Skip to main content

Full text of "The effect of length of blind alleys on maze learning: an experiment on twenty-four white rates"

See other formats


a 


x 
* 


* 


Cornell University 


Library 


The original of this book is in 
the Cornell University Library. 


There are no known copyright restrictions in 
the United States on the use of the text. 


http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924024783957 


Cornell University Library 
QL 785.P48 


On 


BEHAVIOR MONOGRAPHS 


Volume 3, Number 4, 1917 , Serial Number 15 


Edited by JOHN B. WATSON 
The Johns Hopkins University 


The Effect of Length of Blind Alleys on 
Maze Leaming: 
An Experiment on Twenty-Four White 
Rats 


BY 
JOSEPH PETERSON 


Published 
at Cambridge, Boston, Mass. 
HENRY HOLT & COMPANY 
_ 34 West 33d Street, New York 
G. E. STECHERT & CO., London, Paris and Leipzig, Foreign Agents 


The Journal of Animal Behavior 


An organ for the publication of studies concerning the instincts, 
habits and intelligence of organisms 


The Journal contains a Department of Notes in which appear brief accounts of 
especially interesting and valuable observations of behavior. 


Published bi-monthly at Cambridge, Boston, Mass., by Henry 
Holt and Company, New York. Each volume contains at léast 450 
pages with plates and text-figures. 

The subscription price is $5.00 per volume (foreign subscription 
$5.50) postpaid. an 


Manuscripts, subscriptions, and all business correspondence should 
be addressed to f 


THE JOURNAL OF ANIMAL BEHAVIOR 
Emerson Hall, Cambridge, Massachusetts 


Behavior Monog 


For the publication of studies in behavior and intelligence which are 
too lengthy or too expensive for acceptance 
by The Journal of Animal Behavior 


Published at irregular intervals at Cambridge, Boston, Mass., in 
connection with the Journal of Animal Behavior, by Henry Holt and 
Company, New York. Each volume contains approximately 450 
pages with plates and text-figures. 

The subscription price is $3.00 per volume (foreign subscription 
$3.50) postpaid. 

Monographs may be purchased separately at prices varying with 
the cost of manufacture. 

Manuscripts and inquiries concerning terms Lot publication should 
be addressed to the Editor of the Behavior Monographs, 

JOHN B. WATSON, 
The Johns Hopkins University, 
Baltimore, Md. 


Subscription to The Journal of Animal Behavior and the Behavior 
Monographs should be sent to Emerson Hall, Cambridge, Maasa- 
chusetts. 


BEHAVIOR MONOGRAPHS 


Volume 3, Number 4, 1917 Serial Number 15 


Edited by JOHN B. WATSON 
_The Johns Hopkins University 


The Effect of Length of Blind Alleys on 


Maze Learning: 


An Experiment on Twentv-Four White 
Rats 


JOSEPH PETERSON 


Assistant Professor of Psychology, University of Minnesota 


Published 
at Cambridge, Boston, Mass. 
HENRY HOLT & COMPANY 
34 West 33d Street, New York 
G. E. STECHERT & CO., London, Paris and Leipzig, Foreign Agents 


THE EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON 
MAZE LEARNING: AN EXPERIMENT ON 
TWENTY-FOUR WHITE RATS 


THE GENERAL PROBLEM 


“ How far are pleasurable results able to burn in and render 
predominant the association which led to them? This is perhaps 
the greatest problem of both human and animal psychology.” 
So wrote Thorndike in 1898. The problem is not yet solved. 
The problem arises from the fact, clearly pointed out by Thorn- 
dike, that ‘‘ the connection thus stamped in is not contemporaneous 
[with], but prior to the pleasure.”’: “‘ There is no pleasure along 
with the association. The pleasure does not come until after the 
association is done and gone.’ This problem, though raised 
by Lloyd Morgan: in connection with experiments on learning 
by the ‘trial and error ’’ method, has received very little but 
theoretical attention from psychologists to the present time.‘ 
Its importance for the education process, including the informal 
moral development by general social conditions, is certainly 
such as not to be overlooked. The dearth of experimentation 


1 Thorndike, E. L. Psychol. Mon., Ser. No. 8, p. 103. After nineteen years 
of extensive work on certain phases of learning Professor J. B. Watson, who has 
himself taken a considerable part in this experimental work, says practically the 
same thing. In a review of Holt’s The Freudian Wish and Its Place in Ethics, in 
which he considers a few artificial and inadequate illustrations of learning with 
but slight attention by the author to the neural processes involved, Watson says: 
“In these few experiences a genuine learning process is involved and the explana- 
tion of this learning process—regardless of whether the act is acquired in, few or 
many trials—is what I consider one of the chief problems in psychology.”” Jour. 
of Phil. Psychol., etc., 1917, 16,, p. 89. 

2 Tbid., p. 104. 

3 Introduction to Comparative Psychology, 1894, Ch. 12. E.g., on page 213 Morgan 
says: ‘‘ The successful response is repeated because of the satisfaction it gives; 
the unsuccessful response fails to give satisfaction, and is not repeated.” 

4 See, ¢.g., Smith, S. Limits of Educability in Paranecium. Jour. Comp. Neurol. 
and Psychol., 1908, 18, 499-510. Meyer, Max, The Fundamental Laws of Human 
Behavior, 1911. Thorndike, E. L. Animal Intelligence, 1911, Ch. 4. Haggerty, 
M.E. The Laws of Learning. Psychol. Rev., 1913, 20, 411-422. Carr, Harvey A. 
Principles of Selection in Animal Learning. Jbid., 1914, 21, 157-165. Watson, 
J. B. Behavior, 1914, Ch. 7. Peterson, Jos. Completeness of Response as an 
Explanation Principle in Learning. Psychol. Rev., 1916, 23, 153-162. 


1 


2 JOSEPH PETERSON 


on the problem is likely due to the mind-body relations implied 
in the early form of its statement. 

It is desirable to rescue any problem as to how learning goes 
on from mere theoretical discussions. Professor Watson has 
already attempted this for the problem in question, though 
not yet with marked success.’ Two groups of rats were allowed 
to solve individually a certain problem; in the one group each 
animal was fed immediately after the successful movements 
that brought it into the food box, while in the other group each 
rat was not allowed to take food for thirty seconds after entrance 
to the food box. No difference in the learning of the two groups 
was found. The experiment is regarded as only preliminary 
to a further study of the matter. Two criticisms may be offered 
on this experiment. In the first place, it is not on a wholly 
objective basis. As reported the experiment did not seem to 
be free from the assumption that the question at issue is whether 
the pleasure of the eating had a “‘ stamping-in”’ effect, to use a 
term of Thorndike’s, on the processes leading up to the eating. 
‘““ Successful movements ’’ seem to be regarded as movements 
bringing about this pleasure. If, in the second place, this is 
not the true meaning of the author, it may be suggested that 
the test of the effect of ‘‘ successful movements ”’ is not adequate, 
since precisely the same kind of acts was necessary for both 
groups of animals to get out of the situation presented by the 
problem. Experience with rats will certainly suggest that after 
an animal has once been fed in the food box it will for a time 
work energetically and learn to run the maze without further 
feeding of the kind, particularly if the odor is not carefully 
excluded. As the habit becomes partly fixed it is questionable 
whether the feeding, or even the smell of food, has very much 
to do with the energy that the animal displays. So far as the 
writer’s own experience goes—though he has made no definite 
test of the matter—it appears that once the habit is well under 
way the animal will display great energy in the usual way as 
soon as placed into the entrance box; that the habit will unwind 
itself on the basis of the numerous other stimuli which have 
accompanied the process before. However, any criticism of 
Watson's experiment on the basis of his report of the preliminary 

6 An abstract of the experiment, which was reported in the Chicago convention 


of ue American Psychological Association, is printed in Psychol. Bull., 1916, 13, 
p. 77. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 3 


procedure is unfair, and our only purpose here is to point out 
that there are real difficulties involved such as may give trouble 
to even an experienced behaviorist. 4 

The problem must be conceived in some other way, and in 
terms of stimulus sand response as Watson has rightly insisted.s 
Elsewhere the writer has attempted a statement of the general 
problem in a form more acceptable for experimentation. The 
general thought in mind, whatever the degree of success of its 
statement may have been, was this: Response is never, in 
the case of learning, at least, a reaction to a single stimulus. 
The afferent impulse never begins at a given receptor as the 
result of stimulus by a single object and thence passes into 
motor channels from only one particular afferent fiber. The 
situation in all learning is vastly more complex. A complication 
of external stimuli is nearly always to be reckoned with; then 
again, the afferent impulses from these stimuli are greatly 
determined in their relative effects on response by impulses from 
the proprio- and the entero-ceptive systems; and, in addition, 
the responses resulting are to a large extent determined by 
the general conformation of the organism. Different forms of 
animals have different action systems, for example. The 
pleasantness or unpleasantness of an act is only an inner indica- 
tion as to’ whether the response, forced by the complex inner 
organization (inherited and acquired) and the outer circum- 
stances, or stimuli, is or is not in general harmony with the 
conformation of the organism. The question of explanation 
may resolve itself, then, wholly into one of the physical and 
physiological circumstances. It was then,suggested that all such 
factors as recency, frequency, and intensity of stimuli, which 
may be conceived as involving only a single tract, are in them- 
selves inadequate to account for learning. Indeed, they may 
serve in all cases outside of mere chance associative connections 
only as secondary aids to learning. In the usual cases certain 
stimuli and their immediate effects continue for a time and 
operate synchronously with others so that the response is a 
resultant of these various circumstances. It may tentatively 
tend this way and that, but will complete itself in the way that 
is on the whole most consistent, when everything is taken into 


° Behavior, p. 257. ; i F 
7Peterson, Jos. Completeness of Response as an Explanation Principle in 
Learning. Psychol. Rev., 1916, 23, pp. 153-162. 


4 JOSEPH PETERSON 


consideration. The most complete response possible, in this 
sense,—the most consistent—has the advantage and will, other 
things equal, survive over others. The various tentative begin- 
nings of acts this way and that, moreover, are not to be regarded 
as separate acts: they may easily, at a later juncture, be re- 
solved into the ‘‘completest’’ act. Such conditions, it was 
maintained, must be taken into consideration to account for 
the selectiveness manifest in learning. This is a complex 
“principle ’’ both to state and to test out in experiment; but 
the organism and the behavior of an animal are inconceivably 
complex, and over-simplification for the sake of clearness of 
conception and of explanation is often a positive disadvantage 
to progress in the biological sciences. Numerous evidences of 
this statement might be given. 

The experiment reported in the following pages was planned 
in its main features when the article above referred to was 
written, and it is there suggested in the concluding paragraph. 
It was thought that varying the lengths of certain cul de sacs 
in identical mazes might show a difference in behavior not 
explicable on the basis of frequency, recency, and intensity of 
stimulation. If, for instance, a tendency to enter a short cul 
de sac is overcome with fewer errors in that particular case, 
or in fewer runs through the maze, than are required when the 
same cul de sac is lengthened somewhat, it would appear that 
some other explanation than that based on the principles named 
is necessary. On the basis of frequency and recency the animal 
would stand the same chance, on emerging from the blind alley, 
either of turning back-toward the entrance of the maze, on the 
one hand, or of going toward the food box, on the other, that 
it would with the blind alley longer. This would certainly be 
true if acts are the individual and disparate affairs in trial and 
error processes that they are usually assumed to be, each being 
complete as a rule before the next is begun. 

Watson says: “ This factor (frequency) alone is probably 
sufficient to account for the maze habit. Apparently it is 
difficult to obtain any explanation based upon other factors.’’s 

® Op. cit., pp. 267, 268. In a footnote he says: “If it happens by chance that 
any cul de sac is entered as frequently as any segment of the true pathway, it becomes 
as firmly fixed as the true segment.’”” I cannot understand what the warrant is 


for this statement. A careful tabulation of the detailed movements of some of my 
rats in the maze shows that it is altogether contrary to the actual facts. In records 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 5 


After pointing out, successfully to the writer’s mind, the difficulty 
in the way of Thorndike’s principle of ‘“‘ satisfiers,” he contends 
that there is no immediate connection backwards: between the 
obtaining of food and the elimination of errors. Watson attempts 
on the basis of the probability doctrine, suggested in another 
relation by Stevenson Smith, to show how frequency alone may 
suffice in the acquiring of maze habits. He argues that an 
animal, having started along the maze path A, has an equal 
chance on coming to a cul se sac X, all other factors equal, either 
of taking B, the true path beyond the blind alley, or of going 
into X; that on returning from X, in case of the wrong choice 
having been made, it again has an equal chance of taking B. 
It thus has a probability of 3/4 (or 1/2 + 1/2 of 1/2) of keeping 
the right path. 

If no other factor than frequency operates in such a case we 
should expect an animal to continue entering the cul de sacs 
indefinitely; for on turning back from any point toward the 
starting place in the maze the same law must apply. The 
chances are again 1/2 that any cul de sac passed will be entered, 
and 3/4 that the animal will continue in its general direction, 
now toward the starting point in the maze. In a maze with 
several blind alleys, each of which has a chance of 1/4 of turning 
any rat reaching it back toward the maze entrance, the proba- 
bility would be very slim that the animal would at the first 
trial reach the food. The returns would therefore tend to fix 
the habit of entering cul de sacs as strongly as that of going 
toward the food. Mere probability explains truly enough how 
the animal gets to the food each time, but that 1s net the problem 
of learning; it does not explain how it happens that on the whole 
the second trial is better than the first, the third better than 
the second, and so on. Frequency based on probability does 
not bring such a result: it fails utterly to explain learning, even 
in the simple case of the maze.» The real issue has been over- 
picked at random, instances occur in numerous places of violations of the principle 
stated. A detailed presentation of these instances will be reserved for a later 
article, as proper attention to them here would lead us too far away from the main 
purpose of the present paper. Instances are very frequent when the animal takes 
certain blind alleys entirely contrary to the expectations based upon either fre- 
quency or recency or of both combined. 

9 This statement is based on actual data of a supposed case of a rat in a maze 
of six cul de sacs whose “‘ choice’ at each bifurcation of the trail is determined by 


the flipping of a coin. After considerable data by this method has accumulated— 
after most any number of trials—it becomes very evident that if the frequency 


6 JOSEPH PETERSON 


looked as a rule. Watson does not try out his suggestion, or 
follow it far enough to get to the real difficulty. It is not easy, 
as Watson rightly admits,1° to see how the recency principle 
can help out the situation. No one has given more on this 
than the mere name. Both recency and frequency fail to explain 
learning as a gradual change in the way of doing something, 
involving the elimination of random acts. They do not show 
what controls an act, but only that if it is controlled, or directed, 
alike each successive trial it will become easier and more rapid in 
performamcee. 

