Skip to main content

Full text of "Journal of experimental zoology"

See other formats


eee ees 
Sa 


4 hy ae 
it * iA 
ey : 


iit! 

et if tt { 

Bah ses 

f : a} i 
sane $! wid! 


tg 
Hy 


i 


* Pits ie 
He Nerd ath gst eine 
i i APR 
NHR eat 


Sag hy 
Fe 


2 Rete 
i y > 
A Ane va ‘ 

Sug ea 


VEER ca 
Ag 


hoa 


Wee: 


Sra 


neh nk a 
: As i 


rR ng 
ie 
Soe 


ee move 4: 
Sets 
re eS 
eae nd 
Poe pree 

Seo 

= 

PRES 


pRtete. 


: 

ya? 

fp hears 
a 


i 
ated 


hag 
ore tes 


rele 
SHAE it Ar, 


tag aye 
HMA Ta BY 
ety prise eeree 


Maan erin aigediia geet Kit 
Ne fh eat ; , f 
barry ti 5 
Rift: 


Aah aia 
Heal 

js 
Pigg 


0 
peta 
sae 


Se — 


Ae 
et 


fay 
rhe oa 
Hele 


sae) sh 
bie ti oA) 
ine i 


Pte 


Digitized by the Internet Archive 
in 2009 with funding from 
University of Toronto 


http://www.archive.org/details/journalofexperim03broo 


AeA TIN Ad. 


OF 


EXPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY 


EDITED BY 

WILLIAM K. BROOKS FRANK R. LILLIE 

Johns Hopkins University University of Chicago 
WILLIAM E. CASTLE JACQUES LOEB 

Harvard University University of California 
EDWIN G. CONKLIN THOMAS H. MORGAN 

University of Pennsylvania Columbia University 
CHARLES B. DAVENPORT GEORGE H. PARKER 

Carnegie Institution Harvard University 
HERBERT S. JENNINGS CHARLES O. WHITMAN 

Johns Hopkins University University of Chicago 

EDMUND B. WILSON, Columbia University 
AND 


ROSS G. HARRISON 
Johns Hopkins University 
Manacine EpIToR 


VOLUME III 


an 
THE JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY 
| BALTIMORE 


1906 


CONTENTS 


No. 1—February, 1906 
Epmunp B. WILson 
Studies on Chromosomes. III. The Sexual Differences of the Chromosome 
Groups in Hemiptera, with Some Considerations of the Determination 
and imhentance Gt ex. With ix PAPtTes cae koe teh se ne leks oo bye I 


Davin D. WHITNEY 
An Examination of the Effects of Mechanical Shocks and Vibrations upon 
theatre of Mevelopment of Pertiltzed (Peps. oma a) .o0 ss os oa 41 


Joun W. Scortr 
Morphology of the Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite. With 
Houmwaresandslive Hiotices ther Cbs aes ois ss ve 2 eye aes 49 


Cuartes R. SrocKaRD 
The Development of the Fundulus Heteroclitus in Solutions of Lithium 
Chlorid, with Appendix on its Development in Fresh Water. With 
Liner Md SEER TeS aera ee tea neareh  ie  ea ea Rm ee F 99 


E. A. ANDREWS 
Partial Regeneration of the Sperm-Receptacle in Crayfish. With Eleven 
PUIG SmI Sen eSNG. OS CA sed 4: mi ncke cs «antenna as wat cap eaysro ie aut 121 


A. J. GOLDFARB 
Experimental Study of Light as a Factor inthe Regeneration of Hydroids. 129 


No. 2—July, 1906 
FRANK R. LILLie 
Observations and Experiments Concerning the Elementary Phenomena 
of Embryonic Development in Chztopterus. With One Plate and 
Beveney-ciomtenipiresimit hed) Gxt r.. ..\.. 26: e.ciem sles at terete sree cick acts 153 


Littran V. Morcan 
Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians. With Seventeen Figures . 269 


Cuas. W. Harcitr 
Experiments on the Behavior of Tubicolous Annelids. With Three 
CRIES SY OSE AAAS 9S SC a era IRR Bae Sem a ee eS ans ea 295 


IsapEL McCracken 
Inheritance of Dichromatism in Lina and Gastroidea ...............-. 321 


No. 3—September, 1906 


Oris P. DELLINGER 
Locomotion of Amcebze and Allied Forms. With Two Plates and 


‘Twenty-nine Figures m the ext 2.1.2... ee hee ee ee ge 337 
S. O. Mast 
Light Reactions in Lower Organisms. I. Stentor Cceruleus. With Six 
Bipures |. 2: ali sceg mee ie heey ecein Bin > im oe wins ot ie elcl aed eee 359 


G. H. ParKER 
The Influence of Light and Heat on the Movement of the Melanophore 


Pigment, especially in Lizards. With Three Figures ............... 401 


AtFrrep G. Mayer AND CAROLINE G. SOULE 
Some Reactions\ot Caterpillars amd: Moths)... 21... ocisjereie lessee ie 415 


H. S. JENNINGS 
Modifiability in Behavior. II. Factors Determining Direction and Charac- 


ter of Movement.in the Earthworm. .2...0. 02. sachs #gee ss -eker 435 


No. 4—December, 1906 
T. H. Morcan 
The Physiology of Regeneration. With Seven Figures ................ 457 
Hydranth Formation and Polarity in Tubularia............-...-..--.- 501 


D. H. Tennent anp M. J. HoGue 
Studies on the Development of the Starfish Egg. With Five Plates. ..... 517 


GeorGE L. STREETER 
Some Experiments on the Developing Ear Vesicle of the Tadpole with 


Relation to Equilibration. With Twelve Figures .................. 543 
C. M. Cuiip 
The Relation Between Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation .... 559 


W. N. Now in 
Study of the Spermatogenesis of Coptocycla Aurichalcea and Coptocycla 
Guttata, with Especial Reference to the Problem of Sex-Determination. 


With Two'Plates 50 4 ciie aot ayaa tere aoe tence Oo ees ae 583 


Victor E. EMMEL 
Torsion and Other Transitional Phenomena in the Regeneration of the 


Cheliped of the Lobster (Homarus Americanus). With Two Plates .... 603 


Eurayia V. WALLING 
The Influences of Gases and Temperature on the Cardiac and Respira- 
tory Movements in the Grasshopper a2q-4-.2c2 a. eee eee 621 


STUDIES ON CHROMOSOMES 


Ill. THE SEXUAL DIFFERENCES OF THE CHROMOSOME- 
GROUPS IN HEMIPTERA, WITH SOME CONSIDERA- 
TIONS ON THE DETERMINATION AND INHERI- 
ANGE \OF SEX 


EDMUND B. WILSON 


Wirth Six Ficures 


Since the time of Henking’s able paper on the spermatogenesis 
of Pyrrochoris (’g1), it has been known that in certain Hemiptera, 
and in some other insects, a dimorphism exists in the nuclear con- 
stitution of the spermatozoa, one-half of them containing the so- 
called “accessory” or “heterotropic” chromosome, while in the 
other half this chromosome is lacking. ‘The meaning of this fact 
has hitherto remained undetermined. McClung in 1902 devel- 
oped an hypothesis of sex-production based on the conjecture that 
the heterotropic chromosome is a sex-determinant, and more 
specifically that spermatozoa containing this chromosome produce 
males, for the very obvious, yet fallacious, reason that it 1s present 
in the male. This hypothesis was based simply on the fact 
that the spermatozoa are of two numerically equal classes, like 
the sexes of the adults; and it was apparently overthrown by 
subsequent observation. ‘The hypothesis implied that the cells 
of the female must contain one chromosome less than those of 
the male; and although McClung did not specifically place his 
assumption in this form, he considered it extremely improbable 
that the accessory chromosome, or “any such element,” is present 
in the egg. Sutton (02) believed that he had found a 
confirmation of this in the grasshopper Brachystola, where he 
showed that the number in the male (spermatogonia) 1s twenty- 
three, and stated that in the female (odgomia and _follicle- 


Journat or ExperIMENTAL Zoétocy, Vor. 11, No. 1. 


2 Edmund B. Wilson 


cells) the number is twenty-two, supporting this statement by a 
single figure (op. cit., Fig. 11). Sutton was, however, able to 
examine only a very few of the female groups, and the object is 
an unfavorable one as compared with the Hemiptera, owing to 
the less compact form of the chromosomes. McClung’s hypothe- 
sis seemed to be rendered completely untenable by the later obser- 
vations of Montgomery on Anasa (04), and of Gross on Syromas- 
tes ('04), both these authors describing and clearly figuring the 
same number of chromosomes (twenty-two) in the male and the. 
female cells. Gross and Wallace (’05) were thus independently 
led to the conclusion that only one of the two classes of spermat- 
ozoa was functional, namely, that in which the heterotropic 
chromosome is present. Those of the other class were assumed 
to degenerate after the fashion of polar bodies. 

I am now able to bring forward decisive proof that the appar- 
ently adverse evidence brought forward by Montgomery and | 
Gross was based on errors of observation, and that the sexes in 
Hemiptera of this type do in fact show a constant difference in 
the number of chromosomes. As far as these animals are con- 
cerned, however, McClung’s conjecture as to the mode of fertili- 
zation proves to have been the reverse of the truth; for it is the 
female, not the male, that possesses the additional chromosome, 
as I have determined beyond all doubt in four genera, namely, 
Anasa, Alydus, Harmostes and Protenor. The facts leave no 
doubt that both forms of spermatozoa are functional; that all of 
the eggs possess the same number of chromosomes; that all con- 
tain the homologue, or maternal mate, of the accessory or hetero- 
tropic chromosome of the male; and that fertilization by sper- 
matozoa that possess this chromosome produces females, while 
males are produced upon fertilization by spermatozoa that do 
not possess it. 

A second type of dimorphism of the nuclei of the spermatozoa 
was made known in the first of these studies. In this type all 
of the spermatozoa contain the same number of chromosomes, but 
half of them contain a large “idiochromosome” and the other 
half a corresponding small one. | was led in that paper to suggest 
the possibility that the idiochromosomes might play a definite 


Studies on Chromosomes 2 


role in sex-production, but could at that time produce no evidence 
in support of the suggestion. I have now the evidence to show 
that this suggestion was in accordance with the facts; for in at 
least four genera, Lygzeus, Euschistus, Coenus and Podisus, both 
sexes show the same number of chromosomes, but the small 
idiochromosome is present only in the male. Somewhat earlier, 
and independently, Stevens (05) determined a precisely similar 
fact in the case of a beetle, Tenebrio, which indicates that the 
phenomenon is of wide occurrence in the insects. ‘These results 
confirm the correctness of my conclusion that the heterotropic 
or “‘accessory” chromosome has become unpaired in the male 
sex through the disappearance in that sex of its mate, and give a 
complete explanation of the fact that in forms possessing the 
heterotropic chromosome the male number is odd and one less 
than the female number. I believe that these facts may give 
_ the basis for a general theory of sex-production. 


I. DESCRIPTIVE 
A. General Character of the Chromosome-grou ps 


In two preceding papers (Wilson, ’05, 1; 05, 3,) (where due 
acknowledgment is made to previous observers in this field) 
I have described in some detail the general nature of the chro- 
mosomes in these insects. For such an investigation as the present 
one, the Hemiptera present peculiar advantages, owing above all 
to the short and regular form of the chromosomes, and the relative 
lack of crowding in the equatorial plate. | have employed almost 
exclusively Flemming’s strong fluid as a fixative, staining the 
sections with iron-hematoxylin and extracting until the cytoplasm 
is nearly or quite colorless. “The best preparations thus obtained 
leave nothing to be desired in point of brilliancy and clearness, 
and show the chromosomes with a distinctness that is hardly 
exaggerated by the black and white figures here reproduced. 
The very large number of sections now at my disposal (including 
all those of Paulmier and a still greater number of new prepara- 
tions of my own) has enabled me in the case of nearly every 
species to examine numerous division-figures (of which only the 


4 Edmund B. Wilson 


best have been selected for illustration) and to satisfy myself thor- 
oughly of the constancy of the relations as described. Everyone 
familiar with such objects will, however, realize that in regard to 
such matters as the arrangement and size-differences of the 
chromosomes certain apparent variations appear that are due to 
slight differences in the form and position of the chromosomes, 
and to the various degrees of foreshortening thus caused. This 


introduces a slight error, into both the observations and the draw- 


ings, that can hardly be avoided. A second source of error lies 
in the degree of extraction, which produces surprising variations 
in the apparent size of the chromosomes—I have found, for 
instance, that by successive extraction the chromosomes may be 
reduced almost to one-half their original apparent size, and the 
smaller chromosomes may thus be caused almost to disappear from 
view. Camera drawings at successive stages of the extraction show, 
however, that the relative sizes of the chromosomes remain sub- 
stantially unchanged, and the comparison of the same object after 
a shorter and a longer extraction has thus, in a number of cases, 
given a more certain result than could otherwise have been 
obtained. I have, whenever it was possible, figured different 
stages of the same species from the same slide, so as to avoid the 
error due to different degrees of extraction; but this is not always 
possible, since as a rule longer extraction is required to give a 
perfectly clear view of the spermatogonial groups than 1s desirable 
for the spermatocyte-divisions. For the comparison of the two 
sexes, different slides must of course be used, and to this is due, 
I am sure, some of the size-differences between the odgonial and 
spermatogonial groups that appear in the figures. — 

Making all due allowance for the sources of error mentioned, 
it remains perfectly clear that the chromosomes in each species 
show among themselves constant and characteristic size-differ- 
ences; and further, that with the special exceptions in the male 
described beyond, the chromosomes of the unreduced groups 
(1. e., those of the o6gonia and spermatogonia) may be paired off, 
two by two, to form equal or symmetrical pairs. ‘The pairing of 
the chromosomes is most evident in the case of especially small 
chromosomes (such as the m-chromosomes of Anasa, Alydus, 


Studies on Chromosomes 5 


Harmostes, etc., or the small pair of ordinary chromosomes of 
Coenus and Euschistus, described beyond) or especially large ones 
such as the largest pair in Alydus, and in some of the species of 
Euschistus. Those of intermediate size are also obviously paired 
in some of the forms (e. g., in Protenor, Fig. 1); but in many ot 
the species the several pairs are not sufficiently marked in size to 
admit of certain recognition. Nevertheless, a comparative study 
of many species has convinced me of the correctness of the con- 
clusion, first indicated by Montgomery (’01) and afterward more 
fully worked out by Sutton (02), that all the chromosomes (again 
with the special exceptions referred to above) may be thus paired, 
and that the chromosome-group as a whole includes two parallel 
series of chromosomes that undoubtedly represent respectively 
the descendants of those that originally are brought together in 
the union of the gametes. This is very clearly brought out by 
making camera drawings of the chromosomes, and arranging 
them as nearly as practicable in pairs of equal size. “This arrange- 
ment conspicuously shows the sexual differences, as may be seen 
by a comparison of Figs. 2, / and 4 (Anasa) and 5,c and g (Lygzus). 
There is, of course, a large error to be allowed for in the series 
as thus arranged, and no pretense to complete accuracy in the 
selection of the members of most of the pairs can be made. 
Nevertheless, when all due allowance for differences of form, 
foreshortening and the like is made, the fact that such a double 
series exists is unmistakable. When it is borne in mind that the 
spermatid-nuclei in each case contain a single series of chromo- 
somes showing the same size-relations (c}. for instance, Figs. 1, ), 
Ed. 92,.4,d,€; 3,4, ¢,],; 4, b,j, d, b), it becomes in a high depree 
probable that the corresponding pairs of the somatic groups con- 
sist each of a paternal and a maternal member, in accordance 
with Montgomery’s original and fundamental assumption (’01). 
As may be seen bya comparison of the figures, the members of 
each pair when in their natural position, do not as a rule lie in 
juxtaposition but may occupy any relative position. Only at 
the period of synapsis do they actually couple, two by two, to 
form the bivalents whose members are subsequently separated 
by the reducing division. 


6 Edmund B. Wilson 


In order to give a wider basis of comparison | have given new 
figures of the chromosome-groups of nearly all the species, 
even in the case of forms already figured in my preceding papers. 
Since the idiochromosomes or the heterotropic chromosome form 
the distinctive differential between the nuclei of the two sexes, I 
shall in the descriptive part of this paper call them the “ differen- 
tial chromosomes. ”’ 


B. First T ype. Forms Possessing an “Accessory” or Hetero- 


tropic Chromosome 


As stated above, I have compared the males and females in 
respect to the chromosome-groups in four genera, selecting for 
this purpose the most available cells, which are the dividing 
oogonia and ovarian follicle-cells in the female, the spermato- 
gonia and investing cells of the testis-cysts in the male. ‘The 
general result is the same in all, but owing to the conspicuous 
size-difference of the chromosomes in Protenor, this form gives 
the most obvious and striking evidence.! 


a. Protenor belfragei 


Montgomery (or) first made known the general character of 
the chromosome-groups in this interesting species, showing that 
the spermatogonial groups show an odd number, thirteen, that 
the heterotropic chromosome (Montgomery’s “chromosome x’’) 
is immediately recognizable by its enormous size—tt 1s fully twice 
the size of the largest of the other chromosomes—and that it is 
unpaired (though he considered it a bivalent). My own observa- 
tion confirms his description in every point, except that I have 
never seen this chromosome transversely constricted into two 
halves. ‘The first glance at a good preparation of the spermat- 
ogonial metaphase, as seen in polar view, shows this huge chro- 


’There can be no doubt of the identification of the follicle-cells; but there is some uncertainty regard- 
ing the cells here called odgonia, since they are from the undifferentiated region of the ovary in which the 
distinction between odgonia and follicle-cells cannot be made out. It is therefore quite possible that some 
of the groups here described as odgonia may be from very young follicle-cells or nutritive cells; but this 
does not affect the main result. 


Studies on Chromosomes Z 


mosomed a sa long worm-shaped body obviously without a mate, 
(fis. 1, dj). The remaining twelve chromosomes may _ be 
grouped in symmetrical pairs (indicated by numbers ebony, 


y ee °*e 


a h (fs 


= 


/ 


oO 
Ss 


Ficure 1 


Protenor belfragei—a, Anaphase of second spermatocyte-division; 5, c, sister groups, from the 
same spindle, polar view, second spermatocyte-division; d, e, f, spermatogonial groups; g, h, groups 


from immature ovaries, probably odgonia; i, group from dividing follicle-cell. 


d, e), though the members of each pair may occupy any relative 
position. Of these six pairs, one (2, 2) is always much larger than 
the others, its members being approximately half the size of the 


1All the figures are drawn to the same scale. In all, # denotes the heterotropic chromosome, i the 
idiochromosomes (large and small in some cases lettered J and 1 respectively), m the paired micro- 


chromosomes, and s the smallest pair of ordinary chromosomes. 


8 Edmund B. Wilson 


heterotropic. A second pair (3, 3) may usually be distinguished 
as the next largest, and a third pair (7, 7) as the smallest, though 
this is not always obvious. [This pair probably correspond 
to the “m-chromosomes” of my preceding paper. ‘The remain- 
ing three pairs are of nearly equal size, though sometimes they 
clearly show a progressively graded series as in Fig. 1, d, e. In 
synapsis the six paired chromosomes become coupled, as usual, 
to form six corresponding bivalents, while the large chromosome 
remains as an unpaired univalent. During the whole growth- 
period of the spermatocytes this chromosome remains in a con- 
densed spheroidal state, forming a very large chromosome- 
nucleolus. In the prophases of the first division it again elongates 
and divides longitudinally in this division. Each secondary 
spermatocyte accordingly receives seven chromosomes. In the 
second division six of these (the products of the bivalents) again 
divide equally, while the seventh (the large chromosome) passes 
undivided to one pole (Fig. 1, a). One-half of the spermatid 
nuclei accordingly receive six chromosomes, the other half seven, 
the additional one being the large heterotropic chromosome 
(Fie anh ve) 

In the female the chromosome-groups of the dividing odgonia 
and follicle-cells appear with a clearness not inferior to that shown 
in the spermatogonial groups (Fig. 1, g-z). Itis at once apparent 
that in these groups there are two very large chromosomes, equal 
in size, in place of the single one that appears in the male, while 
the remaining chromosomes show the same relations as in the 
male. ‘There are accordingly fourteen chromosomes in all, which 
may be equally paired off, two by two, and no chromosome is 
without a mate of corresponding size. Since the largest two are 
of the same relative size as the single heterotropic chromosome 
of the male, it is quite clear that one of them must have been 
derived from a spermatozo6n containing this chromosome, while 
the other is its maternal mate or homologue. 

I have not been able to follow by actual observation the phe- 
nomena of reduction, maturation and fertilization in the egg; 
but the data are sufhcient to show, with a degree of probability 
only short of certainty, what must be the history of the chromo- 


Studies on Chromosomes 9 


somes in these processes. Since the odgonia contain fourteen 
equally paired chromosomes, synapsis in the odcyte must result 
in the formation of seven symmetrical bivalents—z. e., seven 
couples of equal chromosomes—and each egg after maturation 
contains seven univalent chromosomes, one of which is the 
maternal representative or mate of the heterotropic chromosome 
of the male. ‘This group contains one chromosome of each of the 
original pairs, and is precisely similar to the group present in 
those spermatozoa that contain the heterotropic chromosome 
(Fig. 1, c). Fertilization by such a spermatozoa doubles this 
group, giving the condition observed in the female—z. e¢., fourteen 
chromosomes equally paired, the largest pair consisting of the 
heterotropic chromosome and its maternal mate (J, 7, Fig. 1, g, /). 
Fertilization by a spermatozoon that lacks the heterotropic chro- 
mosome will give the condition observed in the male, namely, 
thirteen chromosomes, of which twelve are equally paired, while 
the thirteenth is the large unpaired one which is obviously derived 
from the egg. ‘There is therefore no escape from the conclusion 
that both forms of spermatozoa are functional, that females are 
produced upon fertilization by spermatozoa that contain, and 
males upon fertilization by spermatozoa that lack, the hetero- 
tropic chromosome. Since the two classes of spermatozoa are 
equal in number, fertilization will in the long run produce males 
and females in approximately equal numbers. 


b. Anasa tristis 


A comparison of the nuclei of the two sexes in this species gives 
a precisely concordant result, though the size-differences do not 
allow of so exact an identification of the differential chromo- 
somes. In the preceding study I showed that the number of 
chromosomes in the male (spermatogonia) is twenty-one, not 
twenty-two as stated by previous observers. Study of the sper- 
matogonial metaphase groups shows that twenty of the chro- 
mosomes may be equally paired, two by two, while the remaining 
one is, of course, without a mate (Fig. 2, e, 7). “The unpaired 
heterotropic chromosome is one of three largest chromosomes, 


IO Edmund B. Wilson 


but which particular one cannot be determined by simple inspec- 
tion, since the three are of nearly equal size. In synapsis two 
of these large chromosomes unite to form the largest of the ten 
bivalents (1, Fig. 2, a) that appear in the first spermatocyte 
division. ‘The third, which retains its compact form as a chro- 
mosome-nucleus during the growth-period, remains as the univ- 
alent heterotropic chromosome (/, Fig. 2, a). ‘The first spermat- 
ocyte division accordingly shows eleven chromosomes, ten of 
which are bivalent, and one (heterotropic) is univalent. ‘The 
distribution of these chromosomes in the maturation-division takes 
the usual course, the heterotropic chromosome dividing equally 
with the ten bivalents in the first mitosis while its products pass 
undivided to one pole of the spindle in the second (Fig. 2, 6). 
Half the spermatozoa accordingly receive ten chromosomes, one 
of which (z, Fig. 2, c) is larger than the others, and half an exactly 
similar group plus the large heterotropic chromosome, or eleven . 
meall(Pig.22, 2). 

The o6gonial groups show invariably twenty-two chromosomes, 
which may be arranged in eleven equal pairs (Fig. 2, g, b). In 
place of the three large chromosomes of the spermatogonial 
groups appear four Senior chromosomes, forming two equal 
pairs. ‘Iwo of these four are obviously the large chromosome, 
common to all the spermatozoa, and its maternal mate, while 
the other two must be the heterotropic chromosome (derived in 
fertilization from the spermatozoon) with its maternal mate. 
It is, therefore, clear that all of the matured eggs must contain 
eleven chromosomes, that females are produced upon fertilization 
by those spermatozoa that contain a similar group—1. e., by those 
containing the heterotropic—males upon fertilization by spermat- 
ozoa that lack the heterotropic. 

The ovarian follicle-cells often show chromosome-groups 
identical with those of the odgonia (Fig. 2, 7). Not infrequently, 
however, the number of chromosomes is much greater, and the 
same is true of the nuclei of the investing cells of the ovary, of 
the oviduct and of the fat-body. In the male similar multiple 
groups are not uncommon in the interstitial and investing cells 
of the testis. Only in a single case have I succeeded in gaining 


Studies on Chromosomes rn 


“Get eoscos 


fs & en 


tg 
: @eees ode: 
Gbacccecee: 


O 
T 
. 


9 § 


h 
% ra a) 
J ae ye ie ese" ; 


FIGURE 2 


Anasa tristis.—a, Metaphase of first spermatocyte-division, in polar view, showing the nine large 
bivalents in a ring, the univalent heterotropic chromosome below it, and the m-chromosome bivalent 
in the center; b, anaphase of second division; c, d, sister-groups from the same spindle, polar view, 
second division (1 the macrochromosome); e, spermatogonial group; f, the same chromosomes arranged 
in pairs; g, odgonial group from a larva; h, the same group arranged in pairs; i, spermatogonial group; 


j, group from a dividing follicle-cell; k, double group, from a cell toward the periphery of a larval ovary. 


12 Edmund B. Wilson 


a clear and complete view of such a group; but this one case 
suffices to give, with great probability, the explanation of the 
increased number of chromosomes. In this case every chro- 
mosome of the metaphase group may be clearly seen, and the 
number is exactly twice the odgonial number, namely, forty-four 
(Fig. 2, k). Careful study clearly shows that this group contains 
four microchromosomes and eight macrochromosomes, in each 
case twice the number of those present in the odgonia. ‘This 
leaves no doubt that in this case all the chromosomes have divided 
once without the occurrence of a cytoplasmic division, and makes 
it probable that the increase in number in the cells in question 1s 
always due to a process of this kind. I have not been able to 
obtain faultless preparations of the dividing cells of other tissues, 
and can only state that in the ectodermal cells of the larva the 
number of chromosomes is approximately the same as in the 
oogonia. The multiple chromosome-groups were only observed 
in the cells mentioned above, all of which, it may be observed, 
are degenerating or highly specialized cells. 


c. Alydus pilosulus 


Despite the small number of chromosomes ( 2 14, & 13, as in 
Protenor) this genus is in some respects less favorable for detailed 
analysis than either of the ones described above, for the size of 
the heterotropic chromosome does not distinguish it sufficiently from 
the other chromosomes to allow of its certain identification in the 
spermatogonia. ‘lhe main fact appears, however, as clearly as 
in Protenor or Anasa that the female has one more chromosome 
than the male. 

In polar views of the second spermatocyte-division this species 
shows the sister spermatid-groups with great beauty, one having 
six chromosomes and one seven (Fig. 3, e, 7). These chromo- 
somes show at least five distinguishable sizes that are constant, 
namely, (1) a largest; (2) an extremely small one (m-chromo- 
some); (3) a second smallest (the heterotropic); (4) a second 
largest, and (5) three others intermediate in size between (3) and 
(4), one of which is frequently a little larger than the other two. 


Studies on Chromosomes t3 


The sister groups are practically exact duplicates save for the 
heterotropic which varies considerably in appearance as seen from 
the pole owing to foreshortening (cj. the side-views given in my 
preceding paper). The spermatogonia correspondingly show 
always thirteen chromosomes (Fig. 3, a), of which the largest and 
the smallest pair are at once distinguishable. Next follow four 
chromosomes nearly equal in size, two of them often appreciably 
smaller than the other two. Of the remaining five, one must be 
the unpaired heterotropic; but, as already stated, it cannot be 
positively identified by inspection. Closely similar groups may 


be) j 
Ge un 
G 

h 
e. <0. 
e2®@ @ 6 


2 


FIGURE 3 


Alydus pilosulus.—a, Spermatogonial group; b, group from a dividing investing cell of the testis; 
c, odgonial group; d, from a dividing cell of an egg-follicle; e, f, two pairs of sister-groups, each from a 
single spindle, anaphase of second spermatocyte-division, in polar view. 


occasionally be found in dividing cells of the enveloping cells of 
the testis (Fig. 3,0) 0 Whether fran groups occur like those 
described in Anasa, I cannot say. 

The dividing oégonia and follicle-cells, of which a large number 
have been observed, always show fourteen chromosomes that may 
be arranged in seven equal pairs (Fig. 3, c, d). As in the sper- 
matogonia, the largest and the smallest pair are usually at once 
recognizable, and also the four second largest. The remaining 
six, of nearly equal size, must of course include the heterotropic 
chromosome and its maternal mate. 


14 Edmund B. Wilson 


d. Harmostes reflexulus 


My material of this species is much less abundant than that of 
the three preceding, and the preparations are not of the same 
excellence. “hey nevertheless show beyond doubt that the num- 
bers are here the same as in Protenor and Alydus, viz., thirteen in 
the male and fourteen in the female. In my sections of both sexes 
the chromosomes appear less regular in contour than in the other 
species examined (probably owing to somewhat defective fixation). 
They show clearly, however, in both sexes a largest pair and a 
smallest (m-chromosomes), as in the other forms. 


C. Second Type. Forms Possessing Unequal Idiochromosomes 


The sexual differences of these forms have been worked out in 
Lygzeus turcicus, five species of Euschistus (variolarius, ictericus, 
tristigmus, fissilis and servus), Coenus delius and Podisus spinosus. 
In the last named species the unreduced number is sixteen, in the 
others fourteen. In all, the number of chromosomes is the same 
in both sexes, but while the males show a large and a small idio- 
chromosome, the females show two large idiochromosomes that 
are equally paired. ‘This difference clearly appears in all the 
species examined but is most conspicuous in Euschistus vario- 
larius, E. ictericus and Lygzeus turcicus, where the inequality 
of the idiochromosomes is most marked. The relative size of 
the idiochromosomes varies somewhat (perhaps owing to differ- 
ences in the degree of extraction of the dye) but on the whole is 
characteristic of the different species, as described below. 

In all of the species of Euschistus examined, and in Coenus 
delius, a largest and a smallest pair of ordinary chromosomes 
(the latter marked s in some of the figures) are readily distinguish- 
able. “hese give rise to corresponding large and small biv Alents 
in the first mitosis, and are recognizable as single chromosomes 
in the spermatid-groups (Figs. 4, 5). The small chromosomes 
are in every case smaller than the large idiochromosome, and in 
Mineus bioculatus (Fig. 4, p, g) are actually smaller than the small 
idiochromosome. It is possible that this pair of chromosomes 


Studtes On Chromosomes [5 


FIGURE 4 


Euschistus, Mineus.—a, E. variolarius, second spermatocyte-division; b, sister-groups, second 
e, second spermatocyte-division, E. tristigmus; f, g, 


h, i, corresponding views of E. servus; 
E. fissilis; p, Mineus bioculatus, 


division; c, d, corresponding views of E. servus; 
E. variolarius, spermatogonial and odgonial groups respectively ; 


j,k, the same, E. ictericus; /, m, the same, E. tristigmus; ,0,the same, 


second spermatocyte-division; q, sister-groups, from the same spindle, second division. 


16 Edmund B. Wilson 


may correspond to the microchromosomes, or m-chromosomes, 
that are so characteristic of the first type (7, in Figs. 2, 3). 


e. Euschistus 


In E. variolarius the inequality of the idiochromosomes (Fig. 4, 
a) is greater than in any other of the observed forms excepting 
Lygzeus turcicus. ‘The sister spermatid-groups (Fig. 4, 6) consist 
in each case of a ring of six ordinary chromosomes with the idio- 
chromosome near its center. In the outer ring may be distin- 
guished as a rule four or five different sizes of chromosomes, the 
largest and smallest (s) being always recognizable, and usually 
also a second largest and second smallest. “The large idiochro- 
mosome 1s imers distinctly larger than the smallest chromosome 
(s) of the outer ring, while the anal idiochromosome is very much 
smaller than Bear and in long extracted preparations looks 
exactly like a centrosome. ‘The spermatogonial groups corre- 
spondingly show seven pairs of chromosomes (Fig. 4, 7), of which 
the small idiochromosome, the smallest pair of ordinary chro- 
mosomes, and two large pairs are recognizable. “The remaining 
seven include three equal pairs, while the seventh is the large 
idiochromosome, but it is impossible to identify this chromosome 
more nearly. ‘The odgonial groups show fourteen equally paired 
chromosomes, as shown in Fig. 4, g; but my preparations do not 
show this so well in this species as in the others. 

E. ictericus shows a similar spermatogonial group (Fig. 4, 7) 
except that the small idiochromosome is relatively a little larger 
and the small pair of ordinary chromosomes but slightly smaller 
than the others. The odgonial groups (Fig. 4, &, an unusually 
open specimen) very clearly show the absence of the small idio- 
chromosome, but the equal pairing of the chromosomes is less 
obvious than in the following species. 

In E. tristigmus (Fig. 4, e, /, m) the small idiochromosome is 
relatively much larger than in the foregoing species, while in 
E. servus, it is usually a little larger still (Fig. 4, c,d, b). In both 
these forms the smallest pair of ordinary chromosomes are at once 
recognizable in the spermatogonia (s, Fig. 4, h, /) and the equal 
pairing of the others is evident. In E. servus the o6gonial groups 


Studies on Chromosomes 17 


show the equal pairing of all the chromosomes with equal clear- 
ness, the absence of the small idiochromosome being evident 
(Fig. 4, 7). The small pair (s) evidently correspond to ‘the small 
pair in the male (4, /) and the large idiochromosome-pair must 
therefore be represented by one of the larger pairs. Fig. 4, 1, 0, 
show the spermatogonial and odgonial groups of E. fissilis, show- 
ing the same relations as in E. servus, save that the small pair are 
relatively larger. 

The above-described species of Euschistus, while agreeing pre- 
cisely in the general relations, present individual differences so 
marked as to show that even the species of a single genus may be 
distinguishable by the chromosome-groups. In this case the 
most interesting feature is the series shown in the inequality 
of the idiochromosomes, which becomes progressively greater 
in the series (1) E. servus, (2) tristigmus, fissilis, (3) ictericus, 
(4). variolarius, the inequality in the last case being fully as great 
as in Lygeus. I may again mention the fact that in the opposite 
direction the genus Brochymena often shows the idiochromosomes 
less unequal than in E. servus; in Mineus they are sometimes 
of nearly equal size (Fig. 4, p, 7), while in Nezara no inequality 
exists. Practically all intermediate conditions are therefore shown 
within the limits of a single family between the extreme inequality 
shown in E. variolarius and no inequality at all. It is quite clear 
from the observations here brought forward that this progressive 
differentiation has occurred only in the male sex, as I conjectured 
in my first paper. 


7. Ccenus delius 


The relations in this form are so closely similar to those seen 
in Euschistus servus or fissilis, as described above, as hardly to 
require separate description. Fig. 5, b, shows the spermatogonial 
metaphase- “group; 5, 1, the corresponding odgonial group. Both 
these preparations show very clearly the eel pair (s) of ordinary 
chromosomes (not so well shown in the figure of the spermat- 
ogonial group in my first paper). Here, as in Euschistus, it is 
evident that the large idiochromosome is much larger than the 
membérs of the small pair. 


18 Edmund B. Wilson 


. Lygzeus turcicus 
S Ve 


In this species the inequality of the idiochromosomes is nearly 
or quite as great as in Euschistus variolarius, but the differentia- 
tion of the chromosome-pairs is less marked than in that species, 
and the small pair cannot be distinguished with certainty in any 
of the stages. In the spermatogonial groups, accordingly, only 
the ‘small idiochromosome is markedly smaller than the others 
(Fig. 534 d); and hence its lack of an equal mate is rendered very 
conspicuous. In the female the small idiochromosome is absent 
as usual and all the chromosomes are equally paired (Fig. 5, 7, ¢). 
The idiochromosomes cannot be distinguished from the ordinary 
chromosomes. 


hb. Podisus spinosus 


In this species both sexes show sixteen chromosomes. In the 
spermatogonial groups (of which I am now able to give a better 
figure than the one in my first paper) the small idiochromosome 
appears relatively larger than in any of the foregoing species, 
though still not more than half the size of any of the others 
(Fig. 5, 7). In the female (follicle-cells, Fig. 5, &) all the chro- 
mosomes are equally paired and the small idiochromosome 1s 
absent, but owing to the relatively large size of the latter in the 
male the chromosome-groups of the two sexes do not show so 
obvious a contrast as in the foregoing cases. 


Resumé and Conclusions Regarding the Second T ype 


In all the forms described under this type the two sexes show 
the same number of chromosomes but differ in that the male 
groups include a large and a small idiochromosome while the 
female groups have two large idiochromosomes of equal size. 
This result agrees with that Acad reached by Stevens (’05) in 
the case of the beetle Tenebrio, and involves the same conclusions 
that she has indicated. Since all the chromosomes of the odgonial 
groups are equally paired, it is evident that all the matured eggs 
must contain half such a group, one of the chromosomes being 
the maternal representative, or mate, of the large idiochromosome 


Studies on Chromosomes 19 


o mM 


FIGURE 5 


Lygeus, Coenus, Podisus, Nezara——a, Lygeus turcicus, second spermatocyte-division; 5, sister- 
groups, second division; c, d, spermatogonial groups; e, the chromosomes of d arranged in pairs; 


f, odgonial group; g, the same in pairs; h,7,Ccenus delius, spermatogonial and follicle-cell groups; 


I, m, Nezara hilaris, spermatogonial and odgonial groups respectively. 


20 Edmund B. Wilson 


of the male. Fertilization of such an egg by a spermatozoon con- 
taining the small idiochromosome will produce a group identical 
with that occurring in the male; fertilization by one containing 
the large idiochromosome will produce the characteristic female 
group. This result is thoroughly consistent with that obtained 
in the first type; for if the small idiochromosome be supposed to 
disappear in the male, the phenomena become in every respect 
identical with those occurring in the first type. The large idio- 
chromosome is therefore undoubtedly homologous with the 
heterotropic chromosome, and the latter owes its unpaired 
character to the fact that its former paternal mate has vanished, 
as I conjectured in my first paper. 

It is further evident that in synapsis, in both sexes, the members 
of each chromosome-pair become coupled to form symmetrical 
bivalents, except in case of the idiochromosomes of the male. 
In this case alone do chromosomes of unequal size couple to form 
an asymmetrical bivalent; and it is a consequence of this coupling 
that the subsequent distribution allots the small idiochromosome 
to one-half of the spermatozoa and the large one to the other half. 


D. Third Type. Forms in which the Idiochromosomes are 
of Equal Size 


Of these forms I have been able to examine only a single case, 
namely, that of Nezara hilaris; and in the course of a whole 
summer's collecting I obtained but a single female in the proper 
stage to show the odgonial divisions. Forttnately both ovaries 
show a considerable number of division-figures which demonstrate 
the facts with perfect clearness. 

A particular interest attaches to this form on account of the 
fact, described in my first paper, that the idiochromosomes are of 
equal size and hence give no visible differential between the two 
classes of spermatozoa. ‘This form gives therefore a test case con- 
cerning my general conclusion that the differentiation of the 
idiochromosomes has occurred only in the male; for since these 
chromosomes are here alike in all the spermatozoa, it might with 
some plausibility be assumed that the differentiation had in this 


Studies on Chromosomes 21 


species taken place in the female. ‘The facts conclusively show 
that such is not the case. 

The spermatogonial groups (Fig. 5, /) show fourteen chromo- 
somes, all of which may be symmetrically paired. ‘The smallest 
pair, 7, 7, (as | showed in my first paper) are the idiochromosomes 
as is shown by their characteristic behavior during the growth- 
period and in the maturation-divisions. In synapsis the twelve 
larger chromosomes couple to form six bivalents, while the idio- 
chromosomes divide as separate univalents in the first spermat- 
ocyte-division. Their products then conjugate as usual to form 
the idiochromosome-dyad, which differs from all the forms hitherto 
observed in being composed of two equal members. All the 
spermatid-nuclei are accordingly exactly similar in appearance 
and no visible dimorphism exists (cf. Fig. 4 of my first paper, 
Wilson, ’05, 1). We should accordingly expect to find the 
odgonial groups exactly similar to the spermatogonial; and such 
is clearly shown to be the fact by the preparations, the odgonial 
groups showing fourteen equally paired chromosomes among 
which the idiochromosomes are readily recognizable by their 
small size (Fig. 5, 7). 

In this case, therefore, alone among all those examined, no 
visible differences are shown by the nuclei of the two sexes. 
One pair of the chromosomes are, however, different in nature 
from the others, as is shown by their different behavior in the 
male in the growth-period and in synapsis; and it is quite clear 
that the two members of this pair are always assigned to different 
spermatozoa. In respect to this chromosome, therefore, the 
spermatozoa fall into two classes as truly as the other forms, 
though they cannot be distinguished by the eye. It is hardly 
necessary to point out how important this case is in giving a firm 
basis of comparison with the more usual forms in which, if we can 
trust the existing accounts, all of the functional spermatozoa are 
exactly alike in appearance, and no sexual differences of the chro- 
mosome-groups are apparent. 


993) Edmund B. Wilson 


Ek. The Differential Chromosomes in the Synaptic and Growth- 
periods 


I will now briefly consider a very marked difference between 
the sexes in respect to the behavior of the differential chromosomes 
during the contraction-phase of synapsis and the succeeding early 
erowth-period.t' In the male, as was fully described in my last 
paper, both the heterotropic chromosome and the idiochromo- 
somes condense early in the growth-period (usually as early as the 
contraction-phase of synapsis) to form rounded, condensed, 
intensely-staining chromosome-nuclei. In this condition they 
persist throughout the whole growth-period of the spermatocyte, 
without ever assuming the looser texture and more elongate form 
of the other chromosomes. In the earlier part of this period they 
are as a rule closely associated with a large pale plasmosome, but 
later become separated from it. 

In the female no trace of such a chromosome-nucleus can be 
found in the contraction-figure of the synaptic period. My best 
preparations of this stage are from the ovaries of the larval Anasa, 
which show a distinct synaptic zone of oocytes intervening between 
the zone of multiplication and the growth-zone; but I have 
observed the same condition in the ovaries of recently emerged 
adults of Harmostes, Alydus, Euschistus, Coenus and Podisus. 
In all these forms the contraction-figure is very similar to that of 
the spermatocytes, the chromosomes being in the form of deeply 
staining, ragged, and apparently longitudinally split loops that 
are crowded into a spheroidal mass toward the center or one side 
of the nucleus and surrounded by a large clear space. ‘The nuclei 
at this time occasionally show one or two small deeply-staining 
nucleolus-like bodies (probably plasmosomes); but these are 
much smaller than the chromosome-nuclei of the spermatocytes 
at this period, and in many of the nuclei are absent. ‘The contrast 
between these nuclei and those of the male at the corresponding 
period is so striking as to be at once apparent. In later stages the 
chromosomes spread through the nuclear cavity, become looser 
in texture and finally give rise to a fine reticular structure. In 


1A fuller presentation of observations on these phenomena is reserved for a subsequent paper. 


Studies on Chromosomes 23 


these stages a variable number of deeply-staining nucleoli make 
their appearance; but their true nature can only be determined 
positively when the whole ovarian life of the egg has been followed 
and the process of maturation observed. I can, therefore, only 
state that no chromosome-nucleolus is present in the contraction 
period of synapsis, or in the early growth-period; and even though 
‘it be present in later stages, which I think is very doubtful, a wide 
difference between the sexes would still exist in respect to the 
earlier period. 


F. General Resumé 


The foregoing results may be given a general formulation as 
follows: If m be the unreduced number of chromosomes in the 
female, the matured eggs in all cases contain half this number ("). 
The males are of three types. In the first, one of the chromo- 
somes (the heterotropic or “accessory’’) is without a mate, and 
the unreduced number is accordingly one less than that of the 
female. Half the spermatozoa possess, and half lack, the hetero- 
tropic chromosome, the first class having the same number as the 
matured eggs (%), the second class one less (?—1). Inthe second 
type the male has the same number of chromosomes as the female, 
but possesses one large and one small idiochromosome while the 
female possesses two large ones. In maturation half the spermat- 
ozoa receive the small and half the large idiochromosome. ‘The 
third type differs from the second in that the idiochromosomes are 
of equal size in both sexes, and no visible differences exist between 
the two classes of spermatozoa or the somatic groups of the two 
sexes. Designating the large and small idiochromosomes as [ 
and 7 respectively, the relations in fertilization and sex-production 
are as follows: 

TYPE I 
(Protenor, Anasa, Atypus, HarMostes) 
Egg % + spermatozoén % (including heterotropic) = (female). 
Egg % + spermatozoén 7 — 1 (heterotropic lacking) = n — 1 (male). 
TYPE II 
(Lycarus, Euscuistus, Cornus, Popisus) 


Egg ® (including I) + spermatozoén ® (including I) = n (including IZ) (female). 
Egg * (including I) + spermatozoén ” (including i) = n (including Ji) (male). 


24 Edmund B. Wilson 


TYPE WT 
(Nezara) 


Egg ™ + spermatozoén ” = n (male or female, including in each case two equal idiochromosomes), 


These relations are graphically shown in the following diagram 


(Fig. 6) in which the differential chromosomes are black and the . 


ordinary ones unshaded (only two pairs of the latter shown). For 
the sake of simplicity only the final result of synapsis (second 
column) and the ensuing process of reduction (third column) are 
shown, without regard to variations of detail. “The matured eggs 
(ov) are represented with a single polar body (the result of the 
reduction-division) which is greatly exaggerated in size. The 
female-producing and male-producing spermatozoa (sp) are 
lettered a and b respectively. It will be evident from an inspection 
of this diagram that the second type may readily be derived from 
the third, and the first from the second by the reduction (second 
type) and final disappearance (first type) of one of the differential 
chromosomes. ‘This I believe to represent the actual relations 
of the three types. 


il GENERAL. 


In recent years evidence has steadily accumulated to strengthen 
the view that the general basis of sex-production is given by a 
predetermination existing at least as early as the fertilized egg, 


but there is a wide divergence of opinion in regard to the condi- 


tions pre€xisting in the gametes prior to their union.! 

The fact that in some organisms (such as Dinophilus, Hyda- 
tina or Phylloxera) the unfertilized eggs, sometimes even in the 
ovary, are visibly distinguishable as male- -producing and female- 
producing forms, has led a number of recent writers to deny that 
the spermatozo6n can play any part in sex-determination. Beard, 
for example, asserts that “The male gamete, the spermatozo6n, 
has and can have absolutely no influence in determining the sex 


‘The general question of sex-determination, with its literature, has within the past five years 
been so ably and thoroughly reviewed by Cuénot, Strasburger, Beard, von Lenhossék, O. Schultze 
and others, that I shall here limit myself in the main to an analysis of the new observations brought 
forward, 


ee 


Studies on Chromosomes 25 


OGGoNnIA FERTILIZATION 
AND SE — ~~ 
SPERMATOGONIA Gametes Spermatozoa Zygotes 


‘a 
| 
es 
= 
we) 
ay 


@|> 
I| 


IT Lygaeus 


®: 


LF Nez ava 


4 
@ © 
= 


26 Edmund B. Wilson 


of the offspring” (02, p. 712); and a similar conclusion, though 
less dogmatically stated, is reached in the general reviews of 
Lenhossék (’03) and O. Schultze (03). The opposite view that 
the spermatozoon alone is concerned in sex-determination (which 
like the preceding one, 1s of very ancient origin) has, however, been 
maintained by some recent writers, for instance, Block (whose 
work I know only from Cuénot’s review) and McClung, as already 
mentioned|. On the other hand, both Cuénot (’g9) and Stras- 
burger (00) in their able reviews, have argued that both gametes 
may be concerned in sex-determination; and the last named 
author urged the view, afterward recognized as probable by 
Bateson and developed in detail by Castle ('03), that sex-produc- 
tion takes place in accordance with the Mendelian principles of 
inheritance. 

The observations here brought forward, together with those of 
Stevens on Tenebrio, establish the predestination (in a descriptive 
sense) of two classes of spermatozoa, equal in number, as male- 
producing and female-producing forms. “Though indistinguish- 
able to the eye in their mature state, these two classes differ visibly 
in nuclear constitution at the time of their formation; and since 
this occurs in the same order of insects as Phylloxera, where the 
eggs are visibly distinguishable (by their size) as male-producing 
and female-producing forms, it is evident that a substantial basis 
now exists for the views expressed by Cuénot and Strasburger, and 
for the Mendelian interpretation of sex-production worked out 
by Castle. Whether in the Hemiptera that form the subject of 
this paper the eggs are, like the spermatozoa, predestined as male- 
producing and female-producing forms can at present be a matter 
of inference only. I have not been able to distinguish such classes 
by their size, and the data show, almost with certainty, that if 
they exist they do not exhibit any visible nuclear differences 
like those present*in the spermatozoa. But this gives no ground 
for denying their existence. No visible nuclear dimorphism of 


1“By exclusion then, it would seem that the determination of this difference (the sexual one) is 
reposed in the male element’’ (McClung, *o2, p. 78). McClung nevertheless maintained the exist- 
ence of a selective power on the part of the egg such that “the condition of the ovum determines 
which sort of spermatozoén shall be allowed entrance into the egg substance”’ (op. cit., p. 76). 


Studies on Chromosomes 27 


the spermatozoa exists in Nezara, yet this condition is con- 
nected, by an almost continuous series of intermediate forms, 
with one in which a conspicuous difference of nuclear con- 
stitution is to be seen. It seems hardly open to doubt that 
sex-production conforms to the same essential type throughout 
this series. At least a possibility is thus established that in 
organisms generally both eggs and spermatozoa may be pre- 
destined as male-producing and female-producing forms, whether 
they are visibly different or not. In any case, it is evident 
that in the Hemiptera the chromosome-combination characteristic 
of each sex is established by union of the gametes and is a result 
of fertilization by one or the other of the two forms of spermatozoa. 
Sex must therefore already be predetermined in the fertilized egg, 
and it is difficult to conceive how it could subsequently be altered 
in these animals by conditions external to the egg or embryo. 
Since the idiochromosomes or heterotropic chromosomes form the 
distinctive differential between the nuclei of the two sexes, it is 
obvious that these chromosomes are definitely coordinated with 
the sexual characters. We must therefore critically inquire into 
the causal relation between sex-production and the chromosomes, 
of which this codrdination is an expression. 

That sex-production may be interpreted as the result of a 
Mendelian segregation, transmission and dominance of the sexual 
characters has been shown by Castle (’03). ‘The history of the 
differential chromosomes in synapsis and reduction evidently 
affords a concrete basis for such an interpretation in the terms of 
the Sutton-Boveri chromosome-theory. Analysis of the facts now 
known will, however, show even more clearly than the more 
general considerations adduced by Castle, that this interpretation 
is only admissible under the assumption that a selective fertiliza- 
tion occurs, such that eggs containing the female-determinant are 
fertilized only by spermatozoa containing the male-determinant 
and vice versa. Until I had read Cuénot’s recent interesting 
paper (’05) on the breeds of mice and their combinations, the 
necessity for making this assumption seemed to me an almost 
fatal difficulty in the way of the interpretation, but if Cuénot’s 
conclusions be well founded the a@ prior: objections to such a 


28 Edmund B. Wilson 


selective fertilization are in large measure set aside. I therefore 
think that the possibility of a SVisadelen interpretation of sex- 
production should be carefully examined, though as will be shown, 
an alternative interpretation is possible. 


I. In such an examination the distinction between sex-deter- 
mination and sex-inheritance should be clearly drawn; for it is 
well known that each sex may contain factors capable of pro- 
ducing the characters of the opposite sex, and it may well be that 
the patency or latency of the sexual characters is determined by 
factors quite distinct from those concerned with their transmission 
from parent to offspring. For the purpose of analysis it will, 
however, be convenient to speak of the idiochromosomes or their 
homologues as “sex-determinants,”’ this term being understood 
to mean that these chromosomes are the bearers of the male and 
female qualities (or the factors essential to the production of these 
qualities) respectively. [hey may also be designated (whenever 
it 1s desirable to avoid circumlocution) as sex-chromosomes or 
“oonochromosomes.” As a basis of discussion the Mendelian 
interpretation may be taken to postulate, further, that the two 
sex-chromosomes, which couple in synapsis and are subsequently 
disjoined by the reducing division, are respectiv ely male-determi- 
nants and female-determinants in the sense just indicated. The 
most convenient approach to the question is offered by the hetero- 
tropic chromosome, since its unpaired condition in one sex renders 
its mode of transmission more clearly obvious than that of the 
idiochromosomes. ‘The facts (especially as observed in Protenor) 
clearly prove that this chromosome alternates between the sexes 
in successive generations, passing from the male to the female in 
the production of females, and from the female to the male in the 
production of males (Fig. 6). The important bearing of this 
on both sex-inheritance and sex-determination will appear beyond. 

Since the heterotropic chromosome is without a fellow in the 
male it must, if it be a sex-determinant at all, be the male-determi- 
nant, which exerts its effect uninfluenced by association with a 
female-determinant. But since the spermatozoa that contain 


1Cf. Watase, ’92. 


Studies on Chromosomes 29 


this chromosome produce only females, it must be assumed that 
the maternal mate or fellow, with which it becomes associated on 
entering the egg, is a dominant female-determinant. Further, 
since sills Hone from fertilization by spermatozoa that do not 
contain the heterotropic chromosome, the latter must in male- 
producing eggs be derived from the egg-nucleus (c7. the diagram, 
Fig. 6). [he general interpretation, ‘therefore, must include the 
assumption that there are two kinds of eggs (presumably in 
approximately equal numbers) that contain respectively the male- 
and the female-determinant,' and that the former are fertilized only 
by spermatozoa that lack the heterotropic chromosome (7. ¢., the 
male determinant) and vice versa,’ giving the combinations (1m)j 
(female) and m—(male). Such a selective fertilization 1s there- 
fore a sine qua non of the assumption that the heterotropic chro- 
mosome is a specific sex-determinant. 

A nearly similar, though somewhat more complex, result follows 
in the case of the idiochromosomes. In respect to sex-production 
the large idiochromosome 1s identical with the heterotropic chro- 
mosome, and the morphological evidence is nearly or quite 
decisive that the heterotropic chromosome is actually a large 
idiochromosome, the smaller mate of which has disappeared. 
The small idiochromosome may therefore be regarded as a 
disappearing, or even vestigial, female-determinant that 1s recessive 
to its larger fellow (the male-determinant); and its reduction in 
size may plausibly be regarded as an atrophy resulting from its 
invariably recessive nature (this chromosome being strictly con- 
fined to the male). Precisely as in case of the heterotropic 
chromosome, the large idiochromosome of the male (male- 
determinant) must be derived in fertilization from the egg-nucleus 
(Fig. 6); and, as before, it must be assumed that eggs that contain 
this chromosome are fertilized only by spermatozoa that contain 
the small idiochromosome, those that contain the female-determi- 


1This would follow from the coupling of the two sex-chromosomes in synapsis to form the bivalent 
(m)f, and its division in such a way as to leave in the egg either the male- or the female-determinant 
indifferently. 

Otherwise the combinations mm or f— might result, which is contrary to observation, since the sex- 


chromosomes are in this type never paired in the male or unpaired in the female. 


30 Edmund B. Wilson 


nant only by spermatozoa containing the large idiochromosome. 
In this type, accordingly, it is clear that the large idiochromosome 
(like the heterotropic chromosome to which it corresponds) passes 
alternately from one sex to the other, while the small one never 
enters the female; and this would remain true even did selective fer- 
tilization not occur (Fig. 6). “The same interpretation may finally 
be extended to Nezara, where the idiochromosomes are of equal size 
in both sexes, the relations of dominance being the same as before. 

The two vital points in this result are first, the assumption of 
selective fertilization, and second the relations of dominance and 
recession in the two sexes. As regards the first point, until the 
appearance of Cuénot’s paper, referred to above, almost no 
definite evidence had been produced of an infertility between 
particular classes of gametes in the same species; though it has 
long been known that many plants are in a greater or less degree 
infertile to their own pollen, and an analogous fact has been more 
recently demonstrated in Ciona by Castle (’96) and Morgan (’04). 
Correns (’02), in his study of hybrid maize, was led to suggest 
that in this case there might be a somewhat diminished fertility 
between the gametes bearing the recessive character (thus account- 
ing for a relative deficiency of extracted recessives in the second 
generation of crosses, F,). In studying the breeds of mice 
Cuénot has found it impossible to obtain pure or homozygous yellow 
forms. Yellow mice are invariably heterozygotes (the yellow 
being dominant over gray, black or brown) and when crossed 
with a pure race of a different color (e. g., gray) give the typical 
Mendelian result, yellow and gray offspring appearing in equal 
numbers. ‘This proves that a complete Mendelian disjunction 
of the yellow and gray determinants takes place in maturation. 
When yellow mice of known constitution (e. g.. Y(G)) are paired 
with like forms, the first offspring include pure gray forms (ex- 
tracted recessives) slightly in excess of the normal ratio of 25 per 
cent., and yellow forms; but contrary to the Mendelian expec- 
tation the latter, when paired with one another, never give pure 
dominants (YY), but again produce pure grays (GG) and 
heterozygous yellows (Y(G)). Cuénot therefore concludes that 
although complete segregation of both the gray and yellow 


Studies on Chromosomes 31 


characters takes place in the gamete-formation, and the resulting 
yellow-bearing gametes unite freely with those bearing the reces- 
sive color, they do not unite with each other: “Ceux-ci (the 
yellow heterozygotes) forment bien des gametes de valeur C] ou 
AJ, mais ces gametes ne peuvent pas s’unir les uns aux autres pour 
donner des zygotes ayant les formules CJCJ, AJAJ ou CJAJ; 
par autre, ils s’unissent facilement a tous les autres gametes que 
jai essayés pour former avec eux des heterozygotes mono- ou 
dihybrides” (op. cit., p. cxxx). This conclusion is sustained by 
the fact that the combination Y(G) x Y(G) (CYCG x CYCG in 
Cuénot’s terminology) produces a relative deficiency of yellows 
in the offspring, as is to be expected.’ In pairing Y(G) with 
Y(G), accordingly, the Y-bearing spermatozoa unite only with 
the G-bearing eggs, and vice versa, which is exactly analogous 
to the selective fertilization assumed in case of the sex-bearing 
gametes. Perhaps it may be possible to find a different expla- 
nation of the facts; but if Cuénot’s interpretation be well-founded 
the case goes far to remove the scepticism which I think one must 
otherwise feel in regard to a selective fertilization of the gametes 
in sex-production. 

An examination of the question of dominance involved in the 
Mendelian interpretation leads to some interesting conclusions. 
In forms possessing unequal idiochromosomes the sexual formulas 
would be for the female (mm) f and for the male m (f) (f being the 
small idiochromosome). Applying the same interpretation to 
Nezara, where the idiochromosomes are of equal size, the corre- 
sponding formulas are (m) f and m (f), giving the gametes (7m), 
j, m and (f). Assuming likewise a selective fertilization the facts 
would be: 


Eccs SPERMATOZOA 
(m) +. (f) = (m) (f), producing a male, m/(f). 
i 3p = mf, producing a female (m) f. 


1The deficiency, though constant, is very slight. Cuénot himself seems to consider this a difficulty, 
but I believe a very simple explanation may be given. With equal numbers of the gametes of both sexes 
the ratio of yellows to grays should be two to one, instead of three to one as in the typical Mendelian case 
(since the class YY is missing). If, however, the spermatozoa be in large excess, as they undoubtedly 
are, all or nearly all the Y-bearing eggs will be fertilized by G-bearing spermatozoa, and vice versa, thus 
bringing the ratio of yellows (Y(G)) to grays (GG) more or less nearly up to three to one. 


32 Edmund B. Wilson 


Now it is clear that if the relations of the chromosomes to sex- 
production be the same here as in the second type, the chromo- 
some m must alternate in successive generations between the male 
and the female (like the large idiochromosome or the heterotropic 
chromosome to which it corresponds), and hence also shows an 
alternation of dominance, being dominant in the former sex and 
recessive in the latter. If, therefore, dominance and recession 
be inherent in the chromosomes, there must be such a relation 
between them that m is always dominant to the chromosome (f) 
of the male, and always recessive to the chromosome 7 of the 
female, and that the latter two chromosomes (/ and (/)) are never 
interchanged between the sexes. ‘This last assumption 1s not so 
improbable as it may at first sight appear; for in the second type 
it 1s certain, as already pointed out, that the small idiochromo- 
some ((f) under the general assumption) never enters the female, 
while the large idiochromosome, m, like the heterotropic, alter- 
nates between the two sexes in successive generations. 

A strict Mendelian interpretation of Speduc ae may unques- 
tionably, I think, be constructed upon the foregoing assumptions. 
But an interesting suggestion for a somewhat modified Mendelian 
interpretation is given by the possibility that the dominance of 
the sex-chromosomes is determined by extrinsic factors, namely, 
by conditions in the protoplasm of the zygote. If this were the 
case it is evident that the idiochromosomes could not be considered 
as sex-determinants in the strict sense of the word. ‘The determi- 
nation of sex would in this case be due to factors preéxisting in 
one or both of the gametes, irrespective of the sex-chromosomes, 
and the latter could only be considered as a means by which the 
sex-characters are transmitted or inherited. ‘The possibility is 
here clearly offered that either or both forms of gametes may be 
predetermined as males or females (or at least male-producing 
and female-producing) prior to fertilization and irrespective of the 
chromosomes; and thus an interpretation of the ordinary forms 
of gametes would be reached in harmony with such cases as 
Dinophilus and other forms in which male-producing and female- 
producing eggs are distinguishable in size prior to fertilization. 
Such an interpretation would further be perfectly consistent with 


Studies on Chromosomes 
5 33 


the modification of sex-production in some cases by external condi- 
tions, and with the production of both males and females in 
parthenogenesis (though this may be otherwise explicable); and 
it might also give the explanation of selective fertilization. 


II. It has not been my intention to advocate the foregoing 
interpretation, but only to set forth as clearly as possible, ihe as- 
sumptions that it inv olves. It is nevertheless my opinion that the 
analysis places no insuperable obstacles in its way, and that, 
however dominance be determined, the Mendelian interpretation 
may in fact give the true solution of the problem. I have, how- 
ever, endeavored to seek for a different interpretation that may 
escape the necessity for assuming a selective fertilization; and 
although I have to offer nothing more than suggestions, some of 
which undoubtedly encounter serious dithculties, I shall make 
them in the hope that they may afford some clue to further inquiry. 
Some of these suggestions are equally applicable to the Mendelian 
interpretation considered above, but for the purpose of discussion 
this interpretation may for the time be laid aside. 

It seems possible that the differential chromosomes may per- 
forma definite and special function in sex-production without being 
in themselves specifically male-determining and female-determin- 
ing or even qualitativ ely different save in Te degree of their special 
activity (whatever be its nature). ‘his suggestion is given by 
the fact that the presence of one heterotropic root toet or 
large idiochromosome is associated with the production of a 
male, while if two such chromosomes are present a female is 
produced. ‘This very obviously suggests that the same kind of 
activity that produces a male will if reinforced or intensified 
produce a female; and with this would accord the production 
of males from unfertilized eggs, and females from fertilized ones, 
in the case of the bee. In these cases the decisive factor may be 
a merely quantitative difference of chromatin between the two 
sexes. But it is obvious that such a difference cannot give the 
basis for a general explanation, since in Nezara, and presumably 
in many other organisms, both the number of chromosomes and 
the quantity of chromatin is the same in both sexes. And yet 


34 Edmund B. Wilson 


the existence of a quantitative difference in some cases raises the 
question whether it is not the result or expression of some more 
deeply lying nuclear difference which may still be present in those 
cases where no quantitative difference exists. I find it altogether 
incredible that two animals as nearly related as Nezara and 
Euschistus should differ fundamentally in the relation of the 
chromosomes to sex-production; and if there is any reason to 
conclude that sex-determination is effected by the idiochromo- 
somes (or by the combination of which they form a part) in the 
case where they are visibly different, | cannot avoid the belief 
that this conclusion applies with equal reason to the case in which 
they appear to the eye alike in all the spermatozoa. It therefore 
seems to me an admissible hypothesis that a physiological or 
functional factor may be present that differentiates the spermat- 
ozoa into male-producing and female-producing forms irrespec- 
tive of the size of the differential chromosomes; and further, 
that the morphological difference that has arisen in some forms 
may have been a consequence of such an antecedent functional 
difference. If we could assume for instance that the differential 
chromosome-pair in the male includes a more active and a less 
active member (the latter having in many cases become, reduced 
in size or even having entirely disappeared) the suggestion might 
be greatly extended in application. Under this assumption the 
facts might receive a general formulation in the statement that 
the association of two more active chromosomes of this class 
produces a female, while the association of a more active and a 
less active one (or the absence of the latter, as in case of the hetero- 
tropic chromosome) produces a male. Reduction of the less 
active member to form a-small idiochromosome would introduce 
a quantitative difference of chromatin as well as a qualitative one. 
Its complete disappearance in the male, leaving only the active 
member as the heterotropic chromosome, would reduce the 
difference to a merely quantitative one. The assumption of 
such a physiological difference is admittedly a purely specula- 
tive construction, and may seem 4a priori very improbable. But 
from the a priori point of view it would seem equally improbable 
that a morphological dimorphism of the spermatozoa, affecting 


Studies on Chromosomes 35 


OW 


only one pair of the chromosomes, should have arisen; yet this 
is an observed fact. I therefore think the suggestion is worthy 
of serious consideration. If it could be adeered the necessity 
of selective fertilization would be avoided, for the observed results 
would follow from the fertilization of any egg by any spermatozoon. 

But even if in accordance with fact the suggestion is still 
obviously incapable of direct application to cases in which sex 
is determined independently of fertilization—for instance, sex- 
production in parthenogenetic development or in hermaphrodites, 
and in forms (such as Dinophilus) where male-producing and 
female-producing eggs are distinguishable in size before fertiliza- 
tion. It is possible that these cases may be explicable (under either 
general interpretation) as a result of some forms of differential 
distribution of the chromosomes occurring at the time of the for- 
mation of the polar bodies (parthenogenesis) or at some earlier 
period in the cell-lineage of the germ-cells; and this possibility 
should of course be tested by a close cytological study of the facts. 
On the other hand, there is nothing in the facts to negative the 
assumption that in some cases the chromosome-combination, 
established at fertilization, may be in something like a balanced 
state that is capable of modification by conditions external to the 
nucleus (as already suggested in the case of dominance). 

Boveri's interesting observations on the dispermic eggs of 
Ascaris (’04) have given direct evidence that the chromosomes 
react to their cytoplasmic surroundings; and the same fact is 
even more clearly shown by the difference of behavior of the 
differential chromosomes in the two sexes of Hemiptera during the 
synaptic and growth-periods. Hence, even though a preéstab- 
lished basis of sex-determination be given in such a physiological 
dimorphism of the spermatozoa as i have suggested, the sex of 
the fertilized eggs may in many cases be only a matter of greater 
or less ciediap oats: and not an immutable predetermination. 
The nuclei, and hence the primordial germ-cells, may in such 
cases be in a state of approximate equilibrium, and still retain the 
power of response to varying conditions in the cellular environ- 
ment. The production of eggs or spermatozoa in hermaphro- 
dites may thus be explicable as a result of greater or less nuclear 


36 Edmund B. Wilson 


activity in the two cases, incited by intra-cellular conditions that 
are external to the chromosome-groups; and a similar explana- 
tion may apply to the related case of the formation of visibly 
different female-producing and male-producing eggs in the same 
organism. 

It would not, [ think, be profitable to speculate further in regard 
to these special cases, but I have wished to indicate that a hypo- 
thesis of sex-production which recognizes in some cases a fixed 
predetermination in the chromosome-groups of the fertilized egg 
is not inconsistent with the control of sex-production in other 
cases by conditions external to the nucleus. The constant 
chromosomal differences of the sexes existing in many Hemiptera, 
therefore, by no means preclude experiments on the modification 
or control of sex-production. 

I have intentionally excluded from the foregoing suggestions 
any discussion of the specific nature of the activities of the differen- 
tial chromosomes, since we are almost wholly ignorant of the 
functions of chromosomes in general. But although we here 
enter upon still more debatable ground, I think we should not 
hesitate to consider such possibilities in this direction as the facts 
may suggest. 

One of the principal, or at least most obvious, differences 
between the germ-cells of the two sexes is their great contrast in 
constructive activity, evinced by the enormous growth of the 
primary oocyte as compared with the primary spermatocyte. 
This growth of the odcyte involves the production of a mass of 
protoplasm (including under this term the yolk or metaplasm as 
well as the active protoplasm) thousands of times the bulk of the 
spermatocyte; and although the latter also increases noticeably 
in size during the growth-period, the accumulation of proto- 
plasm is almost insignificant as compared with that which takes 
place in the female. Now, as described above, the idiochromo- 
somes and heterotropic chromosome remain during this period in 
the male in a relatively passive condition as compared with the 
other chromosomes, while this is not the case in the female. “The 
thought cannot be avoided that there is a definite causal connec- 
tion between the greater activity of these chromosomes in the 


Studies on Chromosomes 27 
odcytes and the great preponderance of constructive activity in 
these cells; and it is especially this coincidence that leads me to 
the general surmise that one of the important physiological 
differences (I do not say the only one), between the chromosome- 
groups of the two sexes, may be one of constructive activity. 
I have elsewhere (The Cell, Chapter VII) reviewed at some 
length the evidence pointing toward the conclusion that the 
nucleus (more specifically, the chromatin) 1s especially concerned 
with the constructive processes of cell metabolism; and while I no 
longer hold the view that the nucleus can be considered as the 
actual formative center of the cell, it still seems to me very 
probable that the formative processes are directly or indirectly 
under its control, as has been advocated by many students of 
cell-physiology. If this view be well-founded, the facts observed 
in Hemiptera give a very definite and concrete basis for assuming 
a greater constructive activity in the cells of the female generally, 
which reaches a climax in the growth-period of the odcyte.’ It 
seems possible that some of the specific differentiations that take 
place in the later history of the germ-cells may be directly trace- 
able to the primary difference in the growth-process. It is well 
known that the young odcytes and spermatocytes show a very 
close similarity, not only in size but also in many details of struc- 
ture. [he enormous accumulation of cytoplasm in the odcyte 
as compared with the spermatocyte leaves the latter with a great 
relative excess of the kinoplasmic or archoplasmic material in which 
the most characteristic differentiations of the spermatozoa—such as 
the acrosome, middle-piece, axial filament and tail-envelopes— 
take their origin. Perhaps a direct causal relation here exists. 


1This suggestion recalls the theory developed by Geddes and Thomson, in their well known work on 
the ‘‘ Evolution of Sex,” that “the female is the outcome and expression of relatively preponderant anabo- 
lism, and the male of relatively preponderant katabolism” (of. cit., revised ed., 1901, p. 140). As de- 
deloped by these authors, this theory has always seemed to me to have too vague and general a character 
to have much practical value, though it expresses a certain physiological contrast between the sexes that 
undoubtedly exists. My suggestion is only remotely connected with that theory, since it refers the differ- 
entiation of the sexes to a functional difference that preéxists in the cells of the male, and involves no 
contrasted processes of anabolism and katabolism. Nevertheless, the observations here brought forward 
may harmonize with that side of the theory which lays stress on the preponderant constructive activity of 
the female cells. 


38 Edmund B. Wilson 


III. Though I have found it convenient to consider the two 
foregoing interpretations separately, they evidently have many 
points of agreement, and perhaps may be reduced to a common 
basis. Both assign to the differential chromosomes a specific 
function in sex-production, both recognize the possibility of a 
determination of sex (as opposed to its transmission), by con- 
ditions external to the chromosome-groups, and both assume, 
in one sex, a specific difference in the sex-chromosomes, followed 
by a Mendelian disjunction in the formation of the gametes. 
The essential point in which the second interpretation diverges 
from the first is that the sex-chromosomes are not conceived as 
bearing the male or female qualities respectively but as differing 
only in the degree of their activity, and this difference is assumed 
to exist in the male only (owing to the relation of fertilization 
to sex-production). It must be admitted that each interpreta- 
tion involves a considerable element of pure conjecture, and that 
each includes assumptions which without additional data must 
be considered.as sericus difficulties. The principal one involved 
in the first interpretation is the assumption of selective fertiliza- 
tion; but if this assumption be granted I believe that it may give 
an adequate solution of the problem of sex-production in the sexual 
reproduction of divecious organisms. ‘The second interpreta- 
tion avoids this difficulty; it may explain the primary difference 
between the gametes of the two sexes, the latency of female 
characters in the male, and the development of such secondary 
female characters as may be regarded as an exaggeration or inten- 
sification of corresponding characters in the male. It seems con- 
spicuously to fail to explain the reverse case of characters that 
are more highly developed in the male; and to many this will 
doubtless appear a fatal difficulty. But we are still ignorant of 
the action and reaction of the chromosomes on the cytoplasm and 
on one another, and have but a vague speculative notion of the 
relations that determine patency and latency in development. 
Additional data will therefore be required, I think, to show 
whether the difficulty in question is a fatal one, and in what meas- 
ure either of the two general interpretations that have been con- 
sidered may approach the truth. ‘The positive result of the 


Studies on Chromosomes 39 


observations of Stevens and myself is to demonstrate the existence 
of a constant and definite correlation between the chromosomes 
and the sexual characters, which is visibly expressed in the relations 
of a single pair of chromosomes. ‘These relations unquestionably 
afford a concrete basis for an interpretation of sex-production 
that assumes a Mendelian segregation and transmission of the 
sex-characters and to this extent they accord with the general 
assumption of Castle. The validity of both this and the alterna- 
tive interpretation suggested must be tested by further inquiry. 


Zodlogical Laboratory of Columbia University, 
December 8, 1905. 


WORKS CITED 


Beard, JOHN, ’02.—The Determination of Sex in Animal Development. Fischer, 
Jena. 
Bovert, I. H., ’04.—Protoplasmadifferenzierung als auslosender Faktor ftir Kern- 
verschiedenheit. Sitzungsber. der Physikal.-med. Ges. Wirz- 
burg, 1904. 
CastLe, W. E., ’96.—The Early Embryology of Ciona intestinalis. Bull. Mus. 
Comp. Zool., xxvii. 
’03.—The Heredity of Sex.. Jbid., xl, 4. 
Correns, C., ’02.—Scheinbare Ausnahme von der Mendel’schen Spaltungsregel 
fur Bastarde. Ber. d. deutschen Bot. Ges., xx. 
Cuénot, L., ’99.—Sur la détermination du sexe chez les animaux. Bull. Sci. de 
la France et de la Belgique, xxxii, v, 1. 
’05.—Les races pures et leurs combinaisons chez les souris. Arch. Zool. 
Exp. et Gén. (4), i11, Notes et Revue, No. 7. 
Gross, J., °04.—Die Spermatogenese von Syromastes marginatus. Zool. Jahrb., 
Anat. und Ontog., xx, 3. 
HenkKING, H., ’91.—Ueber Spermatogenese und deren Beziehung zur Eientwick- 
lung be: Pyrrochoris apterus. Zeitschr. Wiss. Zool., li. 
Lennosskk, M. v., ’03.—Das Problem der geschlechtsbestimmenden Ursachen. 
Fischer, Jena. 
McCiune, C. E., ’02.—The Accessory Chromosome. Sex-determinant/ Biol. 
Ball? ant, 2: 
Montcomery, T. H., ’o01.—A Study of the Germ Cells of Metazoa. ‘Trans. 
Am Phil Soc:, xx: 
*o4.—Some Observations and Considerations on the Maturation Phenom- 
ena of the Germ-cells. Biol. Bull., vi, 3. 


40 Edmund B. Wilson 


Morcan, T. H., ’04.—Self-fertilization Induced by Artificial Means. Jour. 
Exps Zool. a). 

ScHULTZE, O., ’03.—Zur Frage von den geschlechtsbildenden Ursachen. Arch. 
mik. Anat., Ixiil. 

Stevens, N. M., ’05.—Studies in Spermatogenesis with especial Reference to the 
“Accessory Chromosome.” Publication No. 36, Carnegie Insti- 
tution of Washington, Sept., 1905. 

STRASBURGER, E., 1900.—Versuche mit didcischen Pflanzen in Ricksicht auf 
Geschlechtsverteilung. Biol. Centralbl., xx, 20-24. 

Sutton, W. S., ’02—On the Morphology of the Chromosome-group in Brachy- 
stola magna. Biol. Bull., iv, 1. 

Wa tace, L. B., ’05—The Spermatogenesis of the Spider. Biol. Bull., viii, 3. 

Watasg, S., ’92.—On the Phenomena of Sex-differentiation. Journ. of Mor- 
phology, vi, 3. 

Witson, E. B., ’05, 1.—Studies on Chromosomes. I. The Behavior of the Idio- 
chromosomes in Hemiptera. Journ. Exp. Zool., ii, 3. 

*05, 2—The Chromosomes in Relation to the Determination of Sex in 
Insects. Science, xxii, 564, Oct. 20, 1905. 
’05,2. Studies on Chromosomes. II. The Paired Microchromosomes, 

Idiochromosomes and Heterotropic Chromosomes in Hemiptera. 
Journ. Exp. Zool., 1, 4. 


AN EXAMINATION OF THE EFFECTS OF MECHANI- 
CAL SHOCKS AND VIBRATIONS UPON THE RATE 
OF DEVELOPMENT OF FERTILIZED EGGS 


BY 


DAVID DE WHEL NEY 


It has been shown in recent years by several investigators that 
mechanical shocks and vibrations may start the development of 
unfertilized eggs of certain animals.’ It has also been stated by 
Meltzer? that the early development of fertilized eggs of the sea 
urchin is greatly accelerated when the eggs are subjected to me- 
chanical shocks and vibrations. Furthermore, Mathews and 
Whitcher® believe they have obtained results which show that such 
influences may cause the embryos of the sea urchin to be either 
larger or smaller than those of the control eggs, or abnormal in 
shape, or to develop more slowly than normal eggs. 

Assuming these results to be well founded, I undertook, during 
the summer of 1905, some experiments to determine the influence 
of shaking at different stages of development. In order to exclude 
the obvious possibility, that the results of Meltzer, and perhaps 
those of Mathews and Whitcher, were due to differences in tem- 
perature, care was taken to make this factor the same in the shaken 
eggs and in the control. Insomecasesthis was done by means ofa 
water jacket around bothsetsofeggs. Itsoon became apparent that 
no difference at all im the rate occurred when this precaution was 
carried out; so that the work resolved itself into testing the claims 
of these investigators respecting the influence of shock on the 
early stages of development. 

At the suggestion and under the kindly supervision of Prof. 


‘Mathews: Amer. Jour. Physiol., rgo1, vi, p. 142. Fisher: Amer. Jour. Physiol., 1902, vii, 
p- 301. 

"Meltzer: Amer. Jour. Physiol., 1903, ix, p. 245. 

*Mathews and Whitcher: Amer. Jour. Physiol., 1903, viii, p. 301. 


Journat or Exprrimentat Zodiocy, Vor. m1, No. 1. 


42 David D. Whitney 


T. H. Morgan, I have studied the influence of mechanical agita- 
tion and vibration upon the fertilized eggs of Arbacia, Asterias, 
Fundulus heteroclitus and Ctenolabrus. 

Of the fertilized eggs of Arbacia twenty-five lots from six 
individuals were shaken from one to forty-five minutes. Some 
were shaken by hand and others by means of a small hot-air 
engine. [he eggs were placed in a test-tube half full of sea water, 
which in some cases contained small pebbles about the size of an 
ordinary pin head; in other cases water alone was present. The 
tube was then shaken back and forth one hundred to three hundred 
times per minute. 

Six other lots from two other individuals were placed in a thick 
walled test-tube which swung vertically, and, by the use of the 
small engine, the bottom of the tube was made to strike quite hard 
against the side of a board from one hundred and fifty to four 
hundred times a minute. 

In all cases the temperature of the control was the same as that 
of the shaken eggs. When the engine was employed for shaking, 
the test-tube was kept under running sea water, but when shaken by 
hand it was wrapped up in wet cloths. ‘The eggs of the controls 
were also kept under similar conditions. When taken from the 
test-tube, the eggs were all kept at room temperature, 23°-26° C. 
Thus the temperature of the shaken and non-shaken eggs was 
always exactly the same. 

After fertilization, the eggs were usually allowed to stand three 
to five minutes before using. In some cases, however, they were 
shaken immediately after fertilization. In other cases they were 
allowed to stand from five to twenty-five minutes. 

In all these thirty-one sets of shaken eggs from eight individuals 
no acceleration of cleavage was noted in the earlier stages, nor 
were the plutei ever larger than those of the controls. In many 
cases the majority of the embryos of the shaken eggs were small 
and abnormal in shape, and were swimming upon the bottom of 
the dish, thus corroborating some of Mathews’ and Whitcher’s 
observations. ‘This was especially true if the eggs were shaken in 
the test-tube with the small pebbles. Usually a few large plutei 
were found in both the controls and the shaken lots; but in no one 


Effects of Mechanical Shocks upon Development 43 


of these lots did there seem to be more large plutei than in the 
other. 

The following experiments will illustrate the general character 
of the results obtained from Arbacia: 

Experiment I. “fuly 1r—Temperature of sea water 23° C.; of 
room 25°C. All lots of eggs, after fertilization, were kept at 23° 
C. for forty-five minutes, and were then placed in finger bowls of 
water at room temperature. 

Lot A. Control. Great care taken in handling the eggs so as 
to prevent any jarring or shaking. 

Lot B. Three minutes after fertilization the eggs were placed 
in a test-tube the bottom of which struck for forty-five minutes 
against the side of a wooden box two hundred and seventy-five to 
three hundred and fifty times a minute. 

Lot C. ‘Ten minutes after fertilization. Shaken five minutes 
back and forth two hundred and fifty times a minute in a test-tube 
half full of water plus some small pebbles. 

Lot D. Immediately after fertilization. Shaken five minutes 
in same manner as lot C. 

The early stages and the plutei of lots B, C and D were like 
those of the control. 

Experiment II. “fuly 1r.—Conditions and results the same as 
in I. 

Experiment III. “fuly 12.—Yemperature the same as in [ 
and JT. 

Lot A. Control. 

Lot B. Shaken thirty minutes back and forth two hundred and 
forty times per minute, in a test-tube half full of water. “The tube 
was in a jacket of running water. 

Early cleavages and the plutei of B were the same as A. 

Experiment VI. “fuly 13.—Temperature of sea water 23° C.; 
of room 26° C. 

Lot A. Control. 

Lot B. Shaken five minutes back and forth two hundred times 
a minute in test-tube half full of water plus some small pebbles. 

Lot C. Shaken one minute back and forth three hundred 
times per minute in same manner as B. 


44 David D. Whitney 


Early segmentation of all three lots the same. 

Fuly 16.—Lot A. Normal plutei swimming through water in 
the dish. 

Lot B. Normal plutei swimming upon the bottom of the dish. 

Lot C. Normal plutei. A few swimming through the water, 
and the remainder swimming upon the bottom of the dish. 

Experiment VIII. ‘uly 13.—Lot A. Control. 

Lot B. Shaken three minutes by hand back and forth three 
hundred times per minute in a small vial half full of water plus 
small pebbles. 

Lot C. Shaken five minutes in same manner as B. 

B and C were one-half hour later than A in showing the first 
cleavage. 

Fuly r7.—Lot A. Normal plutei swimming through water in 
a dish. 

Lot B. Very few normal plutei. Mostly gastrule, and many 
of them abnormal. Both plutei and gastrula were swimming upon 
the bottom of the dish. 

Lot C. Normal and abnormal plutei swimming upon bottom 
of dish. 

Experiment XII. “fuly 17,—The sea urchins were kept in an 
ice chest at a temperature of 9° C. from five to six hours. ‘Then 
the ovaries were quickly removed to water at 9° C. and fertilized 
by the sperm that had been shed by the males while in the ice 
chest. 

Lot A. Control at 9° C. for thirty minutes. 

Lot B. Shaken thirty minutes back and forth two hundred 
and ten times per minute, in a test-tube half full of water. The 
tube was in an ice water bath at 3° C. 

Lot C. Shaken one minute back and forth three hundred 
times, at a temperature of 11° C. They were allowed to remain 
at this temperature for thirty minutes. At the expiration of this 
time, the three lots were placed in room temperature of 25° to 
262°€- 

The early segmentation and normal plutei of the three lots 
appeared at the same time respectively. 

Some eggs were placed at room temperature—25° to 26° C.— 


Effects of Mechanical Shocks upon Development 45 


and others from the same lot were put under running sea water 
at a temperature of 23° C. ‘The eggs at room temperature devel- 
oped in the early segmentation one stage in advance of those 
which were under running water. 

In all the experiments great care was taken in handling the 
control eggs, both before snd after fertilization, so as to prevent 
jarring which might influence the rate of development of the con- 
trol eggs in such a way that it would be identical with the eggs 
intentionally shaken. 

Meltzer shook the eggs of Arbacia by hand and also by means 
of the piston of a stationary engine. He also placed them in 
dishes on the vibrating part of the same engine. He does not 
state whether the eggs of the control were kept at the same tem- 
perature as those that were being shaken, but only states that the 
temperature of the room was slightly higher than the outside 
temperature. I have made careful records of the temperature of 
the places in the engine house where he performed his experi- 
ments. If the conditions then were the same as they are now, all 
his results can be explained by the different degrees of tempera- 
ture to which the eggs were subjected. 

The eggs that he shook by hand in small vials containing small 
glass beads developed faster. [he temperature in such a case 
would be increased by the friction of the beads and water, and by 
the warmth of the hand, in a remarkably short space of time. 

I found that the temperature in the test-tube which struck 
against a board, when not kept under running water, was raised 
two to four degrees if the experiment was continued for fifteen to 
twenty minutes. 

Meltzer may have avoided all these difficulties and have kept 
all the eggs of a series at the same temperature, but he does not 
state this; and from what he does say, he seems to have paid little 
attention to temperature and consequently concluded that the 
acceleration of development which he obtained was due to vibra- 
tion and shaking. I find on the contrary, that by keeping all the 
eggs of a series at the same temperature no acceleration of develop- 
ment can be obtained by subjecting them to a slight or even to a 
great amount of mechanical agitation or vibration. 


46 David D. Whitney 


Fertilized eggs of Asterias subjected to mechanical shocks gave 
only negative results. About thirty-five lots from twelve individ- 
uals were used. ‘The apparatus employed for producing the 
shocks was a thick glass test-tube, the bottom of which struck 
against two boards from two hundred to three hundred and fifty 
times per minute—the same that was used in connection with the 
eggs of Arbacia. 

‘The eggs were kept in this tube, which was constantly in motion, 
from fifteen minutes to seven hours. Some were subjected to 
shock immediately after fertilization, and others remained undis- 
turbed from five to thirty minutes before being placed in the tube. 
‘The temperature was kept uniform in all lots of eggs of the same 
experiment. 

When many of the eggs had reached the eight-cell stage, about 
two hundred of them were selected at random, and the percentage 
of the eggs in the various segmentation stages was determined by 
counting. ‘The shaken eggs usually varied from I to 3 per cent. 
either above or below the percentage of the same stages in the non- 
shaken lots. As the percentage varied so slightly, sometimes 
above and sometimes below that of the control, I concluded that 
the segmentation was neither hastened nor retarded by mechani- 
cal shocks. 

The eggs of Fundulus heteroclitus were also placed in the test- 
tube of this apparatus and subjected to slight and to severe shocks, 
from a few minutes to ten hours. About forty lots—from 
twenty to twenty-five individuals—were used. As the early 
development of these eggs was found to take place normally in a 
damp chamber, some of them were placed in a test-tube, the air 
of which was kept moist by a piece of wet filter paper, and sub- 
jected to shocks. Similar results were obtained from both 
methods. 

A few lots were placed in a test-tube half filled with water and 
the tube was made to move back and forth in a horizontal position 
from two hundred to three hundred and fifty times per minute. 

In none of these experiments did the early cleavage stages of 
the shaken eggs appear earlier than in those of the controls. 

As the eggs of Ctenolabrus float upon the surface of the water, 


Effects of Mechanical Shocks upon Development 47 


the test-tube was inverted, and its upper end made to strike 
against boards, as in the former experiments. In some of the 
experiments the tube struck against the boards gently, and in 
others it struck severely enough to kill many of the eggs. Inter- 
mediate shocks were also tried. 

About thirty lots—from fifteen to twenty individuals—were 
used and were subjected to shocks from five minutes to several 
hours. In all these experiments the early cleavages appeared at 
the same time both in the controls and in the shaken eggs. 

From the foregoing observations it appears that mechanical 
shocks and vibrations are not effective in accelerating the early 
segmentation of the fertilized eggs of Arbacia, Asterias, Fundulus 
and Ctenolabrus. 

These observations were made at the Marine Biological Labora- 
tory at Wood’s Hole, while occupying one of the tables of the 
Carnegie Institution. 


MORPHOLOGY OF THE PARTHENOGENETIC 
DEVELOPMENT. OF “AMPHITRITE 


BY 


JOHN W. SCOTT 


Wiru Four Piates anp Five Ficures IN THE TEXT 


INTRODUCTION 


I. General Statement as to Object of Work and Results Obtained 


The subject for the following investigation was suggested to 
me by Dr. F. R. Lillie early in the summer of 1902. Some recent 
statements by Loeb and others, in regard to the effects of certain 
salt solutions upon the development oe the eggs of some marine 
Annelids, had led Lillie in the previous year to make a series of 
observations upon the egg of Cheetopterus. Lillie’s chief purpose 
in making such a series of experiments was, “To test what was 
the swnineames of cleavage in the egg, and what was the role of 
cell division in ‘eclosanente * He arrived at the following general 
conclusion: “The process of cell division, as such, is necessary 
neither to growth, differentiation, nor to the earliest correlations; 
but it is accessory, in Metazoa, to all three as a localizing factor, 
often from the earliest stages.” Lillie made the suggestion that 
it would be well to test the questions raised in his paper in some 
other marine Annelids. 

My experiments upon the eggs of Amphitrite, however, have 
involved conditions not met in the egg of Chztopterus and 
necessarily other questions have come up for solution. In start- 
ing out | had in view, among others, the following considerations: 
1. Using the methods adopted by previous investigators to pro- 
duce artificial parthenogenesis, was it possible to syne ae differen- 


1A dissertation submitted to the faculty of the Ogden Graduate School of Science in candidacy for 
the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, Department of Zodlogy, The University of Chicago, June, 1904. 


Journat or ExperimentaL Zoétocy, Vor. ur, No. 1. 


50 ‘fobn W. Scott 


tiation without cleavage in the unfertilized eggs of Amphitrite? 
2. May such differentiation be produced in the fertilized eggs? 
3. Can normal trochophores or normal adult worms be raised 
from unfertilized eggs? 4. If so, how are abnormalities appear- 
ing in the early stages regulated to produce a normal embryo? 
In other words, my object was to learn if possible how this sort 
of parthenogenetic development was related to the normal. 

From observations on living eggs and from material preserved 
during the summer of 1902, I succeeded in demonstrating: 
1. That differentiation without cytoplasmic cleavage may occur 
in the unfertilized eggs of Amphitrite. (Scarcity of material 
prevented testing this question for fertilized eggs.) Cleavage, 
when present, is usually abnormal and is always so in the later 
stages. 2. Strictly speaking, this kind of development cannot 
be termed parthenogenesis, for the differentiation so resulting 
never leads either to normal or to abnormal self-sustaining 
organisms. However, I shall use in this paper the term partheno- 
genesis in a restricted sense to indicate the development that is 
initiated in the eggs of Amphitrite by treatment with salt solutions 
and by agitation. 3. There is no correlation or regulation of 
organs in the later development under the conditions of the experi- 
ment. 4. No specific solution was necessary to produce partheno- 
genesis as was claimed by Fischer (’02), though some solutions 
apparently do not have any effect upon the egg. 5. The egg was 
found to be extremely susceptible to agitation or shaking, espe- 
cially at certain periods after it had been removed from the body 
cavity. 6. It was learned from a study of the preserved eggs 
that the parthenogenetic development was closely connected with 
the method and extent of the nuclear division and the chromatin 
distribution and that much depended upon the ripeness of the egg. 

Considering these results, I found it necessary during the fol- 
lowing summer to make an examination of the very early 
development, both normal and parthenogenetic. In the normal 
egg this included a study of the origin and number of chromo- 
somes, the maturation, and fertilization, none of which had been 
described for Amphitrite. Corresponding stages in the parthen- 
ogenetic eggs were also preserved and studied. In general, it 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 51 


has been found that the early development of the fertilized 
Amphitrite egg conforms to that of the typical Annelid, while in 
the later stages, so far as they were studied, I have confirmed the 
observations of Mead (97). With the unfertilized eggs, on the 
other hand, the form of development varies considerably under 
different conditions. The polar bodies may or may not be 
expelled, and when expelled they may be normal or abnormal. 
Cleavage likewise may be present or absent, and is always 
abnormal in late stages. I have found also that the principal 
conditions which produce variations in the form of development 
are the state of ripeness of the egg and the kind and strength of 
the solution used. 


2. Methods 


For a normal series the usual method of adding sperm to the 
dish of eggs was employed. ‘The care and precautions necessary 
in manipulating and handling the unfertilized eggs have been 
described in a previous paper. Boveri's picroacetic (dilute) and 
Kleinenberg’s picrosulphuric were used as killing fluids, the 
former giving the better results. “The eggs were preserved in 80 
per cent alcohol until used. ‘ 

From each lot of eggs whole mounts and sections were prepared. 
For whole mounts Conklin’s hematoxylin method gave fairly 
satisfactory results. ‘This solution was prepared by adding 20 
parts distilled water to 5 parts Delafield’s hematoxylin; to each 
5 cc. of this mixture was added one drop of Kleinenberg’s (undi- 
luted) picrosulphuric acid. The best results were obtained when 
eggs were stained for twenty minutes and afterward washed for 
about one hour in frequent changes of 50 per cent alcohol. 
Several stains were used for sections, but the best results were 
obtained by staining three to five minutes in strong Delafild’s 
hematoxylin followed by from thirty to fifty seconds in Orange 
G. The iron-alum method proved unsatisfactory on account 
of the great amount of yolk present. For the same reason it 
was found necessary to cut the sections 7 to IO micra in 
thickness. 


he ‘fohn W. Scott 


NORMAL HISTORY OF THE EGG 
I. Periods of Maturation and First Cleavage 


I have tried to determine just when and where the germinal 
vesicle breaks down under normal conditions; whether this occurs 
after the egg is deposited in sea-water or while the egg is still 
floating in the body cavity. ‘The latter view is correct as shown by 
the following facts: Eggs from the ripest female that I obtained 
in two seasons’ work were deposited between 6.15 and 6.50 P. M., 
while I was away from the laboratory. At 6.54 P. M. some of 
these eggs were fertilized and a part of the lot was preserved. 
All of those preserved were in the metaphase, with a few excep- 
tions that showed more advanced development. ‘This could 
not have happened if the vesicle breaks down only after the egg is 
in the sea-water, for the eggs are deposited a few at a time at each 
rhythmic peristalsis of the worm’s body. In other experiments 
where the eggs were cut out of the body of the female and left 
unfertilized, I have noted that the germinal vesicle may break 
down within a few minutes, but frequently does not until several 
hours later. In any case a peri-vitelline space forms and the egg 
flattens or undergoes collapse at the animal pole, the spindle rests 
in metaphase and sperm are very rarely found within the cyto- 
plasm until after this phase. In fertilized eggs the first polar 
body makes its appearance about ten minutes later. ‘This is 
followed after an interval of about eight minutes by the second 
polar body. I have seen the second polar globule thrown off 
within twenty-four minutes after the eggs were deposited and the 
first cleavage occurs eighteen to twenty minutes later. The 
periods here given are minimal; they may be much longer and 
are affected by ripeness of the egg, fertilization, agitation, certain 
chemical agents, and probably other causes. 


2. Orientation of the Egg. Origin and Number of the 


Ch rormosomes 


The rather large germinal vesicle in the ripe egg of Amphitrite 
has a slightly eccentric position, near the animal pole. The yolk 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 53 


next the vegetative pole is somewhat denser than it is in the region 
above the nucleus. This eccentric position of the germinal 
vesicle is present in very immature eggs just after they become 
free in the body cavity, when the egg consists simply of a germinal 
vesicle and a surrounding thick (ares of cytoplasm in which no 
trace of yolk can be fans The nucleolus has no definite position 
within the nucleus. 

As the egg increases in size by the growth of the nucleus and 
the deposition of yolk, the denser portions of the latter substance 
are found in the immediate neighborhood of the nucleus; the 
yolk found nearer the surface of the eg ge 1s composed of small 
granules and there is not so much deposited in the upper side 
of the egg as at other places. At this stage dark masses or 
patches that present a fiber-like appearance are frequently found 
in the cytoplasm; judging from the staining reaction these are 
probably portions of the undifferentiated reticulum. ‘The ger- 
minal vesicle now shows a definite reticulum with small micro- 
meres at the intersections. When the egg is near the mature 
condition chromatin granules, or chromomeres, are found collected 
in various groups. The reticulum of the nucleus has nearly dis- 
appeared. In eggs which seemed to be ripe and in which the 
germinal vesicle was on the point of breaking down, chromatin 
groups were found present in the nucleus and the chromatin 
granules were apparently fusing to form the chromosomes. 

Peculiar difficulties have interfered with getting the stages 
immediately succeeding those just mentioned, stages that show 
the exact origin of the chromosomes and asters. The next stage 
found in my preserved material, where the eggs had been deposited 
as a result of the rough handling of the female, is represented in 
Fig. 1. One distinct aster is present which seems to have arisen 
within the nuclear area, and a few radiating fibers point to the 
origin of the other aster somewhat nearer the animal pole. ‘The 
germinal vesicle has apparently just broken down. A part of 
the contracting, fading nucleolus is still visible, and the chro- 
mosomes or probably chromomeres are in groups or strings. In 
the egg of Fig. 2 both asters are well formed, the chromosomes 
are found in scattered groups, and the nucleolus is still intact. 


54 ‘fohn W. Scott 


The process next observed is the collecting of chromatin to 
form the prophase of the first maturation division. While this 
is in progress the asters enlarge, the centrospheres make a rapid 
growth, and a symmetrical spindle develops and rotates to take 
its definitive position. ‘The chromomeres or chromosomes have 
become quite numerous and there are strong indications that they 
are arranged in eleven groups of four each. In Fig. 3 the clear 
area shows the original position of the nucleolus which has dis- 
appeared; some of the chromosomes are fusing. In Fig. 4 the 
spindle is better formed, the chromomeres are collecting near the 
equator, and some show appearances of fusing. 

In describing the maturation and fertilization of another 
marine annelid, Arenicola, Child figures and describes a “nuclear 
cavity.” I have found the same appearance in the eggs of Amphi- 
trite but my sections have enabled me to prove that the clear area, 
or “cavity,’’ represents the original position of the nucleolus. 
The latter in disappearing gradually shrinks (Fig. 1), grows 
irregular in shape, and apparently goes into solution leaving a 
clear area (Fig. 3). 

In early metaphase it is usually possible to count eleven chro- 
mosomes in the spindle (Fig. 5). A polar view of the chromo- 
somes in the early prophase shows that the chromomeres have 
not all fused. “The reduced number of chromosomes I have 
found to be eleven; this count was made at a stage shortly after 
the expulsion of the second polar globule (Fig. 9). At a later 
stage in one of the cleavage cells twenty-two were counted as the 
entire number. 


3. Nuclear Changes in Maturation 


The nuclear phenomena of maturation in Amphitrite are in 
general like those of Chztopterus as described by Mead. The 
first aster makes its appearance after the germinal vesicle breaks 
down and is first seen in a position that represents a lateral por- 
tion of the upper hemisphere of the nuclear area. “The second 
aster appears a little later, near the surface of the egg, and lies 
directly under the animal pole (Fig. 1). Both asters are now 
growing rapidly, though the second aster always remains behind 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 55 


the first in size. The spindle forms and the chromosomes arrange 
themselves in prophase; but before this process is complete the 
spindle swings around, and when the metaphase is reached it has 
migrated to the surface where it rests in its definitive radial posi- 
tion (Fig. 5). At this time the spindle is long and tapering. 
If not disturbed in any way, practically all eggs will remain indefi- 
nitely in this condition unless fertilized. But if slight agitation 
or certain chemical stimuli be applied further development may 
occur. When fertilized other changes make their appearance 
within five or six minutes. The chromosomes quickly pass 
through the anaphase, and at the time of early telophase the asters 
as well as the spindle fibers begin to fade while the spindle itself 
has become shorter and barrel-shaped (Fig. 6). In later telo- 
phase when the first polar body is being constricted off, the 
chromosomes of the inner group have approached still nearer the 
surface and remnants of the spindle and inner aster have almost 
entirely disappeared (Fig. 7). here are stages just later than 
this when no traces of this aster can be discovered. Very soon the 
second maturation spindle arises nearly parallel to the surface, 
and the chromosomes which have receded a little arrange them- 
selves in prophase. Frequently at this time the pole above this 
region shows a flattening or slight concavity and as the spindle 
turns the chromosomes pass into metaphase. The rest of the 
process in throwing off the second polar globule-is similar to that 
described for the first. Differences, however, have been 
observed. The spindle is smaller than the first and the rays of 
the aster remaining in the egg, instead of fading out in telophase, 
extend further into the cytoplasm, persisting until a later stage 


(Figs. 8 and 9). 


4. Relation of Cytoplasmic to Nuclear Phenomena in Maturation 


The ripe egg with germinal vesicle intact gives no indication of 
the animal pole if judged simply by its shape. But when the 
germinal vesicle breaks down a rapid and remarkable change 
takes place in the shape of the egg, during which the egg undergoes 
collapse or flattening in the polar diameter. Unless fertilization 
is accomplished most eggs remain indefinitely in this condition, 


56 fobn W. Scott 


with the first maturation spindle in metaphase. However one or 
both polar bodies may be thrown off and, as Mead has pointed 
out, some eggs may pass through the early cleavage stages. ‘The 
rapid change just mentioned in the shape of the egg is interesting 
when considered in connection with the coincident movement of 
the spindle to the surface. “The camera lucida drawings of Text— 


Fig. I. Diagrams to show changes in shape of eggs during expulsion of polar globules. Collapse 


in the polar diameter to show how first maturation spindle comes to the surface, in A (typical), and B 
(rather infrequent). First polar body being expelled in C, egg regaining spherical shape; D, anaphase 


of the second maturation spindle, the egg nearly spherical. Camera lucida. Leitz oc. 1, obj. 1-12. 


Fig. | show conditions found at this time. ‘The typical condition 
is shown in Fig. I, 4, where the polar diameter is to the transverse 
diameter about in the proportion of three to five. An extreme but 
frequent condition is shown in Fig. I, B. In both figures there is 
represented a thin ectoplasmic layer; an oval or elongate area, 
marking the region of the germinal vesicle; the spindle in meta- 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 57 


phase occupying a radial position, lying within a differentiated 
area in which there is very little yolk; the area in question being 
in direct communication with or lying partly within the original 
nuclear area. 

The explanation of these phenomena is apparently as follows: 
After the germinal vesicle breaks down, the nuclear sap begins to 
diffuse out through the cytoplasm in which are many yolk oranules, 
and staining reactions show that it very soon reaches the surface 
of the egg. However, owing to the slightly eccentric position of 
the egg nucleus and oye bel also to the structural organization 
of the cytoplasm, this material comes to the animal pole of the 
ege earlier than to the rest of the surface (Figs. 1 and 2). The 
action of the nuclear diffusion seems to render the cytoplasm more 
fluid and therefore less viscous, especially at the animal pole, as is 
shown by the fact that the cytoplasm usually flows out into and 
takes the shape of the inequalities of the enclosing egg membrane. 
The result is what we should expect if the nuclear sap were with- 
drawn from the nuclear area, 7. ¢., the egg flattens or undergoes 
collapse at the animal pole. In this way the surface is carried 
down to the region of the first maturation spindle that up to this 
time has moved very little or not at all from the place of its origin. 
This state of affairs is shown clearly in Text—Fig. I, 4 and B. 
The general mass of the cytoplasm now becomes more uni- 
formly affected by the nuclear sap, as the stains seem to indi- 
cate, and under the influence of surface tension the egg begins 
to slowly assume a globular shape; the spindle remains near the 
surface and so is carried away from its original position. When 
the anaphase is reached the typical condition is shown in Fig. I, C; 
the shape of the egg has become a rounded oval. 

Leaving out of consideration the causes which separate the 
chromosomes into two groups, the idea that the nuclear material 
tends to liquefy the cytoplasm suggests a hypothesis which may 
possibly explain the formation of the polar globules. When the 
chromosomes which are to form the first polar body very nearly 
touch the surface of the egg (Fig. 6), a sort of papilla-like projec- 
tion pushes out ahead of them; soon these chromosomes sur- 
rounded by a small amount of clear cytoplasm are pinched off in 


58 fohn W. Scott 


the form of a little droplet, the first polar globule. Now this is 
just what we should expect if the region immediately surrounding 
the chromosomes is rendered more liquid while the general mass 
of the egg is undergoing slow contraction in resuming the globular 
condition. ‘The formation of the second polar body, which soon 
follows the first, may be explained in the same way. Fig. I, D, 
shows the second maturation spindle in metaphase. ‘The egg has 
apparently resumed its original shape by the time the second polar 
body is completely constricted off. 


5. Fertilization. Fusion of Germ-nuclet 


By the time the second maturation spindle is in anaphase the 
sperm head has penetrated one-third to one-half the distance to 
the center of the egg. It lies slightly in advance or to one side 
of the centrosphere which is beginning to be recognized by its 
characteristic appearance. [he sperm head has begun to enlarge 
and its more pointed end is directed toward the centrosphere. 
During the telophase of the second maturation the male pronu- 
cleus (as we may now call the sperm head) continues to enlarge 
and to advance toward the center of the egg. ‘There is evidence 
to show that it breaks up into a number of chromosomes, each of 
which forms a vesicle, and that these vesicles rapidly fuse to form 
a larger vesicle. At any rate, two, three, or four lobes or vesicles 
are frequently found which fuse at a later period. ‘The sperm 
aster is clear cut and growing; the centrosphere which lies in 
advance of the pronucleus is still hazy without definite outlines. 
Just after the expulsion of the second polar body the male pro- 
nucleus has traversed approximately two-thirds of the distance 
to the center of the egg and appears as a medium-sized vesicle 
with deeply-staining reticulum. ‘There is a rather large vanishing 
aster and the centrosphere has increased in size and clearness. 
About this time the egg chromosomes begin to swell, while the 
small aster, somewhat enlarged and persisting from the matura- 
tion spindle, has at its center a clear area that has the appearance 
of a centrosphere. By the time the male pronucleus reaches a 
region near the center of the egg it takes the form of a clear vesicle 
with deep-staining and sometimes diffusely-staining reticulum. 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 59 


The female pronucleus is moving toward the center with each chro- 
mosome forming a little vesicle. “Ihe female centrosphere is 
small and ill-defined, the male centrosphere is large with well 
marked boundaries (Fig. 10). [he male pronucleus increases 
in size, but it remains almost stationary near the center 
awaiting the approach of the female pronucleus. ‘The female 
chromosome vesicles now fuse to form a vesicle which rapidly 
enlarges. [he female aster and centrosphere have in the mean- 
time entirely disappeared and only traces of the male aster may 
be seen. Fig. 11 shows the germ-nuclei in contact, approximately 
of the same size and appearance. Soon the limiting walls break 
down and the chromatin quickly collects to form the primary 
stage of the first cleavage. 


6. Cleavage 


An early spireme stage of the first cleavage presents a typical 
appearance. When the fully-formed tapering spindle takes its 
characteristic position it lies in or very nearly in a plane perpen- 
dicular to the polar axis slightly nearer the animal pole with the 
chromosomes in metaphase (Fig. 12). While the clear-staining 
area at each end of the spindle (centrosphere) becomes larger, 
the spindle itself becomes broader, blunt at both ends, and 
decidedly shorter. In Amphitrite the centrosome, whatever may 
be its function, is not a permanent organ of the cell; at least it is 
not differentiated at all times by the fixation and stains I have used. 
In Fig. 13 is seen the condition of the egg in telophase. The 
chromosomes with faint outlines are passing into the vesicular 
condition. ‘These collect and fuse in the region of the centro- 
sphere to form the resting nucleus of the daughter-cell. The 
asters are undergoing degeneration, and the cytoplasmic division 
is well under way. Fig. 14 shows the chromatin collecting in the 
vesicular nucleus in preparation for the second cleavage; the 
asters have already divided. 

The cell-lineage of Amphitrite has been described by Mead; 
and it is not necessary to describe later nuclear changes, for the 
Same processes are repeated as have been described for the first 
cleavage. “These processes may be stated briefly as follows: the 


60 Fobn W. Scott 


chromatin, contained in a clear vesicle with a sparsely-staining — 


reticulum, collects into the form of a number of threads or spireme 
which breaks up into a number of chromosomes; these collect, 
divide and pass into a vesicular condition as they approach the 
centrosphere; here their outlines become indistinguishable and 
they fuse to form the vesicular nucleus of the daughter-cell. 
No yolk lobe is formed in cleavage as is the case with ake egg of 
Cheetopterus. 

The first cleavage is unequal and in the 4-cell stage one cell, 
D, is larger than any of the others. The sagittal plane of the 
future embryo passes through this cell and the one diagonally 
opposite. ‘he cleavage is now oblique and alternates in direction 
up to the 64-cell stage face the germ layers are completely sepa- 
rated. At this stage one cell is mesoderm, seven cells are ento- 
derm, and the rest ectoderm. The complete invagination of the 
mesoderm is shown in Fig. 15. The mesoderm in this egg con- 
sists of four cells, one small cell from each large one not being 
shown. The nuclei of certain cells are moving away from the 
periphery, the first indication of the invagination of the entoderm. 
There were 104 nuclei present in this egg 


7. Structure of the T rochophore at Various Stages 


By the time the 64-cell stage is reached, the primary prototroch 
is composed of four separate groups of cells of four cells each; 
these become flattened down, never divide again, and soon become 
ciliated. ‘he completed prototroch, formed by the addition of 
certain other cells, is composed of twenty-five cells which may all 
be recognized after the larva begins to elongate. “The paratroch 
is composed of four cells differentiated at a comparatively early 
period; these persist as a ring of ciliated cells around the body 
until the larva has developed five or six metameres. The cilia 
are at first numerous and fine; later they become larger, stronger, 
and have more vigorous movements. The apical tuft is large and 
well developed in the young trochophore but it slowly atrophies 
when the body begins to seni. The normal round blastulas 

always swim up leu the soins of the dish in which they are 
kept; this is due to the location of the cilia, and the presence of a 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 61 


segmentation cavity no doubt plays a part. [he movement at 
first is chiefly rotary and gradually increases in rate. When the 
trochophore begins to increase in length, the altered shape causes 
it to swim in a more or less winding Teal with rapid movements 
that are evidently produced by deseutely correlated forces. ‘The 
regular shape of the trochophore and the correlated movements 
of the symmetrically arranged cilia are the controlling factors of 
the path through which it swims. 

The shifting of areas in the lower hemisphere—the subumbrella 
region—is the chief means by which the one-layered blastula 
becomes metamorphosed into the three-layered larva. In this 
process the contour of the egg is not very much altered. ‘The 
mesoderm and entoderm sink in, filing the segmentation cavity 
and diminishing the surface area. ‘The cells of the somatic plate 
become thinner and spread over the surface of this region. After 
the blastopore closes the subumbrella region elongates rapidly 
due to the cleavage of cells just in front of the paratroch. ‘The 
formation of metameres, the differentiation of the alimentary 
tract, of the mesoderm, of the mucous glands, of the problematical 
bodies, and of the chief part of the nervous system need no further 
mention for our comparison. 

The rate of early development in Amphitrite is quite rapid. 
The time required for maturation and first cleavage has been 
mentioned. Cell division continues with equal swiftness, cilia 
are developed by the time the 64-cell stage is reached, and under 
favorable conditions the blastula swims in from four to five hours 
after the egg is fertilized. By the time the trochophore is twenty 
hours old it is approximately pear-shaped, a stomadeum has 
developed, a large enteron is very noticeable, vacuoles are found 
in the region of the prototroch, and the brownish-black pigment 
of the eye spots is to be seen in the pretrochal region (Fig. 16). 

The figure just menticned represents a camera lucida drawing 
of a normal, living gastrula twenty-three hours old. Its shape was 
slightly altered by pressure of the cover-glass used to hold it in 
position. Some of the more characteristic details of structure 
are given and in one region the cells lining the enteron are 
shown. 


62 fobn W. Scott 
DEVELOPMENT OF THE UNFERTILIZED EGGS 
A. Description of the Living Material 


1. Methods Used in Producing Parthenogenesis, and Their 


General Effect upon the Eggs 


As soon as the worms were brought into the laboratory, each 
female was washed thoroughly from one to two minutes in fresh 
water to rid it of any chance sperm and then placed in a dish of 
sterilized sea-water until ready for use. “The Amphitrite was 
usually washed several times in fresh water, though this was found 
unnecessary as demonstrated by the control eggs. The body cavity 
of the worms was opened in sterilized sea-water by snipping the 
thin body-wall over the region of the egg pouches. In this way 
very little blood or fluid was mixed with the eggs; they did not de- 
velop so well if much superfluous material was in the solution. The 
eggs were next placed in the desired solutions, usually at once, but 
frequently after various intervals. Care was taken to avoid 
shaking or squirting the eggs unless agitation effects were desired. 

Many solutions with different dilution and time of exposure 
were employed, but the following methods have proved most 
successful in producing differentiation in unfertilized eggs of 
Amphitrite; the eggs were never all in the same stage of ripeness 
and the percentages give the best results found: 


No. oF Time PERCENTAGE OF 
MeErtHop. Soxgzton (Meraop) Exeroren: EXPoseED. Active SwIMMERs. 

2- 5 parts (8 N)Ca(NO3)> + 98-95 parts s.w. Permanently. 25 per cent. 

2 s-ro “ ($N)Ca(NOsz)2 + 95-90 “ s. w. 1 hour. I5 per cent. 

3 5 “ (@iM)KCl + 95 SoS AW. 1 hour. IO per cent. 

4 5 “ (@iM)KNO3 + 95 cSt Wi: 1 hour. 15 per cent. 

5 5 “ (4M)CaCle + 95 G gAge 1 hour. 5 per cent. 

6 By agitation. 20 per cent. 


Of the methods mentioned above the surest and most satis- 
factory is number 1, though number 2 is almost as good. The 
percentage of swimmers varies in different experiments from 
5-25 per cent. The highest percentages of swimming embryos 
found in any of my experiments were obtained by using methods 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 62 


>) 


3 and 4; in the former 30-40 per cent, in the latter 50 per cent. 
However this was from a single experiment and the results given 
were due to two causes, for these solutions were used in connection 
with the disturbance and agitation incident to handling the eggs 
in changing the solutions. Even the results given in he table for 
methods 3 and 4 may have been affected sleuly by agitation, 
as the control seemed to show. In another paper I have shown 
how extremely sensitive the egg of Amphitrite is to agitation. 
This is a very important but uncertain method of producing 
parthenogenesis in these eggs. Usually not more than 2 or 3 per 
cent of the eggs will develop in this way, occasionally 5 to 10 per 
cent, and in one case 20 per cent. In the paper mentioned I 
have called attention to periods of susceptibility to agitation; 
further experiments show that the time of susceptibility to such 
a stimulus varies within wide limits, depending chiefly upon tem- 
perature and the ripeness of the particular lot of eggs. 

A general effect of the calcium nitrate soln | is partially to 
inhibit cytoplasmic cleavage and to stimulate a division of the 
nucleus. If the eggs are nec ripe when removed from the body 
of the female, the. ae solutions do not seem to interfere with 
the expulsion of normal polar bodies or the first few cleavages. 
In the stronger solutions there is sometimes a tendency for the 
eggs to adhere or stick together when in contact, but I have never 
found giant embryos. In the potassium chlorid solution cleavage 
of cytoplasm is more aided than interfered with, especially if the 
eggs have reached the right degree of maturity. Consequently 
cleavage of the cytoplasm frequently keeps pace with that of the 
nucleus until quite a late stage. ‘The first polar body is not formed 
except in very rare cases. ‘The effect on cleavage of the potassium 
nitrate solution is much the same as that of the potassium chlorid 
solution. The effect of the calcium chlorid is more like that of 
the calcium nitrate solution. 

The eggs that develop after agitation usually do not form polar 
bodies and nearly always are unsegmented. ‘The nuclear divisions 
are as extensive and take place in the same manner that they do in 
eggs treated by the salt-solutions. Occasionally an egg will show 
a considerable amount of very irregular and abnormal cleavage 


64 Fobn W. Scott 


(Fig. 39). Cleavage of the cytoplasm without previous cleavage 
of the nucleus has not been found in developing eggs. 


2. Early Stages. 


It would be a needless task to undertake the description of 
individual experiments where much was repeated again and again 
in order to verify results and obtain comparisons of development. 


Fig. II. Diagrams to show. early development. Method No.1 used. A, B and C were types 
forty-one minutes after beginning the experiment; D, another egg sixty minutes later. Camera draw- 
ings from whole mounts. Leitz oc. 1 (15), obj. 7. 


Hence a general description will be given. The germinal vesicle 
breaks down, sometimes within a few minutes, sometimes several 
hours after the eggs are subjected to the solutions. It has been 
found that the riper the eggs are, the more rapidly does the ensuing 
differentiation take place and the nearer it approaches the normal 
development. 


ay 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 65 


One or both polar bodies may be normal, or both abnormal, in 
both size and appearance; or only one is formed, which is always 
abnormal. Most of these abnormal polar bodies can be readily 
distinguished in the living egg by their size which varies greatly 
as II, III; 44-46). When normal polar bodies are found in 
the solutions the time required for this process is also normal. 
But it takes longer to complete the process of throwing off the 
abnormal polar bodies, and the longer the process the more 
abnormal it becomes. When both polar bodies are formed, here 
as under normal conditions the egg undergoes flattening or collapse 
in the polar diameter. When no polar bodies are thrown off, the 
change in shape is never so striking and may not be noticeable 
(Figs. 40-41, 55). 

Cleavage usually begins within one and one-half hours after 
the eggs are placed in the given solution, though I have seen eggs 
in the process of cleavage ne forty minutes ene the beginning 
of an experiment. ‘The first two blastomeres of the egg are ie 
quently abnormal in size and appearance, but a study of the living 
material shows that an apparently normal cleavage may continue 
for several divisions. In every case ameboid movements, or abortive 
attempts at cleavage, occur at this time, even in eggs in which no 
distinct cleavage plane is ever formed; the egg often shows two 
or three lobes, a condition which is more common in the potassium 
chlorid cultures. The cleavage cells of an egg treated with cal- 
cium nitrate are always more compact and fit closer together than 
the normal, while the potassium chlorid eggs have the opposite 
tendency. The different types of cleavage can be best understood 
by consulting the diagrams (Figs. II, III; 17, 49, 50, 58-61). 
In six to twelve hours the cilia become strong enough to cause the 
rotation of the eggs. Up to this time cytoplasmic cleavage, when 
present, occasionally keeps pace with the nuclear divisions, but later 
it seems to lag behind more and more and often entirely ceases. 

Certain unripe eggs after being treated with potassium chlorid 
solution tend to break up rapidly into many small spherules 
(Fig. IV). Occasionally this effect is produced by some other 
solutions if the concentration is sufficient and I have found it 
rarely in the controls. “This phenomenon was noticed by Fischer, 
who says, “Sometimes the eggs go into the morula stage. The 


66 fobn W. Scott 


Y Fig. III. Diagrams to show types of early development. From whole mounts, same magnification 
as Fig. Il. £, one hundred minutes, and F-#, two hundred and twenty-five minutes after beginning 
the experiment. Dotted lines enclose nuclei. 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 67 


origin of the morulas is somewhat obscure. ‘The fact that they 
appear in solutions which have at no time contained any black 
eggs would suggest that nucleated eggs can, in a very short time, 
give rise to morulas without any external signs of cleavage.” 
Fischer thus raises the question whether nucleated eggs may not 
pass into the “morula”’ stage without any external signs of cleav- 
age and so give rise to swimming blastulas or later stages. But 
these eggs do not develop, as we shall see. In one of my experi- 
ments 95 to gg per cent of the eggs showed this condition. At 
11.30 A. M. the eggs were removed from the body of a large female 
and placed for one hour in a potassium chlorid solution. Before 
they were returned to sterilized sea-water, the germinal vesicle 
had broken down in nearly all eggs and a few showed a very small 
peri-vitelline space. ‘Thirty minutes later, at I p. M., the eggs 
showed protuberances over their surfaces as though cells were 
being constricted off. At 2.15 P.M. 
nearly all the eggs were in a multi- 
spherular condition; when pressed 
down slightly with a cover-glass no 
segmentation cavity was found and 
the central portion of the egg was 
still unsegmented. ‘The eggs were 
examined at frequent intervals until 
8.45 P. M., when the spherules were 
not more than half the size of 
those found earlier in the afternoon. Fig. IV. A typical multispherular egg 


The next morning no swimming six hours old from a potassium chlorid 


eges were to be found; many were 
apparently bordering on dissolution 
and all seemed full of small vacuoles. Sections of these eggs showed 
that the germinal vesicle had broken down in most cases and had 
diffused out into the cytoplasm before spherulization began. 
Very infrequently an ege was found with the germinal vesicle 
intact and surrounded by spherules. There was never any 
mitotic division of the nucleus. In eggs where the nuclear sap 
diffused out into the cytoplasm, there was found a more or less 
diffuse achromatin stain in each spherule. It is very evident that 


solution. Camera’ drawing. Leitz oc. I 


(16), obj. 7. 


68 ‘fohn W. Scott 


this peculiar change in some eggs is not one of differentiation, 
but is due to the effect of the potassium chlorid in altering the 
viscosity and lowering the surface tension of the egg-cytoplasm. 
Since the spherulization continues for some hours after it begins, 
it seems that the cytoplasm may undergo a ripening process, 
somewhat as the normal egg must do in order that cleavage into 
small cells may occur; and inasmuch at the process of spheruliza- 
tion continues longer in eggs where the germinal vesicle has pre- 
viously broken down, it indicates that the diffused nuclear sap 
may have something to do with this ripening process. ‘Treadwell 
(02) when working upon Podarke noticed that some of the control 
eggs broke up into a great many “small globules” with no trace 
of nuclear division, the nucleus lying in one cell which was usually 
larger than the others. He found no ciliated embryos among 
these eggs. 


3. Description of Swimming Embryos, Eighteen to Twenty- 
five Hours Old 


At this age the various cultures as a rule show their best develop- 
ment, though the swimming eggs frequently show wide differences 
in structure and appearance. ‘To illustrate these differences we 
may cite a few examples. The egg in Fig. 20, from a potassium 
chlorid solution, was found swimming near the surface of the 
water about twenty-four hours after the beginning of the experi- 
ment. A clear ectoplasmic layer is differentiated, in marked 
contrast to the yolk which lies throughout the central portion of 
the egg. The cilia projecting from the entire surface are large, 
stiff and more strongly developed in certain regions. Segmenta- 
tion is extensive; scarcely ever are eggs found with a greater 
number of cells than shown here, although the cells are not so 
numerous as in the normal egg of fifteen hours. ‘The cell bounda- 
ries are not clearly defined, indicating a tendency to fuse, and cer- 
tain regions do not show cell structure. Clear spots show the 
position of nuclear areas in the cells. “This egg was compressed 
slightly under a cover-glass until it could be examined with a high 
power, but this treatment failed to reveal the segmentation cavity 
which can be easily demonstrated in the same manner in the 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 69 


normal egg (Fig. 16). Vacuoles are sometimes present as shown 
in Fig. 20. Another egg, raised under the same conditions and 
nearly the same age, was taken from the bottom of the dish where 
the great majority of the swimming eggs are found (Fig. 21). 
The size and distribution of cilia and the arrangement of cyto- 
plasm and yolk are practically the same as in the previous case, 
but there is not a trace of segmentation and only two nuclear areas 
are found. We may take as another typical example an egg from 
a calcium nitrate solution (Fig. 28). Here we find the cilia 
arranged irregularly in three distinct groups, a well differentiated 
ectoplasmic layer, the yolk pretty evenly distributed throughout 
the interior of the egg, traces of segmentation, numerous nuclei, 
and some limited patches of brownish pigment. 

Eggs as a rule do not show such uniform distribution of cilia 
as these examples, or such regular shape (Figs. 22, 23, 26, 29, 39). 
The long apical tujt of cilia so characteristic of the normal trocho- 
phore is always absent. In shape the eggs may be comparatively 
globular, oval, or pear-shaped (Figs. 29, 24, 35); ordinarily these 
contours are represented in the active swimmers. On the other 
hand abortive cleavage may produce an irregular outline (Figs. 22, 
36), and cleavage may stop though nuclear division proceeds. 
The number of cells may vary from one to many, and where the 
egg is manifestly composed of more than one cell the boundaries 
of these cells may be indicated by mere indentations, or by com- 
plete cleavage planes; both conditions are found frequently in 
the same egg (Fig. 22). The great majority of the swimming 
eggs have no true segmentation at all and in these unsegmented 
eggs the nuclei are usually very numerous (Figs. 29, 32, 33), but 
are sometimes few in number (Fig. 37); the nuclei also differ in 
size (Fig. 33). In eggs with considerable cytoplasmic segmenta- 
tion there is as a rule one nucleus to each cell though two or three 
may be present. In all cases of swimming embryos the ectoplas- 
mic layer is pretty well differentiated and this differentiation seems 
to bear some relation to the distribution of the cilia. For where 
the cilia extend around the entire circumference of the egg there 
is a correspondingly well developed ectoplasmic layer, and where 
the yolk lies close to the surface the cilia are noticeably absent. 


7O Fohn W. Scott 


However there may be an ectoplasmic layer where no cilia develop. 
The yolk may be broken up into groups in the case of segmenta- 
tion or be scattered by nuclear division (Figs. 29, 32), and very 
frequently it exhibits a tendency to collect near one side of the 
egg, a region which I believe indicates the position of the vegeta- 
tive pole. 

Another important and significant differentiation at this period 
is the development of a boeniek pigment. If one may judge from 
color and appearance this pigment is undoubtedly homologous 
with the eye spots of the normal embryo. In the normal trocho- 
phore this pigment is definitely localized in two small brownish 
or reddish-brown spots (Fig. 16). In the parthenogenetic eggs 
the pigment is never localized in spots. Occasionally two or 
more pigmented areas are found (Figs. 26, 28, 31), but more 
frequently the pigment is found in a single diffuse mass scattered 
along near one side of the egg (Figs. 32, 35, 37). Rather infre- 
quently swimming eggs are facied which have an oblong or blunt 
pear-shape; to a casual observer they might not appear dis- 
similar to the normal trochophore (Figs. 21, 23, 255 32). ‘There 
appears to be such a differentiation in shape in the egg shown in 
Fig. 35, but I think the form is due mainly to an early segrega- 
tion of cleavage materials (yolk, cytoplasm) in which the cleavage 
division had the most prominent effect, this being followed by a 
fusion of the blastomeres. 

From what has been said about the unsymmetrical distribution 
of cilia, the lack of an apical tuft, and the irregular shape of these 
swimming structures, it 1s not surprising to find that their swim- 
ming movements are distinctly different from those of the normal 
trochophore. They do not swim so rapidly, they do not move in 
such a definite path, nor do the movements of the cilia appear so 
well correlated. 


4. Later Stages and Fate of the Parthenogenetic Eggs. 


The maximum percentage of swimming eggs is found between 
the twelfth and twenty-fifth hours, depending upon the tempera- 
ture and the rate of development of a given lot. After the twenty- 
fifth hour they die rapidly and in most experiments all are dead 


ia 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 71 


at thirty-six or thirty-seven hours, although I have raised them 
until over forty-eight hours old. Death is not due to the environ- 
ment, for the normal eggs are readily raised under the same con- 
ditions. ‘Typical conditions of eggs from the calcium nitrate 
solutions are shown in Figs. 29 and 31. In the various cultures 
the amount of segmentation tends to diminish in these later 
stages, due to fusion, wholly or in part, of adjacent blastomeres. 
Frequently, however, eggs retain a condition of definite segmeénta- 
tion which was reached at a much earlier period (Fig. 30). Fig. 32 
is typical of the oldest eggs that came under my observation. 
There is no apparent segmentation of the cytoplasm, a great many 
light areas are present some of which may be vacuoles; the yolk 
is irregularly distributed, and no pigment can be seen. In the 
same experiment quite a large per cent of the eggs were still 
swimming at the age of forty-four hours. Some of these were 
round, some oblong, some with irregular outlines, and all were 
distinctly different from the normal. Later the vacuoles increased 
in number. 

The fate of the parthenogenetic eggs may be summed up as 
follows: Segmentation of the cytoplasm remains until very late 
in substantially the same condition that it reached before the 
development of the cilia. “Those eggs which have extensive and 
strongly developed cilia survive those on which the cilia are 
limited in extent or weakly developed; these surviving eggs also 
have a shape nearest to the normal. After swimming actively 
for some time the cilia do not move so rapidly, apparently shrink 
in size and seem to disappear; the protoplasm assumes a lighter 
appearance, vacuolization sets in, and just before dissolution the 
cells, if present, tend to flow together and the egg as a whole tends 
to assume a more roundish shape. ‘The egg-wall breaks down at 
some point and soon dissolution is complete. I have seen cilia 
still vibrating after the ege-contents began to flow out. 

I have mentioned in the first part of this paper the definite 
change in shape that occurs in eggs at the time of maturation and 
I have referred to the ameboid movements of the cytoplasm that 
are found in early cleavage stages of both fertilized and unferti- 
lized eggs. In the normal eggs these ameboid movements possess 


72 Fobn W. Scott 


a definite character and relate to cleavage. Moreover they take 
place slowly in all eggs. In eggs with no segmentation these 
ameboid movements are very noticeable, for every nuclear division 
that takes place is within the limits of one cell and consequently 
the whole egg is influenced by each division of the nucleus. 
It is for this reason that ameboid movements in the unsegmented 
egg are so much more noticeable than in those eggs in which the 
cleavage iscomplete. In contrast with these movements of the egg 
connected with segmentation phenomena, there are other ameboid 

movements of a different 


a 6 Cc : : 
a kind that are sometimes 
a z : ; 
< found in the salt-solution 
eggs at a comparatively late 
Z ¥ £ h stage of differentiation. An 
ege is occasionally found 
thirty to forty-five hours 


after the beginning of the 


experiment that shows no 

ee segmentation and no cilia- 

tion but is undergoing extra- 

ordinary ameboid activity. 
aves Such an egg has a general 
opaque appearance but the 


Fig. V. Camera drawings to show rapid ameboid border is clearer than the 
movements. From a calcium nitrate solution, forty- rest of the ege. The small 
three hours old. Sketches made at one-half minute inter- pseudopodia somet 1 mes 
vals, a-h; sketches at one-quarter minute intervals, i—p. form and disappear very 
quickly. In Text-Fig. V are shown camera lucida drawings of an 
egg of this kind forty-three hours after the beginning of the 
experiment. In the first two rows, a—h, are eight successive 
sketches made at one-half minute intervals; in the third and 
fourth rows, i—p, are sketches made eleven minutes later of the 
same egg at one-fourth minute intervals. These drawings do 
not show more than half the forms that were taken by this egg 
during the time the sketches were being made. The peculiar 
differentiation Just described may be correlated I believe, with 
processes found in the normal larvas of about the same age. By 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 73 
the time the trochophore is forty hours old it has developed three 
or four well-defined body segments. During this development in 
structure a physiological differentiation has occurred so that the 
larva is now able to make comparatively quick, jerky and wrig- 
gling movements. I think without doubt that this is the sort of 
differentiation that takes place in these eggs treated with salt- 
solutions. ‘The protoplasm presents the same medium-opaque 
appearance and the movements possess the same general character. 


B. Description of the Preserved Material 
1. Early Stages 


If the eggs are quite ripe, that is, in a condition to be fertilized, 
a study of the sections of unfertilized parthenogenetic eggs dis- 
closes no abnormalities until after the expulsion of the polar 
bodies. ‘This applies as a rule only to the weaker calcium nitrate 
solutions. In strong salt-solutions the process of development is 
hindered or considerably modified. Unripe eggs when treated 
with any of the solutions always give abnormal results. Since 
any given lot of eggs removed from the body cavity usually con- 
tains many stages of ripening, there are always many forms of 
differentiation in each experiment. Fig. 45 gives a type in which 
the polar bodies depart slightly from the normal (c/. Figs. 7, 8, 9). 
It will be noticed that the first polar body is slightly larger than 
the normal due to additional cytoplasm and that the second polar 
body is still further increased in size. The chromosomes in the 
second polar body are noticeably ragged in appearance. In 
Fig. 44 these differences are still further accentuated. By refer- 
ring to Fig. 46 we find a very abnormal type of both polar bodies. 
The first polar body is in practically the same condition as the 
second polar body of Fig. 44, while the second polar body has 
increased enormously in size by the addition of yolk granules. 

The state of affairs where only one polar body is formed is 
represented fairly well by Fig. 47. The remaining chromosomes 
have retreated to near the center of the egg and are in a stringy, 
fragmentary, decidedly abnormal condition which probably cor- 
responds to a stage just previous to the first cleavage. It will be 


74 Fohn W. Scott 


noticed that the egg membrane is very thick, a condition which 1s 
produced by the calcium nitrate solutions in unripe eggs and which 
has served to distinguish certain types of differentiation at a later 
period. I have also observed, after the first polar body was 
thrown off, the chromosomes of the second polar body lying in an 
isolated group in the cytoplasm near the animal pole; the remain- 
ing chromosomes retreat to the center of the egg to take part in 
certain phenomena of cleavage. Sometimes the chromosomes 
of the first polar body are thrown of in a separate group which 
lies somewhere near the surface as in Figs 42. 

It frequently happens that no polar Redies are expelled (Figs. 
40,41). Inthiscase the egg does not collapse in its polar diameter, 
the spindle lies in or very near its original position, the chromo- 
somes, spindle and asters all show slight abnormalities, and the 
stain indicates that the egg is immature. [he chromosomes are 
not usually separated permanently by this kind of maturation 
process and frequently do not separate at all. Another abnormal 
condition of the first maturation spindle is shown in Fig. 43. In 
eggs where both polar bodies are expelled the spireme for the 
first cleavage is formed in the same manner as under normal 
conditions (Figs. 44, 45, 46). 

I have mentioned that observations on the living egg sometimes 
failed to disclose any abnormal process in Ves Soe cleavage 
stages and an examination of the sections shows that the same is 
true of the nuclear phenomena. However there is a tendency for 
the chromatin division to be fragmentary or unequal and the 
astral radiations do not possess the distinct, definite outlines of 
the normal condition (Fig. 48). Very rarely there is found a 
tripolar mitotic figure and occasionally the first division results in 
three nearly aptal cells (Fig. 50). Even when the nuclear division 
is approximately normal and complete the cytoplasmic cleavage 
is quite often incomplete or abortive. Frequently therefore more 
than one nucleus is found within the limits of one cell, confirming 
observations made upon the living egg. 

The development of eggs ied treated with potassium chlorid 
solutions does not begin il after the eggs have been returned to 
sea-water. In aoe rare cases the formation of one abnormal 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 75 


polar body has been observed in cultures from this solution. Though 
no polar bodies are formed certain peculiar phenomena take 
place within the egg at this time, the chromosomes pass into the 
vesicular condition and greatly enlarge. he vesicles are less 
numerous at first and some have faint outlines, but later there 
are from 16 to 22 in each egg. Maturation asters form in the 
proper position and a faint spindle may be present, but the spindle- 
rays do not seem to have any important influence upon the chro- 
mosome vesicles (Figs. 55, 56). Sometimes the spindle and 
vesicles move up to a region indicating the position of the animal 
pole. [he maturation asters are rather delicate in appearance 
and fade out before the chromosome vesicles fuse in preparation 
for the first cleavage division. Very commonly the maturation 
asters and spindle Ao not form at all, though the chromosome 
vesicles enlarge in the manner described Ziel e. Fig. 57 shows 
an early condition in the formation of the first cleavage spindle; 
the cleavage asters are distinctly different in size and appearance 
from the maturation asters just described. 

The cleavage phenomena in the potassium chlorid cultures 
show more variability than in solutions where the calcium nitrate 
salt was used. This variability seems to be correlated with the 
fact that an excessive amount of chromatin remains within the 
egg. At the first cleavage the amphiaster is similar but distin- 
guishable from the normal; the chromatin-division takes place in 
the normal manner, but with certain minor irregularities which 
for the most part consist in scattered and fragmentary chromatin- 
granules or chromosomes (Figs. 58, 61). Later cleavage stages 
show more abnormal conditions. Frequently a see aster 1S 
found at the first cleavage and three-celled eggs or triple groups 
of chromosome vesicles are very much more common in this than 
in the calcium solution. I have seen four, five, six, and in one 
case, seven asters all taking part in the chromatin division, and in 
some of these eggs there was no sign of cytoplasmic cleavage. 
In Fig. 61 is shown an egg which may be taken as a type; the part 
that each of five asters is taking in the distribution of the chromatin 
is readily seen, and one is not surprised to find in later cleavage 
that each cell may contain one or many nuclei and that these nucle1 


76 ‘fohn W. Scott 


may be of various sizes (Figs. 62, 63). When cleavage of the 
cytoplasm occurs the nuclear division 1s most nearly normal. 
In general a potassium chlorid culture after five or six hours con- 
tains a few eggs with many cells having one nucleus each, a great 
many more with a smaller number of polynuclear cells more or 
less incompletely divided, and some eggs undivided but multi- 
nuclear. ‘The unequal size of the nuclei in some of the cells 
indicates that the chromosome vesicles in telophase may fuse to 
form two vesicles instead of one. I believe that some of the nuclei 
may arise in this way though I have made no direct observations 
to prove it. 

Some interest attaches to the number of chromosome vesicles 
to be found in potassium chlorid eggs in the late anaphase and 
telophase of the first cleavage. A constant number cannot be 
counted owing to the fact that some usually begin to fuse before 
others have distinct and definite outlines. In the egg of Fig. 59, 
in the left-hand group of vesicles, 18 (+4?) could be counted, 
while in the other group 20 (+4!) were found. ‘The usual number 
that can be definitely counted is from 15 to 20. At a somewhat 
later stage, in an egg where the cleavage of cytoplasm was normal, 
eleven vesicles were counted in each of the two cells (Fig. 60). 
With these conditions may be compared Fig. 49, which represents 
an egg treated with calcium from which both polar bodies have 
been expelled; it is interesting to note that we have here Io (+1) 
vesicles in one group and 11 in the other (not all shown). In the 
calcium nitrate solutions where both polar bodies are formed the 
reduced number of chromosomes, eleven, appears again in the 
daughter-cells of the first cleavage. This number no doubt con- 
tinues in the later cleavage-cells. In the potassium chlorid egg 
(Fig. 59), there is evidently the full number of chromosomes in each 
of the two cells, and as no polar bodies were expelled this number 
can be accounted for on the theory of the individuality of the 
chromosome, since these chromosomes had no maturation division; 
still the number must have been reduced from 44 to 22 by fusion 
before division. But if we take a potassium chlorid egg, like 
that shown in Fig. 60, the eleven vesicles of each daughter-cell 
indicate a bivalent character retained throughout division; or 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 77 


they have assumed this character after division by fusion in twos, 
a view that is improbable from lack of any direct evidence. 
Certain difficulties prevent a thorough analysis of these phenomena 
but the above conditions may be accepted as typical. 


2. Condition of the Eggs as Shown by Sections of Later Stages 


In a general way my observations on the living egg are com- 
pletely confirmed by a study of the sectioned material, which also 
furnishes additional and interesting data. Usually cleavage 1s 
more extensive within the egg than at the surface, probably indi- 
cating fusion at the surface subsequent to cleavage. Some eggs 
are multinuclear without any signs of cytoplasmic cleavage, 
and cleavage of both cytoplasm and nucleus falls farther and 
farther behind the normal as development proceeds. More 
nuclei are uniformly found in the calcium than in the potassium 
cultures, especially in the unsegmented egg, but the potassium 
chlorid solution shows eggs with more distinct and more complete 
cleavage planes. I have been able to discover cilia along the 
edges of the prepared material of all such types of eggs as are 
represented in my drawings of late stages and there is no doubt 
that such eggs represent typical swimmers. ‘The killing fluids 
used injured or destroyed most of the cilia, hence no attempt has 
been made to figure them in the drawings and such ciliation has 
not been found of any benefit in localizing regions of the eggs 

One of the most advanced types of cleavage in the potassium 
chlorid solution at eight to nine hours is represented by Fig. 64, 
and Fig. 65 is a type near the other extreme with very little cleavage. 
In the segmented eggs mentioned there were found portions of a 
segmentation cavity and evidences of an attempt at invagination 
(cf. Fig. 15). In the egg with very little cleavage the nuclei vary 
in size and the largest one has evidently not divided for some time 
since no nuclei of a similar size were found near it. ‘This fact, that 
a nuclear vesicle in an unsegmented egg enlarges extraordinarily 
unless mitosis occurs, is in accord with Lillie’s findings for the egg of 
Cheetopterus and indicates that the size and growth of the nucleus 
are independent of mitotic division. In all developing eggs, 
whether segmented or not, the early differentiated ectoplasmic 


78 fobn W. Scott 


layer is retained. Fig. 51 represents an egg taken at fourteen 
hours from a calcium nitrate culture; the cleavage planes are 
incomplete and there is a clear attempt at invagination. 

No further very important developmental changes occur in most 
of the swimming eggs. However, conditions are occasionally 
found that throw additional light upon parthenogenetic develop- 
ment or upon causes connected with the cessation of development. 
Fig. 67 is an egg from a potassium chlorid culture at the age of 
twenty hours Shick may be taken as ty pical of the most advanced 
differentiation that I have discovered in eggs from this solution, 
although other eggs with more cells and more nuclei have been 
found. While manifest irregularities are noticeable, the invagi- 
nation of the mesoderm is easily made out and is well under way. 
Why the development has advanced no further than where it 
should have been some twelve. hours earlier, if fertilized, seems 
traceable to the following causes: ‘The number of cells is too 
small to complete invagination in the normal manner; only small 
portions of the segmentation cavity were formed, probably due to 
the action of the solution upon the surface tension of the cyto- 
plasm and perhaps to slow and imperfect cleavage; and incom- 
plete cleavage may also have hindered the invagination. In 
another egg 5 the same age from the same solution (Fig. 66), there 
was no trace of segmentation, and vacuoles, which characterize 
the fertilized eggs at about this age, were found. 

The conditions in the calcium solutions at about twenty hours 
are also interesting. Unsegmented eggs frequently show a large 
number of small nuclei (Fig. 52); this fact verifies observations onthe 
living egg. In Fig. 53, the amount of cleavage, the number and 
distribution of nuclei, the lack of any segmentation cavity, the 
ectoplasmic differentiation, and the remnants of the region of the 
germinal vesicle are all characteristic of eggs from this solution at 
fis age. 

vee rarely I have found an egg that showed a striking 
resemblance to the normal, as in Fig. 54. Although this egg is 
twenty and one-half hours of age, it is at a stage that it should have 
reached twelve hours earlier under normal conditions. The 
invagination of the mesoderm is complete; and in each of the 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 79 


two mesoderm cells there has been nuclear division without 
division of the cytoplasm. The nuclei of the entoderm cells 
(partly shown) are migrating toward the segmentation cavity and 
are thus taking the first steps toward the invagination of the ento- 
derm. Large vacuoles are found which are not present in ferti- 
lized eggs of the same number of cells but are found in fertilized 
eggs of the same age. There 1s little essential difference between 
this and a normal egg (Fig. 15). In the normal egg the cleavage 
of the cytoplasm is a little more definite and complete, the meso- 
derm consists of four different cells (two small ones not shown), 
and this egg is more symmetrical in shape. 

