,  6 

Grunion,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154 
Guinea  pig,  corpus  luteum  of,  2,  69-85 

fertility  of,  3,  20 

follicular  atresia  in,  2,  59,  60 

lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  5,  127 

oocytes  in,  2,  36,  39,  41,  42,  54-55 

placenta  of,  2,  110 

uterus  of,  1,  167 
Gums,  brushing  of,  3,  51,  55-57,  71-72 

keratinization  in,  3,  72 
Guppies,  effect  of  diet  on,  5,  1 69 

fecundity  of,  5,  209 

growth  of,  5,  229,  230 

lifespan  of,  3,  15-16,  30-31 

regeneration  in,  5,  208-210 

reproduction  in,  5,  171 
Gynaecomastia,  in  malnutrition,  3,  108- 

109 

Habrobracon  juglandis,  5,  255 
Haddock,  effect  of  age  on  fecundity,  5, 
198-200 

fertility  of,  3,  22 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  148 

relationship    of  fecundity    and    body 
weight,  5,  200 

sex  organs  in,  5,  190 
Haemoglobin,  changes  in  due  to  age,  4, 
203,  206,  207 

foetal,  4,  203,  206,  207 

in  sheep,  4,  202-203 

loss  of  from  red  cells,  2,  218,  219,  220 
Hair,  effect  of  cortisone  on,  2,  175 
Hake,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  148 
Halibut,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  148,  150 
Hamsters,  hypothermia  in,  1,  168;  2,  248 

in  research  on  ageing,  1,  177,  181 

lifespan  of,  1,  177 
Health,  effect  on  sampling,  3,  183,  184 
Hearing,  as  measurement  of  ageing,  1,  6 
Heart,  effect  of  potassium  on,  4,  95-96 

output  of,  3,  191 

changes  with  age,  3,  84-87 

stroke  index  of,  3,  87,  88 
Heart  disease,  in  Africans,  3, 104, 1 1 1-1 12, 
113 

in  man  and  rat,  5,  74-77 
Heart  failure,  congestive  {see  Congestive 
heart  failure) 

in  Africans,  3,  1 1 1 


Heat  regulation,  decline  with  age,  3, 63-64 
Hereditary  haemorrhagic  telangiectasis,  1, 

85,  105 
Heredity,  effect  on  lifespan,  1,  238-241 

role  of  in  ageing,  1,  26,  27,  48 
Herons,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 
Herring,  fecundity  of,  5,  196-197 

growth  of,  5,  156,  157,  158,  160-166 

lifespan,  5,  148,  156,  157,  158,  160-166 

relation    of   gonad    growth    to  body 
weight,  5,  193 

relation  of  size  and  maturity,  5,  172 

size  of,  5,  148 

survival  curves  of,  5,  143,  145 
Hesperoleucus  venustus,  effect  of  environ- 
ment on,  5,  168 
Meter andria  formosa,  reproduction  and 

age  in,  5,  189 
Hibernation,  effect  on  lifespan,  2,  249 ;  5, 

103-104 
Highland  cattle,  lifespan  of,  5,  59,  61,  62 
Hippocampus  hudsonius,  lifespan  of,   5, 

147 
Hippoglossoides  platessoides,  fecundity  in, 

5,  192 
Hippoglossus  spp.,  lifespan  of,  5,  147 
Histidine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 
Holocanthus  bermudensis,  protein  meta- 
bolism in,  5,  183-185 
Homeostasis,  disturbances  of  in  infants, 
4,  154-157 

effect  of  hormones  on,  4,  165-179 

of  water  and  electrolytes,  effect  of  age, 
4,  139-153 
Hormonal  environment,  effect  on  ovary,  1, 

142-146 
Hormones,  effect  on  development  of  re- 
productive tract,  2,  3-18 

effect  on  electrolytes,  4,  313-314 

effect  on  glycogen  storage,  2,  24 

effect  on  homeostasis,  4,  165-179 

ovarian,  2,  48-52 

sensitivity  of  ovary  to,  age  changes  in, 
1, 142-146 
Horses,  breeding  of,  5,  55 

causes  of  death  in,  5,  68 

coat  colour,  and  longevity,  5,  49-50 

death  curves  of,  5,  56 

lifespan  of,  under  various  climatic  con- 
ditions, 5, 65-69 

lifespan  of  English  thoroughbred,  5, 
35-56 

lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  5,  127 

survival  curves  of,  5,  37-42 
Housefly,  ageing  in,  5,  247-262 

effect  of  diet  on,  5,  249-255 

lifespan  of,  5,  249-262,  287-288 
effect  of  paternal  age,  5,  259-262 
sex  differences  in,  5,  255-259 

sex  ratio  in,  5,  249 
Humming-birds,  3,  28 

lifespan  of,  5,  105 
Hyalinization,  in  placenta,  2,  1 10 
Hydrogen  ion  gradients,  4,  34 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


33 


17-Hydroxycorticosteroids,  effect  on  water 

and  electrolytes,  4,  79 
1 7-Hydrox>T)rogesterone,  1,  127,  128 
Hydroxyproline,  in  skin  elastic  tissue,  1, 

88 
Hypercapnia,  renal  effects  of,  4,  265-266 
Hypercholesterolaemia,  3,  136 
Hypernatraemia,    and    cerebral    disturb- 
ances, 4, 36-44 

due  to  diarrhoea,  4,  58 

due  to  water  deficiency,  4,  38-44 
Hypertension,  renal  aspects  of,  4,  258 
Hypertonic  saline,  effect  on  hyponatrae- 

mia,  4,  50 
Hyponatraemia,  4,  44-55,  95 

and  cerebral  disturbances,  4,  36-37 

and  steroid  output,  4,  60 
Hypophysectomy,  effect  on  foetus,  2,  166 

effect  on  ovary,  2,  60-65 
Hypothalamus,  effect  on  thirst,  4,  37 
Hypothermia,  in  hamsters,  1,  168;  2,  248 
Hypoxia,  effect  on  diuresis,  4,  8 

Ide,  lifespan  of,  5,  223 
Inbred  strains,  3,  6-8 

and  parabiosis,  3,  116,  124,  127-129, 
130 

carcinoma  in,  3,  117,  118-119,  120,  121 

cause  of  death  in,  3,  120,  121 

development  of,  3,  115-119,  127 

in  experimental  gerontology,  3,   115- 
130 

lifespan  of,  3,  13,  119-120,  121 

ovarian  function  in,  3,  119-120,  126, 
127 

survival  of,  3,13 

transplantation  in,  3, 1 16, 120, 122-123, 
126,  129 
Index  of  cephalization,  5,  123,  135 
Industrial  performance,  studies  in,  3,  151, 

153,  164-167 
Infants,  electrolyte  metabohsm  in,  4,  78- 
79,  154-164 

water  metabolism  in,  4,  78-79,  154-164 

water  retention  in,  4,  96-98 
Lifection,  resistance  to,  as  measure  of 

ageing,  1,  6 
Infectious  disease,  in  fish,  5,  213-214 
Insects  (see  also  under  names  of  species) 

ageing  in,  5,  247-268 

in  nutritional  research,  1,  174,  175 

lifespan  of,  5,  105 

overwintering  in,  5,  232-234,  240 
Insemination,  artificial,  1,  171 
Insulin,  effect  on  placenta,  2,  141 
Insulin  sensitivity,  1,  237 
Intellectual  behaviour,  3,  175-177,  182 
Intelligence,  changes  in  with  age,  1, 36-39 ; 

3,  170-187 
Interstitial  cells,  in  testis,  2,  91-96 
Involutional  melancholia,  1,  44-45 
Involutional  psychosis,  3,  134 
Iodide,  diffusion  of  through  aortic  tissue, 

1,  72-74 

12 


Iron,  content  of  apocrine  sweat  glands,  2, 
191-193,  195 

in  heart  muscle,  3,  111,  112 

metabolism  of,  3,  109-110,  112 
Irradiation,  effect  on  lifespan,  5,  19-20, 
138-141,282,290,292 

effect  on  onset  of  disease,  5,  87,  88 

effect  on  ovary,  3,  127 

effect  on  thyroid,  3,  55 
Isoleucine,  in  elastin,  1,  91,  94,  96 

Jack,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Jacob-Creutzfeldt's  disease,  3,  134 

Kwashiorkor,  1,  190-191 
Kar>  oplasm,  2,  90,  91 
Keratinization,  in  gums,  3,  72 
Ketosteroids,  in  testis,  2,  98,  99 

in    urine,    relationship    with    adrenal 
steroids,  1,  128-130,  133-139 
17-Ketosteroids,  changes  in  excretion  due 
to  age,  4,  91 
daily  excretion  of,  1,  131-133 
excretion  of,  age  changes  in,  1,  126-140 
in  elderly  and  schizophrenics,  1,  221- 

222,  223-236 
in    elderly    subjects    treated    with 
steroids,  1,  134-136 
Kidney,  age  changes  in,  4,  234-235 
amyloidosis  in,  3,  122,  125,  126 
blood  content  of,  3,  52,  58 
blood  flow  in,  3,  191-192;  4,  248 

age  changes  in,  3,  88 
changes  in  due  to  age,  4,  227-228,  229- 

249,  253-254 
concentrating  ability  of,  changes  in  due 

to  age,  4,  11-12,243-244 
diseases  of,  effects  of  age,  4,  250-263 
effect  of  Dibenamine,  4,  281,  282 
effect  of  obesity,  4,  254,  255,  260 
effects  of  potassium  deficiency  on,  4, 

