Skip to main content

Full text of "Mendelism"

See other formats


R. B. HINMAN 
COLLECTION 


PROFESSOR OF ANIMAL HUSBANDRY 
1921-1943 


New York 
State College of Agriculture 
At Cornell University 
Ithaca, N. Y. 


Cornell University Library 
- QH 431.P83 1912 


wi 


mann 


Cornell University 


Library 


The original of this book is in 
the Cornell University Library. 


There are no known copyright restrictions in 
the United States on the use of the text. 


http:/Awww.archive.org/details/cu31924003077785 


MENDELISM 


MACMILLAN AND CO., Limitep 
LONDON + BOMBAY - CALCUTTA 
MELBOURNE 


THE MACMILLAN COMPANY 


NEW YORK - BOSTON + CHICAGO 
DALLAS - SAN FRANCISCO 


THE MACMILLAN CO. OF CANADA, Ltp. 
TORONTO 


GREGOR MENDEL 


ABBOT OF BRUNN 


Frontispiece 


MENDELISM 


BY 


R. C. PUNNETT, F.R.S. 


FELLOW OF GONVILLE AND CAIUS COLLEGE 
PROFESSOR OF BIOLOGY IN THE UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE 


FOURTH EDITION 


MACMILLAN AND CO., LIMITED 
ST. MARTIN’S STREET, LONDON 


1912 


COPYRIGHT 


First Published 1905. 
Second Edition (revised) 1907. 
Reprinted 1908, 1909. 
American Edition 19c9. 
German Edition 1910. 
Swedish Edition 1911. 
Third Edition (entirely rewritten and much enlarged) 1911. 
Fourth Edition (revised) 1912. 


NOTE TO FOURTH EDITION 


THE chief alteration in the present edition concerns 
the Appendix to Chapter IX., which has been 
revised and extended in accordance with recent 
research. At the same time I have been able to 
correct sundry errors, typographical and other, which 
appeared in the last edition. To those who from 
time to time have been kind enough to point these 
out I would take this opportunity of expressing 


my thanks. 
R. C. P. 


CAMBRIDGE, August 1912. 


PREFACE 


A FEW years ago I published a short sketch of 
Mendel’s discovery in heredity, and of some of the 
recent experiments which had arisen from it. Since 
then progress in these studies has been. rapid, and 
the present account, though bearing the same title, 
has been completely rewritten. A number of illustra- 
tions have been added, and here I may acknowledge 
my indebtedness to Miss Wheldale for the two 
coloured plates of sweet peas, to the Hon. Walter 
Rothschild for the butterflies figured on Plate VI, 
to Professor Wood for photographs of sheep, and to 
Dr. Drinkwater for the figures of human hands. To 
my former publishers also, Messrs. Bowes and Bowes, 
I wish to express my thanks for the courtesy with 
which they acquiesced in my desire that the present 
edition should be published elsewhere. 

As the book is intended to appeal to a wide 
audience, I have not attempted to give more experi- 
mental instances than were necessary to illustrate 
the story, nor have I burdened it with bibliographical 
reference. The reader who desires further informa- 


tion may be referred to Mr. Bateson’s indispensable 
vii 


viii MENDELISM 


‘volume on Mendel’s Principles of Heredity (Cam- 
bridge, 1909), where a full account of these matters 
is readily accessible. Neither have I alluded to 
recent cytological work in so far as it may bear 
upon our problems. Many of the facts connected 
with the division of the chromosomes are striking 
and suggestive, but while so much difference of 
opinion exists as to their interpretation they are 
hardly suited for popular treatment. 

In choosing typical examples to illustrate the 
growth of our ideas it was natural that I should give 
the preference to those with which I was most 
familiar. For this reason the book is in some 
measure a record of the work accomplished by the 
Cambridge School of Genetics, and it is not unfair 
to say that under the leadership of William Bateson 
the contributions of this school have been second to 
none. But it should not be forgotten that workers 
in other European countries, and especially in 
America, have amassed a large and valuable body. 
of evidence with which it is impossible to deal in a 
small volume of this scope. 

It is not long since the English language was 
enriched by two new words—Eugenics and Genetics 
—and their similarity of origin has sometimes led to 
confusion between them on the part of those who 
are innocent of Greek. Genetics is the term applied 
to the experimental study of heredity and variation 
in animals and plants, and the main concern of its 


PREFACE ix 


students is the establishing of law and order among 
the phenomena there encountered. Eugenics, on the 
other hand, deals with the improvement of the 
human race under existing conditions of law and 
sentiment. The Eugenist has to take into account 
the religious and social beliefs and prejudices of 
mankind. Other issues are involved besides the 
purely biological one, though as time goes on it is 
coming to be more clearly recognised that the 
Eugenic ideal is sharply circumscribed by the facts 
of heredity and variation, and by the laws which 
govern the transmission of qualities in living things. 
What these facts, what these laws are, in so far as 
we at present know them, I have endeavoured to 
indicate in the following pages; for I feel convinced 
that if the Eugenist is to achieve anything solid it is 
upon them that he must primarily build. Little 
enough material, it is true, exists at present, but that 
we now see to be largely a question of time and 
means. Whatever be the outcome, whatever the 
form of the structure which is eventually to emerge, 
we owe it first of all to Mendel that the foundations 


can be well and truly laid. 
R. C. P. 


CAMBRIDGE, March 1911. 


CONTENTS 


CHAPTER I 


THE PROBLEM P ‘ 2 ‘ ‘ ‘ I 


CHAPTER II 


HISTORICAL . ; 5 5 : : ‘ 7 


CHAPTER III 


MENDEL’S WORK ; : . P 4 15 


CHAPTER IV 


THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE THEORY . ‘ 26 


CHAPTER V 


INTERACTION OF FACTORS : F : : . 39 


CHAPTER VI 


REVERSION... ‘ : ‘ , , ; ep ibe 


CHAPTER VII 


DOMINANCE . ; : : ; " : . 63 
x1 


xii MENDELISM 


CHAPTER VIII 


WILD FORMS AND DOMESTIC VARIETIES . 


CHAPTER Ix 


REPULSION AND COUPLING OF FACTORS . 


CHAPTER X 


SEX 


CHAPTER XI 


SEX (continued) 


CHAPTER XII 


INTERMEDIATES 


CHAPTER XIII 


VARIATION AND EVOLUTION 


CHAPTER XIV 


ECONOMICAL 


CHAPTER XV 


MAN 


APPENDIX 
INDEX . 


PAGE 


73 


81 


96 


III 


130 


146 


161 


175 
179 


ILLUSTRATIONS 


PLATE 


xiii 


PLATES 
PAGE 
Gregor Mendel ‘ : . , Frontispiece 
. Rabbits. ’ ‘ , To face 55 
. Sweet-Peas » 58 
. Sheep » 72 
Sweet-Peas a | a 
. Fowls » 102 
. Butterflies . » 139 
FIGURES IN TEXT 

Scheme of Inheritance in simple Mendelian Case . 19 
Feathers of Silky and Common Fowl . 27 
Single and Double Primulas 28 
. Fowls’ Combs 30 
. Diagram of Inheritance of Fowls’ Combs 34 
. Fowls’ Combs 36 

Diagram of F, Generation resulting from Cross 
between two White Sweet-Peas 43 
Diagram illustrating 9: 3:4 Ratio in Mice . 48 
. Sections of Primulas . : : 51 


xiv MENDELISM 


FIG, a PAGE 
to, Small and Large-eyed Primulas . ; : ‘ 2 
11, Diagram illustrating Reversion in Pigeons . : 61 
12. Primula sinensis x Primula stellata. x B 64 


13. Diagram illustrating Cross between Dominant and 


Recessive White Fowls . : ; ; 68 
14. Bearded and Beardless Wheat . : F 69 
15. Feet of Fowls . ‘ ; ‘ . . ; 71 
16, Scheme of Inheritance of Horns in Sheep. : Wel 
16A. The “ Cretin” Sweet-Pea : ‘ : go 
17. Abraxas grossulariata and var, lacticolor . ‘ 97 
18, Scheme of Inheritance in Adraxras . : : 99 


19. Scheme of Inheritance of Silky Hen x Brown Leg- 
horn Cock . . : , : ; 102 


20, Scheme of Inheritance of Brown Leghorn Hen x 


Silky Cock ‘ : : . . 103 
21. Scheme of F, (ex Brown Leghorn x Silky Cock) 

crossed with pure Brown Leghorn . ‘ F 104 
22. Scheme for Silky Hen x Brown Leghorn Cock 105 
23. Scheme for Brown Leghorn Hen x Silky Cock 106 
24. Diagram illustrating Nature of Offspring from 

Brown Leghorn Henx F, Cock. 3 . 107 
25. Scheme to illustrate Heterozygous Nature of Brown 

Leghorn Hen : : ‘ : , ‘ 108 
26. Scheme of Inheritance of Colour-blindness  . . 113 
27, Single and Double Stocks . : 3 : . 118 
28. F, Generation ex Silky Hen x Brown Leghorn 

Cock . : er . 5 : - 123 


29. Pedigree of Eurasian Family . : 3 » 126 


FIG. 


30. 
31. 
32. 
33. 
34. 
35- 


ILLUSTRATIONS 


Curve illustrating Influence of Selection 


Curve illustrating Conception of pure Lines . 


Brachydactylous and Normal Hands 
Radiograph of Brachydactylous Hand 
Pedigree of Brachydactylous Family 


Pedigree of Hemophilic Family . 


XV 
PAGE 
152 


154 
162 
163 
164 
166 


For although it be a more new and dif- 
ficult way, to find out the nature of things, 
by the things themfelves ; then by read- 
ing of Books, to take our knowledge up- 
on truft from the opinions of Philofo- 
phers: yet muft it needs be confef- 
fed, that the former is much more open, 
and leffe fraudulent, efpecially in the Se- 
crets relating to Natural Philofophy. 


Witiiam Harvey, 
Anatomical Exercitations, 1653. 


CHAPTER I 
THE PROBLEM 


A CURIOUS thing in the history of human thought, 
so far as literature reveals it to us, is the strange 
lack of interest shown in one of the most interesting 
of all human relationships. Few if any of the more 
primitive peoples seem to have attempted to define 
the part played by either parent in the formation of 
the offspring, or to have assigned peculiar powers 
of transmission to them, even in the vaguest way. 
For ages man must have been more or less con- 
sciously improving his domesticated races of animals 
and plants, yet it is not until the time of Aristotle 
that we have clear evidence of any hypothesis to 
account for these phenomena of heredity. The pro- 
duction of offspring by man was then held to be 
similar to the production of a crop from seed. The 
seed came from the man, the woman provided the 
soil. This remained the generally accepted view for 
many centuries, and it was not until the recognition 
of woman as more than a passive agent that the 
physical basis of heredity became established. That 
recognition was effected by the microscope, for only 
with its advent was actual observation of the minute 
I B 


2 MENDELISM CHAP. 


sexual cells made possible. After more than a 
hundred years of conflict lasting until the end of the 
eighteenth century, scientific men settled down to 
the view that each of the sexes makes a definite 
material contribution to the offspring produced by. 
their joint efforts. Among animals the female con- 
tributes the ovum and the male the spermatozoon ; 
among higher plants the corresponding cells are borne 
by the ovules and pollen grains. 

As a general rule it may be stated that the re- 
productive cells produced by the female are relatively 
large and without the power of independent move- 
ment. In addition to the actual living substance 
which is to take part in the formation of a new 
individual, the ova are more or less heavily loaded 
with the yolk substance that is to provide for the 
nutrition of the developing embryo during the early 
stages of its existence. The size of the ova varies 
enormously in different animals. In birds and 
teptiles, where the contents of the egg form the sole 
resources of the developing young, they are very 
large in comparison with the size of the animal 
which lays them. In mammals, on the other hand, 
where the young are parasitic upon the mother 
during the earlier stages of their growth, the eggs 
are minute and only contain the small amount of 
yolk that enables them to reach the stage at which 
they develop the processes for attaching themselves 
to the wall of the maternal uterus. But whatever 
the differences in the size and appearance of the 
ova produced by different animals, they are all 
comparable in that each is a distinct and separate 
sexual cell which, as a rule, is unable to develop 


1 THE PROBLEM 3 


into a new individual of its species unless it is 
fertilised by union with a sexual cell produced by 
the male. 

The male sexual cells are always of microscopic 
size and are produced in the generative gland or 
testis in exceedingly large numbers. In addition 
to their minuter size they differ from the ova in 
their power of active movement. Animals present 
various mechanisms by which the sexual elements 
may be brought into juxtaposition, but in all cases 
some distance must be traversed in a fluid or semi- 
fluid medium (frequently within the body of the 
female parent) before the necessary fusion can 
occur. To accomplish this latter end of its journey 
the spermatozoon is endowed with some form of 
motile apparatus, and this frequently takes the form 
of a long flagellum, or whip-like process, by the 
lashing of which the little creature propels itself 
much as a tadpole with its tail. 

In plants as in animals the female cells are 
larger than the male cells, though the disparity 
in size is not nearly so marked. Still they are 
always relatively minute since the circumstances of 
their development as parasites upon the mother 
plant render it unnecessary for them to possess any 
great supply of food yolk. The ovules are found 
surrounded by maternal tissue in the ovary, but 
through the stigma and down the pistil a potential 
passage is left for the male cell, The majority of 
flowers are hermaphrodite, and in many cases they 
are also self-fertilising. The anthers burst and the 
contained pollen grains are then shed upon the 
stigma. When this happens, the pollen cell slips 


4 MENDELISM CHAP. 


through a little hole in its coat and bores its way 
down the stigma to reach an ovule in the ovary. 
Complete fusion occurs, and the minute embryo of a 
new plant immediately results. But for some time 
it is incapable of leading a separate existence, and, 
like the embryo mammal, it lives as a parasite upon 
its parent. By the parent it is provided with a 
protective wrapping, the seed coat, and beneath this 
the little embryo swells until it reaches a certain 
size, when as a ripe seed it severs its connection with 
the maternal organism. It is important to realise 
that the seed of a plant is not a sexual cell but a 
young individual which, except for the coat that it 
wears, belongs entirely to the next generation. It is 
with annual plants in some respects as with many 
butterflies. During one summer they are initiated 
by the union of two sexual cells and pass through 
certain stages of larval development—the butterfly 
as a caterpillar, the plant as a parasite upon its 
mother. As the summer draws to a close each 
passes into a resting-stage against the winter cold— 
the butterfly as a pupa and the plant as a seed, with 
the difference that while the caterpillar provides its 
own coat, that of the plant is provided by its mother. 
With the advent of spring both butterfly and plant 
emerge, become mature, and themselves ripen germ 
cells which give rise to a new generation. 

Whatever the details of development one cardinal 
fact is clear. Except for the relatively rare instances 
of parthenogenesis a new individual, whether plant 
or animal, arises as the joint product of two sexual 
cells derived from individuals of different sexes. 
Such sexual cells, whether ova or spermatozoa, 


1 THE PROBLEM 5 


are known by the general term of gametes, or 
marrying cells, and the individual formed by the 
fusion or yoking together of two gametes is spoken 
of as a zygote. Since a zygote arises from the 
yoking together of two separate gametes, the indi- 
vidual so formed must be regarded throughout its 
life as a double structure in which the components 
brought in by each of the gametes remain intimately 
fused in a form of partnership. But when the 
zygote in its turn comes to form gametes, the 
partnership is broken and the process is reversed. 
The component parts of the dual structure are 
resolved with the formation of a set of single struc- 
tures, the gametes. 

The life cycle of a species from among the higher 
plants or animals may be regarded as falling into 
three periods: (1) a period of isolation in the form 
of gametes, each a living unit incapable of further 
development without intimate association with another 
produced by the opposite sex; (2) a period of 
association in which two gametes become yoked 
together into a zygote, and react upon one another 
to give rise by a process of cell division to what we 
ordinarily term an individual with all its various 
attributes and properties; and (3) a period of 
dissociation when the single structured gametes 
separate out from that portion of the double structured 
zygote which constitutes its generative gland. What 
is the relation between gamete and zygote, between 
zygote and gamete? how are the properties of the 
zygote represented in the gamete, and in what 
manner are they distributed from the one to the 
other ?—these are questions which serve to indicate 


6 MENDELISM CHAP. I 


the nature of the problem underlying the process of 
heredity. 

Owing to their peculiar power of growth and 
the relatively large size to which zygotes attain, 
many of their properties are appreciable by observa- 
tion. The colour of an animal or of a flower, the 
shape of a seed, or the pattern on the wings of a 
moth, are all zygotic properties, and all capable of 
direct estimation. It is otherwise with the properties 
of gametes. While the difference between a black 
and a white fowl is sufficiently obvious, no one by 
inspection can tell the difference between the egg 
that will hatch into a black and that which will hatch 
into a white. Nor from a mass of pollen grains can 
anyone to-day pick out those that will produce white 
from those that will produce coloured flowers. 
Nevertheless, we know that in spite of apparent 
similarity there must exist fundamental differences 
among the gametes, even among those that spring 
from the same individual. At present our only way 
of appreciating those differences is to observe the 
properties of the zygotes which they form. And as 
it takes two gametes to form a zygote, we are in the 
position of attempting to decide the properties of 
two unknowns from one known. Fortunately the 
problem is not entirely one of simple mathematics. 
It can be attacked by the experimental method, and 
with what measure of success will appear in the 
following pages. 


CHAPTER II 
HISTORICAL 


To Gregor Mendel, monk and abbot, belongs the 
credit of founding the modern science of heredity. 
Through him there was brought into these problems 
an entirely new idea, an entirely fresh conception 
of the nature of living things. Born in 1822 of 
Austro-Silesian parentage, he early entered the 
monastery of Briinn, and there, in the seclusion 
of the cloister garden, he carried out with the common 
pea the series of experiments which has since become 
so famous. In 1865, after eight years’ work, he 
published the results of his experiments in the 
Proceedings of the Natural History Society of Briinn, 
in a brief paper of some forty pages. But brief as 
it is, the importance of the results and the lucidity 
of the exposition will always give it high rank 
among the classics of biological literature. For 
thirty-five years Mendel’s paper remained unknown, 
and it was not until 1900 that it was simultaneously 
discovered by several distinguished botanists. The 
causes of this curious neglect are not altogether 
without interest. Hybridisation experiments before 
Mendel there had been in plenty. The classificatory 
work of Linnaeus in the latter half of the eighteenth 


7 


8 MENDELISM CHAP. 


century had given a definite significance to the word 
species, and scientific men began to turn their 
attention to attempting to discover how species were 
related to one another. And one obvious way of 
attacking the problem was to cross different species 
together and see what happened. This was largely 
done during the earlier half of the nineteenth 
century, though such work was almost entirely con- 
fined to the botanists. Apart from the fact that 
plants lend themselves to hybridisation work more 
readily than animals, there was probably another 
reason why zoologists neglected this form of investi- 
gation. The field of zoology is a wider one than 
that of botany, presenting a far greater variety of 
type and structure. Partly owing to their importance 
in the study of medicine, and partly owing to their 
smaller numbers, the anatomy of the vegetable was 
far better known than that of the animal kingdom. 
It is, therefore, not surprising that the earlier part of 
the nineteenth century found the zoologists, under 
the influence of Cuvier and his pupils, devoting their 
entire energies to describing the anatomy of the 
new forms of animal life which careful search at 
home and fresh voyages of discovery abroad were 
continually bringing to light. During this period the 
zoologist had little inclination or inducement to 
carry on those investigations in hybridisation which 
were occupying the attention of some botanists. 
Nor did the efforts of the botanists afford much 
encouragement to such work, for in spite of the 
labour devoted to these experiments the results 
offered but a confused tangle of facts, contributing 
in no apparent way to the solution of the problem 


rt HISTORICAL 9 


for which they had been undertaken. After half a 
century of experimental hybridisation the determina- 
tion of the relation of species and varieties to one 
another seemed as remote as ever. Then in 1859 
came the Origin of Species, in which Darwin pre- 
sented to the world a consistent theory to account 
for the manner in which one species might have 
arisen from another by a process of gradual evolution. 
Briefly put, that theory was as follows:—In any 
species of plant or animal the. reproductive capacity 
tends to outrun the available food-supply, and the 
resulting competition leads to an inevitable struggle 
for existence. Of all the individuals born, only a 
portion, and that often a very small one, can survive 
to produce offspring. According to Darwin’s theory, 
the nature of the surviving portion is not determined 
by chance alone. No two individuals of a species 
are precisely alike, and among the variations that 
occur some enable their possessors to cope more 
successfully with the competitive conditions under 
which they exist. In comparison with their less 
favoured brethren they have a better chance of 
surviving in the struggle for existence and, conse- 
quently, of leaving offspring. The argument is 
completed by the further assumption of a principle 
of heredity, in virtue of which offspring tend to 
resemble their parents more than other members 
of the species. Parents possessing a favourable 
variation tend to transmit that variation to their 
offspring, to some in greater, to others in less degree. 
Those possessing it in greater degree will again have 
a better chance of survival, and will transmit the 
favourable variation in even greater degree to 


10 MENDELISM CHAP. 


some of their offspring. A competitive struggle for 
existence working in combination with certain 
principles of variation and heredity results in a slow 
and continuous transformation of species through 
the operation of a process which Darwin termed 
natural selection. 

The coherence and simplicity of the theory, sup- 
ported as it was by the great array of facts which 
Darwin had patiently marshalled together, rapidly 
gained the enthusiastic support of the great majority 
of biologists. The problem of the relation of species 
at last appeared to be solved, and for the next forty 
years zoologists and botanists were busily engaged 
in classifying, by the light of Darwin’s theory, the 
great masses of anatomical facts which had already 
accumulated, and in adding and classifying fresh ones. 
The study of comparative anatomy and embryology 
received a new stimulus, for with the acceptance of 
the theory of descent with modification it became 
incumbent upon the biologist to demonstrate the 
manner in which animals and plants differing widely 
in structure and appearance could be conceivably 
related to one another. Thenceforward the energies 
of both botanists and zoologists have been devoted 
to the construction of hypothetical pedigrees suggest- 
ing the various tracks of evolution by which one 
group of animals or plants may have arisen from 
another through a long-continued process of natural 
selection. The result of such work on the whole 
may be said to have shown that the diverse forms 
under which living things exist to-day, and have 
existed in the past so far as palaeontology can tell 
us, are consistent with the view that they are all 


II HISTORICAL II 


related by the community of descent which the 
accepted theory of evolution demands, though as 
to the exact course of descent for any particular 
group of animals there is often considerable diversity 
of opinion. It is obvious that all this work has 
little or nothing to do with the manner in which 
species are formed. Indeed, the effect of Darwin’s 
Origin of Species was to divert attention from the 
way in which species originate. At the time that it 
was put forward his explanation appeared so satisfy- 
ing that biologists accepted the notions of variation 
and heredity there set forth and ceased to take any 
further interest in the work of the hybridisers. Had 
Mendel’s paper appeared a dozen years earlier it is 
difficult to believe that it could have failed to attract 
the attention it deserved. Coming as it did a few 
years after the publication of Darwin’s great work, 
it found men’s minds set at rest on the problems 
that he raised and their thoughts and energies 
directed to other matters. 

Nevertheless, one interesting and noteworthy 
attempt to give greater precision to the term 
heredity was made about this time. Francis Galton, 
a cousin of Darwin, working upon data relating to 
the breeding of Basset hounds, found that he could 
express on a definite statistical scheme the proportion 
in which the different colours appeared in successive 
generations. Every individual was conceived of as 
possessing a definite heritage which might be .ex- 
pressed as unity. Of this, $ was on the average 
derived from the two parents (ze. + from each parent), 
4 from the four grandparents, § from the eight great- 
grandparents, and so on. The Law of Ancestral 


12 MENDELISM CHAP. 


FTeredity, as it was termed, expresses with fair accu- 
racy some of the statistical phenomena relating to 
the transmission of characters in a mixed population. 
But the problem of the way in which characters are 
distributed from gamete to zygote and from zygote 
to gamete remained as before. Heredity is essen- 
tially a physiological problem, and though statistics 
may be suggestive in the initiation of experiment, 
it is upon the basis of experimental fact that progress 
must ultimately rest. For this reason, in spite of its 
ingenuity and originality, Galton’s theory and the 
subsequent statistical work that has been founded 
upon it failed to give us any deeper insight into the 
nature of the hereditary process. 

While Galton was working in England the 
German zoologist, August Weismann, was elabora- 
ting the complicated theory of heredity which 
eventually appeared in his work on The Germplasm 
(1885), a book which will be remembered for one 
notable contribution to the subject. Until the pub- 
lication of Weismann’s work it had been generally 
accepted that the modifications brought about in the 
individual during its lifetime, through the varying 
conditions of nutrition and environment, could be 
transmitted to the offspring. In this biologists were 
but following Darwin, who held that the changes in 
the parent resulting from increased use or disuse of 
any part or organ were passed on to the children. 
Weismann’s theory involved the conception of a sharp 
cleavage between the general body tissues or somato- 
plasm and the reproductive glands or germplasm. 
The individual was merely a carrier for the essential 
germplasm whose properties had been determined 


WI HISTORICAL 13 


long before he was capable of leading a separate 
existence. As this conception ran counter to the 
possibility of the inheritance of “ acquired characters,” 
Weismann challenged the evidence upon which it 
rested and showed that it broke down wherever 
it was critically examined. By thus compelling 
biologists to revise their ideas as to the inherited 
effects of use and disuse, Weismann rendered a 
valuable service to the study of genetics and did 
much to clear the way for subsequent research. 

A further important step was taken in 1895, when 
Bateson once more drew attention to the problem of 
the origin of species, and questioned whether the 
accepted ideas of variation and heredity were after 
all in consonance with the facts. Speaking generally, 
species do not grade gradually from one to the other, 
but the differences between them are sharp and 
specific. Whence comes this prevalence of discon- 
tinuity if the process by which they have arisen is 
one of accumulation of minute and almost imper- 
ceptible differences? Why are not intermediates of 
all sorts more abundantly produced in nature than 
is actually known to be the case? Bateson saw 
that if we are ever to answer this question we must 
have more definite knowledge of the nature of varia- 
tion and of the nature of the hereditary process by 
which these variations are transmitted. And the 
best way to obtain that knowledge was to let the 
dead alone and to return to the study of the living. 
It was true that the past record of experimental 
breeding had been mainly one of disappointment. 
It was true also that there was no tangible clue by 
which experiments might be directed in the present. 


14 MENDELISM CHAP. 11 


Nevertheless in this kind of work alone seemed there 
any promise of ultimate success. 

A few years later appeared the first volume of 
de Vries’ remarkable book on The Mutation Theory. 
From a prolonged study of the evening primrose 
(Oenothera) de Vries concluded that new varieties 
suddenly arose from older ones by sudden sharp 
steps or mutations, and not by any process involving 
the gradual accumulation of minute differences. The 
number of striking cases from among widely different 
plants which he was able to bring forward went far 
to convincing biologists that discontinuity in varia- 
tion was a more widespread phenomenon than had 
hitherto been suspected, and not a few began to 
question whether the account of the mode of evolu- 
tion so generally accepted for forty years was after 
all the true account. Such, in brief, was the outlook 
in the central problem of biology at the time of the 
rediscovery of Mendel’s work. 


CHAPTER III 
MENDEL’S WORK 


THE task that Mendel set before himself was to 
gain some clear conception of the manner in which 
the definite and fixed varieties found within a species 
are related to one another, and he realised at the 
outset that the best chance of success lay in working 
with material of such a nature as to reduce the 
problem to its simplest terms. He decided that 
the plant with which he was to work must be 
normally self-fertilising and unlikely to be crossed 
through the interference of insects, while at the 
same time it must possess definite fixed varieties 
which bred true to type. In the common pea 
(Pisum. sativum) he found the plant he sought. 
A hardy annual, prolific, easily worked, Pzsum 
has a further advantage in that the insects which 
normally visit flowers are unable to gather pollen 
from it and so to bring about cross feftilisation. 
At the same time it exists in a number of strains 
presenting well-marked and fixed differences. The 
flowers may be purple, or red, or white; the plants 
may be tall or dwarf; the ripe seeds may be yellow 
or green, round or wrinkled,—such are a few of the 


15 


16 MENDELISM CHAP, 


characters in which the various races of peas differ 
from one another. 

In planning his crossing experiments Mendel 
adopted an attitude which marked him off sharply 
from the earlier hybridisers. He realised that their 
failure to elucidate any general principle of heredity 
from the results of cross fertilisation was due to 
their not having concentrated upon particular 
characters or traced them carefully through. a 
sequence of generations. That source of failure 
he was careful to avoid, and throughout his ex- 
periments he crossed plants presenting sharply 
contrasted characters, and devoted his efforts to 
observing the behaviour of these characters in 
successive generations. Thus in one series of ex- 
periments he concentrated his attention on the 
transmission of the characters tallness and dwarf- 
ness, neglecting in so far as these experiments were 
concerned any other characters in which the parent 
plants might differ from one another. For this 
purpose he chose two strains of peas, one of about 
6 feet in height, and another of about 14 feet. 
Previous testing had shown that each strain bred 
true to its peculiar height. These two strains were 
artificially crossed’ with one another, and it was 
found to make no difference which was used as the 
pollen parent and which was used as the ovule 
parent. In either case the result was the same. 
The result of crossing tall with dwarf was in every 
case nothing but talls, as tall or even a little taller 
than the tall parent. For this reason Mendel 
termed tallness the dominant and dwarfness the 


1 Cf. note on p. 171, 


11 MENDEL’S WORK 17 


recessive character. The next stage was to 
collect and sow the seeds of these tall hybrids. 
Such seeds in the following year gave rise to a 
mixed generation consisting of talls and dwarfs dz 
no intermediates. By raising a considerable number 
of such plants Mendel was able to establish the fact 
that the number of talls which occurred in this 
generation was almost exactly three times as great 
as the number of the dwarfs. As in the previous 
year, seed were carefully collected from this, the 
second hybrid generation, and in every case the seeds 
Jrom each individual plant were harvested separately 
and separately sown in the following year. By this 
respect for the individuality of the different plants, 
however closely they resembled one another, Mendel 
found the clue that had eluded the efforts of all his 
predecessors. The seeds collected from the dwarf 
recessives bred true, giving nothing but dwarfs. And 
this was true for every dwarf tested. But with the 
talls it was quite otherwise. Although indistinguish- 
able in appearance, some of them bred true, while 
others behaved like the original tall hybrids, giving a 
generation con- 
sisting of talls | 
and dwarfs in 

. F, 
the proportion of 
three of the for- 
mer to one of the * 
latter. Counting | -——r— 1 i m 
showed that the T TT) TD) DTT(D) TD)D D---F 
number of the iL p-—-F, 
talls which gave 


dwarfs was double that of the talls which bred true. 
Cc 


T(D) T(D) D---Fs 


18 MENDELISM CHAP. 


If we denote a dwarf plant as D, a true breeding 
tall plant as T, and a tall which gives both talls 
and dwarfs in the ratio 3:1 as T(D), the result of 
these experiments may be briefly summarised in the 
foregoing scheme.’ 

