Skip to main content

Full text of "The Wilson bulletin"

See other formats


HARVARD  UNIVERSITY 


Library  of  the 
Museum  of 
Comparative  Zoology 


IhcWilsonBulkUn 

PUBLISHED  BY  THE  WILSON  ORNITHOLOGICAL  SOCIETY 


VOLUME  93 


1981 


QUARTERLY 


EDITOR:  JON  C.  BARLOW 
REVIEW  EDITOR:  ROBERT  RAIKOW 
COLOR  PLATE  EDITOR:  WILLIAM  A.  LUNK 
ASSISTANT  EDITOR:  MARGARET  L.  MAY 
SENIOR  EDITORIAL  ASSISTANTS:  GARY  BORTOLOTTI 

NANCY  FLOOD 

EDITORIAL  ASSISTANTS:  C.  DAVISON  ANKNEY 
KEITH  L.  BILDSTEIN 
JAMES  D.  RISING 
RICHARD  R.  SNELL 


President — Abbot  S.  Gaunt,  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  Columbus, 
Ohio  43210. 

First  Vice-President — Jerome  A.  Jackson,  Department  of  Biological  Sciences,  P.O.  Drawer 
Z,  Mississippi  State  University,  Mississippi  State,  Mississippi  39762. 

Second  Vice-President — Clait  E.  Braun,  Wildlife  Research  Center,  317  West  Prospect  St., 
Fort  Collins,  Colorado  80526. 

Editor — Jon  C.  Barlow,  Department  of  Ornithology,  Royal  Ontario  Museum,  100  Queen’s 
Park,  Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6. 

Secretary — Curtis  S.  Adkisson,  Department  of  Biology,  Virginia  Polytechnic  Institute  and 
State  University,  Blacksburg,  Virginia  24061. 

Treasurer — Robert  D.  Burns,  Department  of  Biology,  Kenyon  College,  Gambler,  Ohio  43022. 

Elected  Council  Members — Richard  C.  Banks  (term  expires  1982);  Mary  H.  Clench  (term 
expires  1983);  Helen  Lapham  (term  expires  1984). 


DATES  OF  ISSUE  OF  VOLUME  93 
OF  THE  WILSON  BULLETIN 

NO.  1—14  May  1981 
NO.  2 — 20  August  1981 
NO.  3 — 29  October  1981 
NO.  4 — 22  February  1982 


CONTENTS  OF  VOLUME  93 


NUMBER  1 

OBSERVATIONS  OF  SEABIRDS  DURING  A CRUISE  FROM  ROSS  ISLAND  TO  ANVERS  ISLAND,  ANT- 
ARCTICA   Robert  M.  Zink  1 

DISPLAY  BEHAVIOR  OF  OVENBIRDS  {Seiurus  aurocapillus)  II.  SONG  VARIATION  AND  SINGING 

BEHAVIOR  M.  Ross  Lein  21 

THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD  OF  ESTIMATING  NESTING  SUCCESS:  A MODEL,  ESTIMATORS  AND  SIMU- 
LATION RESULTS  Gary  L.  Hensler  and  James  D.  Nichols  42 

NARROWLY  DISJUNCT  ALLOPATRY  BETWEEN  BLACK-CAPPED  AND  CAROLINA  CHICKADEES  IN 

NORTHERN  INDIANA  Peter  G.  Merritt  54 

TOE  FUSION  IN  OSCINES George  A.  Clark,  Jr.  67 

ENVIRONMENTAL  EFFECTS  ON  ROOSTING  BEHAVIOR  OF  CHIMNEY  SWIFTS 

Richard  M.  Zammuto  and  Edwin  C.  Franks  77 

GENERAL  NOTES 

OBSERVATION  OF  A BROOD  OF  SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS  IN  ONTARIO,  WITH  COMMENTS 
ON  THE  FUNCTIONS  OF  SEXUAL  DIMORPHISM 

Helmut  C.  Mueller,  Nancy  S.  Mueller  and  Patricia  G.  Parker  85 

FOOD  DEPRIVATION  AND  TEMPERATURE  REGULATION  IN  NESTLING  FERRUGINOUS  HAWKS 

Diana  F.  Tomback  and  Joseph  R.  Murphy  92 

AERIAL  “play”  OF  BLACK  VULTURES  Walter  A.  Thurber  97 

THE  SHOULDER-SPOT  DISPLAY  IN  RUFFED  GROUSE Allan  Garbutt  98 

THE  AGONISTIC  REPERTOIRE  OF  SANDHILL  CRANES 

Stephen  A.  Nesbitt  and  George  W.  Archibald  99 

NOTES  ON  THE  SLENDER  ANTBIRD  Edwin  O.  Willis  and  Yoshika  Oniki  103 

NOTES  ON  THE  UNIFORM  CRAKE  IN  COSTA  RICA F.  G.  Stiles  107 

TRICHOMONIASIS  IN  BALD  EAGLES  Ward  B.  Stone  and  Peter  E.  Nye  109 

Protocalliphora  INFESTATION  IN  BROAD-WINGED  HAWKS 

Scott  Crocoll  and  James  W.  Parker  110 

HERRING  GULL  ATTACKS  AND  EATS  ADULT  MALE  OLDSQUAW  Richard  R.  Snell  110 

RED-LEGGED  KITTIWAKES  FORAGE  IN  MIXED-SPECIES  FLOCKS  IN  SOUTHEASTERN  ALAS- 
KA   Douglas  Siegel-Causey  and  Thomas  E.  Meehan  111 

GROUND-FEEDING  METHODS  AND  NICHE  SEPARATION  IN  THRUSHES  Alan  Tye  112 

AMERICAN  COOT  DISTRIBUTION  AND  MIGRATION  IN  COLORADO 

Warner  P.  Gorenzel,  Ronald  A.  Ryder  and  Clait  E.  Braun  115 

REPRODUCTIVE  RATE  AND  RENESTING  OF  RED-WINGED  BLACKBIRDS  IN  MINNESOTA 

Daniel  W.  Moulton  119 

MIGRATION  SPEEDS  OF  THREE  WATERFOWL  SPECIES 

Frank  C.  Bellrose  and  Robert  C.  Crompton  121 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE  125 

ORNITHOLOGICAL  NEWS  135 


NUMBER  2 


THE  RUFOUS-FACED  CRAKE  {LATERALLUS  XENOPTERUS)  AND  ITS  PARAGUAYAN  CONGENERS 

Robert  W.  Storer  137 

RESOURCE  USE  STRATEGIES  OF  WADING  BIRDS James  A.  Kushlan  145 

AGE  RATIOS  AND  THEIR  POSSIBLE  USE  IN  DETERMINING  AUTUMN  ROUTES  OF  PASSERINE 

MIGRANTS  C.  John  Ralph  164 

WEATHER,  MIGRATION  AND  AUTUMN  BIRD  KILLS  AT  A NORTH  FLORIDA  TV  TOWER 

Robert  L.  Crawford  189 

CLIMATIC  INFLUENCES  ON  PRODUCTIVITY  IN  THE  HOUSE  SPARROW  W.  Brace  McGUUvray  196 

CANADA  GOOSE  BROOD  BEHAVIOR  AND  SURVIVAL  ESTIMATES  AT  CREX  MEADOWS,  WISCONSIN 

Michael  C.  Zicus  207 

BREEDING  BIRD  POPULATIONS  IN  THE  GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAINS,  TENNESSEE  AND  NORTH 

CAROLINA  S.  Charles  Kendeigh  and  Ben  J.  Fawver  218 

NEST-SITE  SELECTION  AMONG  AD6LIE,  CHINSTRAP  AND  GENTOO  PENGUINS  IN  MIXED  SPECIES 

ROOKERIES  Nicholas  J.  Volkman  and  Wayne  Trivelpiece  243 

COWBIRD  PARASITISM  AND  EVOLUTION  OF  ANTI-PARASITE  STRATEGIES  IN  THE  YELLOW 

WARBLER  Karen  L.  Clark  and  Raleigh  J.  Robertson  249 

INTERACTIVE  BEHAVIOR  AMONG  BALD  EAGLES  WINTERING  IN  NORTH-CENTRAL  MISSOURI 

Curtice  R.  Griffin  259 

president’s  message 264 

GENERAL  NOTES 

INTERSPECIFIC  SONG  MIMESIS  BY  A LINCOLN  SPARROW 

Luis  F.  Baptista,  Martin  L.  Morton  and  Maria  E.  Pereyra  265 


NOTES  ON  PURPLE  GALLINULES  IN  COLOMBIAN  RICEFIELDS  Wallace  D.  McKay  267 

AGONISTIC  BEHAVIOR  OF  THE  WHITE-BREASTED  NUTHATCH Lawrence  Kilham  271 


EVASIVE  BEHAVIOR  OF  AMERICAN  COOTS  TO  KLEPTOPARASITISM  BY  WATERFOWL 

Mark  R.  Ryan  274 

ADDITIONAL  EVIDENCE  OF  EGG-MOVING  BEHAVIOR  BY  FEMALE  GADWALLS 

Robert  J.  Blohm  276 

MALLARD  USING  MOVING  VEHICLES  FOR  PREDATOR  AVOIDANCE 

Bruce  C.  Thompson  and  James  E.  Tabor  277 

OCHRACEOUS  WREN  FAILS  TO  RESPOND  TO  MOBBING  CALLS  IN  A HETEROSPECIFIC  FLOCK 

William  H.  Buskirk  278 

FISH  ATTACK  ON  BLACK  GUILLEMOT  AND  COMMON  EIDER  IN  MAINE  Thomas  W.  French  279 

CROWS  STEAL  GOLF  BALLS  IN  BANGLADESH  Richard  M.  Poche  280 

NOTES  ON  THE  STATUS  OF  THE  COMMON  AFRICAN  WAXBILL  IN  AMAZONIA 

David  C.  Oren  and  Nigel  J.  H.  Smith  281 

DISTRIBUTION  AND  REPRODUCTIVE  SUCCESS  OF  ZONE-TAILED  HAWKS  IN  WEST  TEXAS 

Sumner  W.  Matteson  and  John  O.  Riley  282 

THREE  CRESTED  EAGLE  RECORDS  FOR  GUATEMALA 

David  H.  Ellis  and  Wayne  H.  Whaley  284 

286 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


NUMBER  3 


SUBSPECIES  VS  FORGOTTEN  SPECIES:  THE  CASE  OF  GRAYSON’S  ROBIN  {Turdus  graysoni) 

Allan  R.  Phillips  301 

HYPERPHAGIA  AND  SOCIAL  BEHAVIOR  OF  CANADA  GEESE  PRIOR  TO  SPRING  MIGRATION 

M.  Robert  McLandress  and  Dennis  G.  Raveling  310 

A MULTIPLE  SENSOR  SYSTEM  FOR  MONITORING  AVIAN  NESTING  BEHAVIOR 

James  A.  Cooper  and  Alan  D.  Afton  325 

FORAGING  SPEEDS  OF  WARBLERS  IN  LARGE  POPULATIONS  AND  IN  ISOLATION 

Douglass  H.  Morse  334 

DIFFERENTIAL  PASSERINE  DENSITY  AND  DIVERSITY  BETWEEN  NEWFOUNDLAND  AND  OFF- 
SHORE GULL  ISLAND Monique  /.  Vassallo  and  Jake  C.  Rice  340 

ENVIRONMENTAL  INFLUENCE  ON  SOARING  IN  WINTERING  RED-TAILED  HAWKS 

Charles  R.  Preston  350 

BREEDING  SUCCESS  IN  AN  ISOLATED  POPULATION  OF  ROCK  DOVES 

David  E.  Preble  and  Frank  H.  Heppner  357 

RELATIVE  ABUNDANCE  OF  GEORGIA  CAPRIMULGIDS  BASED  ON  CALL-COUNTS 

Robert  J.  Cooper  363 

NON-DRUMMING  MALES  IN  A RUFFED  GROUSE  POPULATION  Gordon  W.  GulUon  372 

GENERAL  NOTES 

BEHAVIORAL  IMPLICATIONS  OF  ABERRANT  SONG  OF  A RED-EYED  VIREO 


Jake  C.  Rice  383 

COURTSHIP  FEEDING  AND  COPULATION  OF  ROYAL  TERNS  Lawrence  Kilham  390 

TWO  CASES  OF  COMMENSAL  FEEDING  BETWEEN  PASSERINES Mark  B.  Robbins  391 


FOOD  FINDING  IN  BLACK-CAPPED  CHICKADEES:  ALTRUISTIC  COMMUNICATION? 

Millicent  S.  Ficken  393 

THE  SENTINEL  CROW  AS  AN  EXTENSION  OF  PARENTAL  CARE 

Gloria  M.  D’Agostino,  Lorraine  E.  Giovinazzo  and  Stephen  W.  Eaton  394 

BEHAVIOR  OF  A MALE  LEAST  BITTERN  INCUBATING  AFTER  LOSS  OF  MATE 

B.  T.  Aniskowicz  395 

NOTES  ON  BROWN  PELICANS  IN  PUERTO  RICO 

Ralph  W.  Schreiber,  David  W.  Belitsky  and  Bruce  A.  Sorrie  397 

EGGS  OF  THE  MARBLED  MURRELET  Lloyd  F.  Kiff  400 

FIRST  DOCUMENTED  CINNAMON  TEAL  NESTING  IN  NORTH  DAKOTA  PRODUCED  HYBRIDS 

John  T.  Lokemoen  and  David  E.  Sharp  403 

FIRST  RECORD  OF  THE  BLACK-CHINNED  HUMMINGBIRD  IN  ALBERTA 

Philip  H.  R.  Stepney  and  Peter  C.  Boxall  405 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE  407 

REPORT  OF  THE  CONSERVATION  COMMITTEE — 1980  438 


NUMBER  4 


ON  AERIAL  AND  GROUND  DISPLAYS  OF  THE  WORLD’S  SNIPES George  Miksch  Sutton 

FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES  IN  TWO  FORESTS  WITH  DIFFERENT  VEGETATION  STRUCTURE 

Brian  A.  Maurer  and  Robert  C.  Whitmore 

AGE  AND  SEX  DIFFERENCES  IN  WING  LOADING  AND  OTHER  AERODYNAMIC  CHARACTERISTICS 
OF  SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS Helmut  C.  Mueller,  Daniel  D.  Berger  and  George  Allez 

EVIDENCE  FOR  AERODYNAMIC  ADVANTAGES  OF  TAIL  KEELING  IN  THE  COMMON  CRACKLE 

Scott  Hickman 

REPRODUCTIVE  CORRELATES  OF  ENVIRONMENTAL  VARIATION  AND  NICHE  EXPANSION  IN  THE 
CAVE  SWALLOW  IN  TEXAS Robert  F.  Martin 

HORNED  LARK  BREEDING  BIOLOGY  AT  CAPE  ST.  MARY’S,  NEWFOUNDLAND 

Richard  J.  Cannings  and  William  Threlfall 

ASPECTS  OF  THE  BREEDING  BIOLOGY  OF  A SUBTROPICAL  ORIOLE,  Icterus  gularis 

Barbara  Yohai  Pleasants 


GENERAL  NOTES 

AN  EXAMPLE  OF  A HYBRID  GREEN  JAY  X BLUE  JAY 

Warren  M.  Pulich  and  Rebecca  M.  Dellinger 
DUSKY  SEASIDE  SPARROW  FEEDS  RED-WINGED  BLACKBIRD  FLEDGLINGS 

James  L.  Rakestraw  and  James  L.  Baker 
STATISTICAL  SIGNIFICANCE  AND  DENSITY-DEPENDENT  NEST  PREDATION 

Stephen  D.  Fretwell  and  Frank  S.  Shipley 
A COMPARISON  OF  NEST-SITE  AND  PERCH-SITE  VEGETATION  STRUCTURE  FOR  SEVEN 

SPECIES  OF  WARBLERS Scott  L.  ColUns 

USE  OF  ARTIFICIAL  PERCHES  ON  BURNED  AND  UNBURNED  TALLGRASS  PRAIRIE 

Janet  Jean  Knodel-Montz 

JUVENILE  PEREGRINE  FALCON  SWOOPS  ON  ROSEATE  SPOONBILLS E.  Scott  Clark 

SYMBIOTIC  INTERACTION  BETWEEN  STARLINGS  AND  DEER  Robert  K.  Murphy 

CATTLE  EGRETS  FEEDING  IN  ASSOCIATION  WITH  HUMAN  WORKERS  G.  K.  Menon 

SCRUB  JAY  CAPTURES  HERMIT  THRUSH  IN  FLIGHT 

M.  Robert  McLandress  and  Use  McLandress 
FOOD  HABITS  OF  BLACK-BELLIED  WHISTLING  DUCKS  OCCUPYING  RICE  CULTURE  HABI- 
TATS   Godfrey  R.  Bourne 

HOUSE  SPARROWS  FLUSHING  PREY  FROM  TREES  AND  SHRUBS 

Harland  D.  Guillory  and  Jack  H.  Deshotels 
DIFFERENTIAL  PREDATION  BY  TWO  SPECIES  OF  PISCIVOROUS  BIRDS 

Fritz  L.  Knopf  and  Joseph  L.  Kennedy 

RED  PHALAROPE  EATING  CARRION  Wade  Wander 

RE-MATING  OF  A LESSER  SNOW  GOOSE 

Kenneth  F.  Abraham,  Pierre  Mineau  and  Fred  Cooke 

COMMON  EIDER  PLAYS  “POSSUM” Douglas  B.  McNair 

TERRITORIAL  ATTACHMENT  AND  MATE  FIDELITY  BY  HORNED  GREBES 

Robert  S.  Ferguson 

EFFECTS  OF  REDHEAD  NEST  PARASITISM  ON  MALLARDS 

Larry  G.  Talent,  Gary  L.  Krapu  and  Robert  L.  Jarvis 

SURVIVAL  OF  A DEMAXILLATE  RED- WINGED  BLACKBIRD Kent  L.  Fiala 

MINIMIZING  INVESTIGATOR  DISTURBANCE  IN  OBSERVATIONAL  STUDIES  OF  COLONIAL 
BIRDS:  ACCESS  TO  BLINDS  THROUGH  TUNNELS 

Gary  W.  Shugart,  Mary  A.  Fitch  and  Vern  M.  Shugart 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE  

PROCEEDINGS  OF  THE  SIXTY-SECOND  ANNUAL  MEETING 
INDEX  


457 

478 

491 

500 

506 

519 

531 


538 

540 

541 

542 

547 

548 

549 

549 

550 

551 

554 

554 

557 

557 

559 

560 

562 

563 

565 

570 

575 

589 


w 1C 
8V2,f 


PUBLISHED  BY  THE  WILSON  ORNITHOLOGICAL  SOCIETY 


VOL.  93,  NO.  1 MARCH  1981  PAGES  1-136 


The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
Founded  December  3,  1888 

Named  after  ALEXANDER  WILSON,  the  first  American  Ornithologist. 

President — George  A.  Hall,  Department  of  Chemistry,  West  Virginia  University,  Morgan- 
town, West  Virginia  26506. 

First  Vice-President — Abbot  S.  Gaunt,  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  Co- 
lumbus, Ohio  43210. 

Second  Vice-President — Jerome  A.  Jackson,  Department  of  Biological  Sciences,  P.O.  Draw- 
er Z,  Mississippi  State  University,  Mississippi  State,  Mississippi  39762. 

Editor — Jon  C.  Barlow,  Department  of  Ornithology,  Royal  Ontario  Museum,  100  Queen’s 
Park,  Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6. 

Secretary — Curtis  S.  Adkisson,  Department  of  Biology,  Virginia  Polytechnic  Institute  and 
State  University,  Blacksburg,  Virginia  24061. 

Treasurer — Robert  D.  Burns,  Department  of  Biology,  Kenyon  College,  Gambler,  Ohio  43022. 

Elected  Council  Members — Sidney  A.  Gauthreaux,  Jr.  (term  expires  1981);  Richard  C.  Banks 
(term  expires  1982);  Mary  H.  Clench  (term  expires  1983). 

Membership  dues  per  calendar  year  are:  Active,  $16.00;  Student,  $14.00;  Sustaining,  $25.00; 
Life  memberships  $250  (payable  in  four  installments). 

The  Wilson  Bulletin  is  sent  to  aU  members  not  in  arrears  for  dues. 

The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library 
The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  housed  in 
the  University  of  Michigan  Museum  of  Zoology,  was  established  in  concurrence  with  the 
University  of  Michigan  in  1930.  Until  1947  the  Library  was  maintained  entirely  by  gifts  and 
bequests  of  books,  reprints,  and  ornithological  magazines  from  members  and  friends  of  the 
Society.  Now  two  members  have  generously  established  a fund  for  the  purchase  of  new 
books;  members  and  friends  are  invited  to  maintain  the  fund  by  regular  contribution,  thus 
making  available  to  all  Society  members  the  more  important  new  books  on  ornithology  and 
related  subjects.  The  fund  will  be  administered  by  the  Library  Committee,  which  will  be 
happy  to  receive  suggestions  on  the  choice  of  new  books  to  be  added  to  the  Library.  William 
A.  Lunk,  University  Museums,  University  of  Michigan,  is  Chairman  of  the  Committee.  The 
Library  currently  receives  195  periodicals  as  gifts  and  in  exchange  for  The  Wilson  Bulletin. 
With  the  usual  exception  of  rare  books,  any  item  in  the  Library  may  be  borrowed  by  members 
of  the  Society  and  will  be  sent  prepaid  (by  the  University  of  Michigan)  to  any  address  in  the 
United  States,  its  possessions,  or  Canada.  Return  postage  is  paid  by  the  borrower.  Inquiries 
and  requests  by  borrowers,  as  well  as  gifts  of  books,  pamphlets,  reprints,  and  magazines, 
should  be  addressed  to:  The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library,  University  of  Michigan 
Museum  of  Zoology,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  Contributions  to  the  New  Book  Fund 
should  be  sent  to  the  Treasurer  (small  sums  in  stamps  are  acceptable).  A complete  index  of 
the  Library’s  holdings  was  printed  in  the  September  1952  issue  of  The  Wilson  Bulletin  and 
newly  acquired  books  are  listed  periodically.  A list  of  currently  received  periodicals  was 
published  in  the  December  1978  issue. 


The  Wilson  Bulletin 
(ISSN  0043-5643) 

The  ofhcid  organ  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  published  quarterly,  in  March,  June,  September,  and  December. 
The  subscription  price,  both  in  the  United  States  and  esewhere,  is  $20.00  per  year.  Single  copies,  $4.00.  Subscriptions, 
changes  of  address  and  claims  for  undelivered  copies  should  be  sent  to  the  Treasurer.  Most  back  issues  of  the  Bulletin  are 
available  and  may  be  ordered  from  the  Treasurer.  Special  prices  wiU  be  quoted  for  quantity  orders. 

All  articles  and  communications  for  publications,  books  and  pubbcations  for  reviews  should  be  addressed  to  the  Editor. 
Exchanges  should  be  addressed  to  The  Joselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library,  Museum  of  Zoology,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan 
48109.  Known  office  of  publication:  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  1827  Neil  Avenue,  Columbus,  Ohio 
43210. 

Second  class  postage  paid  at  Columbus,  Ohio  and  at  additional  mailing  office. 

(c)  Copyright  1981  by  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
Printed  by  Allen  Press,  Inc.,  Lawrence,  Kansas  66044,  U.S.A. 


Antarctic  Petrel  (Thalassoica  antarctica).  Amundsen  Sea,  Antarctica, 
January  1980.  Photograph  by  J.R.  Jehl,  Jr. 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN 

A QUARTERLY  MAGAZINE  OF  ORNITHOLOGY 

Published  by  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 


VoL.  93,  No.  1 March  1981  Pages  1-136 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  1-20 


OBSERVATIONS  OF  SEABIRDS  DURING  A CRUISE 
FROM  ROSS  ISLAND  TO  ANVERS 
ISLAND,  ANTARCTICA 

Robert  M.  Zink 

The  breeding  avifauna  of  the  Sub-Antarctic  and  Antarctic  consists  of 
about  65  species,  mostly  seabirds.  In  the  antarctic  region,  which  includes 
the  Antarctic  continent.  Peninsula  and  adjacent  islands,  there  are  18 
breeding  species;  8 are  restricted  as  breeders  to  the  Antarctic  Peninsula 
(Watson  1975).  These  18  species  depend  largely  on  the  sea  for  their  ex- 
istence and  most  are  highly  pelagic,  only  coming  to  land  for  breeding 
during  the  short  austral  summer.  Sub-adult  and  foraging  adult  seabirds 
from  subantarctic  islands  and  the  wintering  Arctic  Tern  {Sterna  paradi- 
saea)  are  also  found  in  antarctic  waters  during  the  austral  summer.  During 
the  austral  spring,  under  the  influence  of  warming  temperatures,  wind  and 
water  currents,  the  continuous  band  of  ice  which  surrounds  the  Antarctic 
continent  and  much  of  the  Peninsula  in  winter,  breaks  up  into  pack  ice 
and  open  water.  Seabirds  forage  in  continuous  daylight  of  the  austral  sum- 
mer and  exploit  plankton-abundant  regions  of  the  antarctic  oceans. 

Most  information  available  on  antarctic  and  subantarctic  seabirds  has 
resulted  from  terrestrial  studies  of  their  breeding  biology.  Although  little 
is  known  about  their  pelagic  distributions,  behaviors  and  ecologies  (Wat- 
son 1975,  Ainley  et  al.  1978),  these  are  influenced  by  prolonged  sexual 
immaturity,  nonbreeding  periods  when  sexually  mature,  absence  between 
bouts  of  incubation  and  foraging  for  food  for  young.  In  addition,  there  is 
little  known  about  patterns  in  pelagic  seabird  community  composition  and 
species  interactions.  Biotic  and  abiotic  determinants  of  species  occur- 
rences are  also  poorly  understood  (Watson  1975)  although  data  exist  on 
effects  of  sea  water  temperature  (Szijj  1967),  ice  concentration  (Cline  et 


1 


2 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


al.  1969,  Zink  1978,  Ainley  et  al.  1978)  and  local  plankton  blooms  (Cline 
et  al.  1969,  Ainley  et  al.  1978)  on  bird  distributions. 

Early  studies  (Gain  1914;  Siple  and  Lindsey  1937;  Holgerson  1945,  1957; 
Bierman  and  Voous  1950)  established  the  broad  patterns  of  seabird  dis- 
tributions and  general  pelagic  ecology  in  Antarctica;  recent  summaries  of 
many  aspects  of  antarctic  avian  biology  can  be  found  in  Watson  et  al. 
(1971)  and  Watson  (1975).  In  addition,  Watson  (1975)  provides  a useful 
discussion  of  the  maritime  and  terrestrial  physical  environments. 

This  study  contributes  information  on  the  pelagic  distribution,  abun- 
dance and  habits  of  16  species  of  seabirds  obtained  during  an  austral 
summer  cruise  aboard  the  USCGC  Glacier,  while  it  traveled  through  the 
Ross,  Amundsen  and  Bellingshausen  seas,  from  McMurdo  Station  (Ross 
Island)  to  Palmer  Station  (Anvers  Island).  Recent  seabird  observations 
from  the  areas  covered  herein  include  those  of  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  and 
Darby  (1970),  and  are  compared  with  recent  pelagic  seabird  surveys  from 
the  Weddell  Sea  (Cline  et  al.  1969,  Parmelee  1977,  Zink  1978). 

ITINERARY  AND  METHODS 

The  route  and  itinerary  of  the  Glacier  are  shown  in  Fig.  1.  This  study  was  largely  oppor- 
tunistic as  the  purpose  of  this  portion  of  the  1976  International  Weddell  Sea  Oceanographic 
Expedition  (IWSOE)  was  merely  to  reach  Palmer  Station,  consequently,  the  route 
depended  mostly  on  pack  ice  densities.  Of  the  136.1  h of  observation,  99.1  h were 
taken  in  the  Ross  Sea  (170°E-135°W),  23.5  h in  the  Amundsen  Sea  (135°W-90°W)  and  13.5 
h were  mostly  in  ice-free  portions  of  the  Bellingshausen  Sea  (90°W— 66°W). 

Observations  were  made  from  the  “flying  bridge”  of  the  Glacier,  16  m above  the  waterline, 
during  periods  of  various  lengths  (0.5— 4.5  h)  throughout  the  day.  A minute  by  minute  account 
(in  GMT)  was  kept  of  numbers  and  behavior  of  birds  and  seals  sighted  within  approximately 
0.4  km  of  each  side  of  the  ship,  providing  a census  strip  width  of  0.8  km.  The  data  on  seals 
are  discussed  elsewhere  (in  prep.).  To  determine  the  transect  boundaries  a 12.7  x 38.1  cm 
rectangular  hoard  with  a line,  describing  an  angle  of  2.3°,  drawn  diagonally  from  a top  corner 
to  a point  on  the  opposite  12.7  cm  side  was  used.  While  the  top  (i.e.,  38.1  cm)  edge  of  the 
hoard  was  sighted  to  the  horizon  (and  perpendicular  to  the  ship’s  course),  a simultaneous 
sighting  was  taken  along  the  line,  which  intersected  the  water  0.4  km  from  the  observer  (for 
details  see  Cline  et  al.  [1969]).  I tried  to  monitor  specific  birds  following  the  ship  to  prevent 
multiple  entries  of  given  individuals.  When  several  individuals  of  the  same  species  were 
following  the  ship,  all  individuals  seen  at  5-10  min  intervals  were  counted  and  the  approx- 
imate turn-over  rate  was  estimated.  Maximum  number  in  sight  during  any  count  and  turn- 
over rate  were  used  to  estimate  the  number  of  a given  species  seen  during  the  transect 
period.  Estimated  numbers  of  birds  in  large  flocks  were  probably  conservative;  direct  counts 
of  flocks  while  the  Glacier  was  stuck  in  pack  ice  showed  that  initial  estimates  were  low  by 
as  much  as  30-40%. 

Pack  ice  concentration  during  each  observation  period  was  recorded  in  oktas  (0  indicating 
open  seas,  8 representing  solid  pack).  Ice  concentrations  changed  rapidly  during  many  cen- 
suses and  prevented  a precise  correlation  of  each  sighting  with  a specific  okta  value  (see 
discussion  of  Adelie  Penguin  [Pygoscelis  adelioe]  for  an  example).  Also,  the  okta  value 
assigned  to  a flying  bird  or  one  foraging  in  a relatively  narrow  zone  of  several  ice  concentra- 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


3 


Fig.  1.  Approximate  route  of  the  Glacier  from  McMurdo  Station  to  Palmer  Station.  Num- 
bers refer  to  dates  between  16  January  and  7 February  1976.  Crosses  (-I-)  indicate  noon 
(GMT)  positions. 


tions  was  somewhat  arbitrary.  Because  of  these  factors,  and  insufficient  data  for  each  okta, 
data  were  grouped  into  1—4,  5-8  oktas  and  open  seas.  Large  leads  (channels  of  water  through 
ice  floes)  or  polynyas  (areas  of  open  water  in  sea  ice,  distinguished  from  leads)  were  scored 
as  open  seas  even  though  they  existed  within  the  pack  ice,  accounting  for  some  of  the 
apparent  “open  seas”  occurrences  of  pack  ice  species,  as  it  did  in  the  study  by  Cline  et  al. 


4 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  1 

Summary  of  Shipboard  Observation  Periods  from  McMurdo  Station  to  Palmer 
Station  (16  Jan. -6  Feb.,  1976) 


Census  h 

Percent 

Km^  of  census 

Percent 

Open  water 

14.7 

10.8 

243.5 

12.1 

Pack  ice  (1-4  oktas) 

100.2 

73.6 

1438.5 

71.2 

Pack  ice  (5-8  oktas) 

21.2 

15.6 

337.2 

16.7 

Total 

136.1 

100.0 

2019.2 

100.0 

(1969).  The  Glacier  followed  routes  of  easiest  passage  through  pack  ice  and  habitats  sampled 
were  consequently  biased  towards  open  pack,  leads  and  polynyas  (Table  1). 

Sightings  of  each  species  in  the  3 habitats  were  totaled  to  ascertain  general  relationships 
(Table  2).  Information  on  pelagic  zonal  distribution  of  each  species  in  antarctic  (continental 
and  maritime)  and  suhantarctic  (transitional,  cold  and  temperate)  waters  is  given  (after  Wat- 
son 1975).  General  locations  of  known  breeding  sites  along  the  census  route  are  given  in 
Table  2.  Density  estimates  (given  as  mean  ± SD)  were  computed  for  the  3 most  common 
bird  species  by  dividing  the  number  of  individuals  seen  during  a census  by  the  area  censused 
(Table  3);  for  some  other  species  only  maximum  densities  are  given,  and  for  the  remaining 
species  densities  were  not  computed  because  of  low  numbers  or  uncorrected  biases  in  the 
probability  of  detection.  For  instance,  although  albatrosses  were  identifiable  at  0.4  km,  smaller 
birds,  such  as  prions  and  storm-petrels,  were  not.  The  areas  covered  during  the  69  censuses 
ranged  from  8.6-94.5  km^  (x  = 29.3  ± 15.3  knP).  Observation  periods  were  not  uniformly 
spaced,  either  within  or  among  days,  hence  there  was  insufficient  basis  for  a daily  comparison 
of  species  or  abundances.  To  show  general  patterns  in  species  pelagic  ranges,  the  days  for 
which  each  species  was  observed  are  given  in  Table  2.  The  Glacier  often  traveled  300+  km 
per  day,  thus  only  approximate  range  estimates  are  possible  from  this  presentation;  specific 
locations  of  important  sightings  are  given  in  the  text. 

Data  from  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  were  used  to  calculate  density  estimates.  They  censused 
birds  from  23  January-15  February  1972  in  areas  of  pack  ice,  from  its  eastern  edge  in  the 
Bellingshausen  Sea  at  about  85°W  to  its  western  edge  in  the  Amundsen  Sea  at  135°30'W. 
'I'heir  transects  ranged  from  68°S-72°S,  which  represented  distances  into  the  pack  of  from 
39-330  km,  respectively.  Their  23  censuses  sampled  an  area  of  1255  km^  over  88  h;  the 
average  census  covered  54.6  ± 38.5  krn^  and  lasted  3.8  ± 1.9  h.  They  did  not  partition  their 
observations  according  to  ice  concentrations,  hence  throughout  the  present  paper,  references 
to  their  density  and  abundance  figures  are  for  pack  ice  in  general  (however,  they  stated  that 
ice  concentrations  governed  their  penetration  into  the  pack,  hence  their  censuses  were 
probably  in  pack  ice  of  less  than  609b  concentration). 

The  densities  given  by  Gline  et  al.  (1969)  in  their  study  of  birds  of  the  summer  pack  ice 
in  the  Weddell  Sea  were  converted  from  birds/rnile^  to  hirds/km‘^.  They  related  the  occur- 
rence of  birds  to  pack  ice  concentrations  as  follows:  light  (10-309b),  medium  (40—60%)  and 
heavy  (70-100%).  Parmelee  (1977)  provided  observations  of  seabirds  obtained  during  a study 
in  the  Weddell  Sea,  primarily  in  open  water,  between  23  January  and  26  February  1973; 
there  were  no  density  estimates  given  or  derivable  from  this  study.  Zink  (1978)  censused 
birds  in  the  northwestern  Weddell  Sea  during  the  austral  summer  of  1976  and  presented 
data  similar  to  those  in  the  present  report,  however,  pack  ice  densities  represented  averages 


Table  2 

Number,  Dates  Each  Species  was  Sighted  and  Observed  Habitat  Preferences 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


c/2  - 

< 


O S -g 


5 ?o  > E j 2 

h4  ^ 

^ . LO  . . ^ 

^-Ph^^CL  PhQhPh“  OhDh 

<z<<<  z<<zz<z 


4^  <u  a; 

H H H 

U CJ  CJ  CJ 
U(  j-T  j-T  s-T  tT 

Z Z H H H H H 

S S S S S § § 


4J  OJ  OJ 

H H H 


CJ  CJ  CJ  CJ  CJ 

s-T  ^ s-T  s-T  J J I 

H Z H H H H I 


CO 

B 

c 

H S 

OJ 

■B  -S 

y 

S-H 


'y  c 


I ^ 


B 

£ CO 
2 ^ 
o JS 

c 

O -jS 
^ 3 

o 

CO 


rt  o 

£ 2 

•S  t =2 

3 *-  < 
*=^  C _ 
£ .2  ^ 
^ o i 


y 3 
O s-i 
y y 
y _c 

3 

O 


OJ 


y y 

H H 


CJ  CJ  CJ 

H H H 


Cj" 

cj" 

CJ*" 

u 

U 

CJ 

CJ 

cj' 

CJ 

cj'' 

S CJ 

1 

1 

CJ 

§ 

Cj" 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

1 

1 

CM 

CO 

NO 

LO 

NO 

(Jl 

CM 

NO 

CO 

1 

1 

CJ 

CJ 

CJ 

,-h' 

cd 

d 

,— H 

LO 

r-C 

d 

■o’ 

Z 

ON 

d 

CM 

z 

z 

Z 

CO 

o 

CO 

r-- 

NO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

O 

CO 

CO 

CM 

Oh 

LO 

1 — 1 

r-H 

LO 

o 

CO 

1 

1 

CO 

1 

ON 

o 

1 

1 

1 

, 

1 

1 

1 

1 

ON 

o 

CO 

1 

1 

1 

CM 

o 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

rJ 

NO 

ON 

CM 

On 

NO 

'Cf 

On 

CO 

CO 

o 

o 

NO 

1 1 

CO 

1 

o 

1 

1 

o 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

NO 

o 

NO 

LO 

ON 

CO 

1 

r-- 

o 

1 

1 

o 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

LO 

•^" 

cd 

d 

(J 

tJ 

NO 

d 

CM 

cd 

ON 

lO 

CO 

ON 

r- 

CO 

NO 

r- 

CO 

CM 

1 

CM 

1 

ON 

o 

o 

1 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

1 

1 

o 

On 

o 

1 

o 

o 

o 

CO 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

CM 

,J 

CM 

CM 

d 

d 

CM 

d 

d 

tJ 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

CM 

NO 

o 

o 

o 

NO 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

LO 

T 

ON 

CM 

CM 

CO 

CO 

NO 

- 

CO 

f-H 

r- 

CO 

f 

CM 

CO 

NO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

cm" 

r-" 

On" 

cd 

NO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CM 

r-H 

CM 

CM 

o5 

o 

o" 

NO 

lo" 

r-" 

CM 

On" 

o" 

NO 

CM 

CO 

CM 

r— 1 

o 

CM 

o 

CM 

1 

1 

NO 

NO 

, 

NO 

NO 

NO 

r- 

ON 

Ov 

CM 

NO 

CM 

CO 

00 

CM 

1 

On 

CM 

CM 

LO 

lO 

CM 

o 

LO 

lO 

CO 

CO 

NO 

CO 

NO 

lO 

■O' 

O 

r- 

CO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CM 

rH 

,-H 

-H 

CO 

LO 

d 

LO 

cd 

rd 

,-H 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

CO 

CO 

CM 

CO 

r- 

VO 

ON 

NO 

o 

NO 

LO 

CO 

r-- 

lO 

CM 

ON 

CM 

^ 1 

CM 

t" 

'Cf- 

Ov 

CO 

r- 

LO 

CM 

o 

co 

CO 

CM 

CO 

CM 

s| 

M ^3 


O 

o 

a -a 

y 


^£oi3-.. 

CQ  W CO  CQ  O CJ 


CO  3 


-3 

y 

3 

y 

52 

"5  .„ 
P J 


^ § 

< -c 

3 2 •£  .2 

£ y y 

J _Q  "O  ^ 

_3  ::3  3 3 

bC  < 3 g 

^ < 


2 i " 


i ^ i -2 

-3-03 

“D  2 c 
c -r  o C 


^ E as 
E -2  ^ s 


“O  t o 

iu 
« cS  O " 

•S  5 t.  CJ 
o o 
^ i!  -S  § 

1)  u _,  - 
i!  S 5 
-C  ® S C 

3 "O  _c  < 

!■“*== 
^ « -2  -2 

^ : o| 


C J=  -o  3 

N H ^ ^ 
■? 


< 2 

II  S 

^ i 


2 2 
^ *1 
S I 


"O  2; 


Ji 


6 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  3 

Density  Estimates  for  Selected  Species® 

Density 

Open  water  Pack  ice  (1^  oktas) 

(N  = 11)  (N  = 46) 

Pack  ice  (5-8  oktas) 
(N  = 12) 

Species 

X 

SD  (range)  x SD  (range) 

X 

SD 

(range) 

Adelie  Penguin 

— 

_ _ 1.79b  + 3 33  (0-18.92) 

4.05 

± 4.46 

(0-11.65) 

Antarctic  Petrel 

0.31 

± 0.24  (0-0.74)  1.57  ± 4.99  (0-31.87) 

0.23 

± 0.53 

(0-1.78) 

Snow  Petrel 

0.10 

± 0.25  (0-0.79)  0.48  ± 0.45  (0-2.55) 

0.38 

± 0.41 

(0-1.35) 

^ In  birds/km^. 

Based  on  N = 36;  see  range  in  Fig.  2. 


over  all  oktas  because  of  insufficient  pack  ice  census  data.  Variance  in  densities,  not  given 
in  the  original  report,  have  been  calculated  from  the  original  census  data. 

I'here  was  continuous  daylight  from  16  January-1  February,  hut  by  6 February  there  were 
7 h of  darkness  per  day.  The  daily  mean  sea  water  temperature  from  McMurdo  to  Palmer 
varied  from  — 1.9-2. 6°C  (3-12  readings  per  day,  N = 193;  x - -0.95  ± 0.88°C).  Because  of 
the  narrow  range,  bird  distributions  were  not  compared  with  sea  water  temperatures,  as  was 
done  by  Szijj  (1967)  in  Pacific  suhantarctic  waters.  Data  gathered  during  periods  when  vis- 
ibility was  less  than  0.4  km  were  not  included  in  the  abundance  or  density  estimates  but 
were  used  for  distributional  records  and  behavioral  information. 

RESULTS 

A total  of  7786  individuals  representing  at  least  16  species  were  sighted 
and  their  numbers  and  general  habitat  occurrence  are  given  in  Table  2. 
The  hypothesis  that  each  species  was  randomly  distributed  with  respect 
to  area  censused,  within  each  habitat  category,  was  tested  with  Chi-square 
and  rejected  {P  < 0.005)  (see  Table  2)  in  all  species  except  the  Southern 
Giant  Fulmar  {Macronectes  g:ig(int€us).  Thus,  these  arbitrarily  chosen  di- 
visions do  provide  indications  of  general  habitat  preference. 

The  occurrence,  absolute  and  relative  abundance  of  each  species  are 
functions  of  census  effort  per  habitat  and  geographic  region  studied,  as 
well  as  of  species  spatio-temporal  distributions  and  densities.  Since  most 
of  the  census  effort  was  in  pack  ice  (1775.7  of  2019.2  km^),  it  is  not  sur- 
prising that  4 species  typical  of  pack  ice  (Antarctic  Petrel  [Thalassoica 
antarctica]^  Adelie  Penguin,  Snow  Petrel  [Pagodroma  nivea],  Arctic  Tern) 
comprised  90.4%  of  the  total  birds  observed;  these  species  often  occur  in 
high  densities.  Many  of  the  infrequently  sighted  species  are  characteristic 
of  more  open  seas  and/or  lower  latitudes.  However,  of  all  species  observed, 
only  the  Black-browed  {Diomedea  melanophris)^  Gray-headed  {D.  chry- 
sostoma)  and  Wandering  (D.  exulans)  albatrosses  typically  are  not  found 
in  continental  antarctic  waters  (see  zonal  classification  in  Table  2).  A con- 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


7 


siderable  range  of  latitudes  (65°S-77°S)  and  longitudes  (170°E-66°W)  were 
censused,  in  addition  to  different  habitats,  hence  the  absolute  ranking  of 
species;  perhaps  the  relative  relationships  reflect  arbitrary  distribution  of 
sampling  effort. 

Species  typical  of  pack  ice. — Antarctic  Petrel. — This  was  the  most  fre- 
quently sighted  species  during  this  cruise  and  occurred  mostly  in  loose 
pack  ice  (Table  2).  The  73  individuals  sighted  in  open  water  were  in  groups 
of  4 or  fewer.  The  mean  density  of  1.57  ± 4.99/km^  in  1—4  oktas  was 
biased  by  a concentration  of  1750  birds  sighted  during  a 3-h  period  on  27 
January  at  74°30'S,  179°0'W,  in  2-4  oktas  of  pack  ice  (Table  3).  Several 
large  flocks  of  300-400  birds  roosting  on  large  tabular  icebergs  and  nu- 
merous other  smaller  flocks  and  individuals  standing  on  pack  ice  were 
observed,  accounting  for  the  maximum  density  of  31.9/km^.  This  concen- 
tration was  unexplained.  Without  these  flocks  the  mean  density  was  0.90 
± 2.04/km^  and  was  typical  of  most  pack  ice  (1-4  oktas)  areas  censused. 
The  mean  and  maximum  densities  in  5-8  oktas,  0.23  ± 0.53/km^  and  1.78/ 
km^,  respectively,  were  less  than  those  recorded  for  1^  oktas  and  were  less 
variable.  However,  while  the  preference  for  light  pack  ice  was  clear  (Table 
2),  the  densities  in  light  and  heavy  pack  ice  were  not  statistically  different 
(^  - 1.121,  df  = 58,  NS). 

Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  sighted  this  species  on  all  but  2 of  their  23  tran- 
sects and  recorded  a mean  density  of  4.31  ± 6.55/km^  and  a maximum 
density  of  23.06/km^  in  the  pack  ice  of  the  Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen 
seas;  the  4894  individuals  they  counted  constituted  47.7%  of  the  total  birds 
they  observed.  Of  the  23  density  estimates  calculated  from  their  data,  only 
5 exceeded  4.0/km^  (5.76,  12.49,  15.48,  18.56  and  23.06). 

Cline  et  al.  (1969)  observed  no  large  concentrations  of  Antarctic  Petrels 
in  the  Weddell  Sea  and  recorded  a mean  density  of  0.35/km^  (no  variance 
given);  they  noted  this  species  most  often  in  light  pack  ice.  In  the  pack 
ice  of  the  northwestern  Weddell  Sea,  Zink  (1978)  observed  this  species 
most  frequently  in  1-4  oktas  and  recorded  a mean  density  of  3.0  ± 11.03/ 
km^  and  a maximum  density  of  58/km^.  Parmelee  (1977)  also  found  them 
common  in  the  Weddell  Sea.  Present  data  are  insufficient  to  determine  if 
there  are  significant  differences  between  densities  of  Antarctic  Petrels  in 
the  Weddell  Sea  and  the  Ross,  Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen  seas,  al- 
though there  is  a preference  by  this  species  for  light  pack  ice  concentra- 
tions. 

Adelie  Penguin. — Occurrence  of  Adelie  Penguins  during  this  cruise  is 
shown  in  Fig.  2.  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  reported  this  species  as  rare  in  the 
pack  ice  of  the  Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen  seas;  they  observed  24 
individuals  in  1255  km^  of  pack  ice  census.  A possible  reason  for  this  is 
the  apparent  lack  of  breeding  sites  along  the  coasts  of  the  Bellingshausen 


8 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


a region  where  Adelie  densities  were  significantly  higher  than  other  areas  in  which  they  were 
sighted. 


and  Amundsen  seas  (G.  E.  Watson,  pers.  comm.).  The  absence  of  Adelies 
from  these  areas  was  substantiated  during  the  present  study,  as  most  of 
the  Adelies  (2746  of  2773)  were  observed  in  the  Ross  Sea.  Twenty-seven 
Adelies  were  sighted  between  135°W  and  127°W  in  7.3  h (118.1  km^)  and 
none  was  seen  east  of  127°W  during  18.3  h (294.2  km^)  of  pack  ice  census. 
This  species  reportedly  breeds  on  Peter  I Island  (Watson  1975),  however, 
none  was  observed  from  the  Glacier  (see  Fig.  1)  or  during  a helicopter 
seal  census  covering  65  km^  at  approximately  67°47'S,  90°47'W,  which  is 
roughly  100  km  north  of  Peter  I.  Because  of  the  apparent  absence  of 
Adelies  from  the  Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen  seas,  censuses  in  these 
areas  were  not  used  to  calculate  density  estimates.  In  the  Ross  Sea,  den- 
sity estimates  ranged  from  0-18. 9/km^  and  the  mean  densities  were 
1.79  ± 3.38/km^  in  1-4  oktas  and  4.05  ± 4.46/km^  in  5-8  oktas  of  pack 
ice  (Table  3);  the  difference  between  these  mean  densities  was  insignifi- 
cant (^  = 1.85,  df  48,  NS).  On  several  occasions  Adelies  were  observed 
in  3-5  oktas  during  1 census,  and  it  was  difficult  to  assign  these  obser- 
vations to  either  (1^  or  5-8)  pack  ice  category.  Thus,  while  there  appeared 
to  be  more  Adelies  in  heavy  pack  ice  (Table  2),  the  insignificant  relation- 
ship may  reflect  consolidation  of  observations  into  1-4  and  5-8  oktas, 
whereas,  possibly  Adelies  prefer  concentrations  of  3-5  oktas.  Further- 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


9 


more,  the  distribution  of  Adelies  in  the  Ross  Sea  was  not  uniform.  The 
blackened  portion  of  Fig.  2 represents  an  area  where  Adelies  appeared  to 
be  concentrated.  Within  these  areas  a density  of  5.60  ± 5.13/km^  was 
recorded  (N  = 15),  whereas  the  density  was  1.24  ± 1.78/km^  for  the  re- 
mainder (N  = 25)  of  the  Ross  Sea;  using  a t-test  for  unequal  variances 
and  sample  sizes  (Snedecor  and  Cochran  1967:114-116)  these  differences 
were  statistically  significant  (^  = 3.179,  df  = 40,  0.005  < P < 0.01).  This 
concentration  possibly  consisted  of  nonbreeders  attracted  to  a localized 
food  source. 

Cline  et  al.  (1969)  found  Adelies  to  be  the  most  abundant  bird  in  the 
summer  pack  ice  of  the  Weddell  Sea  and  recorded  a mean  density  of 
10.96/km^  (no  variance  given)  and  a maximum  of  90.81/km^;  they  found 
them  most  frequently  in  heavy  pack  ice.  They  noted  that  the  distribution 
of  Adelies  was  uneven  and  that  the  greatest  numbers  were  seen  in  the 
northern  limits  of  the  pack.  Their  maximum  density  estimate  consisted  of 
1360  individuals  observed  on  15  March  1968,  roughly  350  km  from  nearby 
breeding  sites  on  the  Antarctic  Peninsula.  They  noted  that  this  concen- 
tration could  have  comprised  dispersing  adults  from  peninsular  breeding 
colonies.  Zink  (1978)  observed  large  numbers  of  Adelies  in  loose  pack  ice, 
at  the  northern  limits  of  the  pack  ice  in  the  Weddell  Sea,  and  estimated 
mean  and  maximum  densities  of  7.0/km^  and  101.75/km^,  respectively. 
(The  mean  density  was  corrected  downwards  to  reduce  the  effect  of  sev- 
eral large  concentrations;  original  figure  was  mean  density  of  28  ± 30.6/ 
km^  [Zink,  unpubl.].)  The  apparent  occurrence  of  higher  densities  in  the 
Weddell  Sea,  as  compared  to  the  Ross,  Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen 
seas,  needs  further  documentation,  as  both  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  and  Zink 
(1978)  censused  near  breeding  localities  and  high  densities  recorded  in 
these  areas  could  have  inflated  mean  densities. 

Adelies  usually  were  seen  in  groups  of  6—20  birds,  often  on  the  leeward 
side  of  ice  hummocks  and  pressure  ridges.  Like  the  Antarctic  Petrel, 
Adelies  seemed  to  prefer  older  pack  ice,  especially  ice  with  uneven  sur- 
faces providing  shelter.  Few  Adelies  were  observed  in  the  water  except 
when  forced  to  dive  as  the  Glacier  approached  to  within  100  m.  Cline  et 
al.  (1969)  also  observed  this  behavior  in  the  Weddell  Sea  and  noted  that 
Adelies  during  their  post-breeding  molt  typically  remain  out  of  the  water 
and  fast.  I observed  little  direct  evidence  of  molting.  That  few  Adelies 
were  seen  in  the  water  could  be  a result  of  either  restricted  feeding  periods 
or  the  difficulty  in  seeing  swimming  penguins.  On  several  occasions, 
Adelies  were  seen  to  approach  the  ship  when  it  was  stuck  in  ice,  hence, 
curiosity  and/or  lack  of  fear  may  explain  their  reluctance  to  flee  an  ap- 
proaching ship. 

Leopard  seals  {Hydrurga  leptonyx)  prey  on  adult  Adelies  at  sea,  how- 


10 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  I,  March  1981 


ever,  the  incidence  of  this,  and  adult  mortality  in  general,  are  thought  to 
be  low  (Watson  1975).  On  25  January,  2 crew  members  on  the  bridge  of 
the  Glacier  observed  a killer  whale  {Orcinus  orca)  lunge  out  of  the  water 
and  apparently  capture  2 Adelies  standing  at  the  edge  of  a floe.  Further 
observations  are  needed  to  document  the  validity  of  this  claim  and  extent 
of  this  predation. 

Snow  Petrel. — This  species  is  a common  antarctic  bird  generally  re- 
stricted to  cold  continental  and  maritime  waters  with  pack  ice  and/or  ice- 
bergs present  (Watson  1975).  It  was  sighted  on  all  but  2 days.  Most  Snow 
Petrels  (98.58%)  were  seen  over  pack  ice  and  their  distribution  was  usually 
uniform.  The  densities  in  pack  ice  were  0.48  ± 0.45/km^  in  1-4  oktas  and 
0.38  ± 0.41/km^  in  5-8  oktas  (Table  3);  there  was  no  significant  difference 
between  these  densities  {t  = 0.697,  df  = 56,  NS).  Of  the  847  Snow  Petrels 
recorded,  the  largest  flock  had  40  individuals  and  only  5 other  flocks  had 
10  birds  or  more  (10,  12,  13,  19  and  25).  In  the  pack  ice  of  the  Bellings- 
hausen and  Amundsen  seas,  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  recorded  3516  Snow 
Petrels,  which  represented  34.2%  of  the  total  number  of  birds  they  ob- 
served. Their  mean  density  was  2.83  ± 3.53/km^  and  the  maximum  was 
15.92/km^,  suggesting  that  this  species  is  more  abundant  in  the  Bellings- 
hausen and  Amundsen  seas.  The  pelagic  distribution  map  given  in  Watson 
(1975)  appears  to  exclude  the  southern  Ross  Sea.  In  the  Weddell  Sea, 
Cline  et  al.  (1969)  recorded  a mean  density  of  2.82/km^  and  a maximum 
density  of  46.68/km^  and  noted  this  species  most  frequently  in  light  and 
medium  pack  ice  concentrations.  The  Snow  Petrel  was  the  most  common 
(24.5%  of  total)  volant  species  sighted  by  Zink  (1978);  a mean  density  of 
8.0  ± 5.50/krn^  and  a maximum  density  of  98.69/km^  (not  included  in  the 
mean  density  estimate)  were  recorded  in  pack  ice,  primarily  in  ice  con- 
centrations of  from  1—5  oktas.  Few  birds  were  sighted  in  open  waters  and 
a maximum  density  of  5.93/km^  was  recorded.  The  largest  group  of  Snow 
Petrels  observed  by  Parmelee  (1977)  in  the  Weddell  Sea  was  47  and  a total 
of  1588  birds  was  sighted  (sightings  made  on  32  of  35  days). 

The  specific  habitat  preference  of  Snow  Petrels  during  this  study  was 
not  clear  as  the  observed  frequencies  in  pack  ice  (83.7:14.9)  paralleled  the 
relative  areas  censused  in  each  habitat  (71.2:16.7).  Although  this  differ- 
ence was  significant  (x^  100.23,  df  = 1,  P < 0.005)  the  preferred  hab- 

itat may  have  been  masked  by  consolidating  the  pack  ice  observations  into 
2 categories.  As  with  the  Adelie  Penguin,  perhaps  3-5  oktas  is  a better 
approximation  of  their  habitat  (ice  concentration)  preference. 

Snow  Petrels  use  2 types  of  flight.  One  was  “very  erratic,  almost  bat- 
like” (Watson  1975);  the  birds  flew  low  over  the  surface,  often  exhibiting 
high  maneuverability  as  they  closely  followed  ice  edges.  This  apparent 
foraging  behavior  is  consistent  with  the  observation  of  Falla  (1964)  that 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


11 


their  primary  food  consists  of  dead  or  injured  macroplankton.  As  Ainley 
et  al.  (1978)  noted,  such  plankton  would  be  expected  to  accumulate  at  the 
edges  of  cakes  of  ice.  The  other  type  of  flight,  steady  and  direct,  was  used 
about  7-10  m above  open  water  and  occasionally  over  pack  ice. 

Snow  Petrels  frequently  hunted  along  narrow  strips  of  pack  ice  while 
avoiding  adjacent  open  water.  Upon  sighting  a prey  item  a flying  individual 
quickly  veered  upwards  and  then  fluttered  down  to  the  surface.  Feeding 
motions  included  pecking  at  the  surface  and  submerging  the  head  and 
neck  to  catch  subsurface  prey,  which,  according  to  Watson  (1975),  consists 
mainly  of  fish  and  some  invertebrates.  Birds  that  remained  on  the  surface 
for  15  sec  or  longer  folded  their  wings  and  swam  about  after  prey.  Foraging 
individuals  that  alighted  on  the  surface  for  less  than  15  sec  usually  kept 
their  wings  unfolded  and  held  at  about  60°  to  the  surface.  This  was  also 
noted  by  Ainley  et  al.  (1978)  and  apparently  facilitates  more  rapid  or  ef- 
ficient take-offs. 

Arctic  Tern. — Adult  Arctic  Terns  migrate  annually  from  their  arctic 
breeding  grounds  to  “winter”  in  the  Antarctic  during  the  austral  summer. 
The  Arctic  Tern  was  the  fourth  most  abundant  species  observed  and  was 
seen  mostly  in  loose  pack  ice  (Table  2)  along  the  northern  edge  of  the 
pack;  few  were  encountered  south  of  the  pack  edge.  The  maximum  density 
recorded  was  2.72/km^  on  3 February.  Ainley  et  al.  (1978)  noted  that  there 
are  no  records  of  Arctic  Terns  from  the  Ross  Sea.  The  westernmost  ob- 
servations of  terns  were  on  22-23  January,  when  56  birds  were  sighted 
between  72°S-72°50'S  and  144°40'W-151°6' W.  These  observations  per- 
haps establish  the  limit  of  Arctic  Tern  distribution  in  the  eastern  Ross 
Sea.  Darby  (1970)  observed  only  1 Arctic  Tern  in  4 north-south  traverses 
of  the  Ross  Sea  and  that  sighting  was  (actually  in  the  southern  Pacific)  at 
67°55'S,  174°41'E  on  18  January  1968. 

Arctic  Terns  occurred  in  flocks  of  5-20  birds;  5 flocks  of  more  than  20 
were  seen  (60  + , 52,  41,  30  and  25);  a total  of  60  birds  was  observed  in 
groups  of  fewer  than  5.  The  flock  of  60+  terns  was  on  an  iceberg  near 
Peter  I Island  during  a helicopter  seal  census  and  was  not  included  in  the 
shipboard  census  data.  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  recorded  a mean  density  of 
1.27  ± 1.91/km^  and  a maximum  density  of  7.95/km^. 

Cline  et  al.  (1969)  recorded  a mean  density  of  0.97/km^  and  a maximum 
density  of  13.90/km^,  mostly  in  light  and  medium  pack  ice  concentrations. 
Zink  (1978)  observed  a mean  density  of  1.4  ± 1.14/km^  and  a maximum 
of  3.39/km^  in  pack  ice,  as  well  as  a mean  density  of  2.67  ± 5.36/km^  and 
a maximum  density  of  27.54/km^  in  open  seas.  These  observations  support 
the  conclusion  that  Arctic  Terns  are  most  abundant  in  the  Weddell  Sea 
(Watson  1975,  Parmelee  1977). 

Arctic  Terns  often  passed  the  Glacier  at  the  outer  limits  of  the  transect 


12 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


strip  and  it  was  not  always  possible  to  see  plumage  characteristics  clearly. 
Individuals  observed  closely  were  in  non-breeding  plumage  and  most  ap- 
peared to  be  in  heavy  wing  and  tail  molt.  The  few  feeding  individuals 
observed  plunged  headfirst  into  the  water  from  a height  of  about  3.5  m. 
Some  birds  worked  low  over  brash  ice  and  hovered  above  prey  items 
before  picking  them  off  the  surface  in  flight. 