On the other hand, if different ‘“‘ acts’ in a random trial and 

error process are only more or less tentative expressions of the 
one general act of getting food, for example, comparable to the 
out-reachings of the pseudopodia of the ameba, and if in all 
their changing forms these are related to the main performance 
by numerous in-going and out-going impulses, it would seem 
reasonable to suppose that errors of entering blind alleys would 
be overcome, other things equal, in something like a direct 
proportion to the length of the latter. This might be expected 
to hold within certain limits of length, at least. It is not at 
all implied in this view of learning, let it be clearly kept in mind, 
that any conscious states, whether or not they are present, are 
controlling or directing the animal. Indeed, it is just this view 
that we regard as unfruitful, and for which we are seeking a 
successful substitute. Instead of covering up the problem by 
assuming that the animal ‘perceives relations,’ or makes 
“practical judgments,’”’ or “‘ has ideas,’’ we are attempting to 
meet it squasely and to state schematically how the complexity 
of stimuli in the situation favoring learning can function so 
that the animal may “learn by results.’ There can be little 
question in fact that somehow the animal does learn by results. 
Our problem is to understand how and by what kind of results. 
Its solution would seem to have valuable bearings in the way 
of substituting for current erroneous “ social forces’’ factors 
(including ‘‘ pleasure and pain’’) used in explaining human 
conduct, in the absence of better conceptions. 
(or the recency, or both) of running through any unit of the maze be the determ- 
ining factor in subsequent choices the rat never would learn the maze. As the 
previous note states a similar tabulation of actual choices by an animal likewise 
shows the inadequacy of the principles in question. 

19 Op. cil., p. 269. The writer is working, however, with encouraging prospects 


upon a method of testing the influence of recency, and he is finding that influence 
much less potent than he had supposed. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 7 
THE EXPERIMENT 


The experiment was carried out in the University of Chicago 
during the months of July and August, 1916.1: Twenty-four 
white rats, ranging in ages from about five to six weeks at the 
time of the beginning of the experiment, were used. Of these, 
nine were males and fifteen females. These were at first divided 
into two main groups, the one consisting of. the fifteen females 
and one small male and the other of eight males. The first 
group began as untrained animals in the B-mazes, to be described, 
and the second in the A-mazes. They were ear-marked and 
grouped about eleven days before the experiment began, during 
which time they were habituated to handling, and were fed 
‘daily in the food box of the maze (in the separate groups) except 
a couple of days while the maze was out of the laboratory for 
remodeling. The food was bread soaked in milk, a definite 
quantity being given each day to insure uniformity of bodily 
conditions and of hunger. During the entire period of prepara- 
tion and experimentation not a single rat showed any signs of 
illness. The two main groups were again divided into control 
groups, as will be explained later in ‘‘ The General Schedule 
of Experiments.’’ These sub-groups were caged separately for 
convenience of experimentation, but they were fed together 
daily in the food box of the maze throughout the time of the 
experiment and were also interchanged daily in the cages, 1. e., 
each sub-group was on any given day put into the cage occupied 
by its control group during the previous twenty-four hours. 
The purpose of these interchanges was to prevent the develop- 
ment of group odors. 

Only one maze in the laboratory was available. This was, 

uJ desire to express my thanks here to Professors Angell and Carr for the privi- 
leges of the laboratory and for the animals used in the experiment. With the 
exception of aid from my brother, John C. Peterson, a graduate student in the 
University of Chicago, I am wholly responsible for the experiment, both as to prob- 
lem and method. My brother helped me plan the modifications of the maze avail- 
able, and to get started with the experiment, which help I gratefully acknowledge. 
We had planned to carry on the experiment together, but it was found after the 
second day of experimentation that one person could record all the movements 
satisfactorily and could secure greater uniformity in the conditions of the experi- 
ment than was possible to two. : : 

2 Through an oversight at the time of the segregation and ear-marking of the 
animals the small male, No. 10, was classed as a female.’ The error was noticed 
on the fourteenth day of the experiment, and after this time the rat was caged with 
the males, 5, 8, 1, and 7, but it continued to run the IB maze with the females. 
No difference from this change was noted in the behavior of the male or of any 


of the other animals. No. 10 did not continue with the females in any other maze, 
as will be seen in the schedule. 


8 JOSEPH PETERSON 


however, converted into two mazes as shown in figure I, by 
means of a rearrangement of the partitions. Both mazes have 
the same food box, and therefore only one of them can be used 
at any one time in experimentation. IB (figure I) is a maze 
with ten blind alleys, numbered from one to ten. The broken 
line from the entrance, E, indicates the correct path to the 
food. IA is another maze having but six cul de sacs, the entrance 
being at E’. This maze is shown in the figure in heavier outlines. 
Whenever the one maze was in use the entrance to the food 
box from the other was, of course, closed. The mazes were 
made of soft wood, and were stained black just before being 
used in the present experiment. The alleys were uniformly 
four by four inches in cross dimensions, and the partitions were 
approximately one-half inch thick, By means of a number of 
easily removable shutters, braced with triangular supports from 
behind, the cul de sacs could be shortened as desired for the 
purposes of the experiment. By this means each maze could 
be converted quickly into a maze of a slightly different type, 
having the same blind alleys but of relatively different lengths. 
These shutters were also of soft wood stained black and had 
the same cross dimensions as the alleys of the maze, so as to 
fit tightly. In the figure these shutters are indicated by dotted 
cross-lines in the blind alleys. Thus in Maze IB the blind 
alleys 2, 4, 6, and 9 are shortened so that the ten blind alleys 
together have a total length (about eleven feet) approximately 
equaling the ten in Maze IIB, of which 1, 3, 5, 7, 8 and 10 have 
been shortened as indicated. 

Maze IA differs from Maze IIA on another principle: all the 
cul de sacs in the first are of full length, as indicated, while the 
second has them all shortened. In IA the total length of the 
blind alleys is about eight feet, while in ITA it is about four feet. 

The mazes were supplied with glass covers, with wooden 
drop-shutters at the entrances and tin side-sliding shutters at 
the food box entrances. In the experiments each animal was 
first put into the food box and allowed to taste the food before 
the first run, or trial. This was not only to strengthen the 
incentive but also to insure uniformity in incentive and in 
handling of the animal in all trials. In presenting the animal 
to the entrance of the maze the experimenter was always seated, 
with the entrance slightly at his right. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 9 


The groupings of the animals made in the experimental pro- 
cedure are clearly and concisely shown in the general schedule 
following, arranged according to the mazes used. Thus in Maze 
IB the first rats used are those called Group Mu, the u indicating 
that the animals were untrained. The rats of Group St were 
trained, as indicated by the ¢; 2. e., they had learned another 
maze. Frequent reference to figure I, in connection with the 
study of this schedule, will make clear which maze was in use 
for any group in question, and the exact modification of the 
blind alleys. 


FIGURE I.—The fourfmazes used, IB, IIB, IA, and ITA. The heavy lines mark 
the division between the B- and the A-mazes. Dotted lines across blind 
alleys show position of the shutters. E and E’ are the entrances to the mazes. 


GENERAL SCHEDULE OF EXPERIMENTS 


Rats used Practice distributions 
Maze IB 
Mu Group....... 7 females (9, 11, 12, 13, 14, 16, 18) and 1 male (10). 


Two tests, or runs, daily for ten days, then four daily for 
three days, then three daily for each rat until eight runs out of 
ten were correct. (Rat 12 did not complete the habit in time 
available.) 

St Group........ 4 males (2, 3, 4, 6), trained on Maze ITA. 

Three runs daily for three days, then an intermission for six- 
teen days (see explanation on next page), then by the intensive 
method the rats were run Aug. 29th and 30th each three times 
at the following periods of day: 9-9:20, 9:40-9:55, 3-3:15, 3:30- 


10 JOSEPH PETERSON 


3:40, and 8:30-8:40,—total runs, twenty-four for each rat. All 
records were left incomplete, but all rats were equally practiced 
to the point of discontinuance. All animals were eager and 
active. 


Maze IIB 


Nu Group....... 8 females (15, 17, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24). . : 
Rat 20 was blind in left eye. Distribution of practices pre- 
cisely same as for Group Mu, in Maze IB, same days. 
Ri Group........ 4 males (1, 5, 7, 8) trained in IA. 
Distributions of practice same as for Sf, Maze IB, same days, 
Practice periods, Aug. 29th and 30th: 9:25-9:35, 9:55-10:10. 
3:15-3:30, 3:40-3:55, and 8:45-9. All records left incomplete; 
rats eager and active. 


Maze IA 
Ru Group....... 4 males (1, 5, 7, 8). 
Practice distributions same as for Mu, Maze IB. 
Nt, Group....... 4 females (15, 20, 21, 22), trained on Maze IIB. 


Three runs daily for each rat until habit was completed, eight 
runs of ten correct. 

Mt, Group....... 3 females (9, 13, 14), trained on Maze IB. 

By intensive method: the three animals were given three runs 
each, alternating with short periods of rest, during the forenoon 
of Aug. 28th. Rat 14 completed habit in twenty-eight runs, 
or trials; rat 9, in twenty-four runs; rat 13, in forty-one runs, 
eleven of which were made early the morning of the following 
day. All rats were eager and active, except 13 on the last run 
of first day, when it took sixteen seconds following two runs of 
two seconds each. 


Maze IIA 
Su Group. ...... 4 males (4, 6, 3, 2). 
Practice distributions same as for the Rw Group in Maze JA. 
Nf, Group.......4 females (17, 19, 23, 24), trained on Maze IIB. 
Practice distributions same as for Né, in Maze IA. 
Mf, Group....... 3 females (11, 16, 18), trained on Maze IB. 


Practice distributions same as for Mu, and alternating after 
each three trials with them. Each rat completed the habit, 
getting eight out of ten runs correct, in a total of twelve runs. 
All were very active and eager throughout. 


This schedule is given as actually carried out, not exactly 
as originally planned. It will be noted that the programs for 
the two B-mazes are precisely alike, and that the same is true 
of the A-mazes. This affords means of control of a number 
of factors which otherwise might favor one or the other of the 
control groups. Temperature conditions changed considerably; 
it was also necessary to modify occasionally, to suit the time 
at the disposal of the experimenter, the number of runs per 
day by each animal. At the early stages of the learning there 
was not enough time to give each animal more than two runs 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 11 


daily. Later four runs daily were tried, but the eagerness of 
the animals seemed’ in one or two cases to diminish in the last 
‘run. Three runs a day proved to be very satisfactory. It was 
originally supposed that each rat could learn both one of the 
B- and one of the A-mazes during the time available for the 
experiment—July 18th to August 30th—but a difficulty arose, 
which had been underestimated in the planning. When the 
male rats had finished their more simple problems—the A- 
mazves—and were started on the B-maze problems, signs of 
trailing the females appeared. To prevent this possibility the 
male and the female groups of animals had been made to occupy 
the same cages alternately in successive days. It was imprac- 
ticable to wash the maze thoroughly before each experiment 
for each group. The first day that the St and the Rt male 
rats were run in the B-mazes, after the runs of the females, 
there was no difficulty. On the second and the third day, 
however, there seemed to be evidences of trailing and of excite- 
ment, and some of the rats deposited urine drops in the maze 
from the second to the fifth blind alleys. This seemed to 
influence, as a guide, later members of the same groups (i. ¢., 

also males), and to stimulate them to make similar deposits 
along the trail. Thorough washing of the entire maze with 
soap water and Creolin-Pearson, a disinfectant, did not change 
the behavior materially. Consequently, after the third day the 
practices of these males were discontinued for sixteen days, 
until the females had completed their problem. This experience 
with the males seemed in only one (questionable) case to 
influence in the least the runs of the females whose habits had 
been already reduced to the stage of proprioceptive control. 
The mazes, moreover, had. been carefully washed after the 
second and the third day of the experience with the males 
already described. 

The postponement of the experiment with the males in the 
B-mazes made it necessary to run them by the intensive method 
described in the schedule, if at all. It was found that if each 
rat was given three runs and then put back into the cage without 
feed it could again be run soon after with no loss of eagerness. 
In fact, the method worked surprisingly well. The fact that 
the records had nevertheless to be left incomplete on this maze 
so far as these rats were concerned does not affect the data so 


12 JOSEPH PETERSON 


far as they go, as the two comparable groups had identical 
experiences. 

Since the A-mazes were cleared earlier than the B-mazes, it 
was possible to put into them, as indicated in the schedule, 
some of the females—four on each A-maze—which first com- 
pleted their original problem.* Finally,—leaving out the female 
12, which did not complete its original problem until the maze 
was taken over for the males, and the small male 10, which 
had been running with the female group Mu—three females 
were practiced on each of the A-mazes by the intensive method. 
The results of these two groups are, for obvious reasons, strictly 
comparable only to the twelfth trial inclusive, when the rats 
in the IIA maze had completed their problem. 

All comparable, or control groups were then run on the same 
days, the same number of times, and as nearly as possible the 
same time of day. Moreover, to give no possible advantage 
of trailing to either group—and aside from the cases noted, 
not between control groups, no such behavior was observed—the 
group which was practiced first one day was second the next. 

Both time and error results were noted. The experimenter 
devised a system of short signs with which to record the com- 
plete gross behavior of each animal. Returns in the maze were 
noted as accurately as possible; only minor ones not reaching 
cul de sacs or corners of the various maze alleys before being 
corrected were left out of the records. Entrances into the blind 
alleys were all classified by means of appropriate signs, into 
three classes,—complete entrances, entrances about half way in, 
and beginning entrances bringing the animal’s head and fore 
part into the blind alley while the hind feet remained in the 
true path. In the table of results these entrances constituting 
the last class are in the column headed “ Start.” It was also 
noted, as the animal emerged from the cul de sac, whether it 
continued forward toward the food or turned back toward the 
place of beginning. Hesitancies were also noted. Of these a 
peculiar and amusing kind was frequent. Occasionally, an 

1% To be sure that the two groups were of approximately equal ability the ani- 
mals composing them were selected as follows: The first, third, fifth, and seventh 
rats that completed Maze IIB were selected for Maze IA, with the long cul de sacs: 
and the second, fourth, sixth, and eighth were taken for Maze IIA. In case of 
any slight difference in the groups this would put the better animals into the more 


difficult maze, so that the better results expected for IIA could not be due to 
superior animals. : 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 13 


animal would stop quickly at the entrance to a blind alley while 
the head would vibrate very rapidly between the direction of 
the true path and that of the tempting by-way. The record 
here and there shows, for instance, that an animal would stop 
at cul de sac 1, after having nearly inhibited the tendency to 
enter this blind alley, and make, say, three double vibrations 
(3 v. d.) with the head. This behavior is very suggestive and 
will be considered later. On the whole it was found that the 
full description of the animal’s behavior was much more valuable 
for the present problem than the mere recording of time and 
errors. Time records were, however, also kept. 