Mendel experimented with other pairs of con- 
trasted characters and found that in every instance 
they followed the same scheme of inheritance. Thus 
coloured flowers were dominant to white, in the ripe 
seeds yellow was dominant to green, and round 
shape was dominant to wrinkled, and so on. In 
every case where the inheritance of an alternative 
pair of characters was concerned the effect of the 
cross in successive generations was to produce three 
and only three different sorts of individuals, viz. 
dominants which bred true, dominants which gave 
both dominant and recessive offspring in the ratio 
3: 1, and recessives which always bred true. Having 
determined a general scheme of inheritance which 
experiment showed to hold good for each of the 
seven pairs of alternative characters with which he 
worked, Mendel set himself to providing a theoretical 
interpretation of this scheme which, as he clearly 
realised, must be in terms of germ cells. He con- 
ceived of the gametes as bearers of something capable 
of giving rise to the characters of the plant, but he 
regarded any individual gamete as being able to 
carry one and one only of any alternative pair of 
characters. A given gamete could carry tallness or 
dwarfness, but not both, The two were mutually 


1 It has been found convenient to denote the various generations 
resulting from a cross by the signs Fj, Fo, Fy, etc. F, on this system 
denotes the first filial generation, F, the second filial generation pro- 
duced by two parents belonging to the F, generation, and so on. 


HI MENDEL’S WORK 19 


exclusive so far as the gamete was concerned. It 
must be pure for one or the other of such a pair, 
and this conception of the purity of the gametes is 
the most essential part of Mendel’s theory. 

We. may now proceed with the help of the 
accompanying scheme (Fig. 1) to deduce the re- 
sults that should 


flow from Mendel’s parent eS ©- naBeant 
conception of the sanets@ © OQ czametes 
nature of the gam- / 

etes, and to see ‘ ¥ 

how far they are AD----- en 
in accordance with Pe i 

the facts. Since kK @----> & <---@& 

the original tall PA : 
plant belonged to 3 e---- © ee 

a strain which bred Tore © ay 
true, all the gam- a 2 
etes produced by it i Rese © 


must bear the tall 
character. Simi- 
Fic. 2. 


larly all the gam- Scheme of inheritance in the cross of tall with dwarf 
etes of the original pea. (Gametes represented by small and zygotes 


by larger circles. 

dwarf plant must 

bear the dwarf character. A cross between these 
two means the union of a gamete containing 
tallness with one bearing dwarfness)s Owing © 
to the completely dominant nature of the tall 
character, such a plant is in appearance indis- 
tinguishable from the pure tall, but it differs 
markedly from it in the nature of the gametes to 
which it gives rise. When the formation of the 
gametes occurs, the elements representing dwarfness 


2). 
generation 


20 MENDELISM CHAP. 


and tallness segregate from one another, so that half 
of the gametes produced contain the one, and half 
contain the other of these two elements. For on 
hypothesis every gamete must be pure for one or 
other of these two characters. And this is true for 
the ovules as well as for the pollen grains. Such 
hybrid F, plants, therefore, must produce a series of 
ovules consisting of those bearing tallness and those 
bearing dwarfness, and must produce them in equal 
numbers. And similarly for the pollen grains. We 
may now calculate what should happen when such 
a series of pollen grains meets such a series of ovules, 
ze. the nature of the generation that should be pro- 
duced when the hybrid is allowed to fertilise itself. 
Let us suppose that there are 4x ovules so that 2% 
are “tall” and 2% are “dwarf.” These are brought 
in contact with a mass of pollen grains of which 
half are “tall” and half are “dwarf.” It is obvious 
that a “tall” ovule has an equal chance of being 
fertilised by a “tall” or a “dwarf” pollen grain. 
Hence of our 2% “tall” ovules, x will be fertilised by 
“tall” pollen grains and x will be fertilised by 
“dwarf” pollen grains. The former must give rise 
to tall plants, and since the dwarf character has 
been entirely eliminated from them they must in 
_ the future breed true. The latter must also give 
rise to tall plants, but since they carry also the 
recessive dwarf character they must when bred from 
produce both talls and dwarfs. Each of the 2% 
dwarf ovules, again, has an equal chance of being 
fertilised by a “tall” or by a “dwarf” pollen grain. 
Hence +x will give rise to tall plants carrying the 
recessive dwarf character, while x will produce 


I MENDEL’S WORK 21 


plants from which the tall character has been 
eliminated, ze. to pure recessive dwarfs. Conse- 
quently from the 4% ovules of the self- fertilised 
hybrid we ought to obtain 34% tall and « dwarf 
plants. And of the 3 talls + should breed true to 
tallness, while the remaining 2%, having been formed 
like the original hybrid by the union of a “tall” 
and a “dwarf” gamete, ought to behave like it 
when bred from and give talls and dwarfs in the 
ratio 3:1. Now this is precisely the result actually 
obtained by experiment (cf. p. 17), and the close 
accord of the experimental results with those deduced 
on the assumption of the purity of the gametes as 
enunciated by Mendel affords the strongest of 
arguments for regarding the nature of the gametes 
and their relation to the characters of the zygotes in 
the way that he has done. 

It is possible to put the theory to a further test. 
The explanation of the 3:1 ratio of dominants and 
recessives in the F, generation is regarded as due 
to the F, individuals producing equal numbers of 
gametes bearing the dominant and recessive elements 
respectively. If now the F, plant be crossed with 
the pure recessive, we are bringing together a series 
of gametes consisting of equal numbers of dominants 
and recessives with a series consisting solely of 
recessives. We ought from such a cross to obtain 
equal numbers of dominant and recessive individuals, 
and further, the dominants so produced ought all to 
give both dominants and recessives in the ratio 
3:1 when they themselves are bred from. Both 
of these expectations were amply confirmed by 
experiment, and crossing with the recessive is now a 


22 MENDELISM CHAP, 


recognised way of testing whether a plant or animal 
bearing a dominant character is a pure dominant or 
an impure dominant which is carrying the recessive 
character. In the former case the offspring will be 
all of the dominant form, while in the latter they 
will consist on the average of equal numbers of 
dominants and recessives. 

So far we have been concerned with the results 
obtained when two individuals differing in a single 
pair of characters are crossed together and with 
the interpretation of those results. But Mendel also 
used plants which differed in more than a single pair 
of differentiating characters. In such cases he found 
that each pair of characters followed the same definite 
rule, but that the inheritance of each pair was 
absolutely independent of the other. Thus, for 
example, when a tall plant bearing coloured flowers 
was crossed with a dwarf plant bearing white flowers 
the resulting hybrid was a tall plant with coloured 
flowers. For coloured flowers are dominant to white, 
and tallness is dominant to dwarfness. In the 
succeeding generation there are plants with coloured 
flowers and plants with white flowers in the pro- 
portion of 3:1,and at the same time tall plants 
and dwarf plants in the same proportion. Hence the 
chances that a tall plant will have coloured. flowers 
are three times as great as its chance of having 
white flowers. And this is also true for the dwarf 
plants. As the result of this cross, therefore, we 
should expect an F, generation consisting of four 
classes, viz. coloured talls, white talls, coloured 
dwarfs, and white dwarfs, and we should further 
expect these four forms to appear in the ratio of 


ur MENDEL’S WORK 23 


9 coloured talls, 3 white talls, 3 coloured dwarfs, 
and 1 white dwarf. For this is the only ratio 
which satisfies the conditions that the talls should 
be to the dwarfs as 3:1, and at the same time the. 
coloured should be to the whites as 3:1. And 
these are the proportions that Mendel found to 
obtain actually in his experiments. Put in a more 
general form, it may be stated that when two indi- 
viduals are crossed which differ in two pairs of 
differentiating characters the hybrids (F,) are all 
of the same form, exhibiting the dominant character 
of each of the two pairs, while the F, generation 
produced by such hybrids consists on the average 
of g showing both dominants, 3 showing one 
dominant and one recessive, 3 showing the other 
dominant and the other recessive, and 1 showing both 
recessive characters. And, as Mendel pointed out, 
the principle may be extended indefinitely. If, for 
example, the parents differ in three pair of characters 
A, B, and C respectively dominant to a, 4, and c¢, the 
F, individuals will be all of the form ABC, while 
the F, generation will consist of 27 ABC, 9 ABe, 
g AbC, 9 aBC, 3 Abe, 3 aBc, 3 abC, and 1 abe. 
When individuals differing ina number of alternative 
characters are crossed together, the hybrid generation, 
provided that the original parents were of pure 
strains, consists of plants of the same form; but 
when these are bred from, a redistribution of the 
various characters occurs. Taat redistribution follows 
the same definite rule for eacn character, and if the 
constitution of the original parents be known, the 
nature of the F, generation, ze. the number of 
possible forms and the proportions in which they 


24 MENDELISM CHAP. 


occur, can be readily calculated. Moreover, as 
Mendel showed, we can calculate also the chances 
of any given form breeding true. To this point, 
however, we shall return later. 

Of Mendel’s experiments with beans it is sufficient 
to say here that they corroborated his more ample 
work with peas. He is also known to have made 
experiments with many other plants, and a few of 
his results are incidentally given in his series of letters 
to Nageli the botanist. To the breeding and cross- 
ing of bees he also devoted much time and attention, 
but unhappily the record of these experiments 
appears to have been lost. The only other published 
work that we possess dealing with heredity is a brief 
paper on some crossing experiments with the Hawk- 
weeds (/zeracium), a genus that he chose for working 
with because of the enormous number of forms under 
which it naturally exists. By crossing together the 
more distinct varieties, he evidently hoped to pro- 
duce some of these numerous wild forms, and so 
throw light upon their origin and nature. In this 
hope he was disappointed. Owing in part to the 
great technical difficulties attending the cross-fer- 
tilisation of these flowers he succeeded in obtaining 
very few hybrids. Moreover, the behaviour of those 
which he did obtain was quite contrary to what he 
had found in the peas. Instead of giving a variety 
of forms in the F, generation, they bred true and 
continued to do so as long as they were kept under 
observation. More recent research has shown that 
this is due to a peculiar form of parthenogenesis 
(cf. p. 123), and not to any failure of the characters 
to separate clearly from one another in the gametes. 


ree MENDEL’S WORK 25 


Mendel, however, could not have known of this, and 
his inability to discover in Hzeracium any indication 
of the rule which he had found to hold good for 
both peas and beans must have been a source 
of considerable disappointment. Whether for this 
reason, or owing to the utter neglect of his work by 
the scientific world, Mendel gave up his experimental 
researches during the latter part of his life. His 
closing years were shadowed with ill-health and 
embittered by a controversy with the Government on 
a question of the rights of his monastery. He died 
of Bright’s disease in 1884. 


NVote.—Shortly after the rediscovery of Mendel’s paper 
a need was felt for terms of a general nature to express the 
constitution of individuals in respect of inherited characters, 
and Bateson accordingly proposed the words homozygote 
and heterozygote. An individual is said to be homo- 
zygous for a given character when it has been formed by 
two gametes each bearing the character, and all the gametes 
of a homozygote bear the character in respect of which it is 
homozygous. When, however, the zygote is formed by 
two gametes of which one bears the given character while 
the other does not, it is said to be heterozygous for the 
character in question, and only half the gametes produced 
by such a heterozygote bear the character. An individual 
may be homozygous for one or more characters, and at 
the same time may be heterozygous for others. 


CHAPTER IV 
THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE THEORY 


IT was fortunate for the development of biological 
science that the rediscovery of Mendel’s work found 
a small group of biologists deeply interested in the 
problems of heredity, and themselves engaged in 
experimental breeding. To these men the extra- 
ordinary significance of the discovery was at once 
apparent. From their experiments, undertaken in 
ignorance of Mendel’s paper, de Vries, Correns, and 
Tschermak were able to confirm his results in peas 
and other plants, while Bateson was the first to 
demonstrate their application to animals. Thence- 
forward the record has been one of steady progress, 
and the result of ten years’ work has been to 
establish more and more firmly the fundamental 
nature of Mendel’s discovery. The scheme of in- 
heritance, which he was the first to enunciate, has 
been found to hold good for such diverse things as 
height, hairiness, and flower colour and flower form 
in plants, the shape of pollen grains, and the 
structure of fruits; while among animals the coat 
colour of mammals, the form of the feathers and of 
the comb in poultry, the waltzing habit of Japanese 
26 


cH. 1v PRESENCE & ABSENCE THEORY 27 


mice, and eye colour in man are but a few examples 
of the diversity of characters which all follow the 
same law of transmission. And as time went on 
many cases which at first seemed to fall without the 
scheme have been gradually brought into line in the 
light of fuller knowledge. Some of these will be 


FIG. 2. 


A wing feather and a contour feather of an ordinary and a silky fowl. The peculiar 
ragged appearance of the silky feathers is due to the absence of the little hooks 
or barbules which hold the barbs together. The silky condition is recessive. 


dealt with in the succeeding chapters of this book. 
Meanwhile we may concern ourselves with the single 
modification of Mendel’s original views which has 
arisen out of more ample knowledge. 

As we have already seen, Mendel considered that 
in the gamete there was either a definite something 
corresponding to the dominant character or a definite 
something corresponding to the recessive character, 


28 MENDELISM CHAP, 


and that these somethings whatever they were could not 
coexist in any single gamete. For these somethings 
we shall in future use the term factor. The factor, 
then, is what corresponds in the gamete to the unit- 
character that appears in some shape or other in 
the development of the zygote. Tallness in the pea 
is a unit-character, and the gametes in which it is 


TiG. 4, 


Two double and an ordinary single primula Mower. This form of double is recessive 
to the single. 


represented are said to contain the factor for tallness. 
Beyond their existence in the gamete and _ their 
mode of transmission we make no suggestion as to 
the nature of these factors. 

On Mendel’s view there was a factor correspond- 
ing to the dominant character and another factor 
corresponding to the recessive character of each 
alternative pair of unit-characters, and the characters 
were alternative because no gamete could carry more 


vo =©PRESENCE & ABSENCE THEORY 29 


than one of the two factors belonging to the alter- 
native pair. On the other hand, Mendel supposed 
that it always carried either one or the other of such 
a pair. As experimental work proceeded, it soon 
became clear that there were cases which could 
not be expressed in terms of this conception. The 
nature of the difficulty and the way in which it was 
met will perhaps be best understood by considering 
a set of experiments in which it occurred. Many 
of the different breeds of poultry are characterised 
by a particular form of comb, and in certain cases 
the inheritance of these has been carefully worked 
out. It was shown that the rose comb (Fig. 4, B) 
with its flattened papillated upper surface and back- 
wardly projecting pike was dominant in the ordinary 
way to the deeply serrated high single comb (Fig. 4, C) 
which is characteristic of the Mediterranean races. 
Experiment also showed that the pea comb (Fig. 4, A), 
a form with a low central and two well-developed 
lateral ridges such as is found in Indian game, behaves 
as a simple dominant to the single comb. The inter- 
esting question arose as to what would happen when 
the rose and the pea, two forms each dominant to 
the same third form, were mated together. It seemed 
reasonable to suppose that things which were alter- 
native to the same thing would be alternative to one 
another—that either rose or pea would dominate in 
the hybrids, and that the F, generation would consist 
of dominants and recessives in the ratio 3.1. The 
result of the experiment was, however, very different. 
The cross rose x pea led to the production of a comb 
quite unlike either of them. This, the so-called 
walnut comb (Fig. 4, D), from its resemblance to 


30 MENDELISM CHAP. 


the half of a walnut, is a type of comb which is 
normally characteristic of the Malay fowl. Moreover, 
when these F, birds were bred together, a further 
unlooked-for result was obtained. As was expected, 


FIG. 4. 


Fowls’ combs. A, pea; B, rose; C, single ; D, walnut. 


there appeared in the F, generation the three forms 
walnut, rose, and pea. But there also appeared 
a definite proportion of single combed birds, and 
among many hundreds of chickens bred in this way 
the proportions in which the four forms walnut, rose, 
pea, and single appeared was 9:3:3:1. Now this, 


i PRESENCE & ABSENCE THEORY 31 


as Mendel showed, is the ratio found in an F, 
generation when the original parents differ in two 
pairs of alternative characters, and from the propor- 
tions in which the different forms of comb occur we 
must infer that the 

: Rose X Pea 
walnut contains both 
‘dominants, the rose and — 
the pea one dominant Se a 
each, while the single 
- puns tor bets es Walnut Rose Pea Single 
sive characters. This (9) (3) (3) (z) 
accorded with subse- - 
quent breeding experiments, for the singles bred 
perfectly true as soon as they had once made 
their appearance. So far the case is clear. The 
difficulty comes when we attempt to define these 
two pairs of characters. How are we to express the 
fact that while single behaves as a simple recessive 
to either pure rose or to pure pea, it can yet 
appear in F, from a cross between these two pure 
forms, though neither of them should, on Mendel’s 
view, contain the single? An explanation which 
covers the facts in a simple way is that which has 
been termed the “Presence and Absence” theory. 
On this theory the dominant character of an alter- 
native pair owes its dominance to the presence of 
a factor which is absent in the recessive. The tall 
pea is tall owing to the presence in it of the factor 
for tallness, but in the absence of this factor the pea 
remains a dwarf. All peas are dwarf, but the tall 
is a dwarf plus a factor which turns it into a tall. 
Instead of the characters of an alternative pair being 
due. to two separate factors, we now regard them as 


32 MENDELISM CHAP. 


the expression of the only two possible states of a 
single factor, viz. its presence or its absence. The 
conception will probably become clearer if we follow 
its application in detail to the case of the fowl’s 
combs. In this case we are concerned with the 
transmission of the two factors, rose (2), and pea (P), 
the presence of each of which is alternative to its 
absence. The rose-combed bird contains the factor 
for rose but not that for pea, and the pea-combed 
bird contains the factor for pea but not that for rose. 
When both factors are present in a bird, as in the 
hybrid made by crossing rose with pea, the result is 
a walnut. For convenience of argument we may 
denote the presence of a given factor by a capital 
letter and its absence by the corresponding small 
letter. The use of the small letter is merely a 
symbolic way of intimating that a particular factor 
is absent in a gamete or zygote. Represented thus 
the zygotic constitution of a pure rose-combed bird 
is RRpp; for it has been formed by the union of 
two gametes both of which contained # but not P. 
Similarly we may denote the pure pea-combed bird 
as rrPP. On crossing the rose with the pea, union 
occurs between a gamete Ap and a gamete 7#P, 
resulting in the formation of a heterozygote of the 
constitution RrPp. The use of the small letters 
here informs us that such a zygote contains only 
a single dose of each of the factors R and P, although, 
of course, it is possible for a zygote, if made in a 
suitable way, to have a double dose of any factor. 
Now when such a bird comes to form gametes a 
separation takes place between the part of the 
zygotic cell containing R and the part which does 


1 PRESENCE & ABSENCE THEORY 33 


not contain it (vy). Half of its gametes, therefore, 
will contain R and the other half will be without 
it (7). Similarly half of its gametes will contain P 
and the other half will be without it (pf). It is 
obvious that the chances: of R being distributed to 
a gamete with or without P are equal. Hence the 
gametes containing & will be of two sorts, PR and 
Rp, and these will be produced in equal numbers. 
Similarly the gametes without R will also be of two 
sorts, ~P and 7, and these, again, will be produced 
in equal numbers. Each of the hybrid walnut- 
combed birds, therefore, gives rise to a series con- 
sisting of equal numbers of gametes of the four 
different types RP, Rp, rP, and rp; and the breeding 
together of such F, birds means the bringing together 
of two such series of gametes. When this happens 
an ovum of any one of the four types has an equal 
chance of being fertilised by a spermatozoon of any 
one of the four types. A convenient and simple 
method of demonstrating what happens under such 
circumstances is the method sometimes termed the 
“chessboard” method. For two series each con-. 
sisting of four different types of gamete we require 
a square divided up into 16 parts. The four terms 
of the gametic series are first written horizontally 
across the four sets of four squares, so that the 
series is repeated four times. It is then written 
vertically four times, care being taken to keep to 
the same order. In this simple mechanical way all 
the possible combinations are represented and in 
their proper proportions. Fig. 5 shows the’ result 
of applying this method to our series RP, Rd, rP, rp, 
and the 16 squares represent the different kinds of 
D 


3.4 MENDELISM CHAP, 


zygotes formed and the proportions in which they 
occur. As the figure shows, 9 zygotes contain both 
R and P, having a double or a single dose of either 
or both of these factors. Such birds must be all 
walnut combed. Three out of the 16 zygotes contain 

R but not P, and 
RP RP RP RP these must be 
RP Rp P ™P rose-combed birds. 
Walnut! Walnut] Walnut! Walnut] Three, again, con- 


Rp Rp Rp Rp tain P but not &, 
RP Rp rP rp ‘and must be pea- 
Walnut Rose} Walnut} Rose combed birds. 
'P rP =) rP Finally one out 
RP Rp rP rp _of the 16 contains 
Walnut} Walnut Pea Pea neither K nor P. 
me = 5 = It cannot be rose 
RP Rp rP rp —it cannot be 
Walnut Rose Pea | Single pes It must, 

: therefore, be some- 
Fic. 5. _ thing else. Asa 
ae ee tomer of Get 1 


is single. Why 
“it should be single and not something else follows from 
what we already know about the behaviour of these 
various forms of comb. For rose is dominant to 
single ; therefore on the Presence and Absence theory 
a rose is a single plus a factor which turns the single 
into a rose. If we could remove the “rose” factor 
from a rose-combed bird the underlying single would 
come into view. Similarly a pea comb is a single plus 
a factor which turns the single into a pea, and a walnut 
is a single which possesses two additional modifying 
factors. Singleness, in fact, underlies all these combs, 


= PRESENCE & ABSENCE THEORY 35 


and if we write their zygotic constitution in full 
we must denote a walnut as RRPPSS, a rosé as 
RRppSS, a pea as rr PP SS, and a single as rrppSS. 
The crossing of rose with pea results in a reshuffling 
of the factors concerned, and in accordance with the 
principle of segregation some zygotes are formed in 
which neither of the modifying factors R and P are 
present, and the single character can then become 
manifest. : 

The Presence and Absence theory is to-day 
generally accepted by students of these matters. 
Not only does it afford a simple explanation of the 
remarkable fact that in all cases of Mendelian in- 
heritance we should be able to express our unit- 
characters in terms of alternative pairs, but, as we 
shall have occasion to refer to later, it suggests a 
clue as to the course by which the various domesti- 
cated varieties of plants and animals have arisen 
from their wild prototypes. 

Before leaving this topic we may draw attention 
to some experiments which offer a pretty confirma- 
tion of the view that the rose comb is a single to 
which a modifying factor for roseness has been 
added. It was argued that if we could find a type 
of comb in which the factor for singleness was 
absent, then on crossing such a comb with a rose 
we ought, if singleness really underlies rose, to obtain 
some single combs in F, from such a cross. Such 
a comb we had the good fortune to find in the 
Breda fowl, a breed largely used in Holland. This 
fowl is usually spoken of as combless for the place 
of the comb is taken by a covering of short bristle- 
like feathers (Fig. 6, D). In reality it possesses the 


36 MENDELISM CHAP. 


vestige of a comb in the form of two minute lateral 
knobs of comb tissue. Characteristic also of this 
breed is the high development of the horny nostrils, 
a feature probably correlated with the almost com- 


Fic. 6. 


Fowls’ combs. A and B, Fy hen from rose x Breda; C, an Fy cock from the cross 
of single X Breda; D, head of Breda cock. 


plete absence of comb. The first step in the 
experiment was to prove the absence of the factor 
for singleness in the Breda. On crossing Breda 
with single the F, birds exhibit a large comb of 
the form of a double single comb in which the two 


tv PRESENCE & ABSENCE THEORY 37 


portions are united anteriorly, but diverge from one 
another towards the back of the head (Fig. 6, C). 
The Breda contains an element of duplicity which 
is dominant to the simplicity of the ordinary single 
comb. But it cannot contain the factor for the 
single comb, because as soon as that is put into it 
by crossing with a single the comb assumes a large 
size, and is totally distinct in appearance from its 
almost complete absence in the pure Breda. Now 
when the Breda is crossed with the rose duplicity 
is dominant to simplicity, and rose is dominant to 


Rose X Breda 


Duplex y, Duplex 
Rose | Rose 


TT T | T : 1 
Duplex Rose Duplex Single Breda 


Ro: Single (Duplex. 
ce & and Simplex) 


lack of comb, and the F, generation consists of birds 
possessing duplex rose combs (Fig. 6, A and B). 
On breeding such birds together we obtain a genera- 
tion consisting of Bredas, duplex roses, roses, duplex 
singles, and singles. From our previous experiment 
we know that the singles could not have come from 
the Breda, since a Breda comb to which the factor for 
single has been added no longer remains a Breda. 
Therefore it must have come from the rose, thus 
confirming our view that the rose is in reality a 
single comb which contains in addition a dominant 
modifying factor (R) whose presence turns it into 


38 MENDELISM CHAP. IV 


a rose. We shall take it, therefore, that there is 
good experimental evidence for the Presence and 
Absence theory, and we shall express in terms of it 
the various cases which come up for discussion in 
succeeding chapters. 


CHAPTER ¥ 
INTERACTION OF FACTORS 


WE have now reached a point at which it is possible 
to formulate a definite conception of the living 
organism. A plant or animal is a living entity 
whose properties may in large measure be expressed 
in terms of unit-characters, and it is the possession 
of a greater or lesser number of such unit-characters 
renders it possible for us to draw sharp distinctions 
between one individual and another. These unit- 
characters are represented by definite factors in the 
gamete which in the process of heredity behave as 
indivisible entities, and are distributed according to 
a definite scheme. The factor for this or that unit- 
character is gither present in the gamete or it is 
not present. It must be there in its entirety or 
completely absent. Such at any rate is the view to 
which recent experiment has led us. But as to the 
nature of these factors, the conditions under which 
they exist in the gamete, and the manner in which 
they produce their specific effects in the zygote, we 
are at present almost completely in the dark. 

The case of the fowls’ combs opens up the im- 
portant question of the extent to which the various 
factors can influence one another in the zygote. 


39 


40 MENDELISM CHAP. 


The rose and the pea factors are separate entities, 
and each when present alone produces a perfectly 
distinct and characteristic effect upon the single 
comb, turning it into a rose or a pea as the case 
may be. But when both are present in the same 
zygote their combined effect is to produce the 
walnut comb, a comb which is quite distinct from 
either and in no sense intermediate between them. 
The question of the influence of factors upon one 
another did not present itself to Mendel because 
he worked with characters which affected different 
parts of the plant. It was unlikely that the factor 
which led to the production of colour in the flower 
would affect the shape of the pod, or that the height 
of the plant would be influenced by the presence’ or 
absence of the factor that determined the shape of 
the ripe seed. But when several factors can modify 
the same structure it is reasonable to suppose that 
they will influence one another in the effects which 
their simultaneous presence has upon the zygote. 
By themselves the pea and the rose factors each 
produce a definite modification of the single comb, 
but when both are present in the zygote, whether as 
a single or double dose, the modification that results 
is quite different to that produced by either when 
present alone. Thus we are led to the conception 
of characters which depend for their manifestation 
on more than one factor in the zygote, and in the 
present chapter we may consider a few of the 
phenomena which result from such interaction be- 
tween separate and distinct factors. 