Southern  Fulmar  {Fulmarus  glacial oides). — Most  (287  of  289)  of  the 
sightings  of  Southern  Fulmars  occurred  between  3-6  February.  The  2 
birds  seen  west  of  91°W  were  in  loose  pack  ice  on  19  January  at  68°30'S, 
161°30'W  and  68°45'S,  159°0'W.  Watson  (1975)  states  that  this  species  is 
highly  pelagic  and  avoids  pack  ice,  but  Johnstone  and  Kerry  (1974)  remark 
that  Southern  Fulmars  occur  commonly  in  pack  ice  in  the  Australian  sector 
of  the  southern  ocean.  The  majority  of  the  fulmars  sighted  east  of  91°W 
(222  of  287),  during  the  present  study,  were  in  loose  pack  ice  (1-2  oktas), 
and  of  these,  151  were  in  groups  of  fewer  than  5 and  the  remainder  were 
in  9 flocks  of  fewer  than  40.  Most  birds  were  sitting  on  the  water  or  on 
pack  ice  and  few  feeding  birds  were  observed.  Some  fulmars  followed  the 
ship  briefly. 

Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  observed  this  species  on  9 of  23  transects,  and  of 
the  total  of  53  individuals  sighted,  the  maximum  density  was  0.42/km^  (25 
birds  sighted  in  60  km^).  The  scarcity  of  sightings  west  of  91°W  might  be 
explained  by  the  relative  lack  of  censuses  in  more  northerly  ice-free  waters 
(west  of  91°W).  In  the  Weddell  Sea,  Zink  (1978)  observed  3 of  709  birds 
in  pack  ice  and  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  sighted  none  in  pack  ice. 

South  Polar  Skua  (Catharacta  maccormicki). — Watson  (1975)  states  that 
during  the  austral  summer  breeding  season,  adult  South  Polar  Skuas  re- 
main near  breeding  colonies,  while  sub-adults  are  found  at  sea.  Of  the  110 
skuas  observed,  102  were  within  75  km  of  McMurdo  Station,  either  in 
dense  pack  ice  or  along  the  fast  ice  in  McMurdo  Sound.  Skuas  observed 
near  McMurdo  Station  were  light  phase  birds.  Of  the  remaining  8 skuas 
sighted,  6 were  seen  within  115  km  of  McMurdo,  1 in  the  western  Ross 
Sea  and  1 in  the  western  Bellingshausen  Sea.  The  skua  in  the  western 
Ross  Sea  (at  72°10'S,  157°20'W)  was  in  loose  pack  ice  (1  okta),  and  was 
a dark  phase  bird  that  showed  prominent  golden  hackles  and  appeared  to 
be  in  fresh  plumage.  Presumably  an  adult  (based  on  plumage,  see  Watson 
1975),  it  was  some  560  km  from  the  nearest  known  breeding  site. 

Skuas  are  more  often  found  near  land,  thus,  there  are  relatively  few 
pelagic  sightings.  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  observed  8 individuals.  In  the 
Weddell  Sea,  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  observed  4 and  Zink  (1978)  sighted  10.  In 
spite  of  the  paucity  of  pelagic  sightings,  individuals  banded  in  Antarctica 
have  been  recovered  from  temperate  seas  (Watson  1975)  and  Greenland 
(Parmelee  et  al.  1977). 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


13 


Emperor  Penguin  {Aptenodytes  forsteri). — The  largest  number  of  Em- 
peror Penguins  seen  at  1 time  was  4.  Most  sightings  were  of  groups  of  2 
or  3,  widely  dispersed  in  the  pack  ice  of  the  Ross  and  Amundsen  seas. 
Most  individuals  occurred  in  light  pack  ice.  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  recorded 
30  Emperor  Penguins  in  the  Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen  seas.  There 
are  no  known  breeding  sites  along  the  coasts  of  these  seas  and  this  species 
apparently  does  not  range  far  from  its  breedings  sites,  except  for  wander- 
ing young  birds.  Larger  concentrations  were  noted  in  the  Weddell  Sea  by 
Parmelee  (1977,  maximum  number  sighted  at  1 time  was  67,  total  sightings 
were  363).  Also  in  the  Weddell  Sea,  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  found  Emperor 
Penguins  most  often  in  light  and  moderate  pack  ice  concentrations  and 
recorded  a mean  density  of  0.5/km^  and  a maximum  of  5.33/km^.  Zink 
(1978)  observed  a mean  density  of  0.14  ± 0.17/km^  and  a maximum  of 
0.50/km^. 

Southern  Giant  Fulmer. — Watson  (1975)  stated  that  this  species  is  highly 
pelagic  throughout  antarctic  and  subantarctic  waters  and  that  young  in- 
dividuals usually  are  distributed  farther  north  than  adults.  Fourteen  of  17 
(82.3%)  Giant  Fulmars  sighted  in  the  Ross  Sea  were  juveniles,  based  on 
entirely  (or  nearly  so)  dark  plumage  (Watson  1975).  There  are  both  dark 
and  white  phases  of  the  Southern  Giant  Fulmar  and  the  young  of  each 
phase  resemble  the  adult  condition.  The  birds  sighted  during  the  present 
study  were  all  dark  phase.  Siple  and  Lindsey  (1937)  and  Watson  (1975) 
suggested  that  the  percentage  of  white  phase  birds  increases  to  the  south 
or  in  pack  ice;  the  present  observations  do  not  substantiate  this.  Four  of 
the  other  8 Giant  Fulmars  observed  in  the  Amundsen  and  Bellingshausen 
seas  were  adults.  Giant  Fulmars  appeared  to  be  distributed  randomly  with- 
in the  3 habitat  types,  although  this  is  inconclusive  because  of  the  small 
sample  size  (25). 

Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  observed  65  individuals  in  1255  km^  of  pack  ice 
census  in  the  Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen  seas.  In  the  summer  pack 
ice  of  the  Weddell  Sea,  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  sighted  8 individuals.  Zink  (1978) 
observed  106  Southern  Giant  Fulmars  in  the  Weddell  Sea  and  found  them 
randomly  distributed  between  open  seas  and  light  and  heavy  pack  ice. 

Species  typical  of  open  seas. — Wilson’s  Storm-Petrel  {Oceanites  ocean- 
icus). — Wilson’s  Storm-Petrels  were  observed  in  open  water  or  loose  pack 
ice  (Table  2);  their  occurrence  closely  followed  the  pelagic  distribution 
given  by  Watson  (1975).  Twenty-nine  birds  were  sighted  between  178°27'E 
and  176°25'W  (73°10'S-75°S)  and  1 bird  was  seen  between  176°25'W  and 
97°59'W  (at  72°0'S,  145°0'W,  on  22  January  in  1 okta  of  pack  ice)  and  the 
remainder  (86)  were  seen  between  97°59'W  and  Palmer  Station.  The  south- 
ernmost sighting  was  at  76°23'S,  171°53'E,  about  150  km  from  McMurdo 
Station  on  26  January.  This  is  near  the  southwestern  limit  of  the  pelagic 


14 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


range,  and  is  approximately  450  km  from  a breeding  site  on  the  coast  line 
of  Victoria  Land,  at  72°S,  170°E  (Watson  1975).  Ainley  et  al.  (1978)  noted 
Wilson’s  Storm-Petrels  at  Cape  Crozier  (Ross  Island)  and  therefore  the 
limits  of  its  pelagic  range  in  the  Ross  Sea  are  equivocal. 

Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  observed  7 individuals  in  the  pack  ice  of  the 
Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen  seas.  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  observed  few  birds 
in  the  pack  ice  of  the  Weddell  Sea.  Zink  (1978)  recorded  a mean  density 
of  0.44  ± 1.15/km^  and  a maximum  density  of  4.38/km^  in  the  pack  ice  of 
the  Weddell  Sea  in  addition  to  a mean  density  of  1.44  ± 1.97/km^  and  a 
maximum  density  of  9.65/km^  in  open  seas.  These  studies  indicate  that 
the  occurrence  of  Wilson’s  Storm-Petrels  in  pack  ice  is  limited.  Once, 
(about  65°30'S,  67°20'W)  approximately  50  birds  were  in  view,  mostly 
astern  and  following  the  ship.  Otherwise  the  maximum  in  view  was  about  4. 

Blue  Petrel  {Halobaena  caerulea). — All  Blue  Petrels  seen  in  this  study 
and  in  the  Weddell  Sea  by  Zink  (1978)  were  in  open  seas.  A flock  of  55 
Blue  Petrels  was  sighted  at  70°40'S,  136°52'W  on  22  January  as  the  Glacier 
passed  through  an  extensive  patch  of  open  water  in  the  eastern  Ross  Sea. 
This  sighting  is  approximately  300  km  south  of  the  previous  southernmost 
record  (see  Watson  et  al.  1971,  Watson  1975).  The  other  22  individuals 
were  sighted  on  4 February,  in  several  small  flocks,  in  the  Bellingshausen 
Sea. 

Unidentified  petrels. — Unidentified  petrels  were  seen  on  9 occasions  in 
the  southern  Ross  Sea  on  19,  20  and  26  January.  Poor  viewing  conditions 
precluded  positive  species  determination  but  probably  only  the  Mottled 
Petrel  {Pterodroma  inexpectata)  and/or  Sooty  Shearwater  {Puffinus  gris- 
eus)  were  involved.  All  of  these  sightings  are  south  of  the  known  range  of 
the  Sooty  Shearwater  by  at  least  400  km.  Noteworthy  is  the  sighting  on  26 
January  at  76°10'S,  173°30'E,  as  this  location  is  about  100  km  south  of  the 
pelagic  range  of  the  Mottled  Petrel,  but  is  1500  km  south  of  the  known 
pelagic  range  of  the  Sooty  Shearwater.  During  4 cruises  from  New  Zealand 
to  McMurdo,  between  January  and  March  1968,  Darby  (1970)  recorded  her 
southernmost  Sooty  Shearwater  at  68°22'S,  170°18'E,  however,  she  ob- 
served no  Mottled  Petrels. 

Antarctic  Prion  {Pachyptila  desolata). — A single  Antarctic  Prion  was 
seen  on  3 February  at  68°50'S,  100°20'W  over  open  water.  Erickson  et  al. 
(1972)  did  not  see  this  species.  In  the  large  leads  and  polynyas  of  the  Wed- 
dell Sea  pack  ice,  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  recorded  24  prions  and  a mean  density  of 
0.04/km^.  Zink  (1978)  recorded  a mean  density  of  0.21  ± 0.40/km^  during 
open  water  transects  in  the  Weddell  Sea  and  Parmelee  (1977)  observed 
304  prions,  which  included  a single  observation  of  100+  birds. 

Cape  Pigeon  [Daption  capense). — On  22  January,  at  70°40'S,  136°52'W, 
a Cape  Pigeon  was  seen  over  open  seas.  This  sighting  represents  a south- 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


15 


ern  extension  of  approximately  200  km  based  on  the  map  in  Watson  (1975). 
However,  Ainley  et  al.  (1978)  noted  records  of  Cape  Pigeons  in  the  Ross 
Sea  at  72°S  and  76°54'S.  Other  records  include  sightings  by  Darby  (1970) 
at  73°46'S  and  by  the  first  Byrd  Antarctic  Expedition  {in  Siple  and  Lindsey 
1937)  in  Discovery  Inlet,  at  78°30'S.  From  these  records  it  is  possible  to 
conclude  that  only  a few  Cape  Pigeons,  perhaps  sexually  immature  indi- 
viduals, wander  through  the  southern  Ross  Sea.  As  noted  by  Ainley  et  al. 
(1978)  the  observation  of  Spellerberg  (1971)  that  Cape  Pigeons  were  abun- 
dant off  the  northern  tip  of  Ross  Island  during  March  1964,  is  suspect.  Of 
the  other  14  Cape  Pigeons  sighted  during  the  present  study,  5 were  in 
light  pack  ice  and  9 were  over  open  waters  in  the  Bellingshausen  Sea. 
Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  sighted  a Cape  Pigeon  in  the  pack  ice  of  the  Amund- 
sen Sea  at  67°47'S,  128°50'W  on  11  February  1972.  In  the  northwestern 
Weddell  Sea,  Zink  (1978)  sighted  694  Cape  Pigeons  in  open  seas  and 
recorded  a mean  density  of  1.0  ± 1.22/km^  and  a maximum  density  of  4.2/ 
km^;  few  were  observed  in  pack  ice.  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  observed  a total 
of  25  Cape  Pigeons  and  a mean  and  maximum  density  of  0.04/km^  and 
1.70/km^,  respectively. 

Albatrosses. — Four  species  of  albatrosses  (Black-browed,  Gray-headed, 
Wandering  and  Light-mantled  Sooty  [Phoebetria  palpebrata])  were  sighted 
during  open  water  censuses  in  the  Bellingshausen  Sea  from  4-6  February. 
These  species  are  typically  highly  pelagic  and  avoid  pack  ice  (Watson 
1975).  The  largest  number  of  adults  of  either  the  Black-browed  Albatross 
or  Gray-headed  Albatross  in  view  at  1 time  was  3.  The  actual  number  of 
albatrosses  observed  is  difficult  to  determine,  because  of  their  well  known 
ship-following  habits  (except  for  the  gray-headed).  Also,  2 or  3 Black- 
browed  or  Gray-headed  albatrosses  were  observed  (discontinuously)  within 
a short  time  and  there  was  suspicion  as  to  whether  there  were  1,  2 or  3 
individuals  involved.  Immature  Black-browed  and  Gray-headed  albatross- 
es are  difficult  to  distinguish  at  sea,  and  the  records  of  “unidentified  mol- 
lymauk”  in  Table  2 refer  to  juveniles  of  these  species. 

DISCUSSION 

Pelagic  seabird  observations  obtained  during  the  austral  summer  in  Ant- 
arctica are  difficult  to  interpret  because  the  relative  abundances  of  im- 
matures,  unsuccessful  breeders  and  foraging  breeders  are  unclear.  Darby 
(1970)  suggested  that  many  birds  seen  far  from  land  during  the  breeding 
season  were  probably  non-breeding  or  immature  individuals.  Delayed  sex- 
ual maturity  is  typical  for  most  seabirds.  Consequently,  it  is  probable  that 
sexually  immature  procellariids  and  penguins  constitute  a high  proportion 
of  birds  seen  at  sea,  since  they  evidently  avoid  regions  near  breeding  sites 


16 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


(Watson  1975).  Because  of  the  short  austral  breeding  season,  it  is  likely 
that  most  unsuccessful  breeders  depart  to  sea  rather  than  renest.  Brook 
and  Beck  (1972)  discovered  nesting  Antarctic  Petrels,  Snow  Petrels  and 
South  Polar  Skuas  in  the  Theron  Mountains,  250  km  from  the  nearest 
foraging  grounds  in  the  open  waters  of  the  Weddell  Sea.  Warham  et  al. 
(1977)  speculated  that  breeding  Mottled  Petrels,  from  colonies  on  islands 
south  of  New  Zealand,  may  range  up  to  2200  km  into  the  southwestern 
Ross  Sea  during  off-duty  periods.  Beck  (1969)  noted  that  Cape  Pigeons  in 
breeding  condition  have  been  collected  350  km  from  the  nearest  breeding 
site.  Breeding  petrels  range  considerable  distances  from  nest-sites  and 
undoubtedly  contribute  to  the  number  of  birds  seen  at  sea;  this  contri- 
bution may  increase  as  observations  are  made  nearer  to  breeding  sites.  It 
is  not  generally  possible  to  distinguish  ages  or  sexes  of  most  species  on 
the  basis  of  sight  observations.  Therefore,  the  relative  abundance  of  adults 
and  immatures  is  uncertain.  Sightings  of  birds  hundreds  of  km  from  the 
nearest  land  (not  necessarily  breeding  sites;  see  Table  2 for  breeding  sites 
of  species  sighted  during  this  study)  provide  data  for  species-level  regional 
distributions  and  information  about  species'  pelagic  ecology  and  behavior. 
These  data  can  also  be  used  to  ascertain  regional  patterns  in  the  compo- 
sition of  pelagic  seabird  communities. 

Density  estimates  are  useful  for  providing  a quantitative  measure  of  i 

avian  occurrence.  Densities  (birds/area)  are  superior  in  most  instances  to  ' 

measures  of  birds  per  unit  of  time  or  linear  distance  because  of  potentially  I 

serious  biases  in  observer  proficiency  and  ability,  ship  speed  and  uncor-  | 

rected  biases  in  the  nonuniform  probability  of  sighting  different  species  j 

at  a given  distance.  Densities  can  also  be  extrapolated  to  estimate  the  t 

number  of  birds  in  a given  region.  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  stated  that  the 
area  circumscribed  by  the  outer  limits  of  their  sampling  effort  was  about 
250,000  krn‘^.  If  the  mean  density  for  the  Antarctic  Petrel  (4.31/km^)  is 
applied  to  this  area,  the  resulting  number  of  birds  is  1,077,500.  However, 
it  should  be  recognized  that  there  is  considerable  variance  (SD  = 6.55) 
about  this  mean.  The  Antarctic  Petrel  often  occurs  in  dense  aggregates 
and  additional  censuses  are  probably  required.  It  is  necessary  to  establish 
the  uniformity  of  the  habitat  within  the  bounds  of  the  census  effort  and  to 
account  for  such  clumped  distributions  of  birds  (or  at  least  acknowledge 
them).  Also,  because  the  age  structure  of  this  pelagic  “population”  is 
unknown,  such  population  estimates  are  probably  area  specific  and  not  J 
necessarily  applicable  to  other  geographic  regions.  I 

In  antarctic  ecosystems,  there  are  typically  few  species,  but  these  are  1 

often  abundant  (Watson  1975,  Cline  et  al.  1969).  In  the  present  study,  ; 

most  of  the  census  effort  (88%  of  the  total  census  area)  was  in  pack  ice. 

Ten  species  were  observed  in  pack  ice  and  of  these,  4 (Antarctic  Petrel, 


i 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


17 


Adelie  Penguin,  Snow  Petrel  and  Arctic  Tern)  accounted  for  over  90%  of 
birds  observed  in  pack  ice.  Erickson  et  al.  (1972)  found  the  same  10  species 
in  the  pack  ice  of  the  Bellingshausen  and  Amundsen  seas,  although  few 
Adelie  Penguins.  In  order  of  decreasing  abundance,  the  Antarctic  Petrel, 
Snow  Petrel  and  Arctic  Tern  accounted  for  98.2%  of  the  10,270  total  birds 
they  observed.  In  the  pack  ice  of  the  Weddell  Sea,  the  Adelie  Penguin, 
Snow  Petrel  and  Antarctic  Petrel  accounted  for  69.4%  of  the  14,376  total 
birds  (pack  ice  and  open  seas)  observed  by  Zink  (1978)  even  though  only 
21.6%  of  the  698  km^  of  census  area  was  in  pack  ice.  Also  in  the  summer 
pack  ice  of  the  Weddell  Sea,  Cline  et  al.  (1969)  found  that  the  Adelie 
Penguin,  Snow  Petrel,  Arctic  Tern,  Emperor  Penguin  and  Antarctic  Petrel 
accounted  for  98.8%  of  the  9451  total  birds  sighted.  These  studies  sub- 
stantiate the  claim  that  the  pack  ice  environment  is  dominated  by  a few, 
rather  abundant  species.  However,  the  same  species  are  not  equally  dom- 
inant between  areas  nor  are  the  relative  abundances  equal. 

Cline  et  al.  (1969)  concluded  that  pack  ice  concentrations  influenced 
bird  distributions  and  densities  and  they  found  the  highest  densities  of 
most  species  in  ice  concentrations  of  10-60%.  The  observations  of  Ainley 
et  al.  (1978)  further  support  this  conclusion.  During  the  present  study,  ice 
concentrations  of  1-5  oktas  supported  the  greatest  numbers  of  birds.  How- 
ever, the  type  of  pack  ice  also  was  important.  Pack  ice  varies  considerably 
in  thickness,  surface  and  subsurface  structure;  these  vary  with  age  of  ice 
and  amount  of  compacted  snow.  Different  kinds  of  ice  probably  offer  dif- 
ferent potentials  to  birds,  in  terms  of  resources  or  shelter.  Older  pack  ice 
(2  years  and  up)  is  thicker  and  generally  has  a rough,  irregular  surface 
that  provides  wind  shelters  for  resting  or  molting  birds.  In  addition,  its 
subsurface  has  usually  deteriorated  into  a matrix  of  small  holes  and  chan- 
nels which  are  often  frequented  by  various  invertebrates  (Watson  1975) 
such  as  krill  (pers.  obs.).  There  were  qualitative  indications  that  more 
birds  were  in  areas  of  older  pack  ice  than  in  newer  (thinner  and  flatter) 
pack  ice.  Future  studies  should  determine  the  type  of  ice  as  well  as  its 
concentration  for  correlations  with  the  occurrence  of  birds,  seals  and 
whales. 

Voous  (1965)  suggested  that  food  availability  and  abundance  are  prob- 
ably the  primary  factors  influencing  the  distribution  of  birds  in  antarctic 
waters.  From  the  above  discussion,  it  is  apparent  that  the  pelagic  occur- 
rence of  seabirds  results  from  an  interaction  of  food  availability  and  ice 
concentration  and  structure. 

There  was  insufficient  census  effort  in  open  waters  to  determine,  with 
much  certainty,  the  species  characteristic  of  this  environment.  Species 
typical  of  open,  continental  antarctic  waters  are  the  Southern  Fulmar, 
Wilson’s  Storm-Petrel,  Blue  Petrel,  Southern  Giant  Fulmar,  Cape  Pigeon, 


18 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Antarctic  Prion  and  Light-mantled  Sooty  Albatross  (Watson  1975).  In  gen- 
eral, seabird  diversity  is  greater  in  antarctic  maritime  and  subantarctic 
waters  than  in  antarctic  continental  waters  (Darby  1970,  Szijj  1967,  Watson 
1975).  Darby  (1970)  thought  that  the  northern  limit  of  the  pack  ice  consti- 
tuted a barrier  to  the  southward  expansion  of  many  species  and  noted  that 
several  species  were  consistently  80-100  km  north  of  the  pack,  whatever 
its  northern  limit.  On  4 February,  the  Glacier  passed  out  of  what  was 
probably  the  northern  extent  of  pack  ice  (68°S,  87°W).  The  species  com- 
position changed  rapidly  (within  2 km)  from  predominantly  pack  ice 
species  to  speeies  characteristic  of  open  seas.  The  interface  between  pack 
edge  and  open  water  potentially  supports  large  numbers  of  birds  because 
of  an  accumulation  of  plankton  (Routh  1949).  However,  no  such  concen- 
trations of  birds  were  noted. 

The  interactions  and  associations  of  various  species  at  sea  are  poorly 
understood.  In  the  Bellingshausen  and  Weddell  seas  (Zink,  unpubl.)  Wil- 
son’s Storm-Petrel  appeared  to  be  an  “indicator”  of  localized  patches  of 
food.  Individuals  seemed  to  search  independently  and  were  randomly  in- 
terspersed until  a pateh  of  food  was  located.  Others  then  flocked  to  the 
site  and  fed  as  described  by  Alexander  (1954)  and  Murphy  (1936).  They 
headed  into  the  wind,  pattering  their  feet  on  the  surface  and  feeding 
through  the  patch  of  food  and  then  returned  downwind  and  worked  through 
this  specific  area  again.  Such  a grouping  of  Wilson’s  Storm-Petrels  often 
attracted  other  species  such  as  the  Cape  Pigeon  and  Southern  Giant  Ful- 
mar, and  albatrosses  in  more  northerly  areas.  Both  the  nature  of  specific 
roles  in  such  assemblages  and  the  geographic  trends  in  eomposition  of 
interspecific  flocks  are  unknown. 

SUMMARY 

Observations  of  16  species  of  seabirds  in  antarctic  waters  were  obtained  during  the  austral 
summer  of  1976  while  the  USCGC  Glacier  cruised  from  McMurdo  Station  (Ross  Island)  to 
Palmer  Station  (Anvers  Island).  Information  on  distribution,  abundance,  habitat  (ice  con- 
centration) preference  and  behavior  of  these  species  is  given  and  comparisons  are  made  with 
recent  seabird  surveys  in  the  Bellingshausen,  Amundsen  and  Ross  seas  and  the  Weddell 
Sea. 

The  Antarctic  Petrel,  Adelie  Penguin  and  Snow  Petrel  were  the  most  abundant  species, 
respectively,  in  pack  ice,  where  most  of  the  136.1  h of  observation  occurred.  The  Adelie 
Penguin,  which  was  generally  absent  from  the  Amundsen  and  Bellingshausen  seas,  and  the 
Antarctic  Petrel  both  occurred  in  large  concentrations,  while  the  Snow  Petrel  was  more 
uniformly  spread  throughout  the  pack  ice  encountered  on  this  cruise.  These  species,  the 
Arctic  Tern  and  the  Emperor  Penguin,  probably  are  the  primary  species  of  the  antarctic 
pack  ice  ecosystem.  Antarctic  avifaunas  are  dominated  by  relatively  few  species.  However, 
the  same  species  were  not  dominant  throughout  all  regions  examined  and  in  many  cases  the 
relative  dominances  were  different.  A part  of  these  regional  differences  may  be  attributed 
to  the  distribution  of  sampling  effort  between  different  regions  and  habitats.  Pack  ice  con- 


Zink  • SEABIRDS  IN  ANTARCTIC  WATERS 


19 


centrations  of  1-5  oktas  appeared  to  support  the  greatest  numbers  of  birds.  There  were 
indications  that  older,  thicker  ice  is  preferred  by  birds  because  its  irregular  surface  provides 
shelter  and  its  subsurface  structure  is  inhabited  by  various  invertebrate  prey  items  of  sea- 
birds. 

Because  there  were  few  censuses  in  open  seas,  only  general  indications  of  characteristic 
species  were  obtained.  The  Wilson’s  Storm-Petrel,  Blue  Petrel,  Black-browed  Albatross, 
Gray-headed  Albatross,  Cape  Pigeon,  Light-mantled  Sooty  Albatross,  Wandering  Albatross 
and  Antarctic  Prion  were  sighted  either  entirely  or  mostly  over  open  waters. 

The  relative  abundance  of  adults  and  immatures  in  pelagic  observations  is  unknown  be- 
cause they  are  not  distinguishable  at  sea.  Also,  both  sexually  immature  birds  and  foraging 
breeders  can  be  expected  to  occur  long  distances  from  breeding  sites. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

This  study  was  financed  by  NSF  grant  No.  OPP74-21374,  through  the  United  States  Ant- 
arctic Research  Program  (USARP)  to  D.  F.  Parmelee.  I thank  Dr.  Parmelee  for  his  advice 
and  assistance  throughout  the  project  and  Dr.  G.  Llano  for  his  efforts  in  planning  the  field 
season.  The  entire  crew  of  the  USCGC  Glacier  was  very  helpful,  although  I would  like 
especially  to  thank  Captain  C.  P.  Gillett,  Cdr.  J.  Coste  and  Lt.  B.  Genez  for  their  assistance. 
S.  M.  Kozlechar  assisted  with  observations  on  occasion  and  his  help  is  greatly  appreciated. 
Drs.  T.  Foster  and  J.  Middleton,  and  Lt.  R.  Buhl  offered  helpful  advice  throughout  the 
cruise.  I thank  D.  G.  Ainley,  G.  F.  Barrowclough,  L.  M.  Conroy,  D.  F.  Parmelee,  R.  M. 
Timm  and  H.  B.  Tordoff  for  comments  on  earlier  drafts  of  the  manuscript.  I am  especially 
grateful  to  G.  E.  Watson  for  providing  a detailed  and  critical  review  of  this  paper.  Valuable 
discussions  were  held  with  S.  L.  Frye,  P.  H.  Jacobsma,  H.  Kermott,  N.  E.  Lederer,  R.  J. 
Oehlenschlager,  D.  B.  Siniff,  R.  M.  Timm,  D.  W.  Warner  and  M.  D.  Witt  regarding  various 
aspects  of  this  study.  K.  A.  Kohn  prepared  Fig.  1 and  the  map  on  which  Fig.  2 is  based.  A. 
Fosdick  typed  several  drafts. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Ainley,  D.  G.,  R.  C.  Wood  and  W.  J.  L.  Sladen.  1978.  Bird  life  at  Cape  Crozier,  Ross 
Island.  Wilson  Bull.  90:492-510. 

Alexander,  W.  B.  1954.  Birds  of  the  ocean.  2nd  rev.  ed.  Putman,  New  York,  New  York. 
Beck,  J.  R.  1969.  Food,  moult  and  age  of  first  breeding  in  the  Cape  Pigeon,  Daption 
capensis  Linnaeus.  Br.  Ant.  Surv.  Bull.  21:33-44. 

Bierman,  W.  H.  and  K.  H.  Voous.  1950.  Birds  observed  and  collected  during  the  whaling 
expeditions  of  the  ‘Willem  Barendsz’  in  the  Antarctic,  1946-1947  and  1947-1948.  Ardea 
37:1-123. 

Brook,  D.  and  J.  R.  Beck.  1972.  Antarctic  Petrels,  Snow  Petrels,  and  South  Polar  Skuas 
breeding  in  the  Theron  Mountains.  Br.  Ant.  Surv.  Bull.  27:131-137. 

Cline,  D.  R.,  D.  B.  Siniff  and  A.  W.  Erickson.  1969.  Summer  birds  of  the  pack  ice  in 
the  Weddell  Sea,  Antarctica.  Auk  86:701-716. 

Darby,  M.  M.  1970.  Summer  seabirds  between  New  Zealand  and  McMurdo  Sound.  Notornis 
17:28-55. 

Erickson,  A.  W.,  J.  R.  Gilbert,  G.  A.  Petrides,  R.  J.  Oehlenschlager,  A.  S.  Sinha 
AND  J.  Otis.  1972.  Populations  of  seals,  whales  and  birds  in  the  Bellingshausen  and 
Amundsen  seas.  Ant.  J.  of  the  U.S.  7:70-72. 

Falla,  R.  A.  1964.  Distribution  patterns  of  birds  in  the  antarctic  and  high  latitude  suhant- 
arctic.  Pp.  367-378  in  Biologie  Antarctique  (R.  Garrick  et  ah,  eds.).  Hermann,  Paris, 
France. 


20 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Gain,  L.  1914.  Oiseaux  antarctiques.  Deuxieme  Exped.  Ant.  Fr.  1908-1910,  2:1-100. 
Holgerson,  H.  1945.  Antarctic  and  sub-antarctic  birds.  Sci.  Result.  Norw.  Ant.  Exped. 
1927-28,  2(23):  1-100. 

. 1957.  Ornithology  of  the  ‘Brategg’  Expedition.  Sci.  Result.  ‘Brategg'  Exped.  1947- 

48,  4:1-80. 

Johnstone,  G.  W.  and  K.  R.  Kerry.  1974.  Ornithological  observations  in  the  Australian 
sector  of  the  southern  ocean.  Proc.  16th  Int.  Ornithol.  Congr.,  Canberra,  Australia. 
Murphy,  R.  C.  1936.  Oceanic  birds  of  South  America.  Vol.  1,  Am.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.,  New 
York,  New  York. 

Parmelee,  D.  F.  1977.  Adaptations  of  Arctic  Terns  and  Antarctic  Terns  within  Antarctic 
ecosystems.  Pp.  687-702  in  Adaptations  within  Antarctic  ecosystems.  Gulf  Publ.  Co., 
Houston,  Texas. 

, W.  R.  Fraser  and  D.  R.  Neilson.  1977.  Birds  of  the  Palmer  Station  area.  Ant. 

J.  of  the  U.S.  12:14-21. 

Routh,  M.  1949.  Ornithological  observations  in  the  antarctic  seas.  Ibis  91:577-606. 

SiPLE,  P.  A.  AND  A.  Lindsey.  1937.  Ornithology  of  the  second  Byrd  Antarctic  expedition. 
Auk  54:147-159. 

Snedecor,  G.  W.  and  W.  G.  Cochran.  1967.  Statistical  methods.  The  Iowa  State  Univ. 
Press,  Ames,  Iowa. 

Spellerberg,  I.  F.  1971.  Arrival  and  departure  of  birds  at  McMurdo  Sound,  Antarctica. 
Emu  71:161-171. 

SziJJ,  L.  J.  1967.  Notes  on  the  winter  distribution  of  birds  in  the  western  Antarctic  and 
adjacent  Pacific  waters.  Auk  84:366-378. 

VOOUS,  K.  H.  1965.  Antarctic  birds.  Pp.  649-689  in  Biogeography  and  ecology  in  Antarctica 
(P.  van  Oye  and  J.  van  Mieghem,  eds.).  Dr.  Junk,  The  Hague,  Holland. 

Warham,  j.,  B.  R.  Keeley  and  G.  J.  Wilson.  1977.  Breeding  of  the  Mottled  Petrel.  Auk 
94:1-17. 

Watson,  G.  E.  1975.  Birds  of  the  Antarctic  and  Sub-antarctic.  Am.  Geophys.  Union,  Wash- 
ington, D.C. 

, J.  P.  Angle,  P.  C.  Harper,  M.  A.  Bridge,  R.  P.  Schlatter,  W.  L.  N.  Tickell, 

J.  C.  Boyd  and  \1.  M.  Boyd.  1971.  Ant.  Map  Folio  Series  14:1-18. 

Zink,  R.  M.  1978.  Birds  of  the  Weddell  Sea.  Ant.  J.  of  the  U.S.  13:142-145. 

J.  F.  BELL  MUSEUM  OF  NATURAL  HISTORY,  UNIV.  MINNESOTA,  MINNEAPOLIS, 
MINNESOTA  55455.  (PRESENT  ADDRESS:  MUSEUM  VERTEBRATE  ZOOLOGY, 
2593  LIFE  SCIENCES  BLDG.,  UNIV.  CALIFORNIA,  BERKELEY,  CALIFORNIA 
94720.)  ACCEPTED  5 APR.  1979. 


COLOR  PLATE 

The  color  plate  ITontispiece  of  the  Antarctic  Petrel  (Thalassoica  antarctica)  has  been 
made  possible  by  an  endowment  established  by  George  Miksch  Sutton.  Joseph  R.  Jehl,  Jr. 
provided  the  photograph. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  21-41 


DISPLAY  BEHAVIOR  OF  OVENBIRDS  {SEIURUS 
AUROCAPILLUS)  II.  SONG  VARIATION 
AND  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 

M.  Ross  Lein 


Song  of  the  Ovenbird  {Seiurus  aurocapillus)  is  a characteristic  sound  of 
late  spring  and  early  summer  in  woodlands  over  much  of  North  America. 
John  Burroughs  (1871)  first  used  the  onomatopoeic  phrase  teacher  to  de- 
scribe its  apparently  double-syllabled  phrases,  characterizing  it  as  a series 
of  repetitions  of  this  phrase,  beginning  softly  and  building  in  a crescendo. 
Although  the  Ovenbird  was  the  subject  of  an  intensive  life-history  study 
(Hann  1937)  and  of  experimental  investigations  of  song  recognition  (Wee- 
den  and  Falls  1959,  Falls  1963),  there  has  been  no  detailed  examination 
of  song  variation  within  local  populations,  or  of  the  role  of  song  in  behav- 
ioral interactions. 

In  addition,  the  Ovenbird  has  a second  song,  often  referred  to  as  the 
“flight  song,”  heard  far  less  frequently  than  the  normal  teacher  song. 
Many  authors  have  commented  on  this  display  (Burroughs  1871,  Gibbs 
1885,  Wright  1913,  Allen  1919,  Hann  1937,  Kendeigh  1945),  but  it  has  not 
been  described  carefully  on  the  basis  of  tape  recordings  and  its  signifi- 
cance has  been  largely  a matter  of  speculation. 

I previously  have  described  the  nature  and  use  of  non-song  vocalizations 
of  breeding  Ovenbirds  (Lein  1980).  This  paper  describes  the  song  variation 
of  local  populations,  and  documents  singing  behavior  in  detail. 

METHODS 

The  study  areas  and  methods  are  deseribed  in  a previous  paper  (Lein  1980)  and  are 
summarized  only  briefly  here.  Fieldwork  was  conducted  in  1970,  1971  and  1972  in  Bedford, 
Middlesex  Co.,  Massachusetts  and  in  South  Lyndehoro,  Hillsboro  Co.,  New  Hampshire.  The 
habitat  consisted  of  second-growth  mixed  forest  in  both  areas. 

Detailed  behavioral  observations  were  made  on  5-10  pairs  of  Ovenbirds  each  season, 
including  9 individually  color-banded  males.  More  casual  observations  were  made  on  nu- 
merous other  males.  Individual  males  are  identified  by  a 2-letter  species  designation  (OB), 
plus  a suffix  designating  the  individual  (OB-A,  OB-B,  etc.).  Behavior  was  recorded  on  a 
cassette  recorder  and  subsequently  transcribed  for  analysis.  Short-term  singing  rates  (songs 
per  min)  were  calculated  from  the  time  required  for  a bird  to  sing  10  complete  songs,  and 
were  normally  made  only  during  regular  singing  not  interrupted  by  other  activity.  Long-term 
singing  rates  (songs  per  h)  during  different  phases  of  the  breeding  cycle  were  calculated 
from  the  number  of  songs  males  sang  during  entire  observation  periods.  High-fidelity  re- 
cordings of  songs  for  audiospectrographic  analysis  were  made  with  a Nagra  HIB  tape  recorder 
and  a Norelco  D-119ES  cardioid  dynamic  microphone  and  analysed  with  a Kay  Elemetrics 
6061B  audiospectrograph.  Because  of  background  noise  and  echos  in  recordings,  I use  trac- 


21 


22 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


\l 


'1  M 'I  ' 1 '1  '1 

\ \ \ \ \ 1^ 


■ c 

1.0  2.0 
sec 

Fig.  1.  Complete  “primary  songs"  of  male  Ovenbirds:  (A)  song  of  OB-A;  (B)  song  of  OB- 
C;  (C)  song  of  an  unhanded  male.  Note  that  the  fifth  and  tenth  phrases  differ  from  the  others; 
only  5 songs  hy  this  male,  out  of  all  songs  recorded  in  this  study,  showed  this  phenomenon. 


ings  for  illustration,  rather  than  the  actual  sonagrams  themselves.  Terms  describing  the 
structure  of  the  songs  are  used  in  the  manner  proposed  by  Shiovitz  (1975). 

RESULTS 
Song  variation 

Male  Ovenbirds  have  2 vocalizations  referred  to  as  song:  the  normal 
“primary”  or  “territorial  song”  (Thorpe  1961),  and  the  song  referred  to  in 
the  literature  as  the  “flight  song.”  This  latter  name  is  inappropriate  be- 
cause the  vocalization  is  often  given  when  the  bird  is  not  in  flight.  There- 
fore, 1 refer  to  this  song  as  “attenuated  song”  and  restrict  the  term  “flight 
song”  to  those  performed  during  a stereotyped  aerial  display  (see  below). 

Primary  Song 

Description. — The  “primary  song”  consists  of  a series  of  repetitions  of 
a single  phrase  (Fig.  lA,  B).  Each  phrase  consists  of  3-5  separate  notes. 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


23 


Most  notes  sweep  rapidly  downward  in  frequency  with  a concentration  of 
energy  between  3.0  and  5.0  kHz.  The  highest-pitched  notes  start  at  about 
9.0  kHz  and  some  notes  may  fall  as  low  as  2.5  kHz.  The  phrases  range  in 
duration  from  0.16-0.25  sec  in  different  birds.  The  phrase  is  usually  re- 
peated from  8-13  times  in  complete  songs.  Songs  longer  than  14  phrases 
were  heard  only  rarely,  although  once  a song  of  26  phrases  was  recorded. 
The  phrases  are  separated  by  silent  periods  of  about  0.05-0.1  sec.  Com- 
plete songs  vary  in  length  from  about  2.5— 4.0  sec.  Songs  of  an  individual 
can  vary  considerably  in  length,  however,  depending  on  how  many  repe- 
titions of  the  phrase  are  involved. 

Although  the  first  phrase  of  a song  may  be  separated  from  the  second 
by  an  interval  longer  than  those  occurring  later  in  the  song,  the  rate  of 
delivery  is  usually  constant  after  the  second  phrase.  However,  the  ampli- 
tude increases  for  at  least  the  first  5 or  6 phrases,  producing  the  crescendo 
effect.  The  song  is  harsh  and  certainly  could  not  be  called  musical,  but 
it  is  delivered  at  such  a loud  volume  that  it  carries  long  distances  in  the 
woods. 

Intra-individual  variation. — Each  male  Ovenbird  sings  a single  phrase 
type.  I recorded  songs  of  15  males  repeatedly  during  this  study  (some  over 
several  years),  and  approximately  30  other  males  were  recorded  less  reg- 
ularly. Only  once  did  a male  sing  more  than  1 phrase  pattern.  On  24  May 
1971,  an  unbanded  male  at  Bedford  sang  5 songs  which  included  1 or  2 
phrases  of  a type  other  than  the  predominant  one  (Fig.  1C).  This  occurred 
about  5 min  after  a territorial  encounter  on  the  first  day  that  the  male  was 
on  territory.  The  significance  of  these  circumstances  is  uncertain.  All  other 
songs  of  this  male  consisted  of  a single  phrase  type. 

Complete  songs  of  individuals  showed  minor  variation  in  the  number  of 
phrases.  For  example,  during  4 bouts  of  singing  on  28  May  1971,  OB-I 
sang  82  songs.  These  included  songs  with  the  following  numbers  of  phras- 
es: 7 phrases — 1 song;  8 phrases — 11  songs;  9 phrases — 56  songs;  10 
phrases — 14  songs.  Such  variation  was  typical  of  all  males,  although  the 
modal  song  length  differed  between  birds. 

Incomplete  songs  (arbitrarily  defined  as  songs  of  less  than  8 phrases) 
and  muted  songs  were  sung  in  a variety  of  situations  described  and  ana- 
lyzed in  detail  below.  By  definition,  incomplete  songs  can  vary  from  1-7 
phrases  in  length,  but  songs  of  4 or  5 phrases  predominated.  Muted  songs 
were  delivered  at  a much  lower  volume  than  normal.  Most  incomplete 
songs  are  also  muted  since  they  are  not  long  enough  to  develop  the  cres- 
cendo. 

Inter -individual  variation. — There  is  much  inter-individual  variation  in 
the  form  of  the  phrases  (Fig.  2).  1 did  not  attempt  to  sample  all  males  over 
wide  areas,  but  the  samples  from  both  Bedford  (Fig.  2A-L)  and  South 


24 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


sec 


Fig.  2.  Individual  phrases  from  the  “primary  songs”  of  male  Ovenhirds:  (A-L)  phrases 
from  the  songs  of  12  males  recorded  at  Bedford,  Massachusetts;  (M-X)  phrases  from  the 
songs  of  12  males  recorded  at  South  Lyndeboro,  New  Hampshire. 


Lyndeboro  (Fig.  2M-X)  probably  indicate  the  extent  of  variation  within 
local  populations. 

In  some  cases,  aural  recognition  of  individuals  was  possible  on  the  basis 
of  the  rate  of  delivery  of  the  phrases  or  the  quality  of  the  phrases  them- 
selves. For  example,  OB-G  (Fig.  2J)  sang  a recognizably  more  rapid  song 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


25 


than  did  his  neighbors.  Similarly,  OB-I’s  song  (Fig.  2L)  was  distinguishable 
to  the  ear  by  the  squeaky  quality  of  the  phrases.  Such  aural  discrimination 
could  be  made  with  certainty  only  for  individuals  with  extremes  of  varia- 
tion in  these  song  characters. 

The  situation  differs  for  audiospectrographic  analysis.  With  a recording 
of  reasonable  quality  I could  always  identify  the  singer  by  comparison  with 
sonagrams  of  known  individuals,  even  with  males  sharing  a similar  phrase 
structure  and  whose  songs  could  not  be  distinguished  reliably  by  ear.  For 
example,  OB-D  (Fig.  2F)  and  OB-F  (Fig.  2E)  occupied  neighboring  terri- 
tories. Their  song  phrases  are  very  similar,  but  are  characterized  by  a 
number  of  minor  differences.  The  first  2 notes  of  OB-F’s  phrase  terminate 
at  a lower  frequency  than  those  of  OB-D.  The  down-up-down  inflection  of 
the  third  note  is  more  pronounced  in  OB-D’s  phrase.  Finally,  the  terminal 
note  of  OB-F’s  phrase  begins  at  a lower  frequency  than  that  of  OB-D. 
Repeated  recordings  of  these  color-banded  males  established  that  these 
minor  differences  were  constant  and  could  be  used  to  identify  them.  Sim- 
ilar consistent  differences  exist  for  other  pairs  of  males  with  similar  phras- 
es (see  OB-K  and  OB-J,  Fig.  2Q,  R). 

Attenuated  song 

Description. — “Attenuated  song”  is  a highly  variable  vocalization.  Char- 
acteristically it  is  introduced  by  a series  of  whink  notes  and  a pie-bleep 
vocalization  (for  a description  of  these  calls  see  Lein  1980).  Several  normal 
song  phrases  occur  immediately  or  shortly  after  the  ple-bleep.  This  initial 
part  of  the  song  is  followed  by  a rambling  succession  of  other  notes,  many 
possessing  a large  amount  of  gradual  frequency  modulation  (see  Figs.  3 
and  4).  This  “rambling”  section  of  the  song  includes  1 or  more  chip  notes 
and,  in  some  examples,  additional  “primary  song”  phrases  or  ple-bleep 
notes.  “Attenuated  songs”  are  variable  in  length,  ranging  from  4-7  sec. 

“Attenuated  song”  is  frequently  given  as  part  of  an  aerial  display,  var- 
iously referred  to  as  “love-song”  (Burroughs  1871,  Gross  1953),  “passion 
song”  (Jones  1900)  and  most  frequently  “flight  song”  (Chapman  1907, 
Hann  1937,  Saunders  1951,  Gross  1953,  Gunn  and  Borror  1957).  I use  the 
last  term  to  avoid  the  subjective  implication  of  motivation. 

“Flight  song”  was  heard  frequently  and  was  recorded  on  a number  of 
occasions.  Because  it  was  given  irregularly,  and  most  commonly  at  twi- 
light, the  performance  was  rarely  seen.  The  male  was  initially  perched  in 
a tree,  usually  at  the  height  of  the  subcanopy.  The  male  gave  a series  of 
“soft  sip”  calls  (Lein  1980)  while  perched.  The  rate  of  delivery  of  these 
calls  accelerated  until  the  bird  took  flight  and  climbed  to  3-15  m above 
the  treetops.  He  then  flew  in  a hovering  flight  with  spread  wings  and  tail 


26 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


while  delivering  the  song.  The  flight  appeared  labored  and  the  bird  some- 
times circled  as  it  sang.  Immediately  upon  completion  of  the  song  the  bird 
dropped  back  into  the  woods. 

“Attenuated  song”  was  also  frequently  given  by  perched  males  during 
encounters  with  conspecifics.  While  the  songs  in  such  situations  were 
often  incomplete,  they  were  frequently  as  long  as  those  of  the  “flight  song” 
display,  and  match  the  latter  in  form  (Fig.  4B). 

I ntra -individual  variation. — The  “attenuated  songs”  of  individuals  vary 
within  the  constraints  of  the  above  description.  In  several  cases,  “flight 
songs”  of  the  same  individual  recorded  on  different  dates  can  be  matched 
note  by  note  throughout  their  entire  length,  indicating  that  there  is  some 
stereotypy  in  the  form  of  the  song.  However,  other  examples  of  “flight 
songs”  of  the  same  male  show  differences  in  the  rambling  terminal  portion, 
including  variation  in  the  ordering  of  the  elements,  repetition  of  some 
elements,  or  inclusion  of  a second  series  of  phrases  from  “primary  song” 
(Fig.  3A,  B). 

Inter-individual  variation. — “Attenuated  song”  always  includes  2 in- 
dividually-distinctive  elements:  the  pie-bleep  vocalization  (Lein  1980)  and 
the  “primary  song”  phrases.  It  is  perhaps  significant  that  both  these  ele- 
ments occur  together  during  the  more  stereotyped  initial  portion  of  the 
display.  The  terminal  part  of  the  song  is  more  variable  both  within  and 
between  birds,  but  has  the  same  quality  and  form  in  different  males  and 
there  is  much  overlap  in  elements  between  birds  (Figs.  3,  4). 

Singing  behavior 

Primary  song 

General  pattern  of  singing. — Singing  is  almost  the  only  feature  of  Oven- 
bird  biology  that  uses  the  vertical  aspects  of  the  territories.  The  Ovenbird 
is  a ground-nesting,  ground-foraging  species  and  seldom  perches  in  trees, 
except  when  singing  or  when  alarmed.  Prior  to  female  arrival,  undisturbed 
males  sing  regularly  while  sitting  still  on  perches  near  the  bottom  of  the 
canopy.  Bouts  of  singing  range  from  I or  2 to  more  than  20  min  in  length. 
The  mean  height  (estimated  within  I m)  of  95  Ovenbird  song  perches  was 
8.8  m (SE  = ±0.23  m,  range  = 1. 8-15. 2 m),  while  the  mean  height  of  the 
trees  in  which  they  were  perched  was  15.5  m (SE  = ±0.42  m,  range  = 
3.0-24.5  m).  Bouts  of  song  are  interspersed  with  bouts  of  feeding  on  the 
ground,  during  which  males  are  silent,  or  sing  very  sporadically.  Males 
feeding  on  the  forest  floor  frequently  would  hop  or  flit  up  to  a rock,  stump 
or  twig  before  singing  one  of  these  sporadic  “ground”  songs;  then  they 
would  hop  down  and  continue  foraging.  Several  reasons  may  be  suggested 
for  the  obvious  reluctance  of  males  to  sing  from  the  ground.  First,  singing 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


27 


I 


K 


sec 


Fig.  3.  “Attenuated  songs”  of  male  Ovenbirds,  because  of  their  length,  have  been  broken 
for  the  illustrations.  The  solid  triangles  on  the  time  axis  indicate  the  point  of  overlap  of  the 
upper  and  lower  portions  of  each  song.  (A-B)  “Flight  songs”  of  OB-C.  Note  the  similar 
introduction  of  the  2 examples,  consisting  in  A,  of  2 whink  calls,  1 ple-bleep  call  and  3 
“primary  song”  phrases.  Also  note  the  occurrence  of  the  same  elements  in  different  se- 
quences in  the  terminal  parts  of  the  2 examples. 


28 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Fig.  4.  “Attenuated  Songs  " of  male  Ovenbirds,  because  of  their  length,  have  been  broken 
for  the  illustrations.  ▼ on  the  time  axis  indicate  the  point  of  overlap  of  the  upper  and  lower 
portions  of  each  song.  (A)  “Flight  song"  of  OB-A.  Note  the  similarity  of  the  elements  to  those 
of  the  “flight  songs"  of  OB-C,  shown  in  Fig.  3.  (B)  “Attenuated  song"  of  OB-I  given  from 
the  ground.  Note  the  similarity  in  form  to  the  “flight  songs"  illustrated  in  Fig.  3 and  Fig.  4A. 


on  the  ground  may  attract  predators  to  an  inconspicuously  colored  bird  on 
the  forest  floor.  Second,  songs  delivered  from  the  ground  may  not  carry 
well  because  of  ground  attenuation  and  interference  by  dense  vegetation 
with  sound  transmission  (Wiley  and  Richards  1978).  Song  perches  are  at 
a level  of  the  vegetation  profile  which  has  an  open  structure,  consisting 


Lein  • OVEINBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


29 


Table  1 

Song  Rates  of  Male  Ovenbirds  during  Steady  Singing  in  Relation  to  Other 
Features  of  the  Situation"* 


Situation 

Mean  song  rate*" 
(.songs/min 

± SE) 

Range 

(songs/min) 

No. 

observa- 

tions 

Steady  singing  with  no  disturbance 

3.20  ± 0.06 

1.09-4.44 

73 

Steady  singing  with  preening  activity 

2.80  ± 0.11 

2.20-3.33 

16 

Counter-singing  with  another  male 

3.71  ± 0.12 

2.98-4.41 

12 

Rapid  singing  in  first  morning  bout‘s 

4.24  ± 0.33 

2.51-5.40 

8 

Rapid  singing  during  territorial  encounters*" 

6.36  ± 0.93 

4.05-10.34 

6 

® Short-term  song  rates  were  calculated  from  the  times  required  for  10-song  intervals. 

” Song  rates  differ  significantly  between  situations  (Model  1 one-way  ANOVA,  P < 0.001);  rate  for  steady  singing  with 
no  disturbance  differs  significantly  from  those  for  the  other  situations  (Student-Newman-Keuls  multiple  range  test,  P < 
0.05). 

Both  these  situations  involve  many  incomplete  and/or  muted  songs. 


only  of  trunks  and  dead  branches,  with  very  few  leaves  and  little  shrub- 
bery. 

During  the  bouts  of  steady  singing,  Ovenbirds  sing  at  a rate  of  about  3 
or  4 songs  per  min  (Table  1).  Several  features  of  the  external  situation 
influence  the  song  rate.  Bouts  of  intense  preening  activity  result  in  a slight- 
ly slower  rate,  while  several  other  factors,  such  as  counter-singing  with 
another  male,  increase  the  song  rate  (Table  1).  In  2 situations  in  particular, 
during  bouts  of  song  at  dawn,  and  during  mild  territorial  encounters  (and 
at  the  beginning  and  ending  of  more  intense  encounters),  Ovenbirds  may 
sing  very  rapidly.  The  highest  rate  recorded,  10.34  songs  per  min,  oc- 
curred in  such  an  encounter.  However,  in  both  of  these  situations,  most 
of  the  songs  given  were  incomplete  and  many  were  muted. 

Counter-singing  (Armstrong  1963)  was  noted  frequently.  Two  neighbor- 
ing males  would  sing  in  phase,  the  songs  of  1 male  following  immediately 
after,  or  overlapping  with,  the  songs  of  the  leading  male.  Several  facts 
suggest  that  this  is  not  merely  the  fortuitous  result  of  2 males  singing  at 
approximately  the  same  rate.  First,  the  singing  rate  of  such  birds  was 
higher  than  during  normal  singing  (Table  1).  Second,  the  relationship  of 
“song  a — song  b — pause — song  a — song  b — pause  ...”  could  continue  for 
30-40  songs.  When  the  relationship  changed  it  was  not  due  to  the  2 birds 
drifting  out  of  phase,  as  would  be  expected  if  it  were  fortuitous,  but  in 
many  instances  by  the  “b”  bird  not  “waiting”  for  “song  a,”  but  rather 
singing  his  own  song  toward  the  end  of  the  pause.  In  such  cases,  the  birds 
would  sometimes  continue  to  counter-sing  with  the  lead  reversed.  This 
demonstrates  that  the  males  are  “paying  attention”  to  the  songs  of  neigh- 
boring males  and  responding  to  them,  even  at  a considerable  distance. 


30 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  2 

Long-term  Singing  Rates  of  Male  Ovenbirds  in  Relation  to  the  Phase  of  the 

Breeding  Cycle 


Phase  in  cycle 

No. 

observations'* 

Singing  rate** 
(songs/h  ± SE) 

Observation  time 
(min) 

Unmated 

21 

139.4  ± 9.7 

633 

Courtship  period 

18 

65.9  ± 10.2 

692 

Incubation  period 

13 

85.4  ± 12.6 

456 

Nestling  period 

9 

87.9  ± 13.7 

299 

Fledgling  period  and  later 

8 

26.9  ± 5.8 

296 

“ All  observations  during  morning  activity  period. 

Singing  rates  differ  significantly  between  phases  of  the  breeding  cycle  (Model  1 one-way  ANOVA,  P < 0.001);  rates  for 
unmated  and  fledgling  periods  differ  from  those  of  other  periods  (Student-Newman-Keuls  multiple  range  test,  P < 0.05); 
rates  for  courtship,  incubation  and  nestling  period  do  not  differ  significantly. 


Daily  pattern  of  singing. — The  effects  of  daily  activity  rhythms  on  sing- 
ing behavior  have  been  well-described  (Thorpe  1961,  Armstrong  1963).  As 
in  other  birds,  Ovenbirds  show  an  activity  peak  in  early  morning  and  a 
second,  smaller  peak  in  the  evening.  The  morning  peak  is  more  pro- 
nounced early  in  the  breeding  season,  when  the  birds  are  very  active  until 
noon.  Later  in  the  season  strong  singing  becomes  more  restricted  to  early 
hours.  Extremely  hot  or  cold  weather  inhibits  singing,  as  does  heavy  rain. 
Early  in  the  season,  however,  birds  sing  strongly  during  light  rain. 

Many  workers  have  shown  that  the  beginning  of  song  at  dawn,  and  to 
a lesser  extent  its  cessation  at  dusk,  is  closely  correlated  with  the  light 
intensity  (Armstrong  1963).  The  times  of  first  songs  of  Ovenbirds  gradually 
became  earlier  until  the  longest  days  of  June,  then  became  later  as  the 
summer  progressed.  A comparable  relationship  in  the  time  of  the  last  song 
of  the  evening  was  also  noted. 

Light  intensity  appears  to  influence  singing  in  other  ways  as  well.  The 
songs  at  the  beginning  of  the  initial  bouts  of  the  morning  were  character- 
istically incomplete  and/or  muted.  This  was  also  true  of  late  evening  sing- 
ing. Light  level  is  also  important  in  determining  the  occurrence  of  “flight 
song”  (see  below). 

Seasonal  pattern  of  singing. — A seasonal  decline  in  singing  is  known  for 
many  species  (Thorpe  1961,  Armstrong  1963).  Many  studies  have  dem- 
onstrated that  the  long-term  singing  rates  of  a male  bird  are  related  not 
to  the  calendar  date  per  se,  but  rather  to  the  stage  of  the  breeding  cycle 
(see  summary  in  Armstrong  1963:152-156).  Since  the  breeding  cycles  of 
individual  males  are  usually  out  of  phase  with  one  another,  studies  which 
report  only  population  averages  in  relation  to  calendar  date  may  miss 
important  and  abrupt  changes. 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


31 


The  long-term  singing  rates  of  male  Ovenbirds  at  various  phases  in  the 
nesting  cycle  are  shown  in  Table  2.  All  figures  are  based  on  observations 
made  during  the  morning  activity  period  (04:30-10:00  EDT).  Unmated 
males  sing  an  average  of  139.4  songs  per  h.  If  they  were  singing  at  the 
mean  rate  calculated  for  undisturbed  birds,  3.2  songs  per  min  (Table  1), 
this  would  represent  steady  singing  for  about  75%  of  the  time. 

Upon  arrival  of  females,  however,  singing  rate  drops  significantly  to  65.9 
songs  per  h,  a decrease  of  over  50%.  The  decline  is  related  to  the  male’s 
close  association  with  the  female  at  this  time.  During  such  activity  he 
sings  only  sporadically  and  many  of  the  songs  are  incomplete  or  muted, 
a feature  rarely  observed  prior  to  female  arrival. 

Once  incubation  begins,  the  singing  rate  may  increase  slightly,  but  it 
never  regains  the  level  reached  prior  to  mating.  Song  bouts  are  much  more 
irregular  during  incubation  and  feeding  of  young.  By  early  July  regular 
singing  is  heard  only  for  a short  period  at  dawn. 

Incomplete  song. — The  situations  in  which  incomplete  songs  were  used 
range  from  the  occurrence  of  1 or  2 incomplete  songs  during  bouts  of 
regular  singing,  to  periods  of  up  to  1 h when  a male  sang  incomplete  songs 
repeatedly  while  moving  with  his  mate  (Table  3).  Half  the  observations 
involved  association  of  paired  males  and  females.  Most  of  these  were 
during  courtship;  contact  between  mates  during  the  incubation  or  nestling 
periods  is  similar  to  that  during  courtship,  although  much  less  frequent  or 
prolonged.  Males  used  incomplete  songs  when  at  intermediate  distances 
from  their  mates.  When  within  7 m or  less  of  females,  males  rarely  sang 
at  all  (with  the  exception  of  “attenuated  song,”  see  below).  When  more 
than  25-30  m from  their  mates,  males  usually  sang  full  songs.  Males 
changed  from  incomplete  to  full  song  (11  cases)  as  they  moved  away  from 
their  mates.  Switching  from  full  to  incomplete  song  (7  cases),  and  from 
incomplete  song  to  silence  (9  cases),  occurred  as  mates  moved  closer 
together.  Males  gave  incomplete  songs  on  4 occasions  in  association  with 
chases  of  their  mates. 

Incomplete  songs  were  a regular  feature  of  territorial  encounters  be- 
tween males.  Males  gave  incomplete  songs  at  either  a normal  or  a reduced 
and  irregular  rate  during  male-female  interactions;  during  male-male  en- 
counters there  frequently  was  an  elevated  rate  of  singing  (Table  1).  In 
some  encounters,  males  sang  incomplete  songs  almost  continuously,  with 
very  short  pauses  between  them.  Incomplete  songs  were  not  used  during 
intense  interactions  involving  vigorous  or  prolonged  chasing;  these  were 
characterized  by  silence  or  by  the  use  of  non-song  vocalizations  such  as 
chep,  whink,  ple-bleep  and  pink  (Lein  1980).  Incomplete  song  was  used 
during  mild  encounters,  or  at  the  beginning  or  end  of  encounters,  or  during 
pauses  between  bouts  of  active  chasing. 


