Skip to main content

Full text of "Studies of cerebral function in learning"

See other formats


CO 

in 

O) 
O) 
CO 

o 

5 

co 


Lashley,  Karl  Spencer 

Studies  of  cerebral 
function  in  learning 


Archives    of  Neurology   and   Psychiatry 

Vol.  12  SEPTEMBER.  1924  No.  3 


STUDIES     OF     CEREBRAL     FUNCTION     IN     LEARNING 

I 

V.     THE     RETENTION      OF      MOTOR      HABITS     AFTER     DESTRUCTION      OF 
THE     SO-CALLED      MOTOR     AREAS     IN    PRIMATES  * 

K.     S.     LASHLEY.     Ph.D. 

MINNEAPOLIS 

Since  the  area  was  first  described  by  Fritsch  and  Hitzig,1  the 
function  of  the  electrostimulable  cortex  of  the  cerebrum  has  been  the 
subject  of  almost  continuous  controversy.  The  experiments  were 
immediately  called  in  question  through  criticisms  of  the  technic  by 
Dupuy,2  Sanderson,'  Carville  and  Duret,4  and  others,  or  by  abstruse 
metaphysical  deductions  such  as  were  advanced  by  Hermann 5  who 
objected  to  the  motor  area  as  violating  the  "unity  of  mind."  The  work 
of  Ferrier,6  Carville  and  Duret  and  Hitzig  soon  established  the  fact 
of  the  electrical  excitability  of  limited  areas  of  the  cortex,  but  immedi- 
ately a  new  question  arose.  Fritsch  and  Hitzig  had  considered  the 
excitable  zone  as  motor,  if  we  may  translate  the  expression,  "entry  of 
single  psychic  functions  into  material"  by  such  a  term.  In  this  they 
were  followed  by  Carville  and  Duret,  who  described  the  motor  dis- 
turbances following  lesions  in  the  area  as  "paralysie  de  la  motricite 
volontaire  corticale."  Ferrier  also  considered  the  area  as  motor.  But 
most  of  the  early  work  had  been  done  with  dogs,  and  the  sensory 
disturbances  which  appear  in  this  animal  after  ablation  of  the  stim- 
ulable    area    were    emphasized    by    Schifr,7    Nothnagel,8    Munk 9    and 


*  From  the  Department  of  Psychology  of  the  University  of  Minnesota. 

1.  Fritsch,  G.,  and  Hitzig,  E. :  Ueber  die  elektrische  Erregbarkeit  des 
Grosshirns,  Arch.  f.  Anat.  u.  Physiol.  1870,  pp.  300-332. 

2  Dupuy,  E. :  Experiment  sur  les  fonctions  moteur  du  cervean,  Compt. 
rend.  soc.  de  biol.,  1888,  pp.  1025-1027. 

3.  Sanderson,  J.  B.:  Note  on  the  Excitation  of  the  Surface  of  the  Cerebral 
Hemispheres  by  Induced  Currents,  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  22:368-370,  1874. 

4.  Carville,  C,  and  Duret,  H. :  Sur  le  fonctions  des  hemispheres  cerebraux, 
Arch.  d.  Physiol.  7:352-490,  1875. 

5.  Hermann,  L. :  Ueber  elektrische  Reizversuche  an  der  Grosshirnrinde, 
Arch.  f.  d.  ges.  Physiol.  10:77-88,  1875. 

6.  Ferrier.  D. :     The  Functions  of  the  Brain,  London,  1876. 

7.  Schiff,  M. :  Untersuchungen  iiber  die  motorischen  Fnnctionen  des  Gross- 
hirns, Arch.  f.  exper.  Path.  u.  Pharmocol.  3:171-179,  1875. 

8.  Nothnagel,  H. :  Experimentelle  Untersuchungen  iiber  die  Fnnctionen  des 
Gehirns.  Arch.  f.  path.  Anat.  u.  Physiol.  57:184-227,  1873. 

9.  Munk,  H.:     Ueber  die  Funktionen  der  Grosshirnrinde,  Berlin,   1890. 


3 I 


ARCHIVES    OF    SEMIOLOGY    AND    PSYCHIATRY 


o 
>- 


llitzig.1"  who  interpreted  the  disorders  of  movement  variously  as  due 
to  the  loss  of  muscular  and  cutaneous  sensitivity  or  to  the  loss  of 
kinesthetic  images  of  the  movements  to  be  performed. 

Following  the  suggestion  of  Tamburini,  Luciani  and  Seppili 1X 
advanced  the  view  that  the  motor  areas  contained  somesthetic  projec- 
tion fibers  as  well  as  motor  elements,  and  developed  a  theory  of  the 
sensorimotor  function  of  the  sigmoid  gyrus  and  rolandic  areas.  In 
this  they  were  followed  by  Horsley,12  Mott,13  Dana.14  Rothmann  15  and 
many  others. 

De  Barenne,10  in  particular,  has  demonstrated  the  existence  of 
marked  sensory  disturbances  in  the  cat  after  application  of  strychnin 
to  an  area  which  widely  overlaps  the  stimulable  area,  and  the  general 
correctness  of  Luciani's  view  for  the  stimulable  areas  of  lower  mam- 
mals does  not  seem  open  to  question,  although  there  may  be  some  doubt 
as  to  whether  the  motor  functions  of  the  cortex  in  these  forms  are 
comparable  with  those  of  primates. 

Recent  more  critical  work  with  primates,  however,  shows  that  in 
them  a  further  specialization  has  occurred  with  the  development  of  the 
fissure  of  Rolando  as  a  line  of  demarcation  between  centripetal  and 
centrifugal  projection  areas.17     The  work  of  Schafer,is  Mills, 1(l  Griin- 


10.  Hitzig,    E. :      Physiologische    unci    klinische    Untersuchungen    iiber    das 
Gehirn,  Berlin,  1904. 

11.  Luciani,  L.,  and  Seppili,  G. :     Die  Funktion-Localization  auf  der  Gross- 
hirnrinde,  Deutsche  Ausgabe,  Leipzig,  1886. 

12.  Horsley,  V. :  On  the  Analysis  of  Voluntary  Movement,  19th  Century 
29:857-870,  1891. 

13.  Mott,  F.  W. :  The  Sensory-Motor  Functions  of  the  Central  Convolutions 
of  the  Cerebral  Cortex,  J.  Physiol.  15:464-487,  1893-1894. 

14.  Dana,  C.  L. :  A  Study  of  the  Functions  of  the  Cortex  of  the  Motor  Area 
of  the  Brain,  J.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.  21:761-785,  1894. 

15.  Rothmann,  M. :  Leber  die  elektrische  Erregbarkeit  der  Zentral- 
windungen,  Monatschr.  f.  Psychiat.  u.  Neurol.  32:489-502,  1912. 

16.  Barenne,  J.  G.  D.  de:  Sensory  Localization  in  the  Cerebral  Cortex,  Quart. 
J.  Exper.  Physiol.  9:355-390,  1916. 

17.  The  lack  of  any  persistent  paralysis  in  rodents,  carnivora  and  ungulates 
after  destruction  of  the  stimulable  areas  and  the  occurrence  of  partial  paralyses 
after  destruction  of  parts  of  the  corpus  striatum  in  lower  forms  suggest  that 
the  differentiation  of  function  of  precentral  and  postcentral  gyri  in  primates 
may  be  due  rather  to  the  acquisition  of  primative  striate  functions  by  the 
cerebral  cortex  than  to  a  division  of  cortical  functions  which  overlap  in  lower 
forms. 

18.  Schafer,  E.  A.:  On  the  Alleged  Sensory  Functions  of  the  Motor  Cortex 
Cerebri,  J.  Physiol.  23:310-314,  1898. 

19.  Mills,  C.  K. :  The  Separate  Localization  in  the  Cortex  and  Sub-Cortex  of 
the  Cerebrum  of  the  Representation  of  Movements  and  of  Muscular  and  Cutane- 
ous  Sensibility,  J.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.  38:595-619,  1901. 


LASHLEY— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION  251 

baum  and  Sherrington,2"  C.  and  O.  Vogt,21  Lewandowsky  and  Sim- 
mons,22 Hoppe,23  Cushing,24  Franz,23  and  Leyton  and  Sherrington,26 
all  points  to  the  conclusion  that  primary  excitability  is  limited  to  the 
precentral  gyrus  and  that  sensory  disturbances  rarely  result  from  lesions 
confined  to  this  area. 

These  studies  have  defined  the  centrifugal  function  of  the  cells  of  the 
precentral  gyrus  and  have  confirmed  the  view  that  the  area  is  motor 
rather  than  somesthetic.  but  in  spite  of  the  fairly  general  recent  agree- 
ment as  to  the  location  of  "motor"  area,  there  is  a  wide  diversity  of 
opinion  concerning  the  significance  of  the  "motor"  function.  The  area 
is  somehow  concerned  with  the  production  of  movement,  but  how  it 
acts  and  what  kinds  of  movement  it  controls,  are  still  debated  points. 
Ferrier 6  emphasized  the  "automatic"  character  of  movements  after 
destruction  of  the  motor  areas  and  held  that  "All  reactions  not  so 
(automatically)  organized,  and  still  dependent  upon  conscious  discrimi- 
nation and  exercise  of  attentive  volition  are  effectually  and  permanently 
annihilated."  Munk  27  classified  movements  according  to  their  somatic 
distribution  and  maintained  that  small  unilateral  adaptive  movements 
(  Einzelbewegungen  )  are  permanently  lost  after  destruction  of  the  motor 
areas.  His  definition  of  these  movements  is  by  no  means  clear,  but 
his  accounts  seem  to  imply  that  the  movements  had  been  learned,  and 
he  denies  that  a  dog  can  learn  to  give  his  paw  after  destruction  of  both 
motor  areas.  Wagner  28  maintained  that  the  chief  function  of  the  motor 
areas  is  in  learning,  and  that  animals  deprived  of  them  are  incapable  of 
forming  any  new  habits.     Bechterew  29  also  refers  to  experiments  lead- 


20.  Grunbaum,  A.,  and  Sherrington,  C.  S. :  Observations  on  the  Physiology 
of  the  Cerebral  Cortex  of  the  Anthropoid  Apes,  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  72  B:  152-155, 
1903. 

21.  Vogt,  C,  and  Vogt,  O. :  Zur  Kenntnis  der  elektrisch  erregbaren  Hirn- 
rindengebiete  bei  den  Saugetieren,  J.  f.  Psychol,  u.  Neurol.  8:277-456,  1907. 

22.  Lewandowsky,  M.,  and  Simmons,  A. :  Zur  Physiologie  der  vordern  und 
hintern  Zentralwindung,  Arch.  f.  d.  ges.  Physiol.  129:240-254,  1909. 

23.  Hoppe,  H.  H. :  A  Critical  Study  of  the  Sensory  Functions  of  the  Motor 
Zone  (Pre-Rolandic  Area)  :  More  Especially  Stereognosis,  J.  Nerv.  &  Ment. 
Dis.  36:513-527,  1909. 

24.  Cushing,  H. :  A  Note  Upon  the  Faradic  Stimulation  of  the  Postcentral 
Gyrus   in   Conscious  Patients,  Brain  32:44-54,   1909. 