One of the most interesting and instructive cases 
in which the interaction between separate factors has 


¥ INTERACTION OF FACTORS 4I 


been demonstrated is a case in the sweet-pea. All 
white sweet-peas breed true to whiteness. And 
generally speaking the result of crossing different 
whites is to produce nothing but whites whether in 
F, or in succeeding generations. But there are 
certain strains of white sweet-peas which when 
crossed together produce only coloured flowers. The 
colour may be different in different cases, though for 
our present purpose we may take a case in which the 
colour is red. When such reds are allowed to self- 
fertilise themselves in the normal way and the seeds 
sown, the resulting F, : ; 
generation consists of reds White Xx White 

and whites, the former being 
rather more numerous than 
the latter in the proportion Ria White-__F 
of 9:7. The raising of a (9) (7) = 
further generation from the 

seeds of these F, plants shows that the whites always 
breed true to whiteness, but that different reds may 
behave differently. Some breed true, others give reds 
and whites in the ratio 3:1, while others, again, give 
reds and whites in the ratio 9:7. As in the case 
of the fowls’ combs, this case may be interpreted in 
terms of the presence and absence of two factors. 
Red in the sweet-pea results from the interaction of 
two factors, and unless these are both present the 
red colour cannot appear. Each of the white 
parents carried one of the two factors whose inter- 
action is necessary for the production of the red 
colour, and as a cross between them brings these 
two. complementary factors together the F, plants 
must all be red. As this case is of considerable 


42 MENDELISM CHAP. 


importance for the proper understanding of much 
that is to follow, and as it has been completely 
worked out, we shall consider it in some detail. 
Denoting these two colour factors by A and B 
respectively we may proceed to follow out the 
consequences of this cross. Since all the F, plants 
were red the constitution of the parental whites 
must have been AAJ&d and aaBB respectively, and 

their gametes 


White pe consequently Ad 
4 and aB. The 

F g % a ‘canst a ae of 
a of parents the B,. plants 

ae must, therefore, 

jek be AaBb. Such 

; : a plant being 
Sk > Bs heterozygous for 
ac Ab Ab fii two factors pro- 
2% io nee 2 3 duces a series of 

3 = gametes of the 


four kinds AB, 
Ab, aB, ab, and produces them in equal numbers 
(cf. p. 33). To obtain the various types of zygotes 
which are produced when such a series of pollen 
grains meets a similar series of ovules we may make 
use of the same “chessboard” system which we 
have already adopted in the case of the fowls’ 
combs. An examination of this figure (Fig. 7) 
shows that g out of the 16 squares contain both 
A and B, while 7 contain either A or B alone, or 
neither. In other words, on this view of the nature 
of the two white sweet-peas we should in the F, 
generation look for the appearance of coloured and 


v INTERACTION OF FACTORS 43 


white flowers in the ratio 9:7. And this, as we 
have already seen, is what was actually found by 
experiment. Further examination of the figure 
shows that the coloured plants are not all of the 
Same constitution, but are of four kinds with respect 
to their zygotic constitution, viz. 4AABB, AAB2, 


AaBB,and AaBo. 
os Ab 


Since AABB is 
_ 
7 
yo 


homozygous for 


both A and B, all 
Fic. 7. 


the gametes which 
it produces must 
contain both of 
these factors, and 
such a, plant must 
therefore breed 
true to the red 
colour. A plant 
of the constitution 
AABéb is homo- 
zygous for the _ : ; 
factor A; tut” "Bon ae toe ont sven pear ati prea 
coloured Fj. 

heterozygous for 

B. All of its gametes will contain A, but only one- 
half of them will contain B, ze. it produces equal 
numbers of gametes AB and Ad. Two such series 
of gametes coming together must give a generation 
consisting of + AABB, 2x AABb, and x AAbé, 
that is, reds and whites in the ratio 3:1. Lastly 
the red zygotes of the constitution A@Bb have the 
same constitution as the original red made from the 
two whites, and must therefore when bred from give 
reds and whites in the ratio 9:7. The existence 


44 MENDELISM CHAP, 


of all these three sorts of reds was demonstrated by 
experiment, and the proportions in which they were 
met with tallied with the theoretical explanation. 

The theory was further tested by an examination: 
into the properties of the various F, whites which 
come from a coloured plant that has itself been 
produced by the mating of two whites. As 
Fig. 7 shows, these are, in respect of their constitu- 
tion, of five different kinds, viz. AAdé, Aabb, aaBB, 
aaBb, and aabb. Since none of them produce any- 
thing but whites on self-fertilisation it was found 
necessary to test their properties in another way, 
and the method adopted was that of crossing them 
together. It is obvious that when this is done we 
should expect different results in different cases. 
Thus the cross between two whites of the constitution 
AAbb and aaBB should give nothing but coloured 
plants ; for these two whites are of the same ‘con- 
stitution as the original two whites from which the 
experiment started. On the other hand, the cross 
between a white of the constitution aabb and any 
other white can never give anything but whites. 
For no white contains both A and B, or it would 
not be white, and a plant of the constitution aabd 
cannot supply the complementary factor necessary for 
the production of colour. Again, two whites of the 
constitution Aadé and aaBb when crossed should 
give both coloured and white flowers, the latter 
being three times as numerous as the former. 
Without going into further detail it may be stated 
that the results of a long series of crosses between 
the various F, whites accorded closely with the 
theoretical explanation. 


v INTERACTION OF FACTORS 45 


From the evidence afforded by this exhaustive 
set of experiments it is impossible to resist the 
deduction that the appearance of colour in the sweet- 
pea depends upon the interaction of two factors 
which are independently transmitted according to 
the ordinary scheme of Mendelian inheritance. 
What these factors are is still an open question. 
Recent evidence of a chemical nature indicates that 
colour in a flower is due to the interaction of two 
definitive substances: (1) a colourless “ chromogen,” 
or colour basis; and (2) a ferment which behaves as 
an activator of the chromogen, and by inducing 
some process of oxidation, leads to the formation of 
a coloured substance. But whether these two bodies 
exist as such in the gametes, or whether in some 
other form we have as yet no means of deciding. 

Since the elucidation of the nature of colour in 
the sweet-pea phenomena of a similar kind have 
been witnessed in other plants, notably in stocks, 
snapdragons, and orchids. Nor is this class of 
phenomena confined to plants. In the course of a 
series of experiments upon the plumage colour 
of poultry, indications were obtained that different 
white breeds did not always owe their whiteness to 
the same cause. Crosses were accordingly made 
between the white Silky fowl and a pure white 
strain derived from the white Dorking. Each of 
these had been previously shown to behave as a 
simple recessive to colour. When the two were 
crossed only fully coloured birds resulted. From 
analogy with the case of the sweet-pea it was 
anticipated that such F, coloured birds when bred 
together would produce an F, generation consisting 


46 MENDELISM CHAP. 


of coloured and white birds in the ratio 9:7, and 
when the experiment was made this was actually 
shown to be the case. With the growth of know- 
ledge it is probable that further striking parallels 
of this nature between the plant and animal worlds 
will be met with. 

Before quitting the subject of these experiments, 
attention may be drawn to the fact that the 9:7 
ratio is in reality a 9:3:3:1 ratio in which the 
last three terms are indistinguishable owing to the 
special circumstances that neither factor can produce 
a visible effect without the co-operation of the other. 
And we may further emphasise the fact that although 
the two factors thus interact upon one another they 
are nevertheless transmitted quite independently 
and in accordance with the ordinary Mendelian 
scheme. 

One of the earliest sets of experiments demon- 
strating the interaction of separate factors was that 
made by the French 
zoologist Cuénot on the 
coat colours of mice. It 

Agouti X Agouti was shown that in cer- 

tain cases agouti, which 

Aglutt ack Albino |S the colour of the 
(9) (3) (4) ordinary wild grey 
mouse, behaves as a 

dominant to the albino variety, ze. the F, generation 
from such a cross consists of agoutis and albinos in 
the ratio 3:1. But in other cases the cross between 
albino and agouti gave a different result. In the F, 
generation appeared only agoutis as before, but the 
F, generation consisted of three distinct types, viz. 


Agouti X Albino 


Vv INTERACTION OF FACTORS 47 


agoutis, albinos, avd blacks. Whence the sudden 
appearance of the new type? The answer is a simple 
one. The albino parent was really a black. But it 
lacked the factor without which the colour is unable to 
develop, and consequently it remained an albino. If 
we denote this factor by C, then the constitution of an 
albino must be cc, while that of a coloured animal may 
be CC or Cc, according as to whether it breeds true to 
colour or can throw albinos. Agouti was previously 
known to be a simple dominant to black, ze. an 
agouti is a black rabbit plus an additional greying 
factor which modifies the black into agouti. This 
factor we will denote by G, and we will use B for 
the black factor. Our original agouti and albino 
parents we may therefore regard as in constitution 
CCGGBS8 and ceggBB respectively. Both of the 
parents are homozygous for black. The gametes 
produced by the two parents are CGB and cg, and 
the constitution of the F, animals must be CcGgBB. 
Being heterozygous for two factors they will produce 
four kinds of gametes in equal numbers, viz. CGB, 
CgB, cGB, and cgB. The results of the mating of 
two such similar series of gametes when the F, 
animals are bred together we may determine by the 
usual “chessboard” method (Fig. 8). Out of the 
16 squares 9 contain both C and G in addition to 
B. Such animals must be agoutis. Three squares 
contain C but not G. Such animals must be 
coloured, but as they do not contain the modifying 
agouti factor their colour will be black. The remain- 
ing four squares do not contain C, and in the absence 
of this colour-developing factor they must all be 

albinos. Theory demands that the three classes 


48 MENDELISM CHAP. 


agouti, black, and albino should appear in F, in the 
ratio 9 :3:4; experiment has shown that these are 
the only classes that appear, and that the proportions 
in which they are produced accord closely with the 
theoretical ex- 
pectation. Put 
briefly, then, the 
explanation of 
this case is that 
all the animals 
are black, and that 
we are dealing 
with the presence 
and absence of 
two factors, a 
colour developer 
Albino| (C), and a colour 
modifier (G), both 


_ acting, as it were, 
Diagram to illustrate the nature of the F, generation 
which may arise from the mating of agouti with upon asubstratum 


albino in mice or rabbits. of hack. ‘The F, 
generation really consists of the four classes agoutis, 
blacks, albino agoutis, and albino blacks in the ratio 
9:3:3:1. But since in the absence of the colour 
developer C the colour modifier G can produce no 
visible result, the last two classes of the ratio are 
indistinguishable, and our F, generation comes to 
consist of three classes in the ratio 9: 3: 4, instead 
of four classes in the ratio 9: 3:3: 1. 

This explanation was further tested by experi- 
ments with the albinos. In an F, family of this 
nature there ought to be three kinds, viz. albinos 
homozygous for G (ccGGBB), albinos heterozygous 


Fic. 8. 


v INTERACTION OF FACTORS 49 


for G (ccGgBB), and albinos without G (ceggBB). 
These albinos are, as it were, like photographic plates, 
exposed but undeveloped. Their potentialities may 
be quite different, although they all look alike, but 
this can only be tested by treating them with a colour 
developer. In the case of the mice and rabbits the 
potentiality for which we wish to test is the presence 
or absence of the factor G, and in order to develop 
the colour we must introduce the factor C. Our 
developer, therefore, must contain C but not G. In 
other words, it must be a homozygous black mouse 
or rabbit, CCggBB. Since such an animal is pure 
for C it must, when mated with any of the albinos, 
produce only coloured offspring. And since it does 
not contain G the appearance of agoutis among its 
offspring must be attributed to the presence of G 
in the albino. Tested in this way the F, albinos 
were proved, as was expected, to be of three kinds: 
(1) those which gave only agouti, ze. which were 
homozygous for G; (2) those which gave agoutis 
and blacks in approximately equal numbers, ze. 
which were heterozygous for G ; and (3) those which 
gave only blacks, and therefore did not contain G. 
Though albinos, whether mice, rabbits, rats, or 
other animals, breed true to albinism, and though 
albinism behaves as a simple recessive to colour, yet 
albinos may be of many different sorts. There are 
in fact just as many kinds of albinos as there are 
coloured forms—neither more nor less. And all 
these different kinds of albinos may breed together, 
transmitting the various colour factors according to 
the Mendelian scheme of inheritance, and yet the 
visible result will be nothing but albinos. Under 
E 


50 MENDELISM CHAP. 


the mask of albinism is all the while occurring that 
segregation of the different colour factors which would 
result in all the varieties of coloured forms, if only 
the essential factor for colour development were 
present. But put in the developer by crossing 
with a pure coloured form and their variety of con- 
stitution can then at last become manifest. 

So far we have dealt with cases in which the 
production of a character is dependent upon the 
interaction of two factors. But it may be that some 
characters require the simultaneous presence of a 
greater number of factors for their manifestation, 
and Miss Saunders has shown that there is a 
character in ten-week stocks which is unable 
to appear except through the interaction of three 
distinct factors. Coloured stocks may be either 
hoary with the leaves and stem covered by small 
hairs, or they may lack the hairy covering, in which 
case they are termed glabrous. Hoariness is dominant 
to glabrousness ; that is to say, there is a definite 
factor which can turn the glabrous into a hoary plant 
when it is present. But in families where coloured 
and white stocks occur the white are always glabrous, 
while the coloured plants may or may not be hoary. 
Now colour in the stock as in the sweet-pea has 
been proved to be dependent upon the interaction of 
two separate factors. Hence hoariness depends 
upon three separate factors, and a stock cannot be 
hoary unless it contains the hoary factor in addition 
to the two colour factors. It requires the presence 
of all these three factors to produce the hoary 
character, though how this comes about we have not 
at present the least idea. ; 


v INTERACTION OF FACTORS 51 


A somewhat different and less usual form of inter- 
action between factors may be illustrated by a case 
in primulas recently worked out by Bateson and 
Gregory. Like the common primrose, the primula 
exhibits both pin-eyed and thrum-eyed varieties. In 
the former the style is long, and the centre of the 
eye is formed by the end of the stigma which more 
or less plugs up the opening of the corolla (cf. Fig. 
9, A); in the latter the style is short and hidden by 


Fic. 9. 


Sections of primula flowers. The anthers are shown as black. A, “‘ pin” form with 
long style and anthers set low down; B, ‘‘thrum” form with short style and 
anthers set higher up; C, homostyle form with anthers set low down as in 
“pin,” but with short style. This form only occurs with the large eye. 


the five anthers which spring from higher up in the 
corolla and form the centre of the eye (cf. Fig. 9, B). 
The greater part of the “eye” is formed by the 
greenish-yellow patches on each petal just at the 
opening of the corolla. In most primulas the eye 
is small, but there are some in which it is large 
and extends as a flush over a considerable part of 
the petals (Fig. 10). Experiments showed that 
these two pairs of characters behave in simple Men- 
delian fashion, short style (= “thrum”) being dominant 
to long style (=“ pin”) and small eye dominant to 


52 MENDELISM CHAP. 


large. Besides the normal long and short styled 
forms, there occurs a third form, which has been 
termed homostyle. In this form the anthers are 
placed low down in the corolla tube as they are in 
the long-styled form, but the style remains short 
instead of reaching up to the corolla opening (Fig. 
9, C). In the course of their experiments Bateson 


FIG. Io. 


Two primula flowers showing the extent of the small and of the large eye. 


and Gregory crossed a large-eyed homostyle plant 
with a small-eyed thrum (=short style). The F, 
plants were all short styled with small eyes. On 
self-fertilisation these gave an F, generation consist- 
ing of four types, viz. short styled with small eyes, 
short styled with large eyes, Jong styled with small 
eyes, and omostyled with large eyes. The notable 
feature of this generation is the appearance of long- 
styled plants, which, however, occur only in associa- 
tion with the small eye. The proportions in which 
these four types appeared shows that the presence 
or absence of but two factors is concerned, and at 


v INTERACTION OF FACTORS 53 


the same time provides the key to the nature of the 
homostyled plants. These are potentially long styled, 
and the position of the anthers is that of normal long- 
styled plants, but owing to some interaction between 
the factors the style itself is unable to reach its full 
development unless the factor for the small eye is 
present. For this reason long-styled plants with 


Short style x Homo style 
small eye large eye 


Short style 
small eye 


| 


I T T e 7 
Shortstyle Short style Long style Homostyle 
smalleye largeeye (“pin”) large eye 


~ (9) (3) (3) (1) 


the large eye are always of the homostyle form. 
What the connecting-link between these apparently 
unrelated structures may be we cannot yet picture 
to ourselves, any more than we can picture the relation 
between flower colour and hairiness in stocks, It is 
evident, however, that the conception of the inter- 
action of factors, besides clearing up much that is 
paradoxical in heredity, promises to indicate lines of 
research which may lead to valuable extensions in 
our knowledge of the way in which the various parts 
of the living organism are related to one another. 


CHAPTER VI 
REVERSION 


AS soon as the idea was grasped that characters in 
plants and animals might be due to the interaction 
of complementary factors, it became evident that this 
threw clear light upon the hitherto puzzling pheno- 
menon of reversion. We have already seen that in 
certain cases the cross between a black mouse or 
rabbit and an albino, each belonging to true breeding 
strains, might produce nothing but agoutis. In other 
words, the cross between the black and the white in 
certain instances results in a complete reversion to the 
wild grey form. Expressed in Mendelian terms, the 
production of the agouti was the necessary conse- 
quence of the meeting of the factors C and G in the 
same zygote. As soon as they are brought together, 
no matter in what way, the reversion is bound to 
occur. Reversion, therefore, in such cases we may 
regard as the bringing together of complementary 
factors which had somehow in the course of evolution 
become separated from one another. In the simplest 
cases, such as that of the black and the white rabbit, 
only two factors are concerned, and one of them is 
brought in from each of the two parents. But in 


54 


PLATE 


SMOTTAEA ‘S ‘nosy ‘Pb 


i za ‘sadéy 


‘uedepeunpy ‘8 ‘]Teysesioyioy ‘4 !xoeTg ‘9 


aA ‘g- Suorssaaar ‘yf (Aai3=) nosy ‘€ tuedepennpy ‘2 Syqqey yong MoTOR 


2 * 


I 


CHAP. VI REVERSION 55 


other cases the nature of the reversion may be more 
complicated owing to a larger number of factors 
being concerned, though the general principle remains 
the same. Careful breeding from the reversions will 
enable us in each case to determine the number and 
nature of the factors concerned, and in illustration 
of this we may take another example from rabbits. 
The Himalayan rabbit is a well-known breed. In 
appearance it is a white rabbit with pink eyes, but 
the ears, paws, and nose are black (Pl. I, 2). The 
Dutch rabbit is another well-known breed. Generally 
speaking, the anterior portion of the body is white, 
and the posterior part coloured. Anteriorly, how- 
ever, the eyes are surrounded by coloured patches 
extending up to the ears, which are entirely coloured. 
At the same time the hind paws are white (cf. 
Pl. I., 1). Dutch rabbits exist in many varieties of 
colour, though in each one of these the distribution 
of colour and white shows the same relations. In 
the experiments about to be described a yellow 
Dutch rabbit was crossed with a Himalaya. The 
result was a reversion to the wild agouti colour 
(Pl. I., 3). Some of the F, individuals showed 
white patches, while others were self-coloured. On 
breeding from the F, animals a series of coloured 
forms resulted in F, These were agoutis, blacks, 
yellows, and sooty yellows, the so-called tortoise- 
shells of the fancy (PI. I., 4-7). In addition to these 
appeared Himalayans with either black points or 
with lighter brownish ones, and the proportions in 
which they came showed the Himalayan character 
to be a simple recessive. A certain number of the 
coloured forms exhibited the Dutch marking to a 


56 MENDELISM CHAP. 


greater or less extent, but as its inheritance in this 
set of experiments is complicated and has not yet 
been worked out, we may for the present neglect it 
and confine our attention to the coloured types and 
to the Himalayans. The proportion in which the 
four coloured types appeared in F, was very nearly 
g agoutis, 3 blacks, 3 yellows, and 1 tortoiseshell. 
Evidently we are here dealing with two factors: .(1) 
the grey factor (G), which modifies black into agouti, 
or tortoiseshell into yellow; and (2) an intensifying 
factor (7), which intensifies yellow into agouti and 


Yellow X Himalayan 


a 


Agouti X Agouti 


vr T v T 1 
Agouti Yellow Black co Himalayan 


(7) (9) i) (16) 


tortoiseshell into black. It may be mentioned here 
that other experiments confirmed the view that the 
yellow rabbit is a dilute agouti, and the tortoiseshell 
a dilute black. The Himalayan pattern behaves as 
a recessive to self-colour. It is a self-coloured black 
rabbit lacking a factor that allows the colour to 
develop except in the points. That factor we may 
denote by XX, and as far as it is concerned the 
Himalayan is constitutionally zr. The Himalayan 
contains the intensifying factor, for such pigment 
as it possesses in the points is full coloured. At 
the same time it is black, ze. lacking in the factor G. 
With regard to these three factors, therefore, the con- 
stitution of the Himalayan is g¢//ar. The last char- 


vI REVERSION 57 


acter which we have to consider in this cross is the 
Dutch character. This was found by Hurst to 
behave as a recessive to self-colour (S), and for our 
present purpose we will regard it as differing from 
a self-coloured rabbit in the lack of this factor.' The 
Himalayan is really a self-coloured animal, which, 
however, is unable to show itself as a full black 
owing to its not possessing the factor X. The results 
of breeding experiments then suggest that we may 
denote the Himalayan by the formula gg//arSS and 
the yellow Dutch by GGzXXss. Each lacks two 
of the factors upon the full complement of which the 
agouti colour depends. By crossing them the com- 
plete series G/XS is brought into the same zygote, 
and the result is a reversion to the colour of the 
wild rabbit. 

Most of the instances of reversion yet worked out 
are those in which colour characters are concerned. 
The sweet-pea, however, supplies us with a good 
example of reversion in structural characters. A 
dwarf variety known as the “ Cupid” has been exten- 
sively grown for some years. In these little plants 
the internodes are very short and the stems are 
few in number, and attain to a length of only 9-10 
inches. In course of growth they diverge from one 
another, and come to lie prostrate on the ground (PI. 
II., 2). Curiously enough, although the whole plant 
is dwarfed in other respects, this does not seem to 
affect the size of the flower, which is that of a normal 
sweet-pea. Another though less-known variety is 
the “Bush” sweet-pea. Its name is derived from 


1 Hurst’s original cross was between a Belgian hare and an albino 
Angora which turned out to be a masked Dutch. 


53 MENDELISM CHAP. 


its habit of growth. The numerous stems do not 
diverge from one another, but all grow up side by 
side giving the plant the appearance of a compact 
bush (PI.IL, 1). Under ordinary conditions it attains 
a height of 34-4 feet. A number of crosses were 
made between the Bush and Cupid varieties, with 
the somewhat unexpected result that in every 
instance the F, plants showed complete reversion 
to the size and habit of the ordinary tall sweet- 
pea (PI. IL, 3), which is the form of the wild plant 
as it occurs in Sicily to-day. The F, generation from 


Bush X Cupid 


I T T ] 
Tall Bush Cupid Cupid- -- - - I, 
(procumbent) (erect) 
(9) (3) (3) (1) 


these reversionary talls consisted of four different 
types, viz. talls, bushes, Cupids of the procumbent 
type like the original Cupid parent, and Cupids 
with the compact upright Bush habit (PI. II. 4). 
These four types appeared in the ratio 9: 3:3: 1, 
and this, of course, provided the clue to the nature 
of the case. The characters concerned are (1) long 
internode of stem between the leaves which is 
dominant to short internode, and (2) the creeping 
procumbent habit which is dominant to the erect 
bush-like habit. Of these characters length of inter- 
node was carried by the Bush, and the procumbent 
habit by the original Cupid parent. The bringing 
of them together by the cross resulted in a_pro- 


PLATE Il. 


1, Bush Sweet Pea; z, Cupid Sweet Pea; 3, F, reversionary Tall; 
4, Erect Cupid Sweet Pea; s, Purple Invincible ; 6, Painted Lady; 
7, Duke of Westminster (hooded standard). 


VI REVERSION 59 


cumbent plant with long internodes. This is the 
ordinary tall sweet-pea of the wild Sicilian type, 
reversion here, again, being due to the bringing 
together of two complementary factors which had 
somehow become separated in the course of evolution, 

To this interpretation it may be objected that 
the ordinary sweet-pea is a plant of upright habit. 
This, however, is not true. It only appears so 
because the conventional way of growing it is to 
train it up sticks. In reality it is of procumbent 
habit, with divergent stems like the ordinary Cupid, 
a fact which can easily be observed by any one who. 
will watch them grow without the artificial aid of 
prepared supports, 

The cases of reversion with which we have so 
far dealt have been cases in which the reversion 
occurs as an immediate result of a cross, ze. in the 
F, generation. This is perhaps the commonest 
mode of reversion, but instances are known in 
which the reversion that occurs when two pure 
types are crossed does not appear until the F, 
generation. Such a case we have already met 
with in the fowls’ combs. It will be remembered 
that the cross between pure pea and pure rose gave 
walnut combs in F,, while in the F, generation a 
definite proportion, I in 16, of single combs 
appeared (cf. p. 30). Now the single comb is the 
form that is found in the wild jungle fowl, which is 
generally regarded as the ancestor of the domestic 
breeds. If this is so, we have a case of reversion in 
F,; and this in the absence of the two factors 
brought together by the rose-comb and pea-comb 


parents. Instead of the reversion being due to the 


60 MENDELISM CHAP. 


bringing together of two complementary factors, we 
must regard it here as due to the association of two 
complementary absences. To this question, how- 
ever, we shall revert later in discussing the origin of 
domesticated varieties. 

There is one other instance of reversion to which 


Black Barb x White Fantail Black Barb x Spot! 
| 
Dark x Dark 


Among the offspring one very similar 
to the wild blue rock. 


we must allude. This is Darwin’s famous case of 
the occasional appearance of pigeons reverting to 
the wild blue rock (Columba livia) when certain 
domesticated races are crossed together. As is 
well known, Darwin made use of this as an 


Black x White 
Barb | Fantail 


Black % Black 
(White Splashed) (White Splashed) 


{ T I T i 
Black Black Blue Blue White 
(White Splashed) (White Splashed) 


©) ao 


argument for regarding all the domesticated varieties 
as having arisen from the same wild species. The 
original experiment is somewhat complicated, and 
is shown in the accompanying scheme. Essentially 


1 This is an almost white bird, the colour being confined to the 
tail and the characteristic spot on the head. 


vI REVERSION 61 


it lay in following the results flowing from crosses 
between blacks and whites. Experiments recently 
made by Staples-Browne have shown that this case 
of reversion also can be readily interpreted in 
Mendelian terms. In these experiments the cross 
was made between black barbs and white fantails. 
The F, birds were all black with some white 
splashes, evidently due to a separate factor intro- 


duced by the fan- 
CB CB CB CB 
CB Cb cB cb 


tail. On breeding 
BLACK | BLACK| BLACK] BLACK 


these blacks to- 
a age 
CB cB 
BLACK JB BLACK 


gether they gave 
cB cB 


(with or without 
white splashes), 
blues (with or 
without white 
splashes), and 
whites in the ratio 
9:3:4. Thefactors 
concerned are 


an F, generation, 
consisting of blacks 
Cb 
BLACK BLACK 


: Fic, 11. 
colour (C), in the Diagram to illustrate the appearance of the rever- 
absence of which sionary blue pigeon in F. from the cross of black 


‘ . ‘ with white. 
a bird is white, and 


a black modifier (B),in the absence of which a coloured 
bird is blue. The original black barb contained both 
of these factors, being in constitution CCBB. The 
fantail, however, contained neither, and was con- 
stitutionally ccbé. The F, bitds produced by 
crossing were in constitution CcBé, and being 
heterozygous for two factors produced in equal 
numbers the four sorts of gametes CB, Cd, cB, cb. 


62 MENDELISM CHAP, VI 


The results of two such series of gametes being 
brought together are shown in the usual way in Fig. 
11. A blue is a bird containing the colour factor 
but lacking the black modifier, z.e. of the constitution 
CCo6, or Ccbb, and such birds as the figure shows 
appear in the F, generation on the average three 
times out of sixteen. Reversion here comes about 
in F,, when the redistribution of the factors leads to 
the formation of zygotes containing one of the two 
factors but not the other. 


CHAPTER VII 
DOMINANCE 


IN the cases which we have hitherto considered the 
presence of a factor produces its full effect whether 
it is introduced by both of the gametes which go to 
form the zygote, or by one of them alone. The 
heterozygous tall pea, or the heterozygous rose- 
combed fowl cannot be distinguished from the 
homozygous form by mere inspection, however close. 
Breeding tests alone can decide which is the 
heterozygous and which the homozygous form. 
Though this is true for the majority of characters 
’ yet investigated, there are cases known in which the 
heterozygous form differs in appearance from either 
parent. Among plants such a case has been met 
with in the primula. The ordinary Chinese primula 
(P. sinensis) (Fig. 12) has large rather wavy petals 
much crenated at the edges. In the Star Primula 
(P. stellata) the flowers are much smaller, while the 
petals are flat and present only a terminal notch 
instead of the numerous crenations of P. szxenszs. 
The heterozygote produced by crossing these forms 
is intermediate in size and appearance. When self- 
fertilised such plants behave in simple Mendelian 
63 


64 MENDELISM CHAP. 


fashion, giving a generation consisting of szzenszs, 
intermediates, and s¢el/ata in the ratio 1:2:1. 
Subsequent breeding from these plants showed that 
both the szzens¢s and stellata which appeared in the 
F, generation bred true, while the intermediates 


FIG. ¥2, 


Primula flowers to illustrate the intermediate nature of the Fy flower when sinensis 
is crossed with stellata. 


always gave all three forms again in the same 
proportion. But though there is no dominance 
of the character of either parent in such a case as 
this, the Mendelian principle of segregation could 
hardly have a better illustration. 

Among birds a case of similar nature is that of 
the Blue Andalusian fowl. Fanciers have long 


VII DOMINANCE 65 


recognised the difficulty of getting this variety to 
breed true. Of a slaty blue colour itself with 
darker hackles and with black lacing on the 


Sinensis X Stellata 


Intermediate_. ____...-.--- F, 

I T T } 
Sinensis _ Inter. Inter.  Stellata----F2 
Sinensis Sin, Int. Int. Stell. Stellata- - - -F, 
Sinensis Stellata- ---F, 


feathers of the breast, it always throws “wasters” 
of two kinds, viz. blacks, and whites splashed with 
black. Careful breeding from the blues shows that 
the three sorts are always produced in the same 


Blue X Blue 


CT T | 7 
Black Blue i Blue i 
| 
, 


i] T 1 
Black Black Blue Blue White White 


Black Xx ‘White 


Blue 
(all) 


definite proportions, viz. one black, two blues, one 

splashed white. This at once suggests that the 

black and the splashed white are the two homozy- 

gous forms, and that the blues are heterozygous, ze. 
F 


66 MENDELISM CHAP. 


producing equal numbers of “black” and “ white 
splashed” gametes. The view was tested by breed- 
ing the “wasters” together—black with black, and 
splashed white with splashed white—and it was 
found that each bred true to its respective type. 
But when the black and the splashed white were 
crossed they gave, as was expected, nothing but 
blues. In other words, we have the seeming paradox 
of the black and the splashed white producing twice 
as many blues as do the blues when bred together. 
The black and the splashed white “wasters” are 
in reality the pure breeds, while the “ pure” Blue 
Andalusian is a mongrel which no amount of selec- 
tion will ever be able to fix. 