32 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  I,  March  1981 


Table  3 

Summary  of  Situations  in  Which  Male  Ovenbirds  Used  Incomplete  Songs 

Situation 

No.  cases 

Major  category 

Subcategory® 

Male-female  interactions 

49 

During  association 

49 

During  male-female  chasing 

4 

Male-male  interactions 

15 

During  territorial  encounter 

14 

During  male-male  chasing 

3 

Response  to  approach  of  male 

2 

At  territorial  boundary 

1 

Non-encounter  situations 

34 

During  dawn  or  dusk  singing 

27 

While  carrying  food  to  young 

2 

During  vigorous  preening 

3 

At  end  of  singing  hout 

2 

Total 

98 

® Total  for  subcategories  may  be  greater  than  number  of  cases  in  major  category  since  1 observation  may  fall  into  several 
subcategories. 


Birds  also  switched  from  full  to  incomplete  songs  in  other  circum- 
stances. Several  times  a male  began  to  sing  incomplete  songs  when  a 
neighbor,  who  had  been  silent  or  singing  at  a distance,  suddenly  ap- 
proached the  mutual  territory  boundary.  Encounters  did  not  ensue  in  these 
instances  and  the  males  resumed  singing  full  songs  when  they  separated. 
Once,  a male  switched  to  incomplete  song  when  he  moved  near  a known 
territorial  boundary.  The  neighbor  was  not  singing  at  the  time  although  I 
had  previously  observed  several  territorial  encounters  in  the  same  area. 
The  bird  resumed  full  song  again  when  he  moved  away  from  the  boundary. 

Incomplete  song  was  noted  on  34  occasions  not  involving  interactions. 
Twenty-seven  cases  occurred  during  the  first  bouts  of  song  in  the  morning 
or  during  late  evening  singing.  In  view  of  the  influence  of  light  level  on 
the  onset  or  cessation  of  daily  song,  it  is  not  surprising  that  the  birds 
should  show  a transition  from  silence  to  incomplete  song  to  full  song  during 
the  morning  twilight,  or  vice  versa  in  the  evening.  An  important  difference 
was  that  the  morning  performance  usually  involved  incomplete  songs  given 
very  rapidly  and  regularly  (Table  1),  whereas  the  singing  in  the  evening 
was  typically  slow  and  sporadic. 

Other  non-encounter  circumstances  in  which  incomplete  song  was  used 
suggest  that  they  were  correlated  with  a conflict  between  a tendency  to 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


33 


Table  4 

Summary  of  Situations  in  Which  Male  Ovenbirds  Used  Muted  Songs 

Situation 

No.  cases 

Major  category 

Subcategory^ 

Male-female  interactions 

19 

During  association 

19 

During  male-female  chasing 

2 

Male-male  interactions 

10 

During  territorial  encounter 

8 

At  territorial  boundary 

2 

Non-encounter  situations 

4 

During  dawn  singing 

2 

While  carrying  food  to  young 

1 

Response  to  playback  of  song 

1 

Total 

33 

® Total  for  subcategories  may  be  greater  than  number  of  cases  in  major  category  since  1 observation  may  fall  into  several 
subcategories. 


sing  and  a tendency  to  carry  out  some  other  activity.  In  3 cases,  males 
sang  incomplete  songs  during  vigorous  preening  while  on  a song  perch; 
when  the  preening  ceased,  full  song  was  resumed.  Twice  incomplete  songs 
came  at  the  end  of  a bout  of  full  songs,  just  before  the  male  ceased  singing 
and  began  to  forage.  In  2 other  instances,  a male  sang  incomplete  songs 
while  foraging  and  carrying  food  to  its  young.  Males  were  typically  silent 
during  this  activity. 

Muted  songs. — Incomplete  songs  frequently  sound  muted,  but  since  nor- 
mal songs  increase  in  volume  for  the  first  4 or  5 phrases  it  is  impossible 
];  to  determine  this  with  certainty.  Therefore,  only  songs  of  5 or  more  phras- 
I es  are  considered  here.  The  situations  in  which  muted  songs  were  record- 
ed (Table  4)  are  similar  to  those  in  which  incomplete  songs  were  employed 
, (Table  3).  Therefore,  I will  not  consider  the  uses  of  muted  song  in  detail. 

I believe  that  muted  song  is  intermediate  in  motivation  between  incom- 
plete song  and  full  song,  or  that  it  represents  a “less  inhibited”  variation 
[ of  full  song  than  does  incomplete  song.  On  several  occasions,  the  first 
^ morning  bout  of  singing  of  a male  began  with  a series  of  incomplete  (and 

*1  certainly  muted)  songs.  These  gradually  lengthened  until  they  were  rec- 

* ognizable  as  complete,  but  still  muted,  songs.  Then  there  was  a gradual 
^ increase  in  volume  until  the  male  was  singing  normal  volume,  full  songs. 

I believe  that  this  represents  the  usual  pattern  of  onset  of  song  in  the 
I?  morning,  but  I was  rarely  close  enough  to  a singing  male  to  ascertain  that 


34 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  5 

Distribution  of  Ovenbird  Flight  Songs  by  Time  of  Day 

Beginning  h 

^ No.  displays'* 

Beginning  h 

No.  displays 

01:00 

0 

13:00 

6 

02:00 

1 

14:00 

6 

03:00 

0 

15:00 

2 

04:00 

23 

16:00 

5 

05:00 

0 

17:00 

10 

06:00 

1 

18:00 

8 

07:00 

3 

19:00 

53 

08:00 

0 

20:00 

102 

09:00 

3 

21:00 

0 

10:00 

1 

22:00 

0 

11:00 

0 

23:00 

1 

12:00 

0 

24:00 

0 

® All  times  are  Eastern  Daylight  Time. 
Total  of  225  displays  recorded. 


the  intermediate  songs  were  really  muted.  Similar  transitions  from  full  to 
muted  to  incomplete  songs  were  occasionally  noted  at  the  beginning  of 
male-male  or  male-female  interactions;  switching  in  these  situations  was 
often  abrupt  and  frequently  omitted  1 or  more  of  the  intermediate  steps 
from  full  song  to  silence. 

Attenuated  song 

Flight  song. — 1 observed  or  heard  225  performances  of  the  Ovenbird 
“flight  song”  display.  There  is  a major  concentration  of  displays  at  dusk 
(Table  5),  with  155  (68.9%)  occurring  between  19:00  and  21:00.  Most 
“flight  songs”  heard  in  an  evening  occurred  in  a 15-20  min  interval  when 
it  was  quite  dark  in  the  woods,  although  the  sky  was  fairly  bright  and 
visibility  was  good  in  open  areas.  A second,  smaller  peak  occurs  at  sunrise. 
Similar  daily  patterns  of  distribution  of  Ovenbird  “flight  songs”  have  been 
reported  by  Hann  (1937)  and  Eaton  (1957). 

Because  “flight  songs”  were  noted  in  the  course  of  other  activities  and 
there  were  no  controlled  observation  periods,  it  is  difficult  to  assess  the 
remainder  of  the  distribution  pattern.  The  twilight  peaks  are  so  pro- 
nounced as  to  leave  no  question  as  to  their  reality.  The  rarity  of  “flight 
songs”  during  the  morning  is  also  real,  since  the  majority  of  my  fieldwork 
was  between  05:00  and  11:00.  “Flight  songs”  may  occur  at  night  (21:00— 
03:00)  more  frequently  than  recorded  since  almost  no  observations  were 
made  during  this  period. 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


35 


Table  6 

Summary  of  Situations  in  Which  Male  Ovenbirds  Used 
(Excluding  Flight  Song) 

“Attenuated  Song” 

Situation 

No.  cases 

Major  category  Subcategory“ 

Male-female  interactions 

17 

During  association 

17 

During  male-female  chasing 

6 

During  attack  by  male 

2 

During  flight  toward  female 

1 

During  copulation  attempts 

4 

Male-male  interactions 

13 

During  territorial  encounter 

13 

During  male-male  chasing 

6 

After  chasing 

1 

At  end  of  encounter 

1 

Non-encounter  situations 

3 

At  dawn  or  dusk 

2 

Late  in  breeding  season 

2 

Total 

33 

® Total  for  subcategories  may  be  greater  than  number  of  cases  in  major  category  since  1 observation  may  fall  into  several 
subcategories. 


The  predisposing  influence  of  low  light  intensity  on  the  performance  of 
“flight  song”  is  evident  from  the  twilight  peaks.  This  is  also  suggested  by 
the  occurrence  of  “flight  songs”  during  the  middle  of  drizzly  and  heavily- 
overcast  days.  At  least  9 (20%)  of  the  45  “flight  songs”  recorded  between 
06:00  and  19:00  occurred  during  such  weather.  On  a dark  afternoon,  dur- 
ing light  rain,  I sometimes  heard  several  “flight  songs”  in  an  hour.  This 
never  occurred  during  more  clement  weather. 

Attenuated  song  during  encounters. — Incomplete  “attenuated  songs” 
are  often  given,  but  all  cases  considered  here  included  some  of  the  “ram- 
bling” terminal  portion  of  the  song  and  may  thus  be  considered  to  be 
complete.  Of  the  33  instances  when  “attenuated  song”  was  recorded  (ex- 
cluding “flight  song”  performances),  30  involved  interactions  between  con- 
specifics  (Table  6).  Its  use  in  these  situations  is  similar  to  those  of  incom- 
plete and  muted  song,  but  with  one  difference.  Twelve  of  17  records  during 
male-female  interactions  involved  chases,  aggressive  (?)  attacks  by  the 
male,  or  copulation  attempts.  This  is  a much  higher  association  with  very 
intense  interaction  than  was  recorded  for  incomplete  or  muted  song  (see 
Tables  3 and  4). 


36 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


This  association  of  “attenuated  song”  with  high  activity  was  also  noted 
during  male-male  encounters.  Almost  half  (6  of  13)  of  the  records  in  ter- 
ritorial encounters  were  during  aerial  chases.  This  is  in  contrast  to  incom- 
plete or  muted  songs  which  were  rarely  used  during  chases. 

Attenuated  song  during  non-encounter  situations. — On  3 occasions, 
male  Ovenbirds  sang  1 or  more  “attenuated  songs”  from  a perch,  or  from 
the  ground,  in  the  apparent  absence  of  conspecifics  (Table  6).  Twice  it 
occurred  during  the  twilight  of  dusk  or  dawn  when  one  might  expect  to 
hear  “flight  songs.”  The  third  record  was  somewhat  different  and  warrants 
an  extensive  description. 

On  7 July  1971,  1 was  observing  OB-1,  who  had  been  singing  irregularly 
for  at  least  20  min.  At  07:05  he  flew  down  to  the  ground  and  in  the  next 
4 min  sang  at  least  16  “attenuated  songs.”  All  were  muted  in  volume,  but 
were  full-length  and  had  the  form  of  other  “attenuated  songs”  (Fig.  4B). 
Several  times  2 or  more  of  the  songs  were  run  together,  so  that  the  bird 
was  singing  the  soft,  rambling  song  for  periods  of  10-30  sec.  This  perfor- 
mance of  “attenuated  song”  resembles  what  has  been  called  “sub-song” 
(Thorpe  1961:64-70,  Thorpe  and  Pilcher  1958,  Armstrong  1963:58-69). 
Sub-song  in  other  species  is  a low-volume,  rambling  vocalization,  usually 
hearing  little  resemblance  to  the  typical  song,  although  it  frequently  con- 
tains call  notes  and  isolated  elements  of  “primary  song.”  It  may  be  pro- 
duced almost  continuously  for  extended  periods  of  time.  The  similarity  to 
the  performance  of  OB-I  is  striking  and  suggests  that  a relationship  may 
be  involved. 


DISCUSSION 

Song  variation. — The  Ovenbird’s  pattern  of  song  variation  is  a relatively 
simple  one  which  is  exhibited  by  many  passerines  (Borror  1961,  Thielcke 
1969).  Each  male  possesses  1 “primary  song”  pattern,  and  the  song  pat- 
terns of  different  males  in  a local  population  show  considerable  variation. 
This  is  in  contrast  to  the  pattern  shown  by  the  Chestnut-sided  Warbler 
{Dendroica  pensylvanica)  (Lein  1978)  and  some  other  members  of  that 
genus  (Lein  1972;  Morse  1966,  1967,  1970),  in  which  each  male  possesses 
2 or  more  song  patterns  shared  by  all  males  in  a local  population. 

This  type  of  individual  song  variation  may  facilitate  individual  recogni- 
tion of  neighbors’  songs  by  male  Ovenbirds.  The  experiments  of  Weeden 
and  Falls  (1959)  clearly  demonstrated  that  males  discriminate  between  the 
songs  of  familiar  and  strange  birds.  The  aggressive  responses  of  males 
were  more  prompt  and  pronounced  toward  the  songs  of  strangers.  This 
implies  that  territorial  establishment  in  this  species  results  in  a relatively 
stable  relationship  between  neighbors.  The  outcome  is  that  males  are  more 
tolerant  toward  neighbors,  who  presumably  pose  less  of  a threat  to  their 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


37 


territorial  security,  than  they  are  to  strangers,  who  are  likely  to  he  newly- 
arrived  birds  searching  for  a territory.  Subsequent  work  by  Falls  (1963) 
demonstrated  that  the  pitch,  form  and  arrangement  of  the  component 
sounds  of  the  song,  and  the  length  of  the  sounds  and  the  intervals  between 
them,  are  important  in  eliciting  normal  responses  from  male  Ovenbirds 
during  playback  experiments.  These  are  the  features  that  vary  between 
the  songs  of  different  individuals  and  they  probably  form  the  basis  of 
individual  discrimination. 

The  wide  range  of  variation  in  phrase  structure  within  a local  population 
(Fig.  2),  and  the  similarity  of  the  phrases  of  some  individuals  in  different 
populations  (compare  Fig.  2H  and  Q,  and  Fig.  2L  and  V),  suggest  that  a 
system  of  “dialects”  (Marler  and  Tamura  1962,  Baptista  1975)  is  unlikely. 

Little  can  be  said  regarding  the  pattern  of  variation  in  “attenuated  song” 
because  of  the  small  sample  of  recordings.  However,  since  it  contains 
individually-distinctive  “primary  song”  phrases  and  pie-bleep  notes,  it 
probably  shows  patterns  of  variation  comparable  to  those  of  “primary 
song.” 

Singing  behavior. — Several  features  of  the  singing  behavior  of  Oven- 
birds  deserve  comment,  especially  in  relation  to  the  differences  found  in 
a parallel  study  of  the  Chestnut-sided  Warbler  (Lein  1978).  First,  Oven- 
birds  rarely  sing  while  foraging  on  the  ground;  Chestnut-sided  Warblers 
commonly  sing  while  foraging  in  foliage.  Whatever  the  reasons  for  the 
Ovenbird’s  reluctance  to  sing  from  the  ground,  simultaneous  singing  and 
foraging  are  largely  incompatible  in  this  species.  In  contrast,  the  Chestnut- 
sided Warbler  combines  singing  and  foraging  with  only  a slight  decline  in 
song  rate  (Lein  1978)  and  with  little  apparent  interference  with  foraging. 
This  difference  has  interesting  consequences  for  time  and  energy  budgets. 
If  we  assume,  for  the  sake  of  argument,  that  males  of  both  species  require 
the  same  time  and  expend  the  same  energy  in  acquiring  the  food  necessary 
for  subsistence,  then  it  follows  that  Ovenbirds  will  have  less  time  available 
for  territorial  proclamation  than  will  Chestnut-sided  Warblers.  Although 
it  is  improbable  that  this  assumption  is  strictly  true,  it  suggests  a manner 
in  which  the  features  of  the  foraging  niche  of  the  species  could  influence 
territorial  advertisement.  Species  that  can  combine  singing  and  foraging 
may  be  better  suited  to  situations  of  intense  territorial  competition,  or  to 
conditions  which  require  extended  and  continuous  advertisement  for  other 
reasons. 

The  second  difference  is  in  the  influence  of  pair  formation  on  singing 
behavior.  The  large  decline  in  singing  of  Ovenbirds  at  pairing  is  due  to 
the  almost  continuous  association  of  mates  at  this  time,  and  its  inhibitory 
effect  on  song.  In  contrast,  pairing  seems  to  have  little  influence  on  the 
singing  of  the  Chestnut-sided  Warbler,  and  perhaps  on  the  singing  of 


38 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


sexually  dimorphic  warblers  in  general  (Lein  1978,  Morse  1966,  Ficken 
and  Ficken  1969).  Chestnut-sided  Warbler  pairs  spend  relatively  little  time 
in  association  in  the  period  between  female  arrival  and  the  start  of  incu- 
bation (16.4%  of  373  min  of  observations  on  birds  whose  exact  breeding 
status  was  known)  in  contrast  to  the  Ovenbird  (83.4%  of  452  min). 

This  sharp  decline  suggests  that  song  functions  in  attracting  females  to 
unmated  males.  This  is  supported  by  the  observation  that  males  who  lose 
their  mates  resume  singing  in  a manner  similar  to  that  of  newly-arrived 
males.  However,  because  new  males  may  be  arriving  and  establishing 
territories  after  the  first  males  are  already  mated,  song  probably  continues 
to  function  in  territorial  proclamation  and  defense  after  pairing.  The 
amount  of  time  that  a male  Ovenbird  devotes  to  pair-related  activities 
during  the  courtship  period  possibly  decreases  his  efficiency  in  territorial 
defense.  Other  males  frequently  intrude  on  a territory  during  the  courtship 
period,  but  this  may  be  due  to  the  attractiveness  of  the  highly  vocal  fe- 
males to  neighboring  males  at  this  time  (pers.  obs.)  rather  than  to  a decline 
in  the  efficiency  of  defense. 

Communicative  function  of  song. — The  broad  range  of  circumstances  in 
which  “primary  song”  is  used  indicates  that,  as  with  other  Ovenbird  vocal- 
izations (Lein  1980),  it  encodes  rather  general  behavioral  messages  (Smith 
1969,  1977).  It  is  rich  in  identifying  messages,  indicating  that  the  singer 
is  a male  Ovenbird,  in  breeding  condition  and  on  his  territory.  In  addition, 
a recipient  with  previous  experience  may  be  able  to  identify  the  singer  as 
a specific  individual.  By  monitoring  singing  rate  over  a period  of  time,  the 
listener  may  also  be  able  to  determine  the  mating  status  of  the  singing 
male. 

It  is  difficult  to  suggest  which  behavioral  selection  messages  (Smith 
1977)  the  signal  encodes.  Probably  the  message  is  one  relating  to  inter- 
actional behavior,  indicating  that  the  singer  is  prepared  to  interact  in  any 
of  the  ways  typical  of  territorial  male  Ovenbirds.  This  is  perhaps  the  only 
behavioral  selection  message  encoded.  It  could  still  have  a variety  of 
meanings  to  different  recipients.  To  a male  who  was  a potential  intruder 
it  could  signify  a threat;  to  an  unmated  female  it  could  mean  the  presence 
of  a potential  mate. 

Additional  messages  for  muted  or  incomplete  songs  are  equally  difficult 
to  determine.  Although  both  variants  are  used  in  clearly  agonistic  situa- 
tions, they  also  occur  in  circumstances  not  involving  attack  or  escape,  and 
hence  these  agonistic  messages  probably  are  not  encoded.  The  only  gen- 
eral feature  of  their  use  is  that  the  bird  is  experiencing  some  type  of 
inhibition  of,  or  conflict  with,  his  tendency  to  sing.  Hence  they  may  encode 
a message  of  indecisive  behavior.  This  may  provide  information  regarding 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


39 


the  communicator’s  probable  future  behavior,  although  the  meanings  of 
the  signals  are  certainly  dependent  upon  the  class  of  recipient  and  the 
context.  The  frequent  association  of  these  variants  with  interactions  sug- 
gests that  they  may  encode  a supplemental  message  indicating  a greater 
probability  of  interaction  than  that  of  full  “primary  song.” 

“Attenuated  song”  used  during  interactions  probably  encodes  infor- 
mation similar  to  that  borne  by  incomplete  or  muted  “primary  song,”  since 
all  these  vocalizations  are  used  in  similar  circumstances.  In  encounter 
situations  “attenuated  song”  frequently  accompanies  chasing  and  attack. 
However,  its  use  in  other  situations,  such  as  copulation  attempts  and 
“flight  song,”  argues  against  the  encoding  of  a specific  attack  message. 
An  attack  meaning  may  be  available  to  the  recipient  from  the  context  of 
the  signal.  The  main  difference  between  the  messages  of  “attenuated 
song”  and  muted  or  incomplete  songs  probably  relates  to  differences  in 
the  supplemental  messages  of  probability  and  intensity  of  the  behavioral 
selection.  “Attenuated  song”  may  indicate  a higher  probability  of  a more 
intense  interaction,  either  aggressive  or  sexual. 

It  seems  impossible  to  assess  the  communicatory  function  of  the  “flight 
song,”  regardless  of  how  dramatic  it  is.  Its  rare  occurrence  and  peculiar 
situation  of  use,  plus  the  obvious  influence  of  low  light  level  in  its  elici- 
tation, make  it  difficult  to  suggest  its  function. 

SUMMARY 

Song  variation  and  singing  behavior  of  Ovenbirds  were  studied  at  2 locations  in  New 
England.  Each  male  possesses  a single,  distinctive  “primary  song,”  which  may  also  be  given 
in  muted  or  incomplete  forms.  Males  also  possess  a second  type  of  song,  “attenuated  song,” 
often  used  as  part  of  an  aerial  display  (“flight  song”). 

Prior  to  pair  formation,  male  Ovenbirds  sing  strongly  from  perches  near  the  bottom  of  the 
forest  canopy.  Few  songs  are  given  from  the  ground,  where  the  majority  of  foraging  occurs. 
There  is  a sharp  decline  in  singing  at  pairing,  the  result  of  the  inhibitory  effect  of  the  female’s 
presence  on  her  mate’s  singing.  Muted  and  incomplete  “primary  songs“  are  associated  with 
both  male-male  and  male-female  interactions,  but  are  used  in  other  situations  as  well.  “Flight 
song”  is  given  primarily  at  dusk  and  dawn  but  may  occur  at  other  times,  particularly  during 
heavy  overcast.  Other  performances  of  “attenuated  song”  are  associated  almost  entirely  with 
very  intense  interactions. 

The  relatively  simple  pattern  of  song  variation  described  may  facilitate  individual  recog- 
nition of  neighbors’  songs  by  males.  The  incompatibility  of  singing  and  foraging  in  this 
species  suggests  that  features  of  the  foraging  niche  may  influence  the  ability  of  birds  to 
advertise  territories.  Similarly,  the  reduction  in  singing  produced  by  the  extensive  courtship 
interactions  may  affect  the  efficiency  of  territorial  defense  at  this  time. 

The  songs  of  Ovenbirds  appear  to  be  rich  in  the  identifying  messages  they  encode,  but 
their  message  regarding  future  behavioral  selections  is  probably  very  general,  indicating  only 
that  the  singer  is  prepared  to  interact  in  any  manner  typical  of  territorial  male  Ovenbirds. 
Muted  and  incomplete  songs,  and  “attenuated  songs,”  probably  encode  different  supple- 
mental messages  about  the  probability  and  intensity  of  interaction. 


40 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Professor  Ernst  Mayr  supervised  this  study,  and  encouraged  me  in  all  its  phases.  He  and 
Mrs.  Mayr  graciously  allowed  me  to  use  their  summer  retreat  in  New  Hampshire  as  a study 
area.  W.  John  Smith  provided  valuable  assistance  with  techniques  and  theoretical  ap- 
proaches to  animal  communication.  M.  S.  Ficken  and  R.  Zach  improved  the  manuscript  with 
their  comments.  Facilities  and  equipment  were  made  available  by  the  Museum  of  Compar- 
ative Zoology,  Harvard  University.  Financial  support  was  provided  by  Harvard  University, 
the  Carl  A.  and  Katharine  F.  Richmond  Fund,  and  National  Science  Foundation  Grants 
GB7346  and  GB19922  to  Harvard  University  (R.  C.  Rollins,  Principal  Investigator). 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Allen,  F.  H.  1919.  The  evolution  of  bird  song.  Auk  36:528-536. 

Armstrong,  E.  A.  1963.  A study  of  bird  song.  Oxford  Univ.  Press,  London,  England. 
Baptista,  L.  F.  1975.  Song  dialects  and  demes  in  sedentary  populations  of  the  White- 
crowned  Sparrow  (Zonotrichia  leucophrys  nuttalli).  Univ.  Calif.  Publ.  Zool.  105:1-52. 
Borror,  D.  J.  1961.  Intraspecific  variation  in  passerine  bird  songs.  Wilson  BuU.  73:57-78. 
Burroughs,  J.  1871.  Wake-robin.  Hurd  and  Houghton,  New  York,  New  York. 

Chapman,  F.  M.  1907.  The  warblers  of  North  America.  D.  Appleton  and  Co.,  New 
York,  New  York. 

Eaton,  S.  W.  1957.  A life  history  study  of  Seiurus  noveboracensis.  St.  Bonaventure  Univ., 
Sci.  Stud.  19:7-36. 

Falls,  J.  B.  1963.  Properties  of  bird  song  eliciting  responses  from  territorial  males.  Proc. 
13th  Int.  Ornithol.  Congr.  259-271. 

Ficken,  M.  S.  and  R.  W.  Ficken.  1969.  Responses  of  Blue-winged  Warblers  and  Golden- 
winged Warblers  to  their  own  and  the  other  species’  song.  Wilson  Bull.  81:69-74. 
Gibbs,  M.  1885.  Song  of  the  Golden-crowned  Thrush.  (Siurus  auricapillus).  Ornithol.  and 
Ool.  10:191-192. 

Gross,  A.  O.  1953.  Eastern  Ovenhird.  Pp.  457-476  in  Life  histories  of  North  American 
wood  warblers  (A.  C.  Bent,  ed.).  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  203. 

Gunn,  W.  W.  H.  and  D.  j.  Borror.  1957.  Interpretations  of  some  warbler  songs.  Pp.  26- 
36  in  I'he  warblers  of  America.  (L.  Griscom  and  A.  Sprunt,  Jr.,  eds.).  Devin-Adair  Co., 
New  York,  New  York. 

Hann,  H.  W.  1937.  Life  history  of  the  Oven-bird  in  southern  Michigan.  Wilson  Bull.  49:145- 
237. 

Jones,  L.  1900.  Warbler  songs  (Mniotiltidae).  Wilson  Bull.  12:1-57. 

Kendeigh,  S.  C.  1945.  Nesting  behavior  of  wood  warblers.  Wilson  Bull.  57:145-164. 

Lein,  M.  R.  1972.  Territorial  and  courtship  songs  of  birds.  Nature  237:48-49. 

. 1978.  Song  variation  in  a population  of  Chestnut-sided  Warblers  (Dendroica  pen- 

sylvanica):  its  nature  and  suggested  significance.  Can.  J.  Zool.  56:1266-1283. 

. 1980.  Display  behavior  of  Ovenhirds  {Seiurus  aurocapillus).  I.  Non-song  vocaliza- 
tions. Wilson  Bull.  92:312-329. 

Marler,  P.  and  M.  Tamura.  1962.  Song  “dialects”  in  three  populations  of  White-crowned 
Sparrows.  Condor  64:368-377. 

Morse,  D.  H.  1966.  The  context  of  songs  in  the  Yellow  Warbler.  WJlson  Bull.  78:444-455. 

. 1967.  The  contexts  of  song  in  Black-throated  Green  and  Blackburnian  warblers. 

Wilson  Bull.  79:64-74. 

. 1970.  Territorial  and  courtship  songs  of  birds.  Nature  226:659-661. 

Saunders,  A.  A.  1951.  A guide  to  bird  songs.  Rev.  Ed.  Douhleday  and  Co.,  Inc.  Garden 
City,  New  York. 


Lein  • OVENBIRD  SINGING  BEHAVIOR 


41 


Shiovitz,  K.  a.  1975.  The  process  of  species-specific  song  recognition  by  the  Indigo  Bunt- 
ing, Passerina  cyanea,  and  its  relationship  to  the  organization  of  avian  acoustical  be- 
haviour. Behaviour  55:128-179. 

Smith,  W.  J.  1969.  Messages  of  vertebrate  communication.  Science  165:145-150. 

. 1977.  The  behavior  of  communicating.  Harvard  Univ.  Press,  Cambridge,  Massa- 
chusetts. 

Thielcke,  G.  1969.  Geographic  variation  in  bird  vocalizations.  Pp.  311-339  in  Bird  vocal- 
izations (R.  A.  Hinde,  ed.).  Cambridge  Univ.  Press,  Cambridge,  England. 

Thorpe,  W.  H.  1961.  Bird-song.  Cambridge  Univ.  Press,  Cambridge,  England. 

AND  P.  M.  Pilcher.  1958.  The  nature  and  characteristics  of  sub-song.  Br.  Birds 

51:509-514. 

Weeden,  J.  S.  and  J.  B.  Falls.  1959.  Differential  responses  of  male  Ovenbirds  to  recorded 
songs  of  neighboring  and  more  distant  individuals.  Auk  76:343-351. 

Wiley,  R.  H.  and  D.  G.  Richards.  1978.  Physical  constraints  on  acoustic  communication 
in  the  atmosphere:  implications  for  the  evolution  of  animal  vocalizations.  Behav.  Ecol. 
Sociobiol.  3:69-94. 

Wright,  H.  W.  1913.  Morning  awakening  and  even-song.  Second  paper.  Auk  30:512-537. 

DEPT.  BIOLOGY,  UNIV.  CALGARY,  CALGARY,  ALBERTA  T2N  1n4  CANADA.  AC- 
CEPTED 2 AUG.  1979. 


MEANDARRA  ORNITHOLOGICAL  FIELD  STUDY  UNIT 

In  December  1980  the  Meandarra  Ornithological  Field  Study  Unit  (MOFSU)  was  formally 
established  as  a research  group  of  the  University  of  Queensland,  Australia.  Active  in  field 
research  mainly  near  Meandarra,  300  km  west  of  Brisbane,  MOFSU’s  major  interest  is  in 
aspects  of  the  behavior  and  ecology  of  communally  breeding  species.  Visitors  and  volunteer 
field  assistants  are  welcomed,  and  although  no  funding  is  available,  students  are  accepted 
to  work  on  field  projects  towards  an  M.Sc.  or  Ph.D.  For  further  information  write  to:  Dr. 
Douglas  Dow,  Director,  MOFSU,  Dept.  Zoology,  University  of  Queensland,  Brisbane,  Aus- 
tralia, 4067. 


INTERNATIONAL  COMMISSION  ON 
ZOOLOGICAL  NOMENCLATURE 


The  following  opinion  has  been  published  by  the  ICZN  in  the  Bulletin  of  Zoological 
Nomenclature,  Vol.  38,  Pt.  1,  26  Feb.  1981:  Opinion  No.  1168  (p.  69)  ''Cacatua  ducorpsii 
Pucheran,  1853  (Aves):  conserved.”  The  Commission  cannot  supply  separates  of  Opinions. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  42-53 


THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD  OF  ESTIMATING 
NESTING  SUCCESS:  A MODEL,  ESTIMATORS 
AND  SIMULATION  RESULTS 

Gary  L.  Hensler  and  James  D.  Nichols 

Mayfield  (1960,  1961,  1975)  proposed  a method  of  estimating  nesting 
success  which  removes  potential  sources  of  bias  often  associated  with 
other  estimates  of  this  parameter.  Despite  the  intuitive  appeal  of  May- 
field’s method  and  the  general  recognition  that  it  is  appropriate  (e.g..  Mil- 
ler and  Johnson  1978,  Custer  and  Pitelka  1977,  Johnson  1979),  it  is 
still  not  widely  used.  In  this  paper  we  present  a probabilistic  model  for 
the  experimental  situation  considered  by  Mayfield  (1960,  1961,  1975).  We 
then  obtain  maximum  likelihood  estimators  based  on  this  model  and  pre- 
sent results  of  Monte  Carlo  simulations  designed  to  evaluate  the  esti- 
mators. Sample  size  considerations  are  also  discussed. 

Mayfield’s  method  is  based  on  the  concept  of  “nest  days.”  The  model 
he  employs  assumes  the  following:  (a)  the  complete  period  to  success, 
which  we  will  call  the  nesting  period,  (for  example,  the  period  of  incubation 
of  eggs)  is  the  same  number  of  days,  say  J days,  for  all  nests;  (b)  there  is 
a constant  unknown  probability,  p (0  < P < 1),  over  this  period  that  a nest 
observed  on  day  j will  survive  to  day  j + 1;  the  probability  of  a nest  suc- 
ceeding from  day  1 to  full  term  is  then  p-^;  (c)  there  is  a fixed  unknown 
probability,  that  an  observed  nest  will  have  been  first  found  on  day  j 
of  the  nesting  period  of  J days  (for  j = 1,  2,  . . . , J). 

Assume  that  we  observe  K nests  under  the  above  model.  For  each  of 
these  nests  we  observe  a random  vector  = (Y^,  TJ,  k = 1,  2,  . . . , K, 
where  (i)  is  a random  variable  taking  the  value  1 if  the  k‘*^  nest  is 
successful  (i.e.,  survives  the  complete  nesting  period)  and  the  value  0 if 
the  nest  fails  at  any  time,  and  (ii)  is  a random  variable  denoting  the 
number  of  days  the  k**^  nest  is  observed  until  it  either  succeeds  or  fails. 
For  example,  an  observation  of  (0,  10)  would  mean  a nest  was  seen  on  10 
days  but  on  the  IF^  day  visit  it  had  failed,  while  a value  of  (1,  10)  would 
mean  a nest  was  seen  on  10  days  and  on  the  IF^  day  was  observed  to  have 
succeeded  (for  example,  hatchlings  were  present  on  day  11).  Given  the 
random  vectors  X,,  . . . , we  wish  to  estimate  p,  the  daily  probability 
of  survival. 

To  do  this  we  consider  the  joint  distribution  of  (Y^,  TJ: 

(1)  f(y,  t|p)  = [0j-,+,p‘P'[p'“‘(l  - p)  2 

L j=i  J 


42 


Table  1 

Results  of  Monte  Carlo  Simulations  Designed  to  Evaluate  the  Maximum  Likelihood  Estimators  for  Daily  Survival 

Probability  and  Its  Variance^ 


Hensler  and  Nichols  • A TEST  OF  THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD  43 


O' 

ON 

o 

00 

o 

NO 

ON 

(On 

00 

o 

00 

(ON 

c^ 

(On 

nO 

av 

ON 

o 

ON 

o 

ON 

On 

q 

(On 

ON 

(On 

q 

q 

On 

ON 

(On 

ON 

S 

d 

d 

,-H 

d 

,-H 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

r-H 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

V 

£ 2 

-o  fc 

LO 

LO 

lO 

NO 

•rf< 

CM 

CO 

LO 

LO 

00 

CM 

00 

c O 
o w 

On 

On 

ON 

ON 

ON 

q 

ON 

<On 

C^ 

On 

(ON 

ON 

(ON 

On 

ON 

00 

ON 

ON 

Cfl 

O' 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

l-S 

o 

CM 

PO 

<ON 

r- 

r- 

(On 

O 

lO 

CO 

OO 

CO 

CO 

On 

00 

i 

On 

ON 

ON 

<0N 

00 

CO 

q 

CO 

CO 

ON 

q 

(ON 

q 

q 

q 

q 

CO 

00 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

<o. 

O 

CO 

CO 

o 

ON 

o 

LO 

NO 

NO 

ON 

CO 

NO 

8 

o 

rj* 

CM 

LO 

CM 

00 

CM 

lO 

00 

CM 

LO 

LO 

00 

ON 

oo 

CO 

oo 

LO 

NO 

oo 

c 

LO 

NO 

r- 

r- 

00 

r- 

CO 

00 

CO 

ON 

q 

r- 

r- 

q 

r- 

q 

00 

ii 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

<o- 

o 

fO 

fO 

§ 

oo 

CM 

NO 

lO 

(On 

Tf 

(ON 

CO 

8 

Tt 

ON 

CM 

LO 

Tf< 

00 

r- 

r- 

00 

CO 

LO 

NO 

LO 

CM 

% 

CO 

00 

r- 

ON 

CO 

00 

ON 

ON 

ON 

ON 

c^ 

(On 

(ON 

CO 

00 

ON 

(On 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

CM 

CM 

CM 

On 

oo 

r- 

r- 

ON 

ON 

(ON 

00 

o 

CM 

00 

o 

LO 

LO 

lO 

NO 

NO 

NO 

oo 

oo 

oo 

LO 

CO 

CM 

CM 

00 

r^ 

CO 

l-H 

1— 1 

l-H 

CO 

CO 

CO 

LO 

lO 

LO 

q 

r^ 

rH 

l-H 

1 — 1 

CM 

CM 

CM 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

00 

LO 

lO 

00 

o 

CO 

o 

LO 

LO 

00 

CM 

CO 

00 

o 

o 

ON 

ON 

ON 

o 

s 

(ON 

OO 

CM 

OO 

CM 

CM 

lO 

LO 

LO 

00 

CO 

CO 

8 

CM 

CM 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

o 

q 

q 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

O 

O 

o 

o 

O 

alues 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

> 

"2 

o 

LO 

Tf- 

CO 

00 

CM 

(ON 

00 

NO 

oo 

NO 

^ 1 

ON 

o 

fO 

o 

00 

o 

o 

NO 

S 

CM 

o 

■Ct 

00 

NO 

LO 

CM 

LO 

E 

LO 

CM 

oo 

CM 

o 

CO 

CM 

O 

00 

o 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

q 

o 

O 

o 

q 

O 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

<D 

C 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

NO 

r- 

LO 

1 

LO 

CO 

LO 

r- 

o 

8 

NO 

NO 

ON 

ON 

ON 

Tf 

LO 

LO 

lO 

ON 

ON 

LO 

LO 

CO 

oo 

CO 

CO 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

q 

CO 

00 

LO 

NO 

LO 

q 

q 

o 

q 

(—1 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

ON 

NO 

On 

ON 

CM 

r- 

CM 

o 

r- 

o 

ON 

(On 

lO 

lO 

o 

On 

o 

■rf< 

LO 

LO 

LO 

(On 

(On 

o 

<a 

r^ 

r- 

CO 

00 

CO 

q 

00 

ON 

(ON 

q 

q 

q 

CO 

00 

00 

00 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

LO 

o 

o 

LO 

o 

o 

lO 

8 

o 

lO 

8 

o 

LO 

8 

o 

LO 

8 

CM 

o 

CM 

o 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CM 

r- 

lO 

LO 

CM 

LO 

LO 

"(t 

LO 

00 

LO 

LO 

O 

ON 

On 

CM 

>3 

LO 

CO 

8 

CM 

o 

q 

o 

O 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

NO 

r- 

(ON 

8 

ON 

CM 

lO 

ON 

Tf 

CO 

CM 

"L 

o 

CO 

q 

O 

(— ( 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

Q. 

LO 

LO 

CO 

g 

LO 

(ON 

LO 

CO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

O 

O 

o 

O 

o 

o 

CM 

CM 

Actual  confidence 

Mean  estimated  values  interval  coverage 


I 


44 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


d d r-H 


CO  00  fO 
O CvJ  Tf- 
0\ 


o r-  00 

CO  vO  LO 
0^  On  0^ 


CO  00 
lO  lO  lO 
lC  lO  lO 


00  nO  lO 
lO  lO  LO 

o o o 
o o o 


NO  NO  fO 
NO  00  fO 

^ o o 
o o o 


fO  CO  fO 

d d d 


00  ON  o 

Tt  UO 

On  On  On 


On  On  § 

d d d 


^ lO 
CO  lC  NO 
O'  O'  On 

d d d 


lO  lO  NO 
O'  00 
O'  O^  On 


r- 


fO  Cn|  O 
fO  CO  CO 

o o o 
o o o 


lO  Tt  LO 
CSJ  o CM 

^ o o 
o o o 


CO  ^ 

CO  Tf-  Tt 
LO  LO  LO 


o o o 

O 

O O'  O' 

d d d 


CO 

8 

8 

o 

00 

00 

ON 

CO 

ON 

CO 

O' 

ON 

(O 

ON 

(O' 

O' 

(O' 

c^ 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

LO 

lO 

CO 

00 

CO 

00 

8 

LO 

O' 

o 

ON 

ON 

(O' 

(O' 

00 

o 

On 

c^ 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

CM 

o 

ON 

ON 

CM 

o 

NO 

CO 

? 

? 

ON 

O' 

CO 

CO 

(O' 

O' 

CO 

(O' 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

CM 

t^ 

o 

o 

LO 

o 

CM 

CM 

00 

CO 

CM 

NO 

o 

c^ 

CO 

LO 

NO 

O' 

ON 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

C^ 

ON 

ON 

q 

(O' 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

00 

CO 

CO 

o 

00 

NO 

<o 

O' 

LO 

CM 

r- 

r’ 

LO 

8 

00 

On 

On 

CTn 

c^ 

On 

ON 

On 

q 

q 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

O 

00 

O' 

LO 

00 

CO 

8 

CO 

o 

o 

CM 

CO 

CO 

ON 

O 

ON 

o 

ON 

CO 

'—1 

CO 

Tf 

LO 

NO 

NO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

o 

(O' 

NO 

CO 

CM 

o 

lO 

CO 

CM 

o 

o 

CM 

CM 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

O 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

LO 

o 

00 

LO 

NO 

00 

ON 

LO 

CO 

Tf 

CM 

o 

LO 

CM 

8 

8 

CM 

o 

O 

o 

O 

O 

o 

o 

(O 

q 

o 

o 

q 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

00 

00 

CM 

00 

LO 

O' 

CO 

■rf< 

o 

o 

t^ 

00 

o 

00 

5 

O 

O 

O 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CO 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

'-H  O O 
ON  o o 
On  On  O' 


CO  O O 
ON  o o 
OC  CO  o 


CO  o o 
Tt  -rj-  lO 
O'  O'  O' 


00  o o 

NO  NO 
On  O O' 

d d d 


Actual  confidence 

Mean  estimated  values  interval  coverage 


Hensler  and  Nichols  • A TEST  OF  THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD 


45 


r- 

o 

o 

X 

o 

o 

o 

X 

o 

X 

X 

ON 

o 

X 

o 

o 

On 

o 

o 

ON 

o 

q 

On 

ON 

(On 

q 

q 

On 

(On 

q 

q 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

§ 

vO 

o 

ON 

X 

X 

X 

CM 

X 

X 

X 

X 

NO 

o 

8 

00 

o^ 

ON 

ON 

On 

o 

ON 

o 

(On 

(On 

ON 

(On 

X 

O 

q 

o 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

Os 

NO 

8 

X 

X 

On 

o 

X 

NO 

X 

ON 

X 

i 

00 

X 

X 

X 

X 

o 

X 

ON 

X 

c^ 

X 

X 

X 

o 

X 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

<a 

On 

CM 

NO 

X 

X 

X 

CM 

CM 

X 

X 

o 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

8 

o 

X 

rh 

X 

NO 

X 

X 

X 

X 

o 

.E 

O' 

ON 

On 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

ON 

(ON 

q 

o 

o 

o 

o 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

<a 

o 

X 

O 

8 

CM 

X 

X 

O 

•rj' 

O 

X 

CM 

o 

o 

NO 

o 

O' 

8 

8 

ON 

nO 

X 

X 

X 

o 

QS 

8 

o 

o 

o 

cd 

o 

On 

o 

ON 

On 

o 

o 

ON 

o 

(O 

q 

q 

ON 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

d 

d 

d 

00 

t^ 

Tj< 

lO 

X 

X 

X 

Tf 

CM 

X 

r— 

8 

X 

X 

X 

LO 

LO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

ON 

o 

(On 

X 

o 

o 

o 

o 

8 8 

CO 

X 

00 

On 

ON 

O 

CM 

X 

CM 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

o 

r- 

S 

X 

X 

nO 

o 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

-N 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

X 

X 

X 

CM 

o 

o 

8 

o 

o 

<>> 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

q 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

X 

ON 

X 

X 

CM 

X 

X 

ON 

o 

X 

X 

o 

NO 

X 

LO 

CM 

CM 

o 

X 

X 

CM 

o 

X 

CM 

o 

o 

<>- 

o 

8 

8 

o 

8 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

8 

o 

o 

8 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

q 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

q 

q 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

X 

o 

(On 

Tf- 

NO 

o 

o 

o 

X 

X 

X 

Tf 

nO 

nO 

nO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

nO 

X 

o 

NO 

NO 

NO 

<Q. 

r-- 

r-; 

r- 

X 

X 

X 

1— 1 

CM 

CM 

CM 

NO 

NO 

X 

X 

X 

X 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

o 

o 

X 

X 

X 

o 

o 

o 

r- 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

X 

X 

X 

Os 

ON 

ON 

ON 

o 

On 

X 

X 

8 

NO 

t" 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

<a 

On 

ON 

ON 

o 

On 

O 

o 

q 

o 

o 

q 

o 

ON 

q 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

20 

LO 

8 

20 

75 

8 

20 

X 

r- 

8 

20 

X 

8 

20 

X 

o 

o 

20 

75 

o 

o 

lO 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

t B ~ 


" " = 
£ "c  'I 


.Cl.  ^ ~ 
-o  a £ 


-o  -c 

E I 

C 0.. 


k 


46 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


for  y = 0,  1 and  t = 1,  2,  . . . , J. 

The  log  likelihood  function  (see  Cramer  1946:498-506)  for  our  random 
sample  is: 


(2) 


logfl 

k=l 


U-T.+  l 


+ 2 (T.  - D(1  - YK)log  p + (k  - I;  YK)log(l  - p) 

k=i  \ k=i 


+ logf] 


k=l 


J-Tk+l 


■-  j=l 


1-Yk 


Differentiating  (2)  with  respect  to  p,  setting  the  derivative  equal  to  zero 
and  solving  for  p,  yields  the  maximum  likelihood  estimate  (m.l.e.)  of  p, 
say  p (see  Cramer  1946:498-506).  Here  we  have 


(3) 


i T,  + 2 Y.  - K 

k=l  k=l 

K 

I T, 

k = 1 


Mayfield  (1960,  1961,  1975)  proposes  the  following  estimator  for  p: 
Count  the  total  number  of  nest  days  observed  (i.e.,  2]  count 

the  total  number  of  failures  (i.e.,  K — ^ Ykj,  and  estimate  p by 

\ U—  1 / 


1 - K - S Yk 

j-dU which  is  in  fact  p,  the  m.l.e. 

ST, 


The  theory  of  maximum  likelihood  yields  that  the  asymptotic  distribu- 
tion of  (P  - P)  is  Normal  with  mean  zero  and  variance  1/I(p)  where 
l(p)  is  the  Fisher  information  and 


l(p)  = -E 


d^log  f(Y,  T|p) 

dp2 


(see  Cramer  1946:498-506).  As  usual,  E denotes  expected  value.  Thus, 
the  asymptotic  variance  of  p is 

(4)  1 ^ J_ P‘^(l  - P)^ 

K l(p)  K ET(1  - pf  + (EY  - 1)(1  - 2p) 


which  we  can  estimate  as 


Hensler  and  Nichols  • A TEST  OF  THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD 


47 


1 

(K) 


P^d  - P)^ 


T(1  - p)2  + (Y  - 1)(1  - 2p) 

1 


if  Y#1 

K T 


if  Y = 1. 


Here  T and  Y denote  the  sample  means  of  T and  Y,  respectively.  Ap- 
proximate \ — a confidence  intervals  for  p are  then  given  by 


(P  - Za/2V,  P + Z«/2V) 


where  Za/2  is  the  upper  a/2  value  for  the  standard  normal  distribution, 


i.e., 


^0/2  1 

P=exp(-zV2)  dz  = 1 - a/2. 

-OC  ^2t7 


Similarly,  approximate  level  a tests  for  the  equality  of  p values  from  2 
populations  of  nests  are  given  by  the  following:  reject  Hq,  the  null 
hypothesis  that  pi  = p2,  in  favor  of  the  alternative  hypothesis  that 
Pi  7^  P2  if  and  only  if 


(5) 


I Pi  - fe  I 


Za/2  • 


The  behavior  of  these  confidence  intervals  and  tests  depends  on  the 
efficacy  of  T,  Y,  and  p as  estimators  of  ET,  EY  and  p respectively.  To 
investigate  this  behavior  we  performed  Monte  Carlo  simulations  of  a nest- 
ing experiment  which  met  the  assumptions  of  our  model.  We  chose 
several  values  of  J (nesting  period),  K (the  number  of  observed  nests) 
and  p (the  daily  survival  probability).  The  6]  probabilities  were  chosen  to 
be  in  proportion  to  the  available  number  of  nests  from  the  day  of 
nesting  given  that  the  same  number  of  new  nests  are  started  each  day 
and  only  p of  them  survive  to  the  second  day,  p^  to  the  third,  etc.;  i.e.. 


we  set  6j  = 


P^-^1  - P) 

1 - p^  ’ 


j = 1,  2, 


J. 


We  randomly  divided  the  K nests  into  J groups  using  the  distribution 
given  by  the  0^  values.  Each  nest  from  the  group  was  then  followed 
until  it  survived  for  J — j -I-  1 days  or  until  it  failed.  The  probability  of 
daily  survival  was  p,  and  the  probability  of  full  term  success  for  a nest  for 
J days  was  p*^.  The  appropriate  (Y,T)  vector  was  recorded  for  each  of  the 
K nests,  and  p,  v,  and  estimated  confidence  intervals  (90%,  95%,  99%) 
were  calculated.  It  was  then  determined  whether  or  not  the  computed 


Table  2 

Results  of  Simulations  Investigating  the  Power  of  the  Suggested  Test  Statistic,  |p,  - pa]/ 


48 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


NO 

o 

On 

o 

CO 

00 

o 

q 

O 

q 

o 

q 

NO 

r- 

q 

NO 

q 

o 

q 

8 

o 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

CM 

O 

o 

00 

o 

o 

o 

CM 

o 

c^ 

CM 

CM 

LO 

o 

o 

o 

fO 

o 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

to 

o 

ON 

c^ 

q 

q 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

lO 

LO 

o 

00 

CSl 

LO 

o 

CO 

00 

CM 

rf 

o 

00 

00 

o 

ON 

o 

o 

q 

o 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

O 

NO 

CM 

q 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

fO 

NO 

LO 

o 

CM 

NO 

00 

CO 

g 

o 

CM 

Tf- 

00 

o 

o 

o 

O 

LO 

to 

q 

q 

q 

q 

NO 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o^ 

LO 

LO 

LO 

r- 

LO 

o 

'tf 

CM 

LO 

CO 

NO 

o 

o 

o 

i-H 

o 

o 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

to 

CM 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

O' 

to 

o 

On 

Tj- 

00 

CM 

§ 

o 

00 

o 

CM 

g 

o 

ON 

LO 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

>—1 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

q 

O 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

ON 

o 

On 

g 

00 

s 

o 

CM 

o 

to 

o 

o 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

o 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

cc 

to 

o 

o 

LO 

o 

o 

lO 

o 

o 

NO 

o 

o 

On 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

q 

o 

o 

q 

q 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

fO 

S 

o 

q 

nO 

LO 

r- 

CM 

lO 

o 

to 

00 

ON 

00 

q 

q 

q 

q 

o 

O 

o 

CM 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

r- 

o 

to 

to 

lO 

o 

NO 

o 

o 

to 

o 

nO 

o 

o 

o 

to 

o 

o 

q 

q 

q 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

r- 

LO 

o 

nO 

r- 

ON 

to 

CM 

NO 

00 

NO 

ON 

8 

CM 

o 

o 

to 

CM 

o 

o 

cc 

q 

q 

to 

ON 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

fO 

to 

9 

lO 

to 

o 

c^ 

lO 

-rf 

Tj> 

On 

o 

00 

c^ 

8 

o 

o 

CSl 

to 

q 

c^ 

c^ 

to 

CM 

O 

r- 

q 

q 

q 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

r- 

o 

LO 

8 

§ 

g 

o 

nO 

o 

00 

CM 

r- 

o 

8 

8 

fO 

CNI 

o 

NO 

CM 

00 

00 

LO 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

— 1 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

I 


Table  2 
Continued 


Hensler  and  Mchols  • A TEST  OF  THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD 


49 


S3  § 

odd 


lO  Tj-  CSJ 

d d d 


r-  r— I Os 

fO  (M  o 
d d d 


2^8 
d d d 


^ CM 

9 R 
o o o 


8 g 8 8 8 8 


8 


os  lO 
sO 

d d 


2 
d d 


2 8 
d d 


S 8 

d d 


§83 

d d d 


o q 8 

d d d 


o d o 
d d d 


8 8 8 

d d d 


8 8 
d d d 


s s B 

d d d 


3 8 2 

d d o 


8 3 8 


o 
d d 


8 3 

d d 


8 ^ 
d o 


lO  fO 

d d 


oc  o 
d d 


8 8 

d d 


5RR  I I 


o o o ^ ^ 


R R R 

8 8 8 


8 8 8 3 

d d d d 


3 3 8^ 
d d d d 


R R R R 
d d d d 


8 8 


o o o o 

3 8 8 


o o o o 


8 


I I 


I I 


I I 


§ § 

d d 


^ 3 
d d 


§ 8 

o d 


-H  O 

d d 


o o o o o o 


I I I I 
I I I I 
I I I I 
I I I I 


8 § § ^ 
d d d d 


g :::  g § 

d d o d 


3 8 8 8 

d d d d 


8 2 8 3 

d d d d 


8 8 8 3 

d d d d 


0 o o o 

2;  g g g 

d d d d 

1 888 


8 8 8 8 

d r-l  ^ -H 


8 3 

d d 


sO  Tf; 

d d 


3 8 

d d 


I I I 
I I I 
I I I 
I I I 


8 8 3 

^ -H  d 


CM  ^ O 

d d d 


2 3 § 
d d d 


Os  » sO 

d d d 


8 883  888 

d d d d ^ — I ^ 

—I  cc  o O O 

CM  Os  Os  CC  O O O 

d d d d — : 

§ 8 8 § 8 8 8 

O r~:  — I d ^ r-l  ^ 

8 888  888 


■■c  E 


2 c ^ ^ 

■ .£ll‘ 


50 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


confidence  intervals  contained  the  true  value  of  p.  The  entire  above  pro- 
cedure was  repeated  100  times  for  each  combination  of  J,  p and  K values. 
We  computed  the  proportion  of  the  100  times  in  which  the  estimated 
confidence  interval  in  fact  covered  the  true  value.  We  also  determined  the 
mean,  minimum  and  maximum  values  of  the  100  estimates  of  p,  the  mean 
value  of  the  100  estimates  v and  the  mean  of  the  100  estimates  Kv^  (which 
should  estimate  l/l(p)).  These  values  are  compared  with  the  theoretical 
values,  p,  l/l(p),  and  the  exact  confidence  proportions,  in  Table  1.  The 
means  of  the  estimated  values  p and  Kv^  appeared  to  be  good  estimators 
of  p and  l/l(p)  in  virtually  all  simulated  cases.  In  addition,  the  actual 
confidence  interval  coverages  were  close  to  the  theoretical  values,  espe- 
cially when  it  is  remembered  that  proportions  represent  results  of  only 
100  simulations. 

We  also  calculated  p-^  as  an  estimate  of  p^  p*^,  the  probability  of  full 
term  success.  An  alternative  estimate  of  p^  which  is  commonly  used  in 

K 

nesting  studies,  is  Y = ^ Y^/K,  the  ratio  of  the  number  of  observed 

k-l 

successful  nests  to  total  observed  nests.  The  comparisons  of  p*^  and  Y 
as  estimates  of  p^  show  the  superiority  of  p*’  in  cases  where  the  model 
assumptions  are  met  (Table  1). 

All  of  the  results  presented  in  Table  1 were  obtained  assuming 


= £ 


i-‘(i  - p) 


1 - p'’ 


j = 1,  2, 


In  order  to  assess  the  robustness  of  the  above  procedures  to  changes  in 
this  assumption  we  set  all  0^  equal  (i.e.,  dj  = 1/J,  j = 1,  2,  . . . , J)  and 
conducted  additional  simulations  for  many  of  the  situations  examined  in 
Table  1.  Results  were  virtually  identical  to  those  presented  in  Table  1, 
indicating  that  the  estimators  are  quite  robust  with  respect  to  reasonable 
changes  in  the  6j  values. 

In  addition,  we  ran  Monte  Carlo  simulations  of  tests  of  equality  of  two 
p values  using  the  test  statistic  in  (5).  We  assessed  both  type  I and  type 
11  error  probabilities  under  several  experimental  situations.  These  results 
are  presented  in  Table  2 and  can  be  used  as  empirical  approximations  to 
the  power  of  these  hypothesis  tests  under  various  conditions.  It  should  be 
noted  that  the  power  curve  is  not  symmetric.  Thus,  for  a specified  value 
of  A a test  of  the  null  hypothesis  that  pi  = p2  given  that  P2  = Pi  + A (for 
p,  > 0.5)  is  more  powerful  for  A > 0 than  for  A < 0. 

We  note  that  the  estimator  v^  can  be  useful  in  planning  an  experiment 
in  which  the  daily  survival  probability  is  to  be  estimated.  If  we  express 
the  desired  precision  of  p in  terms  of  a specific  coefficient  of  variation,  cv 
(where  cv  = v/p),  then  we  can  substitute  estimates  or  guesses  for  p,  ET,  EY 


Hensler  and  Nichols  • A TEST  OF  THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD 


51 


Table  3 

Sample  Sizes  (Number  of  Nests)  Needed  to  Estimate  Daily  Survival  Probability 
(p)  With  Specified  Levels  of  Precision‘s 


Nesting 
period  (J) 

Daily 

survival 

probability 

(P) 

Desired  coefficient  of  variation  (v/p) 

0.050 

0.040 

0.030 

0.020 

O.OIO 

0.005 

10 

0.75 

39 

61 

Sample 

109 

size  . 

245 

980 

3918 

10 

0.85 

— 

26 

47 

105 

419 

1675 

10 

0.90 

— 

— 

27 

60 

239 

957 

10 

0.95 

— 

— 

— 

26 

104 

415 

20 

0.85 

— 

— 

34 

75 

301 

1205 

20 

0.90 

— 

— 

— 

38 

154 

615 

20 

0.95 

— 

— 

— 

— 

60 

239 

20 

0.97 

— 

— 

— 

— 

33 

130 

20 

0.99 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

40 

30 

0.90 

— 

— 

— 

32 

128 

513 

30 

0.95 

— 

— 

— 

— 

45 

178 

30 

0.97 

— 

— 

— 

— 

23 

93 

30 

0.99 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

27 

40 

0.95 

— 

— 

— 

— 

37 

149 

40 

0.97 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

75 

40 

0.99 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

21 

® Sample  sizes  were  computed  from  (6).  Values  less  than  20  were  not  presented,  because  we  do  not  believe  it  is  appropriate 
to  recommend  such  small  sample  sizes.  Reasons  for  this  belief  are:  (I)  we  question  the  applicability  of  results  relying  on 
asymptotic  theory  to  such  small  sample  sizes;  and  (2)  since  (6)  involves  guesses  of  p,  ET  and  EY,  we  feel  the  resulting 
uncertainty  would  never  warrant  our  recommending  a sample  size  of  less  than  20  nests. 


(denote  these  guesses  by  p*,  T*,  Y*)  into  (4)  and  obtain  the  approximate 
number  of  nests,  K*,  we  need  to  observe: 


(6) 


(1  - P*f 

T*(l  - p)^  + (Y*  - 1)(1  - 2p*)(cv)2 


In  the  absence  of  other  estimates  of  ET  or  EY  we  may  wish  to  specify 
and  p to  compute  EY  and  ET  in  the  standard  manner  using  (1).  As 
an  example  of  sample  sizes  needed  to  estimate  p with  various  levels  of 
precision,  we  have  computed  values  of  K*  using  several  reasonable  com- 
binations of  J and  p (Table  3).  AU  values  in  Table  3 were  computed 
using  ET  and  EY  under  the  assumption  that 


^ - p) 


j = 1,  2,  . 


. . , J. 


Einally,  we  wish  to  indicate  some  uses  of  the  tables  for  the  field  biologist. 
Table  1 shows  that  when  the  model  assumptions  are  met  and  the  field 
biologist  uses  the  approximate  confidence  interval  estimates  herein  sug- 


52 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


gested,  the  actual  frequency  of  coverage  is  quite  close  to  the  theoretical 
in  a wide  range  of  situations.  In  any  one  case,  however,  the  estimate  of 
p (and  hence  also  of  p*^)  can  be  considerably  different  from  the  true  value 
especially  when  the  number  of  nests  in  the  sample  is  small  (see  Max  p 
and  Min  p in  Table  1).  Comparisons  of  p*^  and  Y show  that  p-^  is  always  a 
better  estimate  of  nesting  success  when  the  assumptions  of  the  model 
obtain,  and  that  the  difference  in  these  two  estimates  is  greater  in  cases 
of  lower  overall  nesting  success. 

The  precision  of  the  Mayfield  estimator  in  a field  situation  is  of  course 
dependent  on  how  closely  the  assumptions  of  the  model  are  met  by  the 
population  in  question.  It  is  doubtful  whether  this  model  (or  any  other 
probability  model  of  a biological  phenomenon  for  that  matter)  will  reflect 
exactly  the  reality  of  nature.  However,  the  traditional  estimator  Y is  almost 
sure  to  overestimate  nesting  success  in  all  situations  where  nests  are  found 
on  other  than  the  first  day  of  the  nesting  period  (see  for  example,  Mayfield 
I960,  1961,  1975;  Custer  and  Pitelka  1977).  If  the  assumptions  of  this 
model  approximate  the  reality  of  a population,  then  we  suggest  its  use  to 
correct  for  the  obvious,  known  bias  associated  with  Y.  In  cases  where  this 
model  seems  totally  inappropriate  we  know  of  no  way  to  accurately  esti- 
mate nesting  success  if  nests  other  than  first  day  nests  are  to  be  used. 