From the study of sections of a few well-developed unfertilized 
eggs, it would seem not impossible to raise normal gastrulas and 
even later stages from unfertilized eggs. But I have not succeeded 
in doing so, though I used the same and even better precautions 
than those that gave me success in raising the normal embryos. 


THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS 
She Brief Review of Some Previous Papers 


Before taking up the theoretical discussion it 1s desirable to 
give a short resumé of some papers relating to the subject under 
consideration. 

Mead (98, 2) tried the effect of a weak potassium chlorid 
sea-water solution upon the unfertilized eggs of Chzetopterus. 
He found that the formation of the polar bodies and the yolk lobe 
took place in a normal manner and noticed abortive attempts at 
cleavage. [he abnormal phenomena began after the matura- 
tion mitoses and the reconstruction of the egg nucleus. He came 
to two conclusions in regard to the effect produced by the potas- 
sium chlorid: “First, it is of the nature of a stimulus, compatible 
with the continuance of the normal developmental processes, and 
is not of the nature of a poison or an irritant setting up irregular, 
abnormal, and inconstant changes; second, the stimulus must be 
referred to the specific properties of the salt and not to a change 
in the density of the water in which the eggs are placed.” Morgan 
(00) repeated and verified Mead’s experiments. By increasing 


SO fobn W. Scott 


the concentration of the sea-water (NaCl, MgCl,), he found that 
development may proceed in much the same manner, but no polar 
bodies were extruded. Loeb (01) produced trochophores and 
claimed that “increase in the osmotic pressure or the loss of water _ 
on the part of the egg is the cause of the parthenogenetic develop- 
ment of the egg.” He also believed that potassium chlorid pos- 
sessed a specific effect upon the eggs of Chzetopterus. 

Fischer ('02) succeeded in raising the unfertilized eggs of 
Amphitrite to the “trochophore stage”’ by adding a small amount 
of calcium salt to sea-water and by agitating mechanically the 
sea-water in which the eggs were contained. ‘hough he noticed 
that the developing eggs “present a totally different appearance” 
from the fertilized eggs, his observations were incomplete and 
superficial. In the same paper he states that “the appearance of 
the swimming trochophores, the ciliary activity, and the general 
behavior of the parthenogenetic larvas is exactly that of the nor- 
mally fertilized larvas.” 

‘Treadwell (’02) used a potassium chlorid solution with high 
osmotic pressure to produce artificial parthenogenesis in Podarke. 
No polar bodies were formed; the nuclei were much larger and 
stained more intensely than the normal ones; ciliated structures 
were produced without cleavage; and he concluded, though his 
experiments were “incomplete,” that the cleavage which included 
both nucleus and cytoplasm was not normal, 7. e., was not karyo- 
kinetic. It seemed “as if one of the cells is to be regarded as a 
lobe of protoplasm which contains some of the nuclear material, 
and may later become completely divided by a membrane from 
the other cell.” It seems probable to me, however, that he was 
wrong in this conception. 

Recently Bullot (’04) has worked upon the eggs of the annelid 
Ophelia with the view of solving the problem of the relation 
between differentiation and cleavage. Though his results are 
inconclusive they are entitled to a briet mention here. He asks 
the question, “Do the swimming blastulas found after ten hours 
in the cultures arise from the segmented egg, or do they originate 
from the unsegmented eggst”’ By placing shallow dishes under 
the field of his microscope, he was able to make camera lucida 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite SI 


drawings of individual eggs at various intervals. For one expert- 
ment he gives a series of eight drawings the last one made nine 
and one-fourth hours after the eggs were placed in the solution. 
Out of forty-eight eggs sketched in this series sixteen were swim- 
ming at nine and one- Seutthi hours and these had arisen from eggs 
that had previously shown segmentation. His drawings show, how- 
ever, that the segmentation in these eggs was not always the same; 
yet he concludes, “These experiments show conclusively that in 
the anneiid of the genus Ophelia which was used in these experi- 
ments the parthenogenetic larvas originate from regularly seg- 
menting eggs”? (italics mine). ‘This cian 3 is also open to 
certain objections. First, he says nothing about the later fate 
of the thirty-two eggs which were not swimming at nine and one- 
fourth hours. A see of the individual eggs figured by Bullot 
shows that the segmentation does not pieced in all at the same 
rate and therefore it is unlikely that all should begin swimming 
at the same time. In Amphitrite the maximum number of swim- 
mers is sometimes not reached until ten hours after the first swim- 
ming eggs are seen; the earliest swimming structures always show 
more or less segmentation, but some hours later swimming eggs 
are found with little or no cleavage. ‘This latter condition is partly 
due to fusion of blastomeres but more frequently to the fact that 
the egg has not divided or that cleavage has been abortive. It is 
also difficult to understand what is meant by the phrase, “regu- 
larly segmenting eggs.” He cannot mean the definite, determi- 
nate cleavage characteristic of the fertilized Annelid egg, for his 
figures show otherwise. From his own experiments Bullot 
attempts to cast doubt upon the results obtained by Lillie. It is 
readily seen that no weight should-be attached to this objection. 

I shall take the privilege of referring to the important paper of 
Lillie on Chztopterus (02) as the discussion demands. 


2. Effects of the Salt-solutions and of Shaking 


There are several reasons why the eggs of Amphitrite are 
favorable subjects for experimental studies of this kind. In the 
fertilized egg the characteristic changes in shape at the time when 
the polar bodies are expelled, the determinate type of cleavage 


$2 Fobn W. Scott 


resulting in an early segregation of the germ-layers, the rapid 
ieiliesanaa, and early appearance of important differentiations, 
all furnish favorable points for comparison. The unfertilized 
egg responds not only to treatment with certain salt solutions but 
is also highly susceptible to mechanical agitation. A specific 
salt, calcium nitrate, may be used but the egg develops nearly as 
well, especially if slightly agitated, in solutions where the osmotic 
pressure has been raised (potassium chlorid, potassium nitrate). 
These conditions applied separately to the egg give some important 
modifications in development, but the indirect, or general, effect of 
any one method is to bring about certain differentiations found in 
the fertilized egg. It is not claiming too much then to say that 
these differentiations are independent of the particular method 
used, 7. ¢., they depend upon the organization of the egg. We may 
also speak of the direct or specific effects upon the unfertilized egg 
produced by each particular method. 

Herbst has shown that cells in calcium-free sea-water tend to 
separate from each other, and he (04) regards their tendency to 

“Join together” in calcium-containing sea-water as a reversible 
coagulation process. ‘This effect of salem nitrate then would 
ultimately hinder cleavage and gastrulation as in the eggs of 
Amphitrite. But when the osmotic pressure 1s increased either 
with the calcium or potassium salt, the polar bodies are no longer 
thrown off; the egg does not collapse when the germinal vesicle 
breaks down, thus preventing the maturation spindle from getting 
to the surface. It is probable that an excessive absorption of water 
tends to prevent the collapse of the egg and so prevents the for- 
mation of the polar bodies. Herbst (’04) has shown that potas- 
sium is important for growth and that it leads to the taking up of 
water. The appearance of Amphitrite eggs after treatment with 
potassium chlorid is in this way accounted for if we consider the 
blastomeres as swelling up with absorbed water (Fig. 17). The 
strong solution also destroys, or weakens, some of the processes 
of maturation (asters, spindle) and at the same time causes the 
chromosomes to pass into a vesicular form and grow to an unusual 
size. ‘lhe question arises, Is the suppression of the maturation 
asters due to the chemical effect of the salt, or to an increased 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 83 


osmotic pressure? I am inclined to the latter alternative. ‘The 
effect is only temporary and this would probably not be the case 
if the chemical nature of the egg had been changed. Another 
argument in favor of this view is ‘that i in Cheetopterus eggs weak 
solutions of potassium chlorid do not hinder the process ae matura- 
tion while strong solutions do. ‘There is also evidence to show 
that differentiating processes are taking place at the same time 
though they may not be expressed in the normal morphological 
form; for we find in proper sequence a reconstitution of the egg 
nucleus by a fusion of the enlarged chromosome vesicles and the 
ordinary phenomena of mitotic cleavage division. Besides I have 
shown in an earlier paper that “there are processes of differentia- 
tion going on in the unfertilized eggs of Amphitrite which may be 
started into activity at definite intervals by mechanical agitation.” 

After agitation the unripe eggs do not expel the oalee bodies 
for the reasons which have been ruerconed. In ripe eggs shaking 
causes such a disturbance in the organization of be cy ‘gplnen: 
that cleavage is usually absent, and when present 1s very irregular 
(Fig. 39). It is probable that this treatment so coagulates or at 
least so alters the protoplasm in the ripe egg that it fails to flatten 
in the polar diameter, and the physical conditions are then unfavor- 
able for the expulsion of the polar globules. 

The action of potassium chlorid on cleavage cells and in producing 
spherules may be applied in explanation of certain phenomena 
described by Lillie and Treadwell, who worked with strong con- 
centrations of this solution. “Treadwell neticed in Podarke eggs, 
a great variety of cytoplasmic cleavages. Lillie (02) describes 
as a very common occurrence a sort of pseudo-cleavage which was 
due to the separation of pseudopodia. But he also found in the 
two cell-stage, where the nucleus passed into one cell, that the 
direction of the cleavage plane was normal. In other words we 
have here two factors at work, one a direct effect of the solution 
in altering the viscosity or consistency of the egg cytoplasm, the 
other a process connected with the differentiation of the egg. 
It seems apparent that pseudo-cleavage and the production of 
spherules are independent of cytoplasmic differentiation; these 
conditions are the results of a general effect of the salt upon the 


84. Fobhn W. Scott 


egg protoplasm and have nothing to do with development. 
I may add that I have repeated some of Lillie’s experiments, have 
confirmed his results for the living egg, and find further support 


for this view. 
3. Is this Development Parthenogenesis ? 


In discussing whether this sort of development should be termed 
parthenogenesis or not, this much is clear: we find certain recog- 
nizable differentiations that are found normally in fertilized eggs. 
In the fertilized egg these differentiations are closely correlated 
in time and place, in the unfertilized egg the correlation is less 
complete. In the early development of the calcium-treated 
unfertilized egg of Amphitrite there are found all the processes (as 
indicated by mitotic division, cleavage, cilia, etc.), that are charac- 
teristic of normal development. In so far then we may use the 
term parthenogenesis, and if we wish to distinguish it from par- 
thenogenesis that occurs under normal conditions we may call it 
“artificial.” But on the other hand we must consider that 
physiologically self-sustaining organisms are not produced; we 
must remember that these processes become more and more 
divergent as this pseudo-development proceeds and that a normal 
larva is never produced even under favorable circumstances. 
Therefore we cannot speak of this development as parthenogenesis, 
meaning the production of a normal embryo from an unfertilized 
egg. [he end result is always abnormal. 


4. Relation of Differentiation and Cleavage 


We may speak of differentiation as possessing a morphological 
or a physiological character, and in a wide sense the problem of 
the origin and process of differentiation is the problem of develop- 
ment. But in this discussion the term will be used to mean any 
specific morphological characters or physiological processes that 
are clearly homologous to characters or processes in the develop- 
ment of the fertilized egg. ‘The definition has no reference to the 
accurate localization of organs and substances or to the correlation 
of processes necessary to normal development. Used in this sense 
the important differentiations that I have found in the unfertilized 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 85 


egg of Amphitrite are the following: ‘The early development of a 
layer of ectoplasm and with it a tendency for the yolk to collect 
centrally, the formation of cilia, the development of a brownish- 
black pigment, the appearance of vacuoles that are found in the 
fertilized egg of the same age, ameboid movements that are con- 
nected with cleavage, and ameboid movements at a late stage that 
are independent of cleavage. We have seen that these forms 
of differentiation are relatively independent of cytoplasmic cleavage 
and the expulsion of polar globules. What then 1s the nature of 
these differentiations’ ‘The growth of cilia shows that there has 
arisen in these regions a newspecific composition of the cytoplasm. 
The development of a pigment likewise indicates a chemical com- 
position which was not present before. “The rapid ameboid move- 
ments occurring at a late period suggests a functional difference 
in the cytoplasm, and the slow ameboid movements connected 
with cleavage depend upon processes of differentiation which 
modify the viscosity of the cytoplasm. The presence of vacuoles 
at a definite period after development begins, points to the con- 
clusion that they are caused by processes which have their basis 
in the organization of the egg. [he same may be said of the 
early differentiation of a layer of ectoplasm and the processes 
involved in the transformation of the asters and the mitotic 
division of the nucleus. In all of these differentiations we find 
evidence of preceding morphological organization or chemical 
activity, a fact in agreement with the generally accepted idea that 
morphological differentiation usually involves and is preceded by 
differentiation of a chemical character. “The nature of differentia- 
tion therefore depends ultimately upon the organization of the egg, 
which has its basis in cytological structure and specific chemical 
composition. That the unfertilized egg of Amphitrite gives 
evidence of both morphological and physiological organization 1s 
shown by the following facts. Polarity is present from a very 
early stage and the yolk is deposited in relation to this polarity. 
The cytoplasm shows a difference in permeability and the definite 
way in which the ripe egg collapses preceding maturation 1s cer- 
tainly dependent upon the structure of the egg. But in addition 
to these morphological characters we can prove the presence of 


86 ‘fobn W. Scott 


conditions in the egg which, if disturbed by mechanical agitation, - 
lead to certain kinds of differentiation and development. ‘The 
organization in many other eggs is well known; for example, the 
sea-urchin (Boveri, ’01), Unio (Lillie, ’o1), Crepidula (Conklin, 
’02), the Ctenophore (Fischel, ’03), and the Ascidian (Conklin,’03). 

These phenomena of differentiation are relatively independent 
of each other and, since they are equally independent of cleavage, 
Lillie (02) has pointed out that the cleavage, therefore, cannot be 
considered the cause of any of these differentiations, and we may 
add that the same is true of the processes involved in throwing off 
the polar bodies, unless one excepts the fact that the extra amount 
of chromatin is conducive to the formation of multipolar spindles. 
The development of cilia is relatively independent of nuclear 
division, for cilia are found on eggs that have many or very few 
nuclei. A similar relation holds for the development of vacuoles. 
On the other hand considerable nuclear division seems always to be 
present whenever pigment is found. In like manner transforma- 
tion of the asters is always accompanied by division of the nucleus, 
and both processes are followed by more or less effectual attempts 
at cleavage. In regard to the rapid ameboid movements at a 
late stage, I have studied such a small number of eggs that it would 
be unsafe to draw general conclusions. Whenever observed these 
eges had none of the other differentiations mentioned and were 
without cleavage, but the cytoplasm possessed a light appearance 
which is characteristic of normal development at about the same 
age. 

It is well known that nuclear without cell division may take 
place normally in the eggs of Arthropods. Various methods have 
been used with success to produce the same process by artificial 
means; we have also seen how the ripeness of the egg determines 
whether the two processes shall occur together, the treatment being 
the same in each case. ‘The study of enucleated, fertilized egg- 
fragments has added further data to this question. The multi- 
plication and transformation of asters in such egg-fragments has 
been found to occur independent of the nucleus; it has been shown 
further that this may be found without (Boveri, ’97), or with 
division of the cytoplasm (Zeigler, ’98; Wilson, ’or; Boveri, 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 87 


M. ’03). Boveri (97) and Driesch (’98) have contributed the 
additional fact that the rhythm of cleavage in hybrid-fertilized 
ege-fragments depends upon the egg-cell and is not controlled by 
the sperm. It is probable then that the rhythm of cleavage 
depends upon the cytoplasm and not upon the nucleus. Conklin 
(02) concluded that the position of the spindle is the result of 
movements and stresses in the cytoplasm and that the position of 
the spindle and direction of division are functions of the cyto- 
plasm. He has also called attention to the importance of cyto- 
plasmic movements in causing early differentiations in cell- 
divisions. Lillie (98) demonstrated in the ege of Unio that the 
size of the cells and the tate and direction of cleavage possess a 
prospective significance and he concluded that these phenomena 
were explainable, “by the hypothesis of differentiation of the. 
cytoplasm into materials of different qualities and positions.” 
It will be remembered also that he found cytoplasmic without 
nuclear division in the egg of Chaetopterus. ‘Treadwell (’98) in 
comparing an egg with equal (Podarke) with anosher with unequal 
cleavage (Amphitrite), says, “There can be no question that in 
Podarke there is as great differentiation as in any Annelid of the 
unequal type.’ Wilson (or) found that the nuclear area may 
give rise to a monaster which passes through transformations 
parallel to normal division. During telophase the egg frequently 
becomes ameboid and later it may actually divide into a number 
of irregular masses, one of which contains a nucleus. From this 
study of the parthenogenetic egg of Toxopneustes he drew the 
conclusion, “that any or all of the asters, whether connected with 
the nucleus or not, may operate as centers of cytoplasmic division, 
though if unconnected with the nuclear material the activity ordi- 
narily goes no further than an abortive attempt to divide.” 
Having mentioned some of the facts in regard to these phenomena 
let us return once more to the conditions in Amphitrite. 

My observations show that the more nearly normal the asters 
and the astral radiations are, the nearer the cytoplasmic cleavage 
approaches the normal. ‘That is, when the centrospheres develop 
into the typical large, clear areas and the astral radiations reach 
far into the cytoplasm with straight, strong, and clearly defined 


$8 fobn W. Scott 


rays, che cytoplasmic cleavage is normal. But when the centro- 
sphere and asters are poorly developed, the cleavage of the cyto- 
plasm is more or less incomplete or abortive, whether the egg be 
fertilized or not. “The same thing may be said in regard to the 
relation between abnormalities in the asters and the division of 
the chromatin, only in this case the fragmentary and unequal 
division of the chromatin is apparent in late metaphase before the 
aster has reached its full growth. 

From a study of these facts it is seen that the development and 
transformation of the asters is the most general of all these phe- 
nomena. And since this is the process that invariably precedes 
cleavage, we may consider it the expression of the active forces 
which so alter the viscosity and surface tension of the egg-cell that 
cleavage is the result. ‘his view does not in any way conflict 
with the conditions found in Unio and Crepidula, where the place 
of cleavage is prearranged in the cytoplasm. Looked at from this 
point of view cleavage is an incident correlated with certain 
phenomena of differentiation, and its purpose is adaptive to the 
needs of the organism. And if we regard the egg as a simple 
mosaic, as all recent works seem to indicate, then cleavage is a 
tendency to localize or isolate processes of differentiation by 
separating them with cell walls. When each new cell wall becomes 
established greater differentiation becomes possible. “Uhe form of 
cleavage results from organization and differentiations in the egg, 
and is not itself a process of differentiation. 


5.- Causes of the Cessation of Development 


In our description and discussion of the unfertilized egg, we 
noted various conditions which hindered or stopped further 
development. But the facts given, instead of being causes for no 
further development, were in most cases simply a statement of the 
conditions within the egg after it had become so abnormal that it 
was incapable of regulating or correlating further processes. 
Let us see if we can trace these conditions to more immediate 
causes. From our understanding of the relation of cleavage to 
differentiation, it is easy to see why there is no further develop- 
ment in unripe eggs. For in these eggs the viscosity of the cyto- 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 89 


plasm is unfavorable to cleavage and lack of cleavage prevents 
the localization of various differentiating processes. Even in the 
fertilized eggs where the cytoplasm is not quite ripe we find the 
same retarding effects and ultimately the same cessation in develop- 
ment. ‘lhe interesting question for us then is, Why does the 
development of the unfertilized eggs stop though they are 
apparently in a ripe condition? In such eggs polar bodies are 
formed in quite the normal manner, the early cleavages have the 
same outward appearance and practically the same rhythm as the 
fertilized ege. ‘That development ceases is not from a lack of 
nutritive material for this is sufficient in quantity and in a condi- 
tion suitable for metabolism. It is not from a lack of formative 
material for the entire egg is present and the cytoplasm is of a 
proper ripeness. But whenever abnormalities appear they are 
first found in connection with the asters, and following or accom- 
panying them we find unequal and fragmentary divisions of the 
chromatin. Abnormal phenomena in all other respects appear 
later. It is therefore logical to conclude that the cessation of devel- 
opment has some intimate connection with the developmental 
processes concerned with the growth and transformation of the 
asters, and that the latter process is in close relation with the 
periodic nuclear changes. Let us examine still other conditions 
found in the egg. 

‘The entrance of the sperm certainly causes profound changes in 
the cytoplasm since no other sperm then finds it possible to enter 
the egg. ‘That the effect of the various salts I have used is not the 
same as that of the sperm is proved by the fact that swimmers 
may be developed by these methods from eggs that are too unripe 
to be fertilized by the sperm. In the one case the sperm is kept 
out by the unripe cytoplasm, in the other the solution penetrates 
or alters the cytoplasm and causes the germinal vesicle to break 
down, thus setting free forces that lead to nuclear division and 
other phenomena of development. But the fact that some of 
these eggs may be started in development by mechanical agita- 
tion indicates that simply a change in the state of the protoplasm 
is sufficient to start development. Mathews and Whitcher (’03) 
have concluded that, “For artificial parthenogenesis nothing else 


go ‘fohn W. Scott 


is necessary than that this change be produced.” Nevertheless 
we have seen that something else is necessary in order to insure the 
development of normal larvas, and that this something has to do 
with asters and chromatin. It is probable that agitation in some 
way causes the walls of the germinal vesicle to break down and 
thus permits reactions between nucleus and cytoplasm.’ Now it 
is clear that the effect of the methods I have used to produce 
artificial parthenogenesis 1s not to cause a cessation of develop- 
ment for the end result is practically the same in each case. 
Primarily of course the egg lacks the elements introduced by the 
sperm, the most important being the male chromosomes and the 
sperm centrosome. But inasmuch as normal mitosis may take 
place without the sperm, and on account of the inconstant presence 
of a distinguishable centrosome in eggs of Amphitrite, it is evident 
that we may neglect these factors so far as the question under con- 
sideration is concerned. In any case they can bear only a sub- 
sidiary relation to cleavage and differentiation. 

Perhaps no other fact is so evident, and at the same time so 
little understood, as the reaction of nucleus and cytoplasm in 
development. When these relations are solved there is little doubt 
that we shall obtain thereby at least some of the causes of mor- 
phogenic metabolism. We know that by supplying chemically- 
known food constituents we may produce the growth and repro- 
duction of the yeast-cell, and in its development it follows ordi- 
nary laws of chemical reaction. ‘There are many reasons to 
believe that similar processes take place in the animal cell. But 
here the chemical complexity hides from us at present the exact 
nature of many of the processes of development and differentia- 
tion which could be easily understood. All my experiments indi- 
cate in a most striking manner the intimate relation that exists 
between cytoplasmic and nuclear differentiation; the correlation 
in development between these two factors is very complete where a 
normal organism results. And inasmuch as the cessation of 
development is a culminative process, that is, the abnormalities 
appear in successive transformations of the asters and nucleus, 
we must look upon the cessation of development as due to incom- 
plete reactions between the nucleus and the cytoplasm, each suc- 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite gt 


cessive reaction depending in some measure at least upon the 
preceding one. In applying this conception to Amphitrite, the 
question is still left open as to whether the cessation of develop- 
ment is a qualitative or quantitative divergence from the normal 
course of events. While it is probably true that the nuclear 
material derived from the male is qualitatively different from that 
derived from the female, as is shown by inherited characters, so 
far as the phenomena found in the unfertilized eggs of Amphitrite 
are concerned, the abnormality reveals itself first as a quantitative 
and not as a qualitative difference. I refer to eggs where only 
half the number of chromosomes are present. Now, where two 
chemical substances are combined in definite proportions to pro- 
duce a series of chemical phenomena, if only one-half the required 
amount of one of these be taken, it is evident that the series of 
reactions will be incomplete, and the process will come to an end 
sooner than under normal conditions; we may suppose the reac- 
tion in the beginning differs simply in quantity from the normal, 
and that qualitative differences may possibly appear later due to 
reactions set up with the substance found in excess. ‘This con- 
ception then applies to the egg treated with calcium nitrate where 
both polar bodies are expelled; the first mitotic division does not 
differ in kind from the normal but only in the number of chro- 
mosomes that undergo splitting at metaphase. 

The conditions of my experiments have been such that in ripe 
eggs either both or no polar bodies were expelled. If a means 
could be devised to retain the chromosomes of the second polar 
body within the ripe egg, it would be interesting to note if normal 
development occurred. For we should then have the normal 
number of chromosomes. In the potassium chlorid more than the 
normal number are present; connected with this is a tendency to 
form multipolar asters as in double-fertilized eggs, though the 
normal number of chromosome vesicles or only half this number 
may be present in the telophase of the first cleavage division. 
The frequent occurrence of the multipolar asters might be 
explained as due to a tendency of the excess of chromatin to collect 
in more than one vesicle during the telophase of the preceding 
division. Or, it may be a tendency for the chromosomes that 


92 Fobhn W. Scott 


normally constitute the polar bodies to maintain their individuality 
in groups. But in all cases the development stops, and it cannot 
always be due to an insufficient amount of chromatin. While the 
suggestion of Boveri (02) that the chromosomes are qualitatively 
different, may be accepted as a possibility, it seems probable that 
in Amphitrite the sperm chromosomes are a means of producing 
morphogenic processes which the normal number or half the 
normal number of female chromosomes may start but find it 
impossible to continue for any length of time. However there is 
still the possibility that the strong solution so affects the cyto- 
plasm that it is incapable of reacting on the nucleus in the normal 


manner. 


SUMMARY OF RESULTS 
Normal Egg 


1. In general, the early development (maturation, fertiliza- 
tion) conforms to the typical Annelid type. For cleavage and 
later development, so far as my observations go, I have verified 
the work of Mead. 

2. Under normal conditions the egg is retained in the body 
cavity until the first maturation spindle is in metaphase. When 
deposited and left undisturbed the egg rests in this condition. 
If fertilized the formation of the polar bodies and the ensuing 
development follows. 

3. The ripe egg shows a distinct polarity as evidenced by the 
eccentric position of the germinal vesicle. This polarity is found 
before any yolk is laid down in the cytoplasm and the deposition 
and arrangement of the yolk is apparently determined in relation 
to the nucleus. ‘There is thus indicated a definite structural 
organization of the egg at a very early period. 

4. Peculiar and perfectly definite cytoplasmic changes occur 
in connection with the formation of the polar globules. 

5. The reduced number of chromosomes is eleven, the somatic 
number twenty-two. The eleven chromosomes found at the 
metaphase of the first maturation division are probably derived 
from eleven groups of chromomeres (chromosomes) of four each. 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 93 


6. The rate of development is comparatively rapid. ‘The 
primary germ lay ers are entirely separated at the 64-cell stage, 
and blastulas swim within four to five hours after fertilization. 


Unfertilized Eggs 


1. Under the conditions of the experiments certain forms of de- 
velopment occur with or without cleavage and with or without the 
formation of polar bodies in the unfertilized egg of Amphitrite. 
Such development takes the form of nuclear ee. the early 
differentiation of a layer of cytoplasm, the growth of cilia, the 
appearance of vacuoles that are found in the fertilized ege of the 
same age, the development of a brownish pigment, the ameboid 
movements of the cytoplasm that are connected with cleavage, 
the ameboid movements at a later stage of development that 
appear entirely independent of cleavage, aad the change in shape 
of the egg which in most cases at least 1s connected with incom- 
plete, arrested or abortive division of the cytoplasm. ‘The apical 
tuft of cilia which is characteristic of trochophores from fertilized 
eggs is always absent. 

2. [he means employed for producing this development were, 
(a) the use of certain salt solutions and (6) some method of 
agitating the eggs. 

3. he calcium nitrate solution used stimulated the for- 
mation of polar bodies, produced nuclear division, and tended to 
cause a fusion of the cleavage cells. “The potassium chlorid pre- 
vented the formation of the polar bodies, stimulated division of 
the nucleus, and where cell division was complete tended to sepa- 
rate the blastomeres. 

4. Though some eggs would occasionally stick together no 
actual fusions took place. 

5. Cleavage when present is usually abnormal; it tends to 
grow more abnormal as development proceeds and [| have never 
seen a perfectly normal type of cleavage at a late stage. The 
segmentation cavity is therefore wanting or only partially 
developed. ‘There was no cytoplasmic without preceding nuclear 
division. 

6. The cleavage asters, especially the astral radiations, fre- 


94. Fobn W. Scott 


quently show imperfections at the first cleavage. [he rays tend 
to be weak and fragmentary and do not take such a clear, definite 
stain as the normal. 

7. Nuclear division is always mitotic. It may be equal at 
first but sooner or later becomes unequal and fragmentary. In 
the potassium chlorid solutions multipolar mitoses are common; 
this is undoubtedly associated with the extra amount of chromatin 
retained in the egg. In later stages a nuclear vesicle may enlarge 
extraordinarily unless mitosis occurs. 

8. Normal polar bodies are expelled in the weaker calcium 
nitrate solution if the eggs are in a ripe condition. If the cyto- 
plasm is not ripe the egg does not alter its shape; consequently 
the maturation spindle though normal in other respects does not 
come to the surface and the polar bodies are not expelled. Solu- 
tions with high osmotic pressure prevent entirely or permit only 
partial formation of very weak asters and spindle that are incom- 
petent to cause maturation division. Moreover the chromosomes 
swell up into large vesicles at this time. 

g. The “morula” of Fischer is in all probability not a form 
of development, since no differentiations characteristic of normal 
development are ever found. 

10. Amphitrite eggs are very susceptible to agitation, especially 
at certain periods of development. 

11. Results obtained from any experiment depended much 
upon the ripeness of the given lot of eggs. The riper the eggs, 
the more rapidly does differentiation take place and the nearer 
it approaches the normal development. 

12, The rate of development is always slower than for the 
fertilized eggs. [he germinal vesicle breaks down sometimes 
within a few minutes, sometimes several hours after being treated 
with the solution. ‘The first swimmers are usually noticed seven 
to ten hours after the beginning of the experiment, and ordinarily 
the maximum number of swimming eggs is found between the 
twelfth and twenty-fifth hours. 

13. It has not been found possible to produce physiologically 
self-sustaining organisms by these methods. The differentiation 
always diverges more and more widely from the normal and con- 


= ae 


Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite 95 


sequently we do not find in the late stages any regulation of abnor- 
malities or any true correlation. 

14. That death is not caused by environment is proved by the 
fact that fertilized eggs may be raised under the same conditions. 


REFERENCES. 


Bovert, M., ’03.—Ueber Mitosen bei einseitiger Chromosomenbindung. Jen. 
Zeitsch., Bd. xxx, S. 401. 


Boveri, TH., 97.—Zur Physiologie der Kern. und Zellteilung. Sitz.-Ber. Phys. 
Med. Ges. z. Wiirzburg. 
‘o1.—Die Polaritat von Ovocyte, Ei und Larve des Strongylocentrotus 
lividus. Zodl. Jahr. Abthiel. f. Anat., Bd. xiv. 
*o2.—Mehrpolige Mitosen als Mittel zur Analyse des Zellkerns. Verh. 
d. Phys. med. Gesell. z. Wurzburg, Bd. xxxv, 


Buttot, G., ’o4.—Artificial Parthenogenesis and Regular Segmentation in an 
Annelid (Ophelia). Archiv fur Entwickelungsm. der Organis- 


men, Bd. xviii. 


Cuitp, C. M., ’97.—The Maturation and Fertilization of the Egg of Arenicola 
marina. ‘Trans. N. Y. Acad. Sci., vol. xvi. 


Conkiin, E. G., ’98—Protoplasmic Movement as a Factor of Differentiation. 
Biol. Lect., Woods Hole in 1898-99. 
’o2.—Karyokinesis and Cytokinesis in the Maturation, Fertilization and 
Cleavage of Crepidula and other Gasteropoda. Jour. Acad. 
Nat. Sci., Phila., series 2, vol. xi. 
°03.—Lecture at Marine Biological Laboratory, 1903. Biol. Bull., 
June, 1904. 
Drirscu, H., ’98—Ueber rein-miitterliche Charactere an Bastardlarven von 
Echiniden. Archiv f. Entwickelungsm. d. Organismen, Bd. vii. 


FiscHEL, A., ’03.—Entwickelung und Organ-Differenzirung. Archiv f. Ent- 
wickelungsm. d. Organismen, Bd. xv. 


FiscHer, M. H., ’o2—Further Experiments on Artificial Parthenogenesis in 
Annelids. Am. Jour. Physiol., vol. vii. 


Hersst, C., ’04.—Ueber die zur Entwickelung der Seeigel-larven nothwendigen 
anorganischen Stoffe, ihre Rolle und ihre Vertretbarkeit. III. 
Teil. Die Rolle der nothwendigen anorganischen Stoffe. Archiv 
f. Entwickelungsm., Jan., 1904. 


96 Fobn W. Scott 


Linu, F. R., ’98.—Adaptation in Cleavage. Biol. Lect. Wood’s Hole, 1898. 


°or.—The Organization of the Egg of Unio Based on a Study of the 
Maturation, Fertilization and Cleavage. Jour. Morphol., vol. 17. 


°o2.—Differentiation without Cleavage in the Egg of the Annelid Chz- 
topterus pergamentaceous. Archiv f. Entwickelungsm., Bd. xiv. 


Loes, J., °01.—Experiments on Artificial Parthenogenesis in Annelids (Chztop- 
terus) and the Nature of the Process of Fertilization. Am. Jour- 
Physiol., vol. iv, No. 9. 
’02.—Ueber Methoden und Fehlerquellen der Versuche tiber kiinstliche 
Parthenogenesis. Archiv f. Entwickelungsm., Bd. xiii. 


Loes, J., Fiscuer, M. H., anp Netrtson, H., ’°03.—Weitere Versuche tber kinst- 
liche Parthenogenesis. Vorlaufige Mittheilung. Pfliiger’s Archiv., 


Bd. Ixxxvil. 


Matuews, A. P., anpD WuitcHer, B. R., ’03.—The Importance of Mechanical 
Shock in Protoplasmic Activity. Am. Jour. Physiol., vol. viii. 


Meap, A. D.,’97.—The Early Development of Marine Annelids. Jour. Morphol., 
vol. xiii. 
’98.—The Origin and Behavior of the Centrosomes in the Annelid Egg. 


Jour. Morphol., vol. xiv. 


*98.—The Rate of Cell-Division and the Function of the Centrosome. 
Biol. Lect. Wood’s Hole, 1896-97. 


Morean, T. H., ’95.—Studies of the Partial Larvae of Sphzrechinus. Archiv f. 
Entwickelungsm., Bd. 11. 
‘oo.— Further Studies on the Action of Salt-solutions and of Other Agents 
on the Eggs of Arbacia. Archiv f. Entwickelungsm., Bd. x. 


Scott, J. W., °03.—Periods of Susceptibility in the Differentiation of Unfertilized 
Eggs of Amphitrite. Biol. Bull., June, 1903. 


TREADWELL, A. L., ’98.—Equal and Unequal Cleavage in Annelids. Biol 
Lect., Wood’s Hole, 1898. 


‘o2.—Notes on the Nature of “Artificial Parthenogenesis” in the Eggs 
of Podarke obscura. Biol. Bull., Oct., 1902. 


Witson, E. B., ’or.—Experimental Studies in Cytology I. A Cytological Study 
of Artificial Parthenogenesis in Sea-urchin Eggs. Archiv f. 
Entwickelungsm., Bd. xii. 


Parthenogenetic Development of Ampbitrite 97 


Witson, E. B., ’o1—02.—II. Some Phenomena of Fertilization and Cell-division 
in Etherized Eggs. III. The Effect on Cleavage of Artificial 
Obliteration of the First Cleavage-furrow. Archiv. f. Entwick- 
elungsm., Bd. xiii. 


ZiEGLER, H. E., ’98.—Experimentelle Studien uber die Zelltheilung. I. Archiv 
f. Entwickelungsm., Bd. vi. 


DESCRIPTION OF FIGURES. 


The essential features of all figures were drawn with a camera lucida. The tube length used is 
written in (cm.) immediately after the size of the ocular. 

Reference letters.—a, Apical tuft of cilia; c, chromomeres (chromosomes); d, egg membrane; 
e, brownish pigment; f, female chromosome vesicle (vesicles); g, polar globules; /, layer of ectoplasm; 
m, male pronucleus; me, mesoderm cells; 1, nucleus, nuclear areas; nu, nucleolus; p, peri-vitelline 
space; pr, prototroch; a, paratroch; r, region affected by nuclear sap; s, segmentation cavity; 
v, vacuoles; x, clear spot left by nucleolus; y, yolk granules (yolk). 


Prater I. 


All figures to illustrate the normal development. 

Fig. 1. Unfertilized egg after breaking down of germinal vesicle. Shows collapse of the egg, 
origin of asters, formation of chromosomes, dissolution of nucleolus, and a region differentiated by 
escape of nuclear sap. Leitz oc. 1 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 2. Unfertilized egg; asters better developed but other conditions perhaps not quite so advanced 
asin Fig. 1. Leitz oc. 1 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 3. First maturation spindle forming; nucleolus has dissolved leaving a clear spot. Leitz 
oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 4. First maturation spindle has rotated and now lies in the polar axis; chromosomes in early 
prophase. Leitz oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 5. Metaphase of first maturation spindle; peri-vitelline space formed. Leitz oc. 4 (20), 
obj. I-12. . 

Fig. 6. Late anaphase, first maturation. Leitz oc, 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 7. Condition just after expulsion of the first polar body. Leitz oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 8. Late anaphase, second maturation spindle. Fifteen minutes after fertilization. Leitz 
oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 9. Shortly after expulsion of the second polar body. Fifteen minutes after fertilization. 
Leitz oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 10. Age, after fertilization, twenty minutes. The male pronucleus in central position; female 
chromosomes passing into the vesicular condition and retreating toward the center of the egg. Leitz 
oc. 1 (18), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 11. The greatly enlarged pronuclei just before fusion. Age, twenty-five minutes. Leitz 
oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 12. Metaphase of first cleavage division; age thirty minutes. The asters affecting a large 
area enclosed within a dotted line. Leitz oc. 1 (18), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 13. Early telophase of first cleavage division. Age, forty-one minutes. Leitz oc. 1 (18), 
obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 14. Asters dividing for second cleavage division. Age, forty-eight minutes. Leitz oc. 1 
(38), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 15. Sagittal, horizontal (nearly) section of early gastrula; cilia not shown. The mesoderm 
’ consists of four cells, two small ones not shown in this section. Dotted lines enclose nuclei. Nuclei 
of some cells moving centrally, a characteristic of invaginating entoderm. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 16. Normal gastrula twenty-three hours old; camera drawing from living embryo; only some 
of the more characteristic details shown. 


THENOGENETIC DEVELOPMENT OF AMPHITRITE. Joun W. Scort. PLATE I. 


HE JOURNAL oF ExPERIMENTAL ZoGLoGy, VOL. II. 


Pirate II. 


All figures, except 33 from a whole mount, are from living material. Figs. 17-22 and 30 are from 
eggs treated with potassium chlorid, method 3 of text; Figs. 23-29 and 31-33 are from calcium nitrate 
cultures, method 1; 34-38 from calcium chlorid cultures, method 5; 39 from swimming egg produced 
by shaking. For the age of an egg, the time after the beginning of the experiment is given. 

Fig. 17. Age, four hours and fifty minutes. Loose arrangement of cells; nuclei shown in dotted 
lines. 

Fig. 18. First one seen to move in this experiment; age, six‘hours and twenty-eight minutes. 

Fig. 19. Egg taken from the bottom of the dish; age, nineteen hours. Ectoplasm well differen- 
tiated from the yolk. 

Fig. 20. Age, twenty-three hours and twelve minutes. Swimming at surface. Unusual amount 
of cleavage, one nucleus to each cell; vacuoles present. Type of more active swimmers. 

Fig. 21. Age, twenty-three hours and twenty minutes; from bottom of dish. No cleavage; two 
large, clear spots (nuclear areas) were to be seen. 

Fig. 22. Age, twenty-one hours and twelve minutes. A type intermediate between Figs. 20 and 
21; egg composed of four cells, but incompletely divided; cilia on one cell. 

Fig. 23. Age, twelve hours. No cleavage; bluntly pear-shaped. Leitz oc. 1, obj. 7. 

Fig. 24. Age, twenty-three hours and twenty minutes. Comparatively little cleavage, cells not 
completely separated. Leitz oc. 1, obj. 7. 

Fig. 25. Age, nineteen hours and thirty minutes. Incomplete cleavage. Leitz oc. 1, ob. 7. 

Fig. 26. Age, twenty hours. Common type; no cleavage; several nuclei; brownish pigment in 
two regions; moderately active. Leitz oc. 1, obj. 7. 

Fig. 27. Age, twenty hours. Four cells; many nuclei in the large cell in which pigment is 
developed. Leitz oc. 1, obj. 7. 

Fig. 28. Age, twenty-two hours and thirty minutes. Common type; incomplete and probably 
partly fused cleavage planes; many nuclei; pigment in four regions. Leitz oc. 1, obj. 7. 

Fig. 29. Age, twenty-eight hours. One type of most active swimmers; no cleavage; many nuclei; 
pigment in two regions. Leitz oc. 1., obj. 7. 

Fig. 30. Age, thirty-seven hours and twenty-five minutes. Comparatively few cells and nuclei, 
but these are distinct; from bottom of the dish. 

Fig. 31. Age, forty-one hours. Swimming actively; no cleavage, but with an attached yolk lobe; 
many nuclei; pigment in four regions. Leitz oc. 1, obj. 7. 

Fig. 32. Age, forty-eight hours. Oldest actively swimming embryo found; indications of one 
cleavage plane; very many nuclei (comparatively); diffuse pigment in one region. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 5. 

Fig. 33. Age, fourteen hours. From stained whole mount; no cleavage; nuclei vary greatly in 
size; one or more nucleoli in most of the nuclei. Leitz oc. 1 (15), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 34. Age, nine hours. Intermediate type of cell and nuclear division. 

Fig. 35. Age, twenty hours. Much pigment; bluntly pear-shaped, due to early cleavage 
conditions. 

Fig. 36. Age, twenty hours. From same experiment as preceding; three incompletely divided 
cells, with one nuclear area in each. 

Fig. 37. Age, twenty-five hours. Very active swimmer; no segmentation; three very large 
nuclear areas; much pigment. 

Fig. 38. Age, twenty-three hours. No cleavage; considerable nuclear division. 

Fig. 39. Produced by shaking. Swimming near surface at about twenty hours. 


PA RTHENOGENETIC DEVELOPMENT OF AMPHITRITE. Joun W. Scort. 


Stine aye 


Ses 


17 


: Journat oF ExperiMENTAL ZoGroey, VoL. m1. 


II. 


Prate III. 


All figures of this plate are from sections of eggs treated with calcium nitrate. The age is dated 
from the time the eggs were put in the solution. 

Fig. 40. Unripe egg. Age, fifty-six minutes. No collapse in polar diameter; first maturation 
spindle in metaphase, undergoing mitosis without moving to the surface; thick egg membrane charac- 
teristic of unripe condition. Leitz oc. 1 (20), obj. I-12. 

Fig. 41. Unripeegg. Same age as preceding; in late anaphase. 

Fig. 42. Unripeegg. Age, two hours and twenty-six minutes. First maturation spindle in ana- 
phase has advanced part way to the surface; unequal division of chromosomes. Leitz oc. 4 (20), 
obj. I-12. 

Fig. 43. Very unripe egg. Age, three hours and fifteen minutes. Development very slow; very 
abnormal division of chromosomes; asters still in position of origin. No polar bodies formed, Figs. 
40-43. Leitz oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 44. Both polar bodies expelled, each one slightly abnormal; female chromosomes passing 
into vesicular condition. Age, forty-one minutes. Leitz oc. 1 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 45. First polar body nearly normal, second polar body too large. Age, twenty-four minutes. 
Leitz oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 46. Polar bodies abnormal, the second very much enlarged with yolk granules; the female 
pronucleus in preparation for first cleavage division. Age, sixty-eight minutes. Leitz oc. 1 (20) 
obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 47. Unripeegg. First polar body partly expelled; rest of very abnormal chromosomes have 
retreated to near center of egg. Age, three hours and fifteen minutes. Leitz oc. 4 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 48. Division of the nucleus without cleavage; no polar bodies. Age, sixty-eight minutes. 
Leitz oc. 1 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 49. Polar bodies (not shown); groups of chromosome vesicles removed less than a normal 
distance from one another; eleven vesicles in each group; abortive cleavage results. Age, sixty-eight 
minutes. Leitz oc. 1 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 50. Three-celled egg (drawn from three sections). Age, one hour and forty minutes. Leitz 
oc. I (17), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 51. Typical incomplete cleavage. Age, fourteen hours. Leitz oc. 1 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 52. Typical of condition with no cleavage; many nuclei, which vary greatly in size; some 
vacuoles. Age,twenty hours. Leitz oc. 1 (19), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 53. Typical of condition when cleavage is present; few cleavage planes complete; vacuoles. 
Age, twenty hours. Leitz oc. 1 (20), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 54. The most advanced and most nearly normal type of cleavage found in my sectioned 
material; the mesoderm has invaginated; vacuoles present. Age, twenty hours. Leitz oc. 1 (18) 
obj. I-12. 


RTHENOGENETIC DEVELOPMENT OF AMPHITRITE. Joun W. Scort. PEALE Lie 


di 


Pue JourNar or ExpertMentaL Zo6.oey, Vor. 1. 


Pirate IV. 


Figures of this plate are all from eggs treated with potassium chlorid. 

Fig. 55. Condition during maturation; one aster present; chromosomes in vesicular condition 
and greatly enlarged; no collapse in polar diameter. Age, one hour and thirty-eight minutes. Leitz 
oc. I (18), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 56. Maturation phenomena; both asters and a faint spindle present; egg did not collapse. 
Age, one hour and thirty-eight minutes. Leitz oc. 1 (18), obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 57. Prophase of first cleavage division. Age, two hours and forty-three minutes. Leitz 
oc. 3, obj. I-12. 

Fig. 58. Anaphase of first cleavage division. Cleavage in such eggs usually abortive. Age, 
two hours and seventeen minutes. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 59. Telophase of first cleavage divisions; 20 (+4?) chromosome vesicle in the right hand 
group, 18 (+4?) inthe left hand group. Cleavage in such eggs is frequently complete. Age, two hours 
and twenty-five minutes. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 60. Two-celled stage; division complete; eleven enlarged chromosome vesicles in each cell. 
Probably indicates one origin of multinuclear cells. Age, three hours and fifty-five minutes. Leitz 
OC. 3, obj. I-12. 

Fig. 61. Multipolar asters; anaphase of the second division. Chromatin division irregular and 
unequal. One method of origin of multinuclear cells. Age, three hours and fifty-five minutes. Leitz 
0c. 3, obj. I-12. 

Fig. 62. Cleavage planes complete; irregular and unequal division of chromatin in one cell. 
some cells multinuclear; diffuse chromatin stain near center of egg indicates original position of the 
germinal vesicle. Age, four hours and forty-two minutes. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 63. Several vesicles in each cell; cleavage planes complete. Age, four hours and forty-two 
minutes. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 64. Egg showing typical cleavage. Mitotic figure in one cell; more than one nuclear vesicle 
in others; part of segmentation cavity present. Age, eight hours and forty-seven minutes. Leitz 
Oc. 3, obj. I-12. 

Fig. 65. Egg typical of incomplete cleavage with probably later fusion. Nuclei vary greatly in 
size. Age,eight hours. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 66. Typical of eggs with no cleavage. A few large nuclei; large vacuoles. Age, twenty 
hours. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 1-12. 

Fig. 67. One of the most advanced types of cleavage found in this solution. Evident invagination 
of the mesoderm. Age,twenty hours. Leitz oc. 3, obj. 1-12. 


PLATE IV. 


PA RTHENOGENETIC DEVELOPMENT OF AMPHITRITE. Joun W. Scorr. 


‘Tue Journat or ExperiMentaL Zo6 oey, VOL. Ill. 


ear) ‘faces 
; 


oa 


foe DEVELOPMENT OF FUNDULUS HETEROCLI- 
Lis aN SOLUDIONS, OF LITHIUM CHLORID, 
With, APPENDDSE ON ITS DEVELOPMENT IN 
FRESH WATER 


BY 
CHAREES R. STOCKARD 


With NINETEEN Ficures 


The eggs of Fundulus heteroclitus are hardy and, therefore, 
stand experimental treatment most satisfactorily. Since they 
are capable of development in both salt and fresh water it 1s 
possible to eliminate all of the physical effects of hypertonic 
solutions, for effective solutions of a salt may be applied in fresh 
water that have a final pressure lower than that of normal sea 
water. Mathews has stated that the eggs of this fish are pecu- 
liarly adapted to treatment with solutions because they appear 
to be easily penetrated by nearly all sorts of ions, and they are 
quite insensitive to variations in osmotic pressure of the solutions. 
Whether this statement is absolutely true or not there can be little 
doubt of the fact that the ions resulting from LiCl in solution do 
penetrate the membranes and give an effect that is certainly not 
due to osmotic pressure, since the same result 1s obtained with 
solutions of LiCl in both salt and fresh water, the one being hyper- 
tonic while the other is a hypotonic solution. Whether or not 
the membranes are easily or quickly penetrated is a question 
better answered by a study of the results. 

The fact that lithium salts produce a marked and really specific 
effect on development was observed first in 1893 by Herbst while 
studying the eggs of the sea-urchin. The definite type of embryo 
thus produced was termed by him the lithium larva. Soon after 
this several investigators studied the action of lithium salts on the 
development of the frog’s egg and found that it induced decided 
effects, yet they failed to show that a distinct type of embryo 


Journat or Experimenta Zobtocy, Vor. m1, No. 1. 


100 Charles R. Stockard 


resulted, although Gurwitsch concluded that his radial larva was 
the specific result of the lithium action: Morgan then in 1902 
carried on experiments to test the effects of LiCl on frog’s eggs, 
and again in the following spring he used a number of lithium salts 
along with other solutions for comparison. _ He finally concluded 
that lithium produced in the development of the frog’s egg a 
typical larva just as Herbst had shown for the sea-urchin. Both 
of these workers claimed for similar reasons that the effect was 
due to the chemical action of the lithium ion and not to osmotic 
or pressure effects; hence the specific character of the embryos. 
Loeb, Mathews and others have subjected the eggs of the fish, 
Fundulus heteroclitus, to solutions of lithium salts, but only to 
observe the physiological effects of the solutions, ignoring the 
morphological changes induced. 

With these results in view Prof. I. H. Morgan kindly suggested 
that I study, from a morphological standpoint, the effects of 
lithium salts on the fish’s egg. It is, therefore, a pleasure to avail 
myself of this opportunity of expressing my indebtedness to Pro- 
fessor Morgan for his kindly advice and criticism throughout the 
progress of this work. ‘The experiments were conducted during 
the past summer while occupying a table in the United States 
Fish Commission Laboratory at Wood’s Hole, and I wish to 
thank the authorities of this laboratory, particularly the director, 
Dr. F. B. Sumner, for the courtesies extended me while there. 

The Fundulus egg differs in its mode of development from both 
that of the sea-urchin and the frog, hence it is of interest to note 
whether or not an effect such as that produced by lithium is at 
all comparable in the three cases. Of course it is fully realized 
that no strict embryological comparison should be made among 
types of development so different as these, yet one may find little 
objection to a comparative study of the chemical effects produced 
in the three widely different forms by one and the same element, 
lithium. At present I am not in a position to state that the 
abnormalities which have been induced in the Fundulus egg by 
means of LiCl are specific for lithium, as a number of other salts 
must first be experimented with. The abnormalities described 
are not exceptional, but general, occurring in as large a per cent. 


a 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of LiCl IOI 


of the eggs as could be expected. All of the experiments were 
repeated frequently—some as often as a dozen times—and always 
with constant results. 


METHOD 


The following treatment and precautions were used throughout 
the experiments. It was decided to keep the eggs as nearly as 
possible under normal conditions by using solutions of the LiCl 
in ordinary sea water, and to run experiments with fresh and 
distilled water solutions merely as checks on these. Normal sea 
water controls were carried in each experiment and as a further 
check eggs were kept developing in ordinary fresh and distilled 
water. A series of solutions of LiCl in sea water was prepared 
in strengths }n, $n, $n, 4n, 2n, $n, Zn, and normal, a normal 
solution of LiCl being equivalent to about a 4.25 per cent. solu- 
tion; the eggs were then subjected to these to ascertain the various 
effects of different strength solutions. It was found that those 
solutions weaker than 3 n produced no apparent effect during the 
early stages of deyelopwient. In the ? n, 7 n, and normal colt 
tions the eggs showed varying degrees of abnormalities, the ones 
in the stronger solutions dying after a few hours. A series was 
then prepared between $n and ?n so as to obtain different degrees 
of abnormal development. ‘These solutions were 2.62 per cent., 
mee? Pel cent., 3-02 per cent. and 3.22 per cent. LiCl or .62 n, 
.66n,.71nand.76n. ‘These four strengths were then used in all 
of the experiments. A fresh water series was also prepared and I 
found, as Mathews had, that a $n solution was the minimal 
poisonous dose, that is, the weakest solution preventing the forma- 
tion of an embryo; it will be noted that this is only about one-half 
the strength required to affect the eggs when in a sea water solu- 
tion of LiCl. A +5 nor 1.48 per cent. LiCl solution in distilled 
water was resorted to, though its effect was rather severe, being 
about the same as that produced by a 3.22 per cent. LiCl solution 
in sea water. Controls were always taken from the same bunch 
of eggs as those on which the experiments were performed. It is 
quite noticeable that Fundulus eggs withstand solutions many 


102 Charles R. Stockard 


times stronger than those used to affect the eggs of the sea-urchin 
and frog. 


RESULTS 


When Fundulus eggs are placed in LiCl solutions shortly after 
fertilization, or before the two-cell stage, a very constant modifica- 
tion in the early stages of development will be noticed. ‘The pro- 
toplasmic cap or blastoderm becomes unusually prominent, bulg- 
ing up in an arched fashion that is much more marked than in 
normal eggs. After reaching about the sixty-four cell stage and 
later, a clear bubble-like appearance is noted in the living eggs 
below the disc (Figs. 1 and 2). On sectioning and staining the 
blastoderms at this stage it is found that the bubble-like appearance 
is due to the greatly enlarged condition of the segmentation cavity 
(Fig. 13). The central periblast has been forced from its normal 
place close below the blastoderm (Fig. 14) and pushed down into 
the yolk mass at the same time causing the cap to arch more 
decidedly above on account of the strain thus induced about its 
periphery. ‘his certainly looks like an osmotic effect but one 
fails to see how it could be attributed to such a cause when it is 
remembered that the same condition exists both in the sea water 
LiCl solution and in the distilled water solution (compare Figs. 
1 and 2), the one being hypertonic while the other is hypotonic 
and thus in the two cases opposite, and not equal, effects would be 
expected. 

Lithium chlorid in all cases most obviously delays the develop- 
ment (compare Figs. 1, 2, 3 and 4 with 11; 5 and 8 with 6). In 
the stronger collicione: eggs as old as forty-three hours, show the 
iveeodecnt Still asa paler cap and in these older stages it 
presents a most interesting appearance. On examining such 
living eggs the blastoderm is found to be greatly raised and in 
some cases almost pinched away from the yolk. When one looks 
down on the top of this blastoderm it is seen to suggest very 
strongly a gastrula of some holoblastic egg such as that of a sea- 
urchin or starfish, the center appears thin while the peripheral 
curved surface has a much thicker appearance as if the center 
showed below it the blastopore and the sides represented the 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of I1Cl 103 


folded double wall of the gastrula seen in profile. Stages were 
seen in which this “blastopore”’ appeared very large as if invagina- 
tion was just beginning, and other stages showed various degrees 
of decrease in the size of the blastoporic appearance (Figs. 15 and 
16). These eggs finally died, in many cases on account of the 
blastodermic edges becoming approximated, thus pinching the 
cap entirely away from the yolk. On studying sections of these 
peculiar forms it was found that as the segmentation cavity 
became abnormally large, thus pushing the blastodermic roof up 
into a more decided arch, the edges or periphery of the blasto- 
derm were brought closer together at the same time becoming 
thicker and showing more cell layers than were seen in the crown 
or top of the disc (Figs. 15 and 16). ‘The periblast seems more 
loosely connected with the blastoderm than is usually the case; 
thus it is readily understood how the latter may finally pinch 
away from the yolk mass—compare Figs. 13, 15 and 16. ‘These 
figures also make clear the manner in which the blastopore- 
like image in the living egg seems to decrease in size—as if a 
blastopore was closing; this is merely the visual effect produced 
by the decreasing circumference of the blastodermic periphery as 
it becomes puckered together like the mouth of a sac. No divid- 
ing cells were found in these sections of the blastoderms, and when 
it is recalled how abundant such cells usually are in sections of 
fish blastoderms at this stage we are struck with the inhibition 
or decrease in division rate caused by the LiCl, and hence the slow 
rate of growth. Normally a forty-hour egg would have the young 
fish well formed. No indication of invagination was shown about 
the edges of these blastoderms, but it may be possible that if eggs 
in this condition were transferred from the strong solutions back 
to sea water that the slight recovery thus induced might cause 
them to continue development and perhaps to slightly invaginate 
as this appears to be the easiest direction of growth. Lack of 
material during the latter part of the season prevented the trial 
of such an experiment. 

Many eggs die when the blastoderm becomes confined to the 
polar region as is indicated above, but in rare cases they survive 
and develop to the extent that the blastoderm thins out and 


104 Charles R. Stockard 


EXPLANATION OF FIGURES. 


All were drawn from camera sketches of the living eggs except figures 5, 6, 8 and 9. 

Fig. 1. Egg from a normal 55 LiCl distilled water solution when twenty-two hours and twenty 
minutes old. sg, Segmentation cavity; od, oil drops. 

Fig. 2. When twenty hours old in 3.22 per cent. LiCl in sea water. 

Fig. 3. Twenty-three hours old in 2.82 per cent. LiCl in sea water. gr, Germ ring; es, embryonic 
shield. 

Fig. 4. Twenty-two and one-half hours old in 2.82 per cent. LiCl sea water. 

Fig. 5. Nine days old, taken from 3.02 per cent. LiCl when twenty-eight hours old and placed 
in pure sea water. h, Head; 7, tail; f, fin. 

Fig. 6. Control embryo nine days old. ht, Heart. 

Fig. 7. In 2.82 per cent. LiCl fifty-four hours old. 

Fig. 8. Embryo with no eyes, nine days old, from 2.82 per cent. LiCl into sea water at thirty-two 
hours old. ht, Heart. 

Fig. 9. Cauda bifida normal 3 LiCl forty-eight hours. 1, Tail. 

Fig. 10. In 2.82 per cent. fifty-four hours old. 

Fig. 11. Control twenty-four hours old. em, Embryonic thickening. 

Fig. 12. In 3.02 per cent. thirty-one and one-half hours. 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of LiCl 105 


106 Charles R. Stockard 


flattens slightly, and in some cases forms an embryonic shield in this 
polar position. Fig. 18 shows such a shield which, forming in a 
cap forty-six hours old, extended no further over the yolk than a 
normal high segmentation stage would or than the germ area 
shown in Fig. 1 or 2 does. A longitudinal section of this shield 
is shown in Fig. 17 and it is seen that the folding and cell arrange- 
ments are more or less normal, in this case little or no indication 
of the germ ring was found beyond the edges of the shield. Fig. 19 
shows a section through a blastoderm with the embryonic shield 
thickening well marked. In Figs. 3 and 4, embryos about 
twenty-three hours old, the germ area 1s still high up on the yolk, 
but the germ ring and embryonic shield have reached a stage of 
development commonly seen when the germinal area has descended 
about one-third of the way down the yolk. In these eggs the 
embryonic area never completely covers the yolk, the blastopore 
always remains open and through it the yolk is seen to protrude. 
These instances show most clearly that a second effect of lithium 
chlorid 1s to prevent the down-growth of protoplasm in the egg. 

The embryos that result from the eggs just mentioned are 
peculiar, being necessarily short as the embryonic material only 
extends part of the way down the yolk. ‘They are also poorly 
formed in the head region and the tail seems to dwindle out 
suddenly as a small pointed end. Figure 5 shows such a monster 
in position on the yolk when nine days old; a normal control 
embryo of the same age is shown in Fig. 6. “These short embryos 
were cleared and stained and finally studied in sections. ‘They 
usually lacked all indications of eyes, either optic vesicles or lens, 
but the ears were being formed and the trunk organs were present. 
As is seen in the figure the fins are poorly developed. 

When weaker solutions of LiCl were used, such as 2.62 per 
cent. and 2.82 per cent., the formation of the embryo approached 
nearer the normal, but even in the 2.62 per cent. no embryo was 
ever developed in an entirely normal way. In all cases the rate 
of development was retarded, the heart beat was slower, and the 
blood was colorless, seeming to lack the hemoglobin constituent. 
‘The latter condition must have lessened the respiratory efficiency to 
quite an extent, a fact which would also contribute to their slow 


as 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of LiCl 107 


rate of development. Embryos in these weaker solutions often 
showed cauda bifida (Fig. 9), and others that were more nearly 
normal in form had rather irregular and twisted outlines, some 
of these having the tail end bent at right angles to the body 
axis. [hese malformations of the caudal end are no doubt 
due to the slow descent of the protoplasmic mass over the yolk 
and in some cases to the entire failure of this mass to completely 
enclose the yolk. ‘This causes the embryo to be short, crowded 
into less space, and twisted in appearance. 

A most noticeable feature in these living embryos was the 
apparent absence of eyes (Fig. 8 a nine-day embryo). When one 
looks at a batch of developing fish eggs after the embryos have 
formed, a most conspicuous feature is the large black eyes on each 
egg—the lack of such an appearance is striking in the lithium 
embryos—and the entire mass of eggs has, therefore, a pale, 
unhealthy look. Sections of the heads of these larvas show that 
in many cases no indication whatever of eyes existed, while in 
some a small deep-seated poorly-formed eye was found, which, 
on account of its probable paleness, was entirely unnoticeable in 
the living embryos. 

The germinal area in some eggs advanced over the yolk in a 
very peculiar manner. Figure 10 shows a thin cellular sac that 
extends more than half way down the yolk, constricting it slightly 
about the equator and giving the entire egg a rather unusual 
shape. At the border of the iyeplecns sac little or no 
indication of germ ring or embryonic shield could be distinguished, 
though these structures must have existed to some extent since at 
later stages short malformed embryos would arise appearing in 
life as if their tail ends were entirely lacking. Figure 7 shows a 
circular cap which does not extend so far down the yolk as in the 
last case, but the very strong line of demarcation which often 
exists between yolk and protoplasm is well shown. Figure 12 
shows a rare case in which a small part of the yolk is pulled up into 
the blastodermic cap as a small polar cone. 

The rapidity of action of the LiCl solution, the degree of abnor- 
malities and death rate produced by solutions of different strengths 
as well as the periods in development at which the solutions seemed 


108 Charles R. Stockard 


most and least effective may best be treated by referring directly 
to notes taken while testing these several questions. 

As was mentioned before the rapidity or ease with which metal- 
lic ions penetrate the membranes of Fundulus eggs is uncertain. 
But the examples cited below seem to show that lithium ions, at 
any rate, enter with comparative readiness and produce abnor- 
malities which in these experiments, at least, can not be attributed 
to other causes. Eggs that were fertilized “dry” and then placed 
in sea water LiCl solutions of 2.82 per cent., 3.02 per cent. and 
3.22 per cent. showed characteristic lithium effects after a period 
of only three hours, while in the eight-cell stage, the blastoderms 
bulging up abnormally. ‘The control from the same lot of eggs 
failed to show any such modifications. Again on applying a 
series of solutions of LiCl in sea water of strengths of 1.59 per cent., 
2.12) per cent... 2/05. per cent, 3:16 per cent. 447 lipericcMnmaina 
4.25 per cent. and examining after sixteen hours the eggs in the 
three weaker solutions presented a normal appearance, while those 
in the three stronger solutions contained a large per cent. of abnor- 
mal forms, all in the strongest solution being very abnormal. 
The blastoderm was arched and the segmentation cavity appeared 
as a bubble below. After being in the solutions forty-eight hours 
the 2.65 per cent. lot contained twenty-six per cent. undeveloped 
eggs and many abnormally-shaped embryos, some with cauda 
bifida and irregular outlines. The 1.59 per cent. and 2.12 per 
~ cent. lots also showed malformed embryos though not so many as 
in the 2.65 per cent. lot. ‘Thus it is shown that weak solutions 
do not affect the development in its early stages though they later 
cause the production of deformed embryos. In another experi- 
ment eggs were subjected to 2.82 per cent. LiCl when twenty-two 
hours old, and others when twenty-five hours old were put into 
3.02 per cent. LiCl. When they were all forty-four hours old, the 
ones which had been in the stronger solution, though for three 
hours less, showed more marked modifications than those in the 
weaker. ‘These facts seem to show that LiCl does enter the egg 
with sufficient readiness for all experimental purposes. 

The degree of effectiveness for the different strength solutions 
may be illustrated as follows: When forty-six hours old many eggs 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of LiCl 109 


in 2.62 per cent. LiCl show a faint line indicating the embryo, and 
there are also a large per cent. of dead eggs present. In the 2.82 
per cent. solution at this time there are still more dead ones; the 


Fig. 13. 


Seventeen hours old in LiC! normal solution. bd, Blastoderm; pb, periblast; sg, seg- 


mentation cavity. 


Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 


14. 
1G 
16. 
17e 
18. 


Control ten hours old. sc, Segmentation cavity. 

In 3.02 per cent. LiCl forty-three hours old. sc, Segmentation cavity. 

In 3.02 per cent. LiCl forty-three hours old. sc, Segmentation cavity. 

Longitudinal section embryonic shield from Fig. 18. pb, Periblast. 

Embryonic shield in polar cap forty-six hours old in 3.22 per cent. LiCl. es, Embryonic 


shield; od, oil drop; e, border of blastoderm. 
Fig. 19. Section of polar cap twenty-two hours old in 3.22 per cent. LiCl. es, Embryonic 


shield; sc, segmentation cavity. 


germ ring has not grown completely over the yolk except in rare 
cases, but the embryo is formed in the embryonic shield. . Among 
those in 3.02 per cent. none of the blastopores have closed and the 


IIO Charles R. Stockard 


embryos are all short and abnormal, with a still larger number of 
dead eggs. In the 3.22 per cent. LiCl less than half are still alive; 
all of the blastopores are unclosed; some of the embryos are fairly 
well developed in the embryonic shield, while some of the em- 
bryonic caps are still at the upper pole with the embryos forming 
in a thickening, like an embryonic shield, which often lies imme- 
diately over the pole of the egg (Fig. 18); others have blastoderms 
which may finally pinch away from the yolk and die (Figs. 15 and 
16). 

e another experiment the control embryos are distinctly 
formed when fifty-four hours old; while in the 2.62 per cent. LiCl 
the blastopores are only about two-thirds closed and embryos are 
forming in the embryonic shields. In 2.82 per cent. LiCl blasto- 
pores are still wide open; in 3.02 per cent. caps, with slight embry- 
onic shields, are present; and in the 3.22 per cent. LiCl all seem 
to have stopped developing and many have the blastoderms 
broken away from the yolk and floating freely within the egg 
membrane. 

When the same eggs are sixty-nine hours old forty-five are alive 
in the 2.62 per cent. LiCl solution. “The embryos are all well formed, 
though a few have failed to close the blastopore. In 2.82 per cent. 
LiCl only seven out of thirty-five are still alive, or 20 per cent., 
and only one of these has a definite embryo formed. In 3.02 per 
cent. LiCl only five out of fifty-three eggs are living, or 9.5 percent., 
and these are all deformed. Of those in the 3.22 per cent. LiCl 
solution sixteen are dead and one is living, or 6 per cent. alive. 
In this one the cap is scarcely half way down the yolk. 

At ninety-six hours old these eggs are in the following condi- 
tion: [he 2.62 per cent. LiCl lot have thirteen out of fourteen still 
living, all with well formed embryos though far behind the control 
in development. ‘The 2.82 per cent. ones have not fared so well; 
only three out of seven are still alive but these, also, have well 
formed embryos. ‘The one survivor of the 3.22 per cent. LiCl 
solution is now dead, the cap has pulled back off of the yolk, 
which on the day before it had half covered, and is now a dead 
mass of cells at the upper pole. 

The embryos in the 2.62 per cent. LiCl solution when nine days 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of LiCl III 


old are far behind the control in development; the heart beat is 
only about one-half as fast as normal, and the yolk is badly 
shrunken so that the entire embryo swings about freely in the egg 
membrane. The 2.82 per cent. LiCl embryos are still alive, 
though they also have much shriveled yolks and are otherwise like 
the ones in the 2.62 per cent. solution. 

Eggs of various ages were subjected to the LiCl solutions to 
ascertain whether or not there were periods in development when 
they appeared more sensitive than at other times. As a further 
means of deciding this question, eggs were also removed at inter- 
vals from the LiCl solutions and placed in pure sea water. 
Eggs which were sixteen hours old (early germ-ring stages) were 
placed in 3.02 per cent. LiCl and when they were forty hours old 
they had become very abnormal and many were dead. Eggs put 
into 3.02 per cent. LiCl when seventeen hours old are described 
thus, when eight days old: The pigment is confined almost alto- 
gether to the yolk area and is largely of the black type, little if any of 
the brown being present; the blood 1s colorless and the heart beats 
slowly; the eyes have little or no color and in most cases seem 
absent, and the yolk has decreased greatly in size. Of the eggs 
put into 2.82 per cent. LiCl when nineteen hours old almost the 
entire lot died, the remaining ones resembling those which had 
been in the same solution since fertilization. If put into 2.62 per 
cent. LiCl when twenty hours old they appear, when forty-four 
hours old, similar to those that have spent their entire life in a 
solution of this strength. Those put into 2.82 per cent. when 
twenty-two hours old appear at forty-four hours almost normal 
though a little behind in their development. ‘The extreme sensi- 
tiveness of the nineteen-hour and twenty-hour stages as thus 
indicated is noteworthy. Figure 11 shows a normal egg some- 
what beyond this age, and it is seen that the germ ring has just 
turned the equator of the egg. Other eggs were put into 3.02 per 
cent. LiCl when eighteen, nineteen, twenty, twenty-one and 
twenty-two hours old, respectively, and kept in this solution for 
three days. ‘Then they were transferred to pure sea water again. 
Seventeen days later they were still alive but developing very 
slowly and seemed far from being ready to hatch while the control 


12) Charles R. Stockard 


ones had hatched from three to four days previous. ‘Thus it 
seems that LiCl leaves a lasting effect on these embryos and that 
they are unable to recover, though under normal conditions, for 
as long as seventeen days after. 

Eggs sixty-five hours old with well-formed embryos were placed 
in 3.02 per cent. LiCl solution; twenty-eight hours later on exam- 
ining the living eggs no variations from the normal could be 
detected; fifty-four hours after being in the solution no change was 
observed, though when nine days old they were behind the control 
in their development at least two days. 

To test the time of greatest sensitiveness in another manner, 
eggs were taken from the LiCl solutions after being in them only 
thirty minutes, while in the two-cell stage. [he LiCl in this case 
left no trace of any effect that could be detected in their later 
development. Again eggs taken from 3.02 per cent. LiCl after 
six hours treatment were put into sea water, their age being then 
seven and three-quarter hours, they appeared normal when 
twenty and twenty-four hours old, but when forty-four hours old 
they had fallen behind the control in their development. Eggs 
were taken from 2.82 per cent. LiCl when fifteen and one-half 
hours old, having been in the solution fourteen hours and were 
placed in sea water, twenty-four hours later the eggs showed no 
improvement over those still in the solution. When examined 
after being out of the solution for sixteen days they still showed 
little if any recovery. In another case eggs were placed in sea 
water after being in 2.82 per cent. LiCl for eighteen and one-half 
hours and others in a 3.02 per cent. solution seventeen and three- 
quarter hours. When forty-four hours old they were still abnor- 
mal but far ahead of those remaining in the lithium solutions. 
After being in 3.02 per cent. LiCl for twenty-six and three-quarter 
hours eggs were placed in sea water where they failed to recover, 
though their death rate was materially lowered; after two days 8g 
per cent. still were living while of those left in the solution only 
g.5 per cent. remained alive. ‘This lowering of the death rate was 
noted in many instances where morphological recovery from the 
lithium effect seemed lacking. It appears as though the eggs gain 
in power of resistance, or rather resistance is no longer required 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of LiCl 113 


after being transferred to the sea water and hence though they 
may not recover in one sense, they are better able to survive than 
they would be under the depressing effects of the lithium. 

During one experiment a very strange result was noted: The 
entire series of LiCl solutions failed to give any visible effect for 
more than the first day of development, but later the character- 
istic effects were manifested and those eggs that were removed 
from the solutions and placed in sea water during the day of 
apparent non-effectiveness later showed the effects of the LiCl on 
their subsequent development in the sea water, although no effect 
was observable when they were removed from the lithium solu- 
tions. 

The above experiments were all repeated several times and the 
particular instances cited merely serve as examples to illustrate 
the general results obtained. From such responses to the LiCl 
treatment one may be justified in drawing the following conclu- 
sions. 


CONCLUSIONS 


The rapidity with which lithium chlorid in solution affects the 


egg varies directly as the strength of the solution, eggs being very 


slow to manifest the effects when placed in dilute solutions, while 
a response was obtained on one occasion within three hours in 
rather strong solutions. 

The degree of abnormality and the death rate also vary directly as 
the strength of the lithium chlorid solutions, striking abnormalities 
being found in 3.22 per cent. solutions while similar malformations 
in a less marked degree with a much lower death rate were noted 
in the 2.62 per cent. solutions. 

The lithium chlorid solution affects the development of the embryo 
at any stage in which the eggs may be placed init. But the extent 
of the abnormalities seem to vary inversely with the age at which 
the eggs are subjected to the solution, showing merely a lessened 
rate of development with other minor effects if placed in the 
solution during late stages. 

When eggs are once affected by the lithtum chlorid solution—which 
is found to be the case 1f they remain in it as long as six hours— 


114 Charles R. Stockard 


they are unable to completely recover from this effect even though 
they be placed in pure sea water during the remaining period of 


their development. 


REVIEW OF LITERATURE 


Herbst, in ’92, 93 and 96, conducted extensive experiments to 
test the action of a large number of salts on the developing eggs 
of the sea-urchin. He found that of a number of salts used, those 
of the metal lithium produced the most decided results, and 
further that with the different salts of lithium the results were the 
same. ‘Thus the conclusion was reached that the action of these 
salts was not due to their several acid radicals but to their common 
lithium ion. Herbst, therefore, stated that the abnormalities 
induced were typical lithium effects, not being obtained by the 
use of salts of other metals. He used the term “lithium larva” 
to designate the peculiar monster that resulted. The effect of 
lithium on the sea-urchin’s egg seemed chiefly to cause “exogas- 
trulation,” that is, the endoderm instead of becoming infolded by 
invagination became really inverted and formed a sac connected 
with the ectoderm or outer gastrula wall by a short stalk, thus 
producing a three-part larva, two sacs, the gastrula wall and the 
primitive gut, joined by a short tube or stalk. From the end 
opposite the stalk the primitive gut sac in many instances formed 
another small sac representing, as Herbst claimed, the vaso- 
peritoneal pouch. ‘The ectoderm sac at times became smaller 
and occasionally finally disappeared leaving the entire blastula wall 
endodermal. ‘Uhe larva recovered slightly so as to form the cal- 
cium framework when removed from the lithium solutions; the 
amount of recovery depending upon the length of time it had spent 
in the solution. ‘This briefly is the main action as found by Herbst 
in three genera of sea-urchins treated with, in all, thirteen salts of 
lithium. ‘The features of especial interest to us are the marked 
inversion of the layers and the resulting enlarged embryonic 
cavities. 

Gurwitsch, in ’95 and ’96, treated developing frog’s eggs with 
LiCl and other salts, finding that sodium and lithium salts were 
most effective. His lithium solutions were very weak (0.3 per 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of LiCl rms 


cent., 0.4 per cent. and 0.5 per cent.), only about one-half the 
strength required to affect the Fundulus egg when applied in 
fresh water, and about one-third or one- sou the concentration 
necessary to give a response when used in sea water. Frog’s 
eggs under the influence of LiCl showed a retardation in the seg- 
mentation of the white area, in rare cases this area failing entirely 
to divide. Thus they became marked into upper Sad lower 
hemispheres and the cells at the sides of the floor of the segmenta- 
tion cavity seemed to push in tending to fill this cavity. * At this 
period Gurwitsch claimed that his embryos exhibited a radial 
symmetry but Morgan contradicts this and holds that they are 
really bilateral although the bilaterality may be less evident than 
that shown in the normal egg. Gurwitsch also finds that the 
downgrowth of the substance constituting the upper hemisphere 
is lacking, as indicated above, and this is without doubt a marked 
characteristic of the action of LiCl on the fish’s egg. ‘This also 
gives in the two cases an abnormally high position to the develop- 
ing embryo and may, along with the failure of the blastopore to 


close, cause the malformed caudal areas of many individuals. 


Abnormalities in the position of the blastopore and brain, par- 
ticularly of the former, were found to be the most decided effects 
of the LiCl solutions. 


Madame Rondeau-Luzeau conducted an extensive series of 


‘experiments to test more especially the action of several chlorids 


on development. She found that a double action exists, namely, 
a physical action due to “hypertonicity” of the solutions and a 
chemical action depending upon the kind of salt employed. Only 
in the case of LiCl did the chemical action predominate when weak 
solutions were tested. In my work the fact that solutions in both 
fresh and sea water were used seems to preclude, in this case, all 
chances for the physical or hypertonic effect and leaves the chemi- 
cal effect alone responsible for the abnormal development ob- 
served. Except in the case of LiCl Mm. Rondeau-Luzeau found 
that death was much oftener due to the osmotic pressure of the solu- 
tions than to chemical poisoning. ‘The teratological action of the 
chlorids appeared to be due to their chemical action in the case 
of eggs treated after fertilization. ‘The effects of the solutions 


116 Charles R. Stockard 


were also found to be more marked at some stages than at others, 
particularly so at the time of closure of the blastopore and of the 
formation of the medullary folds. LiCl was found to exert a more 
powerful chemical action than any other salt tried. “The minimal 
amount of this salt that will effect the development of the frog’s 
egg may be represented by an osmotic pressure of 169 cm. (0.3 to 
0.4 per cent.); while for KCl, it it 405 cm. (1.2 per cent.); for NaCl, 
459 cm. (1.9 per cent.); for MgCl,, 485 cm. (1.3 per cent.); and for 
CaCl, 484 cm. (1.5 per cent.). 

Morgan, in ’02 and ’03, subjected frog’s eggs at various stages 
in their development to 0.4 per cent., 0.5 per cent., 0.6 per cent., 
0.7 per cent. and 0.8 per cent. solutions of LiCl. Most of these 
solutions would be too weak, even though prepared in fresh water, 
to affect Fundulus eggs. Morgan found that in the frog’s egg 
LiCl did produce a specific effect which he attributed to the chemi- 
cal action of the lithium ion. Delayed development was noted as 
being a most obvious condition and this 1s equally true of the fish’s 
egg. Egos in two-cell and four-cell stages were more affected 
than those in later segmentation, and those beginning to gastru- 
late were least affected. Stronger solutions were necessary to 
give an equal effect when applied in later stages. Similar facts 
~ are also indicated in my notes above. But it was found that the 
Fundulus eggs placed in the solutions during cleavage and at the 
beginning of gastrulation up to the sixteenth or Sevenreenil hours 
were equally affected without regard to the stages at which they 
were subjected to the action of the salt. Another very noticeable 
effect of this salt was to prevent the downgrowth of protoplasm 
from the upper hemisphere of the frog’s egg, as Gurwitsch also 
found. ‘This makes gastrulation difficult, so that embryos in the 
stronger solutions do not pass beyond this stage. “The absence 
of ye downgrowth of the protoplasm in the fish’s egg has been 
emphasized above and it will be remembered that in the stronger 
solutions the blastoderm folded into a ball upon the upper pole 
and finally pinched itself away from the yolk and died. One type 
of the frog embryo showed a complete inversion of the layers, the 
black area folding down into the yolk cells. The complete accom- 
plishment of such a feat by the fish’s egg could hardly be imagined, 


ah 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of I1Cl 117 


but it seems possible that it may attempt a similar process, when 
it is recalled to what extent the periblast area may be forced down 
into the yolk when the segmentation cavity becomes unusually 
enlarged as it sometimes does. In the frog the black area becomes 
more or less spherical and sinks into or is engulfed by the yolk- 
cells of the white area. ‘The frog eggs often form a sharp line 
between the black and white hemispheres, usually above the 
equator. This line, Morgan states, corresponds to the inturned 
edge of a circular blastopore whose position at or above the 
equator of the egg is explained by the lack of downward move- 
ment of the material of the upper hemisphere. ‘The segmenta- 
tion cavity of the upper cells was obliterated. ‘This is a very 
different state of affairs from that seen in the fish where the seg- 
mentation cavity becomes so enormously exaggerated. Many 
short and folded embryos were found in later stages, some also 
showed cauda bifida; short embryos and forked tails are also 
common among the late effects of LiCl on the fish embryo. 

After considering these investigations upon the effects of 
lithium one is strongly inclined to conclude that the salts of this 
metal do exert a specific influence on developing eggs. But I am 
not now in the position to state that these effects on the Fundulus 
eggs are really specific for lithium, though they have been so 
repeatedly and constantly obtained that one is led to believe that 
they are at least characteristic of lithium action on eggs of this 
form. Other chemical or physical stimuli might possibly pro- 
duce similar abnormalities; this could be definitely stated only after 
the study has been continued with a large number of salts of 
various metals. 


SUMMARY 


1. Lithium chlorid delays development to a most obvious 
degree. 

2. Eggs seem equally affected by the solution when placed in it 
at any time during cleavage stages; at other times the effects 
vary. [hey seem most sensitive about the period when the germ- 
ring circles the equator; though they are always affected to a 
ereater or less degree; the rate of development is slower, the em- 


118 Charles R. Stockard 


bryo presents a pale appearance, since the blood lacks color and 
the pigment spots are fewer than normal. 

3. After having remained for as long a time as six hours in 
LiCl solutions of sufficient strengths to cause abnormalities the 
eggs are incapable of complete recovery when placed in pure sea 
water during the remainder of their dev elopment. 

4. In LiCl solutions the blastoderm is usually prevented from 
growing downward over the yolk, it therefore bulges up as a cap 
on the upper pole of the egg. ‘This cap in the stronger solutions 
constricts its border, thus folding its periphery, and finally pinches 
itself away from the yolk and dies. 

5. The segmentation cavity is enormously enlarged since the 
central periblast pushes down unusually far into the yolk mass 
while the blastoderm bulges up giving the cavity a more arched 
roof. 

6. In many eggs the blastoderm never completely encloses the 
yolk; thus the blastopore remains open and short, peculiarly 
formed, and often cauda bifida embryos result. 

7. In the late embryos the heart beats slowly, the eyes often 
fail to develop, the blood is colorless and, therefore, appears to 
lack hemoglobin. ‘These characters taken with the inability to 
recover from the lithium effect, seems to prove without doubt that 
such an effect 1s due to chemical, and not to physical, causes. The 
fact that similar abnormalities are induced by LiCl solutions pre- 
pared with sea water and fresh water, therefore, giving both 
hypertonic and hypotonic solutions show further its chemical 
rather than its physical, action. 


APPENDIX 


Notes on the Development of Fundulus heteroclitus in Fresh 
W ater 


At Cold Spring Harbor, Long Island, in the summer of 1904, 
I collected material for comparing the development of the eggs of 
Fundulus heteroclitus in fresh water with their development in 
sea water, which is their normal medium. I wished also to ascer- 
tain whether or not those embryos hatched in fresh water would 


Development of Fundulus in Solutions of LiCl 119 


show a higher degree of adaptability for living in this medium 
than the normally hatched fish. ‘The fresh water at this place, 
as indicated by later experiments, evidently contained some sub- 
stance that affected the embryos very strangely, as almost with- 
out exception the late embryo assumed a peculiarly twisted 
position upon the yolk the tail bending up in a circular fashion 
and striking the body about half way from the head. Almost all 
of these eggs died before hatching and those embryos that lived 
to hatch were unable to straighten their bodies and died within 
afew hours. ‘The control embryos began hatching three and one- 
half days before these fresh water ones. 

During the past summer at Woods Hole this experiment was 
repeated with the extra precaution of running a distilled water con- 
trol. Here the results were entirely different in regard to the form 
of the embryo. ‘Those fish that hatched in the fresh and distilled 
water exhibited perfectly normal shapes having also occupied the 
usual position on the yolk. But again the Giech water embryos 
were late in hatching, being in the ailiest case two days behind 
the control and some were five days late in coming out. 

The fresh and distilled water embryos gave an interesting mor- 
tality record for different periods of their development. During 
the first ten to twelve days these eggs were about as hardy as those 
in the sea water, though from this time until hatching began 
they died at a rate of over 5 per cent. per day. In one case where 
there were two hundred and twenty-five eggs when hatching 
began, thirty-nine, or 172 per cent. died that night and only six 
individuals were hatched. During the next twenty-four hours forty- 
three, or 23} per cent. died, leaving only one hundred and forty- 
three still alive. The following day, or three days after hatching 
had begun, only eighteen were alive, 874 per cent. having died that 
night, five dying after hatching. None lived in fresh water longer 
than ten hours after hatching out. From this it is seen that the fish 
become peculiarly sensitive to the unusual medium shortly before 
the hatching time and only a few survive to break through the 
ege membrane. 

The fish that did hatch in the fresh water were certainly no 
better fitted to live in this medium than those hatched in sea water. 


120 Charles R. Stockard 


When embryos that had hatched in fresh water were transferred 
directly to either one-half sea and one-half fresh water, or to pure 
sea water, they began at once to show quicker movements, and 
always lived normally in these media. 

Thus it is seen that although the eggs of Fundulus heteroclitus 
will develop in an apparently normal fashion in fresh water, so far 
as form is concerned, they are slower in hatching. ‘The eggs die 
in large numbers during the hatching period, and those that do 
hatch are unable to survive unless transferred to sea water. At 
this period of late development they probably die from the same 
cause that kills the mature fish if they are put into fresh water. 


Zodlogical Laboratory, Columbia University, 
October 16, 1905. 


LITERATURE 


Gurwitscu, A., ’95.—Ueber die Einwirkung des Lithionchlorids auf der Entwicke- 
lung des Frosch-und Kroteneier (Rana fusca und Bufo vulg.). 
Anat. Anz., xi. 

*96.—Ueber die formative Wirkung des veranderten chemischen Mediums 
auf die embryonale Entwickelung. Arch. f. Entwick., iii. 

Hersst, C., ’92.—Experimentelle Untersuchungen tber den Einfluss der veran- 
derten chemischen. Zusammensetzung des umgebenden Mediums 
auf die Entwickelung der Thiere. I Thiel. Zeits. fiir wissensch. 
Zool., iv, 3. 

"93.-—Experimentelle Untersuchungen. II Thiel. Mittheil. aus der 
Zool. Station zu Naepel. 

*96.—Experimentelle Untersuchungen. III, IV, V, und VI Theil. 
Arch. f. Entwick., iv. 

Martuews, A. P., ’04.—The Relation between Solution Tension, Atomic Volume 
and the Physiological Action of the Elements. Am. Jour. Physiol, - 
re 

Morean, T. H., ’03.—The Relation between Normal and Abnormal Develop- 
ment of the Embryo of the Frog, as Determined by the Effects of 
Lithium Chlorid in Solution. Arch. f. Entwick., xvi. 

Ronpravu-Luzeav, ’02.—Action des Chlorures en Dissolution sur le Développe- 
ment des ceufs de Batraciens. Théses prés. Faculté des Sci. 
de Paris Univ. 


PARTIAL REGENERATION OF THE SPERM-RECEP- 
TPAGI te CRAY BEISH 


BY 


E. A. ANDREWS 


Wir ELeven Ficures 


In American crayfish of the genus Cambarus there is a small 
pocket in the shell of the female that receives sperm from the 
male and subsequently frees it when the eggs are laid. ‘This 1s 
not found in other crayfishes, and as Cambarus is the most 
specialized of the group the sperm-receptacle, or annulus ventralis 
as it has been called, seems to be a new acquisition. At the same 
time it is used as one of the necessary reproductive organs. From 
its character, as a dense part of the shell and from its small size 
and protected position, on the ventral side between the legs, it 
seems improbable that it has been subjected to very many injuries 
in the history of the genus. Yet at the time of shedding all parts 
of the shell are soft, and amongst several hundred females exam- 
ined there were two cases in which the annulus had been injured, 
as if by the claws of some other crayfish pressing against the 
shell when soft. However, these were peripheral injuries and 
did not necessarily prevent the use of the organ. 

That the organ has ever been removed in the history of cray- 
fish without the death of the organism seems highly improbable 
as we have no knowledge of any enemy that could cut out that 
region without destroying the neighboring nerve trunk, at the 
least. 

To determine what would happen if the organs were removed 
twenty-four females were operated upon in May and June, 1904, 
so that all, or most all, of the annulus was extirpated. ‘These 
were adult females, some of which had laid in April and May. 
The telson-rostrum measurements of these were as follows: 


Journat or Experimenta Zoétocy, Vor. 1, No. 1. 


129 E. A. Andrews 


One was 50 mm. long; one 55 mm.; three were 60 mm.; eight 
were 65-80 mm.; one 85 mm.; five 100 mm.; four 110 mm., td 
one 120 mm. in length. 

In July five of these cast their shells; one 100 mm., and four 
8o mm. in length. In each case there was something new formed 
of the nature of an annulus, but so imperfect as to be of no use as 
a sperm receptacle. 

To understand these partial regenerations it is necessary to 
consider the anatomy of the normal annulus. The part of the 
shell so-called has in Cambarus afhnis the form indicated in Fig. 
A, which is a ventral, or external view of the annulus from a 
female 110 mm. long, decalcified and made translucent. ‘There 
are here two prominent elevations or knobs with a groove between, 
and a transverse depression posterior. Running across this 
depression is a ZAg-Zag crack, represented by a heavy black line. 
Following the zig-zag in a general way is a wide dotted streak, 
intended to represent the tubular cavity that contains the sperm. 
This tubular cavity opens to the exterior by a rounded hole, partly 
concealed under the larger of the two knobs; it also opens along its 
whole length by the crack represented by the heavy line. The 
curved broken lines indicate the very thick, chitinous walls of the 
tubular cavity, showing through the translucent shell in this 
preparation. 

All this, however, is only the shell of the annulus; under the 
shell there was a corresponding modification of the epidermis and 
connective tissue. ‘The shell fits like a slightly exaggerated mask 
over the epidermis. The knobs, depressed area and_thick- 
walled tube all have their correspondingly-shaped, underlying 
epidermal counterparts, upon which they were made. 

The male puts the sperm into the hole at one end of the tube 
and it subsequently comes out of the zig-zag crack to meet the 
eggs as they pass over the annulus. 

A comparative study of several species of Cambarus shows that 
the essential part of the annulus is the tubular cavity and that in 
one of the lower species this is more evidently a longitudinal 
groove or pocket with its edges nearly closed together. Also in the 
ontogeny of Cambarus afhnis it is found that the annulus of the 


Partial Regeneration of Sperm-Receptacle in Crayfish 123 
young larva presents only a single longitudinal depression and 
that in successive moults this Becories bent, almost closed, and 
associated, with external sculpturing like that of the adult. 

The essence of the annulus is thus an inpitting of epidermis 
which produces a chitinous pocket, opening only to the exterior. 

In extirpating the annulus the shell represented in Fig. A was 
cut away from the rest of the crayfish’s shell and conver 
dragging with it a variable amount of live epidermis and con- 
nective tissue that remained in the cavities of the knobs and 
adhering to the walls of the tube and to other parts of the removed 


shell. ‘This left in place of the annulus a soft, bleeding, sponge- 
like mass which the pressure of contained blood caused to bulge 
out, more or less. [he wounded surface soon became covered 
over by a layer of material that hardened and turned brown and 
then remained as a firm protection until the crayfish cast its shell. 

The cast-off shells then showed in place of such an annulus 
(Fig. A), which is seen in cast shells from normal crayfish, merely 
a rough, brown membrane, continuous all about its edges with 
the old shell of the crayfish. 

The animals that cast their shells showed new annuli as stiff 
bluish plates of the form of an annulus but without its sculpturing 
and with very little to represent its pocket, or tube. 

The regenerated annuli thus obtained are indicated in Figs. 1, 


I24 E. A. Andrews 


2, 3, 4, 5, drawn to the same scale as Fig. A and enlarged about 
13 diameters. ‘The part in each that seems to represent the essen- 
tial sperm pocket is shown in Figs. 1’, 2’, 3’, 4’, 5’, each, magnified 
about 100 diameters. 

All the regenerated annuli are noticeably short, as in young 
crayfish; but the figures fail to give the full surface since these 
annuli are not flat but have a large posterior face as well as the 
ventral (and anterior) one shown in the figures. ‘The posterior 
and ventral faces make a large angle where they meet in an ele- 

vated ridge, which ridge is ae posterior boundary of the above 
figures. It is near this aie ated ridge that the inpitting of epider- 
mis and shell has taken place to form what is like the early stage 
in the sperm-pocket in the larva. In the larva also the inpitting 
takes place near the posterior edge of the ventral face. 

In the largest female that regenerated, 100 mm. long, there was 
a peculiar abnormal outgrowth from the posterior face of the new 
annulus, Fig. 5. ‘This was a soft protuberance bearing a rounded 
lobe and two papillae that were slightly constricted, or jointed, 
somewhat suggesting rudimentary limbs. ‘The smaller papilla 
was dorsal and is not represented in the hgure. ‘This annulus 
was again abnormal in having some ten pits, or infoldings scat- 
tered over its ventral surface as well as the median lengthwise pit 
that seemed to be homologous with a sperm pocket. On the 
posterior, or dorsal, face there were also nearly as many super- 
numerary ridges and depressions as upon the ventral face. Most 


of these were, in general, transverse, and upon the dorsal face more © 


were upon the right of the median line, while upon the ventral face, 
Fig. 5, more were upon the left. 

The median pits that are thought to represent the beginnings 
of sperm pockets had: the following characters in the five speci- 
mens studied. 

In the first, Figs. 1 and 1’, there was a very simple lengthwise 
groove, or pit, with closed lips and near it elevations that con- 
verged as indicated by the lines in Fig. 1. In Fig. 1’ the central 
line represents the closed lips of the groove as seen at the surface, 
while the cavity of the pit is represented by shading and the very 
thick shell walls of the pit are indicated by the broken lines. 


Partial Regeneration of Sperm-Receptacle in Crayfish 125 


Fic. I Fic. 1’ 


Fic. 2 Fic. 2 


Fic. 3 Fic. 3/ 


126 E. A. Andrews 


Optical sections of this preparation showed that the epidermis 
was turned in as a deep groove in which the epidermal cells 
abutted against the shell lining the pit. 

In another specimen the lengthwise pit was associated with an 
anterior depression indicated by the lines in Fig. 2, and with a 
sharp transverse ridge suggesting the posterior curved edge of a 
normal annulus. The pit, Fig. 2’, opened anteriorly into the 
depressed area and its open mouth branched posteriorly right and 
left. The bottom of the pit, shown by lighter shading, ended 
bluntly posteriorly. “he very thick aeusned shell is indicated 
by broken lines indicating its parallel lamellz. 

In the third specimen ihe pit was at the posterior edge and, as 
in Fig. 3, the preparation showed beneath the shell a wide open 
epidermal groove drawn away from the shell, artificially. “The 
pit, Fig. 3’, was open along its length, ended sharply in front and 
opened out behind between two prominent ridges. ‘The annulus 
was much elevated at its posterior edge and from a more posterior 
view the above ridges looked not unlike the knobs of a normal 
annulus, Fig. A, while posterior to them there was a transverse 
ridge similar to that seen in Fig. 2 and again suggesting the pos- 
terior rim of a normal annulus. 

In specimen four there were two pits close together and some- 
what involved with one another, Figs. 4 and 4’. Both were wide 
open, ended bluntly at their anterior ends and expanded pos- 
teriorly into a wide depression. ‘The more dorsal pit was nearer 
to the median plane, the other more to the animal’s left side. 

In the fifth specimen, Figs. 5 and 5’, the median pit was much 
like that in a very young larva. A depressed area near the pos- 
terior edge of the annulus leads forward into a deep groove, the 
walls of which are very thick inturned shell. In this specimen 
there was a slight pouch on each side where the cavity of the 
groove was wider than the open mouth of the groove. In a 
vague way these side pouches suggest the “recess”’ of the normal 
annulus. 

These imperfect annuli, laid bare at the time of shedding, 
would doubtless have continued outwardly in that shape until the 
next shedding. Whether they would have progressed, during 


Partial Regeneration of Sperm-Rece ptacle in Crayfish 


Fic 4 Fic. 4’ 


127 


128 E. A. Andrews 


successive moults, toward the normal adult form must be deter- 
mined by the results of future experiments. 

In the normal growth of crayfish the annulus has a new shell 
formed over it at each period of shedding and these successive 
shells are more and more complex from the early larva to the adult 
stage. 

These experiments show that when in the adult the shell 
over the annulus was removed with more or less of the epidermis 
and connective tissue (removed, probably, for the first time in the 
history of the organ), a new pit and also a new shell, comparable 
to an early larval annulus in complexity, but to an adult annulus 
in size, were formed. 

Thus there may be a partial restoration, in a few months’ time, 
of an organ that is historically new, though essential, and that 1s 
moreover a single organ without fellow or metameric homolog 
and found only in one sex. 

An ability in the crayfish partially to regenerate this special 
adult organ, to begin to retrace the ontogeny of this organ, would 
seem of use to the species only if it were carried far enough to 
complete a functioning sperm receptacle. Experiments may show 
this to be the case. 

That this special ability could have been evolved by natural 
selection seems difficult to conceive since accidental extirpation 
is but a remote possibility. ‘The ability to start this organ a 
second time in one generation would seem comparable to the 
fundamental ability to make the organ again and again in suc- 
cessive generations. 


November 8, 1905. 


ExPERIMENTAL SPUDY OF LIGHT AS A FACTOR IN 
THE REGENERATION OF HYDROIDS 


BY 


A. J. GOLDFARB 


In a paper entitled “The Influence of Light on the Develop- 
ment of Organs in Animals,” ’95, Loeb states “that light favors 
the development of polyps in Eudendrium (ramosum); that no 
polyps, or only very few, are developed in the dark.” At the 
suggestion of Prof. Morgan, I undertook to determine the mini- 
mum amount of light required by this hydroid to regenerate its 
polyps. ‘[he experiments were conducted during the summer of 
1905 at the Marine Biological Laboratory, at Wood’s Hole, Mass., 
while occupying a room of the Carnegie Institution. 


EXPERIMENTS ON EUDENDRIUM RAMOSUM 


Preliminary Experiments 


Vigorous colonies of Eudendrium ramosum were selected. 
These consist of a main stem or stock and its branches. “These 
primary branches in turn give rise to secondary and tertiary 
branches bearing polyps at their distal ends. ‘These polyps— 
and gonads when present—were separately cut off. The decapi- 
tated colony was placed in a glass bowl containing about 200 cc. 
of normal sea water. These bowls were allowed to remain in the 
light for fifteen, thirty, sixty minutes, respectively. At the expira- 
tion of each of these periods the bowls were placed in a dark cham- 
ber, from which all light was carefully excluded. Other series, of 
six bowls each, prepared in the same way were placed in the dark 
immediately after the removal of the polyps. After forty-eight 

and seventy-two hours, respectively, each series was exposed to 
the light as above mentioned. 

In the following tables, the numbers indicate the regenerated 
hydranths which have free and distinct tentacles. 

Tables A, B and C indicate results of these experiments. 


JournaL or ExperiMentTAL Zoo ocy, Vor. m1, No. 1. 


130 A. Ff. Goldfarb 


A 


ExPERIMENT BEGUN JULY 14, 1905, 12.30 M. 
(Exposed before being placed in dark) 


| 
Time | | 
No. of July 15 | July 16 | July 17 | July 18 | July 19 | July 21 
Exposure. | | 
I 15 min. ° Tee LO. 8 ° ° 
2 30 min. fo) 13 22, Seah omen ) 
3 45 min. | fc) 14 ta) 8 fc) fo) 
4 1 hr. 15 min. ° 19 16 7 ° ° 
5 2 hrs. 15 min. co) TI, 9 21se Meld ia I fo) 
6 3 hrs. ° Tie Rta eit ° 
| | 
B 
ExPEeRIMENT BEGUN JULY 12, 1905, 12.30 M. 
(Exposed after forty-eight hours in dark) 
Time | | | 
No. of July 13 July 14 | July 15 | July 16 | July 18 | July rg July 21 
Exposure. | 
I 5 min. ° 27) : 18 20 3 ° ° 
2 10 min. ° 13 15 18 | I ° 
3 IO min. ° 18 23 II abe || I ° 
4 10 min. OM ne | 15 II 2 ° ° 
5 15 min. ° 27 24 18 7 ° ° 
6 15 min. fo) 10 10 $e) 3 ° ° 
Cc 
ExPERIMENT BEGUN JULY 13, 1905, 4.30 P. M. 
(Exposed after seventy-two hours in dark) 
| 
Time eee Pee) |e 2 
Ne: of Fag fie eee ee ipe |eE | .| 
Exposure. ees es aes SS) = 
2 et | i rie, aes ee eee ee a nl Ne aie [ee ey ee eee | 
I 3 min. ° oP i © 4 4 3 | I ° ° ° ° 
2 5 min. fo) ° ° 4 3 | 2 | 0 ° ° ° ° 
3 8 min. ° ° ° 3 3 3 | “2 ° ° o | ° 
4 10 min. ° I ToeexG 8 6 4 2 2 ie I} 6) 
5 15 min. ° 4 | 7 Gl | 5 4 ° I I I 
6 25 min. 3 6 | 9 9 5 5 2 fo) ° ° 
| 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of A ydroids 131 


It appears from these tables, that the brief exposure of three 
minutes was probably not the minimum required by this hydroid 
for the stimulation of its hydranth formation. It is further to be 
observed, in those colonies kept in the dark forty-eight hours or 
more after removal of polyps, and then exposed, that regeneration 
of hydranths occurred before such exposure. Further, the num- 
ber of newly-formed hydranths may or may not be increased after 
an exposure. 

The appearance of hydranths, formed in the dark, may be 
explained: (1) As the result of the exposure from the moment 
when the hydranths were removed to the moment when the decapi- 
tated colony was placed in the dark. (2) Or, what seemed less 
probable, that hydranths were regenerated normally in the dark. 


First Cycle 


‘To avoid a source of error, all Eudendrium colonies were here- 
after prepared as follows: The room was converted into a photo- 
graphic dark room. Light was admitted only through a pane of 
red glass about six inches square. Particular pains were taken 
to keep out all white light. 

In the preceding experiments each hydranth was separately 
removed and the main stem with all its branches was used. In 
the following experiments each primary branch bearing other 
branches and hydranths was cut off near its point of attachment 
to the main stem or trunk of the colony. The trunks with the 
stumps of the primary branches only, were used. The number 
of primary branches so removed was noted in each case. The 
colonies were separately exposed, then placed in dark chambers. 
These were made of double boxes, one within the other, painted 
black within, and edges protected by bands of black cloth. One 
colony of nearly every series was kept in shaded but continuous 
light of the room. ‘This colony will be termed the “Control.” 

In series D—more or less typical of the results obtained in three 
different series of experiments—the colonies were exposed soon 
after removal of the branches. In series E, F and G the colonies 
were at once placed in the dark, and exposed one, two and three 
days, respectively, after removal of the branches. 


132 A. f. Goldfarb 
D 
EXPERIMENT BEGUN JULY 21, 1905, 2.00 P. M. 
(Colonies exposed before being placed in dark) 
ee mene Are fapa alate oleiis| al 2) a) /2 
Branches of | S| & |e] | =| 2] |B! eB} so) so] so] so] eo] eo 
Removed. Exposure. ee Fa eae fee PEN PS EN eA ee en oa iP | Bh) 
| | | | 
a —— |— |_| — |} —|— |_| |---| || | + — 
15 Not exposed 6} ‘9 | 10.|6°|6 1.4 1.3 13) 03" | aa anontontompontnc 
15 1 min. light COMIN a Ta Ya RC 20 V LS Pec inl cal edt ey eR eo GaP) © 
15 I min. sunlight Ot 7 | 4 4o) se las hg, hrc) aa eae ae von toaline 
15 2 min. light CP I MG Ge ts Ge Wye ey iligy Il 23 to: || 2 | ey || @ 
15 2 min. sunlight PHO CC GAN ON or 234) see | wi ihes elt |S || © |, © 
15 4 min. light © |) 14 | ora 8, | 9) eOn| sealncilieo | © |.) ||0%]) tao cine 
Er 
EXPERIMENT BEGUN JULY 22, 1905, 3.00 P. M. 
(Colonies exposed to light twenty-one hours after removal of branches) 

No. of Time = + a S iS <6 Re 5 = Z aot = 
Branches of ee er er te he ea eer oy Safle Tt os 
Removed: ||) Exposures.) p23} =) eee tee eee ee 

21 | Notexposed | o ° | I I I r 2 I I ° ° ° 
26 I min. ° 5} 13 9 5 5 4 ° ° ° ° ° 
28 2 min. ° Bo He 7 8 4 I ° ° ° ° 
23 4 min © =} 14 13 Se ae | rae” ae 8 6 5 4 4 
22 6 min | ° 2 | nS PG 5G} 9 4 I I I ° ° 
26 8 min | Ou | ae slang 7 9 | I0 4 I ° fo) ° ° 
22 Control | o ch loys. |) sar 7 7 5 I I ° ° ° 


1Six buds formed. 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of H ydroids 133 


EXPERIMENT BrGuN JULY 22, 1905, 3.00 P. M. 


(Colonies exposed to light forty-eight hours after removal of branches) 


F 


| 

No. of Time of ral ae ww Ss = oo a Goines 3 

a aq a a a a a a) | oa) iS 

Branches Exposure. > a a > > => => ex alll ees so 

e =: = E} = E =) el 3 

Removed. a = = = = a | as] < 
a Seale = SS Sede — 

19 Not exposed fe) fo) I I 2 2 I fo) fe) ° 

23 I min. ° 8 II 8 II 13 re bell fa ae ° 

27 2 min. ° 4 7 4 5 4 4 2 | 2 ° 

24 4 min. ° ° 2 4 4 4 ° ° ° ° 

21 I min. sunlight fe) ° I 4 3 2 I fo) fo) 

20 2 min. sunlight fo) Zl 4 9 II fe) 8 3 fo) 

23 Control ° 3 13 14 14 8 7 I 2 8 

G 
EXPERIMENT BEGUN JULY 22, 1905, 3.00 P. M. 
(Colonies exposed to light seventy-two hours after removal of branches) 
Nook CP la : : : ; , ea|| ae 

ee Tansey ssh) cay Wren ss tee eS? Sr sel ee 

ranes<S | __ Exposure. aa Soa ene meee Neb WN ies) Bo) ex lee Batt ade 

Removed. | 1S 2,8) 8) 4) 2) 2)2 212 

| | | 
| | | | 

17 Not exposed § oO fo) ° || 5 Gi) 4 2053 

19 I min. L 2 4 5 7 6 4 Joi Ch |e 

15 2 min. | oO ° ° 4 II 10 || 4 e) || Ye 

17 4 min. fo) I 5 3 I 2 3 2 || @ 

14 | Imin. sunlight | o I ° I 9 12 10 5 2 | 2 

17 | 2min.sunlight | o ° 3 9 8 9 6 4 ° ° 

eee Control once) ° 4 7 7 2 3 apy) ° 


It will be observed: 1. About forty-eight hours after removal 
of the hydranth-bearing branches, new hydranths were formed. 
2. These hydranths may appear as follows: (a) Single hydranths 
may appear at the oral ends of the branches that have been cut; 
(b) single hydranths or group of hydranths may appear at the 
aboral end of the main stem or trunk; (c) one of the branches may 
greatly elongate, give rise to new secondary and tertiary branches, 
each bearing a hydranth; (d) the aboral end of the main stem may 
greatly elongate, give rise to many short branches, each bearing a 


134 A. “f. Goldfarb 


hydranth; (e) any combination of (a), (4), (c), (d), may occur. 

. No record was kept of the history of the individual hydranths. 

This a Is tobe terretted. (Kor, hy dranths on a given colony may 
be regenerated yet will not be recorded if other hydranths on the 
same colony should disintegrate or be absorbed. Such occur- 
rences are probably rare, anal dowict seriously affect the conclu- 
sions. 4. Ihe tables reveal a large degree of variability No 
one table can correctly be said to be ty pica in all its details of the 
phenomena in any given experiment. [he conclusions are not 
based on the ‘ eeical: tables but on all the collected data. 

From the foregoing experiments we may draw the following 
conclusions: 

1. Colonies that had not been exposed to the light during 
or after removal of the branches, regenerate their hydranths. 

2. The difference in the percentage of hydranths regenerated, 
on colonies exposed for brief periods and those not exposed is in 
many cases negligible. 

3. In most cases, there is but little difference in the percentage 
of hydranths regenerated on colonies kept in the dark but exposed 
for brief periods, and on the “control”’ colonies. 


It seemed desirable to determine under what conditions these 
hydroids would thrive best, and to determine if it were possible to 
keep all the colonies under practically uniform conditions. 

Colonies were prepared as previously mentioned. 

(a2) Some were kept in shaded light of the room, cooled by 
evaporation from wet cloths; the water in the bowls was not 
changed. (b) Conditions the same, but water in bowls changed 
daily. (c) Bowls partly immersed in running water. The water 
in bowls was not changed. (d) Colonies placed in the direct rays 
of the sun. Water in bowls changed daily. (e) The hydranths 
were separately cut off, leaving main stem, primary branches and 
a large number of secondary and tertiary branches. Water in 
bowl was not changed. 

This experiment showed that the direct rays of the sun or the 
heat caused by them, were injurious; as no polyps were developed 
in (d). Colonies from which the hydranths had been separately 


Light as a Factor 1n the Regeneration of H1 ydroids ne5 


removed in (e) regenerated very few hydranths. Colonies placed 
in the diffuse light of the room, with the temperature of the water 
lowered by means of wet cloths, and the sea water changed daily, 
throve the best, and regenerated the largest number of hydranths. 

Hereafter the sea water in all bowls was changed daily or every 
other day, and all bowls were covered with wet cloths. 

There was the possibility that the pane of red glass admitted 
too much light into the dark room, and probably such light was 
not monochromatic. Partially to overcome these defects, a large 
sheet of red paper—used in photographic work—was so arranged 
that it could be made to cover to a greater or less extent the red glass. 
This paper covered the red glass completely on bright days, or 
uncovered a very narrow slit of the glass on cloudy days. 

In the following experiments every precaution was taken to 
keep the Eudendrium colonies in the most vigorous condition and 
to admit as little red light during observations as circumstances 
permitted. “Lables H and I may be considered typical of results 
obtained in experiments in which ‘colonies were exposed imme- 
diately after or twenty-four hours after removal of branches. But 
on observing that hydranths were regenerated in colonies not 
exposed to the light, experiments were then undertaken in which 
none of the colonies of the series was exposed. ‘Table J is typical 
of results thus obtained. 

H 


ExpPERIMENT BeGcuNn AuG. 14, 1905 


(Colonies exposed immediately after removal of branches. Two colonies in each bowl!) 


| | 
a 4 Time of Sm See (re 1S | | Nl onl eee 
ranches : ee lw : 5 : ; : Berl ee 
=e Exposure. seieeeleach os kos tos lia | a | ao ae 
poets << | S| Seite foe Naas (rec Ubi (acct ba 
| = || | = a ee 
| 
44 | Not exposed fo) if) 16 | 16 19 iy | © 7 6) ke 5 
44 5 sec. ° ° aa |G | 9 10 II II ie GY I 9 
4 CO 10 sec. ° rae teres aeaer Mes Hees oa Neato allis al aise 0) 
42 | % min. ° r || <0 ° ° 2 2B 2 Zu | 4 
42 I min. fo) © || © Te ee! 8 12 12 10 4 3 
42 5 min. ° fo) Oeil 3 | i 10 | Te) 1) 14 | 3 2 
86 Control ° 3 © |) 13 | 282 40) |50) 540." |. |r 3 
} | 
—- Bs 7 we ‘ite |e os | = ar. | an eke 
Memperature -C\,).cn4.502-.--> ZO ZO erShEx9 | 18 | FON zon i2gah lick Wate aleaz 
fi a Piece leer: 


13 colonies in bowl. 


136 A. fF. Goldfarb 


I 


ExPEeRIMENT Brcun Aue. 8, 1905, 2.00 P. M. 


(Colonies exposed twenty-four hours after removal of branches. Three colonies in each bowl) 


| | l 

Sos raaace ll lees aaa EMEabs ps) oe fo eS 
Removed. “i<}/<e/</<ej<a/</<)/<4]/<|</</<\</</< 
60 | Not exposed! (osha) 4) @| ON (20) 2 Saar e igi tel ROMEO 

60 15 min. OF rom e23h iran Se] 38 il Aes Pie ier fee Pe ac |2|olo 

60 30 min. ° I On| DI ase RON) Grae ear I On| erg 20 eo ecmed! 

60 6o min. |o] 16/17]! 7 6 6: | (90, 0 | ea ella ig elo Mees 

54 3 hours. .-|"0.|'26 | 20 |"14.| 14] 2.] 0! © | ‘o)]\Gor) -o onimralen|eaaiec 

54 Control o | 19 | 44 | 47 | 24 | rz | 12 | 13 | 14 | 18 | 17 |23 [21 |17 | 6 | © 
Temperature °C........ —| 25 | 25 | 24 | 21 | 20 | 20 | 20 18 | 19 | 18 |19 20 21 23 |22 


ExPERIMENT BeGuNn AUuG. 14, 1905, 4.00 P. M. 


(No colonies exposed to light during or after removal of branches. Two colonies in each bow!) 


ae a »l¥l}se|lelealele]ele¢ a) leas 
Branches bo | a6 do dp do a0 do | a0 oo bo bb 
eae ae eed een ee a | | 
| 

54 fe) 2 5 5 2 I fo) ° ° ° fo) 
54 ° 4 13 15 6 4 I ° ° ° 
54 ° 3 14 14 5 2 ° ° ° ° ° 
50 ° 7 15 14 II 7 6 6 3 ° I 
50 ° 5 8 12 7 6 5 2 I ° ° 
5° fo) 10 20 14 6 6 3 6 6 I ° 
Temperature °C. .| 20 20 18 19 | 19 19 20 21 = |), 28 22 


From all of the foregoing experiments we may conclude that: 

1. Hydranths are regenerated about forty-eight hours after 
removal of hydranths or branches. 

2. [he largest number of hydranths observed at any one time 
is recorded from the second to the fourth day after removal of the 
hydranth or branch, or on the first or second day after the appear- 
ance of the regenerated hydranths. 


'The total number of hydranths and branches removed exceeded 2,391. 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of H1 ydroids 1237 


3. Thereafter there is a gradual decline in the number of 
hydranths. 

4. Finally no hydranths are recorded on any of the colonies 
constituting a series, and the close of the first “cycle” is reached. 
It may take from nine to twenty-two days before all the hydranths 
of a series are gone. l’he average time is thirteen days. 

5. [here seems to be no causal relation between the number 
of regenerated hydranths of a colony and the time during which 
such colony had been exposed. Colonies exposed for one minute 
may form an equal, greater or less per cent. of hydranths than 
those colonies exposed for two minutes. 

6. In four experiments the per cent. of hydranths formed on 
colonies kept in the dark but exposed for brief periods, was equal 
to or greater than the per cent. recorded in the control colonies. 
In but two experiments was the per cent. less than in the control 
colonies. 

7. Colonies kept in the dark forty-eight and seventy-two hours, 
respectively, after removal of the branches, and then exposed, 
regenerate their hydranths before such exposures. 

8. Colonies that had at no time during the experiment of 
fifteen days been exposed, nevertheless regenerated a great many 
hydranths. 

om ihe percentage of regenerated hydranths on colonies not 
exposed was in four series equal to or greater than in those colonies 
exposed for brief periods. In four other series the per cent. was 
less than in the colonies that had been exposed. Furthermore, 
in two series the percentage of regenerated hydranths in colonies 
not exposed was greater than in the “control” colonies. 

We may safely conclude that during the first period or cycle of 
about thirteen days, darkness does not prevent the regenerative 
processes In Eudendrium ramosum jrom taking place. Nor does 
darkness necessarily retard the development nor decrease the number 
of hydranths formed. On the contrary exposure to the light for 
short periods may or may not increase the rate of growth nor the 
number of hydranths. 


138 A. “f. Goldfarb 
Second Cycle 


The colonies of Eudendrium ramosum seemed to be, during 
this first period of about thirteen days, in a condition not unlike 
phototonus in plants, 7. ¢., under the influence of previous exposure 
to the light. “In darkness,” Pfeffer’ says, “‘a plant continues to 
erow normally as long as it remains in a condition of phototonus, 
1. e., so long as the iadnene: of the previous exposure persists. 
This period is frequently very prolonged. So long as the plant 
remains in a condition of phototonus, . . . small changes of 
illumination produce no perceptible effect.”” It seemed not 
improbable that the colonies that regenerated hydranths in the , 
dark were under the influence of the light previous to cutting 
away the branches. 

Acting on this supposition the following experiments were 
undertaken. After all the hydranths of a series had disappeared 
and the end of the first cycle was reached, 7. e., when the influence 
of the previous illumination was gone, all the colonies but one 
were subjected to the light for varying periods of time. This one 
colony was carefully ene in the danke There was also in nearly 
every series one control colony placed in the light. 

Tables Ez, K, L and M are typical results obtained in nine 
series. 


E2 
(This Table is continuation of Er) 
First No. of Second | 
No. | Exposure | Branches Exposure Aug. 5. [ous 6. | Aug. 7. | Aug. 9. | Aug. Io. | Aug.1II. 
July 22- \inemovedl> AUue4- 
Time. Time. 
1 | Not exposed 21 Not exposed ° ° | ° ° ° 
2 I min. 26 3 min. 2 4 ° ° 
3) a) 2 min. 28 4 min. o (| fo) ° ° ° ° 
4 4 min. 23 + min. — | = 
5 6 min. 22 I min. 7 5 o | ° 
6 | 8min. | 26 4 min. oo | Q | I o | ° ° 
7 | Control | ° ° Ou o | o | ° 
| 
| | | | 
Memperaturess Cee see eee et rer ee | = 24 eve || Pex |) BS 25 
| | 


Pfeffer: “The Baeieen e Plants,” ron Dp sed 1903. Trans. by Alf. J. Ewart. 
a 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of H ydroids 139 


K 
Date or EXPERIMENT JULY 13, 1905 
First Exposure, | Second Exposure, s Pa ES 3 ey eee | a | a 
No. July 16. July 27. & = = = = Salad ten aah 
: : S| 5) 
Time. Time. = = | zk = ul et & 4 
I 3 min. Not exposed ° ° ° ° ° fo) ° 
2 5 min. I min. ° 4 I ° ° 
3 8 min. 2 min. ° Te || 5 4 I I ° 
| 
4 10 min. 4 min. ° 9 17 17 13 3 ° ° 
5 15 min. 6 min. 2 GN) aug) 9 rie | 6 2 fo) 
6 25 min. I min. sunlight ° 6 | 9 8 2 ° ° fo) 
L 
| No. of | First Exposure, Second Exposure. Hydranths regenerated at end of 
No. Branches Aug. 14. 

‘Removed. Time. Time. Date. | 1 day | 2 days |3 days|4 days 5 days 
SO = iS Ss SO a (eed ee S| ee 
ie ee are Not exposed | Not exposed | Aug. 27 fe) ° ° o | — 
ze || 22 | min. | jhymin Aug. 27 ° ° ° o | — 
| 21 % min. $ min Aug. 25 ° ° fo) fe) 2 
a) | 21 I min. } min Aug. 25 ° 2 2 4 4 
5 21 I min. ¢ min Aug. 26 ° I 6 9 = 
6 21 5 min. Imin. |Aug.26| o | 0o 2 3 — 
7 86 Control 3 I 2 fe) 

1More than one colony in each bowl. 
M 
No. of First Exposure, Second Exposure, Hydranths regenerated after 
No. Branches Aug. 8. Aug. 25. 
Removed. Time. Time. 1 day |2 days|3 days | 4 days 

I 60! Not exposed Not exposed ° ° ° ° 
2 60! 15 min. 4 min. fo) ° I 3 
3 40! 30 min. + min. fo) fo) fo) ° 
4 40! 1 hour 4 min. ° ° I 2 
5 361 3, hours I min fo) fo) I co) 
6 56 Control I I 3 9 
Memperatune Ce gece ere Nees siesatekea) «,folnle eel eM ajnniece sieieiew 18 18 17 18 


1More than one colony in each bowl. 


140 A. Ff. Goldfarb 


As in the first cycle hydranths are regenerated about forty-eight 
hours after exposure. 

There is also no causal relation between the time of exposure 
and the number of hydranths developed. 

Formation of new hydranths occurred with but two exceptions, 
on colonies that were exposed, even for such brief periods as five 
or ten seconds. 

The maximum number of polyps observed at any one time is 
recorded on the third or fourth day after exposure, or on the 
second day after the appearance of the polyps. This is somewhat 
later than in the first cycle. 

Thereafter there is a rapid decline in the number of hydranths, 
more rapid than during the first cycle. 

Finally no more hydranths develop or are observed on any of 
the colonies comprising a series. ‘The end of the second cycle 1s 
reached. It may take from seven to fourteen days before all the 
hydranths are gone. ‘The average is ten days—much shorter than 
in the first cycle. 

The five colonies not exposed at the beginning of the second 
cycle did not regenerate any hydranths during this cycle. In two 
colonies two hydranths were formed, however, though colonies 
were not exposed. 

The control colonies bore no more hydranths by the end and 
in some cases by the middle of the second cycle. 

From these observations we may infer that after the first cycle, 
light was necessary jor the regeneration of the hydranths. Without 
light no regeneration of hydranths, or very little, takes place. It 1s 
surprising to note what a short exposure—in some cases one- 
twelfth minute—was sufficient to start the regenerative processes. 
‘These brief exposures, however, may so stimulate the develop- 
ment of hydranths that the maximum number of hydranths noted 
at any one time during the second cycle is often equal to, or greater 
than, the maximum at a during the first cycle. Those colonies 
that had not been exposed but hon nevertheless formed new 
hydranths during the second cycle, may have been stimulated by 
over-exposure to the red light. In one case at least, according to 
an observation made at the time, a colony which had not been 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of 1 ydroids 


141 


exposed was removed from the bowl. This was thoroughly 
cleaned to remove sediment that had collected. 
then replaced. All this was done in red light. 


The colony was 
Two days later 


a hydranth appeared on the colony. 


Third Cycle 


At the end of the second cycle the colonies of four series were 
again exposed. By this time several colonies were broken into 
two or more pieces, or stolons or branches broken off, or the colony 
was otherwise mutilated. 


Tables E3 and N give tabulated results. 


F3 
(This Table is continuation of Et and Ez) 
No. of First Second Third 
| ail 5 . : Oo : . : . ON) Sele so 
N branches| Exposure, | Exposure, | Exposure,| §| | +) Ww) S| LL] 6) Al Go x] al] Hou 
oO. Padi tittle tal tn | er aa feted =a ea a | al ala 
re- | July 23. | Aug. 4. | Aug. 11. | o5/ oe] wo] eo] so] uo] so) eo] eo| so) eo] eb] eo 
: : ; 4) eh Sh S| er sen) Eh ea el ea Sub aye 
moved. Time. Time. Time. | aici aici alietiael| ele aieia lea 
| | 
ee = = eu ee ERE SN | | |e 
. . . | | | 
1 eer o min. o min 2 min On|ROn MON Se (ia Naaliean eTanO o|O;/oj;0 
2 26 I min. + min I min OD igo" |k0) || or |e) | on On| Fonlonlro) to 
5 1 mi i 6 | | | 
3 | 28 2 min. + min 4 min Oy |) Te ND Wey AI Zee Na | 
4 23 4 min. o min } min || Gr) 2 | eC NSae ire | se se |) se ie |] ee Ie 
| | 
5 | 22 6 min. I min $ min Onl sete 2g) AT ON On| On| tay On Onlin 
6 | 26 8 min. 4 min © min On| FONE 2e eres Onl er PON Onion EON ONO 
7 | 22 Control @) {S| O||. © | OF) |) Oy) | © |-@ | © 1) © 
y e ae =a ee = se ee 7 ma 
fe} 
Ilaria “(Con cpoonnceoqsncassuesosseabar 24 |21 |— |20 |20 |18 |19 |18 |19 |20 |22 |23 |22 
| | 


1. We observe that hydranths may be formed forty-eight or 
seventy-two hours after exposure. They may not, however, 
appear until four or five days after exposure. 

2. [he largest number of hydranths that appeared at any one 
time was in four colonies greater, in two colonies equal to, and in 
twelve colonies less than, the largest number that appeared at any 
time during the preceding cycles. 

3. Asin the first two cycles, the maximum number of hydranths 
was observed on the second or third day after exposure, or on the 
first or second day after the appearance of the hydranths. 


142 


A. f. Goldfarb 


“6Z “‘3ny i am|0O0 0 0 0 - > 
"ez “Sny [When fo 10. oO Ole > 
Lz -3ny GO) 10) OOF te 
‘gz ‘Sny CO) OO mars <2 
‘Sc -sny FO) OO miniares a 
“bz -Sny | 0000 +H <n 
"ZT “any ©; 10) 10!) (G7 cro = 
“iz -Sny - 0 00a 0 ° 
‘oz “Sny | ~0 00H O01 B 
“61 -Sny | a 0000 ~*~ 
“QI “ony | QO) (0) +0) 7-7-0: cS 
“Lr -3ny a 0000 oo 
ATG ca 0" 10: ° 
gi ‘sny nt | 
‘Sr-iny | a2 0 0 000 Q 
“by ny - 00 0 O | 
! 
fr -3ny SOMO Om co mn 
Eon le SOmGnomone + 
zi -sny Sy 
Ay | : 
© : 
=) pf ° 
Se teh x eth Se on. e ; 
a, rs) Sede eae 5 
Ge sg eles] ast ger oe : 
uss fs} ish fet fell fe : 
ce) pons da =e AN se O : 
dst ce : 
a) | 2 
aH : 
£ | 2 : 
3 ae 
Ones | aS 3 
wa OO 5 ss 
A 2 
7 o ics ag a : 
5 BR aide ; 
o [ mm a t+ oH 5 
eA : 
ey | ; 
. 
2 at | eG oe ee 
: sh Wen fer (ee) ; 
a= eo det ga) ss) qei lsh © ‘ 
Aa als: BASS eigen g 
Ss on ° 
=| | 2) 
a S 
o 
j Q, 
° 1 =a A om + Hw & 
@ & 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of H ydroids 143 


4. [he number of hydranths formed bears no causal relation 
to the time of exposure. 

5. The number of hydranths declines, but for no apparent 
cause may increase in number. So that the third cycle may last 
for five days or may last as long as eighteen days. 

6. Colonies not exposed at the beginning of the third cycle 
may or may not regenerate hydranths. Out of the four colonies 
not exposed at the beginning of the third cycle, three had been 
exposed at the beginning of each of the preceding cycles. 

7. [he control colonies regenerated but one hydranth. 

We notice how erratic the response to light has now become. 
Hydranths are developed on colonies not exposed at the beginning 
of the third cycle. The number of hydranths decreases, finally 
all are gone, yet for no apparent cause will increase again. Is it 
possible that at the beginning of the third cycle, arbitrarily chosen, 
the cycle had not actually come to a close, and the hydranths that 
had formed were influenced by the exposure at the beginning of 
the second cycle? Or, had the colonies become so sensitive that 
the slight exposure to red light necessitated by daily observations 
was sufficient to start regeneration? Could the colonies after 
a time dispense with light and thrive in the dark, except for slight 
red illumination? ‘The evidence is not conclusive in support of 
any one of these suppositions. 


Fourth Cycle 


The evidence is practically identical with that obtained for 
third cycle. ‘Table O is typical of results obtained. 

Some control colonies also show periodic increase and decrease 
in the number of hydranths. There may be two, three or even 
four cycles observed in the history of one control colony. Such 
cycles are not explicable by a difference of temperature or change 
of sea water in the bowls, for these cycles occur apparently inde- 
pendently of such changes. A lapse of thirteen or fourteen 
days during which no hydranths appear may intervene between 
cycles. 


A. fF. Goldfarb 


144 


Li 


fof. (ey fo) 


‘6c "Sny | 0 0 0 4 = Oo oo 


co 
~ 


tao mao | 


suru © | suru $1 yystpuns suru 1 ‘urur $z 
“Ulu Z | “UIUI I “UIUI 9 suru $1 
ur g | ‘urul % suru + ‘UII OI 
“uIur “UIUL 7 “UI 7 “UU g 
“UIUL O “uIU O ‘uTu I ‘ur § 
“UIUL I | “UIUT I “UIUI O suru £ 
ee | Pensa US eke me | ae 
Cua “our, “OUT, | oun, 
‘or ‘any | y -3ny ‘gz Atnf | ‘gt Aqnf 
‘ginsodxy yqinoy | ‘aginsodxy paryy, |‘einsodxg puosag ‘ainsodx gq qsaty 


Q cm st Ho 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of A ydrotds 145 


Exposure to Continuous Light 


The colonies of four series were again exposed to the light as in 
the preceding experiments, but no formation of hydranths occurred 
even by the fifth day. It seemed not improbable that the colonies 
were spent. Did the protracted stay in the dark permanently 
injure the hydroids or rather did darkness permanently effect 
the regenerative processes in Eudendrium? ‘This could be deter- 
mined by permanently exposing the colonies to the light. The 
colonies of the four series were removed from their dark chambers 
and placed permanently in the light. Before examining the 
results we should bear in mind that: 

1. Prior to this exposure the colonies bore no hydranths for 
periods of three to fifteen days. 

2. This exposure occurred from twenty to twenty-five days 
after removal of the stems. 

3. Many of the colonies were now badly injured or were but 
remnants of the original colony. 

Tables P and Q are typical of results obtained. 

It will be observed that hydranths are regenerated very fre- 
quently five days after exposure, though it may take nine and even 
twelve days before hydranths make their appearance. This 1s 
much longer than was necessary at any other time. 

2. The number of hydranths formed is surprisingly large. In 
four colonies the maximum number of hydranths equaled, in five 
colonies was greater, and in fourteen colonies was less than the 
maximum noted on any colony at any other time. 

3. [he maximum number of hydranths for any one time was 
recorded from the seventh to the tenth day after exposure, or from 
the third to the fifth day after the appearance of the hydranths, 
much later than during any preceding period. During preceding 
cycles, hydranths regenerated by the second or third day after 
exposure and decreased in numbers thereafter. In these experi- 
ments hydranths were slow in making their appearance, but kept 
on regenerating new ones in some cases for the first six days. 

4. [he per cent.t of newly-formed hydranths on control colo- 


’The per cent. in this and previous cases is obtained by dividing the number of branches removed 
into the number of hydranths regenerated in a colony. 


A. f. Goldfarb 


c 
dz -Sny | mon ate a | 


‘6c-3ny | 0 0 mom HH | 


£z 


‘be any | enone ne | 


It 


A]juoueutiag 


‘gr Sny | Se 
an Bay) || 0 (6 “ollol'o) ola 


‘vn-any || 6 © 6 io ollo le 


146 


OUI, 


‘ol ‘sny 
| fainsodxy pry, 


‘ginsodxq puosag 


joru0g 


qy8iyuns suru z 
qystyuns suru 1 
suru 
“UII Z 
‘uIUT T 
“UIUI O 


‘ginsodxg 44 


soinqeraduta 7, 


(2 
oz 
1Z 
Vz 
LG 
tz 
61 


| 


*pasouray, 
soyouerig 


TOON 


| 
| 
| 
| 


147 


& 
™ 
~ 
S 
SS 
S 
ys 
un. | | | 
3S gl Li gt] gi] zz Sz Iz| oz 61 gl 61 gI| oz| oz| — IZ| $z OO Ost Ox OnIO.0 OnNt OOOO) OO MIT O UOMO nD OU OOo ‘aanqeiadwa J, 
Se eee ——|——|-—|__|__|__|-—_|- oe : 
9 
3 Oh!) 1011/70/50}! 08/6) || 0) || ON|NO) |ON|10) 0" | 10) ||/"0 0/0 Jomuog $1 
© il len teal |e On OR ON MON OTR OR OM On On sO: |O 3 | 0] 0 ut 9 ull g qysipuns *urur z LI 
% | | | “Vyat | : | 
- s Ten | ea |esCO|Co|ona | ON RON ON ROM On |sOnOulo) ne co) uu ¥ ur + yysipuns suru 1 at 
: = : ! ! : 
So Diag |e |e || ON le| eaa|| OMe | eb ea (eed | On| On| KO TO : q ome) ulll 7% ‘Ulu $7 ‘ulur + Li 
. | | ae, ; : ; 
av, | 4 We || Bu Mote AS Mey [heey COI] ale) oul} © eal °°} 0 ulut uu I urur Z $1 
Ajjuoueuiog : : 5 
& HM 2M i AN AS We) ih hl] |) || Cl} S|) oO oa Ke) ur | ulm } uur I 61 
aa CuO Ou onl Wal Walt oh IsOn"ei\so" No) |"O)|"0! ea |olo ult oO ur oO ur oO Lt 
RS eg el saa cae esac oe Gee Gai Sale SS SS Se ee SSS SS SS SS 
= | 
SYS Sl Sl Sl el S| el el ele) Sl el ele : 
a Ele ene We Vecle nl eoe le eae ie We > en ou, ow, QUILT, poaoulayy 
LS Sales Beran oneal liek foots |cetoal icra l|eecaa ce | eah ent ese oRieies 11 -‘sny 1-3ny Sz An sayoursg 
S tv tv isd 1S iss bt iS] v iS Lal _ _ _ Leal _ ~ _ 
3 GE Se NSS a Ge SS EN ei CST ates at ee 2 | P | Omsodxg pany, |‘eansodxg puosag | ‘ainsodxg qsaty jo ‘ON 
3 
ub 
S 
~ 
xo 
50 
N 


148 A. f. Goldfarb 


nies 1s exceeded by the per cent. on colonies previously kept in the 
dark. 

5. The regenerative processes were certainly not impaired by 
the almost continuous confinement in the dark. 

6. After the first cycle light certainly stimulates the regenera- 
tion of hydranths in Eudendrium ramosum. 


EXPERIMENTS ON PENNARIA TIARELLA. 


The hydroid Pennaria tiarella is found abundantly on the same 
piles covered by Eudendrium ramosum. ‘The environment for 
both these hydroids is practically the same, although Pennaria 
may also be found in exceedingly large numbers attached to 
floating eel grass. In the following experiments colonies of Pen- 
naria were taken with one exception from the piles of a dock. 
Vigorous colonies were chosen. ‘The branches were removed and 
the colonies treated in the same way as Eudendrium. A total of 
2,672 branches were cut off. 

Tables R and S are typical of results. 

From these experiments we may conclude that: 

1. If colony with its branches and hydranths be kept in the 
dark for forty-eight hours or more the hydranths are dropped off 
or disintegrated. 

2. If these colonies be kept in the dark they do not regenerate 
any hydranths. 

3. Exposure to daylight or direct sunlight for less than three 
hours did not result in the development of hydranths. Hydranths 
were formed after an exposure of three, four, four and one-half 
and five hours, respectively, only in Experiment R. The hy- 
dranths thus formed disappeared after forty-eight hours in the 
dark. 

4. Exposure to diffuse light or to sunlight for periods varying 
from a few minutes to nineteen hours did not result—with the 
exceptions noted above—in the formation of hydranths. Ex- 
posures for two days invariably induced formation of new hy- 
dranths. Probably exposure to brilliant light for not less than 
two days is in most cases, needed for regeneration to take place. 

5. Confinement in the dark for periods of thirteen to seventeen 


149 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of H ydrotds 


(ey || fe) ||) | o \*dx9j0N 


o| 0] 0] 0] © | © | sinoy $z 
o | sinoy + 


o|0]/0!] 0] 0| o | sanoy $s 


o|lo|o|o]o|o|simoy Z 


>| >| >| >| >| > io 
fet EN ene Eie We eee I) asp tc) 
oa [ca joe jos joe joe | 6 & 
Nn ct 
SIS) WAS WSs Te 
SS jaja ik |S |) ae 
ty 
i 


| 


| oO 


| 
| 
| 


| 


~ 


~ 


° 


| | 

| | 

| | 
| 


sinoy z7| 0 | 0 | Z| Z@ | O | sinoy S| 
sinoy £| 0 | 0 | O | O | O | SInoY Zz 
sinoy | 0} 0}0}]0); 0) AnoY I | 


sinoy $| 0 | 0 | 0 o | o |*dxa jon | 


“gi “Sny 


| 
| 
s | 
| 


“Sr -3ny 
“cI “Sny | 


e 
iS 
i) 
= 
ma 


‘ainsod 
-xq puose 


| 


° 


° 


‘or ‘3ny | 


-dxajon | 0 | o | S$ | Z| © | sanoy $F] 0 | © | 


6 -3ny | 
-L-3ny | 
“9 ‘Sny 


“uIUl O% 


suru $1 


“ulul OT 


suru 


*dxa jon 


: 
| 


ainsod 
“xq jo oulry, 


(ES as) [oUOD 9 


! | | 


“$ -3ny | 
“y -3ny | 
€-Eny | 
7 -Sny | 
“1 -3ny | 
‘ON 


*paaouleyy 
SuIa}g JO ‘ON 


(12.09 yova ul pasvjd $a1u0]09 aad T,) 


So6r ‘1 xtaf ‘INaWiaaaxy 40 ALvd 


U 


A. Ff. Goldfarb 


150 


“yy 31] 03 AUeueUIed pasodxa a19\\y 


|] Gil) Gel er FO) fe) © 
il) || | Oi) eS] © 
Ton lve) cavern bea | oP 
ysl) Gerd sad 5-9 | Sidley 
eM ceall Wey 5 |p Se (Si | xe 
€ | + | + | on] zz] 61) zz 
Slr el er Sle] S 
(S c c i= i=] c c 
Ce OSS Ia Omen a OSs On 
iS N wR & iad vv & 
SSS ESO a as tie 


(e) lized Poe eo) “yt 
Log RA en PL aT 
TON TOMO paseyd 
EU |On On| 0 puau 
11)0 | 0} 0) -puag 
Stlo|lolo 

Pl Srl S| Se 

i= [=] (sj (=] 

a joa jos jo 

ea litical Geet les 

Co tl ea ae 

| 


‘yr ny | 000070 


SGiyce | 
ticsny |) © © oO Ol = = 


g | z| jomuog | € | + |] 0 | & | fonuon| 1 | o 
+ | € | aq8y up| z | $ | on] g |ayqsyuy |,o | o 
o | o | sinoygv | 0 | 0 | 0 | o | ‘urm S1| 0 | o 
o|o/|/smoy &|0}]o0]0]0/-‘urmor/o| o 
o | o | smoy%1|o]o]o0}]o]'urm $ | 0} o 
o | o | -dxajon,| 0 | 0 | 0 | oO |-dxaj0nN | 0 | Oo 
ple Sele eee ede 
we jos | BS los jos jos jos | 3 S joe jos 
Sle hee eles yleqele ss) Fa tes tas 
s[n | o§ Oil | rls gas as: 

. te . esl 

fi # 


fs | 
any | © 0 0 © oO Oo 


ome | 
zsny | 0 0 0 0 0 o 


Jonuog | o | gg 
‘ulm + | 0 | 09 
‘uu z | o}| 68 
‘um 1 |-0 | £6 
‘ull = | O |} 98 
‘dxajon | 0 | 06 
ee aed) ees 
Be la | 2 
Zz ~ 3 mt 
= © 0 op 
Sea an 
# a 


(jm0q 4IDA UL SatuojOI addy T, *SayIUDIG fo jp20mas 491{D Sinoy omj-j40f pasodxs) 


Sob ‘1f xtaf NvOdgG INAWINdaXy 


S 


Light as a Factor in the Regeneration of Hydroids 151 


days does not permanently prevent regeneration, for, on continu- 
ous exposure after such confinement, hydranths are readily 
developed in large numbers. 

6. Pennaria colonies kept in the dark never developed any 
hydranths. Yet these colonies were often literally crowded with 
living, thriving colonies of Bryozoa (species undetermined), with 
many small campanularian hydroids probably Clytia cylindrica, 
and with many young individuals of Eudendrium ramosum. 

7. Light is absolutely essential for the normal growth, develop- 
ment and regeneration of Pennaria tiarella. 


SUMMARY 
Eudendrium 


In examining the effect of light on regeneration of hydranths in 
Eudendrium ramosum, we must distinguish between 
1. Colonies kept in the dark but under the influence of 
previous illumination 
2. Colonies kept in the dark and not under the influence of 
previous illumination. 

1. These colonies regenerate their hydranths two days after 
removal of their branches. Such regeneration is independent of 
light. ‘Uhat light is not essential to the regeneration of hydranths 
during the first thirteen days (during which influence of previous 
illumination obtains) is shown by the fact that, (a) colonies kept 
in the dark and not exposed develop a large number of hydranths; 
(6) colonies kept in the dark and not exposed may regenerate an 
equal or greater per cent. of hydranths than those exposed for 
short periods, or those kept in the continuous light, viz., the con- 
trol colonies. 

2. In colonies not under the influence of previous illumination, 
1. e., after the first thirteen days or thereabouts, the response to 
light is decidedly different. Such colonies do not develop hy- 
dranths or very few hydranths unless exposed. The surprisingly 
short exposure of one-twelfth minute may sufhce to start the 
regenerative processes, but some exposure is essential. 

The illumination prior to removal of branches will stimulate the 


152 A. fF. Goldfarb 


regeneration of hydranths on colonies kept in the dark, and main- 
tain such hydra nths for a long period or cycle of about thirteen 
days. [he second cycle is iaied by a brief exposure which 
stimulates the colony to renewed activity. Similarly short expo- 
sures may rejuvenate the series of colonies during a third and fourth 
cycles. ‘The per cent. of hydranths formed at any one time during 
the second, third or fourth cycle may be equal to, or greater than, 
the per cent. formed at any preceding time. 

There is no causal relation between the number of hydranths 
formed and the time of exposure, 7. ¢., the per cent. of hydranths 
formed after exposure of one-half minute may be greater, 
equal to, or less than per cent. formed after exposure of one minute. 

The minimum diffuse bright light required to cause hydranths 
to regenerate is probably one-twelfth minute. It is not impossible 
that under certain conditions a less exposure could bring about 
regeneration. 

‘The regenerative processes are not impaired by almost continu- 
ous confinement in the dark for from twenty to twenty-five days. 
On continuous exposure the per cent. of hydranths formed may 
equal or exceed that during any preceding time. 

Sometime after removal of hydranths, the colonies kept in the 
dark, but exposed for brief periods, are stimulated to regenerate 
hydranths more readily than those colonies kept in the light, viz., 
the control colonies. 


Pennaria 


Prolonged darkness is inimical to development and maintenance 
of hydranths in colonies of Pennaria tiarella. If kept in the dark 
colonies lose their hydranths within forty-eight hours, and do not 
develop new ones. 