In such cases as this it is obvious that we cannot 
speak of dominance. And with the disappearance 
of this phenomenon we lose one criterion for deter- 
mining which of the two parent forms possesses the 
additional factor. Are we, for example, to regard 
the black Andalusian as a splashed white to which 
has been added a double dose of a colour-intensifying 
factor, or are we to consider the white splashed 
bird as a black which is unable to show its true 
pigmentation owing to the possession of some 
inhibiting factor which prevents the manifestation 
of the black. Either interpretation fits the facts 
equally well, and until further experiments have 
been devised and carried out it is not possible to 
decide which is the correct view. 

Besides these comparatively rare cases where the 
heterozygote cannot be said to bear a closer re- 
semblance to one parent more than to the other, 
there are cases in which it is often possible to draw 


Vil DOMINANCE 67 


a visible distinction between the heterozygote and 
the pure dominant. There are certain white breeds 
of poultry, notably the White Leghorn, in which the 
white behaves as a dominant to colour. But the 
heterozygous whites made by crossing the dominant 
white birds with a pure coloured form (such as the 
Brown Leghorn) almost invariably show a few 
coloured feathers or “ticks” in their plumage. The 
dominance of white is not quite complete, and renders 
it, possible to distinguish the pure from the impure 
dominant without recourse to breeding experiments. 

This case of the dominant white fowl opens up 
another interesting problem in connection with 
dominance. By accepting the Presence and Absence 
hypothesis we are committed to the view that the 
dominant form possesses an extra factor as com- 
pared with the recessive. The natural way of 
looking at this case of the fowl is to regard white 
as the absence of colour. But were this so, colour 
should be dominant to white, which is not the case. 
We are therefore forced to suppose that the absence 
of colour in this instance is due to the presence of 
a factor whose property is to inhibit the production 
of colour in what would otherwise be a pure coloured 
bird. On this view the dominant white fowl is a 
coloured bird plus a factor which inhibits the de- 
velopment of the colour. The view can be put to 
the test of experiment. We have already seen that 
there are other white fowls in which white is reces- 
sive to colour, and that the whiteness of such birds 
is due to the fact that they lack a factor for the 
development of colour. If we denote this factor by 
C and our postulated inhibitor factor in the dominant 


68 MENDELISM CHAP. 


white bird by /, then we must write the constitution 
of the recessive white as cczz, and the dominant white 
as CC//. We may now work out the results we 
ought to obtain when a cross is made between these 
two pure white breeds. The constitution of the F, 
bird must be Ce/z. Such birds being heterozygous 
for the inhibitor factor, should be whites showing 
some coloured “ticks.” Being heterozygous for 
both of the two factors C and /, they will produce 
in equal numbers the four different sorts of gametes 
CT, Cz, cl, ct. The result of bringing two such 
similar series of gametes together is shown in Fig, 
13. Out of the sixteen squares, twelve contain /; 
these will be white birds either with or without a few 

coloured ticks. 


Cl Cl Three contain C 
er = but not /; these 

must be coloured 
Ci Ci i birds. One con- 
CI Ci SSSR 


tains neither C nor 
7; this must be a 
white. From such 
a mating we ought, 
therefore, to obtain 
both white and 
coloured birds in 
the ratio 13:3. 
The results thus 
theoretically de- 


x 
SS 


y 
RS 


FIG. 13. 


Diagram to illustrate the nature of the Fy generation 
from the cross between dominant white and duced were found 


recessive white fowls. 


to accord with the 
actual facts of experiment. The F, birds were all 
“ticked” whites, and in the F, generation came white 


VII DOMINANCE 69 


and coloured birds in the expected ratio. There seems, 
therefore, little reason to doubt that the dominant 
white is a coloured bird in which the absence of 
colour is due to the action of a colour-inhibiting 
factor, though as to the nature of that factor we can 


Fic. 14. 


Ears of beardless and bearded wheat. The beardless condition is dominant 
to the bearded. 


at present make no surmise. It is probable that 
other facts, which at first sight do not appear to be 
in agreement with the “Presence and Absence” 
hypothesis, will eventually be brought into line 
+ + through the action of inhibitor factors. Such a 


70 MENDELISM CHAP. 


case, for instance, is that of bearded and beardless 
wheats. Though the beard is obviously the addi- 
tional character, the bearded condition is recessive to 
the beardless. Probably we ought to regard the 
beardless as a bearded wheat in which there is an 
inhibitor that stops the beard from growing. It is 
not unlikely that as time goes on we shall find 
many more such cases of the action of inhibitor 
factors, and we must be prepared to find that the 
same visible effect may be produced either by 
the addition or by the omission of a factor. The 
dominant and recessive white poultry are indistin- 
guishable in appearance. Yet the one contains a 
factor more and the other a factor less than the 
coloured bird. 

A phenomenon sometimes termed irregularity of 
dominance has been investigated in a few cases, | 
In certain breeds of poultry such as Dorkings there 
occurs an extra toe directed backwards like the hallux 
(cf. Fig. 15), In some families this character behaves 
as an ordinary dominant to the normal, giving the 
expected 3:1 ratio in F, But in other families 
similarly bred the proportions of birds with and with- 
out the extra toe appear to be unusual. It has 
been shown that in such a family some of the birds 
without the extra toe’ may nevertheless transmit the 
peculiarity when mated with birds belonging to 
strains in which the extra toe never occurs. Though 
the external appearance of the bird generally affords 
some indication of the nature of the gametes which 
it is carrying, this is not always the case. Nevertheless 
we have reason to suppose that the character segregates 
in the gametes, though the nature of these cannot 


vil DOMINANCE 71 


always be decided from the appearance of the bird 
which bears them. 

There are cases in which an apparent irregularity 
of dominance has been shown to depend upon 
another character, as in the experiments with sheep 
carried out by Professor Wood. In these experi- 
ments two breeds were crossed, of which one, the 
Dorset, is horned in both sexes, while the other, 
the Suffolk, is without horns in either sex. Which- 


FIG. 15. 


Fowls’ feet. On the right a normal, and on the left one with an extra toe. 


ever way the cross was made the resulting F, 
generation was similar; the rams were horned, and 
the ewes were hornless. In the F, generation raised 
from these F, animals both horned and hornless 
types appeared in both sexes but in.very different pro- 
portions. While the horned rams were about three 
times as numerous as the hornless, this relation was 
reversed among the females, in which the horned 
formed only about one-quarter of the total. The 
simplest explanation of this interesting case is to 


72 MENDELISM CHAP. VII 


suppose that the dominance of the horned character 
depends upon the sex of the animal—that it is 
dominant in the male, but recessive in the female. A 
pretty experiment was devised for putting this view to 
the test. Ifit is true, equal numbers of gametes with 
and without the horned factor must be produced by the 
F, ewes, while the factor should be lacking in all the 
gametes of the hornless F, rams. A hornless ram, 


Dorset Suffolk Suffolk Dorset 
Ram Ewe Ram Ewe 


ca ee 35x? 
Se ee 


é 5 @ 
Fic. 16. 


Scheme to illustrate the inheritance of horns in sheep. Heterozygous males shown 
dark with a white spot, heterozygous females light with a dark spot in the centre. 


therefore, put to a flock of F, ewes should give rise to 
equal numbers of zygotes which are heterozygous 
for the horned character, and of zygotes in which it is 
completely absent. And since the heterozygous 
males are horned, while the heterozygous females 
are hornless, we should expect from this mating 
equal numbers of horned and hornless rams, but only 
hornless ewes. The result of the experiment con- 
firmed this expectation. Of the ram lambs g were 
horned and 8 were hornless, while all the 11 ewe 
lambs were completely destitute of horns. 


PLATE LT 


‘quUT aM Sso[U 


oy Fr 


‘quvy awq pouopy “f 


‘eT NI 


‘OMG YOSIOd ‘S 


ON 


‘query we 


TYVAddV SAdA LT 


A sso WO YY 


‘ue YOHNS ‘1 


“SLNAUVd 


‘t 


‘quevy we 


YT powioY 


‘ury Ty ce 


I 


CHAPTER VIII 
WILD FORMS AND DOMESTIC VARIETIES 


In discussing the phenomena of reversion we have 
seen that in most cases such reversion occurs when 
the two varieties which are crossed each contain 
certain factors lacking in the other, of which the 
full complement is necessary for the production 
of the reversionary wild form. This at once suggests 
the idea that the various domestic forms of animals 
and plants have arisen by the omission from time 
to time of this factor or of that. In some cases 
we have clear evidence that this is the most 
natural interpretation of the relation between the 
cultivated and the wild forms. Probably the species 
in which it is most evident is the sweet-pea (Lathyrus 
odoratus). We have already seen reason to suppose 
that as regards certain structural features the Bush 
variety is a wild lacking the factor for the pro- 
cumbent habit, that the Cupid is a wild without 
the factor for the long internode, and that the 
Bush Cupid is a wild minus both these factors. Nor 
is the evidence less clear for the many colour 
varieties. In illustration we may consider in more 
detail a case in which the cross between two whites 


73 


74. MENDELISM CHAP, 


resulted iri a complete reversion to the purple colour 
characteristic of the wild Sicilian form (PLIV.). In 
this particular instance subsequent breeding from the 
purples resulted in the production of six different 
colour forms in addition to whites. The proportion 
of the coloured forms to the whites was 9:7 
(cf. p. 41), but it is with the relation of the six 
coloured forms that we are concerned here. Of 
these six forms, three were purples and three were 
reds, The three purple forms were (1) the wild 
bicolor purple with blue wings known in cultivation 
as the Purple Invincible (Pl. IV., 4); (2) a deep 
purple with purple wings (PI. 1V., 5); and (3) a very 
dilute purple known as the Picotee (Pl. IV., 6). Cor- 
responding to these three purple forms were three reds: 
(1) a bicolor red known as Painted Lady (PI. IV., 7) ; 
(2) a deep red with red wings known as Miss Hunt 
(Pl. IV., 8); and (3) a very pale red which we have 
termed Tinged White’ (Pl. IV., 9). In the F, 
generation the total number of purples bore to the 
total number of reds the ratio 3:1, and this ratio 
was maintained for each of the corresponding classes. 
Purple, therefore, is dominant to red, and each of the 
three classes of red differs from its corresponding 
purple in not possessing the blue factor (8B) which 
turns it into purple. Again, the proportion in which 
the three classes of purples appeared was g bicolors, 
3 deep purples, 4 picotees. We are, therefore, con- 
cerned here with the operation of two factors: 
(1) a light wing factor, which renders the bicolor 


1 The reader who searches florists’ catalogues for these varieties will 
probably experience disappointment. The sweet-pea has been much 
‘‘improved” in the past few years, and it is unlikely that the modern 
seedsman would list such unfashionable forms, 


PLATE IV. 


10° 

1, 2, Emily Henderson; 3, F, reversionary Purple; 4-10, Various F, 

forms: 4, Purple; 5, Deep Purple; 6, Picotee ; 7, Painted Lady; 
8, Miss Hunt; 9, Tinged White; 10, White. 


vir WILD AND DOMESTIC VARIETIES 75 


dominant to the dark winged form ; and (2) a factor 
for intense colour, which occurs in the bicolor and in 
the deep purple, but is lacking in the dilute picotee. 
And here it should be mentioned that these con- 
clusions rest upon an exhaustive set of experiments 
involving the breeding of many thousands of plants. 
In this cross, therefore, we are concerned with the 
presence or absence of five factors, which we may 
denote as follows :— 


A colour base, 2. 

A colour developer, C. 

A purple factor, 2. 

A light wing factor, Z 

A factor for intense colour, 7 


On this notation our six coloured forms are :— 


(1) Purple bicolor. : . CRBIL} 

(2) Deep purple : ‘ . CRBI. 

(3) Picotee . CRBLIi or CRBU1. 
(4) Red bicolor ( = Painted Lady) CRELI. 

(5) Deep red (=Miss Hunt) . CRO//. 

(6) Tinged white . : . CRbLi or CRO. 


It will be noticed in this series that the various 
coloured forms can be expressed by the omission of 
one or more factors from the purple bicolor of the 
wild type. With the complete omission of each 
factor a new colour type results, and it is difficult 
to resist the inference that the various cultivated 
forms of the sweet-pea have arisen from the wild 
by some process of this kind. Such a view tallies 
with what we know of the behaviour of the wild 


1 It is to be understood that wherever a given factor is present the 
plant may be homozygous or heterozygous for it without alteration in 


its colour. 


76 MENDELISM CHAP, 


form when crossed by any of the garden varieties. 
Wherever such crossing has been made the form of 
the hybrid has been that of the wild, thus supporting 
the view that the wild contains a complete set of all 
the differentiating factors which are to be found in the 
sweet-pea. 

Moreover, this view is in harmony with such 
historical evidence as is to be gleaned from botanical 
literature, and from old seedsmen’s catalogues. The 
wild sweet-pea first reached this country in 1699, 
having been sent from Sicily by the monk Franciscus 
Cupani as a present to a certain Dr. Uvedale in the 
county of Middlesex. Somewhat later we hear of 
two new varieties, the red bicolor, or Painted Lady, 
and the white, each of which may be regarded as 
having “sported” from the wild purple by the 
omission of the purple factor, or of one of the two 
colour factors. In 1793 we find a seedsman offering 
also what he called black and scarlet varieties. It is 
probable that these were our deep purple and Miss 
Hunt varieties, and that somewhere about this time 
the factor for the light wing (Z) was dropped out in 
certain plants. In 1860 we have evidence that the 
pale purple or Picotee, and with it doubtless the 
Tinged White, had come into existence. This time 
it was the factor for intense colour which had 
dropped out. And so the story goes on until the 
present day, and it is now possible to express by 
the same simple method the relation of the modern 
shades, of purples and reds, of blues and pinks, of 
hooded and wavy standards, to one another and 
to the original wild form. The constitution of many 
of these has now been worked out, and to-day it 


vir. WILD AND DOMESTIC VARIETIES 77 


would be a simple though perhaps tedious task to 
denote all the different varieties by a series of letters 
indicating the factors which they contain, instead of 
by the present system of calling them after kings 
and queens, and famous generals, and ladies more or 
less well known. 

From what we know of the history of the various 
strains of sweet-peas one thing stands out clearly. 
The new character does not arise from a pre-existing 
variety by any process of gradual selection, conscious 
or otherwise. It turns up suddenly complete in 
itself, and thereafter it can be associated by crossing 
with other existing characters to produce a gamut 
of new varieties. If, for example, the character of 
hooding in the standard (cf. Pl. IL, 7) suddenly 
turned up in such a family as that shown on Plate 
IV., we should be able to get a hooded form corre- 
sponding to each of the forms with the erect stan- 
dard; in other words, the arrival of the new form 
would give us the possibility of fourteen varieties 
instead of seven. As we know, the hooded char- 
acter already exists. It is recessive to the erect 
standard, and we have reason to suppose that it 
arose as a sudden sport by the omission of the factor 
in whose presence the standard assumes the erect 
shape characteristic of the wild flower. It is largely 
by keeping his eyes open and seizing upon such 
sports for crossing purposes that the horticulturist 
“improves” the plants with which he deals. How 
these sports or mutations come about we can 
now surmise. They must owe their origin to a 
disturbance in the processes of cell division through 
which the gametes originate. At some stage or 


78 MENDELISM CHAP, 


other the normal equal distribution of the various 
factors is upset, and some of the gametes receive 
a factor less than others. From the union of two 
such gametes, provided that they are still capable 
of fertilisation, comes the zygote which in course of 
growth develops the new character. 

Why these mutations arise: what leads to the 
surmised unequal division of the gametes: of this 
we know practically nothing. Nor until we can 
induce the production of mutations at will are we 
likely to understand the conditions which govern 
their formation. Nevertheless there are already 
hints scattered about the recent literature of experi- 
mental biology which lead us to hope that we may 
know more of these matters in the future. 

In respect of the evolution of its now multi- 
tudinous varieties, the story of the sweet-pea is 
clear and straightforward. These have all arisen 
from the wild by a process of continuous loss. 
Everything was there in the beginning, and as the 
wild plant parted with factor after factor there came 
into being the long series of derived forms. Exquisite 
as are the results of civilisation, it is by the degrada- 
tion of the wild that they have been brought about. 
How far are we justified in regarding this as a 
picture of the manner in which evolution works ? 

There are certainly other species in which we 
must suppose that this is the way that the various 
domesticated forms have arisen. Such, for example, 
is the case in the rabbit, where most of the colour 
varieties are recessive to the wild agouti form. Such 
also is the case in the rat, where the black and albino 
varieties and the various pattern forms are also reces- 


vir WILD AND DOMESTIC VARIETIES 79 


sive to the wild agouti type. And with the excep- 
tion of a certain yellow variety to which we shall 
refer later, such is also the case with the many fancy 
varieties of mice. 

Nevertheless there are other cases in which we 
must suppose evolution to have proceeded by the 
interpolation of characters. In discussing reversion 
on crossing, we have already seen that this may not 
occur until the F, generation, as, for example, in the 
instance of the fowls’ combs (cp. p. §9). The rever- 
sion to the single comb occurred as the result of the 
removal of the two factors for rose and pea. These 
two domesticated varieties must be regarded as each 
possessing an additional factor in comparison with 
the wild single-combed bird. During the evolution 
of the fowl, these two factors must be conceived of 
as having been interpolated in some way. And the 
same holds good for the inhibitory factor on which, 
as we have seen, the dominant white character of 
certain poultry depends. In pigeons, too, if we 
regard the blue rock as the ancestor of the domesti- 
cated breeds, we must suppose that an additional 
melanic factor has arisen at some stage. For we 
have already seen that black is dominant to blue, 
and the characters of F,, together with the greater 
number of blacks than blues in F,, negatives the 
possibility that we are here dealing with an inhibitory 
factor. The hornless or polled condition of cattle, 
again, is dominant to the horned condition, and if, 
as seems reasonable, we regard the original ancestors 
of domestic cattle as having been horned, we have 
here again the interpolation of an inhibitory factor 
somewhere in the course of evolution. 


80 MENDELISM CHAP. VIII 


On the whole, therefore, we must be prepared to 
admit that the evolution of domestic varieties may 
come about by a process of addition of factors in 
some cases and of subtraction in others. It may be 
that what we term additional factors fall irf€o distinct 
categories from the rest. So far, experiment seems 
to show that they are either of the nature of melanic 
factors, or of inhibitory factors, or of reduplication 
factors as in the case of the fowls’ combs. But while 
the data remain so scanty, speculation in these 
matters is too hazardous to be profitable. 


CHAPTER IX 
REPULSION AND COUPLING OF FACTORS 


ALTHOUGH different factors may act together to 
produce specific results in the zygote through their 
interaction, yet in all the cases we have hitherto 
considered the heredity of each of the different factors 
is entirely independent. The interaction of the 
factors affects the characters of the zygote, but makes 
no difference to the distribution of the separate 
factors, which is always in strict accordance with 
the ordinary Mendelian scheme. Each factor in 
this respect behaves as though the other were not 
present. 

A few cases have been worked out in which the 
distribution .of the different factors to the gametes 
is affected by their simultaneous presence in the 
zygote. And the influence which they are able to 
exert upon one another in such cases is of two kinds. 
They may repel one another, refusing, as it were, to 
enter into the same gamete, or they may attract one 
another, and, becoming linked together, pass into 
the same gamete, as it were, by preference. For the 
moment we may consider these two sets of pheno- 
mena apart. 

81 G 


82 MENDELISM CHAP. 


One of the best illustrations of repulsion between 
factors occurs in the sweet-pea. We have already 
seen that the loss of the blue or purple factor (B) 
from the wild bicolor results in the formation of the 
red bicolor known as Painted Lady (Pl. IV., 7). 
Further, we have seen that the hooded standard 
is recessive to the ordinary erect standard. The 
omission of the factor for the erect standard (£) 
from the purple bicolor (Pl. II., 5) results in a 
hooded purple known as Duke of Westminster (PI. 
II., 7). And here it should be mentioned that 
in the corresponding hooded forms the difference in 
colour between the wings and standard is not nearly 
so marked as in the forms with the erect standard, 
but the difference in structure appears to affect the 
colour, which becomes nearly uniform. This may 
be readily seen by comparing the picture of the 
purple bicolor on Plate II. with that of the Duke 
of Westminster flower. 

Now when a Duke of Westminster is mated with 
a Painted Lady the factor for erect standard (£) is 
brought in by the red, and that for blue (4) by the 
Duke, and the offspring are consequently all purple 
bicolors. Purples so formed are all heterozygous 
for these two factors, and were the case a simple one, 
such as those which have already been discussed, 
we should expect the F, generation to consist of the 
four forms erect purple, hooded purple, erect red, 
and hooded red in the ratio 9: 3: 3:1. Such, how- 
ever, is not the case. The F, generation actually 
consists of only three forms, viz. erect red, erect 
purple, and hooded purple, and the ratio in which 
these three forms occur is 1:2:1. No hooded red 


Ix REPULSION AND COUPLING 83 


has been known to occur in such a family. More- 
over, further breeding shows that while the erect 
reds and the hooded purples always breed true, the 
erect purples in such families zever breed true, but 
Painted Lady X Duke of Westminster 
(erect red). (hooded purple) 


Purple Invincible 
(erect purple) 


I I | 
Painted Purple Invincible Duke of 
Lady ‘Westminster 


(x) (2) (1) 


always behave like the original F, plant, giving the 
three forms again in the ratio 1:2:1. Yet we 
know that there is no difficulty in getting purple 
bicolors to breed true from other families; and we 
know also that hooded red sweet-peas exist in other 
strains. 

On the assumption that there exists a repulsion 
between the factors for erect standard and blue in 
a plant which is heterozygous for both, this peculiar 
case receives a simple explanation. The constitutions 
of the erect red and the hooded purple are EA6d 
and eeBB respectively, and that of the F, erect 
purple is He5s. Now let us suppose that in such a 
zygote there exists a repulsion between £ and 3B, 
such that when the plant forms gametes these two 
factors will not go into the same gamete. On this 
view it can only form two kinds of gametes, viz. Ed 
and @B, and these, of course, will be formed in equal 
numbers. Such a plant on self-fertilisation must 
give the zygotic series HEG)+2 EeBb+eeBB, te. 


84 MENDELISM CHAP, 


I erect red, 2 erect purples, and 1 hooded purple. 
And because the erect reds and the hooded purples 
are respectively homozygous for & and BS, they 
must thenceforward breed true. The erect purples, 
on the other hand, being always formed by the 
union of a gamete £4 with a gamete eB, are always 
heterozygous for both of these factors. They can, 
consequently, never breed true, but must always give 
erect reds, erect purples, and hooded purples in the 


EEbb eeBB Parents 
Eb Eb eB eB gametes 
EeBb F, 

2 Pa TG = 

‘a ( Zo —> EEbb<— 25)" * 

Q ) x —> EeBb <—eB | 3 

# | eB —> EeBb <— 2b | & 

g eB —>eeBB <—eB a 

er eeipeiey a 


F, generation 


ratio 1:2:1. The experimental facts are readily 
explained on the assumption of repulsion between 
the two factors B and £& during the formation of the 
gametes in a plant which is heterozygous for both. 
Other similar cases of factorial repulsion have 
been demonstrated in the sweet-pea, and two of 
these are also concerned with the two factors with 
‘1 In this account the repulsion between the two factors in question 
has been assumed to be complete. Recent work, however, has shown 
that this is probably not quite true. It is likely that the gametes ZB 


and ed are formed though only very rarely. For further discussion 
the reader is referred to the Appendix at the end of this chapter. 


Ix REPULSION AND COUPLING 85 


which we have just been dealing. Two distinct 
varieties of pollen grains occur in this species, viz. 
the ordinary oblong form and a rather smaller 
rounded grain. The former is dominant to the 
latter.. When a cross is made between a purple 
with round pollen and a red with long pollen the 
F, plant is a long pollened purple. But the F, 
generation consists of purples with round pollen, 
purples with long pollen, and reds with long pollen 
in the ratio 1:2:1. No red with round pollen 
appears in F, owing to repulsion between the factors 
for purple (4) and for long pollen (Z). Similarly 
plants produced by crossing a red hooded long with 
a red round having an erect standard give in 
F, long pollened reds with an erect standard, and 
these } in F, produce the three types round pollened 
erect, long pollened erect, and long pollened hooded 
in the ratio 1:2:1. The repulsion here is between 
the long pollen factor (Z) and the factor for the 
erect standard (4). 

Yet another similar case is known in which we 
are concerned with quite different factors. In some 
sweet-peas the axils whence the leaves and flower- 
stalks spring from the main stem are of a deep red 
colour. In others they are green. The dark 
pigmented axil is dominant to the light one. Again, 
in some sweet-peas the anthers are sterile, setting no 
pollen, and this condition is recessive to the ordinary 
fertile condition. When a sterile plant with a dark 
axil is crossed by a fertile plant with a light axil, the 


1 It should be mentioned that as the shape of the pollen coat, 
like that of the seed coat, is a maternal character, all the grains of any 
given plant are either long or else round. The two kinds do not 
occur together on the same plant. 


86 MENDELISM CHAP. 


F, plants are all fertile with dark axils. But such 
plants in F, give fertiles with light axils, fertiles 
with dark axils, and steriles with dark axils in the 
ratio 1:2:1. No light axilled steriles appear from 
such a cross owing to the repulsion between the 
factor for dark axil (D) and that for the fertile 
anther (7). 

These four cases have already been found in the 
sweet-pea, and similar phenomena have been met 
with in primulas and in peas. To certain seemingly 
analogous cases in animals where sex is concerned 
we shall refer later. 

Now all of these four cases present a common 
feature which probably has not escaped the attention 
of the reader. In all of them ‘the original cross 
was such as to introduce one of the repelling factors 
with each of the two parents. If we denote our two 
factors by A and B, the crosses have always been 
of the nature AdAddxaaBs. Let us now consider 
what happens when both of the factors, which in 
these cases repel one another, are introduced by one 
of ‘the parents, and neither by the other parent. 
And in particular we will take the case in which we 
are concerned with purple and red flower colour, 
and with long and round pollen, ze. with the factors 
Band L. When a purple long (BSLL) is crossed 
with a red round (dd//) the F, (BOLD) is a purple with 
long pollen, identical in appearance with that produced 
by crossing the long pollened red with the round 
pollened purple. But the nature of the F, generation 
is in some respects very different. The ratio of 
purples to reds and of longs to rounds is in each case 
3:1, as before. But instead of an association 


= REPULSION AND COUPLING 87 


between the red and the long pollen characters the 
reverse is the case. The long pollen character is 
now associated with purple and the round pollen 
with red. The association, however, is not quite 
complete, and the examination of a large quantity of 
similarly bred material shows that the purple longs 
are about twelve times as numerous as the purple 
rounds, while the red rounds are rather more than three 
times as many as the red longs. Now this peculiar 
result could be brought about if the gametic 
series produced by the F, plant consisted of 
7 BL+1 Bl+1 6L+7 & out of every 16 gametes. 
Fertilisation between two such similar series of 16 
gametes would result in 256 plants, of which 177 
would be purple longs, 15 purple rounds, 15 red 
longs, and 49 red rounds—a proportion of the 
four different kinds very close to that actually 
found by experiment. It will be noticed that in 
the whole family the purples are to the reds as 3:1, 
and the longs are also three times as numerous as 
the rounds. The peculiarity of the case lies in the 
distribution of these two characters with regard to 
one another. In some way or other the factors for 
blue and for long pollen become linked together 
in the cell divisions that give rise to the gametes, 
but the linking is not complete. This holds good 
for all the four cases in which repulsion between the 
factors occurs when one of the two factors is intro- 
duced by each of the parents. When both of the 
factors ave brought into the cross by the same parent 
we get coupling between them instead of reputszon. 
The phenomena of repulsion and coupling between 
separate factors are intimately related, though 


88 MENDELISM CHAP, 


hitherto we have not been able to decide why this 
should be so. 

Nor for the present can we suggest why certain 
factors should be linked together in the peculiar 
way that we have reason to suppose that they are 
during the process of the formation of the gametes. 
Nevertheless the phenomena are very definite, and it 
is not unlikely that a further study of them may 
throw important light on the architecture of the 
living cell. 


APPENDIX TO CHAPTER IX 


As it is possible that some readers may care, in spite of 
its complexity, to enter rather more fully into the peculiar 
phenomenon of the coupling of characters, I have brought 
together some further data in this Appendix. In the case 
we have already considered, where the factors for blue 
colour and long pollen are concerned, we have been led to 
suppose that the gametes produced by the heterozygous 
plant are of the nature 7 BZL:1 Bl:1 6L£:7 b. Sucha 
series of ova fertilised by a similar series of pollen 
grains will give a-generation of the following com- 
position :— 

49 BBLL+7 BBLi+7 BOLL +49 BoL/ 
+7 BBLI+7 BOLL+ Boll 


+ Boll 
+49 BbL/ 
— 


177 purple, long 
+ BBU+7 Bbll+ bLL+7 b6Li4+ 49 bbu 


+7 Boul +7 O6L1 
-_—_—,_ ————’ —— 
15 purple, 15 red, 49 red, 
round long round 


and as this theoretical result fits closely with the actual 
figures obtained by experiment we have reason for 


Ix REPULSION AND COUPLING 89 


supposing that the heterozygous plant produces a series of 
gametes in which the factors are coupled in this way. The 
intensity of the coupling, however, varies in different cases. 
Where we are dealing with another, viz. fertility (7) and 
the dark axil (D) the experimental numbers accord with 
the view that the gametic series is here 15 #D:1 Fa: 
1fD:15 fd. The coupling is in this instance more 
intense. In the case of the erect standard (Z) and blue- 
ness (#) the coupling is even more intense, and the 
experimental evidence available at present points to the 
gametic series here being 127 £B:1 £b:1 eB:127 eb. 
There is evidence also for supposing that the intensity of the 
coupling may vary in different families for the same pair of 
factors. The coupling between blue and long pollen is 
generally on the 7:1:1:7 basis, but in some cases it may 
be on the 15:1:1:15 basis. But though the intensity 
of the coupling may vary it varies in an orderly way. If 
A and B are the two factors concerned, the results obtained 
in F, are explicable on the assumption that the ratio of the 
four sorts of gametes produced is a term of the series— 


3 AB+Ab+aB+ 3 ab 
7 AB+Ab+aB+ 7 ab 
15 A4B+Ab+aB+15 ab, etc., etc. 


In such a series the number of gametes containing 4 is 
equal to the number lacking 4A, and the same is true for 
&. Consequently the number of zygotes formed contain- 
ing A is three times as great as the number of zygotes which 
do not contain 4; and similarly for &. The proportion 
of dominants to recessives in each case is 3:1. It is 
only in the distribution of the characters with relation to 
one another that these cases differ from a simple Mendelian 
case. 