Table  2 gives  empirical  estimates  of  the  power  in  testing  the  null  hy- 
pothesis that  Pi  = p2  against  the  alternatives  that  P2  = Pi  + A.  This  table 
gives,  for  selected  values  of  A,  the  probability  of  rejecting  the  null  hy- 
pothesis given  that  in  fact  P2  = Pi  + A.  Note  that  for  A 7^  0 rejecting  the 
null  hypothesis  is  the  correct  decision,  and  hence  we  would  hope  the 
probability  of  rejecting  would  be  large.  For  A = 0 this  probability  should 
be  the  level  of  significance  a for  the  test.  Table  2 shows  how  the  power 
function  varies  with  changes  in  A,  Pi , J,  and  K.  A more  detailed  discussion 
of  the  concept  of  the  power  of  a statistical  test  can  be  found  in  Cohen 
(1977). 

Table  3 is  a guide  for  the  field  biologist  to  determine  the  number  of 
nests  needed  in  his  or  her  sample  in  order  to  achieve  a given  precision  in 
the  estimator.  We  feel  that  a sample  size  of  at  least  20  nests  is  needed  in 
all  cases  (our  reasons  are  given  in  Table  3)  so  only  calculated  sample  sizes 
greater  than  20  are  presented.  As  mentioned,  the  biologist  must  first  make 
guesses  of  p,  EY  and  ET  or  of  p and  dj,  j = 1,  . . . , J,  in  order  to  calculate 
the  sample  size  required  for  a specified  coefficient  of  variation  using  equa- 
tion (6).  Table  3 covers  several  cases,  but  direct  calculation  using  (6)  is  a 
simple  matter  in  cases  not  covered  in  the  table. 

SUMMARY 

Using  a nesting  model  proposed  by  Mayfield  (1960,  1961,  1975)  we  show  that  the  estimator 
he  proposes  is  a maximum  likelihood  estimator  (m.l.e.).  M.l.e.  theory  allows  us  to  calculate 


Hensler  and  Nichols  • A TEST  OF  THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD 


53 


the  asymptotic  distribution  of  this  estimator,  and  we  propose  an  estimator  of  the  asymptotic 
variance.  Using  these  estimators  we  give  approximate  confidence  intervals  and  tests  of  sig- 
nificance for  daily  survival.  Monte  Carlo  simulation  results  show  the  performance  of  our 
estimators  and  tests  under  many  sets  of  conditions.  A traditional  estimator  of  nesting  success 
is  shown  to  be  quite  inferior  to  the  Mayfield  estimator.  We  give  sample  sizes  required  for  a 
given  accuracy  under  several  sets  of  conditions. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Jim  Hines  handled  most  of  the  programming  associated  with  the  Monte  Carlo  simulations. 
Madaline  Schumacher  and  Peggy  Bowley  typed  the  manuscript.  We  wish  to  thank  L.  Stokes, 
F.  Percival,  P.  Geissler,  H.  Mayfield  and  A.  Dunham  for  reading  the  manuscript  and  pro- 
viding their  comments. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Cramer,  H.  1946.  Mathematical  methods  of  statistics.  Princeton  Univ.  Press,  Princeton, 
New  Jersey. 

Cohen,  J.  1977.  Statistical  power  analysis  for  the  behavioral  sciences.  Academic  Press, 
New  York,  New  York. 

Custer,  T.  W.  and  F.  A.  Pitelka.  1977.  Demographic  features  of  a Lapland  Longspur 
population  near  Barrow,  Alaska.  Auk  94:505-525. 

Johnson,  D.  H.  1979.  Estimating  nest  success:  the  Mayfield  method  and  an  alternative. 
Auk  96:651-661. 

Mayfield,  H.  1960.  The  Kirtland’s  Warbler.  Cranbrook  Inst.  Sci.,  Bloomfield  Hills,  Mich- 
igan. 

. 1961.  Nesting  success  calculated  from  exposure.  Wilson  Bull.  73:255-261. 

. 1975.  Suggestions  for  calculating  nest  success.  Wilson  Bull.  87:456-466. 

Miller,  H.  W.  and  D.  H.  Johnson.  1978.  Interpreting  the  results  of  nesting  studies.  J. 
Wildl.  Manage.  42:471-476. 

PATUXENT  WILDLIFE  RESEARCH  CENTER,  LAUREL,  MARYLAND  20811  AND 
MIGRATORY  BIRD  AND  HABITAT  RESEARCH  LABORATORY,  LAUREL, 
MARYLAND  20811.  ACCEPTED  15  DEC.  1979. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  54-66 


NARROWLY  DISJUNCT  ALLOPATRY  BETWEEN 
BLACK-CAPPED  AND  CAROLINA  CHICKADEES 
IN  NORTHERN  INDIANA 

Peter  G.  Merritt 

The  Black-capped  Chickadee  {Parus  atricapillus)  inhabits  wooded 
areas  across  the  northern  United  States,  Canada  and  high  elevations  in 
the  southern  Appalachian  Mountains.  This  species  is  replaced  geograph- 
ically by  the  closely  related  Carolina  Chickadee  (P.  carolinensis)  in  the 
mideastern  and  southern  United  States  (A.O.U.  Check-list  1957).  Where 
the  ranges  adjoin,  a variety  of  situations  prevail.  In  Kansas,  the  western- 
most state  where  the  ranges  meet,  the  southern  range  of  P.  atricapillus 
is  contiguous  with  the  northern  range  of  P.  carolinensis.  Rising  (1968) 
provided  evidence  based  on  multivariate  analyses  of  morphological  char- 
acters that  some  interbreeding  resulting  in  hybrid  birds  may  take  place 
there.  Following  the  boundary  eastward,  the  ranges  remain  contiguous 
through  Missouri  and  into  southern  Illinois.  There,  Brewer  (1961,  1963) 
described  a zone  of  interbreeding  where  many  birds  appeared  to  be  hy- 
brids. Eastward  from  Illinois  through  Indiana  and  Ohio,  Brewer  (1963) 
reported  a narrow  gap  (about  24  km  wide  in  eastern  Illinois)  between  the 
breeding  ranges.  Ward  and  Ward  (1974)  reported  contiguous  ranges  in 
southeastern  Pennsylvania  and  they  provided  evidence  based  on  song  that 
hybridization  may  occur  there.  Johnston  (1971)  described  a hybrid  popu- 
lation at  the  contact  zone  in  the  mountains  of  southwestern  Virginia,  where 
the  chickadee  ranges  are  separated  by  elevation.  In  contrast.  Tanner 
(1952)  found  an  elevational  gap  of  about  180  m (855—1035)  between  the 
nesting  ranges  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains. 

The  existence  of  a narrow  gap  between  the  chickadee  ranges  is  peculiar 
because  apparently  suitable  nesting  habitat  occurs  within  the  gap  regions 
(Tanner  1952,  Brewer  1963).  Furthermore,  both  species  appear  to  be  able 
to  exist  under  essentially  the  same  climatic  conditions.  In  the  Great  Smoky 
Mountains  where  P.  atricapillus  occurs  only  on  some  peaks.  Tanner  (1952) 
found  that  P.  carolinensis  nested  at  higher  elevations  on  the  peaks  where 
P.  atricapillus  did  not  occur.  In  Illinois,  Brewer  (1963)  found  that  P. 
atricapillus  and  P.  carolinensis  occurred  along  parallel  river  systems  at 
the  same  latitude  only  32  km  apart. 

On  Mt.  LeConte  (Great  Smoky  Mountains),  Tanner  (1952)  observed  that 
P.  atricapillus  dispersed  to  lower  elevations  in  the  winter,  invading  the 
range  of  P.  carolinensis.  As  the  nesting  season  approached  most  P.  atri- 
capillus withdrew  up  the  slopes  resulting  in  the  formation  of  the  gap. 


54 


\lerritt  • ALLOPATRY  BETWEEN  TWO  CHICKADEES 


55 


Similar  observations  are  lacking  for  the  latitudinal  gap,  but  the  hiatus 
seems  to  be  most  prominent  during  the  breeding  season  (Brewer  1963). 
Brewer  (1963)  suggested  that  the  gap  evolved  as  a reproductive  isolating 
mechanism.  Since  the  hybrid  population  in  Illinois  had  a relatively  low 
rate  of  reproductive  success  (Brewer  1961),  selective  pressures  may  have 
favored  an  annual  movement  of  chickadees  away  from  the  range  interface 
in  certain  regions;  this  would  act  to  increase  the  fitness  of  these  birds. 
This  hypothesis  can  be  falsified  by  demonstrating  that  the  gap  does  not 
form  prior  to  reproduction. 

This  paper  describes  a field  study  examining  the  range  relationship  of 
P.  atricapillus  and  P.  carolinensis  in  northern  Indiana.  The  study  was 
designed  to  test  the  prediction  that  after  a dispersal  of  either  species  of 
chickadee  towards  the  range  interface  during  the  winter,  these  individuals 
withdraw  from  that  region,  forming  a gap  between  the  ranges  before  re- 
production takes  place. 

STUDY  AREA  AND  METHODS 

Fieldwork  was  conducted  in  Kosciusko,  Wabash  and  adjacent  counties  in  Indiana.  Drain- 
age for  most  of  the  study  area  is  by  way  of  the  Wabash  River  system  which  empties  south 
into  the  Ohio  River.  The  northern  part  of  the  study  area  consists  of  gently  rolling  hills 
characteristic  of  glacial  landforms;  the  southern  part  is  a flat  till  plain.  Around  1820,  beech- 
maple  {Fagus-Acer)  and  oak-hickory  {Quercus-Carya)  forests  were  the  dominant  vegetation 
types  in  the  study  area  (Lindsey  et  al.  1965).  Today  most  of  the  land  is  farmed,  the  main 
crops  being  corn,  wheat  and  soybeans.  Wooded  areas  occur  along  river  systems  or  as  isolated 
woodlots.  Lindsey  (1966)  gives  additional  information  on  climate  and  other  aspects  of  natural 
history  of  the  area. 

Preliminary  fieldwork  indicated  that  P.  atricapillus  occurred  near  the  city  of  Warsaw, 
Kosciusko  Co.,  and  P.  carolinensis  occurred  along  the  Wabash  River  in  Wabash  County. 
Six  relatively  undisturbed  stands  of  mesic  forest  were  chosen  as  trapping  stations  along  a 72 
km  north-south  line  transecting  this  region  (Merritt  1977).  Feeders  erected  at  each  trapping 
station  were  filled  with  sunflower  seeds  when  originally  set  up,  and  a constant  supply  of  seed 
was  maintained  until  May  1976.  Trapping  was  conducted  with  6 McCamey  chickadee  traps 
(McCamey  1961)  on  at  least  4 separate  days  totaling  about  25  h at  each  station,  from  24 
December  1975-4  April  1976.  Captured  chickadees  were  held  briefly  for  banding,  measure- 
ment and  observation  of  plumage  coloration.  The  birds  were  marked  with  U.S.  Fish  and 
Wildlife  Service  aluminum  bands  and  from  1-3  plastic  color  leg  bands  for  individual  rec- 
ognition. 

Captured  chickadees  were  identified  on  the  basis  of  tail-to-wing  ratio  and  feather  color- 
ation. Wing  chord  and  tail  measurements  were  taken  as  suggested  by  Baldwin  et  al.  (1931, 
see  Merritt  1978).  Inspection  of  plumage  color  was  limited  to  the  outer  edge  of  the  lateral 
tail  feathers  and  the  lateral  edges  of  the  secondary  wing  feathers.  These  areas  are  charac- 
teristically distinct  and  white  in  P.  atricapillus,  but  are  gray  and  less  distinct  in  P.  caroli- 
nensis. Notes  were  taken  on  the  coloration  of  these  feathers  as  compared  with  a color  chart 
consisting  of  5 gradations  ranging  from  white  to  gray.  The  chart  was  constructed  by  reference 
to  feathers  of  specimens  of  P.  atricapillus,  P.  carolinensis  and  suspected  hybrids  from 
Illinois. 


56 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


TRAPPING  STATION 

Fig.  1.  The  number  of  chickadees  banded  at  each  trapping  station;  P.  atricapillus  is 
shown  in  solid  bars  and  P.  carolinensis  in  open  bars.  The  relative  north-south  distance 
between  stations  is  represented  on  the  abscissa. 


Other  distributional  information  was  gained  by  soliciting  vocal  responses  from  chickadees 
in  non-trapping  areas  in  the  winter  (27  January-26  February  1976)  and  spring  (29  April-31 
May  1976).  P.  atricapillus  typically  sings  a 2-noted  whistle  song,  fee-bee,  which  is  generally 
distinct  from  the  4-noted  song,  fee-bee-febay,  of  P.  carolinensis.  Less  distinct,  yet  diagnostic 
to  each  species  (especially  when  an  on-the-spot  comparison  with  a pre-recorded  tape  can  be 
made),  is  the  general  call  note.  P.  atricapillus  gives  this  as  a relatively  slow  chickadee-dee- 
dee,  whereas  P.  carolinensis  gives  the  same  call  hut  faster. 

A portable  cassette  tape  recorder  was  used  to  broadcast  pre-recorded  chickadee  songs. 
One  min  of  song  and  call  notes  for  each  species  was  recorded  on  separate  cassettes.  Songs 
of  both  species  and  call  notes  for  P.  atricapillus  were  recorded  from  Kellogg  and  Allen 
(1971)  and  call  notes  for  P.  carolinensis  were  recorded  from  Borror  (1970).  I located  wooded 
areas  containing  seemingly  favorable  habitat  for  chickadees.  Up  to  30  min  were  spent  walking 
through  each  area  trying  to  elicit  song  responses  from  chickadees.  This  was  accomplished 
by  continuously  broadcasting  the  pre-recorded  tapes  (once  a minute  I would  pause  to  rewind 
the  tape  and  listen  for  chickadees).  During  the  winter  survey  I broadcast  the  atricapillus 
tape  at  areas  north  of  the  Eel  River,  the  carolinensis  tape  at  areas  south  of  the  Wabash 
River  and  both  tapes,  interchanging  them  at  5-min  intervals,  between  the  Eel  and  Wabash 
rivers.  Because  of  changes  in  the  chickadee  populations  found  at  the  trapping  stations  this 
procedure  was  modified  for  the  spring  survey.  The  atricapillus  tape  was  broadcast  at  areas 
north  of  the  Eel  River,  the  carolinensis  tape  at  areas  south  of  the  Eel  River  and  both  tapes 
at  areas  along  the  Eel  River.  The  trapping  stations  were  also  included  in  the  spring  survey. 

Approximately  10  h of  observation  on  4 separate  visits  were  spent  at  each  of  the  6 trapping 
stations  from  6 April— 22  May  1976.  An  additional  3 h of  observation  were  spent  at  trapping 


Merritt  • ALLOPATHY  BETWEEN  TWO  CHICKADEES 


57 


stations  5 and  6 on  25  May.  The  observation  time  was  spent  searching  for  and  following 
chickadees. 


TRAPPING  RESULTS 

The  winter  ranges  of  the  2 species  were  found  to  overlap  by  at  least  20 
km  (Fig.  1).  Thirty  individuals  of  P.  atricapillus  were  banded  at  trapping 
stations  1-5  and  23  individuals  of  P.  carolinensis  were  banded  at  stations 
3-6.  Characteristics  of  the  captured  chickadees  are  described  and  com- 
pared in  a separate  paper  (Merritt  1978);  only  features  necessary  for 
species  identification  are  included  in  the  following  discussion. 

The  tail-to-wing  ratio  ranged  from  0.846-0.938  (.r  = 0.890,  SD  = 
±0.023)  in  P.  carolinensis  and  0.919-0.992  (x  = 0.955,  SD  = ±0.016)  in 
P.  atricapillus.  The  overlap  in  tail-to-wing  ratio  (9.4%  of  all  chickadees 
captured)  is  not  uncommon;  Lunk  (1952),  Tanner  (1952),  Simon  (1959), 
Brewer  (1963)  and  Johnston  (1971)  have  reported  slight  overlap  in  the  tail- 
to-wing  ratio  of  these  species. 

Accurate  classification  of  plumage  color  was  difficult  in  the  field  because 
lighting  conditions  varied,  but  some  differences  in  coloration  were  evident. 
Four  P.  atricapillus  (13.3%)  and  4 P.  carolinensis  (17.4%)  showed  plum- 
age coloration  tending  towards  intermediacy.  These  percentages  of  indi- 
viduals deviating  from  the  characteristic  forms  are  within  the  range  of 
variation  observed  by  Brewer  (1963:16-17)  in  Illinois. 

One  P.  atricapillus  banded  at  station  2 and  1 P.  carolinensis  banded 
at  station  5 were  intermediate  in  tail-to-wing  ratio  and  feather  coloration. 
Their  intermediate  morphology  may  have  resulted  from  interbreeding; 
however,  it  is  not  likely  that  hybridization  is  widespread  within  the  study 
area  since  these  individuals  represent  only  3.8%  of  all  chickadees  cap- 
tured. 


WINTER  DISTRIBUTION 

The  winter  survey  of  vocal  responses  indicated  that  the  ranges  of  P. 
atricapillus  and  P.  carolinensis  overlapped  by  about  25  km  (Fig.  2).  Dif- 
ferences in  time  and  day  of  sampling  did  not  seem  to  affect  this  survey 
significantly;  chickadees  responded  with  call  notes  or  song  at  all  58  areas 
sampled.  The  distributional  map  in  Fig.  2 may  be  biased  to  some  extent 
by  the  choice  of  songs  broadcast  at  a particular  area.  However,  this  bias 
is  probably  minimal  since  on  several  occasions  an  individual  of  1 species 
responded  to  the  taped  broadcast  of  the  heterospecific  song  (Merritt  1978) 
and  the  amount  of  range  overlap  detected  (Fig.  2)  corresponds  with  that 
found  by  the  trapping  (Fig.  1). 

Typical  vocal  responses  were  elicited  at  most  areas  permitting  species 
identification,  but  at  1 location  an  individual  chickadee  gave  the  song  of 


58 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Fig.  2.  Winter  distribution  of  chickadees  as  determined  by  vocal  responses.  The  trapping 
station  symbols  are  solid  if  P.  atricapillus  was  present,  open  if  P.  carolinensis  was  present 
and  half-filled-in  if  both  species  were  present. 


Merritt  • ALLOPATRY  BETWEEN  TWO  CHICKADEES 


59 


both  species  and  at  3 other  locations  abnormal  songs  were  elicited  (Merritt 
1978).  Tanner  (1952),  Johnston  (1971)  and  Ward  and  Ward  (1974)  also 
observed  individual  chickadees  singing  the  songs  of  both  species.  Similar 
cases  of  closely  related  species  singing  mixed  songs  have  been  described 
in  buntings  {Passerina  cyanea  and  P.  amoena)^  meadowlarks  {Sturnella 
neglecta  and  S.  magna),  towhees  {Pipilo  fuscus  and  P.  albicoLlis),  tree- 
creepers  {Certhia  familiaris  and  C.  brachydactyla).  Old  World  warblers 
{Acrocephalus  scirpaceus  and  A.  palustris,  Phylloscopus  trochilus  and  P. 
collybita,  Sylvia  communis  and  S.  atricapilla)  and  wrens  {Troglodytes 
aedon  and  Thryomanes  bewickii)  (Lemaire  1977:227-230).  As  suggested 
by  Emlen  et  al.  (1975)  for  the  case  of  the  buntings  {Passerina  cyanea  and 
P.  amoena),  it  is  likely  that  this  behavior  promotes  interspecific  recogni- 
tion and  facilitates  heterospecific  spacing. 

The  atypical  songs  given  by  2 chickadees  consisted  of  4 high-pitched 
whistle  notes  of  equal  tone.  The  other  atypical  song  consisted  of  a trill 
followed  by  3 high-pitched  whistle  notes.  In  each  case,  these  songs  seemed 
to  be  given  with  unusually  high  intensity.  Vocal  anomalies  apparently  sim- 
ilar to  these  were  also  reported  in  Illinois  (Brewer  1961,  1963),  Kansas 
(Rising  1968),  southwestern  Virginia  (Johnston  1971)  and  southeastern 
Pennsylvania  (Ward  and  Ward  1974).  Brewer  (1963)  and  Johnston  (1971) 

! noted  an  unusually  high  percentage  of  atypical  vocalizations  near  the  hy- 
brid zones  in  Illinois  and  Virginia,  respectively.  It  seems  probable  that  the 
vocal  anomalies  are  the  result  of  interbreeding.  In  this  study  only  3 indi- 
viduals in  the  58  areas  surveyed  responded  with  abnormal  songs.  This 
suggests,  as  does  the  trapping  data,  that  isolated  cases  of  interbreeding 
may  occur  in  northern  Indiana  but  a significant  zone  of  hybridization  is 
not  present. 

SPRING  DISTRIBUTION 

The  spring  survey  of  vocal  responses  revealed  that  the  ranges  of  P. 
atricapillus  and  P.  carolinensis  were  separated  by  a gap  of  about  30  km 
(Fig.  3).  In  general,  chickadees  of  both  species  were  sparse  throughout 
the  region  surveyed;  rarely  did  more  than  1 chickadee  in  any  particular 
1 area  respond  to  the  taped  broadcast.  Most  chickadees  occurred  in  river 
bottom  forests  or  lowland  woods  along  streams.  This  was  especially  true 
for  P.  atricapillus;  only  twice  were  individuals  located  in  isolated  wood- 

I lots. 

Only  1 P.  atricapillus  was  found  south  of  the  Tippecanoe  River  and 
^ only  1 P.  carolinensis  was  found  north  of  the  Eel  River.  Perhaps  the  most 
? striking  feature  of  the  gap  was  the  absence  of  chickadees  along  the  Eel 
\ River  northeast  of  trapping  station  3.  This  area  offers  habitat  similar  to 
J that  existing  along  the  Elkhart,  Tippecanoe  and  Wabash  rivers,  yet  in 


60 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


) 


spite  of  repeated  tries  at  various  times  on  different  days  no  chickadees 
could  be  found  here. 

TRAPPING  STATION  OBSERVATIONS 

Of  the  30  banded  individuals  of  P.  atricapillus,  only  9 were  seen  past 
the  middle  of  March  and  only  2 were  observed  after  1 April  (Fig.  4).  One 
of  these  (at  station  2)  was  seen  on  both  8 and  15  April  accompanied  by  an 
unbanded  individual.  Of  the  23  banded  individuals  of  P.  carolinensis,  16 
were  observed  in  April  and  4 individuals  (at  stations  4,  5 and  6)  were  seen 
near  the  end  of  May.  During  the  observations  at  stations  5 and  6,  typical 
carolinensis  songs  were  commonly  heard  and  a few  instances  of  what 
appeared  to  be  territorial  combat  were  observed. 

The  only  other  sightings  of  chickadees  at  any  of  the  trapping  stations 
during  April  and  May  were  as  follows:  On  3 April,  1 followed  3 unbanded 
chickadees  from  16:00-16:10  as  they  foraged  from  the  center  of  station  2 
to  the  north  edge  of  the  woods.  At  least  one  of  the  birds  gave  the  2-noted 
whistle  song  characteristic  of  P.  atricapillus  several  times,  but  the  song 
was  given  softly.  When  they  reached  the  edge  of  the  woods  they  hesitated 
for  a minute,  then  flew  north  over  a corn  field  out  of  sight.  On  13  April 
and  16  May,  I observed  an  unbanded  chickadee  foraging  in  woods  along 
the  Eel  River  about  0.4  km  north  of  station  3;  no  vocalizations  were  heard. 
On  25  May,  I saw  a group  of  4 chickadees  as  they  foraged  slowly  through 
some  dense  vegetation  at  station  6.  One  bird  wore  a band  and  appeared 
to  be  a parent  leading  young. 


DISCUSSION 

Comparison  of  the  spring  distribution  (Fig.  3)  with  the  winter  distribution 
(Fig.  2)  makes  it  evident  that  most  P.  atricapillus  withdrew  from  the  range 
interface.  Observations  at  the  trapping  stations  indicate  that  this  took 
place  primarily  during  the  last  2 weeks  of  March  (Fig.  4).  The  withdrawal 
included  P.  atricapillus  occupying  woodlots  in  Elkhart  County  as  far  as 
80  km  north  of  the  range  interface.  Interestingly,  most  of  the  chickadees 
found  in  this  region  were  in  riparian  habitat.  In  Kalamazoo  County,  Mich- 
igan, 175  km  north  of  the  range  interface,  P.  atricapillus  is  common  in 
woodlots  as  well  as  riparian  habitat  at  all  times  of  the  year  (pers.  obs.).  It 
seems  likely  that  in  northern  Indiana  chickadees  were  found  mainly  in 
riparian  habitat  because  the  only  continuous  stretches  of  woodland  habitat 
occur  along  the  river  systems  and  these  areas  act  as  avenues  of  dispersion 
for  the  southernmost  breeding  P.  atricapillus. 

Seasonal  migration  in  P.  atricapillus  has  been  discussed  by  Butts 
(1931),  Wallace  (1941),  Bent  (1946)  and  Lawrence  (1958).  Examination  of 
unpublished  banding  records  obtained  in  December  1978  from  the  U.S. 


Merritt  • ALLOPATHY  BETWEEN  TWO  CHICKADEES 


61 


Fig.  3.  Spring  distribution  of  P.  atricapillus  and  P.  carolinensis.  (Refer  to  legend  in  Fig.  2.) 


62 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


TRAPPING  STATION 

1 

■ 

■ 

■ " 
■ 

2 

■ ■ 

■ ■ 

■ 

■ 

■ 

■ 

■ 

■ 

■ 

3 

□ 

■ 

Q 

4 

r-i 

□ 

■ 

■ 

5 

■ 

r-i  r-i 

□ 

n 

□ 

LJ 

□ 

6 

□ 

Q 

n 

□ 

□— 

— O 
Q— O 

D 

JAN 

FEB 

MAR 

APR 

MAY 

MONTH 

Fig.  4.  Summary  of  captures  and  observations  at  the  trapping  stations.  Squares  indicate 
the  bird  was  captured  and  circles  indicate  it  was  observed.  P.  atricapillus  is  represented  by 
solid  symbols  and  F.  carolinensis  by  open  symbols. 

Fish  and  ildlife  Service  indicates  that  long  distance  movements  of  P. 
atricapillus  are  most  common  in  a northeast-southwest  direction  through- 
out the  eastern  and  midwestern  U.S.  and  southeastern  Canada.  Since 
numerous  handing  studies  show  that  many  individuals  of  P.  atricapillus 
are  permanent  residents  in  this  region  (e.g.,  Odum  1942,  Glase  1973, 


Merritt  • ALLOPATHY  BETWEEN  TWO  CHICKADEES 


63 


Weise  and  Meyer  1979),  it  appears  that  during  the  winter  the  gap  between 
the  ranges  may  become  occupied  by  individuals  of  P.  atricapillus  from 
the  northeast.  The  gap  becomes  apparent  only  after  the  withdrawal  of  P. 
atricapillus  in  the  spring.  Observations  in  Ohio  by  Thomas  (1958)  also 
agree  with  this  conclusion. 

The  disjunct  nature  of  the  breeding  ranges  of  P.  atricapillus  and  P, 
carolinensis  appears  to  be  relatively  stable;  the  gap  has  remained  at  ap- 
proximately the  same  latitude  in  northern  Indiana  for  at  least  40  years. 
From  1935-1939  Ernest  M.  Shull  frequently  went  on  early  morning  walks 
from  Manchester  College  along  the  Eel  River,  upstream  about  3.2  km  to 
the  town  of  Liberty  Mills  and  then  back.  This  area  lies  only  a few  kilo- 
meters northeast  of  trapping  station  3.  ShuU  (unpubl.)  recorded  sightings 
of  all  birds  along  this  route.  Downy  Woodpeckers  {Picoides  pubescens), 
Tufted  Titmice  (P.  bicolor)  and  White-breasted  Nuthatches  {Sitta  caro- 
linensis) were  common  during  every  month  of  the  year.  Chickadees  (most 
reported  to  be  P.  atricapillus)^  however,  were  found  in  January  through 
April  and  September  through  December,  but  none  in  May  or  July  and  only 
1 in  each  of  June  and  August  during  these  years. 

It  is  still  not  clear  what  factors  cause  the  gap  to  occur.  The  fact  that 
the  elevational  gap  found  by  Tanner  (1952)  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains 
and  the  latitudinal  gap  found  in  northern  Indiana  both  occurred  only  during 
the  breeding  season  and  became  obvious  after  a withdrawal  of  P.  atri- 
capillus suggests  that  similar  factors  maintain  the  gap  at  both  locales. 
Considering  ultimate  factors.  Brewer  (1963)  suggested  that  the  gap  evolved 
as  a reproductive  isolating  mechanism  functioning  to  reduce  unsuccessful 
interbreeding.  No  active  chickadee  nests  were  found  in  this  study  so  to 
test  Brewer’s  reproductive  isolation  hypothesis  it  is  necessary  to  estimate 
the  time  of  reproduction.  The  expected  date  of  first  egg-laying  by  chick- 
adees at  the  range  interface  in  northern  Indiana  can  be  approximated  by 
adjusting  data  for  chickadees  nesting  in  Illinois.  Both  species  tend  to  begin 
laying  about  3.5— 4.5  days  later  for  each  degree  of  latitude  northward 
(Brewer  1961),  so  the  first  egg-laying  at  41°N  latitude  should  occur  from 
20  April-26  May  for  P.  atricapillus  and  30  April-21  May  for  P.  caroli- 
nensis. Nest-building,  excavation  and  the  establishment  of  territorial 
boundaries  precede  the  first  egg-laying  by  about  20  days.  Therefore,  chick- 
adees remaining  in  the  study  area  might  engage  in  reproductive  activities 
as  early  as  1 April  for  P.  atricapillus  and  10  April  for  P.  carolinensis.  The 
main  withdrawal  of  chickadees  away  from  the  range  interface  took  place 
prior  to  these  dates  so  the  prediction  that  the  gap  forms  before  nesting 
activities  begin  is  supported.  This,  plus  the  fact  that  only  a few  possible 
hybrid  individuals  were  found  in  this  study,  indicate  that  the  gap  functions 
as  a reproductive  isolating  mechanism,  but  it  is  not  clear  that  the  gap 


64 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


evolved  for  this  purpose.  If  an  individual  chickadee’s  fitness  can  be  in- 
creased by  migrating  to  another  area  to  breed,  then  natural  selection  may 
have  favored  the  spring  withdrawal  of  P.  atricapillus  from  the  range  in- 
terface; however,  this  hypothesis  remains  speculative. 

Two  other  hypotheses  attempting  to  explain  narrowly  disjunct  allopatry 
in  general,  and  thought  possibly  to  apply  to  the  case  of  the  chickadees, 
rely  chiefly  on  proximate  factors.  Cornell  (1974)  suggested  that  parasites 
might  be  transmitted  between  chickadee  species  in  the  overlap  zone.  If 
these  had  an  adverse  effect  on  the  reproductive  success  of  chickadees 
breeding  at  the  range  interface  a gap  could  develop.  Expanding  on 
MacArthur’s  (1972)  model  of  exploitative  competition,  Slade  and  Robert- 
son (1977)  suggested  that  a change  in  resource  availability  as  a result  of 
the  cost  of  interspecific  competition  in  the  overlap  zone  may  have  caused 
a competitively-induced  gap.  These  hypotheses  do  not  assume  that  the 
withdrawal  of  P.  atricapillus  is  related  to  the  maintenance  of  the  gap. 
They  both  require  that  interspecific  contact  occurs  between  chickadees 
that  are  permanent  residents  at  the  range  interface.  If  the  interactions 
severely  reduce  the  fitness  of  chickadees  attempting  to  breed  in  this  region 
a gap  might  develop.  For  an  hypothesis  of  this  type  2 predictions  can  be 
made:  (1)  the  width  of  the  gap  should  be  dependent  upon  the  degree  of 
overlap  exhibited  by  sedentary  individuals  of  both  species;  and  (2)  the 
width  of  the  gap  should  fluctuate  in  time  according  to  the  rate  of  recolo- 
nization. A gap  developing  under  these  circumstances  might  persist  for  a 
relatively  long  period  of  time  if  the  rate  of  recolonization  is  low.  Therefore, 
these  hypotheses  might  only  be  testable  by  a long  term  study  monitoring 
the  distribution  and  abundance  of  permanent  residents  at  the  range  inter- 
face. 

It  is  also  possible  that  the  gap  is  caused  and  maintained  by  the  winter 
influx  of  P.  atricapillus.  These  individuals  might  provide  additional  com- 
petition for  limiting  food  resources,  which  may  result  in  decreasing  the 
winter  survival  rate  of  resident  chickadees  of  both  species  at  the  range 
interface.  This  might  be  especially  important  in  forming  the  gap  if  hybrid 
chickadees  are  inferior  competitors.  This  hypothesis  could  be  examined 
by  testing  the  prediction  that  year-to-year  fluctuations  in  sedentary  chick- 
adee populations  near  the  range  interface  are  directly  related  to  the  in- 
tensity of  the  winter  influx  of  P.  atricapillus. 

SUMMARY 

During  the  winter  and  early  spring  of  1975-1976  the  ranges  of  Parus  atricapillus  and  P. 
carolinensis  overlapped  hy  about  25  km  in  northern  Indiana.  Evidence  based  on  morphology 
and  song  suggest  that  isolated  cases  of  interbreeding  may  occur.  During  the  last  2 weeks  of 
March  most  P.  atricapillus  withdrew  from  the  range  interface.  A survey  of  vocal  responses 


Merritt  • ALLOPATHY  BETWEEN  TWO  CHICKADEES 


65 


conducted  from  29  April-31  May  revealed  that  the  breeding  ranges  were  separated  by  a gap 
of  about  30  km.  The  withdrawal  of  P.  atricapillus  included  individuals  occupying  woodlots 
as  far  as  80  km  north  of  the  range  interface. 

It  is  still  not  clear  what  factors  cause  the  gap  to  occur.  The  resemblance  of  the  elevational 
gap  found  by  Tanner  (1952)  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains  and  the  latitudinal  gap  found  in 
northern  Indiana  suggests  that  the  gap  is  maintained  by  similar  factors  at  both  locations. 
Since  the  gap  becomes  obvious  with  the  withdrawal  of  P.  atricapillus  just  prior  to  the 
breeding  season,  it  is  possible  that  these  movements  were  selected  for  as  a reproductive 
isolating  mechanism. 

Other  hypotheses  attempting  to  explain  the  presence  of  the  gap  rely  chiefly  on  proximate 
factors.  The  most  likely  of  these  hypotheses  are:  (1)  the  gap  forms  as  a direct  result  of 
interspecific  interactions  between  sedentary  chickadees  at  the  range  interface;  and  (2)  the 
gap  is  caused  and  maintained  by  the  winter  movement  of  P.  atricapillus  to  the  range  inter- 
face. Competitive  interactions  reducing  the  fitness  of  sedentary  chickadees  in  and  near  the 
gap  may  be  important  for  either  hypothesis. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

I am  grateful  to  Richard  Brewer  for  providing  guidance  throughout  all  phases  of  this 
project.  Richard  F.  Johnston,  Julian  C.  Lee,  Oscar  T.  Owre,  James  D.  Rising  and  Edwin  O. 
Willis  read  drafts  of  this  manuscript  and  provided  useful  suggestions.  James  Bostwick,  Neil 
Case,  James  B.  Cope,  David  Filer,  Russell  E.  Mumford,  Ernest  M.  Shull,  Paul  Steffen  and 
Rex  Watters  contributed  useful  information.  Doug  and  Christine  Campbell,  John  Olson  and 
John  Stiner  helped  with  the  fieldwork.  This  project  was  completed  in  partial  fulfillment  of 
the  Master  of  Arts  Degree  at  Western  Michigan  University,  Kalamazoo,  Michigan.  Funding 
was  provided  by  grants  from  the  Frank  M.  Chapman  Memorial  Fund  of  the  American  Mu- 
seum of  Natural  History  and  the  Western  Michigan  University  Research  Fund. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

American  Ornithologists’  Union  Check-list  Committee.  1957.  Check-list  of  North 
American  birds,  5th  ed.  Lord  Baltimore  Press,  Baltimore,  Maryland. 

Baldwin,  S.  P.,  H.  C.  Oberholser  and  L.  G.  Worley.  1931.  Measurements  of  birds. 
Sci.  Publ.  Cleveland  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.  2. 

Bent,  A.  C.  1946.  Life  histories  of  North  American  jays,  crows,  and  titmice.  U.S.  Natl. 
Mus.  Bull.  191. 

Borror,  D.  J.  1970.  Common  bird  songs  (sound  recording).  Dover  Publ.,  Inc.,  New  York, 
New  York. 

Brewer,  R.  1961.  Comparative  notes  on  the  life  history  of  the  Carolina  Chickadee.  Wilson 
BuU.  73:348-373. 

. 1963.  Ecological  and  reproductive  relationships  of  Black-capped  and  Carolina 

chickadees.  Auk  80:9-47. 

Butts,  W.  K.  1931.  A study  of  the  chickadee  and  White-breasted  Nuthatch  by  means  of 
marked  individuals.  Pt.  II:  the  chickadee  (Penthestes  atricapillus  atricapillus).  Bird- 
Banding  2: 1-26. 

Cornell,  H.  1974.  Parasitism  and  distributional  gaps  between  allopatric  species.  Am.  Nat. 
108:880-883. 

Emlen,  S.  T.,  J.  D.  Rising  and  W.  L.  Thompson.  1975.  A behavioral  and  morphological 
study  of  sympatry  in  the  Indigo  and  Lazuli  buntings  of  the  Great  Plains.  Wilson  BuU. 
87:145-179. 


66 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  \o.  1,  March  1981 


Gl\SE.  J.  C.  1973.  EcoIog>  and  social  organization  in  the  Black-capped  Chickadee.  Living 
Bird  12:235-267. 

Johnston.  D.  W.  1971.  Ecological  aspects  of  hybridizing  chickadees  {Parus)  in  Virginia. 
Am.  Midi.  Nat.  85:124-134. 

Kellogg.  P.  P.  and  A.  A.  Allen.  1971.  A field  guide  to  bird  songs  of  eastern  and  central 
North  America  (sound  recording).  Houghton  Mifflin  Co..  Boston.  Massachusetts. 

Lawrence,  L.  DE  K.  1958.  On  regional  movements  and  body  weight  of  Black-capped 
Chickadees  in  winter.  Auk  75:415-443. 

Lemaire,  F.  1977.  Mixed  song,  interspecific  competition  and  hybridization  in  the  Reed  and 
Marsh  warblers  {Acrocephalus  scirpaceus  and  palustris).  Behaviour  63:215-240. 

Lindsey.  A.  A.  (ed.)  1966.  Natural  features  of  Indiana.  Indiana  Acad.  Sci.,  Indianapolis, 
Indiana. 

, . B.  Crankshaw  and  S.  A.  Qadir.  1965.  Soil  relations  and  distribution  map  of 

the  vegetation  of  presettlement  Indiana.  Botan.  Gaz.  126:155-163. 

Ll  nk.  . A.  1952.  Notes  on  variation  in  the  Carolina  Chickadee.  ilson  BuU.  64:7-21. 

MacArthlr,  R.  H.  1972.  Geographical  ecology  : patterns  in  the  distribution  of  species. 
Harper  and  Row.  Publishers.  Inc..  New  \ork,  New'  ^ork. 

McCamey,  F.  1961.  The  chickadee  trap.  Bird-Banding  32:51-55. 

Merritt,  P.  G.  1977.  Gap  formation:  a reproductive  isolating  mechanism  for  Parus  atri- 
capillus  and  P.  carolinensis  in  northern  Indiana.  M.  A.  thesis.  estern  Michigan  Univ., 
Kalamazoo,  Michigan. 

. 1978.  Characteristics  of  Black-capped  and  Carolina  chickadees  at  the  range  inter- 
face in  northern  Indiana.  Jack-Pine  arbler  56:171-179. 

Odum,  E.  P.  1942.  Annual  cycle  of  the  Black-capped  Chickadee — 3.  Auk  59:499-531. 

Rising,  J.  D.  1968.  A multivariate  assessment  of  interbreeding  between  the  chickadees, 
Parus  atricapillus  and  P.  carolinensis.  Syst.  Zool.  17:160-169. 

Simon.  S.  W . 1959.  Occurrence  and  measurements  of  Black-capped  Chickadees  at  Monk- 
ton.  Md.  Maryland  Birdlife  15:3^. 

Slade,  N.  A.  and  P.  B.  Robertson.  1977.  Comments  on  competitively-induced  disjunct 
allopatry.  Occ.  Pap.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.,  No.  65,  Univ.  Kansas,  Lawrence,  Kansas. 

Tanner.  J.  T.  1952.  Black-capped  and  Carolina  chickadees  in  the  southern  Appalachian 
Mountains.  Auk  69:407-424. 

Thomas.  E.  S.  1958.  The  Black-capped  Chickadee  in  central  Ohio.  Vi  heaton  Club  Bull.  3 
(new  ser.):8-ll. 

W ALL  ACE,  G.  J.  1941.  \\  inter  studies  of  color-banded  chickadees.  Bird-Banding  12:49-67. 

ARD,  R.  AND  D.  A.  \\  ARD.  1974.  Songs  in  contiguous  populations  of  Black-capped  and 
Carolina  chickadees  in  Pennsylvania.  ilson  Bull.  86:344-356. 

W ELSE,  C.  M.  AND  J.  R.  Meyer.  1979.  Juvenile  dispersal  and  development  of  site-fidelity 
in  the  Black-capped  Chickadee.  Auk  96:40-55. 


DEPT.  BIOLOGY.  UMV.  MIAMI,  CORAL  GABLES,  ELORIDA  33124.  ACCEPTED 
1 DEC.  1979. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  67-76 


TOE  FUSION  IN  OSCINES 

George  A.  Clark,  Jr. 

Although  striking  differences  in  extent  of  webbing  between  the  toes  of 
nonpasserines  are  well-known  and  often  used  to  illustrate  adaptation  in 
birds,  the  degree  of  connection  between  the  toes  in  oscines  has  received 
much  less  attention.  Ridgway  (1901-07)  in  scattered  keys  and  descriptions 
commented  on  integumental  fusion  of  the  toes  of  numerous  New  World 
oscine  taxa,  but  provided  little  interpretation  on  possible  significance  of 
the  taxonomic  variations,  apart  from  their  use  in  distinguishing  taxa.  Al- 
though others  (e.g.,  Rand  and  Traylor  1953)  have  occasionally  commented 
on  fusion  in  oscines,  a comprehensive  survey  of  the  families  is  lacking.  1 
attempt  here  to  interpret  major  taxonomic  differences  in  fusion  in  relation 
to  systematics  and  behavioral  differences  and  to  indicate  problems  for 
future  study. 


MATERIALS  AND  METHODS 

I examined  study  skins  of  1941  species  of  oscines  in  the  collections  of  the  National  Museum 
of  Natural  History  (Washington,  D.C.),  American  Museum  of  Natural  History  (New  York), 
British  Museum  (Nat.  Hist.,  Tring)  and  the  University  of  Connecticut.  In  addition,  I made 
more  than  175  observations  of  toe  positions  in  perching  or  standing  for  wild  or  captive  birds 
representing  30  species.  The  taxonomic  sequence  follows  Morony,  Bock  and  Farrand  (1975). 

To  examine  fusion  among  the  3 forward  toes  I used  a hand  lens  or  binocular  dissecting 
microscope,  except  for  species  of  large  size.  Degree  of  fusion  of  the  middle  (HI)  and  outer 
(IV)  toes  is  primarily  emphasized,  but  extent  of  fusion  of  the  inner  (II)  and  middle  toes  was 
noted  for  349  species,  as  discussed  below.  I selected  as  a major  landmark  for  comparison 
the  articulation  between  the  first  and  second  phalanges  of  the  middle  toe,  located  by  bends 
in  that  toe,  by  the  plantar  flexion  creases  and,  in  many  cases,  by  an  overlying  scute  termed 
the  proximal  cap  (Clark  1977).  Fusion  of  toes  HI  and  IV  is  rated  low  (L)  if  not  reaching 
distally  to  the  region  of  articulation  between  tbe  first  and  second  phalanges  of  HI,  moderate 
(M)  if  reaching  that  region,  and  high  (H)  if  extending  further  distally  (Fig.  1).  These  ratings 
are  arbitrary  divisions  of  a continuum,  but  tbe  extremes  of  low  vs  high  fusion  are  markedly 
different.  This  summary  of  fusion  in  3 categories  provides  less  detail  than  sometimes  given 
by  Ridgway  (1901-07),  but  is  advantageous  in  facilitating  comparisons  across  a wide  range 
of  taxa. 

RESULTS  AND  INTERPRETATION 

In  tabulating  data  on  fusion  of  toes  III  and  IV  (Table  I),  I emphasize 
particularly  the  distribution  of  extreme  differences  (low  vs  high)  and  the 
condition  in  many  genera  considered  atypical  in  their  assigned  families. 
Such  a summarizing  list  (Table  I)  necessarily  obscures  many  finer  taxo- 
nomic differences.  For  example,  nearly  all  genera  of  the  Mimidae  have 


67 


68 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Fig.  1.  Examples  of  toe  fusion,  from  left  to  right:  low.  House  Sparrow  {Passer  domesti- 
cus);  moderate.  White-breasted  Nuthatch  {Sitta  carolinensis);  and  high.  Red-eyed  Vireo 
iVireo  olivaceus).  The  arrows  mark  the  approximate  level  of  the  joint  between  phalanges  1 
and  2 of  the  middle  toe. 


low  fusion,  but  the  inclusion  of  Donacobius  extends  the  mimid  range  to 
the  moderate  level. 

Partial  associations  exist  between  degree  of  fusion  and  behavior.  The 
terrestrial  larks  (Alaudidae)  and  pipits  (Motacillidae)  have  low  to  moderate 
fusion,  whereas  many  predominantly  arboreal  Old  World  families  have 
moderate  to  high  fusion,  e.g.,  Campephagidae,  Irenidae,  Dicaeidae,  Nec- 
tariniidae,  Zosteropidae,  Meliphagidae,  Oriolidae,  Dicruridae.  Families 
that  climb  on  tree  trunks  or  other  vertical  surfaces  often  have  moderate 
to  high  fusion,  e.g.,  Sittidae,  Certhiidae,  Climacteridae.  Still  other  Old 
World  families  or  subfamilies  range  from  low  to  high  fusion,  e.g.,  Laniidae, 
Timaliinae,  Sylviinae,  Malurinae.  Finches,  sparrows  and  buntings 
throughout  the  world  (Emherizinae,  Cardinalinae,  Carduelinae,  Estrildi- 
dae,  Ploceidae)  have  low  to  moderate  fusion,  a level  widespread  in  the 
New  World  9-primaried  assemblage,  including  Parulidae,  Drepanididae 
and  Icteridae.  However,  vireos  (Vireonidae),  including  peppershrikes  (Cy- 
clarhis)  and  shrike-vireos  {Vireolanius),  have  greater  fusion. 

Species  within  a genus  are  usually  similar  in  the  broad  categories  of  toe 
fusion  used  here  (Table  1),  hut  a few  Old  World  genera,  the  warblers 
Cettia  and  Bradypterus  and  the  hush-shrikes  Telophorus,  exhibit  excep- 
tional interspecific  differences.  Within  Cettia,  for  example,  the  low  fusion 
of  C.  squarneiceps,  C.  major  and  C.  brunnifrons  contrasts  with  high  fusion 
of  C.  fortipes;  other  species  are  intermediate.  Among  the  10  examined 
species  of  Bradypterus,  only  B.  seebohmi  has  high  fusion,  the  other  9 
being  low.  Telophorus  bocagei,  T.  sulfureopectus,  T.  olivaceus,  T.  nigri- 
frons  and  T.  multicolor  have  high  fusion  in  contrast  to  low  to  moderate  in 
T.  zeylonus,  T.  viridis,  T.  quadricolor  and  T.  dohertyi.  In  Telophorus, 
separation  of  groups  of  species  by  degree  of  fusion  matches  taxonomic 
units  recognized  on  other  characters  (Hall  and  Moreau  1970),  but  fusion 
differences  within  Cettia  and  Bradypterus  do  not  parallel  taxonomic 


Clark,  Jr.  • OSCINE  TOE  FUSION 


69 


Table  1 

Fusion  of  Toes  III  and  IV  in  Oscines 


Taxa 

No.  species  examined 

Fusion* 

Alaudidae 

39 

L-M 

Hirundinidae 

43 

L-M 

plus 

Atticora 

2 

H 

Neochelidon 

1 

H 

Motacillidae 

47 

L 

Campephagidae 

53 

M-H 

plus 

Chlamydochaera 

1 

L 

Pycnonotidae 

12 

M-H 

plus 

Spizixos 

1 

L 

Hypsipetes 

1 

L 

Irenidae 

4 

M-H 

Laniidae 

Prionopinae 

7 

M-H 

Malaconotinae 

24 

M-H 

plus 

Telophorus 

9 

L-H 

Laniinae 

22 

L-M 

Pityriasinae 

1 

H 

Vangidae 

9 

H 

plus 

Hypositta 

1 

H 

Bombycillidae 

6 

L 

plus 

Phainoptila 

1 

M 

Dulidae 

1 

M 

Cinclidae 

5 

L 

Troglodytidae 

20 

L-M 

Mimidae 

12 

L-M 

Prunellidae 

8 

L 

Muscicapidae 

Turdinae 

261 

L-M 

Orthonychinae 

Orth  onyx 

2 

H 

Androphobus 

1 

H 

Psophodes 

1 

M 

Sphenostoma 

1 

M 

Cinclosoma 

1 

L 

Eupetes 

1 

M 

Melampitta 

1 

L 

I frit  a 

1 

H 

70 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  1 

Continued 


Taxa 

No.  species  examined 

Fusion' 

Timaliinae 

38 

L-M 

plus 

Garritornis 

1 

H 

Stachyris 

10 

M-H 

Rhopocichla 

1 

H 

Macronus 

4 

M-H 

Micromacronus 

1 

H 

Timalia 

1 

H 

Pteruthius 

4 

M-H 

Alcippe 

7 

M-H 

Yuhina 

3 

M-H 

Panurinae 

7 

L-M 

Picathartinae 

1 

L 

Polioptilinae 

Microhates 

1 

H 

Ramphocaenus 

1 

H 

Polioptila 

2 

M 

Sylviinae 

128 

L-M 

plus 

Psamathia 

1 

H 

Cettia 

9 

L-H 

Hradypterus 

10 

L-H 

Acrocephalus 

21 

M-H 

Hippo!  ais 

3 

M-H 

Hathmocerciis 

1 

H 

Macrospheruis 

1 

H 

Malurinae 

Malurini 

12 

M-H 

plus 

A mytornis 

2 

L 

Stipiturus 

1 

L 

Acauthizini 

26 

L-M 

plus 

Gerygone 

5 

M-H 

Moliouini 

3 

M-H 

Eptliianurini 

4 

L-M 

Genus  incertae  sedis 

Larnprolia 

1 

M 

Muscicapinae 

Hradornis 

2 

L-M 

Melaenornis 

3 

L-M 

Fraseria 

1 

M 

Rhinomyias 

2 

L-M 

Ficedii  la 

15 

L-M 

Niltava 

12 

L-M 

Clark,  Jr.  • OSCINE  TOE  FUSION 


71 


Table  1 
Continued 

Taxa 

No.  species  examined 

Fusion'* 

Muscicapa 

14 

L-M 

Myioparus 

2 

L-M 

Humblotia 

1 

M 

Newtonia 

2 

M-H 

Microeca 

4 

M-H 

Peltops 

2 

M-H 

Petroica 

5 

L-M 

Tregellasia 

3 

M 

Eopsaltria 

2 

M 

Philentoma 

1 

H 

Poecilodryas 

4 

M-H 

Peneothello 

1 

M 

Pachycephal  apsis 

1 

H 

Platysteirinae 

16 

M-H 

Monarchinae 

54 

M-H 

Rhipidurinae 

22 

M-H 

Pachycephalinae 

34 

M-H 

plus 

Hylocitrea 

1 

L 

Genus  incertae  sedis 

Turnagra 

1 

M 

Aegithalidae 

7 

M-H 

Remizidae 

7 

M-H 

Paridae 

33 

M 

Sittidae 

22 

M-H 

Certhiidae 

6 

M-H 

Rhabdornithidae 

2 

M 

Climacteridae 

5 

H 

Dicaeidae 

48 

M-H 

Nectariniidae 

94 

M-H 

Zosteropidae 

47 

M-H 

Meliphagidae 

110 

M-H 

Emberizidae 

Emberizinae 

42 

L-M 

Catamblyrbyncbinae 

1 

M 

Cardinalinae 

8 

L 

Tbraupinae 

24 

L-M 

Tersininae 

1 

L 

Parulidae 

41 

L 

plus 

Zeledonia 

1 

L 

Drepanididae 

11 

L-M 

Vireonidae 

38 

M-H 

Icteridae 

19 

L-M 

72 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  1 
Continued 

Taxa 

No.  species  examined 

Fusion® 

Fringillidae 

Fringillinae 

1 

L 

Carduelinae 

17 

L-M 

Estrildidae 

9 

L-M 

Genus  incertae  sedis 

Pholidornis 

1 

M 

Ploceidae 

15 

L-M 

Sturnidae 

14 

L 

plus 

Buphagus 

1 

M 

Oriolidae 

22 

M-H 

Dicruridae 

17 

M-H 

Callaeidae 

3 

L 

(irallinidae 

4 

M 

Artamidae 

9 

M-H 

Cracticidae 

7 

M-H 

Ptilonorhynchidae 

11 

L-H 

Paradisaeidae 

31 

M-H 

Corvidae 

93 

L-M 

plus 

Platylophus 

1 

H 

Crypsirina 

2 

M-H 

Tern  minis 

1 

H 

“ Symbols:  I.  = low,  M = moderate,  H = hifih. 


groups  of  species.  Unfortunately,  little  has  been  reported  about  the  use  of 
the  feet  in  Cettia,  Bradypterus  and  Telophorus;  species  of  the  first  2 are 
widely  noted  as  difficult  to  observe  as  they  skulk  in  brush. 

Fusion  of  toes  II  and  III. — Fusion  of  toes  II  and  III  extends  less  far 
distally  than  that  between  III  and  IV.  The  relatively  few  taxa  with  fusion 
of  II  and  III  reaching  distally  to  the  vicinity  of  the  articulation  of  phalanges 
I and  2 of  toe  III  also  have  high  fusion  of  toes  III  and  IV,  e.g.,  Vangidae, 
Orthonyx,  Microbates,  Ramphocaenus,  certain  muscicapid  flycatchers, 
Hypositta,  Certhia,  Climacteris,  Vireonidae. 

Evolution  and  systematics. — The  taxonomic  distribution  of  different  de- 
grees of  fusion  including  variation  within  genera,  subfamilies  and  families 
shows  that  evolutionary  convergence  has  been  frequent.  Among  birds  as 
a whole,  and  among  oscines,  high  fusion  between  toes  is  probably  usually 
a derived,  rather  than  primitive,  condition.  However,  reduction  of  high 


Clark,  Jr.  • OSCINE  TOE  EUSION 


73 


fusion  remains  a hypothetical  possibility  and  might  have  occurred  occa- 
sionally. Raikow  (1978)  suggested  that  in  situations  where  evolutionarily 
primitive  and  derived  conditions  are  indeterminable,  systematists  should 
use  characters  phenetically  while  recognizing  that  resulting  hypotheses  on 
relationships  will  be  relatively  weak.  Any  systematic  suggestions  for  os- 
cines  based  heavily  on  similarity  of  toe  fusion  would  be  at  best  tentative, 
but  where  fusion  agrees  with  other  characters  in  differing  markedly  be- 
tween genera  traditionally  hypothesized  to  be  closely  related,  reconsider- 
ation of  affinities  seems  warranted,  as  in  certain  of  the  following  examples. 

The  monotypic  Chlamydochaera  from  Borneo  is  the  sole  genus  of  the 
cuckoo-shrikes  (Campephagidae)  with  low  fusion.  Ames  (1975)  found  that 
Chlamydochaera  was  unique  among  examined  campephagids  in  having  a 
thrush-like  syrinx  and  concluded  that  the  genus  belongs  in  the  thrushes 
(Turdidae).  The  low  fusion  of  the  toes  is  also  like  that  of  thrushes.  In 
addition,  Ames  found  thrush-like  syringes  in  the  muscicapine  genera  Bra- 
dornis,  Melaenornis,  Rhinomyias,  Ficedula,  Niltava  and  Muscicapa, 
which,  unlike  most  other  Old  World  flycatchers,  also  have  relatively  low 
toe  fusion  like  that  of  thrushes. 

Harrison  (1967)  suggested  that  the  babbler  Ifrita  from  New  Guinea  is 
closely  related  to  the  blue  wren  group  of  Clytomyias,  Chenorhamphus , 
Todopsis  and  Malurus  of  Australia  and  New  Guinea.  I find  that  all  these 
genera  share  a high  fusion  of  toes  111  and  IV.  In  addition,  Ifrita  has  a 
ridged  culmen  like  that  of  Clytomyias  and  shares  the  unusual  feature  of 
blue  feathering  on  the  head  with  Todopsis,  Malurus  and  male  Chenorham- 
phus. Thus,  several  characters  link  Ifrita  with  the  malurids,  indicating  a 
possible  relationship  not  reflected  in  traditional  classification.  Clytomyias, 
Chenorhamphus,  Todopsis  and  Malurus  have  an  unusual  gap  in  the  inter- 
scapular zone  of  the  spinal  feather  tract,  present  also  in  the  Australian 
grass-wrens  {Amytornis)  and  emu-wrens  {Stipiturus",  Harrison  1969);  the 
condition  of  Ifrita  in  this  regard  has  not  been  reported.  As  the  relatively 
terrestrial  Amytornis  and  Stipiturus  have  low  fusion,  caution  is  necessary 
in  using  fusion  as  a taxonomic  character  in  this  group. 

Dorst  (1960)  proposed  that  the  monotypic  genera  Tylas  and  Hypositta 
from  Madagascar  belong  in  the  family  Vangidae,  which  is  endemic  to  that 
island.  The  high  fusion  in  all  these  birds  is  compatible  with  his  suggestion, 
although  high  fusion  also  occurs  in  other  families  with  which  Tylas  has 
often  been  placed,  including  bulbuls  (Pycnonotidae).  In  Hypositta,  toe 
fusion  is  greater  than  in  typical  nuthatches  (Ridgway  1904:439;  this  study), 
and  Hypositta  is  thus  more  like  the  vangids  in  this  respect.  Also  from 
Madagascar  is  Newtonia,  the  sole  genus  of  the  Muscicapinae  outside  the 
Australian  region  having  moderate  to  high  fusion.  Although  superficial 
appearances  of  study  skins  can  be  highly  misleading  concerning  evolu- 


74 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


tionary  affinities,  Newtonia  brunneicauda  and  females  of  the  vangid  Cal- 
icalicus  madagascariensis  have  some  resemblance.  Further  consideration 
of  the  affinities  of  Newtonia  would  be  desirable. 

The  high  fusion  of  Cyclarhis,  Vireolanius,  Vireo  and  Hylophilus  sup- 
ports the  idea  that  these  genera  constitute  a monophyletic  group  (Barlow 
and  James  1975,  Raikow  1978).  Such  high  fusion  is  unusual  among  New 
World  oscines,  being  known  otherwise  from  Certhia  and  2 genera  of  swal- 
lows, Microbates,  Ramphocaenus.  Vireos  thus  differ  markedly  from  most 
New  World  9-primaried  oscines,  including  warblers,  tanagers,  blackbirds 
and  buntings. 

Use  of  the  feet. — As  relationships  between  the  degree  of  fusion  and  use 
of  the  feet  are  not  well  understood,  I have  emphasized  here  the  most 
conspicuous  taxonomic  differences,  for  associations  between  structure 
and  behavior  might  be  most  prominent  in  such  cases.  My  findings  support 
Riiggeberg’s  (1960)  conclusions,  based  on  a much  smaller  sample  of 
species,  that  high  fusion  often  occurs  in  arboreal  species  and  that  low 
fusion  is  typical  for  terrestrial  species.  Bock  and  Miller  (1959)  indicated 
that  the  high  fusion  of  syndactyly  in  nonpasserines  was  advantageous  in 
arboreal  perching  because  the  parallel  position  of  the  toes  applies  all  the 
force  of  flexion  directly  against  a branch;  separated  toes  would  presumably 
he  mechanically  less  efficient.  In  those  climbing  oscines  with  syndactyly, 
the  forward  toes  are  restrained  in  a roughly  parallel  orientation  that  pos- 
sibly helps  to  ensure  a secure  grasp  on  vertical  surfaces. 