25.  Franz,  S.  I. :  Variations  in  Distribution  of  the  Motor  Centers,  Psychol. 
Monogr.  19:80-162,  1915. 

26.  Leyton,  A.  S.  F.,  and  Sherrington,  C.  S. :  Observations  on  the  Excitable 
Cortex  of  the  Chimpanzee,  Orang-Outan  and  Gorilla,  Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol. 
11:135-222,  1917. 

27.  Munk,  H. :  Ueber  die  Fuhlspharen  der  Grosshirnrinde,  Sitzungsber.  d. 
Berlin  Akad.  Wiss.,  1892,  679-723;  1893,  759-781;  1894,  823-833;  1896,  1131-1159. 

28.  Wagner,  V:    Discussion  in  Neurol.  Zentralbl.  24:1022,  1905. 

29.  Bechterew,  W.  v.:    Die  Funktionen  der  Nervencentra,  Jena  3.  1911. 


2?2  ARCHIVES     OF    NEUROLOGY    AND    PSYCHIATRY 

ing  to  the  same  conclusion.  He  states  that  after  removal  of  the  motor 
centers  associative  motor  reflexes  are  lost.  "With  unilateral  destruction 
of  the  motor  region  in  dogs  the  associative  motor  reflex  may  he  elicited 
in  the  homolateral  fore-leg,  but  in  the  contralateral  fore-leg  the  reflexes 
learned  earlier  are  forever  lost  and  can  not  be  reestablished  even  after 
a  number  of  conditioning  associations  (p.  1551ff.)."  Gierlich 30  also 
supports  this  view  of  the  exclusive  motor  function  of  the  stimulable 
areas. 

In  opposition  to  these  results,  several  writers  have  reported  the 
acquisition  of  habits  after  the  destruction  of  the  motor  areas  or  of  the 
pyramidal  tracts.  Starlinger 31  trained  a  dog  to  give  his  paw  after 
total  destruction  of  both  pyramidal  tracts.  Rothmann  32  observed  learn- 
ing in  a  rhesus  monkey  in  which  one  precentral  gyrus  had  been  extir- 
pated and  the  pyramidal  tract  of  the  other  had  been  sectioned  in  the 
cervical  region.  Franz  and  Lashley 33  and  Lashley 34  found  learning 
ability  in  the  rat  unaltered  by  total  destruction  of  the  stimulable  cortex. 
This  re>ult  has  been  confirmed  by  Jellinek  and  Koppanyi.33 

In  the  contradiction  of  evidence  here,  we  must  favor  the  positive 
results.  Failure  to  learn  may  be  due  to  any  one  of  a  number  of  factors 
in  addition  to  specific  destruction  of  tissue,  and  a  single  positive  case 
with  certain  destruction  of  the  motor  area  is  sufficient  to  discredit  any 
number  of  negative  findings  such  as  are  cited  by  Bechterew  and  Munk. 
It  seems  quite  certain  that  the  formation  of  conditioned  motor  reflexes 
is  possible  in  the  absence  of  the  electrostimulable  cortex,  but  this  fact 
fails  to  reveal  the  normal  function  of  the  area  in  the  performance  of 
complex  activities.  Both  Rothmann  and  Brown38  seem  to  believe  that 
in  the  intact  animal  the  motor  areas  form  the  chief  centrifugal  path  for 
complex  adaptive  reactions  and  that  when  learning  occurs  in  their 
absence  it  is  to  be  considered  as  due  to  vicarious   function   of   other 


30.  Gierlich.  X.:  Ueber  Symptomatologie,  YYesen,  unci  Therapie  der  hemi- 
plegischen  Lahmung,  Wiesbaden,  1913. 

31.  Starlinger,  J.:  Die  durchschneidung  beider  Pyramiden  beim  Hunde, 
Neurol.  Zentralbl.  14:390-394.  1895. 

32.  Rothmann,  M. :  Ueber  die  physiologische  Wertung  der  cortico-spinalen 
(Pyramiden)   Balm,  Arch.  f.  Anat.  u.  Physiol.  (Physiol.  Abt.)   pp.  217-275,  1907. 

33.  Franz,  S.  I.,  and  Lashley,  K.  S. :  The  Retention  of  Habits  by  the  Rat 
After  Destruction  of  the  Frontal  Portion  of  the  Cerebrum,  Psychobiol  1:3-18 
1917. 

34.  Lashley,  K.  S. :  Studies  of  Cerebral  Function  in  Learning,  Psychobiol. 
2:55-135,  1920. 

35.  Jellinek.  A.,  and  Koppanyi,  T. :  Lernfahigkeit  gehirnverletzter  Ratten. 
Anzeiger  d.  Akad.  d.  YViss.,  Wien,  1923,  No.  17. 

36.  Brown,  T.  (i.:  Studies.  XXVII.  (>.  The  Motor  Activation  of  Parts  of 
the  Cerebral  Cortex  Other  Than  Those  Included  in  the  So-Called  "Motor" 
Areas  in  Monkeys,  Quart.  J.  F.xper.  Physiol.  10:103-143,  1916. 


LASHLEY— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION  253 

parts  rather  than  as  an  expression  of  their  normal  function.  On  the 
basis  of  Brown's  work  and  of  clinical  evidence,  however,  Monakow  36a 
is  inclined  to  minimize  the  importance  of  the  pyramidal  areas  for 
••voluntary"  movement.  He  suggests  that  "We  must  give  up  or  essen- 
tiallv  modify  the  view  that  the  precentral  convolution  alone  conducts 
impulses  to  voluntary  movement.  It  is  probable  that  the  pyramidal 
areas  serve  less  for  the  execution  of  voluntary  movements  than  for  the 
inhibition  of  the  kinetic  functions  of  spinal  coordination.  Their  func- 
tion seems  to  be  in  the  class  of  reflex  activity."  Lashley :JT  has 
reported  the  survival  of  visuomotor  habits  after  the  complete  destruction 
of  the  stimulable  area  and  serious  injury  to  the  caudate  and  lenticular 
nuclei 3S  and  has  suggested  that  the  primary  function  of  the  stimulable 
area  is  the  reflex  regulation  of  postural  and  kinetic  mechanisms.  Similar 
results  have  since  been  obtained  for  the  stimulable  cortex  with  problem- 
box  and  maze  habits. 

Thus  we  find  in  the  literature  claims  that  the  electrostimulable  cortex 
is  motor,  that  it  is  sensory,  that  it  is  sensorimotor,  that  its  motor  func- 
tion is  exercised  through  the  storing  of  images  of  movement,  that  it 
is  the  final  common  path  for  all  voluntary  movements,  and  that  it  is  a 
reflex  center  not  primarily  concerned  with  voluntary  activity. 

Much  of  the  literature  on  the  function  of  the  electrostimulable 
cortex,  as  on  cerebral  localization  in  general,  presents  an  inextricable 
tangle  of  physiologic  fact  and  psychologic  speculation.  The  long  con- 
troversy between  Goltz,  Munk,  and  Hitzig  was  largely  due  to  their 
inability  to  grasp  each  other's  psychologic  theories,  and  recent  progress 
in  psychology  tends  to  invalidate  much  of  the  cerebral  localization  which 
was  based  on  older  conceptions  of  mental  faculties. 

Images  have  fallen  into  disrepute,  and  even  the  psychologists  who 
still  deal  with  them  deny  that  kinesthetic  imagery  has  any  demonstrable 
relation  to  the  initiation  of  movement  (Thorndike  39).  The  conception 
of  volitional  activity  is  too  vague  to  have  any  scientific  value.  At  best 
it  represents  an  indefinite  distinction  between  more  or  less  complexly 
conditioned  activities,  and  the  conception  of  conditioned  reflexes  leaves 
the  distinction  without  significance.  The  "will"  has  been  largely  dis- 
carded in  psychology,  although  it  is  still  in  good  standing  in  neurologic 
discussions.      Such   a   statement   as   that  the   stimulable   cortex   is   not 


36a.  Monakow  :     See  footnote  57. 

37.  Lashley,  K.  S. :  Studies  of  Cerebral  Function  in  Learning.  The  Motor 
Areas,  Brain  44:255-286,  1921. 

38.  When  these  experiments  were  reported,  I  believed  that  the  lesions  were 
largely  confined  to  the  caudate  nuclei.  Dr.  J.  B.  Jonhston  has  since  called  my 
attention  to  the  fact  that  the  caudate  nucleus  in  the  rat  comprises  only  a 
narrow  median  band  in  the  corpus  striatum  and  that  the  lesions  reported  actually 
included  a  considerable  portion  of  the  lenticular  nucleus. 

39.  Thorndike.  E.  L. :  The  Mental  Antecedents  of  Voluntary  Movement,  J 
Philos.,  Psychol,  and  Sc.  Meth.  4:40-42,  1907. 


254  ARCHIVES    OF    NEUROLOGY    AXD    PSYCHIATRY 

motor,  but  "psychomotor,"  means  no  more  than  that  the  pyramidal 
cells  excite  patterns  of  spinal  motor  cells  rather  than  individual  cells. 
It  contributes  nothing  to  our  understanding  of  cerebral  function.  In 
the  present  state  of  psychologic  science,  we  can  not  do  better  than  fol- 
low the  dictum  of  Bubnoff  and  Heidenhain,40  "Es  will  uns  uberhaupt 
scheinen  als  miiste  die  Untersuchung  der  physiologischen  Processe  in 
dem  Gehirn  von  den  jene  Vorgange  begleitenden  Bewusstseinsvorgangen 
moglichst  absehen,  wenn  es  sich  urn  eine  Deutung  physischen  Gesche- 
hens  handelt,"  and  rigidly  exclude  from  neurologic  discussion  every 
subjective  concept  which  cannot  be  translated  into  objective  terms. 

There  is  no  evidence  for  the  localization  of  any  "mental  function" 
in  any  part  of  the  cerebrum.  All  that  can  be  concluded  from  the 
existing  evidence  is  that  the  conducting  pathways  concerned  in  par- 
ticular kinds  of  behavior  lead  from  receptor  to  effector  through  certain 
cerebral  areas.  Cerebral  motor  localization  is  a  problem  of  the  origin  . 
and  function  of  the  centrifugal  neural  impulses  of  the  cortex.  Their 
"volitional"  or  "automatic"'  character  can  be  defined  only  in  terms  of 
their  complexity  of  organization  and  their  relative  importance  in  the 
total  motor  integration  or  kinetic  melody,  and  until  so  defined  the  terms 
are  meaningless.  The  "reflex"  conception  of  cerebral  function,  although 
still  a  theory  and  notably  inadequate  to  account  for  all  the  phenomena 
of  cerebral  function  because  of  oversimplification  in  its  formulations,41 
is  too  well  supported  by  evidence  on  nerve  conduction  and  analogy  with 
spinal  functions  to  be  disregarded  in  favor  of  any  speculations  con- 
cerning the  localization  of  "psychic"  functions. 