262-263 
effects  of  pyrogen,  4,  235-238 
effect  of  water  deficiency  of,  4,  43 
en2ymes  in,  4,  245 
function,  4,  229 

in  respiratory  failure,  4,  264-270 
variations  with  age,  1,  220 
glomerular  filtration  rate,  changes  due 
toage,  4,  231,248 
in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  275, 
277 
growthof,  4,  251-252 
hormonal  damage  to,  4,  256-258,  262 
in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  272,  273, 

276,  282,  283,  284,  287 
in  cor  pulmonale,  4,  266-267 
lesions  of,  causing  loss  of  water,  4,  39 
overloading  producing  senile  changes, 

4,  254-255,  260 
plasma  flow  in,  changes  in  due  to  age, 
4,  229-231,  235 
effect  of  pyrogen,  4,  237-238 
regeneration  of,  4,  252-253 


34 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Kidney 

role  of  in  water  and  electrolyte  regula- 
tion, 4,  229-249 
tubular  excretion,  changes  due  to  age, 
4,  233,  247 
Kidney  disease,  in  man  and  rat,  5,  74-77 
Killifish,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 

Labidesthes,  growth  of,  5,  156,  157,  158 
Labidesthes  sicculus,  mortality  rate  of,  5, 

146 
Labour,  dehydration  during,  4,  94,  95 
Lactate,    diffusion    of    through    aortic 

tissue,  1,  72-74 
Lactic  acid,  in  placenta,  2,  137,  141 
Lagonistica  senegala,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 
Lamniformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Larynx,  cancer  of  in  old  people,  1,  21 
Laws  of  mortality,  5,  2-5,  302-311 
Learning,  adaptation  in,  3,  62 

in  old  people,  1,  41,  215 

variation  with  age,  1,  215 
Learning  ability,  in  animals,  3,  62,  64-65 

in  human  beings,  3,  175-180 
Leaves,  amino  acids  in  senescent,  2,  203, 
206,  207,  212 

catabolism  of  protein  in,  2,  202-205, 
206,  207-208,  212,  214 

effect  of  light  on  ageing  of,  2,  211 

metaboUsm  in  senescent,  2,  202-214 

protein  catabolism  in,  2,  202-205,  206, 
207-208,  212,  214 

respiration  in  senescent,  2,  205-209, 
212,213,214 
Lebestes  reticulatiis,  eflFect  of  diet  on,  5, 
169 

fecundity  of,  5,  209 

growth  of,  5,  229,  230 

lifespan  of,  3,  15-16,  30-31 

regeneration  in,  5,  208-210 

reproduction  in,  5,  171 
Lemur,  oogenesis  in,  2,  39-40 
Lepidosteiformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  223,  224 
Leucichthys  kiyi,  survival  curves  of,  5, 143, 

145 
Leucichthys  sardinella,  metabolism  of,  5, 
169 

mortality  rates  of,  5,  145 
Leucine,  in  elastin,  1,  91,  94,  96 
Leuresthes  tenuis,  survival  curves  of,  5, 

143 
Leydig  cells,  in  testis,  2,  91,  92,  93,  94,  95, 

97,  98,  99 
Lipid(s),  content  of  sweat  glands,  2,  191, 
195 

in  aortic  membrane,  1,  74,  78 

in  Leydig  cells,  2,  93 

in  placenta,  2,  122 

loss  of  from  red  cells,  2,  218,  219,  220, 
223,  242 
Lifespan,  allometry  of,  5,  125 

and  delayed  maturation,  1,  27-30 

and  hibernation,  2,  249;  5,  103-104 

and  nutrition,  1,  186-193,  194-199 


Lifespan 

and  parental  age,  5,  21-34,  43-47,  53 
effect  of  antibiotics  on,  3,  36 
effect  of  basal  metabolic  rate  on,  3,  31 
effect  of  brain  and  body  weight,  5, 115- 

139 
effect  of  diet,  1,  30;  5, 169,  249-255,  282 
effect  of  disease  on,  3,  31 ;  5,  72-89 
effect  of  early  maturation,  1,  201-208 
effect  of  egg  laying  on  in  Drosophila,  5, 

275-278 
effect  of  environment,  5,  167-168,  229 
effect  of  growth  and  size,  5,  147-159, 

160-166 
effect  of  growth  hormone,  1,  29 
effect  of  hibernation,  2,  249;  5, 103-104 
effect  of  late  maturation,  1,  196-197 
effect  of  metabohsm  on,  5,  103-104, 

124-126,  129,  136-137,  169-170 
effect  of  mitotic  inhibitors,  5,  136 
effect  of  nutrition  on,  1,  186-193,  194- 

199;3,  24,  25,  28,  34,  35,  36 
effect  of  parental  age,  5,  21-34,  43-47, 

53,  259-262 
effect  of  radiation  on,  5,  19-20,  138- 

141,  282,  290,  292 
effect  of  reproduction,  5,  179,  275-279, 

284-285 
effect  of  temperature,  3,  24-25 
effect  of  temperature  on  in  Drosophila, 

5,  271-279,  283,  284 
effect  of  thyroid  gland  on,  5,  137 
effect  of  weight  on,  1,  203 
genetic  aspects  of,  5,  33,  55,  137 
hereditary  aspects  of,  1,  238-241 
increase  of,  1,  179,  217 
in  nineteenth  century,  5,  23,  32 
mathematical  basis  of,  5,  286-296 
measurement  of,  5,  133-134,  138-141 
methods  of  study,  5,  21-26 
of  Africans,  3,  105 
of  albatross,  5,  100,  103 
of  Ammotragus  lervia,  3,  12,  13,  14,  15 
of  Amphibia,  3,  9 
of  animals,  3,  3,  8-11 

cause  of  decrease  in,  3,  4—6 

methods  of  obtaining  data,  3,  11-17 
of  Arabian  horses,  5,  42 
of  Ardea  cinerea,  5,  99 
of  bats,  3,  27;  5,  103 
of  bees,  factors  influencing,  5,  23 1-246 
ofbirds,  1,4;3,9,  17,  28 

in  captivity,  5,  103 

in  Nature,  5,  90-105 
of  bison,  5,  105 

of  Boselaphus  tragocamelus,  3,  12-13 
of  bull  frog,  3,  26 
ofbuUs,  5,  64-65 
of  Buteo  buteo,  5,  99 
of  buzzards,  5,  99 
of  Canis,  3,  12-13,  14,  17;  5,  134 
of  Capreolus  capreolus,  5,  92-94 
of  caribou,  5,  92-94 
ofcat,  5,  134 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


35 


Lifespan 

ofcattle,  5,  57-65,  70-71 

of  cavy,  3,  12,  13 

ofceIls,2,  216,  227;3,  5 

of  Cervus  elaphus,  5,  92-94 

of  chaffinch,  3,  9 

of  Chiropodomys  gliroides,  5,  96 

of  Citellus  pygmaeus,  5,  97 

of  cockroaches,  5,  253-254 

of  deer,  3,  12;  5,91-94 

of  dingoes,  3,  12 

of  dogs,  3,  12,  13,  14,  17;  5,  134 

of  Dipodomys  heermani,  5,  96 

of  Dolichotis  patagona,  3,  12,  13 

of  Drosophila  subobscura,  5,  262,  266 

of  elephants,  1,  244 

of  EngHsh  thoroughbred  horses,  5,  35- 

56 
offinch,  5,  99,  103 
offish,  1,28,  29;  3,  9,  15-16 

characteristics  of  long,  5,  217-218 

in  captivity,  5,  212-230 

in  Nature,  5,  142-180 

of  different  species,  5,  219-224 
of  flies,  5,  105 
ofgoats,  3,  13,  14,  36 
ofgoldfish,3,  9;5,  223 
of  guppies,  3,  15-16,  30-31 
of  Hafling  mares,  5,  43 
of  hamsters,  1,  177 
of  herons,  5,  99 
of  Hokkaido  ponies,  5,  43 
of  horses,  5,  35-56,  65-69 
of  housefly,  5,  249-262,  287-288 

sex  difi'erences  in,  5,  255-259 
of  human  beings,  actuarial  aspects, 

5,  2-20 
of  humming-birds,  5,  105 
of  inbred  strains,  3,  13,  119-120,  121 
of  insects,  5,  105 
of  Lagonostica  senegala,  5,  99 
of  Lebistes  reticulatus,  3,  15-16,  30-31 
of  Lipitsa  horses,  5,  43 
of  lizards,  3,  26,  27 
of  lupus,  3,  12 
of  mammals,  3,  9,  10 
of  mammals  in  Nature,  5,  90-105 
of  man  and  woman  compared,  5, 10-1 1, 

16 
of  Megadyptes  antipodes,  5,  100 
of  mice,  1,  177;  3,  9;  5,  95-96,  287 
of  Microtus,  3,  10 
of  mosquitoes,  5,  105 
of  Muntiacus  muntjac,  3,  12 
of  Mus,  3,  9 

of  Myotis  mystacinus,  5,  97 
of  nilghaie,  3,  12 
of  Odocoileus  hemionus,  5,  92-94 
ofowls,  5,  99 
of  Ovis  dalli,  5,  92-94 
of  Ovis  musimon,  3,  13,  14 
of  Paramecium,  3,  16 
of  parent  and  progeny  correlated,  5, 