My  observations  of  live  oscines  and  of  published  photographs  indicate 
that  birds  with  low  fusion  vary  the  spread  of  the  forward  toes  considerably 
according  to  the  kind  of  perch.  On  the  ground  or  other  flat  surfaces,  these 
toes  are  widely  separated,  presumably  providing  a stable  base  for  standing 
or  moving.  However,  on  horizontal  perches  of  a small  diameter  relative  to 
foot  size  the  forward  toes  are  held  close  together,  a position  equivalent  to 
that  of  syndactyly,  with  presumably  similar  advantages.  On  sharply  in- 
clined perches  of  small  diameter,  toe  II  of  the  lower  foot  is  often  abducted 
from  III  and  IV,  which  are  held  close  together  (Leisler  1972;  see  also 
Willis  1969,  1972);  the  application  of  forces  in  2 directions  against  the 
perch  by  toe  II,  as  opposed  to  III  and  IV,  presumably  helps  to  prevent 
the  foot  from  slipping  down  the  perch.  Leisler  (1972)  has  provided  further 
details  on  the  relationships  between  toe  position  and  body  orientation  of 
small  oscines  perched  on  vertical  stems. 

A lack  of  absolute  associations  between  degree  of  syndactyly  and  use 
of  the  feet  prevents  the  use  of  structure  to  predict  habits  of  species  not 
studied  alive.  For  example,  the  climbing  Black-and-white  Warbler  {Mni- 
otilta  varia)  has  low  fusion,  like  that  of  allied  nonclimbing  parulids  (Parkes 
1978;  this  study),  in  contrast  to  the  moderate  to  high  fusion  of  many  trunk- 


Clark,  Jr.  • OSCINE  TOE  FUSION 


75 


climbers  in  other  families.  The  tree  creepers  (Certhia),  which  have  highly 
fused  forward  toes  and  stiffened  tail  feathers,  are  climbers.  Because  Or- 
thonyx  from  Australia  and  New  Guinea  have  the  same  structural  charac- 
teristics they  might  be  expected  also  to  be  climbers;  however,  Orthonyx 
actually  forage  terrestrially,  propping  themselves  on  the  ground  with  1 leg 
and  stiffened  tail  while  scratching  in  the  litter  with  the  other  foot  (Zusi 
1978).  As  another  example  of  absence  of  absolute  associations  between 
toe  fusion  and  degree  of  arboreality,  many  arboreal  species  have  only  low 
to  moderate  fusion,  e.g.,  kinglets  (Regulus)  and  New  World  orioles  (Ic- 
terus). Possibly  such  arboreal  birds  with  low  fusion  differ  in  perching  hab- 
its from  those  with  high  fusion,  but  evidence  is  lacking. 

Although  asynchronous  terrestrial  gaits  (walking  and  running)  are  char- 
acteristic for  terrestrial  oscines,  and  synchronous  (hopping)  for  arboreal 
species,  there  are  many  exceptions  (Clark  1975),  and  degree  of  toe  fusion 
is  not  absolutely  associated  with  gait,  except  that  walking  is  apparently 
exceptional  in  oscines  with  high  fusion.  Furthermore,  no  direct  association 
exists  between  degree  of  fusion  and  the  ability  to  hold  food  with  the  feet 
(Clark  1973).  Relationships  between  fusion  and  uses  of  the  feet  may  not 
be  apparent  in  many  cases  without  simultaneously  considering  many  other 
aspects  of  structure  and  behavior. 

SUMMARY 

The  degree  of  toe  fusion,  here  summarized  for  the  oscine  families,  probably  has  only 
limited  taxonomic  usefulness,  but  may  serve  along  with  other  characters  to  detect  genera 
possibly  needing  further  systematic  study,  e.g.,  Chlamydochaera,  Ifrita,  .\eivtonia.  Terres- 
trial species  often  have  lower  fusion  than  do  arboreal  or  climbing  species,  but  there  are 
important  exceptions.  Low  fusion  apparently  aids  balance  on  flat  substrates,  whereas  high 
fusion  facilitates  perching  or  climbing.  The  large  number  of  arboreal  species  with  low  fusion 
has  not  yet  been  satisfactorily  explained. 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

I thank  G.  E.  Watson,  J.  Farrand,  Jr.  and  I.  C.  J.  Galbraith  for  providing  access  to 
specimens  in  their  care.  R.  L.  Zusi  gave  helpful  advice  and  J.  A.  Slater  made  useful  sug- 
gestions on  an  earlier  version  of  the  manuscript.  Miss  Mary  Hubbard  prepared  the  illustra- 
tion. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Ames,  P.  L.  1975.  The  application  of  syringeal  morphology  to  the  classification  of  the  Old 
World  insect  eaters  (Muscicapidae).  Bonn.  Zool.  Beitr.  26:107-134. 

Barlow,  J.  C.  and  R.  D.  James.  1975.  Aspects  of  the  biolog>-  of  the  Chestnut-sided  Shrike- 
vireo.  Wilson  Bull.  87:320-334. 

Bock,  W'.  J.  and  W.  Dew.  Miller.  1959.  The  scansorial  foot  of  the  woodpeckers,  with 
comments  on  the  evolution  of  perching  and  climbing  feet  in  birds.  Am.  Mus.  Novit. 
1931. 


76 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  \o.  1,  March  1981 


Clark,  G.  A.,  Jr.  1973.  Holding  food  with  the  feet  in  passerines.  Bird-Banding  44:91-99. 

. 1975.  Additional  records  of  passerine  terrestrial  gaits.  Wilson  BuU.  87:384-389. 

. 1977.  Foot-scutes  in  North  American  oscines.  Bird-Banding  48:301-308. 

Dorst,  J.  1960.  A propos  des  affinities  systematiques  de  deux  oiseaux  malagaches:  Tylas 
eduardi  et  Hypositta  corallirostris.  L,  Oiseau  30:259-269. 

Hall,  B.  P.  and  R.  E.  Moreau.  1970.  An  atlas  of  speciation  in  African  passerine  birds. 
Br.  Mus.  (Nat.  Hist.),  London,  England. 

Harrison,  C.  J.  O.  1967.  The  apparent  affinities  of  Ifrita.  Bull.  Br.  Orn.  Club  87:97- 

100. 

. 1969.  The  affinities  of  the  blue  wren  genus  Malurus  and  related  genera:  with  special 

reference  to  the  grass-wren  genus  Amytornis.  Emu  69:1-8. 

Leisler,  B.  1972.  Artmerkmale  am  Fuss  adulter  Teich-  und  Sumpfrohrsanger  (Acroceph- 
alus  scirpaceus,  A.  palustris)  und  ihre  Funktion.  J.  F.  Orn.  113:366-373. 

Moron Y,  J.  J.,  Jr.,  W.  J.  Bock  and  J.  Farrand,  Jr.  1975.  Reference  list  of  the  birds  of 
the  world.  Dept.  Ornithology,  Am.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.,  New  York,  New  York. 

Parkes,  K.  C.  1978.  Still  another  parulid  intergeneric  hybrid  (Mniotilta  X Dendroica)  and 
its  taxonomic  and  evolutionar>-  implications.  Auk  95:682-690. 

Raikow,  R.  j.  1978.  Appendicular  myolog>  and  relationships  of  the  New  orld  nine-pri- 
maried  oscines  (Aves:  Passeriformes).  BuU.  Carnegie  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.  7:1-43. 

R-AND,  a.  L.  and  M.  a.  Traylor,  Jr.  1953.  The  systematic  position  of  the  genera  Ram- 
phocaenus  and  Microbates.  Auk  70:334-337. 

Ridgway,  R.  1901-07.  The  birds  of  North  and  Middle  America.  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  BuU.  50, 
Pts.  I-IV. 

Ruggeberg,  T.  1960.  Zur  funktionellen  Anatomic  der  hinteren  Extremitat  einiger  mitteleu- 
rop’aischer  Singvogelarten.  Z.  ^ iss.  Zool.  164:1-106. 

ILLIS,  E.  O.  1969.  On  the  behavior  of  five  species  of  Rhegmatorhina,  ant-followdng  ant- 
hirds  of  the  Amazon  Basin.  \\  ilson  BuU.  81:363-395. 

. 1972.  The  behavior  of  Spotted  Antbirds.  Ornithol.  Monogr.  No.  10. 

Zusi,  R.  L.  1978.  Notes  on  song  and  feeding  behaviour  of  Ort/jony.v  Emu  78:156- 

157. 

BIOLOGICAL  SCIENCES  GROUP,  UMV.  CONNECTICUT,  STORRS,  CONNECTI- 
CUT 06268.  ACCEPTED  25  APR.  1980. 


j 

i 

SEVENTH  INTERNATIONAL  CONEERENCE  ON  BIRD 
CENSUS  ORK  AND  EIETH  MEETING  OE  THE 
EUROPEAN  ATLAS  COMMITTEE 

An  international  conference  for  those  involved  or  interested  in  bird  census  and/or  atlas 
work  will  convene  8-12  September  1981  at  the  Universidad  de  Leon,  Spain.  For  further 
information  write:  Prof.  Francisco  Purroy,  Departamento  de  Zoologia,  Facultad  de  Biologia, 

Leon,  Spain. 


1 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  77-84 


ENVIRONMENTAL  EFFECTS  ON  ROOSTING 
BEHAVIOR  OF  CHIMNEY  SWIFTS 

Richard  M.  Zammuto  and  Edwin  C.  Franks 

The  Chimney  Swift  (Chaetura  pelagica)  is  widespread  and  abundant  in 
eastern  North  America.  The  lengthy  migration  and  long-unknown  winter- 
ing grounds  of  this  species  early  attracted  attention,  but  detailed  ecological 
research  on  this  species  began  only  in  the  middle  of  this  century  (Fischer 
1958).  Others  have  focused  on  responses  by  swifts  to  environmental  con- 
ditions. Ramsey  (1970),  for  example,  studied  the  effect  of  changing  am- 
bient temperatures  upon  internal  body  temperatures,  and  Michael  and 
Chao  (1973)  showed  associations  between  roosting  behavior,  time  of  sunset 
and  light  intensity.  Here  we  relate  roosting  and  other  behaviors  to  several 
environmental  variables. 

STUDY  AREA  AND  METHODS 

Our  studies  were  conducted  in  Macomb,  McDonough  Co.,  Illinois.  Midsummer  daily  tem- 
peratures range  from  18-36°C.  The  center  of  the  city  has  several  blocks  of  contiguous  3-  to 
5-story  buildings;  the  central  area  is  surrounded  on  each  side  by  about  10  blocks  of  1-  to  3- 
story  homes  averaging  10  m apart.  Because  Macomb  is  surrounded  by  cultivated  farmland 
and  is  the  largest  city  (population  23,000)  in  a 60  km  radius,  it  holds  the  major  portion  of  the 
Chimney  Swift  population  in  the  area. 

The  first  Chimney  Swifts  usually  arrive  in  mid-April.  Numbers  are  low  until  late  April  and 
early  May,  when  the  first  large  flocks  are  seen.  The  city  contains  a large  population  of  swifts 
(1000-3000)  until  nest-building  begins  in  late  May,  when  numbers  decline  to  fewer  than  1000. 
During  June  and  July,  most  chimneys  with  swifts  contain  1 breeding  pair  with  occasional 
visitors  or  nest  helpers  (see  Dexter  1952,  1974).  A few  chimneys  contain  flocks  consisting  of 
non-breeding  swifts  (up  to  300)  or  both  a nesting  pair  and  a flock  (Zammuto  and  Franks 
1978).  Nesting  occurs  throughout  the  city  from  early  June  to  August.  Population  numbers 
peak  during  September  and  slowly  decrease  until  mid-October  when  all  the  swifts  are  gone 
for  the  winter. 

Roosts  of  6 or  more  individuals  were  analyzed.  We  located  most  roost-sites  from  a car  at 
dusk  by  watching  for  circling  flocks  of  swifts.  Individual  roost-sites  were  studied  for  several 
mornings  and/or  evenings.  Those  sites  used  most  by  swifts  were  observed  most  often. 

A photometer  placed  on  top  of  a car  roof  facing  the  open  sky  was  used  to  measure  light 
intensity.  For  each  foot-candle  (fc)  change  in  light  intensity  during  exit  or  entrance  by  swifts, 
the  time  and  number  of  birds  (tallied  on  hand  counter)  that  left  or  entered  a chimney  were 
recorded.  These  data  were  tested  using  exponential  curvilinear  correlation  and  regression 
analyses. 

The  times  when  the  majority  of  swifts  left  or  entered  a roost-site  were  determined  with  a 
stopwatch  and  an  average  time,  designated  as  the  time  of  peak  exit  or  entrance,  was  computed. 
Temperatures  and  wind  velocities  were  measured  1.5  m above  ground  near  each  roost-site. 
Cloud  cover  was  estimated  to  the  nearest  5%.  These  measurements  and  the  times  of  sunrise 
and  sunset  were  examined  in  relation  to  the  time  of  peak  exit  and  entrance  at  the  roost-site 
using  multiple  regression  and  correlation  analyses.  The  mean  difference  between  time  of 

77 


78 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  \o.  1,  March  1981 


peak  exit  and  time  of  sunrise  and  between  time  of  peak  entrance  and  time  of  sunset  were 
analysed  with  regard  to  sky  haziness  and  precipitation  using  t-tests.  Sky  haziness  indicated 
that  a fair  sky  was  somewhat  hazy,  not  clear  blue.  Precipitation  was  recorded  as  present  or 
absent.  Throughout  this  report,  means  appear  with  ±1  SD  unless  otherwise  indicated. 

RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

The  mean  number  of  Chimney  Swifts  recorded  leaving  a roost-site  (N  = 

32)  was  70  on  203  mornings,  and  the  mean  number  entering  was  83  on  166 
evenings.  A total  of  224  flock  departures  or  entries  was  observed  at  the 
32  different  chimneys.  Of  the  224,  65%  of  the  departures  or  entries  were 
of  less  than  100  individuals  (Fig.  1). 

Behavior  at  departure. — The  swifts  could  usually  be  heard  calling  inside 
the  chimney  when  we  arrived  at  the  roost  0.5  h before  daylight.  The  calling 
became  louder  as  daylight  approached  (about  0.2  fc),  and  continued  during  | 
exit  by  swifts.  | 

On  many  mornings,  1-10  swifts  arrived  at  a roost-site  about  20  min  j 

before  any  swTfts  left.  Sometimes  they  circled  the  chimney  and  then  flew^  | 

away  but  more  often  they  entered  the  chimney.  The  roosting  birds  called  I 

more  loudly  if  any  of  these  swifts  entered  the  chimney,  but  the  calling  j 

subsided  after  about  15  sec. 

Swifts  departed  roost-sites  singly  or  in  small  groups,  almost  always  in 
a steady  flow  at  rates  of  4-150  per  min.  Sometimes  a 30-min  interval 
occurred  between  departure  of  2 substantial  portions  of  a flock.  ' 

Morning  returns  to  roost-site. — After  swifts  left  a chimney,  some  birds  i 
reentered  on  about  50%  of  the  mornings.  About  18%  of  14,144  departing  | 
birds  reentered  wTthin  30  min  after  first  departure,  and  3%  reentered  j 

between  30  and  60  min.  On  about  3%  of  the  mornings,  more  swifts  entered  ! 

a chimney  than  had  left  it  only  minutes  before. 

Swifts  often  circled  the  chimney  without  reentering  for  many  minutes. 
When  1 circling  swift  entered,  many  other  circling  birds  immediately  fol- 
lowed in  quick  succession;  the  rest  circled  without  reentering  for  several 
more  minutes  until  another  bird  entered,  followed  by  another  portion  of 
the  group. 

Weather  conditions  seemed  to  affect  reentry  into  the  roost.  On  cold  or 
rainy  mornings,  over  90%  of  the  swifts  that  left  a chimney  reentered  it 
within  30  min.  On  these  mornings,  some  were  reentering  while  others  ' 
were  leaving.  A shortage  of  insects  in  the  air  may  also  cause  reentry.  The 
number  of  insects  in  flight  was  likely  very  high  at  daybreak  but  probably  , 
declined  sharply  after  sunrise  (McClure  1938;  Click  1939,  1957).  Reduced  | 
aerial  prey  may  have  caused  the  swifts  to  reenter  the  roost-site  at  sunrise  j 

(on  the  average  of  11  min  after  departure)  where  they  remained  until  later  i 

in  the  morning.  | 

Evening  observations  of  behavior. — Entry  into  the  roost  in  the  evening 


I 


Zammuto  and  Franks  • CHIMNEY  SWIFT  ROOSTING  BEHAVIOR 


79 


NUMBER  OF  SWIFTS  PER  FLOCK 

Fig.  1.  Size  of  224  flocks. 


is  described  in  detail  by  various  authors  (James  1950,  Zammuto  1978). 
Often  more  than  50  swifts  per  min  entered  the  chimney  if  light  levels 
dropped  to  0.5  fc  with  most  of  the  flock  still  outside.  Reentry  rate  in  the 
morning  was  usually  lower,  so  we  believe  approaching  darkness  is  a major 
I stimulus  for  entering  the  roost  at  dusk.  However,  as  reported  by  Calhoun 
(1938),  there  were  many  evenings  when  only  a few  swifts  were  seen  at  any 
one  time  near  a roost-site.  As  soon  as  they  entered,  additional  small  groups 
moved  into  the  area  and  entered  the  chimney  until  the  whole  flock  was 
: inside. 

i Alterations  in  roosting  behavior. — Various  human-related  activity  dis- 
rupted swift  roosting  patterns.  Fumes  from  furnaces,  noises  from  loud 

I vehicles,  slamming  doors  and  voices  sometimes  caused  the  swifts  to  leave 
ji  the  roost  early,  or  caused  circling  swifts  to  disperse  from  the  roost-site. 
4 On  60%  of  the  summer  evenings  loudly  calling  swifts  flying  near  roost- 

ii  sites  seemingly  caused  all  the  swifts  in  the  area  to  disperse,  although  most 
I usually  returned  within  3 min. 

I On  nearly  50%  of  the  evenings,  late-arriving  individual  swifts  circled 
I the  roost  a few  times  but  then  flew  off  (Coffey  1936).  Koskimies  (1950)  felt 
I;  that  such  behavior  in  the  Common  Swift  {Apus  apus)  indicated  it  was  too 
•I  dark  for  the  swifts  to  see  well  enough  to  enter  the  roost,  and  thus  swifts 
'I  overtaken  by  nightfall  spent  the  night  on  the  wing. 


80 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  1 

Monthly  Mean  Light  Intensity  (Foot-Candles)  When  the  Swifts  Entered  the 

Roost 

Date 

No.  swifts 

No.  evenings 
observed 

Mean  ± SE 

Light 

intensity  mode 

Sept.  1976 

207 

3 

0.6  ± 0.35 

0.5 

Oct.  1976 

623 

8 

0.7  ± 0.17 

0.5 

April  1977 

401 

4 

0.5  ± 0.28 

0.5 

May  1977 

1937 

18 

1.8  ± 0.30 

0.5 

June  1977 

2853 

25 

2.7  ± 0.23 

0.5 

July  1977 

2474 

14 

3.3  ± 0.33 

0.5 

-Aug.  1977 

886 

10 

3.5  ± 0.44 

1.5 

Sept.  1977 

1934 

19 

1.0  ± 0.14 

0.5 

Oct.  1977 

183 

2 

1.1  ± 0.37 

0.5 

Total 

11,498 

103  overall  x 

= 1.7  ± 1.2« 

0.6 

® Standard  deviation  of  the  9 monthly  means  shown. 


Light  intensity. — Morning  departure  of  the  swifts  was  more  widely  dis- 
tributed at  very  low  light  intensities  than  was  evening  descent.  Based  on 
12,430  birds,  70%  left  the  chimney  during  light  of  0 and  7 fc,  while  a 
similar  percentage  of  11,498  l)irds  entered  between  a more  restrictive  0 
and  2 fc. 

Light  intensity  was  negatively  correlated  with  the  number  of  swifts  leav- 
ing the  roost  each  morning  [r  = —0.87,  P < 0.001)  and  entering  the  roost 
each  evening  (r  = —0.63,  P < 0.001).  The  regression  formula  for  the 


Table  2 

Monthly  Mean  Light  Intensity  (Foot-Candles)  V\  hen  the  Swifts  Left  the  Roost 


Date 

No.  swifts 

No.  mornings 
observed 

Mean  ± SE 

Light 

intensity  mode 

Oct.  1976 

634 

11 

6.7  ± 0.62 

0.5 

April  1977 

391 

3 

8.0  ± 1.56 

2.5 

May  1977 

1668 

16 

2.3  ± 0.21 

1.5 

June  1977 

2104 

25 

3.7  ± 0.24 

2.5 

July  1977 

2982 

17 

7.0  ± 0.46 

2.5 

Aug.  1977 

1900 

13 

4.1  ± 0.49 

3.5 

Sept.  1977 

2185 

16 

8.1  ± 0.44 

7.5 

Oct.  1977 

394 

4 

4.4  ± 0.57 

2.5 

Total 

12,430 

105  overall  .v 

= 5.5  ± 2.2^ 

2.9 

Standard  deviation  of  the  8 monthly  means  shown. 


Zammuto  and  Franks  • CHIMNEY  SWIFT  ROOSTING  BEHAVIOR 


81 


Table  3 

Equations  and  Correlations  Showing  the  Effect  of  Environmental  Variables  on 
THE  Mean  Time  of  Peak  Exit  and  Entrance  at  the  Roost-site 


Exit  Entrance 


Independent 

variable 

Equation® 

Coeffi- 
cient of 
determi- 
nation** 

No. 

morn- 

ings 

Equation® 

Coeffi- 
cient of 
determi- 
nation** 

No. 

even- 

ings 

Time  of 
sunrise 
Time  of 
sunset 

Temperature 

Y 

= -0.37  + l.OX 

0.96*** 

114 

Y = 0.40  + l.OX 

0.98*** 

114 

(°C) 

Wind  speed 

Y 

= 7.1  - 0.072X 

0.43*** 

114 

Y = 18.2  + 0.092X 

0.32*** 

114 

(km/h) 

Percent  cloud 

Y 

= 6.1  - 0.044X 

0.05* 

113 

Y = 20.1  + 0.06X 

0.05** 

114 

cover 

Y 

= 6.0  - 0.002X 

0.02 

109 

Y = 20.2  + 0.002X 

0.01 

112 

® Where  Y is  the  mean  time  (24-h  clock,  CDT)  of  peak  exit  from  or  entrance  into  the  roost-site  and  where  X is  the 
independent  variable  listed. 

Levels  of  significance  as  determined  by  F tests:  *P  < 0.05,  **P  < 0.01,  ***P  < 0.001. 


number  of  swifts  (Y)  leaving  and  entering  the  roost  with  respect  to  each 
level  of  light  intensity  (fc)  was  Y = 17. at  dawn  and  Y = 13. 
in  the  evening. 

The  swifts  entered  the  roost  at  significantly  higher  light  intensities  in 
the  warmer  months  (May  through  August)  than  in  the  colder  months  (April, 
September,  October)  {t  = 5.68,  df  = 7,  P < 0.001)  (Table  1).  In  a Texas 
study  by  Michael  and  Chao  (1973),  May  through  August  was  also  when 
swifts  entered  the  roost  at  higher  mean  light  intensities,  although  in  their 
study,  light  intensities  at  entrance  were  much  higher  (ranging  between  2.0 
and  14.2  fc,  x = 5.9  ± 3.7)  than  the  ones  we  observed  (0.5-3. 5 fc,  x = 
2.0  ± 1.1;  for  1977,  Table  1).  Mean  monthly  light  intensities  at  which 
swifts  left  roost-sites  are  shown  in  Table  2. 

Sunrise  and  sunset. — The  times  of  sunrise  and  sunset  were  more  closely 
associated  with  the  mean  time  of  peak  exit  or  entrance  at  the  roost  than 
temperature,  wind  speed,  or  cloudiness  (Table  3). 

Swifts  left  the  roost  about  11  min  before  sunrise  and  entered  it  about 
21  min  after  sunset  (Table  4).  There  was  less  than  a 10  min  difference 
among  monthly  means.  In  spring  and  summer  in  Texas,  swifts  entered  the 
roost  much  sooner  after  sunset  (Jc  ==  14  ± 2 min,  range  12-16  min)  (Mi- 
chael and  Chao  1973)  than  did  swifts  in  our  study  for  all  months. 

Temperatures. — Swifts  left  the  roost  significantly  later  and  entered  it 


82 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  4 

Times  Before  Sunrise  and  After  Sunset  When  Swifts  Left  and  Entered  the 
Roost-site  at  the  Fastest  Rate 


Date 

No. 

mornings 

Monthly  mean  i 
before  sunrise 

minutes 

± se 

No. 

evenings 

Monthly  mean  minutes 
after  sunset  ± SE 

Sept.  1976 

11 

9.8 

± 

1.8 

11 

22.4 

± 1.3 

Oct.  1976 

11 

11.7 

1.7 

7 

22.6 

± 1.7 

April  1977 

3 

7.7 

2.9 

4 

21.8 

± 0.8 

May  1977 

15 

17.3 

0.9 

18 

23.1 

± 1.3 

June  1977 

24 

12.2 

1.7 

27 

21.9 

± 1.2 

July  1977 

17 

10.7 

± 

0.9 

15 

18.0 

± 2.1 

Aug.  1977 

13 

12.3 

± 

2.7 

13 

17.1 

± 0.8 

Sept.  1977 

16 

9.0 

-h 

1.2 

18 

18.6 

± 0.7 

Oct.  1977 

4 

8.9 

-h 

1.9 

Total 

114 

overall  x — 11.1 

2.8^ 

total  113 

overall  x — 20.7 

± 2.4b 

“ Standard  deviation  of  the  9 monthly  means  shown. 
.Standard  deviation  of  the  8 monthly  means  shown. 


significantly  earlier  on  colder  days  (Table  3).  Koskimies  (1950)  found  that 
A.  apus  did  not  leave  the  roost-site  until  the  air  temperature  was  high 
enough  for  normal  numbers  of  flying  insects  to  be  available.  Chimney 
Swifts  may  behave  similarly.  In  southern  Texas,  Click  (1939,  1957)  found 
that  the  greatest  numbers  of  insects  were  in  the  air  at  25°C,  and  that 
numbers  of  insects  decreased  considerably  below  18°C;  very  few  were 
flying  at  temperatures  below  15.6°C. 

Wind  speed,  cloud  cover  and  haziness. — With  higher  wind  speeds,  the 
swifts  leave  the  roost  earlier  in  the  morning  and  stay  out  later  in  the 
evening  (Table  3).  Numbers  of  flying  insects  may  be  an  important  factor; 
Click  (1939,  1957)  found  that  the  abundance  of  insects  in  flight  decreased 
as  winds  dropped  below  8 km/h.  Therefore,  fewer  insects  were  flying  and 
presumably  less  food  was  available  for  swifts  on  calmer  days.  It  may  be 
more  advantageous  to  be  roosting  when  the  food  supply  reaches  some 
lower  threshold.  Neither  cloud  cover  (Table  3)  nor  sky  haziness  had  sig- 
nificant {P  > 0.05)  effects  on  the  time  of  peak  exit  or  entrance. 

Precipitation. — Rainfall  signihcantly  delayed  the  time  of  peak  exit  from 
the  roost  (mean  exit  time  on  10  rainy  mornings  = 0.3  min  before  sunrise, 
mean  on  103  fair  mornings  = 12.5  min  before  sunrise,  P < 0.001).  On  rainy 
mornings,  the  swifts  usually  stayed  in  the  chimney  until  the  rain  stopped, 
but  if  they  did  depart  in  the  rain,  most  quickly  reentered.  In  sporadic  rain, 
the  reentry  corresponded  to  the  periods  of  rain.  Rainy  weather  also  delayed 
the  onset  of  daily  activity  in  A.  apus  (Koskimies  1950),  probably  due  to  a re- 
duction of  tbe  food  supply.  If  the  rain  continued  for  several  consecutive 


Zammuto  and  Franks  • CHIMNEY  SWIFT  ROOSTING  BEHAVIOR 


83 


days,  the  Chimney  Swifts  did  not  return  to  the  roost  in  the  morning  after  the 
first  2 days.  They  were  probably  forced  to  hunt  in  the  poor  weather  to  keep 
from  starving,  or  moved  elsewhere. 

On  5 days.  Chimney  Swifts  circled  roost-sites  when  summer  afternoon 
thunderstorms  threatened.  During  most  storm  threats.  Chimney  Swifts 
flew  low,  resuming  their  normal  clear  day  flying  height  when  the  threat 
passed  as  observed  for  Black  Swifts  {Cypseloides  niger)  (Rathbun  1925). 
Between  intermittent  rain  showers,  the  swifts  we  were  studying  usually 
flew  in  small  flocks,  flying  lower  than  usual.  They  may  have  been  following 
their  food  supply;  Koskimies  (1950)  found  that  many  insects  were  washed 
down  to  the  ground  by  rain.  Some  swifts  in  our  study  were  repeatedly 
observed  flying  close  to  lawns  when  it  rained  (Zammuto  and  Franks  1979). 
A.  apus  reduced  foraging  activity  in  rain  (Koskimies  1950,  Lack  and  Lack 
1951). 

When  it  rained.  Chimney  Swifts  entered  the  roost-site  earlier  (mean  of 
8 rainy  evenings  = 16.3  min  after  sunset,  mean  of  104  fair  evenings  = 
20.9  min  after  sunset,  P < 0.05).  On  most  rainy  evenings  about  10%  of 
any  flock  entered  the  roost  30  min  before  the  rest  of  the  birds.  If  the  rain 
stopped  after  such  early  entrances  some  birds  left  the  roost. 

James  (1950),  Bowman  (1952)  and  Fischer  (1958)  noticed  increases,  but 
Dexter  (1966,  1968)  noticed  decreases  in  roosting  flock  sizes  during  pro- 
longed, cool,  rainy  periods.  In  our  study,  swift  numbers  increased  at  10 
' roost-sites  during  10  such  periods.  Some  flock  sizes  doubled,  reaching  200 
' or  more  birds.  Since  in  the  summer  months  these  added  birds  in  our  study 
were  probably  not  migrants  (Zammuto  1978),  we  postulate  that  they  moved 
from  smaller  roosts.  Presumably,  loss  of  body  heat  would  be  minimized 
j when  many  swifts  formed  tight  roosting  clusters  than  when  only  a few 

I swifts  were  present.  This  would  allow  the  swifts  to  survive  longer  without 

I food  since  they  stayed  in  the  roost-site  during  cool,  rainy  weather.  Dexter 

(1966)  noted  that  flocks  he  observed  decreased  in  numbers  in  inclement 
^ weather  and  he  hypothesized  that  absent  birds  were  roosting  in  warmer 

. chimneys  elsewhere.  Our  experience  suggests  that  they  were  in  a larger 

' roost,  but  we  never  studied  small  roosts  during  rainy  periods  to  see  if  swift 

f numbers  decreased. 

SUMMARY 

Roosting  of  Chimney  Swifts  was  studied  in  relation  to  several  environmental  variables  in 
September  and  October  1976  and  April  through  October  1977.  An  average  of  70  swifts  were 
recorded  leaving  and  83  entering  32  different  roost-sites  on  203  mornings  and  166  evenings. 
Of  224  flocks,  65%  numbered  below  100  individuals.  About  18%  of  the  swifts  that  left  the 
roost-site  at  dawn  returned  to  it  to  reenter  around  sunrise.  In  the  morning,  70%  of  the  swifts 
left  the  chimney  during  light  intensities  of  0-7  fc,  but  in  the  evenings  70%  entered  between 
a more  restrictive  0 and  2 fc.  Swifts  left  the  roost-site  at  5.5  ± 2.2  fc,  11.1  ± 2.8  min  before 
sunrise  and  entered  at  1.7  ± 1.2  fc,  20.7  ± 2.4  min  after  sunset.  Light  intensity,  time  of 


84 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


sunrise  and  sunset,  temperature,  wind  speed  and  precipitation  were  all  associated  with  the 
time  of  departure  from  and  entrance  into  the  roost-site.  Swifts  left  the  roost-site  later  and 
entered  it  earlier  on  colder  days,  on  days  with  precipitation,  and  on  calmer  days.  The  effect 
of  sky  haziness  and  cloud  cover  upon  time  of  peak  exit  and  entrance  was  not  significant.  All 
the  environmental  variables  reported  to  affect  flying  insect  abundance  were  similarly  asso- 
ciated with  swift  activity  patterns. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Donald  Campbell  and  Robert  Reason  assisted  with  the  statistical  analyses,  and  Tate  Lin- 
dahl helped  with  the  computer  programming.  Julie  Zammuto  helped  in  the  field,  and  she 
and  Judy  Zielinski  provided  valuable  secretarial  assistance,  Charles  Collins,  Ralph  Dexter, 
Gary  Schnell  and  Jerrold  Zar  suggested  improvements  for  earlier  drafts. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Bowman,  R.  I.  1952.  Chimney  Swift  banding  at  Kingston,  Ontario  from  1928  to  1947.  Can. 
Field-Nat.  66:151-164. 

Calhoun,  J.  B.  1938.  1938  Swift  banding  at  Nashville  and  Clarksville.  Migrant  9:77-81. 
Coffey,  B.  B.  1936.  Chimney  Swift  migration  at  Memphis.  Migrant  7:77-82,  96-98. 
Dexter,  R.  W.  1952.  Extra-parental  cooperation  in  the  nesting  of  Chimney  Swifts.  Wilson 
Bull.  64:133-139. 

. 1966.  Analysis  of  Chimney  Swift  returns  at  Kent,  Ohio,  in  1964  and  1965,  with 

notes  on  a declining  nesting  population.  Bird-Banding  37:120-121. 

. 1968.  Analysis  of  the  1966  and  1967  returns  of  Chimney  Swifts  at  Kent,  Ohio.  Bird- 

Banding  39:56-57. 

. 1974.  Unusually  large  numbers  of  Chimney  Swifts  at  a nest.  Bird-Banding  45:365. 

Fischer,  R.  B.  1958.  The  breeding  biology  of  the  Chimney  Swift,  Chaetura  pelagica  (Lin- 
naeus). N.Y.  State  Mus.  Bull.  368. 

Click,  P.  A.  1939.  The  distribution  of  insects,  spiders  and  mites  in  the  air.  U.S.  Dept. 
Agric.  Tech.  Bull.  673. 

. 1957.  Collecting  insects  by  airplane  in  southern  Texas.  U.S.  Dept.  Agric.  Tech. 

Bull.  1158. 

James,  P.  1950.  Spring  flocking  of  Chimney  Swifts  (Chaetura  pelagica  Linnaeus)  at  Cornell 
University.  Bird-Banding  21:9-11. 

Koskimies,  J.  1950.  The  life  of  the  Swift,  Micropus  apus  (L.)  in  relation  to  the  weather. 
Ann.  Acad.  Sci.  Fennica  15:1-151. 

Lack,  D.  and  E.  Lack.  1951.  The  breeding  biology  of  the  Swift,  Apus  apus.  Ibis  93:501- 
548. 

McClure,  H.  E.  1938.  Insect  aerial  populations.  Ann.  Ent.  Soc.  Am.  31:504-513. 
Michael,  E.  D.  and  W.  H.  Chao.  1973.  Migration  and  roosting  of  Chimney  Swifts  in  east 
Texas.  Auk  90:100-105. 

Ramsey,  J.  J.  1970.  Temperature  changes  in  Chimney  Swifts  (Chaetura  pelagica)  at  lowered 
environmental  temperatures.  Condor  72:264-265. 

Rathbun,  S.  F.  1925.  The  Black  Swift  and  its  habits.  Auk  42:497-516. 

Zammuto,  R.  M.  1978.  Seasonal  activity  of  the  Chimney  Swift  (Chaetura  pelagica)  popu- 
lation in  Macomb,  Illinois.  M.  Sc.  thesis.  Western  Illinois  Univ.,  Macomb,  Illinois. 

AND . 1979.  Chimney  Swifts  apparently  feeding  near  surfaces  of  lawns  and 

from  trees.  Inland  Bird  Banding  51:72-73. 

AND  E.  C.  Franks.  1978.  Forty  adult  Chimney  Swifts  at  an  active  nest.  Bird-Banding 

49:278-279. 

DEPT.  BIOLOGICAL  SCIENCES,  WESTERN  ILLINOIS  UNIV.,  MACOMB,  ILLINOIS 
61455.  (PRESENT  ADDRESS  RMZ:  DEPT.  ZOOLOGY,  UNIV.  WESTERN  ON- 
TARIO, LONDON,  ONTARIO  N6A  5B7  CANADA.)  ACCEPTED  20  DEC.  1979. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  85-92 


GENERAL  NOTES 


Observation  of  a brood  of  Sharp-shinned  Hawks  in  Ontario,  with  comments  on 
the  functions  of  sexual  dimorphism. — The  Sharp-shinned  Hawk  {Accipiter  striatus) 
exhibits  the  greatest  “reversed”  sexual  dimorphism  in  size  of  any  North  American  bird,  with 
males  averaging  100  g and  females  170  g (Mueller  and  Berger,  Auk  87:452-457,  1970).  Of 
the  several,  controversial  hypotheses  to  explain  the  extent  of  sexual  dimorphism  in  raptors, 
that  of  Snyder  and  Wiley  (Ornithol.  Monogr.  20,  1976)  is  perhaps  most  amenable  to  verifi- 
cation. Snyder  and  Wiley  have  suggested  that  food  stress  late  in  the  period  of  dependency 
of  the  young  has  acted  to  select  for  adults  of  greatly  different  body  sizes,  and  thus  different 
feeding  niches.  This  increases  the  range  of  size  of  prey  available  to  maintain  the  adults  and 
their  rapidly  growing  young.  Casual  observations  of  breeding  Sharp-shinned  Hawks  and 
small  bird  populations  in  Ontario  led  us  to  question  this  hypothesis  and  served  as  the  in- 
spiration for  this  study. 

The  senior  author  is  preparing  an  extensive  discussion  of  the  hypothesis  of  Snyder  and 
Wiley.  The  purpose  of  this  paper  is  simply  (1)  to  present  our  intensive  observations  of  1 
brood  of  Sharp-shinned  Hawks,  (2)  to  compare  our  results  with  those  of  3 nests  studied  by 
Snyder  and  Wiley,  and  (3)  to  offer  alternative  explanations  for  some  of  the  data.  Our  obser- 
vations are  unique  in  the  amount  of  time  and  effort  expended  in  observations  during  the 
post-fledgling  period  and  in  that  ours  is  the  first  detailed  study  from  eastern  Canada,  which 
appears  to  be  the  center  of  abundance  of  the  species  (Bent,  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  167,  1937). 

We  found  a brood  of  4 Sharp-shinned  Hawks  (3  females  and  1 male)  near  their  nest  in 
Burpee  Township,  Manitoulin  Island,  Ontario,  on  22  July  1977.  We  began  observations  on 
the  brood  that  afternoon  and  continued  daily  until  noon  on  8 August  when  we  had  to  depart. 
Two  to  5 observers  (usually  3)  watched  for  6.7  ± 2.67  (SD)  h per  day  with  the  least  observation 
(1.95  h)  on  2 August,  when  it  rained  most  of  the  day.  The  nest  was  about  11  m up  in  a white 
spruce  (Picea  glauca)  in  a grove  dominated  by  both  this  species  and  northern  white  cedar 
{Thuja  occidentalis),  with  scattered  small  groups  of  quaking  aspen  (Populus  tremul aides) 
and  an  occasional  balsam  fir  [Abies  balsamea),  white  birch  (Betula  papyrifera)  and  white 
pine  [Pinus  strobus).  The  nest  was  in  the  southwest  corner  of  the  grove  about  35  m from  the 
western  edge  and  30  m from  the  beach  of  Lake  Huron.  The  grove  extended  about  130  m 
north  and  200-350  m east  of  the  nest,  the  edge  was  irregular,  and  a narrow  strip  of  trees 
connected  the  grove  to  2 larger  forests  to  the  east  and  north.  The  area  surrounding  the  grove 
is  locally  called  a “prairie,”  an  open  parkland  covered  with  lichens,  sparse  grasses  and  some 
bare  dolomite  rock.  Individual  and  small  groups  of  trees,  scattered  through  the  prairie, 
formed  5-20%  of  the  vegetative  cover. 

At  least  1 observer  remained  near  the  young;  the  other  observers  were  stationed  at  points 
which  offered  the  best  view  of  approaching  adults.  The  young  habituated  to  our  presence 
rapidly  and  would  tolerate  approach  to  within  less  than  5 m after  the  second  day  of  obser- 
vations. The  adult  female  also  habituated  rapidly.  She  perched  quietly  near  us  on  a number 
of  occasions.  The  adult  male  flew  within  10  m of  us  several  times,  but  was  not  observed 
perching  near  us.  The  young  usually  occupied  rather  exposed  perches,  either  in  aspens  or 
dead  trees  of  various  species,  although  well-fed  young  were  occasionally  found  on  secluded 
perches.  The  center  of  activity  of  the  young,  as  they  grew  older,  moved  northward  from  the 
nest  and  we  moved  our  observation  posts  accordingly. 

On  28  July,  the  young  appeared  to  have  achieved  full  feather  growth  and  flight  skills 
comparable  to  those  of  the  adults;  identification  of  age  was  possible  only  if  we  saw  the 
plumage  or  heard  begging  calls.  Camp  (in  Platt,  unpubl.  M.S.  thesis,  Brigham  Young  Univ., 
Provo,  Utah,  1973)  indicates  that  full  feather  growth  is  attained  at  an  age  of  38-40  days.  We 


85 


86 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


age  of  young 


I^IG.  1.  Mean  prey  deliveries  per  h by  the  adults  and  mean  “predatory  episodes”  per  h 
involving  young.  FDF:  food  deliveries  by  the  female.  FDM:  food  deliveries  by  the  male.  FDT: 
sum  of  food  deliveries  for  both  sexes.  PEY:  predatory  episodes  involving  young  (see  text). 


thus  estimated  the  age  of  our  birds  as  33  days  w^hen  we  discovered  them  on  22  July  and  that 
the  females  had  left  the  nest  about  6 days  and  the  male  about  9 days  before  we  began 
observations. 

Identification  of  prey  items  was  rarely  possible,  particularly  since  most  prey  items  were 


I 


GENERAL  NOTES 


87 


Fig.  2.  Mean  estimated  g of  food  delivered  per  h by  the  adults;  F:  delivered  by  female,  M: 
delivered  by  male,  T;  sum  of  the  2. 


delivered  plucked  and  remains  were  rarely  found.  If  we  obtained  a sufficiently  good  look  at 
the  prey  as  it  was  being  carried  by  the  adult,  we  estimated  its  size  and  weight. 

We  did  not  attempt  to  census  prey-bird  populations  in  the  range  of  the  hawks,  but  a 
subjective  judgement  is  that  the  populations  were  about  the  same  as  on  2 previous  visits  to 
Manitoulin  at  the  same  time  of  year. 

Food  deliveries  and  behavior  of  the  adults. — In  our  120.5  h of  observation,  there  were  73 
deliveries  of  food  by  the  adults,  40  by  the  male,  23  by  the  female  and  in  10  cases  we  could 
not  determine  the  sex  of  the  adult.  In  34  of  the  73  deliveries  (male  22,  female  10,  unknown 
sex  2),  the  prey  item  was  seen  sufficiently  well,  or  sufficient  remains  were  found  to  permit 
estimation  of  size.  The  22  such  prey  items  delivered  by  the  male  had  a mean  weight  of  26  ± 
14.8  g (range  10-80  g).  The  10  items  delivered  by  the  female  had  a mean  weight  of  35  ± 21.5 
g (range  10-90  g).  The  difference  in  weight  of  prey  delivered  by  the  sexes  was  not  statistically 
significant  (t-test,  P > 0.26,  1-tailed). 

To  examine  changes  in  the  feeding  rates  by  the  adults,  we  divided  our  18  days  of  obser- 
vations into  6 three-day  intervals,  thus  insuring  a reasonable  amount  of  observation  time  per 
interval  (mean  19.9  h,  range  14.0-25.9  h).  Within  a 3-day  interval,  food  deliveries  by  adults 
of  unknown  sex  were  assigned  to  the  sexes  in  proportion  to  the  deliveries  by  adults  of  known 
sex.  Weights  of  prey  items  not  seen  were  assigned  to  the  mean  weight  of  prey  delivered  by 
that  sex.  The  male  made  0.53/h  food  deliveries  during  the  first  12  days  of  study  and  only 
0.18/h  during  the  last  6 days  (Fig.  1).  The  female  showed  less  temporal  change  in  food 


88 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


deliveries:  0.24/h  for  the  first  12  days  and  0.16/h  for  the  last  6 days.  The  estimated  weight 
of  food  delivered  per  h shows  similar  trends:  13.6  g/h  by  the  male  and  7.7  g/h  by  the  female 
during  the  first  12  days  and  4.2  g/h  by  the  male  and  5.0  g/h  by  the  female  during  the  last  6 
days  (Fig.  2).  There  was  a precipitous  decline  in  total  food  deliveries  between  the  end  of  the 
sixth  week  of  age  of  the  young  and  the  beginning  of  the  seventh  week:  65%  fewer  deliveries 
per  h and  63%  fewer  g/h.  Daily  mean  deliveries/h  for  this  6-day  period,  when  the  young  were 
42  through  47  days  old  were:  1.05,  0.63,  1.54,  0.42,  0.25,  0.32.  The  dramatic  drop  from  1.54 
deliveries  per  h to  0.42/h  is  further  intersting  because  the  latter  day  (3  August)  marked  the 
first  easily  noticeable  influx  of  migrant  passerines  (mostly  warblers)  into  the  area. 

The  adult  female  was  observed  to  loiter  in  the  nest  grove  on  13  occasions  for  a mean  of 
0.83  ± 0.67  h.  These  13  occasions  of  loitering  occurred  throughout  our  period  of  observation 
and  as  late  as  7 August.  The  female  may  have  spent  even  more  time  in  the  nest  grove.  She 
usually  flew  below  canopy  for  some  distance  before  relinquishing  food  to  the  young  and  then 
exited  the  grove  at  low  altitude,  sometimes  unobserved.  The  male  either  dropped  prey  to 
the  young  above  canopy,  or  dipped  briefly  below  the  treetops.  The  male  was  known  to  loiter 
in  the  grove  on  only  3 occasions  for  a mean  duration  of  0.15  ± 0.08  h.  Neither  adult  showed 
any  behavior  which  appeared  defensive  of  the  young  in  our  presence. 

We  observed  the  female  apparently  hunting  on  2 occasions,  about  0.7  and  1.3  km  from 
the  nest.  She  was  once  observed  carrying  food  1.2  km  from  the  nest.  The  male  was  never 
observed  hunting.  On  the  basis  of  our  observations,  we  suspect  that  most  of  the  hunting  was 
done  more  than  1.5  km  from  the  nest,  although  prey  appeared  to  be  abundant  in  the  im- 
mediate vicinity.  Chipping  Sparrows  (Spizella  passerina),  Yellow-rumped  Warblers  (Den- 
droica  coronata)  and  American  Redstarts  (Setophaga  ruticilla)  were  observed  with  fledged 
young  within  20  m of  the  nest. 

Behavior  of  the  young. — The  first  predatory  efforts  by  the  young  were  observed  on  29  July, 
when  they  were  40  days  old  and  food  deliveries  by  the  adults  were  quite  frequent  (Fig.  1). 
Of  the  80  “predatory  episodes  by  young”  depicted  in  Fig.  1,  52  were  stoops  or  obvious 
pursuit  flights  at  prey  hidden  from  our  view  by  vegetation.  Of  these  52,  23  were  definitely 
unsuccessful,  and  for  the  other  29  cases  we  could  not  determine  the  outcome.  During  the 
last  few  days  of  our  observations,  young  would  often  disappear  for  several  hours,  apparently 
hunting.  Our  efforts  to  follow  and  find  such  young  were  rarely  successful,  probably  because 
of  the  extremely  cryptic  behavior  of  the  birds.  Distress  calls  of  potential  prey  birds  account 
for  16  episodes.  In  2 episodes,  young  birds  were  seen  apparently  eating  a tiny  item  (grass- 
hoppers were  extremely  abundant  in  the  prairie  surrounding  the  nest  grove).  On  1 other 
occasion,  3 young  were  flushed  from  the  ground  by  one  of  us  twice  in  3 minutes.  The  young 
remained  together  and  their  behavior  strongly  suggested  that  one  had  food  hut  the  dense 
vegetation  prevented  us  from  confirming  this.  No  food  delivery  by  an  adult  had  occurred 
during  the  previous  3.13  h.  On  9 occasions,  we  saw  the  bird  at  which  the  young  hawk 
stooped.  These  included  4 pursuits  of  Gray  Jays  {Perisoreus  canadensis),  2 stoops  at  small 
passerines,  1 at  an  unidentified  warbler,  1 at  a \ ellow-rumped  Warbler  and  1 at  a Common 
Flicker  (Colaptes  auratus).  The  most  spectacular  pursuit  of  prey  occurred  on  7 August,  when 
we  saw  the  young  male  suddenly  leave  a perch  about  15  m up  in  a dead  white  pine.  Accel- 
erating rapidly  with  flicking  wing-beats,  he  intercepted  a flying  warbler  about  80  m away, 
missing  by  less  than  30  cm  as  the  warbler  dived.  The  hawk  wheeled  and  plunged  after  the 
warbler  which  disappeared  into  a spruce.  We  did  not  observe  any  successful  capture  of  prey 
by  the  young,  hut  the  hawks  had  ample  opportunities  when  they  were  not  under  surveillance 
by  us.  During  the  last  few  days  of  our  observations,  known  positions  of  individual  young 
were  sometimes  more  than  1.5  km  apart  and  on  our  last  day  1 young  disappeared  2 km  to 
the  north,  flying  high,  and  did  not  return  during  our  last  40  min  of  observation.  It  is  possible 
that  we  witnessed  the  departure  of  1 young. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


89 


On  8 August,  our  last  day  of  observations,  all  4 young  were  under  surveillance  for  only 
0.7%  of  the  5.05  h of  observation.  Three  young  were  under  surveillance  29.7%  of  the  time, 
2 young  40.1%,  1 young  37.0%  and  12.3%  of  the  time  no  young  were  seen. 

Discussion. — The  65%  decline  in  food  deliveries  by  the  adults  that  occurred  between  42- 
44  and  45^7  days  of  age  of  the  young  is  far  too  sudden  to  be  explained  by  a decline  in  prey 
availability  or  vulnerability.  Furthermore,  we  noted  a marked  influx  of  migrant  passerines 
into  the  area  on  day  44,  just  when  the  decrease  in  prey  deliveries  began.  The  decrease  can 
be  explained  by  a reduction  in  hunting  by  the  adults.  Sharp-shinned  Hawks  become  inde- 
pendent of  the  parents  at  an  age  of  about  7 weeks  (Platt  1973,  Snyder  and  Wiley  1976).  A 
reduction  in  food  deliveries  by  the  adults  6 days  before  the  young  become  independent  and 
the  continued  begging  of  the  young  is  consonant  with  the  parent-offspring  conflict  hypothesis 
of  Trivers  (Am.  Zool.  14:249-264,  1974).  During  the  “weaning”  period  the  attempts  of  the 
offspring  to  maximize  their  inclusive  fitness  come  into  conflict  with  that  of  the  parents.  The 
young  selfishly  demand  more  care  than  the  parents,  selfishly,  should  provide.  Davies  (Ibis 
120:509-514,  1978)  has  suggested  that  if  parents  time  the  decline  in  feeding  rates  in  response 
to  cues  from  the  young,  they  are  open  to  being  “cheated”  in  the  conflict  of  maximizing 
fitness.  If,  on  the  other  hand,  the  adults  feed  for  a fixed  period  irrespective  of  the  performance 
of  the  young  in  feeding  themselves,  young  would  be  lost  in  times  of  food  scarcity.  The  optimal 
strategy  would  involve  flexibility  in  the  behavior  of  the  adults  with  parental  care  being  pro- 
longed when  food  is  scarce.  Davies  presents  the  results  of  several  studies  which  indicate 
that  the  length  of  the  parental  care  period  in  birds  is  affected  by  the  availability  of  food.  We 
believe  that  this  is  the  key  to  the  understanding  of  the  differences  in  feeding  behavior  of 
adults  observed  at  various  nests. 

Newton  (J.  Zool.  London  184:465-487,  1978),  in  summarizing  his  observations  of  the  Spar- 
rowhawk  {A.  nisus),  suggested  that  the  male  usually  brings  as  much  food  to  the  young  as  it 
can  and  that  the  female  is  induced  to  hunt  chiefly  when  the  efforts  of  the  male  do  not  meet 
the  needs  of  the  young.  Newton  noted  that  female  A.  nisus  often  loiter  in  the  vicinity  of  the 
nest,  a phenomenon  observed  by  us,  and  by  Platt  (1973)  in  A.  striatus.  The  available  data 
on  the  Sharp-shinned  Hawks  are  in  complete  agreement  with  Newton’s  hypothesis.  The  male 
Puerto  Rican  Sharp-shinned  Hawk  of  Snyder  and  Wiley  (1976)  was  able  to  provide  adequate 
food  for  his  family,  and  the  female  did  no  hunting  (at  least  not  to  provide  food  for  the  young). 
For  the  spruce-fir  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wiley  and  our  Manitoulin  Island  brood,  the  male 
could  not  provide  all  of  the  needs  of  the  young,  and  the  female  did  some  hunting.  In  the 
case  of  the  riparian  canyon  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wiley,  which  we  readily  agree  was  under 
conditions  of  food  stress,  the  female  had  to  do  considerable  hunting  to  help  feed  the  young. 
The  same  was  possibly  true  of  the  oak-juniper  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wiley,  although  the  data 
are  limited. 

The  peak  in  food  deliveries  to  the  Puerto  Rican  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wiley  occurred  during 
week  5 (the  week  after  fledging)  with  a sharp  drop  in  week  6 and  little  further  decline  in 
week  7.  The  spruce-fir  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wiley  showed  a peak  in  week  4,  with  a consid- 
erable and  steady  decline  through  week  5 and  weeks  6 and  7 (the  2 weeks  were  lumped 
because  of  limited  observations).  However,  if  we  look  at  the  amount  of  food  delivered  per 
young,  the  spruce-fir  brood  also  shows  a peak  during  week  5 (1  young  disappeared  at  the 
end  of  the  fourth  week,  cause  unknown).  A recalculation  of  the  data  shown  in  Fig.  2 into 
weekly  intervals  shows  that  our  Manitoulin  Island  brood  received  the  same  amount  of  food 
during  the  last  3 days  of  week  5 (21.2  g/h)  as  they  did  in  week  6 (21.3  g/h)  with  a sharp  drop 
to  11.5  g/h  in  week  7.  The  lack  of  a peak  in  week  5 for  our  brood  might  possibly  be  due  to 
the  fact  that  we  did  not  begin  observations  until  the  last  3 days  of  that  week.  Another  possible 
partial  explanation  for  the  differences  observed  between  nests  in  the  temporal  pattern  of 
food  deliveries  is  in  the  sex  ratios  of  the  brood.  Newton  (1978)  found  no  difference  in  the 


90 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  1 

Per  Cent  Distribution  of  Size  Classes  of  Prey  Taken  and  Prey  Available 


Size  class® 

N 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

Prey  taken 

d 

124 

11.3 

33.1 

40.3 

11.3 

0.8 

3.2 

(Storer  1966,  Table  5) 

9 

125 

1.6 

21.6 

37.6 

23.2 

1.6 

8.0 

Spruce-fir  nest 
(Snyder  and  Wiley  1976) 
Prey  taken 

d 

68 

0 

48.5 

47.1 

4.4 

0 

0 

(Table  5) 

9 

8 

0 

50.0 

50.0 

0 

0 

0 

Prey  available 

(Table  17) 

14.3 

61.0 

12.3 

5.6 

2.9 

3.8 

Riparian  canyon  nest 
(Snyder  and  Wiley  1976) 
Prey  taken 

d 

36 

22.2 

58.3 

16.7 

2.7 

0 

0 

(Table  5) 

9 

15 

0 

20.0 

6.7 

26.7 

26.7 

20.0 

Prey  available 

(Table  17) 

22.0 

44.9 

7.1 

9.5 

7.3 

9.3 

Oak-juniper  nest 
(Snyder  and  Wiley  1976) 
Prey  taken 

d 

19 

0 

36.8 

63.2 

0 

0 

0 

(Table  5) 

9 

4 

0 

25.0 

25.0 

0 

25.0 

25.0 

Prey  available 

(Table  17) 

14.3 

40.7 

20.5 

12.1 

5.1 

7.2 

Manitouiin  Island 

d 

22 

0 

18.2 

50.0 

22.7 

4.5 

4.5 

Prey  taken 

9 

10 

0 

10.0 

40.0 

10.0 

30.0 

10.0 

® The  size  classes  are  those  of  Storer  (1966): 

1 = 3.4-8 

g.  2 = 8- 

-15.6  g,  3 

= 15.6-27  g. 

4 = 27-42.9  g,  5 = 

42.9-64  g. 

6 = 64-91. 1 n. 


food  consumption  of  nestling  male  and  female  Sparrowhawks  and  attributed  the  high  food 
consumption  of  the  smaller  males  to  more  rapid  development  than  the  larger  females.  It  is 
unlikely  that  ecpial  food  consumption  hy  the  sexes  persists  for  very  long  after  fledging;  we 
suspect  that  males  soon  consume  less  than  females.  Our  Manitouiin  Island  brood  consisted 
of  3 females  and  1 male,  the  spruce-fir  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wiley  consisted  of  2 males  and 
1 female  (after  the  loss  of  1 male),  and  the  Puerto  Rican  brood  consisted  of  2 males.  Assuming 
that  the  adults  were  responding  to  the  needs  of  the  young,  the  sex  ratio  of  the  broods  thus 
relates  rather  well  with  changes  in  food  deliveries  after  the  fifth  week.  If  we  take  the  above 
factors  into  consideration,  the  temporal  patterns  of  food  deliveries  to  the  3 broods  discussed 
above  are  basically  similar. 

In  contrast,  the  riparian  canyon  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wiley  showed  a slight  increase  in 
food  deliveries  between  week  5 and  weeks  6 and  7 (the  last  2 weeks  were  lumped  because 
of  limited  observations).  This  high  rate  of  food  delivery  at  the  end  of  fledgling  dependency, 
and  the  considerable  participation  of  the  female  in  providing  food,  suggest  that  this  brood 


GENERAL  NOTES 


91 


was  suffering  from  food  stress  and  that  the  parents  were  responding  in  accord  with  the 
hypotheses  of  Davies  (1978)  and  Newton  (1978).  The  riparian  canyon  nest  was  studied  in 
1971  during  the  worst  drought  in  50  years,  and  bird  populations  were  judged  to  be  low 
(Snyder  and  Wiley  1976).  Our  results  show  a precipitious  drop  in  food  deliveries  early  in  the 
seventh  week.  It  is  possible  that  a similar,  sudden  decline  occurred  in  most  of  the  broods 
studied  by  Snyder  and  Wiley  (1976)  hut  was  not  recognized  because  of  the  very  limited 
observations  conducted  during  week  7. 

Newton’s  (1978)  hypothesis  suggests  that  the  relatively  meager  participation  of  the  female 
in  delivering  food  to  the  spruce-fir  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wdley  (1976)  was  due  to  adequate 
provisioning  by  the  male.  Snyder  and  Wiley  suggest  “.  . . both  sexes  of  adult  accipiters  hunt 
pretty  much  full  time  toward  the  end  of  the  breeding  cycle  . . .”  and  suggest  that  the 
relatively  low  contributions  of  the  female  to  the  provisioning  of  the  spruce-fir  brood  was  due 
to  her  bringing  relatively  small  prey.  She  also  brought  fewer  prey  items  (11%  of  the  total) 
than  did  the  riparian  canyon  female  (29%  of  the  total).  Snyder  and  Wiley  attribute  the 
difference  in  the  feeding  behaviors  of  the  females  at  the  2 nests  to  a greater  abundance  of 
prey  in  the  size  range  preferred  by  females  in  the  riparian  canyon  habitat  than  in  the  spruce- 
fir  habitat.  We  interpret  the  data  in  another  way.  A shortage  in  prey  of  the  size  classes 
preferred  by  the  male  in  the  riparian  canyon  habitat  resulted  in  inadequate  food  deliveries 
by  the  male  and  increased  participation  by  the  female,  while  in  the  spruce-fir  habitat,  a 
reasonable  abundance  of  prey  in  the  size  classes  preferred  by  the  male  resulted  in  reasonable 
food  deliveries  by  the  male  and  the  female  was  not  induced  to  hunt  regularly. 

We  feel  that  the  data  of  Storer  (Auk  83:423-436,  1966)  offer  the  best  estimate  of  prey-size 
preferences  in  sharp-shins.  Storer  examined  the  gut  contents  of  223  sharp-shins  of  which 
82%  were  taken  during  migration,  a period  when  one  might  reasonably  expect  random  avail- 
ability of  the  various  size  classes.  The  analysis  of  249  prey  items  reveals  that  males  prefer 
size  classes  2 and  3 (73%  of  the  total)  and  females  prefer  size  classes  3,  4 and  2 (82%  of  the 
total).  The  preference  of  females  for  size  class  4 is  only  very  slightly  greater  than  for  size 
class  2 (Table  1). 