14 JOSEPH PETERSON 


TABLE I 
GENERAL SUMMARY OF RESULTS IN THE B-MAZES 
Blind alley....... | First | Second 
Degree...........| Compl. | Half | Start. | Compl. | Half Start. 
Direction......... E R FE R FE R FIE R F| E R FE R F 
Runs Group 
1 2 Mu IB }19 10 92 0 212 4 883 19 149 3 66 3 8 
Si IB|6 3 3 1 14 4 10; 1 1 
Nu IIB |18 9 9 3 3}5 3 228 19 910 4 61 1 
Ri IIB | 4 4) 1 TG 17 3 41 1} 1 1 
| 
1- 5 Mu IB (37 15 22) 4 415 5 1044 23 21/17 6 11/11 4 7 
St IB |12 4 8 2 1 #4216 #4 #1271 ~ «21 4 1 8 
Nu IIB /27 15 1210 2 8 7 3 £4141 27 14/18 4 14] 2 2 
Ri IIB|8 3 56 666 1 59 4 4) 3 311 1 
6- 15 Mu 70 16 54455 2 3/2 2778 #5 3/5 5]13 13 
Si 30,0 4 S26) 4 4| 7 7;/5 1 4/2 2| 8 8 
Nu 25 5 2026 2 24/21 2116 2 410 2 95 1 4 
Ri 4 446 1 5380 3 2715 3 2/3 1 216 6 
16- 25 Mu 56 2 54/18 2 1614 45 2 3:5 1 46 6 
Si 12 12)15 15| 7 7/1 Ted a BS 5 
Nu 9 1. 8/10 10/22 1 21;8 1 7 3 311 1 10 
Rt 3 2 13 1 27 «1 «67 6 110 3 73 3 
26- 35 Mu 38 4 34/21 21| 7 7| 3 3] 7 7 4 4 
Nu 4 4) 4 4/10 10) 5 510 1 910 1 9 
36- 45 Mu 40 1 3917 1 167 7\ 2 1 1; 3 3) 4 4 
Nu 1 1| 2 2| 8 8} t 1 7 7|16 16 
46-55 Mu 30 30/24 21) 9 95 1 4/2 21 3 3 
: Nu 1 1) 4 4| 6 6, 2 2) 5 5| 7 7 
56- 65 Mu 19 19/19 19/12 12) 3 3] 2 2| 5 5 
Nu 1 1,1 1 1 114 4 
66- 75 Mu 21 2m}9 1 385 5| 2 2| 2 34 1 8 
Nu 1 lj 1 1} 1 1 
76- 85 Mu 9 9\ 9 97 7| 2 2) 2 2| 4 4 
Nu | 1 al 
86- 95 Mu 4 444 1 3 7 fi 
Nu 
96-105 Mu 3 3) 4 4 2 2 
Nu 
106-115 Mu 1 1| 3 3) 2 2) 1 1] 2 2 
Nu 
116-125 Mu 1 U2 
Nu . 
Totals IB....... 382 46 336/153 7 146) 86 6 80|107 37 70156 9 47/71 6 65 
Totals IIB....... 82 26 561 72 6 66/120 9 111] 85 41 44/71 10 61165 3 62 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IB, 621 234. 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IIB, 275 221 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 15 


TABLE I—Continued 
GENERAL SUMMARY OF RESULTS IN THE B-MazeEs 


Blind alley....... Third Fourth 
Degree........... Compl. | Half Start. Comp). Half Start. 
Direction......... E R FIE R FE R FE R FIE R FE R F 
Runs Group 
1- 2 Mu IB }10 4 65 O 510 3 7/23 8 bbl 4 3 #=1/2 vA 
St IB|6 2 4 5 5.5 8 Be 2B & 2 5 
Nu IIB }10 2 8/3 2 1/4 2 20 3 73 3) 1 1 
Rt IIB 3 1 24 2 24 4 
1- 5 Mu IB /|16 5 11) 6 612 3 930 10 20:6 4 24 1 8 
St IB|7 2 5 6 668 3 52 2 Gt 
Nu IIB 13 3 10/6 3 3/9 2 715 5 10) 5 5| 2 2 
Ri IIB 4 1 34 2 26 1 5 
6- 15 Mu 2 2 2 271 #1 A 
St 2 1 Va 1 2 2 
Nu 1 1 3 3}2 1 1 
Rt 2 1 2 1 15 3 2 2 2 
16- 25 Mu 2 1 #11 1 Law 2 2) 1 1 
St af 1 
Nu 1 1 2 1 15 5| 2 2 
Ri Be 1a 1.2 2 2 
26- 35 Mu 1 yj1 1 3 2 Qi yi ol 2 2 
Nu 1 11 1 3 1 2 
36- 45 Mu 1 1 
Nu 2 2 1 1 2 2 
46- 55 Mu 5 3 2 So I 22 4 4 
Nu de | 1 1 1 1 
56- 65 Mu 1 1 3 3 1 1 
Nu 1 1 1 1 
66- 75 Mu 1 1 1 1 
Nu 1 yi 1 
76 -85 Mu 1 1 2 TOF 2 2 
Nu 
86- 95 Mu 2 2 2 J, J 
Nu 
96-105 Mz - 1 1 
Nu 
106-115 Maz 
Nu 
116-125 Mu 
Nu 
Totals IB....... 40 14 2610 2 823 5 18/54 20 34115 9 618 3 15 
Totals IIB.....: /.|17 4 138}6 3 3/21 5 16/33 13 20/20 2 18/16 2 14 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IB, 73 86 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IIB, 44 69 


2 


16 JOSEPH PETERSON 


TABLE I—Continued 
GENERAL SUMMARY OF RESULTS IN THE B-MAZES 


Blind alley....... Fifth Sixth 
Degree........... Compl. Half Start. Compl. Half Start. 
Direction......... E R FE R FE R FRE R FE R FE R F 
Runs Group ; 
1- 2 Mz IB {30 18 12) 1 119 4 57 3 4 1 1] 
St IB |j12 5 7 5 3 21 1 1 1 
Nw IIB j21 8 13) 1 1}4 2 2:5 5}4 2 2:4 1 3 
Ri WIB/}8 6 2 4 1 3/1 1 
1- 5 Mu IB /46 27 19 1 113 «5 = #«68)10 5 51 12 1 I 
Si IB |19 6 13 5 3 22 2) 1 lj 1 1 
Nu IIB [31 10 21/1 16 3 39 2 75 2 37 2 «5 
Ri WB/8 6 21 14 1 3)2 2) 1 1) 1 1 
6- 15 Mu 5 2 3 5 5} 4 1 3/4 4 
Si 6 2 45 1 43 3} 1 1 2 2 
Nu 3 2 1 4 1 3/2 2| 3 3| 6 6 
Rt A 2. 21:2 2 1 1 
16- 25 ie 3 2 1 1 1) 1 1 3 3 
sf 
Nu 1 1 1 1 2 2 
Ri 2 1 1 1 1j1 1 1 1 
26- 35 Mu 7 & @ 1 1 
Nu Zz 2 i 
36- 45 Mu 2 1 1 1 1 
Nu 11 1 11 1 
46- 55 Mu 4 2 2 1 1 1 1 
Nu 1 A 1 1) 1 1 
56- 65 Mu 3 3 1 oil 
Nu 1 1 
66- 75 Mu 1 172 1 1 
Nu 1 1 
76- 85 Mu 2 1 Wi 1| 2 2) 1 1 1 1 
Nu 
86- 95 Mu 2 1 1 1 1 
Nu 
96-105 Mu 
Nu 
106-115 Mu 1 1 
Nu 
116-125 Mu 
Nu 
Totals IB....... 101 49 52;9 2 7/25 9 1622 6 16/7 1 615 1 14 
Totals IIB....... 53 22 3117 1 618 5 13/16 3 13/12 2 10115 2 13 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IB, 135 44 
Total number of entrances into cui de sacs, IIB, 78 43 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 17 


TABLE I—Continued 
GENERAL SUMMARY OF RESULTS IN THE B-MAZES 


Blind alley....... Seventh 


Eighth 


DORTOB es irecnestens Adare Compl. Half Start. 


Compl. 


Half 


Start. 


Direction......... E R RE R FE R 


E R F 


E R 


Runs Group 
1- 2 Mu IB 


oy 
J 
CO mt bo 


to 


et het 


13 7 
3 
14 232 1 
3 


Nm 


19 
9 
18 
5 


RHO 
—_— 


NNR DS bho et 


NNr LO 


ie) 
Y 
— 
ies] 
Bmw Oo | OMNYW | HOR me 
ioe 
Aye] wowN |] He 
A Al Hw i 
A alHew 


wl] ARO !]WmwO 


to 
bo] mt eH 


—_ 
—_ 


SD RRNN] WWNW 


16- 25 Mu 


rs 
ES 
w 
w 


SED NN] WHEN d 


me | TOM 
-— 


fet et 


po 
— 


26- 35 Mu 


— bn 
me 


= 
ee 


36- 45 Mu 2 2 


Pw wo» - 


Pw Oe Lael 


mH 


46- 55 Mu 1 1 1 1 


56- 65 Mu 


66- 75 Mu 


76- 85 Mu 


86- 95 Mu 


96-105 Mu 


106-115 Mu 


116-125 Mu 


Totals IB....... 22 3 «19)13 13/23 
Totals IIB....... 15 1 14/11 11]13 


1 
3 


22 
10 


42 11 31 
26 4 22 


g100 


Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IB, 
Total number of entrances into czl de sacs, IIB, 


58 


39 


18 JOSEPH PETERSON 


TABLE I—Continued 
GENERAL SUMMARY OF RESULTS IN THE B-MAZES 


Blind alley....... Ninth Tenth 


Degree........... Compl. Half Start. | Compl. | Half | Start. 


Direction......... E R FE R F|E R FIE R FIE R FIE R 


F 


_ 
NPRwWN 


_ 
am 


Z 
ay 
oS 
= 


i 
Obwolwomuw 


Ww 

a 

= 

om! 

ee) 
OR RO] WON 
= bh 
ll ee ee 


et ee | et 


w Bw N ale) 
w 
nH 
PPT NOHO | NNW 


mee | WNTOTCT 


Z 
~ 
a 
mt 


_ 
> 
o 
uo 
s 

i 

i 

_ 

= 


96-105 Mu 


106-115 Mz 


116-125 Mu 


Totals IB....... 15 1 14) 6 6/10 10/27) 3 24) 4 
Totals IIB....... 21 26 1 52 1 1182 4 8/5 


ou 
a 
~ 


Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IB, 31 
Total numher of entrances into cz! de sacs, IIB, 39 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 19 


TABLE II 
GENERAL SUMMARY OF RESULTS IN THE A-MAzES 
Blind alley....... First Second 
Degree of entrance] Compl. Half Start. Compl. Half Start. 
Direction......... E E E E R FE R FE R F 
Runs Group 
1- 2 Ru 15 3 6 3 2 11 1 14°45 9 
TA, Ni 10 1 2 5 SS. De dhs oh. 2 
Mt, 1 1 1 1 1 1 
Su alg 2 2 4 3 12 2 1 1 
IIA, Nt, 5 Z 2 2) 1 11 1 
Mt 2 1 1 1} 2 2 
1- 5 Ru 22 4 9 10 3 73 2 118 5 13 
Ni 13 4 4 8 2 64 2 233 1 2 
Mi 3 1 1 9 6 31 i 
Su 30 3 2 10 6 44 3 #25 5 
Netz 8 4 2 2) 2 2| 3 3 
Mt, 3 3 ak yl 1 
6- 15 Ru 14 6 2 134 94 #1 3/13 13 
Nt 3 2 2 2 1 13 1 24 1 3 
Mt, 5 2 1 10 6 43 3| 2 2 
Su 2 4 3 2 1 14 4! 3 3 
Nin 2 1 2 2 1 2 
Mt 1 
16- 25 Ra 4 4 4 1 1; 3 3 
Nu 1 1 i) io Se, ia, et 
Mi, 1 4 1 35 5 
Su 2 6 6 
Nit 
Mt 
26- 35 Ru 3 4 1 1 1 
Ni 1 1 
Mt 1 1 
Su 8 1 ej at 4 4 
36-45 Ru 2 1 1 
Su 
46- 55 Ru 1 1 
Su 
56- 65 Ru 
Su 
Totals IA....... 68 29 20 59 28 31/27 9 18/51 8 43 
Totals ITA....... 45 20 9. 15 8 711 3 824 1 23 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IA, 117 137 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IIA, 74 50 


20 


JOSEPH PETERSON 
TABLE Il—Continued 


GENERAL SUMMARY OF RESULTS IN THE A-MAZES 


Blind alley....... Third Fourth 
Degree of entrance! Compl. Half Start. Compl. Half Start. 
Direction......... E R FE R FE R FE R FE R FE R F 
Runs Group 
1- 2 Ru | 4 1 3 6 3 312 8 4 2 2 7 § 2 
TAJN | 3 1 2 2 1 #115 9 6 
Mi}1 1 1 1) 1 14 1 3 
Su | 2 2 3 1 2 
IIASNé | 1 1 1 WS 2 32 dT 32 2 
Mr | 1 1 a} 1 1 1 
1-5 Ru |6 1 5 7 3 #414 9 553 5 11 6 5 
Nh |}6 2 4 3 1 218 9 93 1 23 3 
Mi }8 6 22 2); 2 2110 1 91 #1 1 1 
Su |7 3 4 2 212 6 6/3 1 2/5 1 4 
Nb | 2 2) 1 1} 3 3}5 2 3/2 1 1/2 2 
Miz | 1 1 1 1 2 2 
6-15 Ru 15 6 95 5) 4 420 11 93 1 #22 2 
Ni | 7 2 5/5 5) 4 4.6 1 51 1) 6 6 
Mi 10 3 7 2 2) 1 17 2 51 1] 6 6 
Su |6 3 3/6 6 1 U8 3 54 4] 8 8 
Ni | 1 1 5 5) 1 11 14 1 38 
Mi | 1 1 
16-25 Ru /}/5 1 45 5] 6 67 1 62 1 1/6 6 
Na |3 2 1 1 1 6 6 
Mi/1 1 6 664 1 3/1 1 1 1 1 1 
Su | 1 175 1 = 4{10 10 2 2| 4 4 
Nie 2 2 1 1 
Mi, 
26-35 Ru 3 4 4| 4 441 1 3 3 
Nt 
Mi, 2 2}2 1 1 1 1 
Sue |5 3 21 1] 8 8) 1 1| 4 4| 6 6 
36-45 Ru {3 1 2,4 4\ 4 4 1 1) 1 1 
Su at 1j1 1 8 8 
46-55 Ru 1 1) 1 1] 3 3) 1 1} 1 1 
Su | 1 1 1.) a 3 3 
56-65 Ru 
Su 1 1 
Totals IA....... 64 25 39/35 35/43 6 37/90 35 55/21 11 10147 6 41 
Totals IIA....... 25 10 15|14 1 13/33 33/28 12.1618 2 1642 2 40 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IA, 142 158 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, ILA, 72 88 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 21 