Exposure to brilliant light for three or four hours may stimulate 
the development of hydranths, but usually an exposure of not less 
than two days is necessary for such development. 

As in Eudendrium, confinement in the dark for long periods 
does not permanently affect the regenerative powers of these 
colonies. 


OBSERVATIONS AND EXPERIMENTS CONCERNING 
THE ELEMENTARY PHENOMENA OF EMBRY- 
ONIC DEVELOPMENT IN CHAXTOPTERUS' 

BY 
FRANK R. LILLIE 
With One Prare anp SEVENTY-EIGHT FiGcurEs IN THE TEXT 
il, Ilnnonlnetions 66.40 ocaabatcasch ase bees Coma dd et tOer Gee ec merece rae anerrs a 154 
itive, Microscopic Composition of the Protoplasm of the Egg. ..- 2 2.2... ee seo ee 156 
fis “lee Inia IER ES ocootad6asucos5t G00 UU OsaN Up EOS DUS EBOUORADoUanoOUoaGeH 156 
z. Comparison.of the Living and of the Fixed and Stained Protoplasm................ 160 
Lin, “The GUniGihs i Gas ats ehguandar 5 bSanbhuos seb ConeneD Hada eo SnmoeEoneEener Aonoeee 162 
Tie (OLR EONS a6nho Acro came dane Gros DSCC ETO CHECn Oat aaa peer mao See sb or 162 
aa) Betore| Rupture of the Germinal Vesicle <2 2. -!.--------.---. wisp ysis + eee 162 
Goethe se enod om Maturation: and ertilizatiom ~ceiciciy- © 2 1) > = «ole erosion Bere LOT 
Gy Action ol, CentrtugalsPorce in’ the Unsepmented Egg... ..).5.5...s.emee se 4: 179 
peelsiteraturerandabiscussioneseer eee seeee cce fee es SBA as eee ete ss oo pacdonde 187 
a. The Axis of the Ovarian Ovocyte and its Relation to Polarity.................-- 187 
peme Rhesbictoplasnuce nay ermer etme sete er tei taleytatciore lates ole aim) © Sin = a olegrereien selene erate 188 
GaekesidualiSubstancevot the! Germinal Vesicle: «.5.- << oe + ss... - ever ioe eiacl= sls IgI 
ih IPO RRA FON oA o86.o n.56 ce Gouna AO ob nD GOO ee oa ae. 5, ao ation oe rarissoc.2 193 
IY, Clleaiyaras auial Diana thatoing os apo qu dbs cso Done DORE a ADE Cees oc coteponcosoGaTdcc™ 194 
Tr) Hormative Stufis in the Early, Normal) Development. ........-.2..0.-----+eeneeeese- 195 
a. “(Nae ING OLENA esa sso dddadSecdade Hobe SONS BBBMOGMDD 50 - Sneed aauuspoacucac 195 
a, Ine Iden or CleANAies soon ctu scmgnadouees Seas o> BBHe > odocesuaroobdbcasrun ac 204, 
Eee Distributionvorsoupstancesmusthe: Cellseert sila e- = - tebe) ersbels eteiole lois et 207 
Gi, lbatare: Wikia Siemaiers, 5 oo Sia nec can nedde OSD OSERREnT 9 00 Csbr aa Sbacn bod oauobesC 212 
2, IDMiterentaeystoa Wiptdevts (CIENT EIS Jo 55. ¢enncce nae Beeeo> oc aac ce uuoBoonddDoUnSoouT 220 
ae,» Uinta eleeial( OVA po docents baa Sn OEE cs eon oscars hon ien bona 223 
io Ieudhy Saracsuatstrn Ge lNesmeion se SiGe Rep 55 ooo dp oounoeosbadoooDUOnaesor 223 
iia COnceming thes Growthyof the Nucleus. cae mmic cas oleiicies core ie ilsinis el 227 
fie ateractionvols Nucleuseand | C@ytoplasm oer se erseltele ies ate orate) ere) ole mel =)= lore 230 
ive, Water Distribution’ of Formative Stufisst 2.2.2. .-2..cc eens esse tse: 232 
Vee Bormationsob Ciliacand, Other Cell-Constituents. ....-..2..<0--2-++eesr sens 236 
pee Mon iicleatedmUnusermentedes@va).. . <jueiterseteter a= tele ietelele ele lal orin)s eee = 239 
Cupleiteravorerand sl) i1sCUSSIOMs Ae soc « - «3, sperrverra eerste ore sc lejcia(s ls sie isco ereiaysie = eve 243 


1 The experimental parts of this study and the observations dealing with the living egg were car- 
ried on at the Marine Biological Laboratory of Woods Holl, Mass; the study of the preparations 
in the Zcélogical Laboratory of the University of Chicago. 


154 Frank R. Lillie 

V.'; General Discussions refer. vei éc-iese olcln ne /e, 5 axe esas ghee ale «te a en ae es 246 
i. Relation*of Nucleus and!iCytoplasm..«.. .\<sis 6-2. aise ore chee aera renee eee 247 
2. Dhe @Oripinal!Diversity of Organization ......0 1.33 ee eee eee eee nee eee 249 
3. Properties of the Whole. (Principle of Unity)................- fe A ee 251 
A. ithevMosaic Jbheory) of Development... 2) =. 4..9 2 ee eee ee eee eee 255 
5. Concerning Wormativey Stulls.t a5 fe = <2 sos « Scere eee eee ee 258 
6; Nuclear Specification = 2). Sis eis), ys. wis dae 5 Se cat ot Ae Oe I eee eee 259 
7. SUuMMALy Of DISCUSSION 324%. 22 ece--e as ene )etAccerne eee OE Oe Oe 263 


I. INTRODUCTION 


This paper is based on the study of the egg of a single species of 
annelid, Chatopterus pergamentaceus Cuvier, and is a con- 
tribution to the study of the elementary phenomena of develop- 
ment. I have been able to extend my earlier observations on 
differentiation without cleavage (Lillie, ’02) and to demonstrate 
homology of regions and cell-constituents between unsegmented 
eggs that have undergone differentiation and larve normally 
formed. It has been found in the course of the later study that 
the homology is dependent upon a very exact localization of ger- 
minal areas and specific substances in the unsegmented egg, which 
play essentially the same role whether they be divided by cell- 
walls or not. The microscopic composition of these substances 
and the relations of nuclear activity to differentiation have also 
been studied, and it has been necessary to include the ovogenesis, 
maturation, fertilization and cell-lineage in order to control the 
other data. 

In the present paper the attempt will be made to present the 
substance of the entire study without too much detail, leaving the 
elaboration of certain parts for a later contribution. However, I 
shall make no apology for entering into details, because there is 
no other explanation of heredity than a complete account of devel- 
opment, and one cannot describe even a small part of so complex 
a thing without many words, unless one knows in advance what 
is essential and what is not. 

‘The numerous cytological studies of germ-cells of the last few 
years have focused attention especially on the chromosome- 
complex, and the remarkably uniform results have established a 
series of propositions on so firm a basis that they seem destined 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Deve lopme nt 155 


to form the working hypothesis of investigations in heredity and 
development for a eorsiclesalals period of time. These are: (1) 
The constancy in number of the chromosomes in any species. 
(2) The persistence of a descendent of each chromosome in every 
cell throughout the series of cell-generations. (3) The composi- 
tion of the chromosome-complex of the first cleavage-spindle of 
corresponding maternal and paternal groups, equal in number and 
in all other respects, save individual variation. (4) The biparental 
character of the embryonic nuclei, similar to that of the first 
cleavage nucleus. (5) That the individual chromosomes of both 
the maternal and paternal chromosome-complexes are probably 
qualitatively different. (6) That pseudo-reduction (synapsis) 
consists in union of corresponding maternal and paternal chromo- 
somes. (7) That reduction in the number of chromosomes 1s 
effected by separation of the units of such bivalent chromosomes. 

Though all of these propositions are not accepted by all cytolo- 
gists, yet in the main they have become the current opinions. 
Enough, certainly, has been definitely established to put the hypo- 
thesis, that the chromosomes constitute the bearers of the heredi- 
tary qualities, on a definite basis. Tbe embryologist, therefore, 
has his problem defined, and it 1s no less an undertaking than to 
derive the entire body of the individual jrom the chromosome- 
complex as its germ. This 1s the embryological program. It has 
been recognized as such inthe more important recent theories of 
development; but hypothesis has far outstripped observation, and 
it is remarkable how very few observations have been recorded 
establishing definite morphogenic relations between nucleus and 
cytoplasm. Yet, if the hypothesis be true, such relations must be 
of the very essence of embryonic development. 

The observations recorded in this paper have led me to some 
very definite opinions as to the relations of nucleus and cytoplasm 
in embryonic development. | am well aware that they cover only 
a very small part of this embryological program; but I venture to 
express the hope in putting them forward that others may be led 
to test and to extend them. 

Our advance along these lines is very dependent on methods. 
The results of this pay could not have been attained without the 


156 Frank R. Lillie 


methods employed. It is probable that centrifuging and staining 
intra vitam will prove to be very generally applicable, and I 
believe they will yield valuable results. “The method of analyzing 


the developmental processes by suppressing the cleavage without ~ 


prejudice to other morphogenic activities, has been the most 
valuable one on the whole. I believe that it is capable of quite a 
wide application even in cases where the differentiation does not 
proceed to the formation of cilia. Bastian (04) seems to have 
witnessed such a process in rotifers, though, by an incomparable 
blunder, he interpreted the unsegmented ciliated ova as a definite 
genus of ciliate infusorian. It applies in other annelids than 
Cheetopterus as Scott’s (06) and Treadwell’s (02) papers show, 
and I think there is every reason to believe that it will prove 
available in animals of different phyla. 


II. MICROSCOPIC COMPOSITION OF THE PROTOPLASM OF THE EGG 


Tn be Living Protoplasm 


The egg of Chetopterus is a spheroidal mass of a semifluid, 
transparent and homogeneous substance with a large number of 
granules of various sizes and optical properties suspended in it; 
more fluid droplets occur here and there. ‘The living protoplasm 
shows no evidence of a filar, reticular or alveolar structure, except 
that granules or droplets may be so closely set in certain places 
as to produce the appearance of an emulsion. ‘This, however, is 
a secondary character. The primary optical properties of the 
protoplasm are those of a transparent colloidal solution, the 
particles of which are ultramicroscopic; this may assume the 
most various configurations according to the number, size and 
arrangement of the suspended, microscopically visible, granules. 

The ground substance is a suitable name for the fluid that con- 
tains and suspends all the granules and droplets; if all these were 
imagined removed it would preserve a faithful semblance of the 
egg. ‘Thus it is regarded as forming the external pellicle and as 
continuous through the nuclear membrane with the nucleo- 
plasm. Its optical properties are the same everywhere like a drop 
of water, but, unlike such a drop, it may exhibit different physical 
and chemical properties in different parts. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 157 


The Granules.—The varying microscopic ieee of regions 
of the egg is thus due entirely to the granules and droplets sus- 
pended in the ground substance. Droplets of a homogeneous 
transparent liquid, more fluid than the ground substance, are 
especially characteristic of the germinal vesicle and of the pro- 
toplasm in the neighborhood of the vegetative pole (Fig. 1). I 
have not, however, been able to follow TheiE behavior very fully. 
The granules on the other hand are more solid bodies, though i it 1S 
pe bable that some are semifluid; certainly all have aang cis al: 
density. 

By subjecting the living egg to considerable pressure one can 
examine it with the highest powers; my observations were made 
with a Zeiss 2 mm. homogeneous oil-immersion objective, apert. 
130, and compensating oculars 6, 8 and 12. Under such magni- 
fications one sees that the protoplasm of any part consists of the 
ground substance with granules of two kinds, viz: slightly or non- 
refringent granules of ete ely large size, which I shall designate 
spherules, and very minute (less * hae I” in diameter) bode 
refringent granules of approximately uniform size, the microsomes. 
The slveatles vary in size from about 2.25 to about the size of a 
microsome. 

In the living protoplasm the microsomes and the smaller 
spherules are in a constant state of tremulous agitation vibrating 
in rapid rhythm with considerable amplitude. ‘The range of these 
movements is considerably increased if the egg is agai crushed 
so that the protoplasm is broken into small fragments. No one 
who has studied these movements, as I have done for hours at a 
time, could believe that the microsomes are nodal points of a net- 
work, or are connected by filaments as they appear to be in the best 
stained sections. One is forced to conclude that they have free- 
dom of movement in all directions, 7. ¢., that they are suspended 
in a fluid medium which has no filar, reticular or alveolar structure. 

The description so far applies to the substance of the germinal 
vesicle as aptly as to the cytoplasm; though the spherules of the 
germinal vesicle are probably small. droplets in the sense used 
above; they certainly differ in many respects from the spherules of 
the cytoplasm. But the substance of the germinal vesicle con- 


158 Frank R. Lillie 


tains as large a proportion of microsomes as does the cytoplasm, 
and these cannot be distinguished apart in the living condition, 
though they can be differentiated by fixation and staining, as 
described below. ‘The germinal vesicle is of course characterized 
also by the nucleolus described in Part III; the chromosomes were 
not distinguished in the living germinal vesicle. ‘There are also 
large spaces filled with fluid (Fig. 1). 

Microsomes may occur separately or in conjunction with 
spherules; in the latter case a certain number appear to be in- 
separably united to the spherules; no matter how much the pro- 
toplasm is broken up, the union cannot be dissolved. I am not 
sure that this is characteristic of all the various kinds of spherules, 
but it certainly is of those of the ectoplasm. There are different 
kinds of spherules characteristic of different regions of the egg and 
related to different kinds of differentiation. ‘Their differences, 
arrangement and properties are considered later. 

The relation between microsome and spherule is more or less 
problematical. From the fact that spherules may grade down in 
size to approximately the dimensions of microsomes, one is tempted 
to assume that the spherules are produced by growth of micro- 
somes, or by their agglutination and fusion, and the same con- 
clusion follows from other facts considered beyond. ‘The micro- 
somes would thus be the primitive source of these larger granules; 
the spherules a farther step in the processes of differentiation. 

According to this view the microsomes are the primitive formed 
elements of the cytoplasm. ‘The source of the microsomes them- 
selves becomes therefore a vitally important question. There are 
three possibilities: (a) That the microsomes arise from smaller 
ultramicroscopic particles in the ground substance; (>) that they 
are an original cytoplasmic element multiplying by fission; (c) 
that their primitive source is the nucleus, in which event they are 
to be regarded as chromatin derivatives, for all the formed ele- 
ments of the nucleus are chromatin derivatives, or, at least, have 
their base in the chromosomes. 

I have observed no phenomena that indicate either of the first 
two possibilities, but have on the other hand repeatedly observed 
the transformation of nuclear microsomes into cytoplasmic micro- 


i 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development es 
dy . 59 


somes in sufficient quantity to supply all the microsomes found in 
the egg. In addition to these direct observations several im portant 
considerations unite in Support of the hypothesis, t that microsomes 
are chromatin particles. 

The direct observations concerning the structure and fate of the 
residual substance of the germinal vesicle (pp. 172-178) and con- 
cerning chromatin distribution in uninucleated unsegmented eggs 
that undergo differentiation (pp. 230-232) conclusively prove the 
derivation of a large proportion of the microsomes from the nucleus. 
Now each daughter nucleus is built up from a particular chromo- 
some-complex, and it is a commonplace cytological generalization 
that chromosomes arise by the union of chromatin particles (chromo- 
meres) of approximately the size of cytomicrosomes; this 1s readily 
observed in the egg of Chztopterus. When the nucleus comes to 
rest the chromosomes resolve themselves again into these ultimate 
formed elements of the chromatin, after passing through specific 
form changes. ‘Thus the chromosomes are composed of particles 
of about the same size as the cytomicrosomes. ‘The staining 
reaction of the chromomere and cytomicrosome is the same, in 
certain stages at least; newly formed microsomes take the basic 
stain with nearly the same intensity as chromatin particles. In 
the ovogenesis of Chztopterus the main source of the microsomes 
is the structure described by Mead (’98, pp. 193, 194) as the 
paranucleus as follows: “Up to the time when the egg has attained 
about two-thirds its full size, only a part of the protoplasm pre- 
sents the loose reticular appearance; the rest remains as large 
purple masses, which I consider to be equivalent to the Neben- 
kerne or paranuclei of various authors (Fig. 1, c, d, 7). These 
masses are not homogeneous, but resolve themselves into a cyto- 
plasmic network, of which the meshes are much compressed, and 
the strands usually parallel with the surface of the nucleus, though 
at the periphery of the masses they fray out and become continuous 
with the open network which contains the yolk.” I would add 
that the paranucleus forms a kind of cap to the germinal vesicle, 
from which it probably originates. The reticulum, in’ my 
opinion, is an artefact, and the essential thing about the para- 
nucleus is that it is composed of a dense aggregation of micro- 


160 Frank R. Lillie 


somes, that subsequently become distributed to all parts of the 
cytoplasm. 

These various considerations have convinced me that the larger 
proportion of the microsomes are derived from the nucleus. It 
seems improbable that there should be any radical difference in the 
origin of any, for the morphological characters of the cytomicro- 
somes, such as size, density, color and refringibility in life, and 
staining reactions, are remarkably constant. I will not deny that 
they may multiply outside of the nucleus; but, except in the case 
of the centrosome, I know of no evidence in favor of such multi- 
plication of bodies resembling microsomes. On the other hand 
it is certain that the chromomeres multiply within the nucleus. 
If the microsomes are extranuclear chromomeres, as here sug- 
gested, the doctrine of the individuality of chromosomes must be 
understood, as Hacker has pointed out, not in the sense that the 
entire substance of a chromosome is handed on through successive 
generations of cells, but only in the sense of persistence of part of 
the substance in each cell-generation. The ultimate consequences 
of this hypothesis of the microsomes are far-reaching, and need 
not be considered here. 


2. Comparison of Living, and of the Fixed and Stained 
Protoplasm 


In general it may be said that the sections give an extraor- 
dinarily faithful picture of the protoplasm, with one exception, 
viz: the appearance of a reticulum in the ground substance 
(Fig. 1). Practically all methods of fixing and staining employed 
show this reticulum. I have already mentioned one reason for 
supposing it to be an artefact, viz: the rapid ‘‘ Brownian” move- 
ments of the very particles (microsomes) that appear in the prepa- 
rations to be in the nodes of the reticulum. A second reason for 
this belief is the fact that the spherules may be driven through the 
cy toplasm i in any direction by centrifugal force without destroying 
its capacity for development (Part III, 1,c). A third argument is 
found in the formation of pscudopene at certain stages in the 
experiments described in Part IV, of such exceeding tenuity that 


» 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 161 


an alveolar or reticular structure of the pseudopodial protoplasm 
in any way resembling that of the sections is impossible; but such 
delicate strands of protoplasm are living and exhibit character- 
istic movements, though they grade off into invisibility. When 
one adds that no reticulum can be seen by any of the usual devices 
in the living protoplasm, and that a similar reticulum has been 
demonstrated to be formed in colloid solutions known to be 
homogeneous by the usual methods of coagulation employed in 
the preservation of protoplasm, it certainly seems much more 
reasonable to conclude that the reticulum is an artefact in the 
preparations. 

The preparations, however, in other respects greatly extend the 
observations that it is possible to make on the living protoplasm. 
Thus they enable one to distinguish different classes of micro- 
somes and spherules by wiser here reaction, to differentiate 
the chromatin, and to obtain fixed pictures of stages that pass like 
a vision in the living egg. A proper correlation of observations on 
the living protoplasm and the fixed and stained sections greatly 
enlarges the scope of each and is indispensable for thorough inter- 
pretation. 

It is not my intention to enter into a discussion of the various 
theories of the morphological composition of protoplasm; I desire 
only to meet one objection that will certainly occur to most cytolo- 
gists, viz: as Wilson (99) expresses it, ‘A continuous series of size 
gradations exists from the largest deutoplasm spheres down to the 
minutest ‘granules,’ and these bodies and the ‘alveoli,’ which 
form the middle terms of the series, arise in a sensibly homogeneous 
protoplasm.’ As I have already pointed out, the spherules in the 
protoplasm of Chetopterus grade down to the size of microsomes; 
it is also true that granules smaller than microsomes occur. Thus 
microsomes are terms in a series of sizes. This, however, does 
not prove that they may not be specific elements for the size-grada- 
tion may be interpreted in various ways. Microsomes are aggrega- 
tions of molecules, and it is probable that, if we could trace their 
origin morphologically, smaller granules would be recognized as 
their precursors. It might, therefore, be better to use some other 
term than “microsomes” for the granules in question, but any 


162 Frank R. Lillie 


more specific term would be open to other objections. “The word 
microsome is used in this paper only fora class of granules which 
are very numerous, very uniform in size, and with other specific 
characters described in the text and illustrated in the figures. 


Me BEE STRUCTURE OF SDE SEGG 


1. Observations 
a. Before Rupture of the Germinal Vesicle 


The ovaries of Chztopterus occupy the bases of the enlarged 
parapodia in the posterior sexual division of the body, and have 
the form of convoluted tubes. The wall of the tubes is made up 
for the most part by the ova which extend from the lumen. The 
pole of the ovum farthest from the lumen may be called the free 
pole and that next the lumen of the ovarian tube, the attached 
pole. he epithelial arrangement of the ova remains until they 
are full grown, so that there 1s no difficulty in distinguishing free 
and attached poles at any stage. It may be stated at once that the 
free pole becomes the animal pole of the oosperm, and the attached 
pole consequently represents the vegetative pole. Thus it is 
possible to trace the germinal areas recognizable in the odsperm 
back to an early stage of the ovocyte. 

By so doing the so-called period of growth of the ovogenesis 
assumes a new significance as a period in development, and it 
must be studied from this standpoint. ‘This problem, however, 
will be left for later treatment, though certain broad features may 
be mentioned from time to time in the present paper. 

The following account is based on a study of the living egg and 
of sections of material killed in picro-acetic acid, sublimate-acetic, 
Flemming’s fluid and Zenker’s fluid; sections from each kind of 
material were stained in the following ways: (1) Iron hematoxylin 
followed by orange G; (2) thionin and orange G; (3) neutral 
gentian (see Bensley ’00). So that at least twelve different 
combinations were used, and all gave essentially similar results in 
differentiating the structures described. Thus it will not be neces- 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 163 


sary to describe the results of particular methods in detail. Most 
of the features described were seen in the living egg and demon- 
strated also by experimentai méans. 

The cytoplasm and germinal vesicle will be described separately. 

1. Cytoplasm.—The main structural feature of the cytoplasm 
is its differentiation into two layers, an outer or ectoplasmic and an 
inner or endoplasmic. 

The ectoplasm (Fig. 1) covers the free hemisphere and ends a 


short distance below the equator so that the endoplasm comes to 


Fig. 1. Axial section of a full-grown primary ovocyte, which still formed part of the ovarian 
epithelium. Note that the ectoplasm is confined to the upper two-thirds of the egg. 

A, Animal pole or free pole of ovarian ovocyte; c, chromatin; EF, ectoplasm; e.a., endoplasm a; 
e.b., endoplasm b; e.c., endoplasm c; ”, nucleolus; r.s., residual substance of the germinal vesicle; 


s.n., sperm nucleus; /’, vegetative pole or attached pole of ovarian ovocyte. 


Note.—All figures of sections in this paper were drawn with the camera at a magnification of 1900 
diameters (Zeiss comp. oc. 12 and obj. 2 mm. oil immersion). They have been reduced to about 0.6 
of original size. Most of the drawings of living eggs were made with the camera at a magnification 


of about 400 diameters; these were subsequently redrawn, somewhat enlarged, in ink. 


164 Frank R. Lillie 


the surface at the vegetative pole. There is sometimes a defect 
in the ectoplasm in the center of the free or animal pole. This is 
particularly obvious after staining in thionin, for the spherules 
remain unstained and the microsomes stain intensely blue; thus 
is produced the effect of a blue plug in the center of the free hemi- 
sphere. The ectoplasm is covered externally by a very delicate 
membrane, and the vegetative pele where the endoplasm comes to 
the surface is naked. ‘The constant characteristic of the ecto- 
plasm that enables one to follow it throughout the development is 
the presence of a large number of spherules of very uniform size, 
closely set together so as to produce the effect of a pavement when 
one focuses on the surface of the entire egg. ‘These are as large 
as the average spherules of the endoplasm, but they differ from 
them in many respects: (1) In the living egg they are colorless, 
whereas the endoplasmic spherules are yellow in mass; (2) their 
position and arrangement are characteristic in each stage; (3) they 
always stain differently from the endoplasmic spherules; thus the 
osmic acid of Flemming’s fluid leaves them unstained, but stains 
the endoplasmic spherules from gray to solid black; a section of 
such an egg mounted in balsam appears to have a layer of vacuoles 
in the ectoplasm, which close examination shows to be the ecto- 
plasmic spherules. After iron hematoxylin and orange G follow- 
ing either picro-acetic or sublimate-acetic fixation, the ectoplasmic 
spherules are intensely orange, while the larger endoplasmic 
spherules are black; this result 1s easily obtained by proper ex- 
traction of the iron hematoxylin, which at first stains all spherules 
black, but which washes out of the ectoplasmic spherules long 
before it does from the larger endoplasmic ones; if one stops the 
extraction at this point, and stains in orange G a very clear differ- 
entiation of the ectoplasm is obtained. Other methods and stains 
differentiate the ectoplasm in different ways, but as | am unable 
at present to reason from the microchemical reaction to the chem- 
ical nature of the spherules involved, it seems superfluous to 
describe the details. 

There are from one to three layers of these spherules in the 
ectoplasm over the upper hemisphere (Fig. 1). These are em- 
bedded in the ground substance which likewise contains micro- 


i 


+ 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 165 


somes external to the spherules as well as between and on them. 
The layer of the ground substance external to the ectoplasmic 
spherules may be called the surface pellicle; during the maturation 
and cleavage this pellicle forms characteristic waves described 
later on. 

The endoplasm is not so uniform in its composition as the ecto- 
plasm. There is, in the first place, a striking difference between 
the upper and lower hemispheres (Fig. 1). In the upper hemi- 
sphere the protoplasm 1s dense, and contains an aggregation of the 
largest spherules, which stain black in the osmic acid or iron 
hematoxylin (endoplasm a); these are massed largely toward the 
periphery leaving a zone of protoplasm (endoplasm 6); surrounding 
the upper half of the germinal vesicle, relatively free from the 
spherules. Thus thionin (which is an intense microsomal stain) 
shows a deep blue crescentic area surrounding the upper half of 
the germinal vesicle, and sending out processes among the spher- 
ules to the periphery. At the free pole these processes are strong- 
est and may cause an interruption in the ectoplasm, where the 
endoplasm comes to the surface. ‘There is a beautiful reticulum 
in the cytoplasm where it is not concealed by spherules, and the 
microsomes lie in the nodes; I have already given reasons for 
believing that the reticulum is an artefact. In the lower hemi- 
sphere the protoplasm is vacuolated, and contains smaller spherules 
that stain differently from the larger endoplasmic spherules* 
(endoplasm c). 

Thus the endoplasm comes to the surface at two places, (1) 
a small area in the center of the animal pole, where the polar 
globules are later formed;? (2) over a large portion of the vegetative 
hemisphere where the spermatozo6n usually enters. 

2. The Germinal Vesicle—TYhe diameter of the germinal 


1 The endoplasmic spherules have collectively a yellow tinge that is very bright in the eggs of some 
individuals and relatively pale in others. The spherule-bearing endoplasm is, therefore, often named 
in the following pages the yellow endoplasm, or yellow substance. 

2T have not always been able to find this ectoplasmic defect in the primary ovocyte prior to the 
rupture of the germinal vesicle, and at the best it is relatively slight. But I believe it to be significant, 
for it marks the position of a constant exposure of the endoplasm that arises at the time of formation 
of the first maturation spindle, and persists throughout the development. The apical flagella arise 


from this exposed endoplasm. 


166 Frank R. Lillie 


vesicle is approximately half that of the entire egg (Fig. 1), and | 
thus comprises about one-eighth of the substance of the 
latter. Only a small portion of it goes to form the chromo- 
somes of the maturation spindles. ‘There is also a large nucleolus. 
The remainder is a special substance which I shall call the residual 
substance of the germinal vesicle (Fig. 1, r.s.); it plays an impor- 
tant role in the developmental processes and can be assembled, 
even hours after the germinal vesicle has broken down, by the 
action of centrifugal force; thus it is certain that it does not 
“intermingle” with the rest of the cytoplasm. It is important, 
therefore, to describe carefully the structure of the vesicle. 

In the living condition one recognizes easily the membrane and 
the large nucleolus; the peripheral part of the vesicle contains 
more vacuoles than the central mass and thus appears less dense 
(see Figs. 6-16). Apart from the droplets occupying the vacuoles, 
the substance consists of an enormous number of microsomes and 
small spherules embedded in a homogeneous ground substance. 
The chromatin is not separately visible in the living germinal 
vesicle. 

In sections stained with iron hematoxylin and orange G the 
germinal vesicle (Fig. 1) is seen to be occupied by a meshwork of a 
few thick strands, bounding vacuoles. The strands are penetrated 
through and through with microsomes that stain orange instead 
of haematoxylin like the cytomicrosomes. But when the germinal 
vesicle breaks down, these microsomes change their staining 
reaction to that of cytomicrosomes. With thionin and orange the 
cytomicrosomes stain an intense blue and the microsomes of the 
germinal vesicle, orange, until the germinal vesicle breaks down, 
when they, too, take the blue thionin stain. Thus the staining 
reaction sharply differentiates the microsomes of the cytoplasm 
and germinal vesicle. The membrane of the germinal vesicle 
consists of a thin layer of the ground substance of the nucleus in 
which the microsomes take the acid stain, and a thin layer of the 
ground substance of the cytoplasm in which the microsomes take 
the basic stain. 

My study of the nucleolus and chromosomes is incomplete and 
I shall defer a detailed description for another occasion. The 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 167 


close association of certain chromosomes with the nucleolus is a 
very striking and constant character (see Figs. 1 and 4). 

It will be seen, then, that the full grown primary ovocyte is 
highly differentiated: (1) Its polarity is expressed by, (a) the con- 
stant relation of free and attached poles; (>) the extension of the 
ectoplasm over the upper hemisphere mainly; (c) the exposure of 
the endoplasm at a small spot in the free pole and over a large 
area at the attached pole; (d) the contrast between the structure 
of the endoplasm in the free and attached hemispheres. (2) The 
existence of a sharply differentiated ectoplasm, of three kinds of 
endoplasm, and of the various substances of the germinal vesicle, 
gives a degree of orginal diversity that is certainly surprising. 

Our problem is to trace these substances in development, to 
note the appearance of other substances, and from the behavior 
of all in the normal and experimentally modified development, to 
attempt to discover their respective roles in the formation of an 
embryo. We should not attempt to prejudge the matter in ad- 
vance by naming certain substances yolk or food-matters and 
others formative substances. If these are true categories, it 
should be possible to demonstrate it. 

The following account will show that these substances are moved 
by internal forces to definite locations in the embryo and become 
parts of definite systems of organs. It will also be shown that the 
localization, and part of the differentiation, is independent of the 
process of cell division, though closely dependent upon interaction 
with nuclear derivatives. 


b. The Period of Maturation and Fertilization 


I. Polarization..—As long as the germinal vesicle is intact 
the arrangement of the various suleeees in the egg does not 
conform in all respects to the position of the Ciera “alba onic 
areas. [herupture of the germinal vesicle initiates a series of move- 
ments of the substances by means of which they attain to their 


1The substances of the egg are polarized before this process, but have a radically different 
arrangement; a more exact term for the process would, therefore, be re-polarization, which is 


objectionable in other respects. 


168 Frank R. Lillie 


definitive positions during the prophases of the first maturation 
division. ‘This rearrangement of the substances may be called 
the process of polarization, inasmuch as it takes place with refer- 
ence to the polar axis of the egg. It produces a topographical 
arrangement of the substances that corresponds in many essential 
respects to the future embryonic areas. ‘Thus the history of the 
maturation process is an important chapter in the development. 
Fertilization, which occurs during this period, willbe considered only 
incidentally, as I have nothing substantially new to add to Mead’s 
fine account. 

If the eggs of Chetopterus be taken and allowed to stand in sea- 
water, the germinal vesicle breaks down, whether the eggs be 
fertilized or not; the first maturation spindle forms and moves to 
the predelineated animal pole; at the same time the various sub- 
stances become polarized. “Thus polarization is independent of 
fertilization. Unless the eggs be fertilized or stimulated in some 
other definite fashion, they remain in the metaphase of the first 
maturation division indefinitely (Mead ’g8), (Lillie ’o2). If the 
eggs be so fertilized or stimulated, the polar bodies are formed and 
the processes of development follow continuously. 

The movements of the three classes of substances, ectoplasm, 
endoplasm, and the substances of the germinal vesicle will be 
described separately; they have reached practically their definitive 
positions by the time that the first maturation spindle becomes 
fixed at the periphery. 

Ectoplasm.—Before the breaking of the germinal vesicle the 
ectoplasm covers the upper two-thirds of the egg; as soon as the 
germinal vesicle breaks down, it flows toward the vegetative pole, 
and, even before the first maturation spindle has become fixed 
at the animal pole, it has completely overflowed it (Figs. 2 and 4), 
thus covering up the exposed endoplasm. The membrane accom- 
panies the ectoplasm and so covers the entire egg. This move- 
ment of the ectoplasm is clearly visible in the living egg as a series 
of waves of the transparent external pellicle; but, owing to the 
difhculty of orienting the living egg in this stage, I have not been 
able to follow the course of the waves satisfactorily. “The sec- 
tions, however, that enable one to trace the overflow of the vege- 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 169 


tative pole, leave no doubt that they are associated with this 
process. 

At the end of the polarization period the ectoplasm around the 
center of the vegetative hemisphere is as thick as anywhere else. 


eo 
9,9, 6 
"@% 


Cx 


ss 


eat 
isi) 
S59 


Ss 


Fig. 2. Axial section through a primary ovocyte, killed about ten minutes after coming into 
sea-water, and three minutes after fertilization. The ectoplasm has already flowed to the 
vegetative pole, leaving an exposed area of endoplasm at the animal pole. Part of the a endoplasm 
has likewise flowed to the vegetative pole. The germinal vesicle has broken down and the maturation 
spindle isin process of formation between the two primary asters. The residual substance of the ger- 
minal vesicle is clearly seen. The chromosomes do not fall in the plane of the section. Letters same 


as in Fig. 1. 


The original opening in the ectoplasm at the animal pole has 
become enlarged, and the outer end of the maturation spindle is 
fixed here (Figs. 2-5). 

Endoplasm.—Yhe endoplasm consists of three distinct parts 
as already described, (a) the uppermost part laden with large 


ar 


170 Frank R. Lillie 


spherules; (b) the non-spherular part overlying the germinal 
vesicle; (c) the vacuolated part containing small spherules situated 
below the germinal vesicle. In the process of polarization the 
endoplasmic substances, a and }, reverse their relations both 
in regard to one another and also with respect to the germinal 
vesicle. Substance a flows around substance b and the ger- 
minal vesicle toward the vegetative pole where it intermingles 


Fig.3. A later stage of the same series, as in Fig. 2, killed twelve minutes after fertilization. It 
shows the entry of the first maturation spindle into the residual substance of the germinal vesicle. 


Orientation of section uncertain. Letters same as in Bice ie 


with c, so that it is difficult for a time to distinguish them apart, 
except that the two kinds of spherules may be recognized inter- 
mingled (Figs. 2, 4 and 5). Substance 6 on the other hand 
maintains pretty nearly its original position, and the substance of 
the germinal vesicle passes up through it to the animal pole so as 


Elementary Phenomena of Elementary Development 171 


to lie above it. The definitive positions of these substances 
is shown in Fig. 5. An intermediate stage is shown in Figs. 
2 and 4. 

Though endoplasmic substances a and c intermingle, they 
do not lose their identity. This is shown by their subsequent 
behavior; if, for instance, the eggs are allowed to remain unfer- 


‘ r) See R 
C) e ® 4. 2 
at efe prlcc® 
Pet sarcocees! 
ed 90? = 


Fig. 4. Axial section of a primary ovocyte of the same series, as in Fig. 2, killed twelve 
minutes after fertilization. The maturation spindle 1s completely surrounded by the residual 
substance of the germinal vesicle, and the latter is directed toward the animal pole where the ecto- 
plasm is wanting. The nucleolus has become smaller and some of the chromosomes appear to 


have arisen from it (compare Fig. 1). Letters same as in Fig. 1. s, spermatozoa. 


tilized they will be found to have completely separated again in 
the course of about two hours (see Fig. 39, p. 208, representing 
a section of an egg that has stood two hours and seven minutes 
in sea-water). [hese substances also separate out in normally 
segmenting eggs and in unsegmented eggs that are under- 
going differentiation. 


172 Frank R. Lillie 


The spermatozo6n normally enters the egg through the exposed 
endoplasm of the vegetative pole, which usually appears slightly 
collapsed at this time (Fig. 4) and less vacuolated, as though 
there had been loss of fluid. “That the spermatozoon may enter 


°° 
Cy 
° 


Fig. 5. Axial section of a secondary ovocyte thirty-two minutes after fertilization. E. Ecto- 
plasm; e.a., endoplasm a; e.b., endoplasm b; e.c., endoplasm c; r.s., residual substance of the 
germinal vesicle; s.a., sperm aster. 


through the ectoplasm is shown by the fact that the eggs fertilize 
as readily after polarization as before. 

Germinal Vesicle—In the case of the germinal vesicle we 
have changes not only of position, but also in the character of 
the substances concerned: 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 173 


Fic. 10 


Figs. 6 to 10. See text for detailed description. Behavior of substances of the germinal vesicle 
as seen in a single living egg. Polar view. Fig. 6.11 A.M. Rupture of the germinal vesicle. Out- 
flow of cortical substance of the germinal vesicle. Fig. 7. 11.04 A. M. Fig. 8. 11.07 A. M. Fig. 9. 
11.15 A.M. Fig. 10. 11.20. M. a, Aster or polar view of spindle; E, ectoplasm; e, endoplasm; r.s., 


residual substance of the germinal vesicle. 


174 Frank R. Lillie 


As already described, one distinguishes readily in the living 
germinal vesicle a clearer cortical zone and a central more 
granular mass (Fig. 6). Soon after the eggs are in the sea-water 
the membrane weakens at a number of points at once and 
streams of the clear cortical substance can be seen to radiate 
out in all directions into the surrounding protoplasm (Fig. 6); 
these then form little islands of clear substance surrounding the 
germinal vesicle, and around two of these islands radiations 
begin to appear, and a spindle is formed between the two asters 
thus arising and the latter then approach one another until they 
are quite near together. In a polar view one can then see the 
granular substance of the germinal vesicle fold around the spin- 
dle (which contains the chromosomes as sections show) and 
finally form a complete envelope around it and the inner aster. 

Figs. 6 to 10, drawn from the living egg, illustrate this process; 
the entire mass is moving to the animal pole during the process. 
At first the residual substance of the germinal vesicle lies at one 
side of the spindle (Fig. 7, 11.04 A. M.) and it is surrounded by a 
clear zone proceeding from the original cortical layer of the ger- 
minal vesicle; the residual substance of the germinal vesicle is 
horseshoe shaped, and the spindle occupies the opening. Fig. 
8 is a sketch of the same egg three minutes later (11.07); the limbs 
of the horseshoe are bending toward one another so as to enclose 
the spindle. Eight minutes later (Fig. 9, 11.15) the limbs of the 
horseshoe have almost met around the spindle. At 11.20 (Fig. ro) 
the limbs have fused completely and a slight indentation and 
corresponding projection of the endoplasm mark the place of 
union. [he polar view of the forming spindle is always the same. 

If one has a side view instead of a polar view (Figs. 11 to 16), 
some aspects of the process can be seen better; thus the fact that the 
inner end of the spindle is completely surrounded by the granular 
substance comes more clearly to view. Figs. 11-15 show 
the spindle entering the residual substance. “They are drawings 
of a single living egg taken at about two-minute intervals (com- 
pare the section shown in Fig. 3). 

‘Thus the germinal vesicle is transported practically intact to the 
animal pole; during the process a considerable quantity of fluid 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 175 


Fic. 15 ; Fic. 16 


Figs. 11 to 16. For detailed description, see text. Behavior of substances of the germinal vesicle, 


side view. Figs. 11 to 15. Five views of the same living egg drawn with the camera, showing 
the origin of the maturation spindle and behavior of the cortical layer and residual substance of the 
germinal vesicle. Fig. 11. 9.46 4. M. Fig. 12. 9.48 A.M. Fig. 13. 9.51 A. M. Fig. 14. 9-53 A: Me 
Fig. 15. 9.54a.M. Fig. 16. A later stage drawn from another living egg. It will be observed that the 
spindle arises between two separate primary asters and gradually sinks into the residual substance of 
the germinal vesicle, which finally forms a mantle completely surrounding it with the exception of the 
external end. The clear cortical layer of the germinal vesicle retains its original relation. Compare 
also Figs. 6 to 10. a, Aster; E, ectoplasm; e, endoplasm; g.v, germinal vesicle; m.s., maturation 


spindle; r.s., residual substance of the germinal vesicle. 


176 Frank R. Lillie 


has diffused throughout the cytoplasm, but this is the only loss. 
The end-condition is similar in some respects to the “secondary 
germinal vesicle”’ of Cyclops according to Hacker’s description (’02). 

The sections enable one to follow many details that are obscure 
in the living egg and to trace chemical transformations, obvious 
as changes in staining reaction in the residual substance of the 
germinal vesicle. 

In the first place it is shown by the sections that the polarization 
of the spherules begins immediately after the rupture of the ger- 
minal vesicle. In the second place the chromosomes begin to 
separate from the surface of the nucleolus as soon as the wall of 
the germinal vesicle is ruptured, and the nucleolus (in conse- 
quence!) appears shrunken and vacuolated (Fig. 4). In the third 
place the staining reaction of the microsomes of the germinal 
vesicle begins to change from acid to basic, first at the periphery 
and later toward the center. [hus with a thionin and orange 
stain one finds eggs in which the peripheral microsomes of the 
germinal vesicle stain blue, and the central ones still stain orange; 
with hematoxylin and orange the peripheral microsomes at this 
stage take the haematoxylin and the central ones the orange. ‘This 
cannot be satisfactorily shown in a figure without the use of color. 
This change in staining reaction rapidly embraces all the micro- 
somes of the germinal vesicle, and at the same time the whole 
mass shrinks considerably owing to the diffusion of the original 
droplets of the germinal vesicle. 

During this period of diffusion of the fluid substance of the 
germinal vesicle and the ensuing polarization of the ectoplasm 
and endoplasm, the protoplasm as a whole possesses a much 
higher degree of fluidity than before. The evidence for this, 
apart from the observed diffusion of the fluid of the germinal 
vesicle, is found in the change of contour of the sectioned eggs; 
before the rupture of the germinal vesicle, and again after the 
polarization is complete, the contours are perfectly regular in 
practically every egg. But during the period of polarization the 
contours are usually quite irregular, although the method of prepa- 
ration is exactly the same. Moreover, the demarcation of the 
various endoplasmic substances is no longer clear (Figs. 2 and 4). 


' 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 177 


The strongest evidence for greater fluidity at this time is found 
in the fact that the ectoplasmic spherules are much more numerous 
and smaller than they were previously or than they are subse- 
quently (compare Fig. 1 with 2 and 4). Evidently there is a 
reversible process of coagulation concerned, the spherules break- 
ing into smaller particles as the fluidity increases, and setting or 
coagulating again by a process of fusion. 

Mead (’98) has given a very careful account of the origin of the 
asters of the maturation spindle in Chztopterus. According to 
his account a large number of asters arise around the ruptured 
germinal vesicle; two of these, which he calls primary asters, 
become large, and form the asters and create the spindle of the 
maturation divisions. My observations confirm those of Mead in 
almost every particular; though I have not found numerous asters 
inthe sections. Indeed in my preparations Mead’s secondary asters 
appear simply as fluid droplets from the germinal vesicle with very 
slight indication of radiations. But as, according to my observa- 
tions on the living egg, the primary asters likewise arise around 
fluid droplets from the germinal vesicle, it seems quite possible that 
Mead’s interpretation, that there is no essential difference between 
primary and secondary asters, is correct. The primary asters 
certainly arise separately; the observations on the living egg and on 
sections are perfectly conclusive in this respect. They may at first 
be near together or very far apart, but they soon exert an influence 
on one another and a spindle arises between them (Figs. rand 12). 

Figs. 1, 2, 4 and 5 illustrate sufhciently well for our present 
purpose the history and composition of the chromosomes. ‘They 
show at the same time the microscopic structure of the residual 
substance of the germinal vesicle as it appears in sections, and 
confirm the observations of its migration to the animal pole along 
with the spindle. Thus Fig. 3 shows the spindle entering the 
residual substance; Fig. 4 a slightly later stage, and Fig. 5 the 
positions, structure and relations of the spindle and residual sub- 
stance after they have attained the pole. “The microsomes are 
distinguishable from ordinary cytomicrosomes only by their 
slightly larger size. Before the germinal vesicle breaks down the 
residual substance is represented by the coarse reticulum of the 


178 Frank R. Lillie 


germinal vesicle studded with innumerable microsomes that take 
the acid stain. Whenthe germinal vesicle breaks down the retic- 
ulum shrinks together with the partial escape of the contained 
fluid and the staining reaction of the microsomes changes to basic 
like the cytoplasmic microsomes; all the fluid, however, is not 
squeezed out and it retains fluid droplets of considerable size that 
give it a permanent reticular structure. The meshes are thick 
and the microsomes are larger than the cytomicrosomes. It can 
now be traced by virtue of these structural peculiarities. 

The nucleolus is carried along, with the chromosomes that are 
attached to it, by the residual Sb eee, and it 1s therefore found 
at one side of the equatorial plate of the first maturation spindle 
(Fig. 4), where it has been figured by Mead. It gradually dis- 
appears and is lost from view. 

The topography of the polarized ovocyte is shown in Fig. 5. It 
will be seen by comparison with Fig. 1 what a complete rearrange- 
ment of the substances of the unpolarized ovocyte has taken place; 
however, no new substances have been formed, though the sub- 
stances of the germinal vesicle have undergone considerable changes. 

2. The Later Processes of Maturation and Fertilization.—The 
formation of the polar globules and the presence of the sperm 
nucleus in the egg do not involve any important changes in topog- 

raphy of the substances, though the retreat of the egg nucleus to 

the center of the egg after the formation of the second polar body 
involves a more cone localization of the residual substance of 
the germinal vesicle. Thus the topography of the odsperm is 
essentially the same as that of the polarized ovocyte. 

The problems of the nature of the maturation divisions, behavior 
of the sperm and egg nuclei and origin of the cleavage centrosomes 
do not fall within the scope of this paper. So far as my observa- 
tions go they confirm in most respects the account of A. D. Mead 
(98). I expect to take up the behavior of the chromatin in a 
subsequent paper. It should be noted here, however, as bearing 
on the later portions of the present paper that the number of 
chromosomes in the maturation spindle is constantly nine, 
arranged usually in the form of a circle of eight with one central 
chromosome; the same number was found by Mead. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 179 


c. Action of Centrifugal Force in the Unsegmented Egg" 


If the eggs of Chztopterus he taken and allowed to stand in 
sea-water without fertilization the germinal vesicle breaks down, 
the substances become polarized as already described, and the 
first maturation spindle 1 is formed and moves to the animal pole, 
where it remains in metaphase indefinitely (unless the egg be fer- 
tilized or stimulated in some definite fashion). This coud Hone 1S 
reached in from fifteen to thirty minutes, depending on tempera- 
ture and some unknown factors. 

If, at any later time before their death, the eggs be submitted to 
a fairly strong centrifugal action (1500 to 2000 revolutions in one 
minute) and then examined, it will be found that the endoplasm is 
arranged in three distinct layers, viz: a small gray cap, a clear band 
and a yellow hemisphere (Photographs A-H, and Figs. 17-22). 
The gray cap and clear band together take up about one hemi- 
sphere. ‘The ectoplasmic layer is not visibly affected (see photo- 
graphs and Fig. 24) and forms, as before, a continuous layer with 
an aperture at the animal pole. The maturation spindle is 
usually found fixed at the periphery immediately after centri- 
fuging, so that it is obvious that it has not been moved by the 
centrifugal force. 

iliese ageregations of substances bear no definite relation to 
the polarity of the egg, for the maturation spindle may be found 
either in the gray cap, which is then ring-shaped (Photograph H 
and Fig. 19), or in the center of the yellow hemisphere, or in any 
intermediate position. It is, however, most frequently found in 
the hemisphere containing the gray cap. 

If now the eggs be ‘antiga the polar bodies are formed at the 
original animal pole, and thus may appear in the gray cap, or at the 
center of the yellow hemisphere, or in any intermediate position; 
though in this case, again, they are found more frequently in the 
hemisphere containing the gray cap than in the yellow hemisphere 
(see various conditions illustrated in Photographs A—H and in 
Figs. 17-22). 

1 The use of the centrifuge for the purpose of studying the composition of the protoplasm was sug- 


gested to me by Dr. E. P. Lyon, who had already obtained results, about to be published, on the 
ova of sea-urchins by this method. 


180 Frank R. Lillie 


Figs. 17 to 23. Showing the effects of centrifuging upon the living unsegmented egg. Compare 
Photographs A-I. E, Ectoplasm; e, yellow endoplasm; c.b., clear band; g.c., gray cap; g.v. 
germinal vesicle; , nucleolus; p./. polar lobe. 

Fig. 17. Eggs in sea-water at 8.55 a.m. Centrifuged at 10.45 and fertilized. Drawn with camera 
at 12 o’clock noon. The gray cap lies in the vegetative hemisphere. 

Fig 18. Eggs in sea-water at 1.55 p. M. Centrifuged 2.30 p. Mm. Fertilized 2.45 ep. mM. Drawn 
3-40 P.M. The gray cap extends into the polar lobe. Beginning of the first cleavage. 

Fig. 19. Eggs in sea-water 8.50 a.m. Centrifuged 9.40 a.m. Fertilized 9.45 a. mM. Drawn 9.55 
A.M. The gray cap surrounds the maturation spindle and is thus ring-shaped. 

Fig. 20. Eggs in sea-water 1.55 p. M. Fertilized 2.03 p. mM. Centrifuged 2.50 p. M. Drawn 3.00 
p.M. ‘The gray cap has passed entirely into the larger cell CD. 

Fig. 21. Eggs in sea-water 8.50 a. m. Centrifuged 9.40 a. M. Fertilized 9.45 a. mM. Drawn 
10.20 A. M. The gray cap lies opposite to the animal pole. 

Fig. 22. Drawn 10.29 a.m. History otherwise the same as No. 21. Shows intermediate position 
of gray cap. 

Fig. 23. In sea-water 9.08 a.m. Centrifuged at same time before rupture of the germinal vesicle. 
No stratification. Note the position of the nucleolus within the germinal vesicle. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development ISI 


Fic. 18 


Fie. 21 Fig. 23 Fic. 22 


} 


182 Frank R. Lillie 


The polar lobe appears at the usual time preceding the first 
cleavage, and always lies opposite to the polar globules, whatever 
be the position of the yellow substance (Photograph G and Figs. 
17, 18 and 20). ‘Thus, if the gray cap lies at the animal pole, the 
yellow substance extends into the base of the polar lobe, as nar- 
mally; or, inthe opposite case, the polar bodies lying atthe center 
of the yellow hemisphere (Fig. 21), the gray cap may extend into 
the base of the polar lobe (Fig. 18). The structure of the periph- 
eral part of the polar lobe is the same in either case, and does not 
differ from the normal. 

It is therefore clear that the yellow and the gray substances are 
extremes as regards their specific gravity; they move freely through 
the ground substance of the Saauaylaicm. and are thus distri- 
buted with reference to the direction of the centrifugal force, and so 
may occupy any position with reference to the polarity of the egg. 

However, polarity is the force that governs the position of these 
substances in the normal development, causing aggregation of the 
yellow substance in the vegetative hemisphere and of the gray in 
the animal hemisphere. It is thus a stronger force than gravita- 
tion, for these substances aggregate in the same polar sense 
whatever be the position of the egg, but not so strong as the centrif- 
ugal force employed. It is now clear why the gray cap is thrown 
to the animal hemisphere more frequently than to the vegetative, 
for the force of polarity has already caused a partial aggregation 
of the substances before the centrifugal force is applied, and the 
eggs, therefore, tend to rotate with the heavier pole in a distal 
direction; but in a large percentage of the eggs, various causes 
combine to overcome this, the principal one being no doubt the 
crowding of the eggs in the bottom of the tube. 

The process of ine, vage takes place with reference to the polarity 
of the egg (Photograph I and Figs. 18 and 20) and its funda- 
mental form 1s not ajfected by the chance disposition of the endo- 
plasmic substances though minor abnormalities result from certain 
jorms of distribution. anes the first two cleavages are always 
meridional whatever the position of the yellow cbse and the 


gray cap, and the third cleavage is equatorial and spiral in the 
usual sense. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 183 


The appearance of stratification given by centrifugal action is 
extremely striking, and it is so similiar to the banded condition 
described for echinoderms by Boveri (o1a and ’o1b) and for Den- 
talium by Wilson (05), that one cannot fail to be impressed by 
the resemblance. Lyon has found that similar stratification may 
be produced in the eggs of echinoderms, annelids and tunicates by 
centrifugal action. So there can be but little doubt that it isafunda- 
mental feature of egg-organization in the ova of bilateral animals. 

It is a condition normally governed by the polarity of the egg, 
and does not become obvious under normal conditions in the egg 
of Chztopterus simply because the separation is not complete and 
precise, and thus the strata shade into one another, the animal 
pole being relatively light and grayish in hue and the vegetative 
relatively dense and yellow. 

By the aid of the centrifuge one can also demonstrate that the 
substance of the gray cap is the residual substance of the germinal 
vesicle. Ifthe eggs are centrifuged as usual immediately after they 
come into sea-water, the germinal vesicle being still intact, one 
does not get a gray cap or any banded appearance, but instead the 
germinal vesicle is thrown to the surface with such force that it may 
produce a protuberance of the cortical layer (Fig. 23), and the re- 
mainder of the endoplasm appears yellow and not strongly polar- 
ized by the centrifugal force; the cortex remains as before. The - 
nucleolus always occupies the inner end of the germinal vesicle, 
showing that it is undoubtedly heavier than the remainder of the con- 
tents of the vesicle. Within the germinal vesicle there is no strati- 
fication, the granular substance preserving its earlier distributed 
arrangement. ‘These eggs then mature on standing, and the ma- 
turation spindle moves to the periphery of the egg; in doing so.it may 
take the longest course through the egg and become fixed at the 
pole opposite to that occupied by the germinal vesicle, or it may 
move to any other position. Thus it becomes practically certain 
that the polar force governs the migration and leads it to the 
animal pole. ‘The only other assumption would be that the spindle 
determined polarity at whatever point on the surface it happened 
to be fixed, and then it would be perfectly incomprehensible why 
it sometimes migrates through the entire diameter of the egg. 


184 Frank R. Lillie 


If the eggs are allowed to stand eight to fifteen minutes in 
sea-water after being taken from the female, so that in some the 
germinal vesicle is still intact, and in others broken down, the 
latter always show the stratification more or less pronounced after 
centrifuging, and the former never show it, but instead the ger- 
minal vesicle is always peripheral and contains a granular mass 
similar to the substance of the gray cap. 

Thus neither the gray cap nor the clear band are formed by 
centrifugal force unless the germinal vesicle is broken down, and 
the conclusion is inevitable that the entire gray cap is derived from 
the germinal vesicle. The yellow substance does not aggregate 
so closely before as after the germinal vesicle has broken down. 
Hence it is probable that the appearance of the clear band is due 
very largely to concentration of the yellow spherules at one pole. 
It would seem that the endoplasm has become less viscid as a 
result of the diffusion of substance from the germinal vesicle, so 
as to permit closer aggregation of the yellow granules. The 
structure of the gray cap completes the identification for it is the 
same as the residual substance of the germinal vesicle (Fig. 
24; compare figures of normal maturation). 

The maturation spindle is usually found fixed at the periphery 
immediately after centrifuging, whatever the direction of the 
centrifugal force may have been; when, however, it is torn loose, 
as sometimes happens, it is found in the clear band. ‘Thus it is 
intermediate between the gray cap and yellow endoplasm in 
specific gravity. It is clear, therefore, that when it is situated in 
the region of the gray cap or yellow endoplasm, the centrifugal 
force must cause it to exert traction on the cortex of the ovum. 
That this is actually the case is shown by the form of the section 
of many eggs killed immediately after centrifuging, in which there 
is a very pronounced depression at the animal pole where the 
maturation spindle is attached (Fig. 24). 

These sections are very interesting in two other respects: (a) 
When the spindle is torn loose from the periphery, it moves as a 
whole and carries the chromosomes with it—chromosomes and 
spindle are never found separated by the centrifugal force. This 
bears witness to the extreme viscosity of the spindle area, for it 


Elementary Phenomena oy Embryonic Development 185 


can hardly be possible that chromosomes and spindle are of exactly 
the same specific gravity: moreover the spindle preserves its form 
intact, and is not bent or broken in any manner so far as | have 
observed, though it must be subjected to considerable strain. 
(b) If the eggs be fixed (killed) immediately after centrifuging, and 
sectioned, stained and mounted in the usual manner, it is found 
that the asters are almost entirely or entirely wanting, though 
sections of the same eggs prepared without centrifuging show 
well-developed asters at both poles of the spindle. The cyto- 
plasm surrounding the spindle is dense and filled with microsomes 
more numerous and smaller than usual. 


A 


O00 GS 
AA Zp SO 


Fig. 24 Section of an egg killed in picro-sulphuric acid immediately after centrifuging. The clear 
band c.b. is seen to be densely microsomal. The ectoplasm retains its original arrangement. The 
yellow endoplasm and the residual substance of the germinal vesicle are at opposite sides of the 
egg. The maturation spindle retains its original position. A, Animal pole; c.b., clear band; E, 
ectoplasm; e, endoplasm; m.s., maturation spindle; r.s., residual substance of the germinal vesicle; 
V, vegetative pole. 


The residual substance of the germinal vesicle has been stripped 
off the spindle and has given place to more finely microsomal pro- 
toplasm. ‘The sphere and centrosome, however, remain with the 
spindle (Fig. 24). If the eggs are permitted to stand after centri- 


186 Frank R. Lillie 


fuging the radiations appear; but their character is different from 
those of the normal egg, owing to the absence of the residual sub- 
stance of the germinal vesicle around the spindle. 

The most obvious interpretation of the clear band is that it 
represents the pure ground substance cleared of spherules by 
centrifugal force. This was, indeed, the interpretation that I gave 
it when working on the living material. ‘The results obtained by 
staining, in ornare lin, entire eggs killed in picro- -sulphuric or 
picro-acetic acid, shows that this interpretation is only partially 
correct; for in these eggs the original clear band appears intense 
violet in color, and was not, for this reason, at first recognized; 
in such preparations the gray cap is stained relativ ely lightly and 
is seen to be highly vacuolated. ‘The yellow mass is unstained 
and dense. Sections of such eggs (Fig. 24) show that the clear 
band contains most of the snilplasnic microsomes (which, as 
we have seen before, have strong affinity for basic dyes), embed- 
ded in ground substance. Thus the centrifugal force has separated 
the microsomes and spherules of the endoplasm, the former then 
appearing as the clear band and the latter as the yellow mass. This 
appears to me to furnish important evidence for the specific 
nature of the microsomes as argued throughout this paper. 
The result is very clear-cut and striking. The microsomes are 
thus seen to differ from the spherules in specific gravity as well 
as in the other respects already noted. Moreover, when the 
spindle is torn loose from the periphery, it is invariably found with 
the contained chromosomes in the clear band, thus showing that 
the microsomes have the same specific gravity as the chromosomes. 
This observation should be correlated with others described in this 
paper, showing origin of microsomes from chromosomes. 

The definiteness and fixity of polarity is one of the most striking 
results brought out by centrifuging the eggs. The direction of 
polarity is not altered by any arrangement given to the endoplas- 
mic substances, and it asserts its power over the maturation 
spindle under all the various circumstances of the experiments; 
similarly it determines the location of the polar lobe under all 
conditions, and, under normal conditions, regulates the distribu- 
tion of the ectoplasm and the endoplasm. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 187 


The following statements may be made-concerning the polarity 
of the egg of Chetopterus: 

1. Polarity may be traced from the stage of the youngest 
ovocyte continuously. 

2. Inthe ovocyte the free end of the egg is the animal pole and 
the attached end the vegetative pole. 

3. It is not determined whether the odgonia are polarized or 
whether the polarity arises in the ovocyte owing to the relations 
of free and attached poles, or from some other external cause. 
My own view is that the polarity is inherent and is a property of 
the nucleus. 

4. In the full-grown ovocytes and o6tid the polarity is un- 
altered by any change of distribution of the granules of the endo- 
plasm; it must, therefore, be a property of the residual protoplasm. 

5. Polarity, however, determines the normal distribution of 
the granules in the egg, and is thus a force stronger than 
gravitation, because the distribution of the spherules is normally 
fixed with reference to polarity, but may bear any relation to the 
direction of gravitation. 

6. Acentrifugal force may overcome the polarity and distribute 
the spherules correspondingly. 

The effects of polarity in the subsequent development will be 
onsidered in Part IV. 


2. Literature and Discussion 


a. The Axis of the Ovarian Ovocyte and its Relation to Polarity 


It appears to be the rule among annelids and molluscs that the 
attached pole of the egg in the ovary becomes the vegetative pole 
of the odsperm. It is true that the number of observations bear- 
ing on this point is relatively small (Stauffacher ’93, Lillie ’g5, 
Conklin ’o2 and ’03). 

On the other hand Boveri's observations (ota and ’otb) make 
it probable that in Strongylocentrotus the conditions are reversed. 
There is no theoretical objection to such a contrast. On the con- 
trary, as Conklin has argued in his very suggestive paper on 


188 Frank R. Lillie 


“Inverse Symmetry”? (’03), reversal of polarity would furnish a 
very simple and complete explanation of the condition of inverse 
symmetry. The considerations that Conklin brings forward in 
this paper, together with the actual observations in He literature, 
combine to anila: reasonably certain the hypothesis, that in all 
animals the axis of the epithelial ovarian ova becomes the primary 
axis of the segmenting ovum. It seems reasonably certain also 
that the poles of this axis may be interchangeable, owing probably 
to inverse polarization of the formative stuffs of the ovocyte. 


b. The Ectoplasmic Layer 


I have been surprised to find, since my observations on Chzetop- 
terus were completed, what a large number of definite observa- 
tions on the existence of an ectoplasmic layer in the eggs of various 
animals were to be found in the literature. These have not been 
recently collated and their importance 1s, therefore, not fully 
realized by many embryologists. When brought together these 
observations bring into one category the “polar rings,” “yolk- 
lobe” and certain aha phenomena described by various AMOS: 
I therefore give rather full citations. 

In Clevane (Whitman ’78) the “polar rings”’ evidently corre- 
spond to the two divisions of the ectoplasm of Chztopterus 
(see Part IV, 1, a, of the present paper). These rings appear after 
the formation of both polar globules and are situated near the 
upper and lower poles, respectively. Each consists of a “trans- 
parent fluid substance,” radiating lines of which at first extend on 
the surface from each toward the equator of the egg; in my opinion 
these indicate the origin of the rings from an ectoplasmic layer, 
as in Rhynchelmis, though Whitman expresses no opinion con- 
cerning the origin of this substance. The upper ring contracts 
around the animal pole, but retains its central opening, which is 
usually eccentric, thus agreeing exactly with Cheetopterus (see 
Photograph ED ailre substance of the lower ring aggregates at 
the lower pole in the form of a disc. Subseqmantr the substances 
of both rings plunge deep into the egg, and the upper ring sub- 
stance 1s PoAneeniried to the left posterior macromere, D, as in 


39 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 189 


Rhynchelmis; what becomes of the lower ring substance is uncer- 
tain. 

The conditions in the egg of Rhynchelmis (Vejdovsky *88—’92) 
are most like those in Chetopterus. The condition before the 
rupture of the germinal vesicle 1s thus described by Vejdovsky 
(p. 32): “Unter der dusserst feinen Dottermembran, die man 
kunstlich von der Eisubstanz tberhaupt nicht abheben kann, 
erstreckt sich eine 0.003 mm. hohe Schicht des peripherischen 
Plasma, welches sich in Pikrocarmin intensiver farbt und deshalb 
ohne grosse Schwierigkeit wahrzunehmen ist (Holzschnitt Fig. 
1, A). Sie besteht aus einer hyalinen Grundsubstanz, in welches 
ausserst feine, aber doch deutliche, in Pikrocarmin intensiv sich 
tarbende Plasmakornchen, eingebettet sind. Bei starken Ver- 
grosserungen ist es nicht schwierig sicher zu stellen, dass diese 
K6rnchen regelmassig schichtenweise und concentrisch in der 
Grundsubstanz angeordnet sind, in einigen Fallen scheint es aber, 
dass sie hier unregelmassig zerstreut sind. Diese Kornchen sind 
denjenigen gleichzustellen, die man viel deutlicher an den Fasern 
des Cytoplasmanetzes wahrnimmt. Im Leben ist diese Proto- 
plasmaschicht braun.”’ According to this account it would ap- 
pear that the ectoplasmic layer covers the entire ovum before 
the rupture of the germinal vesicle. 

After the formation of the polar globules the ectoplasmic layer 
ruptures near the equator of the egg and flows to the two poles. 
Cases were observed where the aco ates took place entirely 
at the animal pole. The defect at the animal pole produced by 
the maturation spindles appears to be covered up in some cases, 
though it is always indicated for some time by a thinner spot. 
Some eggs have no ectoplasmic layer, and these do not develop 
beyond the stage of the first cleavage spindle. ‘The two ectoplas- 
mic discs are transmitted entirely to the larger cell of the two-cell 
stage. In the four-celled stage they are confined to the large 
posterior macromere, and during this stage they sink into the 
interior and surround the sphere containing the nucleus. 

The behavior of the upper accumulation of ectoplasm is thus 
strikingly different from the condition in Cheetopterus (see IV, 
1,5). But the transmission of the lower accumulation to the large 


190 Frank R. Lillie 


posterior macromere agrees with Chetopterus. Enough has been 
said to show what a remarkable similarity exists; the differences 
will no doubt be explained at some future time. 

In both Clepsine and Rhynchelmis the area of the egg between 
the upper and lower accumulations of the ectoplasm is many times 
larger than in Chetopterus.. This is correlated with the relatively 
very small size of the entoderm cells in Chetopterus. Differences 
in the functions of the ectoderm may also be a factor; thus all the 
ectodermal cells in Chzetopterus are ciliated in contrast to Clepsine 
and Rhynchelmis where cilia are lacking. In Chetopterus the 
presence of ectoplasm seems to be essential fn the formation of cilia. 

Wilson (’04, a) describes an ectoplasmic layer in the egg of 
Dentalium; he finds thatitiscontinuous with the ‘‘lower protoplas- 
mic area’ "(substance of the polar lobe)and with the upper’ ‘ proto- 
plasmic disc.” “As the egg, still unfertilized, lies in sea-water, 
the ectoplasm in the region of the upper disc slowly increases in 
amount, and in some cases this region shows a faintly radiating 
appearance around its periphery as if clear hyaloplasm were 
flowing into it from the surrounding region.” It is evident that 
Wilson’s “upper disc” corresponds to my “ectoplasmic defect” 
at the animal pole; the author calls “attention to the fact that the 
original disc is composed of very dense homogeneous protoplasm 
that differs markedly in character from the alveolar protoplasm 
of the ectoplasmic thickening that afterward extends over the 
whole upper surface of the egg.” The resemblances between the 
ectoplasmic layers in Dentalium and Chetopterus will come out 
more clearly as the description proceeds. At present we may note 
as points in common (1) the “alveolar character”? (Wilson) or 
presence of specific spherules. (2) Connection with the polar 
lobe and upper hemisphere. (3) Severance between the ecto- 
plasm of the polar lobe and upper hemisphere later. (4) Exist- 
‘ence of an ectoplasmic defect (upper protoplasmic disc of Wilson) 
at the animal pole. Vejdovsky’s observations on Rhynchelmis 
agree in all these respects, and Whitman’s on Clepsine 1 in most. 

Mead (’98) finds in the egg of Cheetopterus ° ‘immediately inside 
the outer pellicle a narrow Ore containing a single row of yolk- 
granules regularly arranged.” This applies to the ovocyte of the 


as 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 1g! 


second order, and, at that stage, I also usually find the spherules 
arranged in a single layer (Fig. 15); it will be observed that Mead 
calls them “yolk-granules,” not having recognized their true 
nature. Wheeler ('97) figures an ectoplasmic layer in the egg of 
Myzostoma; it is characterized by the presence of granules larger 
than microsomes, and like them staining very deeply in iron-alum 
hematoxylin. “Vheir number and distribution are quite variable, 
and they may even be entirely absent in some batches of eggs.”’ 
Wheeler believes that they are chromatin granules derived from 
the disintegrating nuclei of nurse-cells. It 1s interesting to notice 
in his figures that the ectoplasmic layer is absent at the outer end 
of the maturation spindles, as in Chetopterus, Dentalium, Clep- 
sine and Rhynchelmis. Conklin finds (’05) in the eggs of the 
ascidian Cynthia a “peripheral layer of deeply-staining proto- 
plasm in which the test cells were formerly embedded and which 
contains no yolk, but numerous refractive spherules much smaller 
than those of the yolk.” ‘The layer contains yellow pigment 
“which seems to be associated with these small refractive spher- 
ules.” As Conklin points out, Sobotta has observed and de- 
scribed a similar layer in the egg of Amphioxus. Conklin has 
traced its fate in Cynthia with the greatest exactness, and has 
found that it forms a yellow crescent on the posterior side of the 
egg, and that its substance enters into the composition of the 
mesoblast cells. 

The existence of so similar a layer in such widely separated 
animals and its great morphogenic importance constitute strong 
reasons for believing that it 1s likely to be found in all the principal 
phyla. As will be shown in another place the assumption of its 
existence in certain forms, in which it has not been described, 
helps to explain other phenomena actually observed; for instance, 
the presence of the polar lobe in many mollusca, and the absence 
of flagella in abnormally differentiated eggs of Amphitrite (Scott) 


(seep. 238), 
c. Residual Substance of the Germinal Vesicle 


There are many observations in the literature bearing on: the 
existence of a large quantity of residual matter derived from the 


192 Frank R. Lillte 


germinal vesicle after the formation of the first maturation spindle, 
though in most cases the observations are fragmentary and no 
definite interpretation is given. Figures by Wheeler (’99), Mead 
(98), Coe (99), Wilson ('o4a), Vejdovsky (°88—’92) and others 
show a large amount of substance of the germinal vesicle remain- 
ing after the formation of the first maturation spindle; but none of 
these authors has traced its behavior in later stages. According 
to Hacker (02) the entire substance of the germinal vesicle in 
Cyclops passes to the animal pole where it forms his “secondary 
germinal vesicle.” ‘This is like the condition in Chetopterus; 
but Hacker fixed his attention on the chromosomes and did not 
follow the residual substance farther. Without going into the details 
of the various accounts it 1s clearly shown in the literature that, as 
a rule, only a small proportion of the substance of the germinal 
vesicle enters into the formation of the first maturation spindle. 
Conklin’s observations (’05) are the most complete ones on the 
subject of the residual substance of the germinal vesicle. He 
finds, in Cynthia, that “as soon as the nuclear membrane has 
dissolved the chromosomes, nucleolus and a granular mass from 
which the spindle fibers are formed gather together into the center 
of this area of nuclear protoplasm.” As the spindle forms the 
entire area moves to the surface of the egg, and the clear proto- 
plasm spreads out “into a cap or peripheral layer (Ciona) or may 
form merely a somewhat flattened disc (Cynthia).’”’ After 
fertilization the clear substance of the germinal vesicle flows to 
the vegetative pole, “leaving the first maturation spindle sur- 
rounded by only a small amount of protoplasm.” Here it receives 
the sperm-nucleus and aster, and subsequently moves to the 
posterior side of the egg and up to the equator; “finally, after the 
meeting of the germ-nuclei near the posterior pole of the ege, these 
nuclei and the clear protoplasm surrounding them move inward 
to the center of the egg.” ‘At the close of the first cleavage the 
nuclei and clear protoplasm move into the upper hemisphere, and 
thereafter, throughout development, this hemisphere contains 
most of the clear protoplasm and gives rise to the ectoderm.’ As 
will be seen from the subsequent description, the behavior of the 
residual substance in Chatopterus shows many points of similarity. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 193 


It seems probable, therefore, that the residual substance of the 
germinal vesicle represents a specific formative stuff of essentially 
the same character 1n different phyla. 


d. Polarization 


The phenomenon that | have termed polarization of the forma- 
tive stuffs likewise appears to be a universal one. Here again 
Conklin’s observations on Cynthia are the most complete; he has 
described definite flowing movements of the ectoplasm, residual 
substance of the germinal vesicle and of the endoplasm, beginning 
with the rupture of the germinal vesicle, by virtue of which the 
topography of the ovocyte is radically changed. ‘There is first a 
movement of the ectoplasm and the residual substance of the 
germinal vesicle to the lower pole of the egg, which corresponds 
to the polarization described in this section. This is followed by 
a bilateral arrangement which corresponds to what I describe in 
Part IV, 1, a, as bilateral polarization. The rearrangement 
of substances in the eggs of Ctenophores described by Fischel 
((03), and in Echinids described by Boveri (ora and ’orb) 
appears to correspond to the bilateral polarization in Chetopterus 
(Part IV, 1, a), for it does not immediately follow the rupture of 
the germinal vesicle but is a result of fertilization. Some of the 
observations of Whitman, Vejdovsky and Wilson, already con- 
sidered, belong here. Many authors have described or figured a 
polar segregation of the yolk in the lower hemisphere following 
the rupture of the germinal vesicle which is undoubtedly only a 
part of the polarization processes taking place at this time. 

Wilson, Yatsu and Zeleny have found that there is a progressive 
limitation of potencies of parts of the egg of nemertines beginning 
with the rupture of the germinal vesicle. Assuming that the vari- 
ous formative stuffs have limited potencies this would be the 
natural consequence of such a process of polarization as I have 
described for Chatopterus, because the new topography is more 
precise than the original one (compare also Conklin ’05). 


194 Frank R. Lillie 


IV. CLEAVAGE AND DIFFERENTIATION | 

The problem now becomes to determine the distribution of the 
various substances in the early development, to follow the forma- 
tion of others and to investigate the relations of all to the various 
forms of differentiation. “[o determine the normal distribution 
of these substances I have studied the cell-lineage up to the time 
of the formation of the mesoblast cell, and with the aid of sections 
have determined the distribution of the various substances to the 
different cells. “The use of intra-vitam staining has likewise con- 
tributed to the study of the distribution of substances and to 
the identification of new ones. For the purpose of ascer- 
taining the relations of the substances to the various forms of 
differentiation, | have made a renewed study of the differentiation 
of unsegmented eggs, which I described in an earlier paper 
(Lillie ’02). By the action of centrifugal force an abnormal 
distribution of the endoplasmic substances has been produced, 
and the effect of this on the form of the cleavage has also been 
studied to a certain extent. 

A statement of the more general conclusions may make the point 
of view of the entire description clear. In the first place the 
observations on cell-lineage show that the process of cleavage 
does not produce an essentially different distribution of substances, 
and that the parts of the trochophore are clearly mapped out in the 
segmented ovum of sixty-four cells. The topography of the 
unsegmented egg 1s, therefore, essentially similar to that of the 
trochophore. In the second place it will be shown that some of 
the substances (spherules) have in all probability a specific mor- 
phogenic value, while others appear to have simply a nutritive 

value, though a clear separation of the substances (spherules) into 
these two categories has not been possible. The morphogenic 
significance of ‘the substances is shown especially well by the 
observations on the differentiation of unsegmented eggs, in which 
clear regional homologies with the trochophore are found includ- 
ing ectodermal, entodermal and mesodermal substances. In the 
third place it will be shown that the activation of these morpho- 
genic substances is accompanied by and probably dependent upon 
interaction with nuclear derivatives. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 195 
t. Formative Stuffs in the Early Normal Development 


Mead has given.a good account of the cell-lineage up to sixty- 
four cells, and I have been able to confirm his results in prac- 
tically all particulars. His description, however, was written 
from a different point of view, and, though complete and accurate 
as to the form of the cleavage, does not deal at all with the dis- 
tribution of substances, which indeed he did not distinguish. It 
is necessary, therefore, to describe some parts in considerable 
detail, while for others I can rely on Mead’s excellent account. 


a. The First Cleavage 


The period of the first cleavage is meant to include the entire 
time from the, dissolution of the membranes of the germ-nuclei 
to the two-celled stage. It 1s characterized by the appearance of 
the polar lobe involving division of the ectoplasm into two parts, 
by the appearance of bilateral symmetry, and by formation of new 
substances. 

1. Polar Lobe and Division of Ectoplasm.—At about the meta- 
phase of the first cleavage the ovum begins to elongate in a polar 
direction and a slight constriction appears in an equatorial plane 
considerably below the equator (Photograph Gand Fig. 26, p. 205); 
thus the ovum becomes decidedly pear-shaped, the stem of the 
pear being at the vegetative pole. In the region of the constriction 
the ectoplasm is divided so that there is a gap between the part 
situated below and that above it (Figs. 25 and 26); this gap is 
permanent, and a vegetative polar group of the ectoplasmic 
spherules is thus finally separated from the remainder. ‘The con- 
striction separating this group deepens as the first cleavage 
furrow begins to appear at the animal pole (Fig. 25); this furrow 
lies to one side of the polar globules and on the same side of the 
ege it passes into the constriction, so that the division becomes 
decidedly unequal and the polar group of ectoplasmic spherules 
(Fig. 25, p./.), passes entirely into the larger cell (CD). ‘The polar 
furrow deepens during the later phases of the first cleavage and 
thus produces a pedunculated lobe, the polar lobe, which contains 
all the polar ectoplasmic spherules (see Photograph I) and also 


196 Frank R. Lillie 


some endoplasmic spherules in its base. At the height of forma- 
ton of the polar lobe the ectoplasmic waves become extremely 
pronounced all over it. During the completion of the first cleav- 
age the constriction around the polar lobe gradually disappears 
and the substance of the polar lobe forms that part of the larger 
cell next the cleavage furrow, where the group of ectoplasmic 
spherules keeps a superficial position. 


Fig. 25. Longitudinal section, first cleavage, late anaphase. Posterior end to the left, anterior 
to the right. The ectoplasm of the polar lobe has been separated from the remainder. c, chroma- 
tin masses cut off from the chromosomes; c.v., chromosomal vesicles of the daughter nuclei. E.d., 


Ectoplasmic defect; p./., polar lobe; r.s., residual substance of the germinal vesicle. 


- Mead has given a good description of the form changes of the 
first cleavage, and has described the polar lobe, which he calls the 
yolk-lobe, following the older terminology. “The term polar lobe, 
introduced by Wilson, seems preferable to the name yolk-lobe, 
and | therefore employ it. 

The polar lobe is fundamentally an ectoplasmic formation. 
The proof of this is experimental. We have seen that the endo- 
plasmic substances may be given any arrangement with reference 


Ele PIILE, ntary Phe r10Mena of Embr yonic Dex elopme nt 197 


to polarity by the centrifuge; but that the ectoplasm is unaffected 
and that the polar ¢ slcniles appear at the original animal pole even 
though this be the center of aggregation of the large endoplasmic 
Ppherules. Now the polar (epee invariably forms opposite to the 
polar bodies, so that in the extreme case, reversal of position of the 
endoplasm, the gray cap extends into the base of the polar lobe; 
or the protoplasm of the clear band may extend into its base in 
case of intermediate position of the endoplasmic substances 
(Photographs G and I and Figs. 17, 18 and 19). Thus the posi- 
tion of the polar lobe is independent of the position of the endo- 
plasmic substances, and is determined by the polarity of the egg 
and the distribution, with reference to the polar axis, of the ecto- 
plasm. Inasmuch as the animal pole is devoid of ectoplasm the ecto- 
plasmic substance of the polar lobe may be termed briefly the 
polar ectoplasm. 

2. Bilateral Symmetry.—The second striking feature of the 
first cleavage is the appearance of bilateral symmetry, which 
comes out “swags ble and permanently during the formation 
of the first cleavage spindle. Prior to this time I ae been unable 
to detect any positive evidence of bilateral symmetry; thus 1 
comes to expression in a few minutes. Like polarity itself the 
bilateral symmetry is independent of the distribution of the endo- 
plasmic substances, and, in my opinion, can only be conceived as 
due to a force like that of polarity. The evidence for this follows: 

The first cleavage spindle forms approximately in the center of 
the egg, but a little above the equatorial plane. Before this time the 
egg shows a perfect radial symmetry so far as the distribution of 
substances is concerned. Ina horizontal section the germ-nuclei 
lie in the center of a mass of non-spherular protoplasm, which 1s 
surrounded by a ring of endoplasm of even thickness, and this 
again by an ectoplasmic ring of even thickness. As the spindle 
forms, however, the endoplasmic ring becomes broader at one end 
and narrower at the opposite end, as though the non-spherular 
protoplasm containing the spindle had moved toward one side 
of the egg; this side is the anterior, and the opposite side the pos- 
terior face of the egg, and the axis of the spindle lies in the plane of 
symmetry thus indicated, and therefore in the longitudinal axis 


198 Frank R. Lillie 


of the embryo. ‘Thus in the early anaphase the first cleavage 
spindle lies eccentrically in a mass of non-spherular protoplasm, 
and the center of the endoplasmic spherules has been shifted 
toward the posterior end (Figs. 25 and 26). 

At the same time the centrosphere at the posterior end becomes 
much larger than that at the anterior end (Fig. 26); this is 
very marked at the end of the prophase, and is, indeed, the 
first clear indication of the posterior end. 

‘Treadwell ('97) has shown that, even when the first cleavage is 
equal in annelids, it bears the same relation to the axis of 
the embryo. But it should be noted that, when the first 
cleavage is unequal, as in Chetopterus, we have demonstrative 
evidence that the bilaterality thus established involves more 
than the mere determination of an axis; it involves also 
certain embryonic proportions of prospective significance. The 
cell CD is not only posterior in position, but it is larger and 
different to a certain extent 1n its composition, and behaves 
radically differently from the cell, 4B; in fact, the first cleavage 
predelineates the proportions and properties of the anterior and 
posterior ends of the embryo. 

The gradual determination of the plane of bilateral symmetry 
furnishes a fascinating problem. It is difficult or impossible to 
say which of the phenomena observed to be involved are primary 
and which secondary; it seems probable that the primary deter- 
mining factor has altogether eluded observation, and that the 
orientation of the spindle, the enlargement of the posterior 
centrosphere, and the shifting of the endoplasm and of the 
spindle are all consequences of some more remote cause, VIZ: 
a second process of polarization’ analogous to that following 
the breaking of the germinal vesicle, but with reference to a new 
axis placed at right angles to the polar axis—the sagittal axis of 
the embryo. 


'T have called the shifting of substances that defines the bilateral symmetry a second process of 
polarization because, so far as I can see, there is no immediate explanation of the phenomenon. 
Certainly the unequal cleavage (and the determination of bilateral symmetry that goes with it) is 
not due to any chance distribution of the substances of the egg; for on no theory of chances can 
one explain the uniformly normal relation of composition and of mass between the cells 4B and CD. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 199 


In centrifuged eggs the first cleavage is invariably normal with 
relation to the sole = , and usually unequal whatever the positions 
of the endoplasmic substances (see Photograph I, and Figs. 18 
and 19). Thus the cell 4B may receive most, or in other cases 
practically none, of the endoplasmic spherules. ‘There is certainly 
a tendency in the former case toward equality in size of the two 
cells, but the tendency toward inequality is usually markedly the 
stronger of the two. Thus, just as the polarity of the egg is inde- 
pendent of the distribution of the endoplasmic substances, so also 
is the bilaterality. And conversely just as polarity determines 
the polar distribution of these substances so does the force of 
bilateral polarization acting at right angles to polarity normally 
determine their bilateral arrangement. 

This conclusion is in full agreement with my earlier opinion on 
the organization of the egg of Unio (or) where I distinguished 
polarity and bilaterality as fundamental primitive features of the 
egg organization. In his fine study of the organization and cell- 
lineage of the Ascidian egg, Conklin (’04) likewise states his 
opinion that the median plane and the posterior pole are deter- 
mined by the “structure of the egg”’ and not by external incidents 
such as the path of the spermatozo6n. I have chosen the terms 
polarity and bilateral polarization as preferable because they 
express the opinion, which, I believe Conklin also had in mind, 
that they represent conditions antecedent to the distribution of 
substances in a polar or bilateral sense, and hence more funda- 
mental. : 

We know polarity and bilateral polarization only as conditions 
or forces that determine definite distributions of certain substances. 
Beyond this they can be defined only in a negative manner; thus 
they are not due to any visible aggregation of unlike substances; 
they are not determined by the path of the spermatozoén, nor by 
chance location of germ-nuclei. They might be conceived to be 
electrical or magnetic phenomena, but all the evidence of the 
influence of electricity and magnetism on development runs 
counter to this idea. But however obscure the nature of such 
forces may be they are clearly demonstrated to exist (see General 
Discussion, * "Principle of Unity”). 


200 Frank R. Lillie 


The first division is a differential one with reference to the sub- 
stance of the polar lobe, and to the amounts of other substances. 
Thus CD receives the entire area surrounding the polar globules 
that is devoid of ectoplasm (Fig. 29), it also receives all of the 
substance of the polar lobe and the larger amount of the endo- 
plasmic spherules. On the 4B side the ectoplasm and the endo- 
plasmic spherules come up to the first cleavage furrow at the 
animal pole; whereas on the C'D side they stop some distance from 
it (Fig. 29). These points are of importance with reference to the 
subsequent development. 

3. Residual Substance of the Germinal Vesicle and Nuclear 
Derivatives.—There remain yet two features of the first cleavage to 
consider, viz: the fate of the residual substance of the ger- 
minal vesicle andthe formation and distribution of new atelier 
derivatives. 

The residual substance of the germinal vesicle forms a mantle 
around the first cleavage spindle (Fig. 25, r.s.) and is thus divided 
approximately equally between 4B and CD. After the first 
cleavage is complete it is difficult to distinguish it by the usual 
means. But by means of the centrifuge it can readily be shown 
that it is still a distinct substance, and that certain of its properties, 
at least are still the same. If the two-celled stage be centrifuged a 
segregation 1s produced in the same manner as in the unsegmented 
naleaed egg; that is, a stratification of three substances appears 
in each cell—a gray cap, a clear band and a yellow mass. ‘These 
may be placed in a polar direction or in any other direction. If 
the centrifugal force acts at right angles to the axis of the egg, the 
eray suberinte goes to the ereett in ae upper cell, and lies against 
the cleavage wall in the lower cell; while the yellow endoplasm 
lies against the cleavage wall in the upper cell, and against the 
most distal surface of the lower cell (Fig. 27). Other variations 
need not be described. It will be seen that it is possible in this 
way to demonstrate the continued separate existence of the resi- 
dual substance of the germinal vesicle. 

Similarly one can demonstrate it in the four-celled and eight- 
celled stages; Fig. 28 illustrates this for the eight-celled stage. 
Later cleavage stages were not centrifuged but I have other evi- 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 201 


dence of its independent persistence to a later stage. In centri- 
fuged eggs in which it passes originally entirely into one cell it is 
possible to follow it, on account of its massed condition, up to 
late cleavage stages in which its position is just internal to the nu- 
cleus. Finally, if the results of staining intra-vitam with neutral 
red are to be trusted, we havea method that enables one to follow 
it step by step with perfect accuracy into the trochophore. 


Fig. 27. To show the effects of centrifuging in the two-celled stage. Drawn from the living mate- 
rial. c.b., clear band; e, endoplasm; g.c., gray cap. 

Fig. 28. To show the effects of centrifuging in the eight-celled stage. Drawn from the living 
material. Explanation of letters same as Fig. 27. 


That the nuclei of cleavage stages set free certain substances at 
each prophase is a familiar idea, that has been best set forth by 
Conklin. It has, however, been impossible to follow these sub- 
stances far by cytological methods, so that their fate is largely a 
matter of conjecture. In the egg of Chetopterus such substances, 
“oxy-chromatin,”’ are similarly set free, and, in addition to these, 
there is liberated at each mitosis a group of large granules that can 
be followed through at least one cell-generation in each case. 
These are the bodies that Mead described in the first cleavage as 
nucleoli. 

Their history in the first cleavage is briefly as follows: “They 
arise in connection with the chromosomes from the segmentation 
nucleus and separate from them in the prophase of the first divi- 
sion. So that the equatorial plate consists of a certain number 


202 Frank R. Lillie 


of chromosomes with intermingled granules. The latter are 
hardly larger at this time than the ends of the chromosomes, and 
they stain in iron-hematoxylin even more intensely than the chro- 
mosomes themselves. Only the chromosomes divide and in the 


Fig. 29. Outline of longitudinal axial section of the completed two-celled stage. To 
show the transmission of chromatin masses cut off from the chromosomes exclusively to the 
larger cell. The fine dotted outline shows the boundary of the spherular endoplasm. £, Ectoplasm; 
E.d., ectoplasmic defect;c, chromatin masses cut off from the chromosomes; N, nucleus; /.E., 


polar ectoplasm. 


anaphase the granules lie midway between the daughter chromo- 
somes among the interzonal fibrils (Fig. 25, c). During the ana- 
phase they become fewer and larger, no doubt by fusion with each 
other. In the telophase the cleavage plane passes anteriorly to 
them, thus leaving them invariably in the larger cell CD. So far 
as I could see, not one of them is left in the smaller cell. Fig. 29 
showing the mid-body of the first cleavage is a characteristic view 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 203 


of this stage, and shows with what certainty these granules may be 
traced into the larger cell. “The origin of the bodies from the 
chromosomes and their differential distribution to one cell are 
suggestive of an important role in differentiation. 

Thus a new group of granules arises during the first cleavage 
from the chromatin. A similar process is repeated in later cleav- 
ages, but whether or not it occurs to the same extent in all the cells 
I am unable to say. These granules attain the size of spherules 
by fusion; they then mingle with the a group of spherules of the 
endoplasm, and can no longer be distinguished. The repetition 
of this process up to sixty-four cells would produce a group of 
spherules staining in haematoxylin as numerous, probably, as the 
original a group of the endoplasm. It is possible, therefore, that 
spherules of the a group are continually disappearing and being 
replaced. Certain appearances in spherules of the a group during 
the cleavage favor this view; many are found that appear eroded or 
wasted to a crescent, a condition that I have not observed prior to 
the beginning of cleavage. It is impossible, therefore, to say in 
the late cleavage, how many of the spherules of the a habitus are 
original members of this group, and how many have been derived 
from the chromosomes during cleavage. 

The observations are instructive as bearing on the question of 
differentiation of nuclei. The assumption that chromosomes 
may divide differentially has not the slightest foundation in the 
observed facts of karyokinesis, excepting in the maturation 
divisions. On the other hand the hypothesis that cytoplasmic 
differentiation is dependent on nuclear differentiation appears to 
me a necessary corollary of our growing knowledge of the charac- 
ters of the chromosome complex. The above observations on 
differential distribution of nuclear derivatives shows a method by 
which not only may nuclear determination be realized, but also by 
which differentiation of nuclei may be obtained. If, for instance, 
this process of chromatin diminution takes place in certain cells 
and not in others, differentiation of the nuclei might result; when, 
on the other hand, the nuclear derivatives receive a differential 
distribution in daughter cells of different prospective tendencies, 
the problem of the determination of these cells seems simplified. 


204 Frank R. Lillie 


Thus nuclear determination and differentiation may exist even 
though every chromosomal fission be integral or equational. 

I have described the first cleavage in considerable detail, because 
it includes part of the process of segregation of substances, par- 
ticularly the bilateral polarization, and also illustrates the origin 
of new substances during the cleavage period. The account of the 
subsequent cleavages may now be divided into two parts. In the 
first will be given a very brief account of the form of the normal 
cleavage (cell-lineage) and the fate of the cells. In the second 
part the distribution of the substances in these cells will be traced. 


b. The Form of Cleavage 


The cleavage follows the usual annelid type (Figs. 30-37), 7. ¢.; 
the ectoderm is derived from three quartets of micromeres formed 
by alternate dexiotropic and leotropic equatorial cleavages of the 
four macromeres, 4 (left anterior), B (right anterior), C (right 
posterior) and D (left posterior). The mesoblast comes from the 
cell 4D; the entoderm from the cells 34, 3B, 3C,and 4D. The 


somatic plate is derived from the cell 2D or X. 


Certain special features of the cleavage deserve particular 


attention. Inthe four-celled stage D is much the largest of the cells, 
C comes next, 4 and B are the smallest (Figs. 30 and 31). ‘The 
differences between the cells 4, B, and C are, however, relatively 
slight. In the second cleavage the polar lobe is indicated by a 
protuberance of the vegetative pole of the cell CD toward the left 
side; its material passes entirely into the cell D. Again in the 
third cleavage there is usually a protuberance at the vegetative 
pole of D (Fig. 32) indicating the location of the substance of the 
polar lobe, which may, therefore, be followed readily into the cell 
1D. The polar globules are attached a little posterior to the first 
cleavage furrow on the line of the furrow between C and D, a 
position that is retained throughout the cleavage (Figs. 31 and 35). 
The ectoplasmic defect is transmitted to the cell rd (Figs, 30, 33, 
36 and 38). 

The third cleavage is dexiotropic and approximately equal, 
though the ectomeres ra, rb, rc, and 1d are slightly smaller than 
the “macromeres” r4, 1B, rC and 1D. ‘Thus in the upper 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 205 


Fie. 35 Fic. 36 Fic. 37 


Figs, 26, 30 to 37. To illustrate the form of the normal cleavage in Chetopterus. Drawn from 
preparations. 

Fig. 26. Anaphase of first cleavage. 

Fig. 30. Four-celled stage from in front. Note that the ectoplasmic defect is confined to the 
quadrant D. 

Fig 31. Four-celled stage from the animal pole. 

Fig. 32. Four-celled stage from behind. Early anaphase of third cleavage showing polar lobe 
in the D quadrant. 

Fig. 33. Formation of the first generation of micromeres, of approximately the same size as the 
macromeres. 

Fig. 34. Eight-celled stage from anterior end. 

Fig. 35. Eight-celled stage from animal pole. 

Fig. 36. Division of eight to sixteen cells to show large size of 2d. 

Fig. 37. Approximately sixty-four-celled stage seen from behind. Outline copied from Mead 
(97). The small circles in the cells represent the ectoplasmic spherules (diagrammatic). 

In all of the figures on this plate except the last, the ectoplasm is indicated by the dotted contour 
line. The relation of this line to the animal and vegetative poles should be noted. 


206 Frank R. Lillie 


quadrant the cell rd is much larger than its mates, similarly in the 
lower quadrant 7D is the largest cell (Figs. 33 and 34). 

In the fourth cleavage (eight cells to sixteen cells) the cell 1D 
divides unequally so that its upper product 2d is larger than the 
lower, the ““macromere” 2D (Fig. 36). The division of 1/4, 1B, 
and rC are approximately equal; thus 2d is by far the largest cell 
of the second quartet. In the sixteen cell-stage the cells rd and 2d 
are much the largest cells in the egg. The orientation of the egg 


Fig. 38. A sagittal section through a stage of about sixty-four cells. The small upper cells are the 
apical cells. The ectoplasmic defect will be noted in the posterior apical cell to the observer’s right. 
E, Ectoplasm; e.c., endoplasm c; En., endoderm cells; E.d., ectoplasmic defect; M., mesoblast cell. 


X derivatives of first somatoblast. 


and the identification of the cells is thus a relatively simple matter. 
Fig. 37 (outline of cells after Mead, 97) illustrates the arrange- 
ment and prospective significance of the cells of the sixty-four cell- 
stage. Only the superficial part of each cell is indicated and this 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 207 


is not always a good measure of relative size. ‘The distribution of 
the ectoplasmic spherules is indicated by the small circles in the 
cells with approximate accuracy. 


c. Distribution of Substances in the Cells 


No cell is pure in regard to the formative stuffs it contains, but 
each cell, up to a late stage at least receives both ectoplasm and 
endoplasm, with the exception of the entoderm cells which appear 
to receive no ectoplasm (Fig. 38). The arrangement of the sub- 
stances is the same in all ectodermal cells: (1) Externally a layer 
of ectoplasmic spherules; (2) the nucleus in a mass of micro- 
somal cytoplasm; (3) just internal to the nucleus a mass of non- 
spherular substance; (4) within this a group of endoplasmic 
spherules staining black in iron haematoxylin (endoplasm a), and 
(5) next to the segmentation cavity a group of large spherules 
staining in orange G formed by segregation and fusion of the 
smaller endoplasmic spherules (endoplasm c) already described. 
The arrangement is similar in the mesoderm cell (though the polar 
ectoplasm was not distinguished in the section figured), and, with 
the exception of the absence of the ectoplasm, in the entoderm cells 
also. 

This arrangement of substances is invariable; each cell is 
polarized; the axis of polarization of each is a radius of the egg, 
and the central ends are homologous. Thus each cell exhibits a 
stratification of substances similar to that of the entire egg. 

There is, however, one difference in the arrangement of the 
substances in the single cells and in the entire ovum. In the latter 
the c endoplasm lies above the a endoplasm, whereas in the former 
the c endoplasm is most distal. It must be remembered that 
there is a polarization of the whole segmented egg as well as of the 
individual cells; the c and a endoplasmic substances retain the 
same relative position in the entire segmented egg as in the unseg- 
mented. It is as though a cavity had been formed in the center 
of the c endoplasm of the unsegmented egg (Fig. 39) and nuclei 
placed around the periphery just within the ectoplasm, and cleav- 
age planes cut through between nuclei to the cavity. ‘Thus the 


208 Frank R. Lillie 


polarity of the cells is directly derived from the entire ovum. 
Nevertheless it is immanent in each cell, for, however much the 
arrangement of the cells may be disturbed, the arrangement of 
substances in the individual cells remains the same. 

We have seen that the polar ectoplasm passes entirely into the 
D quadrant. Between the two and the four-celled stage it forms a 
polar lobe that lies at first against the 4 quadrant, but shifts to the 
B quadrant as the cells come to rest. When the spindles are formed 
for the third division a very small polar lobe appears in D (Figs. 


Fig. 39. Axial section of an unfertilized egg that had stood in the sea-water two hours 
and seven minutes before killing. The a and cspherules of the endoplasm have segregated out 
(e.a., and e.c.; compare Fig. 5). A clear layer has arisen between the ectoplasm (£) and the 
a endoplasm; the outer centrosome of the maturation spindle has divided once, and the inner one 


several times. 


32 and 40). Sections taken at this time show that part of the 
polar ectoplasm plunges into the interior of the egg (Fig. 40, p.£.). 
This is very conspicuous in the four-celled stage containing the 
spindles for the next division. A tongue of endoplasm then 
extends across, severing the portion lying internally from the 
portion that remains superficial in position (Fig. 40). It is possi- 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 209 


ble that the upflow of the polar ectoplasm may carry some of it 
above the line of the next cleavage furrow, so that a certain amount 
may go into rd. An upflow of endoplasmic spherules goes on at 
the same time in all the cells both along the cleavage planes and 
also next the surface. 


go009 
Paste 
: ry : Sinks 


Q 
rey ttt 
e@@ {ee 


Fig. 40. Sagittal section through the cells B and D of the four-celled stage. The section passes 
a little to one side of the middle line; in the median section the spindles of the third cleavage were 
formed in metaphase, and the ectoplasmic defect was confined to D. The small polar lobe is con- 
fined to the D quadrant and contains all of the polar ectoplasm, p.E. Part of the latter is passing 
into the interior of the cell along the plane between B and D. Most of the striated spherules in the 
interior of the cells, however, belong to the c endoplasm, and it is impossible to distinguish exactly 


the boundary line between them and the polar ectoplasm. 


The substance of the polar lobe (polar ectoplasm) is thus far 
from being pure in its distribution, for a small portion of it may 
pass into the cell zd, and the remainder is divided in two parts of 
which the central part is probably transmitted to the first somato- 
blast (2d or X), and the superficial part, to the second somatoblast 
(4d or M). I am not, however, perfectly certain in regard to 
these points, though it is probable from the position of the 
material; both Wilson (’o4a) and Crampton (’96 ), moreover, 


210 Frank R. Lillie 


have shown experimentally that substance of the mesoblast is 
contained in the polar lobe. 

These observations agree closely with Wilson’s interesting 
experimental results on Dentalium; among other things he found 
that removal of the first polar lobe involved absence of the apical 
organ which normally arises in the D quadrant; the fact that some 
of the polar endoplasm probably enters into the composition of 
the cell rd might explain this curious result. Wilson, moreover, 
observed firecthy that substance of the polar lobe entered into the 
formation of both first and second somatoblasts. 

It has been a difficult task to determine the fate of the resi- 
dual substance of the germinal vesicle in the normal cell-lineage 
by direct observation. In the section on staining intra-vitam it 
will be shown that it is possible to trace it with a high degree of 
accuracy by this method. I will, therefore, add pee wrale some 
observations made under experimental conditions that confirm 
the observations, made with vital staining, that this substance is 
distributed mainly to the first generation of micromeres: Seeing 
that cleavage takes place in centrifuged eggs always with reference 
to the original polarity and not to the induced stratification of 
substances, it often happens that the residual substance of the 
germinal vesicle, now in the form of the gray cap (Figs. 17 and 
22 and Photographs), is located in the lower hemisphere. 
In such cases it can be seen to stream quite rapidly toward the 
animal pole during the process of cleavage, so that by the eight- 
celled stage it 1s usually found in the upper quartet. If, however, 
by any chance the third cleavage plane has come in so as to isolate 
it Wholly or in part in the lower quartet it always comes to occupy 
the uppermost corner of the cells in which it is found (Figs. 41 and 
42). his phenomenon may be observed with the greatest ease, 
because this substance in its massed condition contrasts strongly 
with the other cell-constituents. 

There can, therefore, be no doubt of a strong inherent tendency 
for this substance to aggregate in the upper hemisphere. Under 
normal conditions it is originally located in this part of the egg, 
so that there is every reason to believe, on the basis of these 
observations alone, that normally it 1s distributed mainly to the 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development FEMA 


cells of the first quartet of ectomeres and their descendants. The 
reader is referred to the sectionon vital staining for more detailed 
evidence concerning its ultimate fate. 

The distribution of the ectoplasmic spherules, excluding the 
polar group, corresponds exactly to the distribution of cilia (see 
Fig. 37). There can, therefore, be no reasonable doubt that they 
are specially concerned in some way with the production of cilia; 
this conclusion is practically demonstrated by the observations on 
the differentiation of unsegmented eggs. I would expect, there- 
fore, to find in true prototrochal larve of annelids, that the dis- 
tribution of the homologous substance would be confined to the 
prototroch, and would thus form a narrower band than in the 


S) 


Fic. 41 Fic. 42 


Figs. 41 and 42. Two eggs centrifuged before cleavage, now in the eight-celled stage. The stippled 
areas represent the substance of the gray cap, which tends toward the highest point (i.e., nearest to 


the animal pole) in the cells to which it is confined. Drawn from the living eggs. 


uniformly ciliated larvae of Chztopterus. The upper division of 
the ectoplasm in Rhynchelmis has an entirely different fate accord- 
ing to Vejdovsky: like the lower accumulation (my polar ecto- 
plasm) it is restricted to the D quadrant, where it lies near the 
animal pole; in the four-celled stage both polar accumulations flow 
into the interior of the egg and surround the nucleus and _peri- 
plast; subsequently the material appears to be transmitted mainly 
to the “.mesomeres”’ (first and second somatoblasts). 

It would appear, therefore, that the upper division of the 
ectoplasm has fundamentally different functions in Cheetopterus 
and Rhynchelmis. It is possible, however, that it 1s really com- 


212 Frank R. Lillie 


posed of two substances in both forms, and that, in Cheetopterus, 
I have overlooked the part corresponding to the main mass in 
Rhynchelmis, and that Vejdovsky has overlooked the part in 
Rhynchelmis corresponding to the main mass in Chetopterus. 
Seeing that the Rhynchelmis embryo is without locomotor cilta, 
it Sa otaled be natural for the substance forming the superficial 
layer of the ectoderm cells of Chzetopterus and agetu nied with the 
formation of cilia to disappear in its phylogenic history. It 
is difficult to believe that substances having so similar an original 
disposition and structure as the upper and lower divisions of the 
ectoplasm in Chztopterus and Rhynchelmis are entirely different 
in their morphogenic functions (compare Vejdovsky’s Fig. 30, 
Plate IV, with my Fig. 25). 

Undoubtedly it would be possible by a detailed cytological study 
of each cell in each stage of the cleavage to follow more accurately 
the fate of the substances with which we have been concerned. 
A beginning has been made on this problem, but it has proved so 
difficult, that it seemed desirable to present the facts already 
determined, rather than delay their publication, and greatly 
. increase the extent of this already too long publication. 

The main results concerning the distribution of substances in 
the normal development are: 


(1) The ectoplasmic defect at the animal pole is the place of 


formation of the apical organ with its flagella. 

(2) The polar ectoplasm enters into the cells rd, 2d and 4d. 

(3) The remainder of the ectoplasm forms the superficial layer 
of the ectoderm cells. 

(4) The entoderm cells receive no ectoplasm. 

(5) All cells receive some of each kind of endoplasm. 

(6) New substances arise from the nuclei and are differentially 
distributed in some cases at least. 

(7) The residual substance of the germinal vesicle is distributed 
mainly to the first quartet of micromeres. 


d. Intra Vitam Staining 


The capacity of living protoplasm to take up stains has been 
investigated by a large number of workers. Alfred Fischel (’99) 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic De velopment 213 


was the first to apply the method to embryological research; his 
idea was to attempt in this way to differentiate distinct elements of 
the ovum and if successful to follow them in the embryonic devel- 
opment. “Hinsichtlich des letzteren Punktes galt es mir, im 
besonderem, als erwiinschtes Ziel, vielleicht ermitteln zu konnen, 
dass wahrend der Furchung eine Verteilung bestimmter Elemente 
des Eies auf ganz bestimmte Zellen, also gewissermassen eine 
Teilauslese der Plasmaarten der Eizelle statthat.” Fischel 
worked with several stains on the ova of a number of different 
animals, but obtained the best results with neutral red in the 
development of Echinus microtuberculatus. He found that 
stained granules appeared in a zone around the spindles and that 
they had such avidity for the stain, that they would completely 
decolorize weak solutions and become intensely red. During the 
resting stage of the nuclei the granules scattered throughout the 
cell. ‘They were not distributed differentially in the development, 
but each cell contained them. 

Garbowski (’o4) is the only other author, so far as I know, 
who has applied the method to embryological work; his problem 
was to unite a stained portion of one egg with an unstained portion 
of another and to follow the subsequent development of the grafted 
parts. His results, however, do not bear directly on the problem 
under consideration. My own purpose was precisely the same as 
Fischel’s, namely, to find if it were possible to stain constituents of 
the egg protoplasm intra vitam and, if so, to follow their history in 
the subsequent differentiation with special reference to differential 
distribution of such substances. Both results were obtained; cer- 
tain granules take the stain very intensely and are distributed in 
avery precise fashion to a particular region of the embryo. Although 
several stains were used, I shall describe the results from only 
one, neutral red, which I found, like Fischel and Garbowski, 
incomparably the best for the purpose. 

To obtain the results that I describe one must not use a strong 
solution, for then the stain becomes diffuse and the eggs do not 
develop far in it. The best results were obtained from eggs 
placed immediately after fertilization in a solution of 75 parts sea- 
water plus two and one-half parts of a saturated solution of neutral 


214 Frank R. Lillie 


red in sea-water. ‘This solution is a very faint rose color, and the 
eggs develop in it perfectly normally for more then twenty-four 
hours. If there are many eggs in the culture they may entirely 
bleach the solution, all the stain being absorbed by certain gran- 


Fic. 43 Fic 44 


Fic. 45 Fic. 46 


Figs. 43 to 46. Drawings of living eggs reared in neutral red solution. 

Fig. 43. Polar view, stage of first maturation spindle, which is represented by the central circle. 
The parallel lines represent the spherular endoplasm. Eggs fertilized at 10.18 a. M.; put in seventy- 
five parts sea-water plus two and one-half parts of a saturated solution of neutral red in sea-water at 10.20 
A. M.; drawing from the living egg at 10.43 a. M. The dots around the maturation spindle represent 
the red-staining granules. 

Fig. 44. Later stage of another egg seen from the side; the red-staining granules lie above the 
germ-nuclei. 

Fig. 45. Stage of first cleavage spindle seen from the animal pole. The two small circles represent 
the polar globules. The red granules surround the spindle. 

Fig. 46. Two-celled stage. Resting nuclei. Red granules scattered in the upper hemisphere. 


= 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development Dales 


ules that become intensely red; such granules retain the stain 
many hours or permanently, even if the eggs are transferred to pure 
sea-water. If the stain be made about twice this strength, the 
eggs develop abnormally after three or four hours; moreover, a 
diffuse pink color appears in the endoplasm also, but disappears 
from it very quickly after transfer to pure sea-water, though it 
remains in the granules that stain in more dilute solutions. Thus 
the stain is specific for certain granules, and the afhnity of these 
granules for the stain 1s very intense. 

If, then, the stain be of the proper strength one obtains invari- 
ably the following results when the eggs are put in immediately 
after fertilization and are allowed to remain. Up to the time of 
the formation of the first maturation spindle there is practically 
no stain in the egg, though by careful examination with an oil 
immersion lens one can see certain minute, scattered, bright red 
granules in the ectoplasm. In some cases there appears around the 
first maturation spindle a complete mantle of bright red granules 
larger than the ordinary cytomicrosomes and smaller than spher- 
ules (Fig. 43). [hese agree in position with the residual sub- 
stance of the germinal vesicle. In other cases these granules do 
not stain until the second maturation spindle is formed or even 
later. 

When the germ-nuclei are formed, these granules form a band 
above them and later surround them (Fig. 44). When the first 
cleavage spindle is formed, they form a mantle surrounding it 
completely (Fig. 45) and are thus divided between the two cells. 
As the nuclei of the two-celled stage come to rest, the red granules 
spread out in the upper hemisphere 0 each beneath the ectoplas- 
mic spherules, 7. ¢., at the periphery of the non-spherular substance 
(Fig. 46). 

As the nuclei elongate for the second division the red granules 
again accumulate around them and form a mantle to the spindles, 
and are thus divided between the four cells, in which, as the nuclei 
come to rest, they again spread out in the upper hemisphere 
(Fig. 47). 

In the preparation for the third cleavage (four cells to eight 
cells) the red granules are again attracted to the nuclei and spread 


256 Frank R. Lillie 


out along the periphery of the spindles. But in this case the 
larger proportion is aggregated around the upper aster, and a 
relatively small number only around the lower (Fig. 48). Thus 
the first generation of ectomeres receives by far the larger propor- 


_ tion of the red granules, and this produces a most striking orienta-. 


tion of the egg, the upper quartet being a bright red, and the lower 
only faintly tinged, except in the upper left hand corner of each 
macromere, where the red granules congregate after the cleavage ts 
completed, and thus joretell their distribution to the second quartet 
of ectomeres (Fig. 49). The cell D furnishes the most striking 
illustration of this on account of its large size. 

I am not sure that all of the red granules remaining in the 
macromeres are distributed to the cells of the second quartet; 
but certainly the larger number are, so that very few of the original 
red granules remain for the third quartet. 

In the later cleavage the general result is so clear as to be easily 
visible with the low powers; the upper half of the segmented egg 
has an intense red stain, the portion derived from the second 
quartet very little, and below this there is almost none at all. 

This difference continues to increase in intensity during the 
formation and growth of the trochophore. At about twenty hours 
the exumbrella of the trochophore (Fig. 51) is brilliant, the red 
stain is distributed in great blotches that define a broad band 
interrupted dorsally and leaving only a small area at the apical 
pole with smaller red spots, in the center of which is the apical tuft 
of cilia. The subumbrella in comparison to the exumbrella is 
very lightly stained but the better stained specimens show a few 
red granules in the ectoderm that appear to define the somato- 
blastic plate; I am not, however, sure of the distribution of these 
granules in the subumbrella, which vary greatly in different 
cultures. 

The bright red stain is practically confined to the ectoderm; the 
entoderm and mesoderm have at most a faint rose tinge. As the 
trochophore becomes older, the broad prototrochal band becomes 
relatively narrower, owing no doubt largely to the expansion of 
the head vesicle in front of it. 

If now the cells of the prototroch containing the large masses 


. 
j 
i 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development O58 | 


of red are examined with the oil immersion lens, it is found that 
the red mass 1s situated internal to the nucleus. Five substances 
may in fact be recognized in each cell: (1) the layer of ectoplasm 
with its characteristic spherules which are slightly stained; 
(2 and 3) the nuclear area containing a large vacuole; (4) the 
bright red mass, analyzable under the 2 mm. lens into a dense 
group of large red spherules; (5) an aggregation of yellow endo- 
plasmic spherules (Fig. 52). 

Now in a late cleavage stage it will be found that the arrange- 
ment of substances is the same with this exception, that the red is 
in the form of relatively small granules, and the vacuole is not yet 
formed (Fig. 50). From which it follows, either that the red 
granules have grown to the size of spherules, or that agglutination 
and fusion have taken place among them. ‘The origin of spherules 
from smaller granules is thus demonstrated. 

The question now arises, what is the origin of these red-staining 
granules that have so precise a distribution in the development. 
One thinks at once of the residual substance of the germinal 
vesicle with which the aggregation of red granules has the follow- 
ing points in common: (1) the original distribution around the 
maturation and the first cleavage spindles; (2) the position in late 
cleavage just central to the nucleus; (3) the size of the granules, 
intermediate at first between cytomicrosomes and spherules; (4) 
the distribution to the upper hemisphere. 

One is, however, met at once by the difhculty that the amount 
of these granules in the later development exceeds considerably 
the original amount of the residual substance of the germinal 
vesicle. ‘This difficulty may, however, be met by assuming that 
similar granules arise from other nuclei than the germinal vesicle; 
there is very definite evidence for this view detailed in the account 
of the differentiation of unsegmented eggs. 

But a more serious difficulty arises; if these red granules are 
indeed the residual substance of the germinal vesicle, one should 
find that the gray cap formed from this substance in centrifuged 
eggs takes the red stain. Now, this is not the case; and to save 
the hypothesis one can only assume that the denseness of the 
ageregation induced by the centrifugal force is unfavorable to the 


218 Frank R. Lillie 


Figs. 47 to 52. Drawings of living eggs reared in neutral red solution. 

Fig. 47. Four-celled stage. Resting nuclei. Red granules scattered in upper hemisphere. 

Fig. 48. Third cleavage-spindles formed. Majority of red granules accumulated around the 
upper half of the spindles. 

Fig. 49. Eight-celled stage. Resting nuclei. Same egg as Fig. 48, twelve minutes later. Details 
drawn only in D quadrant. In 7rd the red granules are uniformly distributed; in 7D they occupy the 
upper left hand corner, or region of the cell 2d. 

Fig. 50. Single cell of about the sixty-four-celled stage. The red granules are now larger and lie 
just internal to the nucleus. 

Fig. 51. Trochophore of twenty-four hours, reared in neutral red solution, the black areas 
show the distribution of the stain. The largest areas in three irregular rows are inthe prototroch. As 
the larva rotated, the dorsal interruption of the prototroch came repeatedly into view. 

Fig. 52. A single prototrochal cell drawn with oil immersion lens in life. The red granules are 
now very much larger than before. E, Ectoplasm; e, endoplasm; N, nucleus; V7, vacuole. 


hdeediinn dl teens hotel 


219 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 


220 Frank R. Lillie 


penetration of the stain. What one does observe is that similar 
scattered red granules appear on the surface of the clear band, 
and that these become more abundant in the upper part, and 
spread over the entire animal hemisphere, whatever be the position 
of the gray and yellow masses, as in the normal development. 
Now the clear band always lies against the gray cap, and, as this 
gradually dissolves and spreads out, it seems probable that the 
red eranules appearing on the clear band are derived from the 
gray cap. It would thus appear that the residual substance of the 
germinal vesicle does not consist entirely of erythrophilous gran- 
ules; but it is probable that the original red granules are derived 
from this substance. 

This is at any rate the conclusion to which I have come. It 
should be possible to observe this without any uncertainty in the 
living egg, if the hypothesis is correct; but when I began to realize 
the significance of the question the season had passed; and I have 
to rely on the observations made before I realized that the gray 
cap was the residual substance of the germinal vesicle. 

I expect to return to this question another season; but I have 
little doubt of the correctness of this identification of the red 
granules distributed to the exumbrella with part of the residual 
substance of the germinal vesicle and other nuclear derivatives. 
The resemblance in position, size of granules, and general be- 
havior is too precise to leave much room for doubt. 


2. Differentiation Without Cleavage 


In 1902 I published a paper on Differentiation Without Cleav- 
age in the Ege of the Annelid Cheetopterus pergamentaceous, in 
which I showed that under certain conditions, eggs of Cheetop- 
terus might become ciliated, and exhibit other phenomena of 
differentiation without undergoing any process of cleavage. 
Similar results have been obtained for other annelid ova by 
Treadwell (02) and Scott (03 and ’06). One striking fact noted 
in Chetopterus was that such ciliated unsegmented ova some- 
times exhibited what appeared to be regional homology with the 
trochophore. But the structure of the ciliated ova was very 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 221 


variable and such homology could not usually be demonstrated. 
My general conclusion at that time was that cell division is not 
necessary for embryonal differentiation of certain kinds; and that 
the usually disordered and variable character of the forms develop- 
ing from unsegmented ova indicates that the essential role of 
cleavage in normal development is the localization of processes 
of different kinds. 

However, the conclusion in no way explained how differentia- 
tion of specific cell-organs could arise in an unsegmented mass of 
protoplasm with only one nucleus. ‘The most obvious inference 
was that the unsegmented ovum already contained substances of 
specific morphogenic value, that played their roles whether dis- 
tributed in cells or not. ‘This idea has been more or less definitely 
in mind since the original experiments, and I, therefore, took up 
the subject in Cheetopterus again in the summers of 1904 and 1905. 

I found that a variety of methods might be employed to bring 
about differentiation without cleavage: 

(1) By exposing the unfertilized eggs for about an hour to the 
action of potassium chloride in sea-water. (For details of method 
see Lillie *o2.) 

(2) By treating fertilized eggs with the same solution but in 
higher concentration (Lillie ’02). 

(3) If ova are fertilized after standing two to eight hours in sea- 
water a variable proportion develop without undergoing seg- 
mentation, the proportion being greater when the ova have 
remained longer without fertilization. Or such eggs might seg- 
ment at first and the blastomeres fuse together, the subsequent 
development being without cleavage. ‘The fertilization was often 
polyspermic in such cases. 

On July 28, 1904, a batch of eggs from one female was divided 
into three parts, which were fertilized after standing three hours, 
four hours and five hours, respectively, in sea-water. None of 
these segmented to any considerable extent. “The next morning 
the four-hour lot was swarming with ciliated specimens, all oe 
which had developed without cee On July 30 some eggs 
were divided into two lots one of which, A, was fertilized after 
standing in sea-water one and one-half hours and the other B, 


- 


222 Frank R. Lillie 


after two and three-quarter hours. The cleavage of lot A was 
irregular from the start but only about 40 per cent developed 
without cleavage. In lot B, on the other hand, over go per cent 
failed to segment or the cells fused later on, and most of these eggs 
differentiated without cleavage. (For details of these experiments 
see the section on multinucleated, unsegmented eggs.) 

(4) Fertilized eggs placed at a temperature of 10-14° C. for 
about twelve hours remain unsegmented and differentiate when 
restored to the room temperature, usually without segmentation. 

In Experiment 8, 1904, two lots of eggs, A and B, were fertilized 
at 4.52 and 5 P. M., respectively; each was divided into two lots, 
A1and A2, Brand Bz. §8.A1 and 8.B2 were placed at a tem- 
perature of about 14° C. about the time of the formation of the 
first polar body; 8.A2 and 8.B1 were placed at the same time 
at a temperature of about 16° C. When 8A.1 and 8.B2 were 
examined the next morning at about g A. M. they were unsegment- 
ed and they showed almost as sharp a segregation of the residual 
substance of the germinal vesicle as could be produced with the 
centrifuge. The preparations show that the maturation spindle 
remained unchanged in many cases and degenerated in others. 
In 8.A2 and 8.B1 there were two classes of eggs, viz: young 
normal larve and eggs like 8.A1; clearly the temperature in this 
case was near the lower margin of the temperature range for 
cleavage. A large proportion of the unsegmented eggs then un- 
derwent differentiation without cleavage at a very rapid rate. 

(5) I have some evidence that an*abnormally high tempera- 
ture may produce the same effect (only one experiment not quite 
conclusive, as differentiation was not followed to a period of forma- 
tion of cilia). 

Careful examination of this mode of development, however, 
produced, showed, what I had entirely missed before, that the 
differentiation proceeds by the segregation and differentiation of 
the substances described in the preceding parts of this paper, but 
which I had not recognized at the time of my first study. 

In describing the phenomena of differentiation without cleavage 
I shall leave out of account a good many accompanying phenom- 
ena which are not directly related to the problem in hand and which, 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 228 


moreover, were sufhciently noticed in the earlier account (Lillie 
’02), such as the ameboid movements occurring at various stages 
of the process, and the more or less abnormal cleavage of a variable 
proportion of eggs in each experiment. 

The unsegmented eggs that undergo differentiation may be 
divided into two classes according as they are uninucleated or 
multinucleated; the former constitute the type in the KCI cultures 
and are relatively rare in the fertilized cultures; the latter may owe 
the multinucleated condition either to polyspermy or to an original 
cleavage which is subsequently lost by fusion of the blastomeres. 
The multinucleated unsegmented eggs differentiate nearly as 
rapidly as normal ova (cilia formed in six or seven hours) whereas 
the uninucleated ova differentiate more slowly (cilia formed in 
eight to nine hours). 


a. Uninucleated Ova 


The conditions are simpler in the uninucleated ova in some 
respects and the records more complete; they may, therefore, be 
considered first. The process of differentiation depends on segre- 
gation of the cytoplasmic materials already described, growth of 
the nucleus to an enormous size, interaction of nuclear derivatives 
and cytoplasm, and a final rearrangement and differentiation of 
substances. Among the many variable phenomena, these are 
constant, and may, therefore, be considered as primary. 

1. Early Segregation of Formative Stujfs.— The orginal segre- 
gation of material, whether the eggs be fertilized or not, 1s like the 
normal up to the period preceding the first cleavage; the polar 
globules may or may not be formed (Lillie ’02); the polar lobe may 
appear and is then retracted; but cleavage does not take place. 
Then the egg-nucleus together with the nonspherular protoplasm 
begins to sink in toward the center of the egg. At the same time 
endoplasmic spherules slowly arise toward the animal pole, as in 
the normal development, between the nonspherular protoplasm 
and the ectoplasm (Fig. 53). The ectoplasm also begins to flow 
towards the animal pole andis soon aggregated mainly inthe upper 
hemisphere (Figs. 54 and 55); the polar ectoplasm remains at the 
vegetative pole (Fig. 55). ‘The original opening in the ectoplasm 


224. Frank R. Lillie 


at the animal pole may persist and the spherular endoplasm 
reach the surface here (Figs. 53, 55, 56). he layer of spherular 
enodplasm is usually considerably thinner on one side, show. 
ing that there is a bilateral polarization of the substances like 
the normal. The nucleus has enlarged considerably. This 
occurs about one and one-half hours from the beginning of the 
experiment. 


Fig. 53. Section of differentiating uninucleated unsegmented egg. E, Ectoplasm; E.d., ecto- 
plasmic defect; e., spherule bearing endoplasm; N, nucleus; p.E., polar ectoplasm. Eggs put in 
ninety-three parts sea-water plus seven parts 2 M KCl at g a. ., transferred to sea-water 
at 10 A. M. Preserved 10.39 A. M., one hour and thirty-nine minutes from the beginning of the 
experiment. The dotted outline represents the boundary of the spherule-bearing endoplasm; the 
broken line, the ectoplasm. 


The ectoplasm now flows more rapidly towards the animal 
pole and thus forms a relatively narrow polar ring;in some cases 
the opening disappears and the ectoplasm becomes heaped up in 
a mass (Fig. 59); as thereis relatively little endoplasm in this 
part of the egg there appear three strata (Figs. 54, 55 and 56). 
This occurs about two to four hours from the beginning of 
the experiment. Thus in Experiment 3.1, 1904, I put some 
unfertilized eggs into ninety-five parts of sea-water plus five 
parts of 5/2 M KCl at 10.47 a. m., and the eggs were trans- 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 225 


Fie. 54 Fic. 55 


Fic. 56 Fic. 57 


Figs. 54 to 58. Stages in the differentiation of uninucleated unsegmented eggs drawn from living 
material. E, Ectoplasm; e, endoplasm; E.d., ectoplasmic defect; p.E., polar ectoplasm; r.s. re- 
mainder of gray cap (residual substance of germinal vesicle). 

Fig. 54. Eggs put in ninety-five parts sea-water plus five parts of a two and one-half M KCI solu- 
tion at 3.15 p.M. ‘Transferred to sea-water at 5 P.M. Drawn at 5.15 p. M., two hours from the begin- 
ning of the experiment. 

Fig. 55. Eggs put in ninety-five parts sea-water plus five parts of a two and one-half M KCI solu- 
tion at 10.47 a. M. Transferred to sea-water at 11.52 A.M. Drawn about 2.45 p. M., four hours from 
the beginning of the experiment. 

Fig. 56. Same history as Fig. 55. 

Fig. 57. Eggs put in eighty-five parts sea-water plus fifteen parts of a two and one-half M KCl 
solution at 11.10 a. M. Transferred to sea-water at 12 o’clock, noon. Drawn 4.12 p. M., five hours 
from the beginning of the experiment. 


226 Frank R. Lillie 


ferred to normal sea-water at 11.52 A. M., and the conditions 
shown in Figs. 55 and 56 were found at about 2.30 and 
2.45 P. M., respectively. It will be noticed that there is quite 
a pronounced constriction near the equator between the 
unlike substances; this often becomes so deep as to produce the 
impression of a cleavage. The substance of the polar lobe is indi- 
cated in both cases, and in one (Fig. 55) an ameboid prominence 
is formed at the animal pole, owing to extrusion of endoplasm 
through the ectoplasmic defect occurring there. 

In about half an hour to an hour more a very striking change 
takes place in the majority of the eggs (uninucleated, unseg- 
mented). A mass of yellow spherules that have continued to rise 
toward the animal pole pushes out through the defect in the 
ectoplasm and forms a protuberance at 
the animal pole (Figs. 57, 58, and Photo- 
graph L). ‘Thus the ectoplasm becomes 
a broad band encircling the ovum 
between the polar masses of yellow 
endoplasm. (For section of such a 
stage, see Fig. 60.) The nucleus tas 
_now become as large as the original 
germinal vesicle and it lies in the non- 
spherular protoplasm beneath the ecto- 
plasmic band. In some cases the 
ectoplasmic defect at the animal pole 


Fig. 58. Eggs taken from female : 
at g A. M., centrifuged at 10.45 A. M. becomes covered up (Fig. 59), and then: 


Fertilzed 10.50 4m. Drawn about “¢hisg banded form does mot acculinesmen 


3-15 Pp. M., four hours and _ thirty-five 


cases the ectoplasm simply becomes 
more massed. 

There are some reasons for thinking that the polar masses of 
endoplasm are of different composition, thus it will be noticed 
that the original lower mass (Photograph L) affects the photo- 
graphic plate less strongly than the upper mass, though to the eye 
they appeared alike in color. 

The mere separation is also evidence for some difference in 
composition, though it is to be admitted that it does not constitute 
decisive proof. A third reason is found in the presence in the 


minutes after fertilization. 


| 
/ 
| 
i 
| 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 227, 


lower mass of a small reticular area usually clearly marked at this 
stage (Fig. 60) and always absent in the upper mass. ‘These facts 
are mentioned only as indicating that the original (or early 
induced) diversity of substances in the endoplasm may be greater 
than one would suppose. ‘The opposed masses in this case corre- 
spond. roughly to the endoplasm of the first generation of ecto- 
meres (upper mass) and of the other cells (lower mass). The 
distinction is not between endoplasmic substances a and c, for 
these occur in each mass. 

2. Concerning the Growth of the Nucleus—Nuclear growth is 
usually accompanied by nuclear division involving the formation 
and division of a definite number of chromosomes at periodic 
intervals. Now in the eggs that we are describing the nucleus 
does not divide, but it grows and undergoes periodic changes that 
correspond to the periodic changes of dividing nuclei. ‘hus 
in any preparation the nuclei of some eggs are composed mainly 
of nonstaining substances, with only a few scattered stained gran- 
ules similar to the “nucleoli” cast off at each normal division 
(Figs. 59 and 60), and other nuclei in ova on the same slide, and 
thus of precisely the same age and mode of preparation, are dense 
masses of intertwined chromosomes (Fig. 61). “These conditions 
undoubtedly represent successive phases of nuclear activity, and 
demonstrate a periodic succession of chromatic and non-chro- 
matic stages. Inasmuch as the number of chromosomes in the 
older and larger nuclei is much greater than in the younger and 
smaller, it cannot be doubted that there is a periodic formation 
and division of chromosomes as in the normal cleavage. 

The egg shown in Fig. 61 was killed three hours and seventeen 
minutes after the beginning of the experiment. After making due 
allowance for the slower rate of development it would correspond 
in age to about a normal sixty-four celled stage, which would 
contain, if unfertilized, g x 64 = 576 chromosomes. A calcu- 
lation of the chromosomes inthis uninucleated egg gives a roughly 
corresponding number; the nucleus ran through four sections and 
there were considerably over 100 chromosomes in each. (All in 
the one section figured could not be shown in the figure.) The 
number corresponds so closely to the theoretical requirements, 


228 Frank R. Lillie 


Fig. 59. History same as 53, preserved two hours 


that it has the value of an 
actual demonstration of the 
view expressed in the last 
sentence of the preceding 
paragraph. 

The arrangement of the 
chromosomes in Fig. 61 is 
suggestive; they are not uni- 
formly distributed, but are 
arranged in four groups. 
The number of groups in 
the entire nucleus was at 
least double as many. The 
only explanation of such a 


and nineteen minutes from beginning of experiment. grouping that occurs to me 
The ectoplasm aggregated in animal hemisphere; ecto- is contained in the hypoth- 


plasmic defect covered up; a and c endoplasms sepa- 


rating out. 


of which there were nine, 
tend to remain together. 
‘There is no other evidence 
for this than I have given. 
In any event the steady 
increase in number 1s a 
strong support for the in- 
dividuality hypothesis. 
That the nucleus does 
not divide under such cir- 
cumstances may be attrib- 
uted to the absence of cen- 
trosomes and _ spheres, 
which are usually lacking 
in the uninucleated unseg- 


mented eggs in the early 


esis that all descendants of 
each original chromosome, 


Fig. 60. History same as 53, preserved four hours and 


eighteen minutes from beginning of experiment. Longi- 
tudinal section of three-banded form; original animal 
pole probably to left. Nucleus in achromatic condition. 
Vacuolated area to right characteristic of eggs in this 


condition. 


stages. The reason for this defect is not very clear, especially in 
the case of those eggs where the stimulus to development is fur- 
nished by normal fertilization. But, even when the stimulus is 


ae 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 229 


supplied by potassium chloride, certain eggs form asters and spin- 
dles, so that the potentiality of these structures must be sup- 


posed to reside in the egg proto- Se 


plasm. The series of preparations Be x 
upon which I rely for most of ‘a i 
the data concerning the nuclei is, eRe, \ 
from a potassium chloride series, yee \ 
inwhich the polar bodies were not | ee \ 
formed;! it is thus possible that, | Sue | 
in this case, there is a connection \ AUP 
between the omission of the pro- ) 


cess of formation of the polar  \ / 
bodies and the absence of asters. a J 
Such an explanation might also tee, Lees 
apply ue the uninucleated, vee Fig. 61. History same as 53, preserved three 
Memtcdeiec. round ine fertilized joics and seventeen midutes from beginning of 
series; these constitute a relatively experiment. See text for description. 
small proportion of all the eggs in such a culture, and in a 
normal lot of eggs some fail to fertilize, and do not form 
polar bodies; if we supposed that some extract of the sperm 
stimulated differentiation in such eggs we would get the above 
result. 

3. Interaction of Nucleus and Cytoplasm.—After the nucleus 
has reached about the size of the germinal vesicle a strong mutual 
attraction between the nucleus and the ectoplasm begins to be 
apparent, when the nucleus is in the chromatic condition. This 
is shown in one of two ways: Either the ectoplasm is drawn into 
the interior of the egg where it forms a mass in contact with the 
nucleus, or the chromatic part of the nucleusis drawn out to the 
ectoplasm. It is interesting to note that the former corresponds 
to the normal procedure in Clepsine (Whitman ’78) and Rhynchel- 


1Tf the polar bodies are not formed, the looped or elongated form of chromosome does not appear 
in the uninucleated, unsegmented ova until between one and two hours from the beginning of the 
experiment. Previous to this time one finds only irregular forms similar to the chromosomes of 
the maturation spindles. Thus the chromosomes apparently carry out the maturation divisions 
within the egg but without forming daughter nuclei, before the form of chromosome characteristic 


of the cleavage divisions appears. 


230 Frank R. Lillie 


mis (Vejdovsky ’88—’92). 
| When the ectoplasm has 
fil ll na Md lM ee : gathered in the interior 
HA AAT oS it collects on one side on 
the large nucleus, and a 
very important change 
takes place. A la rege 
quantity of the chromatin 
ageregates on the margin 
of the ectoplasm, and, 
while the remainder of 
nucleus returns to the 
achromatic condition, 
this portion diffuses in 
Fig. 62. Eggs put in ninety-five parts sea-water plus five the form of search Patt 
parts 23 M KCL at 11.10 a. M., five hours and ten minutes cles (microsomes) between 
from beginning of experiment. For description see text. the ectoplasmic spherules, 
which are thus completely 


2 


— me 
eee? 
at 
oe 


<68 
oi 
085 


— 28 


0%9 88 


8, 
ts 


66 8, 
eee? 
A — 
= 


Z 
‘e 
eo 0 _@ 
e 


Ve 
/ fo 


interpenetrated by a chromatic 
network. Eggs in which the 
ectoplasm has been attracted 
‘into the interior apparently do 
not develop farther. 

When the ectoplasm remains 
external the chromatic part 
of the nucleus is drawn to it. 
Fig. 62 illustrates a case in 
which there were two accumu- 
lations of the ectoplasm on 
opposite sides of the ege; the 8: 
attraction acting on the nucleus ‘ 
has drawn the latter out into Fig. 63. History same as 62. For description see 
a broad band extending from ee 


one mass of ectoplasm to the other. The chromatic portion of the 
nucleus is next the ectoplasm on each side, leaving an achromatic 
nuclear bandinthe center. The chromatin is broken up into small 
particles which are scattered between the ectoplasmic spherules. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 231 


A single section, such for instance as is shown in Fig. 63, shows 
the outline of the nucleus still intact; the chromatin in strands 
with large varicosities is seen stretching off toward the main mass 
of the ectoplasm, and the farther we trace it from the center of the 
nucleus the more the chromatin is broken up; radiating lines of 
granules can be seen extending from larger masses of chromatin in 
between the ectoplasmic spherules, so that all intermediate stages 
between masses of chromatin and scattered microsomes may be 
seen. Many similar sections occur on the same slides. Such 
granules are very much less abundant between the ectoplasmic 
spherules in stages preceding this. 

The living material furnishes the most conclusive proof of the 
migration of the chromatin halo of the nucleus. _ If the living eggs 
be examined under pressure at this time, one sees a gray halo 
around each large nucleus (Photograph J and Fig. 58); a little 
later this may be seen to stretch out toward a surface aggregation 
of ectoplasm and to flow into it losing its identity in the larger 
mass (Photograph kK). 

The intermingling of a large part of the nucleus, distinguishable 
in the living condition as a gray halo and in sections as a periph- 
eral chromatin layer, is one of the most striking and suggestive 
phenomena concerned in the differentiation of the uninucleated 
unsegmented eggs. Concerning the facts there cannot be a par- 
ticle of doubt. The process can easily be observed in the living 
condition, and even photographed, as Photographs J and K 
prove. he preparations are even more conclusive, if possible, 
for one can study the details more leisurely. Nor is there any 
doubt, in my opinion, that the chromatin particles become micro- 
somes. ‘The gradation from larger chromatin masses to un- 
doubted microsomes is perfect, and proceeds in an orderly direc- 
tion from the nucleus outward (Figs. 62 and 63). 

The interpretation of this phenomenon must proceed from the 
assumption that it is the result of a process occurring in the normal 
development, probably in each generation of cells, but which has 
been inhibited by the abnormal nuclear conditions, until it breaks 
loose by sheer force. Its striking character is due to the summa- 
tion of a number of relatively inconspicuous processes of the nor- 


222 Frank R. Lillie 


mal development. It, therefore, furnishes to some extent a meas- 
ure of the amount of chromatolysis within the same period of the 
normal development. 

The result necessitates a more careful inquiry into the normal 
conditions; at present it does not seem advisable to attempt a 
detailed comparison. I may say, however, that the process ap- 
pears to me to represent more than the formation of the chromatic 
granules that lie between the groups of daughter chromosomes 
in the normal anaphase (see description, p 202,and Fig.25);because 
I believe that such granules escape into the cytoplasm periodically 
in the uninucleated unsegmented eggs, and do not accumulate 
within the nucleus. ‘The matter is still under investigation. 

Finally it should be noted that such distribution of chromatin is 
not limited to the ectoplasm, but occurs probably in the endoplasm 
also in later stages. 

4. Later Distribution of Formative Stuffs —That the ectoplasm 
undergoes a radical change by this intermixture of chromatin is 
proved by two striking phenomena: (1) The mass of chromatin- 
charged ectoplasm immediately overflows the remainder of the 
substance of the egg, usually, but not always, covering it com- 
pletely; (2) the staining reaction of the ectoplasmic spherules 
changes radically; whereas previously they could not be stained 
with iron haematoxylin without overstaining the rest of the egg, 
they now take the hematoxylin strongly. These two phenomena 
will be considered separately. 

Overflow (unicellular gastrulation). I observed this process 
first in the summer of 1904. The most complete records are in 
Experiments 5 and 1 of that season, from which I take the follow- 
ing account: Experiment 5 was started at II.I0 A. M. July 28, 
1904. Division 1 of this experiment was treated as follows: 
The eggs were put into a mixture of ninety-five parts sea-water 
plus five parts 23 M KCl at 11.10 a.m. Part a was transferred 
to sea-water at 12.05 P. M. (fifty-five minutes). The designation 
of this experiment is thus 5.1.a. At 3.20 P. M. many eggs had reached 
the elongated condition shown in Figs. 57 and 58 with two terminal 
masses of yellow endoplasm, and a broad equatorial band of ecto- 
plasm through which the large nucleus could clearly be seen. At 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 233 


5 p. M. the ectoplasmic band in many eggs had ruptured on one 
side transforming it into a saddle-shaped mass, and the two masses 
of endoplasm met and fused on the opposite side of the egg (Fig. 
64). The nuclear area is situated beneath the ectoplasmic 
accumulation. 

It is undoubtedly at this time that the chromatic matter of the 
nucleus intermingles with the ectoplasm, though | did not observe 
this in 1904 for the simple reason that the eggs were not examined 
under pressure. But in 1905 this phenomenon was observed as 


already described. 


Fic. 65 Fic. 64 Fic. 66 


Figs. 64 to 70. Later differentiation of uninucleated unsegmented eggs from life. 

Fig. 64. Form produced by rupture of ectoplasmic band on one side. Eggs put in ninety-five 
parts of sea-water plus five parts 2 M KCl at 11.10 a. mM. Transferred to sea-water at 12.05 P. M.; 
drawn at 5 p. M., five hours from beginning of experiment. 

Fig. 65. Slightly later stage than 64. 

Fig. 66. Same egg as sixty-five drawn three minutes later; the ectoplasm has overflowed a large 
part of the endoplasm. 


All stages of the rupture of the ectoplasmic band were observed; 
the notes read “all stages may be seen; the granular band (ecto- 
plasm) first becomes broader on one side, then the yolk masses 
(endoplasm) approach and fuse and the band ruptures. Thus 
the yolk has the characteristic shape shown in the figure.” 

Then the ectoplasm accumulates in a mass (Fig. 65) and 


234 Frank R. Lillwe 


immediately begins to spread rapidly over the surface of the egg 
(Figs. 66 and 67) until there is a complete surface layer of ecto- | 
plasm. The notes read “This overflow of the gray matter (ecto- 
plasm) can easily be watched as it takes place rapidly, and all | 
stages may be seen at once on a single slide.” “Two drawings of 
the same egg (Figs. 65 and 66) accompanying Experiment I, 1904, 
show that a considerable part of the process of overflow may 
take place in three minutes. An uncovered spot, which prob- 
ably corresponds to the position of the polar ectoplasm, may persist 
for some time, and the egg thus affords a striking picture, as of 
epibolic gastrulation ina unicellular mass of protoplasm (Fig. 67). 


Wf), 


gnu 


Fic. 67 Fic. 68 


Fig 67. Another egg; slightly later stage. Unicellular “‘gastrula.” 
Er Fig. 68. Ciliated uninucleated unsegmented egg drawn about twenty-three hours from beginning 


of experiment. The vacuoles are about in the position of the prototroch of the larva. 


In many cases the process of overflow is incomplete, or does not 
involve the yolk mass at all, thus leading to conditions described 
beyond. ‘There is usually more or less endoplasm included in the 
overflowing mass; in some cases a very considerable quantity, so 
that the layer established by the overflow may be quite thick. 
This is a parallel condition to that found in the ectoderm cells 
normally, in which, as described on p. 207, only the layer external 
to the nucleus is composed of ectoplasm, and the remainder of the 
cell of endoplasm. 

Change of Staining Reaction. Sections made for the purpose 
of studying the process of overgrowth show that it does not begin 
until the ectoplasmic spherules have been so modified by the 


Elementary Phenomena of Embr yonic Dew elo pment 235 


intermixture of chromatin that their staining reaction to iron 
hematoxylin has changed considerably. Although this approxi- 
mates their stain to that of some of the endoplasmic spherules, 
there is not the least difhculty in distinguishing them. ‘The ecto- 
plasmic spherules are smaller, or at least more uniform in size, 
less spherical, and are massed differently; the substance in which 
they are embedded likewise appears different from that of the 
endoplasm, so that the total effect of the ectoplasm even when its 
spherules stain black is entirely different from that of endoplasm. 

It appears as if the ectoplasmic spherules become completely 
impregnated with chromatin, just before the process of over- 
growth takes place; the change in staining reaction is difficult to 
explain otherwise. [he formation of cilia follows very soon after 
the process of overgrowth; thus it cannot be doubted that the 
impregnation of the wenplasine spherules with chromatin stimu- 
lates their latent potencies. 

Sections stained in thionin and orange G show that the dispersion 
of the chromatin is by no means confined to the ectoplasm; but 
that the endoplasm as well is thoroughly interpenetrated with 
particles of chromatin, which are beyond all question microsomes. 
The same thing may also be seen in the iron hematoxylin prepara- 
tions, but it is not shown in so striking a manner as by the thionin 
and orange stain. The bright orange spherules contrast beauti- 
fully with the brilliant blue microsomes clustered around them 
and in all the interspaces. With thionin and orange staining the 
ectoplasmic spherules continue to take the orange stain even after 
the intermixture with chromatin but they take the blue also to a 
certain extent, so that their general effect is olivaceous. 