STATEMENT     OF     PROBLEM 

Stated  objectively,  three  mutually  incompatible  theories  concerning 
the  function  of  the  precentral  gyrus  are  to  be  found  in  the  current 
literature.  They  are:  1.  This  area  is  the  only  centrifugal  outlet  from 
the  cerebral  cortex  for  complexly  integrated  movements  or  for  move- 
ments acquired  as  a  result  of  training  (  voluntary  movements  or  con- 
ditioned reflexes).  2.  In  the  intact  animal,  the  Betz  cells  are  the 
principle  centrifugal  paths,  but  some  neural  impulses  of  like  function 
may  descend  by  extrapyramidal  tracts,  and  these  tracts  may  assume 
vicariously  all  the  functions  of  the  motor  area.  3.  The  motor  area 
is  a  part  of  the  mechanism  functioning  in  the  regulation  of  tonus  and 
posture  and  is  not  directly  concerned  in  conditioned  reflex  activity. 


40.  Bubnoff,  V.  and  Heidenhain,  R. :  Ueber  Erregungs-  und  Hemmungs- 
vorgange  innerhalb  der  motorischen  Hirncentren,  Arch.  f.  d.  ges.  Physiol.  26: 
137-200,  1881. 

41.  The  data  on  direct  adaptation  of  unpracticed  organs  to  the  solution  of 
|irol)lem-boxes  presented  later  in  this  paper  seem  wholly  inexplicable  in  terms 
oi  simple  conditioned  reflexes. 


LASHLEY— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION  255 

The  first  of  these  views  is  definitely  ruled  out  by  the  evidence  cited 
above  for  habit  formation  after  destruction  of  the  areas.  The  evidence 
for  the  second  and  third  views,  as  applied  to  primates,  is  inconclusive, 
although  a  considerable  mass  of  evidence  derived  from  the  cerebral 
paralyses  seems  to  support  the  second.  The  validity  of  this  evidence 
will  be  considered  after  the  experimental  data  are  presented. 

A  simple  test  of  the  hypotheses  is  possible.  If  after  total  destruc- 
tion of  the  precentral  areas  an  animal  shows  undiminished  ability  to 
carry  out  activities  of  all  degrees  of  complexity,  acquired  before  the 
operative  destruction,  this  will  be  conclusive  evidence  that  the  lesion 
did  not  destroy  any  part  of  the  conditioned  reflex  arcs  involved  in  the 
activities,  and,  as  a  corollary,  that  these  arcs  do  not  traverse  the  motor 
areas.  Loss  of  the  conditioned  reflexes  following  the  lesion  with  their 
later  reestablishment  through  training  will  support  the  second  view,  that 
the  pyramidal  cells  of  the  precentral  region  are  the  principal  efferent 
paths  involved  in  habitual  movements. 

Such  a  test  was  made  with  the  rat  and  gave  unmistakable  evidence 
against  the  participation  of  the  motor  areas  in  the  activities  of  the 
simple  maze  and  visual  discrimination  box  ( Franz  and  Lashley, ; 
Lashley37).  Since  the  publication  of  that  report,  the  results  have  been 
confirmed  for  a  complex  maze  and  the  "double-platform  box."  As  the 
rat  shows  no  paralysis  after  lesions  to  the  stimulable  areas  alone,  it  is 
not  possible  to  generalize  from  it  to  higher  forms  which  do  develop  a 
paralysis.  I  have  therefore  repeated  the  tests  in  a  series  of  experiments 
with  monkeys. 

EXPERIMENTAL     METHODS 

The  parlysis  which  follows  lesions  to  the  precentral  gyrus  in  monkeys 
necessitates  a  modification  of  the  technic  used  with  the  rat.  The  general 
procedure  was  as  follows :  The  animals  were  trained  in  rather  simple 
manipulative  acts  involving  a  new  and  easily  recognizable  pattern  and 
sequence  of  movements.  They  were  then  kept  without  practice  for 
about  two  months,  at  the  end  of  which  time  their  retention  of  the  habits 
was  tested.  This  gave  a  measure  of  the  normal  loss  to  be  expected 
from  disuse  of  the  habits  over  a  period  equal  to  that  required  for 
recovery  from  cerebral  paralysis.  After  these  preliminary  retention 
tests,  the  motor  areas  were  destroyed.  The  animals  were  then  kept 
without  further  practice  in  the  habits  until  the  paralysis  was  so  far 
improved  that  they  were  judged  capable  of  making  the  movements 
required.  They  were  then  given  a  final  series  of  retention  tests  (post- 
operative retention  tests)  and  brought  to  necropsy. 

Train ing  Methods. — For  training,  the  familiar  problem-box  method 
was  used.     The  animals  were  confined  in  a  large  cage.  5  by  5  feet,  to 


256  ARCHIVES    OF    NEUROLOGY    AND    PSYCHIATRY 

the  floor  of   which  a  small  problem-box  containing   food  was  bolted. 
Three  problem-boxes  were  used  : 

1.  Pull  box.  The  animal  must  reach  through  a  circular  hole.  2 
inches  in  diameter,  grasp  and  pull  forward  a  rod  which  passed  trans- 
versely 3  inches  behind  the  hole.  This  released  the  lid  of  the  box, 
which  was  thrown  open  by  a  spring. 

2.  Crank  box.  The  animal  must  grasp  and  turn  a  crank  projecting 
from  the  front  of  the  box.  The  crank  handle  described  a  6  inch  circle. 
It  offered  a  resistance  of  about  200  gm.  at  all  points  of  the  circle.  It 
was  set  at  "one  o'clock''  and  must  be  turned  counter-clockwise  through 
270  degrees,  at  which  point  it  released  the  lid  of  the  box.  The  lid  was 
thrown  back  by  a  spring. 

3.  Hasp  box.  The  animal  must  open  an  ordinary  gate  hasp,  closed 
with  a  wooden  plug  inserted  loosely  through  the  staple,  withdrawing 
the  plug  and  lifting  the  hasp  from  the  staple,  over  which  it  would  fall 
again  if  released.  He  must  then  raise  the  lid  of  the  box  and  hold  it 
open  while  he  reached  in  for  the  food. 

Five  trials  a  day  were  given  with  each  box ;  the  time  spent  in  each 
trial  was  recorded,  and  detailed  notes  were  made  as  to  the  use  of  right 
or  left  hand  and  the  exact  method  employed  in  opening  the  boxes. 
Training  was  continued  until  the  latches  were  released  in  a  stereotyped 
manner  without  random  movements.  Several  interruptions  of  training 
occurred  lasting  from  one  to  several  weeks,  so  that  the  learning  curves 
do  not  represent  the  rate  for  continuous  training. 

In  addition  to  the  problem-box  habits,  each  animal  was  trained  to 
pick  out  cubes  of  banana  from  among  cubes  of  wood  of  similar  size 
and  appearance.  The  cubes  were  scattered  in  irregular  order  under  a 
sheet  of  glass  supported  2  inches  above  the  floor  of  the  cage.  They 
were  placed  about  6  inches  back  from  the  edge  so  that  the  animals  had 
to  reach  under  the  glass  to  get  the  cubes,  which  they  could  see  but 
could  not  distinguish  by  odor. 

Operative  Technic. — Destruction  of  the  motor  areas  was  made  under 
ether  anesthesia,  with  aseptic  precautions.  The  region  of  the  precentral 
gyrus  was  exposed  by  trephining  and  identified  by  electrical  stimulation. 
The  opening  was  enlarged  by  bone-forceps  until  the  precentral  gyrus 
and  surrounding  areas  were  exposed.  Arm,  leg  and  face  areas  were 
verified  by  stimulation,  and  the  limits  of  the  excitable  area  determined. 
The  entire  area  was  then  undercut  by  thermocautery  to  a  depth  of 
about  <>  mm.  To  avoid  injury  to  the  longitudinal  sinus,  a  median  bridge 
oi  bone,  1  cm.  in  width,  was  left  intact.  In  one  specimen  the  leg  area  of 
this  region  was  undercut  by  passing  the  cautery  diagonally  mediad  and 
downward  until  the  resistance  of  the  falx  was  felt,  then  cutting  longi- 
tudinally across  the  gyrus.  The  dural  flaps  were  then  replaced  and 
the  wound  closed.  In  the  cases  reported  below  the  wounds  healed  with- 
out infection. 


LASHLEY— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION  257 

Retention  Tests. — The  animals  all  showed  marked  paralysis  after 
operation.  This  cleared  up  gradually,  and  the  retention  tests  were 
given  about  two  months  after  the  operation.  (Numbers  1  and  3,  v.i., 
recovered  somewhat  more  quickly  than  is  usually  the  case  when  the 
lesion  is  restricted  to  one  hemisphere.)  In  the  postoperative  retention 
tests,  the  animals  were  placed  singly  in  the  large  cage  with  each  of  the 
latch  boxes  in  turn.  The  time  required  to  open  the  boxes  was  noted, 
and  the  methods  were  recorded  in  detail  for  comparison  with  methods 
employed  in  learning  and  in  the  preliminary  retention  tests. 

Verification  of  Lesions. — When  the  tests  were  completed,  the  opera- 
tive fields  were  again  exposed  and  explored  by  electrical  stimulation. 
Excitable  points  found  were  mapped.  The  brains  were  then  removed, 
fixed  in  10  per  cent,  formaldehyd.  and  sketches  made  under  a  camera 
lucida.  Serial  sections  of  the  region  of  the  lesions  were  then  prepared. 
Camera  drawings  of  these  were  made  and  the  lesions  reconstructed 
from  them. 

PROTOCOLS 

Number  1. — This  was  a  small  male  cebus,  trained  on  the  crank  box,  pull  box 
and  hasp  box.  The  skull  was  trephined  and  opened  on  both  sides  in  front  of 
the  precentral  gyrus.  The  openings  were  extended  backward  to  the  fissure  of 
Rolando.  The  leg,  arm  and  face  areas  were  located  by  electrical  stimulation 
and  destroyed  by  cautery.  Cauterization  extended  beyond  the  stimulable  area 
except  in  the  median  line.    The  wound  was  covered  with  mica  and  closed. 

On  the  following  day,  there  was  a  partial  paralysis  of  both  sides  with  great 
spasticity.  Coordinated  walking  movements  were  possible,  but  there  was  great 
weakness  of  the  legs.  The  arms  were  extended  toward  food.  He  could  not 
grasp  with  his  left  hand.  Partial  grasping  with  the  right  hand  appeared,  but 
there  was  inability  to  raise  food  to  the  mouth.  He  recognized  a  banana,  and 
made  efforts  to  grasp  it.     The  arms  were  usually  hyperextended. 

Four  days  later,  he  moved  clumsily,  his  arms  and  legs  spread  out  frequently, 
letting  him  fall  prone.  He  grasped  with  his  right  hand,  but  was  unable  to  hold 
food  or  lift  it  to  his  mouth.  He  ate  by  thrusting  his  mouth  against  the  bread. 
He  was  well  oriented  in  the  room.  Two  weeks  before  operation  he  had  learned 
to  slip  out  of  the  crack  as  the  door  was  opened  and  to  run  into  an  adjoining 
room.  He  did  this  twice  on  the  fourth  day.  He  had  a  tendency  to  stay  near  a 
cage  containing  other  monkeys  and  to  hide  under  it  when  pursued.  There  was 
marked  tremor  after  slight  effort. 

Ten  days  after  operation,  he  stood  and  walked  without  falling,  fumbled  in 
grasping,  but  was  able  to  hold  food  in  the  right  hand  or  to  lift  it  to  his  mouth. 