An  examination  of  Table  1 suggests  that  the  male  at  the  spruce-fir  nest  of  Snyder  and 
Wiley  (1976)  was  taking  larger  prey  than  expected  either  in  comparison  to  Storer's  (1966) 
data  or  the  prey  available.  The  female  took  few  prey,  and  it  is  not  surprising  that  none  were 
from  the  presumably  few  individuals  available  in  larger  size  classes.  The  riparian  canyon 
male  took  considerably  smaller  prey  than  the  males  of  Storer  (1966)  or  those  of  any  other 
brood  studied.  We  believe  this  is  strong  evidence  of  food  stress.  It  appears  that  the  male 
was  doing  the  best  he  could  during  this  “worst  drought  in  50  years,*'  low  bird  populations 
and  probably  limited  breeding  by  prey-birds.  The  riparian  canyon  female  brought  a larger 
proportion  of  the  prey  during  the  fledgling  period  than  at  any  other  nest:  53%  (Manitoulin 
37%,  spruce-fir  22%,  Puerto  Rico  0%).  In  view  of  the  high  participation  in  feeding  by  the 
riparian  canyon  female,  and  the  apparent  scarcity  of  prey,  the  high  proportion  of  large-sized 
prey  taken  is  not  surprising.  It  is  possible  that  the  male  considerably  depressed  populations 
of  smaller  birds  early  during  this  unusually  bad  year.  The  limited  data  from  the  oak-juniper 
nest  suggest  that  the  male  was  preying  on  relatively  large  prey.  The  4 prey  items  taken  by 
the  female  are  insufficient  for  comment.  The  Manitoulin  Island  male  took  larger  prey  than 
predicted  on  the  basis  of  Storer’s  data  and  the  same  is  true,  to  a lesser  extent,  for  the  female. 

We  believe  that  our  intensive  observations  on  Manitoulin  Island,  and  our  interpretation  of 
the  data  of  Snyder  and  Wiley,  suggest  that  food  stress  late  in  the  period  of  dependency  of 
the  young  in  Sharp-shinned  Hawks  is  an  exceptional  phenomenon.  We  believe  it  occurred 
only  in  the  riparian  canyon  brood  of  Snyder  and  Wiley  (1976)  during  an  unusually  unfavorable 
year.  We  therefore  conclude  that  food  stress  during  breeding,  ameliorated  by  separate  feed- 
ing niches  of  the  adults,  is  an  unlikely  explanation  for  the  remarkable  reversed  dimorphism 


92 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


in  size  exhibited  by  Sharp-shinned  Hawks.  It  appears  that,  under  most  conditions,  the  female 
could  contribute  whatever  prey  is  needed  by  the  brood  without  having  to  be  1.7  times  as 
large  as  the  male  and  capturing  slightly  larger  prey. 

Acknowledgments. — We  thank  Mary  Tasker  for  her  exceptional  hospitality  to  often  unso- 
ciable field  biologists  and  James  Tasker  for  aid  in  the  observations.  J.  A.  Feduccia,  K.  D. 
Meyer  and  R.  H.  Wiley  offered  helpful  comments  on  the  manuscript.  We  thank  T.  J.  Cade, 
R.  T.  Reynolds  and  N.  F.  R.  Snyder  for  constructive  criticisms  of  an  earlier  version  of  the 
manuscript;  this  does  not  mean  that  we  have  followed  all  of  their  suggestions  or  that  they 
endorse  our  interpretations. — Helmut  C.  Mueller,  Dept.  Zoology  and  Curriculum  in  Ecol- 
ogy, Univ.  North  Carolina,  Chapel  Hill,  North  Carolina  27514,  Nancy  S.  Mueller,  Dept. 
Biology,  North  Carolina  Central  Univ.,  Durham,  North  Carolina  27707,  AND  Patricia  G. 
Parker,  Dept.  Zoology,  Univ.  North  Carolina,  Chapel  Hill,  North  Carolina  27514.  Accepted 
10  Nov.  1979. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  92-97 


Food  deprivation  and  temperature  regulation  in  nestling  Ferruginous  Hawks. — 

Nestling  deaths  from  heat  prostration  may  occur  regularly  in  some  falconiform  species  (e.g., 
Fitch  et  al..  Condor  48:207-237,  1946;  Nelson,  pp.  64-72  in  Peregrine  Falcon  Populations, 
Their  Biology  and  Decline,  Hickey  ed.,  Univ.  Wisconsin  Press,  Madison,  Wisconsin,  1969; 
Olendorff,  U.S.I.B.P.  Rep.  No.  211,  1973;  Beecham  and  Kochert,  Wilson  Bull.  87:506-513, 
1975).  Beecham  and  Kochert  (1975)  concluded  that  41%  of  Golden  Eagle  (Aquila  chrysaetos) 
nestling  mortality  in  their  Snake  River,  Idaho,  study  area  was  caused  by  overheating  and 
observed  that  young  birds  in  nests  with  a southern  or  western  exposure  are  most  vulnerable 
to  heat  stress. 

The  Ferruginous  Hawk  (Buteo  regalis)  nests  in  the  semi-arid  regions  of  southern  Canada 
and  the  western  L^nited  States  (Olendorff  1973;  Tomhack  and  Murphy,  unpubl.).  Throughout 
the  breeding  range  Ferruginous  Hawks  nest  primarily  on  rocky  outcrops,  tops  of  trees  (es- 
pecially junipers  lJuniperus  spp.]  and  cottonwoods  [Populus  spp.R  and  occasionally  on  sage- 
brush {Artemisia  spp.)  or  the  ground  (Smith  and  Murphy,  Brigham  Young  Univ.  Sci.  BuU. 
18:1-76,  1973;  Oldendorff  1973;  W offinden,  Ph.D.  diss.,  Brigham  Young  Univ.,  Provo,  Utah, 
1975;  Howard  and  W olfe,  J.  Range  Manage.  29:33-37,  1976;  Lokemoen  and  Duebbert,  Con- 
dor 78:464-470,  1976;  Fitzner  et  al..  Condor  79:245-249,  1977).  Despite  the  high  tempera- 
tures encountered  in  the  latter  stages  of  the  nesting  cycle  (e.g..  Smith  and  Murphy  1973; 
Fitzner  et  al.,  1977),  apparently  shade  availability  is  not  a nest-site  requirement  for  the 
species.  In  the  Great  Basin  west  of  Utah  Lake,  W offinden  (1975)  examined  56  nests  of 
Ferruginous  Hawks  on  rocky  outcrops,  on  the  ground,  and  in  trees.  Almost  half  of  these 
nests  were  unshaded  throughout  the  day.  The  nests  on  steep  slopes  received  some  shade 
only  in  morning  or  afternoon,  depending  on  slope  aspect. 

Here,  we  present  field  data  suggesting  that  underfed  Ferruginous  Hawk  nestlings  are 
especially  vulnerable  to  heat  stress.  W e predict  that  combined  effects  of  inadequate  food 
provisions  and  high  temperatures  may  cause  much  nestling  mortality  in  years  when  prey 
f)opulations  are  low. 

On  15  June  1977.  we  surveyed  Cedar  and  Rush  valleys,  Utah  Co.  and  Tooele  Co.  (elev. 
1760-1895  m;  40°00'N,  between  111°55'W  and  112°35'W),  for  Ferruginous  Hawk  nests  (for 
detailed  description  of  study  area  see  Smith  and  Murphy  1973).  Thirteen  light  phase  nestlings 
between  ca  5 and  7 weeks  old  remained  in  5 nests,  including  2 tree  nests  in  Utah  juniper 
(Juniperus  osteosperma),  2 nests  on  rocky  outcrops  on  steep  (ca  40°),  west-facing  slopes,  and 


GENERAL  NOTES 


93 


1 ground  nest  on  a southeast-facing  slope  (ca  30°).  Using  a YSl  Tele- thermometer  (Model 
43E),  a YSI  black  bulb  probe  and  2 YSI  plastic-tipped  probes,  we  measured  ambient  and 
cloacal  temperatures  of  2 nestlings  5.5-6  weeks  old  at  a rocky  outcrop  nest  from  14:00-17:00 
on  24  June.  A plastic-tipped  probe  was  inserted  3-4  cm  into  the  cloaca  of  each  nestling, 
taped  in  place  and  checked  each  time  the  nestlings  defecated.  We  erected  a low  cardboard 
barrier,  non-reflective  and  brown  in  color,  to  prevent  the  young  from  leaving  the  nest.  The 
nestlings  showed  no  signs  of  agitation  whenever  we  left  our  tent  blind  and  approached  the 
nest  to  take  temperature  readings. 

On  23  June  1979,  in  the  Raft  River  Valley,  Box  Elder  Co.,  Utah,  and  Cassia  Co.,  Idaho 
(elev.  1400-1700  m;  42°00'N,  113°30'W),  we  found  3 Ferruginous  Hawk  nestlings  in  2 nests 
in  Utah  juniper  (for  description  of  study  area  see  Howard  and  Wolfe  1976).  One  nest  con- 
tained a melanistic  bird,  dubbed  “Othello,”  ca  5 weeks  old,  and  the  second  nest  contained 

2 light  phase  nestlings,  “lago”  and  “Desdemona,”  ca  5.5  and  6 weeks  old,  respectively. 

Prior  to  the  trip,  we  calibrated  3 miniaturized  temperature-sensitive  radio  transmitters 

(Mini-Mitter  Co.,  Inc.,  Model  T).  On  23  June,  a transmitter  was  wrapped  in  Black-tailed  Jack- 
rabbit  (Lepus  californicus)  meat  and  fed  to  each  Ferruginous  Hawk  nestling,  and  the  3 birds 
were  placed  on  a recently  active  Ferruginous  Hawk  ground  nest  which  was  unshaded  through- 
out the  day.  We  hobbled  the  young  so  they  could  not  leave  the  nest  to  find  shelter.  Within  30  min 
the  nestlings  no  longer  struggled  against  the  restraint  and  settled  into  position.  We  monitored 
transmitter  signals  from  the  nestlings  15  m from  the  nest  by  means  of  a Lafayette  3-Channel 
1.5  Watt  Receiver  (Mini-Mitter  Co.,  Inc.,  Model  Ch  receiver)  and  recorded  ambient  tem- 
peratures with  a YSI  black  bulb  probe  and  YSI  Tele-Thermometer  (Model  43TD).  As  indi- 
cated by  signal  strength,  the  transmitters  lodged  in  the  crops  of  the  nestlings  until  they  were 
regurgitated  in  pellets  ca  24  h later.  Transmitter  signals  were  converted  into  body  temper- 
atures as  follows:  the  receiver  was  switched  to  1 of  3 channels  and  the  time  required  for  100 
clicks  was  recorded  on  a stopwatch.  This  time  interval  was  compared  to  the  appropriate 
calibration  curve.  Every  15  min  from  13:30-17:00,  we  took  body  temperature  readings  and 
paired  them  with  a black  bulb  ambient  temperature  reading. 

The  nestlings  were  transported  in  and  maintained  for  2 nights  in  individual  grass-lined 
cardboard  cartons.  We  fed  them  several  times  a day  with  fresh  jackrabbit  meat, 
hamburger  and  water.  Cast  transmitters  were  recalibrated  and  again  fed  to  the  birds.  On  24 
and  25  June  1979,  we  placed  the  nestlings  on  a recently  active  Ferruginous  Hawk  rocky 
outcrop  nest  built  on  a steep  (ca  40°),  west-facing  slope  in  Cedar  Valley,  Utah  Co.,  Utah. 
The  nest  was  not  shaded  in  the  afternoon  and  the  nestlings  were  again  hobbled.  On  24  June, 
we  measured  ambient  and  crop  temperatures  from  14:50-18:00.  We  did  not  feed  the  young 
on  25  June  until  15:00  in  the  hopes  of  duplicating  the  effects  of  low  food  availability;  black 
bulb  ambient  and  crop  temperatures  were  taken  only  from  12:00-14:00  to  avoid  the  extreme 
late  afternoon  temperatures. 

The  behavior  of  the  5-7-week-old  nestlings  encountered  in  Cedar  and  Rush  valleys  in  June 
of  1977  strongly  suggested  that  ambient  temperatures  were  uncomfortable,  if  not  stressful. 
All  of  the  nests  were  unshaded  for  half  the  day  or  longer.  Three  nestlings  had  wandered 
several  meters  from  their  ground  nest  and  crouched  under  a small  sagebrush  {Artemisia 
tridentata).  At  1 rock  outcrop  nest,  the  2 nestlings  had  moved  to  the  shade  of  a nearby 
man-made,  small  rock  shelter.  Two  young  hawks  had  moved  3-5  m from  the  other  outcrop 
nest  to  lay  their  heads  in  the  shade  of  a large  rock.  The  3 nestlings  in  each  of  the  juniper 
nests  were  apparently  unable  to  leave  the  trees.  However,  they  all  tended  to  pant  rapidly 
and  salivate.  Angell  (Living  Bird  8:225-241,  1969)  also  reported  “shade-seeking”  behavior 
in  nestling  Ferruginous  Hawks  4-4.5  weeks  old.  This  thermoregulatory  behavior  is  obviously 
an  important  means  by  which  nestlings  avoid  overheating  in  the  weeks  prior  to  fledging. 

In  1977,  there  were  few  active  Ferruginous  Hawk  nests  in  the  Cedar  and  Rush  valley  study 


94 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  \o.  1,  March  1981 


Fig.  1.  Ambient  temperatures  and  cloacal  temperatures  of  2 Ferruginous  Hawk  nestlings 
vs  time  of  day  at  Cedar  Valley  nest,  24  June  1977. 

areas  (Murphy  et  al.,  unpubl.).  The  primary  reason  for  the  decline  appeared  to  be  a low 
population  of  Black-tailed  Jackrabbits  (Murphy  et  al.,  unpubl.).  This  jackrabbit  is  the 
principal  food  of  the  Ferruginous  Hawk  in  the  Great  Basin  region  (Smith  and  Murphy  1973; 
Smith  and  Murphy,  Raptor  Research  13:1-14,  1979),  Woffinden  and  Murphy  (1977)  showed 
a relationship  between  the  annual  numbers  of  Ferruginous  Hawk  nesting  pairs  and  jackrabbit 
abundance.  They  also  noted  a high  nestling  mortality  (an  average  of  49.59?^)  in  2 years  of  low' 
jackrabbit  density  in  1973  and  1974. 

In  June  1977,  we  found  no  fresh  jackrabbit  remains  at  any  of  the  nests  we  examined, 
although  remnants  of  small  prey  items  such  as  kangaroo  rats  {Dipodomys  sp.),  lizards  (Cne- 
midophorus  sp.,  Phrynosoma  sp.)  and  birds  (Icteridae)  occurred  at  2 nests.  One  or  both 


GENERAL  NOTES 


95 


Ambient  Temperature  °C 

Fig.  2.  Crop  temperatures  monitored  by  telemetry  vs  ambient  temperatures  for  Ferru- 
ginous Hawk  nestlings  lago  (1),  Desdemona  (2)  and  Othello  (3),  June  1979. 


parent  birds  circled  overhead  at  all  the  nests  we  visited,  indicating  that  the  young  were  still 
attended. 

At  the  rock  outcrop  nest  where  we  took  temperature  readings,  no  fresh  food  was  present 
during  the  3 days  of  our  visits.  At  all  nests  it  appeared  that  young  were  inadequately  pro- 
visioned during  a time  when  food  consumption  rates  should  be  maximal  (Olendorff,  Condor 
76:766-768,  1974).  A graph  of  black  bulb  ambient  vs  cloacal  temperatures  for  the  afternoon 
of  24  June  (Fig.  1)  suggests  the  nestlings  had  difficulty  thermoregulating.  The  sky  was  partly 
overcast  that  day,  and  when  the  sun  was  exposed,  black  bulb  temperatures  at  the  nest 
increased  rapidly  from  30°-36.5°C.  Nestling  cloacal  temperatures  (range  33-37°C)  increased 
and  decreased  rapidly  with  ambient  temperature.  These  body  temperatures  are  lower  than 
those  daytime  temperatures  reported  for  other  falconiforms  (e.g.,  McNab,  Condor  68:47-55, 
1966;  Fitch,  Condor  76:331-333,  1974;  Rudeen  and  Powers,  Condor  80:447-449,  1978),  pos- 
sibly a consequence  of  both  the  cloacal  temperature  (rather  than  deep  core  temperature 
measurement)  and  an  impaired  thermoregulatory  capacity.  Both  nestlings  began  panting 


96 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


continuously  each  time  their  temperatures  reached  35°C.  Fluctuations  in  body  temperature 
of  nestling  #2  were  more  extreme  than  those  of  #I  (F-test  NS);  nestling  #2  was  not  found 
during  our  last  visit  on  25  June. 

The  3 nestlings  from  Raft  River  Valley  in  1979  were  seemingly  well-fed  prior  to  our  ex- 
periments, judging  by  the  fresh  jackrabbit  remains.  Body  temperatures  of  the  3 nestlings 
monitored  by  telemetry  ranged  from  39.9-43.6°C  (Fig.  2),  and  black  bulb  ambient  temper- 
atures ranged  from  29.5-43°C.  These  body  temperatures  are  higher  than  those  of  the  1977 
Cedar  Valley  nestlings  and  indicate  either  that  there  is  a difference  between  temperatures 
measured  in  cloaca  and  crop,  that  the  1977  nestlings  were  suffering  from  starvation  and 
could  not  thermoregulate  well,  and/or  that  the  1979  Raft  River  Valley  nestlings  were  stressed 
by  heat  and  restricted  movement  on  the  nest.  This  latter  possibility  is  unlikely,  since  we 
obtained  similar  crop  temperatures  for  the  Raft  River  Valley  nestlings  in  a resting  state  in 
the  laboratory  (Tomback  and  Murphy,  unpubl.).  However,  the  3 initial  23  June  1979  readings 
of  42.7°C  for  lago  and  1 initial  reading  of  43.2°C  for  Desdemona  at  the  Raft  River  Valley 
nest  (Fig.  2)  may  have  been  elevated  by  our  handling. 

Means  and  standard  deviations  (°C)  for  ambient  and  nestling  body  (crop)  temperatures  for 
the  3,  1979  experimental  periods  are  as  follows:  Raft  River  Valley,  23  June — T^  31.4  ± 1.06, 
lago  42.5  ± 0.21,  Desdemona  42.8  ± 0.25,  Othello  42.4  ± 0.24;  Cedar  Valley,  24  June — T^ 
39.4  ± 2.74,  lago  41.2  ± 0. 11,  Desdemona  42.3  ± 0.27,  Othello  42.5  ± 0.20;  and  Cedar  Val- 
ley, 25  June— T^  35.1  ± 1.06,  lago 40.7  ± 0.72,  Desdemona 43.1  ± 0.74,  Othello 43.0  ± 0.29. 
F-tests  indicated  that  ambient  temperature  varied  more  on  24  June  than  on  either  23  June 
{F  ^ 0.01)  or  25  June  {F  ^ 0.01).  Yet,  the  crop  temperatures  of  both  lago  and  Desdemona 
fluctuated  most  extremely  on  25  June,  when  the  nestlings  were  deprived  of  food,  than  on 
either  23  June  {F  = 0.01,  both  nestlings)  or  24  June  (F  = 0.01,  F = 0.05,  respectively).  The 
crop  temperatures  of  lago  varied  more  on  23  June  than  24  June  (F  = 0.05),  whereas  those  of 
Desdemona  varied  much  the  same.  For  Othello  crop  temperature  varied  similarly  on  all  3 
days.  However,  the  crop  temperatures  measured  for  both  Othello  and  Desdemona  under 
conditions  of  food  deprivation  were  significantly  higher  than  those  temperatures  monitored 
on  either  23  or  24  June  ( Mann-Whitney  f/-test,  F ^ 0.001),  even  though  the  ambient  tem- 
peratures of  25  June  were  lower  than  those  of  24  June.  Body  temperatures  for  lago  were 
lower  on  25  June  than  on  previous  days  (Mann-Whitney  f/-test,  F < 0.001),  even  though 
they  fluctuated  more,  lago  rarely  panted  on  24  June,  while  the  other  nestlings  panted  most 
of  the  time  and  occasionally  held  their  wings  out  to  the  sides  to  facilitate  cooling.  The  high 
temperatures  on  24  June  affected  Othello  most  severely,  as  the  panting  and  posturing  of  this 
nestling  were  pronounced  and  almost  continuous.  It  is  possible  that  Othello's  dark  plumage 
increased  his  heat  burden  (Hamilton  and  Hepner,  Science  155:196-197,  1967;  Lustick,  Sci- 
ence 163:387-390,  1969;  hut  see  U ahlsherg  et  al.,  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  126:211-222,  1978). 

In  conclusion,  the  temperature  fluctuations  of  the  underfed  nestlings  at  the  Cedar  Valley 
nest  in  1977  and  the  significantly  higher  and  more  varied  body  temperatures  of  the  underfed 
nestlings  in  1979  suggest  that  food  deprived  nestling  Ferruginous  Hawks  cannot  cope  with 
heat  stress.  Reduction  in  prey  probability  are  known  to  result  in  high  nestling  mortality  in 
the  Ferruginous  Hawk  (WOffinden  and  Murphy  1977).  Heat  prostration  may  he  a major  cause 
of  nestling  deaths  under  such  conditions,  especially  since  Ferruginous  Hawk  breeding 
grounds  are  characterized  by  high  summer  temperatures  and  nests  are  unshaded  for  all  or 
part  of  the  day. 

Acknowledgments. — W e thank  Dan  Johnson,  Kent  Keller  and  Philip  Murphy  for  their  in- 
valuable help  in  the  field  and  Clayton  W hite  for  Raft  River  Valley  nest  information.  Fieldwork 
in  1977  was  funded  in  part  by  a grant  from  the  Chapman  Memorial  Fund  of  the  American 
Museum.  At  that  time  D.  F.  Tomback  held  a postdoctoral  fellowship  from  Brigham  Young 
I'niversity.  Data  and  manuscript  preparation  by  D.  F.  Tomback  was  supported  by  the  De- 


GENERAL  NOTES 


97 


partment  of  Biology,  University  of  California  at  Riverside.  Keith  L.  Bildstein,  a reviewer, 
provided  helpful  suggestions  concerning  the  manuscript. — DiANA  F.  ToMBACK,  Dept.  Zo- 
ology and  Entomology,  Colorado  State  Univ.,  Fort  Collins,  Colorado  80523,  AND  JOSEPH  R. 
Murphy,  Dept.  Zoology,  Brigham  Young  Univ.,  Provo,  Utah  84602.  Accepted  10  Jan.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  p.  97 


Aerial  “play”  of  Black  Vultures. — Occasionally  I have  seen  Black  Vultures  (Coragyps 
atratus)  engage  in  playlike,  aerial  acrobatics  at  Cerro  Verde  (13°50'N,  89°38'W;  2000  m 
elev.),  El  Salvador.  On  15  November  1971,  I was  present  during  a violent  “Norte,”  a pro- 
longed windstorm  which  may  attain  gale  velocity  on  mountain  tops.  I noticed,  without  heeding 
at  first,  sounds  I attributed  to  a child  tooting  a musical  toy.  Then  a trio  of  Black  Vultures 
shrilled  past  my  head,  producing  a sound  like  that  of  an  aeolian  harp,  caused  undoubtedly 
by  wind  passing  through  the  feathers  of  wings  and/or  tails. 

A dozen  or  more  Black  Vultures  were  soaring  in  the  strong  upslope  winds  on  the  north 
side  of  Cerro  Verde,  being  carried  upwards  as  much  as  5(X)  m above  the  summit.  From  time 
to  time  one  or  more  birds  “peeled  off’  to  dive  precipitiously  towards  the  southeast,  the  wind 
behind  them.  Some  leveled  off  where  I could  see  them;  others  continued  out  of  sight,  de- 
scending more  than  700  m.  Recordings  of  their  sounds,  made  at  the  time,  suggest  diving 
wire-strutted  biplanes  of  the  First  World  War. 

One  trio  was  especially  notable,  diving  again  and  again  as  a team.  I watched  them  make 
some  25  dives,  3 of  which  ended  near  me.  The  birds  dove  sometimes  in  V-formation,  some- 
times in  line  and  attained  impressive  speeds.  They  ended  the  dives  in  2 steps;  (1)  a slight 
increase  in  angle  of  attack  which  checked  their  speed  slightly  and  flattened  the  dive;  and 
(2)  a sharp  increase  in  attack  angle  plus  spreading  and  lowering  the  tail  which  forced  them 
into  a shallow  climb.  They  then  used  their  forward  momentum  to  circle  along  the  lee  side 
of  the  mountain  into  the  upward  current  again.  The  leader  of  a dive  also  led  in  the  following 
ascent  but  I could  not  ascertain  whether  it  retained  that  position  in  subsequent  dives. 

I have  heard  since,  under  less  favorable  conditions,  the  shrill  of  diving  Black  Vultures 
during  strong  Nortes.  I add  only  that  twice  I saw  single  vultures  diving  as  described  above. 
I noted  that  a single  bird  produces  several  tones,  suggesting  that  several  feathers  are  in- 
volved. 

Bent  (U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  167:29,  1938)  described  similar  diving  sounds  produced  by 
courting  Black  Vultures.  Brown  and  Amadon  (Eagles,  Hawks  and  Falcons  of  the  World, 
McGraw-Hill,  New  York,  New  York,  1968:181)  reported  sounds  like  ripping  heavy  paper  as 
Black  Vultures  dive  for  food.  The  birds  at  Cerro  Verde  were  not  diving  for  food  and  did  not 
seem  to  be  courting.  Brown  and  Amadon  (1968:101)  refer  to  certain  otherwise  unclassified 
aerial  maneuvers  of  falconiformes  as  communal  displays.  However,  a display  implies  com- 
munication between  a sender  and  an  intended  observer;  this  does  not  apply  to  lone  birds. 
I have  no  notion  of  the  incentive  which  governed  the  vultures  I watched,  but  to  me  the 
exuberant  quality  of  their  behavior,  so  unlike  our  usual  impression  of  the  species,  had  “all 
the  appearances  of  play  and  seemed  to  serve  no  other  function  than  the  release  of  pent-up 
energy”  (Pettingill,  Ornithology  in  Laboratory  and  Field,  Burgess  Publ.  Co.,  Minneapolis, 
Minnesota,  1970:254). — Walter  A.  Thurber,  Cornell  Univ.  Laboratory  of  Ornithology,  159 
Sapsucker  Woods  Road,  Ithaca,  New  York  14853.  Accepted  30  Nov.  1979. 


98 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  98-99 


The  shoulder-spot  display  in  Ruffed  Grouse. — Although  the  “shoulder-spot”  display 
is  widespread  in  Tetraonidae,  Lumsden  (Living  Bird  9:65-74,  1970)  and  Hjorth  (Viltrevy 
7:184-596,  1970)  were  both  unable  to  find  evidence  of  this  display  in  Ruffed  Grouse  {Bonasa 
umbellus).  The  shoulder-spot  display  is  most  frequently  seen  in  ambivalent  (also  termed 
conflict  by  Lumsden  1970)  situations  containing  strong  elements  of  fear.  The  behavior  pat- 
terns associated  with  fear  are  difficult  to  observe  in  wild  Ruffed  Grouse  because  of  the 
general  wariness  of  the  species  and  the  restricted  visibility  characteristic  of  Ruffed  Grouse 
habitat.  These  observational  difficulties  may  have  resulted  in  this  behavior  being  overlooked 
when  Ruffed  Grouse  were  observed  in  a natural  setting. 

A colony  of  captive  Ruffed  Grouse  at  the  University  of  Guelph,  Guelph,  Ontario,  provided 


Fig.  1.  A male  Ruffed  Grouse  displaying  a shoulder  spot. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


99 


the  opportunity  for  extended  observations  of  grouse  behavior.  The  shoulder-spot  display  was 
observed  in  many  males  on  numerous  occasions.  By  contrast,  no  females  were  observed 
using  this  display. 

Copulation  is  the  activity  during  which  the  shoulder-spot  display  has  been  most  frequently 
seen  in  females  of  other  grouse  species  (Lumsden  1970).  However,  our  captive  females  rarely 
permit  normal  copulation,  and  the  observation  of  copulation  is  very  rare  in  captivity.  There- 
fore, failure  to  observe  the  shoulder-spot  display  in  female  Ruffed  Grouse  may  be  attributable 
to  the  rarity  of  copulation  by  captive  hens  rather  than  to  the  absence  of  this  display  in  female 
Ruffed  Grouse. 

The  shoulder-spot  display  was  most  frequently  observed  in  captive  males  performing  what 
has  been  termed  the  “intimidation”  display  (Aubin,  M.Sc.  thesis,  Univ.  Alberta, 
Edmonton,  Alberta,  1970)  or  “upright-cum-ruff’  (Hjorth  1970)  display.  When  a male  per- 
forming the  intimidation  display  is  approached  by  an  observer,  the  male  usually  attacks  or 
retreats  within  a short  time.  However,  some  males  are  reluctant  to  do  either,  leading  to  an 
ambivalent  situation.  If  the  ambivalence  is  sufficiently  intense,  the  male  assumes  a semi- 
upright posture,  with  all  feathers  except  the  crest  sleeked.  In  this  posture,  he  alternately 
approaches  and  retreats  from  an  intruder,  with  his  body  held  at  a slight  angle  to  the  intruder. 
He  may  threaten  to  strike  with  his  bill.  Before  this  strike  intention  movement  is  made,  the 
wings  are  withdrawn  from  beneath  the  contour  feathers,  and  are  slightly  extended.  The 
shoulder-spot  display  is  performed  just  before  the  wings  are  extended  (Fig.  1). 

Close  examination  of  birds  performing  this  display  revealed  that  the  shoulder-spot  is 
formed  by  exposure  of  the  underwing  coverts  on  the  upper  surface  of  the  wing,  as  Lumsden 
(1970)  thought.  From  examination  and  manipulation  of  the  wings  of  live  birds,  it  does  not 
seem  possible  that  a simple  re-alignment  of  the  underwing  coverts  could  produce  a shoulder- 
spot  of  the  dimensions  seen  on  many  males.  The  exposure  seems  to  be  effected  by  movement 
of  the  patagial  skin,  accompanied  by  repositioning  of  the  feathers.  Apparently,  the  skin  is 
drawn  over  the  leading  edge  of  the  wing,  and  onto  the  upper  surface.  The  coverts  are  then 
exposed  to  form  the  shoulder-spot.  By  varying  the  degree  of  skin  movement  and  feather 
rearrangement,  it  would  be  possible  for  the  bird  to  alter  the  dimensions  of  the  shoulder-spot 
displayed. 

Similarities  between  Ruffed  Grouse  and  other  grouse  species  in  the  method  of  effecting 
this  display,  and  the  context  within  which  it  is  performed  indicate  the  origin  of  the  shoulder- 
spot  display  is  similar  in  all  grouse.  These  observations  support  the  suggested  evolutionary 
development,  whereby  the  display  is  derived  from  what  was  originally  a flight  intention 
movement  (Hjorth  1970,  Lumsden  1970). 

Financial  support  for  the  maintenance  of  the  grouse  colony  was  provided  by  the  Ontario 
Ministry  of  Natural  Resources  and  the  University  of  Guelph. — Allan  Garbutt,  Dept. 
Zoology,  Univ.  Guelph,  Guelph,  Ontario  NIG  2W1  Canada.  (Present  address:  1424  Carlyle 
Rd.,  Calgary,  Alberta  T2V  2Vl  Canada.)  Accepted  3 Feb.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  99-103 


The  agonistic  repertoire  of  Sandhill  Cranes. — Detailed  descriptions  of  agonistic  dis- 
plays are  lacking  for  wild  Sandhill  Cranes  (Crus  canadensis).  Walkinshaw  reports  that  all 
cranes  have  some  of  the  same  aggressive  displays  (Walkinshaw,  Cranes  of  the  World,  Win- 
chester Press,  New  York,  New  York,  1973).  Masatomi  and  Kitagawa  (J.  Fac.  Sci.,  Hokkaido 
Univ.,  Ser.  IV,  Zool.  19:834-878,  1975)  give  a thorough  description  of  agonistic  behavior  in 
the  Japanese  Cranes  {G.  japonensis)  that  facilitates  description  of  such  behavior  in  Sandhill 
Cranes.  Voss  (pp.  63-85  in  Eastern  Greater  Sandhill  Crane  Symposium,  R.  D.  Feldt,  com- 


100 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


piler,  Michigan  City,  Indiana,  1977)  describes  agonistic  behavior  in  captive  and  wild  Greater 
Sandhill  Cranes  (G.  c.  tabida)  on  the  summer  range  in  Wisconsin.  Here  we  present  obser- 
vations of  agonistic  behavior  of  migratory  Greater  Sandhill  Cranes  on  their  winter  range  in 
Florida  and  summer  range  in  Wisconsin  and  year-round  observations  of  the  resident  Florida 
Sandhill  Crane  {G.  c.  pratensis)  based  on  approximately  700  h of  fieldwork  at  Paynes  Prairie, 
Alachua  Co.,  Florida  and  at  the  International  Crane  Foundation,  Baraboo,  Wisconsin.  We 
also  include  the  contexts  under  which  these  agonistic  behaviors  were  given.  Approximately 
80%  of  the  birds  observed  in  Florida  had  been  captured  (Nesbitt,  pp.  299-303  in  Proceeding 
of  the  International  Crane  Workshop,  J.  C.  Lewis,  ed.,  Oklahoma  State  Univ.,  Stillwater, 
Oklahoma,  1975),  individually  color  marked  and  their  sex  and  approximate  age  deter- 
mined. Sex  was  determined  by  laporatomy  or  from  postures  assumed  during  Unison  Calling, 
a sexually  distinct  display.  Age  was  determined  from  plumage  characteristics. 

Highly  dominant  displays  are  given  by  individuals  that  have  little  or  no  fear  of  other  cranes. 
If  2 highly  aggressive  cranes  confront  each  other  Bill  Sparring  usually  results.  This  display 
begins  when  the  2 birds  approach  each  other,  some  preliminary  bill  jabbing  ensues,  then 
with  wings  extended,  neck  feathers  erect  and  bill  tips  close  together  (Fig.  LA),  the  birds 
vocalize  and  vault  into  the  air  throwing  feet  and  wings  forward  (Fig.  IB).  Substantial  contact 
does  not  usually  occur,  but  there  is  a risk  of  injury  from  feet  or  bill.  In  many  instances,  this 
display  is  very  brief,  lasting  only  2 or  3 sec  and  does  not  progress  beyond  the  initial  bill 
jabbing.  The  victor  stands  his  ground  while  the  loser  retreats  giving  any  of  the  6 escape 
postures  described  by  Masatomi  and  Kitagawa  (1975).  Archibald  reported  a similar  display 
in  the  Hooded  {Grus  monacha)  (Animal  Kingdom  77:19-24,  1974)  and  White-naped  (Grus 
vipio)  cranes  (Animal  Kingdom  76:17-21,  1973)  as  does  Walkinshaw  (1973)  for  Sandhills.  Bill 
Sparring  probably  is  important  to  the  establishment  of  a dominance  hierarchy.  Kepler  (pp. 
177-196  in  Proceeding  of  the  International  Crane  Workshop,  J.  C.  Lewis,  ed.,  1975)  dis- 
cusses the  occurrence  of  a linear  dominance  hierarchy  in  a flock  of  9 captive-reared  Whoop- 
ing Cranes  {G.  americana).  A similar  hierarchical  system  was  noticed  in  the  wild  among 
adult  male  Sandhill  Cranes  and  may  exist  through  other  social  groups  of  sandhills  as  well. 
During  another  high  intensity  agonistic  display,  the  Head  Lowered  Charge  (Fig.  ID),  the 
aggressor  rushed  quickly  at  another  bird,  neck  extended,  head,  neck  and  body  held  hori- 
zontal, wings  usually  tight  to  the  body.  Sometimes  flapping  begins  as  the  other  bird  is 
approached,  the  bill  is  open,  and  the  aggressor  often  grabs  the  other  bird  by  the  wing  or 
scapular  feathers.  A charge  can  occur  during  feeding  where  the  aggressor,  apparently  feeding 
normally,  moves  closer  to  the  offending  individual.  Then  from  the  feeding  position  a charge 
erupts  catching  the  other  crane  by  surprise.  Walkinshaw  (1973)  has  generally  described  this 
behavior  as  occurring  with  all  cranes.  The  Head  Lowered  Charge  often  leads  to  Aerial 
Pursuit,  especially  during  the  period  when  the  birds  are  defending  nesting  territories.  During 
Aerial  Pursuits  the  aggressor  may  attempt  to  strike  the  fleeing  bird  with  its  feet.  A dominant 
crane  will  displace  a subordinate  individual  from  a feeding  or  drinking  site  with  a Bill  Stab 
(Fig.  IE)  directed  at  the  back  or  back  of  the  neck  with  bill  either  open  or  closed.  The  attacked 
bird  simply  moves  a few  steps,  and  the  dominant  bird  assumes  the  feeding  or  drinking  spot. 
Charges  or  stabs  from  the  dominant  individual  often  follow  a full  Bill  Sparring  episode. 
Following  all  high  intensity  agonistic  displays,  the  dominating  individual  usually  gives  a Low 
Bow  display  (Masatomi  and  Kitagawa  1975,  Voss  1977)  during  which  the  neck  is  arched  and 
the  head  slowly  lowered  toward  the  ground  displaying  an  expanded  bright  red  comb. 
The  display  terminates  with  the  head  between  the  bird’s  legs  and  bill  held  vertically  (Fig. 
IF).  As  the  head  is  lowered  the  bird  emits  a low  growl.  A Low  Bow  is  often  given  by  territorial 
birds  upon  landing  near  an  intruder  before  any  other  signs  of  aggression  are  shown. 

During  agonistic  episodes  where  the  motivation  levels  are  lower,  any  one  of  a series  of 
generalized  ambivalent  displays  may  be  seen.  To  drive  other  cranes  from  a defended  territory 


GENERAL  NOTES 


101 


Fig.  1.  Common  agonistic  displays  of  Sandhill  Cranes:  (A,  B)  Bill  Sparring;  (C)  Directed 
Walk  Threat;  (D)  Head  Lowered  Charge;  (E)  Bill  Stab;  (F)  Low  Bow;  (G)  Generalized  Body- 
Wing  Shaking;  (H)  Neck  Retracted  Submissive  Posture. 


or  a feeding  area,  the  dominant  individual  or  pair  (and  their  chick,  if  present)  give  a Directed 
Walk  Threat  or  Adornment  Walking  (Masatomi  and  Kitagawa  1975,  Voss  1977)  often  with 
a characteristic  vocalization  (Slow-rattle  Family  Call;  Nesbitt  and  Bradley,  in  press)  given 
in  unison,  by  all  defending  cranes.  This  display  is  an  exaggeration  of  the  normal  upright 
walking  movement  directed  at  a particular  individual.  The  displaying  bird  circles  the  op- 
ponent with  a stiff  gait  characterized  by  animated  head  pumping  and  tertials  slightly  raised. 
With  each  step  the  neck  is  extended  and  the  bill  pointed  at  the  other  crane.  Periodically, 
the  bill  is  angled  downward  displaying  an  expanded,  bright  red  comb  (Fig.  1C).  The  Directed 
Walk  Threat  is  apparently  equivalent  to  a display  described  by  Archibald  (1974)  for  the 
Hooded  Crane.  The  individual  toward  which  this  behavior  has  been  addressed  usually  moves 
away  quickly  with  head  lowered  and  body  held  horizontally.  Other  adornment  displays  de- 
scribed by  Masatomi  and  Kitagawa  (1975)  for  G.  japonensis  seem  to  represent  variations  in 


102 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


the  intensity  of  adornment  walking  in  sandhills.  If  the  offending  individual  does  not  retreat 
as  a result  of  the  Directed  Walk  Threat  a charge  or  stab  may  occur.  If  the  aggression  level 
is  not  high,  one  of  several  ritualized  general  threat  displays  may  ensue.  The  Generalized 
Body-Wing  Shaking  (Voss  1977)  (Fig.  IG),  similar  to  the  Low  Bow,  has  been  described  for 
several  species  of  cranes  (Archibald  1974;  Walkinshaw  1973;  Paulsen,  Dansk  Ornithol.  For- 
enings  Tidsskr.  69:119-122,  1975).  But  it  is  less  intense  and  general  rather  than  directed. 
Body-Wing  Shaking  begins  as  a stylized  feather  maintenance  movement,  contains  a less 
dramatic  bow  and  terminates  with  displacement  preening  of  the  legs  or  belly.  Again  a low 
growl  is  given  as  the  head  is  lowered.  In  Sandhill  Cranes  other  forms  of  bowing  described 
by  Masatomi  and  Kitagawa  (1975)  appear  to  be  less  intense  versions  of  the  Low  Bow.  The 
Unison  Call  may  function  as  a generalized  aggressive  display  (Archibald,  Ph.D.  diss.,  Cornell 
Univ.,  Ithaca,  New  York,  1976).  A pair  will  often  Unison  Call  before  aggressive  episodes  or 
after  successfully  driving  intruders  from  a defended  territory.  The  Crouch  Display  observed 
by  Voss  (1977)  principally  in  young  cranes  was  noticed  among  the  Florida  birds  only  once, 
given  by  a young  crane.  In  the  wild  it  is  perhaps  a seldom  given  display  that  in  adult  birds 
transmits  little  or  no  agonistic  information.  When  2 cranes  with  lower  aggressive  motivation 
are  close  together,  they  often  engage  in  displacement  foraging  or  preening.  These  are  tran- 
sitional behaviors  leading  to  normal  feeding  or  preening  and  away  from  aggressive  encoun- 
ters. 

In  fearful  situations,  birds  of  the  year  and  other  subordinant  individuals  wishing  to  avoid 
attack,  assume  a Neck-retracted  Submissive  Posture  (Voss  1977)  (Fig.  IH),  during  which  the 
comb  is  constricted  and  dull  in  color.  A crane,  fearful  of  attack  from  another  crane,  may 
feign  the  precopulatory  display  with  wings  spread  and  beak  held  above  horizontal  at  a 45° 
angle,  thus  changing  the  motivation  of  the  attacker.  When  approached  by  a mammalian 
predator.  Sandhill  Cranes  give  a predator  threatening  Spread-Wing  Display  (Voss  1977), 
consisting  of  an  upright  posture  with  wings  held  high  and  half-open.  The  head  is  held  high 
with  the  beak  pointed  directly  at  the  predator.  If  the  predator  does  not  retreat,  then  the  bird 
approaches,  thrusts  the  bill  forward  and  hisses.  A crane  was  observed  successfully  fending 
off  the  attack  of  a juvenile  Bald  Eagle  (Haliaeetus  leucocephalus)  using  bill  jabs  with  vaults 
and  forward  thrusting  of  the  feet  and  wings.  Such  movements,  similar  to  Chasing  and  Kicking 
(Voss  1977),  are  generally  equivalent  to  the  movements  that  have  been  stylized  into  Bill 
Sparring,  but  without  the  characteristic  vocalizations.  A similar  attack  behavior  and  hissing 
has  been  observed  during  trapping  operations  when  oral  tranquilizers  were  used.  Attacks 
were  directed  at  tranquilized  cranes  that  did  not  react  normally  to  unaffected  birds.  Pre- 
sumably this  is  the  type  of  attack  that  has  resulted  in  severe  injuries  to  cranes  (Walkinshaw, 
Michigan  Acad.  Sci.,  Arts,  Letters  40:75-88,  1965).  The  attack  method  described  by  Altmann 
(J.  Mammal.  41:525,  1960)  employed  by  a pair  of  adult  cranes  with  a chick  to  drive  off  a 
moose  (Alces  alces)  incorporated  these  same  movements. 

Aggression  was  often  observed  during  and  after  unpaired  dancing  bouts  between  non- 
breeding cranes.  Aggressive  attacks  were  also  launched  at  individuals  apparently  preoccu- 
pied in  another  behavior  such  as  Bill-Raising  (Masatomi  and  Kitagawa,  1975:Fig.  119),  uni- 
lateral stretching  or  sitting. 

All  of  these  displays  have  been  observed  in  both  sexes,  though  Bill  Sparring  appears  more 
pronounced  in  males.  Sub-adult  cranes  exhibited  the  same  aggressive  displays,  but  the 
frequency  and  intensity  of  the  displays  were  much  reduced  when  compared  with  paired 
males.  Among  4 of  5 distinctly  marked  breeding  pairs  tbe  male  consistently  took  the  leading 
role  in  territorial  defense.  W ithin  the  fifth  pair,  the  male  and  female  were  equally  aggressive, 
both  initiating  an  equal  number  of  encounters.  Females  and  young  of  the  year  commonly 
participate  in  Directed  Walk  Threats,  and  occasionally  Charges.  Other  displays  appear  much 
less  frequently.  The  hierarchical  position  of  the  pair  or  family  seems  to  depend  on  the 


GENERAL  NOTES 


103 


position  of  the  male.  The  aggressiveness  and  therefore  the  hierarchical  position  of  a male 
relates  to  the  presence  of  a chick.  Pairs  without  chicks  tend  to  be  lower  in  the  order.  For 
example,  the  dominant  pair  of  the  5 marked  pairs  had  1 chick  in  1977.  In  1978,  they  were 
chickless  and  were  dominated  by  2 previously  subordinate  pairs,  both  with  chicks. 

The  frequency  of  agonistic  encounters  involving  adult  Florida  cranes  increased  during  the 
period  when  young  of  the  previous  year  were  separated  from  the  family  group  (February  and 
March).  The  frequency  remained  high  until  just  before  the  eggs  hatched,  then  the  level 
declined  and  the  number  of  encounters  remained  low  until  several  weeks  after  hatching. 
This  lowered  aggression  period  corresponds  with  the  period  in  which  Bennett  (Auk  95:411^13, 
1978)  noticed  little  response  from  territorial  cranes  to  the  play  back  of  tape  recorded  calls. 

We  wish  to  thank  K.  S.  Voss  and  C.  B.  Brownsmith  for  their  comments  on  early  drafts  of 
this  manuscript.  This  study  was  in  part  a contribution  of  the  Federal  Aid  to  the  Wildlife 
Restoration  Program,  Florida  Pittman-Robertson  Project  W-41. — Stephen  A.  Nesbitt, 
Florida  Game  and  Fresh  Water  Fish  Commission,  Wildlife  Research  Laboratory,  4005  S 
Main  St.,  Gainesville,  Florida  32601  AND  George  W.  Archibald,  International  Crane 
Foundation,  Baraboo,  Wisconsin  53913.  Accepted  1 Nov.  1979. 


Wilson  Ball.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  103-107 


Notes  on  the  Slender  Antbird. — The  Slender  Antbird  (Rhopornis  ardesiaca),  first  col- 
lected somewhere  in  eastern  Brazil  by  Prince  Maximilian  von  Wied  (Beitrage  zur  Naturges- 
chichte  von  Brasilien,  Vol.  3,  1831),  was  until  recently  known  from  3 specimens:  the  male 
type,  another  male  from  Ituagu,  in  south-central  Bahia,  and  a female  from  the  town  of  Boa 
Nova  just  down  the  Rio  de  Contas  (Naumberg,  Bull.  Am.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.  76:231-276,  1939). 
Emil  Kaempfer  collected  the  last  2 specimens  in  1928.  Naumberg  suggested  that  Kaempfer’s 
“Boa  Nova”  was  another  town  with  the  same  name,  northwest  across  the  Rio  Sao  Francisco; 
but  Kaempfer  was  at  the  second  Boa  Nova  in  1927,  not  1928.  Moreover,  Wied  is  likely  to 
have  collected  the  type  near  the  first  Boa  Nova,  which  he  passed  en  route  from  Vitoria  da 
Conquista  to  Salvador. 

From  3-9  December  1974,  we  studied  Slender  Antbirds  in  patches  of  dry  forest  on  Fazenda 
Alvorada,  just  north  of  the  first  Boa  Nova  (14°20'S,  40°H'W).  A good,  if  scattered,  population 
exists  in  these  patches,  which  are  gradually  being  cleared  for  cattle  pastures.  In  1977,  H. 
Sick  collected  a male  at  Boa  Nova  after  we  mentioned  our  observations  to  him. 

Habitat  and  foraging. — Boa  Nova  lies  at  700  m elev.,  below  800-1000  m ridges  of  the 
northern  end  of  a broad  plateau  that  stretches  southwest  past  Vitoria  da  Conquista  nearly 
to  the  valley  of  the  Rio  Pardo  in  the  state  of  Minas  Gerais  (Fig.  1).  This  plateau  is  the  main 
ridge  of  southeastern  Bahia,  forming  a border  between  wet  coastal  forests  (which  include 
some  patches  of  dry  forest  in  the  upper  basins  of  small  rivers)  and  the  desert  scrub  or 
“caatinga”  of  the  interior. 

The  natural  vegetation  of  this  rolling  plateau  varied  within  short  distances  from  wet  cloud 
forests  (15(K)-2000  mm  annual  rainfall)  on  the  eastern  escarpments  to  caatinga  in  such  rain- 
shadow  areas  as  the  lee  slopes  around  Boa  Nova,  but  the  summit  was  mainly  a dry  forest 
(8(X)-1000  mm  rainfall)  with  many  “cipos”  or  lianas — a “mata  de  cip6.”  The  scattered  re- 
maining patches  of  dry  forest  have  a strange  appearance,  with  scattered  white  trunks  of 
small  trees  above  a dense  layer  of  midstory  trees  and  vines.  The  understory  is  fairly  open, 
but  blocked  here  and  there  by  lianas  and  by  patches  of  huge  terrestrial  bromeliads  [Aechmea 
sp.).  In  the  forest,  bromeliads  tend  to  sit  high  on  tree  trunks;  but  at  the  borders  between  dry 


104 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Eig.  1.  Coastal  Bahia,  Brazil,  showing  the  region  inhabited  by  Slender  Antbirds.  The 
only  roads  shown  are  ones  traveled  by  the  authors.  ' 


forest  and  caatinga,  or  between  dry  forest  and  pastures,  there  is  enough  light  for  the  hro- 
tneliads  to  descend  to  the  ground.  These  hromeliads,  and  surrounding  undergrowth  near  the 
forest  edge,  are  the  habitat  of  Slender  Antbirds.  j 

Just  inside  the  forest  borders — occasionally  in  tall  scrub  nearby — the  antbirds  hop  on  the  ! 

ground,  low  vines  and  on  the  tops  of  hromeliad  leaves,  pausing  now  and  then  to  swipe  dead  j 

leaves  from  atop  the  hromeliads  or  from  spots  on  the  ground.  They  wander  silently  much  of  j 

the  time,  peering  up  and  down  and  capturing  small  grasshoppers  or  other  insects  by  short  j 

flights  or  by  hopping  down  to  peck.  We  found  them  surprisingly  like  Gray  Catbirds  {Du-  \ 
metella  carolinensis)  of  North  America  in  their  appearance  and  actions,  although  they  stay  j 
on  the  ground  much  more.  j 

Members  of  a pair  wander  separately  or  together,  often  disappearing  among  the  dense 
hromeliads  for  15  min  or  more,  only  to  reappear  less  than  5 m from  where  they  entered. 

Each  pair  we  watched  seemed  to  have  a very  limited  home  range,  barely  50  m across;  but  ■ 

home  ranges  of  pairs  were  usually  separated  by  100  m or  more  because  patches  of  hromeliads  i 


1 


GENERAL  NOTES 


105 


Fig.  2.  Audiospectrographs  of  Slender  Antbird  vocalizations:  (A)  song  of  male;  (B)  alarm 
chip  (logarithmic  scale);  (C)  song  of  female;  (D)  faint  chirps  and  grunts  of  a pair;  (E)  rattle, 
starting  with  a chip;  (F)  rattle  of  another  bird. 


were  seldom  close  together.  Occasionally  we  found  birds  hopping  on  the  ground  through 
fairly  open  undergrowth  between  patches  of  bromeliads,  and  once  2 such  pairs  were  singing 
as  they  hopped  along  a presumed  territorial  boundary.  In  the  center  of  the  main  forest  tract 
of  Fazenda  Alvorada,  we  encountered  only  1 bird  near  a bromeliad  zone  in  a treefall  clearing. 
Maximum  densities  were  in  a rather  scrubby  second-growth  woodlot,  where  pairs  were  100- 
200  m apart. 

We  found  no  Rhopornis  following  army  ants  within  the  main  woodlot;  there  the  related 
White-winged  Fire-Eyes  (Pyriglena  leucoptera)  and  White-bibbed  Antbirds  (Myrmeciza  lor- 
icata)  split  the  niche  of  the  Slender  Antbird.  Its  niche  is  essentially  that  of  another  slender 
and  long-legged  forest-edge  antbird,  the  White-bellied  Antbird  (M.  longipes),  a species  that 
follows  army  ants  occasionally  in  Panama,  Trinidad  and  other  areas. 

Song  and  vocal  behavior. — The  simple  and  loud  peer  peer  peer  peer  peer  peer  song  of 
Slender  Antbirds  (Fig.  2A,  C)  is  audible  up  to  500  m from  the  forest  edge,  and  seems  well 
adapted  for  birds  that  live  widely  scattered  along  forest  edges  in  isolated  patches  of  dense 
cover.  Songs  began  15—60  min  after  the  first  light  and  after  the  first  songs  of  other  diurnal 
birds,  hence  between  05:20  and  06:00.  Sessions  of  song  were  irregular,  mostly  during  the 
morning;  few  birds  sang  at  any  time.  Songs  were  rare  after  13:00  or  14:00.  Often  the  male 
started  singing  when  the  female  disappeared,  she  would  finally  answer  him  a few  times,  and 
the  two  would  move  together  again.  One  male  sang  off-and-on  for  nearly  2 h (05:40-07:30) 


106 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


until  the  female  appeared.  At  times,  2 males  or  2 pairs  sang  back  and  forth  for  short  periods, 
mostly  in  boundary  disputes. 

When  the  pair  met  and  foraged  close  together,  they  exchanged  faint  sif  or  prit  and  other 
notes  (Fig.  2D),  some  rather  like  the  “bubbling”  call  of  White-backed  Fire-Eyes.  The  male 
occasionally  started  a “serpentine-song”  of  the  type  common  in  antbirds:  an  alternating 
series  of  slow  and  fast  chirps,  eu,  eu,  u-u-u-u-u,  eu,  eu,  eu,  eu  . . . and  so  on.  One  male  fed 
his  mate  a small  grasshopper  after  serpentine-singing.  Another  pair  carried  small  insects  to 
a patch  of  bromeliads  and  rattled  repeatedly  at  the  observer;  but  on  other  days  the  female 
rattled  only  near  another  patch  of  bromeliads.  Otherwise  there  was  no  evidence  of  breeding 
activity. 

Individual  birds  varied  widely  in  their  reactions  to  us.  Some  foraged  quietly  and  tamely, 
others  quickly  fled  to  dense  vegetation  and  others  scolded  vigorously.  None  was  as  difficult 
to  observe  as  are  antbirds  of  the  rain  forest,  perhaps  because  Slender  Antbirds  live  next  to 
good  cover  rather  than  in  open  undergrowth.  The  usual  alarm  calls  are  a sharp  chipping  and 
a loud  rattling.  Chipping  is  a loud  sibilant  tsiekl  or  psiefl  (Fig.  2B)  as  the  bird  pounds  the  tail 
downward  before  or  after  fleeing  to  cover.  At  times  the  call  becomes  a loud  feeyoul  or  phewl, 
perhaps  a different  note.  An  approaching  or  mobbing  bird  opens  the  beak  widely  and  rattles 
loudly,  \ (Fig.  2E,  F).  The  song  and  all  these  alarm  calls  are  like  calls  of  Black- 

headed Antbirds  (Percnostola  rufifrons),  but  also  resemble  calls  of  antbirds  of  the  genera 
Myrmeciza  and  Pyriglena.  The  song  and  rattle  are  especially  like  those  of  all  the  species  of 
Pyriglena,  and  can  be  confused  with  notes  of  Pyriglena  leucoptera  at  Boa  Nova.  The  songs 
of  Narrow-billed  Antbirds  (Formicivora  iheringi),  common  in  lower  midlevels  of  dry  forest 
at  Boa  Nova,  are  like  slow  and  faint  versions  of  the  songs  of  Slender  Antbirds.  Competitive 
mimicry  (Cody,  Ann.  Rev.  Ecol.  Syst.  4:189-211,  1973)  in  voice  seems  possible,  although 
the  3 species  do  not  overlap  much  in  foraging. 

Conservation. — Dry  forest  in  central  southern  Bahia  is  rapidly  being  cleared  for  cattle, 
according  to  CEPLAC,  the  Cocoa  Institute  at  Itabuna  and  Renato  Aragao,  former  director 
of  the  Institute  Brasileiro  de  Desenvolvimento  Florestal  at  Salvador.  The  initial  stages  of 
clearing  for  cattle,  creating  many  zones  of  forest  edge,  probably  benefit  Slender  Antbirds. 
However,  patches  of  forest  are  decreasing  in  size  and  length  of  edge.  At  Fazenda  Alvorada, 
cutting  a corner  of  the  woodlot  near  the  ranch  houses  to  plant  corn  and  beans  had  trapped 
1 pair  of  Slender  Antbirds  in  a patch  50  x 100  m.  Early  each  morning  they  hopped  out 
through  newly  felled  trees,  searched  a bit  and  gave  alarm  notes,  and  soon  returned  to 
bromeliads  in  the  decreasing  bit  of  forest.  Another  edge  of  the  main  woods  was  recently 
cleared  to  plant  introduced  grass,  even  though  wide  areas  of  pasture  nearby  were  reverting 
to  scrub.  On  the  far  side  of  the  main  woodlot,  a neighbor  was  also  clearing  new  areas.  One 
can  see  cleared  slopes  all  around  Boa  Nova,  so  the  fate  of  Slender  Antbirds  is  certainly  in 
doubt.  The  pattern  in  similar  areas  in  Sao  Paulo  state  has  been  to  clear  all  forests,  then  plant 
eucalyptus  or  pine  for  crops  when  hills  start  to  erode  and  pastures  are  no  longer  worth 
weeding. 

Slender  Antbirds  should  be  kept  on  the  Endangered  or  Vulnerable  lists  of  the  International 
Council  for  Bird  Preservation.  Another  species  restricted  to  mata-de-cip6,  Formicivora  iher- 
ingi, should  probably  he  added  to  the  list.  A forest  reserve  of  mata-de-cip6  is  certainly 
desirable,  just  as  it  would  be  good  to  have  reserves  in  all  distinctive  types  of  vegetation,  or 
even  better  to  preserve  a natural  framework  of  vegetation  in  all  regions.  One  idea  for  Boa 
Nova  would  be  a reserve  in  conjunction  with  an  agricultural  experiment  station,  which  is 
needed  on  the  plateau  because  of  its  distinctive  climate,  soils,  etc.  The  problem  remains 
that  species  often  disappear  from  reserves  (Willis,  Ecol.  Monogr.  44:153-169,  1974),  espe- 
cially small  ones.  W Idle  the  Slender  Antbird  has  dense  populations  and  may  resist  extinction, 
it  could  be  lost. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


107 


Acknowledgments. — We  appreciate  a grant  from  the  Committee  for  Research  and  Explo- 
ration of  the  National  Geographic  Society,  the  courtesy  of  ranchers  Joel  Almeida  Sampaio 
and  Humberto  Gomes  Sampaio  of  Fazenda  Alvorada  and  of  other  people  of  Boa  Nova  and 
information  furnished  by  the  agronomists  of  CEPLAC  at  Itabuna.  Eugene  Eisenmann  and 
John  Farrand  checked  the  records  of  Rhopornis  and  suggested  the  correct  Boa  Nova  for  us. 
Edwin  O.  Willis  and  Yoshik.4  Oniki,  Dept.  Biology,  Univ.  Miami,  Coral  Gables,  Florida 
33124.  Accepted  15  Jan.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  107-108 

Notes  on  the  Uniform  Crake  in  Costa  Rica. — Although  the  Uniform  Crake  (Amau- 
rolimnas  concolor)  is  found  from  Mexico  to  Bolivia  and  Brazil,  and  formerly  occurred  on 
Jamaica,  virtually  nothing  is  known  of  its  habits.  During  fieldwork  in  1971-1973  at  Finca  La 
Selva,  in  the  wet  lowlands  of  NE  Costa  Rica,  I obtained  information  on  the  behavior  and 
vocalization  of  this  elusive  bird,  as  well  as  the  first  unequivocal  data  on  nesting  of  the  species. 

Although  not  reported  for  La  Selva  by  Slud  (Bull.  Am.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.  121:49,  1960),  the 
Uniform  Crake  is  fairly  common  in  forested  swamps,  heavy  vine-tangled  thickets  along 
forested  streams  and  in  dense  second  growth  adjoining  forest,  especially  favoring  the  maze 
of  hanging  dead  and  decaying  leaves  in  Heliconia  thickets  (cf.  also  Orians  and  Paulson, 
Condor  71:426,  1969;  Kiff,  Condor  77:101,  1975).  In  these  dense,  tangled  habitats,  the  birds 
are  not  particularly  shy  and  may  approach  a motionless  observer  closely,  but  seldom  leave 
the  densest  available  cover. 