We have seen that in some cases the nucleus draws the ectoplasm 
into the interior of the egg, so that it is completely surrounded by 
the endoplasm. It is probable that this is the reason why many 
eggs fail to reach the stage of differentiation of cilia. [hus 
Experiment 9, 1901, very few of the eggs became ciliated; but the 
preparations show that, in practically all, the nucleus grew and 
the ectoplasm segregated in the usual fashion. But the sections also 
show that the internal migration of the ectoplasm is very char- 
acteristic of the series. 


236 Frank R. Lillie 


ie Formation of Cilia and Other Cell Constituents.—The 
formation of cilia follows hard on the heels of the ectoplasmic 
overflow. Some uninucleated eggs begin to revolve eight or nine 
hours after the beginning of the experiment, and others not until 
much later. “The majority, in the case of the potassium series, 
never reach this stage at all, probably, as suggested before, owing 
to failure in the process of overflow. 

Typically the entire ectoplasmic surface becomes ciliated, but 
the cultures show great variety of conditions owing to the following 
circumstances: (1) A good many eggs break into two or more 


U 


\ 
\ 
yy be I 


Sys 


Fic. 69 Fic. 72 


Fig. 69. From the same culture as 68; about same age. 
Fig. 70. From same culture as 68; about same age. The endoplasm of the egg has been con- 
stricted off. 


parts owing to excessive development of constrictions between 
segregated substances (compare Fig. 69). Thus are produced 
some non-nucleated parts, some parts nucleated and without,ecto- 
plasm, other parts nucleated and with relatively little endoplasm. 
(2) Fusions are very common (see Lillie ’02). (3) A larger or 
smaller part of the endoplasm may remain exposed (compare 
Figs. 68 and 69). 

It is, therefore, not surprising that good examples of clear 
regional homology with the entire trochophore are rare. Some 
are, however, found. (Ihe regional homology may be very 
precise in multinucleated unsegmented eggs; see next section.) 
The best example of it in uninucleated eggs is shown in Fig. 1 of 
my Ig02 paper. (See also Figs, 68, 69 and 70 of the present 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 227 


paper.) The reason for the existence of a regional homology is 
so clear that discussion is hardly necessary. Fig. 70 shows a 
ciliated part corresponding only to the upper halves of Figs. 68 
and 69 and resembling only the exumbrella region of the trocho- 
phore. 

In general the following statements may be made concerning 
the differentiation of the ca amc eeed eggs. (1) Organs are never 
formed, but only such structural dsmene as may occur in single 
cells of the trochophore. (2) Organs may, however, be simulated 
by the aggregation of the Bice cnc matter of an organ, for 
instance in the case of the yellow endo- 
plasm, which simulates the gut of the 
trochophore, or the row of large vacu- 
oles situated near the upper margin 
of the yellow endoplasm (e. g., Fig. 
69) which simulates the row of vacu- 
oles of the prototroch. (3) The struc- 
tural elements appear in the same 
order of time as in the trochophore. 
(4) The distribution of structural ele- 
ments tends to resemble that of the Fig. 71. Ciliated unsegmented egg 
trochophore. (5) he yellow Prides about twenty-eight hours old. Most of 


the endoplasm has been consumed. 


plasm (yolk ?) is used up, apparently fae 

for the maintenance of metabolism, 

in the ciliated unsegmented eggs precisely as in the larva (see 
Fig. 71). (6) The apical flagellum is never formed. 

Inasmuch as a number of conditions were figured and described 
in my earlier paper (Lillie ’02) and because multiplication of 
examples would be unprofttable, I shall content myself with 
illustrating in a very few cases the general statements just made. 

The first and second statements really require no further 
elaboration; Figs. 68-71 sufficiently illustrate them. 

The order of appearance of the structural elements is: 
(a) cilia; (b) intermediate vacuoles; (c) ectoplasmic vacuoles. | 
was much interested in attempting to discover the mode of origin 
of the cilia. From the fact that their distribution, both in the nor- 
mal larva and unsegmented eggs, is the same as that of the ectoplas- 


Ze 
Z 
Se 


238 Frank R. Lillie 


mic spherules, I was at first inclined to believe that each spherule 
was the germ of a cilium, and expected, therefore, to find the same 
number of cilia and spherules. The number of cilia seems, how- 
ever, tO exceed the number of spherules, and they do not arise 
directly from the spherules but jrom the microsomes between or on 
the surface of the spherules. This can be seen very clearly by 
focusing on the surface of an egg that 1s acquiring cilia with a 
2 mm. oil immersion lens. But there is a very intimate relation, 
and one can convince himself after the rate has slackened that the 
spherules vibrate in unison with the beat of the cilia. 

The intermediate vacuoles appear between the ectoplasm and 
the endoplasm soon after the formation of cilia. They are prac- 
tically confined to the original ectoplasmic hemisphere, and are 
specially well dev eloped nelly © just above the margin of the endo- 
plasm, thus in a position corresponding to the prototroch. ‘They 
appear to correspond to similar large vacuoles situated internal 
to the nucleus in the cells of the exumbrella and particularly of 
the prototroch in the larva. But, as they are confined by cell- 
walls, they tend to fuse and thus to produce a fluid-filled space 
separating ectoplasm and endoplasm (see figures). 

The ectoplasmic vacuoles are much smaller and more pedeunie en: 
and do not appear until about twenty-four hours from the begin- 
ning of the experiment (Fig. 71). They are usually massed in a 
particular region, corresponding to their distribution in the tro- 
chophore where they are very much more abundant in the exum- 
brella. In the ciliated ova of this age, the yellow endoplasm is 
much reduced in amount, and the eggs as a rule do not live much 
longer. 

(6). It is a curious fact, observed also by Scott in his observa- 
tions on Amphitrite, that the apical flagella are never found in 
these ciliated eggs. ‘This indicates a combination of factors in 
the normal development that is seldom or never realized under 
the conditions of the experiments. The explanation is probably 
as follows: The apical flagella arise at the animal pole, and in 
Chetopterus one can frequently see one polar body at least at 
their base. This is, however, the place where the endoplasm 
comes to the surface; the apical flagella are therefore endoplasmic 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 239 


in their origin. In the overflow of the ectoplasm in the unseg- 
mented eggs this exposure of the endoplasm is seldom, or never, 
preserved. Thus the formation of the apical flagella would be 
prevented. 

The study of the cytological phenomena in sections has been 
carried only as far as the overflow of the ectoplasm (about eight 
hours). Up to this time it is possible to follow the history of the 
original substances of the egg consecutively. I have not studied 
the later cytological details; the changes that take place in the 
structure of the cytoplasm are very profound and complex. 


b. Multinucleated Unsegmented Eggs 


These are characteristic of cultures of fertilized eggs so treated 
as to inhibit the process of cleavage or to induce fusion of blasto- 
meres. [here is a strong tendency towards polyspermy, if eggs are 
allowed to stand for several hours in sea-water before being fer- 
tilized. Such eggs may break up at the first division into a con- 
siderable number of blastomeres, and, in many such cases, the 
blastomeres soon fuse together, and cleavage planes do not 
again appear. The protoplasm of the egg of Chetopterus is 
extremely susceptible to external conditions, and one of the most 
common evidences of this is the fusion of blastomeres. 

In Experiment 7.B., 1904, for instance, the eggs were taken from 
the female at 9 A. M., and were allowed to stand in sea-water until 
11.42, when they were fertilized; a series of eggs was preserved and 
from them entire mounts and sections were prepared. In the 
entire mounts of eggs preserved twelve minutes after the addition 
of the sperm it can readily be seen that there are from three to 
twelve or more sperm nuclei in each egg. These were also seen 
in the living eggs under pressure. [he polar bodies were formed 
normally in about twenty-five minutes. Figs. 72 to 75 show the 
form changes of a single egg, from 12.32 to 12.47 P. M. At first 
this egg became so lobulated that it seemed as though it were divid- 
ing into about ten cells at once; then these lobes gradually dis- 
appeared and the eggs returned to their spherical condition. 
At 12.54 the great majority of the eggs had returned to the spher- 


240 Frank R. Lillie 


ical condition; about 5 per cent appeared like normal two-celled 
stages; these were probably monospermic. 

In Experiment 7.A. some of the same eggs were fertilized at 
10.35 A. M., having remained in the sea-water one hour and thirty- 
five minutes before fertilization. About 60.per cent of these 
segmented fairly normally, the blastomeres fused together in the 
remainder “Intermediate conditions were rare or absent.” 
(Quoted from notes.) 


‘Akal 
Vo 


Fie. 72 Fie. 73 


Mii | | jie 


| 
Wana DD 


i 


| 


Fic. 74 Fic. 75 


Figs. 72 to 75. Four views of the same polyspermic egg. The eggs were put in sea-water at 9 
A. M. and allowed to stand until 11.42 a. M. when they were fertilized. Fig. 72 was drawn at 12.32 
p. M.; Fig. 73 at 12.37; Fig. 74 at 12.44; Fig. 75 at 12.47. At 12.50 the egg was perfectly spherical 


again and showed no sign of cleavage. 


I expect to make a special study of the polyspermy, for which 
I have the preparations. Soin this place I shall consider only 
a few points. In the polyspermic eggs multipolar spindles arise, 
and a very uneven distribution of the chromatin results. ‘This 
is followed by the formation of nuclei of very unequal size, and 
in the following divisions the mitoses are irregular. ‘Thus there 


= 


Se, = ae 


es 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 241 


is an enormous increase of the chromatin, and-soon the most of 
the nuclei cease to be separate in the resting condition, and form 
a vast reticulum situated between the ectoplasm and endoplasm 
in the upper half or two-thirds of the egg (Fig. 76). A few 
small nuclei may remain separate. In the ensuing periods of 
division the individual karyokinetic figures cannot be observed, 
and there is a veritable riot of centrosomes, chromosomes, spindle 
fibers and astral radiations. 


Fig. 76. Optical section of whole mount; multinucleated unsegmented egg, preserved five and 
one-half hours after fertilization. The nuclei formed an extensive reticulum; the apparently separate 
nuclei of the figure are the optical sections of the broad strands of the nuclear reticulum. 


During this period the ectoplasm becomes very thick at the 
animal pole, and the endoplasm forms a ball that is attached to 
the surface at the vegetative pole by the substance of the polar 
lobe (polar ectoplasm) (Figs. 77 and 78). 

Thus it will be seen that the presence of numerous nuclei or of 
an extended nuclear reticulum, situated between ectoplasm and endo- 
plasm, results in a different arrangement of the ectoplasm and endo- 


242 Frank R. Lillie 


plasm from that found in the uninucleated ova where the nucleus 1s 
central in position. In the multinucleated ova the polar distribution 
of substances of the ovum does not differ essentially from the normal. 
It appears to follow that the curious modes of aggregation of the 
ectoplasm and the endoplasm in the uninucleated ova are due, in the 
first place, to the lack of the restraining influence of numerous nuclet, 
and, 1n the second place, to the fact that, when nuclear influence 1s 
established by the enlargement of the nucleus, it proceeds from a 
single center in place of many. 


yy tit 
9009900006 


Fic. 77 


Fig. 77. Multinucleated unsegmented egg drawn from life four hours and ten minutes after fer- 
tilization. The eggshad been allowed to stand in sea-water one hour and fifty minutes before fertilizing. 
Fig. 78. Multinucleated unsegmented egg drawn from life eight hours after fertilization. From 


a culture in which the eggs had been centrifuged one and one-half hours after fertilization. 


The maintenance of the normal distribution of ectoplasm and 
endoplasm in the unsegmented multinucleated ova results in a 
very perfect homology of the ciliated unsegmented ova with the 
trochophore. A few examples may illustrate this. Fig. 77 is 
from a living egg, six hours old. The polar globules show the 
position of the animal pole; the ectoplasm, endoplasm and nuclei 
(appearing almost like vacuoles in the living egg) have the posi- 
tions described for Fig. 76. At the pole of the egg nearly opposite 
to the polar globules is seen a mass of spherular protoplasm, that 
corresponds exactly in position and appearance to the mesoblast 
cells in the normal embryos, which can very readily be seen in the 
living condition. ‘Thus we can readily homologize ectoplasm, 


Elementar yy Phenomena of Embr yonic Der elopme nt 24.3 


endoplasm and mesoblast substance with those of the normal 
larva. Moreover, they have the same positions with relation to 
one another and to the polarity of the egg as in the normal embryo. 
The main difference is that the absence of cell-walls permits all 
of the endoplasm to ageregate in a single mass; whereas in the 
normal larva it is divided between the cells of the ectoderm, ento- 
derm and mesoderm, though occupying in each kind of cell a 
position next the segmentation cavity. 

In some cases the substance of the polar lobe is partly separated 
from the yolk-mass proper (Fig. 78). ‘This affords a fine demon- 
stration of the existence of such a substance at the lower end of 
the yolk-mass. The presence of this substance is perfectly 
characteristic. It is very adhesive, as union by it of two or three 
ova is very common. 

An interesting addition to the information concerning such un- 
segmented Eated eggs 1s given by staining intra vitam with 
Reueral red. It is then found that there is an Aecumulices of red 
granules in the upper hemisphere that correspond precisely in 
position and staining reaction with those of the trochophore 
(see Part IV, d). The only difference is, that in the unseg- 
mented egg they are more massed, a condition naturally resulting 
from the absence of cell-walls. 

For completeness of demonstration of the principle of germinal 
localization these differentiated, and yet wholly unsegmented, ova 
leave little to be desired. 


c. Literature and Discussion 


Differentiation of entirely unsegmented eggs has been observed 
in two other genera of polycheeta, in Podaee by Treadwell (’02) 
and in Amphitrite by Scott (03 and ’o6). Bullot (’04) failed to 
observe it in Ophelia, though he looked for it, and he therefore 
cast some doubt upon its occurrence. I would only point out 
that the occurrence of segmented ciliated embryos in his cultures 
by no means demonstrates the absence of the phenomenon of 
differentiation without segmentation; for Treadwell, Scott and | 
have all observed the occurrence of ciliated ova both segmented 
and unsegmented in the same culture-dish. Bullot’s figures show 


244 Frank R. Lille 


that, in the unfertilized cultures of Ophelia eggs, segmentation 
was seldom or never, even approximately, normal. 

It is clear from the descriptions of these three authors that the 
occurrence of a certain amount of segmentation 1s more common 
under the conditions of the experiment in the forms that they 
described than it is in Chzetopterus, which seems to be peculiarly 
favorable for the observation of differentiation without any 
cleavage. 

To this list of observations may be added, in all probability, 
those of Bastian (’05) on a peculiar form of differentiation in 
Rotifer eggs. Indeed, unless one is willing to adopt Bastian’s 
explanation that particular species of ciliates may arise by direct 
metamorphosis of rotifer eggs, no other explanation is possible 
than that he has observed cases of differentiation without cleavage. 
His peculiar interpretation so colors the description of the observa- 
tions, that it is difficult to utilizethem. But it is probable from the 
figures that in some cases the eggs form cilia without any cleavage, 
and that, in other cases, the eggs segment or break into several 
smaller parts, each of which then differentiates farther without 
cleavage. 

Scott summarizes his results on the differentiation of unfertilized 
eggs of Amphitrite as follows: “ Under the conditions of the experi- 
ments certain forms of development occur with or without cleav- 
age and with or without the formation of polar bodies in the unfer- 
tilized egg of Amphitrite. Such development takes the form of 
nuckear divisions, the early differentiation of a layer of ectoplasm, 
the growth of cilia, the appearance of vacuoles that are found in 
the fertilized egg of the same age, the development of a brownish 
pigment, the ameboid movements of the cytoplasm that are con- 
nected with cleavage, the ameboid movements at a later stage of 
development that appear entirely independent of cleavage, and 
the change in shape of the egg, which in most cases at least is 
connected with incomplete, arrested or abortive division of cyto- 
plasm. The apical tuft of cilia which is characteristic of trocho- 
phores from fertilized eggs is always absent.” 

Scott justly emphasizes in his theoretical considerations “the 
int-mate relation that exists between cytoplasmic and nuclear 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 24.5 


differentiation; the correlation in development between these two 
factors is very complete where a normal organism results. Inas- 
much as the cessation of development is a cumulative process, 
that is the abnormalities appear in successive transformations of 
the asters and nucleus, we must look upon the cessation of develop- 
ment as due to incomplete reactions between nucleus and cyto- 
plasm.” 

Treadwell (’02) observed that ciliated embryos might arise in 
Podarke without cleavage, and that the differentiation might be 
carried very far in such cases. One case that he describes 1s par- 
ticularly interesting (Fig. 12 of Treadwell); two eggs are fused to- 
gether and are entirely unsegmented. ‘Around one portion of 
the fused mass is a ring of cilia, occupying very much the position, 
with respect to the cell, of the prototroch in its relation to the 
trochophore. Not only are the cilia present, but around the em- 
bryo, underneath the ciliated band, is an area free from the ordi- 
nary pigment of the rest of the cell, but containing granules of 
a faint yellow color, agreeing in this respect Sein with the 
prototroch band of the signin egnaglnone In this embryo there 
is, then, not merely a differentiation—without cleavage—of cilia, 
but of the characteristic protoplasm accompanying these cilia. 
Or, in other words, we have here in the unsegmented embryo, 
not merely a differentiation of cilia, but a differentiation of proto- 
troch cilta.” 

The possibility of a considerable amount of embryonal differen- 
tiation without either nuclear or cytoplasmic division may be 
considered established. ‘This in itself is a fact of considerable 
importance, for it disposes effectually of all theories of develop- 
ment that make the process of cell-division the primary factor of 
embryonal differentiation, whether in the form of Weismann’s 
qualitative nuclear division, or of Hertwig’s cellular interaction 
theory. Further, the phenomenon establishes firmly, as I pointed 
out in 1g0T, the view that the role of cell-division in development 
is primarily a process of localization. This view is a fundamental 
part of the doctrine of formative stuffs and is held as such by 
Wilson, Conklin and others. It may of course be variously 


elaborated, according to whether it is held, for instance, that cell- 


246 Frank R. Lillte 


walls are impermeable partitions, as Conklin believes (O05, p2102), 
or may permit passage of materials through (e. g., by perforation) 
as Vejdovsky appears to have observed. ‘The meaning that is 
given to the term “‘cell-division’”’ would be important in elabora- 
ting the theory; for myself I had meant to include in it only the 
processes of division of the chromosomes, separation of daughter 
nuclei and the origin of the cell-wall between the daughter nuclei; 
movements of the substances within the cell, for instance, would 
be excluded. It seems to me, however, that the elaboration of a 
theory concerning the role of cell-division in development has 
to meet with considerable difficulties. I would agree, in general 
with Conklin, that the localization pattern is something more 
fundamental and constant than the cleavage pattern; yet the 
fixity of the latter in all ova of the determinate type of cleavage 
is a marvelous thing, and cannot be fully explained by any mosaic 
principle (see General Discussion, p. 254). 


V. GENERAL DISCUSSION 


Embryological and cytological study has advanced beyond the 
stage of opinion represented by current theories of development. 
It is generally agreed that the complex phenomena cannot be re- 
duced to the operation of any single factor, and many possible 
factors have already been more or less thoroughly discussed, but 
there is no agreement as to the relative importance of the various 
accepted factors, nor as to their relations to one another in the 
various embryonic processes. 


I. Relation of Nucleus and Cytoplasm 


There can be no doubt that the fundamental morphological 
composition of the protoplasm of the egg of Chatopterus corre- 
sponds to the terms of a granule theory and not to those of a filar, 
reticular or emulsion theory. That a threadlike, reticular or 
alveolar formation may arise in such protoplasm is unquestionable, 
but such conditions are secondary ones, due to particular modes of 
ageregation of the elementary constituents. It is not necessary in 
adopting a granule theory to go as far as deVries and Altmann 


Elementary Phenomena of Embr yonic Development 24.7 


and assume that the granules (pangens of deVries) are the only 
living elements in ‘ne cell, like the bacteria in a zooglcea. The 
microsomes appear to be the primary living elements of the cyto- 
plasm; I would not venture to assert that thes are the only living 
elements.! 

Similarly there can be little doubt that the larger granules, the 
spherules, are products of the microsomes. ‘The origin of some 
of these from aggregations or growth of microsome-like bodies 
could be slainise eed by intra vitam staining; moreover, the 
spherules follow the microsomes in time of origin in the ovogenesis. 

Again there can be no doubt that a very large proportion, at 
least, of the microsomes are of nuclear origin. A great many 
occur in the residual substance of the germinal vesicle, and, in the 
history of the uninucleated unsegmented eggs, swarms of micro- 
somes can be seen to proceed from disintegrating chromosomes 
just prior to the period of differentiation. 

Thus my conclusions correspond to certain of the terms of 
deVries’ theory of intracellular pangenesis (1889), provided that 
we identify the microsomes with deVries’ pangens. ‘This theory 
involves the following assumptions: 

1 That the entire living protoplasm is composed of pangens, which consti- 
tute the only living element in it. The pangens are invisibly small. 

Now the microsomes are not invisibly small, and I regard them 
only as the primary living elements; not as the sole living 
elements. 


2. There are as many different kinds of pangens as there are independently 
variable characters, or independent “factors” composing the complex of the char- 
acters of the species. 

The only cytological evidences that the microsomes are of 
different kinds are (a) that they produce different kinds of spher- 
ules, and (>) that they proceed from nine different sources in 
Chzetopterus, viz: the nine chromosomes of the egg-nucleus. 


3. All the various kinds of pangens of a species occur in the nucleus, and 
those existing in the cytoplasm come from the nucleus. 


1 Indeed a large part of the discussion as to what elements of the cell are living and what are not 
living seems to me to be purely academic, and likely to remain so, until we possess much more 
satisfactory knowledge of the mechanics of the vital processes. 


248 Frank R. Lillie 


This agrees with my opinion concerning the microsomes of 


Cheetopterus. 


4.'. Uhe cytoplasm contains essentially only those kinds of pangens that enter 
into activity init. Thus in each variety of cell the immerse majority of the 
pangens remain inactive in the nucleus, and only those leave the nucleus and 
enter into activity in the cytoplasm chat represent the specific cell characters to 
be expressed. 

On the cytological side there is no evidence to correspond to this 
idea. On the contrary all the chromosomes appear to be active 
in each cell. 


5. Pangens multiply both in the nucleus and also in the cytoplasm. 


This is certainly the case as regards microsomes in the nucleus, 
and possibly also in the cytoplasm. 

The theory of intra-cellular pangenesis has anticipated cer- 
tain observations that may be made concerning the elementary 
phenomena of development. But it is defective in two important 
respects: (I) it assumes a degree of original diversity, and certain 
modes of behavior of the pangens (such as the inactivity of the 
vast majority in each species of cell) that find no justification in 
our cytological knowledge, but only in alleged theoretical neces- 
sit es; (2) it provides no explanation of an essential part of embry- 
onic development, viz: the spatial arrangement of organs and 
their sequence in time, in short, the unity of the organism by which 
alone is the possibility of self-sustenance guaranteed. Weis- 
mann’s theory of germ-plasm proceeds yet farther in unwarranted 
assumptions as to he original complexity of the germ-plasm, but 
includes an explanation or the spatial arrangement of organs and 
their sequence in time, by providing for these factors in the 
architecture of the germ-plasm. 


2 The Original Diversity of Organization 


All theories postulate a certain original diversity or complexity 
of organization as the starting point in embryonic development. 
Now we must inquire what we mean by original? For some the 
fertilized ovum constitutes the starting point; but it is clear, on 
consideration, that it is relatively far removed from the actual 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 249 


origin. Only that can be original which is continuous through the 
series of generations, viz: the germ-plasm in Weismann’s termin- 
ology, which we identify with the chromosome group. The 
entire history of the ovogenesis, as Wilson has repeatedly pointed 
out, forms part of the embryonic development. ‘The original 
diversity of organization is, therefore, contained in the specific 
chromosome group, which observation has shown to be transmitted 
from generation to generation. 

The next question is as to the degree of original diversity within 
this chromosome complex. I would maintain that the estima- 
tion of this should rest primarily on the observable facts and not 
on a mental projection to the lower plane of germ-plasm of the 
complexity of the higher plane of the adult organization. ‘The 
visible diversity of the chromosome complex is usually only quan- 
titative, that is, it consists of a definite number of parts, the in- 
dividual chromosomes. In some cases qualitative differences are 
also observable, for the individual chromosomes may differ in size 
and behavior (Montgomery ’o1, Sutton ’02, Wilson ’05); Boveri 
(02) has shown also for the echinids, by some ingenious experi- 
ments that, though all the chromosomes are alike morphologi- 
cally, the individual chromosomes are probably non-equivalent 
physiologically. There is, therefore, good reason for believing 
that there are at least as many different kinds of original substances 
in the germ-plasm as there are chromosomes. Considerations as 
to valency of chromosomes may, perhaps in the future, tend to 
equate differences in the number of chromosomes in different 
species; recent observations of McClung (’05) have furnished 
some arguments along this line. To maintain that there are no 
more original germinal substances than there are actual unit 
chromosomes may, perhaps, be too extreme a position; but it 
seems to me sounder by far and likely to prove more fruitful as 
a working hypothesis than the assumption that the germ-plasm 
is a microcosm of “determinants” of all the characters of the 
species. 

I do not believe that any considerations as to the potencies of 
the germ-plasm are valid as arguments for the amount of the 
original diversity, because, if the validity of such arguments be 


250 Frank R. Lillie 


recognized, there remains no standard but the arbitrary judgment 
of the individual. Apart from the a prior: difficulty of accounting 
for the phenomena of heredity there is no reason for assuming the 
existence of a large number of original germinal qualities. But, 
seeing that any species 1s as distinct from other species in thestage 
of germ-plasm as in the adult condition, the original germinal 
qualities, whatever they may be, must bear the stamp of the species. 

At present we have no accurate means of estimating the degree 
or nature of the differences between the chromosomes, but I believe 
that certain statements may be made about them that follow 
logically from our present knowledge. In the first place, the 
differences cannot correspond to the differences between organs 
or regions, either of the embryo or adult, because the doctrine of 
the individuality of the chromosomes teaches that each cell receives 
a descendent of each chromosome. ‘The whole economy of nature 
forbids us to believe that each cell possesses arm, leg, brain, liver, 
lung, etc., chromosomes, of which only one* class enters into 
activity in any given tissue, the remainder lying idle. The fact of 
the uniform distribution of all chromosomes to all tissues proves 
conclusively, either that all chromosomes are alike, or that each 
represents some character of the entire organism. As we have 
accepted the view that they are originally unlike, we must adopt the 
second alternative. 

The only observations that we have connecting a particular 
chromosome with a particular set of characters are those of 
McClung (’02) and Wilson (’05), according to which the accessory 
or idiochromosome is a sex-determinant. Now sex is preémi- 
nently a character of the entire organism. My hypothesis is, that 
each chromosome represents some such character or group of 
characters. It is difficult to imagine what such characters may 
be; we need a new morphology for their enumeration, and it is 
to be hoped that this will come from the breeders’ experiments; 
for the only clue that we have to the relation between chromosome- 
characters and species-characters, consists in the parallelism 
between the reduction-phenomena in the germ-cells and the 
Mendelian ratios in inheritance. We might hope, therefore, to 
get at the nature of chromosome-characters by an enumeration 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 251 


of the various kinds of characters that are inherited in Mendelian 
proportions. To attempt this in detail would be too great a task, 
but they include such characters as color (e. g., inheritance of 
albinism, or green and yellow endosperm of peas, etc.), stature, 
pubescence, etc., that are not special characters of particular 
organs but of the whole organism. 

We must be careful, however, to avoid a pitfall here in assuming 
that there is any resemblance between species-characters and 
chromosome-characters. [here is at most only correspondence 
due to genetic connection; and any imaginable degree of knowl- 
edge of the unit species-characters would not furnsh a_ particle 
of information as to the nature of the original unit germinal 
characters. 


3. Properties of the Whole (Principle of Unity) 


If the first step in any theory of embryonic development must 
be certain postulates concerning the original diversity of organiza- 
tion, the second step must be an explanation of the physiologi- 
cal unity of the organism in all stages of its development; for the 
two main facts concerning any organism are that its parts are 
diverse, and that it is, nevertheless, a physiological unit. The 
traditional view, held by many embryologists at the present day, 
is that the physiological unity arises in the course of embryonic 
development by the secondary adaptation of originally inde- 
pendent parts to one another. But this explanation has, in my 
opinion, become untenable, and must be replaced by the view 
that there are certain properties of the whole, constituting a prin- 
ciple of unity of organization, that are part of the original inherit- 
ance, and thus continuous through the cycles of the generations, and 
do not arise anew in each (compare Lillie, ’or, p2 275) Weis= 
mann places this principle of unity of organization in the archi- 
tecture of the germ-plasm, but, as [ cannot accept his view of vast 
complexity of the germ-plasm, neither can I accept this princi-_ 
ple in the sense of Weismann. My own views agree most nearly 
with those expressed by Whitman in his paper on the Inadequacy 
of the Cellular Theory of Development (’93). In this paper 


252 Frank R. Lillie 


Whitman uses the term organization to express what I have termed 
above, properties of the whole or principle of unity. 

If any radical conclusion from the immense amount of investiga- 
tion of the elementary phenomena of development be justified, 
this is: that the cells are subordinate to the organism, which pro- 
duces them, and makes them large or small, of a slow or rapid rate 
of division, causes them to divide, now in this direction, now in 
that, and in all respects so disposes them that the latent being 
comes to full expression. We see this in the adaptiveness of the 
process of cleavage of the ovum (Lillie ’95, ’99; Conklin ’96-’97; 
Meisenhemier ’99), in the regeneration of a starving planarian 
constantly suffering a diminution in the number of its cells while 
its structure 1s increasing in complexity (Lillie ’0o, Schultz ’o4), 
in “regulation,” and in all cases of morphallaxis (Morgan ’oo), 
whether in a protozoan or a metazoan. ‘The organism is primary, 
not secondary; it is an individual, not by virtue of the cooperation 
of countless lesser individualities, but an individual that produces 
these lesser individualities on which its full expression depends: 
The persistence of organization is a primary law of embryonic 
development. 

I believe that this conclusion is strongly reinforced by my 
observations on differentiation without cleavage; for here we see 
the various substances of the ovum marshalled in order, disposed 
in a bilateral arrangement and fashioned in the form of a larva; 


and we see the cilia and other cell-constituents arise in the appro- . 


priate locations—and all this without the need of even a single 
nuclear division. ‘The question arises whether these phenomena 
could not be explained by assuming appropriate attractions and 
repulsions between the elements of the different classes of sub- 
stances, the spherules and microsomes. Although such attrac- 
tions and repulsions undoubtedly exist, and although they appear 
to me to constitute an important elementary morphogenic factor, 
yet I find the assumption inadequate to explain the orderly 
arrangement of the processes collectively. It seemed at first that 
the polarity of the ovum might be explained by assuming that 
the arrangement of endoplasmic substances was produced by mutual 
attractions and repulsions; but it was found that no alteration of 


| 
| 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 253 


the arrangement of the endoplasmic substances modified the 
direction of polarity. Similarly the bilateral polarization of the 
first cleavage spindle and many aspects of the later cleavages 
appear to be independent of any chance arrangement of endo- 
plasmic spherules. 

Thus there is an apparent inversion in the sequence of embry- 
onic phenomena, by virtue of which those characters that we 
would expect to appear late, such as the general form and pro- 
portions of the embryo, manifest themselves first, and thus lend 
to the subsequent phenomena an adaptive aspect; as though that 
which was to be explained preceded the phenomena that could 
alone account for it. Jt 1s obvious, however, that the adaptiveness 
of development does not constitute an explanation, but 1s, on the 
contrary, itself one of the chief phenomena to be explained. 

The principle of unity transcends all forms of visible diversity 
hitherto observed; it is a property of the whole distinct from the 
discernible properties of the parts. Undoubtedly it is capable of 
further analysis, and it must ultimately be derived from particular 
relations and properties of material particles. In embryonic 
development it reveals itself first by axial polarization, second by 
bilateral polarization and determination of the localization pattern, 
third by adaptation in cleavage, etc. Analysis of some of these 
phenomena may some day give a clue to this most mysterious of 
embryological phenomena. 

Morgan (04) has attempted an “Analysis of the Phenomena 
of Organic ‘Polarity,’”’ based on the phenomena of regeneration. 
He concludes that “by means of three assumptions—of toti- 
potence, of heterotropy and of organization-power—we can 
explain the main features in the result. Each assumption is, 
moreover, a direct deduction from an experiment or observation.”’ 
In a footnote he adds, that “the same explanation applies to the 
development of the egg.”’ By “‘heterotropy”’ he means that “the 
material,” though totipotent “‘is somewhat different at every level, 
and that this difference corresponds in kind to the character of 
the body at each level.” The “organization power,” as I un- 
derstand it, is the same as the “centripetal influence’? which, 
“acting from the surface inward, determines the organization of 


254 Frank R. Litlie 


the new parts. ‘The action of this centripetal influence is on the 
new parts as a whole, and determines the relative location of 
each organ.” Later on he suggests that “for want of a better 
term, we may provisionally call the property of living material to 
assume a specific form, the property of formative organization.” 

It is clear that Morgan here offers an analysis of much more 
than is generally included under the term polarity; indeed he 
offers an analysis of regeneration and embryonic development 
asawhole. It is interesting to note that he defines the factor of 
“formative organization’’ so as to be similar to what Whitman 
calls organization simply, and what I have called influence of 
the whole. It would be essentially the same idea, if it included 
heterotropy also, and the latter appears to me to be subordi- 
nate to the former principle. 


4. The Mosaic Theory of Development 


The mosaic theory of development of Roux, excepting that 
inessential part concerning qualitative nuclear analysis, has been 
strongly supported by the facts of cell-lineage and the recent 
work of Boveri (ota), Fischel (03), Wilson (04a and b), 
Conklin (05a and b), Zeleny (05), Yatsu (04) and others. 
Wilson, Conklin and Fischel especially have shown conclusively 
that in mollusca, ascidians and ctenophores, the mosaic character 
of development is based on the principle of germinal localization 
originally enunciated by His. The work of these authors is too 
recent and well known to need review here; I only wish to say that 
I fully accept the results, and am prepared to abide by the theo- 
retical necessities resulting from the assumption that the cleavage 
mosaic in Chetopterus is as definite a mosaic of potencies as it is 
in Patella, where each cell of the cleavage-mosaic up to the thirty- 
two celled stage, or later, differentiates after isolation in sub- 
stantially the same manner as when forming part of the whole. 

But Wilson (o4b) thinks that this ool is inconsistent with 
the conclusion stated by me in 1got, “that the entire organism in 
every stage of its development exercises a formative influence on 
all of its parts,” although he does not doubt “that this position, 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 255 


with proper qualifications, is well grounded.” He then goes on 
to say that “it is clear that the primary localization of formative 
stuffs in the unsegmented egg is essentially an act of the “organism 
as a whole;”’ and even though a complete preformation and pre- 
localization of specific stuffs for every cell and tissue were assumed 
—and I believe with Boveri and Fischel that such an assumption is 
not necessary or even probable—we should not escape the neces- 
sity for assuming such action of the whole.” But in the same 
paragraph, while assenting to Whitman’s saying that “organiza- 
tion precedes cell-formation and regulates it,” he takes issue with 
him on the ground “that the cytoplasmic aggregation or “organ- 
ization” is a progressive or epigenetic process.” 

As I understand Whitman, “organization”? is what I have 
called “action of the organism as a whole;”’ Wilson has either 
understood the matter differently, or has forgotten this, when he 
identifies “cytoplasmic segregation” and ‘organization.’ Organ- 
ization, or action of the organism as a whole, 1s something 
that precedes and regulates cytoplasmic localization as much as 
it does cell-formation. If this fact were clearly kept in mind I 
think that Wilson would find less difference between Whitman’s 
views (and mine) and his own, than he suspects; for the same 
mistaken identification of organization with cytoplasmic localiza- 
tion reappears in the succeeding remarks. 

But I do not mean to assert that my views are identical with 
those of Wilson even when this allowance is made, for he seems 
to believe that the action of the organism as a whole ceases when 
once the localization pattern is determined, and that thereafter 
It is a question of self-differentiation of independently developing 
parts with a certain amount of correlative interaction’ of cells. 


1Tt seems necessary to make a special statement of the opinion that I hold concerning the distinc- 
tion between “action of the organism as a whole’’ and the principle of correlative differentiation. By 
the formerI mean, to use the words of my paper on the organization of the egg of Unio, “‘that the 
entire organism in every stage of development exercises a formative influence on all of its parts.”” 
The principle of correlative differentiation, as I understand it, involves all actions of the intraorganic 
environment, as I expressed it in my “Experimental Studies on the Development of the Organ in 
the Embryo of the Fowl” (1903), that is, “that the rate, degree or mode of differentation of any 
embryonic rudiment is dependent on some part or parts of the same organism external to itself.” It 
will be seen that, as thus conceived, there is an important difference between the two principles, and 
that the principle of correlative differentiation would not include the principle of action of the 
organism as a whole without a considera>dle extension of its usual meaning, which seems to me 
undesirable and likely to be confusing. 


256 Frank R. Lillie 


It appears to me on the contrary, that the action of the organism 
as a whole 1s a continuous process; that physiological unity exists 
in every stage, and not merely sporadically, no matter to what extent 
the mosaic principle may apply. We have to deal with diversity 
in unity, and unity in the midst of diversity as the two funda- 
mental properties of organisms. ‘The phenomena of regenera- 
tion and regulation are incomprehensible on the basis of a 
pure mosaic theory of development. Wilson does not fail 
to recognize this principle here and there, as in cases of 
regulation, but it seems to me that he has not given it sufficient 
weight, and has not kept clearly in mind the distinction 
between this principle and. the derivative condition of germinal 
localization. 

I.would like also to give what seems to me the explanation of 
Wilson’s conclusion that “cytoplasmic segregation is a progressive 
or epigenetic process,” based on the results of Boveri, Yatsu, 
Zeleny and himself. In general it may be said that these authors 
have found that there is a progressive limitation in the potencies 
of parts of the egg from the time of the rupture of the germinal 
vesicle to the eight-celled stage or beyond. Now in Chetopterus 
there is a visible segregation of substances already present, de- 
scribed in Part III, 2, beginning with the breaking of the germinal 
vesicle, and continuing to the third cleavage at least, by means of 
which a new germinal topography is produced. Assuming that 
these substances have limited potencies, as has been demon- 
strated for similar substances in other eggs, the redistribution of 
them would inevitably bring about such a result as these authors 
have described, because the new topography is much more precise 
than the original one. The rearrangement is an “epigenet-c 
process,’ if you please; but only the topography is new, not the 
substances. ‘There is a great deal of evidence in the literature 
that a similar redistribution is characteristic of the maturation 
period in most phyla. On the other hand I believe that a true 
epigenetic process begins with the first cleavage in the production 
of new substances from the nuclei, and undoubtedly plays a part 
in the progressive limitation of potency of the blastomeres; but, 
I believe, only a small part at first. (Lo avoid misunderstanding, 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 257 


I would state that it is my view that the specific formative stuffs 
of the later development arise after the process of cleavage has 
begun.) 

Differences in time of origin of formative stuffs no doubt exist 
in different kinds of eggs, baie I do not believe that the differences 
are so extreme as some would appear to think. Determinateness 
of cleavage may be a measure of the extent and precision of their 
localization prior to cleavage, but I think it must be a very inexact 
measure. I fully agree with Whitman that “cell-orientation may 
enable us to infer organization, but to regard it as a measure of 
organization is a serious error;’’ however, one must keep in mind 
that “organization” precedes and controls “localization,” and 
not confuse the two terms. When this is done I can also agree 
with Wilson that “‘a highly differentiated initial cleavage pattern 
is, therefore, ipso facto evidence” (in some degree), “of a high 
degree of initial cytoplasmic /ocalization”’ (last italics mine). 


5. Concerning Formative Stuffs 


Much has been written of late concerning formative stuffs.’ 
(Morphoplasmic stuffs of Wilson, organ-forming substances of 
various authors.) Unfortunately their actual physical character- 
istics have been but little examined; indeed, in most cases the so- 
called organ-forming substances are really germinal areas prob- 
ably including a variety of substances, and distinguishable only 
by their localization and by the useful but superficial character 
of color. As I have attempted to show, the specific character of 
the substances in Chztopterus is given by the spherules. It is 
certainly reasonable to expect that formative stuffs in other ova 
may be differentiated by microchemical methods. Previous 
observations on this point have been more or less incidental, so 
that generalization would not be profitable. 

According to the theory of formative stuffs developed in the 
preceding Pages, a series of stages characterizes each kind before 
it reaches its definitive croleeien condition. First, its origin, 


1These “formative stuffs’? do not conform to the older conceptions of Sachs, who postulated cir- 
culating fluids of formative function. They are supposed to be varieties of protoplasm that probably 


do not circulate exte sively from cell to cell. 


258 Frank R. Lillie 


in the form of microsomes, from the nucleus; second, the forma- 
tion of granules of a different order from the microsomes, the 
spherules; third, the addition of new nuclear derivatives. (The 
third stage is necessary for there is no final stage of differentiation 
in non- maeleed parts; in the uninucleated ceeomeared eggs the 
third stage 1s clearly marked morphologically by the diffusion of 
chromatin particles among the spherules.) Fourth, definitive 
histogenesis (the morphogenic reaction). 

The theory of formative stuffs doesaway with any ‘“‘determinant”’ 
hypothesis. “Characters” are not due to “unfolding” of the 
“potencies”? of “determinants”? but are results of morphogenic 
reactions between two or more formative stuffs. “The “character” 
need no more be preformed in the reagents (formative stuffs) in 
the case of a morphogenic than in the case of a chemical reaction. 
But I do not mean to imply that the morphogenic reaction is a 
simple chemical reaction, nor that, after it has taken place, the 
character need appear at once in its definitive form. ‘The mor- 
phogenic reaction is probably often of the nature of a response to a 
stimulus, a phenomenon of irritability, and the definitive “char- 
acter’’ resulting may come to expression slowly. 


6. Nuclear Specification 


The logical consequences of the preceding conclusions cannot 
be avoided: it 1s clear that the nuclei cannot produce the same 
kinds of formative stuffs in successive stages of ontogenesis, unless 
we assume that the organism as a whole manufactures different 
things out of the same substances at different times and in different 
places. ‘This appears to be so improbable an assumption that 
there is no escape from the conclusion that the nuclet undergo pro- 
gressive differentiation; in other words, that each successive onto- 
genetic stage is preceded by a corresponding nuclear phase. 

The soealble modes of nuclear specification are: (1) that different 
chromosomes represent successive ontogenetic phases, and that, 
therefore, in any given phase the majority are inactive. The 
objections to this view appear to me to be fatal; for, in the first 
place, there is not a sufhcient number of chromosomes to satisfy 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 259 


the conditions of the hypothesis; and in the second place, there are 
grave objections to the hypothesis of inactive chromosomes in any 
stage of development. ‘This is, however, substant ally the view 
that Wilson presents (’05, p. 292). (2) The second possibility 
would be that all chromosomes are active in the various stages of 
ontogenesis, but that only a part of each is active at any given 
stage. In other words, that each chromosome contains the deter- 
minants of all stages, as Weismann supposes, and that these enter 
into activity successively. This would, however, presuppose an 
enormous amount of original diversity, an idea that we have 
already specifically rejected, besides being open to the general 
objection that the major part of each nucleus would necessarily be 
supposed to be inactive. (3) Both the foregoing are essen- 
tially preformation hypotheses. ‘The third possibility is that the 
postulated progressive development of nuclei is essentially an 
epigenetic process. 

It appears to me that all the well-established physiological and 
embryological data point toward this conclusion. Yet, from 
Nageli down, nearly every writer on the subject of heredity postu- 
lates a degree of original preformation in the idioplasm_ or 
nucleus, corresponding to the amount of morphogenic activity 
supposed to be exercised by it in all stages of the life history. Ac- 
cording to Nageli “every perceptible character is represented in the 
idioplasm by a rudiment” (Abstammungslehre, 1884, p. 23). Ac- 
cording to deVries it is necessary to assume original specific pangens 
for every heritable property (Intracellular Pangenesis, 1889). Weis- 
mann postulates a determinant for every independently varying 
part. The embryologists have usually not entered into this ques- 
tion; but Wilson has gone so far as to state (05a) “that the germ 
consists of two elements, one of which undergoes a development 
that is essentially epigenetic, while the other represents an original 
controlling and determining element. ‘The first is represented by 
the protoplasm of the egg. ‘The second is the nucleus, which, as 
I have attempted to show, must apparently be conceived as a kind 
of microcosm or original preformation, consisting of elements 
which correspond, each for each, to particular parts or characters 
of the future organism.” 


260 Frank R. Lillie 


According to these writers, therefore, all the characters that are 
ever to be impressed by the nucleus on the cytoplasm are repre- 
sented by original preformations in the nucleus. Such a conclu- 
sion appears to me to be practically a negation of the evidences of 
our senses. If such a degree of original diversity is really pre- 
formed in the chromosome-complex, it 1s inconceivable that it 
should not reveal itself to some one of our senses by variety of 
behavior or reaction. Moreover, there is not room in the known 
laws of chemical combination for such diversity of substances 
within the chromatin of a species as these preformation hypotheses 
require. It seems to me that all a priori considerations should 
be ruled out of court, unless we are willing to transform 
biology into a branch of metaphysics dealing with potencies and 
latencies. 

If nuclear specification is to be considered an epigenetic process, 
the causes thereof may be conceived either to be in the environ- 
ment of the nucleus, viz: the body of the cell, or to lie within the 
nucleus itself. In the former case we should have to assume that 
the locations of nuclei in different parts of the original germinal 
topography may act as stimuli on the nuclei to differentiate them 
in various directions. ‘The original cause of cytoplasmic diver- 
sity has been traced back to the nucleus, but I do not think that it 
is necessarily illogical to reverse the order and assume that the 
cytoplasmic diversity may be a cause of new nuclear diversity. 
On the contrary it is a widespread biological phenomenon that 
secretions of the organism react on the organism itself in various 
ways. 

On the other hand I cannot avoid the conclusion that the pro- 
gressive development of nuclei is to a great extent a process of self- 
differentiation. ‘here are certain unavoidable corollaries of the 
argument bearing on the question. If descendants of each orig- 
inal chromosome are transmitted to each cell of the organism, and 
if each chromosome, therefore, represents some character of the 
entire organism, as we concluded before, it must follow that each 
has a series of forms of expression suitable to the successive onto- 
genetic stages. Thus, if chromosome x, for instance, expresses 
itself by pigmentation in the adult condition, it must have had 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 261 


some different form of expression in stages prior to the appearance 
of pigment. There is, in other words, a correspondence between 
the mode of expression of a chromosome, and the ontogenetic 
stage reached by the entire organism. There must, therefore, 
be a progressive evolution of the chromosomes of the same general 
character in all cells; but this need not exclude local specialization 
of the nucle also. 

The differentiation of any particular cell would therefore be the 
result of an interaction between a specific formative stuff or stuffs, 
inherited from previous generations of cells, and a new 
material derived from the nucleus. Variation in either factor 
would give a different result. ‘Thus, for instance, all cells of the 
embryonic epidermis might be supposed to contain similar stufts 
originally; their special lines of differentiation would then be 
dependent on the nature of the final nuclear stuff. If, now, we 
suppose that this may vary with the different external conditions, 
the line of differentiation would vary with the latter.. 

To take a specific instance, we may explain Lewis’ (’04) inter- 
esting discovery that a lens may arise in tadpoles from any part 
of the embryonic epidermis that is brought into suitable relations 
to the optic vesicle, by the hypothesis that the stimulus of the 
optic vesicle causes a different kind of nuclear secretion in the 
epidermal cells acted on than in others. In this case the postu- 
lated nuclear differentiation would result from the environment. 
If such a case may be considered typical, we might perhaps 
generalize by saying that the progressive evolution of chromosomes 
common to all cells is a process of self-differentiation, but that 
local specifications may result from action of the environment. 

According to this conception, therefore, there is an orthogenic 
and epigenetic progressive development or evolution of the somatic 
nuclei during the development of the individual, that is common to 
them all, and in addition local specifications characteristic of parti- 
cular regions and organs. ‘The latter are subordinate modifications 
within the limits set by the ontogenic stage reached in the evolu- 
tion of the somatic nuclei of the individual. “There would also be 
involved in the general conception a theory of continuity of the 
germ-plasm, similar to Weismann’s, viz: that the nuclei of the 


ZOD Frank R. Lillie 


“Keimbahn” undergo neither evolution nor specification, except 
such evolution as may be phylogenic in its character. 

Without developing the idea any farther in this place I think it 
will be seen that the view is not inconsistent with the biogenetic 
law, and that it may be made part of a larger theory of phylogenic 
development. It seems to me that some such ideas as these 
result logically from our present cytological knowledge and, indeed 
they have arisen in my mind in the attempt to interpret current 
cytological conceptions. ‘They are presented in no dogmatic 
spirit, but in the hope of stimulating discussion. 


7: Summary of Discussion 


The main points of the discussion may be summarized thus: 

(1) The chromosome group of the species contains the total 
sum of the material transmitted from one generation to another. 

(2) [he microsomes arise from the chromosomes and constitute 
the primary cytoplasmic element. They produce the various 
formative stuffs. 

(3) The original diversity, by which I mean the actual degree 
of heterogeneity of the chromosome group, is probably relatively 
slight. 

(4) There is an original principle of unity, action of the organ- 
ism as a whole, which expresses itself by axial and bilateral 
polarization (thus determining the segregation pattern) by adapta- 
tion in cleavage, and probably in various other ways, and which 
is continuous from generation to generation. Only its mode of 
expression changes and this in accordance with the stage of develop- 
ment of the organism. ‘The unity of the organism does not arise 
by the secondary process of division of labor. 

(5) Apart from the postulated original diversity and the action 
of the organism as a whole, the entire development is epigenetic. 

(6) Each chromosome probably represents in each stage some 
property of the entire organism. 

(7) Each ontogenic stage is preceded by a corresponding nuclear 
phase; in other words, nuclear evolution is the primary factor in 
the determination of embryonic stages. 


ae 


Elementar 1 Phenomena O Embr YONIC Develo brnent 26 
y ; 3 


(8) Nuclei probably also undergo local specification as a result 
of varying intraorganic environment, and possibly also through 
action of the organism as a whole. 

(9) By virtue of the two modes of nuclear evolution and specif- 
cation, different kinds of formative stuffs arise at successive phases 
of the ontogenesis and in different parts of the embryo. 

(10) The final histogenesis of any cell depends upon interaction 
of the formative stuffs already present in the cytoplasm with the 
last formative stuff derived from the nucleus. 

(11) The nuclei of the “ Keimbahn” undergo neither evolution 
nor specification except such as may be of a phylogenic character. 


264 Frank R. Lillie 


LITERATURE 


ALTMANN, RicHARD, ’94.—Die Elementar Organismen und ihre Beziehungen zu 

den Zellen. Zwete vermehrte Auflage. Leipzig, Veit und Co. 

Bastian, H. CHARLTON.—-On the Occurrence of Certain Ciliated Infusoria within 

the Eggs of a Rotifer, Considered from the Point of View of Hetero- 
genesis. 

BensLey, R. R., ’00.—The cesophageal Glands of Urodela. Biol. Bull., II. 

Bovert, Tu., ’o1a.—Ueber die Polaritat des Seeigeleies. Verh, der Phys. Med. 

Ges. zu Wiirzburg, N. F., Bd. xxxiv. 
‘o1rb.—Die Polaritat von Ovocyte, Ei und Larve des Strongylocentrotus 
lividus. Zool. Jahrb. Abth f. Anat., u. Ontog., Bd. xiv, Heft 4. 
’02.—Ueber mehrpolige Mitosis als Mittel zur Analyse des Zellkerns. 
Vehr. der Phys. Med. Ges. zu Wiirzburg, N. F., Bd. xxxv. 

Buttot, G., ’o4.—Artificial Parthenogensis and Regular Segmentation in an 
Annelid (Ophelia). Arch. f. Entw’m. der Organismen, Bd. 
XVIll. 

ConkLin, E. G., ’96—’97.—Cleavage and Differentation. Biological Lectures De- 
livered at the Marine Biological Laboratory of Woods Holl. Bos- 
ton, Ginn & Co. 

’02.—The Cause of Inverse Symmetry. Anat. Anz., Bd. xxiii, Heft 23. 

’03.—Karyokinesis and Cytokinesis, etc. Jour. Acad. Nat. Sci. of Phila- 
delphia, vol. 12. 

’o5a.—The Organization and Cell-lineage of the Ascidian Egg. Jour. 
Acad. Nat. Sci. of Philadelphia, vol. xin, p. 1. 

’05b.—Mosaic Development in Ascidian Eggs. Journ. Exp. Zool., i, 
No. 2. 

Crampton, H. E., ’96.—Experimental Studies on Gasteropod Development. 
Arch. Entw’m. Bd. iii. 

FiscHEL, A., ’97.—Experimentelle Untersuchungen am Ctenophorenei. I. Arch. 
Entw’m. Bd. vi, 1. 

*98.— I-IV. Thid., vii, 4. 

*99.—Ueber vitale Farbung von Echinodermeneiern wahrend ihrer 
Entwickelung. Anat. Hefte, erste Abth., Bd xi, Heft xxxvii. 

’03.—Entwickelung und Organ-Differenzierung. Arch. Entw’m. Bd. xv. 

Garsowski, Tu., ’04.—Ueber Blastomerentransplantation bei Seeigeln. Bull. 

de |’Acad. des. Sc. de Cracovie. 

HAcKER, VALENTINE, ’02.—Ueber das Schicksal der elterlichen und grosselter- 
lichen Kernantheile. Morphologische Beitrage zum Ausbau der 
Vererbungslehre. Jena. Zeitschr. fiir Naturw., N. F. Bd., xxx. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 265 


His, W., ’75.—Unsere Korperform und das Physiologische Problem ihrer Entste- 
hung. Leipzig, F. C. W. Vogel. 
Lewis, Warren H., ’04.—Experimental Studies on the Development of the Eye 
in Amphibia. I. On the Origin of the Lens. The Am. Journ. 
of Anat., i11, 4. 
Lituiz, Frank R., ’95.—The Embryology of the Unionide, A Study in Cell- 
lineage. Journ. of Morph., vol. x, No. 1. 
’99.—Adaptation in Cleavage. Biological Lectures Delivered at the 
Marine Laboratory of Woods Holl. Boston, Ginn & Co. 
°oo.—Some Notes on Regeneration and Regulation in Planarians. I. 
The Source of Material of New Parts and Limits of Size. The 
American Naturalist, vol. xxxiv, No. 399. 

‘o1.—The Organization of the Egg of Unio Based on aStudy of its Matur- 
ation, Fertilization and Cleavage. Journ. of Morph., xvii, No. 1. 

’o2.—Differentiation without Cleavage in the Egg of the Annelid Che- 
topterus pergamentaceus. Arch. Entw’m., xiv, 3 u. 4. 

’03.—Experimental Studies on the Development of the Organs in the 
Embryo of the Fowl (Gallus domesticus). I. Biol. Bull., vol. 
v, No. 2. 

McCuune, C. E., ’02.—The Acessory Chromosome (Sex Determinant?). Biol. 
Bulle iu. teand. 2. 

’05.—The Chromosome Complex of Orthopteran Spermatocytes. 
Biol. Bull., vol. x, No. 5. 

Meap, A. D., ’97——The Early Development of Marine Annelids. Journ. of 
Morph., xiii, 2. 

’98.—The Origin and Behavior of the Centrosomes in the Annelid Egg. 
Journ. of Morph., vol. xiv, No. 2. 

MEISENHEIMER, JOHANNES, ’99.—Entwicklungsgeschichte von Dreissensia poly- 
morpha Pall. I. Bis zur Ausbildung der jungen Trochophora- 
larve. Marburg, Oscar Ehrhardt’s Buchdruckerei (Heinr. Bauer.) 

’oo.—Entwicklungsgeschichte von Dreissensia polymorpha Pall. Zeit- 
schr. f. wis-. Zool., lxix, 1. 

Montcomery, T. H., ’01.—A Study of the Chromosomes of the Germ-Cells of 
Metazoa. Trans. Amer. Phil. Soc., xx. 

Morean, T. H., ’00.—Regeneration in Planarians. Arch. Entw’m., Bd. x, 1. 

’04.—An Analysis of the Phenomena of Organic “Polarity.” Science, 
IN? Ss, xx, No. 518. 

V. Nacett, C., ’84.—Mechanisch physiologische Theorie der Abstammungslehre. 

Minchen und Leipzig, R. Oldenbourg. 


266 Frank R. Lillie 


Roux, Wm., ’95.—Gesammelte Abhandlungen tiber Entwickelungsmechanik der 
Organismen. Leipzig, Wm. Engelmann. 

ScHULTZ, EUGENE, ’04.—Ueber Reduktionen. I. Ueber Hungererscheinungen bei 
Planaria lactea. Arch. Entw’m., xviii, 4. 

Scott, J. W., ’03.—Periods of Susceptibility in the Differentiation of Unfertilized 
Eggs of Amphitrite. Biol. Bull., v, 1. 

°06.—Morphology of the Parthenogenetic Development of Amphitrite. 

Journ. of Exp. Zool., vol. ii, No. 1. 

STAUFFACHER, H., ’93.—Eibildung und Furchung bei Cyclas cornea. Jena. 
Zeitschr. f. Naturw., N. F., xxi. 

Sutton, W. S., ’02.—On the Morphology of the Chromosome Group in Bra- 
chystola magna. Biol. Bull., iv, 1. 

TREADWELL, Aaron L., ’97.—Equal and Unequal Cleavage in Annelids. Bio- 
logical Lectures Delivered at the Marine Biological Laboratory of 
Wood’s Holl. Boston, Ginn & Co. 

’02.—Notes on the Nature of “Artificial Parthenogenesis’ 

Podarke obscura. Biol. Bull., iii, 5. 

Veypovsky, F., °88-’92.—Entwicklungsgeschichtliche Untersuchungen. Prag, 
Druck und Verlag Von J. Otto. 

Vejpovsky, F., unD Mrazek, A., ’03.—Umbildungen des Cytoplasma wahrend 


> 


in the Egg of 


der Befruchtung und Zelltheilung. Nach den Untersuchungen 
am Rhynchelmis Eie. Arch. f. mikr. Anat. u. Entwick., Bd. Ixit. 
DeVries, Huco., ’89.—Intracellulare Pangenesis. Jena, Gustav Fischer. 
WHEELER, W. M., ’97.—The Maturation, Fecundation and Early Cleavage of 
Myzostoma glabrum Leuckart. Arch. de Biol., xv. 
Wurman, C. O., 78.—The Embryology of Clepsine. Quart. Journ. Micr. Sc., 
NZS volesre: 
’93.—The Inadequacy of the Cell-Theory of Development. Biological 
Lectures Delivered at the Marine Biological Laboratory of Woods 
Holl. Boston, Ginn & Co., 1894. 
Wi1son, E. B., ’99.—On Protoplasmic Structure in the Eggs of Echinoderms and 
Some Other Animals. Journ. of Morph., xv, supplement. 
’o4a.—Experimental Studies in Germinal Localization. I. The 
Germ-Regions in the Egg of Dentalium. Journ. of Exp. Zodl., 
vol. i, No. 1. 
’o4b.—Experimental Studies in Germinal Localization. II. Experi- 
ments on the Cleavage Mosaic in Patella and Dentalium. Journ. 


Exp. Zool., i, No. 2. 


Elementary Phenomena of Embryonic Development 267 


Witson, E, B. ’o5a.—The Problem of Development. (Annual Address of the 
President, New York Academy of Sciences, December 19, 1904.) 
Science, N. 8., xxi, No. 530. 

’o5b.—Studies on Chromosomes. I. The Behavior of the Idiochro- 

mosomes in Hemiptera. Journ. Exp. Zool., ii, No. 3. 

Yatsu, N., ’04.—Experiments on the Development of Egg-fragments in Cere- 
bratulus. Biol. Bull., vi, No. 3. 

ZeveNy, C, ’04.—Experiments on the Localization of Developmental Factors in 
the Nemertine Egg. Journ. Exp. Zool, i, No. 2. 


268 | Frank R. Lillie 


DESCRIPTION OF PHOTOGRAPHS. 


The author is greatly indebted to Misses Catharine Foot and E. C. Strobell for the photographs 
which were taken and printed by them. 

All the photographs are from living eggs. The magnification in all is 237 diameters. 

All the eggs shown were centrifuged, about 3000 revolutions in one minute, and (with the excep- 
tion of A and H) then fertilized. 4 to I inclusive, show the early stages of such eggs up to the first 
cleavage, one and one-quarter hours. J, K, L and M were taken from six and one-quarter to seven 
hours from the time of fertilization. 

The first group illustrates the stratification of the egg substances produced by the centrifuge and 
its relation to polarity, the axis of the egg standing vertical in each case (except in Photograph A, where 
the axis is not known). The large, dense area seen in each photograph is the massed spherules of the 
endoplasm, the smaller is the ‘‘gray cap”’ or residual substance of the germinal vesicle. The clear 
band is seen between them. The photographs show the ectoplasmic layer very distinctly, especially, 
by contrast, just external to the endoplasmic mass. 

Except for the outlines of the polar bodies in B, D, F, G and I, the photographs have been repro- 
duced without retouching. 

A. Living egg, unfertilized, shortly after centrifuging. The polar area cannot be seen, hence 
the relation of the strata to the polarity is not known. 

Band C. Two views of one egg with two polar bodies. Centrifuged 8.45 a. mM. Fertilized 
9-17 A. M. Photographed forty-eight minutes after fertilization. C is a high focus to show the gray 
cap, B a lower focus to show the polar bodies. The plane of stratification is nearly at right angles to 
the axis of the egg. 

D. Centrifuged 8.45 a. mM. Fertilized 9.17 A. M. Photographed 10.13. M. There are two polar 
bodies. The plane of stratification is inclined about go° to the axis of the egg. 

E. History same as D. Photographed 10.20 a. mM. There are four polar bodies, a condition 
observed only once. Plane of stratification inclined about 45° to the axis of the egg. 

F. History similar to D. There are two polar bodies. The polar lobe is beginning to form. 
Plane of stratification inclined about 120° to the axis of the egg. 

G. Same egg as shown in F. Photographed five minutes later. 

H. Centrifuged 9.08 a. m. Unfertilized. Photographed 10.45 a. M. The gray cap surrounds 
the maturation spindle and is therefore ring-shaped. During the time that has elapsed since centri- 
fuging the gray cap has spread out considerably. , 

I. History similar to D. Two-celled stage nearly complete. Polar lobe is nearly at its height. 
Its substance contrasts strongly with the endoplasm. Most of the gray cap is in the smaller cell and 
the larger is filled almost entirely with endoplasm. 

J. Centrifuged 9.08 a.m. Fertilized immediately. Photographed 3.30 p. M., six hours and twenty 
minutes after fertilization. This is a uninucleated unsegmented egg showing the large clear nucleus 
and the chromatin halo surrounding it; distorted and flattened by pressure. 

K. History same as J. Photographed 4.10 p. M., seven hours after fertilization. The chromatin 
halo has risen to the surface where it is mingling with the ectoplasm. Egg distorted by pressure. 

L. History same as J. Photographed 3.52 Pp. M., six hours and forty minutes after fertilization. 
Egg is flattened by pressure. Three-banded condition. The shadow of the ectoplasmic band may 
be seen on the surface of the nucleus. 

M. History same as J. Photographed 3.45 p. m., six hours and thirty-five minutes after fertiliza- 
tion. Three eggs are shown in one field of the microscope, the one to the left is segmented (abnormal) 
the central one is multinucleated unsegmented; the egg to the right is uninucleated unsegmented. 
Shown for comparison of the three main conditions found in such cultures. 


ELEMENTARY PHENOMENA OF EMBRYONIC DEVELOPMENT IN CHAETOPTERUS 
FRANK R. LItLieé 


Journat or Experimentar Zoorocy, Vou. U1, 


REGENERATION OF GRAFTED PIECES OF 
PLANARIANS 


BY 


LILIAN V. MORGAN 


Witnh SEVENTEEN FiGurREsS 


Experiments in grafting pieces of Planarians were undertaken in 
order to discover whether regeneration of short pieces grafted on 
to longer pieces would be influenced by the polarization of the 
longer piece. It has been found, in Hydra by Peebles! and by 
ate and possibly in Tubularia by Peebles, that long pieces 
have such an influence on short pieces. 

After repeated attempts in various ways to graft two pieces, 
it was found that in some species of flatworms, a fair proportion 
of pieces would grow together if held in place between wet pieces 
of paper. Thick tissue paper was used, tough enough to stand 
the necessary manipulation, and thin enough to be almost instantly 
soaked in water, and when wet to cling closely to the pieces of 
worm. A small piece of wet paper is first laid on a flat surface of 
parafhn (hardened onthe under side of a small dish). As it 1s very 
important that the pieces to be grafted should be brought together 
immediately after they are cut, a second piece of wet paper 1s 
held in readiness, and the whole worms are placed with a camel’s 
hair brush on the first paper. The worms are then cut at 
the level required, and the pieces are turned in the desired 
relation to one another, and quickly covered by the second piece 
of paper. If the pieces have not stayed in exactly the right posi- 
tion, they can be shifted to a certain extent by pushing with a dull 


1Peedles, F., 00. Experiments in Regeneration and in Grafting of Hydrozoa. Archiv f. 
Entwickelungsmech., Bd. x. 
King, H. D., ’or. Observations and Experiments on Regeneration in Hydra viridis. Archiv f. 


Entwickelungsmech., Bd. xiii. 


JourNat or Exprrmentar ZodxoGy, VoL. 11, No. 2. 


270 Lilian V. Morgan 


knife on the upper piece of paper. For a moment they will remain 
in place, but in order to keep them quiet for the length of time 
necessary for them to grow together, they are held at the sides 
by the edges of pieces of cover-slip pressed down on the outside 
paper close to the worms. ‘lwo or three fine needles stuck 
through the paper into the parafhn at the ends of the worms keep 
them from moving apart. The small dishes are placed in a 
moist chamber—a large tightly-covered dish with a little water 
on the bottom—and kept in the dark. It is all important that the 
erafting pieces should be neither too wet nor too dry. They 
should be transferred to the paraftin plate with very little water, 
and the surplus drops should be drained off the pieces of paper 
after they are soaked. If the worms are too wet they do not 
stick, and if they are too dry they disintegrate. At the time that 
the worms are cut, the tips of both tails are cut off in order that 
the pieces shall be as quiet as possible. The grafting pieces are 
left in position from eighteen to twenty-four hours. At the end 
of that time, after the cover-slips and needles have been removed, 
a little water is added to float the upper paper, which is then very 
carefully taken off. Since it is dificult to handle very small 
pieces, the worms were cut anteriorly at the desired levels, and 
grafted. Only after the pieces had grown together was the one 
which was to be the short piece in regeneration cut off near the 
union to make it the required length. 

The method was first worked out with Planaria maculata, 
collected at Van Courtland, N. Y., and at Cold Spring Harbor, 
L. I. ‘They are apparently the same species but the two forms 
have some constant and marked differences. Only ten grafts of 
these worms were successful. [he main part of the work was 
done with Phagocata gracilis from a fresh water pond at Fal- 
mouth, Massachusetts. 


A. REVERSED GRAFIS PERFECTLY UNITED 


The anterior cut surface of a short piece was grafted to the 
anterior cut surface of a longer piece in order to see whether a 
reversed head or a normal tail would regenerate at the exposed 
posterior cut surface of the short piece. 


ee 


Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians 271 
© 


The worms were cut at three different levels: (1) At an anterior 
level a short distance behind the eyes near the region of the head; 
(2) at a middle level through the pharyngeal chamber; (3) at a 
posterior level behind the pharyngeal chamber through the region 
of the tail. All possible combinations (except two) of these levels 
were made. In each case, the combinations are designated by the 
initials of the regions through which the cuts were made, viz: 
head, middle, and tail regions; the capital letter stands for the 
longer piece, the small letter for the shorter piece. “Thus, “ Mh”’ 
indicates that the combination is made up of two worms, one cut 
through the pharyngeal chamber in the middle region of the 
worm, the other cut behind the eyes through the head region; 
the two pieces were joined by their anterior cut-surfaces and 
when grown together, the second worm was cut off posteriorly, 
leaving only a short piece (h) grafted to a comparatively much 
longer piece (M) of the other worm. The results of the experi- 
ments are noted in the following table: 


TABLE I 


Regeneration of short pieces of Phagocata gracilis grafted in a reverse direction on to longer pieces 


THe SHort ComMpoNENT PRODUCED AT ITS 
Free Posterior SURFACE OTHER Diep 
1 1 ee REsuLTS | 
i | | 
Head Tail | Closed Knob 
= ee: ie | 
Hh 8 I 5 6 4 
Hm == 10 2) 2 = 
(Ht) -— -- — _— = 
Mh 2 — — — = 
Mm Z 2 10 I = 
Mt = 3 8 I I 
(Hh) -- -- — _— = 
Tm 4 = — —_ = 
Ae 2 I = | = 
18 17 33 10 5 
Hh — 5 —_— I = 
(bh long) | | 


DyD. Lilian V. Morgan 


The last column of the table may be left out of account; it 
simply records the cases which died after being grafted, before 
they showed any results one way or the other. The next to the 
last column will be ignored for the present. The first three 
columns give the cases where the graft was good, the cut surfaces 
were squarely attached, and a certain proportion of the short 
pieces regenerated. 

It is clear that the exposed cut surface of the short piece is a 
posterior surface, from which would normally regenerate a tail, 
if the piece were not attached to the other worm. It has been 
shown in Planaria maculata! that a head is formed at both the 
anterior and posterior surfaces of very small free pieces, but this 
I believe has not been found to be the case in Phagocata gracilis; 
even exceedingly small free pieces of this worm regenerate a head 
at the anterior end, but invariably a tail at the posterior end. 
If then from the free posterior surface of a short piece a head 
develops, the probability is that the short piece 1s in some way 
influenced by the longer piece. 

The table shows that out of sixty-eight cases, thirty-three (nearly 
half) closed in, regeneration was prevented, and the combination 
remained headless. Combinations of this sort are always sluggish 
as compared with normal worms or with worms with regenerated 
heads, normal in direction or reversed; they have lived, however, 
for weeks without showing signs of deterioration. Of the 
thirty-hve which regenerated, about half (eighteen) produced 
heads, and the other half (seventeen) produced tails. This pro- 
portion of heads and tails occurred for the various combinations 
of the three regions of the worms all taken together, but if each 
kind of combination is considered by itself the proportions are 
very different. 

Hh. Short pieces from the head region, reversed and grafted 
to the head region, regenerated in nine cases out of fourteen, and 
of these nine cases eight produced heads (see Fig. 14, 15), and 
only one a tail. Varying the size of the shorter piece was tried. 
In five cases, the shorter piece (h) was left about twice as long 


‘Morgan, T. H., ’04. The Control of Heteromorphosis in Planaria maculata. Archiv f. Ent- 
wickelungsmech., Bd. vii. 


Regeneration of Grajted Pieces of Planartans 272 


as the short pieces in all the cases recorded in the first line of the 
table, but yet much shorter than the long piece (H). In all these 
cases, a tail and not a head regenerated. 

Hm. Ten out of twelve pieces from the middle region, reversed 
and grafted to the head region, regenerated, but all ten without one 
exception produced tails. 

Mh. ‘Two short, reversed pieces from the head region grafted 
to the middle region both regenerated and produced heads. 

Mm. Middle region on middle region regenerated in only four 
cases out of fourteen, and of these, half (two) produced heads, 
and half (two) tails. : 

Mt. No heads were produced and only three out of eleven 
pieces regenerated and produced tails. 

Tm. Four cases regenerated, and all produced heads. 

Tt. Regeneration in only three out of eleven cases; two of the 
three produced heads, and one a tail. 

Of the ten grafts of Planaria, one (an Mh) made a head, prob- 
ably from the posterior surface of h, but the history was not closely 
enough followed, to be certain of the origin of the head; another 
graft (Hh), made a tail at first, but when the new tail had been cut 
off, near the line of union with the longer piece, a head developed. 

It appears from the data for Phagocata that the reversed head 
region readily produces a head; it occurred in all the cases, but 
one, when it was grafted to a head region (if the piece h was 
very short), and in both cases that were tried on the middle region, 
On account of this uniformity, it did not seem necessary to try 
ith. 

The results from the reversed middle region are much more 
variable. In the first place, a much larger proportion of these 
grafts, than of grafts of the head region, did not regenerate at all. 
Of those that did regenerate, all ten grafted onto the head region 
produced tails, half of those grafted onto the middle region pro- 
duced heads and half tails, but all four on the tail region pro- 
duced heads. 

The reversed tail region in far greater proportion than the head 
or the middle region closed and did not regenerate, but the experi- 
ments show that even the tail region, if it does regenerate, may 


274. Lilian a Morgan 


produce a reversed head; for although of six regenerated pieces 
(three grafted to the middle, and three to the tail) four produced 
tails, yet two of those grafted to the tail produced heads. 

In these experiments, there is no regularity as to the kind of 
regeneration of the reversed grafted pieces; but the results show 
that using each of the three regions of Phagocata gracilis as stock, 
with some of the other regions as graft, and each as graft, with 
some of the others as stock, heads may be produced. But some 
of the possible combinations of the different regions developed 
only tails. “The head region as graft on different stocks, and the 
tail region as stock with different grafts produced the largest pro- 
portion of heads in these experiments. It is possible. that when 
tails are produced, the smaller piece, although very short com- 
pared with the longer, may be long enough to be beyond a limit 
within which a piece may regenerate a reversed head. On looking 
over the notes to seewhether there were data exact enough to throw 
any light on the point I found in three cases, where the pieces were 
measured or their length noted and heads were produced that the 
small piece was shorter than in two cases where the pieces were 
measured and produced tails. But another piece (which was 
longer to begin with) closed in, and although it was afterward 
cut twice until it Was very short, it even fen produced a tail. 
The closing-in, in itself, could not have affected the kind of 
regeneration, for other pieces which closed-in, and were after- 
ward cut, produced heads; these were not actually measured. 


B. IRREGULAR CASES 


Having considered the regeneration of a head or a tail at the 
exposed posterior surface of squarely erafted and perfectly united 
pieces, the other sorts of regeneration will be examined. ‘The 
results recorded in the fouten column of the first table are ex- 
panded in the first part of a second table. 

‘There are two main classes of examples: 

I. First, when the original cut surfaces did not graft perfectly 
across the cut ends, surface to surface, but one, or the other, or 
each of the cut ends. was not covered completely by the other 


Regeneration of Grajted Pieces of Planarians 275 
TABLE If 
Phagocata gracilis (Column 4 of Table 1) 
I. Heap rrom Exposep Epcr | Il. Heap at ANGLE or Grarr | 
oF ANTERIOR SURFACE OF From | 
| 
_— ‘UNCERTAIN 
Long piece| Short Both Long piece Short | Both 
Hh — -- —_ No. 94 (k) | No. 3 (k) | No. 1 (k) = 
(one eye) | No. 19 (k) 
No. 10 (k)| No. 95 (k) 
H long h = = = = — No. 43 (t) = 
(four eyes) 
Hm No. 18 (t) | 
No. 89 (k)|  — = = a = oh 
Mm =a No. go (k) _ — = ae a 
Mt > = = — —_— — No. 86 
Planaria Maculata 
feels Mens ee a — = _ e —_ a a eee 
Hh | No. 2 (k) | a No. 7 (t) — — | = No. 4 
| No. 3 (k) (two heads) | 
Hen = = — No. 8 (t) = = ey 
Ht No. 13 (k) — No. 11 (t) -- — | No. 12 (t) — 
| (two heads) (three eyes) | 


1The behavior of the exposed posterior surface of the short component is indicated in the table by 
the small letters ‘‘t”? and “‘k;” (t), when a tail was formed; (k), when the piece closed-in, and formed a 


knob, which did not regenerate in the posterior direction. 


surface, but was left in part free. From such a free surface, a 
head regenerated, and whatever its origin, whether from the short 
or from the long piece, it gradually assumed (in the examples 
whose history was followed) the size and position of a head of the 
large piece. 

In Planaria maculata No. 13 Ht (Fig. 24, B,) the small grafted 


piece was from the beginning but a mere fragment at one edge of 


276 Lilian V. Morgan 


Fig. 1. Phagocata gracilis, combination Hh. A, One day after grafting. B, Eleven days after 
grafting. C and D, Diagrams of the anterior nervous systems of combinations Hh, fourteen days after 
grafting, drawn from several sections with the camera lucida. The worm of Diagram C had regenerated 
a reversed head. £, Diagram of the anterior nervous system of a combination Hh, fourteen days after 
grafting, where h had closed in. a, Line between the large and small components of the grafted com- 
bination. 5, Line between old and new tissue. 

Fig. 2. Planaria maculata No. 13 Ht. 4, After regeneration of the large component had begun. 
B, Eight days later than 4. 

Fig. 3. Phagocata gracillis No. 89 Hm. 4, One day after grafting. B, Twelve days after 
grafting. a, Exposed anterior surface of the long component H. 56, Exposed posterior surface of the 
short component m. », Line to which m had been absorbed forty-one days after grafting. 

Fig. 4. Phagocata gracilis No. 18 Hm. 4, Five days after grafting. B, Ten days after grafting. 
C, Forty-three days after grafting. x, Outline of the new head sixteen days after grafting. 

Fig. 5. Phagocata gracilis No. go Mm. a, Exposed anterior surface of m. 

Fig. 6. Planaria maculata No. 2 Hh. 

Fig.7. Planaria maculata No.7 Hh. C, About sixty-eight days later than 4 and B. 

Fig. 8. Phagocata gracilis No. 94 Hh. A, Twenty-two days after grafting. B, Diagram of the 
nervous system of A. 


Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians 


278 Lilian V. Morgan 
/ 5 


the anterior cut surface of the large piece, and the large piece 
regenerated freely; the small piece was gradually absorbed, and 
the combination appeared seventy-one days after grafting like a 
normal worm. 

Phagocata gracilis, No. 89 Hm (Fig. 34, B,) was amore inter- 
esting case, because the union took place in such a way that part 
of the anterior cut surface of the long piece was not covered by the 
small piece and was drawn over to one side. Regeneration took 
place from the exposed portion, and the new tissue grew much 
longer on the short side than on the long. A head was thus 
formed in the normal position of the head of the large piece. 
The small piece closed in at its exposed posterior surface, and was 
at first large enough to take part in sticking to the bottom of the 
dish, but was gradually absorbed, and forty-one days after 
grafting formed merely a small excrescence on the side of the 
large worm. It had the same appearance fifty-six days after 
erafting. 

Phagocata No. 18 Hm (Fig. 44, B, C,) was interesting because 
of morphallaxis which took place involving both pieces. The 
small piece when grafted covered about half the anterior surface 
of the large piece, and was at first pushed before the large piece 
as it crawled. A narrow head regenerated at the exposed half of 
thesurfaceof thelarge piece. “The small piece gradually changed its 
position and turned back at the side of the large piece, developed a 
tail at its free posterior surface, and crawled with the large piece 
and in the same direction. [he head, in the meantime, became 
not only as broad as the large piece, from which it originated, but 
as broad as both components together. “The worm died before 
it became certain whether the smaller tail was going to be absorbed 
although it seemed to have diminished. 

Phagocata No. go Mm (Fig. 5) was a caseof head growing 
from the partly exposed anterior surface of the smaller piece. 
The head assumed the position of the head of the larger piece, 
and the closed-in posterior part of the small piece formed a slight 
protrusion from the side of the large worm. 

Planarians Nos. 2 and 3 produced heads from part of the 
exposed anterior surface of the large piece. ‘Their origin was nor 


Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians 270) 


followed closely. In one (Fig. 6), there seemed to be two heads 
from the large piece, the small piece being stuck to the middle of 
the cut surface between the heads. 

In Planaria No. 7 (Fig. 74, B, C,) the grafted pieces stuck 
merely by their edges, leaving most of the anterior surfaces of 
both pieces exposed. [wo vigorous worms regenerated, with 
complete heads and tails, and crawled or attempted to crawl in 
opposite directions, attached only by the corners of their heads. 
The eyes at first were very irregular and never became perfectly 
normal, but were, except one of them, multiple eye-spots in two 
clusters in each worm. ‘The worms lived for weeks, one of them 
being sometimes dragged along, while the other, which had a 
better hold, crawled normally. Usually they crawled side by 
side, the head of one being doubled up in the angle between the 
two worms (as seen in Fig. 7/4, B). 

Planaria No. 11, Ht, was a two-headed combination, one head 
and one tail were smaller than the other and seemed to be under- 
going absorption. The origin of the regenerated parts was 
uncertain. 

II. The second class of irregulat grafts comprises somewhat 
different phenomena from the first. 

In Phagocata No. 94, Hh (Fig. 84), the new head probably 
came from the large piece, and it may have originated from an 
exposed edge (as in class I, just considered), but in No. ro Hh, 
and No. 3 Hh, the head seems to have originated from the small 
piece, not from a free surface, but in another way. The union of 
the two pieces in both cases seemed to be perfect, but the line of 
union, instead of being straight across the large piece (Figs. 9, 10), 
was at an acute angle with its long axis. No. 1o died before the 
head was complete, but a head (Fig. g) seemed clearly to be 
developing from the small piece, starting with new tissue that 
regenerated in the angle between the old tissue of the small piece 
and the line of union with the large piece, 1. ¢., the head developed 
from one side of the anterior surface of the small piece, though in 
contact with the large piece. [he head grew in the direction 
which was nearly anterior in relation to the large piece, and the 
posterior surface of the small piece closed in, and became a knob 


280 Lilian V. Morgan 


Fig. 9. Phagocata gracilis No. 10 Hh. A, One day after grafting. B, Ten days after grafting. 
C, Eighteen days after grafting. x, Line where the posterior end of h was cut off a second time. 

Fig. 10. Phagocata gracilis No. 3 Hh. 4, One day after grafting.. B, Later stage. C, About 
twenty days after grafting. D, Eighty-three days after grafting. E, Diagram of the anterior nervous 
system of D. 

Fig.11. Planaria maculata No.8 Hm. 4, Five days after grafting. B, Seven days after grafting. 

Fig. 12. Phagocata gracilis No.1 Hh. 4, One day after grafting. B, Fifteen days after grafting. 
C, Twenty-nine days after grafting. 

Fig. 13. Phagocata gracilis No. 19 Hh. A, Four days after grafting. B, Twelve days after 
grafting. C, Forty-seven days after grafting. 

Fig. 14. Phagocata gracilis No. 95 Hh. A, Fourteen days after grafting. B, Diagram of the 
anterior nervous system of I 4. 

Fig. 15. Phagocata gracilis No. 43 H long h. 

Fig. 16. Planaria maculata No. 12 Ht. 

Fig. 17. Phagocata gracilis, dorso-ventral Hh. A, One day after grafting. C, Twenty-nine days 
after grafting. B, Another specimen, six days after grafting. D, E, F, Diagrams of different positions 
of a dorso-ventral graft after regeneration. J, II, Worms. 4, B, Heads; solid lines represent ventral 


surfaces; dotted lines, dorsai surfaces. 


Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians 281 


IIB 


MAKE! 


KID) A / 


282 Lilian V. Morgan 


at the side of the worm. No. 3 (Fig. 10) was somewhat different. 
The small piece was wedge-shaped, narrowest at the anterior end 
of the slanting line of union with the large piece. From this 
narrow part regenerated most of the new tissue from which the 
head developed; the broad side of the piece lengthened a little 
at first as though to make a tail, but later closed in and was being 
absorbed. ‘he head differed from the reversed heads of Table I, 
in that it did not develop in the middle of the exposed posterior 
surface, but at the thin edge of one side. At one period a single 
eye was seen, and also a second pigment spot, which later dis- 
appeared, and only one eye remained in the narrow abnormally 
shaped head. 

Planaria No. 8 Hm (Fig. 114, B) developed a head at the for- 
ward angle of a slanting graft, but its origin was not studied. 
The small piece made a tail. 

In the examples of a single head (next column of Table II) 
made up in part from both components of the compound worm, 
the head (where the history was followed) originated from an 
edge, perhaps slightly exposed of the large piece. 

In Phagocata No. 1 Hh (Fig. 12) the point of origin of the head 
was the anterior edge of a slanting graft. In No. 19 Hh (Fig. 13) 
new tissue developed from the large piece, especially at the sides 
of the straight graft. But only one edge grew anteriorly, and the 
other side of the graft was thrown back. In both No. 19 and 
No. 1, new tissue from the small piece also regenerated at the line 
of the graft, and took part in the formation of the head, and one 
eye came from each component. ‘The posterior edge of the small 
piece closed. In No. 19, the division between the large and the 
small piece was always marked by a notch at the forward end of 
the head; the compound head was always somewhat lateral 
In position. 

The first steps in the regeneration of No. 95 Hh (Fig. 14) were 
not followed; there resulted from a grafted pair, one worm having 
an eye from each component, and with a knob from the small 
piece at the side. A combination of H long h (No. 43, Fig. 15) 
produced a worm with one head, but with four eyes, and with 
two tails. 


Regeneration of Grajted Pieces of Planartans 283 


Planaria No. 12 Ht (Fig. 16) produced one head with three 
eyes, and two tails, and a pharynx developed in the smaller 
regenerated tail. 

Like the perfectly attached unions, the irregular combinations 
also usually resulted in one way or another in the formation of com- 
plete worms derived from parts of two worms. If the point of 
attachment of the two components is very limited, two nearly 
complete worms may be regenerated, as Planaria No. 7 (Fig. 7), 
but usually, even though regeneration of two worms begins, the 
smaller duplicate parts are later absorbed. If part of an anterior 
cut surface remains exposed after grafting, the point of regenera- 
tion is thereby determined, and the new. head grows at that point; 
it may be from the larger component, Pl. No. 13 (Fig. 2), Ph. 
No. 89 (Fig. 3), Ph. No. 18 (Fig. 4), or it may be from the smaller 
Ph. No. go (Fig. 5): 

If a cut anterior surface is not exposed, new tissue may still 
grow at the line of the graft, and, one edge growing forward, form 
a head, at first lateral, then more and more anterior in relation to 
the larger component. Such a head may be derived from one 
component, No. 94 (Fig. 8), No. 3 (Fig. 10), No. 10 (Fig. 9g), or 
from both, No. 1 (Fig. 12), No. 19 (Fig. 13), No. 95 (Fig. 14). 

In all these cases, whether the head be derived from a free sur- 
face, or from the growing edge of the line of the graft, the posterior 
part of the smaller component (whether it closes in or whether it 
regenerates a tail) is almost always gradually absorbed, leaving 
one complete worm whose body and tail are derived from the 
larger component, and whose head is derived from the larger com- 
ponent or from both, or even from the smaller only. In the last 
case, No. go (Fig. 5), No. 3 (Fig. 10), No. ro (Fig. 9), the tail 1s 
absorbed of that same component from which is derived the head 
of the final combination. In the combination worm, however 
the head may arise, the larger body persists as the body of the 
worm. Part of a second head may also regenerate, but it 1s 
usually abnormal and in a crowded position [No. 43 (Fig. 15), 
PieNo. 12 (Fig. 16)| and perhaps degenerates. One of two eyes 
degenerated in No. 3 (Fig. 10). A siege case of morphallaxis i in 
the formation of the final unit is seen in No. 18 (Fig. 4), where the 


284 Lilian V. Morgan 


head (derived from the large piece) at first occupied but half the 
width of the large piece, the small piece regenerated a tail, which 
grew back to a posterior position before it was absorbed, and the 
fewd widened until it extended across the width of both compo- 
nents. Allthe observed cases of a head being formed at one edge 
of the line of graft without a free surface are of thecombination Hh. 


C. DORSO-VENTRAL GRAFTS 


Another set of experiments was carried out to see what sort of 
regeneration would take place if two worms were so attached by 
their anterior cut surfaces that the dorsal surface of one worm was 
continuous with the ventral surface of the other, 7. ¢., one Worm was 
turned over on its back before grafting together the anterior ends. 
After many unsuccessful attempts to get the pieces to stick 
together, six grafted pairs were obtained from pieces cut anteriorly 
in the head region (dorso-ventral Hh), and four combinations of 
tail regions (dorso-ventral Tt). In one Hh, part of an anterior 
surface remained exposed, and two normal worms regenerated 
and pulled apart. After about a week, all the other five Hh com- 
binations without exception, had regenerated in one and the same 
way. From both flat surfaces at the line of union of the two pieces 
a head regenerated; at the same time the small piece had regener- 
ated a tail at the free posterior cut surface (a normal product), and 
the result of such a combination is that a larger worm crawls 
about carrying attached to it, posterior to the eyes, another smaller 
worm at first shorter than itself. In other words, two worms 
appear to be attached to each other by the “napes” of their necks, 
the larger one carrying the smaller one curled up above it (Fig. 17 
4, B,C). As the smaller one grows, it also tries to attach itself 
and sometimes the combination is so twisted that both worms 
crawl at once. The two cases of dorso-ventral graft obtained in 
Planaria maculata regenerated in the same way. ‘The dorso- 
ventral grafts of Phagocata of the combination It behaved like 
those of abe Eoaeion Hh in two cases where they could readily 
be observed; in the other two, two heads showed, but the pieces 
were very small and not carefully made out. 


Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians 285 


The preceding description of the regeneration of dorso-ventral 
combinations does not fully represent the state of the case; for, 
when the worms are of different colors and the grafted combina- 
tion is more carefully examined, it is seen that the head of the 
crawling worm has its dorsal surface pigmented like the dorsal 
surface of the worm carried above it. In other words, the head is 
a composite of ventral material regenerated from one worm, and 
dorsal material regenerated from the other worm, and both heads 
belong in part to both worms. ‘The relations of the heads to the 
old parts are shown in a diagram (Fig. 17D) of a side view of the 
combination; the dorsal surfaces are represented by dotted lines, 
the ventral by continuous lines. If worm J is loosened from its 
crawling surface, the heads may be thrown in opposite directions 
(Fig. 17£), and now the worms appear to be attached by their 
throats, and worm / appears to possess head B instead of head 4. 
If allowed to return to the position of least resistance, head 4 will 
again be thrown forward from the larger worm /, and the ventral 
surfaces of worm J and head 4 will be continuous in the crawling 
position on the bottom of the dish. Diagram F, representing a 
position midway between D and £, willshow most clearly how the 
composite heads have grown from the upper and lower edges of 
the graft. 

In dorso-ventral grafts, there 1s no chance for the nerves of the 
two worms to unite, and this may be a factor in the production 
of two heads at the graft, and hence of no regeneration of a head 
at the free posterior cut surface of the small piece. From this 
point of view, the results of the experiments fall directly in line 
with the results of the plain grafts of Tables I and II. 

The relations of the nervous system have not been worked out 
in all cases, but many combinations were sectioned and showed 
results of interest. As far as the nerves have been studied, the 
general rule holds that whenever a head is developed, its nerves 
are connected with the longitudinal nerve-trunks of one or both 
components of the graft, and if there is a single head involving the 
smaller component, the longitudinal nerves of the small compo- 
nent are connected with those of the larger component; the single 
head may be a reversed head on the small piece, or a laterally 


286 Lilian V. Morgan 


compound head, the derivation of the head being judged by the 
position of the eyes. If a reversed head 1s developed at the pos- 
terior surface of the small piece, it is found in section that the 
longitudinal nerves of small and large components meet squarely 
end to end and thus the nerves are perfectly continuous, and in 
the new head, sometimes (Fig. tC’) apparently normal “brain” 
and intercerebral commissure are developed; sometimes the 
commissure is very small. If a tail is formed again, the longi- 
tudinal nerves of the two pieces unite end to end, but there is no 
brain or commissure. In one specimen, the longitudinal nerves 
in the new tail are connected at intervals by smaller nerves as 
they are throughout a normal worm. 

In a case of what was supposed to be reversed regeneration 
(Fig. 1D), the anterior nerves are in the small component, 
and with a large connecting nerve are shunted off to one side 
connected with only one longitudinal nerve of the large compo- 
nent; the left nerve of the small component is continuous with the 
right nerve of the large component; the connecting nerve leaves 
the longitudinal nerve at the level where the normal commissure 
should connect the two longitudinal nerves of the large piece; 
but exactly through this region passes the line of the na and 
the normal commissure 1s “ha slightly developed. “This worm 
was thought to be among those which showed externally perfect 
head regeneration, but it may have been confused in embedding 
with closed-in pieces since the eyes have not been found in the 
sections. [he chief interest lies in the one-sided connection of 
the nerves of the larger and smaller components. 

In grafts in which the small piece closed in, and in which no 
regeneration occurred, there was found no brainor commissure. In 
the examples that were sectioned, the longitudinal nerves ended 
abruptly, sometimes with a few nerve branches in place of the 
commissure, and in one case, a third large nerve in the knob that 
remained of the small component (Fig. 1£) was connected by 
a few nerves with one of the longitudinal nerves. 

The union of the longitudinal nerves of the two components 
seems to take place at the time that the pieces grow together; the 
nerve-trunks are continuous in surface view when the grafted 


Regeneration of Grajted Pieces of Planarians 237 


pieces are first released from the papers (eighteen to twenty-four 
hours after being placed together), and in sections of a graft then 
killed, the nerves are found united. ‘The line of union of the two 
pieces of worm can sometimes be seen in sections of worms killed 
some time after the pieces grew together because of the accumu- 
lation at the line of union of parenchyme nuclei. 

In Phagocata No. 94 (Fig. 85) the nerve of the closed-in knob 
of the small piece has formed a closed ring with branches. ‘The 
ring is united with the longitudinal nerve of the side of the large 
component on which the knob 1s attached. 

In No. 95 (Fig. 146), where one eye has developed in each com- 
ponent of the graft, the longitudinal nerves of the large com- 
ponent are connected by a commissure, the eye of the ee com- 
ponent is innervated by nerves from the brain connected with one 
longitudinal nerve; from the commissure a branch passes to the 
nerve ring in the knob derived from the small component, and 
anteriorly from this ring a large branch nerve is developed from 
which the eye, derived from the small component, is innervated. 
Again this branch and the brain in the other side of the large 
component are connected by nerves but slightly developed in the 
normal position of a commissure. 

In No. 3 (Fig. 10o£), where there is but one eye which 1s derived 
from the small component of the graft, the longitudinal nerves 
of the large component are connected by a commissure and are 
also connected anteriorly by the nerve of the smaller component 
of the graft, forming with the commissure a complete ring. “Two 
anterior nerves, which also are connected together arise, one from 
the longitudinal nerve of the large component, the other from the 
nerve ring of the small component, and from this a nerve extends 
to the one eye. 

The fact that regeneration at line of graft a slanting occurred in 
cases of the combination Hh only has not been explained. It 
might be supposed that the material of the head region most 
easily forms a head, the heads being here produced even See aN the 
usual outlet for regeneration, 1. e., a free surface, is blocked by the 
graft. In line with this view would be the case of No. 3, w here the 
reversed head grew, not in the middle of the piece, but at the thin 


288 Lilian V. Morgan 


edge of the piece, that is the point where the tissue was nearest 
to the normal place for head-regeneration. ‘The question as to 
whether (as in Tubularia)'a head regenerates sooner or more 
readily at different levels from behind forward, has not been 
studied. It is shown by the results given in Table I, that 
regeneration of a head even in a reverse direction may take place 
at any level of the worm, and the condition of the nervous system 
in two of the class of cases under discussion suggests that a nerve 
ending freely is a factor in regeneration. In neither of the 
worms sectioned (Figs. $8 and 14) do the longitudinal nerves of 
the two components meet squarely as in the worms with reversed 
heads (Fig. 1). In the cases where the line of graft slants, the 
free anterior end of a longitudinal nerve trunk may then, as in 
dorso-ventral grafts, be a factor in determining the position of 
the new head, although there is no exposed cut surface of either 
worm. 

It would be of interest to know the condition of the nerves in a 
case like Planaria No. 2 as compared with Phagocata No. 19, 
and Phagocata No. 58. In Planaria No. 2 (Fig. 6), it seems that 
two heads have regenerated from the anterior surface of the 
large component of the graft, one head each side of the small 
closed piece. In Phagocata No. 19 (Fig. 13), new tissue grew 
from the large component at both sides of the line of graft, but a 
head developed on one side only, and was later composed in part 
of the smaller component of the graft. In Phagocata No. 58, the 
small piece closed in, the anterior surface of the larger component 
regenerated on both sides of the small piece, and the new tissue 
completely surrounded it, but a head never developed. The 
worm was watched for ninety-six days. 

Reviewing the results as far as worked out, it appears that in 
all cases of graft, the eye and its nerve are developed from the 
same component of the grafted worm. With two worms cut off 
anteriorly there are four exposed anterior nerve ends. When 
these, by the conditions of the graft, cannot unite, two perfect 


‘Morgan, T. H., and Stevens, N. M.’o4. Experiments on Polarity in Tubularia. Journ. of Exp. 
Zool., vol. i. 


Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians 289 


heads are formed (dorso-ventral grafts and a very one-sided 
eraft like Planaria No. 7, Fig. 7). When they can unite end to 
Baa (pertect plain gratts), es do so, and regeneration of the 
anterior ends of the nerves is excluded; if a head is then developed 
it is not the anterior, but the posterior, exposed ends of the nerves 
that come to innervate the eyes, and may even form normal brain 
and commissure, or may regenerate in a way less like a normal 
anterior nervous system. ils irregular orafts, various ways of 
combining the nerves of the sell component with those of the 
large lead) to different arrangements of the nerves of the two sides. 
When the small component ¢loses in, its nerves may end freely 
posteriorly, as in square grafts (Fig. 1£), or may form a ring 
attached in various ways to the nerves of the large component. In 
No. 94 (Fig. 8) the ringas shunted in with one longitudinal nerve 
of the large component. In No. 3 (Fig. 10) it is joined with the 
commissure and with one longitudinal nerve and innervates the 
one eye which is derived from the small component. In No. 95 
(Fig. 14) the ring is connected by a large nerve to the commissure, 
sends a large branch to the eye derived from the small component, 
and the branch is joined by a few nerves with the anterior nerve 
of the other side of the large component. 

The relations between the kind of regeneration (as seen in 
surface views) and the condition of the nervous system is what 
would be expected, though apparently perfect regeneration of a 
head and eyes may be accompanied (as in Leptoplana littoralis)* 
by imperfect regeneration of cerebral commissure and brain. 

Some points in regard to the closing-in of the exposed posterior 
surface of the small component may be further examined. Clos- 
ing-in of the posterior end of the small component never occurred 
where there was regeneration of two heads in the anterior tissue 
of the components, either complete heads [dorso-ventral grafts 
(Fig. 17) and Tabie II, column 3, Planaria No. 7 (Fig. 7) and 
No. 11], or partially complete [Table II, Phagocata No. 43 
(Fig. 15), Planaria No. 12 (Fig. 16)]. It always occurred (except 


1Morgan, L. V. Incomplete Anterior Regeneration in the Absence of the Brain in Leptoplana 
littoralis. Biol. Bull. vol. ix. 


290 Lilian Ve Morgan 


in two cases), Table II, Phagocata No. 18, (Fig. 4) and Planaria 
No. 8 (Fig. 11), where a single head originated in some way from 
the anterior end of one or both components (Figs. 2, 3, 5, 8, 9, 
10, 12, 13, 14). It often occurred in plain square grafts (Table I), 
but never when the shorter component was not very short (Table I, 
H long h). Closing-in of the anterior surface of various kinds 
of Planarians, when the worms are simply cut across without 
grafting, 1s a very common phenomenon; it has often been 
observed and is frequently noted in the literature on regenera- 
tion in different species and genera of Planarians.' Closing-in 
of posterior cut surfaces of ungrafted worms, so far as I know, 
has not been observed. 

It might then appear that when the posterior surface of the small 
component closes in (Table I, column 3;- Table I, columns 1, 2, 
4, 5 and 6, except Phagocata No. 18 and the compound-headed 
worms Phagocata No. 43 and Planaria No. 12), it acts like an 
anterior cut surface of an ungrafted worm. If this were always 
true, it would follow that when the anterior surface of the small 
component takes part in the formation of the head, and at the 
same time, the posterior surface closes in, both surfaces of the 
small component act like anterior surfaces of a cut worm. On 
looking closely at the facts, it will be seen that the closing-in of 
the posterior surface of the small component never occurred 
when all of the anterior surface of the small component took part 
in the formation of the head (dorso-ventral grafts and column 3 
of Table II); in those cases a tail was formed from the short com- 
ponent posteriorly. “The cases where head-formation occurred 
anteriorly, and at the same time closing-in occurred posteriorly 
in the small component are cases of slanting grafts and of all other 
grafts where part only of the anterior surface of the short com- 
ponent contributed to the new head (Table II, columns 2, 5 and 6, 


‘Morgan, T. H., ’98. Experimental Studies of the Regeneration of Planaria maculata. Archiv 
f. Entwickelungsmech. d. Organismen, Bd. viii. Lillie, F. R.,’o1. Notes on Regeneration and Regu- 
lation in Planarians. Am. Journ. of Physiol, vol. vi. Schultz, E.,’02. Aus dem Gebiete der Regenera- 
tion. IJ. Ueber die Regeneration bei Turbellarien. Zeitschr. f. wiss. Zool., Bd. Ixxii. Morgan, T. H., 
04. Notes on Regeneration. Biol. Bull., vol. vi. Child, C. M.,’o5. Studies on Regulation. IV, V 
and VI, Journ. of Exp. Zodl., vol. i. Morgan, L. V., loc. cit. : 


Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians 291 


the origin of the parts was not followed in Phagocata No. 43 and 
Planaria No. 12). The condition of the nervous system in slant- 
ing grafts indicates that there is a difference between the closing-in 
of these posterior surfaces and the closing-in of the posterior sur- 
faces of reversed square grafts without regeneration at the line 
of graft. 

The longitudinal nerve trunks of the short component in the 
slanting grafts (Figs. 8, 10 and 14) did not join end to end with 
those of the long component, but formed a closed ring joined in 
some other way with the longitudinal nerves of the long compo- 
nent. In contrast to this the posterior ends of the longitudinal 
nerves of closed-in pieces of square grafts without regeneration at 
the line of graft ended freely. It may then be true that the closing- 
in of reversed square grafts is like the closing-in of the anterior cut 
ends of ungrafted worms, while the behavior of the nerves may be 
a factor in the closing-in of the posterior ends of the small pieces 
in slanting grafts where part of the anterior surface contributes 
to the new head. ‘The hypothesis that the closing-in of reversed 
square grafts without regeneration is a phenomenon of an anterior 
surface or of one acting like an anterior surface leaves unexplained 
the case of a graft where the short piece at first closed in, but after 
it was twice cut off produced a tail; as the shortness of the piece 
seems to be a factor in the production of reversed heads, it is 
unexplained why the piece when longer acted like an anterior 
surface, when shorter made a tail. 


SUMMARY 


1. Short pieces of Phagocata gracilis, cut from different parts 
of the worm, and grafted by the anterior surface to the anterior 
surface of long pieces from different parts of another worm, some- 
times produce heads, sometimes produce tails and sometimes 
close in at the exposed posterior surface. 

2. The numbers of the combinations cut from different regions 
of the worms, which either failed to regenerate, or produced heads, 
or produced tails, are fully summarized in Table I, page 271, and 
on page 272. The number of grafts that was studied of any one 


292 Lilian V. Morgan 


combination was comparatively small, and with large numbers 
the proportions in the results might be different. 

Longer pieces from the head region of a worm, reversed and 
grafted on the head region of another worm, regenerated tails 
at the exposed posterior surface of the short component. 

3. When (because of imperfect grafting or for some other 
cause) regeneration occurs at some point other than the exposed 
posterior surface of the small component, various results follow. 
Examples of Phagocata and Planaria are fully summarized in 
Table II, page 274, and on page 275. 

4. Ifthe conditions of the graft are such that part of an anterior 
surface 1s exposed (even if only a very small fraction of the whole 
surface) a head is regenerated from the exposed part of that sur- 
face. A head may regenerate from each component of the 
graft (Fig. 7), or only from the large component (Figs. 2, 3, 4, 
6), or from the short component (Fig. 5), according to which 
surfaces are partly exposed. 

5. A single head formed in this way (no matter from which 
component or in what position it originates) gradually grows to 
the size and position of a head of the larger component, the 
smaller duplicate parts of the compound worm are absorbed, 
and one complete compound worm results. 

6. Ina few cases where both long and short components were 
cut through the head regionsofthe worms, a head regenerated at one 
edge of the line of graft, although there was apparently no exposed 
(anterior) surface (Figs. 8-14). 

7. In the last cases (paragraph 6) except one (Fig. 13, and 
one where the original line of graft is not recorded, Fig. 8) the line 
of graft was not straight across the worms, but at an acute angle 
with the long axis, and the head regenerated at the forward angle 
of the graft. The heads were derived from one component 
(Figs. 8 and 10), or from both (Figs. 12-14). 

8. Where the nervous system of these grafts was studied, it 
was found that the longitudinal nerves of the two components did 
not squarely unite, and probably anterior ends of the longitudinal 
nerves remained free. 

g. These heads like those of paragraph 4 acquired by degrees 


Regeneration of Grafted Pieces of Planarians 293 


the size and approximate position of a head of the large com- 
ponent, and the small knobs or tails at the exposed end of the 
small components showed (where their history was followed) that 
they were being absorbed. 

10. [wo compound worms with extra eyes were observed 
(Figs. 15, 16) but their history was not recorded. 

11. Another kind of combination, “dorso-ventral” grafts, 
tried with one combination of Planaria and with two combinations 
of different regions of Phagocata, produced uniform results 
(except in one case of imperfect graft). In the grafted combina- 
tion, the ventral surface of one worm was continuous with the 
dorsal surface of the other. All the examples produced double 
worms, the two heads being at the line of graft, on opposite sides 
of the combination, and each head being derived in part from each 
component of the graft (Fig. 17). 

12. [he heads in these cases regenerated at the normal place 
for head regeneration, but not from a free cut surface. ‘The cut 
anterior ends of the longitudinal nerve trunks must, however, 
have ended freely. 

13. he exposed posterior surface of the small component in 
each case of dorso-ventral graft regenerated a tail, and never 
closed in, and the small component became in a short time a more 
or less nearly full-sized worm, attached to the other worm. 

14. In single-headed compound worms where the nervous 
system was studied, it was found that the longitudinal nerve 
trunks of the two components are connected in one way or another 
and the anterior nervous system and the innervation of the eyes 
are derived from the two components according to which part of 
the new head arise from each. 

15. In square grafts, the anterior ends of the longitudinal 
nerves of the two components unite end to end. ‘The posterior 
ends of the longitudinal nerves of the short component may form 
commissure and brain, if a reversed head regenerates, but they 
may end freely if the posterior end of the short component closes 
in. 

16. In slanting grafts, which regenerate a head at the line of 
graft, the longitudinal nerves of the short component form a ring 


204 Lilian V. Morgan 


connected in one way or another with the longitudinal nerves of 
the large component. 

ii mike nervous system of double worms, like that of ne 7s 
and of dorso-ventral grafts (Fig. 17) has not been studied, but the 
conditions of the graft are evidently such that the longitudinal 
nerve trunks of the two components could not unite, and in accord- 
ance with this is the fact that double worms (not one compound 
worm) are formed. 


New York, January, 1906. 


Contributions from the Zodlogical Laboratory, Syracuse University. 


Pee PERIMENTS ON HE BEHAVIOR OF TUBICOLOUS 
ANNELIDS 


BY 


CHAS W. HAR GMa 


With Turee Ficures 


During the past summer it came in my way to collect 
numerous colonies of the serpulid annelid, Hydroides dianthus, 
which is very abundant in the waters about Woods Hole.  Find- 
ing them to be well adapted to aquarium life [ kept colonies under 
observations upon my laboratory table during almost the entire 
summer, and made such experiments and observations on their 
reactions and various forms of behavior as suggested themselves 
from time to time. 

In connection with the observations upon Hydroides, at the 
suggestion of Dr. |. P. Moore, I also included species of Pota- 
milla and Sabella, though they were very much less numerous 
than the former species. I am under obligations to Dr. Moore 
for identifying the several species. 

As is well known, many of these annelids are remarkably 
sensitive to the slightest disturbances of various sorts, such as 
vibrations, the intervention of shadows, etc. In connection with 
interesting observations concerning the habits of various tubico- 
lous worms Dalyell,’ remarked concerning Amphitrite bombyx 
that “it is impatient of light,’’ withdrawing into its tube in- 
stantly upon the interception of the light. 


1The Powers of the Creator Revealed. London, 1853. Quoted from Andrews’ four Mor ph., 
vol. v, p. 287. 


Tue JourNnat or ExperiMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. 111, No. 2. 


296 Chas. W. Hargitt 


Claparede,' has also called attention to a similar feature in 
Branchiomma_ kollikeri, stating that “it is very sensitive to 
changes in the amount of illumination, for a slight movement of 
the hand at a distance of a meter from the aquarium, causes all 
the animals to withdraw into their tubes as soon as the shadow 
falls upon them. Yet Sabellas, having no eyes, remained im- 
mobile and unaffected.” 

It will be seen from some of the following observations that the 
reference to Sabella is more or less incorrect, since our species of 
Sabella, at least, are quite well provided with eyes, and are also 
subject to the same stimuli as are others, differing only in degree. 


Fig. 1. Several individual tubes of Hydroides dianthus, showing general aspects when 


growing freely upon shells or similar substratum. (Somewhat less than natural size.) 


Similar observations have been made also by Darwin? and 
others upon earthworms, the significance of which is probably 
of the same general character as the former. Still later ob- 
servations upon species of Tubicolide have been made by Andrews, 
Loeb, Nagel, and others, which will be considered in detail in a 
later connection. 


1Annelides Chetopodes du Golfes de Naples. 1868. Quoted from Andrews Jour Morph., vol. v,p. 287. 
?The Formation of Vegetable Mould through the Actions of Worms. 1881. 


Behavior of Tubtcolous Annelids 297 


My observations extended to the following named species: 
Hy droides dianthus, Potamilla oculifera, and § Sabella microph- 
See lane. chiefly the first. “hese were available in considerable 
numbers, and collected from various shells about the docks of the 
United States Fish Commission, from shells, various bivalves, 
Venus, Pecten, etc., from rocks dredged from depths varying from 
two or three fathoms to fifteen to twenty in Vineyard Sound and 
Buzzards Bay. The other species were obtained in part from 
among colonies of Cynthia collected from the docks, and in part 
among colonies of Hydroides. ‘Their numbers were smaller 
than those of Hydroides and the observations correspondingly 


Fig. 2. Colony of Hydroides dianthus growing in complex mass from flat rock base. The 
various aspects of the mouth of the tubes may be easily distinguished, showing vertical, lateral 


and downward relations referred toin the paper. (Somewhat less than natural size.) 


less extended. As will be observed in a later connection the 
limitations of experiments on species of Potamilla and Sabella 
were due in part to their comparative indifference to the various 
tests applied. 


EXPERIMENTS ON HYDROIDES DIANTHUS 


The general character of these annelids is so well known that no 
particular account is necessary. The photographs of several typi- 


298 Chas. W. Hargitt 


cal conditions will show quite enough to make clear the habitat and 
modes of growth. Colonies growing upon shells are seldom large, 
while those growing upon rocks are frequently quite large, often 
including from thirty to fifty, or even more, distinct specimens, each 
inhabiting its own tube, but forming inextricable masses vari- 
ously intertwined, and among which are usually various other 
annelids, corals, hydroids, etc., the whole comprising a most 1n- 
teresting ecological community, as well as a most beautiful display 
of richly varied form and color, rarely surpassed among the almost 
infinite variety of marine life. It may be noted in passing that 
most of this richness and variety of coloration is to be found 1n the 


Fig. 3. Colony of Protula intestinum, from Bay of Naples. The serpentine aspects of 


the tubes referred to inthe paper are easily recognized. The coiled tubes to be seen upon the 
central tube of the colony is particularly interesting as clearly indicating the indifference of the 


creature to the influence of gravity. (Somewhat less than the natural size.) 


annelids themselves, a fact which has been long known and 
commented upon, but little understood. ‘This feature will be 
further considered in connection with the several accounts in 
which it may be involved. 


Behavior of T ubtcolous Annelids 299 


Though I had often observed the general sensitiveness of these 
creatures to sudden intervention of shadows of various sorts, my 
attention was particularly attracted to the matter in the present 
instance by the observation that shadows of even the slightest 
degree, such as those produced by a strip of white paper, or even 
a glass rod, seemed quite as effective a stimulus as those produced 
by the hand or other opaque screen. And this was the more 
noticeable in that the specimens were before a north window, and 
thus in diffused light. 

Having determined upon a series of observations and ex- 
periments, the colonies were arranged in several aquaria, certain 
of which were placed upon a table before a window facing south, 
the others upon a table facing northwest, the latter also in such a 
position that a sixteen candle-power incandescent lamp was 
available for certain experiments at night. 


First Series 


The first series of experiments was made _ to determine exactly 
the character of the stimulus to which the reactions of the worms 
were due; that is, whether they were the result of differences of 
the intensity of light, or whether to the suddenness of the stimulus, 
or the apparently negative effects of shadow stimulus. Loeb (’93) 
had pointed out, in a simple experiment made by opening and 
closing the shutter of a window before which specimens of 
Hydroides uncinata were living in an aquarium, that “only the 
decrease in the intensity of light acts as a stimulus upon the 
animals.’’ 

This experiment I repeated many times and under variously 
modified conditions. The general fact that only the decrease of 
light, or in other words the shadow, is effective was abundantly 
confirmed. Drawing the shade before the window, interposing the 
hand or a sheet of paper, always sufhced to insure the prompt re- 
traction of the animals. Now by continuing the presence of the 
shadow, the worms extended themselves quite as before. When 
thus extended the curtain, or other intervening object, may be 
removed, allowing the increase of light to its former intensity. 


300 Chas. W. Har gitt 


This was variously done, in the case of the curtain, vertically; in 
the case of the hand, screen or cardboard, etc., by either lateral, 
downward, or vertical withdrawal; but in every case the added 
increment of light produced no effect whatsoever upon the 
behavior of the animals. 

The experiment was further varied by employing artificial 
light, an incandescent electric light of sixteen candle-power, hung 
just over the table upon which the aquarium was placed. Allow- 
ing the specimens to become fully extended, which was quite as 
common by night as by day , in darkness as in light, the light was 
suddenly Heche) directly i in their faces, so to speak, but in no case 
was there evidence of any specific reaction. On the other hand, the 
sudden extinction of the light almost always gave the same re- 
sponse as the shadow, as could be seen by immediately turning 
on the light again. 

Again the experiment was made in bright sunlight. Allowing 
the specimens to fully expand under an appropriate screen they 
were then subjected to the sudden increment of full sunlight but 
without the slightest reaction. “This was varied by covering the 
aquarium with a dark box; leaving them in total darkness until 
fully expanded, then quickly removing the box and allowing the 
full sunlight to fall directly upon them gave the same negative 
result as in the former. 

There can be no doubt, therefore, that the reaction is not due 
to simply a difference of light-intensity alone. For whether in 
diffused or direct sunlight, whether in natural or artificial light, 
the response is to the shadow, sudden diminution of light, a 
purely negative condition. But it may well be doubted whether 
this can be properly designated as simply negative phototropism 
or heliotropism. ‘The phenomena are much too complex to be 
explained by any single factor. Details on this point will be taken 
up in a later connection, however. 

The experiments were varied by interposing the screens at a 
distance of from a very few inches, perhaps two or three, to five, 
ten, fifteen, twenty, etc., up to forty or fifty, but without materially 
affecting the results. In the diffused light before a north window 
the shadows from the greater distances were not always equally 


Behavior of T ubtcolous Annelids 301 


effective, but before a south window when the light was bright, 
even though diffused, the results were as certain and effective as 
at the shorter distances. No attempt was made to determine the 
limits beyond which the shadows might become too indefinite to 
act as a stimulus. 

Furthermore, the experiments were still varied by varying the 
time or rate of the shadow movements. It was found that 
ordinarily little difference could be detected as to the reactions 
under a swift and a slow motion as ordinarily made by the move- 
ment of the hand. Butif pains were taken to insure a very rapid 
movement, as by propelling an object before the aquarium by 
mechanical means, such as a spring, it was possible to pass the 
shadow of a small object before the specimens without producing 
sufficient effect to act as a stimulus. Likewise it was possible to 
interpose a screen by such very slow degrees as to avoid any direct 
reaction on the part of the specimens. 

Another experiment was tried to test the effects of a constantly 
recurring, or rhythmic shadow. ‘This was effected by arranging 
a pendulum so adjustable as to secure a rhythm of from about a 
quarter of a second to a full second. With the full second move- 
ment there was more or less constant reaction with each passing 
shadow. With the half second movement it was found that, after 
the first few beats, a considerable portion of the worms failed to 
respond at all; and with the quarter second beats almost all the 
colonies became indifferent to the presence of the passing 
shadows. 

These results are extremely interesting as indicating the possible 
relations of the reactions to protective or adaptive ends. May it 
not be possible that in these rhythmic shadows we have a simula- 
tion of the more or less rhythmic shadows resulting from the 
ripples or wave action, which are phenomena of more or less 
constancy and of course affecting in much the same way specimens 
living in the shallower waters ! 

In connection with this matter of rhythmic shadows it was 
observed that where experiments were repeated with any con- 
siderable frequency specimens sooner or later became somewhat 
irresponsive, often failing entirely to react to any of the usual tests. 


302 Chas. W. Hlargitt 


This I am inclined to regard as the result of fatigue. But it may 
naturally be asked if this might not be equally well explained as 
the result of a condition similar to that induced by the rhythm 
just considered ? 

That it is a matter of fatigue, or a closely related phenomenon, 
rather than the latter, seems to me strongly indicated by the fact 
that it results from any form of shadow stimulus which may be 
applied, such as a vertical or lateral; slow or rapid; made by the 
hand, by a small rod or broad screen; in short, by any or all of 
the various tests applied successively or in any order whatsoever. 
These tests applied singly and at irregular intervals rarely failed 
to produce the appropriate reaction. It was only when repeated at 
such rapidly recurring intervals as to leave insufficient time for 
readjustment that the animals gradually became irregular and 
indefinite in their behavior. At almost every point the results 
simulated so intimately the fatigue phenomena of higher organisms 
as to leave a very strong impression of its similarity, or even 
identity, differing, if at all, chiefly in degree rather than in 
character. 


Second Series 


The second series of experiments was directed to a determination 
of the exact localized areas involved in the various reactions. The 
first step toward a solution was the observation that when the 
crown of gill flaments was directed away from the source of light, 
as would be the case when the opening of the tube, and conse- 
quently the direction of the head was turned from the light, it 
frequently happened that the worm failed to respond to the usual 
shadow stimulus. ‘This was variously repeated and with cumula- 
tive evidence to the effect that the outer, or under surface of the 
gills was distinctly less sensitive to these stimuli than the inner 
surface. This is only what might naturally be expected, since 
it is the inner surface which in expansion sustains direct relations 
to sources of contact or approach from the surrounding medium, 
while on the other hand the outer surface in the unfolding and 
expansion of the plumose filaments would be least exposed. 


Behavior of Tubicolous Annelids 303 


Furthermore, it was found by close observation that the terminal 
portions of the filaments were likewise more sensitive than the 
basal portions, which 1s again what might naturally be anticipated. 
If one watched carefully the behavior of a specimen following a 
retraction of the gills it would be seen that the following protru- 
sion Was a very cautious process, so to speak. ‘The tips of 
the gills would be gradually extended, barely protruding beyond 
the margin of the tube, and in this position the creature would 
wait for a time, the filaments in the meantime actively vibrating, 
as if searching for any source of danger. Next they would be 
protruded somewhat farther, with another pause, and finally the 
entire crown would be protruded and gradually expanded. ‘The 
same process Was frequently observed, but in reverse order, in a 
contraction following a very slight stimulus. 

Following an enrol vigorous stimulus, as a very dense 
shadow or a mechanical disturbance, the worms would frequently 
remain for some time deeply withdrawn, and in again expanding 
would do so with unusual caution. 

This was most strikingly demonstrated in the following ex- 
periment of further attempting to locate the sensitive areas by a 
process of graduated excision of the gill filaments. ‘This was 
attempted by a quick snip with scissors as the specimens were 
quietly expanded near the surface of the water. The operation 
was found, however, to be an exceedingly difhcult one, owing to 
the lightning-like rapidity of the contraction of the worm at the 
slightest disturbance. I succeeded in several cases in clipping off 
about one fourth of the terminal portion of the filaments, and 
found in every case that there followed an appreciable loss in the 
acuteness of the sensory reaction, though as will be seen in the 
later discussion of “mixed stimuli,’ it is not beyond doubt that 
this apparent loss was due to the interposition just here of another 
stimulus, that resulting from the mutilation, which for the time 
being superseded that of the light. This is further suggested in 
the fact that immediatelly following such an excision the creature 
seemed to go through various maneuvers, rubbing the filaments 
over each other in the most curious fashion, protruding and with- 
drawing them in very unusual ways. And it was further ob- 


304 Chas. We Hargitt 


served that within perhaps a dozen hours the worm behaved in 
quite a normal fashion, and seemed to have regained much of its 
former sensitiveness, though its responses were less definite or 
exact than before. 

Attempts to excise further portions of the same gills upon a 
following day showed the operation to be a much more difficult 
matter than in the first instance. ‘This was not only due to the 
same extremely rapid reaction mentioned before, but also to the 
fact that the creature had apparently acquired a degree of caution 
in protruding far beyond the tube, and had likewise apparently 
become much more easily disturbed than before. ‘This became 
even more marked upon a third or fourth attempt. It was as if 
it had acquired an experience which served much as in higher 
animals, and will have a peculiar significance in some of the later 
discussions. 

I finally succeeded in excising entirely the gills down to the base 
of the palps in the case of several specimens by substantially the 
same process, except that it was necessary, in order to secure this 
result to place the scissors about a quarter of an inch below the ori- 
fice of the tube, and then to cut off that much of the tube, and in 
the process “catch’’ the crown of the retreating worm. Ina few 
other cases I forced the worms out of the tubes by thrusting a 
bristle into the smaller portion of the tube, posterior to the worm, 
and then gradually irritating it till it emerged entirely, or so far as 
to allow careful excision of the entire gill area, after which the 
worm was allowed to readjust itself in the tube. 

This last expedient was found, however, to give less satisfactory 
results than the former, as a rule, since it often happened that by 
the process of forcing the worm from the tube there was great 
hability of injuring it sufficiently to seriously interfere with the 
subsequent experiments, or to so modify the reactions as to give 
inconclusive results. 

As a result of these several experiments it was clearly demon- 
strated that the sensory centers are within the gill areas, and chiefly 
the more distal portions, though with the basal third of the gills 
intact the creature still retains more or less of sensory activity. 
But with the entire gill area removed close to the palps, the sensory 


Behavior of T ubtcolous Annelids 305 


power is wholly lost for the time being. It must be stated inci- 
dentally that the capacity of regeneration is well developed in 
these worms, and new tissue begins to make its appearance within 
about two days, and with this regeneration there is recovered in a 
more than proportional measure, the sensory function. The 
entire gill is regenerated in about two weeks, or in some cases 
perhaps less. 

It will be evident, I think, from these several experiments, that 
the sensory area is quite definitely circumscribed in this species, 
and it may also be stated in this connection, that it is equally so in 
the other species included within the present account. But it 
remains to consider the further question, whether there be any 
special sensory organ, answering the structural or functional 
purposes of an eye? In reply it may be said that such organs, 
the so-called eyes, have long been known in many of these worms, 
and that they have been generally regarded as having a visual 
function. ‘The citation from Claparéde in an earlier section of 
this paper is one of many which might be given in support of the 
view. 

On the other hand it is likewise equally well known that there 
are not lacking many species, not only among worms, but ccelenter- 
ates, molluscs, etc., which are totally devoid of anything of the sort 
and are nevertheless, quite as sensitive as species having an abun- 
dant supply of these “eyes.’’ Such is the case, for example, with 
species of Hydroides. So far as known the gills are wholly devoid 
of anything like the “‘eyes’’ of Potamilla or Sabella; and yet the 
latter are, so far as my observations go, much less sensitive than 
the former. 

Andrews several years ago made a critical study of the eyes of 
annelids, comparing them with similar organs of molluscs. Com- 
menting on points of the comparisons, he remarks, ““In_ both 
cases the animals respond very quickly to slight sudden changes 
in the intensity of illumination, bivalves seeking safety by retreat 
within the hard shell, the annelid Saiceertae into firm tubes.’ 
But he continues, “The great number and position of these 
organs suggests doubts as to their usefulness as eyes, the same 
that have been made to the like organs of Arca”’ (op. Citi. 2379) 


306 Chas FLargitt 


This author found no structure which was in any degree com- 
parable to the “eyes’’ in the other species investigated, including 
Potamilla and Sabella. Yet he found that “‘the seat of sensation 
is also in the branchiz; when these are cut off more and more, the 
animal still reacts till nothing but the bases of the branchial stems 
remain.’’ ‘This last point differs slightly from my own obser- 
vations as just given above, though it would seem to be rather 
a difference of degree than of kind. 

It may be mentioned in passing that Dr. A. L. Treadwell, who 
has carefully studied the histology of the branchiz of Hydroides, 
assured me that he was not able to distinguish anything compar- 
able with the eye-like organs found in other species of annelids. 

It seems fairly certain, therefore, that we have in these annelids 
a condition of highly sensory activity, more or less definitely 
localized, yet without any specialized organs, as sensory centers, 
or media of photic coérdination. Whether it shall be found upon 
further investigation that there exists in these annelids certain 
specialized cells, similar to those found by Langdon! in the earth- 
worm, which may be regarded as having a similar function must 
be left for the future to determine, though the probability of such 
sensory cells may be confidently anticipated. 


Third Sertes 


A third series of experiments was conducted in relation to the 
effects of colored light upon the reactions of these worms. 

The general influences of various parts of the spectrum on 
organic processes are too well known to call for special details. It 
will be sufficient to refer to the observations of Lubbock,? Graber,’ 
and Engelmann,‘ as examples of many who have given attention 
to the matter. Others will be cited in connection with particular 
phases to be considered later. 

My experiments with colored light were made with dark ruby 
glass such as 1s commonly used in photographic dark rooms, and 

‘Langdon, F. E.: Jour. Morph., vol. xi, 1895, p. 193, etc. 

*Lubbock: Ants, Bees and Wasps, 1882, p. 186, et seq. 

8Graber, V.: Grundlinien des Helligkeits- u. Farbensinnes der Tiere, 1884. 


‘Engelmann, Th. W.: Ueber Licht- u. Farbenperception niederster Organismen, Arch f. d. gesam. 
Phyisol., Bd. xxix, 1882. 


Behavior of Tubtcolous Annelids 397 


with deep blue cobalt glass. No attempt was made to determine 
whether the glass was approximately monochromatic. However, 
for the purposes involved, it answered fairly well. 

The first experiment consisted in the very simple process of 
interposing a plate of colored glass between the aquarium and 
window, with no attempt to protect other portions of the aquarium 
from the diffused light of the room. As might have been antici- 
pated, under the circumstances no definite results were distinguish- 
able. 

The next experiment was made by enclosing the entire aquarium 
under a dark box, one end of which was fitted with a colored 
glass. [he specimens were left here for about a half hour before 
observations were attempted, and then no differences could be 
distinguished. After another half hour further tests were made, 
but with no appreciable differences as compared with those under 
natural light. In this instance the red glass was used. ‘The 
experiment was varied by interposing blue glass, and again the 
aquarium was left for about the same time, and no differences 
being apparent, the apparatus was left for another hour before 
testing, and again with negative results. Finally it was left over 
the entire night and tested about 8 a. M. the next day, but again 
with negative results. As will be seen by the later experiments 
the apparent failures were due to the fact that immediate atten- 
tion was not given to the matter of tests and observations. 

At 8.15 the aquarium was placed under the red glass 
and testing immediately, it was observed that there were no 
responses to any of the former tests, even perfectly opaque screens 
like a thick pasteboard, gave only negative results. hat 1s, 
reactions were wholly inhibited. After an hour, further tests 
showed that specimens were quite recovered and reacted to any 
of the usual tests quite promptly and energetically. Repeating 
the tests at intervals during another hour they gave the same 
positive reaction which had been shown under normal conditions. 
Removing the red screen entirely, in order to repeat the tests under 
blue glass, it was found that the entire colony was strangely 
irresponsive; none of the usual tests making any impression what- 
ever upon them. Repeating the tests in every way possible failed 


308 Chas. W. Hargitt 


to secure any reaction for at least two minutes, when it was noted 
that the specimens were apparently recovering froma sort of 
dazed condition, and within five minutes all were reacting quite as 
promptly as under normal conditions. 

The specimens were left for about forty minutes under natural 
conditions and were then placed under the red light again and 
carefully observed, and with the same results as in the last 
experiment except that it was found that after the first effects of 
inhibition due to the red light, they were found to gradually 
recover sensitiveness in from five toten minutes. Testing as before 
at intervals of from five to ten minutes during half an hour they 
reacted quite as under normal conditions. [hen again the red 
screen was removed and the animals carefully watched and tested 
as before with the same results, namely, that immediately follow- 
ing the removal of the screen they were found to be quite indifferent 
to any of the usual tests but that in from two to five minutes they 
had quite recovered and reacted as under normal conditions. By 
actual count the reactions were as follows: At the end of one half 
minute none; end of one minute one specimen; end of one and one- 
half minutes four specimens; end of two minutes ten specimens; 
end of two and one-half minutes all but five of the colony of 
twenty-five specimens. These did not fully recover normal 
activity until some six minutes had elapsed. 

At two o’clock they were placed under the blue screen and care- 
fully watched as before. ‘There was at first the same inhibition 
of reaction as in the former case. At the end of two minutes the 
first indication of recovery of sensory activity was noticed. Re- 
peated at intervals of one minute it was found that in four minutes 
apparently the entire colony became normally responsive. ‘Tests 
made at intervals of about five minutes for nearly half an hour 
seemed to show that there was a higher average of response than 
under the red light. At 2.25 the screen was carefully removed 
and tests showed specimens to be acutely sensitive, respond- 
ing to the slightest shadow. 

The experiment was repeated, after adding a new and fresh 
colony to the aquarium, and with almost identical results. 


Behavior of Tubicolous Annelids 309 


Again at four o'clock the same experiment was repeated, and 
withthe same results, the creatures losing for about five minutes any 
sensory discrimination, but quickly recovering it with apparently 
greater acuteness than under natural light. “This was likewise the 
case at the conclusion of the experiment at six o’clock, when the 
screen was removed, every specimen reacting with great prompt- 
ness and vigor. 

‘These experiments were repeated again and again, under vary- 
ing conditions of light, temperature, etc., but with so large a 
measure of constancy as to preclude the possibility of the opera- 
tion of merely accidental or incidental causes. 

As has been noted in an earlier section, the coloration of these 
creatures is very variable, ranging from orange-red on through 
yellow, dull brown, blue and purple of all leis of pomiacen 
to almost pure in some cases. In connection an the experiments 
on colored light occurred the query, whether there might be any 
possible relations between the various colors of the worms and the 
exposure to varying intensities of light > While their arrangement 
in the colonies gave no apparent support to an afhrmative probabil- 
ity, still it seemed well to carefully test the individual reactions of 
the more conspicuously colored specimens. ‘This was accordingly 
done in repeated instances but with entirely negative results. 
Whether in natural light or under the influences of artificial and 
colored light; whether among a group of bright orange-colored spe- 
cimens, or in deep purple colored specimens, not the slightest 
individual difference could be distinguished. And the same was 
equally true whether the experiments were made with colonies just 
taken from the natural habitat or those longin the aquarium. — It 
may be assumed therefore that these rather remarkable colors have 
little or no adaptive relations to light, any more than to selective 
protection. It seems most remarkable that in a single colony of 
these worms numbering perhaps thirty or more specimens, and 
probably arising from a single brood or generation, all this range 
of color variation should be found, and that without any apparent 
significance in relation to their habits or life history, yet such seems 
to be the case here as in many other equally well known instances 
among this and other phyla or classes. 


310 Chas. W. Hargitt 


In this connection may be mentioned an illustration of obser- 
vations cited by the writer in an earlier paper,! namely, the decline 
of color brilliance under the artificial conditions of the aquarium. 
In Hydroides this was quite marked, as was also the case with 
Protula. The colors in which this was first noticeable were the 
reds, orange and yellow, though it was not lacking in the bluish 
tints. These changes were less evident in Potamilla and Sabella, 
the colors of which are less striking. 

It might be inferred from what has gone before that these color 
changes would have little effect upon the sensory responses of the 
specimens, and such was the case. While as already shown, any 
evident decline in vigor was likely to involve a corresponding 
decline in sensory qualities, but there was nothing to indicate that 
there was any necessary relation between these phenomena and 
that of decline of color. 


EXPERIMENTS ON OTHER ANNELIDS 


Reference has been made repeatedly to Potamilla and Sabella, 
but no special account has been given of definite experiments made 
upon them. As already stated, the numbers of these specimens 
obtained were comparatively few, and the experiments much less 
extensive than upon Hydroides. But one species of each was 
found, namely, Potamilla oculifera, and Sabella microphthalmia. 
In size and habit they are very similar. ‘Their tubes are not 
calcareous and rigid as with Hydroides, they are much smaller 
in size, with a habitat among sponges, ascidians, etc. [heir general 
behavior is similar to that of Hydroides, but very much less striking. 
The experiments made upon them were the same as have been 
already described for the other species. In various experiments 
with light of varying intensities—to electric and colored light, to 
touch, etc., they gave no reaction which was not much more clear 
and convincing in the former. ‘To colored light their behavior was 
almost uniformly negative, as was also the case with that of the 
electric lamp. Furthermore, their reaction time was much more 
sluggish and uncertain than in the case of Hydroides. Their 


1Science, vol. xix, 132, 1904. 


Behavior of Tubicolous Annelids 311 


reaction to shadow stimuli, while similar to that of Hydroides, was 
as before very much less striking. It was frequently the case that 
only after some two or three more or less dense shadows had been 
cast over them that a reaction followed, and then comparatively 
slow, and so to speak, deliberate-like. ‘Tactile stimuli, and exci- 
sion of portions of the gills induced, asin Hydroides, apparent 
caution and more promptness of reaction. 

Concerning the behavior of Protula intestimum a brief reference 
has already been made in another section. It may be worth while 
to cite a few additional observations which serve to confirm and 
accentuate those of the other species. Protula is a large tubicolous 
annelid, quite common in the Bay of Naples which he Hydroides 
secretes dense calcareous tubes curiously coiled in serpentine as- 
pects, asshownin Fig. 3. “Uhesetubesare often 180 mm. ormorein 
length and from 5 to 8 mm. in diameter at the opening. ‘The color 
is senele of a more or less uniformly bright orange red, and a col- 
ony of these creatures fully expanded “ise a most brilliant 
picture. 

Their reactions to the various stimuli already mentioned are 
very similar to those of Hydroides, though somewhat more 
erratic. hus it frequently happens that while at one time re- 
sponses to shadows are very prompt and decisive, at another time 
they may be apparently quite indifferent. Perhaps on the follow- 
ing day this may all be changed and the colony acutely sensitive 
to the slightest variation in the intensity of light. Furthermore, 
they seem less adapted to continuous aquarium conditions than 
do species of Hydroides, a lowering of vital tone being more or 
less evident after about a fortnight. ‘This is also associated with a 
decline in the color tone of a more marked degree than 1s the case 
with the former species. And with these changes the reactions 
to various stimuli, especially light, become very uncertain or 
even entirely lacking. 


GENERAL DISCUSSION 


The foregoing experiments and observations bring before us 
certain clearly defined facts which call for further consideration 
and explanation. ‘This may be facilitated, perhaps, by a com- 


212 Chas. W. Hargitt 


parison with similar facts from other sources, some of which have 
already been referred to. Patten,’ whose somewhat extended 
observations and study of the eyes of molluscs 1s well known, has 
offered suggestive comments concerning their reactions to varying 
intensity of light. For example, he has observed that in Arca, 
whose sensory organs are very numerous and highly complex, the 
reaction is much less marked than in species of Avicula in which 
the slightest shadows, such as that of a pencil, causes instant 
response. He makes a similar comparison between Cardium and 
Pecten, the former of which is extremely sensitive to varying light 
intensity but only slightly so in reference to mechanical disturb- 
ances like a jar of the aquarium or the action of waves; while the 
latter is highly sensitive to mechanical stimuli but much less so to 
shadows, though in this case again the sensory organs are both 
numerous and complex. 

Hence he concludes that “ We are led to suspect the presence of 
some other factor which must, when known, account for the 
apparent agreement in functional powers between two organs so 
widely different in structure.’’ 

Concerning Pecten, Patten also observed the phenomenon of 
fatigue, to eee reference has been made previously, finding that 
‘specimens experimented upon frequently become erratic in 
responses, “so that finally, even quite deep and sudden shadows 
may produce only restless or uneasy movements, or perhaps no 
effects at all’’ (ibid., p. 614). 

Darwin’s observations concerning the reactions of earthworms 
to light are too well known to call for more than passing notice, ex- 
cept to point out that this critical observer expressed no hesitation 
in ascribing to these creatures definite light perceptions, nor did he 
hesitate to suggest the cooperation aan coordination of other 
factors in nemo about the varying results obtained in his 
experiments. For example, he observed that, under the conditions 
of feeding, mating, etc., or where they were otherwise preoccupied, 
they either failed entirely to react to the light stimulus, or did so in 
very different degrees of promptness or directness. He even 


IMitt. Zod]. Sta. Neapel, Bd. vi, p. 608, et seq. 


Behavior of T ubtcolous Annelids 3 13 


suggests that at times their behavior indicated changes of nervous 
states, “as if their attention were aroused or as if surprise were 
felt.’” 

Under the later development of the theory of tropisms, and its 
extension to the phenomena of animal behavior, its dominance 
has relegated the earlier views to the limbo of discarded anthro- 
pomorphisms so called. Without essaying any review of the pros 
and cons of this problem, it may be said that already a reac- 
tion has taken place and frankness compels a reconsideration 
of some of these discarded and discredited views. Such a review 
has already been made by Jennings’ so far as it relates to the lower 
organisms, and his conclusions must, it seems to me, be equally 
true for many if not most higher animals as well. 

Among those who have given especial attention to the behavior 
of annelids, the most important are Loeb,’ and Nagel,‘ and to a 
less extent Radl.° 

As is well known, Loeb maintains without hesitation that the 
behavior of these annelids in their relations to light is governed 
by the same laws as is that of plants. Indeed, concerning the 
general orientation of these creatures he contends that they are 
governed by two fundamental influences, namely, gravity and 
light, and in support of this view describes various experiments, 
and gives illustrations as to various details. 

Concerning the influences of gravity as a factor in the case of 
the species i am unable to find any adequate confirmation of 
Loeb’s views. An examination of the several photographs pre- 
sented herewith will, it seems to me, show that neither in the case 
of single individuals nor in that of colonies is there any such uni- 
formity as to position or relation as would indicate the predominant 
influence of any single factor or force. Where single specimens 
are found growing either upon a shell, or upon a solid and fixed 


1The Formation of Vegetable Mould. London, 1881. 

2Contributions to the Study of the Behavior of Lower Organisms. Washington, 1904. 

3Der Heliotropismus der Tiere und seine Ueberlinstimmung mit dem Heliotropismus der Pflanzen. 
Wiirzburg, 1890. 

‘Der Lichtsinn augenloser Tiere. Jena, 1896. 

5Untersuchungen tiber den Phototropismus der Tiere. Leipzig, 1903. 


314 Chas. W. Hargitt 


support, as a rock, the general position is more or less prone, with 
a varying serpentine aspect, as shown in Fig. 1. In certain cases 
the mouth of the tube would be found in one direction, and in 
others quite otherwise. Sometimes the head projected upward, 
often downward. This is better shown in some measure in the 
case of colonies. While here the general tendency of the tubes 
is vertical in growth, many are found diverging laterally, and in 
not a few cases directly downward, as may be seen in Fig. 2. 
In this figure the open mouths of several tubes may be observed 
near the top. This colony is attached to a flat stone, whose center 
of gravity would serve to maintain a constant position, hence the 
variously coiled tubes are evidence of a correspondingly shifting 
behavior on the part of the worms during their growth. This 1s 
perhaps still better seen in Fig. 3, a colony of Protula, several 
of which I studied at Naples. While here as in the former 
figure, the general aspect of the tubes 1s toward the vertical, still 
their lower serpentine coils show the varying conditions of 
orientation at different periods of growth. But if further evidence 
be needed it is found in the tube coiled about the larger central one 
the mouth of which opens almost directly downward. But 
furthermore, colonies kept in the aquaria for nearly two months, 
during which time the tubes had grown almost half an inch, showed 
not the slightest evidence of any response to gravity. They did, 
however, show unmistakable evidence of adjustment to conditions 
favorable for respiration, and for the capture of food; and these 
I regard as of far more importance in determining orientation 
than either gravity or light. 

Zeleny,’ who has reared the larva of these serpulids in connec- 
tion with his investigations upon their regeneration and regula- 
tion, has incidentally recorded brief, but very interesting observa- 
tions concerning the behavior of the young worms in relation to 
“gravity, light and food, but was able to find no general rule, 
although some groups seem to be arranged with respect either to 
maximum food-obtaining ability, or with respect to a lateral 
stimulus of unknown character.’’ 


Biol. Bull., vol. viii, p. 309. 


Behavior of Tubicolous Annelids 315 


While not sufficiently extended to warrant definite conclusions, 
these observations go to corroborate my own as to the impor- 
tance of adjustments in reference to respiration and obtaining 
food. 

Concerning the reactions to light Loeb! holds very decided 
views, and has taken Nagel sharply to task concerning a sugges- 
tion that the behavior of such creatures as responded to the 
stimulus of shadows might be due to something akin to a sense of 
apprehension as to the approach of an enemy or other threatening 
danger. In opposition to this he undertakes to show the necessity 
for the existence of some organ of perception similar to a cerebrum 
in order to enable the creature to possess any instinct, or sense of 
self-preservation, such as Nagel’s suggestion implies. Loeb 
suggests, on the other hand, that the effect of light may tend to 
induce a muscular expansion or stretching so that the worm 
emerges from the tube. If now, a sudden diminution of light 
follows, asin the case of a shadow, an opposite muscular reaction 
is Set up, and consequently the worm rapidly withdraws into the 
tube, and the sudden retraction of the worm is only the expres- 
sion of the rapid extension of the contraction wave of the worm’s 
musculature. 

Of the relations of light to many of the phenomena of life there 
is not the slightest doubt. “That in many cases these relations may 
be so intimate as to warrant designating them by such terms as 
phototropism, heliotropism, etc. But that there 1s any such 
correlations involved in the aspects of behavior under review as to 
warrant regarding them in the light of cause and effect seems not 
only doubtful, but wholly inadequate. As Radl has remarked, 
“Diese Erklarung Loebes ist vielleicht ebenso einfach wie 
unrichtig’’ (op. cit., p. 78). 

In the present case there are many difficulties in the way of the 
rigid application of any theory of tropisms. In the first place 
the widely differing degrees of responsiveness among closely 
related species of slightly differing habitat is difficult to explain by 
this means. Again, the varying behavior under slightly differing 


1Archiv f. d. gesam. Physiol., Bd. Ixvi, 1897, p. 461. 