TABLE II—Continued 


GENERAL SUMMARY OF RESULTS IN THE A-MAZES 


Blind alley....... Fifth 
Degree of entrance! Compl. Half Start. — Start. 
Direction......... E R FE R FE R lin R F 
Runs Group 
1- 2 Ru 4 3 
TA, Ni, 6 3 3 
Mi, 1 1 
Su | 2 2| 1 1) a 2 2 
ITA, Nin 2 2 2 2 1 1 
Mi 1 1 
1-5 Ru 10 0| 1 1 4 
Nt 9 7|4 4 
Mt, 1 1 
Su | 3 3) 6 6| 2 2) 8 8 
Ni a 2, 2 2 1 1 
Miz 1 1 
6-15 Ru | 3 3) 3 3) 4 4 1 
Nt, | 1 1 1 
Mi, 
Su | 4 442 1 1/2 
Nt | 1 1 
Mt 
16-25 Ru 1 11 
Ni 
Mh, 
Su 
Ni 
Mt 
26-35 Ru | 1 1 1 
Nt 
Mt 
Su 
36-45 Ru 1 
Su 
46-55 Ru | 1 1 
Su {1 1 1 1 
56-65 Ru 
Su 
Totals IA....... 6 6) 4 4|26 6 6 
Totals ITA....... 9 910 1 95 3 3 
Total number of entrances into cul de sacs, IA, 18 
Total number of entrances into cz de sacs, IIA, 17 


22 JOSEPH PETERSON 
RESULTS 


Tables I and II give in a condensed form the main results 
of the entire experiment. In the separate larger divisions are 
given the reactions to the several blind alleys. These reactions 
are classified in a manner most easily made clear by taking 
up a concrete case. In table I the words “ First,” ‘“‘ Second,” 
etc. at the top stand for the blind alleys of the B-mazes of the 
corresponding numbers. The results of the first blind alley, for 
example, are then divided into three parts, ‘‘ Complete,”’ ‘‘ Half,”’ 
and ‘ Start,” meant to designate the degree of entrance by 
the rats into the blind alleys, as already explained. Complete 
entrance means going entirely to the end of the blind alley, 
or so near the end that the animal might reach the end by means 
of the vibrissae. Frequently the rats ran against the end with 
considerable force. Half entrance means approximately half 
way, or all entrances between complete and beginning. Those 
marked ‘‘ Start ’’ include cases in which the animal either just 
put the head in or entered with the fore half of the body. In 
such cases the hind feet of the animal usually remained in the 
true path, so that the general orientation was not completely 
given up as in the other two cases. The three columns coming 
again under each of these rubrics show respectively, the number 
of entrances into the blind alley in question, E, the number 
of returns toward the place of starting in the maze on the rats’ 
emerging from the blind alley, R, and the number of times the 
animals kept the general orientation, 7. e., continued toward 
the food box, F. The totals for R and F must therefore equal 
the number under E. 

The figures in the left column of the table indicate the number 
of the run, or of the test, of the animals, while the letters Mu, 
St, etc. stand for the group, as Group M untrained, Group S 
trained, and so on. The description of each group and of its 
practice distributions are given in detail in the schedule, pages 9 
and 10,.to which frequent reference is advisable. Now, to illus- 
trate in a concrete case, in the first line of the data, giving results 
of the first two trials of the animals, we find that Group Mu 
(eight rats, untrained, running in Maze IB) made nineteen 
complete entrances into the blind alley No. 1, with ten returns 
and nine cases in which the rat continued forward toward the 
food box. There were two entrances half way, with two forward 
tuns and no returns; thirteen beginning entrances, with four 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 23 


returns and eight cases of the animal keeping its general forward 
orientation; and so on, through the results for all the blind 
alleys in order. 

Note that the figures for groups Mu and St are in bold face 
for blind alley 1, and not for 2, and that these relations are just 
reversed for the groups of animals running in Maze JIB. The 
bold face designates full length cul de sacs, and the figures not in 
bold face indicate that the blind alley was shortened. The amount 
of shortening in any case is shown in figure I, as already explained. 
Careful attention to all these matters will greatly aid the reader 
in getting quickly and conveniently the general results of 
numerous reactions. Without such attention the tables are 
meaningless. The results cannot so well be effectively and 
accurately shown in graphs. 

The totals at the foot of the columns must not be taken too 
seriously, as will be evident in subsequent discussion. These 
are totals only of changing comparative quantities. For this 
reason the results of the experiments have been classified for 
different periods of the training. The results of the first two 
trials are given separately—and are not added in the totals 
because they are again included in the data for the 1st to Sth 
trials—as they are least affected by the animals progressive 
training. They show us approximately whether mere chance, 
or probability laws, can explain the direction that an animal 
beginning in the maze takes on emerging from a cul de sac, 
whether it returns or continues forward keeping its general 
orientation,—not accurately, however, for learning begins from 
the very first experiences in the maze. The progression of the 
learning in the case of each particular cul de sac is shown by a 
gradual decrease in entrances in the summaries of the Ist to 
5th, the 6th to 15th, 16th to 25th, etc. trials; also by the gradual 
decrease in returns and the increase, correspondingly, in the 
number of cases of keeping the general forward orientation. 
These two kinds of changes are very interesting and illuminating 
toward showing, in a manner not hitherto done with data on 
learning, just how the cul de sacs are eventually eliminated." 
This is our main concern in this paper. 


“4 Professor Carr has pointed out that the extent of entrance to cul de_sacs grad- 
ually decreases, as well as the number of entrances. (Hicks, V. C., and Carr, H. A. 
Human Reactions in a Maze. Jour. Animal Behav., 1912, 2, 98-125. See par- 
ticularly page 116.) 


24 JOSEPH PETERSON 


Three important features of the results are to be noted. The 
first is the rapid decrease in the proportion of the returns to 
forward runs, on the rat’s emergence from blind alleys. With 
the exception of blind alleys 2 and 5 in the B-mazes few such 
returns were made after the 15th trial, though the animals 
continued to enter some of the blind alleys beyond the 75th, 
some even beyond the 100th. These cul de sacs, noted as 


AVERAGE PER TRIAL PERIOD 8p 


4-5 6-15 16-25 26-35 46-55 66-75 
TRIAL PERIODS 


as 
7 a 

“ts. 

‘ 1 A 1 : t 
1-5 6-15 16-25 36-45 56-65 76-85 

TRIAL PERIODS : 


L 
96-105 116-125 


FicuRES II AND III.—CE is the curve of decrease in average number of complete 
entrances per trial to Blind Alley 1 full length, Maze IB; R indicates the 
decrease in returns, and 2R twice the returns, from this cul de sac. C’E’, R’ 
and 2R’ show corresponding data for Blind Alley 1 shortened, Maze IIB. 
Eight untrained rats in each case. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 25 


exceptions, have directions such as to favor returns in the case 
of a rat emerging from them. This more rapid decrease in 
returns than in entrances to cul de sacs is least complicated, 
and also shown most emphatically, in the case of the complete 
entrances to cul de sac 1, which is encountered before the rat 
could be confused by running into any other blind alleys. Figure 
II shows the matter graphically. Curve CE represents the 
number of complete entrances of eight untrained rats to the 
first blind alley at full length, as in Maze IB;. curve R, the 
returns; and 2R, twice the returns. 2R is a better curve for 
comparison with CE because originally, 1. ¢. before an animal 
is at all practiced, about half of the entrances are followed by 
returns; twice the returns, therefore, gives a number initially 
about equal to the total number of entrances. Figure III gives 
corresponding curves, C’E’, and 2R’, respectively, for the same 
number (eight) of untrained rats in cul de sac 1, shortened from 
22 inches to 8.5 inches. Here the same result is evident: while 
the elimination of entrances is far more rapid than in the case 
of the longer blind alley, the returns are still more rapidly reduced 
as shown by the 2R’ curve. 

It may also be noted here under our first point that the 
returns in both the B-mazes persisted longer in the cases of the 
blind alleys farther from the food box than of those near it. 
That is, returns from blind alleys first encountered were less 
easily eliminated, as were also entrances, than from those further 
along the true path. This is true even in cases of blind alleys 
nearer the food box that were comparatively long, as 7 and 8, 
even though, as in the case of 8, the direction of movement 
in emerging from the cul de sac favored returns. It is barely 
possible that an odor factor may have entered in case of 8. 
The mazes IA and IIA are not so well adapted to show these 
relationships, as there are fewer blind alleys of various individual 
differences of complexity, but the same conclusions as those 
given for the B-mazes may also be made for them. 

A second important point to note is, that the nature of the 
response to a blind alley gradually changes with practice, as 
well as the relative number of entrances into it. This change 
in the nature of the response is more marked in longer than in 
shorter blind alleys, particularly in those whose elimination was 
most difficult. It is illustrated best in the data from cul de sac 


26 JOSEPH PETERSON 


1 of Maze IB. Many of the entrances to 2, as the observation 
of the animals in their responses and also their individual records 
showed, are clearly due to confusions resulting from entrances 
to 1. The experimental notes supply many evidences. As a 
rule the rat in the early stages of response to such a blind alley 
runs rapidly into it the entire distance, usually coming into 


AVERAGE PER 
TRIAL PERIOD 


1 J FT = 
1-8 98-15 16-25 56-45 56-85 76-85 
TRIAL PERIODS 


: i 1 
36-45 66-65 76-85 96-105 116-125 


TRIAL PERIODS 


a 1 i 
1-5 6-15 16-285 


FIGURES IV AND V.—C shows decrease in average number of’ complete entrances 
per trial to Blind Alley 1 full length in the B-mazes; H arnt for half-way 
entrances, and S for beginning entrances. C’, H’, and S’ show corresponding 
results for the blind alley shortened. Eight untrained rats in each case. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 27 


contact one way or another with the end; but with succeeding 
trials the entrance is less and less complete, until finally the 
impulse to enter is wholly inhibited. Thus in the records of 
responses of two groups each of eight untrained rats to the first 
blind alley in the B-mazes (table I) the large numbers in the 
E-columns shift gradually from the ‘“‘ Complete’’ through the 
“Half” to the “ Start’? column. This shift is graphically 
shown in figure IV and figure V for first blind alley of mazes 
IB and IIB, respectively. C and C’ are the curves representing 
the rate of elimination of complete entrances, H and H’ of half 
entrances, and S and S’ of beginning entrances. Note that 
while the C-curves fall rapidly from the first, especially the 
one (C’) from the shortened cul de sac, there is a decided rise 
in the H- and the S-curves. Specifically, in the case of Maze 
IB (the cul de sac long), C falls gradually, with two minor excep- 
tions, all the way at a nearly uniform rate; H rises almost 
uniformly to the 35th trial, then it keeps almost a uniform 
height to the 65th trial, and finally gradually declines; and S, 
after a rapid initial decline, gradually rises again until the 65th 
trial is reached, when it gradually declines and reaches zero 
before the other two curves. In the case of Maze IIB (cul de 
sac shortened) the same relationship between these respective 
curves is shown, though all these curves drop earlier in the 
process than with the longer blind alley, except that in this 
case the S’ curve holds out longer than either of the other curves. 
A cursory examination of the data for other blind alley records 
shows that this type of transition from complete to only partial 
entrance and then to final elimination is a general feature of the 
results for the different groups of animals in the various mazes. 
A few exceptions only, in cases of very short cul de sacs, are 
noticeable. This is a phenomenon of learning in the maze to 
which little attention has previously been given, and which 
seems to the writer to be inexplicable on the basis of mere fre- 
quency and recency-laws. Several impulses working together, 
some facilitating others inhibiting one another, gradually result 
in the survival of the most consistent, or complete acts. No 
hesitancies in the rats’ behavior in these cases were present, 
such as might be secured from persons in similar circumstances. 
The rats evidently did not have time, nor adequate sense organs 
and conscious memories as a person would have, to recognize 


28 JOSEPH PETERSON 


and take note of external stimuli, but resembled automatic 
machines in the quickness and uniformity of their responses. 

This change appears more significantly in the results of most 
of the individuals than in those of all averaged. Here are some 
examples. Rat 18, of Group Mu made entrances to the first 
blind alley (full length) in this order: 12c (complete), th (half), 
1c, th, 5c, 1s (started), 4c, th, 2c, th, 5c, ih, 4c, th, Ic, 4h, 
2c, 3h, 3s, 1c, lh (total 55 entrances). Rat 10’s record, same 
group, is Is, 1c, 1h, 9c, 1h, 7c, 2h, 3c, th, 1c, 2h, 1s, 2c, 1s, 2c, 
th, 1s, 1c, lh, 2c, 1h, 1c, 1h, 1c, 1h, 1c, 3h, 1s, 1c; the next time 
on passing this cul de sac there was a momentary pause with 
three very rapid in and forward vibrations of the head, causing 
a confusion in which the animal made eleven errors in the other 
nine cul de sacs none of which it had entered, with but one 
exception, for twelve trials; then 1s, 2c, 1s, 2c (after another 
such vibrating pause before the cul de sac), 2s, 1c, th, 1s, 2c, 
lh, 1c, th, 1c, 1s (total 70 entrances). These results are typical. 
In numerous cases when the habit of avoiding the cul de sac 
was nearly complete, so that the animal usually made the ‘“‘s” 
type of entrance, the peculiar rapid vibration of the head noted 
above took place. The pause was, however, but for an instant. 
This response seems to indicate that the impulse to go forward 
at the critical place is still partly checked or impeded by one 
to enter, not quite eliminated. It is important to note, more- 
over, that when finally the rat does succeed in passing the cul 
de sac, even when this hesitant, vibrating behavior does not 
take place, it very frequently runs headlong into some neighbor- 
ing cul de sac which had long since been inhibited, and thereby 
gets considerably confused. Frequently, after such an experience 
it makes a complete entrance into the cul de sac in question the 
next trial, just as a child “ speaking a piece’ must bow again 
and start over when she goes wrong. This is one reason why 
a few complete entrances continue to occur. More than once 
an animal which had successfully passed cul de sac 1 for several 
trials would, without any hesitancy, run into it with great speed 
and against the closed end with terrific impact. In one such 
case the animal’s whole maze habit, just on the finishing stage, 
seemed to have been temporarily jolted wholly out of gear, its 
next trial being much like that of a beginner. All this makes 
it very plain that maze habits are not to be explained on the 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 29 


basis of individual, disparate “‘ acts,” following in their occurrence 
some law of chance. On the contrary, the various impulses 
in the random activity of the early trials are gradually and 
collectively woven into one matrix of successive responses, each 
setting off the next succeeding one, and all shaped by the whole 


\s ~%R MD 
in tom am 


SE 
Pm woh 
wey 


o 6 8 /0 
EYS t 


T 

J 

| 

MTs Bes BO a0 

/ 20 3 4 7 a 

BLIND ALL 

FIGURE VI.—Heavy columns, double lines, single lines, and discontinuous lines 
show, respectively, total entrances by all animals to full length and shortened 
blind alleys in the B-mazes, and to full length and shortened blind alleys in 
the A-mazes. Figures above columns give the totals represented. 