Thirty-five  days  after  operation,  he  climbed  and  ran  accurately,  picked  up 
small  pieces  of  banana  with  the  right  hand  without  noticeable  clumsiness,  and 
made  quick  movements  in  efforts  to  catch  flies.  Retention  of  the  problem-box 
habits  was  tested  at  this  time. 

The  average  time  per  trial  in  each  day's  practice  (five  trials  daily)  is  given 
in  Table  1  for  each  of  the  problem-boxes.  This  is  followed  by  the  average  time 
per  trial  on  each  day  of  the  preliminary  retention  tests,  and  similarly  for  the 
postoperative  retention  tests. 


258 


ARCHIVES    OF    XEUROLOGY    AND    PSYCHIATRY 


Table   1.— Average    Time    in    Seconds   Per    Trial    Consumed    in    Opening   Each 

Problem-Box  in  Each  Day's  Practice  of  Training,  Preliminary  Retention 

Tests  and  Postoperative  Retention   Tests 


Training  Tests 

Prelimi 

nary  Retention 

.  A 

Tests 

l'n II  Box 

<  rank  Box 

Hasp  Box 

Pull  Box 

Crank  Box 

Hasp  Box 

•206.2 

135.2 

1.6 

3.2 

44.4 

."..4 

390.0 

34.4 

3.0 

1.6 

81.8 

1.8 

47.2 

10.0 

2.4 

11.0 

14.0 

•:.o 

12.8 

18.0 

1.2 

2.4 

1.6 

4.6 

5.6 

1.2 

3.S 

2.6 

2.4 

10.8 

2.7 

2.0 

27.6 

.i  .. 

2.6 

1.6 

4.S 
6.0 
1.8 

Postoperative  Retention  Tests 

Pull  Box 

Crank  Box 

Hasp  Box 

2.8 

2.8 

63.6 

31.0 

1.6 

2.2 

7.4 

57.6 

2.2 

4.4 

■2M 

3.8 

Tests  for  visual  discrimination  were  made.  There  was  no  error  in  100 
trials.  At  all  times  following  the  operation  the  animal  was  oriented  in  the 
cage  and  room  and.  with  the  exception  of  the  motor  disturbance,  gave  no 
indication  of  anv  deterioration. 


Fig.    1. — Extent   of    lesions    in   animal    Xo.    1. 
sketch  and  serial  sections. 


Reconstructed    from    camera 


Extent  of  Lesions. — The  areas  destroyed  are  shown  diagrammatically  in 
Figure  1  and  in  sections  in  Plate  1. 

Left  Hemisphere:  Mediad,  the  lesion  began  about  1  mm.  behind  the  end  of 
the  fissure  of  Rolando  and  extended  forward  to  the  level  of  the  knee  of  the 
corpus  callosum.  The  cortex  of  the  precentral  gyrus  was  destroyed  to  within 
2  mm.  of  the  edge  of  the  longitudinal  fissure,  but  that  of  the  median  surface 
was  uninjured.  Caudad,  the  lesion  extended  slightly  onto  the  postcentral  gyrus 
but  did  not  involve  all  of  the  cortex  within  the  fissure  of  Rolando.  Laterad,  it 
extended  to  the  upper  border  of  the  operculum.  The  parts  of  the  stimulable 
area  left  intact  were  the  paracentral  gyrus,  the  cortex  within  the  fissure  of 
Rolando  and  the  lateral  part  of  the  face  area  included  on  the  operculum. 


LASH  LEY— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION 


259 


Plate  1. — Fig.  A:  Outline  of  the  dorsal  aspect  of  the  cerebrum.  The  broken 
areas  outlined  indicate  the  extent  of  dural  adhesions.  The  transverse  broken 
lines  show  the  level  of  the  sections  designated  by  the  corresponding  numbers. 

Figs.  1  to  5. — Camera  sketches  of  sections  showing  extent  of  lesions.  Blood 
clots  and  scar  tissue  are  indicated  in  solid  black.  Obviously  degenerated  cortex 
is  marked  with  coarse  stippling.  R,  fissure  of  Rolando;  S,  fissure  of  Sylvius; 
P,  parieto-occipital  fissure. 


260  ARCHIVES    OF    NEUROLOGY    AXD    PSYCHIATRY 

Right  Hemisphere:  The  lesion  was  similar  to  that  on  the  left  but  slightly 
more  extensive.  The  cortex  within  the  rolandic  fissure  was  destroyed,  and  the 
lesion  extended  farther  over  the  operculum. 

After  destruction  of  almost  all  of  the  arm  areas  of  both  sides  and 
of  most  of  the  areas  of  the  legs  and  face,  this  animal,  on  recovery  from 
paralysis,  showed  perfect  retention  of  visual  and  motor  habits  acquired 
before  injury.  Except  for  the  paralysis  and  later  spasticity,  no  sig- 
nificant change  in  his  behavior  could  be  noted. 

Number  2. — This  was  a  small  male  cebus  too  wild  for  training  at  the  begin- 
ning of  the  experiments.  The  motor  area  of  the  right  hemisphere  was  exposed, 
the  arm,  face  and  leg  areas  identified  and  cauterized  to  a  depth  of  5  mm., 
caudad  to  central  fissure,  laterad  and  cephalad  to  a  line  5  mm.  beyond  the  limits 
of  the  excitable  area. 

Following  operation,  the  left  leg  and  arm  were  not  used.  The  leg  was 
hyperextended  and  gave  some  support  to  the  body  in  standing  or  sitting,  but 
made  no  stepping  movements.  No  movements  of  the  hand  could  be  elicited. 
This  complete  paralysis  of  the  left  hand  persisted  for  two  weeks. 

Four  weeks  after  operation,  the  left  leg  was  used  almost  normally.  The  left 
arm  could  be  used  to  support  the  animal's  weight,  but  tended  to  become  rigid  in 
hyerpextension,  and  the  left  hand  could  not  be  used  for  grasping. 

Eleven  weeks  after  operation,  the  paralysis  had  almost  disappeared;  the 
left  hand  was  somewhat  clumsy  but  could  be  used  in  grasping  food.  It  was 
not  used  when  the  right  hand  was  unrestrained. 

Training  on  the  problem  boxes  was  begun  at  this  stage  of  recovery.  During 
the  next  three  months,  the  problems  were  learned  and  retention  tests  given. 

Seven  months  after  the  first  operation,  the  left  motor  area  was  exposed  and 
similarly  explored  and  destroyed.  Paralysis  of  the  right  arm  and  leg  followed.  It 
appeared  to  be  as  complete  as  that  of  the  left  side  following  the  first  lesion.  It 
improved  more  rapidly,  however.  Six  weeks  after  operation  the  right  hand  was 
used  to  pick  up  food,  and,  although  still  somewhat  spastic,  was  judged  capable 
of  manipulating  the  latch  boxes.  Retention  tests  were  therefore  begun.  The 
average  time  per  trial  for  successive  groups  of  five  trials  in  training,  pre- 
liminary retention  tests,  and  postoperative  retention  tests  are  given  in  Table  2. 
Visual  discrimination  was  unaffected  by  the  operation. 

Table  2. — Average    Time   in   Seconds   Per   Trial   Consumed    in    Opening   Eaeh 

Problem-Box  in  Each  Day's  Practice  in  Training,  Preliminary  Retention 

Tests  and  Postoperative  Retention   Tests 


Tra 

ining  Tests 

Preliminary  Retention 

Tests 

Pull  Box 

Crank  Box 

Hasp  Box 

Pull  Box            Crank  Box 

Hasp  Box 

24  brs.« 

126.0 

102.2+ 

2.2                          28.8 

8.8 

24  hrs.* 

105.0 

lll.Ot 

2.0                           1.6 

6.4 

2,520  sec. 

16.0 

235.0 

24  hrs.* 

5.2 

184.4 

Postoperative  Retention  Tests 

366  sec. 
2.5 

7.S 
7.4 

19.6 
16.0 

A_ 

Pull  Box            Crank  Box 

Hasp  Box 

1.4 

1.4 

55.6 

9.8                        14.8 

102.5 

4.5 

1.2 

6.0 

3.6                        11.4 

93.4 

20.4 

2.8 

6.6 

1.2                             7.2 

35.4 

1.6 

60.6 
22.8 
15.4 

*  Failed  to  open  box  while  under  observation  and  was  left  in  the  cage  over  night. 
+  Time  with  hasp  left  unfastened. 


LASH  LEY— CEREBRAL     FUNCTION  261 

The  short  time  required  for  opening  the  boxes  in  the  postoperative 
retention  tests  gives  clear  evidence  for  some  retention  of  the  habits. 
Much  of  the  delay  apparent  was  due  to  the  weakness  and  clumsiness 
of  the  right  hand.  With  each  problem-box  the  attack  in  the  postopera- 
tive retention  tests  was  directly  on  the  latches.  The  methods  employed 
were  at  first  those  used  before  operation,  and  the  movements  were 
definitely  adapted  to  solving  the  problems,  although  lacking  force  and 
accuracy;  for  example,  efforts  were  all  directed  to  turning  the  crank 
counter-clockwise. 

The  weakness  of  the  right  arm  led  to  a  surprising  readjustment  on 
the  part  of  this  animal.  The  operation  on  the  right  hemisphere  made 
the  left  arm  weak  and  spastic  during  training.  In  all  of  the  trials  of 
training  and  preliminary  retention  tests  the  left  arm  was  used  only  as 
a  pro]),  and  the  left  hand  was  not  once  used  in  manipulating  the  latches 
of  any  of  the  boxes.  After  the  second  operation,  the  right  hand  was 
much  more  affected  than  the  left,  which  had  largely  recovered,  and 
an  almost  immediate  shift  to  the  left  hand  in  opening  all  of  the  boxes 
occurred. 

Pull  Box :  Postoperative  retention  tests.  Trial  1  :  Fumbled  in  hole  with 
right  hand,  removed  hand,  peered  into  hole;  again  inserted  right  hand  and  again 
pulled   lever ;   twenty-seven   seconds. 

Trial  2 :     Right  hand,  fairly  accurately,  seven  seconds. 

Trial  3:  He  fumbled  with  right,  then  inserted  left,  grasped  lever  and  pulled: 
seven  seconds. 

Trial  4 :     He  inserted  left  hand  at  once ;  three  seconds. 

Trial  5:     Left  hand  used  at  once;  five  seconds. 

The  right  hand  was  used  only  four  times  in  the  succeeding  forty  trials. 

Crank  Box:  Trial  1:  He  grasped  the  crank  with  his  right  hand  at  once 
and  turned  counter-clockwise.  The  crank  stuck  in  the  third  quadrant.  He 
pushed  at  it  feebly,  gave  up,  returned  to  the  attack  from  the  side  of  the  box 
and  pulled  it  through  final  segment ;  160  seconds. 

Trial  2:  He  grasped  the  crank  with  the  right  hand.  Apparently,  he  was 
unable  to  move  it.  He  grasped  it  with  both  hands  and  swung  it  around ; 
twenty-five  seconds. 

Trials  3,  4  and  5:  He  used  only  the  right  hand  and  turned  with  difficulty 
moving  to  side  of  box  and  exerting  direct  pull  instead  of  his  former  transverse- 
rotary  movement. 