In the account given in the foregoing chapter we treated 
of repulsion between two factors as being complete, and 
for practical purposes this is generally true. Nevertheless 
the most recent work indicates that repulsion like coupling 
is partial, and the reasons for taking this view may best be 


go MENDELISM CHAP. 


given in a brief account of the experiments that have led 
to it. There exists a monstrous form of sweet-pea in 
which the flowers are malformed and the stigma protrudes 
owing to the cleft condition of the keel which normally 
curves it (cf. Fig. 16a). From its fancied resemblance to 


FIG. 16A. 


The “‘cretin ”” sweet-pea. 


a gaping mouth with a protruding tongue, such a plant has 
been termed a cretin. The pollen of a cretin was used to 
fertilise a sterile plant with normal flowers, and all the 
resulting F, plants were normal and fertile. Fertility (7) 
is dominant to sterility (7), and the normal flower (/V) is 
dominant to the cretin (7). The cross was so made that 
one factor went in from each parent. An F, generation 
was raised from 9 F, plants and consisted of — 


IX REPULSION AND COUPLING gI 


336 normal fertile, 

150 normal sterile, 

143 cretin fertile, 
II cretin sterile. 


Had it not been for the small proportion of sterile cretins 
the case might have been considered as one in which com- 
plete repulsion occurred between the factors # and VV 
during the formation of the gametes of the F, plant. The 
presence of these sterile cretins may, however, be explained 
on the assumption that the repulsion is not complete, but 
that the gametes produced by the F, plant form the series 
NF: 3 Nf:32F:nf The union of such a series of female 
gametes with a similar series of male gametes as occurs 
when the F, plants are selfed would result in a generation 
consisting of— 

33 normal fertile, 

15 normal sterile, 

15 cretin fertile, 

I cretin sterile, 


proportions which are almost precisely those actually found 
by experiment. From this and other experiments there is 
reason for supposing that in cases where repulsion occurs 
between two factors 4 and B the repulsion is not absolute, 
but that the gametes are produced in one or other of the 
series— 

AB:3 Ab:3 aB:ab, 

AB:7 Ab:7 aB: ab, 

AB: 15 Ab: 15 aB: as, etc. 


Such series are in a sense the converse of those in which 
partial coupling occurs, for in the one case it is the two 
terms Aé and aB that are reduplicated, while in the other 
it is in the terms AZ and aé where this reduplication takes 
place. A point of importance in connection with this view 
of the nature of repulsion is that it enables us to bring the 
phenomenon more closely into line with that of coupling. 
Whatever may be the meaning of repulsion and coupling, 


g2 MENDELISM CHAP. 


and this at present is by no means clear, it is now evident 
that they will both ultimately take their place in some 
common scheme. The relations between the two types of 
series may perhaps be more clearly shown in the subjoined 
table which brings out several points of interest in connec- 
tion with them. The series marked with an asterisk are 
those which have already been demonstrated experimentally. 
The middle term in the table, in which all four kinds of 
gametes are produced in equal numbers, is, of course, that 
of a simple Mendelian case where neither repulsion nor 
coupling occur. . 


n 
‘S 2 Distribution of S 3 
oes Factors in Gametic 6 hs Form of F2 Generation. 
Zz Ss Series. Ag 2 
AB. Ab. aB. ab AB. Ab, ab. ab 
2n i (a-xr (m-1) 1 4n2 2n2+r net n2—1 i 
256 THIS 3227) I 65536 32769 16383 16383 I 
128 r: 63: 63 I 16384 8193 4095 4095 I 
64 RE SEs gt I 4096 2049 1023 1023 I 
32 Eh AS A ag I 1024 513 255 255 I 
16 RE OF 7 1 256 129 63 63 1* 
8 ri 3 3: 4. 64 33 15 15 1* 
4 Ps LS fs = 16 9 3 3 I 
8 22 £s FE 3, 64 4 7 7 oF 
16 7 SER OR Boy 256 1977 15 15 49* 
32 5 em aa ot 1024 737 3r 3r 225* 
64 35 Hae or 4096 3009 63 63 g6r 
128 63 ce £2 163 16384 12161 127 127 3060" 
256 12 Li 8 Te! 65536 88 2 255 1607 
by nd Ie 2 ee 1) re) WP Gh 1) (p22) ns) Ga 1) 


As the table shows it is possible to give generalised expres- 
sions by means of which the nature of the generations 
resulting from any of the series can readily be calculated. 
The table brings out clearly the reason why repulsion at 
first appeared to be absolute, for as its intensity increases 
the zygote exhibiting both recessive characters rapidly 
becomes exceedingly rare. Even for a series of such com- 
paratively low intensity as 4B:15 44:15 aB: ab the ex- 
pected proportion of aaéé zygotes is only 1 in 1024, while 
for a series on the 1: 127 basis it is only 1 in 65,536. 

A question of interest is whether the coupling and re- 


Ix REPULSION AND COUPLING 93 


pulsion for the same pair of factors is of the same intensity 
—whether for instance two characters coupled on the 15:1 
basis will exhibit repulsion on the 1:15 basis. The existing 
evidence certainly favours such a view. Experiments made 
during the present year (1912) render it practically certain 
that in the cretin case the coupling between the normal 
flower and the fertile anther is on a 3: 1:1: 3 basis; and 
there is strong reason for supposing that the repulsion 
between long pollen and purple is on a 1:7:7:1 basis. 
In each of these two instances the coupling and repulsion 
appear to be of the same intensity. In the higher series 
very large numbers are needed to determine the intensity 
of the repulsion. Thus for the series 1 4B:127 4b: 127 
aB:1 ab the double recessive is expected only once in 
65,536 plants. It is unlikely that such cases will ever be 
decided by direct experiment, but it may be asserted that 
where the intensity of coupling is high, the evidence avail- 
able certainly points to the intensity of repulsion being 
high also. 

One more point of interest may be noted in connection 
with these series. As the table shows it is possible where 
coupling is concerned to express the various gametic series 
by the general formula (x-1)AB:Ab:aB:(n- 1) ab, 
where 2 is the total number of the gametes in the series 
A plant producing such a series of gametes gives rise to a 
family of zygotes in which 37” — (22-1) show both of the 
dominant characters and 2?—(22—1) show both of the 
recessive characters, while the number of the two classes 
which each show one of the two dominants is (27-1). 
When in such a series the coupling becomes closer the 
value of increases, but in comparison with 7? its value 
becomes less and less. The larger x becomes the more 
negligible is its value relatively to ~*. If, therefore, the 
coupling were very intense, the series 3m*—(2m—1): 
(2m —1):(22-—1):2*-—(22-—1) would approximate more 
and more to the series 377:0:0: 7%, ze. toa simple 3:1 
ratio. Though the point is probably of more theoretical 
than practical interest, it is not impossible that some of the 


94 MENDELISM CHAP. 


cases which have hitherto been regarded as following a 
simple 3 : 1 ratio will turn out on further analysis to belong 
to this more complicated scheme. 

This account of coupling and repulsion is based upon 
the evidence from the sweet-pea, which has been more 
carefully studied in this connection than any other species, 
and it is not pretended that it offers more than a pre- 
liminary account of these interesting and peculiar phen- 
omena. Facts are known which are not entirely covered 
by the scheme suggested. Baur,! for instance, working 
with Antirrhinum, has adduced evidence that in a case 
where coupling in F, resulted from the cross AA BB x aabd, 
the converse cross 4444 x aaBB behaved in a normal 
Mendelian manner without showing repulsion between 
A and B. 

Lastly, we may ask whether these phenomena are con- 
fined to plants or whether they are also to be found among 
animals. That in certain animals something in the nature 
of repulsion takes place between certain factors and the 
sex factor will appear in the next chapter. And evidence 
is beginning to accumulate suggesting that coupling and 
repulsion may occur in animals independent of the sex 
factor. Morgan’s recent work on the inheritance of eye 
colour and wing characters in the little pomace fly ? (Droso- 
phila) is strongly suggestive of the operation of coupling, 
while Hagedoorn ® has described the repulsion between the 
factor for agouti (G) and that for colour (C) in a certain 
strain of mice. Quite recently I have myself obtained 
experimental evidence for the coupling of certain factors 
influencing the coat colour of rabbits. Such evidence then 
as we possess suggests that as we learn more of these 
matters we shall find these phenomena under some form 
or other playing an important part in the heredity of 
animals. And, indeed, this is what, on general considera- 
tions, we might have been led to expect. Among the 

1 Zeit. f. ind. Abst. u. Vererb., 1912. 


2 Journ. Exp. Zool,, 1911. 
3 Zeit. f. ind. Abst. u. Vererb., 1912. 


IX REPULSION AND COUPLING 95 


phenomena of heredity in man few things are more striking 
than the remarkable way in which children so often re- 
semble this or that parent, not in one or two, but simul- 
taneously in a number of characters. The human species 
shows so many heritable features, and man is such a 
mongrel, that it would be reasonable to look for relatively. 
few points of resemblance between parent and offspring. 
The fact that these are frequently so numerous suggests that 
in man characters are transmitted as it were in bunches, 
and it is not improbable that the study of cases in which 
coupling occurs will eventually render clearer what is 
evidently an important feature in the heredity of our own 
kind. 


CHAPTER X 
SEX 


IN their simplest expression the phenomena ex- 
hibited by Mendelian characters are sharp and clean 
cut. Clean cut and sharp also are the phenomena 
of sex. It was natural, therefore, that a comparison 
should have been early instituted between these two 
sets of phenomena. As a general rule, the cross 
between a male and a female results in the produc- 
tion of the two sexes in approximately equal 
numbers. The cross between a heterozygous domin- 
ant and a recessive also leads to equal numbers of 
recessives and of heterozygous dominants. Is it 
not, therefore, possible that one of the sexes is 
heterozygous for a factor which is lacking in the 
other, and that the presence or absence of this factor 
determines the sex of the zygote? The results of 
some recent experiments would appear to justify 
this interpretation, at any rate in particular cases. 
Of these, the simplest is that of the common currant 
moth (Adraxas grossulariata), of which there exists 
a pale variety (Fig. 17) known as Jacticolor. The 
experiments of Doncaster and Raynor showed that 
the variety behaved as a simple recessive to the 
96 


x SEX 97 


normal form. But the distribution of the dominants 
and recessives with regard to the sexes was peculiar. 
The original cross was between a J/acticolor female 
and a normal male. All the F, moths of both sexes 


FIG, 17. 


Abraxas grossulariata, the common currant moth, and (on the right) its paler 
lacticolor variety. 


~were of the normal grossulariata type. The F, 
insects were then paired together and gave a 
generation consisting of 3 normal : 1 /actzcolor. But 
all the J/acticolor were females, and all the males 


Lacticolor X Grossulariata 


3 


Lact.d x Gr.? 4 Gr.d xX Lact. 


| aed 1 q T T i] t T T 1 
Gr.d Lact.? Gr.d Gr.d Gr.@ Lact.? Gr.d Lact.o Gr.? Lact.2 


were of the normal pattern. It was, however, found 

possible to obtain the J/acticolor male by mating a 

lacticolor female with the F, male. The family result- 

ing from this cross consisted of normal males and 

normal females, dactzcolor males and dactzcolor females, 
H 


98 MENDELISM CHAP. 


and the four sorts were produced in approximately 
equal numbers. In such a family there was no 
special association of either of the two colour 
varieties with one sex rather than the other. But 
the reverse cross, F, female by /acticolor male, gave 
a very different result. As in the previous cross, 
such families contained equal numbers of the normal 
form and of the recessive variety. But all of the 
normal grossulariata were males, while all the éact:- 
cq@lor were females. Now this seemingly complex 
collection of facts is readily explained if we make 
the following three assumptions :— 

(1) The grossularzata character (G) is dominant 
to the éactzcolor character (g). This is obviously 
justified by the experiments, for, leaving the sex 
distribution out of account, we get the expected 
3:1 ratio from F, x F,, and also the expected ratio 
of equality when the heterozygote is crossed with 
the recessive. 

(2) The female is heterozygous for a dominant 
factor (/) which is lacking in the male. The con- 
stitution of a female is consequently //, and of a 
male f/f This assumption is in harmony with the 
fact that the sexes are produced in approximately 
equal numbers. 

(3) There exists repulsion between the factors 
G and F in a zygote which is heterozygous for 
them both. Such zygotes (F/Gg) must always be 
females, and on this assumption will produce gametes 
Fg and /G in equal numbers. 

We may now construct a scheme for com- 
parison with that on page 92 to show how these 
assumptions explain the experimental results. The 


x SEX 99 


original parents were J/acticolor female and grossu- 
lariata male, which on our assumptions must be 
Ffgg and /fGG respectively in constitution. Since 
the female is always heterozygous for F, her gametes 
must be of two kinds, viz. #g and fg, while those of 
the pure grossulariata male must be all f/G. When 
an ovum F¢ is fertilised by a spermatozoon /G, the 


Figg [2] 6G [¢] 
gametes { ie \ j te 


[J] feg FiGg(¢@] Gel ¢] Figg [2] 
Bee = Z 


I T T i, t T T 1 


Figg ffGg Figg FiGg ffGg ffGG FiGg Figg fiGg figg 
[2] [8] [9] [9] [6] [6] Ce] [9] [6] [3] 


Fic. 18. 


Scheme of inheritance in the F, and Fy, generations resulting from the cross of 
lacticolor female with grossulariata male. The character of each individual is 
represented by the sex signs in brackets, the black being grossudariata in 
appearance and the light ones dacticolor. 


resulting zygote, F/Gg, is heterozygous for both F 
and G, and in appearance is a female gvossularzata. 
The zygote resulting from the fertilisation of an 
ovum /g by a spermatozoon /G is heterozygous for 
G, but does not contain F, and therefore is a male 
grossulariata. Such a male being in constitution 
JfGg must produce gametes of two kinds, /G and /g, 
in equal numbers. And since we are assuming 
repulsion between F and G, the F, female being in 
constitution, F/Gg must produce equal numbers of 


100 MENDELISM CHAP, 


gametes /g and fG. For on our assumption / and 
G cannot enter into the same gamete. The series 
of gametes produced by the F, moths, therefore, are 
SG, fg by the male and Fg, fG by the female. The 
resulting F, generation consequently consists of 
the four classes of zygotes F/gg, F/Gg, ffGg, and 
J/GG in equal numbers. In other words, the sexes 
are produced in equal numbers, the proportion of 
normal grossulariata to lacticolor is 3:1, and all of 
the J/actzcolor are females; that is to say, the reé- 
sults worked out on our assumptions accord with 
those actually produced by experiment. We may 
now turn to the results which should be obtained 
by crossing the F, moths with the /actzcolor variety. 
And first we will take the cross Jactzcolor female x F, 
male. The gametes produced by the J/actzcolor 
female we have already seen to be /g and fg, while 
those produced by the F, male are f/G and fg. The 
bringing together of these two series of gametes must 
result in equal numbers of the four kinds of zygotes 
FfGg, Ffee, ffGg, and ffyg, 2.e. of female grossulariata 
and /acticolor, and of male grossulariata and lactécolor 
in equal numbers. Here, again, the calculated results 
accord with those of experiment. Lastly, we may 
examine what should happen when the F, female is 
crossed with the Jactzcolor male. The F, female, 
owing to the repulsion between F and G, produces 
only the two kinds of ova #g and /G, and produces 
them in equal numbers. Since the /actécolor male 
can contain neither / nor G, all of its spermatozoa 
must be fg. The results of such a cross, therefore, 
should be to produce equal numbers of the two 
kinds of zygote Ffgg and //Gg, te. of Jacticolor 


x SEX IOI 


females and of grossulariata males. And this, as we 
have already seen, is the actual result of such a cross. 

Before leaving the currant moth we may allude 
to an interesting discovery which arose out of these 
experiments. The /actzcolor variety in Great Britain 
is a southern form and is not known to occur in 
Scotland. Matings were made between wild Scotch 
females and dactzcolor males. The families resulting 
from such matings were precisely the same as those 
from dactzcolor males and F, females, viz. grossulariata 
males and /acticolor females only. We are, therefore, 
forced to regard the constitution of the wild grossu- 
lariata female as identical with that of the F, female, 
ze. aS heterozygous for the grossulariata factor as 
well as for the factor for femaleness. Though from 
a region where éactzcolor is unknown, the “ pure” wild 
grossulaviata female is nevertheless a permanent 
mongrel, but it can never reveal its true colours 
unless it is mated with a male which is either 
heterozygous for G or pure dacticolor. And as all 
the wild northern males are pure for the grossu- 
farviata character this can never happen in a state 
of nature. 

An essential feature of the case of the currant 
moth lies in the different results given by reciprocal 
crosses. Lactzcolor female x grossulariata male gives 
grossulariata alone of both sexes. But grossulariata 
female x /actzcolor male gives only grossulariata males 
and J/actécolor females. Such a difference between 
reciprocal crosses has also been found in other 
animals, and the experimental results, though some- 
times more complicated, are explicable on the same 
lines. An interesting case in which three factors 


102 MENDELISM CHAP. 


are concerned has been recently worked out in 
poultry. The Silky breed of fowls is characterised 
among other peculiarities by a remarkable abundance 
of melanic pigment. The skin is dull black, while 
the comb and wattles are of a deep purple colour 
contrasting sharply with the white plumage (PI. 
V., 3). Dissection shows that this black pigment 
is widely spread throughout the body, being especially 
marked in such membranes as the mesenteries, the 
periosteum, and the pia mater surrounding the brain. 
It also occurs in the connective tissues among the 
muscles. In the Brown Leghorn, on the other 
hand, this pigment is not found. Reciprocal crosses 
between these two breeds gave a remarkable differ- 
ence in result. A cross between the Silky hen and 


Silky BrowaLesthan the Brown Leghorn 


exc cock produced F, birds, 
in which both sexes 
ete exhibited only traces 


Q@ x @------- F, of the pigment. On 
| casual observation they 


fs ae might have passed for 
ieee oy : : 
SSS PLL LE unpigmented birds, for 
FIG. 19. with the exception of 
Scheme illustrating the result of crossing a 


Silky hen with a Brown Leghorn cock. an occasional fleck of 
Black sex signs denote deeply pigmented 


birds, and light sex signs those without pigment their skin, 
Gigck dot in the centredienate bias with Comb, and wattles were 
i aa as clear as in the Brown 
Leghorn (Pl. V., 1 and 4). Dissection revealed the 
presence of a slight amount of internal pigment. 
Such birds bred together gave some offspring with 
the full pigmentation of the Silky, some without any 


pigment, and others showing different degrees of 


PLATE V. 


3 4 


1, 2, F, Cock and Hen, ex Brown Leghorn Hen x Silky Cock; 
3, Silky Cock; 4, Hen ex Silky Hen x Brown Leghorn Cock. 


x SEX 103 


pigment. None of the F , male birds, however, 
showed the full deep pigmentation of the Silky. 


When, however, the cross was made the other way, 
viz. Brown Leg- 


horn hen x Silky Brown Leghorn Silky 


cock, the result ex é 

was different. ad ee 

While the F, male Se igheeassrss F, 
birds were almost 

destitute of pig- = —-——-"7.— >. Tr 
ment as in the € 6 6 6 @ 2? @ QE 
previous cross, the FIG. 20. 


F, hens, on the Scheme illustrating the result of crossing a Brown 
Leghorn hen with a Silky cock (cf. Fig. 19). 

other hand, were 

nearly as deeply pigmented as the pure Silky (Pl. V., 
2). The male Silky transmitted the pigmentation, 
but only to his daughters. Such birds bred together 
gave an F, generation containing chicks with the full 
deep pigment, chicks without pigment, and chicks 
with various grades of pigmentation, all the different 
kinds in both sexes. 

In analysing this complicated case many other 
different crosses were made, but for the present it 
will be sufficient to mention but one of these, viz. 
that between the F, birds and the pure Brown 
Leghorn. The cross between the F, hen and the 
Brown Leghorn cock produced only birds with a 
slight amount of pigment and birds without pigment. 
And this was true for both the deeply pigmented 
and the slightly pigmented types of F, hen. But 
when the F, cock was mated to a Brown Leghorn 
hen, a definite proportion of the chicks, one in eight, 
were deeply pigmented, and these deeply pigmented 


104 MENDELISM CHAP. 


birds were always females (cf. Fig. 21). And in 
this respect all the F, males behaved alike, whether 
they were from the Silky hen or from the Silky 
cock. We have, therefore, the paradox that the F, 
hen, though herself deeply pigmented, cannot trans- 
mit this condition to any of her offspring when she 
is mated to the unpigmented Brown Leghorn, but 
that, when similarly mated, the F, cock can transmit 


1 
this pigmented condition to a quarter of his female 


(Brown Leghorn) Silky 
Xx 


(BrownLegh.)g x ? 6 x Q (Brown Leghorn) 


$322 883852229 
FIG. 21, 


Scheme to illustrate the result of crossing F, birds (e.g. Brown Leghorn x Silky) 
with the pure Brown Leghorn. 


offspring though he himself is almost devoid of 
pigment. 

Now all these apparently complicated results, as 
well as many others to which we have not alluded, 
can be expressed by the following simple scheme. 
There are three factors affecting pigment, viz. (1) 
a pigmentation factor (P); (2) a factor which 
inhibits the production of pigment (/); and (3) a 
factor for femaleness (/), for which the female birds 
are heterozygous, but which is not present in the 
males. Further, we make the assumptions (a) that 
there is repulsion between F and J in the female 
zygote (///z), and (4) that the male Brown Leghorn 


x SEX 105 


is homozygous for the inhibitor factor (/), but that 
the hen Brown Leghorn is always heterozygous for 
this factor just in the same way as the female of the 
currant moth is always heterozygous for the grossu- 
lariata factor. We may now proceed to show how 
this explanation fits the experimental facts which 
we have given. 

The Silky is pure for the pigmentation factor, 
but does not contain the inhibitor factor. The 
Brown Leghorn, on the 
other hand, contains [@] FfPPii ffppli[d ] 
the inhibitor factor, but be ee: 


‘ ik gametes gametes 
not the pigmentation FPi . fp 
factor. In crossing a {Pi fpl 
Silky henwith a Brown 
Leghorn cock we are 
mating two birds of the FfPpli ffPpli 
constitution  &fPPiz [?] é [3] 

1G. 22. 
and ST¢pll, and all the Scheme to illustrate the nature of the Fy 
F. birds are conse- generation from the Silky hen and Brown 
1 Leghorn cock (cf. Fig. aay. 


quently heterozygous 
for both P and 7 In such birds the pigment is 
almost but not completely suppressed, and as both 
sexes are of the same constitution with regard to 
these two factors they are both of similar appearance. 
In the reciprocal cross, on the other hand, we are 
mating a Silky male (/fP 22) with a Brown Leghorn 
hen which on our assumption is heterozygous for the 
inhibitor factor (/), dnd in constitution therefore is 
Ffppli. Owing to the repulsion between F and / 
the gametes produced by such a bird are Fpz and fp/ 
in equal numbers. All the gametes produced by 
’ the Silky cock are fPz Hence the constitution of 


106 MENDELISM CHAP, 


the F, male birds produced by this cross is /fPplz 
as before, but the female birds must be all of the 
constitution #fPpz7. The Silky cock transmits the 
fully pigmented condition to his daughters, because 
the gametes of the Brown Leghorn hen which con- 
tain the factor for femaleness do not contain the 
inhibitory factor owing to the repulsion between 

these factors. The 


[2] Ffppli Pa [8] nature of the F, genera- 
an Pat tion in each case is 
Fpi {Pi in harmony with the 

fpl \ x { above scheme. As, 


however, it serves to 

illustrate certain points 

FfPpii ffPpli in connection with in- 
[9] [3] termediate forms we 
ape shall postpone further 


Scheme to illustrate the nature of the Fy . 2 . . 
generation from the Brown Leghorn hen consideration of it till 


and Silky cock (cf. Fig. 22). . 

we discuss these 
matters, and for the present shall limit ourselves to 
the explanation of the different behaviour of the F, 
males and females when crossed with the Brown 
Leghorn. And, first, the cross of Brown Leghorn 
female by F, male. The Brown Leghorn hen is on 
our hypothesis //pp/z, and produces gametes Fz and 
Spl. The F, cock is on our hypothesis /fPp/7, and 
produces in equal numbers the four kinds of gametes 
SPI, fPi, fol, fot. The result of the mecting of 
these two series of gametes is given in Fig. 24. 
Of the eight different kinds of zygote formed only 
one contains P in the absence of /, and this is a 
female. The result, as we have already seen, is in 
accordance with the experimental facts. 


Xx SEX 107 


On the other hand, the Brown Leghorn cock is 
on our hypothesis //#s/7. All his gametes conse- 
quently contain the inhibitor factor, and when he 
is mated with an 


F, hen all the |Fpi Fpi Fpi Fpi 
zygotes produced | PI! ae fpl fpi 
must contain J. g ? ? "4 
None of his off- | fpr fpI fpl fpl 
spring, therefore, | fP! fPi fp! fpi 
can be fully pig- 3 3 3 3 
mented, for this ; 

ee Fic. 24. 
condition only Diagram showing the nature of the offspring from a 
occurs in the ab- Brown Leghorn hen and an F, cock bred from 


a Silky hen x Brown Leghorn cock, or vice versa. 
sence of the in- 


hibitor factor among zygotes which are either homo- 
zygous or heterozygous for P. 

The interpretation of this case turns upon the 
constitution of the Brown Leghorn hen, upon her 
heterozygous condition with regard to the two factors 
Fand J/, and upon the repulsion that occurs between 
them when the gametes are formed. Through an 
independent set of experiments this view of the 
nature of the Brown Leghorn hen has been con- 
firmed in an interesting way. ‘There are fowls which 
possess-neither the factor for pigment nor the in- 
hibitory factor, which are in constitution ppzz. Such 
birds when crossed with the Silky give dark pig- 
mented birds of both sexes in F,, and the F, genera- 
tion consists of pigmented and unpigmented in the 
ratio 3:1. Now a cock of such a strain crossed 
with a Brown Leghorn hen should give only com- 
pletely unpigmented birds. But if, as we have 
supposed, the Brown Leghorn hen is producing 


108 MENDELISM CHAP. 


gametes /z and /p/, the male birds produced by 
such a cross should be heterozygous for /, ze. in 
constitution //pplz7, while the hen birds, though 
identical in appearance so far as absence of pig- 
mentation goes, should not contain this factor but 
should be constitutionally F/ppzz7. Crossed with the 
pure Silky, the F, birds of opposite sexes should 
give an entirely different result. For while the hens 


[Q] Fippli ffppii [d] 
gives gives A 
gametes gametes 
Fpi fpi 
fp } * Ce 
[él fppri [2] Féppii fppli[6] FfPPi[?] 
gives gives gives gives 
gametes gametes gametes gametes 
fPi Fpi fpl { FPi 
cae { fpi fpi {Pp 
ital — T T 1 
FEPpii , ffPpii FfPpli FfPpii ffPpli ffPpii 
[9] [6] [@] [9] [1 [] 
FIG. 25. 


Scheme to illustrate the heterozygous nature of the pure Brown Leghorn hen. 
For explanation see text. 
should give only deeply pigmented birds of both 
sexes, the cocks should give equal numbers of deeply 
pigmented and slightly pigmented birds (cf. Fig. 
25). These were the results which the experiment 
actually gave, thus affording strong confirmation of 
the view which we have been led to take of the 
Brown Leghorn hen. Essentially the poultry case 
is that of the currant moth. It differs in that the 


x SEX 109 


factor which repels femaleness produces no visible 
effect, and its presence or absence can only be deter- 
mined by the introduction of a third factor, that for 
pigmentation. 

This conception of the nature of the Brown 
Leghorn hen leads to a curious paradox. We have 
stated that the Silky cock transmits the pigmented 
condition, but transmits it to his daughters only. 
Apparently the case is one of unequal transmission 
by the father. Actually, as our analysis has shown, 
it is one of unequal transmission by the mother, the 
father’s contribution to the offspring being identical 
for each sex. The mother transmits to the daughters 
her dominant quality of femaleness, but to balance 
this, as it were, she transmits to her sons another 
quality which her daughters do not receive. It is 
a matter of common experience among human 
families that in respect to particular qualities the 
sons tend to resemble their mothers more than the 
daughters do, and it is not improbable that such 
observations have a real foundation for which the 
clue may be provided by the Brown Leghorn hen. 