In  life,  A.  concolor  resembles  a diminutive  wood-rail  (Aramides  sp.)  in  build,  posture  and 
soft-part  colors:  red  iris,  greenish-yellow  bill  and  orange  to  reddish  legs.  The  bird  usually 
has  an  erect  stance  and  walks  with  head  high  and  tail  cocked,  except  while  foraging.  The 
tail  may  be  pumped  in  agitation  and  is  carried  low  as  the  bird  scurries,  mouselike,  across 
an  opening.  The  birds  forage  deliberately,  walking  slowly  and  pecking  into  leaf  litter,  hanging 
dead  leaves  and  detritus.  Apparently  they  also  dig  in  soft  mud,  as  I have  observed  birds  with 
obviously  muddy  beaks  on  several  occasions.  I have  seen  Uniform  Crakes  seize,  beat  and 
swallow  spiders  (Lycosidae),  a very  small  frog  (Eleutherodactylus  sp.)  and  a small  lizard 
(Anolis  sp.),  which  was  killed  with  a few  swift  pecks,  then  swallowed  headfirst. 

Like  Aramides  wood-rails,  the  Uniform  Crake  possesses  loud,  arresting  whistled  calls  that 
often  provide  the  only  clue  to  its  presence.  To  date  1 have  noted  the  following  vocalizations: 

(a)  A series  of  6—9  clear,  upslurred  whistles,  in  which  successive  notes  first  become  louder 
and  higher  pitched,  then  accelerate,  drop  in  pitch  and  fade  away — tooeee,  Tooeee,  Toooeee, 
TOOOEEE,  Tooee,  tooee-tuee-tui.  A bird  giving  this  call  was  often  answered  by  another  some 
distance  away.  1 could  often  decoy  single  birds  to  within  1—2  m by  imitating  it;  this  call  is 
probably  a territorial  advertisement  or  “song.”  At  very  high  intensities  (as  when  answering 
my  imitation  at  very  close  range),  the  loudest  notes  of  the  call  often  had  a flutelike  break  in 
the  middle:  toourleee,  etc. 

(b)  At  close  range  in  the  last-mentioned  situations  a soft,  low-pitched,  pigeon-like  cu- 
uuuhuuuu  is  audible,  possibly  an  aggressive  note. 

(c)  Two  birds,  perhaps  a mated  pair,  may  call  back  and  forth  with  one  or  several  clear, 
not  very  loud,  whistled  toooo  notes,  either  level  in  pitch  or  slightly  downslurred. 

(d)  A sharp,  nasal  kek  is  given  by  a startled  bird. 

The  loud  “song”  of  A.  concolor  was  heard  at  La  Selva  chiefly  from  late  August  to  Decem- 
ber, which  is  probably  the  breeding  season.  On  14  November  1973,  I found  a nest  with  eggs 
in  an  area  where  I had  seen  and  heard  much  A.  concolor  activity  in  the  preceding  2 months. 


108 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  \o.  1,  March  1981 


The  nest  consisted  of  a loose  cup  of  leaves  filling  a hollow  in  the  top  of  a vine-covered  stump 
beside  a seldom  used  trail  in  a small  treefall  clearing  in  swamp  forest.  The  stump  was  about 
5 m from  a stream,  and  1 m from  the  nearset  dense  thicket,  into  which  the  adult  bird  flushed 
from  the  nest  promptly  disappeared,  allowing  a brief  glimpse  of  reddish-brown  plumage  and 
orange  legs.  The  nest  contained  4 slightly  incubated,  sub-elliptical,  slightly  glossy  eggs  (set 
no.  78177  of  the  estern  Eoundation  of  Vertebrate  Zoolog>  ).  The  ground  color  of  the  eggs 
is  pale  huffy,  marked  mostly  near  the  large  end  with  bold  superficial  reddish-brown  splotches 
and  subsurface  spots  and  blotches  of  grey  and  purplish-brown.  Measurements  (length  and 
largest  diameter)  and  dry  shell  weights  of  each  egg  are:  33.40  x 26.11  mm,  0.774  g;  44.41  x 
25.90  mm,  0.740  g:  33.28  x 26.18  mm,  0.789  g:  and  33.60  x 25.74  mm,  0.675  g. 

The  first  description  of  a putative  .4.  concolor  egg  (from  Brazil)  was  by  Nehrkorn  (Katalog 
der  Eiersammlung  nebst  Beschreibungen  der  Aussereuropaischen  Eier  von  Adolf  Nehrkorn, 
11  Auflage,  R.  Friedlander  und  Sohn.  Berlin,  1910):  “reddish-grey  with  very  sparse  violet 
and  rust-brown  flecks,  33  x 26.5  mm"  (translation  mine).  The  measurements,  but  not  the 
colors,  fit  the  set  described  here.  Schonwetter  (Handbuch  der  Oologie,  Lieferung  5,  Aka- 
demie-verlag,  Berlin,  1961)  repeated  Nehrkorn's  description  but  could  not  locate  the  egg  in 
question,  which  is  probably  lost.  Bond  (Birds  of  the  est  Indies,  averly  Press,  Baltimore, 
Maryland,  1936)  wrote  of  .4.  concolor  that  “the  egg  of  the  continental  form  is  said  to  be  ashy- 
grey  with  a reddish  tinge,  speckled  dusky  (34  x 31.5  mm)."  This  description  was  not  re- 
peated in  later  editions  of  Bond’s  book,  and  its  original  source  is  unknown:  since  neither 
colors  nor  measurements  agree  with  the  present  set,  the  identification  was  probably  erro- 
neous. Finally,  Wetmore  (Birds  of  the  Republic  of  Panama,  Pt.  1,  Smithson.  Misc.  CoU. 
\ ol.  150,  1965)  described  eggs  he  believed  to  pertain  to  .4.  concolor  collected  on  Isla  San 
Jose,  in  the  Las  Perlas  Archipelago  off  the  Pacific  coast  of  Panama  on  1 September  1944. 
The  description,  measurements  and  date  agree  well  with  the  present  set.  but  as  the  eggs 
had  been  picked  up  by  a worker  who  stated  only  that  they  “had  been  found  in  a low  nest," 
the  identification  clearly  required  corroboration. 

The  eggs  of  .4.  concolor  are  quite  similar  to  those  of  Rallus  spp.  and  Aramides  spp.,  but 
differ  markedly  in  these  respects  from  eggs  of  Loterallus  spp.  or  Porzana  spp.  Behavior  and 
vocalizations  of  Amaurolimnas  also  indicate  close  affinities  with  Aramides,  as  suggested  by 
Olson  (\^  ilson  Bull.  85:381.  1973).  Peters  (Birds  of  the  orld.  Vol.  2.  Harvard  Univ.  Press, 
Cambridge.  Massachusetts,  1934)  had  placed  Amaurolimnas  among  a group  of  probably 
unrelated  Old  \\  orld  genera.  Ripley  (Rails  of  the  orld.  David  Godine,  Boston.  Massachu- 
setts. 1977)  recognized  an  Amaurolimnas-Aramides  relationship  by  lumping  both  into  the 
Old  \\  orld  Eulabeornis.  Relationships  to  Old  W orld  rails  are  beyond  the  scope  of  the  present 
paper,  but  my  observations  definitely  support  the  conclusion  that  Aramides  is  the  closest 
relative  of  Amaurolimnas. 

1 thank  L.  F.  Kiff  for  supplying  weights  and  measurements  of  the  eggs  of  A.  concolor  and 
for  bibliographic  assistance  and  critical  commentary,  k.  C.  Parkes  and  S.  Olson  suggested 
several  improvements  in  the  manuscript  and  S.  M.  Smith  provided  help  in  the  field.  ork 
at  La  Selva  was  supported  financially  by  a Chapman-Naumberg  Postdoctoral  Fellowship 
from  the  American  Museum  of  Natural  History,  and  logistically  by  the  Organization  for 
Tropical  Studies. — F.  G.  Stiles,  Escuela  de  Biologia,  L'niversidad  de  Costa  Rica,  Cuidad 
i niiersitaria.  Costa  Rica.  Central  America.  Accepted  14  Jan.  198U. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


109 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  p.  109 


Trichomoniasis  in  Bald  Eapjles. — The  protozoan  parasite  Trichomonas  gallinae  has 
been  reported  causing  disease  in  the  upper  digestive  tract  of  a variety  of  birds  including  the 
Golden  Eagle  (Aquila  chrysaetos);  the  Rock  Dove  (Columba  livia)  is  its  primary  host  (Levine, 
Protozoan  Parasites,  Burgess  Publishing  Co.,  Minneapolis,  Minnesota,  1973).  This  flagellate, 
in  birds  of  prey,  causes  a disease  called  “frounce,”  characterized  by  yellow  caseous  nodules 
in  the  upper  alimentary  canal  and  emaciation  (Stabler,  Exper.  Parasitol.  3:368-402,  1954). 
This  note  reports  2 cases  of  trichomoniasis  found  in  Bald  Eagles  (Haliaeetus  leucocephalus) 
that  were  subsequently  treated  with  Emtryl  (l,2-dimethyl-5-nitroimidazole.  Dr.  Salshury’s 
Laboratory,  Charles  City,  Iowa).  Stabler  and  Kitzmiller  (N.  Am.  Falcon.  J.  7:47-48,  1967) 
recommend  this  drug  as  a safe  and  effective  treatment  for  trichomoniasis  in  hawks. 

On  25  February  1977,  a debilitated  6.3  kg  adult  Bald  Eagle  was  found  in  Sullivan  County, 
New  York.  The  bird  was  treated  for  shock  and  given  an  antibiotic  by  a veterinarian.  When 
the  senior  author  examined  the  bird  on  27  February  1977,  yellow-brown  caseous  lesions  were 
present  over  most  of  the  surface  of  the  hard  palate,  and  the  saliva  was  blood-tinged.  Nu- 
merous live  trichomonads  were  demonstrated  microscopically  from  the  lesions  and  saliva, 
and  lesion  smears  stained  with  Giemsa  stain  showed  organisms  fitting  the  description  of  T. 
gallinae  (Levine  1973).  The  bird  was  given  orally  three  125  mg  tablets  of  Emtryl.  On  1 March 
1977,  the  mouth  lesions  were  nearly  gone.  However,  rare  trichomonads  were  found  micro- 
scopically from  material  taken  from  the  esophagus  on  4 March  and  3 more  125  mg  tablets 
were  given.  This  was  repeated  on  5 March.  No  ill  effects  were  noted  from  the  medication. 
The  eagle  rapidly  gained  strength  and  showed  greatly  increased  aggression  toward  its  care- 
takers. Further  examination  for  trichomonads  were  negative,  and  the  bird  was  released  to 
the  wild  on  11  March. 

The  second  Bald  Eagle  was  an  immature,  captured  in  a weak,  emaciated  condition  in  a 
farm  field  near  Cutchogue,  Suffolk  Co.,  Long  Island  on  26  July  1978,  after  it  flew  weakly 
against  a slow  moving  pick-up  truck.  This  bird  was  treated  for  shock  and  x-rayed  at  the 
North  Fork  Animal  Hospital,  Southhold,  Long  Island.  No  broken  bones,  or  reason  for  the 
sickness  were  found.  On  28  July  1978,  the  bird  was  transferred  to  the  Delmar  Wildlife 
Resources  Center  at  Delmar,  New  York.  The  eagle  was  thin  (3.6  kg),  weak  and  had  numerous 
yellow-brown  circumscribed  lesions  on  the  hard  palate,  pharynx,  tongue  and  anterior  esoph- 
agus. Microscopic  examination  of  oral  scrapings  revealed  numerous  organisms  typical  of  T. 
gallinae.  Five  hundred  mg  of  Emtryl  were  administered  to  the  bird  on  28  July  by  mouth. 
The  dose  was  repeated  24  h later  as  the  lesions  were  disappearing.  The  eagle  showed  no 
I untoward  effects  from  an  approximate  dose  of  278  mg  of  Emtryl  per  kg  of  body  weight  given 

i!  in  a 24  h period.  The  bird  fed  voraciously  on  venison  and  fish,  and  appeared  stronger.  On 
! 31  July  the  lesions  had  almost  entirely  healed  and  the  bird  was  given  another  250  mg  of 
Emtryl.  No  trichomonads  could  be  found  in  the  mouth  or  esophagus.  The  bird  gained  weight 
i and  vigor,  remained  negative  for  trichomonads  and  was  released  on  10  August  at  Shelter 
I Island,  New  York. 

I The  eagles  may  have  contracted  the  trichomoniasis  from  eating  Rock  Doves  and/or  Mourn- 
l)  ing  Doves  {Zenaida  macroura).  Tangredi  (N.Y.  Fish  and  Game  J.  25:89-90,  1978)  reported 
I trichomoniasis  in  Mourning  Doves  from  Long  Island,  and  many  Rock  Doves  were  seen  near 
( the  capture  site  of  the  eagle  from  Sullivan  County. 

These  seem  to  be  the  first  cases  of  trichomoniasis  reported  in  the  Bald  Eagle.  Emtryl 
q appears  to  be  a very  quick  and  effective  way  to  eliminate  trichomoniasis  in  this  species. — 
I Ward  B.  Stone  and  Peter  E.  Nye,  N.Y.S.  Dept.  Environmental  Conservation,  Delmar 
i*  Wildlife  Resources  Center,  Delmar,  New  York  12054.  Accepted  27  Nov.  1979. 


1 


no 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  p.  110 


Protocalliphora  infestation  in  Broad-winged  Hawks. — In  spring  and  summer  1978, 
during  study  of  the  productivity  of  the  Broad-winged  Hawk  {Buteo  platypterus)  in  Chautauqua 
County,  New  York,  we  discovered  infestation  of  nestlings  by  dipteran  larvae  later  identified 
as  Protocalliphora  avium  Shannon  and  Dobroscky  (Calliphoridae).  This  is  the  first  reported 
occurrence  of  infestation  of  the  Broad-winged  Hawk  by  this  ectoparasite.  Bohm  (Wilson  BuU. 
90:297,  1978)  listed  Protocalliphora  spp.  in  Great  Horned  Owls  (Bubo  virginianus).  Long- 
eared Owls  (Asio  otus).  Red-tailed  Hawks  (Buteo  jamaicensus).  Red-shouldered  Hawks  (Bu- 
teo lineatus)  and  Cooper’s  Hawks  (Accipter  cooperii). 

Larvae  were  first  noticed  and  collected  from  the  ear  cavities  of  nestling  broad-wings  from 
18-26  days  old.  Each  nestling  in  3 broods  of  2 averaged  9 larvae  (range  2-15)  per  pair  of  ear 
cavities.  Three  nestlings  in  an  additional  brood,  killed  by  a predator  at  6-9  days  of  age,  were 
not  infestpd.  The  infested  nests  were  widely  separated,  and  the  nest-sites  were  variable  in 
characteristics. 

The  nestlings  appeared  to  suffer  no  major  deleterious  effects  from  infestation  by  the  blood- 
sucking larvae.  However,  bleeding  was  observed  in  the  ear  cavities,  the  skin  around  the  ear 
openings  was  swollen  and  scabs  sometimes  covered  the  ear  cavities.  No  behavioral  abnor- 
malities were  noticed.  Bohm  (1978)  stated  that  infestation  by  these  dipterans  caused  no 
serious  harm  to  large  species,  but  did  cause  some  mortality  in  small  passerines.  Because 
mortality  is  known  in  small  birds,  and  because  at  least  1 other  ectoparasite  (Mexican  chicken 
hug  [Haematosiphon  indorus])  is  known  to  cause  nestling  mortality  in  raptors  (Platt,  Wilson 
Bull.  87:557,  1975),  Protocalliphora  might  be  expected  to  cause  or  contribute  to  mortality 
of  small  or  undernourished  nestling  Broad-winged  Hawks,  and  potentially  other  raptor  nest- 
lings. The  effects  of  ectoparasites  should  be  looked  for  in  bird  species  showing  brood  re- 
duction strategies  for  growth  and  reproduction  (see  O’Conner,  Living  Bird  16:209-239,  1977) 
in  which  the  young  are  often  undernourished  and  weak,  and  this  includes  raptors. 

We  wish  to  thank  Allen  Benton,  Robert  Bohm  and  especially  C.  W.  Sabrosky  for  their  aid 
in  identifying  the  larvae. — ScoTT  Crocoll  and  James  W.  Parker,  Dept.  Biology  and 
Environmental  Resources  Center,  State  Univ.  Coll.,  Fredonia,  New  York  14063.  Accepted  30 
Oct.  1979. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  110-111 


Herring  (iiill  attacks  an«l  eats  adult  male  Oldsquaw. — Herring  Gulls  (Larus  argen- 
tatus)  have  been  observed  preying  on  a wide  variety  of  small  adult  birds  (Witherby,  Jourdain, 
Ticehurst  and  Tucker,  The  Handbook  of  British  Birds,  Vol.  5,  H.  E.  and  G.  Witherby  Ltd., 
London,  England,  1952:  Harris,  Ibis  107:43-53,  1965).  Such  prey  items  are  almost  always 
small  passerines  or  shorebirds,  and  as  such  are  much  smaller  than  the  guUs  themselves. 
Harris  (1965)  mentions  Herring  Gull  predation  on  Manx  Shearwaters  (Puffinus  puffinus). 
Razorbills  (Alca  torda)  and  Common  Puffins  (Fratercula  arctica),  and  Peter  Fetterolf  (pers. 
comm.)  has  observed  Herring  Gulls  preying  on  juvenile  Ring-billed  Gulls  (L.  delawarensis). 
Few,  if  any,  instances  of  Herring  Gulls  preying  on  birds  larger  than  these  have  been  reported. 
This  note  reports  an  instance  of  predation  on  an  adult  male  Oldsquaw  (Clangula  hyemalis) 
by  an  adult  Herring  Gull.  The  average  weight  of  an  adult  male  Oldsquaw  in  December  is 
about  580  g (Peterson  and  Ellarson,  W ilson  Bull.  91:288-300,  1979).  The  average  weight  of 
an  adult  Herring  Gull  is  1098  ± 151  g,  based  on  a sample  of  15  male  and  11  female  specimens 
at  the  Royal  Ontario  Museum,  Toronto,  Canada. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


111 


Daily  mid-December  populations  of  Oldsquaws  in  the  inner  harbor  at  Toronto,  Ontario, 
average  about  1500  individuals  (Alison,  M.Sc.  thesis,  Univ.  Toronto,  Toronto,  Ontario,  1970). 
On  28  December  1978,  at  10:23,  while  watching  Oldsquaw  behavior  at  about  1000  m from 
the  30  m Toronto  Harbor  Police  observation  tower  with  a 20x  spotting  scope,  I observed  an 
adult  Herring  Gull  attack  an  adult  male  Oldsquaw.  The  gull  dived  at  and  struck  the  duck 
from  a height  of  20-30  m.  Similar  attacks  were  observed  at  11:00,  11:19  and  11:55  and  others 
may  have  occurred.  The  Oldsquaw  was  never  observed  diving  or  flying  and  swam  only  when 
attacked.  At  12:06,  aU  the  Oldsquaws  (>100  individuals)  within  1500  m flocked  and  flew  in 
a tight  50-100  m diameter  circle  above  the  stricken  duck.  The  Herring  Gull,  having  appar- 
ently killed  the  duck,  was  seen  using  its  bill  to  bite  and  pick  at  the  carcass,  apparently 
feeding;  after  about  3 min  the  flock  of  circling  Oldsquaws  began  to  disperse.  The  gull  alter- 
nately swam  within  3 m of  the  Oldsquaw  carcass  or  fed  on  it  until  12:47,  at  which  time  the 
gull  departed.  At  13:01,  either  it  or  another  Herring  Gull  landed  beside  the  carcass  and 
remained  within  3 m of  it  occasionally  feeding  until  14:17,  when  the  gull  departed.  No 
subsequent  visits  to  the  carcass  were  made  by  Herring  Gulls  or  any  other  birds,  and  by  14:40 
the  duck  was  no  longer  visible.  Presumably  it  sank;  Schorger  (Wilson  Bull.  59:151-159,  1947) 
reported  that  Oldsquaws  with  completely  water  saturated  plumage  have  negative  buoyancy. 

I thank  the  Toronto  Harbor  Police  for  permitting  access  to  their  tower.  Comments  and 
criticism  by  P.  M.  Fetterolf,  J.  D.  Rising,  N.  J.  Flood,  G.  R.  Bortolotti  and  J.  C.  Barlow  are 
appreciated. — Richard  R.  Snell,  Dept.  Zoology,  Univ.  Toronto,  Toronto,  Ontario  MSS  lAl 
Canada.  Accepted  30  Jan.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  111-112 


Red-legged  Kittiwak.es  forage  in  mixed-species  flocks  in  southeastern  Alaska. — 

The  foraging  behavior  of  Red-legged  Kittiwakes  {Rissa  brevirostris)  away  from  their  breeding 
sites  is  virtually  unknown.  Between  1-7  September  1978,  we  observed  adult  and  juvenile 
Red-legged  Kittiwakes  foraging  in  a mixed-species  flock  of  adult  Mew  Gulls  [Lams  canus), 
juvenile  Bonaparte’s  Gulls  {L.  Philadelphia)  and  juvenile  Glaucous-winged  GuUs  {L.  glau- 
cescens)  in  the  lower  Green’s  Creek  drainage  on  Admiralty  Island  in  southeastern  Alaska. 

During  ebb  tide  the  exposed  delta  mud  flats  at  Green’s  Creek  are  used  by  thousands  of 
guUs  and  shorebirds,  particularly  during  spring  and  fall  migration.  The  lower  portion  of 
Green’s  Creek  is  used  by  spawning  salmon  during  late  July  through  September:  hundreds 
of  humpbacked  salmon  (Oncorhynchus  gorbusche)  were  present  during  our  observations. 

We  observed  5 mixed-species  flocks  of  about  50  individuals  each  at  the  open  meadow 
bordering  Green’s  Creek,  always  during  ebb  tide;  never  when  the  stream  bank  and  adjacent 
meadow  were  flooded.  The  flock  composition  was  nearly  constant:  Glaucous-winged  Gulls, 
10%;  Mew  Gulls,  25%;  Bonaparte’s  Gulls,  35%;  and  Red-legged  Kittiwakes,  30%  (adults, 
10%;  juveniles,  20%).  (The  identification  of  the  kittiwakes  in  the  flocks  was  difficult  at  first; 
however,  the  juveniles  were  discriminated  from  juvenile  Bonaparte’s  Gulls  by  the  kittiwakes’ 
well-marked,  dark  cervical  collar,  their  dusky  eyes,  and  unbarred  tail.  The  adult  kittiwakes 
differ  from  adult  Mew  Gulls  by  their  solid  black  wing  tips  and  red  legs.)  Glaucous-winged 
Gulls  arrived  first  and  remained  near  the  deep,  still  water  or  the  gravel  shore,  coming  closest 
to  the  forest  edge.  Mew  Gulls  arrived  next,  followed  soon  by  Bonaparte’s  Gulls.  Red-legged 
Kittiwakes  were  the  last  to  arrive,  and  they  frequented  the  faster  stream  riffles  and  stayed 
farthest  from  the  forest  edge.  Mew  and  Bonaparte’s  gulls  always  stayed  between  the  Glau- 
cous-winged Gulls  and  Red-legged  Kittiwakes.  None  of  the  gulls  or  kittiwakes  left  the  stream 
banks,  nor  flew  into  the  forest.  We  observed  no  interactions  between  species. 


112 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Glaucous-winged  and  Mew  gulls  spent  most  of  their  time  sitting  on  the  water  or  the  rocky 
shore.  They  pecked  the  exposed  dorsal  surface  of  the  spawning  salmon,  but  we  could  not 
confirm  feeding.  Bonaparte’s  Gulls  and  Red-legged  Kittiwakes  flew  almost  continuously  and 
dived  frequently.  Bonaparte’s  Gulls  appeared  to  be  “pursuit  diving,”  and  the  kittiwakes, 
“dipping”  (terms  from  Ashmole,  pp.  223-286  in  Avian  Biology,  D.  A.  Earner  and  J.  R. 
King,  eds..  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New  York,  1971).  Close  observation  showed  that 
kittiwakes  generally  dived  between  salmon  and  ate  something  from  the  stream  bottom,  pre- 
sumably salmon  eggs.  On  at  least  2 occasions,  kittiwakes  pecked  at  the  exposed  dorsal 
surface  of  salmon  spawning  in  the  stream.  Although  dead  salmon  were  abundant  on  sand 
banks,  we  never  saw  gulls  or  kittiwakes  eat  any  of  them. 

Adult  and  juvenile  kittiwakes  foraged  similarly.  No  pattern  of  dominance,  aggression,  or 
indication  of  feeding  hierarchy  was  detected.  Juveniles  foraged  next  to,  and  independent  of, 
adults. 

Red-legged  Kittiwakes  are  commonly  found  near  the  Pribilof  Islands  during  the  breeding 
season;  they  are  rare,  post-breeding  visitors  to  the  northeastern  Bering  Sea  and  Aleutian 
Islands.  There  are  a few  accidental  records  in  southeastern  Alaska  and  the  Yukon  River 
(Kessel  and  Gibson,  Stud.  Av.  Biol.  1:48-49,  1978),  but  they  are  usually  seen  at  sea,  if  at  all. 
They  have  only  been  reported  to  feed  on  small  fish  and  cephalopoda  in  the  waters  south  of 
the  Pribilofs  (Hunt,  pp.  196-382  in  Environmental  assessment  of  the  Alaskan  continental 
shelf,  Vol.  2,  Natl.  Ocean.  Atmos.  Admin.  Environ.  Res.  Lab.,  Boulder,  Colorado,  1977). 

Bonaparte’s  and  Mew  gulls  are  common  visitors  to  southeastern  Alaska  in  fall,  and  the 
Glaucous-winged  Gull  breeds  there — it  probably  is  not  unusual  to  find  these  3 gulls  foraging 
together  (Bent,  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  113:65-73,  1921).  Our  observations  are  significant  in 
that  range  extensions  in  both  location  and  time  are  established  for  the  Red-legged  Kittiwake, 
and  the  post-breeding  feeding  habits  in  southeastern  Alaska  are  described  for  the  first  time. 

I'his  study  was  partially  supported  by  VTN,  Inc.  We  thank  V.  Byrd,  G.  Hunt,  K.  Vermeer 
and  the  Editor  for  helpful  comments. — DOUGLA.S  Siegel-CauSEY,  Dept.  Ecology  and  Evo- 
lutionary Biology,  Univ.  Arizona,  Tucson,  Arizona  85721  AND  Thomas  E.  Meehan,  VTN 
Consolidated,  Inc.,  2301  Campus  Dr.,  Irvine,  California  92713.  Accepted  10  Eeb.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  112-114 


(iroiincI-f‘e«Mliiig:  methods  arul  niche  separation  in  thrushes. — Recent  papers  by 
Clark  (Wilson  Bull.  83:66-73,  1971)  and  Henty  (Wilson  Bull.  88:497-499,  1976)  have  de- 
scribed a foraging  method  used  by  a number  of  species  of  birds,  consisting  of  lateral  sweeps 
of  the  bill  to  move  aside  loose  material.  They  termed  it  “bill-sweeping”  and  reported  its 
occurrence  in  several  thrushes  (Turdus),  namely  the  White-necked  Thrush  (Turdus  albicol- 
lis),  European  Blackbird  (T.  merula),  American  Robin  (7\  migratorius)  and  Songthrush  (T. 
philornelos).  1 have  recently  observed  such  behavior,  together  with  other  foraging  methods, 
in  2 additional  species,  the  Fieldfare  (7’.  pilaris)  and  European  Redwing  {T.  iliacus)  and 
incidentally  in  the  European  Blackbird.  The  intention  of  this  note  is  to  relate  bill-sweeping 
to  other  principal  foraging  techniques  used  by  thrushes  when  feeding  on  the  ground,  and  to 
comment  on  niche  separation  in  the  genus. 

Observations  were  made  between  October  1975  and  February  1976  on  wild  birds  in  Cam- 
bridgeshire, England,  using  a hide  or  vehicle  stationed  at  the  edge  of  a field.  Birds  regularly 
approached  to  within  20  m,  and  sometimes  to  within  10  m of  a concealed  observer,  providing 
detailed  views  of  their  searching  and  handling  techniques. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


113 


Table  1 

Relative  Use  of  Food  Categories  by  Fieldfares  and  Redwings 

Fieldfare 

Redwing 

N (%) 

N (%) 

Surface  items  (all  <10  mm  long) 

266  (43.7) 

176  (66.9) 

Soil  items 

342  (56.3) 

87  (33.1) 

Soil  items  >10  mm  long 

154  (45.0) 

19  (21.8) 

The  2 main  species  were  wintering  together  on  areas  of  cattle-grazed  permanent  pasture. 
For  most  of  the  winter  this  was  their  preferred  habitat,  in  which  they  collected  invertebrate 
food.  Foods  taken  comprised  2 major  categories:  (1)  “soil  items,”  including  earthworms, 
centipedes,  slugs  and  larvae  of  beetles  and  craneflies  (Tipulidae),  which  required  extraction 
from  soil  or  grass  tufts;  and  (2)  “surface  items,”  exposed  on  the  soil  or  vegetation,  and 
comprising  mainly  flies,  beetles  and  spiders,  for  which  handling  was  minimal.  This  division 
was  based  on  the  difference  in  technique  required  for  the  capture  of  prey  items  in  each 
category. 

Similar  methods  were  used  by  both  Fieldfares  and  Redwings  when  taking  prey  from  a 
given  category.  In  both  species  searching  for  both  categories  consisted  of  running  along  the 
ground  in  short  bursts,  usually  of  1-5  paces  or  hops,  halting  after  each  run,  and  apparently 
scanning  the  ground  in  the  immediate  vicinity.  If  a potential  food  item  was  spotted,  a few 
paces  might  be  taken  towards  it.  In  the  case  of  a surface  item  the  bird  usually  pecked 
immediately  at  it  (although  there  might  be  a brief  hesitation)  during  which  the  bird  might 
cock  its  head  to  one  side  before  the  peck.  Occasionally,  more  than  1 peck  was  made.  With 
a soil  item,  the  behavior  was  similar  to  the  bill-sweeping  described  by  Clark  (1971),  although 
he  found  bill-sweeping  used  “to  move  aside  twigs,  leaves,  dry  soil,  or  snow”  and  did  not 
mention  use  on  relatively  hard-packed  substrates  such  as  the  damp  soil  with  dense  roots 
found  on  the  present  study  area.  On  hard  substrates  the  behavior  is  more  appropriately 
termed  digging,  as  described  by  Heppner  (Condor  67:247-256,  1967)  for  American  Robins 
hunting  earthworms.  As  in  robins,  a Redwing  or  Fieldfare  would  stop  near  potential  prey, 
hesitate,  often  cocking  the  head  to  one  side,  sometimes  take  a short  step  backwards  or  to 
one  side,  then  stab  downwards.  Often  several  pecks  were  made,  when  the  first  few  did  not 
usually  secure  the  food.  Instead  the  downward  stab  was  followed  by  a head  flick,  often 
causing  soil  to  be  thrown  to  one  side.  The  flick  might  be  more  or  less  pronounced,  and 
occasionally  was  not  lateral. 

The  major  difference  between  the  bill-sweeping  described  by  Clark  (1971)  and  Henty  (1976) 
and  digging  as  described  by  Heppner  (1967)  and  the  present  paper  is  that  the  former  is  a 
search  technique  while  the  latter  is  a “pursuit”  technique  used  after  a potential  prey  has 
been  spotted.  However,  the  two  are  seemingly  related. 

Bill-sweeping  was  also  used  by  Fieldfares  and  Redwings  to  move  aside  loose  material. 
Redwings  occasionally  fed  in  dead  leaves  in  the  manner  described  by  Henty  (1976),  walking 
through  the  litter  with  almost  continuous,  rapid,  lateral  head  movements.  Redwings  used  a 
similar  method  when  searching  cattle  dung  for  dipteran  larvae,  except  the  bird  stood  still  by 
a pat  while  searching.  When  Redwings  foraged  in  cowpats  in  this  way,  the  technique  was 
intermediate  in  action  between  bill-sweeping  and  digging,  and  was  used  for  both  search  and 
“pursuit.”  Fieldfares  searched  litter  much  less  frequently  than  Redwings,  and  were  never 
observed  feeding  at  cowpats. 


114 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


The  2 species  differ  in  size — the  Fieldfare  weighing  120-140  g and  the  Redwing  60-80  g. 
These  differences  were  reflected  in  differences  in  diet.  I recorded  the  numbers  of  surface 
and  soil  items  taken  by  thrushes  in  many  large  flocks  over  the  whole  winter  (Table  1),  In 
order  to  avoid  bias  due  to  observations  containing  different  numbers  of  items  captured  I 
calculated  the  proportion  of  surface  items  taken  during  each  feeding  record.  A Mann-Whit- 
ney  U-test  on  the  difference  of  this  proportion  between  the  species  was  highly  significant 
(P  < 0.001).  The  Redwings  took  more  surface  items  than  soil  items  and  the  Fieldfares  took 
more  soil  items  than  surface. 

Digging  required  more  time  and  energy  than  picking  items  from  the  grass.  However,  soil 
items  were  generally  larger  than  surface  items  (Table  1);  also  Fieldfares  took  larger  soil 
items  than  did  Redwings  (Table  1,  Mann-Whitney  U-test,  P < 0.01). 

Thus,  the  2 species  subdivided  the  habitat  primarily  on  spatial  and  behavioral  differences; 
differences  in  prey  size  were  to  some  extent  consequential  upon  these,  supporting  Hespen- 
heide  (pp.  158-180  in  Ecology  and  Evolution  of  Communities,  M.  L.  Cody  and  J.  M.  Dia- 
mond, eds.,  Belknap  Press,  Harvard,  Cambridge,  Massachusetts,  1975).  However,  prey  size 
differences  were  also  evident  within  a foraging  zone,  indicating  either  that  each  species  had 
prey-size  preferences  or  that  some  undetected  difference  in  foraging  technique  was  involved 
(such  as  depth  of  digging). 

In  late  February  and  March,  when  the  availability  of  larger  insects  on  the  surface  and 
vegetation  increased  (Tye,  unpubl.),  the  Fieldfare  took  proportionately  more  surface  items 
(75.4%)  than  in  winter  (43.7%,  October  to  mid-February)  and  more  closely  resembled  the 
Redwing  in  feeding  techniques,  suggesting  that  prey-size  preferences  may  have  been  im- 
portant in  determining  the  Fieldfare’s  feeding  behavior. 

Litter-feeding  was  most  common  when  the  ground  was  frozen  or  snow-covered.  At  such 
times  most  Fieldfares  left  the  study  area  completely,  and  the  remaining  few  fed  on  small 
clear  patches  of  pasture  or  garden  lawns.  In  contrast.  Redwings  moved  into  hedge-bottoms 
and  gardens  and  switched  to  litter-feeding.  Prey  items  found  in  litter  resembled  surface 
items  of  the  open  pasture,  mostly  small  arthropods  and  slugs.  Large  items  were  uncommon 
in  the  litter  which  may  explain  why  Fieldfares  did  not  often  feed  there. 

The  European  Blackbird  apparently  used  the  same  techniques  as  the  Fieldfare  and  Red- 
wing, if  not  in  the  same  proportions.  The  Blackbird  tended  to  bill-sweep  in  a strict  sense 
more  than  the  Fieldfare,  especially  in  litter,  and  to  dig  more  than  the  Redwing,  and  was 
probably  intermediate  in  its  use  of  soil  and  surface  items  on  pastures.  It  is  also  intermediate 
in  size  (ca  90-120  g). 

Therefore,  when  feeding  on  the  ground,  the  Tardus  species  studied  so  far  all  seem  to  use 
the  same  range  of  related  feeding  techniques.  Within  a habitat  they  achieve  niche  separation 
by  the  differential  use  of  these  techniques,  and  therefore  of  the  foraging  zones  for  which 
each  technique  is  appropriate.  Further  niche  separation  may  also  be  achieved  by  prey-size 
preferences  within  a foraging  zone.  The  behavioral,  spatial  and  prey  size  differences  may  be 
related  to  body  size. 

These  observations  were  made  while  working  for  a Ph.D.,  supervised  by  the  late  Prof.  R. 
K.  Murton  and  Dr.  P.  P.  G.  Bateson,  at  Monks  Wood  Experimental  Station,  and  financed 
by  a Natural  Environment  Research  Council  Studentship.  P.  P.  G.  Bateson,  J.  C.  Barlow, 
J.  P.  Dempster,  I.  Newton  and  N.  J.  Westwood  commented  on  the  manuscript.  N.  J.  West- 
wood  provided  some  bird  weights. — Alan  Tye,  Monks  Wood  Experimental  Station,  Abbots 
Ripton,  Huntingdon  PE  17  2LS,  Cambridgeshire,  England.  (Present  address:  Dept.  Zoology, 
Fourah  Bay  Coll.,  Univ.  Sierra  Leone,  Freetorun,  Sierra  Leone.)  Accepted  20  Feb.  1980. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


115 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  115-118 


American  Coot  distribution  and  migration  in  Colorado. — American  Coots  (Fulica 
americana  americana)  are  widespread  in  North  America,  breeding  primarily  on  fresh  water 
wetlands  and  wintering  on  both  brackish  and  fresh  water  habitats  (Fredrickson,  in  Manage- 
ment of  Migratory  Shore  and  Upland  Game  Birds  in  North  America,  Sanderson,  ed.,  Int. 
Assoc.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Agencies,  Washington,  D.C.,  1977). 

Despite  being  one  of  the  more  common  marsh  birds  and  a game  species,  the  coot  is  often 
overlooked  or  ignored  during  waterfowl  inventories  or  marsh  evaluations.  Current  data  con- 
cerning the  coot  in  Colorado  are  limited.  Cooke  (The  Birds  of  Colorado,  Colo.  Agric.  Exper. 
Stat.  Bull.  37,  1897),  Keyser  (Birds  of  the  Rockies,  McClory  & Co.,  Chicago,  Illinois,  1902), 
Sclater  (A  History  of  the  Birds  of  Colorado,  Witherby  and  Co.,  London,  England,  1912), 
Niedrach  and  Rockwell  (The  Birds  of  Denver  and  Mountain  Parks,  Colo.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist. 
Pop.  Ser.  No.  5,  1939)  and  Bailey  and  Niedrach  (Birds  of  Colorado,  Denver  Mus.  Nat.  Hist. 
Vol.  I,  1965)  summarized  general  information  on  nest  observations,  early  and  late  occur- 
rences and  distribution.  More  recently  Kingery  and  Graul  (Colorado  Bird  Distribution  Lati- 
long  Study,  Colo.  Field  Ornithol.  and  Colo.  Div.  Wildl.,  Denver,  Colorado,  1978)  updated 
distribution  data  and  Lane  and  Holt  (A  Birder’s  Guide  to  Eastern  Colorado,  L & P Press, 
Denver,  Colorado,  1979)  gave  average  arrival  and  departure  dates  for  eastern  Colorado.  We 
initiated  surveys  of  wetlands  in  August  1976  to  further  delimit  distribution,  seasonal  abun- 
dance and  aspects  of  coot  biology  in  Colorado. 

Study  areas  and  methods. — Counts  of  coots  were  conducted  on  4 study  areas  to  document 
spring  and  fall  migration.  Study  areas  were  selected  to  represent  the  major  topographic  areas 
of  Colorado  and  were  located  at  Beebe  Draw  near  LaSalle,  Weld  Co.,  on  the  eastern  plains; 
Lake  John,  near  Walden,  Jackson  Co.,  in  North  Park;  Ice  Pond,  near  Buena  Vista,  Chaffee 
Co.,  in  the  central  mountains;  and  Hog  Lake,  part  of  Brown’s  Park  National  Wildlife  Refuge 
(NWR),  Moffat  Co.,  in  extreme  northwestern  Colorado.  Each  study  area  included  an  emer- 
gent marsh  dominated  by  cattail  {Typha  spp.)  and/or  bulrush  {Scirpus  acutus)  and  areas  of 
open  water. 

Weekly  counts  were  initiated  in  1977  to  document  spring  migration  at  Beebe  Draw  and 
Lake  John.  Ice  Pond  and  Hog  Lake  were  not  visited  until  after  the  peak  of  migration. 
However,  counts  by  Brown’s  Park  National  Wildlife  Refuge  personnel  from  14  March  to  22 
May  1977  were  used  to  indicate  coot  migration  at  Hog  Lake.  In  1978,  regular  visits  to  all 
areas  began  by  mid-March. 

Distribution  and  status  were  determined  from  108  questionnaires  returned  by  field  per- 
sonnel of  the  Colorado  Division  of  Wildlife  and  amateur  ornithologists  and  by  observations 
of  230  wetlands  encountered  while  traveling  between  study  areas  throughout  the  state  from 
August  1976  through  November  1978.  Most  regions  of  the  state  were  visited,  with  the  ex- 
ception of  the  far  eastern  plains,  especially  along  the  South  Platte  and  Arkansas  rivers. 

Date  or  period  of  observation,  dominant  emergent  vegetation,  geographic  location,  coot 
numbers  and  status  (breeding,  resident,  migrant  only,  etc.)  were  recorded  for  each  wetland 
visited.  Vegetation  was  classified  as  either  cattail  and/or  bulrush,  or  other.  Omitted  were 
roadside  ditches  and  wetlands  completely  filled  with  cattail  and/or  bulrush  with  no  open 
water.  “Other”  included  areas  with  sedges  {Carex  spp.),  willows  (Salix  spp.),  or  grasses 
(Graminae)  as  the  dominant  emergents,  or  areas  without  any  emergents,  such  as  many  lakes, 
reservoirs,  and  stockponds.  Geographic  location  classifications  were  eastern  plains,  high 
mountain  valleys  and  west  of  the  Continental  Divide.  High  mountain  valleys  refer  specifically 
to  North,  Middle  and  South  parks  (large  inter-mountain  depressions  devoid  of  extensive 
woodlands)  and  the  San  Luis  Valley.  Coots  were  considered  to  be  breeding  if  territorial 


116 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  1 

Relationship  of  Coot  Breeding  Distribution  and  Vegetation  Type  in  Colorado 


Number  of  areas  surveyed 


Dominated  by  Dominated  by 

Typha  or  Scirpus  other  vegetation 


Area 

With 

breed- 

ing 

coots 

None 
or  non- 
breed- 
ers 

Total 

With 

breed- 

ing 

coots 

None 
or  non- 
breed- 
ers 

Total 

(df  = 1) 

Prob- 

ability 

Statewide 

45 

31 

76 

15 

136 

151 

63.13 

<0.001 

Eastern  plains 

17 

10 

27 

1 

58 

59 

42.02 

<0.001 

High  mountain  valleys 
West  of  Continental 

8 

1 

9 

13 

35 

48 

12.44 

<0.001 

Divide 

20 

20 

40 

1 

43 

44 

25.45 

<0.001 

behavior  (as  defined  by  GuIIion,  Wilson  Bull.  64:83-97,  1952)  and  nests  and/or  young  coots 
were  observed  during  15  April  through  15  August,  and  nonbreeding  if  gregarious  and  non- 
territorial. Coots  observed  from  16  August  through  14  April  were  classed  as  migrating  or 
wintering.  Distribution  and  status  classifications  were  assigned  according  to  the  latilong 
system  (Kingery  and  Graul  1978),  with  the  state  divided  into  27  blocks  based  on  lines  of 
latitude  and  longitude. 

Distribution  and  status. — Coots  bred  throughout  most  of  Colorado.  Breeding  was  not  doc- 
umented in  2 latilong  blocks,  one  located  in  the  southcentral  mountain  region  and  the  other 
in  the  southeastern  plains.  Major  breeding  areas  were  Brown’s  Park  on  the  western  slope, 
and  North  Park  and  the  San  Luis  Valley  in  the  high  mountain  valleys.  Breeding  coots  were 
not  observed  in  Middle  or  South  parks,  although  previously  reported  (Cooke  1897,  Lane  and 
Holt  1979)  and  to  3045  m elev.  at  Kenosha  Pass  (Bailey  and  Niedrach  1965).  On  the  plains, 
coots  were  locally  abundant  where  suitable  habitat  occurred. 

Nesting  coots  were  primarily  associated  with  cattail  and  bulrush  marshes  (Table  1).  A Chi- 
square  test  of  independence  between  the  presence  or  absence  of  breeding  coots  and  the 
occurrence  of  cattail  and/or  bulrush  dominated  marshes  indicated  a strong  relationship  {P  < 
0.001)  in  each  region  and  statewide.  A similar  analysis  for  the  migration-wintering  period 
also  indicated  a strong  relationship  (P  < 0.001)  statewide.  Not  all  cattail  or  bulrush  marshes 
were  used  by  breeding,  migrating  or  wintering  coots,  but  they  were  used  in  strong  preference 
to  other  vegetation  types.  Other  vegetation  types  used  for  nesting  included  tamarix  (Tamarix 
gallica),  spikerush  {Eleocharis  macrostachya),  willows  and  sedges.  Sedges  growing  as  emer- 
gents  were  characteristic  of  many  high  elevation  wetlands  used  by  coots,  especially  wetlands 
in  North  Park. 

Coots  were  resident  in  low  numbers  (<  1000),  mainly  along  the  western  boundary  of  the 
plains  from  near  Port  Collins  south  to  Pueblo.  The  presence  of  wintering  coots  was  dependent 
on  mild  winter  weather.  Coots  were  present  near  Port  Collins  during  the  winter  of  1976-77, 
hut  absent  during  the  1977-78  winter  when  water  areas  froze.  In  the  Port  Collins  area,  coots 
associated  with  wintering  waterfowl  and  fed  on  feces  and  waterfowl  carcasses.  Christmas 
bird  counts  sponsored  by  The  National  Audubon  Society  (1960-1977)  and  listed  in  Au- 
dubon Field  Notes  and  American  Birds  have  consistently  noted  coots  along  the  Front  Range 
from  Fort  Collins  to  Pueblo: 

Spring  migration. — At  Beebe  Draw  and  Hog  Lake  coots  arrived  in  late  February  or  early 


Table  2 

Total  Number  of  Coots  Observed  During  Spring  and  Fall  Migrations  1977  and  1978,  4 Study  Areas,  Colorado 


GENERAL  NOTES 


117 


LO 

00 

On 

lO 

(ON 

o 

LO 

rf* 

ON 

lO 

ON 

CM 

00 

LO 

o 

o 

Tf 

CM 

o 

NO 

CM 

1 

LO 

1 

o 

1 

CO 

LO 

LO 

1 

e- 

1 

00 

1 

CO 

i— ' 

CM 

o 

5- 

O 

00 

CM 

lO 

1 

o 

1 

1— 1 

CN 

LO 

o 

1 

00 

1 

LO 

CM 

oo 

CM 

oo 

o 

rf- 

1 

1 

1 

lO 

1 

f-H 

3 

CO 

On 

1 

1 

1 

1 

•-H 

OO 

OO 

LO 

00 

8 

ON 

LO 

1 

NO 

Tt 

CO 

00 

o 

1 

rf 

NO 

oo 

CM 

NO 

CO 

hH 

§§ 

1 

00 

1 

CM 

i;)  ' 

CO 

NO 

On 

LO 

1 

rf 

1 

00 

CO 

-o 

00 

NO 

OO 

r- 

-a 

c 

c 

-Q 

CO 

aj 

s 

a 

j= 

V 

2 

< 

C 

a 

o 

o 

o> 

s 

00 

00 

NO 

1 

3 

o 

NO 

C/) 

cs 

C^ 

o 

1 

00 

rf 

CM 

00 

CM 

NO 

00 

CM 

CO 

o 

o 

1 

CM 

1 

rf 

oo 

LO 

1 

CM 

1 

CM 

NO 

LO 

CM 

1 

o 

1 

rf 

8 

ON 

o 

00 

1 

1 

00 

CO 

LO 

rf 

00 

e- 

1 

o 

1 

C^ 

1 

e- 

ro 

LO 

1 

1 

1 

o 

CO 

-C 

oo 

- 

o 

3 

tL 

5- 

LO 

LO 

o 

r-H 

o 

ON 

3 

< 

CN 

r— 1 

1 

1 

CO 

o 

e- 

CM 

1 

o 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

lO 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

OO 

CO 

c 

On 

e- 

ON 

e- 

ON 

00 

<On 

r- 

On 

00 

e- 

ON 

ON 

00 

On 

c 

ca 

ca 

c 

c 

<v 

ca 

hJ 

O 

Pi! 

ca 

hJ 

_o 

o 

o 

-J 

Lh 

Q 

o 

Sh 

Q 

o 

_c 

o 

I—! 

o 

~a 

c 

o 

■o 

c 

o 

Id 

o 

-Q 

_Q 

o 

0) 

CP 

au 

OJ 

O 

Pi! 

o 

bC 

bC 

o 

o 

ca 

ca 

o 

o 

O 

CQ 

oa 

-J 

u 

CJ 

X 

X 

o ON  o 'O 
^ r—  NO  ON  o 
cs  ^ 


ON  00  lO 
r-l  O ON 
NO  On 


o o lo  csi 

CM  On  UO  On  O 

LO  CM  CM  ^ CM 

CM  CM 


I I 


CO  NO  CM 
Tj-  C^  O CM 
NO  NO  00  CM 
CM  CM 


CM  ^ NO  lO  i-H 

OO  nO  oo  On  lO 

lO  O CO  OO  CM 

^ CM 


OO  lO  O NO 

CM  O uO  Tf  O 

^ OO  CM  00  Tf 

OO  CM 


I-H  r-  O CM  00  CM 

NO  00  LO  Tf  CM 

I CM  NO  CM  Tt  CM 

CM  ^ 


LO  NO  00 
r-  NO  ^ ^ 
Tf'  CO  CO  CM 
CM 


CM  On  On  LO  NO 
ON  CM 
^ CM  NO  oo 


00  O O lO  ON  o 
NOooONCMNor- 
^ ON  LO  CM 


CO  NO 
ON  oo 


I g I ^ I 


CO 

ON  ON  Li  ^ 

Xh  r— I 

On  On 
^ ^ ^ On  On 

''3  ^ C C 

Q Q -i  ^ 1 ^ 

_Q  _Q  OJ  <D  Ph 

O O P»S 

o t>  o ca 

m cQ  j j 


00 


o 

Oh 

OJ  O 


Hog  Lake  1977  — — 575  273  518  367  402  435  336  301  — 215 

Hog  Lake  1978  — 345  307  368  577  499  652  620  678  549  422  349 


118 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


March.  Peak  numbers  were  recorded  in  1977  and  1978  during  the  second  week  of  April 
(Table  2).  At  Lake  John  coots  arrived  in  1977  and  1978  during  the  second  week  of  April, 
although  coots  were  present  elsewhere  in  North  Park  the  previous  week.  Arrival  date  was 
dependent  on  ice  thaw.  In  both  years  numbers  increased  rapidly  for  2 weeks,  then  decreased 
to  resident  levels  within  3-4  weeks.  Although  coots  arrived  at  Ice  Pond  earlier  than  Lake 
John,  peak  numbers  were  also  recorded  the  third  week  of  April.  The  decrease  of  coot 
numbers  from  peak  to  resident  levels  in  1977  and  1978  was  consistent  with  other  areas. 

Timing  of  migration  and  relative  numbers  observed  on  each  area  were  consistent  between 
years.  The  general  pattern  statewide  was  arrival  in  late  February  or  early  March  (first  week 
of  April  at  Lake  John),  increasing  to  peak  numbers  during  the  second  to  third  week  of  April, 
then  decreasing  to  resident  levels  by  the  second  to  fourth  week  of  May.  Timing  of  peak 
migration  was  similar  for  the  eastern  plains  and  west  of  the  Continental  Divide.  The  peak 
occurred  1 week  later  in  the  high  mountain  valleys.  The  literature  suggests  a similar  pattern 
of  coot  migration  in  Colorado. 

Summer  movements. — Numbers  of  adult  coots  increased  gradually  on  the  study  areas  and/ 
or  peripheral  marshes  starting  in  mid-  to  late  July.  Increases  in  numbers  of  immature  coots 
30  days  of  age  or  older  occurred  in  mid-  to  late  August.  An  influx  of  adult  and  immature 
coots  on  Hog  Lake  in  1977  began  about  2 weeks  earlier  than  the  general  pattern  found  on 
other  areas. 

The  late  summer  buildup  was  influenced  by  local  conditions.  Adult  and  immature  coots 
will  leave  a marsh  as  water  levels  recede  and  it  dries  (Ryder,  Ph.D.  thesis,  Utah  State  Univ., 
Logan,  Utah,  1958).  This  situation  was  particularly  applicable  to  Beebe  Draw  and  Hog  Lake, 
where  breeding  coots  occurred  on  nearby  marshes.  Movements  onto  Hog  Lake  were  in  part 
related  to  pumping  schedules  and  water  levels  on  nearby  marshes.  Water  levels  were  allowed 
to  recede  on  some  marshes  in  Brown’s  Park  NWR  as  the  summer  progressed.  Immature 
coots  crossed  the  Green  River  in  the  direction  of  Hog  Lake  on  at  least  1 occasion. 

Fall  migration. — Peak  numbers  were  recorded  in  1977  on  all  areas  during  late  August  to 
mid-September,  before  the  waterfowl  hunting  season  of  1-14  October  (Table  2).  In  1978, 
peak  numbers  were  observed  5 weeks  later  at  Beebe  Draw,  2 weeks  later  at  Lake  John,  1 
week  later  at  Ice  Pond  and  6 weeks  later  at  Hog  Lake.  Peak  numbers  occurred  before  the 
30  September-13  October  1978  waterfowl  season  at  Ice  Pond  and  during  or  after  the 
hunting  season  for  the  other  areas.  In  November  1977  and  1978,  coot  numbers  decreased  to 
minimal  levels.  No  coots  were  known  to  winter  on  the  study  areas. 

Compared  to  spring,  fall  migration  occurred  over  a more  widespread  period,  with  less 
dramatic  changes  in  numbers  and  with  less  consistency  in  timing.  Successive  migratory 
waves  may  explain  the  fluctuating  numbers  observed  on  some  Colorado  areas. 

Acknowledgments. — Financial  assistance  was  provided  by  the  Accelerated  Research  Pro- 
gram for  Migratory  Shore  and  Upland  Game  Birds  and  the  Colorado  Division  of  Wildlife, 
Federal  Aid  to  Wildlife  Restoration  Project  W-88-R.  We  are  grateful  to  J.  Creasey,  refuge 
manager.  Brown's  Park  National  W ildlife  Refuge,  and  his  staff,  G.  Deutscher  and  J.  Sellers, 
for  their  assistance  throughout  the  field  investigations.  We  thank  the  Beebe  Draw  Gun  Club 
for  access  to  its  property  and  the  Mt.  Princeton  Fishing  Club,  especially  R.  M.  Stabler  for 
access  to  Ice  Pond,  (/ratitude  is  expressed  to  all  who  returned  questionnaires  concerning 
coot  observations  in  Colorado. — Warnek  P.  Goreinzel,  Ronaed  A.  Ryder,  Dept.  Fishery 
and  Wildlife  Biology,  Colorado  State  Univ.,  Fort  Collins,  Colorado  80523  AND  Clait  E. 
Braun,  Colorado  Div.  Wildlife,  Wildlife  Research  Center,  317  West  Prospect,  Fort  Collins, 
Colorado  80526.  (Present  address  WPG:  Extension  Wildlife  and  Sea  Crant,  554  Hutchinson 
Hall,  Univ.  California,  Davis,  California  95616.)  Accepted  26  Jan.  1980. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


119 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  119-121 


Reproductive  rate  and  renesting  of  Red-winged  Blackbirds  in  Minnesota. — Red- 
winged Blackbirds  {Agelaias  phoeniceus)  have  an  economic  impact  upon  the  industry  of  wild 
rice  {Zizania  aquatica)  cultivation  in  northern  Minnesota  (Moulton,  J.  Wildl.  Manage. 
43:747-751,  1979).  Red-wings  nest  in  the  marsh-like,  emergent  vegetation  that  borders  the 
peripheral  drainage  ditches  of  the  wild  rice  paddies.  Very  few  Red-winged  Blackbird  studies 
have  provided  data  on  the  number  of  young  fledged  per  female,  per  territorial  male,  or  per 
unit  area;  or  on  the  extent  and  nature  of  renesting  and  movements  of  females  during  the 
nesting  season  (Dolbeer,  Auk  93:343,  1976).  The  objectives  of  this  study  were:  (1)  to  estimate 
the  size  and  reproductive  success  of  a population  of  Red-winged  Blackbirds  on  a typical 
group  of  wild  rice  paddies;  (2)  to  estimate  fledging  rates  per  male  territory  and  per  nesting 
female;  and  (3)  to  estimate  the  extent  of  renesting  and  movement  by  marked  females  during 
the  nesting  season  in  this  habitat. 

The  study  was  conducted  from  late  April  to  early  August  1977  on  commercial  wild  rice 
paddies  (total  area  53.2  ha)  located  185  km  north  of  Minneapolis,  in  Aitkin,  Minnesota.  Nest 
searching  began  in  early  May  and  continued  through  July.  Active  nests  (1  or  more  eggs  or 
nestlings)  were  checked  daily.  Prior  to  nesting,  some  birds  were  captured  in  large  mist  nets 
and  in  a large,  walk-in  decoy  trap  baited  with  oats  and  live  blackbirds.  Territorial  males 
were  captured  in  wire  traps  that  used  a live,  adult  male  as  a decoy  (Bray  et  al..  West.  Bird 
Bander  50:4-7,  1975).  Nesting  females  (with  nestlings)  were  captured  by  placing  small  pieces 
of  mist  net  around  their  nests.  Each  bird  was  marked  with  a USFWS  band  on  1 leg  and  a 
numbered,  plasticized-nylon  streamer,  secured  around  the  tarsus  with  an  aluminum  grommet 
(Arnold  and  Coon,  Bird-Banding  42:49-50,  1971;  DeHaven,  West.  Bird  Bander  50:48-50, 
1975),  on  the  other  leg.  Birds  were  classed  as  either  second-year  (SY)  or  after-second-year 
(ASY)  on  the  basis  of  plumage.  Females  were  aged  by  color  of  marginal  wing  coverts  (Payne, 
Univ.  California  Publ.  Zool.  90:57,  1969).  An  error  rate  of  from  18  (Dolbeer  1976)  to  20% 
(Payne  1969)  must  be  expected  when  using  this  technique. 

Nesting  chronology  and  success. — A total  of  182  nests  (154  active)  was  located  and  marked 
along  the  9566  m of  paddy  ditches.  Dominant  plant  species  bordering  the  drainage  ditches 
were  broad-leaf  cattail  {Typha  latifolia)  and  narrow-leaf  cattail  {T.  angustifolia),  sedges 
{Carex  spp.),  bulrushes  {Scirpus  spp.),  various  grasses  (Gramineae)  and  some  water  plantain 
{Alisma  spp.)  and  arrowhead  {Sagittaria  spp.).  The  first  eggs  were  laid  on  17  May  and  the 


Table  1 

Nesting  and  Fledging  Rates  for  45  Defined  Male  Territories  and  for  the  Entire 

Study  Area 


Male 

territories 

No.  of 
active  nests 

Min. 

nesting 

females 

Max. 

nesting 

females 

Entire 

study 

area® 

45 

120 

97 

115 

53.2^= 

X nests  or  nesting  females 

— 

2.67*’ 

2.16*’ 

2.56*^ 

2.48“ 

X young  fledged 

2.55 

0.95 

1.2 

1.0 

2.6 

® Undetermined  number  of  territories. 
**  Per  male  territory. 

' Ha. 

Nests  per  ha. 


120 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  2 

Nesting  Parameters  for  36  Individually  Marked  Female  Red-winged  Blackbirds 


Nesting 

females 

Young 

fledged/ 

female 

Females 

renesting 

X days 
from  end 
of  first 
nest  to 
start  of 
next  nest 

Females 
attempting 
second 
brood 
on  area 

Renesting 

females 

that 

switched 

male 

territories 

X 

distance 

(m) 

between 

successive 

nest-sites' 

SY  females 

8 (22)^ 

1.5 

2 (33) 

7 

0 

0 

— 

ASY  females 

28  (78) 

2.04 

4(17) 

10 

1 (6.6) 

3(75) 

169 

Total 

36 

1.93 

6 (20) 

9.6 

1 (5.6) 

3 (50) 

169 

“ Parenthetical  values  are  percent  of  total  within  category. 
” For  the  3 females  that  switched  male  territories. 


last  young  fledged  on  16  July — a nesting  season  of  61  days.  The  median  dates  for  nest  starts 
and  fledging  were  24  May  and  18  June,  respectively.  The  average  clutch-size  was  3.7  eggs 
(mode  4).  A total  of  137  young  was  fledged  from  48  of  154  (31.2%)  active  nests.  Predators, 
primarily  raccoons  (Procyon  lotor),  destroyed  99  nests  (54.4%).  About  80%  of  all  egg  mortality 
and  over  50%  of  all  nestling  mortality  was  due  to  predation. 