Trial  6:  He  grasped  with  the  left  hand  and  turned  counter-clockwise; 
six  seconds.    All  later  trials  were  made  with  the  left  hand  only. 

Hasp  Box:  On  the  first  three  days  of  the  postoperative  tests,  he  pulled 
out  the  plug  and  disengaged  the  hasp  promptly  with  his  right  hand,  but  lacked 
strength  to  lift  the  lid.     He  gave  up  after  a  few  attempts  with  his  right  hand. 

On  the  fourth  day,  he  drew  out  the  plug  and  disengaged  the  hasp  with  his 
right  hand,  then  lifted  the  lid  with  his  left  foot.  It  fell  back  as  he  attempted 
to  reach  the  food.  He  lifted  it  again  with  his  left  hand,  climbed  to  the  edge 
of  the  box,  bringing  his  right  side  against  the  lid,  so  holding  it  up  while  he 
grasped  the  food  with  his  left  hand.  On  the  second  trial,  he  lifted  with  the 
left  hand  and  held  it  up  with  his  left  hand,  inserting  his  head  for  the  food.     In 


262  ARCHIVES    OF    NEUROLOGY    AXD    PSYCHIATRY 

all  later  trials,  he  lifted  the  lid  with  his  left  hand,  sometimes  holding  it  back 
with  his  left  knee  or  with  his  head  while  reaching  into  the  box  with  his  left 
hand,  or  with  the  left  hand  while  reaching  with  his  head.  On  the  eighth  and 
all  later  trials,  he  pulled  the  plug  with  his  left  hand  and  used  the  right  only  as 
a  prop. 

Throughout  the  retention  tests  his  activities  were  centered  on  the 
plug,  hasp,  and  lid.  He  never  attempted  to  lift  the  lid  until  the  hasp 
was  disengaged.  When  the  lid  was  raised  the  next  acts  seemed  defi- 
nitely  directed  to  holding  it  up,  and  though  clumsy,  the  movements  were 
clearly  not  random. 

EFFECTS     OF     SUBSEQUENT     DESTRUCTION     OF     THE     CORPUS     STRIATUM 

A  hroad  bladed  cautery  was  next  passed  through  the  old  lesion  into 
the  corpus  striatum  and  drawn  back  and  forth  through  this  nucleus. 
The  wound  was  closed,  and  the  animal  was  kept  under  observation 
until  his  death  eight  days  later. 

On  recovery  from  anesthesia,  the  animal  showed  marked  spasticity 
of  the  left  side.  The  left  arm  was  usually  hyperextended,  although  in 
walking  or  clinging  to  a  perch  the  arm  and  leg  assumed  a  normal 
posture.  The  left  side  was  very  weak,  and  when  he  walked  the  arm 
and  leg  frequently  collapsed  suddenly.  He  took  food  with  his  right 
hand  and  placed  it  in  his  mouth.  When  I  held  a  bit  of  food,  he  drew 
my  fingers  to  his  mouth  with  the  palm  of  his  left  hand,  but  without 
closing  the  fingers,  which  remained  hyperextended. 

On  subsequent  days,  he  used  both  right  and  left  hands  in  walking, 
climbing  and  grasping  food.  The  left  side  was  spastic  and  very  weak 
but  capable  of  a  variety  of  fine  adaptive  movements.  The  right  side 
-bowed  a  coarse  tremor,  and  athetoid  movements  of  the  left  arm 
appeared  when  the  right  was  used.  There  was  marked  paralysis  of  the 
pharynx.  He  kept  his  mouth  stuffed  with  food  or  shavings  but  was 
unable  to  swallow. 

At  no  time  was  the  paralysis  as  marked  as  after  the  destruction  of 
die  cortex.  Indeed  the  condition  showed  no  resemblance  to  hemiplegia, 
but,  except  for  the  pharyngeal  paralysis,  was  essentially  that  described 
by  Wilson  4L'  for  lesions  of  the  striate  nucleus  without  involvement  of 
the  pyramidal  tracts.  The  possibility  that  the  recovery  from  the  initial 
paralysis  was  due  to  vicarious  functioning  of  the  striate  nucleus  seems 
thus  definitely  to  be  ruled  out.  The  animal  did  not  recover  sufficiently 
for  retention  tests  after  this  operation,  but  his  behavior  when  released 
in  the  laboratory  -bowed  that  his  general  orientation   was  unaffected. 


42.  Wilson,    S.   A.   K. :      An    Experimental    Research    Into    the    Anatomy    and 
Physiology  of  the  Corpus  Striatum.  Brain  36:427-492,  1913. 


LASH  LEV— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION  263 

When  given  an  egg,  he  made  efforts  to  break  it  by  pounding  it  on  the 
floor,  as  he  had  done  before  the  operation,  and  in  the  performance  of 
this  habit  both  hands  were  used. 

Extent  of  Lesions. — The  extent  of  the  destructions  is  indicated  in  Figure  2 
and  sections  through  the  area  are  shown  in  Plate  2. 

Right  Hemisphere  :  The  lesion  extended  cephalad  from  the  median  end  of  the 
central  fissure  to  the  middle  of  the  superior  frontal  gyrus,  bordering  the 
longitudinal  fissure  but  leaving  the  cortex  of  the  median  surface  of  the  hemi- 
sphere intact.  Caudad  it  invaded  the  postcentral  gyrus  and  completely  oblit- 
erated the  fissure  of  Rolando.  Laterad  it  extended  well  onto  the  operculum. 
Only  the  paracentral  gyrus  and  the  lateral  portion  of  the  face  area  remained 
intact. 

The  second  operation  destroyed  all  of  the  caudate  nucleus  and  the  greater 
part  of  the  lenticular,  leaving  only  the  posterior  end  of  the  puramen  intact. 


Fig.  2. — The  extent  of  the  lesions  in  animal  No.  2.  Reconstructed  from 
camera  sketch  and  serial  sections.  The  posterior  border  of  the  left  precentral 
gyrus  escaped  injury. 

Left  Hemisphere:  The  lesion  was  less  extensive  than  that  on  the  right.  It 
began  5  mm.  in  front  of  the  median  end  of  the  central  fissure  and  extended  to 
the  frontal  lobe.  All  of  the  cortex  of  the  median  surface  to  the  callosomarginal 
fissure  was  destroyed.  The  posterior  edge  of  the  precentral  gyrus  remained 
intact,  for  a  width  of  about  5  mm. 

In  this  animal,  practically  all  of  the  precentral  gyrus  of  the  right 
hemisphere  was  destroyed.  He  was  then  trained  in  manipulative  move- 
ments of  the  right  hand.  This  was  followed  by  partial  destruction  of 
the  left  precentral  gyrus.  (  )n  recovery  from  paralysis,  he  gave  clear 
evidence  of  retention  of  the  habits  but  owing  to  spasticity  of  the  right 
hand,  made  a  direct  transfer  of  the  habits  to  the  left  hand.  The  right 
precentral  gyrus  was  almost  completely  destroyed,  whereas  a  rather 
large  proportion  of  the  left  precentral  gyrus  escaped  injury,  but  in  spite 


264  ARCH  IlllS     OF     NEUROLOGY    AND     PSYCHIATRY 


Plate  2. — Fig.  A:  Outline  of  the  dorsal  aspect  of  the  cerehrum.  The  broken 
areas  outlined  indicate  the  extent  of  dural  adhesions.  The  transverse  broken 
lines  show  the  level  of  the  sections  designated  by  the  corresponding  numbers. 

Figs.  1  to  5. — Camera  sketches  of  sections  showing  extent  of  lesions.  Blood 
clots  and  scar  tissue  are  indicated  in  solid  black.  Obviously  degenerated  cortex 
is  marked  with  coarse  stippling.  R.  fissure  of  Rolando:  S,  fissure  of  Sylvius: 
/',  parieto -occipital  fissure. 


LAS  HLEY— CEREBRAL     FUNCTION  265 

of  this,  the  animal  shifted  to  the  use  of  his  left  hand  in  opening  the 
problem-boxes.  Subsequent  destruction  of  the  greater  part  of  the 
corpus  striatum  did  not  produce  a  recurrence  of  the  hemiplegic 
symptom  s. 

Number  3. — This  was  a  large  female  rhesus  trained  on  problem-boxes  and 
visual  discrimination.  After  retention  tests,  the  motor  areas  of  both  sides  were 
exposed,  mapped  and  cauterized.  Following  the  operation  the  legs  and  left 
arm  were  completely  paralyzed.  The  right  arm  made  clumsy  pawing  move- 
ments. Twelve  hours  after  operation,  the  animal  walked  a  few  feet  with  stag- 
gering gait,  then  collapsed  with  arms  and  legs  widely  extended,  and  for  several 
days  made  no  further  efforts  to  walk.  The  following  day  she  grasped  a  grape 
with  her  right  hand  and  brought  it  to  her  mouth  after  several  unsuccessful 
trials.    The  movements  were  clumsy  and  slow. 

Four  weeks  after  operation  she  seemed  sufficiently  recovered  for  retention 
tests,  although  still  showing  a  general  clumsiness  and  marked  weakness  of  the 
left  limbs. 

The  average  time  per  trial  for  successive  groups  of  five  trials  in  training, 
preliminary  retention  tests  and  retention  tests  after  operation  is  given  in  Table  3. 
Visual  discrimination  was  unaffected  by  the  operation. 

Table  3. — Average    Time    in    Seconds   Per    Trial   Required   for    Opening   Each 

Problem-Box   in   Each  Day's  Practice   in    Training,  Preliminary   Retention 

Tests,  and  Postoperative  Retention  Tests 


Training  T*  >t- 

Preli: 

minary 

Retention 

Tests 

Pull  Box 

Crank  Box 

H 

asp  Box 

Pull  Box 

Crank  Box 

Hasp  Box 

68.2 

1,595.0 

40.4- 

1.0 

4.2 

24.0 

2. 6 

111.8 

373.2 

1.0 

3.8 

6.6 

1.0 

145.4 

124.<i 

1.6 

5.0 

1.0 

304.2 

24.2 

1.2 

3.6 

140.0 

17.0 

1.0 

2.2 

16.S 

2.8 

Po> 

'toperative 

Retention  Tests 

Pull  Box  Crank  Box  Hasp  Box 
1.0                                                         2.2  49.2 

1.0  1.4  9.8 

2.6  1.4  8.4 

'  Time  with  hasp  unfastened. 