316 Chas. W. Hargitt 


conditions, as for instance, where specimens have been rendered 
wary under attempts to excise the gills, or where a given specimen 
has been placed for a time so near the surface of the water that 
it has been compelled to extend itself in an unusual manner to 
bathe its gills at all, under which circumstances it often fails 
entirely to react to the ordinary stimulus. In this case we have 
apparently what Jennings has designated as “ physiological states’’ 
namely, conditions in which the internal changes involved in 
respiration perhaps, have served to render the animal indifferent 
for the time to stimuli which under normal conditions are very 
constant and effective in their action, or perhaps as, suggested 
above, complex or mixed stimult. 

-That light of itself is not a determining factor in impelling the 
worms to emerge from the tubes is evident in that this attitude 
occurs 1n darkness as well as in light, by night as well as by 
day. 

Furthermore, it must be recalled in this connection that the 
particular stimulus involved in these observations, as previously 
pointed out, is not light at all directly, but the lack of light, or 
the shadow. ‘The response is, therefore, induced by a negative 
stimulus, if such an apparent paradox be tolerable in relation to 
phenomena of behavior. Of course, it is not overlooked that 
Loeb has designated these and similar reactions as due to “nega- 
tive heliotropism.’’ At the same time it is not clear that in the 
present case we are dealing with phenomena at all comparable 
with those associated with negative heliotropism as ordinarily 
understood. For as already observed, the phenomena are not 
in themselves negative. They are not dependent upon any given 
degree of light, or rather darkness, but to the suddenness of the 
change. 

Rawitz' long ago called attention to exactly this peculiarity in 
describing the reactions of species of acalephs. Referring to the 
suggestion of Drost concerning the effect of shadows as stimuli, 
he says “Ein Reiz kann immer nur von etwas positivem, also in 
diesem Falle vom Lichte, ausgetibt werden, niemals aber von 


'Der Mantelrand der Acalephen. Jenaische Zeit. f. Naturwiss, Bd. xxii, xxiv, xxvii. 


Behavior of T ubicolous Annelids 3 17 


einer Negation. Und Schatten ist eine Negation, die des Lichtes 
namlich,”’ etc. 

But it will be asked, if we are not to regard these reactions as 
due to some form of light stimulus, whether it be positive or 
negative, nor perhaps even to photokinesis, how then shall they 
be interpreted? For myself they seem much better explained as 
activities concerned with protective adaptation than by any merely 
mechanical or mechanico-chemical adjustments. If the tubes 
in which these creatures have their homes have been acquired as 
protective adaptations, and if it be granted that they, in common 
with other annelids, have some sensory capacities, including 
light perceptions, then it would not seem a far call to interpret 
the phenomena in question as sensory reflexes which have become 
instinctive through multiplied generations of struggle with 
various predatory enemies, whose approach cast fateful “shadows 
before.”’ 

It only remains to consider, briefly, the reactions involving the 
influence of colored light. Ina general way the principal features 
concerned under this head have long been known. Experiments, 
have shown that violet rays have a definitely higher phototropic 
quality than have those of lower refrangibility, such as orange 
and red. 

Nagel* has called attention to the same facts in relation to 
molluscs, though he enters into few details, and frankly admits 
the desirability of additional observations. He believes that all 
the colors of the spectrum, except red, have a measure of stimulus 
for such organisms as show, like his mussels, sensory reactions 
to the presence of light. 

Loeb? has also made similar observations, and concludes that 
“since the heliotropic phenomena appear only weakly or not at 
all behind dark red glass, while they occur just as in diffuse day- 
light behind dark-blue glass, the few red rays which penetrate 
the dark-blue glass cannot be responsible for the heliotropic 
phenomena which take place so energetically behind this screen 
but can be due only to the activity of the more refrangible rays.’’ 


1Der Lichtsinn augenloser Tiere, Jena, 1896, p. 53- 
*Physiological Studies, vol. i, p. 18. 


318 Chas. W. Hargitt 


A comparison of these views with the facts cited in connection 
with my observations will show that in several particulars there 
are obvious differences. In the first place, my observations show 
that, though there is an inhibitory quality in the dark-red rays 
when first brought into relation with the worms, it is only 
temporary. Within some ten minutes, or after time has allowed 
the specimens to become adjusted in their sensory activities to 
the new conditions, they become almost if not quite as responsive 
to the shadows asunder normal light. And, moreover, the same 
thing happens under the catches of the dark-blue rays. Here 
hare 4 is a brief period of inhibition, as in the case of the red rays 
though the adjustment is more rapid. | Usually within five minutes 
fhe animals have become accustomed to the new light and behave 
quite as under normal light. I have pointed out in the earlier 
connection that it was not certain that the red glass with which 
my experiments was made was monochromatic, bit since it was 
sufficiently so to render the light passing through it nonactinic, 
it may be assumed that it was sufhiciently so to prevent the 
passage of the essentially blue or violet rays, at any rate in sufh- 
cient measure to act as a stimulus. It seems not improbable that 
the apparent discrepancy concerning the effects of the red rays 
may be due to the fact that a longer time is required in adjustment 
to these rays than to the blue. 

In the next place, and here I have seen no similar records, the 
most striking difference appears in the fact, previously mentioned, 
that following the removal of the red screen there was a most 
remarkable inhibition of all sensory responses under the effects 
of normal light. For several minutes, not more than five, the 
animals behaved in the most striking manner, much as if asleep, 
and recovered sensory activity in much the same way as if awaken- 
ing. Nothing of this sort was apparent when suddenly brought 
from under the influences of the blue rays. Here if anything 
their sensory activities seemed even more than normally alert. 

However, while this behavior seems more or less peculiar and 
singular, I am inclined to believe its explanation may be found 
along the same line of influence which has already been cited, 
namely, the inhibitory effects of the colored light when first applied. 


Behavior of Tubicolous Annelids 319 


Under both the blue and red there was a period of inhibition 
for some time varying in the two cases, as pointed out above, but 
most effective in the case of the red. Recovery of sensory activity 
under these changed conditions involved, of course, some physio- 
logical change in the sensory apparatus in response to the 
peered conditions. Now in the sudden emergence from the 
low refrangibility of the red rays which had involved the preceding 
adjustment there was involved likewise another physiological 
change but in the reverse order. 


SUMMARY 


1. Under the varying degrees of light intensity furnished by the 
sixteen candle-power incandescent lamp, the diffuse light from 
north and south windows, and direct sunlight, the results of all 
experiments involving increased intensity of light were uniformly 
negative. On the other hand, experiments involving a sudden 
decrease of light intensity gave results as uniformly positive. 
However, the behavior does not seem to be essentially comparable 
with that usually designated as negative heliotropism. 

2. Experiments continued without interruption for some time 
gave rise to behavior analogous to that of fatigue. 

3. Various experiments involving the direction of light contact, 
the excision of branchia, etc., showed that the sensory areas are 
located in the branchial filaments, chiefly the inner and terminal 
portions. 

4. When the animals are brought under the influence of red 
and blue light sensory activities are for a time inhibited. ‘This 
is more marked under the red than the blue. On the other hand, 
when brought suddenly from the colored light into normal white 
light there is apparently an intensified sensory acuteness due to 
the blue light, while the effects of the red seem to have been just 
the opposite, namely, to positively inhibit sensory response for a 
period of from two to five minutes. 

5. Species of Potamilla and Sabella behave in essentially the 
same manner as do those of Hydroides, though with less acuteness, 
promptness and certainty. This is somewhat remarkable since 


320 Chas. W. Hargitt 


bd 


these species are abundantly supplied with “eyes,’’ which are 
entirely lacking in Hydroides. 

6. Species of Protula likewise behave in essentially the same 
manner as do the others, though apparently somewhat more 
erratic and uncertain in their reactions. 

7. [he experiments tend to discredit the theory of tropisms, 
since no single factor, such as light or gravity, furnishes an 
adequate explanation. 

8. The experiments strongly suggest the presence in the gill 
filaments of these creatures of sensory cells and nerve endings 
through which are coordinated by means of nervous centers the 
various aspects of behavior toward protective or physiological 
ends. 


INHERITANCE OF DICHROMATISM IN LINA AND 
GAs FROMUDEA 


ISABEL McCRACKEN 


In a series of experiments with the dichromatic species of beetle, 
Lina lapponica, as described in a previous paper,’ it was deter- 
mined by breeding through a series of four generations that the 
behavior in heredity of the alternate characters of the species 
spotted-brown” and “black” follows, in general, Mendelian 
behavior. 

This was evidenced by the fact that the broods from a first 
cross between spotted individuals and black individuals are either 
wholly spotted or the broods are made up of individuals, some of 
which (and the majority) are spotted, and some are black. In 
other words, it appeared that “spotted-brown”’ was either com- 
pletely or partially dominant, depending upon unknown causes. 

That black is a recessive character was evidenced by the fact 
that it frequently did not appear in a first cross between S and B, 
in which case it was extracted in the next generation and thereafter 
bred true. 

The proportions between alternate characters from hybrids in 
Lina showed no parallelism with typical Mendelian proportions, 
however, and called for further experimentation. 


oe 


EXPERIMENTS WITH LINA 


I have during the past season (1905), sought to determine the exact 
proportion of dominant to recessive in successive generations bred 
each generation from hybrid dominant parents. ‘The individuals 
used as parents in each generation were spotted individuals, from 
broods composed of both spotted and black individuals, that had 


1 Inheritance of Dichromatism in Lina lapponica, Journal of Experimental Zodlogy, vol. ii, 
pp. 119-136. 


Journat or Experimentat Zoétocy, Vor. 11, No. 2. 


222 Isabel McCracken 


proven their hybrid character by producing black as well as spotted 
offspring. 

Diagram I shows character of matings and color character of off- 
spring throughout four generations from hybrid parents. _S stand- 
ing alone signifies a brood in which S was completely dominant, 
S B standing side by side signifies a brood consisting of both 
spotted and black individuals, the spotted being present in larger 
numbers (or partially dominant). 


Sirs Saxis 
[sexs] eel 
| 
S and S B Smandesmb 
eo SPP aA0 
S andS B Seb 
| : | 
Sand’ SB SeB 
| | 
| 
Sand SB 


Diagram 1. Showing pedigree through four generations from hybrid S. 


Inthe past year’s experiments, as in the previous ones, the hybrid S 
parents of the first generation produced two sorts of broods, broods 
in which S was completely dominant and broods in which S was 
partially dominant. In 1904, by breeding consecutively from 
completely dominant S broods, pure S broods were obtained in 
the “third generation. Breeding in 1905 consecutively from 
hybrid S parents chosen from broods in which S dominated 
partially, in each generation both ‘sorts of broods were obtained, 
that is, completely dominant S broods and partially dominant S 
broods. In the partially dominant S broods the proportion of 
dominant to recessive gradually increased through the fifth genera- 
tion when hibernation began. 

It may be noted here that last year (1905) there occurred in 
broods both from dominant and recessive parents, an occasional 
wholly melanic individual, thorax as well as wing covers being 
totally black. Such individuals were utilized for a study of 
“sport-variation’”’ in heredity. 


Inheritance of Dichromatism 323 


Table I gives a summary of the data in each generation from 
S x S parents, including total number of broods, number of broads 
in which S lease completely, number of broods in which S$ 
dominated partially (designated as “ mixed broods’’), total number 
of melanic or sport individuals, total number of individuals and 
proportion of S to B in partially dominant 5 broods. 


TABLE I 
Total | Propor- 
No. Com- Total Total Total tion of 
- ra pletely No. of No. of No: of, | S28 
ate Domi- Mixed Melanic | Individ- | in Mixed 
eee nant S Broods. Sports. uals. Broods. 
Broods 
Pirsh GER eTAhlOM! ecto e--1= 118 13 105 10 | 3442 | Bas BU 
Second generation ......... 45 27 18 I * 1050 | Heit Bal 
Third generation .......... a2 121 21 I AZO Senet 
Fourth generation.......... 19 16 3 ° 549 |. 26nia 


The increasing proportion of spotted individuals produced in 
successive generations shows a progressive dominance of the S 
character. That B might eventually be excluded from the partial 
dominant S line seems highly probable, but conclusive evidence 
cannot be obtained on this point until success attends hibernation. 

It seems, however, that we may consider S as dominant in a 
greater sense than that denoted by Mendelian terminology. It 1s 
progressively dominant in a way not accounted for by Mendelian 
law. 


EXPERIMENTS WITH GASTROIDEA 


The relation of dominant to recessive in alternative characters 
of the kind studied is more clearly brought out in the following 
series of experiments with Gastroidea dissimilis. 

Gastroidea dissimilis is a dichromatic species belonging to the 
same family of beetles as Lina lapponica, the Chrysomelidz. 

When an adult of this species first issues from its pupal case, 
the wing covers are softand yellow. During the process of harden- 


324 Isabel McCracken 


ing all individuals become at first a smoky brown and later black. 
From this black condition the individual color-development pro- 
ceeds gradually in one of two directions. In one series of indi- 
viduals there is a gradual deepening of the pigment to a shiny 
permanent deep blue-black condition. In the other series there is a 
passing from the black condition to a shiny permanent bright 
green condition. ‘The color is fixed in each series within two or 
three hours of issuing. 

We find, therefore, in Gastroidea, in its color-development, the 
same condition found in Lina, that is, a primary condition through 
which all individuals pass (and in which one series of individuals 
remains), and a secondary condition into which one series of 
individuals only passes. 

The two colors, black and green, are therefore represented in the 
species, no intermediates having been observed during the course 
of the experiment involving the handling of many thousands of 
individuals (about 26,000). The beetle is small, about 5 mm. 
long, feeds on dock or rhubarb and breeds from late in February 
until early in September, producing normally five or six genera- 
tions ina year. Under laboratory conditions the breeding season 
was prolonged through December and seven generations were 
reared from a lot collected March 16, 1905. 

The experiment began with two hundred adults, one hundred 
black, one hundred green. ‘These had been mating out of doors, 
blacks and greens promiscuously, and ovipositing had begun. 
It was comparatively certain that there were no pure bred indi- 
viduals in the collection, since presumably crossmating had been 
going on ever since dichromatism had been established in the 
species. 

The first eggs were obtained March 16. These hatched March 
24 and issued as adults April 23. At this season, therefore, mature 
adults were obtained in less than forty days from the egg. 
Females oviposited five or six days after maturing, each female 
ovipositing ten to fifteen egg masses during the following few 
weeks with from twenty to thirty eggs in a mass, frequently run- 
ning as high as fifty eggs in the mass. With this material as a 
nucleus, | sought to determine whether there was a behavior of 


Inheritance of Dichromatism 325 


the color-alternatives in heredity corresponding to that of Lina 
lapponica. My plan was to breed successively from completely 
dominant hybrids, if such were found to be present, to breed suc- 
cessively from partially dominant hybrids, and, finally, from 
recessives that had from one to several pair of dominant ancestors. 

In the following tables results in total are tabulated for seven 
generations of laboratory reared lots. 

Table II gives the data of the first generation reared from out- 
door collected adults. Individuals used as parents for these data 
were confined in the laboratory with mates of similar color. Since 
it is altogether possible that some or all of the females had mated 
with individuals of alternate color before laboratory isolation, the 
results indicated give little more than known parentage for second 
generation data. 


TABLE II 


First Generation 


Coleen Namber ss |) Number | Number | Total No. 
Character of of of of Proportion of B : G in 
of | Green Black | Mixed Indi- Mixed Broods. 
Matings. Broods. Broods. | Broods. viduals. 
GXG 33 ° | 6% 580 | 1 B in each of six G broods. 
Beeb al fo) | 2 39 1207 ii), 6) 1 


*One B individual in each six broods. 


The single black individuals occurring in otherwise green broods 
of G x G are possibly due to earlier matings of the female parents. 
The data from the Bx B matings seemed to point to B as the 
dominant type, the actual results being presumably somewhat 
modified by uncontrolled matings previous to collection. 

In succeeding generations the same method of mating was 
followed as with Lina lapponica in 1904, that is, females of one 
brood were confined in a breeding-jar with males of another and 
vice Versa. 


326 Isabel McCracken 


For second generation data five categories of matings were 
established as follows: 


A-G X G, each parent from broods of BX B parentage that had produced mixed broods. 

B-G X G, each parent from broods of G X G parentage that had produced broods of green only. 

C-B X B, each parent from a mixed brood of B parentage. 

D-B X B, each parent from broods of BX B parentage that had produced broods of black only. 

E-G X B or B & G, the G parent from a pure G brood, the B parent from a completely dominant 
B brood. 


Table III gives a summary of the data of these matings. 


TABLE III 


Second Generation 


| 


Total 


Mating bs | | No. | No. | No. of “ Proportion of 
Cate- reac | Parents: Green | Black | Mixed |” ® B: Gin 
gory. | aay Broods. | Broods. | Broods ee Mixed Broods. 
ae a | as = ee 
A 133 << 133 GX G (mm!) 16 fo) 337 
B GXG GXG A | fo) 98 
(e BX B BX B(m) cy | 5 25 740 eee 4 
D BX B BX B (cd?) ° | I fo) 22 But 1 brood reared. 
GX Gl|eo>cBg) | | 
: BX Bi or \ | fo) I 15 384 3.6 21 
Bo X GQ J | | 


m! in parenthesis means that the individuals mated were from mixed broods. 
cd? in parenthesis means that the individuals mated were from broods in which B was completely 


dominant. 


This data shows that the recessive greens (G) breed true under 
either condition A or condition B. The dominant blacks (B) 
produce, under condition C, either broods in which B dominates 
completely or mixed broods in which B dominates partially. 
Since but a single brood was reared under condition D, the data 
are insufhcient. B x G produces either completely dominant B 
broods or mixed broods. 

For third generation data five categories of matings were estab- 
lished as indicated by Diagrams 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6. 


Inheritance of Dichromatism 


Diagram 2. Mating category A. 


First matings............ BxB Bx B BxB BxB 
ea [ee ene [ara 
| | | 
irstspenisces.scse.co ce B-BG B-BG B-BG B-BG 
| | oar ed) 
| | 
Second! gene..:---...-.-- G G 
| 
hinds pene -oaneesee 
Diagram 3. Mating category B. 
Birshamatings..) see. BxB Bx B BxB Bix B 
[ena oe eae lea 
| | | | 
IBIS fea Clee eeen an seeeerce= B-BG B-BG B-BG B-B-G 
[ | | | 
| | 
Second! pene sssc-----c-- B-BG B-BG 
| | 
Diagram 4. Mating category C. 
Birst)matings: --:---.-.- BxB Bx B Bx B Bx B 
Partie es |: = lie! fies 
| | | | 
Birst renss.-< 50652-35055 B-BG B-BG B-BG B-BG 
eae acs 2-2 | | 
| | 
Second) gents. ---..-.- B-BG B-BG 
| | 
B-BG 
Diagram 5. Mating category D. 
Mirstymatingss.----.... Biss /B Bx B BEXEB, Bx B 
al ed ee) S| 
| | | | 
Binsthgensen-: acccessss<s B-BG B-BG B-BG B-BG 
| | | | 
| | 
Second penl....---.. ---- B-BG cae 


B-BG 


a2 7 


328 Isabel McCracken 


Diagram 6. Mating category E. 


First matings............ Bx B GxG Bx B GxG 
(Sal (ae Kae Pease 
| | | | 
BKStROUs.-voeresensoes B G B G 
| S| | a 
| | 
Second! gente--.-.---== B B 
[Mi . Rae Et Seal 
| 
‘Mewtistel) Fee ecccbocccenoe BG 


Inspection of these diagrams shows that the original black 
parents (B x B of the first matings, except in Diagram 6) may be 
considered hybrids inasmuch as they produce both black and green 
individuals. 

Table IV gives a summary of the results of third generation 
matings. 


TABLE IV 


Third Generation 


Ma- | | | Total Proportion of 
ting Great- Grand No. | No. No. | No.of | B:A in 
Cate-| gtand- | scents. Parents. Green | Black | Mixed | [pdj- Mixed 
gory. | Parents. | Broods. | Broods. | Broods. | yiduals.| — Broods. 
aos = fe 
A BX B |GXG GXG | 29 ° ° 733 
Be |) BoB | BSB Ga) Goce | 27 ° rk 653 
C BEX Be | BBG) BBG) fo) 8 19 688 27 Ons 
D Bx BBX By Gad) Bee BiEd)) ° 18 3 518 Pie, BT 
BX B \ 420 7, B11 
E W \ 
GxG{ BEX G BX B (cd) eae 15 


*One B individual in a brood of nineteen, otherwise G individuals. 


Comparing categories A and B, we find, with the single excep- 
tion of one individual, that broods of green individuals only are 
produced by G x G matings, whether there are one (Category B) 
or two (Category A) generations of green parentage. 

Comparing Categories C and D, we find that under each of 
these conditions, that is, whether the B parents are chosen from 
broods in which B dominates completely, or from mixed broods, 


Inheritance of Dichromatism 329 


two kinds of broods result, broods in which “‘black”’ dominates 
completely, and mixed broods. ‘The comparative number of 
broods in which “black”? dominates completely is, however, much 
greater in Category D than in Category C, and the comparative 
number of black individuals in mixed broods is much greater in 
Category D than in Category C. ‘This shows a decided difference 
in the influence of the black character in these two categories. 

In the fourth generation, ten categories of matings were estab- 
lished as indicated in Table V. This table gives a summary of 
the results of these matings. 


TABLE V 


Fourth Generation 


SES | G-great- Great No. | No. !No. of} Total Ear 
ting = Grand- i f ; . B:G in 
grand- grand- Parents. G’n | Black | M’d |No. of E 
eat parents. parents. Re Broods | Broods|Broods Indi’d. MGs 
gory | roods. 
A BOG Ben Ga Gi(m)) iG XG: GxXG righ, Wp ey late 457 
B BX B |BX B@)|GXG(@m)|«Exe 17 ro) ° 404 
Cc |BXB |BX Bi) |BX B@)|GxG(m)| 6 ° ° 69 | 
Be By) | 
D Gx Gf BXG BSB Gai) GX G(m) ll, So ° 322, | 
E |BXB |BX B(cd)| BX B(m)|GxG(m)| +4 jae 73 
F BSB BX B(m) BX B(m) | Bx B aS Aisey || eG an 
G | BX 8B |BX Bd)|BX Bcd px B a i og ) 199 
| BaxeBi| | | 
H eExc{i2s% BX B(cd)|B x B(m) Se) 3 for |) as) Or 
Beer) | | 
I excels ~ © BX B(m) |B yx B(m) OF Ny a2 aaltanes ZO STAs pcal 
1 | BX B\||BxX B(cd)| BX Bled) 5 z 3 , ‘ aM 
FEyeGie Gs NGS :f' 


Comparing Category D of Table IV with Category G of Table 
V we find, as in Lina lapponica, that by selecting individuals in 
which there is complete dominance, the recessive character is 
eventually eliminated.. In other words, the fourth generation 
from hybrid parents, along the line of complete dominance, breeds 
true to the dominant character, no recessives appearing in the 


offspring. 


330 Isabel McCracken 
Comparing Categories A, B, C, D and E in Table V we find 


G x G breeding true in the fourth generation whether the imme- 
diate parents only are green or there has been a lineage of green 
for one, two or three generations. 

In the fifth generation, seven categories of matings were estab- 
lished as indicated in Table VI. ‘This table embodies a summary 
of these matings. 


TABLE VI 


Fifth Generation 


| | | 
a \eecs| ce Gréat- | Wes fvoul Neste cles eee 
ting Grand- lpsadtet BE bate __ | No. of|B : G in 
Cate- Fee grand: Brand. parents. | hare Indi- | Mixed 
Oa parents. | parents. parents. | Bd’s Bd’s|/B ’s| wide Bea 
| | | 
at eats | ey 
A | BX BY) GX G GXG GXG GG) 325) onl ones29) 
B |BXB|BXB GXG GXG GX G]}24| o| o| 679 | 
CB eB eBe@eB BX B GXG GG |) 9s) 10) || onl) waz0n| 
Ds | BAcB) | BX B BxXB BX B DAA eileen cll <c75 
D |/BXB|BXB BX B BX B GXG|14/| 0} ©| 370) 
ED | BGBa Bx 8 a) | BS Bi Ga) Bi Bim) PBs 1B) 708935 8 | 1336 eG su 
F |BXB/ BX B (cd)| BX B (cd)| BX B (cd)| BX B 1s | 0 | 397 
BX B(cd)| | BX B(cd)) | BX B(cd) 
Gabe B GxG , GXG ee ey Ole?) 6 | He Gee 


1The letter G in this connection indicates ‘‘ great.” 


Data in Table VI shows G XG continuing to breed true in 
Categories A, B, C and D, that is, whether there has been a line 
of green parentage for four generations or the immediate parents 
only are green. Comparing Category F of ‘Table V with Category 
E of Table VI we find an increasing proportion of broods in which 
black is wholly dominant and an increasing proportion of black 
individuals in the broods in which black is but partially dom- 
inant. In Category F, Table VI (compare Category G, Table 
V) black in the completely dominant line continues to breed 
true. 

In the sixth generation six categories of matings were estab- 
lished as indicated in Table VII. This table embodies a summary 


of the results of these matings. 


338 


Inheritance of Dichromatism 


; (- OxS | “9 <5) sorcol esr oi iinee 3 
ar am ea : ‘ SX |\(p) ax a | (p>) ax a| (p) ax a] \(P) axa) lax a 
1:91 61+ 9 8 ° qed @maxa! @Maxa| ™axa| @™axad| axa a 
1: gz | 6vt 9 5 ° aX @ (P)a Xa! (P)axXal (P)AXa]| (PAXA| AX a a 
Whe | ° lees ° axa (P)axal (P)axal (P)AaXE| (P)AXA| TX 2) 
| {Sz jocaqurgy i ; fsox la] f(a xa| f(a xa | f(P) axa ae F 
8% |\ ge jo-squigr | ° PSO Nore Go eres se saril  eecs 
bor | of jouquigr | o st 9x9 9x9 9X9 sk Se TX dell aed Vv 
Shore "Ss 4103 
eee , |« ‘ d “squared *syuoied *syuaied 
PoXxTN -Ipu] *spooig speends || akon ‘syuareg ‘syuored mate a, : 3 -ayeD 
url paxtyyt Jo “ON org uaaI5 pues -puris -purid pueis puri chat) 
; ‘ON oxI ‘ = ; ; ; : 1 
Sara var Joon | Jo ‘ON PII) JRAQH 999 D999) an 
u,doig 


Uud1IDAIUIAL) YyIniy 


TIA ATavL 


332 Isabel McCracken 


The data in Categories A and B show single black individuals 
in each of three otherwise green broods out of a total of forty-four 
broods, and a total of 1317 individuals of G x G parents. Unless 
this can be attributed to a mishap due to accident in the handling 
of breeding jars (a possibility where hundreds of breeding-jars are 
being daily cleaned and cared for) this is a return of latent B in 
normally recessive G. It will be noticed that such an exception 
to the general behavior in G x G matings was also recorded in 
Table IV (third generation). 

Comparing data in Category F, Table VI, with data in Category 
D, ‘Table VII, we find a discrepancy in the behavior of what was 
apparently pure B, that is likewise unaccounted for unless by 
accident or reversion. Table VII, Category D, records the 
presence of a recessive G (possibly a /atent G in Castle’s termi- 
nology)! in each of six broods from pure B parentage. 

Otherwise the behavior of G and B in the sixth generation is 
consistent with their behavior in preceding generations in similar 
categories. 

In the seventh generation, nine categories of matings were 


established, as indicated in Table VIII. 


*Castle, W. E., 1905: Heredity of Coat-colorin Guinea pigs and Rabbits. Carnegie Inst. Pub., No.23. 


333 


Inheritance of Dichromatism 


9L6 ° bt °o lgxXadi@max a l(P)a xa \(@ax fg i(P)e <a (Pa xa axa 
OSS || © IZ fe) ey SS uli(pa)iqne <aqn (PD) a <q | (PS) eee tn CC) tere (mw) xX € axa 
r:% | géor iv ze o |axXa\|(™axa qoXeO. |e ee, { oxo|f{ oxo|f oxo 
| | eee meactliaceeg erie cmseee le Gres tr 
bee || vo or 0 laxe |pevaxa | (axa) |e xa | (axa Case aire a8 
| \(p?) a xg \(p)a x J ((“)a x F (w)g & g \(@) ¢ x I 
[pi926, |" tO Nee v% |oxo|(™ma xa Ce Gans Se ees J 9x9 aX 
ie ea: ial ae aX d 
if NAanG Sac x o£ x «£ 
ee Paap eae B ee lGecee axo|{ 2x9 xD) oP) oe 2) nae 
\ axa aq xX d d xX a @ Xx 
| )x«£ yxX«£ yx | xXx«£ 
++6 ° ines fits. MI fay Se £9) pea Dae J Duke, pe eek ax 
\ axa Gis | |e GE ea | eae eet 
tez fe) fe) 6 QX«£ yx) yxX«£ 5x9 /() a x a | (a x axXq@ 
€10z Oe | &c 1L 9x9 yxX9 yxX9) XO 9x) |(™) a x a |@ a x a 


*spooig. | 

paxtf *PIAIpuy | ‘spoorg,  ‘spoorg | ‘spoorg sjuared | ‘sjuased ‘sjuared *syuaied ‘syuoied 
D?@ | JOON | PexXIW | 3 | werH | ‘squoieg | «sjuamed -pueis -pueid -pueis -pueis -puris 
ee LEA OU a Ome ON tO ss CIN TOR CIN ~purig “yeaIgy) “09 “999 “9999 “99999 

u,doig 


UOWDAIUIL) 1 UIQ0ay 


IIA ATAVL 


334 Isabel McCracken 


Categories A, B, C and E show consistent behavior of recessives. 

Category F shows the possibility of complete domimation of B 
even from a line of partially dominant ancestry. 

The proportions in Categories D and G remain consistent with 
proportions in corresponding categories of previous tables. 

‘Table [IX embodies a comparison of broods of seven generations 
reared successively from B x B in which the parents were partial 
dominants, that is, black individuals from mixed broods con- 
taining a larger number of black than green individuals. This 
table slows the proportion of B:G (of dominant to recessive) 
in successive generations, and the rate of increase from generation 
to generation of the dominant B. 

This data shows a progressive dominance of B, which either 
reduces G to a latent in the seventh generation or wholly eliminates 
it. 


TABLE IX 
2 Prop’n 
Total eile | Total No. | Total No. of B:G 
Nolce Navara! Broods B ECOL B - 
Broods. | Individs. Conese) ae) Mixed 
o, Dominant. |Dominant. BREGATS 
First generation (Table IT)............. 41 1207 2 39 T2391 
Second generation (Table IG) ....... 30 740 5 25 it guid Bu 
Third generation (TableIVC) ........ 27 868 8 19 2a ONL 
Fourth generation (Table VF) ........ 13 250 8 Hog) 5H 
Fifth generation (Table VIE) ......... 43 1336 35 8 8.531 
Sixth generation (Table VITE) ........ 14 419 8 6 16:1 
Seventh generation (Table VIII F)!..... 10 333 10 ° All B 


‘Not absolutely, but practically, comparable with categories of other generations with which it is 
compared. 


COMPARISON OF RESULTS IN LINA AND GASTROIDEA 


The fact that in one species (Gastroidea) black is the dominant 
character, while in the other species (Lina) black is the recessive 


Inheritance of Dichromatism 235 


character, shows that there is nothing in the character as such 
that makes for its actual behavior. 

That green is the final color in the color-development of the 
individual in Gastroidea has already been pointed out. In the 
paper previously referred to, it was pointed out that black is the 
final color in the color-development of individuals in Lina lap- 
ponica. We have, therefore, a similarity of behavior in the estab- 
lishment of the definitive color in the two-color series in each 
species. [hat is, we have in each species a primary color-con- 
dition through which all individuals pass and in which one series 
remain (“ black”. in Gastroidea, “spotted” in Lina). We have 
in each species a secondary color-condition into which some of 
the individuals pass and there remain (“green”’ in Gastroidea and 
“black” in Lina). It has been shown that it is the final or second- 
ary color-condition that becomes the recessive character in each 
species. 

If it could be aionth that the primary or first condition into which 
each species enters represents the older or ancestral condition, we 
would then have in Lina and Gastroidea a condition found by 
Castle in pigmented versus albino and long versus short-coated 
guinea pigs; that 1s, a dominance of the ancestral or older character. 


SUMMARY OF RESULTS 


1. In Gastroidea dissimilis “black” is dominant, “green” is 
recessive. In Lina lapponica “black” is recessive and “ spotted- 
brown” is dominant, though not typically so. In each species 
the dominant color is that appearing first in the color-development 
of the maturing adult, and the recessive color is that appearing 
last in the color development. 

2. In each species the recessive character breeds true at once 
(with the possibility of the recurrence of the latent dominant). 
The dominant character breeds true in the third or fourth genera- 
tion from a hybrid through the completely dominant line (with 
the possibility of a recurrence of the latent recessive). 

3. In each species there is progressive or accumulative domi- 
nance from generation to generation through the partially domi- 
nant line. 


336 Isabel McCracken 


In conclusion it is evident that under the conditions of the breed- 
ing experiments with Lina lapponica and Gastroidea dissimilis, 
the heredity of the alternate characters in dichromatic species 
differs materially from typical Mendelian heredity of alternate 
characters. In the latter, the relation between dominant and 
recessive characters is a perfectly stable one, assuming the 
definite numerical proportion of 3:1. Inthe former, there is 
apparently an actual prepotency of the dominant character that in 
the long run effectually eliminates or reduces the recessive char- 
acter to a latent one. 


Entomological Laboratory, Stanford University, 
December, 1905 


Contribution from the Biological Laboratory of Clark University 


LOCOMOTION OF AMCEBA AND ALLIED FORMS 


BY 


ORIS P: DELLINGER 


Witn Two Pirates AND TWENTY-NINE FIGURES IN THE TEXT 


Few subjects are more fundamental or have been more 
assiduously studied than the movements of the Amceba. ‘The 
earlier writers maintained that the explanation of these move- 
ments was to be found in the contractility of protoplasm. M. 
Schultz, Briicke, DeBary, Kthne, Haeckel and others held this 
view. They did not fully analyze their theories, however, 
although Briicke did postulate an internal contractile frame- 
work to account for the streaming of protoplasm. 

Early in the discussion, objections were raised to the contractile 
theory. Wallich (’63) called attention to the fact that the 
currents of protoplasm in Amcebe did not begin at the posterior 
end or in the interior but that they commenced at the point of 
advance and extended backward. Biitschli, Hofmeister, Nageli 
and others made similar observations and on that account re- 
linquished the contraction theory. All these observations were 
made by looking down on the Amceba from above, while if it is 
viewed from the side, the difficulties can be readily explained. 


BERTHOLD’S THEORY 


Many other theories followed, none of which need be mentioned 
until Berthold’s. In the year 1886, he advanced the opinion that 
the protoplasm of Amcebe behaves much the same as a drop of 
fluid which is spreading on a solid surface. If a drop of inorganic 
fluid is caused to adhere more strongly at one side than the other, 
it will roll toward the more adherent side. He believed this 
movement to be exactly what we have in Amcebe. Jennings 
('04, p. 208) says: “By a proper arrangement of the conditions 


Tue Journar or Expertmentat Zoéxocy, Vor. 11, No. 3. 


338 Oris P. Dellinger 


almost every detail of amceboid locomotion may be clearly imitated”’ 
(by a drop of fluid). Berthold further compares the movements 
to a drop of water fleeing from a rod wet with ether; but, as 
Butschli points out, the character of the movements here is entirely 
different from those of a drop adhering to a solid surface. 
Butschhi, in discussing Berthold’s view, says (p. 190): “I consider 
it as Incorrect to suppose that Amcebz really adhere to the solid 
substratum. I do not entirely dispute the fact that local adhesions 
at the hinder end, or occasionally also in the pseudopodia during 
their retraction, may come under observation. On_ the other 
hand, I consider it certain that an extensive adhesion is absent.’’ 
Berthold was obliged to offer some other explanation to account 
for the protrusion of a free pseudopod, as in that case there is no 


B 
L ENTS roe 


Fig. 1. Diagram of the currents in a drop of clove oil in which the surface tension is lessened 
on one side. The drop elongates and moves in the direction of a. (After Jennings.) 

Fig. 2. Diagram of the currents in a progressing A. limax. A, top view; B, side view. (After 
Rhumbler from Jennings.) : 


surface to which to adhere. He explained it by a contraction 
theory. We shall see later that neither form, currents, nor 
adhesion at the point of advance, required by Berthold’s theory 
are present. 


SURFACE TENSION THEORY OF BUTSCHLI AND RHUMBLER 


Butschli (’92) and Rhumbler (’98) followed with the surface 
tension theory. According to these two authors, and also many 
others, the Ameceba is a drop of complex fluid which moves about 
as a result of local changes in surface tension. ‘The currents in 
such a drop are forward in the central axis and backward along 
the surface (Fig. 1). Fig. 2 will show the same currents in the 


Locomotion of Am@ebe and Allied Forms 339 


2) 


Ameeba, according to Rhumbler. It is evident that this theory 
would explain the putting out of free pseudopods, but, as Butschli 
points out, it would not account for the very fine pseudopods of 
actinospherium-like forms. Unfortunately for their theory the 
necessary currents are not present. Even Butschli calls attention 
to the fact that he could not always observe them. Neither is 
the Ameeba the shape of a drop of fluid acting under changing 
surface tension and, what is more to the point, there are definite 
points of attachment. As Jennings points out, this theory would 
not explain many phenomena of the free pseudopods. 


MOVEMENTS OF AMCBA AS DESCRIBED BY JENNINGS 


Great credit is due Jennings (’04) for the careful experiments 
and observations by which he throws doubt on the surface tension 
theory of Rhumbler and Butschli, and brings forward his con- 
tractiontheory. Forthis reasona careful review of his work will be 
taken up before going on to my own observations and experiments. 

Jennings bases his view on the following: 

(1) Experiments and observations to determine what move- 
ments take place. 

(2) Observations as to the shape of advancing Amcebe. 

(3) Belief that the anterior edge is closely applied to the sub- 
stratum. 

(4) Observations which seem to indicate contractility of the 
ectosarc. 

His experiment consisted in mixing soot with water in which 
Amoeba verrucosa was moving. Particles of the soot became 
attached to the surface of the Amceba and by watching these, he 
could distinguish what currents were present. He sums up his 
results as follows: 


‘In an advancing Amceba substance flows forward on the upper surface, rolls 
over at the anterior edge, coming in contact with the substratum, then remains 
quiet until the body of the Ameeba has passed over it. It then moves upward at 
the posterior end, and forward again on the upper surface, continuing in rotation 
as long as the Ameeba continues to progress. The motion of the upper surface 


is congruent with that of the endosarc, the two forming a single stream’’ (Fig. 2): 


340 Oris P. Dellinger 


According to his description, Jennings derives the shape of an 
advancing Amceba from two observations. These are as follows : 


“Tn an Ameeba that was creeping on the lower surface of a cover glass, I was 
able to define with some accuracy the parts that were attached and those that 
were not. A small flagellate was moving briskly about between the Amceba and 
the cover glass but its excursions were limited by a visible line-running parallel 
with the anterior edge of the Ameeba and extending at the sides back to about one- 
third the animal’s length from the rear. The zone between this and the margin 
was pressed close to the glass, and was evidently attached to it. The more 
pointed posterior end was held quite away from the glass, leaving a broad passage- 
way through which the flagellate finally escaped. 

“The results of this observation were confirmed by another. An Ameeba 
verrucosa in full career was suddenly turned on one lateral edge by a strong current 
from a rotifer, and its upper edge coming in contact with the cover glass, it 
remained in that position some time without change of form. It could be seen that 
the under surface was concave, the edges very thin and flat, while the posterior 


portion was thick and arched’’ (p. 145). 


Fig. 3. Diagram of the movements of a particle attached to the surface of A. verrucosa in 
side view. As the Amceba moves forward from position 7, the particle moves forward to the positions 
shown in 2, 3, 4,5 and6, (After Jennings.) 

Fig. 4. Jennings’ conception of the shape of an advancing A. verrucosa, seen from the side. * The 
anterior edge is thin and applied closely to the substratum while the posterior end is raised high in 
the water. The Ameba is attached only by the anterior third to the substratum. (After Jennings.) 

Fic. 5. Shape of advancing Amebe in general. (After Jennings.) 


7 


a 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 341 


From these observations Jennings concludes that the form of 
the advancing Amcebz verrucosa is that given in Fig. 4, and of 
Ameoebe in general, that of Fig. 5, in which we see the thin 
anterior edge applied closely to the substratum while the thick 
posterior end 1s raised high above it. On the same page he gives 
a diagram of an advancing anterior edge and on the preceding 
page, the following observation and conclusion: 


“A certain feature of the advance of the anterior edge seems of much significance. 
Each wave seems to arise just behind the previous anterior boundary line and over- 
laps it, leaving it buried. ‘This line often remains visible for a short time after the 
new wave has been formed. ‘The new wave rolls over the preceding one in such 
a way that its original upper surface becomes applied to the substratum. ‘This 
is demonstrated by the rolling under of small objects on the upper surface of the 
advancing wave. A diagram of the movement at the anterior edge is given in Fig. 
45. [he movements can be imitated roughly by making a cylinder of cloth, laying 
it flat on a plane surface, and pulling forward the anterior edge in a series of waves. 
The entire cylinder then rolls forward just as the Amceba does. 

“The essential features of the movement seem to be; (1) the advance of the wave 
from the upper surface at the anterior edge; (2) the pull exercised by the wave on the 
the remainder of the upper surface of the body, bringing it forward. Most of 
the other phenomena follow as consequences of these two. ‘The flowing forward 
of the granules of the endosarc seems to demand no special explanation, since a 
fluid containing granules within a rolling sac must necessarily flow forward as 
the sac rolls. By the movement forward of the anterior end a space is left free; 
by the rolling forward of the posterior end the fluid is piled up and pressed upon 
and must flow forward into the empty space in front. Possibly there may be 
other causes at work in producing the endosarcal currents, but such currents 


would be produced without other cause in a sac moving as Amceba does.’’ 


From the above it will be seen that Jennings’ conception of an 
Ameeba is an elastic sac filled with a fluid. If we give the sac 
contractility, which from observations (p.147) he has a perfect 
right to do, we have the Amceba from which he derives his 
theory of the movements. 

Further discussion of his paper will follow my own observations 
and experiments, but | may add that, although I can confirm 
some of his observations, I think, had he studied good side 
views of his specimens, he would have reached far different 
conclusions. 


4 
342 Oris P. Dellinger 
AMEB AND DIFFLUGIA STUDIED FROM ABOVE AND FROM THE SIDE 


In connection with a comparative study on the cilium, it was 
necessary for me, as many students of contractile protoplasm have 
done, to begin with the pseudopod of the Amceba. Before I had ° 
proceded far, it became evident that I must repeat Jennings’ (04) 
observations; the more so because a study of prepared slides of 
the pseudopod led me to doubt his conclusions as to the structure 
of Amcebe. 

I did not confine myself to Amoebz but gave considerable 
attention to Difflugia, Actinosphaerium and Euglena. Difflugia 
is practically an Ameeba in a shell, and has pseudopods that move 
in a particularly definite way. Apparently little attention has 
been paid to this form. Jennings (’04) and Penard (’go) refer 
to it, the latter calling attention to the activity of its pseudo- 
pods. 

It was necessary to answer the following questions before I 
could proceed: 

1. How does an Amceba form a pseudopod f 

2. What evidence have we for a contractile substance in Amceba 
and how is this substance distributed ? 

3. How does an Ameeba attach itself? 

4. What determines its form while in motion ? 

5. What causes Amcebe to advance in a certain direction? 

My first observations were made in the usual way by watching 
the forms from above. I used Difflugia acuminata, D. spiralis, 
Ameeba proteus, A. limax, A. verrucosa, A. radiosa and several 
small forms of Amoeba which I did not determine. Amoeba 
proteus was especially well suited to my purpose, as it was large 
enough to be easily seen with the unaided eye. 


Difflugia 


Diff_lugia acuminata extends a slender pseudopod to its full 
length free in the water. ‘That it is free is evident from its 
motions. ‘The pseudopod then becomes attached at its tip (Fig. 6) 
and contracts, drawing the mouth of the shell up to the point of 
attachment. A new pseudopod appears pari passu, generally 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 343 


near the base of the first (Fig. 6), and increases in length as the 
first is contracted. ‘The new pseudopod, which is fully extended 
when the mouth of the shell reaches the point of attachment, 
swings into the line of advance and becomes attached. The 
cycle is then repeated. Figs. 6 and 8 show the position of the 


Fig. 6. Diagram of Difflugia acuminata showing changes from one pseudopod to another. 
a, a, a,a,ashow points of attachment; b,b,b, b,b, the new pseudopod at different stages of its formation. 
(The diagram was made with camera lucida by drawing the paper at right angles to the line of 


advance.) 


mouth and pseudopod at different stages in the change from one 
pseudopod to another. Fig. 7 shows four successive positions of 
the shell at the time when a pseudopod becomes attached, and also 
the lines of advance of the shell and of the tips of new pseudopods 
as they form and are carried forward. That the pseudopods 
while free take no part in drawing the shell forward is evident 
from the figure. 

Often Diffugia acuminata apparently moves with a single 
pseudopod, in which case the new pseudopod is extended directly 


34.4 Oris P. Dellinger 


over the tip of the old. I obtained evidence to prove this from a 
specimen in which the second pseudopod appeared at the side 
instead of over the end of the first (Fig. 9). It is evident that the 


} 


Fig. 7. Diagram showing position of mouth at time the pseudopod was attached and the lines of 


advance of mouth and of forming pseudopods. 7,2, 3 and 4 show positions of mouth when attachments 
a,b,c and d are made. Dotted line indicates advance of the mouth and the solid lines follow the tips of 
the forming pseudopods. (Camera lucida.) 

Fig. 8. Diagram of D. acuminata as it advances. At position 7,a is attached; at 2,} is attached 
and c is appearing; at 4, the mouth of the shell is well up to the point of attachment of 6 andc¢ is long; 
at5, cis attached; at 6,c has contracted and d is appearing; at 7, d has become attached. (Camera 
lucida.) 

Fig. 9. D. acuminata with new pseudopod appearing near the tip instead of near the base. 

Fig. 10. Diagram of currents in advancing D. acuminata. The substance of the contracting 


pseudopod flows over into the new pseudopod. 


last two methods of locomotion are but variations of the first. 
‘The movements were sometimes complicated by combining these 
methods. 

The particles in the contracting pseudopod flowed around into 
the new pseudopod (Fig. 10), giving the exact picture of the flow 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 345 


of particles in retracting and protruding pseudopods of Amcebe. 
Another point of resemblance was the shrunken and wrinkled 
appearance of the pseudopod as it contracted. 

Difflugia spiralis carries its shell when in motion much as a 
snail does, and thus the mouth and base of the pseudopods are 
not visible unless viewed from the side. Yet watching it from 
above one sees the end of a pseudopod suddenly appear, wave 
about for a time, and then become attached. ‘The shell then 
moves over it, at the same time another pseudopod appears and 
the above is repeated. 


Amaebe—Absence of Rolling Movement in all Except Ameba 


V errucosa 


My first experiment with the Amceba was the one Jennings per- 
formed with lampblack. I tried at first Amoeba proteus, A. limax 
and some smaller Amcebe which I did not determine. In these 
forms I never obtained his results, although I repeated the 
experiment many times. ‘The attached particles would merely 
oscillate back and forth a little but never revolved as he describes 
(Fig. 11). The position of the particles on the Amcebe made no 
difference. I watched particles attached to the anterior end, to 
the posterior end, and to the middle, but never found any revo- 
lution. 

I next experimented with Amceba verrucosa. ‘The particles 
became attached much more readily in this form and I found the 
currents exactly as Jennings describes them. Diatoms, granules 
and particles of all kinds passed to the anterior end, over the edge, 
then stopped until the Amceba passed over them, when they were 
picked up and the above repeated (Fig. 3). 

‘There may be rotation in other species but | am convinced that 
it is not common. Evidence for this will appear later in the 
paper. 

I next gave my attention to the direction of the currents of 
protoplasm. By watching the granules it is easy to tell just 
what the currents are. In general the granules flow forward 
rapidly in the middle and spread out in all directions at the 


346 Oris P. Dellinger 


anterior end. Some of the granules at the border stop while those 
in the center flow on. ‘These may remain quiet for some time 
and then enter the current, or some may not enter the current until 
the body of the Amceba is almost past, when they enter at the 
posterior end. ‘The large granules of the endosarc oscillate back 


ao 


Fig. 11. A, diagram showing position of the particles of lampblack on an advancing Ameba at 
different stages of the advance. B, the same Amceba twenty minutes later. During all that time 
they had oscillated back and forth but had kept nearly the same position. (Camera lucida drawing.) 

Fig.12. Diagram showing movements of a large particle in the endosarc during different stages 
of the advance. (Camera lucida drawing.) 

Fig. 13. Diagram of A. proteus moving in a clear field. 


Fig. 14. Diagram of A. proteus moving in the presence of small algz on which it is feeding. 


and forth but keep the same relative position (Fig. 12). The 
currents at the anterior end are like those in a pseudopod formed 
free in the water. ‘The granules do not flow forward in straight 
lines but zigzag along as if flowing azound obstacles. In 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 347 


Amoeba verrucosa there are definite streams and channels 
which anastomose, and the flow reminds one of the flow of 
corpuscles in a frog’s web. Diatoms flow forward with the other 
granules if lengthwise with the currents. If lying across the 
channels they appear tobe caught and move with the Ameeba itself. 

In Ameeba verrucosa there is also a peculiar jerking of the 
particles at the posterior end at regular intervals, as if at this point 
there were regular flows forward. I was unable to explain this 
while studying them from above but a side view revealed the 
cause. 

The anterior end of Amceba verrucosa 1s very regular i in contour 
and advances evenly in all direct‘ons. Here again it is an ex- 
ception, for in all other species studied the anterior end advances 
by a series of lunges, whichare never directly forward but alternate 
from side to side of the direct line of advance. When there are 
many pseudopods put out in front, as is often the case, the one 
through which the Amceba advances alternates from side to side. 
This phenomenon probably accounts for the absence of rolling 
of the ectosarc in these forms. 

The various shapes of advancing Amoebe find an explanation 
in the reactions of the animals to food. If Amcebz are traveling 
in a clear field or moving from one mass of debris to another, they 
move by long loops (PI. II, Figs. 7, 8, 9) or maintain a long, slender 
form and are attached at cael points (Fig. 13 and PI. ie Higs1a). 
When moving and feeding on algz they reach out many pseudo- 
pods and then present a palmate form (Fig. 14, and PI. I, Fig.13). 

Other observations made were: 

The shape and wrinkled appearance of the posterior end 
indicate that the substance is contracting in this region. 

2. The particles in Amcebz that are rolled along retain their 
relative position and do not flow as in an advancing Amceba. 
This observation is easily made by rolling an Amceba about with 
a rod. ‘This gives good evidence for two things: First, that the 
Ameeba is not an elastic sac filled with a fluid, which if rolled 
along would produce the endosarcal currents (Jennings, quoted 
above). The flow of the particles in the endosare must be due to 
something else. Second, that the endosarc has a definite struc- 


348 Oris PR Dellinger 


ture that holds the particles firmly, however much the Amceba 
is rolled about. 

There is no question as to the methods of movement in Difflu- 
gia. A slender pseudopod reaches out, attaches at the tip, and 
contracts, drawing the shell forward. A new pseudopod is 
formed by an aurlsos of the substance from the contracting 
pseudopod. The new pseudopod 1 is full length when the Hens 
of the shell reaches the point of attachment of the old and swings 
in to the line of advance and attaches. ‘The above cycle is then 
repeated. . 

The similarity of the two forms suggests that Amoeba move 
by the same general method. [he movements of an unidentified 
Ameeba fies some support for this view. ‘This form extends 
a slender pseudopod free in the water, attaches it by the tip and 
draws the rest of the body up to this point. It then establishes 
a point of attachment close behind the first, and freeing this, the 
above is repeated. My observations before obtaining a side view 
indicated that Amoeba proteus moves in the same way. 


STUDY OF AMC@B AND DIFFLUGIA FROM THE SIDE 


The discussions of Amcebz for the past fifty years call to mind 
the story of the two knights and the shield. Ifa single observa- 
tion of an Amceba accidently turned on its side by a passing 
rotifer has given ground for a theory of the shape, attachment, and 
movements of Amcebze, it becomes clearly necessary to devise 
some means by which Amcebe and similar forms can be studied 
in side view. Such a view will give points of attachment and 
support, and upon these depends an understanding of the strains 
and contractions which produce the movements both for advance 
and for the formation of pseudopods. So far as I can find, no 
mention of the study of Ameeba in this way is to be found in the 
literature. 

‘The apparatus devised is very simple. One edge of an ordinary 
slide is ground squareand polished. Long cover slips are cemented 
to this with the edges extending beyond the polished surface 
so as to form a narrow trough (Fig. 15). With the microscope 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 349 


brought to a horizontal position, specimens pipetted into the 
trough move along the edge of the slide and are easily observed in 
side view.' 
Difflugia 

Difflugia spiralis moves exactly like D. acuminata (Fig. 16); 
that is, it extends a pseudopod free in the water, attaches it near 
the tip (z, Fig. 16) and draws the shell forward. As the shell is 
drawn forward a new pseudopod appears (2, Fig. 16), grows in 
length as the first is shortened (3, Fig. 16), is full length when the 
mouth of the shell reaches the point of attachment, and is then 

Polished Edge 


wy 


WE. 
Fa 


‘7 6 Cover Glasses 


Fig. 15. Diagram of apparatus designed to study Protozoa in side view. 


swung into the line of advance and attached. The attachment 
of the pseudopod in both cases is near the tip and is definite. 

When Difflugia moves with one apparent pseudopod, as 
described above, the new pseudopod is put out as a direct 
continuation of the old. It is always short and the points of 
attachment are closer together than when moving with two 
pseudopods (Fig. 17). 

Difflugia spiralis often creeps on a vertical surface. At such 
times the points of attachment can be easily demonstrated by 
jarring the table. The unattached parts vibrate while those 
attached move with the glass. This same form often creeps on 
the ceiling (Fig. 18), and then the movements are the same as 
when on the floor. 

1The cover slips were cemented with varnish and baked in an oven while closely pressed to the slide. 


Only a trace of cement was used and great care exercised to prevent any of it reaching the polished 


surface. 


350 Oris P. Dellinger 
Amabe 


Advancing Amoeba verrucosa is attached at the anterior and 
posterior ends and is free everywhere else (Fig. 19). The ante- 
rior end is extended along the substratum but is apparently free 


Fig. 16. Diagram of D. spiralis changing from one pseudopod to another. At position 7, the first 
pseudopod is attached; at 2, a new pseudopod is appearing; at 3, the mouth of the shell is well up to 
the point of attachment of the first and the new pseudopod is reaching over toattach; at 4, the new 
pseudopod is attached. 

Fig. 17. D. spiralis moving with one pseudopod. 

Fig. 18. D. spiralis creeping on the ceiling. 

Fig. 19. Side view of advancing Ameeba verrucosa showing the various shapes it assumes at different 
stages of advance. At position 7, a new attachment has just been formed at the posterior end and it 
is quite thick. At position 3, the Amceba is just ready to form a new attachment at the posterior 
end. Jt is seen that the anterior end is not thin, and although put out along the substratum is not 
attached except at definite points. 


from it. Itis thick and blunt (Fig. 19, and PI. I, Figs. 1, 2, 3 and 
4). There is a sudden flow of the substance forward when the 
attachment at the posterior end is released and the Ameeba in this 
region becomes much thicker (z, Fig. 19, and Plate I, Fig. 1). 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 351 


This accounts for the jerking motion noted when observed from 
above. ‘The flow of the substance forward is due to the pull from 
the attachment at the anterior end and contraction of the pos- 
terior end. As the substance flows forward the posterior end 
becomes thinner andis drawn up to the point of attachment (2, 3, 
Fig. 19). A new attachment is then formed at the anterior end 
and the one at the posterior end is released. ‘The strain due to 
the pull from in front causes the sudden flow forward. ‘The old 
attachment at the anterior end becomes in part the new attachment 
at the posterior end, but when the sudden flow forward occurs, the 
attached surface is extended back much farther. 

It is difficult to understand how Jennings arrived at the con- 
clusion that the anterior end is thin. On the other hand the ob- 
servations from which he derives the shape, and on which he bases 
his view as to the thickness of the posterior end and point of attach- 
ment, are easily explained. If the Amceba 1s seen in the stage of 
its movement shown in Fig. 19, the posterior end is thick but 
the anterior end is not thin, neither is it the only point of attach- 
ment. The escape of the flagellate, which he describes might be 
explained by the change of attachment at the posterior end, or by 
the Ameeba being jarred loose (PI. I, Fig. 4). 

Admitting the shape as he gives it, it does not seem to me that 
he explains the endosarcal currents. It is true they would occur 
in a sac rolling on the floor, but unfortunately for his explanation 
the sac he observed was hanging from the ceiling, in which case 
the particles should fall down toward the posterior end instead of 
flowing forward as he finds them. If Fig. 4 is inverted we have 
the Amceba as he saw it. 

All other forms of Amoebe advance much more like Difflugia. 
They extend the anterior end free inthe water and attach it at or 
near the tip and then contract. At the same time the posterior 
end is contracting and the substance thus pushed and pulled 
forward goes to form the new anterior end. ‘This continues as 
long asthe Amoeba advances (Figs. 20, 21, 22 and 23, also Plt Ts 
Figs. 6, 7 and 8). Often the anterior end is pushed along the 
substratum but no attachments form except at definite points. 

In other cases the anterior end is lifted free and then curves 


259 Oris P. Dellinger 


2)2) 


down to the substratum and attaches, forming a long loop (PI. II, 
Figs. 1 to 10). ‘The posterior end is then released and the sub- 
stance flows over to the anteriorend. At the same time another 
anterior end is extended. Amoeba proteus frequently uses this 
method (Fig. 24) and a small undetermined form appears to move 


a ai ere 
A Bae 
Zi 
aA B A B GC Deemed 


mu 2. 3 4 5 4 
A A B B25.A B B B 


Fig. 20. A. proteus, side view showing shape and points of attachment and the method generally 
used by this form in advancing. At position r, the Ameeba is attached at a and } and forms a long 
loop; at position 2, a new anterior end has been extended; the points of attachment at a and b are well 
defined and the substance is drawn up into posts; at position 3, attachment a has been released and a 
new attachment formed at ¢; a new anterior end has been pushed out at 4 and brought to the 
substratum and attached. The Ameeba continues this as long as it advances. 

Figs. 21 and 22. Side views of small undetermined Amoeba showing points of attachment. 

Fig. 23. Side view of A. limax showing points of attachment. 

Fig. 24. Side view of A. proteus showing how it moves by long loops. 

Fig. 25. Side view of the small Ameeba referred to above, showing how it loops along in changing 
from one point of attachment a, to another. At2,attachment b is just forming; at 4, attachment a 


is released. 


in this way altogether (Fig. 25). The points of attachment are 
always well defined and never extensive (Fig. 20, and PI. I, Figs. 
6, 7 and 8). Sometimes they are far apart, the Amoeba forming 
a long loop, or they may be close together (Fig. 26). 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 353 


The substance is often drawn up into well defined posts. The 
attachments are formed quickly and at no time was there any 
evidence that a viscid substance is present. An Amceba was 
jarred loose from the slide by striking the table when the parts of 
the body that had been in contact were easily seen. ‘The sur- 
face at these points was perfectly flat and looked as if it had been 
planed off. 

The granules at the anterior end move outward in all directions 
but never backward. At the posterior end the granules flow 


eee 
2G. Pa ah 28 


1 2 3 


a 
Fig. 26. A. proteus, side view, showing points of attachment close together. The anterior end is 
just reaching out to form a new attachment. 
Fig. 28. Side view A. proteus creeping on the ceiling. 


Fig. 29. A. proteus at rest showing points of contact. The Ameba does not seem to touch with an 


extensive contact. 


forward in a steady, even stream. ‘The currents are really the 
same as seen from above, the upper and lower surface then be- 
coming the lateral edges. Particles attached to the surface oscillate 
but are never carried around the Ameeba. 

An Ameeba proteus covered with carmine granules was advancing 
in the usual way. ‘The particles moved along with the Amceba, 
keeping the same relative position. “wo particles almost oppo- 
site each other on the upper and lower surface of the advancing 
anterior end kept the same distance from the tip as it advanced. 
The one on top never rolled over and passed to the under side. 


354 Oris P. Dellinger 


Particles attached to the tip would sometimes pass to the upper 
surface. At no time did particles move along the upper surface 
to the anterior end and then pass underneath. On the other 
hand, particles on the under surface flowed along with particles 
on the upper surface. I am convinced that in species studied 
there was no flowing of the upper surface over the anterior edge 
to the under side. | 

Ameoebe often creep on a vertical surface or, by inverting the 
cell, on the ceiling, and at suchtimes the shape is the sameas when 
creeping on the floor (Fig. 28). A large Ameeba proteus creeping 
on the ceiling was jarred loose and tried to attach itself again. 
It had partly succeeded when it was loosened at the new attach- 
ment. In rounding up it then went through the stages shown in 
Fig. 27. The entire process took about fifteen minutes. If the 


l 9 3 4 


Fig. 27. Side view of A. proteus that was jarred loose from the ceiling at all but two points of 
attachment, which were near the anterior end. Shapes it assumed in rounding up. It hung in position 


3 for ten minutes during which time all the large granules in the endosarc kept the same position. 


Ameeba is a sac filled with a fluid in which the endosarcal particles 
are suspended it seems that these particles would settle to the 
bottom. ‘This was not the case. Since particles do not flow as they 
would in a sac, we must assume a more definite structure in the 
endosarc. Whatever structure is assumed must account for the 
behavior of the endosarcal particles while the Amoeba is moving 
and at rest. In other words, the endosare must be so constituted 
as to allow the particles to flow at one time and to hold them 
securely at another. 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 355 


Other observations were: 

1. Amoebz at rest do not touch with an extensive contact 
(ig220). 

2. Amoebe often change form without advancing. They do 
this in the presence of Paramoecia and appear to be fishing for 
them. 

3. Amoebe when rolled about, or when starting to move, after 
a rest, extend pseudopods in all directions. Diffugia when 
turned on its back will do the same. In either case as soon as a 
pseudopod becomes attached the animal moves off in the direc- 
tion of the point attached. 


CONCLUSIONS 


What answer to the questions asked at the beginning of this 
inquiry are to be found in the above observations f 

1. Advancing Amcebz at no time approximate the shape of a 
drop of fluid adhering more closely at one side than the other. 
Neither do they behave like a drop acting under changing surface 
tension. I think we are warranted in saying also that they are 
not sacs of contractile ectosarce rolling about. Such an assump- 
tion would not explain the phenomena in moving Amcebe as we 
have found them from a side view. 

2. The definiteness with which Difflugia extends a pseudopod, 
swings it about, brings it into the line of advance and attaches tus 
the manner in which the shell is drawn up to this point of attach- 
ment; and the creeping about over a vertical surface and the 
ceiling, indicate that in this form there 1s a contractile substance. 

3. In advancing Amcebe the movements are essentially those 
of Diffugia. Often they are exactly the same. The anterior 
end is extended free in the water and attached. ‘There is then a 
contraction of the substance back of this point and a flow of the 
substance toward the anterior end results. The movements of 
Amoebz are due to the presence of a contractile substance. 

From observation we are warranted in assuming that such a 
substance is present. 


356 Oris P. Dellinger 


4. The granules of the endosare do not- flow as if they were 
suspended in a fluid in a contractile sac. On the contrary they 
are held in definite positions under conditions that would indicate 
that the endosarce has definite structure. A coarse reticulum 
of contractile substance distributed through the endosare would 
account for the phenomena as we have observed them. 

I wish to acknowledge my indebtedness to Dr. C. F. Hodge, 
under whose direction the research was made, for the many sug- 
gestions and the encouragement given me. Also to Mrs. A. 
Forrest Dellinger for help in preparing the drawings and plates. 


Clark University, Worcester, Mass. 
January 23, 1906. 


Locomotion of Amebe and Allied Forms 357 


LITERATURE CITED 


BERTHOLD, G., ’86.—Studien ttber Protoplasmamechanik. 332 pp., 7 pl. Leipzig. 

BrtckeE, E., ’62.—Die Elementarorganismen. Sitzb. d. K. Akad. Wien. Bd. 
xliv, II, Abth. 

Burscu1t, O., ’92.—Untersuchungen tiber mikroskopische Schaume und das Proto- 
plasma. 234 pp., 23 fig., 6 pl. Leipzig. 

HormetstER, W., ’65.—Ueber die Mechanik der Bewegungen des Protoplasmas. 
Flora, pp. 7-12. 

Jennincs, H. S.,’04.—Contributions to the Study of the Behavior of Lower Organ- 
isms. pp. 130-230. Carnegie Institution, Washington. 

Kutune, W., 64.—Unters. ber das Protoplasma u. die Contractilitat. Leipzig. 

NAGELI, C.,’55.—Die Glitschbewegung, eine besondere Art der periodischen Bewe- 
gung des Inhalts in Pflanzenzellen. Pflanzenphysiol. Unters. 
Heft I. Pp. 49-53. 

PENARD, Euc., ’90.—Etudes sur les Rhizopodes d’eau douce. Mem. de la Soc. de 
Physique et d’Histoire Naturelle de Genéve. T. 31, pp. 1-230. 

’o02.—Faune rhizopodique du bassinsdu Leman. pp. 714. Geneve. 

Ruumster, L., ’98.—Physikalische Analyse von Lebenserscheinungen der Zelle. 
Bewegung, Nahrungsaufnahme, Defakation, Vacuolen-Pulsation, 
und Gehausebau bei lobosen Rhizopoden. Arch. f. Entw.-mech. 
der Organismen, Bd, vii, pp. 130-350. 

Scuuttze, M., ’63.—Das Protoplasma der Rhizopoden und der Pflanzenzellen. 
Leipzig. 

Wa tticu, G. C., °63.—On an Undescribed Indigenous Form of Amceba. Ann. 
and Mag. of Nat. His. Bd. ii, pp. 287. 


358 Oris P. Dellinger 


DESCRIPTION OF PLATES 


Plates I and II are photographs of living Amceba taken with B & L 3 objective and No. 1 eyepiece 
All but Nos. 10 and 13 in Plate II are of the same Ameba. 


Pirate I 


Fig. 1. Amoeba verrucosa, side view. Just afterattachment is formed at the posterior end. The 
Ameeba is thick at the posterior end and has a definite point of attachment in that region. The 
anterior end is also attached and the lines observed from above are seen to run to the point of 
attachment at the anterior end. 

Figs. 2 and 4. A. verrucosa, side view. In Fig. 4 the Ameba is just ready to form a new attachment 
at the posterior end. It is noticed that the anterior end is not thin but is thick. 

Fig. 3. A. verrucosa side view. Was creeping on the ceiling and was jarred loose except at the 
anterior end. The anterior end is rounded and is not thin. 

Fig. 5. A. verrucosa, top view. 

Fig. 6. . A. proteus, side view. The Ameeba is attached at a and b and the anterior end is being 
advanced to form a new attachment. Note the definiteness of the points of attachment. 

Fig. 7. A. proteus, side view. The Ameeba is attached at a and b. The substance at bis drawn 
upinto a well-defined post. Theanteriorendis coming to the substratum to form a new attachment. 
It moved from ¢ to d while the plate was being exposed. A new pseudopod is appearing at e. 

Fig. 8. The samie Amceba a few seconds later. e has grown in length. Attachment at a is 
releasing and another has been formed at f. 


Prate II 


Figs. 1 to 6. A. proteus, side view. Changing from one attachment to another. At position 7, the 
anterior end has reached over to attach. At 2, the attachment at the posterior end is released. 
At 3,4 and 5 the substance is flowing over to the anterior end. At 6, the substance is well over, and 
the Ameeba is putting out a new pseudopod at the anterior end. 

Figs. 7, 8 and 9. Ameeba proteus,side view. Moving by long loops. The Ameeba has just brought 
the anterior end to the substratum but has not attached it. It slides along and is not attached until 
the position in Fig.g is reached. The substance then flowed over to the anterior end as in Figs. 1 to 6. 
Note the smallness of the pseudopods at the posterior end on which the Ameba is resting. 

Fig. 10. A. proteus, top view. Showing form taken when moving in a clear field. The Amoeba © 
in this photograph is attached at the anterior and posterior ends and probably forms a loop. 

Figs. 11 and 12. A.proteus, side view. Upon small pseudopods, which is often the case. A new 
anterior end is being extended in 77 and is brought to the substratum and attached in 72. 

Fig. 3. A. proteus, top view. Moving in the presence of alge, with numerous pseudopods at the 
anterior end. 


LOCOMOTION OF AMCBA AND ALLIED FORMS. Oris P. De.iincer. LAL ANATID, I 


Tue JourNar or Expermmentar Zoorocy, Vor. 1, No. 3. 


LOCOMOTION OF AMCEBZ AND ALLIED FORMS. Oris P. DeLLINGER. PLATE II 


Tue JourNat oF ExprertmMentar Zoérocy, Vor. m1, No. 3. 


. 
= 
. 
{ 
f 
J 
' 
* 
1 
a aa 
i 


CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE 
MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY AT HARVARD COLLEGE, 
E. L. MARK, Direcror.—No. 180. 7 


LIGHT REACTIONS IN LOWER ORGANISMS 
I. STENTOR CCERULEUS 


BY 


Ss. O. MAST 
Professor of Biological Science at Hope College, Holland, Mich., U. S. A. 


Wirth Six Ficures 


RempLITCrOd UCHOLME Ee Reiter = cis eo. Ne o.ciake aa ole wad o tYs those ototoa ed oa ere Senet Sees 359 
2. Material and apparatus ........ SO HOC Oe Io Ae BOO ric aan Gc ca OBA acoE 360 
QMO) SERV ACLON SH peeeey eats choi clexcien= ons io) tiyouele Ves sieie ehsvaveiaPe arco aah eid scree ae noes RE RE 366 
PA oimae VEG VITUS SO LEDIEOLS Meee se soit oie iy yie reveal yess ciel ON sie 6 eieie [oasis lo mice fea raie Seer ae 366 

Ame loni ght ormaniin tensity. <ctacs see ate «ec reeeieiets eke aco tts s Secale aioe ete 366 
bemuovlightipraded tan initensi Gyiscts (yese)=ive 2) iste shes see eios cae Sos erevelehave ol stove Ie cia alee pleats 371 

BaP ACEACHCOnS COHLOLS faiy crevasse Sato, ior Se os REIS toe wera oer as mee Cacia om tere 377 

Ore bhresholdetorlrchitss tum ilay sec syst ca hers see tei nePensioie ce cietese Sao cleree PeeO EN 381 

ZN Vigiecneranimales sided! lumina ted ictmx\s)<screiers lave Ae are @ eich eve levon clare eicietsisareeaisters 381 

IBITS UETHGEROG Sperqereyeun arererelalersc ore tele Mine «ae IE ero cis eet ee ee 382 

Slacoiays (aetilaveyel Soh Bcc Gian ae ta SERRA en a A AMG mermaid Seas hc 383 

intial hat oboe ata JEBe atte Done DOE H TOC eed ore SOB neAG ata sa0c 384 

Due vathitheanimal:sranterior end iJlluminateds2-4--c- s01- een ee eieeeee earn 384 

AMR S UI TLLTIVALS MRRP C TR eo eeeroFer vies ars: ti clove cs ciaisteiie'w Sualln eosiGidione dierelengs peers ais avavostanae sete ere 392 
Fem DIDO pT ap i varrraraates foystop-farsvers ci wievasr Si cieoatatsiora Sg ays sjorcle.evele Gisra.slerelelere Meee oa ail eree elms 393 
Os. ARENGIDY. 36 so. bow Sa adea Oo COCOA CORES FEO ances Sea Anan ON en cape anGunad ce doe 394 


I. INTRODUCTION 


The light reactions of Stentor cceruleus have been described 
in detail by Jennings (’o4a, p. 29) in a paper, entitled “Re- 
actions to Light in Ciliates and Flagellates.’’ His work shows 
conclusively that such reactions do not take place in accordance 
with the tropism theory as set forth by Loeb, Verworn, or Holt 
and Lee, but that the animals orient by means of “motor 
reactions,’ 2. e., by turning toward a structurally defined side 
(aboral), when stimulated, and then proceeding on a new path 
at an angle with the old one. My experimental results, while 


THE JOURNAL OF ExPERIMENTAL Zo6 oGy, Vor. 111, No. 3. 


360 S. O. Mast 


differing in a few minor details from those of Jennings, lead to 
the same general conclusions. Our methods, however, have been 
quite different. Jennings laid particular stress on the detailed 
movements of the individuals, while I directed most careful 
attention to the regulation of the stimulus. Moreover, he dealt 
with some phases of the subject more extensively than I did, 
while I dealt with others more extensively than he did. My 
experiments were, however, entirely completed and much of 
this paper was written before the work of Jennings was publish- 
ed. The facts that, working entirely independently of each 
other on the same organism and with different methods, we 
have obtained results which lead to the same general conclusion 
and that this conclusion is not in agreement with the commonly 
accepted tropism theories, have led me to prepare this paper for 
publication even at the risk of repeating some matters already 
published by others. 

I wish, before continuing further, to express my appreciation of 
many valuable suggestions by Dr. G. H. Parker, under whose 
direction the work was pursued, and my indebtedness to Prof. 
E. L. Mark, not only for granting exceptional facilities for 
carrying on the work but also for his encouraging interest. 
I am also under obligation to Dr. H. 8. Jennings for helpful 
criticism. 


2. MATERIAL AND APPARATUS 


The animals used in the following experiments were obtained 
by letting aquatic plants collected in a pond known to contain 
Stentor, decay in battery jars nearly filled with water. Stentors 
begin to multiply rapidly after Paramecium disappears or while 
it is decreasing. If, however, the jars contain more than about 
one-tenth as much vegetable matter as water, fermentation 
takes place to such an extent that the Stentors are apparently 
all killed, for while in such cultures there are usually numerous 
Paramecia, Stentors never appear, or if they do, there are very 
few of them. The cultures should be kept in diffused daylight 
or total darkness at a temperature of about 22° C. If condi- 
tions are favorable, Stentors usually appear about ten days after 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 361 


the cultures are put up, and then multiply very rapidly, frequently 
becoming so numerous that the walls of the jars to which they 
attach themselves, especially near the surface of the water, 
become densely covered with them in the course of a few days; 
but they soon decrease in numbers again and frequently dis- 
appear altogether within a week. It was found that if a few 
stalks of hay were added to cultures about to decrease in 
numbers they could be kept in good condition much longer than 
otherwise. Frequently it happens that after the Stentors seem 
to have completely disappeared in a given culture they reappear 
and multiply very rapidly. The disappearance and reappear- 
ance of Stentors in a culture is due largely, as Peters ('02) has 
shown, to chemical changes brought about by the decaying 
vegetable matter. While working on the following experiments, 
several cultures were constantly kept on hand. ‘This insured 
an abundance of good material at all times. 

Stentors, as is well known, are usually found most abundantly 
on the shady side of the vessel in which they are kept. If 
put into a dish containing water and placed in front of a window, 
they swim about for a time seemingly at random, but soon orient 
and then swim from the source of light, apparently as nearly 
parallel with the light rays as possible. How and why do they 
orient? Do they move parallel with the rays of light, and if so, 
why? What is the minimum light intensity which causes orienta- 
tion? Is this a constant? 

Answering these questions involves quantitative as well as 
qualitative work. In order to make quantitative work worthy 
of the name, it is, as Jennings (04b, p. 507) has well stated, at 
least necessary to know what we are trying to measure, 1. €., We 
must understand the reactions of the organism with which we 
are working in detail, and know the precise value of the stimulus 
as it reaches the organism; and to know this we must understand 
the physical and chemical properties of the stimulus. The 
results and conclusions of the following experiments present a 
striking illustration of the importance of the latter. Strasburger 
(’78) found that certain swarm spores were positive in large 
vessels and negative in hanging drops in the same light intensity. 


362 S. O. Mast 


Rothert (’03) obtained similar results, and both offered quite 
extensive theoretical explanations for the cause of this; but 
neither got at the truth of the matter because they did not 
recognize the effect of the curvature of the surface of the hanging 
drop on the light as it entered the water. Chmielevsky (’04) 
proved conclusively by theoretical considerations and by means 
of photographs that the intensity of a hanging drop of liquid is 
frequently greatest in parts of the drop farthest from the source 
of light, so that it is more than probable that the organisms in 
hanging drops in the experiments of Strasburger and of Rothert 
reacted to light precisely as they did in the vessels. 

In the following experiments particular stress was laid on 
the study and regulation of the light used as a stimulus. The 
work was carried on in a large basement dark-room in which 
the temperature varied but little. “This room was supplied with 
gas, carbon filament incandescent lamps of various candle-power, 
a carbon arc lamp, and six-glower and _ single-glower Nernst 
lamps. Moreover, the room was so situated that sunlight, direct 
and diffused, could also be made use of. The Nernst single- 
glower lamp was found to be the most satisfactory source of 
light for all experiments, both quantitative and qualitative, pro- 
viding the intensity required was not great. Such a glower 
consists of a single, small, straight rod composed of oxide of 
zircon. ‘Lhe rods are about I mm. in diameter and vary from 
Icm. to 2.5cm. in length, thus producing when heated a compact 
source of light. ‘They are heated in air by an electric current 
and need not be protected by a globe, so that much reflection 
and refraction are avoided. ‘The glowers were generally not used 
in connection with the regular Nernst lamp, but were mounted 
in front of an opening in a box the inside of which was painted 
dead black (Fig. 1, d, c), to prevent reflection from the back- 
ground. The box used was nearly cubical in form, each side being 
about 35 cm. long. The end containing the opening was made of a 
heavy asbestos pad to avoid danger from fire, since the tempera- 
ture of the glower becomes very high. In case one glower did 
not -produce light of sufficient intensity, three were so grouped 
that a cross-section would form a small equilateral triangle. 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 363 


When these were heated they appeared much like a single 
glower somewhat enlarged. We have, then, in the Nernst 
glower, arranged as described above, a light the direction of 
the rays of which can be pretty accurately controlled, certainly 
more nearly so than the direction of rays from carbon 
incandescent or arc lamps. But the light intensity of the 
Nernst glower, while it probably varies less than that of 
the carbon arc, certainly varies more than that of the carbon 
incandescent. It varies with the voltage, the age and composi- 
tion of the glower, the temperature of the room and especially 
with currents of air. Most of these factors were, however, under 
control. The light intensity with known voltage and tempera- 
ture was frequently measured; the voltage was recorded from 
time to time during the progress of nearly all quantitative ex- 
periments by means of a meter in the circuit; the temperature of 
the room was nearly constant and drafts of air were avoided as 
much as possible. But as a matter of fact the variation under 
the most favorable conditions was so great that delicate quantita- 
tive results must be accepted with caution. The variation 
in light intensity, and its relation to the variation in voltage are 
indicated in the Appendix (p. 394). 

A modification of a piece of apparatus devised by Sabine and 
Yerkes (Yerkes, ’03) was used extensively in the following experi- 
ments and found very serviceable. By means of it a field of light 
can be produced which either is uniform in intensity through- 
out or gradually increases in intensity from one end to the 
other. he rays producing the field are all practically perpen- 
dicular to the plane of the field and there is little diffusion. The 
defects in Oltmanns’’ method of producing light of graded 


1Oltmanns (’92) produced in an aquarium light gradually increasing in intensity from one end t 
the other, by placing a hollow prism filled with a mixture of India ink and glycerine-gelatine between 
the source of light and the aquarium. The India-ink mixture of course absorbed only a little light 
at the thin end of the prism, but gradually more toward the thickerend. Oltmanns and others assumed 
that the light rays in the aquarium under such conditions were parallel with each other and perpen- 
dicular to the side through which they entered. That this is not true, is clear from a theoretical as 
well as from a practical standpoint. The India-ink mixture contains numerous solid particles of 
carbon in suspension, which, together with particles in suspension in the water in the aquarium, 


unquestionably diffuse the light more or less. 


364 S. O. Mast 


intensity, which have been pointed out by Strasburger (’78, 
p- 588), Miss ‘Towle (’oo), and others, seem tobe largely if 
not entirely avoided in this method. ‘The piece of apparatus 
used in these experiments will be referred to as the light-grader. 
Its construction will be readily understood from the accompany- 
ing diagrams (Figs. 1, 2). 

The walls of the apparatus are all light-proof and dead black 
inside, so as to prevent reflection. ‘The outline of a cross-section 
at any point is square. ‘he upper portion of the front wall of 
the vertical part of the apparatus is hung on hinges forming a 
door. From the bottom of this door is hung a loose vertical 
curtain, which can be so opened that observations can be made 
without admitting light. The glower is parallel with the minor 
axis of the lens. It is mounted in front of a small opening 
in a light-proof box, painted inside dead black, which thus 
forms a non-reflecting background. The glower and stage 
are at the conjugate focal points of the lens, and, therefore, at 
equal distances (50 cm.) from it. ‘The plano-convex cylindrical 
lens used is 25 cm. long, 10 cm. wide and has a radius of 
curvature of 12.5 cm. 

A cylindrical lens will not form a single definite image of an 
object, but rather a series of images, since by means of it light 
is focused only in reference to one plane. If, then, the object, 
e. g., a Nernst glower, is placed at one of the conjugate focal 
points so that the distance from the lens to the glower is equal to 
that from the lens to the image, and the glower is so arranged that 
it is pependicular to the axis of the lens, the image will not 
consist of a narrow band of light as large as a glower, which would 
be true if the segment of a sphere were used as the lens, but it 
will consist of a comparatively large field of light, the length of 
which is proportional to the functional length of the lens, while 
the width is equal to the length of the glower, regardless of the 
functional width of the lens (see Fig. 2). But since the amount 
of light which passes through the lens is directly proportional 
to the functional width of the lens and the width of the field is 
constant, it is clear that the intensity of light in the field, if we 
disregard the amount of light absorbed by the lens, must also be 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 365 


Fic. 1 


Fig. 1. Avvertical section of the light-grader. The lens a, which is a segment of a cylinder, has 
its longitudinal axis lying in the plane of the section; b, stage; c, Nernst glower; d, non-reflecting 
background; e, mirror; f, light rays; g, opaque screens. 

Fig. 2. Stereographic view of light, lens and image. a, Lens; b, field of light graded in intensity and 
produced by the image of the glower; c, Nernst glower; d, opaque screen containing a triangular 


opening and lying on the upper flat surface of the lens; f, light rays. 


366 S. O. Mast 


theoretically proportional to its functional width. Direct meas- 
urements of the light intensity with different functional widths 
of the lens proved this to be true within the limits of error. If, 
then, the lens be covered with an opaque screen containing a tri- 
angular opening, the base of which is parallel with the minor axis 
of the lens as represented in Fig. 2, there will result a rectangular 
field of light in which the intensity gradually diminishes from the 
end produced by light which passes through the base of the trian- 
gular opening to the opposite end, where theoretically it fades 
into darkness. Practically, however, it was found to be impos- 
sible to cut the apex of the triangular opening so as to prevent an 
apparent line at the end of least intensity. Since the light 
intensity of the field is proportional to the functional width of 
the lens, it is evident that the rate of diminution in inten- 
sity depends upon the ratio of the altitude of the triangular 
opening to the length of its base, 7. e., decreasing the altitude 
or increasing the base causes an increase in the rate of diminu- 
tion and vice versa. ‘The facts that a field of light, either uni- 
form in intensity or graded in intensity, can be produced by 
merely changing the form of the opening in the screen over 
the lens, and that the intensity can be changed by altering the 
width of the opening, and readily calculated for any width if it 
is known for a given width, make this a very desirable piece 
of apparatus for quantitative as well as qualitative work. 


3. OBSERVATIONS 


A. Moving Stentors 


a. In Light Uniform in Intensity 


In studying the reactions of Stentors to light-rays_per- 
pendicular to the plane of the field, animals were put on 
the stage of the light-grader in a shallow aquarium! contain- 
ing about 3 mm. of water taken from the culture jar and 


‘The aquarium used was made by enclosing a space of desired size on a clear piece of plate glass 
with a ridge of paraffin attached to the glass with balsam. 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 367 


carefully filtered. A field of light of the desired size and 
intensity, produced by regulating the opening in the screen, 
was thrown into the middle of the aquarium. The shallow- 
ness of the water restricted the movements of the animals 
almost entirely to the plane of the field and thus prevented 
orientation. « If, under these conditions, a uniform field of light 
about 16 candle-meters in intensity is thrown into the aquarium 
and a considerable number of Stentors introduced and evenly 
scattered, it will be noticed in the course of a few seconds that 
nearly all of the Stentors have left the light area (a), as repre- 
sented in Fig. 3. These animals, as stated above, can not orient 
toward the source of light; how, then, do they get out of the 
light area and how do they keep out? 


Fig. 3. Distribution of Stentors as seen in the light-grader 30 seconds after a field of light 16 
candle-meters in intensity throughout was thrown into the middle of the aquarium containing 
water 2 mm.deep. a, Field of light, natural size; b, shaded area of aquarium. In this experiment the 
animals could move only in directions perpendicular to the rays of light. 


After being stirred up, as they are when transported from 
the culture jar to the aquarium, they move rapidly, so that those 
in the light area soon swim out of it, but without any noticeable 
change in their course as they pass from light into darkness. The 
animals in that case get out of the light area by mere random 
movements. But when animals in the dark happen in their 
random movements to come in contact with the light area, they 
suddenly stop, swing their anterior end towards one side, and then 
continue on a new path which forms a definite angle with the old 
one. The new course may take them back into the dark area 
at once, but if it does not, as frequently happens, they soon stop, 
turn again towards one side and ‘begin again on a new path. 


368 S. O. Mast 


This reaction may be repeated several times before the animal 
succeeds in getting out of the light area; indeed, Stentors were 
frequently seen to repeat this reaction until they finally got 
out of the light area at the side opposite the one at which they 
entered. ‘hus we see that the animals are kept out of the 
light area by means of what Jennings has called the motor 
reaction. : 

It is evident from the preceding description that the light 
rays in this experiment are practically perpendicular to the 
plane of the field, and that the movements of the animals are 
almost entirely restricted to thisplane, so that the rays strike them 
perpendicular to their longitudinal axis; therefore the direction 
of the direct rays certainly can not affect their movements. 
But it may be argued that there is sufficient light reflected 
from other individuals and from particles of various other 
substance in the water to affect the direction of movement of 
Stentors and that they leave the light area because of this 
reflected light, to which they orient. That this is not true is 
clearly shown by the fact that Stentors are found swimming 
at random in the shaded regions close to the edge of the light area, 
as represented in Fig. 3. “The shaded area contains practi- 
cally as much reflected light as the light area, so that if the re- 
flected light affected the direction of movement at all, we should 
expect it to affect that of the Stentors in the shaded area as well 
as that of those in the light. But if this were true, we should 
expect the an'mals in the shaded regions to be less numerous near 
the edge of the light area than elsewhere; we should also expect 
to find them oriented toward the light area, but this is not the 
case. The animals are as numerous in this region as they are 
farther from the light area and are here found swimming at 
random. ‘That the motor reaction is not due to the temperature 
is evident from the fact that Paramecium, which is more sensi- 
tive to heat than Stentor (Jennings, ’04a, p. 31), does not respond 
when it passes into the light area. Thus we must conclude that 
the animals do not leave the light area by orienting to reflected 
rays, but that they are carried out of this area by random move- 
ments caused by the stimulation of the light, and that they are 


Light Reactions 1n Lower Organisms 369 
Oo _= 


kept out by means of a motor reaction induced by an increase 
in light intensity as they attempt to enter. 

If the light is not very intense many of the animals, after hav- 
ing given the motor reaction several times without succeeding 
in getting out of the field, become quiet or continue on in a 
straight course, no longer responding with the motor reaction. 
Such animals have apparently become acclimated. Some ani- 
mals give the motor reaction apparently as soon as their ante- 
rior end touches the field of light, others do not respond until 
they are entirely within the field of light, while still others 
pass into the field three or four millimeters before reacting, and 
if the light is not extremely intense, there are always some which 
apparently are not affected by the light at all. “hese responses 
show clearly that there is great variability in the sensitiveness 
to light among animals from the same culture, and this is true 
even under the most favorable circumstances. “The variability, 
together with the fact that Stentors readily become acclimated 
to light, makes quantitative work exceedingly difhcult. 

The fact that the motor reaction is given by some animals as 
soon as their anterior ends reach the light area, shows that stimu- 
lation of this endis sufficient to induce a reaction, butit does 
not show that the posterior end 1s not sensitive, nor does it show 
that the posterior end is less sensitive than the anterior. This 
question will be dealt with later. 

In the experiment described above, Stentors were frequently 
seen to enter the field of light so that their paths form rather 
acute angles w:th the edge of the field. Animals thus entering 
the field, in responding with the motor reaction, often turned 
toward the center of the field, and in so doing were of course 
carried farther into the light area; whereas, had they turned 
toward the edge they would immediately have been carried 
back into the dark area again. This indicates, as Jennings (‘04a, 
pp. 33-35) very clearly showed by direct observation, that they 
always turn toward a structurally defined side regardless of the 
region stimulated. That this is really what takes place, can be 
demonstrated as follows: Water to the depth of about one 
centimeter is put into the aquarium in the light-grader so that 


370 S. O. Mast 


the Stentors can readily swim in all directions. If, now, by 
means of a mirror held beneath the aquarium, a field of light 
about 20 candle-meters in intensity is suddenly flashed on 
animals swimming at random in the dark, nearly every animal, 
no matter in which direction it is moving,—from the source of 
light or toward it,—stops almost instantly, turns, and then starts 
on a new path, 7.e., responds with the motor reaction. If the 
animals are numerous, they may be seen literally to turn in every 
direction at the same instant. If reaction takes place in accor- 
dance with the tropism theory as defined by Loeb, Verworn, or 
Holt and Lee, we should of course expect all animals not oriented 
to turn from the source of light, but we should not expect those 
which happen to be already oriented when the light is flashed 
on them to turn at all. This, however, is not the case. As 
stated above, practically all turn; some of those moving at right 
angles to the rays turn toward the source of light and others 
from it; and of those already oriented, some turn to the right, 
others to the left. This is a most striking and convincing 
experiment. It strongly supports the direct observations of 
Jennings by which he demonstrated that Stentor always turns 
toward a structurally defined side and shows clearly that there 
is no apparent relation between the direction of the rays and the 
direction of turning. 

When Stentors which are oriented to a given light respond with 
the motor reaction to an increase in intensity of the light, they 
are for the time being thrown out of orientation, but by repeated 
response to the more intense light they soon become oriented 
again. If, however, the light intensity is increased very much 
(125+ candle-meters), they may again respond after having be- 
come oriented. As a matter of observation, under such condi- 
tions the responses in many individuals are repeated in such 
rapid succession that the animals appear to be turning about a 
pivot at their posterior end. 

Jennings (’o4a, pp. 49-50) writes: “When a large number of 
Euglena are swimming toward the source of light, if the illumi- 
nation is suddenly decreased in any way, they give the typical 
motor reaction described in my previous paper as a response to 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 371 


other classes of stimuli (Jennings, ’00, p. 235). ‘That is, they 
turn at once toward the dorsal side.’’ Euglena are positive, 
so that when they are swimming toward the source of light 
they are oriented; when, under such conditions, they respond 
to a decrease in light intensity, they are, as was found to be true 
in case of Stentor, thrown out of orientation. ‘These reactions 
seem decidedly fatal to any theory which assumes that animals 
remain oriented because when in this position similar surfaces 
on opposite sides of the body, or locomotor appendages on such 
surfaces, are equally stimulated. 

We have thus far shown that when Stentors pass from a dark 
into a light region they respond with the motor reaction. Will 
they respond likewise when they pass from a region of one 
intensity to that of another? 

Two adjoining regions of different light intensity can readily 
be produced by reflecting the field of light in the light-grader 
so that the reflected field overlaps the field produced by direct 
light. Where the two fields overlap, the light will of course be 
more intense than elsewhere, although it is exceedingly difb- 
cult to see any line of demarcation between these regions of 
different light intensity; but when Stentors swimming in the 
regions of lower intensity happen to strike this plane they respond 
with the motor reaction much as if they had come in contact 
with a glass wall. If, however, they strike the plane while 
swimming from the more intensely illuminated region, they pass 
on apparently unaffected. It is decidedly interesting as well 
as instructive to watch the results when these animals reach 
the practically invisible plane between the regions of different 
light intensity; if moving in one direction, without any response 
whatever; if moving in the opposite direction, with a marked 
reaction. 


b. In Light Graded in Intensity 
Holt and Lee (01, pp. 471-475) found that if Stentors are put 


into an elongated aquarium in which the light becomes gradually 
more intense from one end to the other, they collect at the dark 


37/2 Se OMViast 


end of the aquarium. ‘These investigators produced such light 
conditions by means of Oltmanns’ prism, as follows: “Parallel 
rays of light fell horizontally at right angles to the long axis of a 
narrow trough destined to receive the organisms. Between 
the trough and the incoming light there intervened a prismatic 
screen. Thin at one end, it there let almost all the light 
through; but becoming gradually thicker it gradually diminished 
the intensity of the rays till at the opposite end the screen was 
opaque and intercepted all the light.” ‘If numbers of blue 
Stentors are put into a trough that is moderately illuminated 
under the conditions described above, the animals at first swim 
away from the light until they encounter the farther wall of the 
trough. ‘They then swim backward a little distance and start 
off in a new direction, as Jennings has described for other species 
of Infusoria, some toward the light end of the trough and some 
toward the dark end. Soon they strike the wall again, and 
again start off in one direction or the other, and this series of 
movements is repeated many times. The preponderance of 
movement is toward the dark end, and in time by far the 
majority are found there swimming about.’’ Holt and Lee 
explain the cause of the preponderance of movement toward 
the dark end by applying Verworn’s hypothesis. “The animals 
swimming from the side of the aquarium nearest the source of 
light toward the opposite side are, they say, so deflected toward 
the dark end, because the side of the animals facing the light end 
of the aquarium is more strongly illuminated than the opposite 
side. ‘his is due in part to the decrease in light intensity from 
one end of the aquarium to the other, but more especially to 
the fact that the side facing the more highly illuminated end of 
the aquarium receives considerable light which is reflected from 
the water and sides of the trough at the well lighted end. The 
fact that an appreciable quantity of light is thus reflected is 
sufficiently attested by Strasburger (’78) and Miss Towle (’00). 

‘Thus it is clear that Holt and Lee in their explanation of the 
cause of the “preponderance of movement toward the dark end” 
make use largely of two factors, the swimming from the source 
of light and the light reflection from the more highly illuminated 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 373 


end of the aquarium. Can these factors be eliminated and, if 
so, will the animals still collect at the darker end of the aqua- 
rium? If, with these factors eliminated, they still collect at the 
darker end, it is clear that the hypothesis of Verworn, as applied 
by Holt and Lee, will not explain the cause of such a collection. 

A field consisting of rays practically perpendicular to the plane 
of the field and graded in intensity can readily be produced in 
the light-grader by placing a screen containing a triangular open- 
ing over the lens, as described above (pp. 364-366). If, now, a 
considerable number of Stentors are put on to the stage in the 
light-grader in an aquarium containing thoroughly filtered water 
about two millimeters in depth, and such a field of light is thrown 


Fig. 4. Distribution of Stentors as seen in the light-grader one minute after a field of light graded 
in intensity was thrown into the middle of the aquarium containing water 2 mm. deep. The shallow- 
ness of the waterforces the animals to move at right angles to the light rays. a, Field of light; b, shaded 
area; the light intensity atc is 16 candle-meters; at d, 4 candle-meters and ate, 1.6 candle- 
meters. The intensity at f was so low that it could barely be distinguished from that in the 
shaded area. 


into the aquarium perpendicular to the bottom, it is found that 
in a short time nearly all the animals have left the more in- 
tensely illuminated regions of the field, while there are as many 
in the region of lower intensity as there are in the shaded area. 
This result is represented in Fig. 4. 

If the light area is made as large as the aquarium, the animals 
collect at the darker end. In this experiment there are a good 
many animals in the light area when the light is first turned on 
and each one will cause diffusion of light, 7. e., will be a secondary 
source of light, reflecting rays parallel with the plane of the field. 


374 Se Oo Masi 


Those at the most highly illuminated end of the field will reflect 
more light than those at the darker end. May not, then, the 
collection of the Stentors at the darker end be due to orientation 
to these reflected rays, as has been suggested by some who believe 
ray-direction to be the primal cause of orientation? An argu- 
ment has already been presented in this paper (p. 368) which 
seems to indicate very clearly that the reflected light would not 
under the above conditions affect the direction of movement 
of the animals. But to test this more in detail and to learn 
precisely how the animals react in collecting at the darker end 
of the aquarium, a square field of light 2.5 cm. ona side was 
thrown into the middle of an aquarium on the stage in the light- 
erader. The field of light was 125+ candle-meters at one end 
and o+ candle-meters at the other. The aquarium contained 
water about two millimeters deep, taken from the culture jar 
and carefully filtered. Special precautions were taken to pre- 
vent diffusion of light by dust particles on the lens, on the water, 
or on the mirror used to reflect the light after it had passed 
through the aquarium into a dark chamber, where it was absorbed 
(see Fig. 1, p. 365). One Stentor at a time was taken with a fine 
pipette from a dish kept in the dark, then carefully dropped 
into the center of the field, and its reactions studied. In this 
way the effect of light reflected from numerous animals was of 
course eliminated. The movements of the animals, practically 
restricted to the plane of the field by the shallowness of the water 
in the aquarium, could be distinctly seen with the naked eye, but 
of course their structure could not be made out. As soon as an 
animal was released in the center of the field it responded with 
the motor reaction several times in rapid succession, moving 
but a short distance between successive responses, until it appar- 
ently became acclimated to the intensity of light to which it 
was subjected. If at the end of this period it happened to be 
headed so that in moving forward it did not pass from regions 
of lower to regions of higher light intensity, it usually continued 
making nearly a straight path until it got out of the light area. 
If, however, it happened to be oriented so that its movements 
carried itinto regions of higher light intensity, it continued only 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 375 


“SI 


a short distance before again responding with the motor reaction, 
and such response was repeated at short intervals, until the 
animal happened to become so directed that when it moved 
forward it no longer passed from regions of lower to regions of 
higher intensity. It thus continued on a straight course out 
of the field, there being no longer any stimulation to induce the 


l / 


eo 
yy 


yY ~ 

, x Se 
Re “ | : 

Ss : ~ 


~ 
™s 


da 


Fig. 5. Each square represents the field of light at its actual size. The rays of light strike the 


plane of the field practically perpendicularly. The higher light intensity at the side 7 was 125+ candle- 
meters, and that at the opposite side, d,o-+candle-meters. The lines within the square represent, 
approximately, the paths of twelve Stentors taken at random from a dish in the dark and dropped 
one at a time as nearly into the center of the field as possible. The angles in the paths indicate 


points where the animals gave the motor reaction. 


motor reaction. It will be seen from this description that 
Stentors in light graded in intensity, moving perpendicular to 
the direction of the rays, do not orient and then continue in a 
definite direction. “They may continue moving in any direction 
excepting in such as would carry them from regions of lower to 
regions of higher light intensity. ‘This fact will become more evi- 
dent by referring to the accompanying figures (Fig. 5), which were 


376 S. O. Mast 


made by tracing the paths of the animals on a sheet of paper as 
they proceeded on their course. With a little practice it was 
possible to watch the animals and draw at the same time, so 
that while these tracings do not represent the paths in detail very 
accurately, they at least represent the general course taken by 
the animals. 

It will be seen from these figures that only one of the twelve 
animals left the light area at the most highly illuminated end, 
and that this one continued responding with the motor reaction 
at short intervals until it got out of the area. Evidently it did 
not happen to become headed in such a direction that movement 
after a reaction would no longer carry it into regions of higher 
light intensity. It will also be seen that there is absolutely 
no indication of orientation with reference to degrees of light 
intensity, 7. ¢., the animals move continuously in any direction 
except such as would carry them rapidly into regions of higher 
light intensity. Subsequently 64 additional animals were put 
into the graded field one at a time. Of these, four left the feld 
at the end of highest light intensity, 26 at the end of lowest 
intensity, and 34 at the two sides. 

We have in the above experiments eliminated the two prin- 
cipal factors (p. 372) upon which Holt and Lee based their 
explanation of “preponderance of movement toward the dark 
end,’ and have found that such movement still takes place. 
Evidently, then, their explanation will not hold. Jennings 
(o4a, p. 48), after stating this explanation and describing the 
motor reaction of Stentor due to light stimulations, continues 
with the following paragraph: 

“There is evidently nothing in this account [referring to the 
observations of Holt and Lee] which is inconsistent with the 
method of light reaction which I have described. On the 
contrary, the reason why the organisms finally swim toward the 
dark end and gather there becomes much more evident when 
the reaction method that I have described is taken into con- 
sideration. Let us suppose that the Stentors, after striking 
the back of the trough, turn in equal numbers toward D [dark 
end of trough] and toward L [light end of trough]. In those 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 277 


swimming toward D the anterior end is directed away from the 
source of strongest light (due to reflection from the lighted end 
of the dish L), and the animals are passing into a region of less 
intense light. ‘hereis thus nothing to cause the ‘motor reaction,’ 
with its accompanying change in the direction of movement. 
In the Stentors swimming toward L, on the other hand, the 
strongest light falls on fae anterior end, and the organisms are 
passing into a region of more intense light. Either of these 
factors taken separately may, as we have seen, cause the motor 
reaction (the turning toward the right aboral side), thus changing 
the directionin which the Stentors swim. ‘The animals which 
start to swim toward L will therefore soon be turned, and only 
when the direction of movement is toward D will there be no 
cause for further change.”’ 

In the experiments described above, we have substantiated 
Jennings’ ideas as set forth in this paragraph; we have shown 
that the preponderance of movement toward the darker end is due 
to the motor reaction induced by an increase in light intensity, 
which consequently prevents continuous movement toward the 
more highly illuminated end of the aquarium. 

It should, however, be kept in mind that these experiments 
were completed before Jennings’ paper was published, and there- 
fore were in no way influenced by his explanation. 


B. Attached Stentors 


In cultures many of the Stentors are usually found attached 
by the posterior end to solid objects of various kinds near the 
surface of the water. The relative number of attached and free 
individuals, however, depends upon the condition of the culture. 
In a given culture under certain conditions nearly all the animals 
may be free, whereas, in the same culture perhaps only a few 
days later, nearly all may be attached. From casual observa- 
tions, it was thought that the number of free animals depends 
upon the rate of reproduction. In some cultures it was difficult 
to study the reactions of free-swimming animals, because they 
attached themselves again almost immediately after being de- 


378 S. O. Mast 


tached. On the other hand it was difficult in some cultures 
to study reactions of fixed Stentors because the least disturbance 
seemed to cause them to separate from their support. 

Attached Stentors exhibit great variability in sensitiveness to 
light. For while the reactions are marked in some cultures when 
they are subjected toa sudden change—. g., either to an intensity 
of 120 candle-meters or to rather weak diffused daylight—in others 
only slight reactions are induced by sudden exposure either to 
strong direct sunlight or to the light from a carbon are of 250 
candle-power at a distance of 25 cm. (4000 candle-meters). In 
testing quite a number of cultures at different times, from Decem- 
ber to the following August, I did not find any in which there was 
no response to sudden exposure to direct sunlight; but in many 
cultures there were individuals that did not appear to be affected 
at all, and, as stated above, there were some cultures 1n which the 
response was not definite. It is, therefore, not surprising that 
Jennings was unable to get any light reactions from attached Sten- 
tors in the cultures that he studied. He says (’o4a, p. 32): “Such 
individuals do not react at all to light. When light is thrown on 
them they remain in the positions in which they are found at the 
beginning, neither contracting nor in any way changing their posi- 
tion. No matter whether the light is weak or strong, and with- 
out regard to the direction from which it comes, fixed Stentors give 
no reaction and show no orientation with reference to light. “The 
contact reaction apparently inhibits the light reaction completely.’ ’ 

This, as we have seen, is true only under certain conditions, 
but the interference of contact reactions probably has something 
to do with the variation, which is much greater in attached than 
in free swimming animals. Contact reactions probably also tend 
to keep the animals in such a position that their longitudinal 
axis is perpendicular to the surface to which they are attached, 
for they are usually found in this position-even when attached to 
vertical surfaces or to the upper surface of solids, and when attached 
to such surfaces they must maintain their position against gravi- 
tation. 

The reactions in detail are as follows: If Stentors in favorable 


Light Reactions 1n Lower Organisms 379 


condition are put into a dimly lighted aquarium containing water 
a few millimeters deep, they soon become attached to the bottom, 
take a position such that their longitudinal axis is approximately 
vertical and soon become quiet. If, now, the light is slightly in- 
creased, they begin to swing about their point of attachment, pre- 
sumably turning toward their aboral sides. If, however, the light 
is suddenly increased considerably, they at once contract, but 
in the course of a few moments they expand again and then 
slowly swing as they do when the light intensity 1s only slightly 
increased; after this they soon become quiet or break loose. 
These reactions are entirely independent of the direction of the 
light rays. ‘They are precisely the same in animals hanging from 
the surface film or from a cover glass floating on it, whether 
illuminated from above, below or from the side. ‘There is, how- 
ever, some evidence that the same reaction is induced by light 
of lower intensity when thrown on the anterior end than when 
thrown on the posterior, indicating that the anterior end 1s more 
sensitive than the posterior (p. 389.) 

Attached Stentors illuminated from the side do not orient, as 
might be expected at first thought. Experiments with reference 
to this point were repeated many times with animals in various 
conditions and with light varying in intensity from very strong 
direct sunlight striking the animals at various angles, to that 
which would barely induce a reaction; but in no case was there 
any definite indication of orientation. Assuming the tropism 
theories to be correct, we should, of course, expect the animals 
to orient, 7.¢., to turn until symmetrical portions are equally 
stimulated and then stop, and we should not expect any reaction 
in animals already oriented when subjected to light stimulation, 
unless the stimulations were intense enough to cause contraction. 
But, as stated above, such 1s not the fact; when slightly stimulated, 
the Stentors swing about their point of attachment regardless of 
the direction of the rays, and if illuminated from the side they do’ 
not stop swinging when the anterior end becomes directed away 
from the source of light. Evidently, then, the reactions of attached 
Stentors are not in accordance with the tropism theories. Are they 
in harmony with orientation by means of motor reactions? 


380 S. O. Mast 


The anterior end of Stentor, as will be shown later (p. 389),1s more 
sensitive to light than any other portion of the surface of the body, 
so that when it is turned from the source of light the stimulation 
onthe animal as a whole is weaker than when it is turned in any 
other direction. It is this which keeps free-swimming animals 
oriented after they have once attained such a position by means 
of the motor reaction (Jennings, ’o4a, p. 45). Why, then, do not 
attached Stentors remain oriented, when, in swinging about their 
point of attachment, they happen to reach a position in which 
their anterior ends are directed from the source of light? We 
have shown that free-swimming animals which are already 
oriented respond with the motor reaction if the light intensity 1s 
suddenly increased and thus are thrown out of orientation, and 
that if animals hanging from a surface film are suddenly illumin- 
ated from above they begin to swing about their points of attach- 
ment; so that the mere fact that the anterior end is directed from 
the source of light is not sufficient to prevent the motor reaction, 
1. €., turning toward a structurally defined side. We have also 
seen that attached Stentors are readily acclimated to light and, 
as this process of acclimation proceeds, we should expect to find 
a stage at which the turning of the anterior end from the source of 
light would cause enough reduction in. stimulation to prevent fur- 
ther swinging, 7.¢., to inhibit the motor reaction, and this is 
probably true. We find attached Stentors when not stimulated 
arranged so that the longitudinal axis is perpendicular to the 
surface to which they are attached. With these animals accli- 
mation has proceeded so far that there is no longer sufficient stimu- 
lation when the anterior end is directed from the source of light 
to induce the motor reaction. We should, therefore, expect them 
to maintain a position perpendicular to the surface to which they 
are attached. . 

If the light intensity, then, is great enough to cause a sufhcient 

-stimulation when the anterior end is turned from the source of 
light, attached Stentors respond with the motor reaction which 
prevents orientation; but if it is not intense enough to induce the 
motor reaction under these conditions, the tendency to take a 
position perpendicular to the surface on which they are attached 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 381 


again prevents orientation, so that the fact that attached Stentors 
do not orient is precisely what we should expect if the animals 
orient by means of motor reactions. 

In closing this section of the work, let me state clearly that, 
while I have found absolutely no evidence of tropic responses in 
any of my experiments with Stentor, I do not wish to be under- 
stood as intimating that difference in intensity on opposite sides 
of these organisms, or even that the direction in which the light 
rays pass through them, may not affect the direction of their 
motion. I shall, however, state again, as I have stated several 
times in the preceding pages, that the reactions of Stentors are not 
in accord with the tropism theory as defined by either Loeb, 
Verworn, or Holt and Lee. 


C. Threshold for Light Stimuli 
a. With the Animal’s Side Illuminated 


Since the threshold for light stimuli varies greatly with different 
individuals and in the same individual under different conditions, 
it is evident that the mere determination of it without correlating 
the physiological conditions of the animals with the individual 
variation in the threshold can be of little value. We have, how- 
ever, one problem that can be attacked without such correlation. 
I have assumed in some of the preceding discussions, that the 
anterior end of Stentor is more sensitive to light stimuli than 
any other portion of the surface. Now, this assumption can be 
tested if the threshold can be ascertained when animals under 
any given conditions are illuminated on the side, and again when, 
under the same conditions, they are illuminated on the anterior 
end. ‘To obtain the threshold for animals with the side illumin- 
ated, they were put on the stage of the light-grader in an aquarium 
containing water only about two millimeters deep. ‘Then the 
intensity of a small field of light thrown into the middle of the 
aquarium was reduced until the animals when passing from 
darkness to light no longer responded with the motor reaction. 


382 S: On Mast 


The movements of the animals were practically restricted to the 
plane of the bottom, so that when the animals passed into the 
held of light their sides only were fully illuminated. 

Three methods were used to ascertain the lowest intensity to 
which animals, under the above conditions, react. 

First Method.—A field of light of uniform intensity throughout 
was thrown into the aquarium and the intensity reduced by 
decreasing the functional width of the lens. The reactions of 
the animals as they passed into the light area were then studied. 
While theoretically this method, as well as those which follow, 
should yield accurate quantitative results, practically the results 
must be considered as at best only rather gross approximations, 
for in the first place it was impossible to prevent fluctuations in the 
light intensity; in the’second place Stentors are very readily accli- 
mated to light of low intensity; in the third place it was difficult 
to see at times if a response was given or not, owing to the low 
intensity to which many responded; and, finally, the individual 
Variation was so marked that without statistical methods the 
point of lowest intensity to which a definite number responded 
could be only roughly determined. The threshold also varies 
greatly in different cultures and in the same culture under different 
conditions. For example, a culture was tested March 3, at 3 
o’clock, and definite reactions to a change in intensity of 1.6 
candle-meters were obtained, although only a small proportion 
of the whole number reacted. The same Stentors were tested 
again after having been in the light-grader go minutes in dark- 
ness undisturbed, and then only an occasional animal responded 
to a change in intensity of 4.8 candle-meters. 

It was found that the movements of the animals could be 
followed best by studying their shadows cast on a piece of white 
paper held about 20 cm. below the aquarium. ‘The paper, if 
unglazed, diffuses the light, and therefore reflects it very nearly 
equally over both the dark and the light area, so that the intensity 
difference is not appreciably affected. 

A large number of Stentors from different cultures were tested 
for their threshold to light stimuli, as described above, on the 
following days: February 20, 21, 22 and 23; March 4, 10 and 16; 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 383 


also August I, 2, 3 and 4. In all the cultures experimented with 
animals were found that gave unquestionable reactions when they 
passed from darkness into a light intensity of 1.5 candle-meters 
and in some culture when they passed into an intensity of 1.2 
candle-meters. But no definite reactions were obtained under 
any conditions from animals which passed from darkness into 
light of an intensity lower than 1.2 candle-meters. 

Second Method.—In this method, as in the one described above, 
the field of light in the aquarium was uniformly illuminated, 
but in place of studying the reactions directly, the animals were 
left undisturbed for a given time (15 to 60 minutes) after the light 
was turned on, and then their distribution ascertained by counting 
those in the field of light and those in dark areas equal in size to 
that of the field of light. The results of thirty such experiments 
were recorded. “There were, however, a considerable number more 
in which the results were so evident that the distribution was not 
studied by actual count and no record was made of them. ‘These 
tests were performed on the following days: February 20, 21, 22 
and 23, and on March 3 and 4, the same days on which expert- 
ments described under “First Method’’ were performed, and 
the animals used were taken from the same cultures in both 
cases. The light intensity of the field in these tests varied from 
less than 1.2 to 3.2 candle-meters. In five tests it was 3.2 
candle-meters; in four of these tests there were decidedly fewer 
Stentors in the field at the close of the test than in an equal area 
outside; in the other one the number was about equal in the two 
areas, but these animals had been used in the preceding experiment 
and were therefore probably acclimated to the light. In twelve 
of the tests, the light intensity was 1.6 candle-meters; in eight of 
these there were definitely fewer animals in the light area than 
elsewhere; in the remaining four the distribution was about uni- 
form throughout the aquarium. In eight tests the intensity was 
1.2 candle-meters; in four of these there were fewer Stentors in 
the light area than elsewhere, but the difference was not very 
marked, while in the other four it was questionable which con- 
tained the greater number. In the five remaining tests the light 
intensity was slightly less than 1.2 candle-meters; in all but one 


384. S. O. Mast 


of these, the light did not effect the distribution and in this one the 
outcome was questionable. ‘These results agree very well with 
those obtained by the first method, in which, as here, it was found 
that the least change in intensity which would produce any re- 
action under most favorable conditions was 1.2 candle-meters. 
The tabulated results of these experiments, as well as of those 
that follow, will be found in the Appendix (pp. 394-399). 

Third Method.—A field of light graded in intensity from one 
end to the other was produced by means of a screen containing a 
triangular opening placed over the cylindrical lens. ‘The point of 
least light intensity along the side of the field to which the animals 
responded when passing into the light was then ascertained, 
either directly by observing the movements of the animals, or in- 
directly by studying the effect of the light on the distribution of 
the animals after having been exposed to it for a given length of 
time. ‘The intensity of any area in the field could readily be 
calculated by measuring the functional width of the lens as 
determined by the opening in the screen at the point through which 
the light passed to produce the given area. A large number of 
experiments on animals from various cultures and under various 
conditions were carried out in accordance with this method, the 
result being that the threshold was found to vary from 1.2 to 4 
candle-meters. In no instance was there any definite evidence of 
reaction to a change in light intensity of less than 1.2 candle-meters. 

In all of these experiments individuals were met with which 
were apparently not affected in the least in passing from darkness 
into a light intensity as high as 125 candle-meters. ‘The threshold 
of the most sensitive animals also varied considerably in different 
cultures, but I think we may safely conclude that the threshold 
for Stentors from the cultures tested under the most favorable 
conditions was not less than 1.2 candle-meters when the sides of 
the animals were illuminated. 


b. With the Animal’s Anterior End Illuminated 


Let us now consider the threshold of Stentors from the same 
cultures under similar conditions, but with the anterior end illumi- 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 385 


nated in place of one side. Stentors are approximately conical 
in form, the posterior end being somewhat pointed while the 
anterior end is an almost flat surface approximately perpendicu- 
lar to the longitudinal axis of the animal. If, then, the light rays 
are perpendicular to the longitudinal axis, they must be parallel 
with the anterior end, and this end therefore will not be exposed 
fully to light, but the posterior end being pointed will be practically 
as highly illuminated as if it were turned toward the source of 
light. In the experiments discussed above, the light rays were 
practically perpendicular to the longitudinal axis of the animals, 
therefore the only portion of the body not fully exposed to light 
in these experiments was the anterior end. If, however, the 
movements of the Stentors in an aquarium are not restricted in 
relation to the source of light, it is evident that sooner or later, 
in random swimming, the anterior end, as well as the sides 
and posterior end, may be turned toward the source of light and 
thus fully exposed. If, now, the animals leave that part of the 
aquarium nearest the light, it may be due to motor reactions 
induced by stimulation of any part of the surface. But if the 
anterior end of the animal is more sensitive than any other portion 
of its surface and the light intensity is gradually reduced, it 1s 
clear that a condition of illumination will be reached in which the 
motor reaction will be induced only when the anterior end is turned 
toward the source of light. If, then, the threshold is found to 
be higher when the movements of the animals are restricted to 
a plane perpendicular to the light rays than when they are not 
thus restricted, we may conclude that the anterior end is more 
sensitive than any other portion of the surface. The following 
experiments prove this to be true. 

In order to ascertain the threshold when there was no restriction 
of movements, the Stentors were put into an elongated aquarium, 
one end of which faced a Nernst glower that was on a level with 
the bottom of the aquarium. The aquarium was then moved 
from the source of light until a place was reached at which the 
animals no longer left the end nearest the light. “Theoretically 
this method seems very simple, but practically it is quite other- 
wise; as in the case of the experiments described above, only 


386 S. O. Mast 


rather gross approximations can be looked for. We have here 
to deal with the fluctuations in the intensity of the source of 
light, with reflection of light from the ends and sides of the 
aquarium, with absorption of light by the water in the aquarium, 
and with the difficulty of determining, without statistical methods, 
the lowest intensity to which the animals respond, a difficult prob- 
lem because of the marked individual variation of the animals 
and the ease with which they become acclimated. 

These difficulties were controlled as far as possible in the fol- 
lowing way. ‘The variation in light intensity in the Nernst 
Jones. is due largely to variations in voltage. Having ascertained 
die intensity for a given voltage, the intensity for any other 
voltage can be approximately calculated, so that by means of 
a volt-meter in the circuit it was possible to ascertain the 
approximate light intensity at any given time. Four per cent of 
the light which reaches the end of the aquarium nearest the 
source is reflected and again four per cent of that which passes 
through the opposite end; moreover, there is considerable re- 
flection from the sides of the aquarium and from the surface of 
the water. Owing to such reflection and to the absorption of 
light, the variation in intensity in an aquarium illuminated from 
the end by light, even from a single point becomes exceedingly 
complicated. With sucha complicated field it 1s of course utterly 
impossible to do quantitative work worthy of the name. For use 
in the following experiments an aquarium was constructed 
accordance with the plan shown in Fig. 6. ‘The construction 
will be readily understood by referring to the figure. 

In these experiments the light was placed on a level with the 
bottom of the aquarium, so that there was no reflection from this 
surface. ‘he space between the walls was filled with water to 
prevent reflection from the sides. ‘The velvet between the end 
walls absorbed most of the light which passed through the 
aquarium and consequently prevented most of the reflection from 
the end farthest from the glower. Reflection from the surface of the 
water was prevented by cutting off the light which would other- 
wise reach it by means of a screen in front of the aquarium. ‘The 
Nernst glower was entirely surrounded by screens, only one of 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 387 


which contained an opening. Through this, light escaped pro- 
ducing a field just large enough to cover the front end of the 
aquarium, and since the whole apparatus was in a dark room no 
other light reached the aquarium. ‘The heat rays were cut out 
by passing the light through 7 cm. of distilled water! in a vessel 
with parallel sides. 


ee a AS EL] 


Fig. 6. Plan of aquarium, natural size. a, Screen; b, piece of black velvet; c, vertical walls 2.5 
cm. high. This aquarium was made by gluing glass slides together with balsam. It was contructed 
for the purpose of eliminating as far as possible the effect of reflected light (see text). 


The aquarium was filled to a depth of five millimeters with 
water taken from the culture jar and thoroughly filtered, so as to 
eliminate absorption as much as possible. ‘Then, after properly 


1To those who still use alum solutions for absorbing rays, the following statements by Nichols 
(793, p- 15) may be of interest: 

““Melloni (Thermochrose, p. 165), using an Argand lamp with a glass chimney as a source, found 
that a layer of alum solution 9.21 mm. thick transmitted 12 per cent of the total incident radiation 
and that distilled water transmitted only 11 per cent.” 

“<Shelford Bidwell (Nature, Vol. xliv, p. 565), using a paraffin lamp as a light source and the thermo- 
pile, obtained the following results: 


Solutions Diathermacy 
HBsraniys yg cell lierercbey ferctostoter ct atetof¥ ets iofet-\ayo! =o «1cveh «i ishajal eJehe/alotels¥shsvel sla) e)2)ato 1000 
\Watiar, Cieailice!,ooononsspacododubocodo ese soaucoGoDobDos0oaGL 199 
MAID SAUER a5 co cb aodacoedn andandoOO UDG EESoooDDU gE soAdGC0ae 200 
MMumbesatinated esOlm tion's :a\s\clere aise <i- </cie'ele/elareioc\ sae chelelalelelsiela\aie) ole 204, 


‘‘Neither potassium alum nor ammoniumalum in solution alters thediathermacy of distilled water 
in the region studied,” 7. e., wave lengths 0.776 to 1.414. 

Nichols and Coblentz (03, p. 272) state that water transmits 67 per cent at wave lengths 14 and 
becomes completely opaque only when wave lengths 1.8 is reached. The upper limit of wave length 


in red is usually considered to be 0.76 ys. 


388 S. O. Mast 


arranging the aquarium with reference to the light, quite a 
number of Stentors taken directly from the culture jar were in- 
troduced and evenly scattered. ‘The animals were then left un- 
disturbed for some time, 10 to 60 minutes, before their distribution 
was studied. It was soon found that Io minutes was sufhcient 
time to produce an effect, if the light intensity was not below the 
threshold. If, on studying their distribution, it was found that 
there were definitely fewer within an area, about five millimeters 
wide at the end of the aquarium nearest the light than elsewhere, 
and that those in the remaining area were still practically equally 
scattered, the intensity at this end was considered the threshold. 
It will be noticed that owing (1) to reflection from the end, (2) to 
absorption, (3) to the difficulty in deciding whether a change in 
distribution has occurred, and (4) to possible acclimation, the thresh- 
old as read would tend to be slightly too high. Sometimes, if the 
threshold was not found at the first trial, as was usually the case, the 
animals were thoroughly stirred up and a second trial made with the 
same animals, but it was soon found that the threshold of such 
animals was higher than that of animals fresh from the culture 
jar, even if the light on the first exposure was not above the 
threshold in intensity, so that in nearly all experiments fresh 
Stentors were introduced for each trial. 

In all, 62 exposures were made in accordance with the method 
just described (see Appendix, pp. 397-399). These experiments 
were performed, on February 25, 26, 29; March 1, 2, 3, 8, 11, 13, 15, 
16, 17, and August 1, 2, 3 and 4. The Stentors used were taken 
from the same cultures from which those used in ascertaining 
the threshold in the light-grader had been taken. Many of the 
experiments in the two series were performed on the same days, 
or nearly so, and in the same room, so that the conditions in both 
were practically the same with the exception of illumination. 
But, as already stated, the threshold in the light-grader was found 
to vary from 1.2 to 4.8 candle-meters, whereas in these experi- 
ments it was found to vary from 0.235 to 0.646 candle-meters. 
This contrast is brought out more strikingly in the following tests: 
During the period from August 1 to 4, inclusive, Stentors fresh 
from the culture jar were repeatedlytested under various conditions 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 389 


in the light-grader and no definite reactions were obtained to 
intensities less than 1.78 candle-meters, while animals from the 
same culture jar tested at different times during the same period 
in the elongated aquarium illuminated from the end, reacted defin- 
itely to an intensity as low as 0.287 candle-meters. 

It will be remembered that in the first of these series all portions 
of the surface of the animals excepting the anterior end were 
subjected to illumination, while in the second series the entire 
surface, including the anterior end, was exposed to light. The 
difference in the threshold in the two series must, then, be due to 
the exposure of the anterior end in the second series; and since 
the threshold is lower in this series than in the first, we can safely 
conclude that with reference to stimulation by light, the anterior 
end is the more sensitive portion of the surface of the animal. 

The difference in sensitiveness between the anterior end and 
other portions of the surface of Stentor was further tested in the 
following experiments. In these experiments the light was placed 
on a level with the animals, which were hanging from a cover 
glass floating on the surface film in a small aquarium. A mirror 
was fastened above the aquarium and another below it, each at 
such an angle that the light reflected from above fell on the 
posterior end, and that from below, on the anterior end of the 
animal. Then, by properly screening the light and using the 
mirrors independently, the threshold was obtained when the 
animals were illuminated on either end. While the results thus 
obtained indicate a threshold of lower intensity when the anterior 
end is illuminated than when the posterior end is illuminated, I 
cannot consider these indications conclusive, since, on account of 
the extreme individual variations and the high intensity of light 
required, it was very difficult indeed to locate the threshold. 

Jennings (’04a) demonstrated by direct observation that ciliary 
action in Stentor, Paramecium, Oxytricha and other organisms 
produces currents which carry liquid from some distance ahead 
of the animals to the oral groove. Now, if the stimulating agent 
can be carried with the liquid, “The result is a stimulation on 
the oral side of the body, not elsewhere.’’ In giving the motor 
reaction, these organisms always turn toward the aboral surface, 


390 S. O. Mast 


1.e., away from the side stimulated, whenever the stimulating 
agent is such that it can be carried in the current, 7. ¢., chemical 
or thermal. Roesle (’02) showed that the peristome region is 
more sensitive to mechanical stimuli than any other portion of the 
surface of the body, and it is also probably true that the same 
region is more sensitive to chemical and thermal stimuli. If so, it 
is clear that when animals are put into a chemical solution which 
acts as a stimulus, or into a liquid at a temperature above the 
threshold, the oral side is more strongly stimulated than the 
aboral, and we should expect precisely what is found to be true, 
1.e.,the animals under these conditions respond with the motor 
reaction and in so doing turn the anterior end away from the oral 
side, which is more strongly stimulated than the aboral. 

The effect of a stimulating agent on a given surface depends 
upon the sensitiveness of the surface and upon its degree of 
exposure. Mechanical and radiant-energy stimuli are produced 
by agents which are in general not affected by currents and thus 
can be applied to any desired portion of the surface. It is, 
however, found that the animals always turn toward the aboral 
side regardless of the surface to which these agents are applied. 
Can these facts be explained f 

In Euglena the eye spot is in all probability more sensitive to 
light than any other portion of the body. It is located in the 
dorsal lip near the surface of the body. ‘These organisms are 
positive to light of moderate intensity. “hey swim with their 
anterior end facing the light, and in giving the motor reaction 
they turn toward the dorsal lip, in which the eyespot is situated, 
i. €., toward the region most sensitive and consequently most 
strongly stimulated, just as Stentor, Paramecium and Oxytricha, 
all of which are negative, turn from the side most strongly stimu- 
lated in case of thermal or chemical stimuli. But it will be re- 
called that here the location of stimulation is not so much due to 
Variation in sensitiveness as to variation in exposure of different 
regions of the surface, owing to currents. We have demonstrated 
that the anterior end of Stentor is more sensitive to light than any 
other portion of the surface. Roesle showed, as stated above, 
that the peristomal region in certain Infusoria is more sensitive 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 391 


to mechanical stimuli than any other surface area. Is it not 
probable, then, that this region in Stentor is also more sensitive 
to light than any other? If this is true, then in stimulating the 
anterior end with light the oral region will be more strongly 
affected than any other, and, in turning from the oral side when 
the motor .reaction is given, the animals turn from the part most 
strongly stimulated precisely as in response to thermal and 
chemical stimuli. This would likewise be true if they were 
stimulated by light striking any other part of the surface of the 
body than the anterior end, for these animals are translucent, 
so that if they were illuminated from the side, for example, light 
could reach the oral region by passing through the animal. As 
a matter of fact, we have as yet no experimental results with 
regard to light reactions in Stentor which cannot be explained 
by assuming a given structure in the region of the oral groove 
to be the only portion of the body which is sensitive to light. 

In givingthe motor reaction to all light, chemical, or thermal, 
stimulations, Stentor probably turns always from the side most 
strongly stimulated, but apparently not so with regard to mechani- 
cal stimulations, forin such stimulations the motor reaction can 
be induced by touching any part of the surface. Does this mean 
that a certain physiological change induces a given definite reac- 
tion, which has become fixed in the race, possibly by “survival 
of the fittest,’’ and that all local stimulations are effective only 
in so far as they tend to cause general physiological changes? Or 
are the local stimulations produced by some agents (chemicals, 
change in temperature, light) still in a measure local signs which 
induce reactions in harmony with them, as well as cause a definite 
physiological change, while in those produced by other agents 
(mechanical) the local sign is lost and the response is induced 
because of a given physiological state? Or may not the effect of 
stimulating mechanically any point on the surface be transmitted 
to the region of the peristome and there produce changes greater 
than were produced at the point actually touched and thus call 
forth a local sign which may regulate the reaction ? 

Whatever the final answer to these questions may be, the facts 
thus far established by experiment seem to indicate that the motor 


392 S. O. Mast 


reaction, 7.¢., the turning toward a structurally defined side, 
originated in response to stimuli which produced local signs. 


4. SUMMARY 


1. Stentors free to swim in all directions orient and swim from 
the source of light. 

2. They orient by means of motor reactions, 1.¢., by turning 
toward a structurally defined side and then proceeding on a new 
path which forms an angle with the old one. If a single reaction 
does not result in orientation, it is repeated until the anterior end 
of the animal happens to become directed from the source of 
light. 

3. The motor reaction is induced by a sudden increase in light 
intensity regardless of the relation between the direction of the 
rays and the direction of movement of the animals at the time the 
intensity 1s increased. 

4. If a source of light to which Stentors are oriented is in- 
creased in intensity, the animals respond with the motor reaction 
and are thus thrown out of orientation, but by repeating the motor 
react.on they soon become oriented again. 

5. The anterior end of Stentor is more sensitive to light than 
any other part of the surface of the body. “The minimum thresh- 
old in animals stimulated by rays perpendicular to the longi- 
tudinal axis is 1.2 candle-meters, but in those stimulated by light 
striking the anterior end it is only 0.25 candle-meter. 

6. The threshold varies greatly in individuals under the same 
conditions, and in the same individuals under different conditions. 
Stentors readily become acclimated to light, but much more 
readily under some conditions than under others. 

7. Stentors once oriented remain oriented, if the light intensity 
is not too high, because they are least sensitive to light when the 
rays strike the posterior end. 

8. Attached Stentors respond to increase in light intensity by 
contracting or by swinging about their point of attachment. If 
the increase is great and sudden, they contract; if it is not very 
great nor sudden, they swing about their point of attachment. 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 393 


These reactions are independent of any relation between the posi- 
tion of the animals and the direction of the light rays. 

g. Attached Stentors do not orient, for if the light intensity is 
above the threshold when the anterior end is turned from the 
source of light, the stimulation induces the motor reaction, which 
prevents orientation; andif it is below the threshold, the tendency 
to take a position such that their longitudinal axis is perpendicular 
to the surface on which they are attached, again prevents it. 

10. [he variation in the threshold to light stimuli in attached 
Stentors is much greater than in free-swimming ones. In some 
physiological conditions they respond definitely to a light intensity 
of 1.20 candle-meters, whereas in others they respond very inde- 
finitely when exposed to an intensity of 4000 candle-meters. 

11. The threshold in attached Stentors is probably lower when 
Ight strikes the anterior end than when it strikes the posterior 
end. 

12. The light reactions of Stentor, both free-swimming and 
fixed, cannot be explained by the application of the tropism theory 
as defined by either Loeb, Verworn, or Holt and Lee. 


5. BIBLIOGRAPHY 


CHMIELEVSKY, V., ’04.—Ueber Phototaxis und die physikalischen Eigenschaften 
der Kulturtropfen. Beihefte z. Bot. Centralbl., Bd. xvi., pp. 53- 
Go, lat, i. . 
Hott, E. B., anp Ler, F. S., ’01.—The Theory of Photatactic Response. Amer. 
Jour. Physiol., vol. iv, No. 9, pp. 460-481. 
Jennincs, H. S., ’04a.—Contributions to the Study of the Behavior of Lower 
Organisms. Carnegie Institution of Washington, Publication No. 
16, 256 pp., 81 Figs. 
’°04b.—The Behavior of Paramecium. Additional Features and Gen- 
eral Relations. Jour. Comp. Neurol. Psychol., vol. xiv, No. 6, 
Pp. 441-510. 
Nicuots, E. L.,’93—A Study of the Transmission Spectra of Certain Substances 
in the Infra-red. Phys. Review, vol. i, No. 1, pp. 1-18, pl. 1. 
Nicuois, E. L., anD CosBLentz, W. W.,'03—On Methods of Measuring Radiant 
Efficiency. Phys. Review, vol. xvii, No. 4, pp. 267-276. 
Otrmanns, F., ’92.—Ueber die photometrischen Bewegungen der Pflanzen. 
Flora, Bd. Ixxv, pp. 183-266, Taf. 4. 


394. S. O. Mast 


Peters, A. W., ’02—Metabolism and Division in Protozoa. Proceed. Amer. 
Acad. Arts and Sci., vol. xxxix, No. 20, pp. 439-515. 

Rogste, E.,’02—Die Reaktion einiger Infusorien auf einzelne Induktions— 
schlage. Zeitschr. f. allg. Physiol., Bd. u, Heft 1, pp. 139-168. 

RotuHeErtT, W., ’03—Ueber die Wirkung des Aethers und Chloroforms auf die 
Reizbewegungen der Mikroorganismen. Jahrb. f. wiss. Bot., Bd. 
xxxix, Heft.1, pp. 1-70. 

STRASBURGER, E., ’78—Wirkung des Lichtes und der Warme auf Schwarmsporen. 
Jena. Zeitschr., Bd. xii, pp. 551-625. 

Tow e, Evizaneta W.,’00—A study in the Heliotropism of Cypridopsis. Amer. 
Jour. Physiol., vol iii, No. 7, pp. 345-365. 

Yerkes, R. M., ’03—Reactions of Daphnia pulex to Light and Heat. Mark 
Anniversary Volume, article 18, pp. 359-377. 


6. APPENDIX 


TABLE I 


Light Intensity, 222-Volt Nernst Glower 


Photometer to Photometer to | Candle per Standard 
Date Glower Candle Hour in Voltage Candle- 
in cm. in cm. Grains power 
Rebs 25 200 52.78 ? ? HO} —= 
Feb. 26 200 52.25 ? P 19+ 
Feb. 26 200 54.1 ? 211+ 18+ 
Reb.) 27 201 63.89 165.72 QM ie 13.62 
Mian 07 200 56.5 154-3 204-209 16.15 
TABLE II 
Light Intensity, 110-Volt Nernst Glower 
| | 
Photometer to | Photometer to | Candle per Standard 
Date Glower Candle Hour in Voltage Candle- 
in cm. incm. Grains Power 
July 15 200 47-8 138.87 108 . 5-110 20.27 
July.15 200 44.89 154.3 IIO-III 24.42 
July. 27 200 47-8 154-3 IIo 22.48 
Aug. I 200 44.17 157.38 LDI5 5118 26.88 
Aug. 3 200 48 .32 163.56 109.5-I1I 23.35 
Aug. 27 200 45-2 140.96 IIO-III.5 23.00 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 395 


It will be noticed that the light intensity of the 110-volt 
glower is higher than that of the 222-volt clower. This is due 
to the comparatively low voltage. Later, in comparing the in- 
tensity of the 110-volt glower with that of the 222-volt glower 
directly, with a voltage of 110+ and 222+, respectively, it was 
found that the candle power of the latter was 1.62 times as 
great as that of the former. 


TABLE It 


Light Intensity at ee in Light-grader,222-Volt Nernst Glower. 


Photometer | Photometer Candle per Functional Standard 

Date to Candle at Hour in Voltage width of Candle- 
in cm. Stage Grains | Lens meters 
Mar. ? 106.78 es 161.86 206-208? o.75mm. 1.193 
Mar. ? 71.6 | S 161.40 | 206-208? I.5 mm. 2.52 
Aug. 2 60.37 4 157-39 | 221-223 1.5 mm. 25557 
Aug. 27 42.41 | n | 138.87 | 224-225 3.0mm. 6.5+ 


The light 1 intensities given in the tables were obtained by taking 
30 successive readings at intervals of one minute. The distance 
from the photometer to the candle is in every case an average of 
30 readings. A Lummer-Brodhun photometer was used. 


TABLE IV 


Threshold for Stentors with the Side Illuminated 
First Meruop (see p. 382) 


Reaction in Light-grader as Stentors pass from Darkness into Light 


Date Voltage of the following Intensities in Candle-meters 
re 1.2 1.6 | geo 

Feb. 20 ; no no ? | marked 
Feb. 21 ? no no definite, but not in all 
Feb. 22 ? no P large majority 
Feb. 23 ? no no about rin § 
Mar. 4 208+ no definite 
Mar.10 205-207 no about 1 in 10 many 
Mar.16 209-210 no ? a few 
Mar.16 206-208 no ? definite, but only a few!| 


1after these Stentors had been in the aquarium in the light-grader 1.5 hours, no reactions were 


found to lower intensity than 4.8 candle-meters. 


396 S. O. Mast 


August I to 4, voltage 220-222. During this period Stentors 
fresh from culture jars were tested several times under various 
conditions and no definite reactions were seen to intensities 
lower than 1.78 candle-meters. 


TABLE V 


Threshold for Stentors with the Side Illuminated 


Seconp Meruop (see p. 383) 


Time of Expo- | Light Intensity | No. of Sten- | In Equal In Equal 
Date sure in in Candle- tors in Field | Field to Field to 
Minutes meters of Light | Right Left 
Feb. 20 48 Rue definitely definitely + 
Feb. 20 33 Buz 4 16 18 
Feb. 20 7° 32 6 12 16 
Feb. 21 63 1.6 7 13 12 
Feb. 22 ? 1.6 B 8 9 
Feb. 22 56 1.6 8 15 16 
Feb; 22 53 12 II 15 9 
Feb. 22 38 | 1.2 10 16 15 
Feb. 23 go 1.6 9 9+ 9+ 
Feb. 23 60 1.6 12 18 24 
Mar. 3 65 | 1.6 19 30 
Mar. 3 65 Ta, 22 27 
Mar. 3 65 | ae 32 31 
Mart. 3 go T6- 17 30 
Mar. 3 go Tez 14 23 
Mar. 3 go i2— 28 32 
Mar. 3 65 1.6 30 24 
Mart. 3 65 1.2 29 32 
Mar. 3 65 tea 44 44 
Mar. 3 60 1.6 24 18 
Mar. 3 60 1.2 27 32 
Mar. 3 60 I.2- 22 32 
Mar. 4 120 1.6 9 24 
Mar. 4 120 se 16 | 16 
Mar. 4 120 1.2- 24 | 16 


The temperature varied from 19° C. to 24° C. in these experi- 
ments, but this change in temperature produced no apparent 
effect on the reactions. 


Feb. 


Feb. 
Feb. 
Feb. 
Feb. 
Feb. 
Feb. 
Feb. 


Feb. 


Feb. 
Feb. 
Feb. 


27 


27 
2) 
By 
233) 
29 
=) 
2 


2) 


ay 
29 
29) 


Mar. 1 


Mar. 1 


Mar. 1 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 


TABLE VI 


Threshold for Stentors, the Anterior End Illuminated (see p. 384) 


Time of 
Exposure 
in Minutes 
60 
7° 


48 


20 


60 


Tempera- 
ture in 


Degrees C. 


21 


Voltage 


209 


208 


397 


Condition 
of 
Stentors 


| 


used for 


fresh 
used for 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 


fresh 
fresh 


some time 


some time 


same again 
same again 


same again 
same again 


same again 
same again 


same again 


same again 
same again 


same again 


same again 


same again 


| same again | 


same again | 


Distance 
from Light} Reactions 
in cm. 
588 | none 
588 | marked 
588 | none 
588 marked 
650 | quite definite 
806 none 
806 | none 
806 none 
806 none 
723 none 
723 | none 
670 none 
670 slight 
670 | marked 
( | very few within 
J | 5mm. from 
779.) | tight end of 
| | aquarium 
goo | none 
goo | none 
goo none 
806 none 
806 marked 
825 none 
825 none 
Rae if | very few within | 
\| 3 mm. of light 
| end 
800 definite 
800 definite 
836 none 
336 if | marked; many | 


836 


836 


U 


at dark end 


marked; many | 


at dark end 


none 


Intensity 
in Candle- 
meters 


0.26 


0.209 


0.26 


0.24 


0.265-+ 


0.265 
0.265+ 


398 


Date 


onl 
i) 
A 
_ 


2 
Mar. 2 


Time of 
Exposure 
in Minutes 


135 


20 


60 


40 


60 
60 


25 


Tempera- 
ture in 


Degrees C. 


22 


21 


20 


22 


S. O. Mast 


TABLE VI—Continued 


Voltage 


212 


213 
211 


210 


Condition 
of 
Stentors 


same again 


fresh 
same again 


fresh 


same again 


fresh 


fresh 


used before 


fresh 
same again 
same again 


fresh 


used before 
used before 


used before 


fresh 
used before 


fresh 


fresh 
fresh 
fresh 
fresh 
fresh | 
fresh | 
used before | 
used before 
fresh 


used before | 


fresh 


used before 


in cm. 


800 


800 


835 


835 


850 
850 


goo 


goo 


goo 
825 
75° 


75° 


800 
goo 


800 


goo 
800 


800 


800 
800 
600 
600 
500 
300 
500 
500 
500 
500 


500 


500 


Distance 
from Light 


(a 


Reactions 


none 
very few near 
light end 
few near light 
end 
many at dark 
end; marked 
none 
marked 
very few within 
1 cm. of light 
end 


very many near 
dark end 


quite definite 
> 


marked; few 
| near light end 
none 
quite definite 
| definite,but not 
| very marked 
marked 
none 
marked 
marked 
| ? 
| very marked 
marked 
? 
very definite 
none 
few within 1 cm. 
of light end 
| definite 


Intensity 
in Candle- 
meters 


0.235 


0.209 


very low; 
light filter- 
ed through 
thin paper 
ditto 


0.209 
0.265 


0.265 


0.446 


0.646 
0.646 
0.646 
0.646 


0.646 
0.646 


Light Reactions in Lower Organisms 399 
TABLE VI—Continued 
Time of | Tempera- Condition | Distance Intensity 

Date | Exposure | ture in Voltage of from Light} Reactions jin Candle- 

in Midutes|Degrees C.| Stentors in cm. meters 
| ( |very few within 
Aug. 1 ; ? III-113 fresh 800 { | rem.oflight 0.365 
end 

Aug. 4 ? ? IIO-III fresh 800 definite 0.365 
Aug. 4 ? ? TIO-112 fresh goo f See) 0 call 0.287 


\ 


very evident. 


A 222-volt Nernst glower was used in all the experiments in this 
series excepting the last three, in which a 110-volt glower was used. 
For intensity of light, see table on intensity, p. 395. 


side ¥. i 


cr rf 


: Gas 
mh SRT ig musty ge pm 


CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE 
MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY AT HARVARD COLLEGE, 
E. L. MARK, Director—No. 183. 


THEANFLUENCE OF LIGHT AND HEAT ON THE 
MOVEMENT OF THE MELANOPHORE 
PIGMENT, ESPECIALLY IN LIZARDS 


BY 
G. H. PARKER 


Wirth Turee Ficures 


It is now generally recognized that, of the various factors con- 
cerned in the integumentary color changes in lizards, none is so 
important as the migration of the pigment granules in the large 
pigment cells of the derma, the melanophores, erythrophores, 
etc. These cells are situated as a rule in the deeper part of the 
derma, and their bodies are either embedded in the more or less 
opaque guanine layer or lie proximal to this layer. Their pro- 
cesses extend distally between the guanine particles, and, as finely 
divided branches, form a rich arborization on the proximal face 
of the epidermis. When these distal processes are filled with 
pigment granules from the bodies of the cells, they form a dark 
covering on the distal face of the guanine, and the skin in conse- 
quence is dark-colored. When, however, the pigment migrates 
fromthe distal processes proximally into the bodies of the cells, the 
guanine layer becomes exposed to the light, and, since this layer 
is made of reflecting particles, the skin under these circumstances 
has a lightish appearance. ‘Thus a distal migration of the pig- 
ment results in a dark-colored skin, a proximal migration in a 
light-colored one. 