30 JOSEPH PETERSON 


circumstance of the maze environment.'® This seems to imply 
that the effect of one stimulus holds over into and conditions 
effects of later stimuli. 

The third point to note in our results is that when any given 
cul de sac is shortened it is eliminated more readily than when 
left at full length. That is to say, other things equal, and 
within certain limits, a long cul de sac is eliminated less readily 
than a short one. This statement is amply borne out in our 
data both from the A- and the B-mazes. The general results 
of all our experiments are shown roughly in the accompanying 
diagram, figure VI, representing the total number of entrances 
to each of the blind alleys in the various mazes. The heavy 
black columns and the double lines represent the totals for 
the full length and the shortened blind alleys, respectively, in 
the B-mazes; the single continuous and the broken lines stand 
for the corresponding totals for the A-maze blind alleys. In 
the B-mazes the total entrances to the full length cul de sacs 
is 1311, while the total number of entrances to the same cul 
de sacs when shortened, by an equal number of animals under 
the same conditions, is 929, a decrease of 29%. This decrease 
would doubtless be considerably greater but for the fact that 
confusions by the long blind alleys resulted in random behavior 
which increased the totals for the shortened cul de sacs. For 
instance, table I shows that more entrances were made into 2 
short than into 2 full length. This was very clearly due to 
the fact that as long as the habit to avoid 1 was incomplete 
the animals in the confusion also entered 2. It will be recalled 
that in the A-mazes the cul de sacs were all full length in the 
one and all shortened to about half their length in the other case. 
Here we do not have the confusion noted in the B-mazes. The 
shortened blind alleys were entered 47% fewer times than those 
of full length. This bears out the conclusions drawn from the 
B-mazes. 

The effect of shortening the cul de sacs was most noticeable 
in the case of 1 in the B-mazes, which was by all means the most 
difficult to eliminate. Being the first to encounter, it was 

16 On this point our results agree with some aspects of those by Peckstein, L. A. 
Whole vs. Part Methods in Motor Learning: a comparative Study. Psych. Mon., 
Ser. No. 99, 1917. ‘‘ Each aspect of the course is no doubt associated with and 


located in reference to all the details of the course and to the entire objective en- 
vironment as well.” P. 30. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 31 


likely the least complicated by the results of entering other 
blind alleys. Table III and figure VII show comparatively the 
rate of elimination of all entrances to this cul de sac full length 
and shortened, 22 and 8.5 inches respectively, by the two groups 
of rats, Mu and Nu. While the two curves start near together 
the one, 5, representing the entrances to the shortened cul de sac 
drops rapidly after the 15th trial; the other one, L, after the 
initial decline keeps nearly the same height to the 55th trial. 
The percentage eliminations are shown for the long and for 
the shortened cul de sac, respectively, by curves E and E’. 


TABLE III 


ELIMINATION OF ALL ENTRANCES TO BLIND ALLEY 1. Two GROUPS OF 
EIGHT Rats EacH. Mazes IB ann IIB 


Wilalsuisi)datacdieayaees 1-2 1-5 6-15 | 16-25 26-35 | 36-45 | 46-55 


Av. No. of entrances to | 
blind alley 1, long...| 16.5) 11.1 The HEB 6.6 6.4 6.0 


Percent wovac ees deen es 100.0 | 67.3 | 46.6} 47.3 | 40.0] 38.8) 36.4 


Av. No. of entrances to 
blind alley 1, short’d.| 13.0 8.8 7.2 4.1 1.8 1.1 1.1 


POP COM Gaia scarier as iso 100.0 | 67.7 | 55.4] 30.8] 13.8 8.5 8.5 


TABLE IlI—Continued 


ELIMINATION OF ALL ENTRANCES TO BLIND ALLEY 1. Two Grours or 
Eicut Rats EacH. Mazes IB ann IIB 


(ENaIS. osc ce Sees 56-65 | 66-75 | 76-85 | 86-95 | 96-105 106-115]116-125 
Av. No. of entrances to 

blind alley 1, long... 5.0 3.5 2.5 8 6 6 
a a ee es | 22) 2) 428| @8| 8.6] © 
Av. No. of entrances to | 

blind alley, 1 short’d. a2 | al 0) 0 0 0 
Per cent...........05. 1.2 8 s/o | 0 0 0 


The results from cul de sac 2 are 221 entrances to the full 
length (40 inches) and 234 to the shortened form. This would 
appear to contradict our general conclusion. However, it must 
be remembered that the rats for which 2 was shortened made 

3 


32 JOSEPH PETERSON 


346 more entrances to 1 (long) than did the control animals 
for which 2 was left full length; they also made 18 more returns 
to the starting place in the maze. This not only required that 
2 shortened be passed more times than 2 long, but also with 
greater probability of entrance for each time. It was noted 
that rats entering 1 were likely thereby to be thrown out of 


Per Av. number 
Cent Per Period 
100 
90 
BO ALG) 
70 
60 
50 
4or8 NALA, 
30 


20 


Lo 
e 


1 1 L uz. i 
1-2 1-5 6-15 16-25 36-45 56-65 76-85 96 
- -1L05 116-125 
TRIAL PERIODS 


FIGURE VII.—L shows rate of elimination of all entrances to cul de sac 1 long, 
Maze IB, and S same for 1 short, Maze IIB. E’ and E show corresponding 
percentage eliminations. 


orientation and to make other errors. There can therefore be 
little doubt that if 2 had been the only blind alley in the maze, 
it would have formed no exception to the general rule. The 
greater number of entrances to 4 shortened than to 4 long 
(86 to 69) is due to the fact that rats emerging from 5 had a 
strong tendency to run into 4. A glance at the maze will show 
why this is to be expected. There were 32 more returns from 
5 long than from 5 short; furthermore, entrance to 5 long had 
the greater tendency to disorient the animal so that entrance 
to 4 would be an increased probability. Just why 8 short 
should have been entered 51% more than 8 long is not easy to 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 33 


determine. There were, moreover, 14% more returns from the 
entrances in the former than in the latter case. It is possible 
that the rats entering 8 at full length, which runs along side 
the food box, had time and opportunity to get sufficient odor 
from the food to influence them against returning. Accidental 
factors may have been the cause in part; half of the entrances 
were made in the first two trials, and the total numbers are 
too small to indicate with much probability the actual trends. 

On the whole there can be no question that, other things 
equal, entrances to short cul de sacs are more easily eliminated 
than entrances to long ones. 

The results from both types of mazes used in this experiment 
(see tables I and II) show that on the whole cul de sacs first 
encountered in the maze were entered more frequently, and 
that the impulses to enter them were overcome with more 
difficulty, than were those occurring further along the true 
path, or nearer the food. In this respect our results are in 
agreement with those of Miss Vincent'* and contrary to those 
of Miss Hubbert.17 While in the present experiment, ‘not 
intended especially to test this point, the bearing of the results 
is necessarily complicated by an inequality of the lengths of 
the various blind alleys, there is no evidence to show that results 
would have been different with cul de sacs of equal lengths and 
of equal direction difficulties. In the B-mazes, for example, 
‘6 and 7 were much less troublesome than 3 and 4, in many 
respects similarly located with respect to the correct path, and 
all of equal length. By all means the most difficult cul de sac 
to avoid entering was 1, even when shortened to 8.5 inches. 
The total entrances to 6 and 7 long are 101, against 142 to 
3 and 4 long; to 6 and 7 shortened 83, against 130 to 3 and 4 
shortened. The total number of entrances to 1 short are 275, 
whereas the totals to 6, 7, 8, 9, 10 full length amount only to 
192. It seemed that the rats got rather firmly registered in 
their proprio-ceptive system of controls the tendency to make 
two successive turns of 90 degrees each to the right, beginning 

16 Vincent, Stella B. The White Rat and the Maze Problem—IV. The Number 
and Distribution of Errors: a Comparative Study. Jour. Animal Behav., 1915, 
5, 367-374. “‘ The final members of the cul de sacs were entered less frequently 
and eliminated first.” P, 374. 


17 Hubbert, Helen B. Elimination of Errors in the Maze. Jour. Animal Behav., 
1915, 5, 66-72. 


34 JOSEPH PETERSON 


at the corner of the maze before cul de sac 1, and that since the 
turns were so close together they tended very persistently to 
fuse together into a single turn of 180 degrees, thus taking the 
rat into the blind alley. It was very interesting to see certain 
rats continue to run into 1 with almost monotonous regularity 
for three weeks, three trials each day, while other errors, errors 
of entering other cul de sacs, occurred very seldom. Thus from 
the 10th to the 79th trial, inclusive, rat 9 made 60 errors of 
entering 1 with only 11 entrances to all the other nine blind 
alleys; rat 11 from the 24th to the 83rd trials made corresponding 
errors of 47 to 15. 

In the A-mazes cul de sacs 5 and 6 were likewise entered 
fewer times and eliminated more easily than 1, 2, and 3, all 
of length equal to that of 5 and shorter than 6. It is, of course, 
not contended here that the two sets of blind alleys compared 
are of equal difficulty in all respects other than that here con- 
sidered. At the same time, they may be approximately equal; 
that is a matter which can be determined only empirically. 

The accompanying table (table IV) shows that not only is 
the number of entrances to blind alleys first to be passed along 
the true path greater than that nearer the food box, but also 
that the percentage rate of elimination is greater in the latter. 
This is shown by comparing the number of entrances to the 
different groups of cul de sacs in question for different successive 
periods in the learning process from the first to the last trial. 
In the first five trials of all the animals, trained and untrained, 
the average number of entrances per trial into cul de sacs 1-4 
of the B-mazes is twice that of entrances into 6-10. Calling 
these numbers for the first period (the average of the Ist to the 
Sth trial) 100% each, to get a common basis for comparison, 
we find that there is a much more rapid percentage drop of elim- 
ination of entrances in the case of the blind alleys nearer the 
food box. Since the trained rats discontinued the experiment 
with the 25th trial without finishing the habit, the percentages 
for the two groups in the B-mazes are not correct after the 
25th trial, though they are strictly comparable. An additional 
line is given, in the case of each of these groups, of the accurate 
percentages of elimination of entrances for the untrained rats 
(eight in each group) alone. It will be noted that in the case 
of the five cul de sacs nearest to the food box the percentage 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 35 


of elimination is considerably more rapid than in that of the 
first four blind alleys encountered. In the case of the A-mazes 
the percentage elimination is considerably greater for cul de 
sacs § and 6 than for 2 and 3. Figures VIII and IX represent 
graphically the data of Table IV. 

There is no room to doubt that the blind alleys first to be 
passed along the true paths in the mazes used are both more 
frequently entered and more slowly eliminated than are those 
further along the trail. 


TABLE IV 
Periods of trials............... 1-5 6-15 | 16-25 | 26-35 | 36-45 | 46-55 
Blind alley 1............... 134 210 166 84 75 71 
Blind alley 2............... 147 76 68 39 33 24 
cn Blind alley 3............... 79 11 5 6 2 7 
| Blind alley 4............... 89 16 19 8 6 6 
N 
wy | Totals: i<e stgauuviar caved 449; 313] 258} 137, 116] 108 
© Av. per trial. cases on ona 89.8] 31.3] 25.8] 13.7] 11.6] 10.8 
‘Per cent, 24 rats............] 100.0 | 34.7] 28.6] 15.2] 12.9] 12.0 
(Per cent, 16 untr’d rats... .. 100.0 | 27.6] 25.5}; 19.7] 16. 15.6 
Blind alley 6.............. 42 28 9 1 2 2 
Blind alley 7.............. 28 33 18 10 6 2 
Blind alley 8.............. 62 15 5 3 2 3 
om |Blind alley 9.............. 49 7 4 5 3 2 
g )Blind alley 10.............. 36 14 3 1 1 1 
GI 
Bi | TVORALS: oe eesed vaaws yeaa nas 217 97 39 20 14 10 
Av. per trial..........0.... 43.7 9.7 3.9 2.0 1.4 1.0 
Per cent, 24 rats............ 100.0 | 21.3 9.0 4.6 3.2 2.3 
Per cent, 16 untr’d rats. ....| 100.0 15.6 6.8 4.0 2.8 2.0 
(Blind alley 2............... 84 65 28 8 3 1 
< | Blind alley 3............... 51 73 49 25 13 3 
s SPO CALS 2s. ccseriac: saw aaselta 135 138 77 33 16 4 
=& |Av. per trial..........0.... 27.0} 13.8 7.7 33. 1.6 4 
bPeF CONC. 3s suee cies de anione 100.0} 46.9] 26.5] 11.2 5.6 1.4 
Blind alley 5............... 32 21 2 1 1 3 
< |Blind alley 6............... 23 9 0 2 0 1 
vo 
& )Totals......0.. 0. cece eee eee 55 30 2 3 1 4 
& | Av. per trial.......0.00.... 11.0] 3.0 2 3 il 4 
Per CEN ic aicardiun ere naters<| 100.0) 23 1.8 Dk 9 3.6 


36 JOSEPH PETERSON 
TABLE IV—Conlinued 


Period of trials........ 56-65 | 66-75 | 76-85 | 86-95 | 96-105 |106-115)116-125 
Blind alley 1....... 52 36 26 8 q 6 0) 
Blind alley 2....... 15 10 8 7 2 3 3 

oc Blind alley 3....... 1 2 1 2 1 0) 0 

» | Blind alley 4....... 6 2 4 2 0 0 0) 

N 

S| Totals encase wesc uns 74 50 39 19 10 9 3 

| Ave per trials sassei.ce 7.4 5.0 3.9 1.9 1.0 9 33 
Per cent, 24 rats.... 8.2 5.6 4.3 Dek 1.1 1.0 13) 
{Per ct.,16 untr’d rats} 10.6 7.2 5.6 27 1.4 1.3 4 
Blind alley 6...... 1 0 1 1 0 0 0) 
Blind alley 7...... 0 0 0) (0) 0) 0 0 
Blind alley 8...... 0 1 0 2 0 0 0 

fQ|Blind alley 9...... 0 0 0 0 0 0) 0 

@ |Blind alley 10...... 0 0 0 1 0 0 0) 

i) 

= |Totals............. 1 1 1 4 0 0 0 
Av. per trial........ 1 i eal A 1) 6 0 
Per cent, 24 rats.... “y my) gh 9 0) 0 0) 
Per ct.,16 untr’d rats 2 22, 4 8 0 0 0 


It may be that the odor of the food is a factor that at least 
partly explains the more easy elimination of the cul de sacs 
nearer the food box. However, there is very little, if any, real 
evidence that such is the case. A crucial test would be to use 
anosmic rats, though other means of controlling the odor factor 
are easily possible. Some facts in the present experiment count 
against the influence of odor as suggested. For example, errors 
of entrance into cul de sac 10 are nearly as numerous as those 
of entrance into 9, although to get to 10 the animal had to pass 
a short alley of 8.5 inches leading directly into the food box. 
Moreover, all the rats, with occasional exceptions,'* ran so 
rapidly after the first trial that it is improbable that food odor 
had any immediate direct influence in the behavior in the maze. 
There was no evidence in the behavior of the animals that they 
were attracted to the food box by such odors.1® In the cases 
of supposed trailing, already noted, the animal which appeared 

18 Occasionally, without any apparent external condition to explain the behavior, 
an animal would sneak slowly and cautiously all the way through the maze. In 
a few cases such activity seemed to be due to recent fights with other rats or to 
noises from fights between other animals. 