47-49 


Lifespan 

of  Parus  major,  5,  100 

of  Passer  domesticus,  5,  99 

of  Passerines,  5,  99-100 

of  Perognathus,  3,  9,  10 

of  Peromyscus,  3,  9,  10 

of  Peromyscus  leucopus,  5,  95-96 

of  rabbits,  1,  181 ;  3,  11 ;  5,  97 

of  Rangifer  articus,  5,  92-94 

of  rats,  1,  177,  178,  179,  180;  5,  95-96, 
251-252 
eff'ect  of  diet,  5,  78 
eff"ect  of  disease,  5,  72-89 

of  red  cells,  2,  217,  233,  234,  238,  239, 
241,245 

of  sheep,  3,  12,  13,  14,  15;  5,  91-94 

of  shrews,  5,  97 

of  Sorex  araneus,  5,  97 

of  souslik,  5,  97 

of  sparrows,  5,  99 

of  Sterna  hirundo,  5,  97-99 

of  Strix  aluco,  5,  99 

of  swifts,  5,  100,  103,  104 

of  terns,  5,  97-99,  104 

of  tits,  5,  100 

of  Tokophyra,  1,  187,  191 

of  trout,  1,29;  5,222,  265 

of  twins,  3,  140,  141 

of  ungulates,  5,  91-95 

of  vertebrates,  3,  9,  10,  11-17 

of  wolfhound,  3,  12,  13 

of  wolves,  3,  12,  14 

relationship  to  index  of  cephalization, 
5,  123-125 

relationship  with  growth,  5,  160-166 

sex  difi'erences  in,  3,  30 

sexual  maturity  and,  1,  29,  30 
Life-tables,  1,  9-10;  5,  9-11 

limitations  of,  5,  11-12,  18 
Light,  effect  on  ageing  of  leaves,  2,  211 

efi'ect  on  deer  antler  growth,  2,  181- 
182 
Liver,  carcinoma  of,  3,  106,  107,  113 

foetal,  glucose  production  in,  2,  136, 
145 
relationship  with  placenta,  2,   108, 
109 

glycogen  storage  in,  age  efi"ects  of,  2, 
23-25,  27,  29 

in  fish,  5,  227 

mitochondria  in,  2,  101 
Liver  disease,  in  Africans,  3,   107-111, 
188-190 

infish,  5,  215-216 
Lizards,  metabolism  and  lifespan  in,  3, 26, 
27 

parasites  in,  3,  33 
Longevity  (see  also  Lifespan) 

onset  of  disease  and,  5,  72-89 
Longitudinal  studies,  3,  163-164,  172,  173, 

174 
Look-down,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Lophius  piscatorius,  elastase  in,  1,  99 
Lovettia  seali,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  146 


36 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Lung,  age  changes  in,  1,  65 

blood  flow  in,  1,  67 

cancer  of  in  old  people,  1,  21 

diseases  of,  3,  77-79 
in  rats,  5,  82 

effects  of  age  on,  4,  264 

elastic  resistance  of,  1,  58,  60,  61,  63, 
65 

function  of,  3,  77,  78 

mixing  of  air  in,  1,  68 

volume  of,  variations  with  age,  1,  58, 
60,  65,  66,  67,  68 
Lungfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Lupus,  lifespan  of,  3,  12 
Lytnantria  dispar,  3,  24 
Lymphocytes,  levels  of,  variations  with 

age,  1,224,231,232 
Lysine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 

in  plants,  2,  212 

Mackerel,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154 
Magnesium,  deficiency  of,  4,  301-310 
signs  of,  4,  304,  307,  309-310 

in  bone,  4,  309 

in  plasma,  4,  99-100 

in  rat  body,  4,  120,  121 
Makeham's  Law,  5,  2,  302 
Malayan  flying  barb,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
MaUotus  villosus,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  146 
Malnutrition,  eft'ect  of,  1,  205 

effect  on  body  fluids,  4,  156-157 

in  Africans,  3,  188-190 

water  metabolism  in,  4,  156-157 
Mammals,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 

lifespan  of,  3,  9,  10 
in  Nature,  5,  90-105 

life-tables  of,  5,  117-118 

relationship  of  brain  and  body  weight 
to  Hfespan,  5,  115-139 
Man,  disease  in,  effect  on  lifespan,  5, 72-89 

lifetime  energy  expenditure  of,  5,  127 
Mandibular  glands,  in  bees,  5,  233 
Manual    work,    performed    by     elderly 

people,  1,  216 
Maternal  age,  effect  on  lifespan,  5,  23-27, 

31,32 
Maturation,  definition  of,  1,  202 

delay  of  and  lifespan,  1,  27-30 

effect  of  nutrition  on,  1,  189-190 

effect  on  lifespan,  1,  27-30,  201-208 

rapid,  as  cause  of  ageing,  1,  194—199 
Mealworm,  effect  of  paternal  age  on  life- 
span, 5,  262 
Megadyptes  antipodes,  lifespan  of,  5,  100 
Melanogrammus  aeglefinis,  fecundity  of, 
5, 198-200 

sex  organs  in,  5,  190 
Membranes,     diffusion    coefficients    for 
solutes  for,  1,  69-79 

permeabilitv  of,  effect  of  enzyme  in- 
hibitors, i,  78 
Memory,  3,  187 

age  changes  in,  3,  65 

in  centenarians,  1,  25 


Memory 

in  old  people,  1,  40-41 
in  rats,  1,  215 
Menarche,  age  of,  1,  202 
Menopause,  ovarian  changes  in,  1,   144- 

146 
Menstrual  cycle,  acid-base  balance  in,  4, 
93 
effect  on  water  and  electrolytes,  4,  81- 

88 
sodium/potassium    ratios    during,    4, 
83-88 
Menstruation,  effect  on  apocrine  sweat 
glands,  2,  196-198,  199-200 
restoration  of  after  cessation,  1,  144- 
145 
Mental  ability,  adaptation  in,  3,  62 

decline  of  with  age,  3,  64—65 
Mental  aspects  of  ageing,  1,  32-52 
Mental  disease,  genetics  of,  3,  133-134, 
137,  144 
in  Africans,  3,  145 
in  old  people,  1,  45-46 
Mental  excitement,   due  to  magnesium 

deficiency,  4,  304,  307,  309-310 
Mental  testing,  3,  176,  181,  182,  183-184, 

186 
Mercury  poisoning,  excretion  of  sweat  in, 

4,99 
Mer tones  libycus,  3,  51,  57 
Metabolic  disease,  in  fish,  5,  216 
Metabolic  disturbances,  in  infants,  4,  1 54- 

164 
Metabolism,  comparison  between  infant 
and  adult,  4,  157-159 
effect  of  undernutrition,  1,  189 
effect  on  lifespan,  5,  103-104,  124-126, 

129,  136-137,  169-170 
in  senescent  leaves,  2,  202-214 
Metabolic  rate,  effect  on  lifespan,  3,  31 

relationship  with  ageing,  3,  23-28 
Methionine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 
Mice,  amyloidosis  in,  5,  80-81 
effect  of  diet  on,  5,  79-80 
eflfect  of  X-irradiation  on  ovary  of,  2, 

49-50,  57 
growth  in,  1,  204 
lifespan   of,   1,   177;  3,  9;  5,  95-96, 

287 
ovarian  changes  in,  1,  146 
white-footed,  lifespan  of,  5,  95-96 
Microtus,  lifespan  of,  3,  10 
Miller's  thumb,  lifespan  of,  5,  223 
Minerals,  in  pollen,  5,  237 

intake  of,  effect  on  body  composition, 
4, 116-138 
Minnow,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  152 
Mitochondria,  appearance  of,  2,  100 
division  of,  2,  103,  104 
in  adrenal  cortex,  2,  101-102,  103 
in  adrenal  glands,  2,  101,  102 
in  different  physiological  states,  2,  100- 

104 
in  Leydig  cells,  2,  93 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


37 


Mitochondria 

in  liver,  2,  101 

in  pancreas,  2,  101 

in  placenta,  2,  107 

in  yolk-sac,  2,  101 
Mitosis,  2,  250,  251 

in  apocrine  sweat  glands,  2,  196,  199 
Mitotic  inhibitors,  5,  136 
Moina  macrocopa,  3,  24 
Molecular  changes  in  ageing,  5,  129-131 
Mollusca,  3,  25 
Monkeys,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  108 

in  research  on  ageing,  1,  182 

menstruation  in,  1,  145-146 

oogenesis  in,  2,  44,  55 

ovulation  in,  2,  67 
Moonfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Mortality,  relation  with  age  and  growth, 
5,  160-166 

theory  of,  5,  127-129 
Mortality  rates,  5,  286-296 

laws  governing,  5,  2-5 

mathematical  models  for,  5,  302-311 

sex  differences  in,  5,  81 
Mosquito,  effect  of  diet  on,  5,  255 

lifespan  of,  5,  105 
Mosquito  fish,  mortality  rates  of,  5,  167 

reproduction  and  senescence  in,  5, 187- 
189 
Mothers,  age  of,  eflfect  on  lifespan,  5,  23- 

27,  31,  32 
Motivation,  3,  154-156,  185,  186,  187 
Mugiloidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 54 
MiiUerian  ducts,  normal  development  of, 