Nor is this the only reflection that the Brown 
Leghorn suggests. Owing to the repulsion between 
the factors for femaleness and for pigment inhibition, 
it is impossible by any form of mating to make a 
hen which is homozygous for the inhibitor factor. 
She has bartered away for femaleness the possibility 
of ever receiving a double dose of this factor. We 
know that in some cases, as, for example, that of the 
blue Andalusian fowl, the qualities of the individual 
are markedly different according as to whether he 
or she has received a single or a double dose of a 


IIO MENDELISM CHAP. X 


given factor. It is not inconceivable that some of 
the qualities in which a man differs from a woman 
are founded upon a distinction of this nature. Certain 
qualities of intellect, for example, may depend upon 
the existence in the individual] of a double dose of 
some factor which is repelled by femaleness. If this 
is so, and if woman is bent upon achieving the results 
which such qualities of intellect imply, it is not 
education or training that will help her. Her problem 
is to get the factor on which the quality depends 
into an ovum that carries also the factor for female- 
ness, 


CHAPTER XI 
SEX (continued) 


THE cases which we have considered in the last 
chapter belong to a group in which the peculiarities 
of inheritance are most easily explained by supposing 
that the female is heterozygous for some factor that 
is not found in the male. Femaleness is an addi- 
tional character superposed upon a basis of maleness, 
and as we imagine that there is a separate factor 
for each the full constitutional formula for a female 
is F{MM, and for a male ff. Both sexes are 
homozygous for the male element, and the difference 
between them is due to the presence or absence of 
the female element /. ; 
There are, however, other cases for which the 
explanation will not suffice, but can be best inter- 
preted on the view that the male is heterozygous 
for a factor which is not found in the female. Such 
a case is that recently described by Morgan in 
America for the pomace fly (Drosophila ampelophila). 
Normally this little insect has a red eye, but white- 
eyed individuals are known to occur as rare sports. 
Red eye is dominant to white. In their relation to 
sex the eye colours of the pomace fly are inherited 
IIr 


112 MENDELISM CHAP. 


on the same lines as the grossulariata and lacticolor 
patterns of the currant moth, but with one essential 
difference. The factor which repels the red-eye 
factor is in this case to be found in the male, 
and here consequently it is the male which must 
be regarded as heterozygous for a sex factor that 
is lacking in the female. 

In order to bring these cases and others into 
line an interesting suggestion has recently been 
put forward by Bateson. On this suggestion each 
sex is heterozygous for its own sex factor only, and 
does not contain the factor proper to the opposite 

sex. The male is of the 


Matt Bana constitution Mimff and the 
gives gives 

gametes gametes female Ffmm. Each sex 
pay pied 


Mf ——> fn (peas produces two sorts of 
mf ———><Frd } fertiluations gametes, JZ and my in 
as pean 5 eee the case of the male, and 
Fim, fm in that of the 

female. But on this view a further supposition is 
necessary. If each of the two kinds of spermatozoa 
were capable of fertilising each of the two kinds of 
ova, we should get individuals of the constitution 
MmFf and mmff, as well as the normal males and 
females, Wmff and Ffmm. As the facts of ordinary 
bisexual reproduction afford us no grounds for 
assuming the existence of these two classes of indi- 
viduals, whatever they may be, we must suppose that 
fertilisation is productive only between the sperma- 
tozoa carrying MM and the ova without F/, or between 
the spermatozoa without 47 and the ova containing 
F. In other words, we must on this view suppose that 
fertilisations between certain forms of gametes, even 


XI SEX 113 


if they can occur, are incapable of giving rise to 
zygotes with the capacity for further development. 
If we admit this supposition, the scheme just 
given will cover such cases as those of the currant 
moth and the fowl, equally as well as that of the 
pomace fly. In the moth there is repulsion between 
either the grossulariata factor.and F, in the fowl 
between the pigment inhibitor factor and F in the 
female, while in the fly there is repulsion between 
the factor for red eye and M in the male! 
Whatever the merits or demerits of such a 
scheme it certainly does offer an explanation of a 
peculiar form of sex limited gx? 
inheritance in man. It has 
long been a matter of COMMON ig 56:3 @ x 
knowledge that colour-blind- 
ness is much more common 5}? 6€h62Q 
among men than among Tie oe 
women,and also that unaffected Scheme to illustrate the probable 
women can transmit it to their Plime The dalcaete ne 
sons. At first sight the case, Bers, ateded, individuals, 
is not unlike that of the WP tSpetic of aacsmieng ie 
sheep, where the horned char- — ndition to her sons. 
acter is apparently dominant in the male but recessive 
in the female. The hypothesis that the colour-blind 


1 Still more recently Doncaster (Journal of Genetics, 1911) has 
proposed that the female should be regarded as A/A7/Z and the male 
as //Mm, each sex being heterozygous for its own sex factor, while 
the female is also homozygous for that of the male. The view has 
much to commend it, for it offers a basis of explanation for the not 
infrequent assumption of male characters by females which are old or 
suffering from ovarial disease. In such cases it may be supposed that 
the factor F in some way loses its potency and allows the underlying 
M factors to come into fuller play. In its general working the hypo- 
thesis resembles that given in the text. 


I 


114 MENDELISM CHAP. 


condition is due to the presence of an extra factor 
as compared with the normal, and that a single dose 
of it will produce colour-blindness in the male but 
not in the female, will cover a good many of the 
observed facts (cf. Fig. 26). Moreover, it serves to 
explain the remarkable fact that a// the sons of colour- 
blind women are also colour-blind. For a woman 
cannot be colour-blind unless she is homozygous for 
the colour-blind factor, in which case all her children 
must get a single dose of it even if she marries a 
normal male. And this is sufficient to produce 
colour-blindness in the male though not in the female. 

But there is one notable difference in this case as 
compared with that of the sheep. When crossed 
with pure hornless ewes the heterozygous horned 
ram transmits the horned character to half his 
male offspring (cf. p. 71). But the heterozygous 
colour-blind man does not behave altogether like a 
sheep, for he apparently does not transmit the colour- 
blind condition to any of his male offspring. If, 
however, we suppose that the colour-blind factor is 
repelled by the factor for maleness, the amended 
scheme will cover the observed facts. For, denoting 
the colour-blind factor by .Y, the gametes produced 
by the colour-blind male are of two sorts only, viz. 
Mfx and mfX. If he marries a normal woman 
(Ff/mmxx), the spermatozoa Mx unite with ova fur 
to give normal males, while the spermatozoa mfX 
unite with ova Fmzx to give females which are 
heterozygous for the colour-blind factor. These 
daughters are themselves normal, but transmit the 
condition to about half their sons. 

The attempt to discover a simple explanation of 


XI SEX 115 


the nature of sex has led us to assume that certain 
combinations between gametes are incapable of 
giving rise to zygotes which can develop further. 
In the various cases hitherto considered there is no 
reason to suppose that anything of the sort occurs, 
or that the different gametes are otherwise than 
completely fertile one with another. One peculiar 
case, however, has been known for several years in 
which some of the gametes are apparently incapable 
of uniting to produce offspring. Yellow in the mouse 
is dominant to agouti, but hitherto a homozygous 
yellow has never been met with. The yellows from 
families where only yellows and agoutis occur pro- 
duce, when bred together, yellows and agoutis in 
the ratio 2:1. If it were an ordinary Mendelian 
case the ratio should be 3:1, and one out of every 
three yellows so bred should be homozygous and 
give only yellows when crossed with agouti. But 
Cuénot and others have shown that a// of the yellows 
are heterozygous, and when crossed with agoutis 
give both yellows and agoutis. We are led, there- 
fore, to suppose that an ovum carrying the yellow 
factor is unproductive if fertilised by a spermatozoon 
which also bears this factor. In this way alone does 
it seem possible to explain the deficiency of 
yellows and the absence of homozygous ones in the 
families arising from the mating of yellows together. 
At present, however, it remains the only definite 
instance among animals in which we have grounds 
for assuming that anything in the nature of unpro- 
ductive fertilisation takes place. 


1 For the most recent discussion of this peculiar case the reader is 
referred to Professor Castle’s paper in Scdece, December 16, 1910. 


116 MENDELISM CHAP. 


If we turn from animals to plants we find a more 
* complicated state of affairs. Generally speaking, the 
higher plants are hermaphrodite, both ovules and 
pollen grains occurring on the same flower. Some 
plants, however, like most animals, are of separate 
sexes, a single plant bearing only male or female 
flowers. In other plants the separate flowers are 
either male or female, though both are borne on the 
same individual. In others, again, the conditions are 
even more complex, for the same plant may bear 
flowers of three kinds, viz. male, female, and herma- 
phrodite. Or it may be that these three forms 
occur in the same species but in different individuals 
—female and hermaphrodites in one species ; males, 
females, and hermaphrodites in another. One case, 
however, must be mentioned as it suggests a possi- 
bility which we have not hitherto encountered. In 
the common English bryony (Bryonia dioica) the 
sexes are separate, some plants having only male 
and others only female flowers. In another Euro- 
pean species, &. alba, both male and female flowers 
occur on the same plant. Correns crossed these 
two species reciprocally, and also fertilised B. dzozica 
by its own male with the following results :— 


dioica 9 x dioicagd gave 9 9 and 6 6 
45 xalba ¢ 4, 92 @ only 
alba @ xdioicad ,, 9 @ and 36. 


The point of chief interest lies in the striking differ- 
ence shown by the reciprocal crosses between dzozca 
and alba. Males appear when alba is used as the 
female parent but not when the female dozca is 
crossed by male alba. It is possible to suggest 


XI SEX 117 


more than one scheme to. cover these facts, but we 
may confine ourselves here to that which seems 
most in accord with the general trend of other 
cases. We will suppose that in adzozca femaleness 
is dominant to maleness, and that the female is 
heterozygous for this additional factor. In_ this 
species, then, the female produces equal numbers of 
ovules with and without the female factor, while 
this factor is absent in all the pollen grains. Alba? 
x dioicad gives the same result as dzorca? x 
dioicad , and we must therefore suppose that alba 
produces male and female ovules in equal numbers. 
Albas x dioica? , however, gives nothing but females. 
Unless, therefore, we assume that there is selective 
fertilisation we must suppose that all the pollen 
grains of alba carry the female factor—in other 
words, that so far as the sex factors are concerned 
there is a difference between the ovules and pollen 
grains borne by the same plant. Unfortunately 
further investigation of this case is rendered im- 
possible owing to the complete sterility of the F, 
plants. 

That the possibility of a difference between the 
ovules and pollen grains of the same individual must 
be taken into account in future work there is 
evidence from quite a different source. The double 
stock is an old horticultural favourite, and for centuries 
it has been known that of itself it sets no seed, but 
must be raised from special strains of the single 
variety. “You must understand withall,”’ wrote 
John Parkinson of his gilloflowers,' “that those 
plants that beare double flowers, doe beare no seed 


1 Paradisus Terrestrts, London, 1629, p. 261. 


118 MENDELISM CHAP. 


at all . . . but the onely way to have double flowers 
any yeare is to save the seedes of those plants of 
this kinde that beare single flowers, for from that 
scede will rise, some that will beare single, and some 
double flowers.” With regard to the nature of these 
double-throwing strains of singles, Miss Saunders 
has recently brought out some interesting facts. She 


Fic. 27. 


Single and double stocks raised from the same single parent. 


crossed the double-throwing singles with pure 
singles belonging to strains in which doubles never 
occur. The cross was made both ways, and in 
both cases all the F’, plants were single. A distinc- 
tion, however, appeared when a further generation 
was raised from the F, plants. All the F, plants 
from the pollen of the double throwing single behaved 
like double throwing singles, but of the F, plants 


XI SEX 119 


from the ovules of the double throwers some behaved 
as double throwers, and some as pure singles. We 
are led to infer, therefore, that the ovules and pollen 
grains of the double throwers, though both produced 
by the same plant, differ in their relation to the 
factor (or factors) for doubleness. Doubleness is 
apparently carried by all the pollen grains of such 
plants, but only by some of the ovules. Though 
the nature of doubleness in stocks is not yet clearly 


Single 


— 


Single Double 


x oe 
Pollen of Ovule of 
pure single X Ovule Pollen X pure single 


Single Single Single 
Single Single Double Single Double 
Single Single Double Single Double 


understood, the facts discovered by Miss Saunders 
suggest strongly that the ovules and pollen grains 
of the same plant may differ in their transmitting 
properties, probably owing to some process of 
segregation in the growing plant which leads to an 
unequal distribution of some or other factors to 
the cells which give rise to the ovules as compared 
with those from which the pollen grains eventually 
spring. Whether this may turn out to be the true 
account or not, the possibility must not be over- 
looked in future work. 


120 MENDELISM CHAP, XI 


From all this it is clear enough that there is 
much to be done before the problem of sex is solved 
even so far as the biologist can ever expect to solve 
it. The possibilities are many, and many a fresh 
set of facts is needed before we can hope to decide 
among them. Yet the occasional glimpses of clear- 
cut and orderly phenomena, which Mendelian 
spectacles have already enabled us to catch, offer a 
fair hope that some day they may all be brought 
into focus, and assigned their proper places in a 
general scheme which shall embrace them ll. 
Then, though not till then, will the problem of the 
nature of sex pass from the hands of the biologist 
into those of the physicist and the chemist. 


CHAPTER XII 
INTERMEDIATES 


So far as we have gone we have found it possible to 
express the various characters of animals and plants 
in terms of definite factors which are carried by the 
gametes, and are distributed according to a definite 
scheme. Whatever may be the nature of these 
factors it is possible for purposes of analysis to treat 
them as indivisible entities which may or may 
not be present in any given gamete. When the 
factor is present it is present as a whole. The 
visible properties developed by a zygote in the course 
of its growth depend upon the nature and variety of 
the factors carried in by the two gametes which 
went to its making, and to a less degree upon 
whether each factor was brought in by both gametes 
or by one only. If the given factor is brought in by 
one gamete only, the resulting heterozygote may be 
.more or less intermediate between the homozygous 
form with a double dose of the factor and the 
homozygous form which is entirely destitute of the 
factor.’ Cases in point are those of the primula 
flowers and the Andalusian fowls. Nevertheless 
these intermediates produce only pure gametes as is 
121 


122 MENDELISM CHAP, 


shown by the fact that the pure parental types 
appear in a certain proportion of their offspring. 
In such cases as these there is but a single type of 
intermediate, and the simple ratio in which this and 
the two homozygous forms appear renders the 
interpretation obvious. But the nature of the F, 
generation may be much more complex and, where 
we are dealing with factors which interact upon one 
another, may even present the appearance of a series 
of intermediate forms grading from the condition 
found in one of the original parents to that which 
occurred in the other. As an illustration we may 
consider the cross between the Brown Leghorn and 
Silky fowls which we have already dealt with in 
connection with the inheritance of sex. The offspring 
of a Silky hen mated with a Brown Leghorn are in 
both sexes birds with but a trace of the Silky 
pigmentation. But when such birds are bred 
together they produce a generation consisting of 
chicks as deeply pigmented as the original Silky 
parent, chicks devoid of pigment like the Brown 
Leghorn, and chicks in which the pigmentation 
shows itself in a variety of intermediate stages. 
Indeed from a hundred chicks bred in this way it 
would be possible to pick out a number of indi- 
viduals and arrange them in an apparently continuous 
series of gradually increasing pigmentation, with the 
completely unpigmented at one end and the most 
deeply pigmented at the other. Nevertheless, the case 
is one in which complete segregation of the different 
factors takes place, and the apparently continuous 
series of intermediates is the result of the interaction 
of the different factors upon one another. The con- 


XI INTERMEDIATES 123 


stitution of the F,¢ is a ffPplz, and such a bird 
produces in equal numbers the four sorts of gametes 
SPI, fPi, fol, foi. The constitution of the F,@ in 
this case is FfPplz. Owing to the repulsion between 
F and / she produces the four kinds of gametes 
FPi, Hpi, fPI, fol, and produces them in equal 
numbers. The 


result of bringing ree el ig 
two such series of 

gametes together ? iJ ? ? 
is shown in Fig. |Fpi Fpi Fpi Fpi 
28. Out of the |‘! i fpl fpi 
sixteen types of : . y ? 
zygote formed one | fPI {PI {PI {PI 
(F/PPii)ishomo- |! | fPi_— | PT | ft 
zygous for the pig- 3 é 3 3 
mentation factor, | fpI fpI fpl fpl 
and does not con- | ‘PI {Pi fpl fpi 
tain the inhibitor é é rei 3 
factor. Such a 

Fic. 28. 


bird is as 
i t deeply Diagram to illustrate the nature and composition of 
pigmented as the the F, generations arising from the cross of Silky 


hen with Brown Leghorn cock. 

pure Silky parent. 

Two, again, contain a single dose of P in the absence 
of 7. These are nearly as dark as the pure Silky. 
Four zygotes are destitute of P though they may or 
may notcontain /. These birds are completely devoid 
of pigment like the Brown Leghorn. The remaining 
nine zygotes show various combinations of the two 
factors P and J/, being either PP/i, PPI/, Pell, or 
Pplt, and in each of these cases the pigment is more 
or less intense according to the constitution of the 
bird. Thus a bird of the constitution PP 


124 MENDELISM CHAP. 


approaches in pigmentation a bird of the constitution 
Ppit, while a bird of the constitution Pp// has but 
little more pigment than the unpigmented bird. In 
this way we have seven distinct grades of pigmenta- 
tion, and the series is further complicated by the fact 
that these various grades exhibit a rather different 
amount of pigmentation according as they occur in a 
male or a female bird, for, generally speaking, the 
female of a given grade exhibits rather more pigment 
than the corresponding male. The examination of 
a number of birds bred in this way might quite well 
suggest that in this case we were dealing with a 
character which could break up, as it were, to give a 
continuous series of intergrading forms between the 
two extremes. With the constant handling of large 
numbers it becomes possible to recognise most of 
the different grades, though even so it is possible 
to make mistakes. Nevertheless, as breeding tests 
have amply shown, we are dealing with but two 
interacting factors which segregate cleanly from one 
another according to the strict Mendelian rule. The 
approach to continuity in variation exhibited by the 
F, generation depends upon the fact that these two 
factors interact upon one another, and to different 
degrees according as the zygote is for one or other 
or both of them in a homozygous or a heterozygous 
state. Moreover, certain of these intermediates will 
breed true to an intermediate condition of the 
pigmentation. A male of the constitution //PP/I 
when bred with females of the constitution A/PP/7 
will produce only males like itself and females like 
the maternal parent. We have dealt with this case 
in some detail, because the existence of families 


XII INTERMEDIATES 125 


showing a series of intermediate stages between two 
characters has sometimes been brought forward in 
opposition to the view that the characters of 
organisms depend upon specific factors which are 
transmitted according to the Mendelian rule. But, 
as this case from poultry shows clearly, neither 
the existence of such a continuous series of inter- 
mediates, nor the fact that some of them may breed 
true to the intermediate condition, are incompatible 
with the Mendelian principle of segregation. 

In connection with intermediates a more cogent 
objection to the Mendelian view is the case of the 
first cross between two definite varieties thence- 
forward breeding true. The case that will naturally 
occur to the reader is that of the mulatto, which 
‘results from the cross between the negro and the 
white. According to general opinion, these mulattos, 
of intermediate pigmentation, continue to produce 
mulattos. Unfortunately this interesting case has 
never been critically investigated, and the statement 
that the mulatto breeds true rests almost entirely 
upon information that is general and often vague. It 
may be that the inheritance of skin pigmentation 
in this instance is a genuine exception to the normal 
rule, but at the same time it must not be forgotten 
that it may be one in which several interacting 
factors are concerned, and that the pure white and 
the pure black are the result of combinations which 
from their rarity are apt to be overlooked. But 
until we are in possession of accurate information it 
is impossible to pronounce definitely upon the nature 
of the inheritance in this case. 

On the other hand, from the cross between the 


126 MENDELISM CHAP. 


darkly pigmented Eastern races and the white 
segregation seems to occur in subsequent genera- 
tions. Families are to be found in which one parent is 
a pure white, while the other has arisen from the cross 
between the dark and light in the first or some 
subsequent generation. Such families may contain 


é o Qxd a 3 ie 
3 5x9 QS SSSELS? QP 
| | 
3 3 Several children 
al © or O 
FIG. 29. 


Pedigree of a family which originated from a cross between a Hindu and a European. 
Black signs denote individuals as dark as average Hindus. Plain signs denote 
quite fair members, while those with a dot in the centre are intermediate. R 


children indistinguishable from pure blonds as well 
as children of very dark and of intermediate shades. 
As an example, I may give the following pedigree, 
which was kindly communicated to me by an Anglo- 
Indian friend (Fig. 29). The family had resided in 
England for several generations, so that in this case 
there. was no question ofa further admixture of black. 
Most noticeable is the family produced by a very dark 


xu INTERMEDIATES 127 


lady who had married a white man. Some of the 
children were intermediate in colour, but two were 
fair whites and two were dark as dark Hindus. 
This sharp segregation or splitting out of blacks and 
whites in addition to intermediates strongly suggests 
that the nature of the inheritance is Mendelian, 
though it may be complicated by the existence of 
several factors which may also react upon one 
another. Nor must it be forgotten that in so far as 
these different factors are concerned the whites them- 
selves may differ in constitution without showing 
any trace of it in their appearance. Before the case 
can be regarded as settled all these different 
possibilities will have to be definitely tested. With 
the dark Eastern races as with the negro we cannot 
hope to come to any conclusion until we have 
evidence collected by critical and competent 
observers. 

Though for the present we must regard the case 
of the negro as not proven, there are nevertheless 
two others in which the heredity would appear not 
to follow the Mendelian rule. Castle in America 
crossed the lop-eared rabbit with the normal form, 
and found that the F, animals were intermediate 
with respect to their ears. And subsequent experi- 
ment showed that, on the whole, they bred true to 
this intermediate condition. The other case relates 
to Lepidoptera. The speckled wood butterfly 
(Pararge egerta) has a southern form which differs 
from the northern one in the greater brightness and 
depth of its yellow-brown markings. The northern 
form is generally distinguished as var. egerzades. 
Bateson crossed the southern form from the south 


128 MENDELISM CHAP. 


of France with the paler British form, and found 
that the offspring were more or less intermediate in 
colour, and that in subsequent generations the parental 
types did not recur. These cases at present stand 
alone. It is possible that further research may 
reveal complications which mask or interfere with 
an underlying process of segregation. Or it may 
be that segregation does not occur owing to some 
definite physiological reason which at present we do 
not understand. ; 

And here it is impossible not to recall Mendel’s 
own experiences with the Hawkweeds (Hzeracium). 
This genus of plants exhibits an extraordinary pro- 
fusion of forms differing from one another sometimes 
in a single feature, sometimes in several. The 
question as to how far these numerous forms were 
to be classified as distinct species, how far as varieties, 
and how far as products of chance hybridisation, was 
even at that time a source of keen controversy among 
botanists. There is little doubt that Mendel under- 
took his experiments on the Hawkweeds in the hope 
that the conception of unit-characters so brilliantly 
demonstrated for the pea would serve to explain the 
great profusion of forms among the Hieraciums. 
Owing to the minute size of their florets, these 
plants offer very considerable technical difficulties 
in the way of cross-fertilisation. By dint of great 
perseverance and labour, however, Mendel succeeded 
in obtaining a few crosses between different forms. 
These hybrids were reared and a further generation 
produced from them, and, no doubt somewhat to 
Mendel’s chagrin, every one of them proved to breed 
true. There was a complete absence of that segrega- 


Xu INTERMEDIATES 129 


tion of characters which he had shown to exist in 
peas and beans, and had probably looked forward 
with some confidence to finding in Hzeracium. More 
than thirty years passed before the matter was 
cleared up. To-day we know that the peculiar 
behaviour of the hybrid Hieraciums is due to the 
fact that they normally produce seed by a peculiar 
process of parthenogenesis. It is possible to take 
an unopened flower and to shear off with a razor all 
the male organs together with the stigmata through 
which the pollen reaches the ovules. The flower, 
nevertheless, sets perfectly good seed. But the cells 
from which the seeds develop are not of the same 
nature as the normal ova of a plant. They are 
not gametes, but retain the double structure of the 
maternal cells. They are rather to be regarded as 
of the nature of buds which early become detached 
from the parent stock to lead an independent exist- 
ence, and, like buds, they reproduce exactly the 
maternal characteristics. The discovery of the true 
nature of this case was only rendered possible by 
the development of the study of cytology, and it 
was not given to Mendel to live long enough to learn 
why his hybrid Hieraciums all bred true. 


CHAPTER XIII 
VARIATION AND EVOLUTION 


THROUGH the facts of heredity we have reached a 
new conception of the individual. Hitherto we have 
been accustomed to distinguish between the members 
of a family of rabbits like that illustrated on Plate 
I. by assigning to each an individuality, and by 
making use of certain external features, such as the 
coat colour or the markings, as convenient outward 
signs to express our idea that the individuality of these 
different animals is different. Apart from this, our 
notions as to what constituted the individuality in 
each case were at best but vague. Mendelian analysis 
has placed in our hands a more precise method of 
estimating and expressing the variations that are 
to be found between one individual and another. 
Instead of looking at the individual as a whole, 
which is in some vague way endowed with an indi- 
viduality marking it off from its fellows, we now 
regard it as an organism built up of definite char- 
acters superimposed on a basis beyond which for the 
moment our analysis will not take us. We have 
begun to realise that each individual has a definite 
architecture, and that this architecture depends 
130 


cu.xnr1 VARIATION AND EVOLUTION 131 


primarily upon the number and variety of the 
factors that existed in the two gametes that went 
to its building. Now most species exhibit consider- 
able variation and exist in a number, often very 
large, of more or less well-defined varieties. How 
far can this great variety be explained in terms 
of a comparatively small number of factors if the 
number of possible forms depends upon the number 
of the factors which may be present or absent ? 

In the simple case where the homozygous and 
heterozygous conditions are indistinguishable in 
appearance the number of possible forms is 2, 
raised to the power of the number of factors con- 
cerned. Thus where one factor is concerned there 
are only 2'= 2 possible forms, where ten factors are 
concerned there are 2‘°= 1024 possible forms differ- 
ing from one another in at most ten and at least one 
character. Where the factors interact upon one 
another this number will, of course, be considerably 
increased. If the heterozygous form is different in 
appearance from the homozygous form, there are 
three possible forms connected with each factor ; 
for ten such factors the possible number of indi- 
viduals would be 3°=59,049; for twenty such 
factors the possible number of different individuals 
would be 37°= 3,486,784,401. The presence or 
absence of a comparatively small number of factors 
in a species carries with it the possibility of an 
enormous range of individual variation. But every 
one of these individuals has a perfectly definite con- 
stitution which can be determined in each case by 
the ordinary methods of Mendelian analysis. For 
in every instance the variation depends upon the 


132 MENDELISM CHAP. 


presence or absence of definite factors carried in by 
the gametes from whose union the individual results. 
And as these factors separate out cleanly in the 
gametes which the individual forms, such variations 
as depend upon them are transmitted strictly accord- 
ing to the Mendelian scheme. Provided that the 
constitution of the gametes is unchanged, the heredity 
of such variation is independent of any change in 
the conditions of nutrition or environment which may 
operate upon the individual producing the gametes. 

But, as everybody knows, an individual organism, 
whether plant or animal, reacts, and often reacts 
markedly, to the environmental conditions under 
which its life is passed. More especially is this 
to be seen where such characters as size or weight 
are concerned. More sunlight or a richer soil may 
mean stronger growth in a plant, better nutrition 
may result in a finer animal, superior education may 
lead to a more intelligent man. But although the 
changed conditions produce a direct effect upon the 
individual, we have no indisputable evidence that 
such alterations are connected with alterations in 
the nature of the gametes which the individual pro- 
duces. And without this such variations cannot be 
perpetuated through heredity, but the conditions 
which produce the effect must always be renewed 
in each successive generation. We are led, there- 
fore, to the conclusion that two sorts of variations 
exist, those which are due to the presence of specific 
factors in the organism and those which are due to 
the direct effect of the environment during its life- 
time. The former are known as mutations, and are 
inherited according to the Mendelian scheme; the 


xm VARIATION AND EVOLUTION 133 


latter have been termed fluctuations, and at present 
we have no valid reason for supposing that they are 
ever inherited. For though instances may be found 
in which effects produced during the lifetime of the 
individual would appear to affect the offspring, this 
is not necessarily due to heredity. Thus plants 
which are poorly nourished and grown under adverse 
conditions may set seed from which come plants 
that are smaller than the normal although grown 
under most favourable conditions. It is natural 
to attribute the smaller size of the offspring to the 
conditions under which the parents were grown, and 
there is no doubt that we should be quite right in 
doing so. Nevertheless, it need have nothing to do 
with heredity. As we have already pointed out, the 
seed is a larval plant which draws its nourishment 
from the mother. The size of the offspring is 
affected because the poorly nourished parent offered 
a bad environment to the young plant, and not 
because the gametes of the parent were changed 
through the adverse conditions under which it grew. 
The parent in this case is not only the producer of 
gametes, but also a part of the environment of the 
young plant, and it is in this latter capacity that it 
affects its offspring. Wherever, as in plants and 
mammals, the organism is parasitic upon the mother 
during its earlier stages the state of nutrition of the 
latter will almost certainly react upon it, and in this 
way a semblance of transmitted weakness or vigour 
is brought about. Such a connection between mother 
and offspring is purely one of environment, and it 
cannot be too strongly emphasised that it has nothing 
to do with the ordinary process of heredity. 


134 MENDELISM CHAP. 


The distinction between these two kinds of varia- 
tion, so entirely different in their causation, renders 
it possible to obtain a clearer view of the process of 
evolution than that recently prevalent. As Darwin 
long ago realised, any theory of evolution must be 
based upon the facts of heredity and variation. 
Evolution only comes about through the survival 
of certain variations and the elimination of others. 
But to be of any moment in evolutionary change 
a variation must be inherited. And to be inherited 
it must be represented in the gametes. This, as we 
have seen, is the case for those variations which we 
have termed mutations. For the inheritance of 
fluctuations, on the other hand, of the variations 
which result from the direct action of the environ- 
ment upon the individual, there is no indisputable 
evidence. Consequently we have no reason for 
regarding them as playing any part in the pro- 
duction of that succession of temporarily stable forms 
which we term evolution. In the light of our present 
knowledge we must regard the mutation as the basis 
of evolution—as the material upon which natural 
selection works. For it is the only form of variation 
of whose heredity we have any certain knowledge. 