_1* An exception should be made here of the case of returns from cul de sac 8, already 
discussed. The floor of the food box was covered with paper (double thickness) 


during the feeding each day, and during the experiment the food was kept in a 
dish in the extreme corner of the food box away from cul de sacs 8, 9, and 10. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 37 


to be following a scent of any kind moved perceptibly more 
slowly, holding the nose continuously or frequently to the floor. 
The writer does not believe that the more rapid elimination of 
the cul de sacs nearer the food is to be explained on the basis 
of scenting the food. The matter, however, needs further test. 


Per Cent 


100 
80 
80 


Per Cent 7 


1006 
Bo 
sot 

ro bh 


6OL \ 


Sor 


\ 
nes <~ 
Qee LF Se 
1-5 6-15 16-25 36-45 56-65 
TRIAL PERIODS 


40> 
30+ 


20 + 


“| . Ser see had, tee an Fig. V1. 


1-5 6-15 16-25 36-45 66-65 76-85 86-105 116-125 
TRIAL PERIODS 


FicuRES VIII AND IX.—F and L show percentage elimination of all entrances to 
cul de sacs 1-4 and 6-19 combined, respectively, by twenty-four rats in the 
B-mazes; UF and UL the corresponding data for sixteen untrained rats. F’ 
and L’ show the percentage elimination, respectively, of all entrances to blind 
alleys 2 and 3, and 5 and 6 combined, by twenty-two rats in the A-mazes. 


Do pure probability laws govern the returns of the rat on 
emergence from blind alleys? In the tables of results (tables I 
and II) the totals of the first two trials have been kept separate 
so that the percentage of returns from blind alleys toward the 
starting place in the maze could be found for a period little 


38 JOSEPH PETERSON 


influenced by the effects of training. The following table (table 
V) classifies for easy comparison the results of all the rats on 
the first two trials. The entrances to cul de sac 1 in the A- 
mazes are not included as all emergences from this blind alley 
brought the rat to the place of the entrance to the maze. 


TABLE V 
Full Length Cul De Sacs 
Compl. Ent. | Half Ent. Start. Ent. ne 
et. 
Rats Maze of all 
E’s. 
Ent. | Ret. | Ent. | Ret. | Ent. | Ret. 
Set dis omer anucctiis s B 170 76 35 6 54 16 38 
AAT Ce sicn tte se 4 eae 8 B 44 16 Cd 0 | 14 3 29 
4amte’d s ven eisay ane y A 51 24 @ 4 46 16 42 
i Gh aca s (arene mene A 33 13 5 2 12 3 36 
| 
Average per cent returns.........0.0.00 000. c ccc eee eens 33.75 
TABLE V—Continued 
Shortened Cil De Sacs 
Compl. Ent. | Half Ent. Start. Ent. 8 
Rats Maze Ret. 
of all 
Ent. | Ret. | Ent. | Ret. | Ent. | Ret. E’s. 
8 UTE nasa econ B 182 72 27 11 48 18 39 
BEE Cove cson loess crreae B 42 13 4 2 20 5 30 
A TCE Cision saunas A 23 10 6 3 12 0 32 
A ALE: Cictatten chines itae htt A 1l 2 6 1 10 0 ll 
Average per cent returns. ....... 0. ce ec eee eee eee ena 23 


It will be noted that in the B-maze the per cent of returns 
from the shortened cul de sacs are practically equal to those 
from the full length ones, both for the trained and for the un- 
trained rats. The returns for untrained rats are not far short 
of 50%. The shortage is mostly due, no doubt, to the small 
degree of learning that took place in the process of the first 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 39 


two trials, during which there was considerable random activity 
and reduction of excess movements. It would seem that at 
first—before any learning has taken place—the chance of a rat’s 
returning on emergency from a blind alley is about one to one. 
There may be a greater tendency to go forward, keeping the 
general orientation rather than to return; if so, the excess for- 
ward tendency is but slight. The returns from cul de sacs first 
to be passed seem slightly to exceed in percentage those from 
blind alleys further on toward the food box. In the B-mazes 
the returns from cul de sac 1 (both full length and shortened) 
are 44% of the total number of entrances; the corresponding 
percentages for the other blind alleys in order from 2 to 10 are 
55, 31, 32, 48, 33, 50, 34, 13, 33. These figures are taken, of 
course, only from the records of the untrained rats, sixteen alto- 
gether. Those most favorably situated for returns, so far as 
the rat’s keeping the general direction on emergence from the 
blind alley is concerned, are 2 and 5. This judgment is sup- 
ported by the data. It is not clear why the returns from 7 
should run so high. The percentage of returns by the eight 
untrained rats in the A-mazes are, for the 2nd to the 6th blind 
alley, in order: 36, 33, 67, 0, and 0. The large number for 4 
was to be expected. The greater number of returns from the 
cul de sacs first encountered is likely due to the fact that the 
animals had already learned something of keeping the general 
orientation before the other blind alleys were entered. 

In the B-mazes there appears to be a slight decrease in the 
returns of the first two trials by the trained rats as compared 
with the untrained. This seems to be due to a sort of “ transfer 
of training.” It is likely, as the writer suggested in the earlier 
article already referred to, due to a tendency of animals with 
experience in mazes to proceed with less whole-souled response 
into cul de sacs. Let us suppose that as an animal enters a cul 
de sac it also receives certain stimuli of various kinds from the 
true path from which it departs. These stimuli may produce 
a weak partial response, or tendency to response, which does 
not immediately fade away. If this tendency persists until the 
. tat emerges from the cul de sac it will, of course, enhance the 
impulse to take the true path and thus increase the probability 
of continued forward movement. It is not inconceivable that 
a trained animal may have developed a habit of keeping the 


40 JOSEPH PETERSON 


correct general orientation by some such means as this. Such 
habits would.then have common factors for all cul de sacs, and 
in mazes of different kinds. It would seem, too, that on some 
such basis as this the returns would be eliminated more readily 
than entrances to the blind alleys, as has already been shown 
to be the case. This explanation may involve an interaction 
of sensory and motor impulses in the nerve fibres—each sys- 
tem, sensory and motor, interacting upon and stimulating the 
other—in such a manner as to make comprehensible how the 
effect of stimuli may be carried over into later responses and 
partly condition them as suggested below. 

Possibly the animals also learn with training to utilize better 
such factors as vague visual stimuli of the closed end of the 
cul de sac. Certainly the speed of the rat running into the 
blind alley would make one cautious in assuming that such 
factors are explicitly reacted to by the animal. That there 
was a real transfer of some kind is, in any event, a conclusion 
which also finds support in the results of the A-mazes. For 
the full length and the shortened cul de sacs, the per cent returns 
for untrained rats are 42 and 32, respectively, agreeing rather 
closely with the B-maze results, whereas the corresponding per- 
centage returns by the trained rats—seven in each A-maze— 
are 36 and 11, a decrease from that of the untrained animals 
of 14% for the full length and of 66% for the shortened cul 
de sacs. In the B-mazes the percentage returns from the full 
length cul de sacs by trained rats is 24% less than that by un- 
trained rats; for the shortened blind alleys the percentage re- 
turns by the trained rats is 21% less than that by untrained 
rats. 


THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE RESULTS 


It may be urged by the reader that the more rapid elimination 
of entrances to the shortened cul de sacs than to the full length 
ones is due to the fact that the rat, in the case of the short blind 
alleys at least, sees the closed end and thereby avoids entering 
so frequently, or so completely. In one sense this begs the 
whole question. Seeing is not some thing that stimulates or 
directs the animal; it is only a mode of being stimulated. Its 
possibility in the present study is not at all denied. The whole 
question with which we are concerned is: How do all possible 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 41 


kinds of stimuli operate, directly or indirectly, toward the learning 
to avoid entering cul de sacs? 

That the rat is not wholly blind has been demonstrated in 
a number of cases,?° but there is no clear evidence to show that 
the presence of such visual factors as are possible to the rat 
could operate on the principle of frequency, recency, or intensity, 
or all combined, in such a manner as to eliminate the impulses 
to enter the cul de sacs under the conditions of the present prob- 
lem. They might, of course, aid the rat in getting to the food 
at any one time, but how could they operate toward cutting 
short the random processes in successive trials, i.¢., in bringing 
about what is called learning? A brief review of the work on 
visual controls in the rat’s behavior is to be found in Miss Vin- 
cent’s paper. Waugh found: that though the mouse could 
perceive the distance of objects “ within a range of 15 cm.,” 
it nevertheless seemed not to make use of the ‘‘ visual percep- 
tion of depth”’ in getting past two partitions each from oppo- 
site sides reaching half way across the problem box, the one 
being nearer than the other. 

In the present experiment, it will be recalled, the interior of 
the maze was stained black, and even if it be granted that the 
rat could see the ends of some of the shortest cul de sacs there 
would be but little difference in the visual stimuli between the 
“blind” and the open alleys, in as much as both were obstructed 
alike in the further end and the side opening of the latter was 
not directly visible. Differences in brightness would be irregular 
and but slight, as the room was lighted from three sides—south, 
west, and slightly from the north—and an electric light was 
directly over the maze. It should be said that no difference 
in behavior between the rat blind in the left eye—No. 20—and 
the other rats was noticeable though the experimenter kept 
watch for such difference. More careful visual controls are of 
course desirable. 

‘But the real question is how any stimulus, visual or otherwise, 
must operate together with other stimuli so as to inhibit 
unsuccessful acts and to cause to survive those acts which bring 


20 See Richardson, Florence. A Study of the Sensory Control of the White 
Rat. Psychol. Mon., Ser. No. 48, 1909. Vincent, Stella B. The White Rat and 
the Maze Problem—I. The Introduction of a Visual Control. Jour. Animal 
Behav., 19 15, 5, 1-24. re ; 

2 Waugh, K. T. The Roéle of Vision in the Mental Life of the Mouse. Jour. 
Comp. Neurol. and Psychol., 1910, 20, 549-599. 


42 JOSEPH PETERSON 


success, in this case those acts which bring the animal to the 
food box. The results follow the series of stimuli and responses 
which take the animal through the maze. How can the result 
work backwards? The writer believes that in the foregoing 
pages he has presented plausible reasons and data to show the 
absolute inadequacy of frequency and recency laws as the direct- 
ing factors in maze learning. Frequency fails to give any basis 
not only for this kind of learning in general but particularly 
for the specific kinds of results obtained in the experiments 
considered. In a complex situation like this, frequency explains 
only how within a certain probability the rat will finally reach 
the food, but it fails to explain why subsequent trials should 
be improvements on the first one. It is not clear how recency, 
as ordinarily understood, can aid the learning. The principle 
of intensity needs re-interpretation. When several stimuli act 
on an animal bringing about a series of responses as in this 
case, the final one of which is the successful one, it appears that 
somehow, not well understood yet, the various effects of these 
stimuli hold over into that of the final stimulus and that all 
together simultaneously act to direct the energy of the animal 
into the most consistent channels. In the large, these channels 
offer the least resistance and afford the most complete response. 
It is in this sense that the successful acts are more intense than 
others, and thus their effect is greater toward shaping the neural 
pathways for their repetition and for the gradual elimination 
of the more inconsistent and tentative responses leading up to 
them. On this assumption it becomes somewhat comprehensible 
why the maze is learned to a large extent ‘‘as a whole,” so 
that small errors may throw the animal out completely, or 
at some other part of the maze, when the habit is nearly per- 
fected. The specific results of the present experiment are also 
intelligible. These various hold-over effects in the extero- and 
the proprio-ceptive systems afford the basis of imagery in human 
behavior, and supply the ‘‘ large situation ’’ to which one reacts 
ideally. They may function, so far as we can know, wholly 
unconsciously or with but vague consciousness in the case of 
the rat. In the human being habits of responding to separate 
groupings of these factors may be acquired, and such exciting 
factors may be aroused indirectly by association. Nothing is 
gainedtin psychological explanation by assuming ‘‘ ideas” to 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 43. 


explain behavior, unless in such cases we understand how the 
ideal dispositions themsélves are acquired. The use of the 
term zdea in the higher forms of behavior is justified then only 
on the basis of simplicity of statement. There is none but 
questionable evidence thus far that ideational behavior is different. 
in any way but degree from sensori-motor, or the well known 
trial and error, behavior. “Ideas” can function only when 
the somewhat detachable dispositions, of which they are the 
imperfect, subjective aspects, have been built up by experience, 
and such dispositions require a rather complex nervous mechan- 
ism. It is needless to say that no evidence of ideational behavior 
has been found in the white rat. While, as has been pointed 
out in the foregoing, there are likely some hold-over effects of 
stimuli in the case of the rat, these likely operate more or less 
mechanically and en masse so that the animal enjoys little 
independence of action and is subject rather completely to the 
dominance of the group of stimuli present or immediately past. 
That is to say, the animal can respond only to present situations 
though with a considerable number of random variations, until 
the most consistent responses to that situation have fixed them- 
selves to the exclusion of all others, after many repetitions of 
trials. Then the response becomes uniform and mechanical to 
a high degree. 