The  time  required  to  open  the  problem-boxes  in  the  postoperative 
retention  tests  gives  certain  evidence  of  the  retention  of  the  habits. 
An  average  of  678.8  seconds  was  consumed  in  each  of  the  first  five 
trials  of  training  in  opening  the  boxes  by  the  method  of  random  activity. 
( )nly  17.5  seconds'  average  were  required  for  the  first  five  trials  of 
the  postoperative  retention  tests.  The  methods  of  opening  the  pull  and 
crank  boxes  were  the  same  before  and  after  the  operation.  The  per- 
sistent weakness  of  the  left  arm  called  for  a  change  in  method  of  open- 
ing the  hasp  box.  Before  operation,  the  animal  had  used  the  same 
method  in  twenty  consecutive  trials.  The  plug  was  pulled  out  of  the 
staple  with  the  right  hand.  The  hasp  was  lifted  from  the  staple  with 
the  right  hand,  turned  back  against  the  lid,  and  then  transferred  to  the 
left  hand.     The  lid  was  lifted  with  the  left  hand  and  the  right  hand 


266  ARCHIVES    OF    NEUROLOGY    AXD    PSYCHIATRY 

thrust  into  the  box  tor  the  food.  In  the  postoperative  retention  tests, 
the  left  hand  was  not  used  at  all.  She  first  lifted  the  lid  with  her  right 
hand,  then  released  it  and  attempted  to  grasp  the  food  with  the  same 
hand,  but  the  lid  falling  back  prevented  this.  On  the  first  trial,  she 
finally  held  the  lid  back  with  her  right  hand,  inserted  her  head  in  the 
box  and  took  the  food  in  her  teeth.  On  the  second  trial,  she  thrust 
her  head  against  the  lid  after  raising  it  with  the  right  hand  and  so  held 
it  open  while  the  hand  was  inserted  in  the  box.  The  same  method  was 
used  on  the  third  and  fourth  trials.  On  the  fifth  trial,  she  attempted 
to  hold  up  the  lid  with  her  left  foot  and  finally  succeeded  in  this  after 
overbalancing  twice.  In  all  later  trials,  she  released  the  lid  and  allowed 
it  to  fall  against  her  right  arm  as  this  was  thrust  into  the  box.     These 


Fig.  3. — The  extent  of  the  lesions  in  animal  Xo.  J.  Reconstructed  from 
camera  sketch  and  serial  sections.  Practically  the  whole  of  both  precentral 
gyri  destroyed,  including  the  paracentral  gyrus  and  cortex  within  the  central 
sulcus. 

various  acts  were  carried  out  with  definite  adaptation  to  the  contour 
of  the  box  and  had  none  of  the  elements  of  random  pulling  and  thrust- 
ing which  characterize  the  initial  stages  of  learning.  As  in  the  case  of 
Number  2,  there  seemed  to  be  an  immediate  adaptation  of  movements 
to  opening  the  box,  which  had  not  been  employed  at  any  time  in  the 
previous  practice. 

Extent  of  Lesions. — The  injured  areas  are  shown  in  Figure  3  and  sections  in 
Plate  3. 

Right  Hemisphere:  The  lesion  began  at  the  posterior  median  end  of  the 
central  fissure  and  included  practically  all  of  the  precentral  gyrus.  On  the 
median  surface,  all  of  the  cortex  above  the  callosomarginal  fissure  was  destroyed. 
Practically  all  of  the  gyrus  within  the  rolandic  fissure  was  involved.     Laterad. 


I. ASHLEY— CEREBRAL     FUNCTION 


267 


Plate  3. — Fig.  A:  Outline  of  the  dorsal  aspect  of  the  cerebrum.  The  broken 
areas  outlined  indicate  the  extent  of  dural  adhesions.  In  a  preliminary  exam- 
ination, the  left  hemisphere  was  cut  through  along  the  longitudinal  dotted  line. 
The  sections  of  its  two  halves  are  therefore  from  somewhat  different  levels. 
The  transverse  broken  lines  show  the  level  of  the  sections  designated  by  the 
corresponding  numbers. 

Figs.  1  to  5. — Camera  sketches  of  sections  showing  extent  of  lesions.  Blood 
clots  and  scar  tissue  are  indicated  in  solid  black.  Obviously  degenerated  cortex 
is  marked  with  coarse  stippling.  R,  fissure  of  Rolando;  S,  fissure  of  Sylvius: 
P ,  parieto-occipital  fissure. 


268  ARCHIVES    OF    NEUROLOGY    AND    PSYCHIATRY 

the  lesion  included  the  upper  half  of  the  operculum.     At  most,  only  the  lateral 
facial  area  escaped  destruction. 

Left  Hemisphere:  The  lesion  was  almost  coextensive  with  that  on  the  right. 
More  of  the  paracentral  gyrus  was  destroyed  and  somewhat  less  of  the 
i  iperculum. 

After  practically  complete  destruction  of  both  precentral  gyri,  this 
animal  gave  evidence  of  perfect  retention  of  visual  habits  and  habits 
of  manipulation.  Direct  adaptive  changes  in  behavior  were  made  to 
compensate  for  weakness  of  the  left  arm. 

DISCUSSION     OF     EXPERIMENTS 

After  extensive  lesions  to  the  precentral  gyri  of  both  hemispheres, 
each  of  tbe  animals  studied  gave  clear  evidence  of  the  retention  of 
patterns  of  movement  which  had  been  acquired  before  the  operative 
destructions.  Evidence  of  this  retention  was  obtained  from  a  com- 
parison of  the  time  required  for  opening  the  problem  boxes  in  initial 
training  with  that  for  the  postoperative  retention  tests,  from  the  restric- 
tion of  reactions  in  the  postoperative  retention  tests  to  the  catches  of  the 
problem  boxes,  and  from  the  persistence  of  individual  peculiarities  of 
opening  the  boxes. 

The  average  time  required  by  all  animals  for  opening  each  box  in 
the  first  five  trials  of  training  was  584  43  seconds.  An  average  of  only 
30.7  seconds  was  required  in  the  first  rive  trials  of  the  postoperative 
tests.  The  animals  all  failed  the  hasp  box  in  the  preliminary  training 
until  first  trained  without  the  plug  in  the  staple.  In  the  postoperative 
tests,  they  all  opened  this  box  promptly  with  the  hasp  closed  by  the 
plug.  At  the  beginning  of  the  postoperative  tests,  each  animal  (except 
Number  3  with  the  hasp  box)  used  the  same  hand  or  hands  for  each 
part  of  the  manipulation  as  he  had  before  the  operation,  and  attacked 
the  latches  in  his  former  manner,  although  the  methods  of  attack  were 
modified  rapidly  to  compensate  for  the  persistent  motor  difficulties.  All 
the  animals  showed  perfect  retention  in  the  visual  discrimination  test. 

These  results  establish  conclusively  that  the  cerebral  areas  destroyed 
were  not  essential  to  the  performance  of  the  habits  studied,  and  observa- 
tions on  the  general  behavior  of  the  animals  following  recovery  from 
the  paralysis  justify  the  further  conclusion  that  the  areas  are  not  essen- 
tial to  the  performance  of  any  type  of  complex  adaptive  or  habitual 
activity. 

Four  possible  explanations  of  the  results  must  be  considered: 

1.  In  no  case  did  the  operation  destroy  the  entire  precentral  gyrus 
of  both  sides.     The  parts  remaining  intact  may  have  contained  a  suffi- 


-13.  This  does  not  include  the   failures  with  the  has])  box  or  the  times  when 
Number  1  was  left  in  the  case  over  night. 


LASHLEY— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION  269 

cient  number  of  fibers  previously  integrated  in  tbe  habit  to  produce  the 
conditioned  reflexes,  in  spite  of  the  great  destruction  of  other  fibers  of 
equivalent  function.  Such  a  possibility  is  supported  by  data  on  other 
functional  areas  in  which  the  various  parts  seem  equipotential  (  Franz, 4::" 
Lashley34)  and  by  the  apparent  equipotentiality  of  parts  of  tbe  motor 
area  revealed  by  electrical  stimulation  (Lashley44).  but  several  facts 
speak  strongly  against  this  explanation. 

Partial  destructions  usually  entail  a  certain  confusion  in  the  per- 
formance of  all  the  functions  of  an  area,  which  seems  to  exceed  any- 
thing of  the  sort  noted  in  these  animals.4"' 

In  Number  3,  the  destruction  on  both  sides  was  so  nearly  complete 
that  only  a  part  of  the  face  areas  could  have  remained  functional. 
If  we  attempt  to  explain  the  survival  of  habits  as  being  due  to  the 
activity  of  undestroyed  parts  of  the  motor  area,  we  must  assume  that 
a  part  of  the  face  area  is  capable  of  performing  all  the  functions  of  the 
entire  motor  cortex — an  assumption  which  is  as  far  from  the  accepted 
views  of  localization  as  is  the  denial  of  all  habit  function  to  tbe  motor 
areas. 

2.  It  might  be  urged  that  in  the  recovery  from  the  motor  paralysis, 
the  vicarious  functions  assumed  by  other  areas  included  the  movements 
involved  in  the  problem-box  habits ;  that  the  habits  were  relearned  dur- 
ing the  period  of  recovery  from  paralysis.  The  habits,  howrever,  con- 
sist of  particular  patterns  of  movement  associated  with  the  stimuli 
presented  by  the  latch  boxes.  During  the  postoperative  period,  there 
was  no  occasion  for  the  animals  to  reacquire  these  particular  patterns 
of  movement  and  no  opportunity  for  the  movements  to  be  associated 
with  the  latch   boxes. 

3.  The  long  controversy  concerning  the  sensorimotor  function  of 
both  the  precentral  and  postcentral  gyri  suggests  that  the  two  may  both 
include  centrifugal  cells  for  the  performance  of  habits.  The  literature 
cited  in  the  first  part  of  this  paper  seems  to  establish  the  differential 
function  of  the  two  areas,  however,  and  the  lack  of  paralysis  after 
lesions    to    the    postcenral    gyrus    makes    the     hypothesis     untenable. 


43a.  Franz,  S.  I.:  On  the  Functions  of  the  Cerebrum:  The  Frontal  Lobes, 
Arch.  Psychol,  1907.  No.  2,  pp.  1-64. 

44.  Lashley,  K.  S. :  Temporal  Variation  in  the  Function  of  the  (iyrus  Pre- 
centralis  in  Primates,  Am.  J.  Physiol.  65:585-602,  1923. 

45.  I  am  collecting  data  on  this  question  at  present.  The  evidence  is  not 
complete,  but  there  is  indication  that.  e.  g.,  any  extensive  but  incomplete 
destruction  of  the  visual  areas  of  both  hemispheres  in  the  rat  is  followed  by 
inaccuracy  of  brightness  discrimination,  with  great  variability  from  day  to 
day,  such  as  has  been  reported  by  Franz  in  1916  for  aphasia,  yet  without  any 
complete  loss  of  any  phase  of  the  visual  function.  Such  loss  as  appeared  in 
the  motor  hahits  of  the  monkeys  was  almost  certainly  ascribable  to  the  simple 
motor  weakness,  and  gave  no  indication  ol  an  apraxia. 


270  ARCHIVES    OF    NEUROLOGY    AND    PSYCHIATRY 

Nanagas  46  found  a  few  islands  of  large  pyramidal  cells  in  the  postcen- 
tral gyrus,  but  the  great  mass  of  them  was  restricted  to  the  precentral. 
Finally,  Brown  36  reported  that  the  destruction  of  the  postcentral  gyrus 
did  not  abolish  learning  ability  or  interfere  seriously  with  habits  formed 
before  the  operation  in  the  chimpanzee. 