The migration of the pigment is influenced both by internal 
and external factors. Not only do the emotional states and 
other nervous conditions of the lizard make themselves evident 


Tue Journat or Experimentar Zoéiocy, Vor. 11. No. 3. 


402 G. A Parker 


in the color changes of its skin, but external factors, such as heat 
and light also induce these changes. It has been pointed out 
recently by Parker and Starratt (’04, p. 464) that, in all lizards 
on which they could get records, a high temperature is accom- 
panied by a proximal migration of the pigment, whereby a light- 
colored skin is produced, and a /ow temperature by a distal move- 
ment, thus giving rise to a dark coloration. Light on the other 
hand seemed to affect different lizards differently. Thus in 
Chameleon and Anolis light induces a distal migration of pigment, 
while in Stellio, according to Filippi (’66), and in Varanus and 
Uromastix, according to Thilenius (’97), light calls forth a proxi- 
mal migration. After having studied Anolis and become familiar, 
in this lizard, with one in which light produced a distal migration 
of pigment, I was naturally desirous of examining a species in 
which the reverse took place. Unfortunately the lizards studied 
by Filippi and by Thilenius were all old-world species and hence 
were not readily accessible to me in a living condition. I was, 
therefore, obliged to seek for a representative of this type of color 
change among more available forms. Hoffmann (’99, p. 1353) 
states on the authority of Wiedersheim, that the horned toad, 
Phrynosoma orbiculare, on cool overcast days is dark colored, 
and that it changes to silvery gray when sunlight falls upon it. 
Wiedersheim believed that this change was caused by a difference 
of temperature and not by illumination, but as no proof of this 
view was given, Keller (’95, p.132) was free to assert that these 
changes were due in his opinion to light. In that case Phrynosoma 
would show agreement with Stellio, Varanus and Uromastix, 
in that a strong light would induce a proximal migration of the 
pigment and a weak light, or none, a distal movement. Since 
Phrynosoma is abundant in the southern and western parts of the 
United States and since its color changes gave promise of being 
the reverse of those in Anolis, so far’as their relation to light was 
concerned, | decided to make it an object of study. 

Preliminary trials made at my suggestion by Mr. A. S. Pearse 
showed that horned toads obtained oo dealers were not usually 
ina very satisfactory condition for work of this kind, and further 
that the color changes, though present in Phrynosoma, were not 


Influence of Light and Heat on Melanophores 403 


of a very pronounced type. ‘The subject was, therefore, aban- 
doned till through the kindness of Miss S. R. Armington I came 
into the possession of a large vigorous specimen of Phrynosoma 
blainvillei Gray, from San Diego, California. This specimen 
had been well fed, had recently shed its skin, and showed in a 
clear and unmistakable way a series of color changes that had 
been only faintly indicated in other specimens. Btn results 
recorded in this paper refer in the main to the reactions of this 
one specimen, though they have been checked by observations on 
other horned toads in the laboratory. I am under obligation 
to Miss Armington for the privilege of working with this animal. 


Fig. 1. Dorsal view of Phrynosoma blainvillei, showing the dark coloration due to exposure to day~ 
light. <2. 

Since both light and heat influence the migration of the pigment 
in the melanophores of lizards, 1t was bvieus that in experi- 
menting on Phrynosoma, both factors were to be kept in mind. 
In a room at 19° C., after an exposure for several hours to bright 
but diffuse daylight, the horned toad was deep brownish, mottled 
with black and white (Fig. 1). The head was brownish gray. 
Down the middle of the back from the head to the tail ran a 
broad brownish streak, which was much lighter on the neck than 
elsewhere. Right and left of this lighter portion and covering 
most of the neck were two large patches almost black in hue. 
The trunk was marked by four chestnut-brown transverse bands 


404 GH. Parker 


alternating with darker bands. ‘The most anterior of the brown 
bands was immediately behind the neck and crossed the trunk 
from foreleg to foreleg. “The most posterior one was on the 
pelvis. ‘The tail was also marked by transverse bands, five dark 
ones alternating with four brown ones. On the lateral edges 
of the trunk the color was chestnut-brown, and the claw-like 
scales of the edge, numbering about twenty to a side, had dark, 
almost black bases with white edges and tips (Fig. 2). The legs 
were banded transversely dark brown and black. ‘The ventral 
side of the animal was light-colored, the trunk being yellowish 
with numerous gray splotches. 

After the horned toad had been kept in the dark at 19° C. for 
several hours, it became distinctly lighter excepting on the ven- 
tral surface, which remained unchanged. ‘The head assumed a 
light yellowish-gray tint, the brownish dorsal line and the brown- 
ish transverse bands on the trunk and the tail also became lighter, 
as did the chestnut-brown edges of the body. But the most 
marked change was in the lateral claw-like scales; these lost their 
dark bases and became yellowish white (Fig. 3). 

Since 1t was not always easy to follow the color changes in the 
complicated pattern of this animal, [ selected for further study 

the lateral claw-like scales, in which these changes went on in a 
clear and conspicuous way. Each scale is Soe wene triangular 
in form with its claw-like apex turned posteriorly. As already 
stated, these scales in bright diffuse daylight at 19° C. are deep 
gray or even black except on their free edges and tips, which are 
yellowish white (Fig.2). After an hour or so in the dark at 19° C. 
the whole scale became yellowish white (Fig. 3). These scales 
have a well developed guanine layer and a deep-seated layer of 
melanophores whose pigment when in the distal branches pro- 
duces the dark central areas and when withdrawn leaves the 
whole scale whitish. In some of the experiments the dark central 
areas did not disappear entirely even after the animal had been 
in the dark for some time, but they remained visible as faintly 
grayish or greenish spots. On examining these spots under a 
hand lens they were found to contain scattered, minute, dark 
points, evidently particles of pigment which had not yet been 


Influence of Light and Heat on Melanophores 405 


carried proximally far enough to be hidden by the guanine layer 


and which thus gave rise to the grayish or greenish tint. 


S 


ds 


Fig. 3 


Figs. 2 and 3. Dorsal views of the right side of Phrynosoma blainvillei, showing (Fig. 2) the dark 
coloration due to exposure to very bright daylight and (Fig. 3) the light coloration due to retention in 
the dark. The negatives from which these figures were made were taken from the same anima)—Fig. 
3 immediately after the animal had been taken from the dark, and Fig. 2 after exposure for an hour and 
a half to bright daylight. The illumination for photographing was diffuse daylight and the exposure 
in each instance was 7 seconds. The two plates were then developed for the same length of time in 
the same fluid. In Fig. 3 the very faint bands and patches on the lateral claw-like scales are shadows. 
These also occur in Fig. 2; otherwise the dark color on these scales in Fig. 2 is due to the melanophore 
pigment. The effect of this pigment can be most fully appreciated by comparing the last three lateral 


scales in the two figures. The magnification is only slightly over natural size. 


406 G. H. Parker 


The complete proximal migration of the pigment is carried 
out at a somewhat different rate from the distal migration. 
At 19° C. the distal migration was accomplished in about fifteen 
minutes and the proximal one ina little over half an hour. ‘Thus 
in Phrynosoma, as in Anolis (Carlton, ’03), the distal migration 
takes less time than the proximal one. 

I next tried the effect of temperature changes on the migration 
of the pigment. At 15° C. in bright diffuse daylight, the lateral 
scales had very pronounced dark centers, much as at 19° C. under 
similar illumination. ‘These dark centers were partly but not 
completely lost when the animal was kept in the dark at 15° C. 
As these centers usually disappeared completely at 19° C. in the 
dark, it is clear that a low temperature favors a distal position 
for the pigment. 

At 32° C. the animal when in the dark was light-colored and 
the lateral scales showed no traces of dark centers. ‘Their tint 
was a clear ivory white. When the lizard was transferred to the 
light at this temperature, it became rapidly darker, the lateral 
scales always showing gray centers, which sometimes became 
almost black, but not so much so as at 19°C. Hence a high 
temperature favors a proximal position for the pigment. 

Similar results, both as respects light and temperature, were 
obtained from two other horned toads in the laboratory, but 
these animals were much less satisfactory for experimental pur- 
poses because of the conditions of their skin. In both the epi- 
dermis was filled with fine particles of dirt that rendered the 
pigment changes visible only with difficulty, nor were these two 
lizards as quick in response as the first one was, a fact that may 
have been due to their lack of proper food and surroundings. 

From these observations it is evident that both heat and light 
influence the pigment migration in the skin of Phrynosoma 
blainvillei, a bright light and a low temperature calling forth a 
relatively rapid distal migration of the pigment and the absence 
of light and a high temperature inducing a proximal migration. 
Bright light is antagonistic to high temperature and no light to 
low temperature. Since the animals were dark-colored in a 
bright light at a high temperature and light-colored in the dark 


Influence of Light and Heat on Melanophores 407 


at a low temperature, it follows that between 15° C. and 32°C. 
light or its absence is a more effective stimulus than heat or cold 
and in the main controls the resulting color. I believe, therefore, 
that the dark coloration noticed by Wiedersheim (Hoffmann 
’90, p.1353) in Phrynosoma orbiculare on sunless, cool days was 
probably a light reaction, though the low temperature doubtless 
favored it. It seems to me improbable, however, that Keller 
(95, p-132) is correct in assuming the reaction to be due exclu- 
sively to light. The blanching of ae animal, which is stated by 
Wiedersheim to occur when sunlight falls upon it, is not so readily 
explained. My three specimens of Phrynosoma remained quite 
dark in-full sunlight even after several hours of exposure. Had 
they turned light, I should have suspected that it was due to heat, 
but in all my experiments with these animals the effect of heat 
has invariably been found to be subordinate to that of light and 
the animal has always been dark-colored in daylight. How we 
are to explain Wiedersheim’s observation, I am at a loss to say, 
but it is not impossible that Phrynosoma orbiculare differs from 
P. blainvillei in its relation to light and heat, and that like Anolis 
it may react to a high temperature by withdrawing its pigment 
even when illuminated. In such a case the blanching in sun- 
light would be a temperature effect and not due to light. That 
such a condition is not improbable may be inferred from the 
observations of de Grijs (’99, p-51) on Phrynosoma cornutum. 
In this species the individuals are said to be dark-colored in an 
unheated cage and light-colored in a warm one, even though 
illuminated. 

If the light color assumed by some species of Phrynosoma in 
bright sunlight is not due to a reversal of the ordinary light 
reactions, such as are seen in Chamzleon and Anolis, but to a 
temperature reaction, may it not be that the blanching of the 
other supposedly exceptional cases, such as Stellio, Uromastix 
and Varanus, is also to be explained in this way? Unfortunately 
there is not sufficient evidence at hand to settle this question, but 


1[n this respect my results agree with the observations of Gadow (or, pp. 510, 521), who states that 
Agama stellio when basking and geckos when in broiling hot sunlight are almost black. 


408 G. H. Parker 


in two of the best known cases, Uromastix and Varanus, this 
interpretation is not impossible. 

Uromastix, according to Thilenius (’97, p.536), 1s dark colored 
at night, but whitens after the morning sunlight has been on it 
for an hour or more, and remains so till evening, when it again 
becomes dark. No conclusive evidence is given as to whether 
this reaction is due to heat or to light, but if, as I suspect, tempera- 
ture is the controlling factor, these changes are precisely what 
should be expected, and the fact that Uromastix has been observed 
by de Grijs (99, p. 51) to become light-colored in a warm cage 
favors this view. 

The relations of Varanus to heat and light have been stated by 
Thilenius (’97, p. 536) more fully than those of Uromastix. 
Varanus is said to be dark-colored in the shade at a temperature 
of 45° C. to 50° C., but to blanch in the morning sunlight before 
the thermometer had reached 30° C. Such an instance might 
seem to be a conclusive case of reversed light action, but in my 
opinion the dark color in this instance is due, as in Phrynosoma 
blainvillei, to the action of the diffuse daylight (shade) irrespective 
of the relatively high temperature. The blanching in sunlight, 
however, notwithstanding the statement as to temperature made 
by Thilenius, I believe to have been due to heat. It is true that 
Thilenius states that the temperature of the morning sunlight 
had not yet risen to 30° C. when the change took place, but it 
seems to me hardly possible that such could have been the case. 
Even the early morning sunlight usually has much more heat in 
it than this. Possibly TUhilenius took the temperature with an 
ordinary mercury-bulb thermometer, and it may be that the 
reflection from such a bulb would be sufficient to allow so low a 
reading to be recorded, but with a black-bulb thermometer I feel 
conhdent that a higher temperature would have been found, and 
such a bulb would have imitated much more nearly the conditions 
in the dark absorbing skin of the lizard than the reflecting surface 
of a glass-and-mercury thermometer would. I, therefore, believe 
it probable that the blanching of the skin of Varanus in sunlight 
is a temperature reaction, such as de Grijs has observed in other 
lizards, and not a reversed light reaction. 


Influence of Light and Heat on Melanophores 4.09 


If these conclusions are correct, the reactions to light and to heat 
of the melanophores and other like cells in the skins of lizards can 
be stated very simply, as follows: Light causes a distal migration 
of the pigment granules of these cells; its absence a proximal one. 
A high temperature causes a proximal migration of the pigment; 
a low temperature a distal one. So far as | am aware, there are 
no real exceptions to these rules. Light causes a distal migration 
of pigment and its absence a proximal one in Chameleon (Briicke, 
°52; Keller, ’95, etc.), Anolis (Carlton, ’03; Parker and Starratt 
’o4), and Phrynosoma blainvillei; and I know of no lizard of 
which the reverse is true. Heat causes a proximal migration of 
pigment and cold a distal one in‘Chamzleon (Briicke, ’52; Keller 
95); Eumeces schneideri, [arentola annularis, Uromastix, Scelop- 
orus undulatus, Crotaphytus collaris, Phrynosoma cornutum, 
Amphibolurus barbatus, Agama mossambica, A. stellio, A. iner- 
mis and Cachryx defensor (de Grijs, ’99, pp. 51-54), and Anolis 
(Parker and Starratt, ’04). Here, too, | know of no exceptions. 
At unusually high temperatures, such as obtain in the full 
sunlight of torrid deserts, the light reactions of the melano- 
phores of certain species of lizards are apparently subordinate to 
their temperature reactions, and hence these forms may appear 
light-colored in full sunlight. Thus Amphibolurus barbatus, 
according to de Grijs (’99, p. 54), darkens in the early morning 
sunlight but changes to light gray as the heat of midday comes 
on, and Anolis (Parker and Starratt, ’04) becomes green, 1. €.5 1tS 
melanophore pigment migrates proximally, at 40° C. to 45° C. 
irrespective of illumination. The same is probably true, for 
reasons already given, of Stellio (Filippi, 66) Phrynosoma orbicu- 
lare (Hoffmann, ’90), Varanus and Uromastix (Thilenius, °97) 
Calotes emma (Gadow, ’or, p. 519), and most desert-inhabiting 
agamids and iguanids (de Grijs, ’99, p. 54). “Thus, in my opinion, 
the apparently exceptional cases of light reactions in melanophores 
are really instances of normal temperature reactions and do not 
show any reversal of the regular processes. I believe, therefore, 
that the simple rules already stated as to the relation of heat and 
light to the pigment migration in the melanophores of lizards 
will hold for all those lacertilians in which such changes occur. 


AIO | G. H. Parker 


Not only do the melanophores of lizards show uniformity in 
the migration of their pigment under the influence of light, but 
the rule that holds in these cases appears to be of much wider 
application. It is well known that in the eyes of most verte- 
brates the pigment of the retinal pigment-cells migrates back 
and forth under varying illumination, and here, as in the mela- 
nophores, light causes a distal migration and its absence a proxi- 
mal one. ‘The same has been shown to be true by Hess (’05, p. 
421) for the retinal pigment in the eyes of cephalopods. ‘The 
compound eyes of many arthropods show similar changes. Ac- 
cording to Exner (’91) and others, the dark pigment of these 
eyes is often divided into two layers, one distal and the other 
proximal. ‘The proximal layer consists of dark pigment gran- 
ules within the retinular cells and these granules migrate back 
and forth under changes of illumination. The distal migra- 
tion is made in the light and the proximal one in the dark exactly 
as with the melanophores. Not only is there agreement in the 
direction of the migration but the relative rates in the few cases 
known show a certain similarity. ‘Thus, in the retinular cells in 
Palzemonetes (Parker, ’97), the distal migration is more quickly 
accomplished than the proximal one, a condition parallel with 
that seen in the migration of the pigment in the melanophores 
of Anolis (Carlton, ’03; Parker and Starratt, ’04) and of Phry- 
nosoma. 

In all these instances the direction of the migration is in 
strict relation to the source of light and is not determined by such 
obvious structures of the cell as the nucleus. Thus in the 
melanophores of the lizards, in the retinal pigment cells of the 
vertebrates, and in the retinular cells of certain crustaceans, 
Gammarus (Parker, ’99), for instance, the distal migration of 
pigment is in a direction away from the nucleus, while in the 
retinular cells of most crustaceans and insects (Exner, ’91; Parker 
’97) it is toward that organ. ‘Thus the position of the nucleus 
seems in no way to influence the direction taken by the migrating 
pigment. 

As the pigment particles of the melanophores are within cell 
limits and, when illuminated, move toward the source of light, 


Influence of Light and Heat on Melanophores 4Il 


the phenomenon may be described as a form of intracellular 
phototropism positive in character. It is, however, highly im- 
probable that the pigment granules take any active part in this 
operation; in my opinion they are simply transported by the 
protoplasm, which in some instances certainly receives directly 
from the light the stimulus to motion. That the pigment par- 
ticles are not necessarily concerned in the motion is seen in the 
retinular cells of Gammarus, whose distal pigment, which is most 
exposed to light, fails to migrate, whereas that which is some- 
what more proximal in the same cell performs the characteristic 
migration (Parker, ’99). 

It is probable that the forms of pigment migration already 
discussed are more or less adaptive in their character. “The dark 
color of the lizard’s skin in moderate illumination at a moderate 
temperature insures, possibly, among other things, a certain 
degree of warmth which would be superfluous, if not dangerous, 
at a higher temperature, and in consequence the skin becomes 
light-colored in hot sunlight. The movement of the retinal 
pigment in both vertebrates and arthropods is well calculated 
to protect the receptive organs of their retinas from overstimula- 
tion by light and to improve the sharpness of their retinal images. 
Thus these pigment changes are not without adaptive character. 
Admitting such to be true, it might be supposed that if a case 
arose in which a reversed migration of pigment would be of service 
to the organism, such a form of migration would be evolved and 
a set of pigment cells in whichthe pigment granules under illumi- 
nation would migrate away from the source of light instead of 
toward it would be produced. In this connection it is interesting 
to observe that such a reversed movement of pigment does occur 
in the distal pigment cells of the compound eyes of many arthro- 
pods, but that where this has been studied with fullness, as in 
Palzmonetes, it has been shown that these cells, though they 
contain the same kind of pigment as the proximal cells do, have 
a pigment change based upon a wholly different principle; the 
cell as a whole migrates distally and proximally and the pigment 
granules within show no intracellular rearrangement (Parker, 
’97). Hence it seems probable that the melanophores, retinal 


412 G. H. Parker 


pigment cells, and other like structures in which dark pigment 
eranules exhibit migratory movements, are restricted as to these 
possibilities, and that in light they always transport their pigment 
toward the source and never in the reverse direction. 

~ Whether heat is as uniform in this particular as light seems 
to be, cannot be stated with certainty. Parker and Starratt (’04) 
have pointed out the uniformity of its effects on the melanophores 
of the vertebrate skin and it is possible that this uniformity also 
extends to the retinal pigment, for Herzog (’05), following up 
certain observations made by Kuhne (’79, p. 334), has shown 
that in the frog for a range of temperatures between 18° Cyand 
o° C. the retinal pigment migrates distally with a decreasing 
temperature and proximally with an increasing one, as the pigment 
of the integumentary melanophores does, but in ranges above 
18° C., according to Herzog, the reverse takes place. However, 
the relation of the retinal pigment migration to temperature has 
been so little studied that further investigation will be necessary 
before safe general statements can be formulated. 


SUMMARY 


1. Phrynosoma blainvillei can change the color of its skin from 
a light yellowish gray with dark bands and spots to a dark chestnut 
a mottled with black. 

The light coloration is produced by the proximal migration 
of the pigment granules out of the processes of the melanophore 
and other like cells into their bodies, thus exposing to the light 
the reflecting guanine layer. 

The dark coloration is produced by the distal migration 
of these pigment granules from the cell bodies into their processes, 
whereby the guanine layer becomes covered and cut off from the 
light. 

4. In Phrynosoma blainvillei the proximal migration is favored 
by heat and the absence of light, and the distal one by cold and 
light. 

5. In Phrynosoma blainyillei between 15° C. and 32° C. light 
or its absence 1s more effective as a stimulus to color change than 
heat or cold. 


Influence of Light and Heat on Melanophores 413 


6. The blanching of certain lizards in strong sunlight, which 
has been supposed to be evidence of a reversed light reaction, 
is probably not a light reaction at all, but a temperature reaction 
normal in direction. 

7. As inthe retinular cells of Palzmonetes, the distal migra- 
tion of the melanophore pigment of lizards is more quickly accom- 
plished than the proximal migration. 

Swe is probable that in all melanophores in which there 1s 
a migration of pigment, light or a low temperature will induce 
a migration toward the source of illumination and the absence 
of light or a high temperature a migration in the reverse direc- 
tion. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


Bricke, E., ’52.—Untersuchungen tiber den Farbenwechsel des afrikanischen 
Chamialeons. Denkschr. kais. Akad. Wiss., Wien, math.-naturw. 
Cl., Bd. iv, Abt. 1, pp. 179-210, 1 Taf. 

Cartton, F. C., ’03.—The Color Changes in the Skin of the So-called Florida 
Chameleon, Anolis carolinensis Cuv. Proc. Amer. Acad. Arts 
and Sci., vol. xxxix. No. 10, pp. 257-276, 1 pl. 

Exner, S.,’91.—Die Physiologie der facettirten Augen von Krebsen und Insec- 
ten. Deuticke, Leipzig und Wien, vi +206, pp., 7 Taf. 

Fiurept, F. pe.,’66.—Sulla struttura della cute dello Stellio Caucasicus. Mem. 
Accad. Sci., Torino, ser. 2, tom. 23, pp. 363-373, I tav. 

Gavow, H., ’o1.—Amphibia and Reptiles. The Cambridge Natural History, 
vol. viii, xi1 + 668 pp. 

Grys, P. pr, ’99.—Einiges iiber Farbwechsel-Vermogen bei Reptilien. Zool. 
Garten, Jahrg. 40, No. 2, pp. 49-55. 

Herzoc, H., ’05.—Experimentelle Untersuchungen zur Physiologie der Bewe- 
gungsvorgange in der Netzhaut. Arch. f. Anat.u.Physiol., Physiol. 
Abt., Jahrg. 1905, Heft 5-6, pp. 413-464, Taf. 5. 

Hess, C., ’05.—Beitrige zur Physiologie und Anatomie des Cephalopoden- 
auges. Arch. f. ges. Physiol., Bd. cix, pp. 393-439, Dat. 5-8: 

Horrmann, C. K., ’90.—Fidechsen und Wasserechsen. Bronn ’s Klassen und 
Ordnungen des Thier-Reichs, Bd. vi, Abt. 3, Reptilia 2, pp. 
443-1399, Taf. 49-107. 

Keizer, R., ’95.—Ueber den Farbenwechsel des Chamaeleons und _ einiger 
anderer Reptilien. Arch. f. ges. Physiol., Bd. Ixi, pp. 123-168, 
saiey4: 


414 G. H. Parker 


Ktune, W., ’79.—Chemische Vorgange in der Netzhaut. Jn L. Hermann, 
Handbuch der Physiol., Bd. ii, Theil 1, pp. 235-342. 

Parker, G. H., ’97.—Photomechanical Changes in the Retinal Pigment Cells 
of Palamonetes, and their Relation to the Central Nervous System. 
Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool., Harvard Coll., vol. xxx, No. 6, pp. 273-300, 
I pl. 

’99.—The Photomechanical Changes in the Retinal Pigment of 

Gammarus. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool., Harvard Coll., vol. xxxv, 
No. 6, pp. 141-148, 1 pl. 

Parker, G. H., and Srarratt, S. A., ’04.—The Effect of Heat on the Color 
Changes in the Skin of Anolis carolinensis Cuv. Proc. Amer. 
Acad. Arts and Sci., vol. xl, No. 10, pp. 455-466. 

Tuirenius, G., ’97.—Der Farbenwechsel von Varanus griseus, Uromastix 
acanthinurus und Agame inermis. Morph. Arb., Bd.vii, Heft 3, 
pp- 515-544, Taf. 17-18. 


SOME REACTIONS OF CATERPILLARS AND MOTHS 


BY 
ALFRED G. MAYER AND CAROLINE G. SOULE 


It is believed by many naturalists that the larva of Danais 
plexippus is warningly colored, for it is conspicuously ringed in 
black, white and yellow; often displays itself openly upon the 
leaves of its food plant, and it is not known to be subject to the 
attacks of birds or other vertebrates. “Vhe larva normally feeds 
only upon species of milkweed (Asclepias), although in captivity 
it may sometimes be induced to feed upon the leaves of the carrot. 
Each individual larva commonly spends its entire life upon a 
single plant. It usually lives upon or near the upper half of the 
plant, and only rarely does it descend to a point below the middle 
of the plant. 

In fact, were the larva to crawl down the stem and escape from 
the plant it might possibly starve before finding another Asclepias. 
We have frequently placed larve upon the lower part of the stem 
of a milkweed (Asclepias) and in all but one instance they crawled 
up the stem and remained for days at or near the top of the plant 
feeding upon the leaves. In one case, however, the larva crawled 
upward but moved away from the milkweed upon some blades 
of grass which touched it. 

It must be borne in mind, however, that the caterpillar of 
D. plexippus is sometimes found upon very small or immature 
milkweed plants; too small to provide them with sufficient food 
upon which to mature. Under these conditions it would seem that 
they must either find other milkweeds or starve. If this be the 
case their success in finding other milkweeds must be facilitated 
by the fact that these plants usually grow in clusters, but we have 
no evidence leading us to conclude that the larva of D. plexippus 
has the ability to direct its course to a milkweed rather than to any 
other vlant. 


Tue Journat or Experimenta Zootocy, Vor 111, No. 3. 


416 Alfred G. Mayer and Caroline G. Soule 


The larve of Argynnis, Catocala, and some other lepidoptera 
feed only at night, and crawl a considerable distance away from 
their food, and remain hidden during the day, and these cater- 
pillars must in some way be guided pack to their food. 

That the larva of D. plexippus may also have this ability, seems 
probable from the fact that if the caterpillar be repeatedly dis- 
turbed it finally curls up and drops off the milkweed. 

Our experiments show that under normal conditions the cater- 
pillar of D. plexippus is constrained to remain upon the milkweed 
in obedience to two reactions which the creature displays with 
almost machine-like regularity. It is negatively geotactic, and 
positively phototactic. If the larva be placed head downward, 
in darkness, upon a vertically suspended string, it will usually 
turn at once, and crawl upward. If nowthe string be reversed so 
as to cause the larva to be again head downward, it again reverses 
its direction and crawls upward. This may be repeated time 
after time with the same result. Often when the string is reversed 
the larva continues to crawl downward for a short distance, but 
it is restless in so doing, and shows a tendency to frequently stretch 
its anterior segments outward at right angles to the body, and to 
sweep widely to and fro with its head. ‘This is sure to result soon 
in the head being bent completely back upon the body, and in 
consequence the larva crawls upward. In crawling upward the 
creature 1s more at ease and rarely stretches its head horizontally 
outward. Thus in its travels the larva is almost certain to go 
farther in an upward than in a downward direction. ‘This is 
especially true if the larva be in ordinary daylight but it also takes 
place in the dark. Larvz were placed head downward upon a 
vertical string which was placed in an absolutely dark chamber, 
and at the eal of three minutes the string was observed. If the 
larva had turned and crawled upward the string was reversed, and 
observed again at the end of another three minutes. ‘Thirty-seven 
experiments upon three larve gave the following results. “The 
larvee, at the expiration of three minutes, had turned and crawled 
up to a point above their original position in twenty-seven cases. 
In four cases the larve continued downward, and were found head 
downward and still descending, while in six other cases the larvae 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars and Moths 417 


had ascended above the original position, but had again turned 
and begun to descend. It is apparent that the larva shows a 
tendency to crawl upward against the force of gravity, and this 
reaction alone would tend to retain it at or near the top of its food 
plant. No difference is apparent in the reactions of well-fed or 
hungry larve, excepting that the latter are more active. 

In another series of experiments a milkweed was planted in a 
flower-pot and suspended upside down. Larve placed upon the 
young leaves, at what was now the lowest part of the plant, crawled 
upward, and sooner or later they went up the stem of the plant 
and crawled off upoh the flower-pot. One larva crawled up the 
entire length of the plant (nineteen inches) and off upon the flower- 
pot in four minutes and twenty-seven seconds. Upon being again 
replaced upon the young leaves, it again crawled off the plant in 
about six and one-half minutes. ‘The plant was then reversed and 
placed in its normal upright position. ‘The same larva was then 
placed at the base of the stem, and promptly crawled upward to 
the top of the plant where it remained feeding for fifty-three hours, 
when it was removed for further experiments. Frequent repeti- 
tions of this experiment showed that the larve do not long remain 
upon a plant which has been turned upside down 

We may conclude that the larva has no inherent tendency to 
seek out the young leaves, and that its remaining near the top of 
the plant it not a matter of “judgment” or conscious preference, 
but is due to negative geotaxis, and may be a wholly unconscious 
reaction. It is worthy of note that the tendency to turn and crawl 
upward only affects the larva when in motion. — It will often remain 
at rest head downward for hours at a time, but upon beginning 
to move 1s almost certain soon to turn upward. 

But, another influence contributes to cause the caterpillar to 
remain at or near the top of the plant. ‘This is due to the fact 
that the creature is positively phototactic, and is particularly sensi- 
tive to the ultra-violet rays. Ifthe larva be placed in the middle 
of a horizontally placed opaque tube, one end of which is stopped 
with a cover impervious to light, while the other end is open to 
admit diffused daylight, the larva always turns and crawls toward 
the open, lighted, end of the tube. Experiments were made upon 


AI8 Alfred G. Mayer and Caroline G. Soule 


six larvae under the above conditions, using a pasteboard tube 
eleven inches long and one and one-half inches in caliber, and they 
crawled fifty-one times to the open end of the tube and never to the 
dark closedend. ‘The larvae were pushed back into the center of the 
tube after each trial and the experiment so arranged that the head- 
end of the creature faced the dark end of the tube but invariably 
the larva turned and crawled toward the daylight. ‘This positive 
heliotropism in caterpillars is well known, and Loeb (’05, p. 42) 
states that it was shown by all of those upon which he experimented 
even by the larvae of the willow-borer which live in the stems of 
willows where they are not exposed to light. When this experi- 
ment with the horizontal tube is tried, substituting the light of a 36- 
candle power kerosene lamp, four feet distant, for ‘pylon a very 
different reaction takes place, for under these conditions the larvze 
crawled to the light end of the tube twenty-eight times, and to the 
dark end twenty-one times. The light of a kerosene lamp is 
deficient in ultra-violet rays, and it seemed probable that the larve 
might be sensitive to these rays and hence practically indifferent 
to the light of kerosene, while they always crawled toward diffused 
daylight. In order to test this the diffused daylight was obliged 
to pass through a layer of bisulphide of carbon one and one-half 
inches thick before entering the light end of the tube. Experiments 
were made upon four larve under these conditions. One of them 
crawled five times to the light end, and eight times to the dark end 
of the tube; another larva went twenty-one times to the light and 
one time to the dark end, another went once to the light and ten 
times to the dark end, and the fourth larva went three times to the 
light end and never to the dark end of the tube. 

Thus in forty-nine trials the caterpillars went nineteen times to 
the dark end of the tube, and thirty times to the end which admitted 
diffused daylight deprived of its ultra-violet rays. Now, if ordi- 
nary diffused daylight be admitted at one end of the tube the larve 
always crawl toward it, and are also much more active than when 
under the influence of the daylight deprived of the ultra-violet 
rays. We see, then, that they are attracted chiefly if not wholly 
by the ultra-violet rays and are but little if at all influenced by the 
rays that constitute, to us, the visible spectrum. 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars and Moths 419 


Even hungry larve in diffused daylight deprived of the ultra- 
violet rays move with extreme slowness, and often remain for hours 
at a time motionless; and apparently unstimulated. ‘They act 
similarly in the dark. In ordinary diffused daylight, however, 
they are very active, and rarely remain quiescent more than a few 
minutes at atime. Intense sunlight renders the larva very restless 
but it becomes less active when in the shade. When hungry the 
larve are much more active than when well fed. 

At night when its negative geotropism is apparently the only 
factor tending to maintain the caterpillar near the top of the plant, 
the sluggishness of its movements in the darkness must be a safe- 
guard, preventing its moving to any considerable distance. 

The negative geotropism of the caterpillar is, however, more 
potent than the influence of the diffused daylight of an ordinary 
room, as is shown by the following experiment. If we place the 
larva in the middle of a vertical pasteboard tube having the upper 
end closed with a cap so as to prevent the entrance of light, while 
the lower end remains open admitting the diffused daylight of the 
room, the larve show a greater tendency to go upward into the 
darkness than downward toward the light. Thus two larve, 
placed in such a tube, went fifteen times to the dark (upper) end 
of the tube, and eight times to the light (lower) end, although they 
were invariably placed head downward, nearer the lower than 
the upper end, and facing the light at the beginning of each 
trial. 

It will be remembered that Loeb showed that the starving larvae 
of Porthesia chrysorrhcea are positively phototactic, while they 
become practically indifferent to the influence of light when well fed. 
Larve of D. plexippus are, however, always positively phototactic 
to moderate daylight, whether well fed or starving. Lubbock 
(Avebury) (83) showed that ants are always sensitive to the ultra- 
violet rays, reacting strongly against them, and Loeb found that 
nocturnal moths are attracted mainly if not wholly by the more 
refrangible rays of the spectrum. 

In 1903 Parker published a beautiful series of observations, 
showing that when the mourning-cloak butterfly, Vanessa antiopa, 
alights in sunlight it comes to rest with its head turned away 


420 Alfred G. Mayer and Caroline G. Soule 


fromthe sun, and with its body in line with the rays of light. On 
cloudy days or in shaded places this reaction is not displayed, 
and even in brilliant sunshine it is overruled by the chemotropic 
response to food. ‘This negative phototropism is seen only in 
intense sunlight, and then only after the butterfly has been upon the 
wing; 7. ¢., after a certain state of metabolism has been established. 
The butterfly will creep or fly toward a source of moderate light, 
such asa lamp in a darkened room, or a window admitting light; 
and it is even attracted toward intense sunlight until it has begun 
to fly about, when it becomes negatively phototropic. When the 
eyes are blackened no reaction to light occurs, and the butterfly 
flies upward. 

It appears that positive phototaxis and negative geotaxis are all 
that are required to maintain the larva of D. plexippus upon the 
young leaves of its food plant, and to practically prevent its wander- 
ing away from the plant itself. The larva certainly displays no 
“judgment” in finding its most nutritious food, and probably its 
reactions are almost if not quite unconscious, for they are dis- 
played with almost machine-like regularity at every recurrence of 
the stimulus, however as Jennings (’04) has shown, animals are 
not machines for their method of behavior is often that of trial and 
error and internal as well as external factors modify their behavior. 

Another criterion of consciousness is the presence of associative 
memory, but all of our attempts to detect the presence of such 
memory have failed. 

A large number of experiments were made upon larve which 
will eat only certain definite kinds of leaves, and will starve to 
death without taking a single bite of other sorts of leaves. These 
larve can be induced to eat sparingly of previously uneatable leaves, 
however, if the sap of their proper food plant be pressed into the 
previously distasteful leaves. For example, the larva of D. 
plexippus can be induced to eat sumach (Rhus) leaves if the latter 
be thickly covered with the milky juice of Asclepias. ‘This only 
occurs, however, when the larva has become very hungry. 

Conversely, milkweed leaves can be rendered uneatable by 
covering them thickly with the milky juice of sumach. ‘The larva 
appears not to be guided by any sense of color in seeking its food, 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars and Moths 421 


for it will eat Asclepias leaves which have been painted over with 
aniline red. 

We find that any caterpillar can be induced to bite at or devour 
any foreign substance, if one proceed as follows: Allow the larva 
to begin eating its proper food plant, then slide up in front of 
it a distasteful leaf, sheet of paper, piece of tinfoil, etc. “The larva 
will then take a few bites out of the foreign substance, but will 
soon withdraw its head, often snapping its mandibles and thrash- 
ing from side to side. Very soon, however, it reeommences to 
devour its proper food in a normal manner. Under these condi- 
tions if the foreign substance or distasteful leaf be presented to the 
larva at intervals of not more than one and one-half minutes, about 
the same number of bites are taken at each presentation. The 
instinctive mechanism of eating once set in motion continues as if it 
possessed momentum. ‘This may be taken to show that the larva 
does not retain the memory of its disagreeable’ experience for an 
interval of a minute anda half. If, however, we make the interval 
about thirty seconds, the larva will take fewer and fewer bites of the 
distasteful object and will soon refuse it altogether, and stop eating 
whenever it is presented. 

The following table gives the results of observations of the above 
sort upon larve of D. plexippus; the intervals of presentation of 
the distasteful leaf being as nearly as possible one and one-half 
minutes apart. The table shows the number of bites taken by the 
larve at successive presentations of a leaf of the Virginia Creeper, 
Ampelopsis. 

An inspection of the table shows that the successive reactions 
in any single series of experiments are very irregular, but that 
these irregularities tend to disappear when we take the sum of 
the corresponding reactions in a large number of experiments. 
For example, in the column headed “Summation of series show- 
ing at least nine reactions,’ we see that the numbers are relatively 
more nearly equal each to each than are those of any one of the 


1 The words “‘ disagreeable,” “‘agreeable,’’ ‘distasteful,’ etc.,are used in default of better expressions, 
We must not forget that the larva may be quite unconscious, and may act by reflexes, which may be 
very complex and controlled by varying internal and external conditions, thus being capable of much 


modifiability, although possibly unconscious. 


TABLE I 


Showing the Number of Bites taken by Larve of D. plexippus at each Presentation of a Leaf of Ampelopsis, the Ampelopsis 


being Presented at Intervals of 13 Minutes 


suonoray 6 | 


> t+ 
4svay 4B JO satiag Bee SS AS SS 
: Se =e fey alr Ss 
[2 jo uoyeuruimns 
‘ ad 
zobl ‘gz “\das ES cc ies 6 whee i Ee 
IA ‘ON vare’y Saitoh ase a 
. . a 
zobr ‘gr -3dag 1) 10069) (E~=G01) "OO NOy 31) (OF col all OL OR 1E~) a Oss ees 
A ‘ON PAreT = sad = 7 re ee 
zobr ‘Sr -3dag OorHOMMODAHVWO A 
A “ON PAIe’T = t+ A Ae i 
¢ é 
zobr ‘hi -ydag hee c) et GylGPeGana CaS ort © wy tase en we 
A ‘ON Paley tare ia mas bahia 
zobr ‘tr -3dag CeO OO 1 OO 
A ‘OND Parey 
zobr ‘zr -adag Oo ++ MO O © O 
A ‘ON BAle'T 
zobr ‘L -ydag Le erica eS re ei 
AI ‘ON PAre'T 
1061 ‘F¥-0O 00 OD MH tO AO —| 
TI ‘ON PAre’'T i” a ie eas 
1 61 *F -190 | COMA 1) NOL! BORN a oa) 
I “ON PAreyT = 
1261 ‘7 "pO mamanmaoand oa 
TIT ON BAre'T | 
‘ . 
1.61 ‘£30 weta tnmmOmanvo nittoOoOanaAamMtH OO OO 
TI ‘ON Bale’, 7 te TAs < a 
“ . 
1061 ‘9 90 ly. (eG as OF ON On Coaten sO 
TI (ON Barley Sy 5 
1061 “9 “390 ootoo ano 
TI ‘ON BaAre’T | AS, TA 
‘ . 
robr ‘S$ “pO ©Or RA RAO OMS 
TE ‘ON bare'y =~ — =a A HA 
1061 ‘Z "390 mo 00 KR Ne HO 
II “ON FAreT 
1061 “z *O onto naa at 
I ‘ON earey 
‘ . 
1061 ‘$390 © 0 0 0 0 6 0 mim 0 
ION eareyT 
9) 08h 9220 QA Se ean AO ican 
( [ON earey 
Cj. 
1061 ‘L “390 ooeoenrnrotannomo oO Oo 
< . 
1061 ‘g "~O | maw no S OF a O aE AN oO) mM mt OS 
J ‘ON Paley = 
s[@l1y, jo ‘ON Grew tas) SC) OOo & eet tase mca & OE 
Sob, ee ee << 


: 


*From the twenty-first to the fortieth trial, inclusive, this series gave the following for the number of bites at each trial 


6, 0, 2, 5, 9, 7, I, O, O, O, O, O, I, O, O, O, 2, 0, 4, O. 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars. and Moths 423 


component series. [he fact comes to light that there is no sensible 
falling off of the reaction, the number of bites taken at the ninth 
reaction being fully equal to those taken at the first. It can hardly 
be affirmed that the larve must take a certain definite number of 
bites from a foreign leaf before being able to “recognize” or react 
against its “injurious” or “distasteful” nature, for when larve 
are placed in a chamber surrounded with these same leaves they 
will starve to death before taking even a single bite. ‘The instinct 
to eat must first be initiated by the presence of food proper to the 
larva, but once the mechanism of this instinct is called into play, 
it continues as if by its own momentum. ‘The phenomenon might, 
therefore, be termed the ““momentum of reaction.”” As the value 
of this momentum remains the same, and does not decrease with 
successive reactions it probably represents an unconscious reflex 
on the part of the larva. ‘The presence of its proper food plant 
causes the eating reaction to come into play, and this reaction con- 
tinues for a constant and apparently unmodified time against a 
regularly recurring succession of counteracting stimuli. 

Larve do, however, become accustomed to certain stimuli, and 
if such a stimulus be repeated, it finally ceases to produce any reac- 
tion. If one blows a current of air upon a moving larva, or raps 
sharply upon the surface on which it is crawling, the creature con- 
tracts and remains motionless for some time. If this stimulus be 
repeated every time the larva begins to move, it finally loses all 
effect. For example, a single blow of the breath sufficed to halt a 
caterpillar of D. plexippus every time it started for four successive 
times; at the fifth start, however, two blows were necessary to cause 
It to cease moving; then, however, one blow sufficed until the nine- 
teenth attempt to start, when eleven blows were necessary to stop 
it. [hen one blow sufficed until the forty-seventh attempt at start 
when five blows were necessary; on the forty-eighth attempt to start, 
one blow still sufficed to stop it; but when it started for the forty- 
ninth time, one hundred blows had no effect upon it whatsoever. 

Usually, however, the results are not so irregular; for example, 
one blow sufficed to stop a caterpillar in thirty-seven attempts to 
start, but at its thirty-eighth attempt one hundred blows had no 
effect and it moved steadily onward. 


A24 Alfred G. Mayer and Caroline G. Soule 


When the larva of D. plexippus has become insensitive to blows 
of the breath, it still reacts to mechanical shocks, stopping when 
the twig upon which it crawls is sharply struck, mechanical shocks 
can, therefore, be distinguished from other mechanical or chemical 
stimull. 

Davenport (97), Loeb ('00), Massart (’01) and Jennings (’02, 
’o4) have studied this final failure to react to a repeated stimulus. 
The studies of Jennings upon various fixed Infusoria are especially 
instructive, and he concludes ‘that the phenomenon is not neces- 
sarily due to fatigue either of the muscles or of the sensory or per- 
ceptive power, but as Davenport has expressed it, “When an 
organism has been stimulated by contact for some time it becomes 
changed so that it no longer responds as it did at first.” In other 
words, internal factors come to modify its behavior. We must 
bear in mind that in some cases this failure to respond to repeated 
stimuli may be due to fatigue as has been shown to be the case in 
the rejection of pieces of filter paper by the tentacles of Metridium 
(see L. F. Allabach (’05); Biol. Bulletin, vol. x, p. 35). 

In another series of experiments designed to test the presence 
or absence of associative memory, larve of Pyrrharctia isabella 
were placed in a wooden box sixteen inches long, five inches wide 
and four inches deep, which was divided into two compartments 
by means of a central wooden partition. ‘This partition was 
pierced by a small opening. The chamber on one side of the 
partition contained moist earth and growing food plants while the 
chamberon the other side was barren. ‘The larvz were placed inthe 
barren chamber and after wandering about they found their way 
through the opening into the food chamber. ‘Then after being 
allowed to eat for a few minutes they were thrust back through the 
opening into the center of the barren chamber. Apparently, how- 
ever, they failed to learn the direct path to the food, but always 
wandered about upon successive trials. For example, one larva 
was placed in the barren chamber twenty-one times; during the 
first ten times it found the food in the average time of 50.8 minutes. 
The average of the next ten times was, however, 101 minutes and 
the final trial consumed 434 minutes. Another larva which was 
placed thirty-eight times in the barren chamber appeared to do 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars and Moths 425 


better for it made an average of 88.8 minutes for the first ten, 66.1 
minutes for the second ten, 52.8 for the third ten, and 129.2 minutes 
on the last eight trials. Another larva made twenty-five trials 
giving an average of 29.3 minutes on the first ten, 17.3 minutes 
on the second ten, and 18.8 minutes on the last five. Another 
larva made the following averages in forty-two trials. For the 
first ten 58.7 minutes, second ten 58.1, third ten 39.1, fourth ten 
32.2. On the whole it appears that the larve do not learn, even 
after repeated trials, to shorten their paths to the food. 

It was evident that the larva are attracted by the presence of 
the food, however, for when the food chamber contained earth with 
no plants the larve entered it at much more protracted intervals 
than when food was present. For example, when the food-cham- 
ber contained growing plants, a larva entered it forty times, the 
average time for each trial being forty-seven minutes. When the 
plants were removed leaving only the moist earth the larva crawled 
into the chamber less frequently, the average time in nine trials 
being 228 minutes, and when both earth and plants were removed 
it made an average during seven trials of 417 minutes. 

It will be remembered that Yerkes (02) and Yerkes and Hug- 
gins (’03) found that the green crab and the crawfish are able to | 
learn simple labyrinth habits. 

A study was made of the instinct to spin the cocoon, in order to, 
if possible, reduce it to a set of reactions to definite stimuli. ‘The 
experiments were carried out upon Samia cynthia and Callosamia 
promethea. In both of these moths, however, the larvae accommo- 
date themselves to very varied conditions. For example, we have 
seen several cocoons of S. cynthia which were in all respects similar 
to normal cocoons excepting that they were much larger than the 
average. These large cocoons, however, contained each a single 
large inner chamber in which there were two pupe. Apparently 
two larve had started to spin side by side at about the same time, 
using one and the same leaf and had mutually accommodated them- 
selves to the conditions so as to contribute each its share toward 
the spinning of a single cocoon large enough to accommodate both 
of them. If all leaves be removed, the larvae of C. promethea and 
S. cynthia will still spin cocoons even upon a perfectly flat surface. 


426 Alfred G. Mayer and Caroline G. Soule 


These larvae normally start the cocoon by covering the leaf-stalk 
with a layer of silk. “hey then spin down the middle of the upper 
side of the leaf and draw the edges of the leaf together, forming a 
tube. After spinning an outer case of moderately loose silk, the 
inner case of densely spun silk 1s constructed. ‘This inner case is 
ellipsoidal, and its innermost surface smooth and polished. ‘The 
silk at its upper end is, however, loose and the strands are longi- 
tudinal so as to allow for the exit of the moth. ‘The lower end of 
the inner case 1s densely woven and its innermost surface polished. 
The cocoon hangs vertically downward and the larva always 
pupates with its head turned upward. We find that this upward 
turning of the head of the larva 1s in response to gravity, for if the 
cocoon Tbe reversed in the dark immediately after the outer envelope 
is completed, the larve often become reversed and still pupate head 
upward, but facing what would under normal conditions be the 
lower end of the cocoon. Miss Caroline G. Soule first tried this 
experiment in 1900 with twenty-eight larva of S. cynthia, reversing 
the cocoons of nineteen immediately after the formation of the 
outer envelope, and of nine a few hours after it had been completed. 
In all of the cocoons which were turned upside down immediately 
after the formation of the outer envelope, the larva were reversed 
in their cocoons and pupated head upward. In thirteen of these 
cocoons the silk of the inner chamber was loose at both ends. In 
one cocoon the larva had bitten a hole in the lower end when it 
became the uppermost, and had spun the other solid. Five cocoons 
had no exit arranged, and their inner capsules were solid at 
both ends. In all of the nine cocoons which were turned upside 
down, a few hours after the completion of the outer envelope the 
larve pupated head downward, and the cocoons were normal in 
all respects. 

In 190102, a similar experiment was tried upon twenty-seven 
cocoons of C. promethea. “Twenty-one of these were not affected; 
the larve pupating head downward. In six other cocoons the 
larve pupated head upward, thus being reversed in reference to 
the cocoon. In one of these cocoons the inner case was loosely 
spun at both ends, while in the five others it was spun as in the 
normal cocoon and thus the head of the pupa faced the densely 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars and Moths 42 


“SI 


woven end of the inner capsule which apparently allowed of no 
ready exit. We tried enclosing larve in capsules of glass and of 
cotton network resembling the inner capsules of the cocoon in 
shape but the larve still spun an inner capsule of silk in a normal 
manner. We then opened a normal cocoon and after removing 
the pupa placed a spinning larva within it. The larva spun a new 
inner capsule which was nowhere attached to the cocoon within 
which it was made. Evidently the instinct to spin the cocoon 
may be adjusted to widely different environmental conditions, 
some of which are never met with in nature. It cannot be reduced 
to a simple reaction of thigmotaxis. ‘That is to say, the larva does 
not cease to spin merely because it feels itself pressed upon by the 
confined space of the cocoon, for even if it be placed with a cocoon 
already spun, it re-spins it. he larva does, however, orient itself 
in reference to gravity and normally pupates head upward. 

Darwin (71) supposed that the peculiar and often brilliant 
coloration of male Lepidoptera had been brought about as the 
result of long continued selection upon the part of the females. 

Studies were carried out upon the mating instinct in order to 
determine whether the females exercised any selection in the choice 
of their mates. 

In 1900 Mayer studied the mating instinct in Callosamia pro- 
methea in which it appeared that the female exercised no choice 
in the matter. It seemed desirable to repeat these experiments 
upon a large scale and with this in view more than 1500 cocoons 
of C. promethea were collected during the winter of IogI—o2. 
Large numbers being gathered by our friends Messrs. Clarence 
Riker and William F. Patterson. “The cocoons were hung under 
trees and the moths allowed to fly about unconfined. It is im- 
portant in such experiments to maintain the conditions as close 
to those of nature as possible for confinement often produces 
remarkable alterations in behavior. For example, larvz which feed 
only during the night, in a state of nature, will often eat during the 
entire day in captivity. About six hundred males emerged from 
the cocoons and the wings of about one-half of them were painted 
with scarlet or green ink while the others were allowed to remain 
normal in color. It was evident that the males whose wings were 


428 Alfred G. Mayer and Caroline G. Soule 


scarlet or green succeeded fully as well in their attempts to mate as 
did the normal males. More than three hundred attempts to 
mate were observed. Seventy per cent were successful and thirty 
per cent gave rise to no visible resistance on the part of the female 
although no mating occurred. In one instance only was a normal 
male resisted successfully by the female, while in another case the 
male succeeded in mating although the female made some show 
of resistance. ‘l'wo of the green colored males were successfully 
resisted by two females, although even a slight showing of resist- 
ance on the part of the female was too rare and exceptional to be 
of any moment in selection. ‘Therefore, the peculiar black colora- 
tion of the male appears not to have been caused by sexual selec- 
tion on the part of the female, or at any rate the female promethea 
moths of the present time show no dislike for abnormal coloration 
in the male. 

In Porthetria dispar the male is brown and the female white, 
and it 1s possible that the peculiar coloration of the male may have 
been developed under the influence of sexual selection on the part 
of the female. The experiments of A. H. Kirkland (96) upon 
the mating habits of P. dispar, show that in common with C. pro- 
methea the males are attracted solely by the odor of the female. In 
C. promethea, however, Mayer found that old females are more 
attractive than young, whereas the reverse is the case with P. 
dispar. Moreover, the severed wings or scales of the females of 
C. promethea do not attract the male while those of the female of 
P. dispar are highly attractive. 

Experiments were made to determine whether the females of P. 
dispar exercised selection against maimed or abnormally colored 
males. “The wings of an equal number of males and females were 
clipped off, and the success achieved by the wingless males in their 
attempts to mate was much less than that of the perfect males, as 
will appear from the following table which records the results of 
one hundred attempts to mate. ; 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars and Moths 429 


TABLE JI. 


Showing the Reactions of Females of P. dispar whose Sight 1s Ncrmal 


} 
| 


| 


shen =o 2 Ah Hey tS 80 4 rs) 
| 3 te ye) et coe eels 2 oe 
a a z Salfakee: o& = 
aw © mone pens af Hive 
= ryercl is oy 0s S .2 0 =) 1S 
| Be ra) 7~e “ig ei es wen 
| a Y = es o Sad o & OS 
moe 2 -a QO Seas | seas 4s 
a4 oe “ee ait Om, ° 5) ey 
D Oya tuelen ow q ros oe re) o 
ance oS 8 0 -= + Do ay Wt 
4 aoe geo |eesF | gas as 
3 o 38 038 Sn ~ Sz 5 
i net YAg eet) ONS = OW a 0 
= 30 & ana ke 82S .o m Fo uO 
= ve o o 
= = om ey Ay 
Perfect G + perfect Q 19 3 7 17 65.5 34.5 
Wingless o’ + perfect Q | 4 2 II | eters 76.5 
A | 
Perfect G' + wingless 9 | 10 I 3 4 yea 28.6 
Wingless G'+ wingless Q | 6 ° 6 | 2 | oe: ixXe) 


The above table shows that the perfect males were much less 
apt to meet with resistance from the females than were the wing- 
less males. Some further experiments were made to discover 
whether the selection against the wingless males was determined 
by sight. The females were blinded by painting their eyes with a 
thick layer of quick-drying asphaltum. The results are shown 
in the following table: 


TABLE III. 


Showing the Reactions of Blind Females of P. dispar 


| 
| 
| 


een 
Soe | ga eR aren) feces 82 
~ Sir 5m a8 | 3 8 as 
. = o won LO), || silery 
Ss =o) = —o ives St | S475 4) 7S) 
qo 8 oO > one TB) a o¢ rs 
av » — — os 2 2 ist = 
po} > ~ | 5 5s | Swe o wk 
2g Ss 22 = m | 8 3 
ISS a Ow 2 wn ay a | iS ee 
2 1 On tal (S| tre 2 3 y ° =) 
G Mon ~~ Y Be OS aos 2 o's 
z PASE One Pre cohad ECs teen emi bees 
a #59 2o% a Scat) eeaee onan 
= fy S a= Sree | ks heer 
2 cs a a 
Pertectag) = pertect 9) — 10 2 2 15 (hie |) 2305 
Wingless o + perfect 9 7 2 4 apea 2283 
Perfect Go + wingless Q — I. 6 | 9) 50 
. ; | | 
Wingless G' + wingless 9 4 I 4 ||» 80 | 20 


It appears from the above tables that when the female is blinded 
the wingless males succeed fully as well as do those which are 
normal. Selection on the part of the female is therefore conditional 
upon the possession of sight. 


430 Alfred G. Mayer and Caroline G. Soule 


Another series of experiments was carried out in which the 
females were normal in all respects while the wings of the males 
were painted scarlet or bright green. ‘This abnormal coloration, 
however, failed to affect the results, and the colored males suc- 
ceeded quite as well as did normal males. Apparently the female 
selects against wingless males but not against those showing 
abnormal colors. 

These experiments upon P. dispar would have been made more 
extensive had it not been that they simply confirm the excellent 
and very extensive experiments of Crampton upon Saturnide. 
Crampton shows that normal males are more apt to mate with 
normal than with abnormal females, and also that abnormal males 
mate less readily than normal males. Moreover, abnormal 
females lay fewer eggs. In a word the actual basis of elimination 
is correlation, not sexual selection exercised by the female in 
respect to color. 

The mating instinct in the males of C. promethea and P. dispar 
is a phenomenon of chemotaxis. Sexual selection on the ground 
of color alone does not affect it, and there is no associative memory 
connected with it. We frequently placed a normal male among 
females, and after mating had taken place, the antennz of the male 
was covered with flour paste. Under these conditions the male 
never again mated, but often did so immediately when the flour paste 
was dissolved with water. It appears, therefore, that the mating 
instinct can only be called forth through the sense of smell, and not 
through associative memory. 

‘The males fly toward the females against the wind. Frequently 
we have observed a male flying up against the wind until he passed 
by the side of and beyond the female. Under these conditions he 
would often remain poised on his wings, and the wind would drifthim 
back until he came to leeward of the female, when a few vigorous 
strokes of his wings would bring him more or less toward her 
again. Inother words, the male pursued the method of trial and 
error so ably shown by Jennings ('04) to be prevalent in the ani- 
mal kingdom. 

Miss Soule found that if the wind be allowed to blow from a 
female toward a male Saturnid moth both of the same species, the 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars and Moths 431 


male may be induced to mate with a female of another species 
confined in a cage with him, thus demonstrating that the mating 
instinct is called forth through chemotropism. 


SUMMARY 


1. The caterpillar of the milkweed butterfly, D. plexippus 
is positively heliotropic to the ultra-violet rays, but almost if 
not quite unresponsive to the rays which to us constitute the visible 
spectrum. This caterpillar is also negatively geotropic. These 
two reactions serve to maintain it near the upper part of its food 
plant, and to lessen the risk of its wandering down the stem and 
starving before being able to find another milkweed. 

2. The caterpillar has no inherent perception of the proper form 
or color of its food but is guided by a chemical sense. Once the 
eating reaction be set into play it tends to continue so that the larva 
may then be induced to eat substances which it would never have 
commenced to eat inthe first instance. ‘This tendency to continue 
its activity in the face of a non-stimulus, we have called the 
momentum of its reaction. 

3. After a larva D. plexippus has commenced to eat a milkweed 
leaf, we may cause it to bite at leaves which it would not normally 
eat. If this “distasteful” leaf be presented to the feeding larva 
at intervals of one and one-half minutes, the caterpillar takes 
about the same number of bites each time, showing that it 1s not 
influenced after one and one-half minutes by the events of its past 
experience. If, however, the distasteful leaf be presented at 
intervals of about thirty seconds the larva takes fewer and 
fewer bites at each successive presentation and soon refuses it 
altogether, and ceases to eat the “distasteful” leaf when it 1s 
presented. 

4. No associative memory of more than one and one-half minutes’ 
duration can be demonstrated in larve of lepidoptera. 

5. A constantly repeated stimulus loses its effect, and this may 
be due not to fatigue, but to internal changes which express them- 
selves in modified behavior. 


4:32 Alfred G. Mayer and Caroline G. Soule 

6. The caterpillar of Samia cynthia and Callosamia promethea 
are negatively geotropic when about to pupate, and always pupate 
head upw ard. Ifthe cocoon be turned upside down after the outer 
case has been spun they will often pupate head upward, thus facing 
what in nature. would have been the lower, closed part of the 
cocoon. 

7. The mating instinct of Porthetria dispar is, on the part of the 
male, a reaction of chemotaxis, but the normal females display a 
decided selection against wingless males, although not against 
abnormally colored males. When the female is blinded she does 
not select against wingless males. 


AUTHORS ‘CITED 


ALLABACH, L. F. ’05.—Some Points Regarding the Behavior of Metridium, Bio- 
logical Bulletin, vol. x, p. 35-43. 

Crampton, H. E., ’04.—Variation and Selection in Saturnid Lepidoptera, Biolog- 
ical Bulletin, vol. vi, pp. 310-311. Also: Science, ser. 2, vol. 
xix, p. 459. Also: On a General Theory of Adaptation and 
Selection. Journal of Experimental Zodlogy, vol. 11, pp. 
425430. 

Davenport, C. B., ’97—Experimental Morphology, pt. 1; Effect of Chemical 
and Physical Agents upon Protoplasm, pp. 294, London. 

Jennincs, H. S., ’02.—Studies on Reactions to Stimuli in Unicellular Organ- 
isms, IX. On the Behavior of Fixed Infusoria (Stentor and 
Vorticella). Amer. Journ. Physiol., vol. viii, pp. 23-60. Also: 
Contributions to the Study of the Behavior of the Lower 
Organisms. Carnegie Institution of Washington, Publication 
No. 16, pp. 256, 1904. Also: 1905; The Method of Regu- 
lation in Behavior, and in Other Fields, Journal of Experi- 
mental Zodlogy vol. 11 pp. 473-494. 

KirKLAND, A. H., ’96—Assembling of the Gypsy Moth: In Forbush and 
Fernald’s Report on the Gypsy Moth. Mass. Board of Agri- 
culture, pp. 345-357, Boston. 

Lors, J., 00.—The Physiology of the Brain and General Psychology, pp. 309, 
New York. Also, ’05: Studies in General Physiology, 2 vols. 
Decennial Publications of the University of Chicago. 

Massart, J., ’01.—Annals de |’Institut Pasteur, August 25. 


Some Reactions of Caterpillars and Moths 433 


Lussock, J. (Avebury), ’83. Ants, Bees and Wasps. International Scientific 
Series. 

Mayer, A. G.,’00.—On the Mating Instinct in Moths. Annals and Mag. Nat. 
Hist., ser. 7, vol. v, pp. 183-190. Also: Psyche, vol. ix, pp. 
14-20. 

Parker, G. H., ’03.—The Phototropism of the Mourning- cloak Butterfly, 
Vanessa Antiopa, Linn. Mark Anniversary Volume, pp. 453- 
470, pl. 33, New York. 

Soute, C. G., ’02.—Notes on Hybrids of Samia cynthia and Attacus prome- 
thea. Psyche, vol. ix, pp. 411-413. 

YerKES, R. M.,’02.—Habit-formation in the Green Crab, Carcinus granulatus, 
Biological Bulletin, vol. iii, pp. 241-244. 

YERKES, R. M., and Huccins, G. E., ’03.—Habit-formation in the Crawfish, 
Cambarus affinis. Harvard Psychological Studies, vol. i, pp. 


565-577. 


_ 

my a 
“sada Eis 
“oy 
Sie f 


“ 


i ie Pe 


MODIFIABILITY IN BEHAVIOR 


II. FACTORS DETERMINING DIRECTION AND CHARACTER OF 
MOVEMENT IN THE EARTHWORM 


BY 


H. S. JENNINGS 
Fohns Hopkins University Baltimore, Md., U.S.A, 


When an organism moves in a certain direction, what are the 
various factors by which this direction is determined? ‘This is the 
question with which the present paper deals, using the earthworm 
asatype. The problem, as will appear, is a complex one, even for 
so low an organism.! ; 

According to certain theories of tropisms, much held up to recent 
times, the direction of movement in any lower organism is always 
determined in a precise and simple way by the impact of external 
agents. For a brief statement of this view we may quote the 
words of Davenport and Perkins (’97) in the introductory para- 
graph of a paper on the directed reactions of the slug: 

“Tt is almost an axiom in modern zodlogy, that whenever an 
organism, or any mass of protoplasm whatsoever, migrates in a 
definite direction, it does so because it is guided from without by 
the direction of impact of an irritant.” 

Just how the external irritant acts in guiding the organism has 
likewise been formulated in a simple way. The formulation most 
commonly accepted has been that which represents the result as 
due to the local action of the stimulating agent on the motor organs 


1The author had in hand a more extensive piece of work on the behavior of the earthworm, when 
the excellent paper of Harper (’o5) appeared. This made a statement of many of my results super- 
fluous. But while the work of Harper went farther in certain directions than my own, I have brought 
out certain facts, and especially have dealt with certain general relations which Harper did not take 
up. It seems worth while, therefore, to publish a summary of my results, bringing out the general 
relations in which I was chiefly interested. 


Tue Journat or Exprrimentat Zootocy, Vor. 111, No. 3. 


430 H. 8S. fennings 


of the region on which the stimulus impinges, as set forth especially 
by Loeb and by Verworn. Davenport again has made a con- 
venient summary of this theory, in direct application to the earth- 
worm, in a discussion of the way in which this animal reacts to 


light: 


“Represent the worm by an arrow whose head indicates the head end (Fig. 1, 4). 
Let solar rays SS fall upon it horizontally and perpendicularly to its axis. Then 
the impinging ray strikes it laterally, or, in other words, it is illuminated on one side 
and not on the other. Since, now, the protoplasm of both sides is attuned to an 


Ss S 


Low light attunement. 


A 


Low light attunement. 


Fig. 1. Diagram to explain a tropism in a muscular organism, such as the earthworm. (After 
Davenport.) 


equal intensity of light, that which is the less illuminated is nearer its optimum 
intensity. Its protoplasm isina phototonic condition. ‘That which is strongly illu- 
minated has lost its phototonic condition. Only the darkened muscles, then, are 
capable of rormal contraction; the brightly illuminated ones are relaxed. Under - 
these conditions the organism curves toward the darker side; and since its head 
region is the most sersitive, resporse begins there. Owirg to a continuance of the 
causes, the organism will cortinue to turn from the light until both sides are equally 
illuminated, 7. ¢., until it is in the light ray. Subsequent locomotion will carry the 
organism in a straight line, since the muscles of the two sides now act similarly. 
‘Thus orientation of the organism is effected. “The same explanation, which is modi - 
fied from one of Loeb (’93, p. 86), will account, mutatis mutandis, for positive pho- 
totaxis” (Davenport, ’97, p. 209)." 


Both this axiomatic view that the direction of movement is 
always precisely determined by external agents, and the theory that 
this determination is due to direct local action of the agent in ques- 
tion, have begun to give way before a careful examination of the 


4In making the above quotations, there is no intention of representing Davenport as a sponsor or 
defender of the views set forth. In his valuable general works, Davenport has summed up in an 
unsurpassed way the general views prevailing at the time, and our quotations are given merely as such 
summaries. 


Modipfiability in Behavior 437 


facts as to just what organisms do. A thorough acquaintance with 
the behavior of Paramecium, or of any other typical free-moving 
lower animal, renders the above quoted axiom quite unten: ble. 
If the animal is placed in such a position that the externzl condi- 
tions are uniform in all directions, so that there is nothing in them 
to guide it, it does not remain quiet. Paramecium when plzced 
in such a situation simply goes straight ahead, and the sz me is 
true of most other lower animals. In other words, the differentia- 
tions of the animal’s body are sufficient to determine the direction 
of locomotion, provided there is nothing else to do so, and often 
this factor prevails even when there are external factors which 
work decidedly against it. An animal often goes in a certzin 
direction merely because there is nothing to prevent its forward 
movement. ‘The animal is a going machine, and need not be 
prodded up by a present outside stimulus in order to move; it 
carries within itself both the impulse to move, and factors deter- 
mining the direction and character of the movement. ‘The move- 
ment of such a creature as Paramecium is indeed defined in mzny 
ways, even in a uniform environment. 

If such relations are evident in examining even so low an animal | 
as the infusorian, it will not be surprising if we find them to hold 
in even higher degree of so complex an animal as the earthworm. 

Of course external agents frequently do determine the direction 
of movement. But thorough study shows that their action 1s 
rarely of the simple and direct character set forth in the summz riz- 
ing statement above quoted; it is usually, on the contrary, very 
indirect and complex. For the earthworm, this was perhzps first 
made evident by the work of Miss Smith (02). “This work showed 
that the directive action of light, heat, chemicals, etc., on the e: rth- 
worm Allolobophora is far from being of the precise, unequivocal 
character that the local action theory would require. She found 
that in place of orienting itself by bending steadily to one side under 
the unilateral action of a stimulus, the earthworm frequently 
alternated movements in various directions, till finally one direction 
was selected for further progress. ‘This direction was by no means 
usually one of precise orientation. This result was confirmed by 
Holmes (’05), who finds that “random movements” play a large 


438 | H. 8. ‘fennings 


part in determining the direction of locomotion in the earthworm, 
as well as in various other lower organisms, under the action of 
light and other stimuli. Holmes’ observations inclined him even 
to doubt that the earthworm has any localizing power in the nega- 
tive reactions to light, in the sense that it is anywise more likely to 
turn first directly away from the lighted side. 

In essential agreement with such results are the statistics of 
Parker and Arkin (01), who found that in reacting to one-sided 
illumination the earthworm usually (in 65.5 per cent of all cases) 
merely starts ahead, and that when it turns, it may turn either 
toward or away from the source of light, the number of turns away 
from the light being only 26 per cent greater than the number of 
turns toward the light. The large overplus of movements that are 
not as required by the theory of direct local action of course require 
explanation. Harper (’05) showed clearly that under strong light 
the earthworm Perichzta may turn directly away from the light 
and he brought out some of the factors on which depend the other 
movements, that are not determined in direction by the impact of 
the light rays. It is a further inquiry into the determining factors 
of movements that do not depend directly on the localization of the 
stimulus, that concerns us in the present paper. 

In the paper of Holmes (’05) the movements that are not in 
relation with the direction of the source of stimulation are charac- 
terized simply as “random.” But, of course, the movements are 
random only with relation to the external stimulus considered; it 1s 
evident that they must be determined in some way. It cannot 
be admitted in a scientific treatment of the matter that any move- 
ments are random in the sense of undetermined. If we leave the 
matter here, calling the movements merely random, we leave our 
treatment open to the charge of inexactness, and of the use of non- 
scientific concepts; the movements are left without a determining 
cause. Why does the animal turn sometimes to the right, some- 
times to the left, sometimes go straight ahead, or perform other 
activities, even though the external conditions remain the same? 

We are forced then to take up the fundamental question, What 
factors determine the direction of movement of the given organism 
at a given moment? This question the local action theory of 


Modipability in Behavior 439 


tropisms tried to settle in an axiomatic, a priori way, taking into 
consideration only external factors. What is required is that the 
question shall be taken up as a problem for objective investigation, 
attempting by analytical experimentation to differentiate the 
various factors involved, both internal and external. ‘The paper 
of Harper made incidentally a first advance in this direction; the 
present paper endeavors to deal with the problem more explicitly, 
and to advance the analysis farther. 

As will appear, the problem is really an extraordinarily complex 
one, instead of being of the simple character assumed by the local 
action theory, so fie the present essay can give only a general out- 
line of the various classes of factors involv ae taking into considera- 
tion situations where the external conditions are made as simple 
as possible. 


EXPERIMENTS 


The common earthworm, Lumbricus terrestris L. (L. herculeus 
Say.), was used in the present experiments. ‘The method of work 
was the simplest, not to say crudest, possible. “The earthworm 
was placed on a large sheet of wet filter paper in a dimly lighted 
room, pains being taken to keep it moist; then the simplest possi- 
ble stimuli, precisely localized, were applied to it. ‘The stimulus 
chiefly used was a touch with the tip of a finely pointed glass rod. 
Precisely localized chemical and thermal stimuli were also used 
but essentially the same result were obtained with all. It is evident 
that in these experiments (as in those of other recent authors) the 
animals are under most unnatural conditions, and for certain pur- 
poses this would of course be a serious disadvantage. For our 
present object, however, this is not the case, since these circum- 
stances must be taken into consideration as well as all others, in a 
general investigation of the factors that determine the direction 
of movement. 

When a localized stimulus is applied to one side of the earth- 
worm, does it respond by a simple contraction or relaxation of the 
muscles of the side of the region affected, thus turning the head 
from or toward the stimulated side? 

Analysis of the results of a large number of experiments shows 


440 H. S. “fennings 


that when a local stimulus is applied to one side of the anterior 
part of the earthworm—-say to the sixth metamere—any one of the 
following varied methods of action may result: 

1. There may be merely a slight swelling of the region stimu- 
lated. 

2. The worm may turn the anterior end away from the side 
stimulated. 

3. It may turn the head toward the side stimulated. 
4. It may creep backward. 


5. It may creep forward. 

6. It may creep first backward, then forward. 

7. The head may be merely retracted. 

8. The animal may make a sudden right-about-face, interchang- 
ing the position of anterior and posterior ends, in the way to be 
described later. 

g. The anterior fourth of the body of the worm may be raised 
in the air and waved wildly about. 

From this list it is clear that the reaction must depend on many 
factors, since we can distinguish at least nine different methods of 
behavior which may follow on the stimulus. Of course the dif- 
ferent actions performed in different cases are not matters of chance 
but are determined in some way. What are the determining 
factors? 

So far as I have been able to determine these factors by ana- 
lytical experimentation, we can distinguish at least the following 
groups: 

I. External Factors 


1. Intensity of the Stimulation. This is, of course, universally 
recognized as a determining factor in behavior. Other conditions 
remzining the same, in the earthworm a weak stimulus may induce 
a mere swelling of the body at the region stimulated (this varying in 
extent in different cases); a stronger stimulus may cause a slight 
turning of the head away from (or sometimes toward) the point 
stimulated; a still stronger one a retraction of the head, without 
turning; a very strong one an immediate rapid crawling backward. 
In connection with variations in the other conditions an almost 


Modipfiability in Behavior 441 


infinite variety of combinations of reactions may be produced by 
varying the intensity of the stimulus. 

2. Localization of the Stimulus. The different effects of dif- 
ferently localized stimuli are of course universally recognized; 
this is indeed one of the corner stones of the orthodox tropism 
theory. Certain of the relations of reaction to localization of the 
stimulus in the earthworm are of interest. 

Stimulation first of one side, then of the other, may induce in 
the two cases movement in opposite directions. But this is by no 
means always the result; the direction of turning depends, as we 
shall see later, on a multiplicity of factors. Other conditions 
remaining the same, a stimulus of a certain intensity on the right 
side of one of the anterior twenty metameres usually causes a turn 
to the left; the same stimulus on the left causes a turn to the right. 
It is thus clear that there exists the possibility of an immediate 
relation of the direction of the movement to the side stimulated, as 
Harper showed. ‘This is the factor which the local action theory 
of tropisms takes into consideration, though there seems to be no 
evidence that the turning is due merely to direct local action. 
But in any case it is only one factor out of many. In the region 
back of about the anterior third of the body, this factor plays little 
or no part, as we shall immediately see. 

Comparing stimuli applied to anterior and posterior parts of 
the body, very different results are reached. A rather strong 
stimulus on the dorsal surface of the anterior third of the body 
(to just behind the clitellum) usually causes the animal to creep 
backward. This backward movement is usually soon followed 
by a forward one. A similar stimulus on the posterior half of the 
body usually causes the worm to creep rapidly forward. ‘Between 
these two regions there is an area comprising five to ten metameres 
in which a strong dorsal stimulus causes an immediate stretching 
of the entire worm, the anterior region starting forward, the pos- 
terior region backward. ‘The anterior movement soon prevails, 
as a rule, so that the worm crawls forward. ‘The final result then 
is in all these cases a movement forward; this is preceded in the 
case of anterior stimulation by a movement backward, in inter- 
mediate stimulation by a movement in both directions, while 


442 1. 8. ‘fennings 


only in the case of posterior stimulation does the forward movement 
take place directly. Under other conditions, to be mentioned 
presently, the worm may crawl steadily backward. Further, 
which of these three results we get in a given case depends largely 
on the intensity of the stimulus and on other factors not yet consid- 
ered, as well as upon the localization of the stimulus. 

Lateral stimulation on the anterior one-fourth or one-third of the 
body may, as we have already seen, cause a turning away from the 
side stimulated. Often, however, it causes merely a backward 
movement; sometimes a forward one (for the conditions on which 
these depend, see later). But lateral stimulation of the posterior 
two-thirds of the body never causes turning away from the side stimu- 
lated, so far as I have observed. In this region a strong lateral 
stimulus causes merely a rapid movement forward. ‘The adap- 
tive relation of these facts to the movements of the animal under 
natural conditions is evident. An attack in the anterior region is 
best escaped by moving backward; in the posterior region by mov- 
ing forward. Forward movement is more rapid than backward 
movement, so a stimulus at the middle of the body, or even a little 
in front of it, is more quickly escaped by a forward than by a 
backward movement. Lateral attacks in the anterior region 
may be escaped through a sidewise turn, but lateral stimulation in 
the posterior region will be much more quickly avoided by a rush 
forward. ‘These adaptive relations of course do not show us 
why the earthworm moves as it does, but their existence is inter- 
esting, and it is possible that in the long run they may have some- 
thing to do with determining what movements shall occur. 

The fact that strong stimuli on the lateral surface of the posterior 
half of the body do not tend to cause turning to one side is one that 
needs to be kept in mind in practical experimentation. Thus, 
Parker and Arkin (or) found that light directed against the side 
of the anterior end of the worm causes turning away, but directed 
against the posterior part it does not cause the turning. ‘This, of 
course, does not in itself indicate that the posterior half is less 
sensitive to light than the anterior half (though this may be prob- 
able on other grounds), for even if the light acted most power- 
fully on the posterior half of the animal, it would not cause a 


Modipfiability in Behavior 443 


bending, but merely a movement forward. The argument for 
less sensitiveness in the posterior half from the results of Parker 
and Arkin rests on the assumption of the correctness of the local 
action theory of tropisms for light reactions; this assumption can 
hardly be said to have shown itself correct. 

A very powerful stimulus, constituting a severe injury, usually 
causes the part anterior to the injury to crawl rapidly straight 
ahead, while the part behind the injury squirms about violently. 
Close examination of this “squirming”’ shows that it is exactly the 
movement that is required, in the natural conditions under which 
the worm lives, for urging the body forward with great rapidity. 
The worm is, of course, usually in a narrow burrow. Under the 
circumstances the “squirming” throws first one side of the body, 
then the other, against the sides of the burrow, while at the same 
time the setz are extended and moved in such a way as to exercise 
a powerful leverage against the sides. In this way, by repeated 
successive impulses from each side, the worm shoots forward 
through its burrow, tending thus to escape from the injurious 
agent. The anterior end is not turned about from side to side, but 
is held more nearly straight, so that it does not interfere with the 
movement induced by the powerful impulses coming from behind. 
At the same time it aids the flight by creeping as rapidly forward 
as possible. 

In a well known paper Norman (97) described the fact that 
when the strong stimulusis a cut, actually dividing the worm into 
two halves, the parts anterior and posterior to the cut behave 
respectively in the two ways above described, and brought this 
into relation with the question of the existence of pain in the 
lower animals. It was argued that, since only the part behind 
the cut shows thé squirming reaction, while the part in front does 
not, it cannot be held that there is any indication of pain in the 
worm, since it must be supposed that the anterior region is at 
least as sensitive as the posterior one. But since the behavior of 
both anterior and posterior pieces is of precisely the character that 
would under natural conditions most assist the worm to escape | 
from the stimulating agent, it may be questioned whether the dif- 
ference between the two has any bearing on the probable exist- 


444 Hi. S. fennings 


ence of pain. There would seem to be no more and no less 
reason for supposing the strong lateral movements which urge the 
animal forward in the “squirming” to be accompanied by pain, 
than the rapid forward creeping of the anterior part. 

The remaining more important determining factors in the move- 
ments of the earthworm must be classified as 


IT. Internal Factors 


These are extremely varied, and a complete classification is 
impossible at present. Some of the chief ones are the following: 

3. The reaction to a given stimulus depends partly on what the 
animal has done, and on its position, just before receiving the 
stimulus. 

‘This factor shows itself as follows: “Che earthworm makes with 
its anterior end side to side movements in creeping, turning first to 
the right, then to the left. If now we stimulate it Just after it has 
turned its head far to the right, it at once as a rule jerks its head 
to the left. [his occurs whether the stimulus is applied to the 
right side, to the left side, or to the dorsal surface, so that the 
direction of turning becomes independent of the localization of the 
stimulus. If the animal is lying bent to the right, it is most likely 
to bend to the left when stimulated, without regard to the precise 
point of stimulation. 

4. The reaction to a given stimulus depends partly on a general 
tendency of the animal to move in a certain way, namely, forward 
rather than backward. ‘This is seen in the different results of 
localized stimuli on the posterior and anterior parts of the body. 
In the former case the animal moves away from the source of 
stimulus (forward), and continues to do so. In the latter case it 
first moves away from the source of stimulation (backward), then 


1In describing movements that depend upon such a multiplicity of factors as do those of the earth- 
worm, it is difficult to treat of any one factor separately without making the effects of that factor 
appear more absolute and independent of the presence of others than the experimental results show to 
be the case. Thus, while as a rule the results are as given above, different results may be reached 
when other factors than those considered in this paragraph become the determining ones. This 
remark applies to the discussion of each of the factors taken up separately in the text. 


Modipfiability in Behavior 44.5 


this movement is as a rule supplanted by movement toward the 
source of stimulation (forward). 

5. The reaction to a given stimulus depends partly on the direc- 
tion in which the animal is crawling at the moment when it is 
stimulated. In a quiet worm, posterior stimulation causes move- 
ment forward, anterior stimulation movement backward. The same 
results are usually observed when the animal is very quietly crawl- 
ing forward or backward. But at times, when creeping forward 
it is found that stimuli on any part of the body—even at the 
anterior tip—merely cause the worm to hasten the forward move- 
ment. In the same way, a worm that is creeping backward may 
persist in this movement, and even hasten it, in spite of repeated 
strong stimulation at the posterior end. ‘The physiological state 
seems to have taken a sort of set, causing the worm to obsti- 
nately persist in following the direction in which it has started. 
Sometimes, when the worm is in a similar condition, a stimulus at 
the advancing end of the animal (anterior or posterior) causes a 
cessation of the movement for a few seconds, then the worm starts 
again in the direction it was pursuing. Stimulation causes it to 
suspend operations for a time, but it is not to be easily turned aside 
from the course it is following, and it soon resumes this course. 

6. The reaction to a given stimulus depends partly on previous 
stimuli received. I have not by any means worked out in detail 
all the manifestations of this principle; they are very numerous. 
A worm which has not received marked stimuli for some time, and 
is at complete rest, may not react at all to a slight stimulation. 
Two or three similar stimulations, however, rouse it up, and now 
it reacts readily. After more stimuli, or more intense ones, its 
reaction to any given stimulus changes in character and increases in 
intensity. We can distinguish from this point of view a series of 
different physiological states, each manifesting itself by a different 
reaction. A partial list of these, with the characteristic behavior, 
is as follows: 

(a) The state of rest, in which the worm does not react readily 
to slight stimuli, such as a touch with the tip of a glass rod. 

(b) A state of moderate activity, in which a touch at the anterior 
end causes movement backward; at the posterior end movement 


446 TS. fennings 


forward, while lateral stimuli (in the anterior region) cause turning 
away from the side stimulated. 

(c) A state of excitement, after repeated stimuli, in which the 
animal persists in the direction of movement once begun, merely 
stopping for a few seconds when stimulated at the end which 1s 
advancing. 

(d) A state of greater excitement, in which stimuli merely cause 
the animal to hasten its movements in the direction in which it has 
started, without regard to the localization of the stimulus. 

(c) A state of still greater excitement, after long-continued and 
intense stimulation. Now the worm responds to a stimulus at the 
anterior end, that would in a resting worm cause only a compara- 
tively slight reaction, by a rapid “right-about-face.” ‘The body 
is suddenly doubled at its middle, so that the anterior and pos- 
terior halves become parallel, with the two ends pointing in the 
same direction, then the posterior half 1s quickly whipped about, 
so that the whole worm is again straight, but is facing the opposite 
direction from that in which it was pointed before the reaction. 
This peculiar reaction takes place with such rapidity that one can 
distinguish the way in which it occurs only after many repetitions. 
After this right-about-face the worm usually crawls rapidly in the 
new direction. 

In the natural condition, within the burrow, this reaction would, 
of course, instantaneously direct the animal downward, if attacked 
as it creeps toward the surface. 

(f) A state of still more intense excitement, after repeated 
strong stimulation that is of such a character as to actually injure . 
the tissues. “he worm now responds to a repetition of the stimu- 
lus (and often when the new stimulus is only slight) by lifting the 
anterior fourth of the body into a vertical position, and waving it 
about in a frantic manner. ‘This behavior is usually alternated 
with the right-about-face reactions, and with persistent rapid 
crawling forward or backward. ‘The spectator is involuntarily 
inclined to feel that the animal is tormented, and that continuation 
of the experiment is cruelty; this may, of course, be due only to the 
peculiar constitution of the spectator, and not to that of the worm. 

It is beyond question that many other physiological states could 


Modipability in Behavior 447 


J 


— 


be distinguished, each with its characteristic method of action. [ 
have considered only those appearing under relatively simple con- 
ditions of stimulation, and such as can be formulated in a more or 
less definite way. Many other variations in reaction that may 
easily be observed I have not taken up above, because recount- 
ing them would add merely a heap of details, the interrela- 
tions of which are not clear. I have further omitted all physiologi- 
cal states resulting from varied states of metabolism, and have not 
attempted to study possible lasting modifications of physiological 
state (“habits,” etc.), but have merely dealt with the movements 
from moment to moment. 

Yet with even this limitation of the field, it is clear that the 
cause for movement in a given direction, or of a certain character, 
is in the earthworm not simple, but excessively complex. The 
present external stimulus is only one of the numerous variable 
factors involved. ‘The movement at a given time demonstrably 
depends, not alone on present external conditions, but also on 
former external conditions, former actions of the organisms, and 
present internal physiological conditions that are determined in 
many different ways. ‘The direction of movement of one of these 
organisms cannot be represented as a simple function of the direc- 
tion of impact of some external force, but is the complex resultant 
of many different factors. 


GENERAL 


Cause of Change of Reaction Under Uniform External Stimulation. 


In the present and in previous papers I have shown that 
even while the external conditions remain the same, the reaction 
of the organism changes. Such a statement appears to be looked 
upon by some as leaving the movements without determining fac- 
tors, and as therefore leading to vitalism and mysticism. But 
such an idea results only from a failure to realize what animals are. 
They are not static structures, but are bundles of processes, and 
it is this that gives us a key to the understanding of changes in 
behavior even under uniform external conditions. In every animal 


448 IS) feuniaes 
processes of the most varied character are occurring: the taking 
of oxygen and other substances, digestion, assimilation, dissimila- 
tion, secretion, excretion, usually some form of circulation, ete. 
Whenever we think of an animal—an earthworm or an infuso- 
rian—we need, in order to understand its behavior, to think of it 
as a little engine of intense activity. [he movements of the 
organism we know to be the results of the production of energy in 
these internal processes. We know further that these processes 
depend for their normal course upon each other, and upon the 
environment, and that this is as true of the movements as of the 
other processes. Disturbance of the internal processes we know 
to result in changes in the movements. In former papers (05, 
’o5a) I have given many cases of this and of the observed 
dependence of behavior on the relation of external conditions to 
these processes. 

What then will happen if some external condition acts upon the 
organism in such a way as to modify one of these processes ! ? Sup- 
pose, for example,that pressure acts in a given case in such a way as 
to interfere with the circulation—of the protoplasm in Paramecium 
or of the blood in the earthworm. ‘The animal will perhaps first 
respond directly to the pressure as a primary stimulus, giving the 
reaction A. ‘The pressure continuing uniformly soon impedes 
the circulation. We know that such interference with an internal 
process is in itself a cause of reaction; the animal,therefore, now 
responds to this interference, giving the reaction B. But circula- 
tion cannot be long impeded without interfering with respiration, 
and this again is a cause of reaction, giving us perhaps the new 
response C. The external conditions meanwhile remain uniform, 
but, as we see, the internal condition changes from one state to 
another. The interference with respiration is bound ere long to 
induce changes in assimilation; these necessarily entrain changes 
in dissimiliation, and these changes in excretion. Excretion is 
likewise more directly impeded by the stoppage of the circulation. 
As a result thus of a uniform external condition modifying prima- 
rily but a single bodily process, the internal state of the animal 
passes from one change to another, since the internal processes 
are bound up in mutual interdependence. We know that the 


Modifiability in Behavior 44.9 


bodily movements are the result of these internal processes, so 
that as one after another of these is blocked, it appears not merely 
natural but inevitable that repeated alterations of movement should 
result; we Semtnewehicsson reactions 4,5, Gus. 2-2. ~ Lhisvasil 
have shown in previous papers, 1s exactly what occurs in many 
lower organisms. The action of a uniform external stimulus that 
affects any of the life processes 1s necessarily cumulative, since a 
change in one process is bound to result in changes in the others. 
As a result of the cumulative internal changes we get repeated 
changes in movement, even though the external condition remains 
the same. There is no difficulty, therefore, in finding determining 
factors for such changes of behavior; they are induced by preced- 
ing changes in precisely what we know on other grounds to be the 
source of movement—the internal physiological processes. 

There is thus no reason when we consider the fact that organ- 
isms are bundles of interdependent processes, for supposing that 
they should always behave in the same way under the same 
external conditions. Persistence inthe same movement as one after 
another of the internal processes is disturbed would itself be a 
puzzle, requiring very precise internal compensatory regulations. 
Change of movement is what might be expected, and is what we 
commonly find. ‘There is, of course, as much reason for finding 
such changes in the lower organisms as in higher ones, and this 
again corresponds with the results of experimentation. 


“Method of Trial and Error” 


In a previous paper (04a) I spoke of the reaction method in 
which the organism varies its movements under a given stimulus 
as the “method of trial and error,” in order to bring out its simi- 
larity to the behavior for which that expression is commonly used 
in higher animals. ‘The essential point which I intended to bring 
out by using this term in connection with the way in which these 
animals move toward or away from certain agents was the follow- 
ing: the organism performs varied movements, some features of 


450 H. S. fennings 


which are not determined! by the localization of the stimulus, but 
by other factors; it then continues those movements which bring 
it into or toward a certain condition, this condition being usually 
either a greater or a less action of the stimulating agents, as 
the case may be.? Holmes (’05, p.111) objects to the use of this 
expression for some of the cases for which I employed it, as, for 
example, the reaction to light in Euglena, while proposing to 
reserve it for such cases as the earthworm. His reason for this is 
that ‘‘ Euglena does not react by a number of indiscriminate move- 
ments until the right one is accidentally hit upon, but by a direct 
reflex, whose effect is to bring the organism more nearly parallel 
to the direction of the rays.’ He continues, “The phototaxis of 
Euglena is not so manifestly the outcome of the trial and error 
method as that of the earthworm. In the latter case light does 
not cause directly a movement which makes for orientation. ‘The 
direct response may or may not have that effect. “The successful 
response is accidentally hit upon.” He further suggests that the 
expression “trial and error’ be “limited to those cases in which 
the adapted movements may be regarded as chance successes.”’ 

I have, of course, no desire to enter into an academic discussion 
as to the proper definition to be given to the hackneyed expression 
“trial and error,’ and I should be quite willing to drop that 
expression, not only for Euglena but for the earthworm and other 
lower organisms; it has perhaps gained in its history implications 
of various sorts which find no corresponding factors in lower 
organisms. But what does seem to me worth while is to try to make 
clear the points on which the various methods of behavior that I 
classified together under this name actually agree, these being the 
points which impelled me to class them together. It appears to 

1The word determine is of use in experimental work only when it expresses a concrete experimental 
result, namely, that when one factor varies, another (the determined one) varies in a corresponding 
way. The factor A (e. g., localization of the stimulus) is said to determine the factor B (e. g., direc- 
tion of movement) when a change in A involves experimentally a change in B. If B remains the same 
even when 4 is changed or disappears (as in the case of the movements referred to above), then B is 
not determined by A. 

?The essential feature of this type of behavior could be expressed more generally, so as to 
include other than directed reactions, as follows: the organism performs varied movements, some of 


which do not tend to produce the result that is finally brought about by the behavior as a whole; of 
these varied actions, those are followed up which do tend to produce this final result. 


Modipfiability in Behavior 451 


the writer most unfortunate to attempt to make a distinction on 
the basis of the emphasis of such terms as “indiscriminate,” “ac- 
cidentally,” “chance.” The “random” movements of the earth- 
worm are of course no more a matter of chance than are the varied 
movements of Euglena. ‘They are merely determined in a dif- 
ferent way, and in very complex ways, as I have attempted to 
show in the present paper. Where the two agree is in the fact that 
they are partly determined by some other factor than the localiza- 
tion of the source of stimulus, and that they do not all tend to pro- 
duce the finally resulting orientation. In both cases, if the stimu- 
lus were differently localized the first movements would still be the 
same. ‘This is as true of the movements of Euglena as it is of 
those of the earthworm, in becoming oriented to light. Euglena 
when stimulated as a result of light coming from one side swerves 
more than usual toward its own dorsal side. “This movement is 
at first just as likely to take the anterior end away from the source 
of light as toward it (see Jennings, ’04). In precisely the same 
way, according to Holmes’ account, the earthworm in reacting by 
the “random movement”’ method is as likely to turn its anterior 
end in one direction as in the other. But in both these organ- 
isms, after swerving for a certain distance in one direction, the 
creature swerves in the opposite direction. In Euglena this is due 
to the fact that the animal revolves on its long axis, so that the 
dorsal side, toward which it is swerving, soon reverses its position; 
in the earthworm it is due (partly at least) to the fact that a previous 
turn to the left itself serves to induce a succeeding turn to the right. 
The movement is fully determined throughout, in both cases equally. 
Now in both the organisms, since there is successive swerving 
in opposite directions, when the light is coming from one side 
one of the movements is bound to take the anterior end in general 
toward the source of light, the other away from it, and there is no 
more accident about this in one case than in the other. In both 
organisms, of these movements in different directions, “only those 
are followed up which bring the animal out of the undesirable 
situation” (Holmes).!| In Euglena “light does not cause directly 


1That is to say, in both cases the following up of a certain motion 7s determined by the localization 
of the stimulus. 


452 H. S. fennings 


a movement that makes for orientation,’ any more than it does 
in the earthworm. Part of the movement is directly away from 
the position of orientation (see Fig. 23, Jennings, ’o4), and this 
very phase of the swerving is more pronounced when the animal 
is becoming oriented to light than in the usual spiral path when 
it is not reacting at all. Movever, this phase of the movement is 
a necessary one in bringing about the reaction, if the analysis I 
have given in the paper just cited 1s correct. The stimulus of 
light causes pronounced movements both away from and toward 
the source of stimulation, and the organism follows up more 
decidedly those which lead toward the optimum condition, just as 
happens in the earthworm. 

Thus, while it appears to me immaterial whether we choose to 
call either or neither of these cases reaction by “trial and error,” 
the essential point lies in the fact that in these as in many other 
organisms stimulation causes varied movements, which do not all 
lead toward the condition finally attained, and that those move- 
ments which do lead toward this final condition (the “optimum’’) 
are followed up more decidedly than the others. The behavior 
may perhaps be most accurately characterized as “selection from 
among the conditions produced by varied movements.” In general 
we find that many organisms are so constituted that internal 
conditions (permanent or temporary) will produce under stimu- 
lation movements that are varied in precisely such a way as to sub- 
ject the creature to as varied environmental conditions as possible, 
and thus give it an opportunity to select what is nearest the opti- 
mum.! Every one of these movements 1s, of course, as absolutely 
determined as is the most orthodox tropism, only the determining 
factor is not the localization of the stimulus (or other external 
factor) alone. 

Certain recent writers have seemed to imply that there is a con- 
trast between the “‘trial and error’? method, and behavior that is 
definitely determined by structural and other internal conditions. 
It needs to be emphasized, perhaps, that the behavior which I and 
others have characterized by this phrase is very precisely deter- 
mined by structural and other internal conditions; indeed, its dis- 


1As to how this selection occurs, see the author’s paper on the “Method of Regulation in 
Behavior” (05a). 


Modipfrability in Behavior 453 


tinguishing feature is the fact that it is thus determined by such 
conditions, rather than exclusively by the external conditions. It 
is, of course, very true, as Harper (’05) remarks, that definitely 
localized reaction methods are developed as we rise higher in the 
scale, yet it appears to be equally true that if we mean by “trial 
and error’ the performance of varied movements, subjecting the 
organism to varied conditions, certain of which are selected, then 
this also becomes more highly developed and more used by organ- 
isms as we ascend the scale. We must not forget that this 
expression “trial and error” was originally based on the behavior 
of such highly developed organisms as the cat, dog and monkey; 
and doubtless there is no organism which uses this method to any 
such extent as does man. Whenever the external conditions do 
not furnish a precise determining factor for the movements, yet 
some sort of reaction is required, any organism is forced to have 
recourse to this style of behavior, performing varied movements 
till a condition is reached that relieves the organism of the neces- 
sity of continuing these movements. In its highest form we call 
this experimentation. 

In his recent Experimentelle Biologie, v. Uexkiill ('05) is unable 
to understand how we gain anything by characterizing certain 
behavior as “trial and error” (p. 128). Whether we gain Het by 
a certain procedure depends largely on what the problems are in 
which we are interested, and we find that v. Uexkill limits the 
problems of biology in so extraordinary a way that he could not 
possibly be expected to see any value in such a procedure. For 
him, “There is thus for animal biology only one point of view— 
to understand the purposive structure of every animal on.the basis 
of its reflex arcs” (J.c., p.g6). He is, moreover, convinced a priori 
that we can never understand the purposiveness of organisms 
froma causal standpoint; “ Die beiden Betrachtungsarten [Ursach- 
tlichkeit und Zweckmissigkeit] auf einander zuriickzufiihren 1s 
unmoglich” (p. 129). When we add that v. Uexkiill holds that 
any action of organisms may equally be characterized as trial and 
error,’ it is evidently not surprising that he can make nothing of 


1We have perhaps here further evidence that this expression is one that lends itself too readily 
P P P 


to misinterpretation to make it the best characterization for the behavior in question. 


4.54 H. 8S. “fennings 


the characterization of certain kinds of behavior as trial and error. 
On the other hand, some of us are interested in the question as to 
how it happens that organisms do those things that aid them in 
carrying on their physiological processes and thus keep them in 
existence—a matter which does not fall within the field of biol- 
ogy at all, as limited by v. Uexkill. To such as are interested in 
this problem it may very well appear that we have really made a 
certain advance when we recognize the fact that organisms do not 
in response to stimulation always perform at once a directly regu- 
latory action; do not react merely by a stereotyped reflex, as has 
often been represented; but that on the contrary they perform 
varied movements, some of which are not directly regulatory, but 
which do subject them to varied conditions, certain of which con- 
ditions are selected, through cessation of the varied movements, 
thus resulting in regulation. If we are further able to find out the 
determining factors for setting in operation these varied move- 
ments, and for their cessation when a favorable condition is reached 
it will naturally appear to those interested in the problem sketched, 
that we have made a further advance. Recognition of a physio- 
logical law that results finally in the persistence of only the one 
movement that is directly regulatory, so that the animal later 
reacts by a stereotyped regulatory reflex, and no longer by the 
varied movements (“trial and error’’), will seem a further advance 
to those interested in precisely the problem of how such definite 
regulatory reactions happen to exist. It is not necessary to hold 
that these determining factors and laws have been completely 
unveiled (see Jennings ’o5a, for a tentative statement of them), in 
order to recognize the importance and interest of the line of work 
that is concerned with their investigation. On the other hand, 
those for whom regulation is a priori an unsolvable problem, and 
who limit the field of biology to understanding the purposive 
structure of animals on the basis of their reflex arcs, will of course 
find these matters outside the field of their interest. But those 
whose interests are thus narrowly bounded are certainly in the 
minority; the writer does not happen to know of any biologist 
aside from Dr. v. Uexkiill that holds to such limitations. 


zyr 


Modifiability in Behavior 455 
Pith ReARURE CEhED 


Davenport, C. B., ’97.—Experimental Morphology, vol. 1, 280 pp. 
Davenport, C. B., and Perkins, HELEN, ’97.—A Contribution to the Study of 
Geotaxis in the Higher Animals. Journ. Physiol., xxii, gg—1Io. 
Harper, E. H., ’05.—Reactions to Light and Mechanical Stimuli in the Earth- 
worm, Pericheta bermudensis (Beddard). Biol. Bul., x, 17-34. 
Hoimes, S. J., °05.—The Selection of Random Movements as a Factor in Photo- 
taxis. Journ. Comp. Neurol. and Psychol., xv, 98-112. 
Jennincs, H. S., ’04.—Reactions to Light in Ciliates and Flagellates. Contr. 
to the Study of the Behavior of the Lower Organisms. Carnegie 
Institution of Washington, Publ. 16, pp. 29-71. 
?04a.—The Method of Trial and Error in the Behavior of Lower 
Organisms. Jbid., pp. 235-252. 
’05.—Modifiability in Behavior. I. Behavior of Sea Anemones. Journ. 
Exp. Zool., 11, 447-472. 
’o5a.—The Method of Regulation in Behavior and in Other Fields. 
Tbid., 11, 448-494. 
Norman, W. W., ’97.—Diirfen wir aus den Reactionen niederer Thiere auf das 
Vorhandensein von Schmerzempfindungen schliessen. Arch. f. d. 
ges. Physiol. Ixvii, 137-140. 
Parker, G. H. and Arkin, L., ’o1.—The Directive Influence of Light on the 
Earthworm Allolobophora feetida (Sav.). Amer. Journ. Physiol., 
iv, I51-157. 
SmirH, AMELIA C., ’02.—The Influence of Temperature, Odors, Light and Con- 
tact on the Movements of the Earthworm. Amer. Journ. Physiol., 
vi, 459-486. 
UexkuLL, J. v.,’05.—Leitfaden in das Studium der experimentellen Biologie 
der Wassertiere. Wiesbaden. 130 pp. 


a 


x ie : id 
Pee | 


* 
ta 
e 


HE SPEYSIOLOGY OF REGENERATION 


BY 
T. H. MORGAN 


With Seven Ficures 


Lest the title mislead someone expecting to find in_ the follow- 
ing pages an account of the processes of assimilation and of resp1- 
ration, that presumably take place during regeneration, I ought to 
state that I shall deal only with the physiology of the growth pro- 
cess as shown in the regeneration of a new part. Morphogenesis 
does not express my meaning in all respects, for | am not con- 
cerned so much with changes in form as with the rate of growth and 
of differentiation. If I have taken a liberty in using the term phy- 
siology to cover these kinds of changes, my excuse must be that we 
are dealing with phenomena that lie on the borderland, where 
physiology and morphology overlap, and appear to merge into each 
other. 

It is generally, if tacitly, assumed that when undifferentiated 
cells are supplied with food materials growth must follow, but I 
shall try to show, on the contrary, that whether or not growth takes 
place depends not so much on the available food sueply as ona 
formative influence that regulates both the kind and the amount 
of growth. The nature of this formative influence is the most dif- 
ficult and problematical factor with which we shall have to deal. 


RATE OF REGENERATION IN STARVED AND WELL-FED 
SALAMANDERS 


If the rate of regeneration is in any way connected with the food 
supply, the fact ought to become at once apparent by comparing 
the process in well-fed and in starved individuals. I have carried 
out such an experiment with the salamander, Diemyctylus virides- 
cens. The results show that we must be careful to distinguish, 
in the use of the word rate, between a simple increase in size, and 


Tue JourNat or ExpPeRIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. Il, NO. 4. 


458 T. H. Morgan 


the rapidity of the process of differentiation (or development in 
the narrower sense) of the new part. 

Zeleny' has recently shown that the increase in size of regener- 
ating arms of the brittle-star and of the legs of the crayfish is deter- 
mined, in part, by the number of the appendages removed. ‘The 
more parts removed, the faster each regenerates. Zeleny discusses 
amongst other factors, the possible relation of this result to the 
food supply, and points out that the larger the number of append- 
ages removed the greater will be the temporary surplus of food, 
for the amount necessary to nourish the entire leg may be greater 
than that used at first in the growth of the small new part. Whilst 
pointing out the possibility of this interpretation Zeleny carefully 
refrains from committing himself to it as the only explanation of 
his results. 

It seemed to me that this question might be tested in the follow- 
ing way. Ifthe rate 1s determined by the food supply, then if two 
sets of individuals are selected, and one set starved and the other 
fed, the latter should be in a better condition for regeneration than 
the former. If the same number of parts 1s removed in each, the 
well-fed individuals should regenerate faster than the starved ones. 

If fewer parts are removed from the well-fed individuals than 
from the starved ones, nevertheless the well-fed individuals should 
regenerate faster, for, the greater amount of food given ought to 
much more than outweigh the surplus in the starved ones due to 
the absence of more parts. 

‘The experiment was carried out with salamanders collected in the 
autumn. ‘They were in excellent condition when caught, although 
not so large as they soon became when fed on pieces of beef 
The individuals were kept for several weeks, without much food, 
before the experiments began. In some individuals one leg was 
removed, in others two, in others three, and in still others one, two, 
or three legs and also the tail, which was cut off near the base. 
Duplicate sets were prepared, each containing several of these dif- 
ferent kinds of individuals. One lot was kept without food and 
the other fed about every other day on small pieces of raw beef. 


1Zeleny, C. A Study of the Regeneration of the Arms of the Brittle-Star. Biol. Bull., vi, 1903. 


Compensatory Regulation. Jour. Exp. Zodl., ii, 1905. 


T he Physiology of Regeneration 459 


As the new limbs developed they were carefully compared and in 
some cases measured. It was soon seen that no constant differ- 
ence could be detected in the two sets, or between different kinds 
of individuals of the same set, if the regeneration of the new legs 
is measured by their rate of differentiation. “Therefore, food does 
not seem to be the main factor in the result. But another fact also 
came to light. ‘The new legs of the well-fed individuals were larger 
than those of the starved ones. ‘This difference is correlated with 
the great difference in size between the two sets, for the well-fed 
animals grew to a large size, while the starved ones dwindled 
almost to a skeleton. By actual weight after eleven weeks one 
well-fed individual weighed 3.48 gms. and a starved one .67; the 
latter having, therefore, only one-fifth the weight of the former. 
This difference in weight is not due to the storage of fat; but all of 
the organs of the body, heart, liver, pancreas, intestine, skin, 
muscles, etc., are larger in well-fed animals. “The new limbs also 
partake of the general condition of well being, so far as size is con- 
cerned; in other words, they developed in proportion to the size of 
the old part. Measurements of the new limbs show that those of 
the well-fed individualshave outstripped those of the starved indi- 
viduals. ‘The difference in diameter was especially marked, while 
the length of the new limb seemed to show less difference. 

It has been stated that no difference in the rate of differentiation 
was found, but owing to the very considerable individual variation, 
small differences, if they exist, might have been easily overlooked, 
and while this must be freely granted, the main result was quite 
definite that no appreciable difference was seen, while the differ- 
ence in size was quite apparent. ‘There is another consideration 
in this connection. If the difference in size of the new parts, in 
relation to the number of parts removed, depends on the surplus 
of food, the detection of the difference might largely depend on the 
size of the part removed. In a form like the salamander, where 
the legs are relatively very small in proportion to the rest of the 
body, the difference in the amount of surplus food would be so 
small that we would not expect to detect any difference in the 
relative sizes, even if it exists, when one or when three legs are 
cut off. In fact, I could detect no such difference in these forms, 


460 T. H. Morgan 


when individuals lacking one, two or three legs were compared 
with each other; neither in the starved nor in the well-fed sets. 
When the whole of the tail is removed the loss becomes proportion- 
ately greater but still I failed to note any differences. It 1s to be 
remembered that Zeleny’s results show only an increase in size of 
the new part, and not in rate of differentiation, and my own re- 
sults show to some extent the same thing; at least, this difference 
was found between the well-fed and starved sets, if not between the 
individuals with one or with more parts removed. In the latter 
case measurable results might depend, as stated above, on the rela- 
tion between the relative size of the body and of the parts removed. 

Zeleny’s important discovery, regarding the relation between the 
size of the new parts and the number of parts removed, bears a 
close resemblance to another curious fact in regard to the rate of 
regeneration. If the distal end of the tail is removed it regenerates 
more slowly than when more of the tail is cut off. “Thus the more 
the material removed the greater the rate of regeneration of the 
new part. Stated in this form the two results appear to be identi- 
cal. ‘This question may now be considered. 


RATE OF REGENERATION OF THE TAIL OF DIEMYCTYLUS AT 
DIFFERENT LEVELS 


If the tail of one individual is cut off near the base and of another 
near the outer end, a great difference in the rate of growth of the 
new tail becomes apparent. The nearer the cut to the outer end 
the slower the rate of regeneration In general it may be said that 
the rate of development of the new tail is directly proportional to 
the distance of the cut surface from the distal end of the tail. A 
few actual measurements will bear out this statement. 


1Spallanzani observed that it takes as long for the toe of a salamander to regenerate as it does for an 
entire leg. King found in the starfish that regeneration is more rapid from the base of the arm than 
from its tip. The results are in accord with the fact just stated for the tail of the salamander. 


The Physiology of Regeneration 461 


INCREASE IN LENGTH OF REGENERATING TAIL 


Jan. 29, Cut Off at Base Cut Off Near Tip 


A — 


Guia aN aa =o 

IMMER ‘Gn eso acoocbedooogsans BounG RTO BORE y OMUROE 3 4 44 I 2 
Veloaa.co ogo combs COO cine Sone trae ener 3 33 4 2 2 
li See DUCED OSS Do cacti CRO n aE Onc Ca eeR ete 4 53 6 13 33 

Missa bacdes6od HOcd 100d DOGO OOD EAD Roar 4 6 64 2 4 

Slee Dy Old aca HATO Dodo ao HetO Con eee aay are 43 6 Gf a, 4 

Nell “Gls coco qscoR ode CeOO hak eME ADO R See Temenos 43 6 7 2 4 
LAN Pelepetee Setar a aferelorererataters ain eters sioreisaraietnseleo.omel 5 6 7 2 4 
ISDS nips BH CRROD OO LOC RE GOE A San ert aeeeesee 6 7 7 — 44 

Increase in Lenetu or ReEGENERATING Tait 

Feb, 3. Cut Off at Base Cut Off Near Tip 

SSS (SS aN 

IMPs ahd aoc oct OBE On De aGae Berane 3 3 4 I I 13 
nie S athccty BORO ae See ea ee 4 34 5 14 I 14 

TO jdtre Sc SORES OS CoE Oe ee me 53 6 7 2 1% 3 
EEN RoI DOLE Oph oC HOC ROrt 6 — 8 24 2 — 
MICs ey eT Cisiee Sos eses Sissies 7 — 8 24 24 _ 
ESM raleoat tere ese crop sieleaeseres- Pla Houses saiasen ws 1% — 8 _ 2+ — 
1/1 bored Seto poe ine ia ie ee aaa 7 — 7% — 24 _— 
ith Sh cdi hale COCA BEAD Care REE 7 -- 1B — 3 _ 


What is the meaning of this result? Is it due to the larger 
amount of food-material available when more of the old tail is 
removed? ‘This possibility was tested by starving one lot of ani- 
mals. ‘The results are not in accord with the assumption. For 
example: In the second half of the preceding table in the last two 
columns of the series ‘‘cut off at base” and in the last column of 
the series ‘“‘cut off near tip,” the records of individuals are given 
that were not fed after March 6. No decrease in the rate of regen- 
eration is to be found. In fact these individuals as long as they 
lived did even better than the others. “The salamanders were in a 
well-fed condition at the beginning of the experiment and the ma- 
terials derived from their own bodies sufhiced, during the time of 
the experiment, to give sufficient materials for maximum regener- 
ative growth. In the experiment in which the legs as well as the 
tail were cut off, it was apparent that the new tails in the starved 
set were not as large, as in the well-fed set. In the starved animal 
the old tail also was very emaciated and much smaller in the ver- 


462 T. H. Morgan 


tical and transverse diameters than was the tail of the well-fed indi- 
viduals. ‘Ihe difference in length was not so apparent, which 1s 
probably due to the loss in the bones being less than that in the 
other tissues. 

The normal tail of Diemyctylus is much bigger at its base than 
nearer its distal end, so that the cross-section of the base is larger 
than that of the tip. Can this difference account for the difference 
in the rate of regeneration from the two levels? At the beginning 
of the new growth this relation may account for the difference in 
size of the new part, because the amount of material proliferated 
from a large surface may be greater than that from a small one. 
This, in itself, would not account for the results, if, at each level, 
the new part, from beginning to end, wereas broad as the old part, 
but such is not the case. “The new part has more the shape of a 
somewhat flattened cone, with a broader base in one case than in 
the other, but tapering quickly to its apex. Therefore, if more 
material were proliferated from a broader base the cone would be 
longer 1. ¢., it would be in proportion to the base. On the other 
hand the period of proliferation is short, and_ the basal parts soon 
differentiate intotheir ofgans. Subsequent growth takes place near 
the tip. Hence after the first period is passed, the new tail must, 
in both cases, continue to grow in length through its own activity, 
and its increase in length must henceforth be due to this activity 
and not to proliferation from the base. It may appear that the 
difference in rate is due to some initial difference in the material 
at different levels of the old tail. If it were simply a question of 
material, per se, we should expect the new growth from a basal 
surface to be as rapid during the later stages of formation of the 
new tail as at first, since the material for both came from the same 
level, but this is not the case. Hence, I conclude, that the cause 
of the difference observed is not due to a difference in the old ma- 
terials that go to produce the new part. The analysis leads here 
to the same conclusion as in other cases of posterior growth to be 
described, in all of which the result appears to be due to some re- 
tarding influence that appears as the growth approaches its natural 
terminus. ‘The retardation is the same for the growth at the end 
of a new part (that arises from the base) and for the new growth 


The Physiology of Regeneration 463 


that begins from the old part near its end. A discussion of this 
point will be left until other cases have been considered. 


THE RATE OF REGENERATION OF THE EARTHWORM AT 
DIFFEREND LEVELS 


In order to study the rate of posterior regeneration in the earth- 
worm, Allolobophora feetida, at different levels the worms were 
cut in two at the following places; (1), near the posterior end, re- 
moving 20 to 25 segments; (2) near the middle, 7. ¢., at about the 
5oth segment; and (3), just behind the girdle at about the 33d 
segment. It is not advantageous to cut further forward, for, as I 
have shown elsewhere, the power of regenerating a posterior end 
ceases rather suddenly about the level of the 15th segment. ‘The 
following tables give the results of three experiments of this kind. 
Table I is for a set of worms 55 days old (September 30 to Novem- 
ber 24). Table IL is for the same set 35 days old (September 30 to 
November 4). Table III is another set 57 days old (December 
28 to February 23). 


TABLE I 
September 30 to November 24 
Worms Cur Near Posterior Enp, at Mipp1e anp Back or GirDLE, ANTERIOR Enp InTAcT 


Posterior End Middle Behind Girdle 

Old New Old New Old New 
83 7 49 34 35 45 
84 7 53 27 33 54 
67 12 49 38 37 37 
73 12 32 fr) 
64 17 31 25 

81 6 32 ° 

65 16 34 30+ 


If we consider the data given in these tables we find in the first 
series, Table I, that from the posterior /evel the number of new seg- 
ments regenerated is small, the maximum number being 17. In this 
case there were 64 old segments, showing that the regenerating end 
was not very near the posterior end, since about 36 segments must 
have been removed, or what is more probable some of the posterior 
segments pinched off after the operation. In the other cases where 


464 T. H. Morgan 


25 were absent six or seven new segments regenerated. From the 
middle region the number of new segments is 27, 34, 38; a much 
larger number than in the last case. From the region behind the 
girdle the number of new segments is 25, 30, 37, 45, 543 which is 
on the whole a still larger number than the last, although the dif- 
ference 1s not very great. ‘he highest number, 54, is higher than 
the greatest number for the middle region which is 38. 

In ‘Table Il only one worm is recorded for the posterior level, 
and this has only 8+ new segments. In the middle region the 
numbers for the clearest cases are 26 -+, 34+, 34+. Inthe region 
behind the girdle the only worm that regenerated normally had 
52 new segments. ‘The results in this table are few but they agree 
with those of the last one. 


TABLEII 
September 30 to November 4. 
Worms Cut as in Taste Tl. Intact Anterior Enps 


Posterior End Middle Behind Girdle 
Old New Old New Old New 
77 8+ 50 26+ 38 52+ 

55 34+ 

47 34+ 

51 eS 

53 18+ 


In the series recorded in Table III, the number of new segments 
for the posterior region is variable, owing in part to the fact that 
the levels from which the regeneration occurs are somewhat dif- 
ferent, as shown by counting the old segments. ‘There is one case 
with a very large number, viz: 24 (with 72 old segments) which 
gives almost the complete number, but in the other cases where 
the cut was made at about the 80th segment, only from 5 to II 
new segments regenerated. From the middle region the number 
of new segments is greater than for the last level, giving a maxi- 
mum of 51, but most of the other cases produced about 30 to 40. 
From the region of the girdle the numbers are still larger, with a 
maximum of 61, the others varying from 31 to 40, or thereabouts. 
In this table also the data show the same relation between the rates 
at anterior and posterior levels as do the other two. 


The Physiology of Regeneration 465 


TABLE III 
December 28 to February 23 
Twenty-Five Worms IN £AcH Set were Cur (A) 1N Front or Girpte; (B) Beninp Girpre; 
(C) ar Mippte; (D) near Posterior END 


Posterior End (D) At Middle (C) Behind Girdle (B) In Front of Girdle (A) 
Old New Old New Old New Old New 
72 13 about 50 42 about 34 61 19 ° 
76 8 about 50 35 37 19 ° 
74 bf) about 50 33 31 20 oO. 
75 II about 50 34 19 (abn) 21 22 (abn) 
78 8 about 50 38 36-40 (abn) 23 +17-20 (abn) 
88 5 about 50 «= 42 55 (abn) 26 23 
71 17 about 50 ~=s«<II Abn. 25 50 
68 4 about 50 551 40+ (abn) 
123 24 about 50 40 


about 50 24 
about 50 30 


There is also another series, Table IV (56 days old), including 
only two levels. From the middle level of the worm the maximum 
number of new segments is 58 (with only 41 old segments present), 
while the other individuals have between 23 and 43. From the 
region in front of the girdle (22 to 26 old segments) the numbers 
are very variable; 40 new segments being the maximum. Com- 
parison of these results with those in the other tables shows again 
that in the middle and anterior regions the number of new-seg- 
ments is large, and much greater in number than when the worm 
is cut in two nearer the posterior end. 

TABLE IV 
January 6 to March 3 


Intact ANTERIOR END, 


ANTERIOR Enp Intact; Cur at A Few Anterior SEGMENTS Cut Orr Cur 1 Front or 
Mippte or Worm Axso Cur at Mippie or Worm. GIRDLE 

Old New Old New Old New 

56 39 about 50 30 22 ° 

55 43 about 50 45 24 ° 

41 58 about 50 35 24 12-+ 

47 44 about 50 18 24 3+ 
23 about 50 50 24 40 (about) 
32 about 50 45 26 30 (about) 
38 about 50 26 
30 about 50 42 

about 50 48 

30 about 50 30 
30 about 50 48 48 
24 about 50 23 


about 35 55 


466 T. H. Morgan 


What 1s the cause of this difference in the rate of regeneration 
at different levels? “The amount of food available at different 
levels might appear to furnish the most probable explanation of 
such differences. For instance, if the food is digested in the an- 
terior part of the body, let us say in the stomach and the anterior 
part of the stomach-intestine, there will be the same amount 
present in the worms cut at the three levels; but since on the hypoth- 
esis the posterior end uses up more food than it digests, the surplus 
for regenerative purposes will be greater the less there remains of 
this posterior region. Hence at he level of the girdle, the regen- 
eration will be more rapid than at the middle, and at the cell: 
more rapid than at the posterior end. I tried to test this possible 
interpretation in thefollowing way. ‘The head ends of some of the 
worms were cut off, and also at the same time the posterior ends at 
the same three levels as before. For two or three weeks, or more, 
the worms were unable to obtain food, hence if the same difference 
in the rate of regeneration at the three levels were to occur the 
assumption that the difference is due to a food relation is dis- 
proven. Such was found to be the case, as the following data 
show. ‘The series were made at the same time as were those given 
in the preceding tables with which, therefore, they are to be com- 
pared. Thus Table V should be compared with Table I; Table 
VI with Table I]; and Table VII with Table IV. 


TABLE V 
September 30 to November 24 
Worms Cur Near Posterior Enp, at Mippie AND BeuIND GirpLE; Atso A Frew ANTERIOR 
SecMents REMOVED 


Posterior End Middle Behind Girdle 

Old New Old New Old New 

69 9 60 39 34 29 

84 7 64 29 37 40 

83 9 54 42 35 48 

76 13 46 38 34 33+ 

87 5 33 35 (about) 
64 17 34 49 

79 16 34 35 


As shown in this table there is a marked difference in rate of 
regeneration between the posterior and middle levels, but not be- 
tween the middle and the girdle levels; in fact, rather in favor of 


The Physiology of Regeneration 467 


the latter, but since the same difference in rate is found between the 
girdle level and the tail level as is found in intact worms, the 
results show that the difference in rate is not due to a difference 
in the food supply. 

In the next series, Table VI, the only survivors were those cut 
at the level of the girdle (except one worm cut at the middle which 
did not regenerate at all). 


TABLE VI 
September 30 to November 24 
Worms Cut BenInp GirpDLE AND A Few ANTERIOR SEGMENTS Cut Orr 


Old New 
35 58+ 
29 
fo) 
_— abnormal 
= very short 


Owing to the difficulty in counting the segments in these worms 
the results are unsatisfactory; but so far as they go the results show, 
when compared with those of Table II, that the rate is about the 
same in both, yet while confirmatory the number of cases is too 
small to be of much value. In the middle columns of Table IV 
there are recorded other cases of worms cut in two at the middle, 
and at the same time some of the head-segments were also re- 
moved. ‘The number of posterior segments that regenerated is 
about the same as in the corresponding set for the same level with 
intact anterior ends as recorded in the first columns of Table IV. 
These results, taken in connection with those given above, seem to 
show that whether the worms are with or without food for a con- 
siderable period of the time the number of the segments produced 
is about the same. Suppose we reverse the argument and assume 
that, since in the starved worms the materials for regeneration 
must be supplied by the reserve materials in the worms themselves, 
then the longer the piece the greater will be the sum total of the 
reserve supply and hence we should expect more regeneration; but 
the facts contradict this assumption, for the longer the worms the 
slower the regeneration. Here again we find that the results can- 
not be explained as due to the food factor. If we assumed that 
the anterior end is the storehouse for the reserve, and that the 


468 T. H. Morgan 


posterior end merely uses up the food, and, therefore, the longer the 
piece the less material available for posterior regeneration, we 
might appear to offer a formal explanation of the results, but there 
are no facts in favor of this assumption, and the experimental results 
that are next to be described negative such a conclusion. 

In order to examine the relation between the rate of regeneration 
and the size of the piece the following experiment was made. ‘The 
worms were cut near the middle and at about the 20th segment 
from the posterior end in one series and in the other series at the 
middle and in front of the girdle. ‘The pieces between these levels 
were used in both cases. In the former only a very few posterior 
segments regenerated (only one or two), and in the latter cases only 
6,7 and 12. ‘The results are shown in Table VII. This isa dis- 
tinct difference, to be sure, although the number of segments in 
both cases falls below those of the check series. “The small num- 
ber of cases is no doubt partly responsible for the few new seg- 
ments at the middle region, as shown by other experiments to be 
described, but that this 1s not the whole question is also shown by 
the next series. 


TABLE VII 
January 6 to March 3 
Cur 1n Two at Mipp1Le AND IN FRONT OF 


AxnouTt 20 PosTERIOR SeGcMENts Cut Orr; ALso Girpie; New SeGMENTs From MIDDLE 
Cur at Mippte or Worm. LEVEL 
New Segments New Segments 
° 6 
I 12 
I 7 
° 
° 
I 
° 
° 
2 
I 
I 
° 
° 


In order to obtain a series of still shorter pieces, the worms were 
cut into five pieces, each in the following way. Each worm was 


o> 


The Physiology of Regeneration 469 


first cut in two in the middle (about the 50th segment). ‘The an- 
terior end was then cut into three pieces, by one cut behind the 
girdle and by another halfway between the anterior end and the 
girdle. [he posterior end was also cut into three pieces; thus, 20 
segments were cut from the posterior end and thrown away, the 
remaining part was then cut in two. All of the pieces of one kind 
were kept together, and were killed and examined after two months 
(January 6 to March 3). The check set of longer pieces will be 
found in Table IV. 


TABLE VIII 
January 6 to March 3 


Worms Curt into Six Pieces (Tam Piece THrown Away). For Derarts, srr TExt 


1p D Cc B A 

Old New Old New Old New Old New Old New 
13 15 12 18 14 14 26 15 (about) 19 knob 
17 4 II 18 13 17 23 18 
16 5 12 17 15 14 22 ° 
I5 2 21 6 8 14 19 
15 ° 19 20 15 8 
17 ° 16 14 15 knob 
17 ° TOenE SG 14 14 
17 re) II 15 14 18 (about) 

17 fo) = fo) 

21 5 — ° 

12 8 = ° 


For convenience the pieces will be called A, B, C, D, E; the first 
being nearer the anterior end, etc. If we examine the piece in the 
reverse order, beginning with E, we find relatively few new pos- 
terior segments in these pieces, although in one case the surprising 
number of 15 segments have developed. It seems not unlikely that 
in this case a piece was misplaced. In the D series the number of 
segments is very variable, ranging from 5 to 35, the latter number 
giving nearly the entire number lacking at the posterior end. In 
the C series, whose posterior end is at the middle of the worm, 
the number of segments is about the same as in the last case, except 
there is no such extreme case as that mentioned. In the B series 
only one piece was alive (of the 25 cut off) with about the same num- 
ber of new segments as in the last cases. In the A series only one 


470 I. H. Morgan 


piece had regenerated (from the posterior end of the 23d segment) 
with 18 poorly developed segments. 

With the exception of the E pieces this series does not show any 
very marked difference for the different levels. “There is a possible 
source of error in the pieces from the posterior region, for I have 
sometimes found these breaking up into smaller pieces, or pinch- 
ing off pieces from the posterior end. ‘The small number of the 
old segments in some of these pieces, where the expectation is 
about 15, may be due to this; but granting the possibility of such 
occurrences the main results cannot be due to this factor as will be 
seen by examining the number of old segments present in the 
pieces. | 

If we compare this table with the longer pieces of Table IV, com- 
paring, for example, those cut at the middle of the worm in the two 
cases, we find that fewer segments regenerate as a rule in these very 
short pieces. It will be recalled that in none of these pieces does a 
head develop at the anterior end; on the contrary, most of the more 
posterior pieces develop a heteromorphic tail on the anterior end. 
How far this heteromorphic development may affect the result 1s 
not clear, but that the small number of posterior segments cannot be 
be due to this factor is shown by comparison with somewhat longer 
pieces that may also develop heteromorphic tails. The great 
mortality of these short pieces indicates that they are not under 
very favorable conditions, although the death rate is especially 
high at first, and while later the small pieces appear to be in a 
healthy condition and may live for several months, yet after two 
or three months they all die of starvation. One point, however, 1s 
fairly clear that although the small pieces lack the power to produce 
new parts at the maximum rate for a given level, yet the retarda- 
tion is far from being proportionate to their small size. From this 
fact we may safely conclude that the amount of the food supply 
in the piece is not the main factor in its rate of regeneration, 
although when it has decreased belowa certain point regeneration 
may stop or be much retarded for want of materials. “These nega- 
tive conclusions in regard to the rate of growth are useful in so far 
as they clear the way for the discussion of the main problem of: 
what factors regulate the rate of growth, for, if it is not due to 


The Physiology of Regeneration 4A7E 


food the road is clear to search in some other direction for the mean- 
ing of the facts. Before attacking this fundamental question I 
should like briefly to bring into relation with the foregoing results 
certain other facts that I have already published.1 

I have shown that if 10 to 12 posterior segments are cut from 
the posterior end of a worm, and then the next Io to 12 segments 
are cut off, the piece lying between these cuts does not as a rule 
produce any new posterior segments, even after three and a half 
months. Whole worms, however, that lack the last Io or 12 seg- 
ments regenerate a few segments in this time. In another experi- 
ment, long pieces from the middle of the worm, having the ante- 
rior end also removed, were compared with similar pieces with the 
head end intact. The rate of posterior regeneration was the same 
for both. Again, some worms with only a few posterior segments 
removed were compared with similar ones which lacked also the 
head end. ‘The regeneration at the posterior end was the same 
in both after a month showing that the results do not depend on 
the question of the taking in of food. ‘These and a few other 
experiments are in harmony with the results described above. 

So far the results have been judged by the number of the 
new segments produced; in other words, by the differentiation of the 
new part. If, however, we test the results by the size of the new 
part certain differences are apparent. [he most general result 1s 
seen at once when well-fed worms are compared with starving 
pieces. 

The new part in the well-fed individual is larger, and this is 
especially noticeable in the diameter of the piece, that often 
approaches that of the old part. The individual segments are 
also larger in the well-fed worms, so that for an equivalent num- 
ber of segments the new part is longer. In the starved worms the 
new segments are often very small, especially when the old part ~ 
is a very short piece. In striking contrast to this difference in size, 
which is so apparent that it is not necessary to take measurements 
to discover it, the number of segments is, as we have seen, approx- 
imately the same in a well-fed and ina starved individual. ‘These 


‘Morgan, T.H. Regeneration in Allolobophora feetida. Archiv f. Entw. Mech., 1897. 


472 feel is Morgan 


results show that while the size of the new part 1s dependent on 
the food supply, the growth and differentiation of the new part is 
to a large extent independent of this factor. ‘The difference in the 
size of the new part in the two cases shows, nevertheless, that the 
new part is affected by the conditions of the food supply, and it is 
probable that the smaller number of posterior segments regener- 
ated by very short pieces is the result of the lack of food for further 
growth, hence the pieces from different regions show approxi- 
mately the same number of segments, but the difference in rate 
of regeneration of larger pieces at different levels cannot be ac- 
counted for in this way as the experiments first described have 
shown. So long as there is enough food material in the blood or 
other fluids of the body to allow growth to take place at all it goes 
on at a rate determined by the peculiarities of each level, and largely 
independent of the food supply. 

If my conclusions from the data are correct, and the difference 
in the rate of development of the new parts at different levels is not, 
in the main, due to differences in the food supply, to what is the 
difference due? The answer that first suggests itself is that the 
difference must be due to differences in the materials of the worm 
at different levels. If by differences at different levels we mean 
the differentiation of the parts, or the kind of material that 
exists at each level, then the conclusion may express a part of the 
truth, but if we mean that each level possesses limitations in its 
powers of growth, not possessed by other levels the answer can be 
shown to be inadequate. For example, if the rate of regeneration 
from the middle of the worm were determined by certain peculiari- 
ties of its material, we should expect the same rate to continue 
until the entire missing part was replaced; and if we were then to 
cut off the tip of the new tail after it had completed itself it ought 
to regenerate as rapidly as does the new part from the middle of the 
worm, because its material has come from that level. While I 
have not actually carried out this experiment there can be no doubt 
that the newly regenerated tail would show at its tip the same 
retardation shown by the unregenerated tail. Furthermore, if 
this view were correct how could we explain the termination of the 
growth process when the normal number of segments has been 


The Physiology of Regeneration 473 


replaced? Why should not the new part continue indefinitely to 
grow? If we can find the explanation for the cessation of growth 
at the proper terminus we can probably find also an explanation 
for the difference in the rate at different levels, for, as can be shown, 
the two things appear to be one and the same. In other words, as 
the new part grows longer its materials change, and this change 
is of such a kind that it leads to the cessation of growth. Hence 
starting under different conditions at different levels the same end 
result will be reached in all cases, and when the terminus is reached 
the growth should slowly decline as we find in fact that it does. 
It is this idea that will be developed later. 

There is another point that must be mentioned here. The dif- 
ference in rate 1s not so much due to an initial difference in the 
appearance of new material at different levels, as to the relatively 
greater slowness of the growth of a terminal part after it has been 
once started. [his seems to mean that the stimulus for the form- 
ation of the new part is so much greater than the difference in the 
rate for each level, that the bicrer’ becomes entirely overshadowed 
in the formation of the first new tissue. Moreover, the prolifera- 
tion of new material as the result of an injury may depend on a 
different set of factors than the subsequent rate of growth of a new 
part after the original stimulus that led to the proliferation has dis- 
appeared. 

In order to compare the rate of regeneration at the beginning 
and toward the end of its period of growth some of the same lot 


‘of worms that gave the records of Table IV were kept alive for 


another month (January 6 to April 6) in order to see how much 
further the regeneration of new segments would continue. The 
results are given in the following Table IX. 


TABLE Ix 
Ste CuT NEAR 
Cur at MippLe Cur Bexinp GirDLE Posterior Enp 

Old New Old New Old New 
48 52 33 13 (abn) 75 10 
59 32 32 72 75 8 
41 53 36 60 63 17 
52 36 34 52 
gt 40 34 53 
1 46 23 45 


A74 T. H. Morgan 


The number of new segments from the middle region is about 
the same as it was a month earlier; the full number having been 
nearly reached, although not quite in a few cases. More new seg- 
ments are found in the worms cut just behind the girdle; the num- 
ber approaching completion here also. In the cases where about 
25 segments had been cut from the posterior end, the number 
removed had not yet been made good, and the same holds for 
the single case where about 37 old posterior segments had been 
removed. The results show a much smaller proportionate 
increase during the third month than during the two preceding 
ones. In other words, as the new part approaches completion its 
rate of regeneration declines. 


REGENERATION OF THE CAUDAL FIN OF FUNDULUS 


Experiments on the regeneration of the caudal fin of Fundulus 
were carried out during the summer of 1905, while occupying the 
Table of Columbia University at the Marine Laboratory at Woods 
Hole, and continued throughout the present winter at the New 
York Aquarium, where, thanks to the admirable arrangements and 
to the opportunity for scientific work extended by the Director, 
Mr. Chas. Townsend, it has been possible to carry on the work 
under the most favorable conditions. 

In two former papers! dealing with the regeneration of the fins of 
Fundulus and of some other species of fish I have described the 
differences in rate of regeneration from the basal and distal parts of 
an oblique cut made through the tail. ‘This difference I referred 
correctly, as further results have shown, to the retardation of the 
growth on the more distal parts. ‘This retardation, however, did 
not seem to me to belong to the same category of facts as the 
retardation from a more distal squarely-cut surface, and it was 
primarily in order to account, if possible, for this difference that 
Tagain undertook a re-examination of the facts. “The new results 
have cleared up this point satisfactorily, and I have gone further 
and been able to obtain data that bear on the more fundamental 
question of the rate of growth from different levels. 


‘Morgan, T. H. Regeneration in Teleosts. Archiv f. Ent. mech., x, 1900. Further Experiments 
on the Regeneration of the Tail of Fishes. Archiv f. Ent. mech., xiv, 1902. 


The Physiology of Regeneration 475 


This problem of the rate of growth at different levels would seem 
to be a comparatively simple one, but I have found it somewhat 
baffling, owing to the great individual differences in the rate of 
growth in different fish. “Iwo methods of study have been fol- 
lowed. In the one, thetails were removed from a number of fish 
at two or three different levels, and the rate of regeneration meas- 
ured from time to time in the living fish, or a few fish were killed 
at intervals and then measured. In the other case the records of 
the rate of growth of the same fish were kept which gives more 
accurate data. : 

The results may be briefly summarized in the following state- 
ments. [he rate of growth 1s, at first, nearly the same whether the 
cut is made near the base or near the outer end of the tail. This 
period covers that of the first proliferation of new material. Very 
soon, the new parts grow more rapidly from the basal than from 
the more distal cut surface. In general, the nearer the new part 
approaches its completion, the slower its regeneration, so that the 
new part from a distal cut surface very soon grows with extreme 
slowness, while that from the basal cut continues for a longer time 
to grow, but it, too, as it gets longer, shows an ever increasing 
retardation in its growth. 

In regard to obliquely cut surfaces, ] can confirm my former 
statements, namely, that regeneration from the more distal part 
of the oblique surface is much retarded after the first period of pro- 
liferation is over. Moreover, this retardation is far greater than 
that seen in a cross-cut surface at the same distance from the end 
of the tail, and consequently is not due to the same factor. I can 
now give some further experiments that throw a great deal of light 
on this retarding factor in oblique regeneration. 

If the tail of a fish is cut off in such a way (Fig. 1) that two 
cross-cut surfaces a and b are exposed at different levels, it will be 
found that on a, regeneration progresses much more slowly (after 
its first beginning) thanon b. The rate of growth on 5 is as fast or 
faster than that on a cross-cut surface of the whole tail at this level, 
while that on a is very much slower, and seems almost to come to 
an end after a time unless the new part from / catches up to the 


476 T. H. Morgan 


new part ona, after which the two new parts continue to grow 
forward together until the tail is completed. 

In the reverse case the cut: surface a is made the broader of the 
two,as shown in Fig.2._ Under these circumstances the retarda- 
tion is smaller on a, while that on b is the same as in the former 
case, which is the same as that of an entire cross cut for this level. 
The results show that the retardation of the distal surface 1s in pro- 
portion to its height, while the growth on the basal cut surface 
is the same as the regeneration for an entire cross-cut surface at 
this level. “The resultsare the same whether the distal partial sur- 
face is at the top or at the bottom of the tail. 


FiG.et , Fic. 2 Fic. 3 


It might be supposed that the retardation on the outer partial cut 
a is due to this part lying at one side of the tail, where it might 
be supposed to receive less nourishment. I tested and disproved 
this possibility in the following way. The tail was cut off as shown 
in Fig. 3. Here the middle portion has a small and independent 
cut surface. Regeneration takes place from all three surfaces, but 
the growth from the outer one a was markedly less than that from 
the other two. After a time the new upper and the lower parts, 
from the upper and the lower basal cut surfaces, catch up to the 
middle part, and after this has occurred the three parts, com- 
pletely united, grow forward as a single organ to complete the 
length of the tail. In order to prevent this latter result I have cut 
away after a time the new parts above and below, leaving the 
middle part intact, and in some cases this was done two or three 
times before the upper and lower parts had caught up to the middle 
part. In this way I hoped to discover whether the middle part 
would finally grow out to its full length, or whether it would not go 


nr 


The Physiology of Regeneration 477 


beyond a size proportionate to the width of the piece from which 
itarose. It was found that the new middle part continued to grow, 
but with extreme slowness. Whether it would finally reach its full 
length I do not know, but it is not improbable that it would do so. - 

These results explain, in a way, the regeneration from an oblique 
surface. If we consider any part of the outer oblique surface by 
itself alone, it is in the same condition as the outer part in the pre- 
ceding cases of partial cut surfaces, and the retardation of the 
growth in both would seem to be due to the same factors, whatever 
these may be. The outer part of an oblique surface cannot com- 
plete itself, except very slowly if at all, until the new tissue from 
the more proximal parts of the same cut surface has caught up, 
and set it free, so to speak. ‘This is, in fact, what occurs. The 
nature of the retarding influence will be discussed after some fur- 
ther facts have been considered. 

The details of the experiments in which the rate of regeneration 
at different levels was examined may now be given. 

The first experiment was with Fundulus majalis. ‘The opera- 
tion was carried out on July 7, and the first measurements were 
made July 26. The same lot of living fish gave all of the measure- 
ments. The tails were cut near the outer or distal end, near the 
base, and obliquely between these two levels. From the basal cut 
surface the new parts measured 14, 2, 2, 24, 23, 24, 3, 3. From 
the distal cut surface the new parts measured 2, 2, 1%, I¥, 14, 2. 
Already at this time some of the basal cuts had regenerated faster 
than the distal ones. The oblique cut surfaces were measured 
near the top and near the base where the length was greatest. The 
two measurements for each fish are bracketed, the distal one stand- 


ing above. They are iG ee The basal measure 


ments correspond with those of the same level given above. The 


distal measurements are about half as much as the distal measure- 
ment given above. Five days later, August 1, the following meas- 
urements were taken. 

Basal, 33, 45 35 3) 3h: 43) 3% 45 4 44 

Distal, 4, 33, 345 3, 34 32 


Oblique, : { 


478 T. H. Morgan 


There seems to be little or no difference at this time between the 
basal and the distal cut surfaces. “The oblique surfaces are much 
shorter at the distal part than at the basal, as in the last case. 

Eight days later the following measurements were taken: 

Basal, 7, 5, 6, 7, 5, 6, 6, 7, 6 
Distal; 0,055°502,.4564 


B tes care ae ane 
Bene: 8 f \5 B (4 

The new parts from the basal surfaces are noticeably ahead in 
most cases of those from the distal surfaces. ‘The oblique cuts 
show the same relation as before. 

Eight days later, August 17, the results were as follows: 

Basaly: 7... 0,05. 7.055510 050 
Distal, 5, 7, 45 55 445 4 

2 
Oblias 1619 18 18 13 

The relation is the same here as before. It is noticeable that 
even at this time the distal cut surfaces had not regained their full 
length. Eleven days later the measurements were about the same, 
no appreciable increase in length being noted. As the fish were 
not in as good condition as at first the last results are no doubt due 
to this. Other results, to be described later, where the fish were 
under better conditions, show that the new growth continues after 
this time. 

The next series of experiments were made with Fundulus hetero- 
clitus. The tails were cut off on July 7 and the first measurements 
made July 20. 

Basal, 2 +, 1, 14, to 12, I} to 14, 2, I 
Distal, 14, 12, 2, 14, 14 to 13, 14, 14, 14, 14 


CeO Ta eT an Parca Ma Ter 
eta ee palace ae a ee hr rae 8 
Ts cies | aie sea aet 
2ei\eg ee | ones, 


At this time, 13 days after the operation, there is nothing to show 
that there is any difference between basal and distal rates of growth. 
In the measurements from the oblique surface the basal growth 


is noticeably greater than the distal, and even also greater than 
the basal growth from the basal cut surface. Whether this is a 


The Physiology of Regeneration 479 


real difference, or one due to unintentional cutting nearer to the 
base in oblique cuts, is difficult to decide, but the latter view is the 
more probable. The following experiments were carried out on 
Fundulus heteroclitus in the New York Aquarium between Decem- 
ber 8, 1905,and January 30,1906. [he waterin whichthe fish lived 
was warmed to about 69° F. Measurements of the amount of 
old material cut off were made. ‘They are given below: 
Basal, 10, 10;.94,'9, 9; 9, 9,95 8 
Distal, 53, 535 55 5 5) 59 43 
Large fish were used in this experiment. The distance from 
the rounded end of the scales covering the base of the tail to the 
end of the tail measured, as shown above, about 10. Hence the 
basal cut passes just at the edge of the scales. It was found better 
not to cut closer than this level to the base; for further forward 
there is soon reached a region from which regeneration is abnormal 
and often delayed. ‘The first measurements (of the tails of a few 
fish that were killed and put into formalin) after 21 days (December 
29) were as follows: 
Basal, 33, 3, 3, 3 
Distal, 34, 34, 24, 24 
The rate at the two levels seems to be nearly the same at this 
time. The next measurements were made after 38 days (Jan- 
uary I5). 
Basal 54,54, 5, 4 
Distal, 4, 4, 34, 32, 34 
The new part from the base has now outstripped that from the 
distal level, although the latter has not yet reached its full growth. 
The third and last set of measurements were made after 58 days, 
January 30. 
Basal, 6, 6, 54, 5, 5 
Distal, 33, 3 
By this time the basal surfaces have regenerated nearly twice as 
fast as have the distal ones, although the latter still fall short of 
their full length, which they would have attained had they regener- 
ated as fast as the basal cut surfaces. 
In the last series the new tails had not regained their full length 
after 58 days. In order to obtain later stages for comparison a 


480 T. H. Morgan 


new series was started February 16 and continued until May 11, a 
period of 84 days. ‘The results are given in the following tables. 
For comparison I give a few measurements of the lengths of the 
old pieces cut off February 16. The pieces cut off from the distal 
end measured in I0 cases 3, 3, 34, 33, 34, 34) 34) 33) 4, 44; and 
from the basal end 7, 7, 74, 74, 8, 8, 8, 9. Subsequent examina- 
tion showed that some of the distal cuts had been unintentionally 
made nearer the middle part of the tail (those measuring 4 mm, 
above, for instance) and these have been included in the tables 
in the column marked “Middle.” 