It is evident that this view of the process of 
evolution is in some respects at variance with that 
generally held during the past half century. There 
we were given the conception of an abstract type 
representing the species, and from it most of the 
individuals diverged in various directions, though, 
generally speaking, only to a very small extent. It 
was assumed that any variation, however small, 
might have a selection value, that is to say, could be 


xu1 VARIATION AND EVOLUTION 135 


transmitted to the offspring. Some of these would 
possess it in a less and some ina greater degree 
than the parent. If the variation were a useful one, 
those possessing to a rather greater extent would be 
favoured through the action of natural selection at 
the expense of their less fortunate brethren, and 
would leave a greater number of offspring, of whom 
some possessed it in an even more marked degree 
than themselves. And so it would go on. The 
process was acumulative one. The slightest variation 
in a favourable direction gave natural selection a 
starting-point to work on. Through the continued 
action of natural selection on each successive genera- 
tion the useful variation was gradually worked up, 
until at last it reached the magnitude of a specific 
distinction. Were it possible in such a case to 
have all the forms before us, they would present the 
appearance of a long series imperceptibly grading 
from one extreme to the other. 

Upon this view are made two assumptions not 
unnatural in the absence of any exact knowledge of 
the nature of heredity and variation. It was assumed, 
in the first place, that variation was a continuous 
process, and, second, that any variation could be 
transmitted to the offspring. Both of these assump- 
tions have since been shown to be unjustified. Even 
before Mendel’s work became known Bateson had 
begun to call attention to the prevalence of dis- 
continuity in variation, and a few years later this 
was emphasised by the Dutch botanist Hugo de 
Vries in his great work on The Mutation Theory. 
The ferment of new ideas was already working in 
the solution, and under the stimulus of Mendel’s 


136 MENDELISM CHAP, 


work they have rapidly crystallised out. With the 
advent of heredity as a definite science we have 
been led to revise our views as to the nature of 
variation, and consequently in some respects as to 
the trend of evolution. Heritable variation has a 
definite basis in the gamete, and it is to the gamete, 
therefore, not to the individual, that we must look 
for the initiation of this process. Somewhere or 
other in the course of their production is added or 
removed the factor upon whose removal or addition 
the new variation owes its existence. The new 
variation springs into being by a sudden step, not 
by a process of gradual and almost imperceptible 
augmentation. It is not continuous but discon- 
tinuous because it is based upon the presence or 
absence of some definite factor or factors—upon 
discontinuity in the gametes from which it sprang. 
Once formed, its continued existence is subject to 
the arbitrament of natural selection. If of value in 
the struggle for existence natural selection will 
decide that those who possess it shall have a better 
chance of survival and of leaving offspring than 
those who do not possess it. If it is harmful to 
the individual natural selection will soon bring about 
its elimination. But if the new variation is neither 
harmful nor useful there seems no reason why it 
should not persist. 

In this way we avoid a difficulty which beset the 
older view. For on that view no new character 
could be developed except by the piling up of 
minute variations through the action of natural 
selection. Consequently any character found in 
animals and plants must be supposed to be of 


xur VARIATION AND EVOLUTION — 137 


some definite use to the individual. Otherwise it 
could not have developed through the action of 
natural selection. But there are plenty of characters 
to which it is exceedingly difficult to ascribe any 
utility, and the ingenuity of the supporters of this 
view has often been severely taxed to account for 
their existence. On the more modern view this 
difficulty is avoided. The origin of a new variation 
is independent of natural selection, and’ provided 
that it is not directly harmful there is no reason 
why it should not persist. In this way we are 
released from the burden of discovering a utilitarian 
motive behind all the multitudinous characters of 
living organisms. For we now recognise that the 
function of natural selection is selection and not 
creation. It has nothing to do with the formation 
of the new variation. It merely decides whether it 
is to survive or to be eliminated. 

One of the arguments made use of by supporters 
of the older view is that drawn from the study of 
adaptation. Animals and plants are as a rule re- 
markably well adapted to living the life which their 
surroundings impose upon them, and in some cases 
this adaptation is exceedingly striking. Especially 
is this so in the many instances of what is called 
protective coloration, where the animal comes to 
resemble its surroundings so closely that it may 
reasonably be supposed to cheat even the keenest 
sighted enemy. Surely, we are told, such perfect 
adaptation could hardly have arisen through the 
mere survival of chance sports. Surely there must 
be some guiding hand moulding the species into the 
required shape. The argument is an old one. For 


138 MENDELISM CHAP. 


John Ray that guiding hand was the superior 
wisdom of the Creator: for the modern Darwinian 
it is Natural Selection controlling the direction of 
variation. Mendelism certainly offers no suggestion 
of any such controlling force. It interprets the 
variations of living forms in terms of definite physio- 
logical factors, and the diversity of animal and plant 
life is due to the gain or loss of these factors, to the 
origination of new ones, or to fresh combinations 
among those already in existence. Nor is there any 
valid reason against the supposition that even the 
most remarkable cases of resemblance, such as that 
of the leaf insect, may have arisen through a process 
of mutation. Experience with domestic plants and 
animals shows that the most bizarre forms may arise 
as sports and perpetuate themselves. Were such 
forms, arising under natural conditions, to be favoured 
by natural selection owing to a resemblance to some- 
thing in their environment we should obtain a striking 
case of protective adaptation. And here it must not 
be forgotten that those striking cases to which our 
attention is generally called are but a very small 
minority of the existing forms of life. 

For that special group of adaptation phenomena 
classed under the head of Mimicry, Mendelism seems 
to offer an interpretation simpler than that at present 
in vogue. This perhaps may be more clearly ex- 
pressed by taking a specific case. There is in Africa 
a genus of Danaine butterflies known as Amaurzs, 
and there are reasons for considering that the group 
to which it belongs possesses properties which render 
it unpalatable to vertebrate enemies such as birds or 
monkeys. In the same region is also found the 


PLATE VI. 


iBraqryem eByesng 


SnUBdIUTULOp STUNBULY 


Risayda SlINRWy 


xmt VARIATION AND EVOLUTION 139 


genus Euralia belonging to the entirely different 
family of the Nymphalidae, to which there is no 
evidence for assigning the disagreeable properties of 
the Danaines. Now the different species of Euralia 
show remarkably close resemblances to the species 
of Amaurzs, which are found flying in the same 
region, and it is supposed that by “ mimicking” the 
unpalatable forms they impose upon their enemies 
and thereby acquire immunity from attack. The 
point at issue is the way in which this seemingly 
purposeful resemblance has been brought about. 

One of the species of Euralia occurs in two very 
distinct forms (Pl. VI.) which were previously re- 
garded as separate species under the names &. 
wahlbergt and E. mima. These two forms respec- 
tively resemble Amauris dominicanus and A. echeria. 
For purposes of argument we will assume that one of 
these forms has been derived from the other and that 
A. dominicanus is the more recent form of the two. 
On the modern Darwinian view certain individuals 
of A. echeria gradually diverged from the echerza type 
and eventually reached the domznicanus type, though 
why this should have happened does not appear to 
be clear. At the same time those specimens of the 
Euralia which tended to vary in the direction of A. 
dominicanus in places where this species was more 
abundant than A. echerza were encouraged by natural 
selection, and under its guiding hand the form wahl- 
bergé eventually arose from mma. 

According to Mendelian views, on the other hand, 
A. dominicanus arose suddenly from A. echeria (or vice 
versa), and similarly wahlbergi arose suddenly from 
mima. If wahlberg? occurred where A. dominicanus 


140 MENDELISM CHAP. 


was common and A, echerda was rare, its resemblance 
to the more plentiful distasteful form would give it 
the advantage over mzma and allow it to establish 
itself in place of the latter. On the modern Dar- 
winian view natural selection gradually shapes mzma 
into the wahklbergd form owing to the presence of A. 
dominicanus ; on the Mendelian view natural selec- 
tion merely conserves the wahlberg¢ form when once 
it has arisen. Now this case of mimicry is one 
of especial interest, because we have experimental 
evidence that the relation between mzma and 
wahlbergi is a simple Mendelian one, mzma here 
being the dominant and wahlbergd the recessive 
form. The two have been proved to occur in 
families bred from the same female without the 
occurrence of any intermediates, and the fact that 
the two segregate cleanly is strong evidence in 
favour of the Mendelian view. On this view the 
genera Amauris and Euralza contain a similar set 
of pattern factors, and the conditions, whatever they 
may be, which bring about mutation in the former 
lead to the production of a similar mutation in the 
latter. Of the different forms of Euvala produced 
in any region that one has the best chance of 
survival, through the operation of natural selection, 
which resembles the most plentiful Amaurzs form. 
Mimetic resemblance is a true phenomenon, but 
natural selection plays the part of a conservative, 
not of a formative agent. 

It is interesting to recall that in earlier years 
Darwin was inclined to ascribe more importance to 
“sports” as opposed to continuous minute variation, 
and to consider that they might play a not incon- 


xr VARIATION AND EVOLUTION § 141 


siderable part in the formation of new varieties in 
nature. This view, however, he gave up later, be- 
cause he thought that the relatively rare sport or 
mutation would rapidly disappear through the 
swamping effects of crossing with the more abund- 
ant normal form, and so, even though favoured by 
natural selection, would never succeed in establishing 
itself. Mendel’s discovery has eliminated this diffi- 
culty. For suppose that the sport differed from the 
normal in the loss of a factor and were recessive. 
When mated with the normal this character would 
seem to disappear, though, of course, half of the 
gametes of its progeny would bear it. By continual 
crossing with normals a small proportion of hetero- 
zygotes would eventually be scattered among the 
population, and as soon as any two of these mated 
together the recessive sport would appear in one 
quarter of their offspring. 

A suggestive contribution to this subject was 
recently made by G. H. Hardy. Considering the 
distribution of a single factor in a mixed population 
consisting of the heterozygous and the two 
homozygous forms he showed that such a population 
breeding at random rapidly fell into a stable con- 
dition with regard to the proportion of these three 
forms, whatever may have been the proportion of 
the three forms to start with. Let us suppose, for 
instance, that the population consists of homozygotes 
of one kind, y homozygotes of the other kind, and 2g 
heterozygotes. Hardy pointed out that, other 
things being equal, such a population would be in 
equilibrium for this particular factor so long as the 
condition g’=gr was fulfilled. If the condition is 


142 MENDELISM CHAP. 


fulfilled to start with the population remains in 
equilibrium. If the condition is not fulfilled to start 
with, Hardy showed that a position of equilibrium 
becomes established after a single generation, and 
that this position is thereafter maintained. The 
proportions of the three classes which satisfy the 
equation g*=yr are exceedingly numerous, and 
populations in which they existed in the proportions 
shown in the appended table would remain in stable 
equilibrium generation after generation :— 


p. 29. Te 
I 2 I 
I 4 4 
I 6 9 
I 8 16 
I 20,000 100,000,000 
I 20 ne 


This, of course, assumes that all three classes are 
equally fertile, and that no form of selection is 
taking place to the benefit of one class more than 
of another. Moreover, it makes no difference 
whether / represents the homozygous dominants or 
whether it stands for the recessives. A population 
containing a very small proportion of dominants 
and one containing a similar proportion of recessives 
are equally stable. The term dominant is in some 
respects apt to be misleading, for a dominant 
character cannot in virtue of its dominance establish 
itself at the expense of a recessive one. Brown 
eyes in man are dominant to blue, but there is no 
reason to suppose that as years go on the population 
of these islands will become increasingly brown eyed. 
Given equality of conditions both are on an equal 


xur =VARIATION AND EVOLUTION 143 


footing. If, however, either dominant or recessive 
be favoured by selection the conditions are altered, 
and it can be shown that even a small advantage 
possessed by the one will rapidly lead to the 
elimination of the other. Even with but a 5 per 
cent selection advantage in its favour it can be shown 
that a rare sport will oust the normal form in a few 
hundred generations. In this way we are freed 
from a difficulty inherent in the older view that 
varieties arose through a long-continued process 
involving the accumulation of very slight variations. 
On that view the establishing of a new type was of 
necessity a very long and tedious business involving 
many thousands of generations. For this reason 
the biologist has been accustomed to demand a very 
large supply of time, often a great deal more than 
the physicist is disposed to grant, and this has 
sometimes led him to expostulate with the latter for 
cutting off the supply. On the newer views, however, 
this difficulty need not arise, for we realise that the 
origin and establishing of a new form may be a very 
much more rapid process than has hitherto been 
deemed possible. 

One last question with regard to evolution. How 
far does Mendelism help us in connection with the 
problem of the origin of species? Among the plants 
and animals with which we have dealt we have been 
able to show that distinct differences, often con- 
siderable, in colour, size, and structure, may be 
interpreted in terms of Mendelian factors. It is not 
unlikely that most of the various characters which 
the systematist uses to mark off one species from 
another, the so-called specific characters, are of this 


144 MENDELISM CHAP. 


nature. They serve as convenient labels, but are 
not essential to the conception of species. A 
systematist who defined the wild sweet pea could 
hardly fail to include in his definition such characters 
as the procumbent habit, the tendrils, the form of 
the pollen, the shape of the flower, and its purple 
colour. Yet all these and other characters have 
been proved to depend upon the presence of definite 
factors which can be removed by appropriate cross- 
ing. By this means we can produce a small plant 
a few inches in height with an erect habit of growth, 
without tendrils, with round instead of oblong pollen, 
and with colourless deformed flowers quite different 
in appearance from those of the wild form. Such a 
plant would breed perfectly true, and a botanist to 
whom it was presented, if ignorant of its origin, 
might easily relegate it to a different genus. Never- 
theless, though so widely divergent in structure, 
such a plant must yet be regarded as belonging to the 
species Lathyrus odoratus. For it still remains fertile 
with the many different varieties of sweet-pea. It 
is not visible attributes that constitute the essential 
difference between one species and another. The 
essential difference, whatever it may be, is that 
underlying the phenomenon of sterility. The visible 
attributes are those made use of by the’ systematist 
in cataloguing the different forms of animal and 
plant life, for he has no other choice. But it must 
not be forgotten that they are often misleading. 
Until they were bred together Euralia wahlbergi 
and £. mima were regarded as perfectly valid species, 
and there is little doubt that numbers of recognised 
species will eventually fall to the ground in the same 


xt VARIATION AND EVOLUTION § 145 


way as soon as we are in a position to apply the 
test of breeding. Mendelism has helped us to 
realise that specific characters may be but incidental 
to a species—that the true criterion of what con- 
stitutes a species is sterility, and that particular form 
of sterility which prevents two healthy gametes on 
uniting from producing a zygote with normal powers 
of growth and reproduction. For there are forms of 
sterility which are purely mechanical. The pollen 
of Mirabilis jalapa cannot fertilise M. longifiora, 
because the pollen tubes of the former are not long 
enough to penetrate down to the ovules of the latter. 
Hybrids can nevertheless be obtained from the 
reciprocal cross. Nor should we expect offspring 
from a St. Bernard and a toy terrier without recourse 
to artificial fertilisation. Or sterility may be due to 
pathological causes which prevent the gametes from 
meeting one another in a healthy state. But in 
most cases it is probable that the sterility is due to 
some other cause. It is not inconceivable that 
definite differences in chemical composition render 
the protoplasm of one species toxic to the gametes 
of the other, and if this is so it is not impossible 
that we may some day be able to express these 
differences in terms of Mendelian factors. The very 
nature of the case makes it one of extreme difficulty 
for experimental investigation. At any rate, we 
realise more clearly than before that the problem of 
species is not one that can be resolved by the study 
of morphology or of systematics. It is a problem 
in physiology. 


CHAPTER XIV 
ECONOMICAL 


SINCE heredity lies at the basis of the breeder’s 
work, it is evident that any contribution to a more 
exact knowledge of this subject must prove of service 
to him, and there is no doubt that he will be able 
to profit by Mendelian knowledge in the conduct of 
his operations. Indeed, as we shall see later, these 
ideas have already led to striking results in the 
raising of new and more profitable varieties. In 
the first place, heredity is a question of individuals. 
Identity of appearance is no sure guide to repro- 
ductive qualities. Two individuals similarly bred 
and indistinguishable in outward form may never- 
theless behave entirely differently when bred from. 
Take, for instance, the family of sweet - peas 
shown on Plate 1V. The F, generation here con- 
sists of seven distinct types, three sorts of purples, 
three sorts of reds, and whites. Let us suppose that 
our object is to obtain, a true breeding strain of the 
pale purple picotee form. Now from the pro- 
portions in which they come we know that the 
dilute colour is due to the absence of the factor 
which intensifies the colour. Consequently the 
146 


CHAP. XIV ECONOMICAL 147 


picotee cannot throw the two deeper shades of red 
or purple. But it may be heterozygous for the 
purpling factor, when it will throw the dilute red 
(Tinged white), or it may be heterozygous for either 
or both of the two colour factors (cf. p. 41), in 
which case it will throw whites. Of the picotees 
which come in such a family, therefore, some will 
give picotees, tinged whites, and whites, others will 
give picotees and tinged whites only, others will give 
picotees and whites only, while others, again, and 
these the least numerous, will give nothing but 
picotees. The new variety is already fixed in a 
certain definite proportion of the plants; in this 
particular instance in 1 out of every 27. All that 
remains to be done is to pick out these plants. 
Since all the picotees look alike, whatever their 
breeding capacity, the only way to do this is to save 
the seed from a number of such plants zxdividually, 
and to raise a further generation. Some of them 
will be found to breed true. The variety is then 
established, and may at once be put on the market 
with full confidence that it will hereafter throw none 
of the other forms. The all-important thing is to 
save and sow the seed of separate individuals 
separately. However alike they look, the seed from 
different individuals must on no account be mixed. 
Provided that due care is taken in this respect no 
long and tedious process of selection is required for 
the fixation of any given variety. Every possible 
variety arising from a cross appears in the F, 
generation if only a sufficient number is raised, and 
of all these different varieties a certain proportion of 
each is already fixed. Heredity is a question of 


148 MENDELISM CHAP. 


individuals, and the recognition of this will save the 
breeder much labour, and enable him to fix his 
varieties in the shortest possible time. 

Such cases as these of the sweet-pea throw a 
fresh light upon another of the breeder’s conceptions, 
that of purity of type. Hitherto the criterion of a 
“pure-bred” thing, whether plant or animal, has 
been its pedigree, and the individual was regarded 
as more or less pure bred for a given quality accord- 
ing as it could show a longer or shorter list of 
ancestors possessing this quality. To-day we realise 
that this is not essential. The pure-bred picotee 
appears in our F, family though its parent was a 
purple bicolor, and its remoter ancestors whites for 
generations. So also from the cross between pure 
strains of black and albino rabbits we may obtain in 
the F, generation animals of the wild agouti colour 
which breed as true to type as the pure wild rabbit 
of irreproachable pedigree. The true test of the 
pure breeding thing lies not in its ancestry but in 
the nature of the gametes which have gone to its 
making. Whenever two similarly constituted gametes 
unite, whatever the nature of the parents from which 
they arose, the resulting individual is homozygous 
in all respects and must consequently breed true. 
In deciding questions of purity it is to the gamete, 
and not to ancestry, that our appeal must hence- 
forth be made. 

Improvement is after all the keynote to the 
breeder’s operations. He is aiming at the produc- 
tion of a strain which shall combine the greatest 
number of desirable properties with the least number 
of undesirable ones. This good quality he must 


XIV ECONOMICAL 149 


take from one strain, that from another, and that, 
again, from a third, while at the same time avoiding 
all’ the poor qualities that these different strains 
possess. It is evident that the Mendelian concep- 
tion of characters based upon definite factors which 
are transmitted on a definite scheme must prove of 
the greatest service to him. For once these factors 
have been determined their distribution is brought 
under control, and they can be associated together 
or dissociated at the breeder’s will. The chief 
labour involved is that necessary for the determina- 
tion of the factors upon which the various characters 
depend. For it often happens that what appears to 
be a simple character turns out when analysed to 
depend upon the simultaneous presence of several 
distinct factors. Thus the Malay fowl breeds true 
to the walnut comb, as does also the Leghorn to the 
single comb, and when pure strains are crossed all 
the offspring have walnut combs. At first sight it 
would be not unnatural to regard the difference as 
dependent upon the presence or absence of a single 
factor. Yet, as we have already seen,.:two other 
types of comb, the pea and the rose, make their 
appearance in the F, generation. Analysis shows 
that the difference between the walnut and the single 
is a difference of two factors, and it is not until this 
has been determined that we can proceed with 
certainty to transfer the walnut character to a single- 
combed breed. Moreover, in his process of analysis 
the breeder. must be prepared to’ encounter the 
various phenomena that we have described under 
the headings of interaction of factors, coupling, and 
repulsion, and the recognition of these phenomena 


150 MENDELISM CHAP. 


will naturally influence his procedure. Or, again, 
his experiments may show him that one of the 
characters he wants, like the blue of the Andalusian 
fowl, is dependent upon the heterozygous nature of 
the individual which exhibits it, and if such is the 
case he will be wise to refrain from any futile 
attempt at fixing it. If it is essential it must be 
built up again in each generation, and he will 
recognise that the most economical way of doing 
this is to cross the two pure strains so that all the 
offspring may possess the desired character. The 
labour of analysis is often an intricate and tedious 
business. But once done it is done once for all. 
As soon as the various factors are determined upon 
which the various characters of the individual 
depend, as soon as the material to be made use 
of has been properly analysed, the production and 
fixation of the required combinations becomes a 
matter of simple detail. 

An excellent example of the practical application 
of Mendelian principles is afforded by the experi- 
ments which Professor Biffen has recently carried 
out in Cambridge. Taken as a whole English 
wheats compare favourably with foreign ones in 
respect of their cropping power. On the other 
hand, they have two serious defects. They are liable 
to suffer from the attacks of the fungus which causes 
rust, and they do not bake into a good loaf. 
This last property depends upon the amount of 
gluten present, and it is the greater proportion of 
this which gives to the “hard” foreign wheat its 
quality of causing the loaf to rise well when baked. 
For some time it was held that “hard” wheat with 


x1v ECONOMICAL 181 


a high glutinous content could not be grown in 
the English climate, and undoubtedly most of the 
hard varieties imported for trial deteriorated greatly 
in a very short time. Professor Biffen managed to 
obtain a hard wheat which kept its qualities when 
grown in England. But in spite of the superior 
quality of its grain from the baker’s point of view, 
its cropping capacity was too low for it to be grown 
profitably in competition with English wheats. 
Like the latter, it was also subject to rust. Among 
the many varieties which Professor Biffen collected 
and grew for observation he managed to find one 
which was completely immune to the attacks of the 
rust fungus, though in other respects it had no 
desirable quality to recommend it. Now as the 
result of an elaborate series of investigations, he was 
able to show that the qualities of heavy cropping 
capacity, “hardness” of grain, and immunity to rust 
can all be expressed in terms of Mendelian factors. 
Having once analysed his material the rest was 
comparatively simple, and in a few years he has 
been able to build up a strain of wheat which com- 
bines the cropping capacity of the best English 
varieties with the hardness of the foreign kinds, and 
at the same time is completely immune to rust. 
This wheat has already been shown to keep its 
qualities unchanged for several years, and there is 
little doubt that when it comes to be grown in 
quantity it will exert an appreciable influence on 
wheat-growing in Great Britain. 

It may be objected that it is often with small 
differences rather than with the larger and more 
striking ones that the breeder is mainly concerned. 


152 MENDELISM CHAP. 


It does not matter much to him whether the colour 
of a pea flower is purple or pink or white. But it 
does matter whether the plant bears rather larger 
seeds than usual, or rather more of them. Even a 
small difference when multiplied by the size of the 
crop will effect a considerable difference in the profit. 
It is the general experience of seedsmen and others 
that differences of this nature are often capable of 
being developed up to a certain point by a process 


3 400 a SS 

o 7 S. 

8 Var IN 

00 

33 V7 7 \ . 

la | / \ \ 

® 200 7 v 

a } \ 

5 1 N 

yz, 100 fr ‘ a 
N 


6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 
Weight of individual seeds 


FIG. 30. 


Curves to illustrate the influence of selection. 


of careful selection each generation. At first sight 
this appears to be something very like the gradual 
accumulation of minute variations through the con- 
tinuous application of a selective process. Some 
recent experiments by Professor Johannsen of Copen- 
hagen set the matter in a different light. One of 
his investigations deals with the inheritance of the 
weight of beans, but as an account of these experi- 
ments would involve us in the consideration of a 
large amount of detail we may take a simple 
imaginary case to illustrate the nature of the con- 


XIV ECONOMICAL 153 


clusions at which he arrived. If we weigh a 
number of seeds collected from a patch of plants 
such as Johannsen’s beans we should find that they 
varied considerably in size. The majority would 
probably not diverge very greatly from the general 
average, and as we approached the high or low 
extreme we should find a constantly decreasing 
number of individuals with these weights. Let us 
suppose that the weight of our seed varied between 
4 and 20 grains, that the greatest number of 
seeds were of the mean weight, viz. 12 grains, 
and that as we passed to either extreme at 4 
and 20 the number became regularly less. The 
weight relation of such a collection of seeds can be 
expressed by the accompanying curve (Fig. 30). 
Now if we select for sowing only that seed which 
weighs over 12 grains, we shall find that in the 
next generation the average weight of the seed is 
‘raised and the curve becomes somewhat shifted to 
the right as in the dotted line of Fig. 30. By con- 
tinually selecting we can shift our curve a little more 
to the right, ze. we can increase the average weight 
of the seeds until at last we come to a limit be- 
yond which further selection has no effect. This 
phenomenon has been long known, and it was 
customary to regard these variations as of a con- 
tinuous nature, ze. as all chance fluctuations in a 
homogeneous mass, and the effect of selection was 
supposed to afford evidence that small continuous 
variations could be increased by this process. But 
Johannsen’s results point to another interpretation. 
Instead of our material being homogeneous it is 
probably a mixture of several strains each with its 


154 MENDELISM CHAP. 


own average weight about which the varying con- 
ditions of the environment cause it to fluctuate. 
Each of these strains is termed a pure line. If we 
imagine that there are three such pure lines in our 
imaginary case, with average weights 10, 12, 14 
grains respectively, and if the range of fluctuation 
of each of these pure lines is 12 grains, then our 


3 
i=} 


w 
{=} 
i=) 


Number of seeds 


” 
° 
° 


= 


{ 

H 

4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 
Weight of individual seeds 


FIG. 31. 


Curves to illustrate the conception of pure lines in a population. 


curve must be represented as made up of the three 
components 


A fluctuating between 4 and 16 with a mean of 10 
B ” ” 6 Prd 18 ” bE) 12 
Cc 2 ” 8 ” 20 ” ” 14 


as is shown in Fig. 31. A seed that weighs 12 
grains may belong to any of these three strains. It 
may be an average seed of B, or a rather large seed 
of A, or a rather small seed of C. If it belongs to 
B its offspring will average 12 grains, if to A 
they will average 10 grains, and if to C they 
will average 14 grains. Seeds of similar weight 


XIV ECONOMICAL 155 


may give a different result because they happen to 
be fluctuations of different pure lines. But within 
the pure line any seed, large or small, produces the 
average result for that line. Thus a seed of line 
C which weighs 20 grains will give practically the 
same result as one that weighs Io grains. 

On this view we can understand why selection 
of the largest seed raises the average weight in the 
next generation. We are picking out more of C 
and less of A and B, and as this process is repeated 
the proportion of C gradually increases and we get 
the appearance of selection acting on a continuously 
varying homogeneous material and producing a 
permanent effect. This is because the interval 
between the average weight of the different pure 
lines is small compared with the environmental 
fluctuations. None the less it is there, and the 
secret of separating and fixing any of these pure 
lines is again to breed from the individual separately. 
As soon as the pure line is separated further selection 
becomes superfluous. 

Since the publication of Darwin’s famous work 
upon the.effects of cross- and self-fertilisation, it has 
been generally accepted that the effect of a cross is 
commonly, though not always, to introduce fresh 
vigour into the offspring, though why this should be 
so we are quite at a loss to explain. Continued 
close inbreeding, on the contrary, eventually leads 
to deterioration, though, as in many self-fertilised 
plants, a considerable number of generations may 
elapse before it shows itself in any marked degree. 
The fine quality of many of the seedsman’s choice 
varieties of vegetables probably depends upon the 


156 MENDELISM CHAP. 


fact that they have resulted from a cross but a few 
generations back, and it is possible that they often 
oust the older kinds not because they started as 
something intrinsically better, but because the latter 
had gradually deteriorated through continuous self- 
fertilisation. Most breeders are fully alive to the 
beneficial results of a cross so far as vigour is con- 
cerned, but they often hesitate to embark upon it 
owing to what was held to be the inevitably lengthy 
and laborious business of recovering the original 
variety and refixing it, even if in the process it was 
not altogether lost. That danger Mendelism has 
removed, and we now know that by working on 
these lines it is possible in three or four generations 
to recover the original variety in a fixed state with 
all the superadded vigour that follows from a cross. 
Nor is the problem one that concerns self-fertilised 
plants only. Plants that are reproduced asexually 
often appear to deteriorate after a few generations 
unless a sexual generation is introduced. New 
varieties of potato, for example, are frequently put 
upon the market, and their excellent qualities give 
them a considerable vogue. Much is expected of 
them, but time after time they deteriorate in a dis- 
appointing way and are lost to sight. It is not 
improbable that we are here concerned with a case 
in which the plants lose their vigour after a few 
asexual generations of reproduction from tubers, and 
can only recover it with the stimulus that results 
from the interpolation of a sexual generation. Un- 
fortunately this generally means that the variety is 
lost, for owing to the haphazard way in which new 
kinds of potatoes are reproduced it is probable that 


XIV ECONOMICAL 157 


most cultivated varieties are complex heterozygotes. 
Were the potato plant subjected to careful analysis 
and the various factors determined upon which its 
variations depend, we should be in a position to 
remake continually any good potato without running 
the risk of losing it altogether, as is now so often 
the case. 