4.  The  only  remaining  possibility  seems  to  be  that  the  electro- 
stimulable  areas  do  not  include  the  centrifugal  elements  of  conditioned 
reflex  arcs  of  any  sort.  (  It  is  of  course  possible  that  they  contain 
some  such  elements,  but  these  cannot  comprise  any  significant  propor- 
tion of  the  total  number  of  centrifugal  cells,  since  their  destruction 
leaves  the  habits  completely  unaffected.)  In  this,  the  experiments  con- 
firm for  primates  the  results  previously  reported  for  the  rat.  The 
neural  impulses  involved  in  conditioned  reactions  do  not  pass  from 
sensory  projection  areas  to  the  precentral  gyrus  and  thence  to  lower 
centers,  but  must  be  conducted  by  centrifugal  cells  lying  outside  of  the 
pyramidal  system.  In  the  rat.  the  evidence  points  to  the  view  that  the 
centrifugal  fibers  of  the  sensory  projection  area  itself  are  primarily 
involved  in  this  motor  function,  since  the  destruction  of  any  fourth  of  the 
cerebrum  exclusive  of  the  visual  areas  does  not  affect  the  performance 
of  visual  habits.47  Whether  or  not  the  same  lack  of  important  tran>- 
cortical  conduction  holds  true  for  the  monkey  is  questionable  in  view  of 
the  greater  proportionate  development  of  the  transcortical  association 
tracts  in  this  animal,  but  it  seems  established  that  the  "motor  areas"  are 
not  concerned  in  the  initiation  of  habitual  movements. 

THE     CORPUS     STRIATUM      AND     VICARIOUS     FUNCTION 

All  recent  students  of  the  question  agree  that  recovery  from  cerebral 
paralysis  is  not  due  to  the  assumption  of  the  function  of  the  destroyed 
motor  cortex  by  the  corresponding  area  of  the  opposite  side.  On  the 
contrary,  the  simultaneous  destruction  of  the  areas  in  both  hemispheres 
seems  to  be  followed  by  a  rather  more  rapid  recovery  than  follows  the 
destruction  of  either  alone.  The  fact  has  been  noted  by  Grunbaum  and 
Sherrington  20  and  by  Wagner  -\  It  was  apparent  in  the  slower  recov- 
ery of  Number  2  from  the  first  operation  than  from  the  second.     The 


46.  Nanagas,  J.  C. :  Anatomical  Studies  on  the  Motor  Cortex  of  Macacus 
rhesus,  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  35:67-96.  1922. 

47.  Experiments  now  in  progress,  which  indicate  that  extensive  fronto- 
parieto-temporal  lesions  may  also  abolish  visual  habits  without  producing 
a  general  deterioriation  of  learning  ability,  indicate  that  a  mass  action  of  the 
cerebrum  is  also  somehow  involved,  but  they  do  not  seem  to  invalidate  the  con- 
clusion that  the  efferent  fibers  of  the  sensory  projection  area  are  primarilv 
concerned  in  the  subcortical  initiation  of  movements  associated  with  the  receptor 
for  that  area. 


LASHLEY— CEREBRAL     FUNCTION  271 

explanation    is    probably    to    be    found    in    the    forced    practice    which 
diplegia  imposes  on  the  paralyzed  limbs  (Odin  and  Franz4"). 

Other  restricted  cerebral  areas  have  also  been  rather  definitely 
excluded  from  participation  in  the  vicarious  function  of  the  stimulable 
areas,  by  the  work  of  Leyton  and  Sherrington  2,i  (  Lashley  49) .  Luciani  :"' 
has  suggested  that  recovery  may  be  due  to  the  activity  of  the  corpus 
striatum,  which  has  homologies  with  the  stimulable  cortex.  This  was 
tested  in  animal  Number  2  of  the  present  series  by  destruction  of  the 
right  striatum  after  recovery  from  diplegia.  Hemiplegia  symptoms  did 
not  recur,  so  that  we  may  conclude  that  the  recovery  had  not  been  due 
to  the  vicarious  activity  of  the  striate  nucleus. 

THE     FUNCTION     OF     THE     ELECTROSTIMULABLE     AREAS 

The  conclusions  which  may  be  drawn  from  these  experiments  are 
wholly  negative.  They  seem  to  prove  that  the  precentral  gyrus  does 
not  include  the  efferent  paths  for  learned  activities ;  in  the  current 
localization  terminology,  it  is  not  the  center  for  "voluntary  movements/' 
as  is  almost  universally  assumed.  But  if  this  is  true,  what  is  the 
significance  of  the  movements  elicitated  by  electrical  stimulation?  How 
may  we  interpret  the  cerebral  paralyses,  and  why  do  they  especially 
affect  the  finer  manipulative  movements?  A  number  of  lines  of  evi- 
dence may  help  to  answer  these  questions  and  clear  up  tbe  function  of 
the  precentral  gyri. 

The  Postural  Function  of  the  Stimulable  Areas. — Many  investi- 
gators have  pointed  out  the  similarity  between  the  movements  elicited 
by  cortical  stimulation  and  "voluntary  movements."  I  am  convinced 
that  this  is  an  error  due  to  contrasting  these  movements  with  those 
which  are  elicited  by  stimulation  of  motor  nerves  or  spinal  cord.  In 
the  latter  cases,  the  movements  are  wholly  incoordinated.  whereas  the 
movements  following  cortical  stimulation  involve  synergic  groups  of 
muscles.  But  in  all  cortical  stimulation  experiments  which  I  have  seen, 
the  movements  have  been  slow  and  rather  massive,  i.  e.,  chiefly  involving 
the  larger  musculature  of  the  limbs.  When  smaller  segments  are 
moved,  the  movements  are  never  coordinated  as  they  are.  for  example, 
in  grasping  small  objects.  They  never  show  the  fineness  of  gradation 
and  accuracy  of  adjustment  which  is  characteristic  of  the  movements 
of  the  intact  animal.  This  has  been  observed  by  various  investigators 
and  interpreted  as  showing  that  the  finer  adjustments  are  integrated 


48.  Odin,  R.,  and  Franz,  S.  I :     On  Cerebral   Motor  Control:    The  Recovery 
from  Experimentally  Produced  Hemiplegia,  Psychobiol.  1:33-50,   1917. 

49.  Lashley,  K.  S.:     Studies  of  Cerebral  Function   in  Learning.     Vicarious 
Function  After  Destruction  of  the  Visual  areas,  Am.   1.  Physiol.  59:44-71.  1922. 

50.  Luciani,  L. :     Human  Physiology,  London  3.  1915. 


272  ARCHIVES     OF    NEUROLOGY    AXD    PSYCHIATRY 

at  some  higher  level  and  imposed  through  it  on  the  motor  area.  But 
there  is  no  direct  evidence  that  this  is  the  case.  The  movements  fol- 
lowing excitation  are  far  more  like  the  gross  changes  of  posture  which 
one  may  observe  in  the  intact  animal — the  raising  of  an  arm  preparatory 
to  snatching  at  food,  bracing  against  a  pressure,  or  the  like.  It  seems 
significant  that  coordinated  movements  of  the  eyes  are  among  the  most 
easilv  elicited  movements  on  electrical  stimulation  (  although  their  stimu- 
late points  lie  outside  of  the  precentral  areas),  and  that  these  move- 
ments in  the  intact  man  or  animal  are  almost  always  a  reflex  fixation 
(postural  adjustment)  called  out  directly  by  exterostimulation  and,  in 
fact,  can  not  be  accurately  performed  in  the  absence  of  such  stimulation, 
as  with  lids  closed. 

Wilson  51  has  pointed  out  the  similarity  of  the  contractures  in  cere- 
bral paralysis  to  the  postural  reflexes  of  decerebrate  rigidity,  and  from 
this  it  seems  certain  that  a  part  of  the  function  of  the  stimulable  areas 
is  the  regulation  of  these  spinal  and  cerebellar  postures.  It  seems  rather 
probable  that  the  movements  obtained  on  electrical  stimulation  are  only 
a  further  exhibition  of  this  postural  activity  and  are  unrelated  to  the 
finer  coordinations  of  conditioned  motor  reflexes,  or  motor  habits. 

The  Dynamic  Function  of  the  Stimulable  Areas. — The  condition 
following  lesions  to  the  precentral  gyrus  or  internal  capsule,  even  in 
man,  should  be  described  rather  as  an  enormous  difficulty  in  making 
movements  than  as  an  absolute  paralysis  of  movement.  The  degree  of 
paralysis  varies  somewhat  from  day  to  day.  Excitement  seems  to 
increase  motor  control  (Minkowski.52  Lashley37),  and  the  paralvsis 
may  in  part  or  wholly  disappear  during  emotional  disturbance,  only  to 
recur  when  the  disturbing  situation  is  past.  If  we  may  judge  from  the 
tonic  condition  of  the  muscles,  there  must  be  in  excitement  a  ireneral 
facilitation  of  lower  motor  centers  which  temporarily  reinstates  cerebral 
control.  Further,  if  this  is  the  case,  in  the  intact  animal  in  the  absence 
of  emotional  stimulation  cerebral  control  must  likewise  be  conditioned 
by  some  such  facilitation  derived  from  the  precentral  gvrus.  The  work 
of  Brown  53  and  of  Leyton  and  Sherrington  26  has  shown  that  stimula- 
tion of  the  motor  area  does   facilitate  the  centrifugal   paths  of  other 


51.  Wilson,  S.  A.  K. :  On  Decerebrate  Rigidity  in  Man  and  the  Occurrence 
of  Tonic  Fits,  Brain  43:220-268,  1920. 

52.  Minkowski,  M. :  Etude  physiologique  des  circonvolutions  rolandique  et 
parietal.  Arch.  Suisse  de  Neurol,  et  Psychiat.  1:389-459,  1917. 

53.  Brown,  T.  G. :  Studies  in  the  Physiology  of  the  Nervous  System.  XXV. 
On  the  Phenomenon  of  Facilitation.  4.  Its  Occurrence  in  the  Subcortical 
Mechanism  by  the  Action  of  Which  Motor  Effects  Are  Produced  on  Artificial 
Stimulation  of  the  "Motor"  Cortex.  J.  Physiol.  9:131-145.  1915;  also  Footnote  36. 


LASHLEY— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION  273 

areas,  either  at  cortical  or  subcortical  levels,  since  stimulation  of  the 
precentral  gyrus  renders  the  otherwise  inexcitable  postcentral  gyrus 
excitable  for  corresponding  movements. 

After  partial  recovery  from  cerebral  paralysis,  the  most  prominent 
symptom  is  the  weakness  of  the  formerly  paralyzed  limbs.  The  greater 
part  of  their  repertoire  of  movements  may  be  restored,  speed  may  be 
nearly  normal,  but  only  a  slight  force  can  be  exerted,  and  fatigue  occurs 
readily.  What  is  lacking  in  this  condition  is  not  an  adequate  integra- 
tion of  the  motor  impulses,  but  a  sufficient  mass  of  neural  impulses  to 
maintain  muscular  activity.  This  may  be  ascribed  either  to  a  reduction 
in  the  number  of  functional  nerve  cells,  or  to  inadequate  facilitation. 
The  ready  fatigability  is  evidence  for  the  latter.54 

There  is  evidence  that  the  withdrawal  of  facilitation  derived  from 
other  sources  will  produce  similar  weakness  and  fatigability  and  a  ten- 
dency not  to  use  the  affected  parts.  Thus  Munk  55  has  shown  that 
denervation  of  a  limb  has  such  effects,  and  Sherrington's  50  work  has 
demonstrated  that  they  are  due  to  the  withdrawal  of  impulses  derived 
largely  from  the  denervated  muscles. 