Basal Middle Distal 
Marchicg' is iitpme muita crete tes Ibe? 25 ee 132 1% 1% 
UAH E mE CE eae 2ege Bar ak 14 I 2 
Bi aerials td eda 44 4h 22 24 
April arG cca cams iuaes seen ae 4t 4% 43 pie Lei arey 2a 2 ae oe 
BT agen trata ste ore Cites at enat ete BE 3k 4 23 ihc 2 ark roe 
Minty Gets ce conceit ieee Ay 3h Fgh at 3 -I 24 24 2 


The results show that even after 84 days the new parts from the 
distal end had not reached their full growth, neither had the new 
part from the basal cut, although it was longer than the former, and 
enough had been produced to have completed the new parts of the 
distal cut had it regenerated as fast as the basal. ‘There is for both 
levels a marked decrease in rate in the later stages which is the 
greater the more distal the cut surface.. 

The slow rate of regeneration from distal cut surfaces, shown in 
the preceding results, led me to examine this question further. A 
terminal piece was cut from the tip of the tail much shorter than 
in any of the preceding cases in order to see whether the same delay 
‘would manifest itself in this case, or whether the initial growth 
would replace at once so small a part. On March 3 a very 
narrow piece measuring 2 mm. was cut from the tip of the tail. 
On April 6 the new part measured $ mm.; on April 14, 13;0n April 
28,1 mm.~° Ina control, in which as much as 7 mm. was cut off, the 
new part measured 2 mm. on April 6, and 3 mm. on April 28. In 
both cases the fish had been kept in dishes in the laboratory and 
were under rather poor conditions, lacking food and sufficient 
air. Nevertheless the regeneration went on at about the same rate 
as when the conditions are better. 


The Physiology of Regeneration 481 

In another case, 2 mm. was cut off from the distal end of the tail 
on February 17. On March 1 the new part measured 1 mm.; on 
March 17 it measured 3 mm.; on March 24, 1} mm.; on April 6, 
14, on April 31, 2mm. Thus it took two and a half months to 
reproduce 2 mm. at the distal end, an amount that could be pro- 
duced in two weeks from a basal cut. In another case 14 mm. was 
. cut off on February 17. On March 17 the new part measured 
about I mm.; on March 24 it still measured 1 mm.;and on April 31, 
Imm. Thus at the end of this time the part cut off had not been 
completely restored. 

In the same series as the one of December $ to January 30, 
other kinds of operations were also carried out. In several cases 
cross-cut surfaces were made at two levels, as shown in Figs. 4 and 
5. The rate of regeneration from the two levels is shown in the 
following figures, with the numbers attached. ‘The first series is 
from December 8 to December 29. 


1 13 
} Cy © ee A I © 
4 3- ) : 
ety Me aw ly 5 
2 Fie: 4 Fic, 5 


It will be seen if these results are compared with those of the pre- 
ceding tables, in which entire cross-cut surfaces are involved, that 
the regeneration from the distal partial surface in these cases 
is greatly inhibited, being not more than one-third as much as 
from an entire surface at the same level. [he statement holds 
both when the partial surface is at the edge and when it 1s in the 
middle of the tail. The next series (December 8 to January 5) 
gives further cases of this sort. 


at 
nn 
tol 
nN 
wal 
ne 


iS) 
v 
ne 
wv 
wv 


wn 

n 
we 
an 

wal 


482 T. H. Morgan 


During the interval of 38 days between these two series the new 
part from the outer cut surface has grown very little, but that from 
the inner cut surface has grown as fast, and apparently a little 
faster, than from the entire cut surface. Here also the greater 
regeneration from the base is probably due to unintentionally 
cutting the partial basal surface nearer the base of the tail as 


examination showed. ‘The third series (January 30) gave the. 


following measurements: 


~~ YS 


Fic, 4b Fic. 5b 


The new part from the outer surface is longer in some cases, 
and this is undoubtedly due to the new growth from the basal sur- 
faces having reached and passed the level of the distal cut. Other 
series show that in this way the tail may finally complete itself. 

Three other series of experiments gave essentially the same 
results, and, therefore, need not be cited. One point of some addi- 
tional interest is furnished by these other series. ‘The rate of 
regeneration from the outer partial cut surface is greater the 
broader, 7. ¢., the higher dorso-ventrally, the cut surface. ‘This 
result shows that the retardation is directly connected with the 
height of the cut surface, and only secondarily with its distance 
from the base of the tail. 


REGENERATION OF THE TAIL OF THE GOLD FISH CARASSIUS 
AURATUS 


This fish has a forked tail which introduces some new factors 
into the problem. ‘The most important fact connected with the 
regeneration of a forked tail is that the new part becomes forked 
very early, before, in fact, the new part has grown out to the level 
of the old notch. ‘This shows that the distal end of the new tail 


The Physiology of Regeneration 483 


is very early laid down, although the later growth must take place 
very near the outer edge of the new tail; the tail retaining its forked 
form during the whole of the subsequent growth. ‘The results for 
three cases in which the tails were cut squarely off, one near the 
base, the other two nearer the distal end, are shown in Fig. 6. 
The distance of the first from the notch is 8 divisions (= 4 mm.), 
and of the latter 2 divisions (=I mm.), and of a third 4 (=2 mm.) 
The measurements give the rate in growth in the middle and at 
the upper and lower parts where the lobes grow out. 

The results show that the growth is rapid at first, especially 
when we consider that the healing over of the cut surface and that 
the arrangement of the new materials take place during this time. 
The notch soon appears in the new tail owing to less rapid growth 
in this region. It will be noticed that the middle region is nearer 
its definitive end than the upper and lower parts of the tail and 
this may account for its retardation. It also seems, as far as | 
could judge, that the notch appears sooner when the regeneration 
is from a distal than from a more basal cut surface, and this is in 
accord with the idea just expressed. [tis not improbable, however, 
that the appearance of the notch is due to other factors. The 
upper and the lower parts continue to grow longer after the 
middle part has reached or nearly reached its goal. At least 
this seems to be the case, although the middle part may also 
continue to grow, but so slowly that its progress was not observed 
in the somewhat rough measurements that I have made. 

In another experiment with gold fish, measurements were not 
taken but sketches of each fish were made at intervals, and as the 
results illustrate some other points, they may be briefly mentioned 
here. In one set the exceedingly slow growth of the middle part 
was noted, the notch had not appeared after 45 days, although the 
middle had nearly reached its outer level. ‘The upper and the 
lower parts had grown faster, but had not reached their limit at 
this time. By January 13 the notch was present, although the 
level of the old notch had not been reached. Its appearance is 
due to the greater growth above and below. In another set the 
notch appeared soon because the cut was made nearer to the old 
notch. ‘The upper and the lower parts had grown past the level 


JAN 


27 


= THT. Morgan 


2% 

; L 

DEC = 
2 


: =i) 
H 


JAN al a ; d+ 
3 > 22 
‘lL 3 


JAN 


12 


Fic. 6 


LV Se Oe ee 


~ 


The Physiology of Regeneration 485 


of the old notch. In another case a small outer and a large inner 
surface was left. “The new growth on the latter was removed once. 
The development was delayed, and even on February 15 it had not 
reached its full length. At this time the notch was present and 
the upper and the lower parts were of the same length. 


AN EXAMINATION OF THE RELATION BETWEEN THE INCREASE IN 
SIZE IN NORMAL AND IN REGENERATING SALAMANDERS 


The rate of growth at neat stages of embryonic and larval 
development and its cessation in many species when a certain size 
or condition has been reached furnishes some of the most perplexing 


and important problems of morphology and physiology. It has 


proven difficult in the case of normal development to attack this 
problem except by measuring the different rates of growth at dif- 
ferent stages of development, but this offers little opportunity 
to test experimentally any conclusions that the measurements may 
lead one to draw. It would seem that a chance to study the 
problem experimentally is afforded in the remarkable power 
to regenerate shown by many animals. ‘The surprising fact has 
never, I believe, been sufficiently appreciated, that regeneration 
means a sudden and rapid renewal of the growth process, which 
takes place not only in those animals that have unlimited powers 
of normal growth, but also in those whose normal growth is limited 
within rather narrow boundaries. ‘The fact that an animal that 
has ceased to grow larger will replace a lost part shows that its 
growth has come to an end not because of the loss of the power to 
grow, but because of some retardation of normal growth that has 
taken place. 

It is commonly pee in the case of an animal that has 
reached adult size that an equilibrium has become established be- 
tween the amount of food that can be digested and the body weight. 
Suppose, if this is true, that a part of the body of appreciable 
weight is suddenly removed without affecting the surface of absorp- 
tion of the digestive tract, will the animal quickly regain its lost 
weight before the missing part has regenerated? In the case 
of an animal that has not reached its upper limit of adult size, and 


486 T. H. Morgan. 


in the case of one having no well defined upper limit the problem 
is more complicated, but if the size at any one time is due to an 
equilibrium between the food digested and the body weight, we 
might anticipate on the theory that an animal lacking a part of the 
body would increase more rapidly in proportion to its weight than 
an intact animal. The salamander that I have used to test 
this point belongs to the latter class. “The experiment was as 
follows: ‘The tails of a few salamanders were cut off at the base, 
the animals being weighed both before and after the operation. 
They were then weighed at intervals of a week during the period 
of regeneration of a new tail. Control, intact animals were also 
kept under the same conditions and their increase in weight also 
recorded. ‘The tailless animals have had their body weight sud- 
denly decreased by the loss of the tail, while the organs of digestion 
remain as before, hence, since the tailless animals have less body 
weight to keep up, they might be expected to increase in propor- 
tion to their weight faster than the check series. If both sets 
increase in weight, as the results show to be the case, the body 
weight of both would be expected to increase at the same rate, but 
the amount of material that goes to nourish the tail in the tailed 
animals might go as additional increase to the body of the tailless 
ones. Hence, as I have said, we might expect at most only a 
greater increase in the tailless set, in proportion to the amount 
removed in the tail which is about one-seventh to one-tenth of the 
total weight. Unless the weights show a very constant although 
small difference it would not be possible to detect the supposed 
influence. Unfortunately, the outcome has shown that the increase 
is too variable to furnish very definite information on this ques- 
tion. 

There is a further possible complication in the results. The 
presence of a regenerating stump may in itself react on the rest 
of the body, and cause the general growth to take place faster, 
in somewhat the same way that the presence of young in the uterus 
of a guinea pig, itself not fully grown, does not interfere with the 
normal growth of the parent,’ but on the contrary, the powers of 


‘Minot, C.S. Seriescence and Rejuvenation, Jour. Physiol., xii, 891- 


The Physiology of Regeneration 487 


assimilation seem to be increased, since under the conditions the 
mother digests enough material not only for her own normal growth 
but also for that of her rapidly growing progeny as well. In the 
case of the guinea pigs the result is complicated by the amount 
of food taken by the mother, which may be greater during preg- 
nancy since the amount does not appear to have been regulated; 
but in the case of the salamanders the amount given was carefully 
controlled. 

The salamanders were kept in flat dishes containing half an 
inch to an inch of water, and were fed on small pieces of raw beef 
about every other day. As they take food from the forceps the 
amount that each takes can be regulated, which would not be the 
case if the food were simply left in the dishes. As the animals grow 
larger they can swallow larger pieces, hence for some time their 
weight can be steadily increased by increasing the size of the piece 
of beef given to each. 

The animals were not young, so that their increase in size is not 
to be ascribed to the increase from youth to age. ‘The life history 
of this species insures the maturity of all the individuals, since 
Gage! has shown that they pass two years on land before under- 
going the final changes that transform them into the aquatic Die- 
myctylus. ‘The susceptibility of the animals to the amount of food 
is shown not only in their rate of increase, but also by the rapid 
loss in weight when food is scarce or absent, although they may be 
kept alive for several months deprived of food. Hence in weigh- 
ing the animals care must be taken to weigh them at corresponding 
times from the last feeding. Failure in this respect is at once 
shown in the weight, asa few cases in the tables indicate. Asa rule 
the last feeding was 24 hours before the time of weighing. ‘The 
average of the five heaviest animals that I obtained by a steady and 
abundant feeding for three months was 3.14 grams and the mini- 
mum average weight for five starved individuals kept ina similar 
dish wast.oo. Inthe well-fed set there were considerable individ- 
ual differences. “[husthe largest individual in the same set weighed 
3.25 and the smallest 1.62 grams or about half. I isolated one of 


"Gage,S.H. Life History of the Vermilion Spotted Newt. -Am. Natural., xxv, 1891. 


488 T. H. Morgan 


the smallest individuals in the hope of determining the cause of 
this difference and found that it took less food than the others. 
No doubt its smaller size is directly connected with this condition. 

The most important fact connected with the increase in size of 
Diemyctylus is that the increase is not due to the storage of fat, 
as the weight of a man who had reached adult size might be in- 
creased by over feeding, but to an actual increase in size of nearly 
all of the organs of the body. The increase 1s, therefore, to be 
ascribed to growth, not the storage of reserve material. The 
starved animal may live for a long time at the expense of its formed 
tissues. [his possibility indicates that an animal of average size, 
kept without food, may still be able to supply a regenerating part 
with materials for growth—the interesting point being that one 
part is rapidly growing at the expense of the rest of the body. ‘The 
simplest interpretation of this result is, I think, that there must be 
present in the blood at all times a certain, although perhaps vari- 
able, amount of nourishing materials, and that a ‘newly regener- 
ating part has the power to take from the blood the materials that 
it needs for growth, even when the 2mount present in the blood 
has fallen so low that the rest of the tissues cannot mzintain them- 
selves, but break down to supply the blood with a certzin amount 
of nutriment. If this idea expresses approximately the relation 
that exists, it follows that while the new part requires a certain 
amount of food in order to continue growing, it can take advan- 
tage of a condition that the older or differentiated tissues cannot 
make use of; in fact, when the latter slowly lose ground. ‘There 
is apparently a similarity in this respect between an embryo and 
the newly regenerating part. Since in regeneration the new part 
is formed directly out of the old tissues we may assume that this 
property of young parts is something connected with their lack 
of differentiation, which is lost when differentiation takes place, 
and is regained again when the differentiation 1s lost. 

In the first experiment the tails of six salamanders (three males 
and three females) were cut off near the base. In another set (also 
of three males and three females) the animals were intact. ~The 
former weighed 1.46 grams a piece (average weight) and the tails 
cut off weighed about 0.16 gram. Owing to a loss of blood, etc., 


, 
] 
\ 


The Physiology of Regeneration 489 


the tailless animals were weighed again the next day, and their 
weight (1.28 grams) was found to be in this case about the same 
or a little greater, due perhaps to the absorption of water by the 
cut surfaces. The control set were at the beginning a little heavier, 
weighing 1.85 grams. The following table gives the average 
individual weight of these two sets of animals from December 12 
to February 3. On January 5 one of the tailless animals died, and 
on January 10 another. 


TABLE SHOWING THE INCREASE IN WEIGHT OF TAILLESS AND INTACT SALAMANDERS 


TABLE A 
Rateof | Control Rate of Rate of 
Tailless Tailless 
Increase | Intact Increase Increase 
MDC Ge 2 a yaya ta -totss21=)s%0)6)otele olor: <1- 1.46 | 1.85 
(intact) 
MD lercvecarcsersic es oe orate alels <a eo 
(tailless) 
IOs Ag So toos oe GOnC Ou aEne 1.28 .06 
IS cecécaobbqaododdEeoge 1.53 25 1.74 sii 
Pirlo ea oe ERE Das .O4 1.65 =09. | {2:28 
[EtS) BE eee ee epee ee 1.88 31 1.98 533 | (intact) 
Wooésccopseaacs beneaeson 1.95 -O7 2.06 08 || 2.08 
DONA ee eer ae niar: 1.96 .O1 2.13 .07 [ (tailless) 
7 |p POS SORE CLD Exe OOO 2.13 5G 2.36 23 [Feb.4 
ihGis /<ligeaaon sees saanec sane 2.08 =9)5 2/323 ait \2.01 
Gln oueattdidea a abe caeoe oes 2.18 -I0 2.30 -O7 Die diges ai ili 
1/6 BUGS BO ORC ore an 222 04 2.34: 04 2533 Bi 
DY ental Abode 8 De cer ena 2.36 14 2.45 II 2ER4 -O1 
Wt eooae bocca ooonaeosoee Pieyn —.02 2.56 II 2.41 .O7 
MOM nae eres Bae ae eeerereas 2.45 sii 2.43 51 je (5 .10 
11 fe ie Dae Bee eet SLA 2.38 ey 2.41 16? 2.45 —.0o6 
DAP A eet ata eae ah ehs ape ete as eats 2.64 .26 2.78 37 Dielfe) 28 
Hilsoge oton Bota aoa eee 2.66 .02 2.66 | —.12 2.80 -O7 
PN tl i eanrteya iar sees oPereas erste hovers sway 2.83 Aa) 2.78 12 2.84 04 


From this table it appears that both sets steadily increased in 
weight, but the increase was greater in the tailless set. ‘The dif- 
ference in rate during the first two weeks, before the new tail has 
appreciably developed is much greater in the tailless set. “There 
is a gain of .35 for the tailless animals and a loss of .20 in the 
intact set. If the next week is taken into account when both sets 
made great gains the balance remains nearly the same. ‘Their 


490 T. H. Morgan 


differences may be due to uncontrolled factors—and in part I 
suspect this must be so, for there is no assignable cause for the 
decrease in the intact series—but if the difference is directly con- 
nected with the absence of the tail in one set it is obvious that the 
entire loss was made good, which isa greater increase than we 
should anticipate since the expectation would only be a propor- 
tionately greater increase in the tailless set. 

The tables also show that after four months the tailless set had 
doubled the weight while the intact ones had increased only one 
and a half times. 

The greater increase in weight in the tailless set might be sup- 
posed to be due to the stimulus of the growing part on the body asa 
whole, as said above; orthe difference may be a purely fortuitous 
one, the tailless animals happening to be a faster growing set. By 
January 27 the new tails were about one-half to one-third of an 
inch long, and their increase in length continuously adds new 
weight to the tailless set. In order to see if the same result would 
follow if some of the intact individuals had their tails removed, they 
were separated in two lots; one lot kept intact and the other cur- 
tailed. The last two columns of the table give the rate of growth 
of the tailless set. The average weight before the operation was 
2.28 grams; afterward 2.08 and the following day 2.01. ‘The 
data are controlled by those of the middle columns. It will be 
noticed that the control, February 3, weighed a little less than 
the animals whose tails were to be cut off. During the two weeks 
following the operation the tailless salamanders gained .32 gram 
while the intact ones gained only .11 gram. If the third week 
is also taken into account the tailless animals gained .33 gram, 
and the intact ones .22 gram, which gives still a difference in 
favor of the former. When the experiment closed the tailless ani- 
mals had gained .83 gram and the intact .55 gram. 

Still not convinced that the difference in these two cases might 
not be due to uncontrolled conditions, I started two new sets on 
March 2. The results are given in the next two tables. ‘The ani- 
mals used had been kept all winter in the laboratory, and were 
under-fed, but for ten days before the operation they had been well- 
fed. ‘Iwo of the animals in this set had had their legs cut off 


The Physiology of Regeneration 491 


some time before and were regenerating new ones, which intro- 
duces, perhaps, a disturbing factor in the result. 


Tasies SHOWING THE INCREASE IN WEIGHT OF TAILLESS AND INTACT SALAMANDERS 


TABLE B TABLE C 
PS Paess: 4 Controt SS Sie pulstarsreseh | ContTROL 
Rate | | Rate Rate | | Rate 
Ole lok Of s| of 
sl. ing | | inc. | ince} inc. 
ies | | (Intact 1.24 
1.46 | | | | 1.68 = = a 
atch 2 \tailless : | WEEE oe es .OI 1.40 16 
Aes | 2-05 1.48 on 1.53 13 
Al 1.30 — | — | _ BOA BE G6 .20 1.63 -20 
Hel] Lez S65 2.41 .36 10 1.57 -35 1.67 04 
17 |e k= 40 EN |) Baie | .29 17 eis 7 04 1.67 -00 
Mion || UXty rad eee 10 2 Ari LO .O2 ep +04 
Z1..| 1-96 -29 cee We = oti! BWiSol| eG yu | 
one sick | one died | April 7..| 1.96 
April7..| 1-93 OC ez i57 | 24 14..| 1.98 
one died | 
igo! Uae SOO 22.70) | 1g | 


The left hand table (B) shows that during the first two weeks 
the tailless set again gained faster, but there is a strange rise and 
fall in the control set that probably makes the result of little value. 
If we take another week into account when the disturbance may 
have had time to subside the rate of increase of the tailless set is 
still double that of the intact ones. 

In the right hand table (C) the control animals increased in 
weight at first much faster than the tailless, but later the tailless 
ones gained much more rapidly than the control. 

Our examination of the tables shows that it would be hazardous 
to ascribe the greater initial gain (in three of the former cases) 
in the tailless animals to the loss of the tail, although this may be 
the case. ‘The difference when it occurs seems too great to be due 
to a proportionately greater increase as the result of the loss of a 
part; and if not due to variable factors, 7. ¢., accidental, it may 
mean that the changes taking place at the cut surface incite the 
digestive tract to greater activity or the cells of the body to greater 
assimilation. 

In the first series (Table A) there is not only an initial greater 


492 T. H. Morgan 


gain in the tailless set but an actually greater increase in weight 
throughout the series. This might be attributed to the influence 
of the regenerating tail on the growth of the rest of the body, but 
as the difference is not found in the other three series, and in one 
set, in fact, the intact animals grew faster, we must conclude that 
there is no clear evidence in favor of the view that a regenerating 
part has in its later stages at least an influence on the digestive or 
assimilative changes that take place in other parts of the body. 
The great powers of growth in a regenerating part may be local in 
their influence and not transferable to other parts. “The question: 
is, however, worthy of further examination. 


CONCLUSIONS 


In connection with the description of the experiments a partial 
analysis of the results has been attempted, and much of the ground 
gone over need not be traversed again here, but the more general 
bearings of the facts may now be discussed. ‘The problems of 
special interest are those connected with the rate of growth at differ- 
ent levels, the rate of growth from partial as compared with entire 
cut surfaces, and the rate of growth on different parts of the same 
oblique surface. 

The question whether the differences in rate can be explained as 
due to the amount of food available at each level has been suf- 
ficiently examined. Ample evidence was found showing that the 
differences in rate of growth are not due to differences in the avail- 
able food supply. It would be erroneous to conclude from this 
that the available food supply has no influence on any of the phe- 
nomena of regeneration, for it has been shown that the size of the 
new part, for example, is affected by the amount of food, in the 
same way as the rest of the body, and it has also been shown that 
when starvation has gone beyond a certain point, even the forma- 
tion of new parts may be delayed, or stopped before the animal 
perishes from hunger. But despite these effects the experiments 
show that the rate of formation of new parts as seen in the regen- 
eration of the limb of Diemyctylus, and in the growth in length of 
the tail of the earthworm and of the salamander takes place at the 
same rate, whether the animal is fed or starved, provided there 


The Physiology of Regeneration 4.93 


still remains enough food for the formation of new material. The 
meaning of this relation seems to be that the greater power of 
assimilation of a young part makes it possible for this part to draw 
the necessary nourishment from the blood, although the amount 
present in the blood is below that which is necessary to maintain 
in statu quo the differentiated tissues. “These slowly decrease in 
size and in the number of their cells, while the new part is increas- 
ing in size and in the number of its cells. “The ultimate physio- 
logical-chemical basis on which this difference between differ- 
entiated and undifferentiated materials rests is entirely unknown 
at present. [he most important consideration in this case is that 
the material of the new part is derived directly from that of the old, 
so that the difference is one of condition only, and is, therefore, a 
reversible process. In other words, because a tissue has become 
differentiated it has not lost the potentiality of becoming young 
again, provided it gives up its differentiation. “This consideration 
has a bearing on the problem of the difference of rate at different 
levels, as will be apparent later on. 

It has been shown in the fish that the rate of growth is retarded 
on a partial surface, provided the surface does not connect at one 
or at both ends with the rest of the tail. For instance if the tail is 
cut off, as shown in Fig. 7A, the outer free cut surface a regen- 
erates more slowly than does an entire cross-cut at the same level; 
but if the partial surface is continuous with the rest of the tail at 
the same level as in Fig. 7A at d, it regenerates at the same rate as 
does the entire cut surface at this level. Results similar to the last 
are found when a square piece is cut out of the middle of the tail,as 
inFig.7B. The proximal, partial cross-cut surface }, continuous 
both above and below with the rest of the tail, regenerates as fast 
(or faster than) an entire cross-cut at the same level. It is to be 
remembered that the longitudinally exposed edge connecting the 
cross-cut surface a and J, does not proliferate in a vertical direc- 
tion except in so far as to cover the exposed surface and to com- 
plete the structure as far as the next fin ray.!. Additions to the 


1The explanation of this seems to be that fin rays cannot develop new ones except from the cut 
ends of the old ones. If one were split lengthwise it would probably complete itself, but not produce 
new ones, 


4.04. T. H. Morgan 


new part from the basal cut surface are not made from this source, 
and the rate of growth of the basal part is not, therefore, increased 
in this way, but the growth from the base seems to be faster, never- 
theless, along the line of the longitudinal cut surface, as seen for 
example in Figs. 7A and B, than at the opposite edge where the 
new part is free. In some way the presence of the new material 
along the longitudinal edge accelerates in its vicinity the growth of 
the new part from the base. 


1) 


Fic. 7 


What factor retards the development of a free, partial cut sur- 
face? The retardation is not due to the level, for it occurs at all 
levels alike. It is in proportion to the height of the free cut sur- 
face, hence the retardation must be in some way connected with 
the height of the base from which the new part arises. One might 
be inclined to interpret this result as due to a proportionate devel- 


The Physiology of Regeneration 495 


opment, by which I mean that the regulation of the growth is such 
that its rate is proportional to the base from which it arises. But 
even if this were the case, it would give no causal explanation of 
the results, unless it be assumed that proportionate development 
is in itself a vitalistic explanation and a causal one in that sense. 
But the facts do not seem to bear out this interpretation, for, while 
the growth on a free partial surface is undoubtedly delayed, there 
are some indications that it might continue slowly toward the 
natural terminus of its development. At least in the gold fish I 
have obtained evidence of this sort in two cases, where the lower 
lobe grew out slowly to its original length. In Fundulus the evi- 
dence is not so clear, but mainly on account of insufficient material. 

Now the factor that seems to be responsible for the retardation 
in the growth from a free partial surface appears to be one involving 
the pressure relations in the new part. Observations show that 
the new part from a partial surface is rounded at the sides (as 1s 
also the new part from an entire surface), and this condition at the 
sides appears to be responsible for the retardation of the rest of 
the new part, for, since the rounding 1s nearly the same for a new 
part from a partial and from an entire surface, the retarding 
influence will be the greater the shorter the base. In contrast with 
this retardation from a free partial surface we have seen that when 
a partial surface is not free but continuous at one or at both ends 
with the rest of the tail, as at b in Fig. 7A, there 1s no retardation. 
In this case the side of the new part, that is continuous with the 
old part, is not rounded, but even with the rest of the new growth, 
hence the rate of growth is not retarded on this side, while on the 
free side the relation is the same as when the growth takes place 
from an entire cut surface. In fact, the growth of the new part on 
the side in connection with the old tail often seems to be accelerated, 
especially along the longitudinal cut surface that 1s also proliferat- 
ing alittle new material, and this condition is in accord with my 
interpretation. 

The new part from an oblique surface also shows a retardation 
of growth on the more distal parts of the cut surface, and the differ- 
ence between the rate of growth on the more distal and the more 
basal parts is in proportion to the degree of obliqueness of the 


4096 T. H. Morgan 


cut surface, as seen in Figs. 7C and D. ‘The retardation in this 
case seems to call for the same explanation as that observed on a 
partial surface, for, if we consider by itself alone any part of the 
more distal surface it may be treated in the same way as a partial 
cut surface, because, despite the fact the parts lying next to it at 
a lower level grow faster than the part in question, yet owing to 
the fact that the part below is still behind that of the lower end of 
the postulated part. there will be a retarding influence onthe growth 
of the latter. The pull or tension that exists will, on the theory, 
hold back the parts just above it. If we imagine this same 
influence existing throughout the whole of the new growth on an 
oblique surface, we can get an insight into the factor at work, and 
see how this case falls under the same head as that of the growth 
froma partial surface. Our analysis and experiments with oblique 
cut surfaces lead, therefore, to the conclusion that the slower growth 
over the more distal part of the oblique surface is not due to its 
more distal position, for comparison with cross-cuts at the same 
level disprove this interpretation, but the result is due to another 
factor. This factor is a formative one in the sense that the fail- 
ure of the maximum potential of growth over the more distal part of 
an oblique surface is due directly to the new growth below it not hav- 
ing reached the same level, and owing to this difference there arises 
a pull ortension on the part that retards its maximum possible rate. 

Before taking up again for further analysis the principal ques- 
tion of the retardation of growth at different levels, I should like 
to clear the way still further by referring briefly to the kind of 
regeneration from the anterior cut surface of the earthworm (or of 
Lumbriculus) and from the oraland basal ends of a piece of the 
stem of Tubularia. Both of these cases may appear to stand in 
contradiction to the conclusions so far reached in regard to poste- 
rior growth in Diemycytlus, Fundulus and Lumbricus. In reality, 
as I hope to show, there is no contradiction between the interpre- 
tation of the two classes of facts. 

When one segment is cut from the anterior end of the earth- 
worm, it is replaced by one; when two are cut off, two regenerate; 
when three are removed, three regenerate, and so on up to five, 
although for five sometimes only four come back. When more 


_ The Physiology of Regeneration 497 


than five are cut off only five (or sometimes only four) regenerate. 
This rule holds only for the anterior end. As the region of the 
gizzard is approached (about the 15th segment) the head, if it 
develops at all, is abnormal; and behind this level there regenerates 
a heteromorphic tail from the anterior end. In Lumbriculus, 
when from one to seven anterior segments are removed, the same 
number is regenerated; when more than seven, six or seven come 
back; and this rule holds for the greater part of the length of the 
worm, since there is no such regional limitation in this species for 
head formation as in the earthworm. 

In the regeneration of the anterior parts in these two species 
there is to be observed no such difference in rate from different 
levels as seen in posterior regeneration, and this is the relation 
referred to above that may seem to be in contradiciton to the con- 
clusions reached in the case of posterior growth. But it is to be 
recalled that we are dealing here only with the part that is first 
laid down, and not with subsequent growth after the terminal or- 
gans have been formed. ‘The comparison should properly be made 
in the two cases only between the terminal organs. ‘The forma- 
tion of the head segments that are all laid down at the same time 
is comparable with the formation of the terminal posterior seg- 
ments that are formed in allcasesof posterior growth. I have no 
observations in the earthworm showing that there is any difference 
in the time of formation of the posterior terminal segment from 
cuts at different levels, and in the salamander I could detect no 
such difference. If such exists the difference is slight and in this 
respect the conditions are similar to the formation of a head at 
different levels, which also seems to take place at the same time, 
although the possibility of slight differences that were not detected 
must be granted for both cases. In both head and terminal seg- 
ment the centripetal influences seem to be the predominating ones. 

These considerations show that, in principle, there is no conflict 
between anterior and posterior regeneration. The difference 
found in the latter case is due to later growth in the posterior end, 
and no such growth takes place in the anterior end. 

It has been shown in Tubularia that the time required for the 
formation of a new hydranth depends on the distance of the cut 


498 T. H. Morgan 


surface from the old hydranth.t' ‘The nearer the cut surface to the 
oral end the quicker the regeneration. “Thesame law holds also for 
the development of the aboral hydranth from the aboral end of a 
piece. In both of these cases we are again dealing with the devel- 
opment of a terminal organ in the formation of which the centri- 
petal influences predominate. ‘Therefore, the difference in the rate 
of appearance of the hydranths at different levels involves only the 
appearance of a terminal organ, which is, as I have tried to show, 
a different problem from that of the growth of an organ as it ap- 
proaches the terminus of its growth. This difference in the forma- 
tion of the hydranth of ‘Vubularia is due, I believe, to the amount 
of stem differentiation at different levels. ‘The gradation of this 
differentiation is from the oral to the aboral end of a piece of the 
stem. Whether after the hydranth has emerged, the stalk grows 
faster from a cut surface nearer the base remains to be examined. 
Let us return now to the main problem of the factors involved in 
the growth of the new part in the posterior regeneration of the 
salamander, fish and earthworm. As a result of removal of 
the posterior end there is a proliferation of new materials, and this, 
as we have seen, appears to take place at about the same time for 
all levels. “The exposure itself may appear to give the stimulus . 
that calls forth the proliferation, but it seems improbable that this 
is the immediate cause, since the greater part of the proliferation 
takes place after the closure of the skin over the wound. It seems 
more probable that the real stimulus is to be sought for in the loss 
of the connection with the old parts; in other words, to the loss of 
the normal pressure relations essential for the normal equilibrium. 
I base this inference mainly on the results of grafting experi- 
ments in hydra where, when dissimilar regions are united, each 
part completes itself at the line of graft, although the actual cut 
surfaces are completely united, and the subsequent changes may 
not take place for a week or more after the operation, when the 
effects of the injury as such must have long since passed away. 
The terminal part is quickly formed in the proliferated mate- 
rials. Between the terminal part and the old part there is also laid 


1This has been shown by Driesch, Stevens and Morgan. 


The Physiology of Regeneration 499 


down a growing zone that is a normal structure for the posterior 
end. I have already given my reasons for supposing that the 
growing region has the same potentialities for all levels,t and that 
it continues to grow until some retarding influence delays, and 
then prevents its further growth. We have also seen that the 
retarding influence is connected with the completion of the normal 
form, hence it is in the nature of a formative influence. I have 
also compared the retardation of a regenerating part to the retarda- 
tion seen in the growth of the whale organism. The growth of 
many animals slowly decreases as the typical form or size 1s 
approached. 

In the case of the posterior growth under consideration, the clue 
to the solution of the manner of growth is to be found, I think, in 
the relation of the new segments or parts to the parts lying prox- 
imal to them. At first this i is the old part, and the first segment 
develops in relation to this part, the next one develops in relation 
to the first new one, and so on for the whole stries. But what, it 
may be asked, is the nature of this relation that determines the 
formation of the successive parts? ‘The old part has a certain dif- 
ferentiation as well as the potentiality of forming the whole of the 
distal or other regions. ‘The relation in question must depend 
in some way upon differentiation, but differentiation in itself can- 
not be assumed to be a formative factor, since we know of no such 
influence extending from cell to cell. If, however, the differentia- 
tion is an expression of certain pressure relations that have deter- 
mined the differentiation and which since they still remain, deter- 
mine the pressure relations of the neighboring parts, and determine 
the kind of new differentiation that will take place, the new part 
thus formed will, in turn, influence the differentiation of the next 
new part that develops, and the process will continue until the 
completion of the typical form has been accomplished. 

The new growth will come to an end when the last formed part 
has developed, whose differentiation is of such a kind that the 
resulting pressures, thereby established, no longer act as a stimulus 
on the growing region to produce another new part. In the forma- 


‘An exception for tail formation must be made for the most anterior end, and for head forma- 


tion in the region behind the gizzard of the earthworm. 


500 TY. H. Morgan 


tion of a new tail the pressure relation is a gradually decreasing 
quantity, and along with this decrease there goes a decrease in the 
stimulus to further growth that ultimately comes to an end. This 
analysis shows why there should be a gradual slowing down of the 
regeneration as the normal form 1s approached, and it 1s apparent 
thet this retardation will be the same whether it occurs near the 
end of an old part, or, as a new part approaches completion; for, 
on the hypothesis, the conditions will be the same in each. ‘The 
hypothesis gives at least a formal explanation of the facts,an@l 
can find no other that will. The most problematical part of the 
hypothesis is, I think, the assumption regarding the nature of the 
influence of the formed part upon the unformed part. I have 
assumed this to be a pressure relation of some kind. Possibly 
some other condition may be found that expresses this relation 
more correctly, but the remainder of the argument may stand even 
if it be found that the nature of the influence is different from that 
which I have assumed. My assumption has, however, the advan- 
tage that it puts into the same category the influences that deter- 
minate the formation of a terminal part, and the subsequent 
growth of a posterior end, namely, a condition of pressure or ten- 
sion. My pressure hypothesis has also the advantage, I think, 
that it involves only a known quantity. It appeals on the 
whole to phenomena that are known to occur in living things; 
for, response to pressure, or stereotropism in adult animals and 
plants is well known. ‘That growth is influenced by pressure 
is also known. Less familiar perhaps is the assumption that dif- 
ferentiation is itself a response to a pressure relation rather than 
due only to the kind of material contained in a cell, although the 
latter also may be a factor that enters at times into the result. 

I have expressed elsewhere the idea that polarity is an expres- 
sion of the gradation of differentiated materials. We may now push 
the analysis further and refer the polarity to a gradation in the 
pressure relations, since these are the dynamical expression of the 
eradation of the materials, as shown in their differentiation. 
These differences can be traced to the egg where the differences 
in the pressure relations of the cells give rise to the later 
differentiation. 


HYDRANT FORMATION: AND° POLARITY IN 
TUBULARIA 


BY 


T. H. MORGAN 


The two problems of hydranth formation and of polarity in 
Tubularia have much in common, yet, as | shall try to show they 
may sometimes involve fundamentally distinct factors. Failure 
to distinguish the proper field of each has led, I believe, to unfor- 
tunate and unnecessary confusion. 

By polarity we mean in Tubularia the formation of a hydranth 
on the oral end, and of a stolon on the basal end of a piece. But 
since in Tubularia a hydranth not infrequently develops also on 
the basal end of the piece, it may appear that our definition of 
polarity has no real significance. I do not think that this is the 
case, although unquestionably the problem needs further analysis. 

Inasmuch as a hydranth may develop at every level, it follows 
that every part has the capacity to produce this organ. Why then 
does it not develop equally on an oral or on a basal end? ‘Theo- 
retically both ends have the possibility of forming a hydranth, but 
there are two facts that show that the conditions are not the same 
on these two cut surfaces. First, when an oral and a basal end of 
halves of the same piece are exposed at the same level (the other 
ends respectively of the pieces being tied) the cut oral end develops 
its hydranth before the cut basal end. Second, while a stolon often 
develops from the basal end, and can easily be called forth, it is 
the rarest occurrence for a stolon to develop from the oral end, 
even when a hydranth 1s present on the basal end and a new cut 
is exposed near the oral end. Hence I| believe we are warranted 
in using the term polarity in Tubularia, although the distinction 
between the two ends is less apparent than in other cases in which 
axial heteromorphosis does not occur. 


JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL ZoOLoGy, Vou. 111, No. 4. 


502 T. H. Morgan 


In my last paper’ on regeneration in Tubularia I gave a number 
of experiments to show that when a piece is tied at its oral and its 
basal end, and is then cut in two in the middle, the oral cut end of 
the basal half develops its hydranth before the basal cut end of the 
oral half. I have repeated this experiment again on a much larger 
scale. “There was one difference in the conditions of the two ex- 
periments, viz: in that in the present case the basal piece was not 
tied at its basal end because another experiment had shown that 
a basal ligature has no influence on the rate of development of 
the oral hydranth. ‘The basal piece developed in practically all 
cases its oral hydranth before the basal hydranth of the oral half 
appeared. ‘The result is, therefore, the same as when the basal 
end of the basal piece is ligated. 


THE CONDITIONS ON WHICH THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE ABORAL 
HYDRANTH DEPEND 


In a former paper? I suggested that the suppression of the hy- 
dranth at the basal end of a piece, open at both ends, is due to the 
more rapid development of a hydranth at the oral end. If this 
hypothesis is correct we should expect the converse to be true and 
anticipate a delay in the formation of the oral hydranth if the basal 
hydranth could be made to develop first. This can be done by 
tying the oral end of a piece first, and then, as soon as the 
basal hydranth has begun to develop, or even after it has emerged, 
by cutting off the oral end of the piece below the ligature. Under 
these conditions I find that the development of the oral hydranth 
is delayed or suppressed for a time. 

It would seem, therefore, that something 1s used up in the form- 
ation of the hydranth that is necessary for its development, and 
that whichever hydranth develops first uses up this something so 
that the other hydranth fails for the time to develop. But the con- 
ditions seem to be more complicated than this, as I pointed out, 
and as the following considerations will show: 


“Polarity” considered as a Phenomenon of Gradation of Materials. Jour. Exp. Zodl, ii, 1905. 
?Morgan, T.H. An Attempt to Analyze the Phenomena of Polarity in Tubularia, Jour, Exp. 
Zool, i, 1904. 


Hydranth Formation and Polarity in Tubularia 503 


If a long piece is cut into a number of pieces 2 to 5 mm. in length, 
each will develop its oral hydranth in the same time that a long 
piece cut off at a corresponding level develops its hydranth. 
Therefore, a long piece must be capable of forming a far greater 
amount of the something necessary for hydranth formation than 
required for its oral hydranth alone. Why then does not the basal 
hydranth also develop? 

By means of a separate experiment I found that a short piece 
(cut off below the hydranth) and a very long piece (cut off with 
its distal cut end at the same level) develop their oral hydranths in 
the same time. It might seem, therefore, that the effect produced 
involves only the immediate region near the cut surface, and the 
breaking down of the ridges in this region during the time of 
hydranth formation suggests that the active factor is the material 
of the ridges that is thrown into the circulation, but I have come to 
question whether this material is the something demanded; for as 
I have already pointed out, it is not obvious why the basal end 
might not also break down, and develop its hydranth by the use of 
the material thus set free, at the same time in which it develops 
its basal hydranth when the oral end is tied. In fact, in a certain 
small percentage of cases an oral and a basal hydranth develop on 
a plece at the same time—or very nearly so—and, more rarely, a 
basal hydranth may develop while the oral hydranth is delayed 
or suppressed. In very short pieces, less than the length of the 
normal primordium of the hydranth, double hydranths and par- 
tial double hydranths are of common occurrence. ‘Therefore, I 
conclude that the basal hydranth fails to develop because it does 
not receive the stimulus necessary to start its development what- 
ever the nature of this stimulus may be. Driesch has assumed 
that the stimulus calling forth the hydranth is the action of the salt 
water on the cut end. [I tried to test this view by sticking the basal 
ends of long pieces into sand with only the basal part of the 
pieces surrounded by water. ‘The oral ends were an inch or two 
above the water and surrounded by air. The air was kept satu- 
rated with moisture by covering the dish. Under these conditions 
the oral hydranth developed as rapidly, possibly more so, than 
when surrounded by water. The result does not conclusively 


504. Terie Morgan 


show, however, that the action of the water may not be an impor- 
tant factor, since the surfaces above water were of course moist; yet 
the results do seem to show that we must be cautious in accepting 
Driesch’s explanation as the only interpretation. 

Another experiment bearing on the question under considera- 
tion is the following. ‘The oral end of a piece was tied and then 
after twelve hours, before the basal hydranth had developed, the 
oral end was cut off below the ligature and the basal end tied distal 
to the region that produces the hydranth. The oral hydranth did 
not develop any sooner (1. e., in actual time) or at most very little 
sooner than it does when simply cut off. “The result shows that in 
a tied piece the oral end does not undergo the changes prepara- 
tory to hydranth formation (as I thought probable at one time), 
and the more rapid development of the basal hydranth when the 
oral end is tied cannot be due to materials set free from the oral 
end that act as a stimulus for the basal development. 

I must here go over again two experiments described in my last 
paper that appear to have a further bearing on this problem. I 
found that if pieces were cut off and allowed to remain with both 
ends open for four, six, eight or twelve hours, and then the oral 
ends were tied, the basal hydranths often developed as fast, or 
nearly as fast, as the control pieces tied at once. It might ap- 
pear that changes are taking place in the aboral end of such a 
piece, open at both ends, that are leading to hydranth formation, 
and that ordinarily the changes are only retarded by the more rapid 
development of the oralend. That this is not the real explanation, 
plausible as it may seem, is shown by another experiment. If, as 
before, pieces are cut off and allowed to stand for several hours, 
then tied at the oral end as before, but at the same time several 
millimeters of the basal ends are cut off, these pieces produce their 
aboral hydranths in the same time, or nearly so, as do those whose 
basal end is not cut off after tying. The result shows that the ac- 
celeration of the aboral hydranth is not due to preliminary changes 
in that end, or at most only in a minor degree. Of course, if too 
much time is allowed to elapse before the oral end istied, the con- 
trol develops first, but a distinct hastening of the aboral develop- 
ment 1s nevertheless observable. I have found, for instance, that 


Hydranth Formation and Polarity in Tubularia 505 


if the tying is delayed for twelve hours, the aboral hydranth is 
generally later in appearing than that of the control, but not twelve 
hours later, and in very favorable cases I have found them develop- 
ing at the same time. 

Evidently then the results cannot be ascribed to changes in the 
aboral end, and probably not to changes in the oral end, but must 
be due to something that takes place throughout the entire piece. 
On this “something” depends the more rapid aboral development 
when the oral end 1s tied. 

Another experiment, designed to test this supposition, 1s impor- 
tant, and although I have repeated it time after time I still feel the 
same doubts as to the result that I spoke of in my last paper. 
Pieces of the same length were cut off near the oral end of the stalk. 
It is of much importance to have the pieces of the same length znd 
from the same region, since the rate depends on the distance of the 
aboral cut-surface from the cut end. For comparison, therefore, 
the cut ends must be at the same level; but in addition to this the 
rate differs in pieces of different ages, and although I have 
attempted to pick out similar pieces, it is practically impossible to 
determine accurately the age of the piece. A small piece that has 
newly arisen as a bud develops more promptly than one of the same 
diameter that has failed to develop fully owing to crowding by the- 
larger pieces. Pieces of the latter kind are unfavorable for com- 
parison. With this understanding concerning a source of error the 
experiment may now be described. After four, six, eight and twelve 
hours, or at other intervals, ligatures are tied around the pieces; 
in some of them near the oral end, in others near the middle, znd 
in others near the base. “The development of the basal hydranth 
in these three kinds of pieces was then compared. ‘The results 
indicate that the basal hydranths in the three kinds of pieces 
develop at nearly the same rate, although the shorter bzsal pieces 
are often behind the other two, as I found in previous experiments 
of this kind, but the delay when it occurs does not seem to be in 
proportion to the relative lengths of the pieces between the liga- 
ture and the basal end. In my former paper | stated that the 
experiment might show whether the materials set free in the circu- 
lation affect the development at the baszl end, because in the longer 


506 1. H. Morgan 


piece tied near the oral end there would be more of such material 
shut off than in the shorter pieces tied nearer the base. ‘This inter- 
pretation of the experiment now seems to me erroneous. ‘There 
would be quantitatively (7. ¢., absolutely) more material in the 
longer piece, yet the relative amount is the same and it is the 
relative amount to a given volume that must act as a stimulus 
on the basal end. Consequently we should not anticipate quicker 
stimulation in one case than in another. On the other hand if the 
substance 1s notgiven off equally fromthe entire wall of the piece and 
if relatively less is given off from the basal region than from the oral 
region some difference in the time of basal development might 
follow. It may be questioned, however, whether the difference, 
sometimes found, is due to this rather than to some other factor. 
It seems to me that more emphasis should be laid on that side of 
the results that shows clearly there is no proportion between the 
length of the shorter piece and the basal development, rather than 
on the apparently slight difference sometimes observed in the dif- 
ferent cases. 

Before coming to closer quarters with our problem a few addi- 
tional results must be briefly given: 

In a few cases a series of ligatures were tied at intervals around 
the oral end of a piece. If the changes induced by closing the 
anterior end introduces a factor that accelerates the development, 
a number of successive ligatures might be imagined to accelerate 
the aboral hydranth, but no acceleration was observed. 

It has been stated that a single ligature at the basal end does 
not hasten the oral development. Also a series of successive liga- 
tures, several hours apart, does not accelerate the oral develop- 
ment. 

If the development of the oral end retards the basal develop- 
ment, it might be supposed, conversely, that, if any changes take 
place at the basal end, successive removals of the basal end might 
accelerate the oral development; but no such result was found. 
This observation is in accord with other experiments that seem to 
show that as a rule no development, or very little, takes place for 
a time at the aboral end. 

On the other hand, removal of short millimeter pieces from the 


e) oa 


Hydranth Formation and Polarity in Tubularia 507 


oral end at intervals delays the development of the oral end but 
only a very little provided the intervals are not too long. For 
example, 1 in one case a short piece was cut from the oral sel of a 
long piece after five hours and another from the same piece after 
eight hours. ‘The primordia appeared thirty hours after the stems 
had been removed from the colony, and at the same time (or so 
nearly so that little or no difference could be detected) in the con- 
trol and in the pieces twice operated upon. In another experi- 
ment short pieces, I to 3 millimeters in length, were cut from the 
oral end of different pieces at different times, 7. ¢., not from the 
same piece, successively as before, at different times. ‘The tips 
of some pieces were cut off after six hours, from other pieces after 
nine hours, and from other pieces after twelve hours. In the con- 
trol a short piece of the same length was cut off at once. It was 
found, when the primordia appeared that the six and the nine hour 
pieces developed at nearly the same rate; the twelve hour pieces 
were somewhat behind, but not, apparently, twelve hours behind. 
The controls were like the six and the nine hour pieces, but pos- 
sibly a little ahead of them. ‘Thus it appears that some effect is 
produced by cutting off the oral parts, out of which the hydranth 
develops, but not in proportion to the intervals. ‘There was 
another series of operations in the same experiment. Long pieces, 
similar to the last ones, were removed at a much greater distance 
from the oral end, entirely beyond the region of polyp-formation. 
It was to be expected that the new oral ends would develop 
mote slowly, because the cut surface is more basally situated. It 
was found, in fact, that these pieces were somewhat behind the 
preceding ones, but the six and the nine hour pieces developed at 
nearly the same rate, while the eleven hour pieces were a little 
retarded. There was need of a control here that was not made, so 
that any deduction from the results is unsafe; but it did not appear 
that the six and nine hour operation had much delayed the devel- 
opment within the period of thirty hours. 

From these last experiments it seems probable that a change 
takes place in the whole piece that leads up to the formation of the 
hydranth, as well as changes at the oral end itself. An accelera- 
tion in the formation of the hydranth results even when the oral 


508 T. H. Morgan 


end out of which the hydranth develops is removed, provided the 
removal is not too long deferred. ; 

The rate of development in most of these cases had been judged 
by the time of the first appearance of the primordia as indicated 
by the end of the piece becoming red. ‘There is allowed here 
some leeway, unfortunately, for personal judgment. In all cases 
the time of emergence of the polyps was also recorded, and in gen- 
eral the two results are found to coincide. ‘The latter method is 
more exact in some ways, but since the polyp may not emerge until 
twelve to twenty-four hours after the first appearance of the pri- 
mordia, it has been found better, on the whole, to judge the rate 
by the appearance of the primordia rather than by the emergence 
of the polyp, since there is a shorter time between the operation 
and its result, so that the effects are less likely to be complicated 
by other conditions. 

It has been stated that in a small percentage of cases, when the 
pieces were left open at both ends, there was, in this species, an 
almost simultaneous appearance of oral and basal primordia. We 
must assume that pieces of this kind were also present in the experi- 
ments and these may tend to confuse the results. It is, therefore, 
unsafe to rely on the result of one or two pieces in a series and a 
larger number of cases must be recorded. In practice this source 
of error is difhcult to control, but I do not think it has vitiated the 
results seriously. 

When pieces are cut at intervals from the oral end there is. a 
noticeable, and often a marked, increase in the number of basal 
hydranths, especially if the pieces are not too long. If very long, 
the time required for the basal end to produce its hydranth is so 
much greater than that of the oral end, that the latter is stimulated 
to produce a new hydranth before the aboral end can begin, hence 
the latter is kept incheck. Very short pieces, especially those from 
the oral region, often produce “double hydranths,” I have com- 
pared the development of such pieces with that of similar pieces 
producing only the oral structure, and, so far as I could make out, 
both developed at the same rate. In other words, the simulta- 
neous development of the basal polyp does not seem to hold in 
check the oral polyp, provided both start at the same time. I 


Hydranth Formation and Polarity in T ubularia 509 


have also observed in longer pieces that when both oral and _ basal 
primordia appear at the same time that such pieces produce their 
polyps as soon as do pieces that make only oral polyps. ‘The re- 
sult seems somewhat paradoxical, but goes to show, I think, that 
the retardation of the basal polyp is ordinarily due to its failure to 
receive the proper stimulus to development, rather than to any in- 
herent lack of latent food-producing properties in the piece. “The 
stimulus once received, however, the development can go on simul- 
taneously with that of the oral polyp, neither suffering retardation. 
If this interpretation is correct it brings us a step nearer the solu- 
tion of our problem. 

Such is the evidence that I have been able to collect. The ap- 
proach of warm weather has prevented further experimentation, 
since the Woods Hole Tubularia develops poorly above a certain 
temperature. Enough has been gathered, however, to throw some 
further light on the two questions of polarity and hydranth forma- 
tion. 

CONCLUSION 


The polarity of Tubularia has something to do, I think, with the 
differentiation and stratificationof the materials as shown bythe dif- 
ference in the behavior of the two cut ends of a piece. ‘The entire 
stem at every level has the potency to produce hydranths and 
stolons, but the kind of structure produced, and the rate of appear- 
ance of the hydranths at the oral and the aboral ends shows clearly 
that there is something in the sequence of the layers or in the direc- 
tion that is a factor in the result. “This influence may be overcome 
by more powerful factors as when a hydranth develops from a 
basal end, or, as occurs more rarely, when a stolon develops from 
anoralend. ‘This reversal does not, however, mean that no polar- 
ity exists. The question arises as to the nature of the postulated 
stratification. At one time it seemed to me not improbable that 
it might relate simply to the relative age of the stem at different 
levels. The growth in length of the stem takes place apparently 
just below the hydranth so that the younger parts are always the 
more distal parts. Hence it might appear that the age of the ma- 
terial gives the stratification. I soon abandoned this idea because 


510 T. H. Morgan 


Stevens and I found that when a newly formed stolon, whose grow- 
ing end is its newest part, is cut off the new hydranth appears on 
the end originally nearer the old part; in other words on its oldest 
end. The new stolon shows, therefore, the same polarity as the 
stem; in fact, the stolon is only a continuation of the stem, and is 
a root only in the sense that it sticks to the substratum. 

That younger pieces regenerate more quickly than older pieces 
was shown in an experiment in which old and young pieces 
from the same colony were compared. I also removed some long 
pieces with short lateral branches that arose near the base. ‘The 
polyps were cut from the old stems and from the young branches. 
‘The young branches formed their primordia much sooner than the 
old stems. Each had been cut off just below the hydranths. This 
result shows that the old tissue of the stem becomes young again 
when it produces a new branch, at least so far as the material of 
the branch itself is involved. 

The youthfulness of the stems is, therefore, an important influ- 
ence in determining its rate of regeneration, but will it explain the 
phenomena of the polarity of the stem? I have suggested that the 
stratification is due to the relative amount of hydranth forming 
material at every level, without attempting to define more precisely 
what this material may be. I can now, I think, give a more sat- 
isfactory definition of this relation. ‘The farther the level of the 
stem from the hydranth the greater its differentiation as stem, 
hence its gradation of differentiated materials and hence the longer 
road it must retrace to produce another structure, the hydranth. 
This differentiation into stem means that the latent capacity to 
form a hydranth can be less easily called into action. ‘That it 
can be awakened, however, is shown by the regeneration of a 
hydranth at each level when the stem 1s cut; and also by the form- 
ation of a bud, which means the local awakening of a hydranth 
at the expense of the old differentiated material. In the new 
branch, therefore, we also get a quicker response in hydranth 
formation than in the old stem at the same level. 

It may appear that the behavior of pieces of the stolon, men- 
tioned above, contradicts my hypothesis, because the part of the 
piece that develops its hydranth, while nearer, it is true, to the 


Hydranth Formation and Polarity in Tubularta 511 


old hydranth, is a new formation not connected with hydranth 
development at all, but with stem formation. In reality there is 
no contradiction here, because the tip of the stolon is a structure 
sui generis, and its stratification is from its tip inward. ‘The part 
nearer the old stem has, therefore, less developed the stolon making 
qualities and more those of stem, hence the hydranth is more easily 
developed at this oral end where the conditions that call forth stolon 
formation are less active. 

Loeb stated that when a ligature is tied at the oral end and an 
abora] hydranth develops the polarity of the whole piece has been 
reversed. ‘That this is not the case was shown, convincingly, I 
think, by an experiment of Stevens and myself. We tied a ligature 
around the oral end, and then, when zn a2 sboral hydranth had evel 
oped, we cut the piece into small parts kept carefully oriented. 
Nearly all the small pieces produced new hydranths at the original 
oral ends, and the only exceptions were those from the region near 
the new basal polyp. In other words, the polarity has been 
changed only in the immediate vicinity of the new basal polyp, and 
the rest of the stem retains its original orientation or stratification. 
That its polarity might in time become changed is patent, but 
that it is not immediately changed by the presence of an aboral 
hydranth is shown by the experiment. In other words, the devel- 
opment of the basal polyp in a piece tied at the oral end is not due 
to a reversal of the entire polarity, but due to local conditions at the 
basal end, calling forth the development there of a hydranth, which 
leads to local changes in the material involved. 

Loeb’s theory? that polarity as an expression of the direction of 
the current in the digestive tract has been fully considered by 
Stevens and myself in an earlier paper. Loeb’s idea that the red 
pigment is a hydranth forming substance has also been there con- 
sidered. His rejoinder to our criticisms Is that it is inconsequent 
on our part to imply that some of the red pigment may not be 
hydranth forming stuff because a large part of it 1s thrown away 
but this reply fails to showin the least that the red pigment assumed 


‘Loeb, I, Concerning Dynamic Conditions which Contribute Towards the Determination of the 
Morphological Polarity of Organisms, University of California Publications. Physiology, i, 1904. 


512 T. H. Morgan 


to be remaining hashad sucha function. ‘The reply of Loeb ignores 
also a number of other results that we obtained that indicate that 
the red pigment has no such role, for our criticism was not based 
on the ejection of the red pigment alone. 

Ihave tried not to lose sight of the possibility that the polarity may 
be an expression of a fundamental stereometrical arrangement of 
the ultimate structure of the cytoplasm. ‘To imagine a network of 
this sort running through the differentiated organs might form an 
attractive speculation, but would be simply fanciful in the present 
state of our knowledge. If we imagine a stereometric network as a 
part of the specialized structure, we must be prepared to admit that 
it changes at each level as the structure changes. ‘Therefore, it 
seems to me simpler to base our hypothesis of polarity on the differ- 
ence in differentiation itself, and not on an imaginary polarized 
system associated with the living materials. 

The other question, with which the present experiments are 
more particularly concerned, relates to the factors that hold in 
check the development of the aboral polyp. This may seem a 
trivial question in itself, yet in principle it involves some of the 
most obscure points in regeneration, and for this reason I have 
studied it in detail, for it seemed to me that if we could give an 
answer to this question we have made a step in advance in the 
study of regeneration in general. While I do not pretend to have 
solved this problem, still the experiments permit us, I think, to push 
the analysis further than was possible before. 

Without going over the ground already covered in the preceding 
account of the experiments let us attempt to scrutinize the results 
more closely. It has been shown that the development of the oral 
polyp is responsible for the retardation of the basal polyp. Con- 
versely if a basal polyp is caused to develop first 1t may temporarily 
hold in check the formation of the oral polyp. Our problem has 
narrowed itself to the determination of the nature ofthisfactor. The 
analysis seems to show that something must be set free in the stem 
that is necessary to stimulate the formation of the polyp, and also 
that the something is used up by the developing polyp. ‘The 
stimulus is internal not external. If it were external we could not 


explain why the basal polyp 1s delayed when the oral polyp develops, 


Hydranth Formation and Polarity in Tubularia 513 


or why it begins to develop as soon as the oral end is tied. It seems 
plausible that the stimulating agent is some material, set free either 
at the ends or by the entire wall, that is a storehouse of reserve 
materials. ‘To call it hydranth forming material begs the question, 
and introduces an unnecessary assumption since there 1s no need 
to postulate such a substance, inasmuch as the cells of the stem in 
every part are themselves capable of developing into a hydranth, 
the more rapidly the less they are differentiated in other directions. 
If we assume that a stimulating substance is set free, we must then 
assume that it 1s used up in the development of the hydranth, 
which after all is exactly the sort of thing a food substance is 
expected to do. 

When the oral end is tied no hydranth develops there, hence the 
food substance accumulating soon starts the aboral development. 
Once started the development continues without further need of 
the stimulus, because possibly in the changes that have been initi- 
ated enough material has been set free to give all that is needed for 
development, or possibly because a process of this kind once started 
can and must continue even if the stimulus that started it is removed. 

This view is necessary in order to explain the simultaneous 
development of oral and aboral hydranths that sometimes occurs. 

It may be asked whether the stimulating material is set free only 
near the cut ends or throughout the piece. Since the piece has 
potentially the power of starting a dozen or more polyps, as shown 
when it is cut into many pieces, | think that it is more probable 
that the materials are set free throughout the piece, although possi- 
bly more near the cut ends. The material must be soluble and 
pass into circulation, for otherwise the basal hydranth would 
develop irrespective of what is taking place at the oral end. The 
experiment of cutting off short pieces from the oral end after certain 
intervals shows that the region involved must be more extensive 
than that occupied by the primordium of the hydranth. The 
greater frequency of basal hydranths under these conditions shows 
that initial changes have taken place at the basal end also, but that 
they are not very great is shown by the experiment of cutting off 
the basal end, and finding that the basal hydranth of a piece tied at 
the oral end still develops almost as fast as when left uncut. 


514 T. H. Morgan 


Why, it may be asked, does the basal hydranth begin to develop 
when the oral end is tied? Must we suppose that whether it 
develops or not (as when the oral hydranth is developing), it is still 
setting free materials? “To answer this question we must turn to the 
experiments that involve tying off different lengths of basal ends. 
These experiments show that a longer tied-off part produces a basal 
hydranth only a little sooner than a shorter tied-off part. If 
these results are confirmed on a larger scale with more abundant 
and favorable material they would seem to mean that the 
material set free, that acts as a stimulus involves a greater part 
of the stem than that of the immediate hydranth forming region. 
That the stem has the capacity to set free much more than this 
has been indicated. It appears, therefore, that we are dealing 
here with one of those characteristic cases of organic equilibrium, 
not uncommon in growth phenomena and starvation periods. ‘The 
stem, isolated from its feeding organ, the hydranth, slowly sets free 
in the fluid food materials from its reserve supply. ‘This material is 
drawn upon by the first hydranth to develop, usually the oral one. 
The balance must be continually made good by the stem until the 
hydranth is finished. Should two hydranths start at the same time 
double the material is used up, and in order to maintain the 
equilibrium, double the amount must be set free by the rest of the 
stem. But if the amount set free is used up at the same rate by the 
oral hydranth, the aboral hydranth does not get the stimulus 
necessary to begin its development. Should it once begin, how- 
ever, it proceeds without regard, or with little regard, to the 
amount set free by the stem, which will tend nevertheless to become 
greater, the greater the difference between the amount in reserve 
and that in the circulating fluids. 

A number of experiments were made to test whether when a 
piece (open at both ends so that the oral end begins to develop) is 
tied after a time, the basal development is hastened even more than 
when the oral end is tied at once. This seems to be the case, but I 
do not think that it can be due to the materials set free at the oral 
end. Whatever materials are set free must be used up by the oral 
end as soon as it is in excess, otherwise the basal end would start. 


Hyrdanth Formation and Polarity in Tubularia 515 


It seems much more probable that the basal acceleration is due to 
changes having been initiated in the stem that leads to the rapid 
formation of materials that have been taken from the fluids by the 
oral end. ‘That end being suddenly closed the surplus becomes 
quickly sufficient to stimulate the basal polyp. But, as explained, 
the difficulties and uncertainties of this experiment make it unde- 
sirable to lay too much stress upon its results. 

My analysis leads, therefore, to the following interpretation of 
polyp formation. ‘The regeneration is due to changes set up in the 
stem resulting from the separation of theold polyp. Thestimulusis 
largely internal, although another factor, the presence of an open 
end, is also essential, as shown by closing the end by means of a 
ligature in which case no polvp develops.t_ The oral end develops 
first, both because it is a younger part (1. ¢., less differentiated 
stemward), and because it has the direction of differentiation for 
hydranth formation. Its development holds in check for a time 
the basal hydranths, because the hydranth that first develops uses 
up or may even deplete the circulating fluids of its surplus food 
supply. Itis well known in other cases of regeneration that a grow- 
ing part will growat the expense of old parts. Whenthe oral end is 
tied the food supply in the fluids of the stem soon rise to a point 
sufficient to start the basal development. The growth process 
once started is powerful enough to draw from the common body 
fluids or other sources sufficient material for its further develop- 
ment. 


‘If tied very near the old polyp, where the cuticle is thin, an oral polyp sometimes develops 
behind the ligature, 


. a 
¢ 
a ih 
‘ 
a 
be ; 
let 
y v A Md (| 
4 i 
Y 1 Ae . 
‘ es ks 
he Wt at ay 
f ip Ee 
8 , al orks 
Ca a Ava! 
\ 
i 
i 
y 
‘ 
4 i q 
eI 
0) 
: ar, 
ot 
: I i 
ek 1 
; i 
3 
j 
a a teh + ee 
f : mys 2 
i 
eae + 
a; ; 
; 
om 1 
7 t mi ‘Sone 
if WT PR RNG Ge 
} eg] Sal 
} ber niet ri es ¥) 
i \ a F , 
Fe a en 
Ney 6 i es yer) 
i ‘ if ae ath 
f * j 
: af * 


fort. Ba ha 


PRUE aK 


SlUDIES ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE STARFISH 
EGG 


BY 
D. HoH: TENNENT AND M. J. HOGUE 


With Five Pirates 


INTRODUCTION 


The studies made in the preparation of this paper have led to 
the view that a conjugation of sperm and egg chromosomes takes 
place soon after fertilization in eggs which have been treated with 
CO, and subsequently fertilized. “Thee de process takes place in 
normally fertilized eggs is suggested by the similarity in shape of 
chromosomes in eggs “oe these two classes, but no detailed study of 
the processes occurring in normally fertilized eggs has been made. 

This interpretation is made with caution and it is recognized 
that its truth can be determined only by a reinvestigation of the 
processes occurring in normally fertilized eggs, or better, by a care- 
ful study of the changes taking place during the formation of the 
germ cells. 


Inasmuch as most of the accounts of the cytological processes 
occurring during artificial parthenogenesis have been based on 
experiments performed on eggs which had given off their polar 
bodies while the egg was still within the ovary, it seemed that 
further observations on an egg which might be subjected, either 
before or after the extrusion of the polar bodies, to influences cap- 
able of causing parthenogenetic development might be of interest. 


'The experimental work on starfish eggs, described in this paper, was done by the senior author 
in the summer of 1905, at the Marine Biological Laboratory, at Woods Hole. Some of the 
material then obtained has been studied during the year 1905-06 in the Biological Laboratory of 
Bryn Mawr College. Miss Hogue has studied the eggs developing as a result of treatment with 
CO, sea-water and has written the account of the nuclear changes, seen in sections of these eggs, 
given in section 1 of this paper. For the remainder of the paper the senior author is responsible, 


THE JouRNAL OF ExPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. III, NO. 4. 


518 D. H. Tennent and M. fF. Hogue 


The eggs of the starfish (Asterias forbesii) lend themselves to 
such eigen ations. These eggs, as is well known, if ripe when 
removed from the ovaries and allowed to remain in sea-water, soon 
mature, and further, these eggs, like the eggs of other Echinoderms 
and unlike the eggs of Molluscs and Annelids, may complete their 
maturation phenomena before the entrance of the spermatozoon. 

Delage’s accounts (02, ’04) of the use of CO, in the treatment of 
the eggs of Asterias glacialis naturally suggested that his conveni- 
ent method might be found useful in experiments on the eggs of 
Asterias forbesil. 

Delage ('02) made use of a siphon in which sea-water was 
charged with CO, by means of sparklet bulbs. His best results 
were obtained with eggs which were subjected when in the “‘stade 
critique”’ to the action of charged water for an hour, after which 
the eggs were removed to ordinary sea-water. 

Doses the work of which this paper is an account it was soon 
recognized that the duration of immersion mentioned by Delage as 
most favorable for the eggs of Asterias glacialis is much too long 
for the eggs of Asterias forbes. The eggs of this starfish when 
allowed to remain in CO, sea-water' for more than half an hour 
were apparently killed, as they disintegrated without undergoing 
development. 

Eggs in various stages of maturity were subjected to the action of 
CO, sea-water for varying periods of time. ‘The details of these 
trials are unnecessary. It is sufficient to say that the egg of 
Asterias forbes, like the egg of Asterias glacialis, is in its most 
favorable condition during the time that elapses between the 
extrusion of the first and of the second polar body. With the eggs 
of Asterias forbesii it was found that uniformly the best results 
were obtained when the eggs were subjected to the action of the 
CO, sea-water immediately after the appearance of the first polar 
body as a protrusion from the surface of the egg. 

The best length of time of immersion was about five minutes. 
Good results were obtained by the immersion of the eggs in CO, 


‘Throughout this paper the term “CO, sea-water” will be used instead of the longer phrase 
“sea-water charged with CO,.” 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 519 


sea-water for from three to ten minutes, but five or six minutes gave 
uniformly the best results. However, in one lot of eggs in which the 
time was ten minutes, fully 95 per cent of the eggs segmented 
regularly and gave rise to normal swimming embryos which were 
kept alive and under observation for more than a month. 

It seemed that the best results were obtained when the sea-water 
was charged and allowed to stand in the siphon for from ten to 
twelve hours before using. 


Method of Treatment 


The eggs were shaken from the ovaries into large dishes of sea- 
water, in which they were allowed to remain until the first polar 
body had made its appearance. ‘They were then drawn with a 
pipette from the bottom of the dish in which they had partially 
matured and transferred gently and with as little water as possible 
to finger-bowls. “The CO, sea-water was then run slowly into the 
finger-bowl until the bowl was about half-filled. During the whole 
operation, care was taken to avoid violent agitation of the eggs. 

The eggs settled to the bottom of the bowls in about three 
minutes. At the end of the desired period of immersion, the CO, 
sea-water was withdrawn and replaced by ordinary sea-water, this 
in turn being changed for fresh sea-water as soon as the eggs had 
again settled. 

The method is convenient in its application and for use with the 
starfish egg offers an ideal reagent. 


T he Scope of the Investigations 


Since the method is so sure in its action, and since many of the 
developmental processes reproduce so faithfully the processes 
occurring in the normally fertilized egg, it seemed that we might 
have in the starfish egg treated with CO, the means of imitating 
the processes of normal parthenogenesis occurring among rotifers, 
crustaceans and insects. 

Delage (01) as a conclusion from experiments in which the 
eggs of Asterias glacialis were subjected while in the critical 
stage (in this case the time when the germinal vesicle loses its 
membrane and prepares for the emission of the polar globules), 


520 D. H. Tennent and M. Ff. Hogue 


to the action of a solution of KCl, expressed the idea that arti- 
ficially parthenogenetic eggs, like those in which parthenogen- 
esis occurs naturally, emit but one polar body, and that the agent 
producing parthenogenetic development acts by the inhibition 
of the formation of the second polar body, the second polar body 
playing the role of a spermatozoon. 

Delage in 1902 found that eggs submitted to the action of CO, 
during the time between the disappearance of the nuclear mem- 
brane immediately preceding maturation and of the return of the 
nucleus to the resting condition ordinarily preceding fertilization, 
developed independently of the polar globules. Parthenogenesis 
(02, p. 231) resulted whether the egg had given off neither, or 
one, or both of the polar globules. 

Subsequently (04), by a somewhat more complicated method 
of treatment, he was able to produce parthenogenetic develop- 
ment, using CO, as a reagent, in sea urchin eggs in which he 
knew that iherh polar bodies had been given off. 

In my work on the eggs of Asterias forbesii it became apparent 
that although the sean polar body could not be seen in many 
of the eggs, it made its appearance in many, and that the eggs 
developed into swimming embryos in both cases. It was found 
later that the second polar body was formed in every case, 
although it might remain within the egg membrane in a cup- 
shaped depression in the surface of the cytoplasm. 

But, although both polar bodies were extruded, thus removing 
the possibility of imitating exactly the processes of normal par- 
thenogenesis, there was as yet no evidence that part of the 
chromatin normally extruded in the second polar body was not 
retained within the egg and that this might later assume the 
functions of the sperm nucleus. 

In consequence of this possibility, it seemed that if it were 
possible to fertilize the egg after its subjection to the action of 
CO,, this retained chromatin might be rejected or at least some 
series of changes might be caused that would be of interest when 
compared with the normal maturation and fertilization stages 
and with those occurring in the egg which had been induced to 
develop by treatment with CO, sea-water. 


Fal 


2 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 521 


This idea involved the question of the comparative effective- 
ness of the CO, solution and of the starfish spermatozoon on the 
egg of the starfish. 

After determining that it was possible to fertilize eggs after 
they had been immersed in CO, sea-water, the question quite 
naturally arose: What will be the result of treating fertilized eggs 
with CO, sea-water ? 

The results of the investigations may be discussed to the best 
advantage in three sections. 

Section 1 embodies the observations on living eggs after their 
treatment with CO, sea-water and the data obtained from a 
study of the sections of the eggs so treated. 

Section 2 contains the data obtained from a study of eggs 
which were treated with CO, and subsequently fertilized. 

Section 3 gives the result of the examination of eggs that were 
fertilized and subsequently subjected to the action of CO, sea- 
water. 


I. UNFERTILIZED EGGS TREATED WITH CO, SEA-WATER 


a. Observations on the Living Eggs 


The time required for maturation varies, as is well known, 
with the condition of the egg, the surrounding temperature, etc., 
so that any facts which were observed as to variations of this 
kind are without value in this connection, but it is of importance 
to notice the influence of CO, sea-water in delaying the comple- 
tion of maturation. 

In one lot of eggs the first polar body was given off one hour 
and ten minutes after the removal of the eggs from the ovary. 
In the eggs of this lot treated with CO, sea-water the second 
polar body appeared one hour and fifteen minutes after the first 
had been extruded or two hours and twenty-five minutes after 
removal from the ovary. 

In the stock dish a quantity of the same lot of eggs were allowed 
to complete their maturation undisturbed and the second polar 
body appeared thirty-five minutes after the first or one hour and 
forty-five minutes after the eggs were removed from the ovary. 


pv D. H. Tennent and M. f. Hogue 


The CO, sea-water is thus seen to have delayed the process of 
maturation forty minutes. 

In some lots of eggs, but not in all, a distinct membrane, some- 
what thicker than the ordinary fertilization membrane, was 
formed and pushed out from the surface of the eggs when these 
were transferred from the CO, sea-water to ordinary sea-water, 
this activity recalling the facts observed by Lefevre (’05, ’06) 
on eggs of Thalassema treated with acid solutions. ‘This mem- 
brane carried the first polar body out with it, the second polar 
body being extruded into the space between the surface of the 
egg and the membrane. 

In the average lot of eggs the series of changes preceding 
the first cleavage follows very closely, those described by Wilson 
(01) as occurring in Toxopneustes eggs, the important difference 
being that in the eggs of Asterias forbesii that have received the 
best degree of treatment with CO, sea-water no cytasters were to 
be observed. In eggs which had received too prolonged a treat- 
ment, numerous cytasters might be seen. 

In about three and a half hours after removal from the CO,~ 
sea-water the cytoplasm becomes coarser and looser in appear- 
ance, apparently becoming more fluid in character in the region 
of the nucleus. Radiations appear stretching out from the 
vaguely defined clearer area into the denser cytoplasm. As a 
result of these changes the nucleus becomes very distinct. 

These primary radiations then become fainter and new radia- 
tions growing in from the sides of the nucleus are seen, the nuclear 
membrane breaks down and the definite mitotic figure is seen to 
be forming, its centers gradually enlarging as the figure becomes 
definitely established. ‘The division of the egg into two cells 
is completed in about four hours after removal from the CO, 
sea-water. On one or two occasions, the temperature being 
low, the first division was completed only after five hours. 

It is of interest to notice that in the eggs of Asterias glacialis 
observed by Delage (’02) segmentation commenced about three 
hours after removal to the ordinary sea-water, the eggs having 
remained in the CO, sea-water for an hour, that is, segmentation 
commenced four hours after the beginning of the treatment, while 


a 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 523 


in the eggs of Asterias forbesii, observed by me, the segmentation 
began in approximately the same time, about four hours from 
the beginning of the treatment, although the eggs had remained 
in the CO, sea-water for but five or six minutes. 


b. A Study of the Nuclear Changes as Seen in Sections 


Delage in his work on Asterias glacialis did not make a detailed 
study of these phenomena. ‘The purpose of his work being finally 
(04) to raise the larve until they would metamorphose. 

Morgan’s (99) observations on the eggs of Asterias forbesil 
when treated with solutions of NaCl, MgCl and KCl (’99, p. 
499) although brief, since the eggs were immature, are of interest. 
He mentions the appearance of areas of cyanoplasm and the fact 
that in some unfertilized eggs that had been in a solution of mag- 
nesium chloride for three hours, stars with delicate rays made their 
appearance in these areas of cyanoplasm. 

Kostanecki’s work on the eggs of Mactra (04) is of especial 
interest in the present connection, since in this egg, which nor- 
mally does not extrude its polar bodies until after the entrance of 
the spermatozoan, he traced a series of phenomena analogous to 
those observed in the starfish egg. 

The eggs of Mactra were treated with solutions of KCI, NaCl, 
CaCl or with concentrated sea-water. ‘The most normal develop- 
ment was obtained in eggs which remained in a weak solution of 
KCl thirty minutes. Here both polar bodies were given off 
(04, Fig. 34) and from the chromosomes remaining in the egg a 
nucleus was formed quite as in the fertilized egg, although it 
contained but twelve chromosomes, one-half the normal number 
(twenty-four), as was later shown in the first segmentation spindle. 

The polar spindles developed deeper within the cytoplasm 
and the polar bodies were larger than those of fertilized eggs. In 
the manner of division of the segmentation nucleus and in the 
absence of centrosomes and centrosome-like structures in these 
divisions, the eggs of Mactra differ from those of Asterias forbesil. 

Scott (’06), in his observations on the parthenogenetic develop- 
ment of Amphitrite eggs after treatment with salt solutions, 
shows that the ripeness of the eggs, the strength of the solutions, 


524 D. H. Tennent and M. ‘fF. Hogue 


etc., were the factors determining whether development would 
be nearly normal or very abnormal. If the Amphitrite eggs are 
ripe, normal polar bodies are given off in a weak solution of 
calcium nitrate, although the subsequent segmentation 1s abnor- 
mal. Here, again, little comparison can be made between these 
eggs and those of Asterias forbesii, the most important point of 
agreement being that both polar bodies are given off in each 
case. 

Lefevre’s (’06) work on artificial parthenogenesis in the eggs 
of Thalassema shows many points of agreement with my obser- 
vations on starfish eggs, the formation of the “fertilization mem- 
brane,” the extrusion, as a rule, of both polar bodies may be 
mentioned here and further comparisons reserved for a later 
mention. 

Inthe present experiment the eggs, after the extrusion of the first 
polar body, were covered with sea-water charged with carbon 
dioxide. ‘They remained in this four minutes and were then 
transferred to ordinary sea-water. 

The series consisted of twenty-five stages, the earlier numbers 
of which were fixed at five minute and the later at ten minute 
intervals in Boveri’s picro-acetic acid. 

The eggs were cut in sections 3 microns thick and stained with 
Heidenhain’s iron-hzematoxylin, long method. Eosin, erythrosin, 
and Bordeaux red were tried as counter-stains but the iron- 
hematoxylin gave the clearest and best effects. “Total mounts 
were stained by Conklin’s hamatoxylin method. 

Throughout the series a few eggs were found with the germi- 
nal vesicle intact, which is due to the fact that the nuclear mem- 
brane had not begun to fade when the eggs were treated with 
the CO: sea-water, 7. e., theywere‘not in Delage’s “critical stage.” 

After the first polar body is given off, a cone of cytoplasm is 
often seen projecting from the surface of the egg at the side of 
the first body during the time that the second polar body is forming. 
This cone frequently persists until the first segmentation spindle 
is well formed (Figs. 21 and 26) and is occasionally seen in the 
two-cell stage at the edge of the cleavage plane. 

The polar bodies are of the same size as those given off in fer- 


The Development of the Starfish Egg SpE 


tilized eggs, differing in this respect from those observed by 
Kostanecki in the artificially parthenogenetic eggs of Mactra. 

The cleavage is normal, dividing the egg into halves and the 
second forming four equal-sized blastomeres. 

Shortly after the extrusion of the first polar body the chromo- 
somes are found lying on the spindle fibers, which have already 
begun to degenerate at their polarends (Fig. 1). In a later stage, 
eighteen chromosomes may be counted (Fig. 2), and with them 
are seen a few spindle fibers. ‘These fibers disappear and the 
chromatin is left free in the cytoplasm (Fig. 3), more or less massed 
together. In the cytoplasm directly beneath the first polar body 
is a darkly staining region which is not represented in the draw- 
ings. It does not take the chromatin stain. 

The formation of the second polar body proceeds slowly and 
the time of the division of the chromosomes varies. In an egg 
from stage 5 (Fig. 4) the chromosomes, undivided, are still attached 
to the fibers of the first polar spindle. In stage 11 (Fig. 5) the 
chromosomes are divided for the second polar body although 
they are still attached to the fibers of the first polar spindle. 

From a study of the preserved material, it seems evident that 
the second polar spindle is formed tangentially to the surface 
of the egg (Fig. 6) and that it later revolves until it has taken a 
radial position (Figs. 7 and 8). The centrosomes, single and 
double, may be seen clearly in these spindles. (Figs. 6, 7 and 8.) 

In the early anaphase of the second polar spindles (Fig. g) the 
chromosomes are scattered irregularly over the whole spindle. 
In the late anaphase (Fig. 10) they are collected in plates at the 
two poles of the spindle, one or two of the chromosomes being 
later than the others in taking their positions. 

After the second polar body is formed, the chromatin is more 
or less free in the cytoplasm, some of it lying on the astral rays 
as though passing down these into the cytoplasm (Fig. 12), and 
the remainder forming five or six vesicles which fuse to form the 
female pronucleus. At each division of the nucleus, chromatin 
is thrown out into the cytoplasm. Occasionally the chromatin 
is found lying free in the cytoplasm, without a trace of astral 
radiation or of vesicle formation (Fig. IT). 


526 D. H. Tennent and M. ‘f. Hogue 


The newly formed nucleus now moves to the center of the 
egg and at the same time begins to divide. ‘The walls of the 
vesicle become indented (Fig. 13), and on the side nearest the 
surface of the egg two centrosomes appear and move around the 
nucleus until he: lie one at each end of the slightly elongated 
nucleus. This process differs from that described by Lefevre 
(06) for Thalassema in which the cleavage asters with their cen- 
ters arise simultaneously at opposite poles of the egg nucleus. 

At the same time the astral fibers are forming in the cytoplasm. 
This growth apparently begins at the nuclear membrane and 
extends into the cytoplasm of the egg as described by Wilson 
( ‘a1) and Morgan (’99) for Echinodermeggs. Later, these radia- 
tions collect at the two ends of the nuclear vesicle whither the 
centrosomes have migrated. 

Following this stage the nuclear membrane breaks down and 
particles of chromatin, seen in Fig. 15 on the astral rays, pass into 
the cytoplasm. 

As the nuclear membrane disappears (Fig. 16), the conspic- 
uous nucleolus unravels and its chromatin, together with that 
derived from the chromatin network of the nucleus becomes 
broken up into short threads, the discharge of chromatin into the 
cytoplasm continuing meantime. The study of the sections 
has not shown whether all of the chromosomes of the equatorial 
plate are derived from the nucleolus. ‘The spindle fibers grow 
into the nucleus and may be seen stretching from the poles to the 
center of the nucleus. 

At this stage the nuclear sap has been poured out into the 
cytoplasm and a difference in staining reaction of the areas 
indicated by the dotted line may be seen. 

Figs. 14 and 17 represent later stages of the first segmenta- 
tion spindle. 

In the late anaphase the chromosomes become enclosed in 
vesicles (Fig. 18), which fuse to form the daughter nuclei. ‘The 
nuclear membrane forms as the astral rays are disappearing. 
During this process chromatin rejection continues, the rejected 
chromatin being seen enclosed in little vesicles lying in the cyto- 


plasm (Fig. 22). 


The Development of the Starfish Egg Saf 


After the daughter nuclei are formed they move apart and the 
constriction appears which divides the egg into halves. It is 
interesting to note that the cytoplasm does not begin to divide 
until the daughter nuclei are completely formed. Figs. 23 and 
25 represent the spindle and equatorial plate of the second segmen- 
tation. 

Mathews (’95) was unable to find centrosomes in the seg- 
mentation spindles of Asterias forbesii. In the material from 
which these sections were made two kinds of centrosomes were 
found; one granular, containing several small deeply staining 
bodies (Figs. 17 and 23); the other, sometimes a single body, 
more often two, from which the radiation extend (Fig. 16). In 
Fig. 18 the double centrosomes appear in the cytoplasm while 
the astral rays are disappearing. 

It has been impossible to count the chromosomes in the seg- 
mentation spindle as they do not take definite form (Fig. 14) 
until they line up in the equatorial plate, ready for division. 
Here, again, it was useless to try to count them since they were 
massed together and of irregular shape. Often part of one 
chromosome is in one section and part in another. Again, 
some divide before others and frequently when the majority of 
the chromosomes have separated, a long chain of chromatic 
material, as yet unsegmented, extends down the middle of the 
spindle. A few chromosomes seem to have a. characteristic 
form. 

It will be remembered that in the second polar spindle there 
were eighteen chromosomes (Figs. 1,2 and 8). From Figs. 9g 
and Io it seems evident that at least eighteen chromosomes will be 
left in the egg after the second polar body is given off. Fig. 21 
is drawn from a section showing the two polar bodies at the sur- 
face and the equatorial plate at the center of the egg. This 
is shown again drawn under higher magnification in Fig. 20. 
While it is impossible to count the number of chromosomes 
exactly, it is evident that there are at least eighteen here. 
Another egg (Fig. 24) shows the two polar bodies at the surface 
and the equatorial plate at the center, with the spindle fibers in 
cross-section. This equatorial plate is shown again in Fig. 19. 


528 D. H. Tennent and M. fF. Hogue 


In this the chromosomes have divided for the daughter nuclei 
of the first division. 

Delage maintained a doubling in the number of chromosomes 
in the parthenogenetic eggs of Strongylocentrotus lividus. ‘The 
eggs had given off both polar bodies when treated with the solution 
and yet later they contained the normal somatic number of chromo- 
somes. He held that at some time, as yet undetermined, the 
chromosomes divide again and so establish the normal number 
(autoregulation), these in turn dividing to form the chromosomes 
of the daughter nuclei. Boveri has since shown that eighteen 
is not the somatic number of chromosomes for Strongylocentrotus 
lividus, but the reduced number. 

In the parthenogenetic eggs of Mactra, Kostanecki (’04) 
found the reduced number of chromosomes in the segmentation 
spindle when the egg had given off both polar bodies. ‘These 
eges did not segment many times. 

Boveri (’04) experimenting with the fertilization of nucleated 
and non-nucleated fragments of sea-urchin eggs, found that when 
a non-nucleated fragment is fertilized it contains less chrom- 
atin; z. e., the nuclei are smaller than the fertilized nucleated 
fragments. Morgan (’95) performed similar experiments with 
similar results. 

Stevens (02) while working on the eggs of Echinus micro- 
tuberculatus which were cut into pieces while in the anaphase of 
the first division, found that fragments containing a centrosome 
and a small number of chromosomes may divide five or six times 
without the chromosomes returning to the constitutional number. 

Wilson (’o1) showed that in parthenogenetic Toxopneustes 
eggs the number of chromosomes is eighteen, one-half the number 
occurring in fertilized eggs. 

The evidence at present, seems in favor of the permanence of 
the number of chromosomes occurring in the unfertilized egg 
which is caused to develop parthenogenetically after two polar 
bodies have been given off. 

Another idea of Delage’s, that the presence of two polar bodies 
might be due to the division of the first polar body, seems incor- 
rect. In Asterias forbesii the formation of the second polar body 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 529 


has been traced and its actual cutting off has been noted. The 
second polar body is slightly smaller (Figs. 21 and 24) while the 
first polar body contains the greater amount of chromatin (Fig.11). 

One egg was found in stage 24 (Fig. 24) in which the second 
polar body had been formed but not extruded from the egg 
before the nucleus began to divide. Here it is certain that the 
polar body does not again enter the nucleus. ‘This retention of 
the second polar body within the egg is not the usual method of 
procedure but may be accounted for by the tardiness with which 
this egg began development. ‘There is, of course, a possibility 
that later the chromatin of this retained polar body might mix 
with that of the egg nucleus in a manner similar to the behavior 
of the sperm nucleus in partial fertilization as noted by Boveri. 

One cannot but be impressed with the normal procedure of 
development in the eggs treated by Delage’s CO, method. In the 
thousands of eggs examined in the study of these sections, there 
were not more than a dozen abnormal structures. (It is to be 
remembered, of course, that this series which has been studied 
with greatest detail was selected primarily because of its perfec- 
tion.) 

Two cases of multipolar spindles were observed, one with 
three poles, the other with four. A few eggs, undivided, con- 
tained three nuclei, each with its amphiaster ready for division. 
There were also two or three eggs which had divided into three 
blastomeres. The cleavage in the early stages was normal. 
No cytasters were formed in the eggs. 


SUMMARY 


1 Two polar bodies are given off. 

2 Eighteen single chromosomes are left in the egg after the 
extrusion of the second polar body. (The number counted varies 
within slight limits.) 

3 Centrosomes are present in the polar and segmentation 
spindles. 


4. Cleavage is normal though slower than in fertilized eggs. 


530 D. H. Tennent and M. fF. Hogue 


II. OBSERVATIONS ON EGGS WHICH WERE FIRST TREATED WITH 
CO, SEA-WATER AND SUBSEQUENTLY FERTILIZED 


Description of the Experiments 


Since it was not known whether the eggs were capable of 
fertilization after removal from the CO, sea-water, some pre- 
liminary work was necessary in order to determine, first, whether 
it was possible to fertilize such eggs, and, second, the optimum 
time for such fertilization after the eggs had been removed from 
the CO, sea-water. 

A set of eggs was subjected immediately after the appearance 
of the first polar body, to the action of CO, sea-water for six 
minutes and then transferred to a large dish of sea-water and 
used as stock from which at five minute intervals during a period 
of two hours, eggs were taken and treated with active sperm. 

A series of these eggs (24 stages in all), each stage taken ten 
minutes after the addition of the sperm, was fixed for sectioning 
and the remainder of each stage were allowed to continue their 
development. The study of these sections is now but partially 
completed. 

Some data of this work may be of interest. I give brief notes 
of observations on the stock and four stages from the twenty-four 
which were under observation. 

Stock—Eggs remained in sea-water for one hour and eleven 
minutes when the first polar body was seen pushing out from 
the surface of the majority of the eggs (9:15—-10:26). 

(1) Eggs subjected to CO, sea-water for 8 minutes (10:26— 
10:34) 

(2) 11:15, still but one polar body. 

‘(3) 11:30, two polar bodies and irregular membrane. 

(4) 3:00, ten per cent in two-cell stage. 

Stage A—Stock eggs treated with sperm at 10:40 (six minutes 
after removal from CO, sea-water). 

(1) 10:43, fertilization membrane distinct. 

(2) 11:35, two polar bodies, one inside, one outside of fertili- 
zation membrane. 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 531 


(3) 12:00, beginning two-cell stage. 

(4) 3:00, twenty per cent past eight-cell stage. 

Stage C—Stock eggs treated with sperm at 10:50 (sixteen 
minutes after removal from CO, sea-water). 

(1) 12:08, beginning two-cell stage. 

(2) 3:00, twenty per cent past eight-cell stage. 

Stone D—Stock eggs treated with sperm at 10:55 (twenty-one 
minutes after removal from CO, sea-water). 

(I) 12:10, some segmented. 

(2) 3:00, thirty-five per cent segmented. 

Stage H—Stock eggs treated with sperm at 11:15 (forty-one 
minutes after removal from CO, sea-water.) 

(1) 12:15, beginning two-cell stage. 

(2) 3:00, thirty per cent eight-cell stage. 

In A eggs began to divide one hour and twenty minutes after 
fertilization. 

In B eggs began to divide one hour and eighteen minutes after 
fertilization. 

In D eggs began to divide one hour and fifteen minutes after 
fertilization. 

In H eggs began to divide one hour after fertilization. 

In this experiment the highest percentage of dividing eggs was 
obtained from those treated with sperm between twenty and 
forty-five minutes after removal from CQO, sea-water. After 
11:15 through the four succeeding stages 11:20, 11:25, 11:30, 
11:35, the percentage of eggs dividing in approximately one 
hour after fertilization rapidly diminished, being practically 
zero at 11:35 (5 minutes after the second polar body had been 
given off in the CO, stock). 

In all of the succeeding stages, segmentation began between 
2:20 and 2:40 or roughly four hours after treatment with CO,,. 

From these observations it seemed possible to conclude that eggs 
treated with CO, sea-water were capable of fertilization during 
the hour immediately succeeding removal from CO, sea-water. 
After that time had passed the spermatozoa, although still exceed- 
ingly active, were either not able to enter the egg or entering it 
produced no effect and the eggs proceeded to their parthen- 


532 D. H. Tennent and M. f. Hogue 


ogenetic division. Fresh spermatozoa were tried but without 
avail. 

For the sake of comparison, a table showing the percentage of 
eggs developing from a set allowed to mature for one hour and 
forty minutes (10:00-11:40), in sea-water (until the extrusion 
of the first polar body), and then fertilized at the intervals noted, 
is given. These eggs were not treated with CO, but were simply 
fertilized with sperm. 

Fertilized 11:40, ninety-five per cent segmented. 

Fertilized 12:03, ninety-five per cent segmented. 

Fertilized 12:25, ninety per cent segmented. 

Fertilized 1:40, thirty per cent segmented. 

Fertilized 3:00, no eggs segmented. 

Fertilized 4:00, no eggs segmented. 

In this set of normal eggs a considerable percentage was 
capable of fertilization for more than two hours after the extru- 
sion of the first polar body. 

Comparison of these sets of results, then, show that apparently 
treatment of the eggs with CO, sea-water shortened the time 
during which the sperm might be effective. 

From the observations mentioned it was thus found that the 
most normal results were obtained by fertilizing the CO, eggs 
from twenty to forty minutes after their removal from the CO, 
sea-water. These eggs when successfully fertilized commenced 
their segmentation in approximately one hour and thirty minutes 
after fertilisation: The ‘treatment with sperm shortened the 
time elapsing between the treatment with CO, sea-water and 
the beginning of segmentation, or, stating it more concretely : 

The time usually required for segmentation of CO, eggs was 
about four hours from the extrusion of the first polar body. 

The time required for the beginning of segmentation of CO, 
eggs subsequently fertilized was about one and one-half hours. 

The eggs that had been treated with CO, and subsequently 
fertilized gained in time, then, two and one-half hours. ~ 

In work with other lots it was found that the greatest percentage 
of fertilized eggs was to be obtained, if the CO, eggs were treated 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 528 


with sperm twenty or thirty minutes after removal from the 
CO, sea-water. 

Several sets of eggs were thus treated and from these, series 
were fixed which have served as the basis of the cytological study. 

It is of considerable interest to notice that the thick vitelline 
membrane pushed out from the egg, did not prevent the entrance 
of the spermatozoan, and that in eggs fertilized after the appear- 
ance of this membrane, a second membrane, somewhat thinner 
than the first, was formed, so that the eggs were surrounded by 
concentric membranes. 

In several cases, the actual passage of the spermatozoan through 
the outer membrane was observed, while in another lot of par- 
thenogenetic eggs which were treated with sperm when in the 
two- and four-cell stages, the spermatozoa were seen swimming 
actively between the blastomeres. 

The sections of these eggs have not been studied so that it 1s 
impossible to state whether or not the spermatozoa entered the 
blastomeres, or their effect on subsequent developments. 


The Study of Sections of the Eggs 


The facts determined by the study of sections of eggs treated 
with CO, sea-water and later fertilized, show that the processes 
occurring are so like the normal that a detailed description is 
unnecessary. I shall, therefore,content myself witha description 
of the figures and later make a comparison between the facts 
brought out by Miss Hogue in her study of the parthenogenetic 
development and those determined in my own study of the eggs 
which have received the double treatment. 

In some of the eggs sectioned, the first polar body was in process 
of formation. Here, there seemed absolute evidence of the long1- 
tudinal division of bivalent chromosomes. 

Fig. 30 shows a section through the remains of the first polar 
spindle immediately after the extrusion of the first polar body. 
In the polar body and in the egg the dumb-bell-shaped bivalent 
chromosomes are seen (Fig. 31). In some cases a tetrad effect 
(Fig. 32) due, I believe, to the grouping of the double chromo- 


somes was to be seen. 


534 D. H. Tennent and M. ‘f. Hogue 


In some cases the chromosomes of the second polar body 
remained within the egg (Fig. 33), clustered beneath the first 
polar body where they apparently degenerated, since in slightly 
later stages they were seen to be broken into small fragments, and 
in still later stages a few slightly stained granules were found in 
this position. ‘There was absolutely no evidence that as chromo- 
somes they took any part in subsequent nuclear transformations. 

The best evidence shows that in the second maturation division 
the bivalent chromosomes are divided to form univalent chromo- 
somes (Figs. 34-37) as has been described by Mathews (’95). 
The division is transverse. I have been unable to find evidence 
of the double longitudinal division which has been described by 
Bryce (02) for Echinus esculentus. 

In many cases abnormalities occur (Figs. 38-42). In some 
cases tripolar spindles are seen, in which event it seems possible 
that both polar bodies are being formed in the same division. 

The chromosomes remaining within the egg soon form five or 
six vesicles which are drawn together and fuse to form the female 
pronucleus which lies surrounded by some of the degenerating 
fibers of the second polar spindles (Figs. 44, 48, 49 and. 59). 
Meantime the sperm nucleus, which has become vesicular, has 
been moving, accompanied by its aster toward the egg nucleus, the 
centrosome and aster dividing as the two approach ( Figs. 45 and 
46), the two nuclei finally fusing to form the segmentation nucleus. 

In a very few cases the egg nucleus seems to be provided with 
an aster of its own (Fig 44). In most cases it seems simply lying 
among some of the fibers remaining from the second polar spindle. 

The segmentation nucleus remains in a resting condition for 
some time, during which the progression of the asters to opposite 
sides of the nucleus may be observed. From their inner sides 
fibers may be seen projecting into the nuclear membrane (Fig. 58). 
and in slightly later stages these fibers may be seen within the 
nucleus although the membrane seems intact. ‘The nucleus has 
meantime become decidedly elongated in outline. 

Succeeding this stage the conditions represented in Figs. 59- 
61 rapidly succeed one another. The nuclear membrane appar- 
ently dissolves first at the poles and finally dissolves throughout, 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 535 


during which process the achromatic figure increases greatly in 
size. 

The chromatic reticulum and the nucleolus break down into 
coarse threads which in succeeding stages becomes finer, these 
threads ultimately becoming broken up into short rods. (Figs. 54— 
56). These rods become rounded (Fig. 57) and during the for- 
_ mation of the equatorial plate are replaced by bivalent structures 
(Figs. 62-68), which lie with their long axis at right angles to the 
long axis of the spindle. 

All of the observations point to a conjugation of rounded unt- 
valent chromosomes to form elongated bivalent chromosomes. 

In Figs. 54 and 55 the chromosomes are seen to be rod-like. 
In Fig. 57 the form has changed, all of the chromosomes having 
‘become rounded, a few showing the bivalent form. In Figs. 62 
and 63, 66 and 67, it is seen that the bivalent form has become 
more common while the actual number of chromosomes has 
diminished, while in Fig. 68, which represents all of the chromo- 
somes of the equatorial plate, it is seenthat the number of chromo- 
somes has been still farther reduced and that with the exception 
of six univalent chromosomes all of the chromosomes show the 
bivalent form. 

These chromosomes retain their bivalent form during the seg- 
mentation divisions (Figs. 64 and 65 and 61 and 69). In Figs. 
61 and 60 it is seen that some of the chromosomes are still of the 
univalent form. ‘This was true of all of the segmentation stages 
examined. 

The division of the chromosomes in the segmentation is longi- 
tudinal, the chromosomes which are at first placed with their 
long axes at right angles to the long axis of the spindle becoming 
pulled out so that their long axes lie parallel to that of the spindle 
(Figs. 64, 65 and 53). 

The daughter nuclei are formed by the fusion of chromosomal 
vesicles. 

Granules of chromatin are thrown out into the cytoplasm from 
the time of the breaking down of the nuclear membrane preced- 
ing segmentation until the formation of the daughter nuclei is 
completed. 


536 D. H. Tennent and M. ‘fF. Hogue 


Further reference will be made to some of these changes in the 
general considerations at the end of this paper. 


III. EGGS FERTILIZED AND SUBSEQUENTLY TREATED WITH CO, 


In these eggs the sperm was allowed to act for ten minutes 
and the eggs were then transferred to CO, sea-water for five 
minutes. I give the data of but one lot. 

Fertilized eggs placed in CO, sea-water (12 :28—12:33). 

No eggs segmented until 3:40 or three hours and seven minutes 
from the time of removal from the CO, sea-water. 

Since the sections of these eggs have not yet been studied it is 
impossible to say definitely whether the eggs developed as a 
result of the CO, treatment or as a result of fertilization by the 
sperm. 

It is probable that since segmentation began within three hours 
after the eggs were removed from the CO, sea-water the sperm 
fertilization was effective, in which event the action of the CO, 
sea-water was simply to delay development. 


SUMMARY 


The observations on the behavior of eggs treated with CO, may 
be summarized as follows: 

1 Unfertilized eggs subjected to the action of CO, sea-water 
for from 3 to 10 minutes commenced segmentation about four 
hours after treatment. 

2 (a) Unfertilized eggs subjected to the action of CO, sea- 
water for from three to ten minutes and fertilized twenty to 
thirty minutes after removal from the CO, sea-water began seg- 
mentation about one hour after fertilization. 

(b) Unfertilized eggs subjected to the action of CQ, sea- 
water for from three to ten minutes when treated with sperm 
one hour or more after removal from the CO, sea-water segmented 
four hours after removal from the CO, sea-water. 

3 Fertilized eggs subjected to the action of CO, sea-water 
segmented three hours after removal from the CO, sea-water. 


A 


The Dev lopment of the Starfis cb ik Egg 537 
GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS ° 


As to the cause of the parthenogenetic development of eggs 
treated with CO,, whether it is by reason of a change of osmotic 
pressure, or because of agitation, or of the exposure to some 
specific chemical substance, or is due to the presence in the sea- 
water of new compounds formed in reactions which may take 
place between the CO, or impurities which may be present in the 
materials used and substances present in sea-water, this paper 
has nothing to say. 

The simple fact remains that eggs treated with CO, as has been 
described, segment regularly and develop into embryos which in 
form and structure cannot be distinguished from embryos obtained 
from fertilized eggs. 

Whether the reagent acts as a stimulus, or a shock, or a poison, 
the author cannot say. The evidence, however, is sufficient to 
show that Delage was correct in his view that maturation is 
arrested temporarily by the action of the CO,,. 

It is in the comparison of sections of CO, eggs with those 
of CO, eggs which were subsequently fertilized and with those 
of normally fertilized eggs that the most interesting facts come 
to light. All behave in essentially the same manner in the 
maturation processes, these processes apparently agreeing with 
those described by Mathews (’05) but which he has not figured 
in detail. In the material studied, which it is to be remembered 
must be regarded as having received somewhat artificial treat- 
ment from the beginning, the number of chromosomes remaining 
in the egg, after the extrusion of the second polar body, varies 
slightly, the number ranging from eighteen to twenty-three, 
although eighteen seems to be the common number. 

It is seen that the maturation processes in both the CO, eggs 
and in the CO, eggs which were subsequently fertilized, differ 
remarkably from those observed in compressed eggs by King 
(06). Here, although the first polar body may be extruded, the 
second is retained, all of the retained chromatin going into the 
formation of one or several vesicles which unite to form the egg 
nucleus, but “the retention of chromatin that is normally extruded 


538 D. H. Tennent and M. F. Hogue 


in the polar bodies does not lead to a parthenogenetic develop- 
ment of the egg. ” In compressed eggs which were fertilized, 
great differences in size in the two pronuclei were noted, a phenom- 
enon which does not occur in CO, eggs subsequently fertilized. 
The polyspermy observed in fertilized compressed eggs has been 
noted in CO, eggs subsequently fertilized which did not receive 
the treatment which results in normal development. 

It is worthy of note again that in the CO, eggs which were sub- 
sequently fertilized and in ordinarily fertilized eggs the chromo- 
somes during the maturation divisions are bivalent (Mathews, 
double chromosomes) and at the close of the maturation divisions 
eighteen univalent (Mathews, seventeen [ ! | single) chromocnni 
remain within the egg. 

It will be noticed that the straight CO, treatment ou has 
some influence on the maturation processes, for while the number 
of chromosomes remains the same as in the fertilized eggs, the 
shape of the chromosomes varies. 

‘The process of reconstruction of the egg nucleus is by the forma- 
tion and fusion of chromosomal vesicles in eggs of the three 
classes mentioned. 

The appearance of the segmentation nucleus in the CO, eggs 
and in the CO, eggs which were subsequently fertilized shows no 
difference. ‘This nucleus breaks down in the same manner in 
both cases, the nuclear reticulum becomes threadlike and breaks 
up into fragments, the mass of chromatic material being the same, 
so far as the eye can judge, in the one case as in the other. 

In neither case can the exact number of fragments be counted, 
but it is clearly to be seen that they are not to be regarded as 
individual chromosomes since the numbers counted are more 
than double those found at any earlier or later stages when the 
count may be made with reasonable certainty, and since many of 
these fragments may be seen in various stages of withdrawal into 
the cytoplasm. 

But at the time of the completion of the equatorial plate, funda- 
mental differences between the chromosomes of the CO, eggs and 
the eggs which were subsequently fertilized may be seen. In the 
one case the chromosomes are irregular and in the other, of a 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 539 


distinct bivalent form; a form which is preserved during the longi- 
tudinal splitting of the chromosomes as it divides to form the 
daughter nuclei, a form which persists in the later divisions of the 
egg so far as observed and a form which is again seen in the 
maturation divisions until the final separation of bivalent into 
univalent chromosomes during the formation of the second polar 
body. 

These facts, it seems to me, are to be explained only by the 
suggestion that a conjugation or synapsis of egg chromosomes 
and sperm chromosomes takes place immediately before the 
formation of the equatorial plate of the first segmentation spindle. 

That this process takes place seems very probable when, after 
comparing Figs. 19 and 20, representing sections through the 
equatorial plate in CO, eggs, with Figs. 54, 56 and 57, repre- 
senting sections through the equatorial plate of CO, + sperm eggs, 
one follows the series of changes shown in Figs. 62, 63, 66, 
67 and 68, in which the rounded chromosomes are shown, some 
lying free, some drawn closely together in pairs and still others 
showing the completed bivalent form. ‘This form is retained 
from the equatorial plate stage (Fig. 68), through the division 
hgures ( Figs. 64, 65, 69, 61) of the CO, + sperm eggs and may be 
seen in the = ee figures of the normally fertilized eggs ( Figs. 
28 and 29). 

As a result of such a conjugation the number of chromosomes 
would remain the same in the parthenogenetic as in the fertilized 
eggs, with the difference that in the parthenogenetic eggs there 
remain eighteen univalent chromosomes and in the fertilized eggs 
eighteen bivalent chromosomes.' 


1During the summer of 1906 the experiments which have been described in this paper were repeated 
and some further observations made. Inthis work it was demonstrated satisfactorily that the equa- 
torial plate in the eggs from some individuals contained, within slight variations, thirty-six chromo- 
somes. The Woods Hole starfishes thus show a variation as to the number of chromosomes similar to 
that pointed out in Echinus microtuberculatus by Stevens (’o2). In such eggs as in the eggs that have 
been considered in this paper the number of chromosomes remains the same in both fertilized and in 
parthenogenetic eggs. A point of disagreement between these eggs and those obtained during the pre- 
vious summer is that dumb-bell-shaped chromosomes were found in parthenogenetic eggs. Little de- 
pendence, therefore, as has been suggested by several authors recently, can be placed upon the shape 


of the chromosomes as an indication of valency. 


540 D. H. Tennent and M. F. Hogue 


It is possible that the objection will be made that the activity 
of the CO, sea-water is such as to change the shape of the chromo- 
somes in the equatorial plate of the first segmentation spindle, 
since we have seen that the shape of the chromosomes in the 
maturation divisions was thus influenced. 

In replying to such an objection, it can only be pointed out that 
the number of chromosomes, although these chromosomes vary 
in size, shape, etc., remains apparently the same in both the CO, 
eggs and in the CO, eggs which were subsequently fertilized, and 
this in spite of the fact that we should expect that the number in 
the fertilized egg to be doubled. 

An additional point of interest comes out in the comparison of 
these eggs, and this in respect to the cleavage asters. “The 
cleavage asters with their centers have the same appearance in 
both the CO, eggs and in the CO, eggs which were subsequently 
fertilized. In eggs which were fertilized at the optimum time 
these were the only asters formed, while in eggs fertilized later 
than this time many cytasters and additional sperm asters were 
formed, the subsequent divisions being exceedingly abnormal. 
The study of these phenomena has been partly completed and 
its results will be submitted in a later contribution. 


Bryn Mawr College 
June, 1906 


The Development of the Starfish Egg 541 


LITERATURE 


Boveri, [H., ’04.—Ergebnisse iiber die Konstitution der Chromatischen Substanz 
des Zellkerns. Gustav Fischer. Jena, 1904. 
Bryce, T. H., ’02.—Maturation of the Ovum in Echinus esculentus. Quart. Jour. 
Mic. Sci., vol. xlvi. 
DeacE, YVES, ’o1.—Etudes Expérimentales sur la Maturation Cytoplasmique 
et sur la Parthénogénése artificielle chez les Echinoderms. Archiv. 
Zool. Exp. 3d series, vol. ix, 1901. 
’02.—Nouvelles Recherches sur la Parthénogénése Expérimentale chez 
Asterias glacialis. Archiv Zool. Exp. 3d series vol. x, 1902. 
’04.—Elevage des Larves Parthénogénétiques D’Asterias  glacialis. 
Archiv Zool. Exp. 4th series, vol. ii, p. 27-42. 1904, 
’04.—La Parthénogénése par L’ Acide Carbonique obtenu chez les Oeufs 
aprés l’émission des Globules Polaires. Archiv Zool. Exp. 4th 
series, vol. 11, p. 43-46. 
’05.—Nouvelles Expériences de Parthénogénése Expérimentale. Archiv 
Zool. Exp. 4th series, vol. iii, Notes et Revue, clxiv-clxviii. 
KostaneckI, K., ’04.—Cytologische Studien an kiinstlich parthenogenetisch sich 
entwickelnden Eiern von Mactra. Archiv f. Mikr. Anat., vol. xiv. 
Kina, H. D., ’06.—The Effects of Compression on the Maturation and Early 
Development of the Eggs of Asterias forbesii. Archiv fiir Entwick. 
der Organismen, vol. xxi. 
LEFEVRE, GEORGE, ’05.—Artificial Parthenogenesis in Thalassemamellita. Science 
P- 379» 1905- 
- ’06.—Further Observations on Artificial Parthenogenesis. Science, pp. 
522-524, 1906. 
Morean, T. H., ’96.—The Fertilization of non-nucleated Fragments of Echino- 
derm Eggs. Archiv fiir Entwick. der Organismen, vol. ii. 
'99.—Action of Salt Solutions on the Unfertilized and Fertilized Eggs 
of Arbacia and of other Animals. Archiv fiir Entwick. der Orga- 
nismen, vol. vill. 
Scott, J. M., ’06.—Morphology of the Parthenogenetic Development of Amphi- 
trite. Jour. Exp. Zodl., vol. ii. 
Stevens, N. M., ’02.—Experimental Studies on Eggs of Echinus microtubercu- 
latus. Archiv fiir Entwick. der Organismen, vol. xv. 
Witson, E. B., ’01.—Experimental Studies in Cytology. A Cytological Study of 
Artificial Parthenogenesis in Sea Urchin Eggs. Archiv fiir Ent- 
wick. der Organismen, vol. xii. 
Witson, FE. B., and Matuews, A. P., ’95.—Maturation, Fertilization and Polarity 
in the Echinoderm Egg. Jour. Morph., vol. x. 


DESCRIPTION OF PLATES 


The drawings were all made from camera lucida sketches. Zeiss compensating ocular 12, with 2 mm. 


oil immersion objective, giving a magnification of 1500 diameters, were used for all of the figures except 


those hereinafter noted. All of the drawings are reduced one-half in reproduction. 


Fig. 1. 


Fig. 2 


Fig. 


egg. 


Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 


(Figs 1-27 from co, eggs) 


Prate I 


First polar body formed. Remains of degenerating spindle. Eighteen chromosomes present. 


. Later stage of degenerating spindle. Eighteen chromosomes. 
. Complete disappearance of spindle. Additional chromosomes were in next section of the 


4. Chromosomes remaining on a half spindle. 

5. Same as Fig. 4, except that the chromosomes have divided. 
6. 
7 
8 


Second polar spindle tangential to surface of egg. 


. Radial position of second polar spindle as seen in later stages. 
. Second polar spindle with eighteen chromosomes. 


Figs. 9 and 10. Early and late anaphase of second polar division. 


Fig. 11. Two polar bodies given off; chromatin lying free in cytoplasm. 


Fig. 12. Formation of female pronucleus through fusion of vesicles. 


Fig. 13. Female pronucleus preparing to divide; chromosomes and fibers appearing. 


Fig. 14. Late prophase of first segmentation division. 


Figs. 15 and 16. Early prophase of first segmentation. Ocular 4, 2 mm. oil immersion objective. 


ee 


DEVELOPMENT OF THE STARFISH EGG PLATE I 
D. H. Tennent anv M. J. Hocur 


16 


Tue Journat or ExperiMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL, III, NO. 4 M J.H. del. 


Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 
Fig. 


Prate II 


17. Anaphase of first segmentation. 

18. Vesicle formation preceding formation of daughter nuclei. 

1g. Equatorial plate of Fig. 24. 

20. Equatorial plate of Fig. 21. 

21. Section of entire egg through equatorial plate. Ocular 4, 2 mm. oil immersion objective 
22. Formation of daughter nuclei. 

23. Metaphase of second segmentation. 

24. Section of entire egg with chromosomes of equatorial plate divided in first segmentation. 


Ocular 4, 2 mm. oil immersion objective. 


Fig. 
Fig. 


25. Section through equatorial plate of second segmentation spindle. 
26. Second polar body retained within the egg. Nucleus dividing. Ocular 4, 2 mm. oil immer- 


sion objective. 


DEVELOPMENT OF THE STARFISH EGG 


PLATE II 
D.H. TENNENT AND M. J. Hocure 


Tue JourNat or ExperiMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL, III, NO. 4 


Pirate IIT 


Fig. 27. Second segmentation, slightly earlier than Fig. 23. . 
Figs. 28 and 29. Adjoining sections of normally fertilized egg in anaphase of first segmentation. 
(Figs 30-69 from CO, eggs which were subsequently fertilized) 

Fig. 30. Section through portion of egg immediately after extrusion of first polar body. 

Fig. 31. Chromosomes lying free in cytoplasm. 

Fig. 32. Tetrad effect due to grouping of bivalent chromosomes. 

Fig. 33. Second polar body not extruded. Chromosomes of second polar body lying in cytoplasm 
beneath the first polar body. 

Fig. 34. Second polar spindle. Chromosomes in process of transverse division. 

Figs. 35 and 36. Adjoining sections through second polar spindle. Chromosomes in transverse 
division. 

Fig. 37. Second polar spindle. Chromosomes separated into univalent elements. 

Fig. 38. Univalent chromosomes remaining in egg. 

Fig. 39. Section through long axis of second polar spindle which was lying tangentially to surface 
of egg. 

Figs. 40 and 41. Sections through long axis of multipolar spindle. 


Fig. 42. Three sections in series through chromosomes remaining in egg after.extrusion of second: 


polar body. 


DEVELOPMENT OF THE STARFISH EGG PLATE III 
D.H.TEnNeEnT anv M. J. Hocure 


\ 
\ < =e) 
\ - # eles 
\ . -" Sa 
\ Pa ~ - oe 
- < - =_~ 
\ ~ < x e . 
\ 7 Co ee 
~ a ba = 
~ — . = D 
2 —— = a - Ss ba} 
~ a 2-3 Glin = 2 
pe Ur eee 28 
a = : : 
= ue 
a ~ 
—— Oe SS 
\ 7 Jae > —— 
\ oo onl 
\ ——_—_—____sa__ —_-—--+_ 
\ = A 
27 Os 
- oe 
= Sifies z 


/ 


39 
e 
t ) 
ys J 
gh e C4 : 
oe e eee 
@ 
40 oun 4D 


21 


Tue JourNat or ExPeRiNENTAL ZO6LOGY, VoL. Ill, NO, 4 D.H.T. del. 


Fig. 43. 


Prate IV 


Second polar spindle formed near center of egg. 


Fig. 44. Female pronucleus formed by fusion of chromosomal vesicles. Part of second polar 
spindle lying in cytoplasm beneath first polar body. 


Fig. 45. 
Fig. 46. 
Fig. 47. 
Fig. 48. 


Male and female pronuclei. Aster divided. 

Male and female pronuclei. Centrosome divided. 

Segmentation nucleus. 

Male pronucleus approaching partially formed female pronucleus. Ocular 4, 2 mm. oil 


immersion objective. 


Fig. 49. 
Fig. 50. 
Bigs 51. 
membrane. 
Fig. 52. 
Fig. 53. 


Same as Fig. 48. 

Vesicles which are to fuse and form female pronucleus. 

Segmentation nucleus elongated; a chromatic fibers are to be seen within the nuclear 
Ocular 4, 2 mm. oil immersion objective. 

Male pronucleus. 

Portion of first segmentation spindle. 


Figs. 54 and 55. Adjoining sections showing thread broken into short, rod-like segments. 


Fig. 56. 
Fig. 57. 


somes, 


Part of section through nuclear material. Three bivalent chromosomes present. 
Rod-shaped chromosomes rounded up into spherical structures. Some bivalent chromo- 


DEVELOPMENT OF THE STARFISH EGG PLATE IV 
D.H. Tennent anv M, J. Hocur 


Tue JourNat or ExperiMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. 111, NO. 4 D. H. T. del. 


Pirate V 


Figs. 58, 59 and 60. Stages in breaking up of segmentation nucleus and of formation of first 
segmentation spindle. 

Fig. 61. Anaphase of first segmentation. 

Figs. 62 and 63. Equatorial plates. Univalent chromosomes pairing. 

Fig. 64 and 65. Adjoining sections of chromosomes of same spindle showing chromosomes drawn 
out in first segmentation division. 

Fig. 66. Equatorial plate. Chromosomes pairing. 

Fig. 67. Same as Fig. 66. 

Fig. 68. Equatorial plate just before division, nearly all of the chromosomes being of bivalent 
form. 


Fig. 69. Late anaphase of first segmentation. Same as Fig. 61. 


PLATE V 


D. H. T.del, 


DEVELOPMENT OF THE STARFISH EGG 


D. H. TENNENT AND M, J. Hocue 


s 
e y $ Hay ° 
1% ads vl Magy 
- or 45: A ‘ : 
ca ode tos ot ‘ate uy 
fetes V Rae 203" 
Ey Sieeges ania pent 
: © . b - co vay 2 
EN 
[if Ay a \ 
oy soe fe) 
eee 1-0) 
rs) A o ise / © 
\ 5 
pay, Ao tom pice 
See its aaah rae 
eee st: 
Mm, Ra 
wt 
© © 


THe JourNAL OF ExPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. III, NO. 4 


. 


io ; 
AS; i in iy _ 


From the Anatomical Laboratory of the Johns Hopkins University 


SOME EXPERIMENTS ON THE DEVELOPING EAR 
VE Wee OF FHE TADPOLE WITH RELATION 
TO LOUMIBRATION: 


BY 
GEORGE L. STREETER, M.D. 


Associate, Wistar Institute of Anatomy 


With Twe ve Ficures 


The eventual object of the experiments reported in the following 
paper was the rearing of some tadpoles which had been deprived 
of their auditory vesicle and acoustic ganglion, either on one side 
alone or on both sides; that is to say, an artificial production of a 
unilateral and bilateral absence of the acoustic apparatus. ‘This 
was done in the expectation that it might be possible to trace the 
central acoustic path, in this new way, and perhaps throw further 
light upon its course and relations. ‘The absence of these sense 
organs, however, produced such definite abnormalities in the 
behavior of the growing larve and in the development of their swim- 
ming «bilities that it became at once apparent that I was dealing 
with valuable evidence in respect to their function and its bearing 
on the mechanism of equilibrium. It is, therefore, deemed advisable 
to restrict the following paper to the physiological features of these 
experiments, and reserve the study of the central nervous system 
of the reared specimens for a later communication. 

What we already know concerning the function of the vertebrate 
ear is based principally on experimental sectioning or stimula- 


tion of the semicircular canals, or the nerves to their ampullz, in 
adult birds and fishes? 


‘Read in part before the Section of Anatomy of the British Medical Association, at the meeting held 
in Toronto, August 21-25, 1906. 

? For experimental work on fishes we are for the most part indebted to Lee (’93 and °98) and Lyon 
(00), both of whom carried on their experiments at the Woods Hole Laboratories. Further work on 
fishes has just been completed at the same place by Professor Parker, whose paper I am told is now in 
press and will appear in the Bulletin of the U, S. Fisheries Bureau. An abstract of part of his 
work was read before the American Zodlogical Society (Parker, ’o5). A voluminous literature exists 
concerning experiments on higher vertebrates, particularly the pigeon, but it need not be considered 
here. 


THE JourNAL oF ExPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. III, NO. 4. 


544 George L. Streeter 


The fact that it is possible to experiment on the embryo and to 
produce at will practically a congenital absence of thisorgan, besides 
serving as a control over the experiments on adult animals, intro- 
duces a direct advantage both as regards the ease with which the 
operation is performed and as regards its completeness and per- 
manence and freedom from injury of adjoining structures, the latter 
point beingof particular importance to those who are still in doubt 
as to how much 1s due in the experiments on adults to injuries and 
stimulationsassociated with the operation and how much is purely 
the result of the cessation of the stimuli which normally originate in 
thelabyrinth. Furthermore, since the labyrinth is removed during 
the early formative period at a time when it may be presumed that 
the various organs possess their greatest adaptability, it will be seen 
that such embryonic interference affords a most complete test 
of the power of functional compensation on the part of other 
organs. 


Behavior of Normal T ad poles 


In analyzing the behavior of operated specimens it was found 
necessary to make a preliminary study of control tadpoles, in order 
to determine the normal development of motor reflexes and their 
coordination and the consequent establishment of equilibrium. 
This was done by removing the larve from their gelatinous cap- 
sule shortly after fertilization and following their development in 
tap water. In this way it was seen that in the process of learning 
to swim they pass through three periods, which may be named as 
follows: 


1. Stage of non-motility, first three days. 
2. Stage of spinal reflexes, fourth to sixth days. 
3. Stage of equilibrium, sixth day to maturity. 


The first stage, with a favorable temperature, lasts from the 
time of fertilization to the third or fourth day. During this time 
the larve, aside from the movement due to cilia, lie motionless on 
their side on the bottom of the dish and do not respond to stimuli. 
The second stage begins at the time when they first respond to 


2 


Experiments on the Developing Ear Vesicle 545 


mechanical stimuli by flexion of the body and tail. These reac- 
tions consist of simple motor reflexes at first, but they soon become 
combined and coordinated so that by a series of such body flexions 
they are able to wiggle rapidly forward on the bottom of the dish. 
This manner of progression evidently consists entirely of spinal 
cord reflexes and is not controlled by higher centers. In order to 
perform it, it is necessary for the tadpoles to touch the bottom or 


Fic. I 


Fic. 2 


Fig. 1, Outline drawing of normal tadpole (Rana sylvatica) of the second stage or stage of spinal 
reflexes. Enlarged 8 diameters. 

Fig.2, Outline drawing of normal tadpole (Rana sylvatica) at the beginning of the third stage. This 
specimen had the power of equilibration, although sections of the ear vesicle showed that the 
development of the semicircular canals was not yet complete. Enlarged 8 diameters. 


side of the dish; when they are driven up into the free water with 
a pipette, where there is no contact with solid objects, they make 
no effort at movement, but sink inertly to the bottom; on striking 
the bottom they run forward again. ‘The third stage begins when 
they are first able to move freely about without touching solid 


1T have been informed by Dr. R. G. Harrison that it is just at this time that the motor nerve 
roots make their appearance, and this may determine the onset of the second stage. According to 
his observations the power of muscle contraction follows almost immediately after the development 
of the motor roots; but it never precedes their development, as is maintained by some. He has 


found the motor root present in specimens that had not yet moved. 


546 George L. Streeter 


objects. At this time a new controlover their movements is devel- 
oped, in virtue of which they become able to leave the bottom of the 
dish and swim up into free water with maintenance of what may 
then be called equilibrium. ‘The form of the tadpole during the 
latter part of the first stage 1s shown in Fig. 3. The second and 
third stages are shown in Figs. 1 and 2. 

The correlation between the histological development of the 
labyrinth and the development of the power of equilibrium was 
studied by selecting specimens of the second and the beginning of 
the third stages, carefully noting their behavior, and then cutting 
them in serial sections.! 

From these series it could be seen that shortly before the animal 
enters the stage of equilibrium the labyrinth consists of a closed 
epithelial sac incompletely subdivided into compartments and 
possessing differentiated nerve endings which are connected with 
the brain by the acoustic nerve and ganglion. ‘That at least one 
such apparatus is essential for equilibrium will be seen when I 
describe the behavior of tadpoles that have been completely 
deprived of the same. As regards the semicircular canals it 1s a 
different matter; they can already be seen in the process of 
development, but are not completely pocketed off until after equili- 
brium is already established. Consequently the semicircular canals 
as such are not an essential factor in equilibration. 


Method of Operation 


Larve of Rana sylvatica measuring about 3 mm. long were 
selected as being most suitable for the operation. ‘Their general 
form at this time 1s shown in Fig. 3. There is a distinct tail bud, 
and on the head the eminences caused by the optic cup and head 
ganglia are visible. ‘he structure that is to form the future laby- 
rinth is situated just dorsal to the ganglionic eminence and is shown 


The correlation between the histogenesis of organs and the development of their functional activity 
forms a fruitful field which has been explored by comparatively few investigators. It may be approached 
both through ontogeny and phylogeny. Prentiss (o1) by this means worked out important facts 
regarding the crustacean otocyst. Many details concerning the verte brate ear which do not belong to 


the scope of the present paper could doubtless be learned in the same way. 


Ex periments on the Developing Ear Vesicle 547 


in Fig. 3 by the mark +. It consists of a cup-shaped mass of 
cells (auditory cup) which have differentiated themselves from 
the deeper layer of epidermis, and are just 


in the process of closing in at the edges to 


a 


form the completed ear vesicle. In size eo 
this ear cup or ear vesicle is about one- : 
half that of the optic cup. 

For performing the operation it 1s not 
necessary to anesthetize the specimen as 
itis stillin the non-motile stage and does aiaicemor cone 
not respond to stimulation. After remov- _ operation, just at the end of the non- 
inethe lakvatromiits eelatinous capsule it motlestage. The tail budis present 
is placed under a binocular microscope Sele oe eS 

5 nences due to the optic cup and head 
and anincision made nearthe place indi- ganglia. Above the latter is the 
cated in Fig.3. The edge of the incision _ point of operation shown by a cross. 
is then raised a little and the auditory cup PE 
is picked out witha needle. After a little 
practice one learns to make the incision directly at the edge of the 
cup so that it comes away easily and intact, resembling somewhat 
a thimbleberry. Lying just in front of it is the acoustic ganglion 
which is not so sharply outlined. This is also removed and, 
in order to make sure that it is all taken out, the surrounding 
mesoderm is cleaned out as far in as the brain. Where but one 
vesicle is to be removed the operation is then complete, and the 
specimen is left to proceed in its development. The wound 
immediately closes of itself and heals in the course of a few hours 
leaving no trace of the operation. Where both sides are operated 
on, the same procedure is carried out on both sides. The ear 
vesicle never regenerates following complete removal. 

The ear vesicle was removed on one side from thirty specimens 
and on both sides from twenty specimens. ‘The animals were then 
kept under observation and their behavior recorded through the 
whole larval period and until the completion of metamorphosis. 
The following notes were selected from these records. 


Fig. 3. Outline drawing of Rana 


548 George L. Streeter 


Removal of One Ear Fesicle 


Twenty-four hours after operation: Specimens are 5.5 mm. long 
and show presence of gill buds. In appearance and behavior, no 
difference can be detected between them and normal tadpoles. 
They lie on their side and on stimulation flex their body, but make 
no attempt at swimming. 

Forty-eight hours after operation: Specimens are 7 mm. long, 
gills are branched and the blood can be seen circulating through 
them. In appearance and behavior they still show no departure 
from that seen in normal control specimens. While at rest they 
lie on theit side. On stimulation (sunlight, jarring the dish, or 
touching with needle) by a rapid flexion of the body and tail from 
side to side they swim forward, 5-10 cm., on the bottom of the 
dish in a straight or slightly curved line, and then come to rest on 
their side, and remain so until a new stimulation excites another 
such excursion. ‘Their course is directed either by the side or 
bottom of the dish. When forced up into free water the flexions 
stop and they sink inertly to the bottom. 

Third day after operation: Specimens average about 8 mm. 
long, abdominal epidermis differentiated from that of the dorsal 
parts of the body by being less pigmented. Appearance and be- 
havior is still practically normal. ‘They begin to show a tendency 
to assume the upright position while at rest, but no great impor- 
tance can be attached to this feature as throughout the early days 
of the tadpole period, preserved specimens lie in the same positions 
as living ones. ‘Their posture in water may be entirely determined 
by their body proportions. ‘Their movements remain of the spinal 
cord type seen on the previous day, the response being more prompt. 

Fourth day after operation: Specimens 9-9.5 mm. long. In 
appearance the operated specimens are the same as the normal 
ones, but in behavior they present a difference. ‘The normal ones 
still confine their movements to the bottom or side of the dish; 
when stirred up into free water, though most of them still roll about 
inertly, some of them are able to maintain a direct course. On the 
other hand the operated ones, as soon as they are driven from the 
bottom, swim in a spiral or circular manner as shown in the 


Experiments on the Developing Ear Vesicle 549 


accompanying Fig. 4. “The tendency is to swim with the operated 
side under, and in the rolling movements around the long axis of 
the body it is from the operated side under to the opposite. When 
these same specimens touch the bottom they are able to direct their 
course as on the previous two days. Evidently, a functional union 
is normally established at about this time between the ear vesicle 
and the spinal cord reflex centers, upon which the individual is 
dependent for maintaining its position in free water, and it 1s not 
until this occurs that the removal of the ear vesicle causes any 
symptoms. 


Fig. 4. Sketch showing three typical swimming movements made by specimens on the fourth day 
after removal of their left ear vesicle. 


Sixth day after operation: Specimens about 12 mm. long, and 
have commenced to nibble at food and pass faeces. The charac- 
teristic movements which first appeared on the fourth day have 
become stronger and stand out in more marked contrast to the 
behavior of normal specimens which at this time can swim easily 
up into free water with accurate maintenance of equilibrium. 

Seventh day after operation: ‘The operated specimens show 
distinct improvement in swimming ability; many of them are now 
able to maintain a fairly direct course in free water, but on excita- 
tion they renew the spirals and circles which characterized the 


fourth, fifth and sixth days. 


550 George L. Streeter 


Eighth day after operation: nearly all the specimens now 
swim freely and directly in all parts of the water, and irregularity 
of swimming is only elicited by excitement. 

Tenth day after operation: Swimming 1s practically normal. 
Their movements are under such control as to enable them to sup- 
port themselves in free water and nibble at floating stems and 
leaves. It can be seen, however, that in swimming they lean 


Fig. 5. Photograph of a frog whose left ear vesicle was removed when a tadpole 3 mm. long. The 
only asymmetry noticeable is the absence of the ear elevation on the left side normally caused by the 
labyrinth and its cartilaginous capsule; the lateral line on that side is straight from the eye back, while 


on the right or normal side it is deflected. The posture is normal. Enlarged 34 diameters. 
g gece 32 


slightly toward the operated side, a sympton which persists through- 
out their larval period. 

Twelfth day after operation: Specimens are normal as regards 
size, nourishment, and symmetry, except for the absence on the 
operated side of the elevation which is caused normally by the 
labyrinth and its cartilaginous capsule. In behavior they differ 
from the normal only in the slight leaning toward the operated side 


Experiments on the Developing Ear Vesicle 551 


and a momentary loss of equilibrium which can be elicited by 
excitement. 

Three months after operation: ‘The specimens passed through 
a normal metamorphosis at the end of the third month. A photo- 
graph made of one of them a few days after the completion of the 
process is shown in the accompanying Fig. 5. 

As long as they continued as swimming tadpoles the slight lean- 
ing toward the operated side persisted and it was possible through 
excitement to cause a momentary disturbance in equilibrium, but 
the latter became gradually more difhcult to demonstrate. As 
soon as they commenced to make use of their legs the character 
of the swimming changed; it then became a series of leg strokes 
instead of the sinuous flexions of the body and tail. After that it 
was no longer possible to detect the leaning toward the operated 
side; both when swimming and when at rest their behavior was to 
all appearance normal. When taken out of water they jumped 
normally and came to rest in a normal posture. When turned 
over on their backs they righted themselves promptly. 

The fact that the slight disturbance of equilibrium, which could 
be still detected in the tail-swimming tadpole, could no longer be 
seen in the leg-swimming frog, a change completed within four or 
five days, probably does not signify the cure of the condition, but 
rather that under the latter circumstances a slight defect 1s more 
difficult to recognize. ‘The corollary of this sould be that equili- 
brium in the swimming tadpole is a more delicately balanced 
mechanism than in the Ticking and jumping frog. 


Removal of Both Ear Vesicles 


During the first three days after the operation the appearance 
and behavior of these specimens are the same as seen in the normal 
ones, and in those from which one ear vesicle was removed. The 
response to stimuli 1s perhaps a trifle less prompt, but otherwise 
they could not be distinguished one from the other. 

Fourth day after operation: It was seen that in one-sided oper- 
ations the specimens commenced about this time to make excur- 


552 George L. Streeter 


sions into free water, and in doing it they departed from the nor- 
mal by swimming in spirals and circles. “Tadpoles with both ear 
vesicles taken out make no such excursions and show decidedly 
less activity. Occasionally they flex their body and tail from side 
to side producing a snapping effect which does not result in any 
forward progress. Like the other specimens they are, however, 
able to wiggle along in contact with the side and bottom of the 
dish. 

Seventh day after operation: The specimens are smaller and 
are retarded in development as compared with the normal and 
one-sided specimens. ‘They are, however, symmetrical in form 
and are normal as regards the appearance and movements of the 
eyes, mouth, heart and intestine. ‘They are decidedly less active 
and stimuli produce irregular attempts at swimming, sometimes 
somewhat spiral in character but usually nothing more than a 
series of awkward flexions of the body. ‘These flexions also 
occasionally occur with no apparent stimulus. They make a 
partially successful effort at nibbling on the bottom of the dish. 

Twelfth day after operation: Absolutely no improvement in 
swimming; any attempt at it results in a series of somersaults. 
they throw their body up into the water and then promptly sink 
to the bottom in almost any position. When at rest, they lie on 
their side, back, or normally on their belly, depending apparently 
on whether their intestine is filled with sand, etc., to properly 
balance the body. ‘The intestine is very apt to be empty because 
of the difficulty they experience in feeding. ‘They do not wiggle 
along on the bottom as well as they did on the fourth and fifth 
days. 

‘Two months after operation: The specimens could not be 
carried much beyond this point, the difficulty apparently being 
starvation from inability to wander around and collect food. 
Perhaps also the respiration was involved, for they were unable 
to go to the surface for oxygen as the normal tadpole does. 

In behavior they showno improvement. For the most part they 
lie stiff and inert in various positions on the bottom, and their 
occasional attempts at swimming have never developed into anything 
more successful than was described on the seventh and twelfth 


Experiments on the Development Ear. Vesicle 553 


days after operation. ‘Their appearance departs from the nor- 
mal principally in the small contracted character of the abdominal 
region. In volume they are about one-third as large as the normal 
specimen, varying from2.5 to 4.cm. in length. They have a hind 
leg bud 2.5 to 3 mm. long. As some of them commenced to 
die at this time the rest were put in preserving fluids for micro- 
scopial purposes. 

A summary of the above notes on the operated individuals may 
perhaps be best formulated by making the following comparison 
with the three stages of normal behavior. 

First stage: [he operation was performed during the latter 
part, while the animals were still non-motile. 

Second stage: During this period they behave exactly like 
normal specimens, both those having one vesicle removed and 
those that have been deprived of both vesicles. They respond to 
stimuli and learn to wiggle along in contact with the bottom of the 
dish in the normal manner. 

Third stage: It is at the beginning of this period that they 
depart fromthenormal. Itcanbe plainly seen from their conduct 
that something has happened to that controlling influence from 
above, which they require in order to leave the bottom and to swim 
and maintain their position in free water. In case but one ear 
vesicle is gone they swim in spirals, circles, or straight while roll- 
ing around their long axis. ‘This, however, lasts only a few days 
and then it is gradually overcome. From then on they swim 
almost perfectly; there may be a slight tilting toward the operated 
side and on excitement a momentary loss of equilibrium, but this 
would only be seen on careful examination. It is a different mat- 
ter where both labyrinths are absent; the animals in that case are 
completely and permanently incapacitated for swimming. ‘There 
Is no apparent sense of equilibrium and they never develop any. 
The animals were kept alive about two months, at the end of 
which time their movements were as irregular as at the beginning. 


Transplantation of Ear Vesicle After Bilateral Removal 


From the above experiments it became evident that a tadpole 
having but one labyrinth proceeds in its general growth and 


554 George L. Streeter 


develops swimming abilities about as wellas the normal animal; but 
specimens deprived of both ear vesicles never learn to swim and 
never develop any sense of equilibrium. ‘The next step was to 
see if it would be possible to remove both vesicles and atthe same 
time transplant one of them into a new position, having in mind 
the successful results obtained by Lewis (04) in transplantation of 
the optic cup. 

After that operation if the tadpole succeeded in developing 
equilibrium and the power of swimming then it would prove that 
a transplanted ear vesicle could establish new connections with the 
central nervous system and develop its normal functions; the ship 
would simply be sailing with its compass set up in a different place. 


Fig. 6. Tadpole showing elevation in front of eye caused by the transplanted left ear vesicle, the 
right ear vesicle having been entirely removed. Drawing made three months after operation. Enlarged 


4 diameters. 


The operation was one that could be performed without diff- 
culty. A tadpole about 3 mm. long is selected and the ear 
vesicle taken out on one side in the manner described above. ‘The 
specimen is then turned over and the opposite ear vesicle is 
uncovered and loosened from the epidermis. Before actually 
removing it a straight incision 1s made with scissors or needles in 
front of the eye and a pocket is created by gently spreading the sub- 
jacent mesoderm apart until the brain is exposed. The loosened 
ear vesicle is then lifted from its natural place and slipped into 
this pocket. If the incision is carefully made the edges of the 
wound close at once and on the following day there is no trace of 
the operation left. Nine operations of this kind were made and 
seven of the tadpoles successfully reared. While they were grow- 
ing it could be seen from a surface view that the transplanted 
vesicle was developing and causing a corresponding elevation in 


Ex periments on the Developing Ear Vesicle 555 


front of the eye. A sketch of one of these at the end of the third 
month is shown in Fig. 6. 

Their behavior during the first week following the operation was 
identical with that of specimens deprived of both vesicles as was 
to be expected. ‘Toward the fifth and sixth days they could 
make progress while touching the side or bottom of the dish, but 
any attempt at swimming in free water resulted only in irregular 
flexions of the body and somersaults. It was hoped that the trans- 
planted vesicle might then begin to function and make it possible 
for them to perceive their position while in free water, but this did 
not occur. They continued to behave in all respects like tadpoles 
having no labyrinth and never gave evidence of possessing any 
trace of equilibrium. 

At the end of the third, fourth and twelfth weeks specimens 
were killed in preserving fluid and prepared in serial sections. 
Examination of the sections showed that in six out of the seven 
specimens which were cut, the transplanted vesicle had developed 
to a greater or less extent, and it was these vesicles that formed the 
surface elevations that had been macroscopically visible in front 
of the tadpole’s eyes. Graphic reconstructions of them are repre- 
sented in Figs. 7 to 12. It will be seen that none of these consti- 
tute a perfect labyrinth, but on closer study it 1s found that they 
all possess certain features which are characteristic of it. Inthe 
first place, that which was transplanted in the form of an open 
auditory cup developed after the operation into a closed vesicle 
containing endolymph. This did not then remain a simple vesicle, 
but exhibited the tendency to subdivision into two or more com- 
partments, the utriculus and sacculus, as seen in Figs. 7,8, 12. Inthe. 
walls of these compartments there are areas of specialized epithelium 
representing the maculz acustice. In Fig. 7 there opens out of 
the more dorsal compartment a distinct endolymphatic appendage. 
A typical semicircular canal is not present in any of them; but 
what may be called a canal tendency is seen in Fig. 8, where 
there is a tube uniting the two principal compartments. The 
small blind pouches leading off the main vestibule, three of which 
are present in Fig. 10, doubtless represent abortive canals. In 
transverse section they are perfectly round and look like typical 


556 George L. Streeter 


canals. It may be recalled that Ridinger (’88) described the 
semicircular canals as developing in the form of blind tubes sprout- 
ing out from the general vesicle. It is quite possible that he was 
dealing with an abnormal embryo and had the same form of canals 
that we see in Fig. 10. 

The ear vesicles are more or less completely enveloped in con- 
nective tissue membranes and they are partly incorporated in 
masses of cartilage, some of which belongs to the normal cartilagi- 
nous cranium and some of it is the regular cartilaginous capsule 
of the labyrinth, the two fusing together in some places. 

In four cases (Figs. 7, 8, 9 and 10) a group of ganglion cells and 
nerve fibers are attached to the median side of the vesicle near its 
caudal end and extend toward the central nervous system. In 
one instance (Fig. 7) the nervous connection between vesicle and 
brain at the junction of olfactory lobe and fore-brain, is complete, 
though it 1s only a few fibers that actually enter the brain. As the 
acoustic ganglion at the time of the operation is attached to the 
auditory cup some of its ganglion cells are undoubtedly carried 
along with it, and it is probable that it is these cells that furnished 
the nerve connections just described. At the time the transplanted 
ear cup was slipped into its pocket the adherent ganglion cells 
must have been lodged in various positions as regards the ear cup 
and the fact that they all come finally to lie on the median side of 
the vesicle and lead toward the brain must be explained by some 
theory of an attraction existing between brain and nerve. 