The more advanced behavior as we see it in the case of 
man—ideational behavior—differs from the lower forms illus- 
trated in the present study in that it is less fixed and less 
dependent upon immediate situations. Stimulus-response organ- 
izations, or tendencies, are more detachable in their separate 
smaller functional components; and the latter have richer pos- 
sibilities of combinations among themselves, on the one hand, 
and on the other there is less dependence for their functioning 
upon direct or immediate stimulation. Various indirect and 
vicarious stimuli come to serve adequately. Thus various. 
systems of stimulus-response mechanisms may become organized 
into inconceivably complex relationships about certain symbolic 
stimuli, such as written or spoken words, various kinds of gestures 
and attitudes of the stimulating individuals, associated objects, 
sounds, contacts, and so on. It then becomes practically impos- 
sible to predict which of the various aspects of the situation 
will succeed in calling out its particular response. We shall 


44 JOSEPH PETERSON 


not here enter into further consideration of this complex behavior, 
except to point out that when the various stimulus-response 
mechanisms have become sufficiently well associated with 
certain muscular strains or neural excitations, the revival of the 
latter by favorable stimuli will call out the acts themselves. 
Thus a stimulus may have entirely ceased to play upon the 
sense ofgans from without and long periods of time may have 
elapsed, and yet, because of this acquired organization, the 
recurrence of any significant aspect of the outer situation, even 
such as a sound associated with it, may revive the crucial exci- 
tation and thus call'out the act. Something of this kind— 
stimulated, however, by the original situation minus the light 
when the animal is allowed to respond—likely takes place in 
the delayed reactions of animals, though this assumption leaves 
entirely open the question as to whether or not the animals 
have ideas, a rather infertile question for science, it must be 
confessed. More elaborate systems of acquired associations 
make possible the continual thinking of absent situations which 
we know that we ourselves experience. In these more «dvanced 
forms of behavior groups of response systems may come so to 
interact upon one another by associations and by stimulation 
from the inward bodily conditions that rehearsal of a problem 
mentally may take place long after actual practice has ceased, 
thus changing behavior materially between practices. It is 
yet questionable whether there are any such cases in animal 
behavior. 

In the foregoing pages we have called into question the prin- 
ciples of frequency, recency, and intensity of stimulation as 
usually understood in relation to the fixing of associations, so 
far as their value in explaining learning is concerned. They do 
not seem to account for the change in successive trials called 
learning. This seems to be true at any rate for maze learning; 
probably it holds for all kinds of learning. All that these fac- 
tors do is, likely, to make more and more easy any associations 
and acts brought about by the real directing factors. That 
is, they tend to fix any series of acts in the order that they are 
gone through, not to change the order of the acts. Some other 
directing factors and some vis a tergo must be found to account 


_* Cf. Yerkes, R. M. The Mental Life of Monkeys and Apes: A Study of Idea- 
tional Behavior. Behav. Mon., 1916, 3, No. 1. Yerkes thinks Julius, an orang- 
utan, solved a problem ideationally; see particularly pp. 68 and 131. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 45 


for the changes in behavior which gradually make response 
more and more direct and which gradually eliminates the use- 
less random acts. We must not forget that the numerous 
internal life processes, e.g., the contractions of the muscles of 
the stomach with hunger, serve as the motivation to activity. 
They determine the stimulating value, as do also modifications 
in the proprio-ceptive system by past behavior, of various 
outside factors. The organism continues to respond by vary- 
ing behavior until successes are attained which modify these 
internal conditions and change the inner motivating factors. 
But the failures also change the organism. The directing fac- 
tors of the response seem to be the inner organic processes 
and the total combination of stimuli from external conditions 
and from muscular contractions, all these overlapping in their 
several effects as has been suggested. The neural channels 
involved in the most consistent acts become the most opera- 
tive through the compelling effects of all these factors, and 
these acts, or directions of response, in time survive over all 
others and ‘gradually acquire an ease and automaticity of func- 
tioning characteristic of habits. The stimuli to action even in 
as simple an organism as a rat are infinitely more complex than 
usually imagined in our ‘‘ neural explanations.’”’ Mere contin- 
gency in the combinations of acts of a rat brought about in 
the maze, or in other problem boxes, for that matter, cannot 
be regarded as the important factor that it has sometimes been 
supposed to be. It is true that some useless acts may occa- 
sionally survive with the more consistent ones by chance asso- 
ciations, but such acts are really not vital parts of the system 
of learned acts. 

The precise nature of the hold-over effects of various stimuli 
posited in the explanations of learning here suggested must be 
left to physiology and neurology. There is undoubtedly a close 
connection between sensory and motor impulses. Sensory stim- 
uli bring about responses which in their different stages of expres- 
sion set up new afferent impulses, or either facilitate or tend to 
inhibit old ones; these again modify the motor tendencies. We. 
are a long way yet from a satisfactory knowledge of nerve im- 
pulses and their effects upon one another,—Are they periodic 
or continuous? What relations obtain between stimulus changes 
and nerve impulse changes? What is the nature of inhibition 


46 JOSEPH PETERSON 


and of facilitation? These and many other problems not yet 
solved have important bearings upon our knowledge of the learn- 
ing act. But psychologists cannot wait for the solution of these 
problems before attempting to formulate more satisfactory con- 
ceptions of the processes with which they must deal at every 
turn. It must be apparent that chaos now reigns with respect 
to this matter. Some writers invoke imitation to explain most 
modifications in behavior; others use pleasure and pain for the 
same purpose; while ideas, purposes, the effects of random acts, 
and so on, are freely used directly or indirectly by most writers. 
All of these factors may have real parts to play in the learning 
process, in some one or more of its various aspects, but they 
are all more or less vaguely conceived and frequently erroneously 
referred to, almost as some sort of original or spontaneous causes, 
rather than complex aspects of the very thing that is to be better 
understood and analyzed. Popular, educational, and soz‘ologi- 
cal writers may be forgiven for their own sins in this part cular 
so long as psychologists have nothing more satisfactory to offer 
than at present. The great problem of how learning takes 
place is yet largely unsolved. 

For the best progress, experiments in behavior modification 
must go hand in hand with physiological investigations into the 
nature of the nerve impulse. A few rather suggestive studies 
have been carried out by psychologists upon the mutual effects 
of successive acts on one another. It appears that while one 
particular kind of act is being learned a second contrary one 
is inhibited by it more than after the first has been completed. 
The extensive investigations of Professor T. G. Brown,?4 on the 
physiological side, have shown a summation of successive liminal 
stimuli (facilitation) of intervals up to about ten seconds. Such 
neural overlapping effects may well function to bring about a 

* Pillsbury, discussing experiments on associative inhibition by Miiller and 
Schumann (1894), concludes that ‘‘ where several things are to be learned in the 
same connection, it is found that inhibition ceases to be effective if the first is 
thoroughly learned before the second is begun.” Fundamentals of Psychology, 
1916, p. 359. See also p. 365. Especially interesting in this regard is a study 
recently reported by Hunter,—Hunter, W. S., and Yarbrough, Jos. U. The In- 
- terference of Auditory Habits in the White Rat. Jour. Animal Behav., 1917, 7, 
49-65. See especially pages 60 ff. One must be careful not to generalize too 
much from these experiments on contrary acts. 

* Brown, T. G. On the Phenomenon of Facilitation. I. Its Occurrence in 
Reactions Induced by Stimulation of the “Motor” Cortex of the Cerebrum in 


Monkeys. Quart. Jour. of Exper. Physiol., 1915, 9, 81-99. Other articles by the 
same authority in the same journal. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 47 


simultaneous operation to some extent of the various experi- 
ences that a rat has in finding the food box in the maze. In 
many types of learning we have been much in the dark as to 
how later effects of the successful results could work back and 
stamp in these successful acts to the exclusion of the various 
unsuccessful ones. By the conception of the overlapping of 
effects of successive nerve functionings may we not be getting 
a start in the right direction? 


DISCUSSION 


Peckstein?s has recently tried to explain the transfer effects 
found in his experiments on the basis of factors which the writer 
finds extremely vague, subjective, and otherwise technically 
objectionable. The general factors of his explanation are: 
(1) ‘General maze habits ’’—reduction of tendency to return, 
knowledge of the nature of errors, improved sense of direction; 
(2) “ consciousness of power;’’ and (3) ‘‘ proper emotional atti- 
tude.” Specific factors are such as common specific identities, 
or near-identities, in the different mazes. We are told that 
return is due to the general ‘“‘ dominance of the familiar.” ‘‘ The 
return pathway is known to be safe. The rats seem natively 
inclined to return to the closed entrance.’”’ This return ten- 
dency—due to knowledge or instinct?—is actually inhibited by 
any maze for any other. The knowledge of the nature of errors 
is a ‘‘ concept,” we are told, developed in the earlier sections of 
the total maze. <A cul de sac “‘ ceases to be a detail that must 
be cautiously explored,” and ‘comes to mean a detail that 
must be left as soon as possible.’’ At first—now we are at the 
“sense of direction’’ factor—some learners ‘“‘ have almost a 
‘going ahead’ instinct,’’ while others have a greater tendency 
to return. This latter tendency is gradually overcome. This 
seems, then, only to be another name for the factor mentioned 
under “returns.” ‘‘In subsequent mazes, the truly sophisti- 
cated learner will enter the cul de sac, but will proceed along 
the forward pathway when he returns to the true course.”’ 

The “‘ consciousness of power ’”’ in the rats seems to manifest 
itself, after all, in some objective behavior change, such as 
increased activity. ‘‘In subsequent mazes, however, the con- 
sciousness of power is clearly seen (!). No ‘warming-up’ period 


% Op. cil., pp. 50-54. 
4 


(a3 


48 JOSEPH PETERSON 


is needed. There is no delay at the entrance. Work has come 
to mean invariable accomplishment and reward. The entire 
attack upon the new problem is aggressive. The learner has 
learned to do by doing.” The proper emotional attitude, the 
last of the general factors, means the overcoming of an attitude 
complex, ‘“‘a mixture of fear, indecision, curiosity, and perhaps 
anger.’’ All this after some really valuable experiments! Surely 
this is only a complication in subjective terms of facts to be 
accounted for, which facts practically all authorities are willing 
to accept more or less completely. These “ factors” of transfer 
do not take us anywhere. 

Dr. Peckstein finds?* that the difficultness of mazes is not 
proportional to their lengths, nor to the number of their blind 
alleys. But why should it be? As to the number of cul de 
sacs, it is obvious that if at first a rat tends in its ‘‘ choices’ at 
bifurcations to follow chance laws—and our present results 
point that way—difficultness ought to increase on some other 
principle. The probability of passing any single cul de sac 
successfully is 3/4, as Watson has pointed out, i. e., if we mean 
by “ successfully ”’ that the animal either goes on in the correct 
path, or, if it enters the blind alley, that on emergence from it 
it keeps the general forward direction. The chance of passing 
two blind alleys is therefore 9/16 (= 3/4 x 3/4); that of passing 
three cul de sacs, (3/4)8; and so on. On this basis the chance 
of getting through one unit of Peckstein’s maze—3 blind alleys 
—is (3/4)3, while that of getting through the entire maze without 
a return is (3/4)". In the former case the probability of a 
success without returns is therefore over thirteen times that 
in the latter. Complications from returns at any cul de sac 
will be brought about by additions of other forward movements 
beyond the point to which return is made, but each of these 
additional forward runs may again be assumed to follow, before 
any training sets in, the same probability law at each cul de sac 
- that is followed in the original run. Thus the above calculation 
may stand roughly as approximately correct. Its results—a 
difficultness of the whole maze of over thirteen times that of 
the quarter maze—agrees more closely with Peckstein’s actual 
results, as estimated by him, than do those based on the 
assumption of a direct proportionate increase in difficultness 


2 Tbid., pp. 59-57. 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 49 


with distance through the maze and number of cul de sacs. 
Peckstein found the whole maze over twenty times more difficult 
than the average of the four quarter mazes, whereas such a 
direct proportionate increase of difficultness as he assumed 
should make it but four times more difficult. Of course, many 
other factors in any such calculations must be taken into 
consideration. With Peckstein’s rats the relative degrees of 
difficultness of the four sections of the maze were found, in 
order from the first to the fourth quarter, to be 15, 1, 3, 2, as 
determined by his combined trials-time-error formula. This 
does not look much like equality. The excess-distance run by 
the rats is certainly a factor as well worth while as any to con- 
sider. Why was it not included? 

So far, then, Dr. Peckstein’s results, as presented by himself, 
seem roughly to agree with our own in supporting the view 
that mere probability laws account for the original ‘‘ choices ”’ 
of the untrained rat at the several bifurcations in the maze. 
This is our own interpretation of his data, not his, it should be 
stated. He merely seems to hold that there is some law of 
diminishing returns, which he does not clearly state, that deter- 
mines the degree of energy expended for the learning of mazes 
of varying complexity. 

Dr. G. V. Hamilton in his interesting study of perseverance 
reactions, by the multiple choice method as he has developed 
it, finds frequency and recency of an advantageous response 
more strongly effective toward learning than either frequency 
or recency with no advantage or with actual disadvantage.:’. 
He finds that frequency with invariable advantage is stronger 
in effect toward the building of a habit than even greater fre- 
quency without an invariable advantage. But, as in most 
experiments on learning, he has his conditions so arranged that 
the animal can end up only with the “ successful” act. E. g., 
Hamilton says: ‘‘ During the twenty habit forming trials under 
discussion she [Rat No. 1] manifested only three recency first 
choices, but after these trials [that is, when the habit was learned] 
during which the operation of the factor of recency was invariably 
advantageous she manifested 100% of recency first choices.’ 

ar A Study of Perseverance Reactions in Primates and Rodents. Behav. Mon., 
Ser. No. 13, 1916. 


28 Ibid., pp. 38-46. 
29 Tbid., p. 40. 


50 JOSEPH PETERSON 


How could she do otherwise after learning is accomplished? 
Here the success of the final act was inevitable by the con- 
ditions of the experiment. The final high degree of success is 
the result of the learning, how, then, can it get around to 
come in at the front door as one of the causative factors? 


SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 


The “principles of learning’ frequency, recency, and intensity, 
in their usually accepted meaning, have been found inadequate 
to account for learning in the maze. Probability laws alone 
make possible a sufficient number of right choices for the rat 
to reach the food box finally in the ordinary maze. The 
probability of reaching the food box by mere chance rapidly 
decreases with the increase in the number of cul de sacs in the 
maze. But it is found that on laws of pure fortuity there is no 
explanation for the elimination of cul de sacs; for since the 
probability of entering any blind alley on returns as well as on 
forward runs is 1/2, the habit of continuing to enter them should 
be as strong as that of keeping the right trail toward the food 
box. For learning to be possible, some sort of short-circuiting 
process must take place by which the true path may be suggested 
for the line of action when the animal gets to the entrance of 
any blind alley. It is not clear how any of the usually accepted 
laws of learning—frequency, recency, and intensity—can operate 
to bring this about. Frequency and recency fail entirely to 
account for the behavior of the rat in the maze. The real 
process of learning, the gradual elimination of unsuccessful 
random acts, such as entrances to cul de sacs and returns toward 
the entrance place in the maze, must be accounted for on the 
basis of some entirely different principle. The principles named 
show only how an act, directed by some other factor, becomes 
gradually more mechanically reflex.» 