2,3,13-14 
Muntiacus  muntjac,  lifespan  of,  3,  12 
Mus,  lifespan  of,  3,  9 
Musca  domestica  {see  Housefly) 
Muscle,  analysis  of,  in  sodium  deficiency, 
4,  49-50 

composition  of,  4,  23 

electrolytes  in,  4,  21-22,  164,  224-225 

potassium  in,  4,  289-291 

power  of,  1,  137 

as  measure  of  ageing,  1,  6 

water  in,  4,  21-22,  113,  163-164 
Muscular  degeneration,  5,  77 

in  rats,  5,  74-76 
Muskallunge,  lifespan  of,  5,  222 
Mutation,  3,  43-44,  45 
Myleran,  effect  on  oogenesis,  2,  43 
Myocardial  degeneration,  5,  77 

in  rats,  5,  74-76 
Myotis  mystacinus,  lifespan  of,  5,  97 

Naevi,  spider,  1,  81-82 
Nasturtium,  proteins  in,  2,  203 
Matrix  natrix,  fertility  of,  3,  2,  22 
Naturalists,  longevity  in,  1,  52 
Neothinmus  macropterus,  fecundity  of,  5, 

193 
Nephrectomy,  effects  of,  4,  252,  255,  257 
Nephrosis,  in  man,  5,  77 

in  rats,  5,  74-76,  83 


Nerve  cells,  2,  250,  251 

in  bees,  5,  234,  244 

pigment  in,  3,  45 
Nervous  disease,  incidence  of  in  old  people, 

1,20 
Nervous  system,  adaptation  in,  3,  61 
Nilghaie,  lifespan  of,  3,  12 
Nitrogen,    diffusion    of   through    aortic 
tissue,  1,  72-74 

excretion  of,  changes  due  to  age,  4,  243 

in  pollen,  5,  236 

inrat  body,  4,  120,  121 
Normal,  definition  of,  3,  74,  94,  190-191 
Nucleic  acids,  in  cells,  3,  48,  49 
Nutrition,  and  ageing,  1,  186-193 

and  disease  in  Africans,  3, 104-1 14, 145, 
188-190 

effect  on  cell,  1,  191 

effect  on  deer  antlers,  2,  184 

effect  on  growth,  1,  186,  188 

eff"ect  on  lifespan,  1,  186-193,  194-199; 
3,  24,  25,  28,  34,  35,  36 

effect  on  pregnant  ewes,  2,  184 

in  Africans,  3,  188-190 

neglected  research  areas  in,  1,  173-185 

Obesity,  effect  on  kidney,  4,  254,  255 
Odocoileus  hemionus,  lifespan  of,  5,  92-94 
Oesophagus,  cancer  of  in  old  people,  1,  21 
Oestradiol,    effect    on    development    of 

reproductive  tract,  2,  11,  17 
Oestrogen,  effect  on  ovary,  2,  60-65 

effect  on  placenta,  2,  142 

effect  on  water  retention,  4,  79,  84,  86 

excretion  of,  changes  in  due  to  age,  4, 
91 

secretion  of,  2,  49-50,  51-52 
Oestrus,  in  X-irradiated  mice,  2,  49-50 
Oncorhynchus  nerka,  fecundity  in,  5,  197- 
198 

growth  of,  5,  156,  157,  158 
Oncorhynchus  spp.,  mortality  rates  of,  5, 

145 
Oocytes,  atresia  of,  2,  44-45 

decline  of  with  age,  2,  32 

effect  of  X-irradiation  on,  2,  49,  50,  51 

formation  of  in  ovary,  2,  32-48 

number  of,  2,  43,  47,  54-55 
Oogenesis,  1,  147-149;  2,  32-48,  55-56 

effect  of  Myleran  on,  2,  43 

effect  on  of  hypophysectomy,  2,  25,  41, 
44 

in  armadillo,  2,  39,  40 

in  Dasypus,  2,  56 

in  dog,  2,  43 

in  galago,  2,  56 

in  guinea  pig,  2,  36,  39,  41,  42 

in  lemur,  2,  39-40 

in  monkey,  2,  44,  55 

in  rabbit,  2,  36-37 

in  rats,  2,  42-43 

in  seal,  2,  37 
Ophiocephaliformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Orange  chromide,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 


38 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Organ  culture  studies  of  foetal  reproduc- 
tive tract,  2,  3-17 
Osier's  disease,  1,  85,  105 
Osmotic  diuresis,  4,  40 
Osteoporosis,  calcium  metabolism  in,  1, 
109,  110,  116-118,  121,  122,  123,  125 
senile,  incidence  of,  1,  125 
Ova,  age  changes  in,  3,  126-127,  149-150 
age  of,  1,  172 

age  of  at  time  of  ovulation,  1,  147-149 
ageing  of,  transplantation  techniques  in 

study  of,  1,  150-159 
lifespan  of,  1,  148,  149 
transfer  of,  1,  150 
Ovaries,  age  changes  in,  3,  1 19,  126 

transplantation  techniques  in  study 
of,  1,  150-159 
amyloidosis  of,  3,  125 
androgen  production  in,  2,  15-16 
changes  in,  1,  148 
changes  in  at  end  of  reproductive  life, 

1,  144-146 
changes  in  at  puberty,  1,  143-144 
changes  in  before  puberty,  1,  142-143 
effect  of  age  on  in  fish,  5,  201 
effect  of  gonadotrophin  on,  1, 150, 151, 

153;2,  31,35,  55 
effect  of  hypophysectomy  on,  2,  60 
effect  of  irradiation  on,  2,  34,  49-51; 

3,127 
effect  of  oestrogen  on,  2,  60-65 
formation  of  oocytes  in,  2,  32-48 
grafting  of,  1,  150-152,  154-157 

oocyte  survival  in,  2,  33 
growth  of  in  herring,  5,  195 
hormonal  secretions  of,  2,  48-52 
hypertrophy  of,  effect  on  oocytes,  2, 

34-35 
in  bees,  5,  233,  235,  237,  238,  239,  243 
in  Drosophila,  5,  275-277,  285 
in  fish,  5,  193 

changes  due  to  age,  5,  182 
life  of,  1,  142-147 
lifespan  of  cells  of,  1,  142 
low-temperature  storage  of,  1,  165 
regenerative  capacity  of,  2,  31-58 
sensitivity  of  to  hormonal  environment, 

1, 142-146 
sensitivity  to  hormones,  age  changes,  1, 

142 
transplantation  of,  1,  165-166;  3,  116, 

122-123,  126 
tumours  of,  development,  1,  153-154 
Overwintering,  5,  232-234,  240 
Ovis  dalli,  lifespan  of,  5,  92-94 
Ovis  musimon,  lifespan  of,  3,  13,  14 
Ovogenesis,  1,  172 

Ovulation,  age  of  ova  at  time  of,  1, 147-149 
in  guinea  pigs,  2,  71,  72,  75,  83,  84 
induced  by  gonadotrophin,  2,  75,  79,  82 
Owls,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 
Oxygen,    diffusion    of    through    aortic 
tissue,  1,  72-74,  77 
in  cell  metabolism,  2,  243 


Oxygen  consumption,  in  placenta,  2,  131- 

132 
Oxygen  pressure,  adaptation  to  changes, 

3,64 
Oxygen  requirements  of  tissue,  1,  76,  77 

Palmar  erythema,  1,  82 
Palometa,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Pancreas,  elastase  in,  1,  98,  99,  100;  3, 
101-102 
mitochondria  in,  2,  101 
Pancreatic  juice,  bicarbonate  in,  4,  64 
sodium  excretion  in,  4,  63,  65,  71 
urea  in,  4,  68-69 
Parabiosis,  1, 30, 170;  3, 116, 124, 127-128 
Paramecium,  lifespan  of,  3,  16 
Parasites,  in  fish,  5,  213-216,  228 
Parental  age,  effect  on  fertility  of  offspring, 
5,56 
effect  on  lifespan,  5,  21-34,  43-47,  53, 
259-262 
"Parental  death",  in  fish,  5,  189 
Parenteral  fluid  therapy,  4,  144,  146-148, 

151 
Parotid  saliva,  bicarbonate  in,  4,  64 
potassium  excretion  in,  4, 63, 64, 65,  74, 

75 
sodium  excretion  from,  4, 62-63, 65, 66, 

69,71 
urea  in,  4,  67-69,  75 
Parrots,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109,  110 
Parturition,  effect  of  corpus  luteum  on,  2, 

79,85 
Par  us  major,  lifespan  of,  5,  100 
Passer  domesticus,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 
Passeres,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Passerines,  lifespan  of,  5,  99-100 
Pelecaniformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Pelicans,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  109 
Penguin,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  100 
Peptic  ulcers,  incidence  of  in  old  people, 

1,19 
Perca  fluviatilis,  survival  curves  of,  5, 143, 

144 
Perch,  effect  of  diet,  5,  178 
lifespan  of,  5,  220,  223 
lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154 
survival  curves  of,  5,  143,  144,  145 
Perciformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  220,  223,  224 
Percoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 54 
Performance,  changes  in  with  age,  3, 149- 
169 
experiments  in  study  of,  3,  150-152 
in  old  people,  1,  42 
peak  of,  1,50-51 

problems  of  study  of,  3,  152-153,  156- 
159 
Perognathus,  lifespan  of,  3,  9,  10 
Peromyscus,  lifespan  of,  3,  9,  10 
Peromyscus  leucopus,  lifespan  of,  5,  95-96 
Personality  changes,  in  old  age,  1,  42-44 
Pharyngeal  glands,  in  bees,  5,  233,  235, 

237,  238,  239,  244 
Pharynx,  cancer  of  in  old  people,  1,  21 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


39 


Phenylalanine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 

Phosphate,  excretion  of,  in  elderly  and 
schizophrenics,  1,  221-222,  223-236 
excretion  in  respiratory  acidosis,  4,  266 
in  babies' urine,  4,  211,  213,  215,216 

Phospholipsin,  in  placenta,  2,  122 

Phosphorus,  effect  of  protein  intake,  4, 
121,  127-128 
excess  of,  4,  144,  145-146 
in  body  of  rat,  4,  120,  121,  127-128 
intake  of,  4,  142 