When we have to deal with a transplanted labyrinth that has 
reached a development equal to those that function in young tad- 
poles, and has established communication with the central ner- 
vous system, we might expect that it would show some sign of 
physiological activity. The failure of it to do so is perhaps best 
accounted for by the fact that the point of entrance into the brain 
is so far away from the hind-brain centers and the spinal cord that 
connections with these are not established. If the experiments were 
varied and the vesicle transplanted to some point in the neighbor- 
hood of the occipital nerves this difficulty would be obviated. 


Fig. 7 Fig. 8 Fig. 9 


Fig. 10 Fig. 11 Fig. 12 


Figs. 7 to 12. Graphic reconstructions showing the form and relations developed by transplanted ear 
vesicles, one to three months after the operation. In all six cases the right ear vesicle was removed and 
the left vesicle transplanted into a subdermal pocket between eye and nostril. In Figs. 7,8, 9 and 10 
the acoustic nerve and ganglion extended from ear vesicle toward brain; in Fig. 7 the connection was 
complete, the fibers entering at junction of fore-brain and olfactory lobe. Central nervous system, 


shaded; ear vesicle, solid black. 


558 George L. Streeter 


Conclusions 


In the tadpole the ear vesicles are essential for the development 
of the power of equilibration, but the study of normal specimens 
shows that well developed equilibration may be present before 
the completion of the semicircular canals; the latter as such are 
therefore not essential. 

When both vesicles are removed no other organ compensates 
for their loss and the animal is completely and permanently help- 
less as regards the maintenance of equilibrium. When only one 
ear vesicle is taken out the remaining vesicle 1s capable of perform- 
ing the work of both so perfectly, that the casual observer would 
mistake them for normal individuals. 

Transplantation of the ear vesicle shows that the group of cells 
forming the auditory cup or primitive ear vesicle is specialized to 
that degree that although removed from their natural relations 
and placed in a new environment they still continue to differentiate 
themselves into a structure approximating the normal labyrinth. 
A nerve and ganglion develops, and complete nervous connection 
may be established between the transplanted vesicle and the brain 
at an abnormal place. Where the latter occurred it did not give 
evidence of any functional ability. 


LITERATURE 


Lee, F.S.,’93.—A Study of the Sense of Equilibriumin Fishes. Journ. of Physiol., 

vol. xv and xvil. 
”98.—The Function of the Ear and Lateral Line in Fishes. Amer. Journ. 

of Physiol., vol. i. 

Lewis, W. H., ’04.—Experimental Studies on the Development of the Eye in 
Amphibia. Amer. Journ. Anat., vol. u1. 

Lyon, E. P.,’00.—A Contribution to the Comparative Physiology of Compensatory 
Motions. Amer. Journ. of Physiol., vol. 11. 

Parker, G. H., ’05.—The Skin, Lateral-Line Organs and Ear as Organs of 
Equilibration. Proceedings of Amer. Zo]. Soc., Science, vol. xxi. 

Prentiss, C. W., ’o1.—The Otocyst of Decapod Crustacea; its Structure, Develop- 
ment and Functions. Bull. Museum Compar. Zool., Harvard 
College, vol. xxxvi. 

RupinceER, ’08.—Zur Anatomie u. Entwickelung desinneren Ohres. Berlin, 1888. 


asain 


iaiE RELATION BETWEEN FUNCTIONAL REGU- 
LATION AND FORM-REGULATION 


BY 


C.-M. CHILD 


The phenomena of regulation in the organic world have received 
much attention in recent years and it has become more and more 
evident that a consideration of these phenomena involves some of 
the most fundamental problems of biology. It is perhaps not 
going too far to say that the solution of the problems of regula- 
tion will constitute a solution of most of the other problems which 
underlie physiological biology. Moreover, we have, in the possi- 
bility of analyzing and modifying regulatory processes by experi- 
mental conditions, a means of attacking the problems involved, 
which is more exact and has already proven more fruitful than 
other efforts directed toward the same end. 

Inorganic life structure and form are to the observer perhaps 
the most salient features and so constitute the most available 
criterion for distinction and recognition not only of the compo- 
nent parts of theorganism butof differentorganisms. Any method 
of procedure which permits the control, analysis, and modification 
of the processes which give rise to structure and form is, therefore, 
of the greatest importance, since it affords an insight into some of 
the phenomena most characteristic of and peculiar to organic life. 
The experimental method as applied in the field of form-regula- 
tion is of this character, and it has already demonstrated that 
many of the formative processes are dependent upon conditions 
which can be altered experimentally. Thus we are able in many 
cases to modify and alter the form and structure very widely. 
We are at present only on the threshold of this field of investiga- 
tion but the outlook for the future is most promising. As our 
methods improve and as we come to comprehend more clearly 
the character of the phenomena with which we are dealing the 


THE JourNAL oF ExprrIMENTAL ZOOLOGY .VOL. Il, NO. 4. 


560 CoM -Ghid 


field within which these methods are applicable will extend andthe 
value of the results obtained will increase. 

Certain workers in the field of form-regulation have, it is true, 
reached somewhat disappointing conclusions. Some maintain 
that the data are as yet insufficient to permit any interpretation, 
others that the phenomena of regulation belong to a totally differ- 
ent category from those of the inorganic world and that, therefore, 
we cannot hope to interpret them in terms of physics and chemis- 
try. he reason for these conclusions is, the writer believes, to 
be found in the fact that these authorities assume the formative 
processes to be a special series or complex of processes in the 
organism differing in nature from other so-called functional pro- 
cesses. The organism is regarded as possessing two groups or 
complexes of activities, the one giv ing rise to structure, the other 
concerned with the dynamic activities in the structure. Any 
such distinction seems to the writer entirely artificial and without 
basis in fact. Numerous examples of the dependence of struc- 
ture for its existence upon dynamic or functional conditions are 
before us, and the only reason why the data are not still more 
abundant is that those already obtained have failed to attract 
due attention. 

It becomes increasingly evident that the organism 1s primarily a 
dynamic or functional complex and the structure and form are 
merely visible expressions of the dynamic conditions. The 
writer has attempted in various papers during the last three years 
('02—’o6a) to develop this idea and apply it to specific cases of 
form-regulation but it seems worth while to present in more gen- 
eral form some of the conclusions reached, together with some of 
the facts on which they are based, and to show how the ideas 
involved may be applied to other cases. 

The process of form-regulation is commonlyregarded as consist- 
ing essentially in the replacement of a lost part—hence the term 
regeneration is used by some authors as synonymous with form- 
regulation. But form-regulation includes not only the replacement 
of lost parts: it may also involve hypertrophy or atrophy of parts 
remaining, the substitution of one part for another, the develop- 
ment of a part widely different from that lost, partial replace- 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 561 


ment or no replacement at all. ‘The recognition of the fact that 
these various phenomena, some of which are widely different from 
regeneration in the original sense are fundamentally similar justi- 
fies the use of a term which has not the disadvantage of possessing 
another and different meaning. Moreover, the term regulation 
serves or should serve to call attention to the fact that there is 
something similar in these processes to what has long been known 
as regulation in functional processes. 

In the following paragraphs some of the writer's conclusions 
are presented, aaditias often in somewhat positive fashion, but the 
positive form of statement has its value in the presentation of 


hypotheses. 


T he Initial Factor in Replacement of a Lost Part 


Driesch (’01) maintains that the initial factor in regulation is a 
disturbance in function or in constitution. While the writer can- 
not agree with Driesch in regard to the sharp distinction which he 
makes between function and constitution, it is sufhciently obvious 
that the initial factor in the process of replacement consists in 
some change in that part of the dynamic system which remains. 

Have we any data which afford a clue as to the character of 
this change and the regions involved? In this connection 
certain experiments on flatworms may be mentioned first. In 
Stenostoma (Child, ’02) or Cestoplana (Child ,’05b) after removal 
of the posterior end of the body at any levels except immediately 
posterior to the cephalic ganglia the posterior portion of the 
remaining part is used by the animal in much the same manner as 
the posterior portion of the part removed. ‘The functional activ- 
ities of this region which are involved in locomotion are readily 
observable since it forms the chief region of attachment and 1s 
employed almost constantly during progression. ‘These forms 
possess means of adhesion along practically the whole length of 
the ventral surface but under normal conditions the posterior end 
is most used and the organs of adhesion are most highly devel- 
oped there. ‘This functional substitution is often very imperfect 
at first, but rapidly becomes more perfect and after a few hours 


562 C. M. Child 


the posterior end of the piece is employed in much the same 
manner as the original posterior end, though adhesion is apparently 
not as firm. 

In short, the animal has altered its behavior and in response to 
the new conditions has ‘‘learned”’ to use this part in place of the 
part removed. ‘This altered function involves not merely the 
portion immediately adjoining the cut surface, but other parts for 
a greater or less distance anterior to this (Child, ’o5b). In other 
mane. a portion of the piece becomes functionally posterior 
even though its original position may have been far from the pos- 
terior end. If we follow the process of form-regulation which 
succeeds this change in function we find that this part becomes in 
the course of time by a process of redifferentiation structurally as 
_ well as functionally a posterior end. In Stenostoma the process 
of formation of the new posterior end is very rapid, and if we pre- 
vent the piece from attaching itself in the characteristic manner 
form-regulation is delayed (Child, ’03a). In this latter case then 
we have demonstrative evidence that the functional conditions 
connected with the use of the part in a certain manner are impor- 
tant factors in form-regulation. Moreover, facts which cannot be 

cited here lead us to the same conclusion in regard to Cestoplana. 

In some of the flatworms, e. g., Planaria maculata (Morgan, 

’98, etc., and others) a new head region can be formed at any level 
of the body, in others, like Dendroccelum, only in the more 
anterior regions (Lillie, ’or), and in still others, Leptoplana 
(Child, ’o4c), Cestoplana (Child, ’o5b), etc., only from levels 
immediately posterior to, through, or anterior to the cephalic 
ganglia. Lillie was first to note that the pieces of Dendroccelum 
capable of producing a new head react much more like the normal 
animal than those incapable of such regulation. ‘The writer has 
observed the same difference in reaction in Leptoplana, Cesto- 
plana, and many other Turbellaria. Undoubtedly in these cases 
the character of the reactions is dependent in large measure on the 
physiological character of those portions of the nervous system 
which are present. In all of these pieces where formation of a 
new head is possible the anterior end of the piece behaves in some 
degree, often very slightly it is true, like a head. We find, more- 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 563 


over, that this ““head-likeness”’ increases as time goes on and long 
before the new head is fully formed the behavior of the growing 
parts is very distinctly head-like. In this case the piece is “learn- 
ing’’ to use the region as a head and the morphological develop- 
ment of the head follows. 

In these cases as well as in many others the behavior of the 
pieces and the use of the parts affords us valuable evidence as to 
what is really taking place. It must not be supposed that the 
movements themselves are the only or most important factors 
involved. They may and undoubtedly do play a part in many 
cases where particular regions are subjected to particular mechan- 
ical or other conditions in consequence of characteristic move- 
ments (Child, ’02, ’03a, ’o4a, ’o4b, ’o4c, ’05a, ’05b), but their 
chief value is that they serve as an index to internal conditions. 
and changes. 

These cases are perhaps sufficient to illustrate the point of view 
and to afford a certain basis in fact forit. Other facts which bear 
upon the same point have been cited and discussed in the papers 
above referred to. According to this point of view the first step 
in the process of form-regulation is functional regulation, an 
alteration in internal dynamic conditions in consequence of the 
disturbance of what we may call the physiological equilibrium of 
the system. ‘The process of functional regulation 1s not, however, 
fundamentally a process of “trial and error’ (see pp. 578, 579), 
for its nature is in large measure predetermined by the dominant 
reactive capacities of he system. 

But can we bring such cases as the regeneration Of anvabmm 
the starfish, a leg in the crayfish, or the amphibian, into the same 


category’ Is there any functional regulation in the direction of 


substitution of a part remaining for a part removed. 

In such cases as these the first visible change is the closure of 
the wound either by a mass of coagulated blood, leucocytes, etc., 
or by cellular material and in all cases sooner or later by a mass of 
tissue composed of cells capable of division and growth. This 
process of wound-closure, while regulatory in nature in that it is 
the result of reactions in response to new conditions 1s not directly 


connected with the replacement of the lost part, but is primarily 


564 C. M. Child 


the result of purely local conditions. As 1s well-known, it occurs 
not only where replacement takes place later but also in those 
cases where there is no approach toward replacement. It is evi- 
dent then that the closure of the wound is not, strictly speaking, 
the first step in replacement of the lost part, although it may be 
the first morphological step in regulation This distinction is 
important, for, as was pointed out above, regulation and even 
form-regulation involves much more than the replacement of lost 
parts. 

But the small mass of growing, physiologically plastic tissue 
which closes the wound may stand, as soon as it forms, or strictly 
speaking as soon as it begins to form, in a relation to the whole 
system or organism more or less similar to that in which the part 
removed or some portion of it originally stood. “This region must 
be subjected to many conditions—internal and sometimes exter- 
nal—similar in greater or less degree to those to which the part 
removed or some portion of it was subjected. ‘The nerves which 
formerly led to the part or some of them now lead to this region 
and the regeneration of the nerves begins very early. It stands in 
the same or somewhat similar relations to other parts of the body 
as the part removed and must, as it forms, use a certain amount of 
energy which is derived from other regions in much the same 
manner as the part removed derived its energy. In short, 
this region may by virtue of its relations to the whole become the 
physiological representative in greater or less degree of the part 
removed. In case this physiological substitution takes place up 
to a certain degree further regulation occurs and the region begins 

to develop into the part removed. In case the substitution does 
not occur or is insufhcient in degree to bring about further 
growth no replacement of the lost part occurs. Ths growth pro- 
ceeds the dynamic conditions are more and more modified so that 
the substitution becomes more and more complete until the miss- 
ing part 1s fully formed. 

In many cases the growth of the regenerating part may proceed 
at least for a time without actual exercise or use of the part, 
though complete differentiation apparently does not occur with- 
out some degree of use. ‘The regenerating arthropod appendage 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 565 


is coiled beneath the closed end of the stump until the first moult 
following the operation. It is clear, therefore, that the conditions 
which determine the earlier stages of the growth and differentia- 
tion of the part are not identical with those to which the part 1s 
subjected later though they are just as truly functional. The 
growth of the new leg is not the result of the attempt to use the 
leg which is missing. ‘The growing tissue begins to develop into 
a leg because its relations to the other parts of the system are in 
some degree similar to those of the leg removed. As it grows, the 
conditions approach more and more nearly those to which the 
normal leg is subjected, 7. ¢., there is a gradual return of the 
functional conditions to the normal. 

This condition, functional substitution in the tissue closing the 
wound, constitutes the one extreme as regards the process of 
replacement and results in the formation of the lost part wholly 
by the outgrowth of new tissue. At the other extreme are the 
cases similar to those first mentioned where the functional sub- 
stitution involves a considerable portion of the piece and the 
process of replacement is one of redifferentiation of this region 
without the outgrowth of new tissue except that closing the 
wound. Between these extremes are found the various interme- 
diate forms of form-regulation. 


T he Common Methods of Form-Regulation 


If the functional substitution for the part removed is accom- 
plished within the part remaining, the region involved undergoes 
a process of ‘‘redifferentiation”’ (Child, ’06a) and the conditions 
determining the growth of new tissue from the cut surface may be 
largely or wholly absent except so far as closure of the wound is 
concerned. This is the case in Stenostoma (Child, ’02) and 
in posterior regulation in Cestoplana (Child ’osb). If, on the 
other hand, the substitution is confined to the region immediately 
adjoining the cut surface or to the tissue closing the wound form- 
regulation is almost wholly or wholly a process of regeneration. 
The substitution in the old part may, however, be incomplete and 
the distal portion of the part removed may be formed by the 


566 C. M. Child 


growth of new tissue, 7. ¢., regeneration, while its basal portion is 
formed by redifferentiation as in Planaria maculata (Morgan, ’98, 
etc.; Child, ’06b). Again the substitution in the old part may be 
more complete at one level than at another and the relative 
amounts of regeneration and redifferentiation will vary at differ- 
ent levels. ‘This is to some extent the case in Planaria maculata, 
for example, where the regenerated anterior region is shorter and 
the redifferentiated region is longer in pieces from levels near the 
head, while the reverse is true in pieces from the middle regions 
(Child, ’06b). The difference is still more marked in Polyche- 
rus where the writer has found! that the new posterior end is 
formed almost wholly by regeneration in pieces from levels near 
the head and almost wholly by redifferentiation in pieces from 
levels near the posterior end, and the relative amounts of rediffer- 
entiation and regeneration show all intermediate conditions in 
pieces between these two extremes. And finally in some cases 
form-regulation at the posterior end may be largely or wholly a 
process of redifferentiation and at the anterior end a process of 
regeneration as is the case in Cestoplana (Child, ’o5b). In all 
these cases of ‘‘mixed”’ form-regulation the redifferentiation occurs 
in the region which is physiologically similar to the part removed 
to such a degree that substitution occurs readily and with relative 
completeness, while regeneration occurs where the substitution is 
confined to the tissue closing the wound or to regions immediately 
adjoining the cut surface. 

In short, the greater the physiological similarity between the old 
part or a given region of it and the new, the greater the amount of 
redifferentiation andthe less the amount of regeneration. The 
reverse 1s also true up toa certain point. When the part remaining 
is so widely different from the part removed that no substitution is 
possible in any region not even regeneration occurs except in 
closure of the wound, and the missing part is not replaced. All 
intermediate stages between complete regeneration and mere 
wound-closure may occur in a single individual at different levels. 
This is well illustrated in Leptoplana (Child, ’o4c) and in Cesto- 


1 Not yet published. 


. ha 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 567 


plana (Child, ’o5b), where the regeneration of the head is complete 
from levels anterior to or through the cephalic ganglia and in 
Cestoplana also from levels immediately posterior to the ganglia, 
and becomes increasingly incomplete with increasing distance 
from the ganglia, until in pieces from the posterior regions scarcely 
more than wound-closure occurs. The absence of regeneration 
beyond wound-closure in many higher forms is doubtless due 
largely to the fact that the physiological specification of the tissues 
is so great that no appreciable substitution occurs in any region 
after removal of a part. 

But form-regulation may occur, nevertheless, even in such cases 
and may be represented by hypertrophy of other parts. A good 
illustration of this is the hypertrophy of one kidney following the 
removal of the other. 

A process of destruction often occurs during form-regulation. 
This is commonly the case in redifferentiation where the structures 
formed and maintained by a given complex of conditions cannot 
persist under the altered conditions and so degenerate or atrophy. 
In a piece of Cestoplana from the prepharyngeal region, for exam- 
ple, the new pharynx appears a considerable distance from the 
posterior end of the piece and all the intestinal branches posterior 
to the new pharynx disintegrate and are replaced by others formed 
anew. It is probable that the movement of intestinal contents 
during contraction and extension plays an important part in this 
rather remarkable change. ‘The data in this case are as yet un- 


published. 


The Rate of Form-Regulation and the Limit of Size 


The development of a part reproduced by regulation is often 
greatly accelerated as compared with normal ontogenetic develop- 
ment. ‘This rapid development can be due to nothing but a dis- 
proportion between size and intensity of physiological conditions 
to which the part is subjected. It is a familiar fact that increase in 
intensity of functional conditions brings about increase in size or 
hypertrophy of the part involved and decrease in intensity a 
decrease in size or atrophy. ‘The new part in regeneration and 
often also in redifferentiation is at first much ‘‘toosmall,”’ 2. e., the 


568 C. M. Child 


intensity of the physiological conditions to which it is subjected 
corresponds to a region of much larger size and hence increase in 
size takes place very rapidly. Dunne growth the relative inten- 
sity of the physiological conditions decreases and so continually 
approaches that existing in the original part before removal. 
Hence unless other factors prevent, the part will grow with decreas- 
ing rapidity until it attains approximately the size of the part re- 
moved. 

In the flatworms where form-regulation will occur in starving 
pieces we find that the increase in size often ceases even before the 
new part has attained the proper proportions with respect to the 
old. In these cases the new part grows at the expense of the old 
and may undergo absolute as well as relative increase in size while 
the old part is undergoing absolute decrease. In such cases an 
equilibrium between old and new parts different from that existing 
in the normal animal is attained. As the new part grows the 
conditions for further growth decrease in intensity and as the old 
part is reduced its demand for nutritive material becomes rela- 
tively more intense and a larger proportion of the material avail- 
able is used up in its own activities. Therefore, growth in the new 
part must cease before it reaches normal proportions, even with 
respect to the reduced size of the old part. 


The Relation between Rate of Regulation and the Degree of Injury 


Comparison of two series of pieces of Leptoplana, the one set 
consisting of the anterior one-fourth or one-fifth of the body, the 
other of the anterior four-fifths or three-fourths, both kept without 
food, shows that the pieces of the first series regenerate from their 
posterior ends very much more rapidly and a much larger amount 
of new tissue than the pieces of the second series (Child, ’o4b). 
Yet the pieces showing the greater and more rapid regeneration 
are only one-fourth as large as the others. 

If we compare the behavior of the two sets of pieces during regu- 
lation, especially during the earlier stages after the Tegeneration has 
begun, we find that in ae first series pie new part is used to a much 
greater extent than inthe second. Not only is this the case but the 
activity in the old parts also is much greater in the first series than 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 569 


in the second or in the normal animal. ‘The movements are more 
violent and the irritability appears to be greater. In short, to all 
appearances, marked modification in the dynamic conditions has 
resulted from the removal of the posterior four-fifths and a scarcely 
appreciable modification from the removal of the one-fifth. 

When the new tissue has appeared it becomes in the first series 
the functional representative of the four-fifths removed, and in the 
second only of the one-fifth. ‘The early stages of its growth are of 
approximately the same size in both cases but in the first series the 
intensity of dynamic conditions must be much greater in this 
region, since its role in the complex is much more extensive and 
important than in the second. It must demand and receive a 
much greater proportion of the energy of the complex. Conse- 
quently growth or hypertrophy is much more rapid and greater in 
amount. Moreover, if it is true that removal of the four-fifths has 
brought about an increase in the intensity of dynamic processes in 
the old part this will doubtless also tend to increase the rapidity of 
growth in the new part, especially during earlier stages, since it 
is involved in the various reactions. As the new part develops, the 
intensity of activity appears to decrease. 

Here as in other similar cases it is not simply the degree of move- 
ments or exercise of the part that determines the rapidity and 
amount of growth, though this of course may constitute one factor. 
It is rather the conditions underlying the motor activity, the nerve 
stimuli, the metabolic conditions, etc., of which the motor activity 
is an index (Child, ’o4b, ’os5b). 

Zeleny has recently obtained somewhat similar results in several 
species of decapod crustacea and in a species of ophiurid (Zeleny, 
"05a, 05b). In all of these cases the rate of regeneration increases 
with the degree of injury up to a certain point. These cases differ, 
however, from the case of Leptoplana in that here regeneration 
occurs from two or more different regions while there only one 
region is involved. Strictly speaking the case of Leptoplana 1s 
comparable to removal of larger or smaller parts of a single arm 
in the starfish or of a single leg inthe crustacea. Butif we remove 
three or four arms from the starfish or several legs from the crayfish 
each one of the newprimordia represents, when it appears, no larger 


ae C. M. Child 


portion of the complex than does the primordium of a single arm or 
leg cut off at the same level, yet it regenerates more rapidly. 

It was noted above that the short piece of Leptoplana showed 
much more intense activity than the long piece. It is perhaps not 
too much to say that this increased activ ity is a response or reaction 
to the absence of the four-fifths. On receiving a stimulus the piece 
reacts but reaction in the normal manner 1s impossible and the 
result is ““irradiation”’ of the stimulus and the appearance of other 
more or less violent reactions. ‘This fact of ‘‘irradiation” has long 
been recognized in nerve physiology and Jennings’ recent experi- 
ments on the modifiability of behavior show that it is an important 
factor in behavior. 

Zeleny has been unable to observe any characteristic difference 
in activity connected with the degree of injury. But the reaction to 
the injury need not necessarily appear as actual motor activity; It 
may take the form of increased rapidity of metabolism or other 
forms. In those cases where’ increased motor activity is present 
it serves as an index to internal conditions but its absence does not 
necessarily indicate the absence of increased dynamic activity of 
other kinds. 

There can be no doubt that a normal reaction normally carried 
out has a certain effect on the individual and that an attempt and 
failure to carry out the reaction has another and very different 
effect, viz: in the direction of altered character and increased inten- 
sity of reaction. [he old experiments upon the reflexes of the 
decapitated frog are sufhcient demonstration of the fact. The 
larger the number of arms or legs removed in the cases cited above, 
the more impossible is the accomplishment of the normal reactions 
and consequently the more intense the effect on the animal as 
regards other reactions. As the new tissue appears it becomes 
a more or less complete functional substitute for the parts 
removed very incomplete at first no doubt. In the conditions of 
intensified activity resulting from the operation the functional 
conditions to which the regenerating structures are subjected must 
increase in intensity with the degree of injury. ‘This increase will 
be all the greater because their presence is the first step in the 
approach to a normal method of reaction to which the system 1s, so 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 571 


to speak, “‘seeking.”’ As regeneration goes on the functional sub- 
stitution becomes more complete, and reactions take place more 
nearly in the normal manner, functional conditions become rela- 
tively less intense, and the rapidity of regeneration decreases. 


Compensatory Hypertrophy 


In a number of cases among the Crustacea (Przibram, ’o1, ’02, 
05; Zeleny, ’o5a), where an asymmetery of the chelz exists, 
removal of the more highly specialized chela 1s followed by a more 
or less complete transformation of the other into the more highly 
specialized type, and the regenerating chela develops into the less 
specialized form. Thus a reversal of asymmetry results. If the 
less specialized chela alone is removed no reversal occurs. Zeleny 
(05a) has obtained somewhat similar results with certain species 
of serpulids where two opercula, one large and functional, the 
other small and rudimentary, are present. Removal of the func- 
tional operculum results in a reversal of the asymmetry while no 
reversal occurs after removal of the rudimentary operculum. When 
the whole head region, including both opercula is removed both 
regenerate in the form of the functional structure. Wilson (’03) 
has found in Alpheus grounds for believing that when the nerve to 
the chela is cut the reversal does not occur, but further experiments 
along this line are needed. 

A satisfactory interpretation of these cases on a functional basis 
appears possible. When the specialized chela or operculum is 
removed the disturbance of the system thus produced results in 
changes in reactions and it seems probable that the easiest and 
most natural change will be such that the less specialized or rudi- 
mentary organs will receive stimuli and be subjected to conditions 
which originally affected the specialized or functional structures. 
In fact, it is dificult to see how any other interpretation can be 
made to serve. Zeleny (’c5a) postulates for the serpulids the 
existence of a ‘‘retardation stimulus” from the functional oper- 
culum which, so long as this is present, inhibits the development of 
the other. The nature of such a retardation stimulus is highly 
problematical. It seems to the writer much more probable that 
the failure of the rudimentary or less specialized part to develop 


572 C. M. Child 


beyond a certain point under normal conditions is due rather 
to the absence of adequate stimuli than to a positive inhibitory 
stimulus. So long as the other more highly specialized part 
is present the animal reacts in a characteristic manner which 
involves the less specialized part only to a certain degree; and the 
structure of that part expresses the character of the conditions to 
which it has been subjected. Removal of the less specialized part 
does not alter the reactions to any such extent as removal of the 
other, hence the part removed regenerates in the same form. 
Removal of both places the new primordia on equal terms and both 
may producethe more highly specialized or the functional structure. 
Here, as elsewhere, the functional conditions involved are not 
primarily those connected with use or exercise of the part but the 
sum total of its dynamic relations to the whole system. 


Polarity and Axial Heteromorphosts 


Physiological polarity in the developed organism may be defined 
provisionally as a habit of reaction resulting from past or present 
relations. ‘This polarity of the organism may be a consequence of 
the polarity of the egg and this in turn a consequence of the rela- 
tions of the egg-cell to the body of the parent or of other con- 
ditions affecting the egg. At any rate there seems to be no good 
ground for believing that polarity is a permanent and funda- 
mental property of the cell or of protoplasm, although certain 
authorities have urged this view. 

Polarity may be rendered visible by structural differentiation 
along the axis or it may not, but in any case the structure is prima- 
rily aresult not a cause. It may, however, become the determining 
condition in that it determines the character of the reactions. 

The original polarity is commonly maintained in form-regula- 
tion simply because each of the parts reacts most readily in a 
manner approaching that in which it has reacted in the past. But 
this habit of reaction can be altered in certain cases. In Tubu- 
laria, for example, pieces very commonly produce a hydranth at 
both ends, the oral hydranth, first the aboral later. The delay in 
the appearance of the aboral hydranth represents the time neces- 
sary to alter the character of the reaction or in other words to 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation Ge) 


change the habit. Moreover, when the oral hydranth is pre- 
vented from forming, the aboral hydranth appears more quickly 
than when the oral hydranth is permitted to form. Here we have 
an example of the case discussed above in which failure to carry 
out a ‘“‘normal” reaction or the reaction most readily performed 
results in increased intensity of other reactions. ‘That the habit 
is really changed is shown by the fact that a second aboral hy- 
dranth develops more rapidly than the first. Undoubtedly struc- 
tural changes in the various regions result from the changes in 
reaction but these are not visible in this form except in the struc- 
tures produced at the cut surfaces. 

Axial heteromorphosis can be brought about in various ways in 
many of the lower forms. ‘The isolation of very short pieces 1s per- 
haps the simplest method. In these cases the piece is not large 
enough to represent the whole complex and the reaction in which the 
piece has been most intimately involved in the past becomes the 
chief or only possible reaction and similar structures are produced 
at both ends. We find, therefore, that pieces of this kind often 
produce at both ends the structures characteristic of the regions 
with which they have been most closely associated in the past. 
Sometimes other special factors modify this result, ¢.g., preparation 
for fission in Planaria maculata (Child, ’o6b), or as in Tubularia 
and some other hydroids the dominance of a particular reaction- 
complex throughout the individual. This latter case requires a 
word of comment: pieces of Tubularia from any part of the stem 
or from the stolons produce hydranths much more frequently than 
they do stolons when the ends are not in contact with a surface or 
solid. Polarity in this form is commonly indicated by the relative 
size and rapidity of formation of the hydranths from different ends 
and at different levels of the piece rather than by the formation of 
typically different structures, though occasionally stolon-forma- 
tion occurs at the aboral end even without contact. It is possible, 
however, as the writer has found, to increase experimentally and 
without contact the intensity of the stolon-forming reaction to such 
an extent that almost every piece will give rise to stolons at the 
aboral end. Many of these stolons lb: after a considerable time 
produce hydranths at their tips. The writer is inclined to believe 


ens C. M. Child 


that the hydranth-forming reactions may be interpreted as prima- 
rily a reaction to lack of nutriment. ‘This reaction occurs most 
readily of course in those parts which have been most closely asso- 
ciated with it in the past. 

In a number of forms short pieces are not necessary for the 
occurrence of heteromorphosis. ‘These include Tubularia and 
various other hydroids, some other ccelenterates, the earthworm 
and some other forms. In some of these as in Tubularia one form 
of reaction is dominant throughout, and in others, like the earth- 
worm, this dominance is regional. Inthe earthworm, for example, 
pieces from the posterior region commonly produce tails at both 
ends (Morgan, ’02). ‘The head-forming reactions can occur only 
in the more anterior regions. The very close relation between 
head-formation and the nervous system in this as well as in many 
other cases indicates that important factors in polarity are situated 
in the nervous system. 

Heteromorphosis may also occur in various forms when a short 
piece is grafted in reverse position on a much longer piece. In 
such cases the short piece produces at its free end the structures 
characteristic of the end of the longer piece with which it is united. 
These are undoubtedly simply cases of physiological dominance of 
the larger component. ‘The greater intensity of its reactions alters 
the conditions in the small piece so that this becomes functionally a 
part of the other. ‘This is doubtless accomplished by the passage 
of stimuli from the larger into the smaller piece and it may be by 
the actual growth of nerves in some cases. 

The regulation of a piece into a complete individual with typical 
axial differentiation and maintenance of the original polarity 
depends upon the relative difference in reactive capacity in the 
different regions. In all such cases the piece must possess the 
power to react in some degree like the whole. Its isolation brings 
into play potentialities not evident while normal relations were 
maintained, and the regions best fitted by past associations, for 
particular reactions perform them most readily and thus the origi- 
nal polarity persists. 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 575 


The Relation Between the Nervous System and Form-Regulation 


This relation which 1s very general, though of course not univer- 
sal, affords the strongest evidence in favor of the view that form- 
regulation is a functional process. No good ground has been dis- 
covered for assuming the existence of a peculiar trophic or forma- 
tive influence originating in the nervous system. Goldstein, in a 
recent paper (’04), gives a most interesting discussion of the subject 
and concludes that the relation between the nervous system and 
growth and differentiation is essentially functional in character. 

It must be remembered, however, that the nervous system adds 
nothing fundamentally new to the phenomena of life. ‘The trans- 
ference of stimuli in definite directions and along more or less 
definite paths occurs where no visible nervous system exists. In- 
deed the development of the nervous system itself is probably in 
greater or less degree the result of such conditions. ‘The problem 
of form-regulation is, therefore, not necessarily different in its fun- 
damental features in those cases where no nervous system exists or 
where regulation occurs in the absence of visible nerves, as com- 
pared with those where visible nervous structures are present. 


Conclusion 


Regulation in general may be defined as a return to physiological 
equilibrium after such equilibrium has been altered by external 
conditions (Child, ’06a). Holmes (’04) has recently given a very 
similar definition which he developed from the idea of sym- 
biotic relations or “social pressure.” As the writer has attempted 
to show in a previous paper (06a) the idea of symbiotic relations 
does not afford a basis for the replacement in form-regulation of a 
part similar to that removed since removal of an element or a group 
of elements in the symbiotic complex must, according to Holmes’ 
assumption, alter the condition of the whole in such manner that 
the ‘‘social pressure” upon the part which becomes the substitute 
for the part removed will be different from that originally exerted 
upon this part, and something else rather than a duplicate of the 
part removed must be formed. But a characteristic feature of 
form-regulation is a return or approximation to the original con- 


576 CoM AGhila 


dition and any interpretation must recognize this fact. In the 
paper above referred to the writer has called attention to the fact 
that replacement of the missing part cannot occur unless that 
portion of the system remaining 1s capable of reacting in a manner 
essentially similar in some degree to the part removed. But mere 
capacity to react in this manner is not sufficient in most cases. 
‘This method of reaction must be the predominant or characteristic 
method, for if it is not, then a structure different from the part 
removed or nothing at all is replaced. ‘This idea affords a basis 
for understanding why the ability to replace lost parts is limited in 
many cases. When by removal of a part the system is altered to 
such an extent that the previous method of reaction becomes 
impossible or is no longer the dominant method, replacement can- 
not occur. In such cases, however, provided the system is not 
altered to such an extent that continued existence is impossible, 
some other form of regulation may occur and a new equilibrium 
may be established differing more or less widely fromthe old. ‘Thus 
when we remove the ganglia from Leptoplana and various other 
polyclads the processes characteristic of the head-region can no 
longer occur and the head 1s not replaced. In Planayia,on the other 
hand, removal of the ganglia does not alter the complex to such an 
extent as in Leptoplana since the nervous system is not as highly 
cephalized here and the headless piece or certain parts of it still 
retain the ability to react like a head in sufficient degree to initiate 
the process of head-formation. When this process is once started 
the ability to perform the characteristic reaction increases and 
the return to the original condition is gradually accomplished. 
The removal of the part brings into play various potentialities of 
reactions which exist in the remaining piece by virtue of its past 
relations to the whole and those processes which are predominant 
determine the character of form-regulation. But objection may be 
raised that a region in the middle of the body of Planaria, for exam- 
ple, has not been associated in the past with reactions or processes 
characteristic of the head and tail regions. _ If we observe the living 
animal, however, we find that the more intense motor reaction of 
either end may visibly involve the middle region in greater or less 
degree. They must also involve it in many ways not visible. It is 


a 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 577 


probably true that the middle region does not while still a part of 
the whole initiate such reactions, because whenever conditions 
which give rise to such reactions are present other parts of the body 
react much more readily than this. But when we sever its connec- 
tion with other parts we find that it is visibly capable of initiating 
such reactions, though of course very imperfectly and much less 
promptly than the original head and tail regions. ‘The ability to 
develop a new head and tail cannot arise from anything else than 
this ability. In this particular case—Planaria—this ability evi- 
dently depends in large measure upon the nervous system. ‘This 
idea does not conflict in any way with that developed in the fore- 
going sections, viz: that a functional substitution of some degree 
must precede and initiate the process of replacement. It is evi- 
dent that such functional substitution cannot occur unless the sys- 
tem retains the ability to react to some extent like the part removed. 
The substitution may alter the reaction of a larger or smaller part 
of the piece but the very fact of substitution depends on the occur- 
rence in the system of reactions resembling in greater or less degree 
those of the part removed. 

In connection with the views expressed above a recent paper by 
pe nes (05), and also certain parts of his book (06), are of inter- 

Jennings’ recent work has demonstrated the high degree of 
modifiability in the behavior even of the lower organisms and 
here he attempts to apply the conclusions reached to other fields. 
His views on the individual adjustment or regulation of behavior 
are based on the following premises. 

‘“‘t Definite internal processes are occurring in organisms. 

“9 Interference with these processes causes a change of be- 
havior and varied movements, subjecting the organism to many 
different conditions. 

‘“*2- One of these conditions relieves the interference with the 
internal processes so that the changes in behavior cease and the 
relieving condition is thus retained.”’ 

The selection of the process or reaction which relieves the inter- 
ference depends upon the fact of relief. 

Jennings attempts to apply this ‘‘method of trial and error” in 
reaction to the phenomena of regulation in other fields, pointing 


578 C. M. Child 


out the possibility that the selection of the apparently advantageous 
reaction is to be found in the fact that it relieves disturbing condi- 
tions. 

It is perhaps sufficiently evident from what has been said above 
that Jennings’ views and the writer’s are somewhat similar in certain 
respects. ‘lhe physiological equilibrium which forms the basis of 
Holmes’ and the writer’s definition of regulation corresponds to the 
condition postulated by Jennings in which the various physiological 
processes are occurring without interference: the alteration of equi- 
librium is brought about by some interference—in the cases under 
discussion in this paper usually by removal of a part. In con- 
sequence of this interference a more or less extensive rearrangement 
of dynamic processes occurs which becomes manifest in alteration of 
the localization and perhaps also of the character of the reactions: 
because of the occurrence and continuation of certain of these pro- 
cesses a return to equlibrium occurs; but the writer does not believe 
with Jennings that the continuation of particular processes is neces- 
sarily due to the fact that they “relieve the interference,” their 
relation to relief may, however, bring about their intensification. 

‘There is one point in connection with form-regulation which 
Jennings has not discussed but which is of considerable impor- 
tance in this connection and that is the frequency of return or 
approximation to the original condition. ‘This is so characteristic 
of regulation that certain authors like Driesch (’01) have made it 
the basis of their definition. In the regulation of behavior and of 
chemical processes it cannot be supposed that only a single method 
of reaction exists whereby the interference can be removed or dimin- 
ished. In many cases, indeed, this is visibly not the case and the 
organism may attain in consequence of the interference an equilib- 
rium widely different from the original. It seems probable, for 
example, that the formation of a new head at the posterior end anda 
tailat the anterior end or of these structures on the right and left side 
respectively of the piece in Planaria would remove or diminish the 
interference and so constitute a return to equilibrium, but this does 
not usuallyoccur. Itisnecessary to recognize the fact thatthe possi- 
bility of reaction is limited somewhat strictly by the past relations - 
of the part or, in other words, by its reactivecapacity. In short, the 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 579 


reaction leading to replacement of the lost part 1s usually the reac- 
tion which the piece can perform most readily, 1.e., that for which 
it is best adapted. The endeavor has been made to direct atten- 
tion to this fact in the preceding paragraphs. Hence the process of 
trial and error does not occur to any great extent in most cases and 
a very high degree of uniformity in the processes of form-regulation 
exists. Moreover, a particular reaction-complex must continue 
fairly constant for a considerable period of time before it becomes 
manifest in such extensive structural development as appears in 
form-regulation. If the characters of such reactions were not very 
strictly limited we should expect a much greater range of variation 
in the processes of form-regulation than occurs. ‘That variation in 
the results does occur has been pointed out by various authors. 
The writer has called attention to the fact that in Leptoplana 
(Child, ’o4c) some individuals are more capable than others of 
producing head-like structures in the absence of the ganglia, as well 
as to numerous other cases. In a recent paper (Child, ’o6b) a 
rather remarkable case of this kind has been noted: in certain 
species of Planaria very small pieces from the middle region of the 
body may produce any one of five different results, viz: single tail- 
less anterior regions, single headless posterior regions, double 
anterior regions, double posterior regions, or normal animals. 
This region is apparently ‘“ndifferent,” 7.c., neither anterior nor 
posterior reactions are dominant in particular parts of the piece 
though both are possible in some degree, and the result depends 
apparently upon a “chance,” /.e., upon certain internal conditions 
not at present recognized. These contribute to determine what 
reactions shall become dominant in each piece. 

But the variation in processes of form-regulation in general 1s 
not sufficiently great to indicate that “trial and error’’ plays any 
important role. The reactions which bring about form-regulation 
do not continue because they “relieve the interference’’ but because 
they are dominant in the part in consequence of its past relations 
to other parts. In the regulation of behavior there is doubtless 
often a greater range of possibilities, but even here it appears 
probable that the character of the regulatory reaction is deter- 
mined, in many cases at least, not by the fact that it “relieves the 


580 C. M. Child 


interference” but by the character of the “ apparatus” or system 
under the existing conditions. 

All the evidence points us to the conclusion that the phe- 
nomena of form-regulation are not essentially different from other 
dynamic phenomena in organisms and that they must be inter- 
preted on a dynamic basis. As the writer has maintained in 
various papers the organism is primarily a dynamic complex and 
visible structural foarte: are primarily incidents or by-products of 
the dynamic processes. Formative processes, so-called, do not 
differ fundamentally from processes of behavior. 

Objection to such interpretation, may be made on the ground 
that it is not really an explanation but rather a step away from 
explanation in that it groups phenomena apparently relatively 
simple in the same category with the most complex phenomena of 
life. In answer to this it may be said that the first step in true 
interpretation is the recognition of the nature of the problem. 
With increasing knowledge of vital phenomena it is becoming more 
and more evident that we cannot select a single group of these 
phenomena and ‘‘explain”’ them without reference to other groups. 
The organism is not a mosaic of independent complexes acting in 
different ways. ‘There is not a particular group of dynamic pro- 
cesses concerned in building the machine and another in keeping it 
going. We shall find similar laws governing all whatever may 
be our final conception as to the nature of these laws. Semon (04) 
has recently proceeded froma somewhat similar idea, in his exceed- 
ingly interesting attempt to interpret the phenomena of heredity, 
habit and memory on a common basis. 

Recognition of the fact that the fundamental problems are the 
same in fields before regarded as distinct or only remotely related is 
always an advance and when different observers in these different 
fields recognize this fact independently of each other, the signific- 
acer oF the conclusions is necessarily greater than it aeula be 
otherwise. 


Hull Zodlogical Laboratory 
University of Chicago 
May, 1906 


Functional Regulation and Form-Regulation 581 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


Cup, C. M., ’02.—Studies on Regulation, I. Fission and Regulation in Steno- 
stoma, Pts. 1, 1] and III. Archiv fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. xv, 
H. 2 and 3, 1902. 

’03a.—Studies on Regulation, II. Experimental Control of Form-Reg- 
ulation in Stenostoma. Archiv fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. xy, 
H. 4, 1903. 

’03b.—Studies on Regulation, II]. Regulative Destruction in Zooids 
and Parts of Zooids in Stenostoma. Archiv fiir Entwickelungs- 
mech., Bd. xvii, H. 1, 1903. 

’o4a.—Studies on Regulation, [V. Some Experimental Modifications of 
Form-Regulation in Leptoplana. Journ. Exp. Zodl., vol. i, No. 
I, 1904. 

’o4b.—Studies on Regulation, V. The Relation between the Central 
Nervous System and Regeneration in Leptoplana. Posterior Re- 
generation. Journ. of Exp. Zool., vol.1, No. 3, 1904. 

‘o4c.—Studies on Regulation, VI. The Relation between the Central 
Nervous System and Regeneration in Leptoplana. Anterior Regen- 
eration. Journ. of Exp. Zodl., vol. i, No. 4, 1g04. 

’05a.—Studies on Regulation, VII. Further Experiments on Form- 
Regulation in Leptoplana. Journ. of Exp. Zodl., vol. ii, No. 2, 
1905. 

‘o5b.—Studies on Regulation, VIII. Functional Regulation and 
Regeneration in Cestoplana. Archiv fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. 
RIK Ld .¥3s LOS. 

‘o5c.—Studies on Regulation, IX. The Positions and Proportions of 
Parts During Regulation in Cestoplana in the Presence of the Ceph- 
alic Ganglia. Archiv ftir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. xx, H. 1, 1905. 

’o5d.—Studies on Regulation, X. The Positions and Proportions of 
Parts during Regulation in Cestoplana in the Absence of the Ceph- 
alic Ganglia. Archiv fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. xx, H. 2, 
1905. 

’o6a.—Contributions Toward a Theory of Regulation, I. The Signi- 
ficance of the Different Methods of Regulation in Turbellaria. 
Archiv fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. xx, H. 3, 1906. 

’06b.—The Relation Between Regulation and Fission in Planaria. Biol. 
Bull. vol. xi, No. 3 ,1906. 

Drtescu, H., ’o1.—Die Organischen Regulationen. Leipzig, 1901. 


582 C. M. Child 


Go psTEIN, K., ’04.—Kritische und Experimentelle Beitrage zur Frage nach dem 
Einfluss des Zentralnervensystems auf die embryonale Entwickel- 
ung und die Regeneration. Archiv fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. 
xvii, H. 1, 1904. 

Hormes, S. J., °04.—The Problem of Form-Regulation. Archiv fiir Entwicke- 

lungsmech., Bd. xvii, H. 2 und 3, 1904. 

Liture, F. R., ’o1.—Notes on Regeneration and Regulation in Planarians, II. 
Amer. Journ. of Physiol., vol. vi, No. 2, 1901. 

Jenninos, H. S., ’05.—The Method of Regulation in Behavior and in Other Fields. 
Journ. of Exp. ZoGl., vol. 11, No. 4, 1905. 

’06.—Behavior of the Lower Organisms, New York, 1906. 
Morean, T. H., ’°98.—Experimental Studies of the Regeneration of Planaria Macu- 
lata. Archiv fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. vu, H. 2 und 3, 1898. 
’02.—Experimental Studies of the Internal Factors of Regeneration in 
the Earthworm. Archiv fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. xiv, H. 3 
und 4, Igo2. 

PrzipraM, H., ’o1.—Experimentelle Studien tiber Regeneration. Archiv fiir Ent- 
wickelungsmech. Bd. xi, H. 2, Igo. 

’o2.—Experimentelle Studien tiber Regeneration (Zweite Mittheilung). 
Archiy fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. xi, H. 4, 1902. 

’05.—Die “Heterochelie’”’ bei decapoden Crustaceen (zugleich: experi- 
mentelle Studien tiber Regeneration. Dritte Mittheilung). Archiv 
fiir Entwickelungsmech. Bd. xix, H. 2, 1905. 

Semon, R., ’04.—Die Mneme als erhaltendes Prinzip im Wechsel des Organischen 
Geschehens. Leipzig, 1904. 

Wison, E. B., ’03.—Notes on the Reversal of Asymmetry in the Regeneration of 

the Chelz in Alpheus heterochelis. Biol. Bull., vol. iv, No. 4, 19. 

ZELENY, C., ’05a.—Compensatory Regulation. Journ. of Exp. Zodl., vol. 11, No. 
I, 1905. 

’05b.—The Relation of the Degree of Injury to the Rate of Regeneration. 
Journ. of Exp. Zodl., vol. 11, No. 3, 1905. 


A STUDY OF THE SPERMATOGENESIS OF COPTO- 
Sverre AURICHALGCEA AND COPTOCYCLA GUI- 
ama LT ESPECIAL. REFERENCE. TO; “THE 
PROBLEM OF SEX-DETERMINATION 


BY 


W. N. NOWLIN 


Witu Two Pirates 


Very little work on the germ cells of the Coleoptera had been 
done until last year, when the form Tenebrio molitor was inves- 
tigated by Dr. N. M. Stevens. The results obtained were so 
suggestive from the standpoint of sex-determination that she is at 
present engaged in examining a number of species of this order 
of insects. 

The following paper is a contribution to this series of investi- 
gations, and I take pleasure in acknowledging my indebtedness to 
Dr. Stevens, both for material used and for direction in the work. 


I. MATERIAL AND METHODS 


The beetles under discussion are Coptocycla aurichalcea and 
Coptocycla guttata, both collected from Convolvulus arvensis at 
Woods Hole, Mass., September 5, 1905. 

The testes were fixed in Flemming’s strong solution, Hermann’s 
platino-aceto-osmic, and Gilson’s mercuro-nitric, each of which 
gave excellent results. 

Heidenhain’s iron-haematoxylin method was used for most of 
the preparations. Other stains, employed principally for chemi- 
cal tests, were thionin, Delafield’s haematoxylin and orange G, and 
with the material fixed in Hermann’s and Flemming’s fluids, the 
safranin-gentian stain of Hermann. ‘The iron-hematoxylin slides 
were often counterstained with orange G. 


Tue JourNat oF ExprerIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. III, NO. 4. 


584 W. N. Nowlin 


II. OBSERVATIONS 


The testes of these beetles consist of many follicles radiating 
from a central duct, the vas deferens, into which are poured the 
spermatozoa. We should expect then to find ripe spermatozoa 
at the inner end of the follicles and spermatogonia at the outer, 
with the intervening stages between. ‘This is true in most cases 
but it is by no means an inviolable rule. Cysts containing ripe 
spermatozoa may be found mingled with those containing first or 
second spermatocytes, and second spermatocytes may occur in the 
same cyst with the first. The classification then cannot be made 
by the location but purely from the character of the cell itself. 


I. Coptocycla aurichalcea 


The equatorial plate of a spermatogonium of Coptocycla auri- 
chalcea contains twenty-two chromosomes, most of which are V- 
shape, and radially arranged with the concave side outward (pl. I, 
Fig. 1). Near the center usually, though it may take any other 
position, is a very small chromosome, which is the small member 
of an “‘idiochromosome”’ group, and while we cannot here be sure 
of its mate we are safe in assuming it to be one of the five smaller 
chromosomes. An attempt was made to arrange the chromo- 
somes in pairs according to form and size, but it soon became 
evident that they differed in different plates; the form, whether 
rod-shaped or V-shaped, depending largely on position with respect 
to the otherchromosomes. As is unusual the behavior of the differ- 
ential chromosomes in the spermatogonium is in every respect like 
that of the others: each divides longitudinally and passes to the 
poles, where condensed masses are formed. 

The earliest spermatocyte shows a true condensation stage or 
bouquet stage [synizesis of McClung (’05)], in which the chroma- 
tin is in threads closely looped together at one side of the nuclear 
space (Fig. 2). The number of loops is here very evidently more 
than the reduced number and probably each loop corresponds to 
a spermatogonial ;chromosome. In a slightly later stage the loops 
straighten until many free ends are seen above the condensed mass 
(Figs. 3 and 5). Each of these strands appears to be a single 


Spermatogenesis of Coptocycla 585 


chromosome, and this idea is strengthened when in the next phase 
the distal ends are seen to bend toward one another in pairs and 
unite (Figs. 5 and 6). Fig. 4 is a cross-section of a nucleus at this 
stage. [hus synapsis takes place in the early part of the growth 
period soon after the last spermatogonial division. ‘This agrees 
with Montgomery’s (’03) description of the synapsis stage in 
Amphibia and furnishes more detailed evidence of the process. 

Up to this period the idiochromosomes have been indistinguish- 
able from the others, but a few very clear sections were obtained 
which showed at the base of the loops, close to the nuclear mem- 
brane, the larger member of the unequal pair (Fig. 5). “This sug- 
gests the probability that the idiochromosomes retain their charac- 
teristic form during the greater part if not all of the synizesis and 
synapsis stages. All the chromatin has a strong afhnity for stains 
during these periods, and the spireme is most distinct when first 
formed (Fig. 7), but almost immediately it becomes very faint (Fig. 
8). The differential chromosomes still stain deeply, however. ‘The 
spireme soon breaks up into separate rods, each showing a longi- 
tudinal split (Figs. 9,10), even at this time there seems to be a 
characteristic form, one chromosome assumes a U-shape, one a 
triangular form, and one that of a nearly closed ring. ‘These show 
still more clearly in the later prophase (Fig. 12). 

The split disappears (Fig. 11), the chromosomes condense, grow- 
ing slightly thicker and shorter (Fig. 12), until we get such forms 
as appear in Fig. 13. The centrosomes are large and distinct in 
this material, and at this stage two may be seen lying close against 
the nuclear membrane. In Fig. 13 we see them nearly 180° apart, 
and as yet no spindle fibers have appeared. ‘[hroughout the 
spireme stage the idiochromosomes are dense spherical bodies. 
In many cases only the larger member is visible (Figs. 7, 8, 9). 
In Fig. 11 both are plainly seen. In the later stages while the 
ordinary chromosomes are undergoing great changes the differen- 
tial chromosomes remain the same (Figs. 12, 13). 

An equatorial plate of the first spermatocyte (Fig. 15) shows 
eleven chromosomes, and since the spindle fibers are very large, 
these may often be seen in cross-section attached to the periphery 
of the chromosomes (Fig. 15). When the material is faintly 


586 W.N. Nowlin 


stained one chromosome stands out prominently, due to its 
retention of the stain (Fig. 14). ‘This is the larger member of the 
differential group, as may be seen in a lateral view of the spindle 
(Fig. 21). The small idiochromosome fails to show here, due prob- 
ably to the angle at which the pair is lying; in late prophase (Figs. 
17, 22) the pair is easily distinguished. While in numerous meta- 
phases the small chromosome 1s difficult to discern, there are afew 
cases in which it is clearly seen (Fig. 18). This unequal pair 
exhibits no difference in behavior during mitosis, but like the others, 
and at the same time, with the exception of one which always 
divides early (Figs. 18, 21), it divides transversely, the large mem- 
ber passing to one pole and the small member to the other (Fig. 
25) 

We have in this division one of the clearest cases of reduction 
division possible. In the spermatogonium the chromosomes are 
V-shaped. During the condensation stage they elongate into 
loops, at first closely massed together, but later these straighten and 
unite in synapsis to form longer loops. ‘This is followed by the 
formation of a continuous spireme (Figs. 7 and 8) which soon 
breaks up into segments, each split longitudinally (Figs. 9, 10). In 
the prophase of the first spermatocyte many of the chromosomes 
are thick rods, which upon examination are seen to be two V’s 
joined end to end, giving in profile a typical E-shape (Fig. 16, ). 
The spindle fiber is attached at the vertex of each V of the bivalent 
chromosome and in anaphase this element separates at the orig- 
inal place of union, at right angles to the initial longitudinal split 
(Figs. 18, 20). The V-shaped chromosomes observed in the 
spermatogonium are thus restored and carried into the second 
spermatocyte. Exactly the same thing is true in the separation of 
the components of the cross-shaped chromosome (Figs. 16, a—d), 
and of the ring-shaped one (Fig. 16, /—o, and Figs. 22, 23, 24). 

There is no rest stage between the two generations of sperma- 
tocytes, but almost immediately the massed chromatin in the late 
anaphase (Fig. 26) separates into chromosomes which begin to 
arrange themselves in the equatorial plate (Figs. 27, 28, 29). In 
Fig. 30 we see this in progress, the small chromosome being very 
distinct. In Figs. 27 and 28, we again see eleven chromosomes 


Spermatogenesis of Coptocycla 587 


of which one is the small idiochromosome. Having seen that in 
the first division the large idiochromosome goes to one pole and 
the small to the other, we should expect to find half the daughter 
cells containing the large and half the small chromosome of this 
pair. ‘his is exactlythe case. Fig. 29 shows a representative of 
the cells possessing the large member. ‘This is especially clear in 
material very much destained, for here as in earlier stages this 
peculiar chromosome holds the color much longer than the others. 
In other respects the differential chromosomes are not different, 
but divide longitudinally with the ordinary chromosomes. 

In early anaphase each V_ shows a distinct split which 
lies in the plane of the equatorial plate (Fig. 31). ‘That this is 
the original longitudinal split seen in the early prophase 
(Figs. 9, 10) there can be no doubt. One chromosome which 
maintains its identity from the early prophase through the first 
spermatocyte as a cross is sufficient evidence. In the late prophase 
and even in metaphase this chromosome (Fig. 16, a—e) may be 
seen to be longitudinally split (Fig. 16, d). ‘The line of separation 
comes at right angles to this split and the univalent chromosomes 
pass to the poles as V’s, each possessing a longitudinal split, though 
it closes and cannot be seen again until the beginning of the follow- 
ing anaphase. [he chromosomes are typically arranged with the 
vertices attached to the fibers and pointing inward. In division 
they are drawn apart, at first with the ends of the V’s pointing pole- 
ward (Figs. 31, 32) but later the vertices turn toward the poles, 
though disorder often reigns until a comparatively late anaphase 
(Fig. 33). 

In the spermatids there is a great amount of archoplasm, and 
in the late telophase of the second division it occupies the greater 
part of the cytoplasmic area (Fig. 36). At a slightly later stage 
the mass loses entirely its fibrillar structure and, condensed some- 
what, lies as a gray sphere (iron haematoxylin) against the nucleus 
(Fig. 38). This is the so-called nebenkern of the spermatid. The 
entire cell now elongates, and with it the axial filament. ‘The 
archoplasm has assumed the form of a large pennant attached to 
the nuclear membrane, and the axial fiber runs throughout its 


length and often beyond (Fig. 39). 


588 W.N. Nowlin 


A cross-section of the tail at this time shows that the archo- 
plasmic sheath 1s folding about the axial fiber (Fig. 50, a). When 
the transformation is nearly complete the two cannot be distin- 
guished from each other, the sheath lies close against the filament 
with a thin layer of cytoplasm outside (Fig. 50, c). Often in cross- 
section the sheath appears to be split (Fig. 50, 5). Since longitu- 
dinal views of the tails reveal nothing of the kind we are left to 
conjecture that this appearance is due to oblique sections of 
the trough-shape sheath. The fact that many of these appear 
together does not alter the explanation, for the spermatozoa and 
late spermatids lie parallel in large numbers. 

‘There is one peculiar but apparently typical stage in the develop- 
ment of the tail: while the head is yet round, though the chromatin 
is much condensed, the axial filament has assumed a vacuolated 
appearance (Fig. 40). ‘This lasts until the head has begun to elon- 
gate, and is just over in Fig. 41. This is not to be confused with 
the rare occurrences of double filaments seen in Fig. 48. ‘These 
are the products of giant cells, due to a failure of one of the divi- 
sions of the spermatocytes or the spermatogonia to complete itself. 
‘They consequently have two centrosomes and two axial filaments. 
Paulmier (’99) found similar conditions in Anasa. As in other 
insects the axial filament in the Coleoptera seems to arise in close 
relation with the centrosome. 

After the vacuolated stage the tail narrows and lengthens much 
more and we see it in its final form in Fig. 44. All this time the 
head of the spermatid has been changing. The dense chromatin 
mass begins to lose its afhnity for haematoxylin until a deep gray 
results. Astructure is now revealed in the nucleus, not heretofore 
seen, a small densely staining nucleolus-like body (Fig. 36). “The 
nucleus now widens its circumference and the chromatin condenses 
around the membrane in the form of a ragged border (Fig. 38). 
The nucleolus-like formation is not seen at this stage, but the sup- 
position is that it is merely obscured by the dense patches of chro- 
matin, for in the next stage it again appears and occupies a most 
characteristic position: the chromatin is arranged in a crescent 
shape and the nucleolus lies in a clear area between the arms of 
the crescent (Fig. 37). The chromatin becomes diffuse and min- 


Spermatogenesis of Coptocycla 589 


gles with the karyolymph until a uniform light gray results (iron 
hematoxylin). The nucleolus is unaffected, changing neither in 
size nor in staining reaction. It makes a change, however, in 
position about this time, moving from a point opposite the cen- 
trosome around the periphery of the nucleus until it often lies 
very near the centrosome (Fig. 49). 

The head of the spermatid lengthens and becomes vacuolated 
(Fig. 41), but as the elongation increases the vacuoles disappear 
and the deeply staining nucleolus-like body is apparent, lying 
closely against the wren membrane (Fig. 45, y). Very soon this 
single hae breaks up into two, three or four parts and later, it 
disappears (Figs. 46 and 47). The head condenses, lengthens, 
stains intensely (Fig. 42), and in its final form is spirally twisted, 
resembling the Bacterium spirillum (Figs. 43, 44). 

It is impossible at the present time to say just what may be the 
function of this peculiar, deeply-staining spermatid element. 
It suggests the accessory of Orthoptera which can be seen 
in one-half the spermatids (McClung, ’99, ’oo, ’02a), and at 
first it seemed probable that it might be one of the idiochromo- 
somes in the beetle as it, in one stage, is about the size of the 
larger member of the pair. Instead of being found in half the 
cells, however, it is without doubt in all, so that this explanation 
was relinquished. It is characteristic of the spermatids of all the 
beetles thus far studied and of some of the Orthoptera. 


2. Coptocycla guttata 


In external appearance Coptocycla guttata is very different from 
C. aurichalcea, but in their germ cells there are many points of 
resemblance. Guttata has the smaller number of chromosomes, 
eighteen in the spermatogonia instead of twenty-two, but the 
size and form are much the same in the two species (Figs. 1 and 
51). The unequal pair is also present here. 

The suggestion that synapsis takes place immediately after the 
condensation stage in C. aurichalcea is strongly confirmed by gut- 
tata. In the condensation or synizesis stage, the chromatin is in 


590 W.N. Nowlin 


the form of loops with the ends against the side of the nuclear mem- 
brane (Fig. 52). [his point Montgomery calls the distal pole of 
the nucleus and the opposite point on the nuclear membrane, the 
central pole. ‘The process is the same as in aurichalcea; the loops 
straighten, thrust the free ends toward the central pole (Fig. 53), 
bend toward each other in pairs and unite end to end (Fig. 54). 
The end of the chromatin thread either bears an enlargement or 
stains more deeply in cross-section for there is at this point the 
appearance of a deeply staining bead. This dark spot thus con- 
veniently marks the place of union in the bivalent chromosomes, 
and we are led to the conclusion that the short loops first observed 
in the synizesis stage are univalent chromosomes. ‘The idiochro- 
mosome pair is véry distinct at the base of the loops as shown in 
Big.952: 

The growth stages are not unlike those of ©. aurichalcea. A 
spireme (Fig. 55) is formed which varies in its staining reactions 
exactly as in C. aurichalcea. ‘The thread finally breaks up into 
bivalent elements which stain deeply and exhibit a longitudinal 
split, and at last condense into forms which they retain during the 
first division. In Fig. 57 are seen two bivalent chromosomes 
assuming the form of rings, and in Fig. 58 these have closed 
together. ‘This figure also exhibits crosses which later change to 
the form found in aurichalcea. Most of the chromosomes of this 
species are dumb-bell-shaped in the first spermatocyte mitosis and 
of the usual V-shape in the second. ‘The idiochromosomes main- 
tain their spherical form throughout all the stages. 

An equatorial plate of the as maturation division shows nine 
chromosomes arranged usually in the order seen in Fig. 60, with 
seven in a circle about the other two. All nine chromosomes may 
be seen in lateral view in Fig. 59, where they are just coming into 
the spindle. ‘The dumb-bell shape of four is here quite evident 
and the unequal pair is conspicuous. ‘Three of the others appear 
as straight rods and the other one bent in V-form. 

During metaphase the chromosomes arrange themselves with 
their long axes parallel with the axis of the spindle, and later they 
divide at right angles to their length. In other words, they exhibit 
qualitative division, bivalent chromosomes separating at the point 


S permatogenesis of Coptocycla 591 


of union made during synapsis. Asa result the small chromosome 
of the idiochromosome pair goes to one pole while the large mem- 
ber goes to the other (Fig. 63). [he equatorial plates of the 
second division confirm this, half possessing nine V-shaped chro- 
mosomes of approximately equal size (Fig. 71), the other half hav- 
ing eight large and one small chromosome (Fig. 65). Fig. 66 
shows the second maturation spindle with this small chromosome 
in metaphase, and in Fig. 64 it is seen dividing. 

Here, then, as in C. aurichalcea and ‘Tenebrio molitor 
(Stevens, 05) half the spermatids will possess the small idiochro- 
mosome, and half the large. 

The transformation of the spermatid is essentially the same 
as in aurichalcea. ‘The deeply staining nucleolus-like body is 
present but no clue to its function or nature is given. ‘The ripe 
spermatozoa are not spirally twisted but resemble that stage of 
aurichalcea seen in Fig. 42. 

Owing to lack of material it was impossible to study the divid- 
ing somatic cells of the male and female forms. ‘That they would 
exhibit the same conditions found repeatedly in other beetles there 
is no doubt. By permission I have reproduced four drawings 
from Dr. Stevens’ originals, which show the chromosome of 
(Fig. 67) a somatic cell from the digestive tract of a male pupa 
of Tenebrio molitor, and (Fig. 68), a female somatic cell of the 
same form found in the egg follicle; (Fig. 69) a female somatic 
cell of Trirhabda virgata from the egg follicle, and (Fig. 70) a 
male somatic cell taken from the larval body of the same species. 

Here, as in other forms that Dr. Stevens has investigated, the 
small idiochromosome goes to the male and the large one to the 
female. 


SUMMARY OF OBSERVATIONS 


Coptocycla aurichalcea 


1 The spermatogonial number of chromosomes in Coptocycla 
aurichalcea is twenty-two, twenty-one of which are V-shaped and 
one very small one, spherical in form. 


592 W. N. Nowlin 


2 Synapsis takes place in a manner, so far not described but 
also observed in certain beetles now being investigated by Dr. 
N. M. Stevens. The loops seen in the synizesis stage, which 
represent individual chromosomes, straighten and unite in pairs 
by the free ends which are pushed up into the nuclear space. 
Pseudo-reduction, therefore, occurs just after synizesis and_ just 
before the formation of the spireme. 

3 The first maturation mitosis is a transverse, reducing divi- 
sion, the second longitudinal, occurring along a lengthwise split 
formed early in the prophase. 

4 There is present a typical pair of idiochromosomes [accord- 
ing to Wilson’s definition (’05 and ’06)| which, with the others. 
divides qualitatively in the first division and quantitatively in the 
second, the small member, therefore, going to one-half the sperma- 
tozoa and the large member to the other half. 

5 The chromosomes show marked individuality from the 
beginning of the prophase, one having the form of a ring, two of a 
cross, several the shape of an E, and finally the unequal pair. 


Coptocycla guttata 


1 The spermatogonial number of chromosomes is eighteen, 
seventeen large V-shaped chromosomes and one which is small 
and spherical. 

2 All other observations on this species confirm those on auri- 
chalcea. 


GENERAL DISCUSSION 


Individuality of the Chromosomes 


Convincing results have been published in regard to the individ- 
uality of the chromosomes by Boveri ('02), who found a difference 
in their function; by Sutton (’02), who found a difference in size; 
and by Baumgartner (04), and others, who have discovered a dif- 
ference of form. 

The idiochromosomes indicate so clearly a difference in function 
as well as in size that it is unnecessary to go into detail on this 


S permatogenesis of Coptocycla 593 


point. [he ordinary chromosomes also confirm the size difference; 
though no careful measurements were made, it is obvious that 
such differences exist at least in most cases (Fig. 19). However, 
the difference in form is most evident in Coptocycla aurichalcea, 
and furnishes another strong support for the doctrine of the 
individuality of the chromosomes. 

Of the eleven bivalent chromosomes several possess forms charac- 
teristic enough to mark them as the same in different generations; 
there are two or three crosses, several E’s,a ring and an unequal 
pair (Fig. 16). ‘There is, doubtless, a size difference that separ- 
ates those of a group though this is not as obvious in some cases 
as in others. ‘The ring form occurs only once in this species, but 
twice in guttata and maintains its identity of shape from an early 
prophase, being much more expanded then, however, than later. 
It is formed by the V-chromosomes of the spermatogonium uniting 
by both ends, as is plainly shown in Fig. 16, /, where two ends are 
not firmly joined. In a late metaphase the bivalent pair seems 
to elongate slightly; this closes the opening and gives the 
appearance seen in Fig. 23. In profile the ring assumes an oval 
form, with the central opening much smaller (Fig. 16, 7), but this 
is distinguished from the actually elongated phase by the length 


(lite. 23). 


Mendel’s Law 


Assuming that the idiochromosomes are sex-chromosomes, or 
represent the sex-characters, they furnish excellent opportunities 
for speculation on the application of Mendel’s law to chromo- 
somes. 

If the Mendelian principles of segregation apply to sex, there 
should be in the second generation 25 per cent males, 50 per cent 
hybrids, and 25 per cent females. Castle (03) concludes that 
there are no individuals pure in regard to the sex-character, but 
only hybrids are produced. Now to apply this theory to the bee- 
tles. We know by actual observation that the male somatic cells 
possess the small idiochromosomes, and the female somatic cells 
the large one. Below are given in a schematic representation 


594. W. N. Nowlin 


two kinds of eggs and two kinds of spermatozoa with the dot 
above to indicate the idiochromosome and the letter to show the sex 


ae 


There are four possible combinations here: 


er (FERTILE MALE) = 4 


—@e (INFERTILE FEMALE) =f 


@ 
ic =@@ ([\WFERTILE MALE) = 
: 
& 
? 


— @@e (Fee Br FEMALE ) =4 


Now by actual examination of the male and female somatic 
cells we know that no such combinations as (2) and (3) exist. 
Our observations, therefore, strongly support the theory of selec- 
tive fertilization, only gametes bearing opposite sex-characters 
fusing. 

While the beetle furnishes no explanation why femaleness as 
a character often dominates (Castle ’03) yet its chromosomes 
indicate why it 1s recessive a part of the time in these insects. In 
the 50 percent of spermatozoa that carry the female character in 
the form of the small idiochromosome, this is overpowered by the 
much larger male idiochromosome in the eggs with which they 
unite. Whenthe chance is even, 7. e., when the idiochromosomes 
are equal in size, then femaleness invariably dominates. 


Spermatogenesis of Coptocycla 595 


The Idiochromosomes 


But two groups of animals thus far investigated are known to 
possess the idiochromosomes; these are the Hemiptera and 
the Coleoptera. It is probable that they exist in other forms 
and have been wrongly classified as was the case with Paulmier’s 
(99) work on Anasa, and Montgomery’s (01) on Coenus delius 
and Euschistus tristigmus. Paulmier failed to distinguish between 
the accessory and the microchromosomes, while Montgomery saw 
the small idiochromosomes in the resting stages and the different 
number of chromosomes in different cysts, but misinterpreted 
them. 

To Wilson is due the correct interpretation of these idiochro- 
mosomes in the Hemiptera heteroptera, asa distinct pair of chromo- 
somes with a definite and characteristic behavior in the different 
generations. 

Slightly earlier than this and independently Dr. Stevens work- 
ing on one of the Coleoptera, ‘Tenebrio molitor, found an unequal 
pair of chromosomes, which proves upon comparison to be essen- 
tially the same as those of the Hemiptera. 

In details of behavior, however, these chromosomes differ mark- 
edly in the two groups. As in Lygzeus and Coenus (Wilson, ’05) 
the idiochromosomesof Coptocycla maintain their identity through- 
out the growth period. ‘They are first distinguished in the synize- 
sis stage as a compact, spheroidal body at the base of the chromatin 
loops (Fig. 52). The small member has not been observed at this 
stage, but it is doubtless present and is obscured by the larger of 
the idiochromosome pair. As this pair appears in the spheroidal 
form, so early in the growth stage, it is a question whether it ever 
assumes the form of loops with the other chromosomes. 

In Lygzeus in the synizesis stage the larger differential chromo- 
some is elongated and bent U-shape. In the post-synaptic phase 
instead of lengthening and splitting longitudinally as in Lygzus, 
both of the idiochromosomes of Coptocycla maintain their com- 
pact, rounded form. In early prophase these chromosomes in 
Hemiptera exhibit a bipartite structure and sometimes remain 
separated while the ordinary chromosomes have fused in synapsis. 


596 W. N. Nowlin 


In Coptocycla the unequal pair has fused, but neither member 
shows any tendency toward being bipartite. In fact, at no time 
is there a hint of a longitudinal split, unless the vacuolated 
appearance of the large idiochromosome in the early growth stage 
may be considered as such. 

The later behavior of the idiochromosomes is markedly different 
in the two groups. In the Hemiptera they remain separated and 
univalent in the first maturation mitosis, but at the close of this 
division their products conjugate to form a dyad, which in all but 
one form, Nezara, is asymmetrical. Inthe Coleoptera the behavior 
during mitosis is exactly like that of the ordinary chromosomes; 
having united in the synapsis stage to form a bivalent, they divide 
transversely in the first mitosis and longitudinally in the second. 
The result is, of course, the same in both cases; the spermatids 
are of two kinds as regards the idiochromosomes, half possessing 
the small and half the large one. The distribution of these chro- 
mosomes to the somatic cells of the two sexes is also the same in 
the two groups; the male cells contain the smaller, the females 
cells the larger idiochromosome. 


Sex-determination 


The observations recorded in the present paper add nothing 
new to the subject of sex-determination, and their chief value con- 
sists in their confirmation of the very suggestive work on Tenebrio 
molitor (Stevens, ’05). 

Since McClung (00) advanced his theory that the accessory 
chromosome of the Orthoptera is a sex-determinant, numerous 
investigators have sought evidence for or against it. The first in 
its favor was that of Sutton (02), who found the odd chromosome 
present in the male somatic cells and absent in the female cells of 
Brachystola magna (23 in & cells, 22 in @ cells). Since that 
time, however, Wilson has found the reverse true for Hemiptera, 
1. e., theadditional chromosome in the female somatic cells (Anasa 
21 #, 22 2), and this leads him to question Sutton’s count for the 
Orthoptera. Wilson suggests that the accessory chromosome of 
Anasa, Protenor and the Orthoptera is the homologue of the larger 


Spermatogenesis of Coptocycla 597 


member of the idiochromosome group found in Coenus, Lygzus 
and certain other Hemiptera, and its missing mate is the homologue 
of the small idiochromosome. He thus supports the view of Paul- 
mier and Montgomery in regard to degenerating chromosomes. 

Knowing from his observations on Anasa that the accessory 
goes to the female in the Hemiptera, he, therefore, conjectured 
that the larger of the idiochromosomes would be found in the 
female somatic cells, and the small one in the male somatic cells, 
and he later found positive proof of this. 

As stated before Dr. Stevens while investigating a form of beetle, 
Tenebrio molitor, found that an unequal pair of chromosomes is 
present, the large one of which goes to the female somatic cells, 
and the small one to the male somatic cells. Since then she has 
confirmed this in other species of Coleoptera, so that at last there 
seems to be much indisputable evidence for the chromosome-sex- 
determinant theory. 

The question arises, what is to be done with such forms as Ther- 
mopsis (Stevens, 05) and Banasa (Wilson, ’05). In the former 
there are no chromosomes which have any external peculiarity 
that would mark them as sex-determinants; in the latter there are 
two sets of chromosomes that we interpret as sex-determinants 
when they appear separately in other forms. 

The solution for Thermopsis is perhaps less difhcult than for 
Banasa; one pair of the chromosomes may bear the sex-character 
although there are no external differences. ‘This does not conflict 
with present views. In Banasa, the situation is different. Here 
we have a pair of idiochromosomes as well as a typical accessory. 
In other cases Wilson has suggested that the large idiochromo- 
some is the homologue of the accessory inasmuch as both are 
members of pairs, one member of which is degenerating or has 
already disappeared. ‘This seems plausible and if taken from 
the standpoint of degeneration only, there is nothing conflicting 
in the fact that two pairs of chromosomes even in the same cell 
are undergoing the change. In fact, as Wilson suggests, it adds 
weight to the idea of degenerating chromatin, for we have no 
reason to suppose that degeneration is necessarily limited to one 
pair. 


598 W.N. Nowlin 


However, taken from the standpoint of sex-determination it 
seems to offer serious difficulties. While we are perhaps not justi- 
fied in assuming that the sex character is confined to one chro- 
mosome, or to one pair of chromosomes, all recent observations 
point that way; then on a priori grounds it seems improbable that 
the same function should be assigned to two pairs of chromosomes 
in the same cell. With the numerous characters to be transmitted 
it would seem more likely that one pair of chromosomes must be 
the bearer of many qualities. 

In Banasa there are four classes of spermatozoa which contain: 


(1) the small idiochromosome + the accessory; 
(2) the large idiochromosome + the accessory; 
(3) the small idiochromosome — the accessory; 
(4) the large idiochromosome — the accessory. 


If the chromosome relations in the male somatic cells of Banasa 
correspond to those in Lygzus and Anasa, only one of these classes 
of spermatozoa (3) can be used. Any combination of (1), (2), 
(4) would refute the idea of homology of these two types of chro- 
mosomes. ‘The same thing is true as to the production of females; 
but one kind of spermatozoon (2) can be functional. 

‘This means that three-fourths of the spermatozoa are function- 
less as regards production of males, three-fourths as regards pro- 
duction of females and one-half absolutely functionless. Such a 
condition seems most improbable, but, of course, the only way of 
determining it is to study the male and female somatic cells of 
Banasa. ‘Ihis promises interesting results as it will either refute 
the homology theory (for this form, at least) or reveal new facts in 
regard to selective fertilization and a functional and non-functional 
condition of spermatozoa. 

While Castle believes there is no hard and fast rule for domi- 
nance in the sex character in dicecious individuals, it seems that 
in the beetles we have a clear case of female dominance. In cells 
where the male and female sex chromosomes are equal in size, 
femaleness invariably dominates. Where they are unequal (the 
female small, as in Coleoptera and some Hemiptera, or entirely 
missing as in Orthoptera) then the female character is visibly 


Spermatogenesis of Coptocycla 599 


recessive, due merely to its reduced strength or entire disappear- 
ance in the male. 

The results pointed out for Tenebrio and confirmed in this paper 
are briefly as follows: an unequal pair of chromosomes is present 
which, we have strong evidence for believing, transmit or deter- 
mine the character of sex. The fact that the small one invariably 
occurs in the male somatic cells and the large one in the female 
somatic cells seems on first thought to mean that the small one 
carries maleness and the large one femaleness. But Wilson’s 
thorough analysis of the conditions in Hemiptera (’06) has shown 
that the larger idiochromosome which alternates between the 
sexes must bear the male character, while the small idiochro- 
mosome, which is confined to the male sex, must represent the 
recessive form of the female character. 

While it has been proved beyond doubt that certain chromo- 
somes are concerned in the determination of sex among insects, 
a general application of the theory cannot yet be made, but it must 
for the present be limited to those forms possessing the accessory 
or the idiochromosomes. Without doubt further investigation 
will reveal either similar differential chromosomes in all forms, or 
show something homologous to them. It may not be a difference 
of size or shape that will distinguish them from the ordinary chro- 
mosomes, but probably one of behavior. Even in case the last 
difference is not found it need not disprove the chromosome-sex- 
determinant theory, for the sex character doubtless can be carried 
like other pairs of antagonistic characters without affecting the 
chromosome visibly. ‘The eggs of comparatively few forms have 
been investigated, and it is possible that a dimorphism of the 
chromosomes in the maturated oocytes may be found in certain 
groups. 


Biological Laboratory, Bryn Mawr College 
April 30, 1906 


600 W.N. Nowlin 


LITERATURE 


BaumGarTNER, W. J., ’04.—Some New Evidences for the Individuality of the 
Chromosomes. Biol. Bull., vol. viii, No. 1. 
Bovert, Tu., ’02.—Ueber Mehrpolige mitosen als mittel zur Analyse des Zell- 
kerns. Vehr. d. Phys. Med. Ges. zu Wiirzburg, N. F., Bd. xxxv. 
CastLE, W. E., ’03.—The Heredity of Sex. Bull of the Mus. of Comp. Zool., 
Harvard College, vol. xl, No. 4. 
McCuune, C. E., ’99.—A Peculiar Nuclear Element in the Male Reproduc- 
tion Cells of Insects. Zool. Bull., vol. ii. 
’00.—The Spermatocyte Divisions of the Acrididz. Kans. Univ. Quart., 
vol. ix, No. 1. 
’02.—The Accessory Chromosome—Sex-Determinant ? Biol. Bull.,vol. iii, 
Nos. I and II. 
’o2a.—The Spermatocyte Divisions of the Locustide. Kans. Univ. 
Quart, vol. i, No. 8. 
Montcom_ery, Tu., ’03.—The Heterotypic Maturation Mitosis in Amphibia 
and its General Significance. Biol. Bull., vol. iv, ’02—’03. 
Stevens, N. M., ’05.—Studies in Spermatogenesis with Especial Reference to 
the Accessory Chromosome. Carnegie Institute of Wash., Pub. 
No. 36. 
Sutton, W. S., ’°02.—On the Morphology of the Chromosome Group in Brachy- 
stola magna. Biol. Bull., vol. iv, No. 1. 
Witson, E. B., ’05.—I. The Behavior of the Idiochromosomes in Hemiptera. 
Journ. Exp. Zool., vol. ii, No. 3. 
’05.—II. The Paired Microchromosomes and Heterotropic Chromo- 
somes in Hemiptera. Journ. Exp. Zool., vol. ii, No. 4. 
06. III. The Sexual Differences of the Chromosome-Groups in 
Hemiptera, with Some Considerations on the Determination 
and Inheritance of Sex. Journ. Exp. Zool., vol. tii, No. 1. 


DESCRIPTION OF PLATES 


All drawings were made with the aid of a camera lucida. A Zeiss oil-immersion 2 mm. objec- 
tive and oc. 12 were used throughout, and the drawings have been reduced one-third. 


Pirate I 
Coptocycla aurichalcea 


Fig. 1. Equatorial’ plate of spermatogonial mitosis, 22 chromosomes. 

Fig. 2. Bouquet-stage or synizesis, showing univalent chromosomes in loop form. 

Fig. 3. Later stage in which loops are straightening. 

Fig. 4. Cross-section of the stage in Fig. 2. 

Fig. 5. Synapsis. Fusion of univalent chromosomes end to end at y. Large idiochromosome 
x, at base of loops. 

Fig. 6. Late synaptic stage, showing the relative lengths of the bivalent elements. 

Fig. 7. Formation of spireme; x, the large idiochromosome. 

Fig. 8. A slightly later stage; spireme very pale, « deeply stained. 

Figs. 9,10. Very early prophase, showing the longitudinal split in the chromosomes, idiochro- 
mosome excepted. 

Fig. 11. Still later prophase. Both members of the unequal pair visible (x). 

Figs.12,13. Prophase in which the chromosomes are contracting into their characteristic forms. 

Fig. 14. An equatorial plate of the first division showing nine of the eleven chromosomes. The 
idiochromosomes retain the stain much longer than the others. 

Fig. 15. An equatorial plate of the first mitosis with the full number, eleven chromosomes, pres- 
ent. The large linen fibers are seen attached to the chromosomes. 

Fig. 16. Three types of chromosomes, found in C. aurichalcea, from various points of view, 
a—e, the cross-shaped chromosome; a, between a front and side view; b, front view when the longi- 
tudinal split is closed; c and e, profile; d, front view showing split, f/—h, the E-shaped chromosome 
f, front view; g, profile, bivalent chromosome dividing; h, profile; /—o, ring-shaped chromosomes: 
], the two univalent chromosomes not well fused; m, probably later stage in which union is more com- 
plete; 7, ring closed and seen in profile; 0, central opening growing smaller. 

Fig. 17. Late prophase of first spematocyte; chromosomes coming into equatorial plate; «, the 
idiochromosome pair. 

Fig. 18. Equatorial plate formed and one chromosome divided ahead of the others. 

Fig. 19. Shows the size difference in some of the chromosomes. 

Fig.20. Anaphase of the first division, showing distinctly the form of the univalent chromosomes, 
and the manner of separation. 

Fig. 21. Side view, first spindle, showing the densely staining idiochromosome when the others 
are very pale. 

Figs. 22-24. Different views of the ring chromosome. 

Fig. 25. Idiochromosomes dividing in first mitosis; /, large chromosome; s, small chromosome. 

Fig. 26. Late anaphase of first spermatocyte. 

Figs. 27,28. Equatorial plates of second mitosis, showing 10 large and one small (s)chromosome. 

Fig. 29. Equatorial plate of same mitosis, showing 11 large chromosomes, /, the large idiochro- 
mosome. 

Fig. 30. Prophase of second mitosis, showing 10 large chromosomes and the small idiochro- 
mosome. 

Fig. 31. Anaphase in which the large idiochromosome is dividing. 


SPERMATOGENESIS OF COPTOCYCLA 
W. N. Now iin 


= A‘ a, 
; é 
Sie 

~ Se 


igs) VIE 
Saree vi 


“15 
ar Yi TE. 
18 


r) af 
: 28 29 


Journat or ExperIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. II, NO. 4 


PLATE I 


/4 


ve 


19 


Prate II 


Coptocycla aurichalcea 


Fig. 32. Anaphase of second maturation division, showing the position assumed by the chro- 
mosomes when first separated. 

Fig. 33. Late anaphase; small idiochromosomes dividing. 

Fig. 34. Late anaphase; unusual case in which the form of the chromosomes is not obscured by 
massing. 

Fig. 35. The usual late anaphase. 

Fig. 36. Very early spermatid showing a great amount of archoplasm. 

Fig. 37. Nucleus at a slightly later stage, showing the peculiar nucleolus-like body. 

Fig. 38. Archoplasmic substance in the form of a sphere applied to nuclear membrane. 

Fig. 39. Archoplasm elongated into a pennant form with axial filament running throughout. 

Fig. 40. Vacuolated stage in formation of the tail. 

Fig. 41. Vacuolated appearance of head c, centrosome. 

Figs. 42-44. Later stage in the transformation of the head. Ripe spermatozoa (44) possess a 
spirally twisted head. 

Figs. 45-47. Later behavior of the nucleolus-like body. 

Fig. 48. Giant spermatozoa, with two centrosomes and two axial filaments. 

Fig. 49. Shows the migration of the nucleolus-like body. 

Fig. 50. Cross-sections of the tail of the spermatid, a—b, at about the stages shown in Fig. 41; 
c, in Fig. 44. 3 


Coptocycla guttata 


Fig. 51. Equatorial plate of spermatogonial mitosis; 18 chromosomes. 

Fig. 52. Synizesis stage showing univalent chromosomes in loop form. Large idiochromosome 
at base of loops. 

Fig. 53. Later stage in which loops are straightening and bending toward each other. 

Fig. 54. Synapsis. The dark bead at the highest part of loop marks point of union. 

Fig. 55. Spireme stage; chromatin very pale with exception of unequal pair (x). 

Fig. 56. Same stage, showing small idiochromosome somewhat removed from the large member 
and connected with it by a chromatin strand. 

Figs. 57, 58. Formation of ring and cross-shaped chromosomes. 

Fig. 59. Late prophase in which all nine chromosomes are seen from a side view. «, the idio- 
chromosome pair. 

Fig. 60. Equatorial plate of the first maturation mitosis; 9 chromosomes. 

Figs. 61,62. Prophase and metaphase, respectively, showing the idiochromosome pair («). 

Fig. 63. Anaphase of first mitosis; the idiochromosome pair, dividing qualitatively. s, small 
idiochromosome; /, large. 

Fig. 64. Very early anaphase of second mitosis, showing division of the small (s) idiochromosome. 

Fig. 65. An equatorial plate of the second spermatocyte in which the small idiochromosome 
(s) is present. 

Fig. 66. Metaphase of second spermatocyte. The small idiochromosome (s) undivided. 

Fig. 67. Equatorial plate from dividing somatic cell of male pupa (Tenebrio molitor), showing 
nineteen large and one small chromosome. 

Fig. 68. Equatorial plate of a dividing cell of follicle of a young egg (Tenebrio molitor), showing 
twenty large chromosomes. 

Fig. 69. Equatorial plate of a dividing follicle cell of a young egg (Trirhabda virgata), showing 
twenty-eight large chromosomes. ; 

Fig. 70. Equatorial plate from the somatic cells of a male larva (Trirhabda virgata), showing 
twenty-seven large and one small chromosome. 

Fig. 71. An equatorial plate of same spermatocyte in which all nine chromosomes are of approxi- 


mately equal size. 


SPERMATOGENESIS OF COPTOCYCLA 
W. N. Now.in 


> 


A 


2 


U4,~° 
te 


PLATE II 


Mim 


o3 of 


Journat or ExPeriMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. II, NO. 4, 


TORSION AND OTHER TRANSITIONAL PHENOM- 
ENA IN THE, REGENERATION OF THE CHELIPED 
OF THE LOBSTER (HOMARUS AMERICANUS) 


BY 


VICTOR E. EMMEL 
Wirn Two Pirates 
INTRODUCTION 


In the course of a series of experiments and observations on 
the phenomena of regeneration in the lobster, which necessitated 
a detailed study of the external morphological characteristics of 
the regenerating cheliped (’05, ’06), it was noticed that in the 
successive stages of development there occurred a gradual torsion 
of the regenerating chelz. 

This and some other closely related phenomena do not seem 
to have been hitherto recorded, although the regeneration of the 
crustacean limbs has been the subject of extensive investigation 
by Herrick (’95), Morgan (’02), Przibram (’o1, 02), Steele (’04), 
Schultz (05), Wilson (703), and other writers. “These phenom- 
ena offer new material for the study of the phylogenetic signifi- 
cance of the regenerative process. 

The problems of development are now being approached by 
the experimental method, and interpretations are based on 
physiological and mechanical princlples. And yet while the 
processes of development may be essentially physiological in 
nature and affected by mechanical factors, still the important 
truth remains that the organization of the cells is an “inheritance 
from the past,” and that consequently the method of develop- 
ment may still furnish evidence of phylogenetic value. Wilson’s 
(’94) valuation of the embryological phenomena may apply 
equally well to those of regeneration: “the idioplasm of every 
species has arisen through the modification of pre-existing 


Tue JourNat or EXPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. III, NO. 4. 


604 Victor E. Emmel 


idioplasm, and every response that it gives to stimulus is an 
expression of its past history. Hence, we need not despair of 
ultimate success in the attempt to decipher the meaning of the 
embryological record, and to find in ontogeny a real criterion 
of homology, and it is here that we find encouragement, if any is 
needed, not to relax our efforts to investigate the normal phenom- 
enon of comparative embryology on the largest scale and down 
to the minutest detail” (p.123). It is with the purpose of contri- 
buting something to the facts bearing on these problems that the 
following observations are presented. 

These observations were made at the Anatomical Laboratory of | 
Brown University, and at the Experiment Station of the Rhode 
Island Commission of Inland Fisheries, where the lobster hatchery 
furnished unsurpassed facilities to obtain material for expert 
mental work. I desire here to express my thanks to Dr. Mead 
for his interest and helpful suggestions in the present study. 


I. ON THE REGENERATION OF THE CHELIPED 


a. Method 


In order to trace the transformations involved in the regenera- 
tion of the cheliped, a series of drawings have been made to 
illustrate successive stages in the process. 

The material for this was obtained from about 75 fifth-stage 
lobsters. ‘These lobsters were practically equal in size and age, 
and had all molted to the fifth stage, July 30 and 31. On August 1, 
the right and left chelipeds were autotomously removed from 
each individual. Every twelve hours, two or three of these 
lobsters were killed in corrosive acetic and preserved in alcohol. 
This was continued until August 13, when some of the remaining 
lobsters had begun to moult to the sixth stage. By this method, 
regenerating finbe were obtained in a series, ranging from 
minute papillae to fully formed functional structure. These 
preserved lobsters were then carefully examined and eight 
specimens selected which presented typical stages in the differen- 
tiation and development. 


Regeneration of the Cheliped of the Lobster 605 


The data for these eight specimens may be tabulated as follows: 


Removed | a No. of days re- | ; 
No. Stage Chetpeds) | Killed montane | Drawing 
I Vi |) Aug. 1, 8\p.m. Aug. 2,8 p.m. Tee dayaunta| ieesbie wT 
2 Vs |sAug. 1,8 p.m. Aug. 5,8 a.m. 3h days | Fig. IIT 
3 V | Aug.1,8p.m. | Aug. 6,8 p.m. 5 days | Figs.I,IV, XV 
a Vv Aug.1,8p.m. | Aug. 7,8 p.m. 6 days | Fig.V 
5 V | Aug. 1, 8 p.m. Aug. 8 7 days) Vio BigsVil 
6 V | Aug. 1, 8 p.m. Aug. 9,8 p.m. Sedaysiy eles ehigeViel 
7 Vv Aug.1,8p.m. | Aug. 13,8 p.m. 12 days | Figs, VIII, XVI 
8 VI | Aug. 1, 8 p.m. Aug. 15, 8 a.m. 133 days | Figs. IX, XVII. 


Camera lucida drawings were made of a right cheliped of each 
lobster. ‘The drawings were taken as nearly as possible from a 
constant plane (Fig. I), which shows a regenerating bud (reg) 
on the right basipodite (bs) of lobster No. 3. Each lobster 
was laid flat on its left side upon the microscopic stand, thus 
giving in every case, a postero-lateral view of the basipodite and 
regenerating cheliped. 


b. Torsion of the Regenerating Cheliped (Plate I) . 
Kus 
Hirst Day CLlobster No. 1, Fig. Il). . During’ the fist and 


second days after the removal of the cheliped, only a very 
slight regeneration, if any, can be detected over the exposed 
surface of the basipodite (bs). ‘The first external indication of 
activity among the regenerating cells, is a minute light colored 
papilla pushing up through the dark clot of blood in the region (b&). 

Third Day (Lobster No. 2, Fig. III). By the third day a distinct 
bud has appeared. Seen from the exterior, it is a simple club- 
shaped mass enclosed within an epithelial membrane or sac. 
The only indication of future segments and joints is a slight 
cleavage or groove (a), forming at the apex of the bud. This 
groove marks the first development of the claw; 1.e., the dactyl- 
opodite (dc), and propodite (pr). 

Fifth Day (Lobster No. 3, Fig. IV). At this stage of develop- 
ment the outlines of the dactylopodite (dc), and propodite (pr) are 
becoming more definite, as indicated by the constrictions for the 


606 Victor E. Emmel 


first (1) and second (2) joints, (counting from the distal segment). 
The groove (a) for the two jaws of the claw has grown deeper. 

Sixth Day (Lobster No. 4, Fig. V). The anlage of all the 
segments of the appendage have appeared; 7. e., the dactyl- 
opodite (dc), propodite (pr), carpopodite (cp), meropodite (me), 
and ischiopodite (7s). “The dactylopodite is now more completely 
differentiated from the propodite. 

It is interesting to note here that the direction of differentiation 
is from the distal portion backward toward the proximal region 
of the bud, a method of differentiation characteristic of regener- 
ation in many animal forms (see Zeleny, ’06; Child ’o6, p. 410). 

Seventh and Eighth Days (Lobsters Nos. 5 and 6, Figs. VI and 
VIL) The four joints and five segments are clearly defined and 
the whole bud is now assuming a forward curvature. 

The important fact for our present purpose in the six stages 
just described, is the position or spatial relations of the two 
terminal segments (7. ¢., the dactylopodite and propodite) with 
reference to the basal segment of the limb. 

It should be observed that each figure represents a nearly 
constant point of view—the postero-lateral aspect shown in Fig. I. 
The plane of the paper thus presents a common plane of reference 
and comparison in each case. Let this plane be called the 
“reference plane.”’ 

Now if in Figs. III and IV, we compare the position of the two 
segments of the claw (dc and pr), it may be seen that the plane 
of cleavage for the two jaws of the claw, or groove (a) is practi- 
cally at right angles to our “reference plane.” In other words, 
if we compare the “biting plane” of the claw at this early period 
with the normal position of the body, the claw will open almost 
vertically upward. 

The same is practically true for Figs. Vand VI. In Fig. VII, 
however, the plane of cleavage (a) is no longer vertical to the 
“reference plane.’”’ ‘This is due to the fact that a slight twisting 
or torsion of the terminal segments has begun. ‘The dactylopo- 
dite (dc) and propodite (pr), and possibly the carpopodite (cp), 
are gradually turning in such a way as to throw the dactylopcdite 
farther inward toward the median line of the body; so that at 


Regeneration of the Cheliped of the Lobster 607 


this stage, the “biting plane” of the claw is inclined away from 
the “reference plane” at an angle of about 20°. The later 
phases of this torsion appear in the further development. 

Twelfth Day (Lobster No.7, Fig. VIII). At this period of the 
regenerating cheliped the lobster is rapidly approaching the molt. 
The segments and joints are now almost completely developed. 
The regenerating limb is assuming a rounded, plump appearance, 
as the result of the large growth of tissue compressed within the 
epithelial membrane. ‘The significant fact here is the position of 
the claw. ‘The torsion has progressed to such a degree that the 
plane passing through the dactylopodite (dc) and propodite (pr) 
or “biting plane,” is now inclined at an angle of about 45° to its 
original position. 

Thirteenth Day (Lobster No. 8, Fig. EX). ‘This phase of the 
development marks the culmination of both the regenerative 
process and the torsion of the chela. At this stage the lobster 
has moulted its old shell, and Fig. 1X, represents the completely 
regenerated limb with its joints and segments expanded to their 
normal shape. 

The complete torsion of the chela is now readily perceived. 
Instead of being parallel, as in Figs. III and IV, the “biting 
plane” of the dactylopodite (dc) and propodite (pr) is at right 
angles to the “reference plane.” Thus it is clearly evident, 
that beginning with the earliest phase of the regenerating cheliped, 
with the claw nearly vertical in position, a torsion of the terminal 
segments has gradually taken place during the development, so 
that the dactylopodite and propodite have rotated through an angle 
of g0°, and attained the horizontal position of the normal claw. 


c. Relative Size of the two Segments of the Claw (Plate I) 


In the earliest period at which the differentiation of the claw 
is apparent in the regenerating bud (Fig. III), the partially 
developed propodite (pr’) of the claw is relatively smaller than 
the opposing dactylopodite (dc), a relation which 1s just opposite 
to that which holds in the adult structure. 

On the fifth day (Fig. IV) there is less difference in size, although 


608 Victor E. Emmel 


the dactylopodite (dc) is still clearly larger and farther developed 
than the propodite (pr’) element of the claw. By the sixth day, 
however (Fig. V), this relative inequality has disappeared and 
the two segments are practically equal. ; 

The seventh day (Fig. V1) marks the transition to a relation in 
which the propodite jaw (pr’) is slightly more developed, and by 
the eighth day (Fig. VII) it clearly forms the larger segment of 
the claw. During the remaining development, the propodite and 
dactylopodite gradually acquire the proportions of the normally 
developed chela (Figs. VII] and IX). ‘Thus, according to these 
observations on the regenerating chela, it is evident that in the 
course of its differentiation the claw develops from an early stage 
in which the propodite portion of the claw 1s proportionately 
smaller, to a stage in which it is proportionately larger than the 
opposing dactylopodite. 


d. Transitional Characters in the Regenerated Crushing Claw 
(Plate IT) 


In the lobster the chelipeds are differentiated into two types of 
claws, the “toothed” or “nipping” claw, and the “‘crushing” claw. 
When the “crushing” cheliped has been amputated and another 
cheliped regenerated from the breaking plane, the claw of the 
latter frequently displays such a close resemblance to a “nipping”’ 
claw that it is not easy to distinguish the regenerated crusher from 
a normal “nipping” claw. 

Figs. XXI, XXII and XXIII represent the normal and regener- 
ated chelipeds of a seventh(?) stage lobster. ‘The normal left 
“nipping’ and the right “crushing” claws are shown in Figs. 


"At this stage the two types of claws are clearly differentiated, although it should be observed that the 
older the animal the more highly developed the ‘‘crusher” becomes. This specimen was one of a 
number of lobsters used in an experiment upon the reversal of the chele, and in this individual the ‘‘ crusher” 
had been removed later than the “‘nipper”’ in order, if possible, to give an advantage to the regener- 
ating “‘nipper.”” It might be added that although the regenerated chelipeds frequently appeared so 
much alike that it was necessary to wait until another moult before it could be determined whether a 
crusher had been developed or not, still no conclusive evidence was obtained that ‘‘reversal’’ ever 
occurs in either young or old lobsters, a result similar to that of Przibram’s (’02) on the European 


lobster. 


Regeneration of the Cheliped of the Lobster 609 
XXI and XXII, respectively, and the regenerated right “crusher” 
in Fig. XXIII. 

Both chelipeds were autotomously removed, the left on July 30 
and the right August 4. ‘Iwenty-seven days later, the lobster 
moulted and measured26.5mm._ Both chelipeds had regenerated, 
but in this individual as well as in many of the other lobsters used 
in the experiment indicatedin the footnote, the claws looked very 
much alike. ‘The regenerated left chela (“nipper’’) had all the 
characteristics of the original “nipper;” the right chela, on the 
contrary, was unlike the original “crusher” and showed characters 
transitional between the original “nipping” and “crushing” 
claws. 

Ina morphological comparison of the regenerated right “crush- 
ing”’ claw with the original “nipping” and “crushing” claws the 
following contrasts may be observed (see Plate IJ) 


OriGINAL Ricut Craw | REGENERATED RiGut CLAw 


(Fig. XXII) (Fig. XXIII) 


| Oricrnat Lert CLaw 


(Fig. XX1) 


Hormof claw... ..6.- |Proportionately elon- | Proportionately thicker | Proportionately elongated 


gated and slender and more massive and slender 


Ratio of breadth to 


6 ay : : : 
length of claw....... ee Se 26 SS EaRs = .34 HEED .27 
1omm g.8 mm. 9-3 mm. 
Dactylopodite ......... Slender and nearly | Distinctly crooked and | Almost straight and com- 
straight stubby paratively slender 
| 
Dentition ............ Pointed cutting teeth, | Characteristic broad | Cutting teeth predominate. 


grouped in periodic | 
sequence (p,p) with 


formula 1:3:5:7* 


Number of spines (sp) 
on inner border of 


LOO emer ereyeeieters 5 


Tactile hairs on inner | 
and outer border of | 


Glawsyere rors 2 | Very numerous 


crushing tubercle + (t.t. 
etc.), with only a rudi- 


ment of periodic se- 


quence (f’) 


wa 


Sparse { ' | 


Sequence of 1:3:5:7 well 


marked (p) 


Quite numerous 


| 
Dominant morphologi- 
“Nipping” type 


cal characters 


“Crushing” type | 


Transitional between 
“nipping” and ‘‘crush- 


ing” types 


*According to Stahr’s schema. (Stahr, °98, p. 459) 


{Formed by a fusion of periodic teeth. 


(Herrick, ’o5) 


tin an older lobster, tactile hairs on the crushing claw almost entirely disappear. 


610 Victor E. Emmel 


From this comparison, it is evident that the claw on the regen- 
erated right cheliped differs from the original “crusher” and 
closely resembles the “nipping” claw, in general form and 
proportions, the character and arrangement of teeth, and even in 
the number of spines and tactile hairs. The conclusion seems 
clear that the cheliped, which regenerates after the removal of the 
“crusher,” is a type transitional between the original “nipping” 
and “crushing” claws. 

In regard to the universal character of this rule it as already 
been intimated that the transitional type may not always be pres- 
ent in the regenerated “crusher,”’ especially in the older lobsters. 
This may be partially due to the conditions under which the limb 
was removed; 7.¢., if it is removed early in the interval between 
two moults, the regenerating bud will have a longer time to develop, 
and may consequently be farther advanced in its differentiation 
toward the original type before the next moult occurs. 


II. COMPARISON WITH THE NORMAL DEVELOPMENT OF THE 
CHELIPED 


a. Torsion in the Larval Stages (Plate IT) 


The torsion of the limb in regeneration which has just been 
described, was discovered before I had seen Herrick’s (05a) 
work on normal torsion. I have since gone over the larval 
development of the lobster on some carefully preserved material 
which Mr. P. B. Hadley kindly gave me, and have verified 
Herrick’s work and made drawings to facilitate comparison with 
the regenerating appendages. 

In the first larval stage; 7.e., just after hatching, the claw of the 
cheliped, as well as the claws of the first and second thoracic 
legs, opens in a vertical plane with a slight inclination outward. 
A dorsal view (Fig. XVIII) shows the dactylopodite (dc) in a 
position vertically above the propodite (pr). 

In the second larval stage a change in position begins, and in 
the third stage (Fig. XIX) the dactylopodite (dc) and propodite 
(pr) have turned over and inward, so that the claw now opens 
at anangle of about 45° to its former plane. ‘The rotation of the 


Regeneration of the Cheliped of the Lobster 611 


cheliped is completed at the fourth stage (Fig. XX), the dactyl- 
opodite (dc) and propedite (pr) are at right angles to their original 
position and the claw now opens inward on the nearly horizontal 
plane characteristic of the normal limb. Atthe same time it may be 
observed that the claws of the first (1’) and second (1°) thoracic 
legs have retained their original vertical position. ‘Thus “the 
position of the great forceps has been reversed by rotation through 
go°, in consequence of which their inner or anterior faces have 
become their under sides” (Herrick, p. 130). 

A direct comparison of the rotation of the regenerating cheliped 
with the torsion in the larval stages may be made by means of 
Figs. XV, XVI and XVII. ‘These figures represent the dorsal 
view of the regenerating lobsters, Nos. 3, 7 and 8, respectively 
(see p. 605). “The dactylopodite and propoditeon the fifth day stage 
of regeneration (Fig. XV) lie in a plane vertical and outward 
similar to the position for the normal claw in the first larval stage 
(Fig. XVIII). On the twelfth day (Fig. XVI) the two segments of 
the claw (dc and pr) lie ina plane at 45° from their former position 
and correspond closely to the third larval stage type (Fig. XIX). 
On the thirteenth day the regenerated claws of the moulted lobster 
open inward on the horizontal plane shown in Fig. XVII. 

Thus in both the regenerative and the ontogenetic development, 
the “great claw” of the cheliped rotates through an angle of go° 
to the horizontal plane of the normal claw. 


b. Relative Size of the Dactylopodite and the Opposing Propodite 
in Normal Development (Plate 1) 


Having observed that in the regenerating claw the propodite 
part of the claw which was at first smaller, gradually surpassed 
in size the opposing dactylopodite, the question naturally arose 
whether this, too, might be a feature of the ontogenetic develop- 
ment. If so, it was evident that it must be sought in the larval 
stages, for at the fourth stage the propodite, part of the claw, 
is already slightly larger than the dactylopodite. 

The larval specimens upon examination showed just such a 
morphological transition in the development of the claw. In 


the first larval stage the propodite part of the claw (Fig. X, pr’) 


612 Victor E. Emmel 


is nearly a third smaller than the opposing dactylopodite (dc). 
In the second stage (Fig. XI) the propodite Jaw (pr’) is only 
slightly the smaller, while in the third larval stage (Fig. XII) the 
two jaws of the claw are practically equal in size. At the fourth 
stage (Fig. XX, Plate II) the propodite jaw (pr’) is larger than the 
opposing dactylopodite, and the claw is now assuming character- 
istic normal proportions. Thus early in the development of the 
cheliped, the dactylopodite is the larger of the two jaws ofthe 
claw. Then by a gradual transformation the propodite jaw 
becomes equal, and finally inthe adult structure is much the larger 
of the two segments of the claw. 

Nor, indeed, is this an unexpected course of ontogeny. For 
the comparative morphology of the crustacean appendages 
certainly indicates that the cheliped is merely a highly modified 
leg appendage. In the lobster a transitional series, almost equal 
to a demonstration, exists between the terminal segments of the 
thoracic legs and the claw of the cheliped. ‘The third and fourth 
legs have no claws (Fig. XIII), but in the first and second thoracic 
legs (Fig. XIV) an elongated process (pr’) has grown out from the 
distal part of the propodite, thus forming a Jaw to meet the oppos- 
ing dactylopodite (dc). In the cheliped this process becomes the 
propodite segment of the highly developed claw. ‘That this 
propodite part of the claw is a true process which has grown out 
from the second segment independently of the joint for the 
dactylopodite, is clearly shown by the well marked groove or 
shoulder (g, Figs. XIV, X, XI,) between the propodite jaw (pr’) 
and the dactylopodite in the early stages of the developing chela. 
From this series it 1s evident that the “great” claw of the cheliped 
has been evolved by a distal elongation of the propodite process 
or “claw-like” projection, against which the terminal segment 
“bites,” thus forming a claw with greater facilities for grasping 
prey. 

c. Development of the “Crushing” Claw 
The development of the external morphological features of 


the chelipeds has been carefully described by Herrick (’05), and 


my observations do not add anything of an original character. 


Regeneration of the Cheliped of the Lobster 613 


But a brief description seems necessary here in order to complete 
the comparison between the regenerative and normal method of 
developing this appendage. 

In the adult lobster two distinct types of claws are found, the 
more primitive “toothed” or “nipping” claw, and the larger 
and phylogenetically younger (according to Stahr, ’98; and Przi- 
bram, ’or) “crushing” claw. But in the larval stages and up to 
the fifth moult, on the contrary, both claws are “similar” and of 
the “toothed” type. At about the sixth stage (Hadley, ’05) the 
“crushing” claw begins to differentiate. “The claw becomes 
wider, broad tubercle-like teeth develop, and the tactile hair of 
the toothed type (Plate II, Fig. XXI) gradually disappear in succes- 
sive moults. “Thus the adult ‘‘crushing”’ claw comes to be char- 
acterized by the almost entire absence of tactile hairs, and the 
presence of the broad crushing teeth; by a dactylopodite relatively 
smaller, and by being on the whole larger and more massive than 
the “toothed” or “nipping” type. 

‘These facts in the ontogenetic development serve to show the 
relation between“ nipping” and “crushing” claws, and give signifi- 
cance to the fact that the regenerating crusher for a long period 
has a form intermediate between these two types. 


III. RESUME 


Three characteristic phenomena may be observed in the 
regeneration of the cheliped, each of which resembles corre- 
sponding stages of the ontogenetic development. 

(a) In the very early stages of differentiation the cleavage of 
the claw first appears at the apex of the regenerating bud insuch 
a manner that the dactylopodite is vertically above the propodite. 
The claw would, therefore, open upward and outward. In the 
later course of development a marked transition in_ position 
takes place. The terminal segments gradually rotate over and 
inward, so that when the moult occurs, a torsion of about go° has 
been produced, and the claw now opens inward on the naturally 
characteristic horizontal plane. 

(b) Contemporaneously with this torsion, a transition in the 
form of the claw also occurs. At an early stage the dactylopodite 


614 Victor E. Emmel 


is larger than the opposing segment of the claw. Later the pro- 
podite part of the claw rapidly develops, and becomes equal and 
finally larger than the dactylopodite. By the time of moulting 
the terminal segments have practically assumed their normal 
proportions. 

(c) During the regeneration of the “crushing” cheliped it 
was observed that after the first moult, the regenerated “crushing” 
claw could not always be recognized as such, but is of a type 
intermediate between the “nipping” and “crushing” claws. 

The ontogenetic development of the cheliped to which this 
series of changes correspond is as follows: 

(a) At the first larval stage the claw of the cheliped together 
with the claws of the first and second chelate legs open upward 
and outward, the dactylopodite being in a position vertically 
above the propodite. The chele legs retain their original 
vertical position, but in the cheliped a gradual change occurs 
during the second and third larval stage, so that at the fourth 
moult the claw has rotated through go° and assumed a horizontal 
position. | 

(b) In the first larval stage the propodite jaw of the claw is 
much smaller than the opposing dactylopodite, but in the second 
and third stages it increases in size and ultimately becomes pro- 
portionately much the larger of the two segments of the claw. 

(c) In the first four larval stages both claws are similar in 
type. At about the sixth moult one claw begins to differentiate 
toward a “crushing” claw. During ensuing moults this claw 
passes through transitional stages and is finally completely 
transformed into a normal “crushing”’ claw. 

In a word, the torsion and other transitional morphological 
changes in the regeneration of the cheliped are parallel to similar 
phenomena in the ontogentic development of this appendage. 


IV. DISCUSSION 


In view of the fact that there is such a close similarity or paral- 
lelism between the regenerative and ontological processes just 
described, the question arises whether this parallelism has any 
phylogenetic significance. 


Regeneration of the Cheliped of the Lobster 615 


The general question of the repetition of phylogenetic and 
ontogenetic processes in regeneration is still open. Such facts 
as the regeneration of an mecca type of claw in the shrimp 
(Miller, *80); the reproduction of a walking leg in place of a 
maxilliped in Portenus (Przibram, ’o1); the formation of a lens 
from the iris in Urodeles (Wolff, ’94); the regeneration of a five, 
instead of four-fingered hand in the Axolotl (Barfurth, ’94); have 
led to a diversity of interpretation. Weismann (’02), on the 
basis of his “determinant” theory, speaking of the differences 
between the regenerated and the original structure, holds that 
“there remains nothing for it but the assumption that the regen- 
eration determinants have remained at a phyletic lower level, 
while the determinants which direct embryogenesis have varied 
and either developed farther or retrogressed”’ (p. 28). 

On the other hand Herbst, Driesch, Morgan, and others, do not 
see a dependence of regenerative processes upon fundamental 
biogenetic laws, but rather hold that a similarity in development 
is due to a similarity in conditions; “that the mistake is not in 
stating that the two processes are sometimes similar or even 
identical, but in stating the matter as though the regenerative 
process repeats the embryonic method of development,” and 
“that it may be entirely misleading to infer that ancestral charac- 
ters have reappeared” (Morgan, ’O1, pp. 213, 214). 

Quite recently, Schultz (’05) has reafhrmed the atavistic signif- 
cance of the process in a striking case of regeneration in the claws 
of five species of Russian crayfish: A. fluvitalis, A. pachypur, A. 
colchicus, A. kessleriand A. leptodactylus. The regenerated claws 
in each species were not only unlike the normal type of claw in each 
case, but, moreover, the remarkable fact was observed that they all 
resembled more or less closely a single type of claw, namely, the 
type characteristic of A. leptodactylus. Schultz’s atavistic inter- 
pretation of the case is supported by the recent work of Skorikows, 
whose study of the genetic relationship among the crayfish from 
the standpoint of geographical distribution, etc., makes A. leptodac- 
tylus var. colchica the parent form of all Russian crayfish. Schultz 
accordingly maintains that “we see here a dependence of regenera- 
tion upon fundamental biogenetic laws, a dependence which 


616 Victor E. Emmel 


Herbst, Delage, Driesch, and others, can vainly deny or slight, 
as “explaining nothing”’ (p. 46). 

In regard to this controverted question, the present observa- 
tions on the lobster seems to favor a recapitulation theory in 
regeneration. Since it is difficult adequately to account for the 
similarity between the ontogenetic and regenerative processes on 
the basis of a “similarity of conditions” in the organism. ‘The 
ontogenetic torsion and proportional development of the claw 
take place in the larval stages of this crustacean. At this period 
the organic conditions must be widely different from those under 
which the same phenomena occur in the regenerative process 
of the adult lobster, for during the larval stages the organism 
undergoes such an extensive metamorphosis in both function 
ang eae (Hadley, ’05) that the fourth moult of the young animal 
‘ BLESS the most surprising leap in the whole history of develop- 
ment”’ (Herrick, ’05). Even the differentiation of the claws into 
the “nipping” and “crushing” types, which occurs after the larval 
stages, marks the change from a “symmetrical” to an “asym- 
metrical” “equilibrium” in these appendages, and 1s correlated 
with the establisment of the “bottom-living”’ habits of the lobster 
(see Przibram, ’05, p. 238; Herrick, ’95, p. 180). 

‘This parallelism between the regenerative and normal develop- 
ment, therefore, seems to be more adequately interpreted by the 
conclusion that im the regenerative process of the cheliped we 
meet with a recapitulation of characteristic phases of its ontogeny; 
phases which, if investigators are at all correct in interpreting the 
origin of the crustacean cheliped through a modification of the 
thoracic leg appendages, are themselves in turn the recapitula- 
tion of a phylogenetic development. 

Anatomical Laboratory 


Brown University, Providence, R. I. 


May 23, 1906 


Regeneration of the Cheliped of the Lobster 617 


LITERATURE CITED 


BarFutH, D., ’94—Die experimentelle regeneration tiberschiissiger Gliedmassen- 
teile bei den Amphibien. Archiv. f. Entw.-Mech., i, pp. 91-116, 
1894. 
Cuitp, C. M., ’06.—Contributions toward a Theory of Regulation. I. Archiv. 
f. Entw-Mech., Bd. 20, pp. 380-426, 1906. 
Emmet, V. E. ’05.—The Regeneration of Lost Parts in the Lobster. Report of 
Rhode Island Commission of Inland Fisheries, pp. 81-117, 1905. 
Sp. paper, No. 20. 
’06.—The Relation of Regeneration to the Moulting Process of the 
Lobster. Report of Rhode Island Commission of Inland Fisheries, 
pp- 258-313, 1906. Sp. paper, No 27. 
Haptey, P. B., ’05.—Changes in Form and Color in Successive Stages of the 
American Lobster. ‘Report of Rhode Island Commission of 
Inland Fisheries, pp. 44-80, 1905. Sp. paper, No. 19. 
Herrick, F. H., ’95:—The American Lobster. Bull. U. S. Fish Commission, 
pp- 1-252, 1895. 
’05.—The “Great Forceps” of the American Lobster. Science, n. s., vol. 
xxi, Pp. 375, 1905. 
’o§ a.—Torsion of the Crustacean Limb. Biol. Bull., vol. ix, No. 2. 
pp- 130-137, 1905. 
Moreau, ’o1.—Regeneration. New York, r1got. 
o2—Regeneration of the Appendages of the Hermit Crab and Crayfish. 
Anat. Anz., Bd. 20, pp., 598-605, 1902. 
Mutter, F., *80.—Haeckel’s Biogenetisches Grundgesetz bei der neubildung ver- 
lorener Glieder. Kosmos, viii, 1880-81. 
PrzipraM, H., ’o1.—Experimentelle Studien tiber Regeneration. Archiv. f. Entw- 
Mech., Bd. xi, pp. 320-345. 1901. 
’o2.—Experimentelle Studien tiber Regeneration. Archiv. f. Entw.- 
Mech., Bd. xii, pp. 507-527, 1902. 
’05.—Die “‘Heterochelie” bei decapoden Crustacean. Archiv. f. Entw.- 
Mech., Bd. xix, pp.181-247, 1905. 
ScHuttz, E., ’o1—Ueber das Verhiltnis der Regeneration zur Embryonalent- 
wicklung und Knospung. Biol. Centralblatt, Bd. xxi, pp. 360- 
368, 1902. 
’05.—Ueber atavistische Regeneration bei Flusskrebsen. Archiv. f. 
Entw—Mech., Bd. xx, pp. 38-47, 1905. 


Sraur, ’98.—Neue Beitrage zur Morphologie der Hummerschere mit physio- 


logischen und phylogenetischen Bemerkungen. Jena. Zeitschr. 
fiir Naturwissenschaft. Bd. xxxil, pp. 387-482, 1898. 


618 Victor E. Emmel 


STEELE, M. I., ’04.—Regeneration of Crayfish Appendages. Univ. of Missouri 
Studies, vol. 11, No. 4, 1904. ; 
WEIsMANN, A., ’02.—The Theory of Evolution. London, 1904. 
Witson, E. B., ’94. The Embryological Criterion of Homology. Biol. Lectures, 
Marine Biol. Laboratory, Woods Hole, 1894. 
"03.—Notes on the Reversal of Asymmetry in the Regeneration of the 
Chelz in Alpheus Heterochelis. Biol. Bull., vol. iv, pp. 197-214, 
1902-03. 
Wo rr, G., ’94.—Entwickelungphysiologische Studien. I. Die Regeneration der 
Urodelenlinse. Archiv. f. Entw.-Mech., i, pp. 380-390, 1894-95. 
ZELENY, C., ’05.—Compensatory Regulation. Journ. of Exp. Zool., vol. i, 
pp. I-102. 
’06.—The Direction of Differentiation in a Regenerating Appendage. 
Science, n. s., vol. xxill, p. 527, 1906. 


EXPLANATION OF PLATES 


(All figures are from camera drawings) 


a—Cleavage or groove between the two jaws of the claw. 

bs—Basipodite. 

c—Cheliped. 

cp—Carpopodite. 

dc—Dactylopodite. 

ex—Exopodite. : 

g—Groove or projection between the dactylopodite and the claw-process of (pr’) the propodite in 

early development. 

h—Tactile hairs. . 

is—Ischiopodite. 

’—First thoracic leg. 

’—Second thoracic leg. 

me—Meropodite. 

p—Group of teeth in periodic sequence. 

p’ —Indications of a former sequence in the teeth of the ‘‘crushing” claw. 

pr—Propodite. 
pr’ —Propodite-part of claw opposing the corresponding jaw or dactylopodite. 

sp—Spines on lateral edge of propodite. 

t—Tubercle-like teeth one crushing claw. 1, 2, 3 and 4, respectively, the first, second, third 

and fourth joints of the cheliped. 


Prate I 


I-IX—Successive stages in the regeneration of the right cheliped of fifth stage lobsters. 
I—Side view of Lobster No. 3 (see table, p. 605). Gives the postero-lateral view of the 
regenerating cheliped (reg) typical for Figs. II-IX. 4.5 
II—Stump of cheliped one day after autotomy, before any regeneration is apparent. bk 
‘‘breaking-plane”’ or surface of the basipodite, at the center of which the regenerating 
bud will appear. (Lobster, No 1) 12.8% 
I1J—Regeneration, 3.5 days. (Lobster, No 2) 12.8X 
IV—Regeneration, 5 days. (Lobster, No3) 12.8% 
V—Regeneration, 6 days. (Lobster, No 4) 12,8 
ViI—Regeneration, 7 days. (Lobster, No 5) 12.8% 
VII —Regeneration, 8 days. (Lobster, No 6) 12.8X 
VilI—Regeneration, 12 days. (Lobster, No 7) 12.8% 
X1IX—Regeneration, 13.5 days. Completely regenerated cheliped, just after lobster had molted 
to sixth stage. (Lobster No 8) 9X 
X-XII—Show relative size of the two segments of the claw of the right cheliped in the larval 
stages (posterior view). 12,8 
X—First larval stage. 
XI—Second larval stage. 
XII—Third larval stage. 
XIII-XIV—Terminal segments of the third leg (XIII), and the claw of the first leg (XIV) of a 
first stage larval lobster (12.8% ). These figures, together with Figs. X, XI and XII, 


evidently indicate successive phases in the phylogeny of the ‘“‘great forceps’ of the 
cheliped. 


Oe ie 


REGENERATION OF THE CHELIPED OF THE LOBSTER 
Victor E, EMMeL 


PATE KTV 


Tue JourNat or ExpERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. III, NO. 4 


PLATE I 


Prate IT 


XV-XVII—Torsion in the regeneration of the right cheliped. Dorsal view of fifth stage lobsters, 
showing different phases of the regenerating limb. 
XV—Regeneration, 5 days, dactylopodite (dc) outward and above the propodite (pr?). 
(Lobster No3) 4.5 
XVI—Regeneration, 12 days, “‘biting plane” of claw turned inward at angle of about 45° to 
its original position. (Lobster No7) 4.5 
XVII—Cheliped completely regenerated, 13.5 days. Lobster had just moulted and the complete 
torsion of the cheliped permits the claw to open inward on a nearly horizontal 
plane. (Lobster No 8) 4.5 
XVIII-XX—Torsion of the right cheliped in normal development. 
XVITI—First larval stage; claw of cheliped (c) opens upward and outward. 9X 
XIX—Third larval stage; claw opens inward at an angle of about 45° to its original position. 9X 
XX—Fourth stage; claw opens inward on nearly horizontal plane. 4.5 
XXI-XXIII—Transitional characters in the regeneration of a ‘‘crushing” claw. Lobster in seventh 
(?) stage. (Dorsal view) 
XXI—Original left “‘nipping” claw. 4.5 
XXII—Original right “‘crushing” claw. 4.5 
XXTII—Regenerated right ‘‘crushing” claw. 4.5% 


REGENERATION OF THE CHELIPED OF THE LOBSTER PLATE II 
Victor E. EMMEL 


XV XVI 


XXI XXII XXIII 


Tue JourNAL oF ExperiMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. III, NO. 4 


Bi it 


a 4 fo 


As) * 
=) Sara : 
Bl soe tee a2 


a 


From the Physiological Laboratory of the University of Kansas 


THE INFLUENCES OF GASES AND TEMPERATURE 
ONE THE CARDIAC AND -RESPIRATORY> MOVE- 
MENTS IN THE GRASSHOPPER 


BY 


EULALIA V2 WALLING 


The object of the experiments recorded in the following pages 
was to ascertain the influences of certain gases upon the cardiac and 
respiratory movements of the grasshopper, both in the uninjured 
animal and when the heart and respiratory centers were isolated 
from all or part of the body. 

The principal parts of the respiratory organs are the paired 
spiracles, with their air sacs, and the tracheal tubes, that ramify 
from these to all parts of the body. ‘The mechanical respiratory 
movements consist of an expiratory and an inspiratory phase. 
During the latter, the active contraction of the abdominal muscles 
increases the breathing space by enlarging the antero-posterior 
and dorso-ventral diameters of the abdomen. In consequence of 
the lowered pressure therein produced, air passes into the tracheal 
tubes. During expiration the muscles relax and air enters the 
air sacs. 

The heart extends as a delicate tube longitudinally above the 
intestine, directly beneath the chitin, along the whole length of the 
abdomen. When entirely exposed to view it is seen to pulsate 
rhythmically throughout its whole extent. By a system of valves 
it is divided into a series of eight chambers, that communicate 
through slits or ostia with a surrounding pericardial sinus. 
Through the aorta, which arises at the anterior end of the heart, 
and the aortic branches, blood is conveyed to all parts of the body, 
along the paths of the tracheal tubes. 

It was demonstrated by Ewing" that the respiratory movements 
persist not only in decapitated animals but also in the isolated 
abdominal segments, in which are contained the ganglia of the ven- 


1Ewing,H.Z. Kansas University Science Bulletin, 1904, Vol. ii, p. 305. 


Tue JourNat or ExperIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. 11, NO. 4. 


822 Eulalia V. Walling 


tral nerve cord, that are the centers for the reflex and respiratory 
movements of the special segments to which they belong. I found 
that the heart also will continue its rhythmical contractions for 
hours, when left attached to a narrow strip of chitin along its mid- 
dorsal line and entirely removed from the body; moreover, that 
segments cut from the heart will continue their pulsations for long 
periods of time, especially if kept moist with Ringer’s solution. 
Whether there are dorsal ganglia as described by Carlson? for 
Limulus, is a question that I am now investigating. Certain it 1s, 
that electrical stimulation on the dorsal side accelerates, and burn- 
ing this region with a very fine needle destroys the action of the 
special segment that was injured. 

More than a thousand grasshoppers of different species were 
employed in this investigation. Besides normal, decapitated and 
deviscerated specimens, abdominal segments and isolated heart 
pieces were employed. In the deviscerated animals, the viscera 
and air sacs were removed, thus leaving the heart, ventral nerve 
cord and respiratory muscles intact. ‘The experiments were 
repeated many times and the data for each set of experiments 
represent average results. 

I take this opportunity to express my thanks for valuable sug- 
gestions and help to Professor Hyde, under whose direction this 
work was conducted. 

Vitality Varies with Conditions.—When kept under the most 
favorable conditions; that is, when placed in a wire cage containing 
sodand growing grass, the animals live from seventeen to twenty- 
five days, according to the species; they live on an average three 
days, and certain species five days, under the same conditions 
when decapitated or without food. Respiration and heart action 
last about thirty-three hours in deviscerated animals and ab- 
dominal segments. When the isolated heart is kept moist in a 
moist chamber, or with Ringer’s solution, it pulsates two days, but 
it becomes dry and stops beating i in nine hours when exposed to 
the air. Post-mortem examinations showed, as a rule, that heart 
action outlasts that of respiration. 


‘Carlson, A. J. | American Journal of Physiology, 1904, vol. xii, p. 67. 


Cardiac and Respiratory Movements in the Grasshopper 623 


Effect of Vitiated Air.—In each of these experiments, a single 
grasshopper was placed in a hermetically sealed tube ten inches 
long and one-half inch in diameter. One grasshopper was con- 
fined with each isolated heart, to furnish the vitiated air. 

The results from many experiments were, that normal animals 
continued to breathe forty-two hours; those of one species (Arphia) 
lived four days. Often when placed in fresh air, the hearts con- 
tinued their movements nine hours after the breathing had ceased, 
and frequently both cardiac and respiratory activities were resumed 
in fresh air, after they had stopped for many hours in vitiated air. 
The decapitated animal and the isolated heart not protected from 
drying, lived as long as in fresh air when not kept moist and often 
when the isolated heart was moistened with Ringer’s solution, 
after removal from the vitiated air its activity would return and 
continue ten hours longer. It was interesting to learn that both 
the respiration and heart action could be resumed after they had 
ceased for many hours. 

Ether.—One end of a glass tube containing the grasshoppers was 
placed under water and the other end was joined to a bottle 
partly filled with commercial ether. ‘Through this system air was 
forced under constant pressure. Both normal and decapitated 
animals stopped breathing in this atmosphere of ether in from 
five to ten minutes, indicating that the brain exercised no influence 
under these conditions. ‘Three hours’ exposure to the ether did 
not inhibit the heart action, but after four hours the ether had 
injured the tissue so that neither respiration nor heart action 
returned when the animals were placed in fresh air. Isolated hearts 
ceased beating in one minute in the ether and when exposed longer 
than forty minutes, were past recovery in fresh air. Inthe uninjured 
animal the ether reaches the heart indirectly during respiration. 
When that ceases, it is fed with venous blood, as in the previous 
and following experiments, with the addition, however, of some 
ether in solution. 

Carbon Dioxide-—The carbon dioxide, which was purified by 
passing through solutions of sodium carbonate, alkaline and acid 
potassium permanganate and alkaline pyrogallic acid, constantly 
flowed through the glass tube in which the grasshoppers were 


624 Eulalia V. Walling 


confined. All connections were tested and made perfectly air 
tight. ‘The exit end of the tube dipped under oil. 

Observations from many experiments showed that, although 
the respiratory movements in the normal animal ceased in from 
twenty to sixty seconds, the heart action continued about six hours 
longer. Moreover, if the grasshoppers were subjected fifteen hours 
to the gas, the respiration would return, provided the animals were 
then removed to fresh air; and what is more remarkable, the heart 
action, but not the respiration, will be resumed in fresh air after 
an exposure to the carbon dioxide for forty-eight hours. In some 
cases only a few minutes, in others several hours, and ina few 
instances, it was necessary to expose them to fresh air for twenty- 
four hours before the heart resumed its rhythm. Isolated hearts 
directly exposed to the gas unlike those in the uninjured body cease 
their activity in from twenty to sixty seconds and will, like those in 
the body, resume their rhythm in fresh air after an exposure of 
forty-eight hours to the carbon dioxide gas. ‘The gas inhibits the 
respiratory movements and those of the isolated heart in exactly 
the same time. ‘The results indicate that the tissue was not 
injured by long exposure to the gas, but probably its oxidative or 
metabolic action was inhibited. 

Carbon Monoxide.—Carbon monoxide was obtained by heat- 
ing a mixture of sulphuric and oxalic acids, and purified by pass- 
ing through the same solutions that were employed to purify 
the carbon dioxide. ‘The gas passed under a slight pressure con- 
stantly through the glass tube containing the grasshoppers, into 
a vessel containing a solution of cuprous chloride. Grasshoppers 
exposed to this gas behave much the same as do those in car- 
bon dioxide. Respiration stops in the same time in both gases, 
but animals may recover respiratory movements after an exposure 
of thirty hours to carbon monoxide, while they cannot recover if 
they remain in carbon dioxide more than fifteen hours. On the 
other hand, the heart action in the normal animal and in the iso- 
lated hearts, directly exposed to carbon monoxide for forty-eight 
hours, will not recover in fresh air, indicating that carbon monoxide 
is more damaging to cardiac activity than 1s carbon dioxide. 

H ydrogen.—Hydrogen obtained by the interaction of HCl on 


Cardiac and Respiratory Movements 1n the Grasshopper 625 


zinc, was passed through solutions of sodium carbonate, sodium 
hydroxide, acid and alkaline potassium permanganate and alkaline 
pyrogallol and further through sulphuric acid when dry hydrogen 
wasneeded. Under slight pressure the hydrogen passed constantly 
through the glass tube containing the grasshoppers. ‘The open 
end of the tube was drawn out into a long point that dipped under 
oil. The connections were all sealed, tested, and made perfectly 
air tight. 

A study of the data obtained from many experiments on the 
influence of hydrogen upon the respiratory and heart movements, 
reveals interesting results. In some of the experiments the cardiac 
and respiratory activity continued five days in an atmosphere of 
dry hydrogen. It will be remembered that this 1s as long as these 
functions persisted in the grasshoppers kept in fresh air without 
food. Theaverage duration of activity, however, 1s not as great in 
hydrogen as it is in air, because the animals are often in a comatose 
condition. ‘That is, they are very restless and active for a few 
minutes, then they become quiet and respiration seems to stop 
for from five minutes to two hours. Occasionally an appendage 
is moved and then they revive, become active and move about in the 
tube, so that periods of activity and lethargy alternate until they die. 

Isolated hearts directly exposed to the hydrogen, behave in some- 
what the same manner. The first effect is an acceleration of 
the heart beat. For example, the heart may contract sixty times 
a minute before it is put into the hydrogen, but one hundred times 
per minute after one minute exposure, eighty-six per minute after 
five minutes, only thirty times per minute after ten minutes, and 
in about twenty minutes all contractions have ceased, only to 
begin again in from two to four hours. They then continue to beat 
in the dry hydrogen for about twenty-seven hours or in moist 
hydrogen twenty-three hours. It was interesting to note, that 
the hearts exposed to the hydrogen did not become dry although 
they were not moistened with Ringer’s solution. 

It would appear from these experiments, that the absence of 
oxygen is not so injurious to the heart and nerve tissue in the grass- 
hopper as might have been inferred from the experimental results 
on higher forms. 


626 Eulalia T. Walling 


Oxygen.—The gas from a cylinder of oxygen was purified by 
washing through solutions of sodium carbonate, sodium hydrox- 
ide, acid and alkaline potassium permanganate and distilled water, 
respectively. ’o obtain dry oxygen, the gas was further washed 
through sulphuric acid. As in the other experiments, the gas passed 
constantly under slight pressure through the glass tubes contain- 
ing the grasshoppers, and the free end of the tube dipped under 
oil. 

An inspection of the many results secured from the influence of 
oxygen upon the cardiac and respiratory movements, disclosed the 
fact that these functions persist longer in animals kept in that gas 
than when living in fresh air without food. In some normal grass- 
hoppers, these functions continued eighty-nine hours, while in fresh 
air, the same species lived but seventy-two hours. ‘There was not so 
much difference in the effects of dry and moist oxygen as there was 
between the dry and moist hydrogen. 

Isolated hearts, moistened with Ringer’s solution when first 
placed in oxygen, continued to pulsate actively for about thirty 
hours. ‘They could not be kept moist as could those which were 
placed in moist chambers in fresh air, and which contracted on an 
average forty-eight hours; therefore, a direct comparison between 
these and those subjected to oxygen cannot be made. 

While the grasshoppers are inactive, breathing faintly and peri- 
odically in hydrogen, they are active, moving about in the tube 
frequently, in an atmosphere of oxygen, but at times do not breathe 
at all, being in what appears to be an apneeic state. Possibly if 
they had had food, they would have lived much longer than did 
those in air. 

High Temperature.—In these experiments the grasshoppers - 
and isolated hearts moistened with Ringer’s solution were observed 
through the glass door of a digester in which the temperature could 
be carefully regulated and observed. 

In one series of experiments, the temperature was gradually 
raised during three hours from 25° C. to 68° C. It was a note- 
worthy fact that as the temperature was raised, the first effect upon 
the isolated heart was to retard its contractions for a few minutes. 
A heart that contracted rhythmically sixty times a minute at 25° C., 


Cardiac and Respiratory Movements in the Grasshopper 627 


contracted only forty-eight times a minute at 30° C. and then the 
action increased rapidly, so that at 48° C., it was beating at more 
than twice its normal rate. In Coloptena femur rubrum the pul- 
sations were increased to about two hundred per minute. As the 
temperature was raised beyond 48° C., the contractions became 
slower and stopped entirely in temperatures between 62° C. and 
68°C. On an average the respiratory rate increased from 48 
per minute gradually to 66 per minute at 41° C., then the rate de- 
creased to 60 at 43° C. It then increased to 112 at 51° C., from 
there on decreased and ceased at 57° C. 

In another set of experiments, the isolated heart was removed 
froma temperature of 25° C., in which the contractions were on an 
average fifty-six per minute, and subjected directly to a tempera- 
ture of 57°C. Here the heart’s action was instantly increased to 
one hundred and fifty contractions per minute. During the next 
hour the temperature was gradually raised from 57° C. to 68°C., 
with the result that the rhythmical contractions decreased and 
ceased entirely at 68° C., but when the heart was then placed 
in a temperature of 25° C., its action was again resumed. 

In a third set of experiments, normal grasshoppers, abdominal 
segments and isolated hearts were subjected to a temperature of 
14° C., that was gradually raised during four hours to 63° C., and 
then lowered again to 14° C., or room temperature. Ina tempera- 
ture of 14° C., there were, in the normal insect, on an average forty 
respiratory contractions per minute and in the abdominal seg- 
ments twenty-five contractions per minute. In-that time the 1so- 
lated heart contracted about thirty-six times. As the temperature 
was raised from 14° C. to 54° C., the respiratory movements in the 
normal animal, and in the abdominal segments increased from 40 
to 110 and 50 contractions per minute, respectively, then decreased 
and ceased at 59° C., while the isolated heart increased its action 
from 36 beats in a temperature of 14° C. to 160 beats in a tempera- 
ture of 48° C., then gradually slowed, and stopped entirely at 
62° C., one-half hour after the respiratory movements had ceased. 
The specimens were then subjected to a gradually but rather rap- 
idly falling temperature, but the cardiac and respiratory activities 
were not again resumed. 


628 Eulalia V. Walling 


Low Temperatures.—When the effect of a gradually lowered 
temperature was to be studied, the grasshoppers were placed with 
a thermometer in a test tube around which the temperature was 
gradually lowered, first by placing the tube with its contents near 
a large dish containing a mixture of chopped 1 ice and salt, and, as 
the temperature oa lower in the tube, it was gradually sub- 
merged in the salt and ice mixture, until a temperature of —17° C. 
was obtained. For studying the influence of still lower tempera- 
tures the tubes were placed in a liquid air chamber, where a 
temperature of —100° C. was obtained. 

The following are the results of many experiments upon normal 
and decapitated grasshoppers and upon abdominal segments and 
isolated hearts. 

The respiratory and heart action becomes slower and fainter as 
the temperature falls from 20° C. to 5° C. At this temperature, the 
animals are usually inactive and breathe faintly only five or six 
times a minute, if at all, while the isolated heart ceases to contract. 
The respiratory movements in the normal animal, however, may 
not stop until the temperature falls to o° C. Normal animals and 
isolated hearts kept in a temperature of —10° C. for about one hour 
will recover their activity if the temperature is very gradually 
raised to that of the room. Several nymphs, young immature 
grasshoppers, were kept for one-half hour in a temperature of —30° 
C., then the temperature surrounding them was gradually raised 
during the following eighteen hours to that of the room. Respira- 
tion did not return but the heart action did. Probably those that 
were subjected to —30° C., or even —100° C. would have recovered 
if the temperature had been more gradually raised during several 
days to that of the room. 


SUMMARY 


Normal grasshoppers can live under most favorable conditions 
in the laboratory twenty-five days, and under the same conditions 
but without food three to five days; whereas the isolated heart beats 
nine hours if unprotected, but if kept moist with Ringer’s solution 
it will beat for two days. 


Cardiac and Respiratory Movements in the Grasshopper 629 


‘ In vitiated air in sealed tubes the normal grasshopper lives 
two days, but the heart beats as long as it does in fresh air only 
more slowly and at irregular intervals. 

In carbon dioxide the heart and respiratory movements cease 
within a minute and may remain inactive as long as two days, 
and then when the specimens are placed in fresh air for about two 
hours the movements are again resumed. Carbon dioxide may 
act upon the cardiac cells and nerve centers as a reaction product, 
inhibiting enzymes or metabolic activity in the cells. Carbon 
monoxide is slightly more toxic than carbon dioxide in its effect 
upon the heart and respiratory centers. 

Hydrogen has a most remarkable effect upon the respiratory 
and cardiac action. For the first few hours it has an inhibitory 
influence upon the heart and respiratory movements both in the 
normal and isolated states. After the comatose condition has 
lasted about four hours, the respiratory and cardiac actions revive, 
and in some cases the movements continue for four days. The 
contractions are conspicuous in consisting of long intervals of cessa- 
tion alternating with shorter ones of activity. In moist hydrogen 
the activities cease sooner than in dry hydrogen. If the energy 
of the nerve, heart and muscle cells depends upon oxidative pro- 
cesses, then the necessary oxygen for the four days activity must 
have been supplied by an amount stored in some manner in the 
cells. . 

In normal grasshoppers and isolated hearts the contractions of 
the heart continued in some cases longer in oxygen than in air. 
It was observed, however, that periods of inactivity alternated with 
periods of strong respiratory contractions. 

It is interesting to note that the cardiac cells can contract in a 
temperature as high as 66°C., and cease at about 0° C. More- 
over, that they can withstand for at least half an hour, a tempera- 
ture of —30°C., and probably if the thawing was carried on very 
slowly, they could withstand a temperature of —100° c. 

Whether the heart tissue in the isolated heart is capable of con- 
traction independent of the influence of intrinsic nerve cells, I 
have as yet not been able fully to determine, but hope soon to 
obtain definite proofs in regard to this question. 


a fata 
pita 


ae 


ca ] 
ay ek Resets os hee 7 
“to 


7 es aren) oe: fon ah se 


Ts 


* 


Lb hs 
i se 
i \3 
wae} 


oe 


tomes 
2) 
» 


QL The Jaurnal of experimental 


rE zoology 
J68 
ne 306 
COp.2 
Biological 
x Medical 


rials 


PLEASE DO NOT REMOVE 
CARDS OR SLIPS FROM THIS POCKET 


UNIVERSITY OF TORONTO LIBRARY 


Fgh et ots 
ntti 
Hy TE i 
a 


data 
we 
es | 


oes 


Pa ey 6c) 2 ote 
Pertran 


he Py. yt , 
ti $ if 
ye a 


Hid 
v if r 
Rava 


i b 
: eit 
ete cH TCS oe eT Le Hittin 
he ain Po Ar eet CASH ear Hei 1 
bref redgen ents halatala tae He AC pteS danced Kia ON RUT Ve 
ey yt i Nef dey 
ys 
n 


fie’ 


tere tes eecikir 
bois ahds 
PM nan aunt 
wee LW Per “a 7 
Bieri 
ott 


ua pee 
% 


nial 
te ry 
Pea aR Og 
Pita ehh gs fe 
i 

% 


his 
See 


-- eye = 
mes as 
Se we 
. Tey 


<~ 
os 


S35 SS 


vee 
a 
ao 


aeons 


S24 


hat 
Waianae 
arava seieaiteers 
ae ve) i. 
wy 
yet 
yy ae 


5 eee. 


SPs ets 
PS 
eas 


Kelpie 
== 
= 


Soete 


= 
SES