%0 Statistically the statement in this paragraph, as well as the one in the first 
part of the monograph, is inaccurate. An animal coming the first time to a blind 
alley has a probability of 1/2 of entering it; a probability of 3/4 of continuing in 
the right direction, whether or not the blind alley is entered; and a probability of 
1/4 of entering the CUL DE SAC and, from it, returning toward the starting place 
in the maze. If the animal actually gets by the blind alley in question, enters one 
farther on and returns in the maze, the conditions are reversed at the first blind 
alley. Now the probability of continuing back to the starting place in the maze, 
ie., either of not entering the blind alley at all or of entering and then continuing 
in the return direction, is 3/4; that of getting reériented in the right direction toward 


the food is 1/4; and that of entering the blind alley is 1/2. Adding these fractions 
to those above for the respective directions in which the animal can possibly go 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 51 


The present experiment was devised to present conditions 
which might test the efficiency of the ‘‘ completeness of response ”’ 
theory outlined recently by the writer, suggesting a means of 
learning based on the overlapping and thereby simultaneously 
operative effects of successive stimuli. Identical mazes were 
used for separate groups of animals, but they were so arranged 
that their several cul de sacs could be conveniently varied in 
length. By this means control groups of rats were run in mazes 
differing only in the relative lengths of their cul de sacs, certain 
of these being long for one group and short for the other. Two 
modifications of such differences were used; the one pair of 
control groups having all the blind alleys short in the one maze 
and all long in the other, while the other pair of controls each 
had some long and some short blind alleys making the total 
length of blind alleys equal for both members of the pair. In 
all, twenty-four rats were used. Groups of rats were interchanged 
in the mazes, after the first problems were completed, so that 
each problem was tried both by trained and by untrained animals. 

Detailed records were kept of the behavior of the animals. 
Complete entrances, half way entrances, and beginning entrances 
to cul de sacs were indicated; complete returns and returns to 
blind alleys already passed were noted; and the direction of the 
rat’s movement on emerging from blind alleys, whether forward 
or back, were recorded. The exact time for each trial was 
kept but not used in the present report. 
we have: forward from the blind alley 3/4 + 1/41; into the blind alley 1/2 + 
1/21; and return toward the starting place in the maze 1/4 + 3/4—=1. This 
gives equal exercise to the acts in all these three directions, on pure probability 
laws, when returns are considered. But the animal is taken out of the maze only 
at the food end of the trail; hence for each trial there must be one more forward 
run at any given place in the maze than return runs. This gives the forward direc- 
tion an advantage statistically of 1/4 runs for each trial over the entrance to the 
cul de sac and of 1/2 over returns. This advantage is proportionately small where 
many returns are made, as near the maze entrance, and large where this is not 
the case, as near the food and later in the learning process at any given point. No 
returns will be made to the last cul de sac, and when entrance to it has been elim- 
nated none will be made to the one next to it; and so on. This condition, then, 
affords a fine theoretical basis for explaining learning in the maze, and also for the 
backward elimination of errors of entrance to blind alleys. There are, however, 
serious flaws in this argument when given in support of frequency, either alone or 
combined with recency, as the only principle operative in the learning of the maze. 
Frequency and recency factors really operate against this explanation, rather than 
in favor of it; for they favor the mere repetition of the choices first made at any 
of these critical positions in the maze, and therefore the strengthening of the im- 


pulses to enter blind alleys rather than their weakening. The force of this point 
will be shown concretely in the paper now in preparation. 


52 JOSEPH PETERSON 


1. The decrease in the percentage of returns by the animal 
emerging from blind alleys is very rapid in the early part of the 
learning, and as a rule the rat continues to enter blind alleys, 
even to their full length, long after returns are discontinued; 
i. e., the curve of returns from blind alleys drops much more 
rapidly than that representing the number of entrances to blind 
alleys. These returns persist longer in the case of cul de sacs 
encountered along the first part of the correct path than in that 
of those nearer the food box. 

2. The elimination of entrances to blind alleys does not come 
about mainly by a decrease in the number of entrances, but 
principally, especially in the case of the longer cul de sacs, by 
a gradual decrease in the degree, or the distance, of entrance. 
Just before entrance is eliminated completely, there frequently 
occurs a peculiar and very rapid vibration of the rat’s head 
between the direction of the true path and that of the tempting 
blind alley. Frequently, after the first success in passing any 
such blind alley, the rat runs headlong into some cul de sac 
farther along the correct trail, which it had previously learned 
to avoid. These and other facts of similar import indicate 
that the maze is learned ‘“‘as a whole”’ to a large extent, and 
that entrances to blind alleys are not properly to be regarded 
as separate acts, as is frequently done in speculations on learning. 

3. Entrances to short cul de sacs are eliminated more readily, 
other things equal, than entrances to long ones. Not only are 
the total entrances to the short blind alleys fewer than to the 
longer ones, but the percentage elimination of them is greater. 
The curve of decrease of entrances drops more rapidly in the 
case of short than in that of long cul de sacs. 

4. Blind alleys first to be passed along the true path are 
entered more frequently than those further along—nearer the 
food box—and their percentage rate of elimination is less. That 
is, entrances to the first cul de sacs encountered are more persis- 
tent, harder to overcome, than those to cul de sacs nearer the 
food box. 

5. With untrained rats the number of returns toward the 
entrance door in the maze, on the rats’ emergence from blind 
alleys, nearly, if not quite, equals in the beginning of the experi- 
ment the number of cases of keeping the general forward direction 
toward the food box. It appears that at the beginning stage 
of learning in the maze mere probability determines whether 


EFFECT OF LENGTH OF BLIND ALLEYS ON MAZE LEARNING 53 


the animal goes forward or back on emerging from a blind 
alley. In this respect, however, as in many others, there are 
rather large individual differences. From the very first ex- 
perience in the maze, and this makes the test of the probability 
law rather difficult, the learning factors enter in and rapidly 
decrease the returns in favor of the general forward orientation. 

6. It has been found desirable in work of this kind to study 
individual reactions in detail. Mere averages do not show the 
significant aspects of the behavior in many cases. A detailed 
report of individual ‘‘ choices’ in the maze, by a method which 
promises to be fruitful, is being prepared to justify further the 
statement in the present paper regarding the inadequacy of 
frequency and recency laws as explanations of the rat’s maze- 
learning. 

7. Responses to stimuli cannot .take place instantaneously, 
neither do stimulation effects fade away momentarily. Besides 
this, response tendencies and muscular strains, maintained for 
a shorter or longer time, constantly set up new sensory impulses 
(proprio-ceptive stimuli) which again stimulate reactions. It 
is suggested that by such means as these, and possibly by others 
not yet known, the effects of successive stimuli, such as an 
animal encounters in getting through a problem box to food, 
operate in a measure simultaneously, and the resulting response 
is on the whole the most consistent or complete one under the 
whole circumstance. The channels to this most complete 
response are gradually forced most open or permeable; their 
greater consistency of operation (facilitation) brings about an 
intensity of activity through them which in repeated trials 
gradually short-circuits through the infinitely numerous path- 
ways involved and thus brings about the gradual elimination 
of useless random acts. It is suggested that learning comes 
about by this means. It is hoped that this suggestion may be 
fruitful toward an understanding of how a final success can 
operate back (as it appears externally to do) upon the random 
acts leading to it so as gradually to bring about their elimination. 
This is the theory which the writer has called the ‘‘ completeness 
of response ”’ principle in learning, and it seems to him to account 
for the results obtained in the present experiment as well as for 
others which have been uncritically attributed to the stamping-in 
effects of pleasantness. 


The Behavior Monographs 
Edited by JOHN B. WATSON 
The Johns Hopkins University, nian Md. 


VOLUME i. 
No. 1 The development of certain instincts and habits in chicks, By 
Frederick S. Breed. Pp. iv + 78, $1.00, postpaid. 

No. 2 Methods of studying vision in animals. By Robert M. Yerkes 
, and John B. Watson. Pp. iv + 90, $1.25, postpaid. 

No. 8 An experimental study on the death- feigning of Belostoma. 
(—Zaitha Aucet.) flumineum Say and Nepa apiculata Uhler. 
_By Henry H. P. Severin and Harry C. Severin. Pp. iti + 

. 47, $.65, postpaid. 
ie biology of Physa. By Jean Dawson., Pp. iii + 120, $1.50, 

postpaid. i , 


No. 5 The function of the vibrissae in the behavior of the white rat. 
By Stella Burnham Vincent. Pp. iv+81, $1.15, postpaid. 
; : VOLUME 2 : 
No. 6 The delayed reaction in animals and children. By Walter S. 
Hunter. Pp. v + 86, $1,15, postpaid. — 
No. 7 The Canada porcupine: a study of the learning process. By 
' Leroy Walter Sackett. Pp. iii + 84, $1.15, postpaid. 
No. 8 Audition and habit formation in the dog.. By Harry Miles John- 
, son. Pp. iv + 78, $1.00, postpaid. . 
No. 9 Habit formation in a strain of albino rats of less than normal 
brain weight. By Gardner Cheney Basset. Pp. iv + 46, 
; $.65, postpaid. 
No. 10 Distribution of ‘effort\in learning in the white rat. By John 
. Linck Ulrich. Pp. iii + 51, $.65, postpaid. 
No. 11. The effect of age on habit formation in .the albino rat. By 
Helen B..Hubbert. Pp. v + 55, $.65, postpaid. 
VOLUME 8 
No. 12 - The mental life of monkeys and apés: a study of ideational be- 
havior.» By Robert M. Yerkes. Pp. iv + 145, $1.50, postpaid. 
No. 13° A study of perseverance reactions in primates and rodents. ,; By , 
G. V. Hamilton. Pp. iv ++ 65. $.75, postpaid. 
No. 14 An analysis of the learning process in the snail, physa gyrina Say. 
By Elizabeth Lockwood, Thompson Pp. iii -+ 97, $1.25, 
postpaid. 


No..15 The effect of length of blind alleys on maze learning: An ex- 
periment on twenty-four white rats. By Joseph Peterson. 
Pp. iii + 53, $.75, postpaid. 


Regular subscription $5 per volume, postpaid. 
THE BEHAVIOR MONOGRAPHS 
Emerson Hall, . Cambridge, Mass. 


Directory of American Psychological Periodicals 


American Journal of Peychology—Worcester, Mass.: Florence Chandler. 
- Subscription $5. | 600 pages annually. Edited by G. Stanley Hall. 
Quarterly. General and experimental psychology. Fourided 1887. 


Pedagogical Seminary—Worcester, Mass.: Florence Chandler. 
Subscription $5. 575 pages annually. Edited by G. Stanley Hall. 
Quarterly. Pedagogy and educational psychology. Founded 1891. 


Psychological Review—Princeton, N. J.: ' Psychological Review Company. 
ubscniption (with Psychological Bulletin) $6. 480 pages annually. 
Bi-monthly. General. Founded 1894.. Edited by Howard.C. Warren. 


Psychological Bulletin—Princeton, N. J.: _Psychological Review Company. 
Subscription $3.25. 480 pages annually. Psychological literature. 
Monthly. Founded 1904. Edited by Shepherd I. Franz. . 


Psychological Monographs—Princeton, N. J.:_ Psychological Review Companv. 
, Subscription $4 per vol. 500 pages. Founded 1895. Ed. by James R. Angell. 
Published without fixed dates, each issue one or more researches. 


Psychological Index—Princeton, N. J.:_ Psychological Review Company. 
Subscription $1.25 200 pages. Founded 1895. Edited by Madison Bentley. 
An annual bibliography of psychological literature. 


Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods—New York: 
~ Sub-Station 84. Bi-weekly. 728 pages per volume. Founded 1904. 
Subscription $3. Edited by F. J. E. Woodbridge and Wendell T. Bush. 


‘Archives of Psychology—Substation 84, New York: Archives of Psychology. 
Subscription $5.. 600 pp. ann. Founded 1906. Edited by R. S. Woodworth. 
Published without fixed dates, each number a single experimental study. 


Journal of Abnormal Psychology—Boston: Richard G. Badge 
Subscription $4. 480 pages annually. Edited by Morton Princ 
Bi-monthly. Founded 1808. Entire field of abnormal psycholo 


Psychological Clinic—Philadclphia: . Psychological Clinic Press. 
ubscription $1.50. 280 pages annually. Edited by Lightner Witmer. 
Monthly (9 numbers). Orthogenics, psychology, hygiene. Founded 1907. 


Training School Bulletin—Vineland, N. J.: The Training School. 
Subscription $1. 160 pages annually. Ed. by E. R. Johnstone. Founded 1901. 
Monthly (10 numbers). Psychology and training of defectives. 


Journal of Race Development—Worcester, Mass.: Louis N. Wilson. 
Subscription:$3. 460 pages agnually. Founded 1910. 
Quarterly. Edited by George H. Blakeslee and G. Stanley Hall. 


Journal of Educational Psychology—Baltimore: Warwick & York. 
Subscription $3. 600 pages annually. Founded 1910. 
Monthly (10 numbers). Managing Editor, J. Carleton Bell. 
(Educational Psychology Monographs. Edited by Guy M. Whipple. 
Published separately at varying prices. Same publishers.) 


Journal of Animal Behavior—Cambridge, Mass.: Emerson Hall. 
Subscription $5. 450 pages annually. Founded 1911. 
Bi-monthly. Robert M. Yerkes, Managing Editor. 


The Behavior Monographs—Cambridge, Mass.: Emerson Hall. 
Subscription $5. 450 pages per volume. Edited by John B. Watson. _ 
Published without fixed dates, each number a single research. 


Psychoanalytic Review—New York: 64 West 56th Street. 
Subscription $5. 500 pages annually. Psychoanalysis. 
Quarterly, . Founded 1913. Ed. by W. A. White and S. E. Jelliffe. 


Journal of Experimental Psychology—Princeton, N. J.: Psychological Review 
Company. 480 pages annually. Experimental. Founded 1916. 
Subscription $3.25 . Bi-monthly. Edited by John B. Watson. 


Journal of Applied Psychology—Worcester, Mass.: Florence Chandler. * 
Subsea pling $4. 400 Ba annually. Quarterly. Founded 1917. 
Edited by G. Stanley all, John Wallace Baird and L. R. Geissler. 


Tux BRaNDow PRINTING Company 
ALBANY, N. ¥