Phosphorylation,  in  plant  cells,  2,  208 

Physiological  function,  stability  of,  5,  145 

Pick's  disease,  3,  134,  135-136,  143-144, 
146 

Pigmentation,  role  of  in  ageing  processes, 
1,  53,  54 

Pigments,  in  nerve  cells,  3,  44 

Pigs,  effect  of  nutrition  on  growth,  1,  188 

Pike,  lifespan  of,  5,  222 

Pike-killie,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 

Pilchards,  3,  26 

Pilot  fish,  lifespan  of,  5,  22 1 

Pitressin,  effect  on  hyponatraemia,  4,  53- 
55 
effect  on  water  diuresis,  4,  7-8,  1 1 
variation  of  effect  due  to  age,  4, 239-240 

Pituitary  gland,  control  of  by  testis,  2,  20- 
21,28 
control  of  on  embryo,  2,  165,  166,  174 
effect  on  electrolytes,  4,  166,  167 
excision  of,  effect  on  oogenesis,  2,  35, 

41,44 
function  of,  age  factor  in,  2,  20-23 
gonadotrophic  hormone  produced  by, 

2,  21-22,  28 
relationship  with  adrenal  cortex,  2,  22 
relationship  with  thyroid,  2,  21 
thyrotrophic  hormone  produced  by,  2, 

21-22 
transplantation  of,  1,  152-153 

Pituitary  hormones,  effect  on  ovary,  1, 
142, 143, 144-145 

Placenta,  acid  phosphatase  in,  2,  107,  108 
adenosine  triphosphate  in,  2,  107,  108 
ageing  in,  biochemical  evidence,  2, 129- 
147 
morphological  aspects,  2,  105-117 
biochemical  changes  in,  2,  107-109 
blood  flow  in,  2,  125,  126 
calcium  in,  2,  110 
cholesterol  in,  2,  122 
collagen  in,  2,  146 
cytological  changes  in,  2,  107 
effect  of  corpus  luteum  on,  2,  84 
effect  of  cortisone  on,  2,  141 
effect  of  hormones  on  glucose  metabol- 
ism, 2,  141-142,  146 
effect  of  insulin  on,  2,  141 
effect  of  oestrogens  on,  2,  142 
enzymes  in,  2,  107-108,  141,  145 
fibrin  in,  2,  110 
fibrinoid  in,  2,  110 
fructose  in,  2,  120,  121,  122 


Placenta 

function  of,  chronological  changes  in, 
2, 118-128 

glucose  in,  2,  135-137,  144,  145 

glucose  production  in,  2,  120,  121 

glycerophosphatase  in,  2,  107,  108 

glycogen  in,  2,  27-28,   107,  108,  122, 
123,  130,  133-134,  137,  159 
effect  of  progesterone,  2,  123,  126 

hyalinization  in,  2,  110 

in  postmaturity,  2,  112-113 

in  toxaemia  of  pregnancy,  2,  112 

lactic  acid  in,  2,  137,  141 

length  of  gestation  and,  2,  110,  111 

lipids  in,  2,  122 

mitochondria  in,  2,  107 

of  goat,  2,  154-158 

of  guinea  pig,  2,  110 

of  rabbit,  2,  108 

of  rat,  2,  110 

oxygen  consumption  in,  2,  131-132 

permeability  of,  2,  118-119,  126 

permeability  of  barrier,  2,  109,  116 

phosphohpsin  in,  2,  122 

pregnancy  changes  in,  2,  110-111,  115 

pyruvate  in,  2,  137 

radio-potassium   uptake   of  in   preg- 
nancy, 2, 148-160 

relationship  with  foetal  liver,  2,  108, 
109 

sodium  transfer  in,  2,  111 

structure  of,  2,  105-106,  116 

succinic  dehydrogenase  in,  2,  107,  108 

surface  area  of,  2,  115 

villi  of,  in  goat,  2,  155-158 

weight  of,  2,  120,  124,  125,  127,  146 
Placentome,  in  goat,  uptake  of  radio- 
potassium  in,  2,  151-154,  159 
Plaice,  fecundity  of,  5,  202-206 

growth  of,  5,  157,  158,  159 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 50 

mortality  rates  of,  5,  167 

reproduction  and  growth  in,  5, 189-190 
Plants,  amino  acids  in,  2,  212 
Plasma,  concentrations  of  ions  in,  4, 25-26 

magnesium  in,  4,  99-100 

potassium  in,  4,  65 

sodium  in,  4,  65 

urea  in,  4,  67 
Plasma  proteins,  in  hver  disease,  3,  112 
Pleuronectes  platessa,    fecundity    in,    5, 
202-206 

growth  of,  5,  157,  158 

reproduction  and  growth  in,  5,  189- 
190 
Pleuronectoidei,  lifespan  and  growth  of, 
5,  161-166 

hfespan  and  size  of,  5,  148 
Pneumonia,  incidence  of  in  old  people,  1, 

19 
Poeciliidae,  effects  of  age  on  reproduction 

of,  5,  181,  186-189 
Pollen,  content  of,  5,  237 

effect  on  bees,  5,  236-240 


40 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Polysaccharides,  in  elastin,  1,  104,  107 
Pompano,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Population,    cross-sectional    surveys,    3, 
158-159,  171-172,  173-174, 179, 186 
horizontal  surveys,  3,  94-95 
longitudinal  surveys,  3,  95,  158,  163- 

164, 172-173,  174,  187 
sampling,  3,  73-79,  81,  94-96,  156-159, 
171-172,  174,  183-184 
Porcupine  fish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Porgy,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Postmaturity,  placenta  in,  2,  1 12-113 
Potassium,  accumulation  of  in  cell,  4,  32- 
33 
activity  of,  in  pregnant  rat  and  goat,  2, 

148-154,  159 
deficiency  of,  effects  due  to,  4,  140 
effects  on  kidney,  4,  262-263 
in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  289- 
292,  299-300 
deprivation  of,  causing  cellular  oedema, 

4,32 
effect  ofprotein  intake,  4, 121,  126, 129, 

133 
effect  on  heart,  4,  95-96 
excess  of,  effects  due  to,  4,  140,  141, 
144-146,  152 
in  adrenal  hyperplasia,  4,  80,  95 
exchangeable  amounts  in  body,  4,  108, 

109,  111,  114 
exchange  of  in  cell,  4,  18,  19,  20,  21,  22, 

24,  30,  34 
excretion  of,  after  water  loading,  4, 
167-169 
effect  of  ACTH  and  cortisone,  4, 

176,  177,  178 
effect  of  aldosterone,  4, 183-184,  186, 

192-194,  196-197 
effect  of  cortexone,  4,  174,  175 
effect  of  Cortisol,  4, 188, 189, 192-194, 

196 
effect  of  cortisone,  4,  171,  172,  176, 

178 
effect  of  vasopressin,  4,  170 
in  elderly  and  schizophrenics,  1,  221- 

222,  223-236 
in  parotid  saliva,  4,  63,  64,  65,  74,  75 
in  respiratory  acidosis,  4,  266 
in  sweat,  4,  63,  64,  65,  74,  76,  77 
in  body  of  rat,  4, 120,  121,  126,  129,  133 
in  erythrocytes,  4,  200-202,  204,  206, 

207,  208 
in  foetal  urine,  4,  218 
in  muscle,  4,  224-225,  289-292 
in  plasma,  4,  65 
in  saliva,  during  menstrual  cycle,  4, 

83-88 
loss  of,  4,  226 

during  labour,  4,  90 
ranges  of  intake,  4,  142 
Potassium  chloride,  effect  on  hyponatrae- 

mia,  4,  53 
Potassium  pump,  4,  204 
Poultry,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  110,  113 


Pregnancy,  aldosterone  excretion  in,  4, 
89-90 

duration  of  and  placenta,  2,  110,  111 

effect  of  corpus  luteum  on,  2,  78,  79,  84 

effect  of  cortisone  on  foetus,  2,  167-175 

effect  of  growth  hormone  on  foetus,  2, 
161-167,  171, 173-175 

effect  of  progesterone,  2,  126,  127 

effect  on  apocrine  sweat  glands,  2,  197- 
198, 199-200 

endometrial  changes  in,  2,  115 

placental  changes  in,  2,  110^-111,  115 

placental   uptake   of  radio-potassium 
during,  2,  148-160 

prolongation  of,  2,  163 

sodium  retention  during,  4,  88-89 

toxaemias  of,  2,  1 1 1-1 12,  142,  144 

uterine  uptake  of  radio-potassium  in, 
2,  148-160 

water  and  electrolyte  changes  in,  4,  88- 
90 

water  retention  during,  4,  88-89 
Premenstrual  oedema,  4,  81-83 
Preservation  of  tissue,  1,  162-172 
Procellariiformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5, 101 
Progesterone,  1,  127,  128 

effect  on  placental  glycogen,  2,  123,  126 

effect  on  pregnancy,  2,  126,  127 
Proline,  in  elastin,  1,  91,  94,  95,  96 
Prostate  gland,  cancer  of  in  old  people,  1, 
21 

in  female  animals,  2,  14,  15,  16 

normal  development  of,  2,  5-6,  9,  10, 
11,  14,20 
Proteins,  age  changes  in,  3,  66 

breakdown  of,  causing  osmotic  diuresis, 
4,41 

catabolism  in  senescent  leaves,  2,  202- 
205,206,207-208,212,214 

effect  of  age  on,  1,  103 

effect  on  bees,  5,  254 

effect  on  Drosophila,  5,  266-267 

in  barley  leaves,  2,  203-205 

in  bees,  5,  234 

in  cells,  3,  48,  49 

in  children's  diets,  1,  198 

in  diet,  5,  252,  253,  254 

in  elastin,  1,  94-96 

in  pollen,  5,  237,  238 

in  renal  disease,  4,  260-262 

intake  of,  effect  on  body  composition, 
4, 116-138 

storage  in  bees,  5,  237,  238,  239 

utilization  of  in  growth  offish,  5,  182- 
186,210 
Protein  metabolism,  relationship  with  cell 

respiration,  2,  207-208 
Protoplasm,  1,  244 

Protozoa,  nutrition  and  growth,  1, 186-187 
Pseudopregnancy,  3,  117,  126,  129-130 
Psittaci,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,   108-109, 