The application of Mendelian principles is likely 
to prove of more immediate service for plants than 
animals, for owing to the large numbers which can 
be rapidly raised from a single individual and the 
prevalence of self-fertilisation, the process of analysis 
is greatly simplified. Even apart from the circum- 
stance that the two sexes may sometimes differ in 
their powers of transmission, the. mere fact of their 
separation renders the analysis of their properties 
more difficult. And as the constitution of the indi- 
vidual is determined by the nature and quality of its 
offspring, it is not easy to obtain this knowledge 
where the offspring, as in most animals, are relatively 
few. Still, as has been abundantly shown, the same 
principles hold good here also, and there is no reason 
why the process of analysis, though more trouble- 
some, should not be effectively carried out. At the 
same time, it affords the breeder a rational basis for 
some familiar but puzzling phenomena. The fact, 
for instance, that certain characters often “skip a 
generation” is simply the effect of dominance in F, 
and the reappearance of the recessive character in the 
following generation. “Reversion” and “atavism,” 
again, are phenomena which are no longer mysterious, 
but can be simply expressed in Mendelian terms 
as we have already suggested in Chap. VI. The 


158 MENDELISM CHAP. 


occasional appearance of a sport in a supposedly 
pure strain is often due to the reappearance of a 
recessive character. Thus even in the most highly 
pedigreed strains of polled cattle such as the Aber- 
deen-Angus, occasional individuals with horns appear. 
The polled character is dominant to the horned, and 
the occasional reappearance of the horned animal 
is due to the fact that some of the polled herd are 
heterozygous in this character. When two such indi- 
viduals are mated, the chances are 1 in 4 that the 
offspring will be horned. Though the heterozygous 
individuals may be indistinguishable in appearance 
from the pure dominant, they can be readily separated 
by the breeding test. For when crossed by the 
recessive, in this case horned animals, the pure 
dominant gives only polled’ beasts, while the hetero- 
zygous individual gives equal numbers of polled and 
horned ones. In this particular instance it would 
probably be impracticable to test all the cows by 
crossing with a horned bull. For in each case it 
would be necessary to have several polled calves 
from each before they could with reasonable certainty 
be regarded as pure dominants. But to ensure that 
no horned calves should come, it is enough to use a 
bull which is pure for that character. This can 
easily be tested by crossing him with a dozen or so 
horned cows. If he gets no horned calves out of 
these he may be regarded as a pure dominant and 
thenceforward put to his own cows, whether horned 
or polled, with the certainty that all his calves will 
be polled. 

Or, again, suppose that a breeder has a chestnut 
mare and wishes to make certain of a bay foal from 


XIV ECONOMICAL 159 


her. We know that bay is dominant to chestnut, 
and that if a homozygous bay stallion is used a bay 
foal must result. In his choice of a sire, therefore, 
the breeder must be guided by the previous record 
of the animal, and select one that has never given 
anything but bays when put to either bay or chest- 
nut mares. In this way he will assure himself of a 
bay foal from his chestnut mare, whereas if the record 
of the sire shows that he has given chestnuts he will 
be heterozygous, and the chances of his getting a 
bay or a chestnut out of a chestnut mare are equal. 

It is not impossible that the breeder may be 
unwilling to test his animals by crossing them with 
a different breed through fear that their purity may 
be thereby impaired, and that the influence of the 
previous cross may show itself in succeeding genera- 
tions. He might hesitate, for instance, to test his 
polled cows by crossing them with a horned bull for 
fear of getting horned calves when the cows were 
afterwards put to a polled bull of their own breed. 
The belief in the power of a sire to influence sub- 
sequent generations, or telegony as it is sometimes 
called, is not uncommon even to-day. Nevertheless, 
carefully conducted experiments by more than one 
competent observer have failed to elicit a single shred 
of unequivocal evidence in favour of the view. Until 
we have evidence based upon experiments which are 
capable of repetition, we may safely ignore telegony 
as a factor in heredity. 

Heterozygous forms play a greater part in the 
breeding of animals than of plants, for many of the 
qualities sought after by the breeder are of this 
nature. Such is the blue of the Andalusian fowl, 


160 MENDELISM CHAP. XIV 


and, according to Professor Wilson, the roan of the 
Shorthorn is similar, being the heterozygous form 
produced by mating red with white. The characters 
of certain breeds of canaries and pigeons again 
appear to depend upon their heterozygous nature. 
Such forms cannot, of course, ever be bred true, 
and where several factors are concerned they may 
when bred together produce but a small proportion 
of offspring like themselves. As soon, however, as 
their constitution has been analysed and expressed 
in terms of Mendelian factors, pure strains can be 
built up which when crossed will give nothing but 
offspring of the desired heterozygous form. 

The points with which the breeder is concerned 
are often fine ones, not very evident except to the 
practised eye. Between an ordinary Dutch rabbit 
and a winner, or between the comb of a Hamburgh 
that is fit to show and one that is not, the differences 
are not very apparent to the uninitiated. Whether 
Mendelism will assist the breeder in the production 
of these finer points is at present doubtful. It may 
be that these small differences are heritable, such as 
those that form the basis ‘of Johannsen’s pure lines. 
In this case the breeder’s outlook is hopeful. But it 
may be that the variations which he seeks to per- 
petuate are of the nature of fluctuations, dependent 
upon the earlier life conditions of the individual, and 
not upon the constitution of the gametes by which 
it was formed. If such is the case, he will get no 
help from the science of heredity, for we know of 
no evidence which might lead us to suppose that 
variations of this sort can ever become fixed and 
heritable. 


CHAPTER XV 
MAN 


THOUGH the interest attaching to heredity in man 
is more widespread than in other animals, it is far 
more difficult to obtain evidence that is both com- 
‘plete and accurate. The species is one in which 
the differentiating characters separating individual 
from individual are very numerous, while the number 
of the offspring is comparatively few, and the gener- 
ations are far between. For these reasons, even if 
it were possible, direct experimental work with man 
would be likely to prove both tedious and expensive. 
There is, however, another method besides the direct 
one from which something can be learned. This 
consists in collecting all the evidence possible, ar- 
‘ranging it in the form of pedigrees, and. comparing 
“it with standard cases already worked out in animals 
and plants. In this way it has been possible to 
demonstrate in man the existence of several char- 
acters showing simple Mendelian inheritance. As 
few besides medical men have hitherto been con- 
cerned practically with heredity, such records as 
exist are, for the most part, records of deformity or 
of disease. So it happens that most of the pedigrees 
at present available deal with characters which are 
161 M 


162 MENDELISM CHAP. 


usually classed as abnormal. In some of these the 
inheritance is clearly Mendelian. One of the cases 
which has been most fully worked out is that of a 


FiG. 32. 


Normal and brachydactylous hands placed together for comparison. 
(From Drinkwater.) 


deformity known as brachydactyly. In brachy- 
dactylous people the whole of the body is much 
stunted, and the fingers and toes appear to have two 
joints only instead of three (cf. Figs. 32 and 33). 
The inheritance of this peculiarity has been carefully 


XV MAN 163 


investigated by Dr. Drinkwater, who collected all the 
data he was able to find among the members of a 
large family in which it occurred. The result is the 
pedigree shown on p. 159. It is assumed that all 
who are recorded as having offspring were married 
to normals. Examination of the pedigree brings 
out the facts (1) that all affected individuals have an 
affected parent; (2) that none of the unaffected in- 


Fic. 33. 


Radiograph of a brachydactylous hand. 


dividuals, though sprung from the affected, ever 
have descendants who are affected ; and (3) that in 
families ;' where both affected and unaffected occur, 
the numbers of the two classes are, on the average, 
equal. (The sum of such families in the complete 
pedigree is thirty-nine affected and thirty-six normals.) 
It is obvious that these are the conditions which are 
fulfilled in a simple Mendelian case, and there is 
nothing in this pedigree to contradict the assertion 
that brachydactyly, whatever it may be due to, 
M 2 


) 


(4 unknown) 


é3 
é 


é 


é 


ex and type 
uncertain 


f 


9° 


MENDELISM CHAP. 


behaves as a simple 
dominant to the normal 
form, ze. that it depends 
upon a factor which the 
normal does not con- 
tain. The recessive nor- 
mals cannot transmit the 
affected condition what- 
evér their ancestry. 
Once free they are always 
free, and can marry other 
normals with full confi- 
dence that none of their 
children will show the 
deformity. 


te 3 The evidence avail- 
ot 4 able from pedigrees has 
& = 3 revealed the simplest 
poss form of Mendelian in- 
re —tee heritance in several 
lee human defects and dis- 
CS eases, among which may 
be be mentioned presenile 
ie cataract of the eyes, an 
Leet abnormal form of skin 
*0 thickening in the palms 
sian hs of the hands and soles of 


the feet, known as tylosis, 
and epidermolysis bul- 
losa, a disease in which 
the skin rises up into 
numerous bursting 
blisters. 


Pedigree of Drinkwater's brachydactylous family. The affected members are indicated by black and the normals by light circles. 


XV MAN ' 165 


Among the most interesting of all human pedi- 
grees is one recently built up by Mr. Nettleship 
from the records of a night-blind family living 
near Montpellier in the south of France. In night- 
blind people the retina is insensitive to light which 
falls below a certain intensity, and such people 
are consequently blind in failing daylight or in 
moonlight. As the Montpellier case had excited 
interest for some time, the records are unusually 
complete. They commence with a certain Jean 
Nougaret, who was born in 1637, and suffered from 
night-blindness, and they end for the present with 
children who are to-day but a few years of age. 
Particulars are known of over 2000 of the descend- 
ants of Jean Nougaret. Through ten generations and 
nearly three centuries the affection has behaved as a 
Mendelian dominant, and there is no sign that long- 
continued marriage with folk of normal vision has 
produced any amelioration of the night-blind state. 

Besides cases such as these where a simple form 
of Mendelian inheritance is obviously indicated, there 
are others which are more difficult to read. Of some 
it may be said that on the whole the peculiarity 
behaves as though it were an ordinary dominant ; 
but that exceptions occur in which affected children 
are born to unaffected parents. It is not impossible 
that the condition may, like colour in the sweet-pea, 
depend upon the presence or absence of more than 
one factor. In none of these cases, however, are 
the data sufficient for determining with certainty 
whether this is so or not. 

A group of cases of exceptional interest is that 
in which the incidence of disease is largely, if not 


166 MENDELISM CHAP. 


absolutely, restricted to one sex, and so far as is 
hitherto known the burden is invariably borne by 
the male. In the inheritance of colour-blindness 
(p. 108) we have already discussed an instance in 
which the defect is rare, though not unknown, in the 
female. Sex-limited inheritance of a similar nature 
is known for one or two ocular defects, and for 
several diseases of the nervous system. In the 
peculiarly male disease known as hemophilia the 
blood refuses to clot when shed, and there is nothing 


Ob 
+O 
+O 4 


g 
—Tt { T 1 tr T T 1 } T T TT 
$dd22 2 FB eo¢g¢ 22? P&P dé 
Mh ey te hh dite 
ééeé eee odds? 
Children ae Se 
(*)o gd ¢ all healthy 76 3?) F 
Fie. 35. 


Pedigree of a hemophilic family. Affected (all males) represented by black, 
and normals of both sexes by light circles, (From Stahel.) 
to prevent great loss from even a superficial scratch. 
In its general trend the inheritance of hemophilia is 
not unlike that of horns among sheep, and it is possible 
that we are here again dealing with a character which 
is dominant in one sex and recessive in the other. 
But the evidence so far collected points to a differ- 
ence somewhere, for in hemophilic families the 
affected males, instead of being equal in number to 
the unaffected, show a considerable preponderance. 
The unfortunate nature of the defect, however, forces 
us to rely for our interpretation almost entirely upon 
the families produced by the unaffected females who 


xv MAN 167 


can transmit it. Our knowledge of the offspring 
of “bleeding” males is as yet far too scanty, and 
until it is improved, or until we can find some 
parallel case in animals or plants, the precise 
scheme of inheritance for hemophilia must remain 
undecided. 

Though by far the greater part of the human 
evidence relates to abnormal or diseased conditions, 
a start has been made in obtaining pedigrees of 
normal characters. From the ease with which it 
can be observed, it was natural that eye-colour 
should be early selected as a subject of investigation, 
and the work of Hurst and others has clearly demon- 
strated the existence of one Mendelian factor in 
operation here. Eyes are of many colours, and the 
colour depends upon the pigment in the iris. Some 
eyes have pigment on both sides of the iris—on the 
side that faces the retina as well as on the side that 
looks out upon the world. Other eyes have pigment 
on the retinal side only. To this class belong the 
blues and clear greys ; while the eyes with pigment 
in front of the iris also are brown, hazel, or green in 
various shades according to the amount of pigment 
present. In albino animals the pigment is entirely 
absent, and as the little blood-vessels are not ob- 
scured the iris takes on its characteristic pinkish-red 
appearance. The condition in which pigment is 
present in front of the iris is dominant to that in 
which it is absent. Greens, browns, or hazels mated 
together may, if heterozygous, give the recessive 
blue, but no individuals of the brown class are to 
be looked for among the offspring of blues mated 
together. The blues, however, may carry factors 


168 MENDELISM CHAP. 


which are capable of modifying the brown. Just 
as the pale pink-tinged sweet-pea (PI. IV., 9) 
when mated with a suitable white gives only deep 
purples, so an eye with very little brown pigment 
mated with certain blues produces progeny of a 
deep brown, far darker than either parent. The 
blue may carry a factor which brings about intensi- 
fication of the brown pigment. There are doubtless 
other factors which modify the brown when present, 
but we do not yet know enough of the inheritance of 
the various shades to justify any statement other than 
that the heredity of the pigment in front of the iris 
behaves as though it were due to a Mendelian factor. 

Even this fact is of considerable importance, for 
it at once suggests that the present systems of classi- 
fication of eye-colours, to which some anthropologists 
attach considerable weight, are founded on a purely 
empirical and unsatisfactory basis. Intensity of 
colour is the criterion at present in vogue, and it is 
customary to arrange the eye-colours in a scale of 
increasing depth of shade, starting with pale greys 
and ending with the deepest browns. On this 
system the lighter greens are placed among the 
blues. But we now know that blues may differ from 
the deep browns in the absence of only a single 
factor, while, on the other hand, the difference 
between a blue and a green may be a difference 
dependent upon more than one factor. To what 
extent eye-colour may be valuable as a criterion of 
race it is at present impossible to say, but if it is 
ever to become so, it will only be after a searching 
Mendelian analysis has disclosed the factors upon 
which the numerous varieties depend. 


xv MAN 169 


A discussion of eye-colour suggests reflections of 
another kind. It is difficult to believe that the 
markedly different states of pigmentation which 
occur in the same species are not associated with 
deep-seated chemical differences influencing the 
character and bent of the individual. May not these 
differences in pigmentation be coupled with and so 
become in some measure a guide to mental and 
temperamental characteristics? In the National 
Portrait Gallery in London the pictures of cele- 
brated men and women are largely grouped accord- 
ing to the vocations in which they have succeeded. 
The observant will probably have noticed that there 
is a tendency for a given type of eye-colour to pre- 
dominate in some of the larger groups. It is rare 
to find anything but a blue among the soldiers and 
sailors, while among the actors, preachers, and 
orators the dark eye is predominant, although for 
the population as a whole it is far scarcer than the 
light. The facts are suggestive, and it is not im- 
possible that future research may reveal an intimate 
connection between peculiarities of pigmentation and 
peculiarities of mind. 

The inheritance of mental characters is often 
elusive, for it is frequently difficult to appraise the 
effects of early environment in determining a man’s 
bent. That ability can be transmitted there is no 
doubt, for this is borne out by general experience, 
as well as by the numerous cases of able families 
brought together by Galton and others. But when 
we come to inquire more precisely what it is that is 
transmitted we are baffled. A distinguished son 
follows in the footsteps ofa distinguished father. Is 


‘170 MENDELISM CHAP. 


this due to the inheritance of a particular mental apti- 
tude, or is it an instance of general mental ability 
displayed in a field rendered attractive by early 
association? We have at present very little definite 
evidence for supposing that what appear to be 
special forms of ability may be due to specific 
factors. Hurst, indeed, has brought forward some 
facts which suggest that musical sense sometimes 
behaves as a recessive character, and it is likely 
that the study of some clean-cut faculty such as the 
mathematical one would yield interesting results. 
The analysis of mental characters will no doubt 
be very difficult, and possibly the best line of attack 
is to search for cases where they are associated with 
some physical feature such as pigmentation. If an 
association of this kind be found, and the pigmenta- 
tion factors be determined, it is evident that we 
should thereby obtain an insight into the nature of 
the units upon which mental conditions depend. 
Nor must it be forgotten that mental qualities, such 
as quickness, generosity, instability, etc—dqualities 
which we are accustomed to regard as convenient 
units in classifying the different minds with which 
we are daily brought into contact—are not neces- 
sarily qualities that correspond to heritable units. 
Effective mental ability is largely a matter of tem- 
perament, and this in turn is quite possibly dependent 
upon the various secretions produced by the different 
tissues of the body. Similar nervous systems associ- 
ated with different livers might conceivably result in 
individuals upon whose mental ability the world 
would pass a very different judgment. Indeed, it is 
not at all impossible that a particular form of 


XV MAN 171 


mental ability may depend for its manifestation, not 
so much upon an essential difference in the structure 
of the nervous system, as upon the production by 
another tissue of some specific poison which causes 
the nervous system to react in a definite way. We 
have mentioned these possibilities merely to indicate 
how complex the problem may turn out to be. 
Though there is no doubt that mental ability is 
inherited, what it is that is transmitted, whether 
factors involving the quality and structure of the 
nervous system itself, or factors involving the pro- 
duction of specific poisons by other tissues, or both 
together, is at present uncertain. 

Little as is known to-day of heredity in man, 
that little is of extraordinary significance. The 
qualities of men and women, physical and mental, 
depend primarily upon the inherent properties of the 
gametes which went to their making. Within limits 
these qualities are elastic, and can be modified to a 
greater or lesser extent by influences brought to bear 
upon the growing zygote, provided always that the 
necessary basis is present upon which these influences 
can work. If the mathematical faculty has been 
carried in by the gamete, the education of the zygote 
will enable him to make the most of it. But if the 
basis is not there, no amount of education can trans- 
form that zygote into a mathematician This isa 
matter of common experience. Neither is there any 
reason for supposing that the superior education of a 
mathematical zygote will thereby increase the mathe- 
matical propensities of the gametes which live within 
him. For the gamete recks little of quaternions. It is 
true that there is progress of a kind in the world, and 


172 MENDELISM CHAP. 


that this progress is largely due to improvements in 
education and hygiene. The people of to-day are 
better fitted to cope with their material surroundings 
than were the people of even a few thousand years 
ago. And as time goes on they are able more and 
more to control the workings of the world around 
them. But there is no reason for supposing that 
this is because the effects of education are inherited. 
Man stores knowledge as a bee stores honey or a 
squirrel stores nuts. With man, however, the hoard 
is of a more lasting nature. Each generation in 
using it sifts, adds, and rejects, and passes it on to 
the next a little better and a little fuller. When 
we speak of progress we generally mean that the 
hoard has been improved, and is of more service to 
man in his attempts to control his surroundings. 
Sometimes this hoarded knowledge is spoken of as 
the inheritance which a generation receives from 
those who have gone before. This is misleading. 
The handing on of such knowledge has nothing 
more to do with heredity in the biological sense than 
has the handing on from parent to offspring of a 
picture, or a title, or a pair of boots. All these 
things are but the transfer from zygote to zygote 
of something extrinsic to the species. Heredity, on 
the other hand, deals with the transmission of some- 
thing intrinsic from gamete to zygote and from 
zygote to gamete. It is the participation of the 
gamete in the process that is our criterion of what 
is and what is not heredity. 

Better hygiene and better education, then, are 
good for the zygote, because they help him to make 
the fullest use of his inherent qualities. But the 


XV MAN 173 


qualities themselves remain unchanged in so far as 
the gamete is concerned, since the gamete pays no 
heed to the intellectual development of the zygote 
in whom he happens to dwell. Nevertheless, upon 
the gamete depend those inherent faculties which 
enable the zygote to profit by his opportunities, and, 
unless the zygote has received them from the gamete, 
the advantages of education are of little worth. If 
we are bent upon producing a permanent betterment 
that shall be independent of external circumstances, 
if we wish the national stock to become inherently 
more vigorous in mind and body, more free from 
congenital physical defect and feeble mentality, better 
able to assimilate and act upon the stores of know- 
ledge which have been accumulated through the 
centuries, then it is the gamete that we must con- 
sult. The saving grace is with the gamete, and 
with the gamete alone. 

People generally look upon the human species as 
having two kinds of individuals, males and females, 
and it is for them that the sociologists and legislators 
frame their schemes. This, however, is but an im- 
perfect view to take of ourselves. In reality we are 
of four kinds, male zygotes and female zygotes, large 
gametes and small gametes, and heredity is the link 
that binds us together. Jf our lives were like those 
of the starfish or the sea-urchin, we should probably 
have realised this sooner. For the gametes of these 
animals live freely, and contract their marriages in 
the waters of the sea. With us it is different, 
because half of us must live within the other half or 
perish. Parasites upon the rest, levying a daily toll 
of nutriment upon their hosts, they are yet in some 


174 MENDELISM CHAP. XV 


measure the arbiters of the destiny of those within 
whom they dwell. At the moment of union of two 
gametes is decided the character of another zygote, 
as well as the nature of the population of gametes 
which must make its home within him. The union 
once effected the inevitable sequence takes its course, 
and whether it be good, or whether it be evil, we, the 
zygotes, have no longer power to alter it. We are in 
the hands of the gamete; yet not entirely. For 
though we cannot influence their behaviour we can 
nevertheless control their unions if we choose to do 
so. By regulating their marriages, by encouraging 
the desirable to come together, and by keeping the 
undesirable apart we could go far towards ridding 
the world of the squalor and the misery that come 
through disease and weakness and vice. But before 
we can be prepared to act, except, perhaps, in the 
simplest cases, we must learn far more about them. 
At present we are woefully ignorant of much, though 
we do know that full knowledge is largely a matter 
of time and means. One day we shall have it, and 
the day may be nearer than most suspect. Whether 
we make use of it will depend in great measure upon 
whether we are prepared to recognise facts, and to 
modify or even destroy some of ‘the conventions 
which we have become accustomed to regard as the 
foundations of our social life. Whatever be the 
outcome, there can be little doubt that the future of 
our civilisation, perhaps even the possibility of a 
future at all, is wrapped up with the recognition we 
accord to those who live unseen and inarticulate 
within us—the fateful race of gametes so irrevocably 
bound to us by that closest of all ties, heredity. 


APPENDIX 


AS some readers may possibly care to repeat 
Mendel’s experiments for themselves, a few words 
on the methods used in crossing may not be super- 
fluous. The flower of the pea with its standard, 
wings, and median keel is too familiar to need 
description. Like most flowers it is hermaphrodite. 
Both male and female organs occur on the same 
flower, and are covered by the keel. The anthers, 
ten in number, are arranged in a circle round the 
pistil. As soon as they are ripe they burst and shed 
their pollen on the style. The pollen tubes then 
penetrate the stigma, pass down the style, and 
eventually reach the ovules in the lower part of the 
pistil. Fertilisation occurs here. Each ovule, which 
is reached by a pollen tube, swells up and becomes a 
seed. At the same time the fused carpels enclosing 
the ovules enlarge to form the pod. When this, the 
normal mode of fertilisation, takes place, the flower 
is said to be selfed. 

In crossing, it is necessary to emasculate a 
flower on the plant chosen to be the female parent. 
For this purpose a young flower must be taken 
in which the anthers have not yet burst. The 


175 


176 MENDELISM 


keel is depressed, and the stamens bearing the 
anthers are removed at their base by a pair of fine 
forceps. It will probably be found necessary to tear 
the keel slightly in order to do this. The pistil is 
then covered up again with the keel, and the flower 
is enclosed in a bag of waxed paper until the follow- 
ing day. The stigma is then again exposed and 
dusted with ripe pollen from a flower of the plant 
selected as the male parent. This done, the keel is 
replaced, and the flower again enclosed in its bag to 
protect it from the possible attentions of insects until 
it has set seed. The bag may be removed in about a 
week after fertilisation. It is perhaps hardly necessary 
to add that strict biological cleanliness must be 
exercised during the fertilising operations. This is 
readily attained by sterilising fingers and forceps 
with a little strong spirit before each operation, there- 
by ensuring the death of any foreign pollen grains 
which may be present. 

The above method applies also to sweet-peas, 
with these slight modifications. As the anthers ripen 
relatively sooner in this species, emasculation must 
be performed at a rather earlier stage. It is generally 
safe to choose a bud about three parts grown. The 
interval between emasculation and fertilisation must 
be rather longer. Two to three days is generally 
sufficient. Further, the sweet-pea is visited by the 
leaf-cutter bee, Megachile, which, unlike the honey 
bee, is able to depress the keel and gather pollen. 
If the presence of this insect is suspected, it is 
desirable to guard against the risk of admixture of 
foreign pollen by selecting for pollinating purposes 
a flower which has not quite opened. If the 


APPENDIX 177 


standard is not erected, it is unlikely to have been 
visited by Megachile. Lastly, it not infrequently 
happens that the little beetle Meligethes is found 
inside the keel. Such flowers should be rejected 
for crossing purposes. 


INDEX 


Abraxas grossutariata, 96 

‘« Acquired” characters, 13 
Adaptation, 137 

Agouti mice, 46 

Albino mice, 46 

Albinos, nature of, 49 
Amauris, 138 

Analysis of types, 149 
Ancestral Heredity, Law of, 11 
Andalusian fowls, 64 

Axil colour in sweet-peas, 85 


Bateson, W., 13, 25, 26, 51, 112, 
127, 135 

Biffen, R. H., 150 

Blue Andalusian fowls, 65 

Brachydactyly, 162 - 

Bryony, 116 

Bush sweet-peas, 57 


Castle, 127 

Cattle, horns in, 79, 158 

Colour, nature of, in flowers, 45 

Colour-blindness, 113 

Combs of fowls, 29, 40 

Correns, C., 26, 116 

Coupling of characters in gametes, 
86 

Cuénot, 46, 115 

‘Cupid ” sweet-peas, 57 

Currant moth, 96 


Darwin, C., 9, 60, 140, 155 
De Vries, H., 14, 26, 135 
Discontinuity in variation, 13 
Dominant characters, 16 
Doncaster, L., 96 
Drinkwater, H., 163 


Dutch rabbits, 55 


Eggs, 2 

Environment, influence of, 132 
Euralia, 139 

Evolution, 9, 78, 134 

Eye, in primulas, 51 
Eye-colour, in man, 167 


Factor, definition of, 28 

Factors, interaction of, 39 
Fertilisation, 3 

Fertilisation, self- and cross-, 155 
Fixation of varieties, 146 
Fluctuations, 133 

Fowls, coloured from whites, 45, 68 


Galton, 11, 169 
Gametes, nature of, 5 
Gregory, R. P., 51 


Hemophilia, 166 

Hardy, G. H., 141 

Heterozygote, definition of, 25 

Heterozygote, of intermediate 
form, 63 

Hieracium, 24, 128 

Himalayan rabbits, 55 

Homostyle primulas, 52 

Homozygote, definition of, 25 

Hooded sweet-peas, 82 

Horses, bay and chestnut in, 158 

Hurst, C. C., 57, 167, 170 


Immunity in wheat, 151 
Individuality, 130 

Inhibition, factors for, 69, 104 
Intermediates, 121 


179 


180 
Johannsen, W., 152 
Lop-eared rabbits, 127 


Mendel, 7, 15, 24, 128 

Mental characters, 170 

Mice, inheritance of coat colour in, 
46 

Mimicry, 138 

Mirabilis, 145 

Morgan, T. H., 111 

Mulattos, 125 

Mutation, 77, 132 


Nageli, C., 24 

Natural selection, 10, 135, 137, 143 
Nettleship, E., 165 
Night-blindness, 165 


Pararge egeria, 127 
Parkinson, J., 117 

Pea comb, 29 

Peas, coloured flowers in, 22 
Peas, tall and dwarf, 16 


Pigeons, 79 

Pin-eye in primulas, 51 
Pisum, 15 

Primulas, 28, 51, 63, 86 
Pollen, 3 


Pollen of sweet-peas, 84 

Pomace fly, rr1 

Population, inheritance of char- 
acters in a, 141 

Presence and Absence theory, 31 

Pure lines, 154 

Purity of gametes, 21 

Purity of type, 148 


Rabbits, 49, 55 
Ratios, Mendelian— 


ge a 31a 

9: 3:3: 4, 23, 30 
9: 3:4 48 
9:7, 46 


Ray, John, 138 
Recessive characters, 17 
Repulsion between factors, 83 


MENDELISM 


Reversion, 54, 157 
in rabbits, 55 
in sweet-peas, 57 
in fowls, 59 
in pigeons, 60 
Rose comb, 29 


Saunders, E. R., 50, 118 
Seeds, nature of, 4 
Segregation, 20 
Selection, 155 
Sheep, horns in, 71 
Silky fowls, 27, 102 
Single comb, 29 
Species, nature of, 143 
Species, origin of, 10 
Speckled wood butterfly, 127 
Spermatozoa, 3 
Sports, 140 
Staples-Browne, R., 61 
Sterility, 145 
Sterility in sweet-peas, 85 
Stocks, double, 117 
Stocks, hoariness in, 50 
Sweet-pea, colour in, 41, 74 
history of, 76 
inheritance of hood in, 82 
inheritance of size in, 57 


Telegony, 159 

Thrum-eye in primulas, 51 
Toe, extra toe in poultry, 70 
Tschermak, E., 26 


Unit-character, definition of, 28 

Variation, 13, 132, 134 

Walnut comb, 29 

Weismann, A., 12 

Wheat, beard in, 69 
experiments with, 150 

White, dominant in poultry, 67 

Wilson, J., 160 

Yellow mice, 115 


Zygotes, nature of, 5 


Printed by R. & R. Crark, Limitep, Edinburgh. 


Haiti 
a . ¥ 
: hs | _