The  importance  of  such  facilitating  systems  has  been  emphasized 
by  a  number  of  recent  investigators  (Monakow,57  Wilson,42  Sherring- 
ton,58 Tournay,59  Hunt00).  The  general  conception  of  these  investi- 
gators is  of  a  series  of  hierarchies  of  motor  reflexes,  all  exerting  a 
facilitating  influence  on  the  final  common  path.  These  involve  at  least 
the  following  elements. 


54.  The  all  or  nothing  principle  of  nerve  and  muscle  activity  requires  the 
assumption  that  strength  of  muscular  contraction  is  dependent  upon  the  number 
of  motor  fibers  involved  and  the  rate  of  succession  of  propagated  disturbance-. 
Piper's  work  ( Electrophysiologic  menschlicher  Muskeln,  Berlin,  1912)  indi- 
cates that  fatigue  involves  a  decrease  in  this  rate  rather  than  a  reduction  in  the 
total  number  of  muscle  cells  activated. 

55.  Munk,  H. :  Ueber  die  Folgen  des  Sensibilitatsverlustes  der  Extremist 
fur  deren  Motilitat,  Sitzungsber.  d.  Berlin  Akad.  Wiss.,  pp.  1038-1077,  1903. 

56.  Sherrington,  C.  S. :  The  Integrative  Action  of  the  Nervous  System. 
London,  1911. 

57.  Monakow,  C.  von:  Aufbau  und  Lokalisati.m  der  Bewegungen  brim 
Menschen,  Ber.  uber  d.  iv.  Kongress  f.  exp.  Psychol,  in   Innsbruck.  1910. 

58.  Sherrington.  C.  S. :  Postural  Activity  of  Muscle  and  Nerve,  Brain  38: 
191-234,  1915. 

59.  Tournay,  A.:  Conception  actuelle  des  grande  fonctions  motrice,  J.  de 
Psychol.  17:904-930,  1920. 

60.  Hunt.  R. :  The  Static  and  Kinetic  Systems  of  Motility.  Arch.  Neurol. 
&  Psychiat.  4:353,  1920. 


274  ARCHIVES    OF    NEUROLOGY    AXD    PSYCHIATRY 

1.  Excitation  of  the  motor  cells  supplying  a  muscle  by  impulses 
derived  from  the  receptors  in  the  muscle  itself  (Sherrington56). 

2.  Long  spinal  reflexes  from  synergic  muscles  (Sherrington,56 
Magnus  61). 

3.  Other  proprioceptive  and  general  exteroceptive  facilitation  whose 
central  mechanism  is  as  yet  rather  obscure   (Yerkes,62  Richter63). 

4.  Vestibular  and  proprioceptive  influences  exerted  through  the 
mechanisms  of  the  cerebellum. 

5.  Probably  facilitation  derived  from  thalamic  mechanisms  in  emo- 
tional excitement  (Head'14). 

6.  Kinetic  influences  of  obscure  origin  integrated  in  the  corpus 
striatum   (Wilson,42  Hunt60). 

Interference  with  any  of  these  mechanisms  is  able  to  produce  a 
change  in  the  excitability  of  the  final  common  path,  and  in  the  intact 
organism  it  seems  certain  that  every  act  involves  the  participation  of 
all  of  them,  both  by  excitation  and  inhibition. 

These  considerations  make  it  possible  to  form  a  tentative  hypothesis 
concerning  the  function  of  the  precentral  gyrus.  Its  demonstrated 
facilitating  effects,  and  its  lack  of  direct  participation  in  the  conditioned 
reflex  arc  seem  to  throw  it  into  a  class  with  these  other  postural  and 
tonic  systems.  Cerebral  paralysis,  is,  I  believe,  to  be  interpreted  as 
showing  that  a  normal  function  of  the  stimulable  cortex  is  to  supply  a 
substratum  of  facilitating  impulses  which  act  in  some  way  to  render 
the  final  common  paths  excitable  by  the  more  finely  graduated  impulses, 
descending  from  the  cortex  by  extrapyramidal  paths  and  producing  the 
finer  shades  of  adaptive  movement.  In  other  words,  impulses  descend- 
ing from  the  precentral  gyrus  do  not  initiate  the  finer  adaptive  move- 
ments through  the  lower  motor  neurons,  but  only  "prime"  these  cells 
so  that  they  may  be  excited  bv  impulses  from  other  sources.  The 
source  of  this  activity  and  the  probable  interrelations  of  the  stimulable 
areas  with  other  parts  of  the  motor  system  and  with  sensory  projection 
areas  present  problems  too  complex  for  discussion  here.     Unquestion- 


61.  Magnus.  R.,  and  de  Kleijn,  A.:  Die  Abhangigkeit  des  Tonus  der  Extre- 
mitatenmuskeln  von  der  Kopfstellung,  Arch.  f.  d.  ges.  Physiol.  145:455-548,  1912. 
Magnus,  R. :  Welche  Teile  des  Centralnervensystems  mussen  f  iir  das  Zustande- 
kommen  der  tonischen  Hals-  und  Labyrinthreflexe  auf  die  Korpermusku- 
latur  vorhanden  sein  ?    Arch.  f.  d.  ges.   Physiol.   153:224-250,   1914. 

62.  Yerkes,  R.  M. :  Inhibition  and  Reinforcement  of  Reactions  in  the  Frog, 
J.  Comp.  Xeurol.  &  Psychol.  14:124,  1904. 

63.  Richter,  C.  P.:  A  Behavioristic  Study  of  the  Activity  of  the  Rat,  Comp. 
Psychol.  Monogr.  1:1-55.  1922. 

64.  Head,  H.:  Studies  in  Neurology,  London,  1920;  Release  of  Functions  in 
the  Nervous  System,  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  92  B:  184-209,  1921. 


LASHLEY— CEREBRAL    FUNCTION  275 

ably,  the  areas  receive  excitations  from  other  parts  of  the  cerebrum  65 
and  it  is  probable  that  all  parts  of  the  kinetic  system  ,irt  are  capable  of 
mutual  influence.  Postural  facilitation  and  inhibition  may  themselves 
be  habitual  responses,  but  the  present  experiments  indicate  that  they 
are  rather  generalized  and  not  independently  organized  for  each  specific 
manipulative  habit. 

Recent  work  in  general  tends  to  emphasize  the  complexity  of  neural 
functions.  We  must  hesitate  to  ascribe  an  exclusive  or  precise  function 
to  any  neural  structure,  for  the  evidence  points  rather  to  the  view  that 
observable  behavior  is  always  the  product  of  the  interaction  of  many 
neural  systems  and  that  the  function  of  any  system  is  dependent  on  its 
temporary  physiologic  relation  to  other  systems.  This  is  particularly 
true  of  the  finer  adaptive  responses  of  the  intact  animal  which  are 
subject  to  inhibition  and  facilitation  by  innumerable  factors.  Their 
execution  depends  on  preparatory  postural  adjustments,  emotional  and 
other  dynamic  facilitation,  as  well  as  integration  of  impulses  from  many 
exteroceptors.67  The  total  mass  of  excitation  is  effective  both  through 
the  specific  efferent  patterns  activated  ar.d  also  through  the  general 
dynamic  effects  which  alone  a?e  incapable  of  producing  the  overt  motor 
reactions  elicited.  The  experiments  reported  here  indicate  that  the 
dc'vtrostimulable  areas  are  rather  more  concerned  with  the  maintenance 
of  excitability  and  the  regulation  of  postural  reflexes  than  with  the 
excitation  and  control  of  finely  integrated  adaptive  movements. 


65.  I  have  made  several  attempts  to  isolate  the  area  from  other  parts  of  the 
cortex  by  circumsection  but  have  not  yet  been  successful.  The  literature  on  this 
point  is  conflicting.  Marique  (Brain  8:536-538,  1885)  reported  the  same  results 
from  circumsection  as  from  excision  of  the  area.  Exner  and  Panetli  (Arch.  f. 
d.  ges.  Physiol.  44:544-555.  1889)  found  similar  results  but  were  inclined  to 
ascrihe  them  to  interference  with  the  blood  supply  of  the  area.  Schafer  (Jour. 
Physiol.  26:23-25.  1901)  reported  one  case  of  complete  circumsection  without 
paralysis.  He  does  not  report  histologic  examination  of  the  lesion,  however, 
and  in  view  of  the  difficulty  of  the  operation  there  is  not  sufficient  evidence 
that  the  isolation  was  complete. 

66.  In  this  discussion,  I  have  disregarded  the  important  conception  of  static 
and  kinetic  functions  advanced  by  Hunt  (Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  4:353.  1920) 
because  the  evidence  does  not  show  clearly  to  which  of  his  systems  the  elect  n>- 
stimulable  cortex  is  to  be  referred.  The  postural  influences  of  the  area  would 
indicate  a  static  function.  What  I  have  called  the  dynamic  or  "priming"  func- 
tion is  rather  a  kinetic  function,  but  is  more  primitive  than  the  activities  implied 
in  Hunt's  conception  of  the  neokinetic  system. 

67.  The  statement  that  every  act  of  the  intact  organism  involves  the  par- 
ticipation of  every  neuron  within  the  central  nervous  system  is  probably  no 
more  of  an  exaggeration  than  are  the  extreme  theories  of  precise  localization  of 
function  or  of  isolated  conditioned  reflex  paths. 


276  ARCHIVES    OF    XEUROLOGY    AND    PSYCHIATRY 

SUMMARY 

The  greater  part  of  the  precentral  gyrus  of  both  hemispheres  was 
destroyed  in  monkeys  which  had  been  trained  previously  in  habits  of 
manipulation  and  visual  discrimination.  When  the  animals  recovered 
from  paralysis,  it  was  found  that  they  showed  perfect  retention  of  these 
habits.  From  this  it  is  concluded  that  the  so-called  motor  areas  are  not 
directly  concerned  with  the  performance  of  complex  learned  activities. 
The  motor  impulses  of  conditioned  reflexes  must  descend  from  other 
areas  of  the  cerebral  cortex  than  the  precentral  gyri,  and  the  latter 
cannot  be  regarded  as  the  source  of  impulses  to  "voluntary  movements.'* 

Destruction  of  the  corpus  striatum  subsequent  to  recovery  from 
diplegia  produced  only  the  usual  symptoms  of  striate  lesion  without 
recurrence  of  the  symptoms  of  cerebral  paralysis.  Recovery  from 
paralysis  was  therefore  not  due  to  vicarious  function  of  this  nucleus. 

The  evidence  for  considering  the  precentral  gyrus  as  a  part  of  the 
kinetic  mechanism  for  reflex  control  of  spinal  posture  and  for  mainte- 
nance of  excitability  of  lower  motor  centers  is  summarized. 


I 


QL 
785 
L37 
v.5 


Lash ley,  Karl  Spencer 
Studies  of  cerebral 
function  in  learning 


Biological 


PLEASE  DO  NOT  REMOVE 
CARDS  OR  SLIPS  FROM  THIS  POCKET 


UNIVERSITY  OF  TORONTO  LIBRARY