110 
Psychological  aspects  of  ageing,  1, 209-218 
Pudding  wife,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


41 


Puffer,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Pulmonary  artery,  calcification  of  elastic 
tissue  in,  1,  98 

elastin  content  of,  1,  93-94 
Pulmonary  oedema,  4,  284—285 
Pygosteus pungitius,  growth  of,  5, 156, 157, 

158 
Pyrogen,  effects  of  kidney,  4,  235-238 
Pyruvate,  in  placenta,  2,  137 
Pyruvic  acid,  in  urine,  4,  221 

Rabbit,  adrenal  cortex  of,  2,  22 

cottontail,  lifespan  of,  5,  97 

elastin  in  aorta  of,  1,  101 

in  research  on  ageing,  1,  181 

lifespan  of,  1,  181 ;  3,  11 ;  5,  97 

oogenesis  in,  2,  36-37 

ovulation  in,  1,  143 

pituitary  function  in  foetal,  2,  20-22 

placenta  of,  2,  108,  126 

renal  blood  content  of,  3,  52,  58 
Radioiodine,  deposition  of  in  thyroid   3, 

52-58 
Radio-phosphorus,    uptake    of    in    deer 

antlers,  2,  177-178 
Radio-potassium,  uptake  of  in  uterus  and 

placenta,  2,  148-160 
Rajiformes,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 54 

lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Rangifer  arcticus,  lifespan  of,  5,  92-94 
Rasbora,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Rat,  adrenal  cortex  of,  2,  22 

brown,  lifespan  of,  5,  95-96 

calcium  metabohsm  in,  1,  111-114, 
124-125 

causes  of  death  in,  5,  82-83 

disease  in,  effect  on  lifespan,  5,  72-89 

effect  of  diet  on,  5,  78,  83-85,  87,  88, 
251-252,  254,  265,  268 

effect  of  somatotrophic  hormone  and 
cortisone  on  foetal  growth  of,  2, 161- 
175 

foetal,  organ  culture  studies  in  repro- 
ductive tracts  of,  2,  3-17 

growth  in,  1,  204 

hypothermia  in,  1,  168 

in  research  on  ageing,  1,  178-180 

Kangaroo,  hfespan  of,  5,  96 

lifespan  of,  1,  177,  178-179,  180;  5, 
95-96,  251-252 

lung  disease  in,  5,  82 

maze  experiments  with,  1,  215-216 

memory  in,  1,  215 

nephrosis  in,  5,  83 

oestrus  in,  2,  56-57 

oogenesis  in,  2,  42-43 

ovarian  changes  in,  1,  146 

placenta  of,  2,  110 

placental  enzymes  of,  2,  107-108 

radio-potassium  uptake  during  preg- 
nancy, 2, 148-160 

redundant  follicles  in,  2,  59-68 

response  to  stimuli,  3,  51,  57 

thyroid  function  in,  3,  52-55 


Rattlesnake,  fertility  in,  3,  22 
Rat t us  rat t us,  lifespan  of,  5,  95-96 
Rays,  growth  of,  5,  163 

lifespan  of,  5,  154,  163,221 

size  of,  5,  154 
Rectum,  cancer  of  in  old  people,  1,  21 
Red  cells,  adenosine  triphosphate  in,  2, 
238 

ageing  in,  2,  233-238 

cholinesterase  activity  in,  2,  235 

dissolution,  2,  218-229 

effect  of  cold  on,  2,  224-229,  236-237, 
244 

enzymes  in,  2,  235,  241 

freezing  of,  2,  215-216,  236-237 

haemolysis  of,  2,  222,  225,  226,  227 

lifespan  of,  2,  217,  233,  234,  238,  239, 
241,245 

low  temperature  storage  of,  1,  164,  171 

membrane  of,  2,  222,  223 

metabolism  of,  2,  217,  227,  228,  240 

ofinfants,  2,  233,  239 

physical  instability  of,  2,  215-231 

preservation  of,  2,  215 

self-repair  in,  2,  234 

structure  of,  2,  223 
Red  hind,  hfespan  of,  5,  220 

protein  metabolism  in,  5,  183-185 
Regeneration,  in  guppies,  5,  208-210 
Regenerative  capacity  of  the  ovary,  2,  31- 

58 
Regression,  1,  33-34 
Reinke,  crystalloids  of,  2,  92,  94,  95 
Renal  artery  disease,  in  cases  of  cancer,  1, 

21 
Renal  blood  flow,  3,  193-194 

age  changes  in,  3,  88 
Reproduction,  effect  of  age  on  in  fish,  5, 
181-182,  186-206 

effect  of  nutrition  on,  1,  192 

effect  of  ovarian  transplantation  on,  1, 
158,  159 

effect  on  hfespan,  5, 179,  275-279,  284- 
285 

infish,  5,  170-174 
Reproductive   organs,    ageing   of,    tissue 

transplantation  techniques  in,  1,  141- 

161 
Reproductive  tract,  organ  culture  studies 

in,  2,  3-17 
Respiration,  in  barley  leaves,  2,  206 

in  senescent  leaves,  2,  205-209,  212, 
213,214 
Respiratory  acidosis,  4,  265-266 
Respiratory  disease,  incidence  of  in  old 

people,  1,  20 
Respiratory  failure,  renal  function  in,  4, 

264-270 
Respiratory  function,  effects  of  ageing  on, 

1,  58-68 

Retirement,  effects  of,  1,  49-50 

Rhinolophus,  3,  27 

Ribonucleic  acid,  in  apocrine  sweat  glands, 

2,  194 


42 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Roach,  lifespan  of,  5,  222 

Rockfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 

Rodents,  ageing  in,  3,  9-10,  11,  12-13 

Rosy  tetra,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 

Rubner's  theory  of  ageing,  5,  125-126,  128 

Rudder  fish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 

Sailfish,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154 
Saliva,  bicarbonate  in,  4, 63, 64, 65, 74, 75, 
83-88 

sodium  in,  4,  62-63,  65  66,  69,  71,  83- 
88 

sodium/potassium  ratios  in,  4,  94 

urea  in,  4,  67-69,  75 
Salivary  glands,  in  bees,  5,  233,  235,  237, 

238,  239 
Sahnon,  fecundity  of,  5,  192,  197-198 

growth  of,  5,  156,  157,  158,  160-166 

lifespan  of,  5,  150,  152,  160-166,  222 

mortality  rates  of,  5,  145,  166 

natural  death  in,  5,  174 

relation  of  size  and  maturity,  5,  172 

reproduction  in,  5,  179 

size  of,  5,  150,  152 
Salmo  gairdneri,  growth  rates  of,  5,  182 
Salmo  salar,  fecundity  in,  5,  197-198 
Svlmonoidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 50 

lifespan  and  growth  of,  5,  161-166 
Sahelinus  alpinus,  lifespan  of,  5,  147 
Sampling,  3,  74,  77,  79,  81,  171-172,  174, 

183 
Sand  dab,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  148 
Sardina  pilchardis,  effect  of  environment 

on,  5,  168 
Sardines,  eflfect  of  environment  on,  5,  168 
Sanger,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 54 
Scandinavians,  lifespan  of,  5,  21-26 
Scar  tissue,  collagen  in,  3,  70 
Scat,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Sceloporus,  3,  36,  27 

Schizophrenics,  adrenal  cortex  activity  in, 
1,  219-238 

cortin  excretion  in  elderly,  1,  221-222, 
223-236 

creatinine  excretion  in  elderly,  1,  221, 
222, 223-236 

effect  of  ACTH  on,  1,  229 

17-ketosteroid  excretion  in  elderly,  1, 
221-222,  223-236 

phosphate  excretion  in  elderly,  1,  221- 
222,  223-236 

potassium  excretion  in  elderly,  1,  221- 
222,  223-236 

sodium  excretion  in  elderly,  1,  221-222, 
223-236 

uric  acid  excretion  in  elderly,  1,  221- 
222,  223-236 
Scombroidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 54 
Sea  anemone,  1,  244 
Sea  horse,  lifespan  of,  5,  147 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154 
Seal,  oogenesis  in,  2,  37 
"Second  childhood",  1,  34 
Seeds,  ageing  of,  2,  211 


Seminal  vesicles,  normal  development  of, 

2,  5-6,  8-9,  10 
Senescence,  {see  also  Ageing) 

definition  and  measurement  of,  1, 4-15, 
31 
"Senile  death",  in  fish,  5,  189 
Senile  elastosis,  1,  101-103,  106-108 
Serine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 
Serotonin  metabolism,  3,  145,  146 
Sex  differences,  in  body  water,  4, 107-1 10, 
113 

in  growth  offish,  5,  159 

in  lifespan,  3,  30 

in  mortality  among  fish,  5, 145, 166-167 

in  reactions  to  environment  in  fish,  5, 
178 
Sex  distribution,  in  emphysema,  1,  68 
Sex  organs,  changes  in  due  to  age  in  fish, 
5,218 

in  fish,  5,  229 

relationship  to  body  weight  in  fish,  5, 
193-196 
Sexual  maturity,  age  of,  1,  202-203 

and  lifespan,  1,  29,  30 

effect  of  diet,  5,  84-85 

infish,  5,  170-171 
Shad,  fecundity  of,  5,  191-192 
Shark,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Sheep,  erythrocytes,  4,  200-203,  204,  206 

in  research  on  ageing,  1,  182 

lifespan  of,  3, 12, 13, 14, 15, 30;  5, 91-94 

placenta  in,  2,  126 
Sheepshead,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Shrews,  3,  28 

lifespan  of,  5,  97 
Siderosis,  3,  107-112,  188,  189 
Siluroidei,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154 
Size,  effect  on  lifespan,  5,  147-159 
Skeleton,  ageing  of,  1,  109-125 
Skill,  effect  on  performance,  3,  160-161 

variations  with  age,  1,  209-218 
Skin,  age  changes  in,  1,  53 ;  3,  96 

ectopic  flexure  lines  of,  1,  12-13 

elastic  tissue  in,  1,  100-102,  105,  106, 
107,  108 

elastic  tissue  of,  ageing  of,  1,  88-108 

temperature  of,  age  changes  in,  3, 80-84 

vascular  lesions  of,  incidence  of  in  old 
age,  1,  80-87 

water  absorption  by,  4,  100-101 
Skipjack,  metabolism  of,  5,  170 
Smelt,  lifespan  of,  5,  147 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  150 
Snakes,  fertility  of,  3,  21,  22 
Snake-head,  lifespan  of,  5,  224 
Snapper,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Social  adjustment,  3,  175 
Sodium,  and  adrenal  function,  4,  166 

deficiency  of  {see  also  Hyponatraemia) 
causing  hyponatraemia,  4,  44-45 

effect  of  ACTH  and  cortisone,  4,  176, 
177,  178 

effect  of  protein  intake  on,  4,  121,  126, 
132 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


43 


Sodium 

eflFect  on  water  intake,  4,  37 
excess  of  {see  Hyperaatraemia) 
exchangeable  amounts  in  body,  4,  108 
exchange  of  in  cell,  4,  18,  19,  20,  21,  22, 

24,  30,  34 
excretion  of,  4,  62-63 

after  water  loading,  4,  167-169 

during  exercise,  4,  279 

effect  of  aldosterone,  4,   183,   185, 

192-194,  196 
eflFect  of  cortexone,  4,  174,  175,  177 
eflfect  of  Cortisol,  4,  187-188,   189, 

192-194 
effect  of  cortisone,  4,  171,  172,  173, 

178 
eflfect  of  vasopressin,  4,  170 
in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  277, 

288,  289,  299 
in  elderly  and  schizophrenics,  1, 221- 

222,  223-236 
in  pancreatic  juice,  4,  63,  65,  71 
in  parotid  saliva,  4,  62-63,  65,  66,  69, 

71 
in  respiratory  acidosis,  4,  266 
in  sweat,  4,  62-63,  65,  66,  69,  71,  74, 

75,  76,  77 
in  tears,  4,  63,  65,  71,  75,  76 
in  body  of  rat,  4,  120,  121,  126,  132 
in  erythrocytes,  4,  200,  201-202,  203, 

206,  207,  208 
in  foetal  urine,  4,  217 
in  plasma,  4,  65 
in  saliva,  during  menstrual  cycle,  4,  83- 

88 
in  submaxillary  gland,  4,  71-72 
loss  of,  in  adrenal  hyperplasia,  4,  79-80 
ranges  of  intake,  4,  142 
retention  of,  in  congestive  heart  failure, 
4,288 
in  pregnancy,  4,  88-89 
transfer  of,  in  placenta,  2,  1 1 1 
Sodium/potassium  ratios,  during  menstrual 
cycle,  4,  83-88 
eflfect  of  aldosterone  on,  4,  184-185, 

186-187,  192-194,  196-197 
eflfect  of  Cortisol  on,  4,  190-192,  193- 

194, 196 
in  saliva,  4,  94 
Sodium  pump,  4,  203,  207 
Sole,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 50 
Sorex  araneus,  lifespan  of,  5,  97 
Souslik,  lifespan  of,  5,  97 
Soya  flour,  eflfect  on  bees,  5,  235 
Spadeflsh,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Sparrows,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 
Spermatids,  2,  87-96 
Spermatocytes,  division  of,  2,  87 
Spermatogenesis,  2,  86-91 
age  changes  in,  2,  97 
condensation  of  karyoplasm  in,  2,  90- 

91 
formation  of  aerosome  and  head  cap, 
2,  88-89 


Spermatozoa,  de%elopment  of,  2,  87-91 

low-temperature  storage  of,  1,  164,  171 
Sphenisciformes,  mortality  rate  of,  5,  101 
Spidernaevi,  1,  81-82 
Spirocercosis,  3,  33,  102 
Sprat,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  148 
Squirrel  fish,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Stallions,  causes  of  death  in,  5,  68 

lifespan  of,  5,  65-67 
Starvation,  eflfect  of  age,  4,  266 
Steady  state  system,  2,  216,  228 
Stentor,  3,  46 
Sterility,  in  cows,  5,  63,  64 
Sterna  hirundo,  lifespan  of,  5,  97-99 
Steroid  metabolism,  changes  due  to  age, 

4,  90-92 
Steroids,  administration  of  in  old  age,  1, 
134-136 

excretion  of,  in  urine,  1,  220 
Sticklebacks,  3,  26 

eflfect  of  temperature  on,  5,  168 

growth  of,  5,  156,  157,  158 

lifespan  of,  5,  152,  220,  223 

size  of,  5,  152 
Stimuli,  reactions  to  at  diflferent  ages,  3, 

51-58 
Stomach,  cancer  of  in  old  people,  1,  21,  24 
Strength,  in  old  age,  1,  137-138 
Stress,  5,  88 

eflfect  on  kidney,  4,  260 

recurrent,  1,  12,  13,  27 
Strigiformes,  mortahty  rate  of,  5,  101 
Strix  aluco,  lifespan  of,  5,  99 
Sturgeon,  growth  of,  5,  156,  157,  158,  163 

lifespan  of,  5,  147,  152,  163,  220,  222, 
226 

oldest  age  of,  5,  191 

reproduction  in,  5,  171 

size  of,  5,  152 

survival  curves  of,  5,  143,  144 
Sublingual  gland,  electrolytes  in,  4,  69-70 
Submaxillary  gland,  electrolytes  in,  4,  69- 

70 
Succinic  dehvdrogenase,  in  placenta,  2, 

107,  108 
Sugar,  eflfect  on  bees,  5,  238 

excretion   of,   in  elderly  and   schizo- 
phrenics, 1,  231 
Sunfish,  eflfect  of  environment  on,  5,  168 

lifespan  of,  5,  223 

protein  metaboUsm  of,  5,  182-183 
Sweat,  4,  100 

chlorides  in,  4,  64,  74 

in  mercury  poisoning,  4,  99 

potassium  excretion  in,  4,  63, 64,  65,  74, 
76,77 

sodium  excretion  in,  4,  62-63,  65,  66, 
69,  71,  74,  75,  76,  77 

urea  in,  4,  67-69 
Sweat  glands,   apocrine,   (see  Apocrine 

sweat  glands) 
Swifts,  lifespan  of,  5,  100,  103,  104 
Syngnathiformes,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5, 

154 


44 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Tarpon,  lifespan  of,  5,  220 
Tears,  chloride  in,  4,  64,  71 

sodium  excretion  in,  4,  63,  65,  71,  75,  76 

urea  in,  4,  67-69 
Temperature,   adaptation   to  change,   3, 
63,  64 

effect  on  bees,  5,  245,  246 

effect  on  cell,  5,  281 

effect  on  dissolution  of  red  cells,  2, 218- 
229 

effect  on  lifespan,  3,  24-25;  5,  167-168 
in  Drosophila,  5,  271-279,  283,  284 

effect  on  protein  metabolism,  5,  185- 
186 

effect  on  senescence  in  plants,  2,  210 
Tench,  lifespan  of,  5,  223 
Tenebrio  molitor,  effect  of  parental  age  on 

lifespan,  5,  262 
Tentorium  cerebelli,  diffusion  of  solutes 

through,  1,  74-75 
Terns,  lifespan  of,  5,  97-99,  104 
Testis,  ageing  in,  2,  97 

cytomorphosis  of  cells  in,  2,  86-99 

effect  on  development  of  reproductive 
tract,  2,  7-17,  19-20 

interstitial  cells  of,  2,  91-96 

ketosteroids  in,  2,  98,  99 

Leydig  cell  in,  2,  91,  92,  93,  94,  95,  97, 
98,99 

of  deer,  2,  179 

pituitary  control  by,  2,  20-21,  28 

transplantation  of,  3,  123 
Testosterone,  1,  128,  129 

administration  of  in  old  age,  1,  134- 
136 

effect  on  deer  antlers,  2,  179-180,  186 

effect  on  foetal  growth,  2,  173 
Tests,  mental,  1,  137 
Tests  of  ability  and  function,  1,  137 
Tetrodontiformes,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Thalamus,  in  old  people,  1,  35-36,  49 
Thirst,  effect  of,  4,  143,  144 

failure  of,  4,  41-43 
Thirst  centre,  4,  37 
Threonine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 
Thrombosis,  in  centenarians,  1,  16 
Thunniformes,  lifespan  and  growth  of,  5, 
163 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 54 
Thymallus  signifer,  3,  26 

effect  of  environment  on,  5,  168 
Thyroid,  functions,  3,  52-55,  58 

radio-iodine  uptake  in,  3,  52-58 

relationship  with  pituitary  gland,  2,  21 

transplantation  of,  1,  157-158 
Thyrotrophic     hormone,     produced     by 

pituitary  gland,  2,  21-22 
Time,  appreciation  of,  age  differences  in, 

1,217-218 
Tissue,  biophysical  changes  during  freez- 
ing, 1,  171,  172 

oxygen  requirements  of,  1,  76,  77 

preservation  of,  in  study  of  ageing,  1, 
162-172 


Tissue  changes,  in  old  age,  1,  55,  78 

in  old  people,  1,  27 
Tissue  transplantation  techniques,  in  the 

ageing  of  reproductive  organs,  1,  141- 

161 
Tits,  lifespan  of,  5,  100 
Toadfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Tobacco  leaves,  protein  in,  2,  203 
Tortoise,  ageing  in,  1,  28 
Totoaba,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154 
Toxaemia  of  pregnancy,  4,  88,  89-90 
Transplantation,  in  inbred  strains,  3,  116, 

120, 122-123,  126,  129 
Trigger  fish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Triton,  lifespan  of,  3,  9 
Trout,  effect  of  diet  on,  5,  169,  253,  254 

effect  of  reproduction  on,  5,  179 

fecundity  in,  5,  192,  197 

food  conversion  in,  5,  186 

growth  rates  of,  5,  182 

lifespan  of,  1,  29;  5,  150,  152,  222,  265 

size  of,  5,  150,  152 

mortality  rates  of,  5,  145 
Trypsin,  3,  101 

Tryptophan,  in  elastin,  1,  91,  96 
Tuberculosis,  in  cows,  5,  63,  64 
Tuna,  fecundity  in,  5,  193 

lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  1 54 
Turtles,  in  research  on  ageing,  1,  177 
Twins,  data  from,  in  genetics  of  ageing,  3, 
131-148 

lifespan  of,  3,  140,  141 
Tyrosine,  in  elastin,  1,  96 

Undernutrition,  effect  on  children's  weight, 
1,  195 

Ungulates,  Ufespan  of,  5,  91-95 

Uterus,  effect  of  corpus  luteum  on,  2,  78- 
79 
isolated,  1,  167 

low-temperature  storage  of,  1,  167 
radio-potassium  uptake  of  in  pregnancy, 
2,  148-160 

Urea,  excretion  of,  4,  40-41,  73 
in  pancreatic  juice,  4,  68-69 
in  parotid  saliva,  4,  67-69,  75 
in  plasma,  4,  67 
in  sweat,  4,  67-69 
in  tears,  4,  67-69 

Uric  acid,  excretion  of,  in  elderly  and 
schizophrenics,  1,  221-222,  223-236 

Urinary  disease,  incidence  of,  in  old  peo- 
ple, 1,  18,  19,  20 

Urine,  acids  excreted  in,  4,  210,  215-217, 
220-222 
ammonium  salts  in,  4,  209-210 
calcium  excretion  in,  1,  120,  121,  122, 

123 
cychc  changes  in,  1,  236-237 
inbabies,  4,  210-211 
in  foetus,  4,  217 

magnesium  excretion  in,  4,  305,  309 
output,  effect  of  aldosterone  on,  4,  182, 
192-194,  196 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


45 


Urine 

output  (continued) 

effect  of  Cortisol,  4, 187, 188, 192-194, 
196 
pH  of,  4,  209-210,  211-212,  215,  221, 

222 
potassium  in  during  menstrual  cycle,  4, 

84-88 
sodium  in,  during  menstrual  cycle,  4, 

84-88 
steroid  excretion  in,  1,  220 
Urogenital  sinus,  development  of,  2,  20 

Valine,  in  elastin,  1,  91,  94,  95,  96 
Variability,  3,  71 
Vascular  lesions  of  skin,  1,  80-87 
Vasopressin,  effect  on  electrolytes,  4,  167, 
170 

effect  on  water  diuresis,  4,  12,  13,  169, 
170,  195 
Venous  lakes,  1,85 
Venous    pressure,    in    congestive    heart 

failure,  4,  272,  274,  275 
Venous  stars,  1,  82,  86 
Ventilation,  measurement  of,   1,  62,   63, 

66 
Vertebrates,  lifespan  of,  3,  9,  10,  11-17 
Virus  infection,  in  leaves,  2,  21 1 
Vital  capacity,  variations  with  age,  1,  58, 

59,66 
Vitamin  A,  effect  on  kidney  function,  4, 

247 
Vitamin  B,  deficiency  in  animals,  3,  35 
Vitamin  B12,  importance  of,  1,  197,  198, 

199 
Vitamin  D,  in  calcium  absorption,  1,  110, 

111-114,  124 
Vitamins,  effect  on  bees,  5,  238 

in  pollen,  5,  237,  238,  239 
Vultures,  arteriosclerosis  in,  5,  110 

Walleye,  fecundity/length  relationship  in, 

5,  197 
Wasps,  5,  232,  255 
Water,  cellular  aspects  of  in  body,  4, 

15-35 
content,  control  of,  4,  10-11 

variations  with  age,  1,  207 
deficiency  of,  causing  hypernatraemia, 
4,  38-44 

effects  of,  4,  140,  160,  163 

in  children,  4,  160 

symptoms,  4,  39 
deprivati  o  n  of ,  effect  on  hyponatraemia, 

4,  51-52 
diuresis,  at  various  ages,  4,  6-10 

effect  of  adrenahne,  4,  9,  14 

effectofage,  4,  238-240 

effect  of  pitressin,  4,  7-8,  11 

in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  272- 
273,  275 
effect  of  load  in  rats,  4,  167-168 
effect  of  vasopressin  on  loss  of,  4,  169, 

170 


Water 

excess  of,  effects  due  to,  4,  46-47,  140, 
144,  145-146,  150,  151 
effect  on  diuresis,  4,  6,  8 
effect  on  hyponatraemia,  4,  52 
effect  on  urine  output,  4,  4 
in  children,  4,  159 
excretion,  during  exercise,  4,  279 

response  to  adrenal  steroids,  4,  180- 
194,  196-198 
exchanges  of  in  body,  4,  3 
extracellular,  changes  in,  3,  191,  192 
in  adults,  4,  106-110 
in  children,  4,  103-106 
variations  with  age,  4,  31,  110-112, 
114,  115 
in  body,  4,  102-115 

effect  of  age  on,  4,  110-112,  114,  115, 

180 
effect  of  growth,  4,  103-106 
measurements  of,  4,  102-103 
in  muscles,  4,  113,  163-164 
in  parenteral  fluid  therapy,  4,  146-148 
in  rat  body,  4,  119,  122,  123 
intake  of,  control  of,  4,  9-10 
intracellular,  and  cardiac  function,  3, 
88 
effects  of  age  on,  4,  110-112,  114, 

115 
in  adults,  4,  106-110 
in  children,  4,  103-106 
lossof,  4,  39-41,  195 
during  labour,  4,  90 
effect  of  ACTH  and  cortisone,  4, 176,. 

177,  178 
effect  of  cortexone,  4,  174,  175 
effect  of  cortisone,  4,  171,  172,  176,. 

178 
following  adrenalectomy,  4,  172 
metabolism,  hormonal  aspects  of,  4, 
78-98 
in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  271— 

300 
in  infants,  4,  96-98,  154-164 
in  malnutrition,  4,  156-157 
in  pregnancy,  4,  88-90 
movement  of  in  cell,  4,  19,  20,  22,  25, 

27-29,  34 
physiological  regulation  of,  4,  3-14 
ranges  of  intake,  4,  142 
regulation  of,  4,  37-38 

by  kidney,  4,  229-249 
retention  of,   eff'ects  of  oestrogen,  4^ 
79 
in  congestive  heart  failure,  4,  288 
in  pregnancy,  4,  88-89 
in  premenstrual  period,  4,  150,  151 
tolerance  to  excess,  4,  150,  151 
Water  load,  effects  of,  4,  170 
Wax  glands  in  bees,  5,  233,  239 
Weakfish,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Weight,  effect  on  lifespan,  1,  203 
Whitebait,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  150 
mortahty  rate  of,  5,  146 


46 


Cumulative  Subject  Index 


Whitefish,  growth  of,  5,  156,  157,  158 

lifespan  of,  5,  1 50,  222 

size  of,  5,  150 

survival  curves  of,  5,  143,  144 
Whiting,  lifespan  and  size  of,  5,  154 
Wolffian  ducts,  effect  on  Miillerian  ducts, 
2,16 

normal  development  of,  2,  5-6,  7-8, 10, 
19 
Wolfhound,  lifespan  of,  3,  12,  13 


Wolves,  lifespan  of,  3,  12,  14 
Wrinkling,  1,  12-13,  23,  27 

X-irradiation,  effect  on  follicular  atresia, 
2,  67,  68 
effect  on  oocytes,  2,  34,  46,  49-51 

Yellow  tail,  lifespan  of,  5,  221 
Yolk-sac,  mitochondria  in,  2,  101 


Printed  by  Spottiswoode,  Ballantyne  <&  Co.  Ltd.,  London  and  Colchester 


V