Territoriality  of  marked  males. — Most  males  began  territorial  defense  in  late  April.  Of  5 
ASY  males  captured  in  the  walk-in  decoy  trap  and  tagged  in  early  May,  2 later  established 
territories  on  the  area.  Between  12  and  26  May,  30  ASY  males  were  captured  on-territory, 
in  small  decoy  traps,  and  tagged.  Seven  marked  males  abandoned  their  territories  and  were 
not  seen  again.  Table  1 gives  nesting  and  fledging  rates  observed  on  45  male  territories  (25 
marked  and  20  unmarked  males).  About  2.6  young  were  fledged  per  male  territory,  a low 
reproductive  rate  compared  to  most  other  studies  (Dolbeer  1976).  One  ASY  male  abandoned 
its  initial  territory  and  established  a second  territory  1.4  km  away.  The  first  territory  con- 
tained 1 nest  which  was  depredated  with  3 eggs  in  it  on  the  night  of  23-24  May.  The  second 
territory  contained  1 nest  in  which  the  first  egg  was  laid  on  6 June  and  which  fledged  3 young 
on  30  June.  The  nesting  female  on  the  first  territory  was  not  marked  so  it  is  not  known 
whether  or  not  it  moved  with  the  male.  Two  males  in  S\  plumage  successfully  defended 
territories  that  attracted  nesting  females.  One  of  the  S\-male  territories  fledged  3 young. 

Nesting  and  renesting  hy  marked  females. — Prior  to  the  start  of  nesting  in  mid-May,  11 
females  (5  AS\  and  6S\  ) were  captured  in  large  mist  nets  and  marked.  Of  those,  4 remained 
on  the  study  area  and  3 (2  ASY  and  1 SY)  nested,  hut  no  nest  was  located  for  the  other  SY 
female.  Between  2 and  21  June,  33  nesting  females  (26  .\SY  and  7 SY),  with  nestlings,  were 
captured  on  their  nests  and  marked.  Of  the  36  marked  females  that  nested,  8 (22%)  were 
classed  as  SY  birds.  The  36  marked  females  accounted  for  42  nests  (41  active)  on  the  study 
area.  One  SY  female  built  a second  nest  after  the  loss  of  its  first  nest,  but  did  not  lay  a 
second  clutch.  Table  2 gives  values  for  young  fledged  per  marked  female.  Of  30  marked 
females  that  could  have  been  observed  renesting,  6 (20%)  renested  on  the  study  area.  Eighteen 
marked  females  successfully  fledged  young  from  nests  started  on  or  before  3 June.  Only  1 
marked  female  successfully  produced  a second  brood  on  the  study  area.  Three  of  6 females 
that  renested  switched  male  territories,  moving  considerable  distances  in  the  process.  The 
phenomenon  of  territory  switching  by  individually  marked,  renesting,  red-wing  females  was 
also  observed  by  Dolbeer  (1976)  and  Fankhauser  (Bird-Banding  35:120,  1964).  This  study  and 
that  of  Dolbeer  (1976)  suggest  that  this  kind  of  movement  may  be  common.  Renesting  and 
second-nest  values  (Table  2)  represent  minimum  estimates  since  females  that  left  the  study 


GENERAL  NOTES 


121 


area  may  have  renested  elsewhere.  Eour  cases  of  renesting  where  clutch-sizes  were  known 
for  both  initial  and  subsequent  nests  were  observed.  In  all  cases,  the  females  involved  were 
ASY  and  laid  initial  clutches  of  4 eggs.  Only  1 female  laid  4 eggs  in  its  second  clutch;  the 
other  3 females  each  laid  only  3 eggs  in  their  second  clutches. 

Acknowledgments. — I thank  the  C.  K.  Blandin  Foundation  and  Wild  Rice  Growers’  As- 
sociation, Inc.  for  financial  support;  K.  C.  Carr  and  R.  S.  Wetzel  (USFWS)  for  field  support; 
H.  Jacobson  for  permission  to  study  the  rice  paddies;  and  M.  W.  Weller  (University  of 
Minnesota)  for  reviewing  the  manuscript.  This  paper  is  No.  10401,  Scientific  Journal  Series, 
University  of  Minnesota  Agricultural  Experiment  Station,  St.  Paul,  Project  17-92. — Daniel 
W.  Moulton,  Dept.  Entomology,  Fisheries  and  Wildlife,  Univ.  of  Minnesota,  St.  Paul, 
Minnesota  55108.  (Present  address:  70  Edgewood  Dr.,  Central  Valley,  New  York  10917.) 
Accepted  1 Nov.  1979. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  121-124 


Migration  speeds  of  three  waterfowl  species. — To  better  understand  the  physical 
nature  of  bird  migration  and  its  energy  requirements,  it  is  important  to  evaluate  the  speed 
at  which  various  species  fly,  and  how  speed  may  be  affected  by  environmental  factors.  This 
paper  reports  on  ground  and  air  speed  of  migrating  flocks  of  Canada  Geese  (Branta  cana- 
densis interior).  Lesser  Snow  Geese  {Anser  c.  caerulescens)  and  Mallards  (Anas  platyrhyn- 
chos). 

In  1966,  we  began  recording  ground  speeds  of  migrating  waterfowl  in  central  Illinois.  By 
late  1978,  we  had  obtained  160  records,  all  but  3 from  a car  driven  for  1.5-24  km  parallel 
to  the  birds’  flight.  The  3 additional  records  were  obtained  from  mapping  the  course  and 
time  interval  of  migrating  birds  observed  from  a light  aircraft.  Migrating  flocks  of  Canada 
Geese  composed  79%  of  the  records.  Lesser  Snow  Geese  16%  and  Mallards  5%  (Table  1). 

At  the  time  the  waterfowl  were  observed,  the  wind  direction  and  velocity  were  estimated. 
Direction  data  were  more  reliable  than  velocity  estimates,  which  were  based  on  radio  reports 
and  local  clues  (flag,  foliage,  smoke  and  the  like).  Most  flocks  were  between  100  and  365  m 
above  the  ground.  At  those  altitudes,  wind  direction  was  approximately  the  same  as  at  ground 
level,  but  average  wind  velocity  was  probably  higher.  The  wind  force  striking  the  migrating 
flocks  was  vectored  on  the  basis  of  cosine  of  the  angle  of  wind  to  migration  track  X wind 
velocity. 

Table  1 shows  the  ground  speed  of  the  migrants,  the  vectored  air  velocity  assisting  or 
impeding  their  passage,  the  calculated  air  speed  that  resulted  from  the  deletion  of  the  wind 
force  and  the  statistical  significance  of  the  results.  Data  for  the  Canada  Goose  were  separated 
into  fall  and  spring  periods  to  determine  whether  the  stronger  winds  in  the  spring  or  the 
proximity  of  the  wintering  grounds  were  factors  affecting  the  air  speed  of  these  geese. 

A comparison  of  ground  speed  to  the  vectored  wind  speed  shows  that  migrating  Canada 
Geese  adjusted  their  flight  speed  within  certain  constraints  to  compensate  for  wind  velocity. 
Although  the  ground  speeds  of  Canada  and  Snow  geese  flying  into  the  wind  were  reduced, 
their  effort  (as  measured  by  air  speed)  averaged  13.1  km/h;  more  when  they  flew  against  the 
wind  than  when  they  flew  with  it.  The  F value  derived  from  an  analysis  of  variance  dem- 
onstrated a highly  significant  relationship  between  wind  speed  and  the  air  speed  of  Canada 
Geese  (F  = 18.5,  P < 0.01  for  fall  and  20.7,  P < 0.01  for  spring),  but  no  statistically  signif- 
icant difference  in  the  Snow  Goose  (F  = 3.6,  NS).  (The  small  sample  measured  in  the  op- 
posed-wind category  appears  responsible.) 


122 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


Table  1 

The  Effect  of  Vectored  Wind  Velocity  on  the  Ground  Speed  of  Migr.\ting 
Flocks  of  Waterfowl  in  Central  Illinois,  1966-1978 


No. 

of 

flocks 

Ground 
speed  (km/h) 

Wind 

speed  (km/h) 

Corr. 

coeff.  P 

Calcu- 
lated air 
speed 
( km/h) 

Species 

Season 

X ± SD 

X ± 

sd 

Canada  Goose 

fall 

48 

72.7  ± 6.4 

+9.3  ± 

7.6 

0.45  < 0.01 

63.4 

fall 

26 

64.7  ± 9.2 

-12.2  ± 

9.8 

-0.69  < 0.01 

76.9 

spring 

30 

70.3  ± 10.0 

+ 13.0  ± 

10.5 

0.22  < 0.10 

57.3 

spring 

22 

60.7  ± 6.0 

-12.2  ± 

7.6 

-0.39  < 0.10 

72.9 

Lesser  Snow  Goose 

fall 

21 

83.3  ± 5.1 

+ 12.7  ± 

6.6 

0.17  < 0.10 

70.6 

fall 

4 

67.3  ± 24.0 

-13.4  ± 

8.8 

-0.77  < 0.10 

80.7 

Mallard 

fall 

9 

71.8  ± 11.1 

-18.8  ± 

12.2 

-0.78  < 0.01 

90.6 

Within  each  wind  force  category  (Table  1),  linear  regression  demonstrated  change  in 
ground  speed  with  wind  speed.  As  the  tail-wind  force  increased,  migrants  failed  to  corre- 
spondingly increase  their  ground  speed.  The  correlation  coefficient  showed  the  best  linear 
fit  for  the  Canada  Goose  fall  data.  Spring  wind  data  were  more  variable  than  faU  data, 
perhaps  as  a result  of  a greater  frequency  of  strong  gusts  at  that  season.  Fig.  1 shows  the 
effect  of  vectored  wind  velocity  on  the  ground  speed  of  migrating  Canada  Geese  during  the 
fall  and  the  ground  speed  that  might  be  expected  without  adjustment  by  the  geese  for  wind 
force.  The  difference  between  the  actual  and  projected  ground  speeds  suggests  that  as 
favorable  winds  increase,  the  birds  decrease  their  air  speed  and  that  as  head  winds  increase, 
they  fly  faster.  Several  different  plot  tests  showed  that  the  relationship  was  linear  rather 
than  curvilinear. 

The  air  speed  of  individual  flocks  of  Canada  Geese  varied  between  25  and  88  km/h.  The 
instance  of  the  highest  speed  recorded  is  especially  interesting.  At  07:15,  10  March  1977, 
Glen  Sanderson  and  Bellrose  were  driving  north  on  1-57  at  Dix,  Jefferson  Co.,  Illinois,  when 
they  noticed  500  Canada  Geese  in  2 flocks  migrating  due  north  at  an  estimated  altitude  of 
300  m.  The  geese  had  a ground  speed  of  96  km/h  which  they  steadily  maintained  for  the 
next  24  km.  Local  weather  on  the  car  radio  indicated  a south  wind  at  8 km/h.  These  geese 
had  apparently  just  departed  nearby  Rend  Lake,  Jefferson  Co.,  on  a flight  of  about  185  km 
to  a traditional  spring  concentration  point  on  the  llliniois  River  near  Bureau,  Bureau  Co. 

Because  of  this  high  air  speed  at  the  apparent  onset  of  a migratory  flight,  we  thought  that 
spring  air  speeds  might  be  higher  than  those  in  the  fall  because  of  energy  expended  on  the 
longer  fall  flights.  Central  Illinois  is  closer  to  the  winter  grounds  in  southern  Illinois  than  to 
James  Bay  or  even  Horicon  Marsh,  Wisconsin,  points  from  which  fall  flights  through  Illinois 
emanate.  However,  air  speeds  of  migrants  were  4.2  km/h  lower  in  the  spring  than  in  the  fall 
(F  = 4.4,  P < 0.05).  Apparently  distance  of  flight  does  not  appear  to  be  a factor  affecting 
flight  effort. 

With  a following  wind  in  the  fall,  the  air  speed  of  Snow  Geese  was  7.2  km/h  greater  (P  < 
0.01)  than  that  of  Canada  Geese.  The  difference  was  significant  (F  = 12.0,  P < 0.01).  Flying 
against  the  wind  in  the  fall.  Mallard  air  speed  also  averaged  13.7  km/h  faster  than  Canada 
Geese  (F  = 21.4,  P < 0.01). 

There  is  conflicting  evidence  regarding  the  effect  of  wind  speed  on  the  air  speed  of  mi- 
grating birds.  Blokpoel  (Can.  \^  ildl.  Serv.  Rept.  Ser.  28:1-30,  1974)  compared  a small  num- 


1 


GENERAL  NOTES 


123 


Fig.  1.  Linear  regression  of  the  response  of  Canada  Geese  in  ground  speed  to  favorable 
wind  speed  and  adverse  wind  speed.  Dashed  line  indicates  projected  ground  speed  with  no 
change  in  air  speed. 


her  of  air  speeds  with  wind  speed  records  for  migrating  Lesser  Snow  Geese  and  concluded 
that  wind  speed  resulted  in  little,  if  any,  change  in  the  air  speed.  Although  Tucker  and 
Schmidt-Koenig  (Auk  88:97-107,  1971)  found  that  air  speeds  of  local  birds  varied  with  the 
component  of  the  wind  (head,  tail  or  cross),  they  minimized  the  importance  of  energy  con- 
servation resulting  from  a bird  altering  its  air  speed,  stating:  “The  air  speeds  we  measured 
are  too  variable  to  support  the  hypothesis  that  birds  fly  at  closely  regulated  air  speeds  to 
conserve  energy.” 

On  the  other  hand,  Schnell  (Living  Bird  4:79-87,  1965)  measured  the  local  flight  speeds 
of  birds  near  the  ground  by  Doppler  radar  and  found  that  wind  velocity  affected  the  air  speed 
of  most  birds.  Bellrose  (pp.  281-309  in  Proc.  XIV  Int.  Ornithol.  Congr.,  1967)  noted  that  the 
speed  of  bird  migrants  on  radar  was  not  proportional  to  increases  in  wind  speed,  and  sug- 
gested that  migrants  adjust  their  energy  output  in  relation  to  the  degree  of  wind  assistance 
or  resistance.  Bruderer  and  Steidinger  (pp.  223-258  in  Animal  Orientation  and  Navigation, 
S.  R.  Galler  et  ah,  eds.,  SP-262,  U.S.  Gov't.  Print  Off.,  Washington,  D.C.,  1972)  used  radar 
to  ascertain  that  ground  speeds  of  Chaffinches  {Fringilla  coelebs)  in  migration  did  not  parallel 
increases  in  wind  velocity;  their  decrease  in  air  speed  equalled  about  one-third  the  increase 
in  wind  force.  In  a further  study  of  the  flight  speeds  of  12  species  of  gulls  (Lams  sp.),  terns 
(Sterna  sp.)  and  skimmers  (Rynchops  sp.)  near  their  nesting  colonies,  Schnell  and  Hellack 
(Am.  Nat.  113:53-66,  1979)  obtained  additional  evidence  that  bird  air  speeds  usually  varied 
inversely  to  wind  speeds.  Additional  work  by  Tucker  (pp.  298-333  in  Avian  Energetics,  R. 
A.  Paynter,  Jr.,  ed.,  Publ.  Nutt.  Ornithol.  Club  No.  15,  1974)  apparently  led  him  to  modify 
his  views  on  the  energy  expended  in  flight:  “At  both  sea  level  and  altitudes  of  6000  m,  it  is 
beneficial  from  an  energetic  point  of  view  to  fly  faster  into  a head  wind,  and  slower  with  a 
tail  wind  than  is  the  case  in  still  air.” 


124 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  \o.  1,  March  1981 


Varying  results  obtained  when  comparing  air  speed  of  birds  with  wind  speed  may  be  a 
function  of  the  wide  range  of  air  speeds  that  birds  can  use  without  the  undue  loss  of  energy. 
Schnell  and  Hellack  (1979)  concluded  that  . . air  speeds  can  be  increased  or  decreased 
considerably  with  only  a relatively  small  increase  in  metabolic  rate  or  cost  of  transport."  As 
shown  by  Greenewalt  (Trans.  Am.  Philos.  Soc.  65:1-67,  1975)  cost  of  transport  curves  are 
relatively  flat  near  their  minima. — Frank  C.  Bellrose  and  Robert  C.  Crompton,  Illinois 
.\atural  History  Survey,  Havana,  Illinois  62644.  Accepted  19  Dec.  1979. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  125-134 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


Two  diverftent  reviews  of  the  followingj  title,  Nomina  Anatomica  Avium:  an  An- 
notated Anatomical  Dictionary  of  Birds,  have  been  received  by  the  Editor.  Both 
are  published  for  the  benefit  of  the  readers. 


Nomina  An.atomica  Avium:  an  Annotated  Anatomical  Dictionary  of  Birds.  By  J. 
J.  Baumel,  A.  S.  King,  A.  M.  Lucas,  J.  E.  Breazile  and  H.  E.  Evans,  (eds.),  with  R.  L.  Zusi 
(consultant  for  taxonomy)  and  L.  Malinovsky  (consultant  for  classical  languages).  Academic 
Press,  London  and  New  York,  1979;xxv  + 637  pp.  $64.50. — The  study  of  avian  anatomy  is 
pursued  by  workers  in  a variety  of  fields  including  systematics,  neurobiology,  veterinary 
medicine,  physiology,  poultry  science  and  others.  Communication  between  workers  in  dif- 
ferent areas,  and  even  in  the  same  area,  has  long  been  hampered  by  the  lack  of  a universal 
system  of  names  for  the  parts  of  the  avian  body.  This  problem  has  now  been  remedied  by 
the  publication  of  the  Nomina  Anatomica  Avium  (NAA)  after  a decade  of  work  by  the  ap- 
proximately 80  scientists  constituting  the  International  Committee  on  Avian  Anatomical  No- 
menclature. The  purpose  of  NAA  is  to  provide  a list  of  terms  for  the  parts  of  the  avian  body, 
and  thereby  to  advance  the  anatomical  study  of  birds.  Subcommittees  dealing  with  the 
different  body  systems  attempted  to  minimize  changes  in  well-established  terms  while  pro- 
viding for  each  structure  a single  term  that  is  short,  easy  to  remember  and  informative. 
Topographically  related  structures  are  given  similar  names,  and  eponyms  are  avoided.  The 
nomenclature  is  in  Latin  for  the  sake  of  international  communication. 

The  book  opens  with  a brief  history  of  the  project  and  information  on  the  use  of  the  NAA, 
followed  by  a series  of  chapters  dealing  with  the  individual  organ  systems.  Few  readers  will 
make  use  of  the  whole  book,  but  workers  who  study  avian  anatomy  for  any  reason  should 
find  one  or  more  chapters  dealing  with  organ  systems  relevant  to  their  investigations,  and 
which  will  provide  them  with  a standardized  terminology  for  use  in  publication  and  other 
technical  communications.  The  book  is  much  more  than  just  a collection  of  terms,  however. 
It  is  extensively  illustrated  with  labeled  drawings  of  various  anatomical  structures,  some  of 
them  taken  from  published  research  reports,  but  many  of  them  newly  drawn  for  this  volume. 
The  lists  of  terms  are  heavily  annotated  with  explanations  of  the  reasons  for  the  choice  of 
terms,  homologies,  synonyms  and  variations  in  different  groups  of  birds.  There  is  an  exten- 
sive list  of  references  establishing  the  authority  for  the  decisions  made  in  the  choice  of  terms, 
and  a lengthy  index  to  the  terms  themselves  and  their  major  synonyms.  The  content  of  the 
book  is  accurately  described  by  its  subtitle. 

The  ultimate  success  of  this  venture  will  depend  upon  the  extent  to  which  the  NAA 
nomenclature  is  adopted  by  researchers  and  writers.  If  the  ideal  of  a unified  nomenclature 
is  to  be  achieved,  it  will  be  necessary  for  virtually  all  workers  to  use  the  system  in  their 
publications,  even  when  it  differs  significantly  from  that  to  which  they  are  accustomed.  This 
may  pose  some  temporary  inconvenience  but  should  be  beneficial  in  the  long  run.  Most 
investigators  will  probably  find  that  the  new  nomenclature  is  not  radically  different  from 
older  ones  because  changes  were  not  made  for  the  sake  of  novelty  but  only  to  introduce 
clarity  and  eliminate  confusion.  Nevertheless,  there  will  be  some  workers  who  will  find 
themselves  unwilling  to  adopt  the  NAA  nomenclature.  In  such  cases  it  would  be  useful  to 
provide  a table  correlating  the  terms  used  with  their  NAA  counterparts.  If  the  reason  for 
reluctance  to  adopt  NAA  terms  is  substantive  then  I recommend  strongly  that  the  individual 
communicate  with  the  appropriate  subcommittee  chairman  (listed  in  the  book)  and  explain 
the  objections  to  the  NAA  terms.  If  the  reasons  for  rejection  are  sufficiently  compelling,  it 


125 


126 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


may  be  possible  to  change  them  in  a future  edition  of  the  NAA.  Workers  whose  investigations 
reveal  new  variations  in  anatomical  structure,  or  who  discover  that  the  current  terminology 
is  based  on  incorrect  or  inadequate  information  should  also  communicate  their  discoveries 
by  sending  reprints  or  comments  to  the  appropriate  subcommittee  chairman.  The  first  edition 
of  NAA  is  not  intended  to  be  the  final  word  on  avian  anatomical  nomenclature.  It  is  intended 
that  revised  editions  will  be  prepared  in  future  years  so  that  the  work  will  increase  in 
effectiveness  as  a basis  for  communication  among  avian  anatomists. — Robert  J.  Raikow. 


Nomina  Anatomica  Avium:  an  Annotated  Anatomical  Dictionary  of  Birds.  A sec- 
ond review. — The  Nomina  Anatomica  Avium  (NAA)  is  an  ambitious  work,  necessitated  by 
the  nomenclaturally  confusing  mosaic  of  over-  and  under-represented  areas  confronting  avian 
morphologists.  Eighty  contributors  labored  for  more  than  a decade  to  produce  the  NAA, 
taking  as  their  objectives  the  “promotion  of  international  communication  by  establishing  an 
agreed  list  of  terms  in  a universally  acceptable  language’"  and  “the  advancement  of  anatom- 
ical knowledge  of  birds.”  If  these  intentions  had  been  met  the  NAA  would  indeed  be  a pearl 
beyond  price  and  justify  the  claim  on  the  flyleaf  that  “it  is  an  essential  work  of  reference  for 
all  avian  scientists  and  every  zoological  library.'"  Unfortunately,  neither  objective  has  been 
completely  fulfilled  and  while  many  sections  are  excellent,  the  resultant  hybrid  may  be 
problematical  enough  to  dissuade  ornithologists  from  perusing  subsequent  editions  or  follow- 
ing current  recommendations. 

There  is  no  question  that  communication  is  enhanced  by  nomenclatural  stability,  so  any 
work,  such  as  the  NAA,  that  attempts  to  provide  the  framework  for  standardization  is  to  be 
commended.  However,  if  stability  is  to  be  maintained  in  derivative  studies,  it  is  imperative 
that  the  syntax  of  the  terminology  be  intelligible,  and  this  is  a facet  of  standardization  for 
which  little  provision  has  been  made  in  the  NAA.  In  common  with  other  anatomical  reference 
works  (e.g.,  Nomina  Anatomica  Veterinaria,  Jena  Nomina  Anatomica,  etc.),  Latin  was  the 
nomenclatural  source  chosen  by  the  International  Committee  on  Avian  Anatomical  Nomen- 
clature (ICAAN)  for  the  NAA. 

The  fact  that  most  ornithologists  (including  anatomists,  who  tend  to  work  in  their  own 
vernacular)  are  unfamiliar  with  the  language  was  recognized  in  the  introduction  to  the  NAA, 
where  it  was  suggested  nevertheless  that  “the  Nomina  should  not  be  neglected  simply  be- 
cause of  the  unfamiliarity  of  Latin.  It  bears  repeating  that  the  official  Latin  terms  should  be 
used  in  scientific  articles  and  books  in  order  to  enhance  international  scientific  communi- 
cation." It  may  also  reasonably  be  argued  that  many  researchers  are  already  familiar  with 
the  vernacularized  Latin  of  textbooks  and  that  attempts  have  been  made  in  the  NAA  to 
provide  some  Latin  equivalents  at  least  suggestive  of  the  terms  already  used  in  the  Romance 
languages  and  English.  However,  textbook  Latin  is  hardly  adequate  preparation  for  the 
nomenclature  of  the  N AA,  where  nouns  and  associated  adjectives  mostly  exhibit  the  number, 
gender  and  case  of  the  unadulterated  forms.  To  ornithologists  with  little  or  no  formal  knowl- 
edge of  the  language,  the  rules  which  distinguish  Os  palatinum  (palatine  bone).  Processus 
palatinus  (palatine  process)  and  Facies  articularis  palatina  (surface  which  articulates  with 
the  palatine)  are  not  clear.  In  addition,  those  unfamiliar  with  the  manner  in  which  word 
stems  affect  the  declension  of  a noun  would  he  hard  pressed  to  recreate  the  procedure  which 
derives  a nominative  plural  of  tractus  from  Tractus  but  musculi  from  Musculus.  As  the  list 
of  terms  given  in  the  NAA  is  deliberately  not  exhaustive,  the  potential  for  a new  era  of 
nomenclatural  confusion  in  subsequent  studies  is  obvious.  The  remedy  for  this  difficulty  is 
simple  enough,  and  it  is  hard  to  rationalize  the  absence  (in  a work  already  comprising  637 
pages)  of  a short  appendix  which  comprehensively  lists  plural  and  adjectival  forms  and  which 
explains  the  syntax  of  the  present  terminology  clearly  enough  to  serve  as  a guide  for  future 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


127 


use.  Criticism  of  the  NAA  for  failure  to  provide  a Latin  grammar,  however  brief,  should  not 
be  dismissed  as  yet  another  example  of  pedantic  nit-picking.  Haphazard  or  arbitrary  ‘Latin- 
ization’  of  terms  by  ornithologists  anxious  to  comply  with  NAA  principles,  but  unable  to 
locate  appropriate  sources  or  to  deduce  suitable  terms  from  questionable  analogues  will 
promote  neither  communication  nor  standardization. 

The  second  intent,  that  of  advancing  anatomical  knowledge  of  birds,  suffers  from  the  same 
theme  of  critical  omission  as  the  first  objective.  The  NAA  is  subtitled  ‘an  annotated  anatom- 
ical dictionary  of  birds’  giving  the  misleading  impression  that  the  annotations  are  supple- 
mental to  the  regular  dictionary  format  of  comprehensive  definition.  Although  there  is  some 
variation  (for  example,  the  chapter  on  the  respiratory  system  is  comprehensively  annotated 
and  illustrated),  in  general  the  list  of  terms  which  prefaces  each  chapter  is  incompletely 
annotated,  and  even  fewer  structures  are  figured.  As  a result,  many  features  are  both  un- 
described and  unillustrated.  The  practice  of  differential  annotation  is  easily  justified  when 
a system  of  nomenclature  has  stabilized  sufficiently  that  some  terms  require  no  further 
definition.  It  is  an  inappropriate  principle  for  any  study  that  attempts  to  be  the  definitive 
work  within  a discipline,  especially  if  the  raison  d’etre  for  that  study  has  been  long-standing 
nomenclatural  confusion.  Non-morphologically  oriented  ornithologists  seeking,  for  example, 
to  extend  their  knowledge  of  the  oral  cavity  will  find  the  listing  of  terms  such  as  Radix  linguae 
or  Fenulum  lingualis  less  than  informative  in  the  absence  of  further  description.  The  dic- 
tionaries (Donath  and  Crawford  1969,  Kenneth  1966)  cited  in  the  introduction  as  useful  in 
the  development  (in  the  sense  of  etymology,  not  syntax)  of  Latin  terms,  are  limited  in  the 
extent  to  which  they  may  be  used  to  supplement  the  NAA.  Many  terms  (e.g.,  Ovogonium, 
Polocytus  secundarius)  undescribed  and  unfigured  in  the  NAA  are  similarly  absent  in  the 
references  mentioned.  In  addition,  the  dictionary  by  Donath  and  Crawford  is  pertinent  to 
human,  not  avian,  anatomy  and  there  are  often  radical  differences  in  the  meaning  of  similar 
terms.  For  example,  the  principal  aortic  vessel  in  birds  is  derived  from  the  right  member  of 
the  embryonic  fourth  pair  of  aortic  arches  while  the  principal  vessel  in  mammals  is  the  left 
member  of  the  fourth  pair.  Extreme  caution  is  therefore  required  in  extrapolating  explana- 
tions from  other  dictionaries  to  undefined  terms  of  the  NAA. 

Differential  annotation  is  equally  problematical  for  avian  anatomists  familiar  only  with  the 
vernacular  terms  of  their  specialties.  As  the  index  is  exclusively  in  Latin  and  as  vernacular 
synonyms  are  generally  (though  again,  variably)  lacking  in  the  text,  a dubious  combination 
of  translation  and  elimination  is  required  to  match  undescribed  Latin  terms  with  their  ver- 
nacular equivalents.  Anatomical  knowledge  of  birds  seems  hardly  likely  to  be  advanced  while 
ornithologists  are  unable  to  readily  locate  or  identify  features  of  interest.  Exhaustive  anno- 
tation and  a vernacular  index  would  do  much  to  eliminate  these  problems. 

Quite  apart  from  the  difficulties  described  above,  the  NAA  contains  a number  of  incon- 
i sistencies,  errors  and  ambiguities,  illustrated  by  the  following  examples.  In  the  chapter  on 
I muscles,  the  term  ‘Pars’  (to  denote  a distinct  and  consistent  subdivision  of  a muscle)  is 
^ capitalized  throughout  the  list  of  terms,  whereas  the  lower  case  is  used  throughout  the 

I annotations;  in  the  list  of  terms  the  muscles  of  the  jaw  are  referred  to  Figs.  2 and  3,  which 

A in  fact  illustrate  the  muscles  of  the  hyoid  apparatus  and  tongue,  and  Musculus  pectoralis  is 

^ used  both  to  indicate  an  entire  muscle  (said  to  comprise  3 parts:  pars  subcutanea  thoracica, 
I pars  subcutanea  abdominalis  and  pars  propatagialis)  and,  in  Figs.  5 and  6,  a prominent, 

I previously  unidentified,  subdivision  (?  M.  pectoralis  pars  thoracicus,  sensu  George  and  Ber- 

I ger  1966)  ventrocaudal  to  the  M.  pectoralis  pars  propatagialis.  Other  examples  abound:  the 
k Lamina  parabasisphenoidalis  (p.  90,  annotation  98)  was  previously  referred  to  as  the  Lamina 
i basiparasphenoidalis  (p.  89,  annotation  96)  while  the  Tuba  pharyngotympanica  communis 
Ij  (p.  89,  annotation  94)  is  shortened  to  Tuba  pharyngotympanica  on  p.  90  (annotation  98);  the 
ik  Canalis  olfactorius  and  Palatum  are  not  on  the  pages  (108  and  282,  respectively)  cited  in  the 


1 


128 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


index;  the  Rostrum  sphenoidale  is  missing  from  Fig.  I of  the  osteology  section,  in  contrast 
to  the  reference  given  in  the  list  of  terms,  and  although  Musculus  subscapularis  is  described, 
no  corresponding  description  is  given  of  Musculus  subcoracoideus.  The  list  could  be  ex- 
tended, but  the  examples  given  are  sufficient  to  demonstrate  that  the  NAA  could  have 
benefited  from  more  careful  proof-reading. 

Despite  these  difficulties,  there  is  valuable  information  to  be  gleaned  from  the  NAA  by 
those  ornithologists  willing  to  brave  the  problems  imposed  by  the  format.  The  notes  appended 
to  the  chapter  on  the  respiratory  system,  for  example,  are  models  of  informed  comment 
unimpeded  by  jargon,  while  critical  analysis  of  the  literature  in  the  light  of  new  (often  pre- 
viously unpublished)  information  elevates  many  other  annotations  from  the  realm  of  simple 
description.  Many  of  the  illustrations  throughout  the  book  are  superb,  although  it  must  be 
admitted  that  others  (e.g..  Fig.  1)  seem  more  likely  to  amuse  than  enlighten.  Finally,  more 
than  900  citations  in  the  bibliography  ensure  adequate  extension  of  the  anatomical  foundation 
laid  by  the  text,  regardless  of  the  area  of  specialty.  Given  that  the  problems  previously 
described  can  be  resolved,  the  excellence  of  much  of  the  text  and  many  of  the  figures 
indicates  the  potential  of  subsequent  editions  of  the  NAA  for  meeting  the  objectives  of  the 
current  contributors.  For  the  present,  let  the  buyer  beware. — R.  JOHNSON. 


Donath,  T.  and  G.  N.  C.  Crawford.  1969.  Anatomical  dictionary  with  nomenclatures 
and  explanatory  notes.  1st  English  Edition.  Pergamon  Press,  London,  England. 
George,  J.  C.  and  A.  J.  Berger.  1966.  Avian  Myology.  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New 
York. 

Kenneth,  J.  H.  1966.  Henderson’s  dictionary  of  biological  terms.  8th  Edition.  Van  Nos- 
trand, New  York,  New  York. 


British  Birds — A Field  Guide.  By  Alan  J.  Richards.  David  & Charles  Ltd.,  North 
Pomfret,  Vermont,  1979:192pp.,  186  color  photographs,  186  line  drawings.  $14.00.  — This 
is  an  attractive  hook  in  many  ways.  Well  produced,  with  good  illustrations,  clear  type  on 
good  paper,  and  a sturdy  binding,  it  is  a pleasure  to  thumb  through.  It  contains  species 
accounts  of  186  British  birds,  one  to  a page,  each  with  a color  photograph  and  a line  drawing. 
I'he  photographs  are  of  excellent  quality  and  accurately  reproduced;  the  adequate,  if  not 
inspiring  drawings  by  Rob  Hume  supplement  the  photographs  by  showing  the  birds  in  flight 
or  in  different  plumages  or  poses.  The  species  accounts  are  divided  into  five  sections:  char- 
acteristics, voice,  habitat,  nest  and  status.  Under  “characteristics"  are  included  a descrip- 
tion, often  rather  abbreviated,  and  notes  on  locomotion,  behavior,  food,  roosting  and  other 
points  of  general  interest.  Under  “status  " are  included  population  figures  for  each  species, 
taken  from  Sharrock's  Atlas  of  Breeding  Birds  in  Britain  and  Ireland  (1976).  This  is  a useful 
feature  for  the  layman  who  may  not  have  access  to  the  Atlas.  Some  species  accounts  have 
a sixth  section,  “similar  or  allied  species,"  where  some  of  the  more  common  British  birds 
not  among  the  favored  186  are  briefly  described. 

As  far  as  it  goes,  this  is  a worthwhile  book.  But  how  far  does  it  go?  For  an  ornithologist 
or  a keen  birder,  not  nearly  far  enough.  The  British  list  contains  ca.  475  species,  of  which 
some  270-290  occur  every  year.  A great  many  have  been  left  out  of  this  hook,  thus  rendering 
it  useless  to  the  person  who  wants  to  identify  every  bird  he  sees.  According  to  the  very 
skimpy  introduction,  the  hook  is  aimed  at  “those  thousands  of  people  interested  in  the  birds 
in  their  gardens  and  local  parks,  in  the  countryside  or  on  the  moors  ..."  etc.;  in  other 
words,  the  armchair  or  junior-intermediate  birder  who  has  perhaps  graduated  from  the  bunny 
slopes  hut  is  not  yet  ready  for  the  advanced  runs.  The  186  species  treated  are  described  as 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


129 


“those  which  might  be  seen  in  Britain  without  too  much  effort  over  the  course  of  a year,” 
although  the  choice  sometimes  favors  distinctive  or  photogenic  species  over  those  less  well 
endowed.  The  drab  Rock  Pipit  (50,000  breeding  pairs)  only  appears  under  “similar  or  allied 
species,”  whereas  the  Dotterel  (100  pairs).  Red  Kite  (30  pairs)  and  Osprey  (several  pairs)  are 
accorded  full  treatment. 

In  spite  of  its  title,  this  is  only  a partial  field  guide  to  British  birds,  and  it  will  certainly  not 
replace  any  of  the  existing  works.  It  is  a book  for  the  educated  layman.  My  advice  to  readers 
of  The  Wilson  Bulletin  is:  if  someone  gives  it  to  you  for  Christmas,  accept  and  enjoy;  hut 
don’t  spend  hard-earned  money  adding  it  to  your  library. — Stuart  Keith. 


Hawks  and  Owls  of  North  America.  By  Donald  S.  Heintzelman.  Universe  Books,  New 
York,  New  York,  1979:197  pp.,  68  photographs  (8  color),  4 figs.,  1 table.  $18.50. — While  the 
title  of  this  book  recalls  the  excellent  but  out-of-print  classic  Hawks  of  North  America  by 
Dr.  John  B.  May,  and  its  successor  North  American  Birds  of  Prey  by  Alexander  Sprunt,  Jr., 
the  present  book  is  less  a systematic  treatment  than  a more  popular  book  addressed,  in  the 
author’s  words,  “to  raptor  enthusiasts  at  less  than  the  professional  level.”  Its  chief  feature 
is  its  display  of  photographs,  some  in  color,  of  most  of  our  raptors.  The  informal  text  of 
discursive,  rather  than  ordered  systematic  accounts,  is  a vehicle  to  show  more  of  the  rapidly- 
growing  files  of  hawk  photographs.  Many  of  these  are  excellent  (but  that  of  a disheveled 
captive  White  Gyrfalcon  is  not).  Some  half  of  these  photographs  are  by  the  author,  but  the 
rest  are  by  many  different  photographers.  The  author  acknowledges  his  debt  to  many 
sources,  but  for  the  most  part  there  is  little  to  indicate  sources  of  information.  (Who  supplied 
the  information  about  the  subspecies  of  Black  Hawk  which  lives  “in  southern  Florida”?) 

There  are  separate  chapters  on  vultures,  kites,  accipiters,  soaring  hawks,  etc.,  and  the 
individual  species  are  afforded  a variable  selection  of  information  and  comment,  but  I find 
little  here  to  commend  it  to  other  than  the  general  reader.  The  species  accounts  are  followed 
by  chapters  on  matters  of  current  interest,  such  as  endangered  species,  habitat  loss,  ecology 
(an  over-worked  term  in  this  text),  migration,  conservation  and  chemical  pollution. 

At  recent  meetings  of  Raptor  Research,  the  number  of  attending  raptor  enthusiasts  doubled 
that  of  A.O.U.  and  Wilson  Society  meetings.  Many  of  these  have  seemingly  little  other 
contact  with  wildlife  biology,  but  my  acquaintance  with  them  leads  me  to  believe  they  are 
more  sophisticated  than  the  audience  to  which  this  book  is  addressed.  It  seems  unfortunate 
that  we  in  North  America  do  not  have  a volume  in  any  way  comparable  to  Leslie  Brown’s 
superb  British  Birds  of  Prey. — Walter  R.  Spofford. 


The  Peregrine  Falcon  in  Greenland:  Observing  an  Endangered  Species.  By  James 
T.  Harris.  University  of  Missouri  Press,  Columbia  and  London,  1979:255  pp.,  39  plates,  1 
map.  $15.95. — This  is  neither  a scientific  treatise  nor  the  definitive  monograph  on  Green- 
land’s Peregrine  Falcons  {Falco  peregrinus).  A “narrative  of  a summer’s  research  in  Green- 
land,” it  is  much  more  than  an  ordinary  diary.  Harris  has  written  an  unusual  and  fascinating 
book  that  should  be  a must  for  all  collectors  of  good  bird  books. 

The  title  and  subtitle  should  be  reversed,  as  the  book  deals  more  with  the  adventure  of 
observing  falcon  cliffs,  their  birds  and  their  weather  than  with  the  Peregrine  Falcon  in 
Greenland.  Thus,  at  first  glance,  I was  disappointed:  I expected  graphs,  tables  and  statistics. 
Many  of  these  data  are  included,  woven  skillfully  into  the  personal  narrative  of  the  Greenland 


130 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


experience  of  1972.  It  is  difficult,  however,  to  find  them  quickly:  the  index  is  short  and 
emphasizes  the  work  of  other  authors  rather  than  Harris’  own  observations.  Apart  from 
these  slight  disappointments  for  the  raptor  expert  there  is  much  to  admire  in  this  handsome 
book,  and  we  must  congratulate  the  publisher  for  an  excellent  and  esthetic  production.  The 
cover  design  (by  Jerry  Dadds),  the  selection  of  black-and-white  photographs  and  even  the 
italic  print  of  the  whole  book  enhance  the  author’s  objectives  and  style.  Harris  attempts  to 
blend  three  different  stories  together:  the  search  for  falcon  eyries  (partly  a travelogue  and 
diary),  the  observations  of  falcon  activities  and  behaviors  at  one  eyrie  (the  proper  topic  of 
the  book)  and  “the  development  of  my  feelings  toward  the  species  and  the  individual  falcon” 
(the  most  difficult  part  to  write). 

The  book  serves  as  an  excellent  introduction  to  the  tundra  ecosystem  and  to  the  care  and 
preparation  that  must  go  into  such  a wilderness  study.  Thus,  I recommend  it  highly  as 
background  reading  for  anyone  bound  for  the  Arctic.  Songbirds,  mammals  and  other  raptors 
are  not  only  described  but  also  followed  through  the  seasonal  cycle  as  long  as  they  occur 
within  the  “falconscape.” 

The  peregrine  story  is  two-fold.  The  author  makes  an  admirable  attempt  to  provide  ex- 
tensive background  notes  on  the  species.  He  begins  by  recounting  the  general  population 
decline  and  its  inter-relationship  with  the  use  of  pesticides,  then  introduces  the  species  and 
subspecies  concepts,  and  talks  about  distribution,  population  surveys  and  other  topics.  Fi- 
nally, we  learn  about  captive  breeding  at  Cornell  up  to  1977,  and  the  latest  breeding  data  for 
Greenland.  These  sections  come  somewhat  abruptly  and  distract  from  the  ongoing  narrative. 
Yet,  for  most  readers,  Harris  has  written  an  excellent  summary  of  the  complex  peregrine 
problem. 

J'he  account  of  breeding  activities  is  rather  general  and  contains  only  the  occasional  “bon- 
bon” for  the  specialist,  e.g.,  the  observation  of  “hop  hunting”  Lapland  Longspurs  (Calcarius 
lapponicus)  on  the  ground.  This  is  not  mentioned  in  the  index.  The  interested  reader  must  go  to 
various  journals  where  scientific  papers  by  some  of  Harris’  companions  have  been  published 
in  the  meantime.  Thus,  the  jacket's  promise,  “Harris'  reporting  is  a model  of  scientific 
accuracy,”  is  certainly  exaggerated.  Today,  the  term  scientific  accuracy  should  mean  more 
than  occasional  behavioral  anecdotes. 

Finally,  the  most  difficult  part  of  this  review,  an  evaluation  of  Harris’  feelings  and  his 
writing  about  them.  Everyone  who  has  watched  birds  in  the  wilderness  will  share  some  of 
the  conscious  and  unconscious  thoughts  and  moods  that  he  felt  sitting  day  after  day  amidst 
flowers  and  mosquitos.  Occasionally,  however,  he  slips  into  metaphors  and  statements  too 
grandiose  for  this  context.  Examples  are:  “A  sense  of  kinship  had  crept  upon  me,  a feeling 
that  we  shared  the  difficulty  of  living,  and  their  presence,  their  successes,  even  the  humming 
of  moscpiitoes,  encouraged  me.”  (p.  63);  “The  mammals  and  I seemed  to  share  an  immediate 
recognition.  An  illusion  took  hold  of  me,  that  we  had  a mutual  understanding  of  and  interest 
in  our  parallel  lives,  because  we  all  had  fur  or  four-chambered  hearts  or  some  bond.”  (p. 
64);  “Most  oologists  alive  today  are  aging  retired  men  with  rich  memories.”  (p.  85);  “Almost 
always  humans  are  individuals.  Animals  are  not.”  (p.  193). 

There  are  many  dialogues  in  this  book  that  do  not  sound  at  all  like  field  ornithologists 
talking  in  the  field.  They  are  too  clean,  too  literate  compared  to  my  experience  of  many 
years.  J'his  introduces  some  element  of  artificiality,  and  quite  a bit  of  distance  from  such 
writers  as  Matthiessen  and  .\bbey,  to  whom  Harris  is  compared  on  the  book  jacket.  Still,  we 
learn  quite  a bit  about  the  author.  He  immerses  himself  in  the  tundra  wilderness,  becomes 
extremely  protective  of  the  falcons,  and  finds  himself  ill  at  ease  with  the  sudden  appearance 
of  human  visitors.  Harris — in  one  of  the  best  dialogue  areas  of  the  book — is  caught  breath- 
lessly almost  stunned  by  the  sudden  confrontation  with  another  world,  and  escapes  back  into 
the  tundra  night. 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


131 


This  beautifully  produced  book  is  a successful  and  rare  (albeit  not  perfect)  blend  of  or- 
nithological adventure,  scientific  information  and  personal  touch.  It  is  w^ell  worth  its  price. — 
Hartmut  Walter. 

Shorebirds  in  Marine  Environments.  By  Frank  A.  Pitelka  (ed.).  Studies  in  Avian 
Biology,  No.  2,  Cooper  Ornithological  Society,  1979:261  pp.,  70  figs.,  52  tables.  $8.00. — This 
volume  is  a collection  of  papers  from  a symposium  on  shorebirds  held  at  the  1977  meeting 
of  the  Pacific  Seabird  Group.  The  symposium,  organized  by  F.  Pitelka,  was  composed  of 
two  sessions:  one  on  distribution,  migration  and  conservation  (15  papers);  and  the  other  on 
ecology  of  shorebirds  (5  papers,  4 abstracts).  The  objectives  of  the  symposium  were  to  look 
at  distribution,  migration  and  ecology  of  shorebirds  in  terms  of  the  basic  information  currently 
available,  and  to  examine  shorebird  biology  in  terms  of  the  conservation  and  management 
of  coastal  wetlands.  Pitelka  sets  the  stage  for  the  discussion  by  examining  shorebird  distri- 
bution on  the  Pacific  Coast.  Such  a larger  view  of  an  entire  coast  is  essential  since  shorebirds 
are  long-distance  migrants.  Pitelka  lists  the  new  world  shorebirds,  and  comments  on  tbeir 
status  on  the  Pacific  Coast.  He  then  analyzes  the  world  shorebird  fauna  (fig.  1).  One  partic- 
ularly helpful  figure  shows  the  occurrence  by  five  degree  latitude  intervals  of  shorebird 
species  in  North  and  South  America.  He  briefly  outlines  tbe  current  problems  in  shorebird 
distribution:  the  importance  and  occurrence  of  staging  areas,  migration  behavior  (group 
dynamics),  age  and  sex  differences  in  patterns,  and  winter  site  tenacity.  His  introduction  is 
an  excellent  starting  point  for  the  collection  of  papers,  and  for  graduate  students  interested 
in  shorebirds. 

The  papers  on  distribution,  migration  and  conservation  cover  a wide  range  of  topics  in- 
cluding censuses  of  restricted  areas  and  wide-ranging  areas,  habitat  use,  timing  of  migration, 
migration  patterns  of  particular  species  and  tbe  evaluation  and  conservation  of  coastal  wet- 
lands. Some  papers  merely  report  on  census  techniques;  illustrating  the  kinds  of  information 
available  from  large-scale  censuses  (see  Prater),  or  in-depth  censuses  of  small  bays  (see 
Gerstenberg).  Other  papers  illustrate  the  importance  of  particular  bays  for  migrating  shore- 
birds.  The  Copper  River  Delta  is  a critical  habitat  for  migrating  shorebirds  as  some  20  million 

I shorebirds  pass  through  this  area  each  spring  (see  Isleib).  The  delta  is  particularly  important 
to  species  such  as  Western  Sandpiper  (Colidris  mauri)  and  Dunlin  (C.  alpina)  that  forage 
j there  and  lay  down  fat  reserves  for  continued  northward  migration  (Senner).  Senner  found 
I that  Dunlin  migrate  collectively,  shift  from  one  place  to  another  as  flocks,  and  show  greater 

I weight  gain  than  do  Western  Sandpipers  that  migrate  independently  of  each  other  in  less 

) organized  flocks  while  in  the  Copper  River  Delta.  The  information  provided  by  Senner  is 

5!  critical  to  protecting  these  areas  against  human  activities  (such  as  oil  spills).  Similarly,  Gill 
t and  Jorgensen  present  quantitative  data  on  shorebirds'  use  of  another  Alaskan  bay. 

The  paper  by  Page  et  al.  is  an  excellent  example  of  tbe  information  that  can  be  gained  by 
censuses  for  many  years.  The  mass  of  data  fall  into  species  patterns,  and  provide  insights 
■I  into  habitat  requirements  and  site  tenacity.  Many  species  returned  to  the  same  foraging 
1 areas  year  after  year.  Winter  feeding  site  fidelity  was  also  found  by  Smith  and  Stiles,  who 
banded  shorebirds  in  2.5  years  at  a mudflat  in  Costa  Rica. 

Two  excellent  papers  on  migration  patterns  deserve  special  mention:  Jehl's  on  the  autum- 
' nal  migration  of  Baird’s  Sandpiper  (C.  bairdii),  and  Harrington  and  Morrison's  on  migration 

' of  Semipalmated  Sandpiper  (C.  pusilla).  Jehl's  is  an  innovative  examination  of  the  migration 

pattern  of  Baird’s  Sandpiper  using  specimens  from  35  museum  and  university  collections 
(if  I count  the  acknowledgments  correctly!).  The  project  involved  a good  deal  of  work  and 
imagination,  and  the  end  results  of  the  country-wide  survey  indicate  sexual  and  age  differ- 
ences in  the  migration  pattern  of  this  species.  Adults  may  migrate  some  9000  miles  in  5 


132 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  Vo.  I,  March  1981 


weeks,  whereas  juveniles  migrate  more  leisurely  over  a broad  front  (Jehl).  Although  I would 
have  liked  to  know  exactly  how  many  specimens  were  examined,  the  paper  illustrates  how- 
museum  specimens  can  be  used  to  answer  ecological  questions. 

Harrington  and  Morrison's  paper  on  the  Semipalmated  Sandpiper  uses  a variety  of  tech- 
niques (banding  studies  at  James  Bay  and  Massachusetts,  and  examinations  of  museum 
specimens)  to  show  that  different  populations  have  different  morphological  characteristics 
that  allow  for  identification  of  the  source  of  migrants.  The  paper  clearly  indicates  that  a 
variety  of  strategies  are  employed  by  one  species  while  migrating.  Their  migration  pattern 
is  a very  complex  system  in  which  breeders  from  different  geographical  areas  use  different 
routes  and  their  patterns  vary  seasonally.  Such  large-scale,  cooperative  studies  can  provide 
information  necessary  for  an  overview  of  migration  and  habitat  use  that  is  simply  not  possible 
by  individual  investigators  working  by  themselves  in  one  bay. 

Two  other  papers  in  this  section  (Speth,  Goss-Custard)  comment  on  the  management  of 
wetlands,  and  the  effects  of  habitat  loss  on  overwintering  shorebirds.  The  paper  by  Goss- 
Custard  is  excellent  in  that  it  makes  predictions  about  the  food  base  of  shorebirds,  and 
provides  data  relevant  to  them.  JehlN  concluding  remarks  are  concise  and  point  out  topics 
requiring  extensive  work. 

The  second  part  of  the  book  deals  with  the  ecology  of  shorebirds  and  includes  papers  on 
flocking  behavior,  winter  and  summer  ecology,  feeding  ecology-,  energetics,  and  territoriality 
of  wintering  shorebirds.  This  section  is  mainly  papers  on  particular  research  topics  with  one 
or  a few  species.  As  such,  the  papers  are  excellent,  and  provide  good  reviews  of  topics, 
quantitative  data,  and  fruitful  discussions.  The  inclusion  of  the  abstracts  was  frustrating,  as 
I wished  to  see  the  entire  papers. 

Strauch  and  Abele's  paper  on  feeding  ecology  of  plovers  wintering  in  Panama  was  partic- 
ularly interesting  in  illustrating  that  some  species  feed  only  at  low  tide  whereas  others  are 
tidally  independent.  The  study  by  Myers,  Connors  and  Pitelka  on  territoriality  in  non-breed- 
ing shorebirds  is  a landmark  study  showing  that  a wide  variety  of  species  will  defend  terri- 
tories during  the  winter,  although  its  expression  differs  among  species,  individuals  and  hab- 
itats. These  data  suggest  that  extensive  cost-benefit  analysis  of  foraging  may  lead  to  more 
(juantitative  predictions  of  the  point  at  which  particular  species  will  stop  defending  territo- 
ries. (b)ss-Custard's  analysis  of  the  energetics  of  foraging  Redshanks  {Tringa  totanus)  is  an 
excellent  example  of  an  examination  of  these  cost-benefit  considerations.  J.  Wiens’  con- 
cluding remarks  point  out  some  of  the  areas  that  need  further  research,  making  a plea  for 
long-term  studies. 

Altogether  this  volume  is  essential  for  all  ornithologists,  particularly  those  working  with 
shorel)irds,  migration  patterns,  foraging  behavior,  territoriality,  habitat  selection  and  con- 
servation. Although  papers  vary  from  brief  essays  without  quantitative  data,  to  hypothesis- 
testing  and  data-based  papers,  the  overall  quality  is  excellent.  The  papers  provide  docu- 
mentation on  several  important  aspects  of  shorebird  biology  (i.e.,  age  and  sex  differences  in 
migration  patterns,  site  tenacity  during  the  winter,  winter  territoriality).  In  the  very  least 
they  provide  quantitative  data  on  migration  patterns  essential  to  understanding  the  impor- 
tance of  coastal  wetlands.  I was  disappointed  in  the  lack  of  any  papers  on  non-Pacific  coast 
areas  (except  for  Harrington  and  Morrison).  Although  it  would  have  taken  a lot  of  time,  an 
index  would  have  made  this  volume  more  useful  for  beginning  graduate  students  or  others 
interested  in  tracing  particular  ideas  (for  example,  feeding  site  tenacity  over  the  winter). 
.'Similarly,  the  volume  was  a long  time  in  coming  out,  which  resulted  in  some  key  papers  not 
being  cited.  All  in  all,  I cannot  recommend  this  volume  more  highly.  And  for  $8.00,  it's 
clearly  the  bargain  of  the  year. — Joanna  Burger. 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


133 


A Field  Guide  to  Western  Birds*  Nests.  By  Hal  H.  Harrison,  illus.  by  the  author 
unless  otherwise  credited;  map,  endpapers  and  logo  by  Mada  Harrison.  Houghton  Mifflin 
Company,  Boston,  1979:xxx  + 279  pp.,  32  color  plates  with  captions,  153  black-and-white 
photographs,  1 map,  glossary.  $11.95  (hard  cover). — Here  is  another  attractive  addition  to 
The  Peterson  Field  Guide  Series — the  eagerly  awaited  companion  volume  to  Hal  Harrison's 
I 1975  A Field  Guide  to  Birds’  Nests  of  285  species  found  breeding  in  the  United  States  east 
of  the  Mississippi  River.  The  latest  work  represents  a more  exhaustive  review  of  the  literature 
than  the  earlier  one;  included  are  all  species,  even  the  casual  or  rare  ones,  known  to  breed 
in  the  contiguous  United  States  west  of  the  Mississippi  River.  For  nearly  each  of  the  520 
species  there  is  a succinct  description  of  its  breeding  range,  habitat,  nest,  eggs  and  other  per- 
tinent notes.  This  handy  pocket-sized  book  is  packed  full  of  practical  information  that  I am  al- 
ready putting  to  good  use. 

The  color  illustrations  are  excellent  despite  their  tiny  size  (only  40  x 53  mm).  The  nests 
centered  in  the  256  color  photographs  are  so  graphically  portrayed  that  one  can  identify 
many  of  them  without  using  the  captions;  the  same  can  be  said  of  the  eggs  whose  markings 
and  colors  for  the  most  part  are  clearly  defined  and  lifelike.  The  eggs  especially  are  so  well 
illustrated  and  described  that  one  wonders  why  the  author  failed  to  include  them  in  the 
book’s  title.  However  well  done,  I find  it  somewhat  disappointing  to  look  at  photographs  of 
' nests  situated  in  artificial  nest  boxes,  but  so  many  birds  these  days  raise  broods  in  human- 
contrived  sites  that  the  author  logically  illustrated  a number  of  them.  If  that  unavoidable 
little  problem  over  natural  vs  artificial  sites  disturbed  me,  I find  it  hard  to  fault  the  numerous 
black-and-white  photos — all  of  very  high  quality  and  nicely  placed  throughout  the  text. 

Harrison’s  use  of  the  Mississippi  as  a boundary  between  east  and  west — a method  devised 
earlier  by  Olin  Sewall  Pettingill,  Jr. — seems  to  work  well.  Inasmuch  as  I spend  a fair  share 
of  my  time  on  the  upper  Mississippi,  I can  vouch  for  Harrison’s  accuracy  for  this  region.  I 
also  note  that  he  left  few  stones  unturned  in  other  areas  of  the  country  familiar  to  me,  e.g., 
Kansas  with  its  many  unusual  records,  including  that  remarkable  Harris’  Hawk  {Parabutea 
unicinctus)  nesting  far  beyond  the  species’  usual  breeding  range.  The  few  places  where  I find 
omissions  in  the  text  are  readily  referrable  to  unpublished  material — hardly  the  fault  of  the 
author!  By  and  large,  Harrison’s  careful  coverage  of  the  huge  block  of  22  states  appears  to  be 
nothing  less  than  outstanding. 

One  is  impressed  by  the  fact  that  nearly  all  of  the  photographs  were  taken  by  the  author. 
Only  those  who  know  how  difficult  it  is  to  find  the  nests  of  a good  many  of  our  birds  will 
appreciate  Harrison’s  special  talents,  perseverance  and  endurance.  I personally  have  seen 
i so  few  nesting  birds  west  of  the  Rocky  Mountains  that  I hope  to  spend  my  retirement  days 
I chasing  them  down  in  the  far  west.  One  reliable  reference  that  will  accompany  me  will  be 
li  this  western  guide. — David  F.  Parmelee. 


i North  American  Ducks,  Geese  and  Swans.  By  Donald  S.  Heintzelman.  Winchester 
Press,  New  York,  New  York,  1978:xiv  -I-  236  pp.,  color  and  black-and-white  photographs, 
t wildlife  refuge  maps.  $15.00. — A Guide  to  North  American  Waterfowl.  By  Paul  A. 
Johnsgard.  Indiana  Univ.  Press,  Bloomington  and  London,  1979:274  pp.,  color  plates,  black- 
and-white  drawings,  range  maps.  $15.95. 

In  these  volumes  we  have  a mixed  bag  of  information  concerning  the  waterfowl  of  North 
America.  One  offers  an  invariably  sparse  treatment  of  field  recognition,  size,  flight  habits 
, and  ranges  in  North  America,  and  the  second  a relatively  complete  coverage  of  each  species 


134 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


(food  habits,  range  maps,  behavior,  etc.)-  Both  books  contain  numerous  photographs  of 
varying  quality. 

Heintzelman  states  his  purpose  as  helping  hunters  and  birders  to  identify  waterfowl  and 
to  highlight  selected  national  and  state  wildlife  refuges  where  waterfowl  are  prominent.  The 
species  accounts  are  brief  and  general  with  most  of  the  limited  text  devoted  to  field  recog- 
nition. Habitat  for  the  Gadwall  {Anas  strepera)  is  “ponds,  lakes,  rivers,  freshwater  marshes” 
and  its  North  American  range  described  as  “southern  Canada  and  the  United  States.”  I 
doubt  that  the  comments  under  the  rubric  “flight  style”  (e.g. , “direct  and  rapid,”  “graceful 
and  rapid,”  etc.)  will  really  be  of  much  aid  to  even  novice  observers.  The  photographs 
accompanying  the  species  accounts  are  black-and-white,  and  average  in  quality,  but  a se- 
lection of  better  quality  color  photographs  graces  the  book’s  centerfold.  The  author  employs 
a taxonomic  scheme  of  subfamilies  that  is  generally  outdated  in  usage  (e.g.,  Aythyinae)  but 
otherwise  has  presented  current  nomenclature  for  the  species. 

A naive  chapter  devoted  to  “Techniques  of  Waterfowl  Study”  tells  us  that  “a  pair  of 
binoculars  is  very  helpful  to  people  looking  at  waterfowl,”  mentions  that  telescopes  are  also 
useful  (“a  minimum  magnification  of  20x  is  necessary”),  notes  that  blinds  are  also  helpful, 
since  hunters  have  long  used  them,  and  sketches  the  vicissitudes  of  common  and  scientific 
names  and  “the  species  problem.”  Subsequent  chapters  briefly — some  are  scarcely  more 
than  two  pages  in  length — describe  eclipse  plumages  and  hybrids,  migrations  (with  flyway 
maps),  prairie  pothole  breeding  grounds  and  local  viewing  areas.  The  final  chapter  is  more 
extensive,  and  presents  maps  and  succinct  descriptions  of  waterfowl  refuges  in  the  U.S. 
Eour  appendices  treat  accidental  sightings,  Canadian  wildlife  areas,  conservation  organiza- 
tions and  homes  for  Wood  Ducks  (Aix  sponsa). 

The  book  contains  some  errors  (e.g.,  Hockbaum  for  Hochbaum  in  the  suggested  reading 
section)  and  poor  layout  in  some  places  (e.g.,  the  figure  on  p.  226  describing  construction  of 
predator  guards  for  Wood  Duck  nest  boxes  has  no  caption  or  cross-reference  with  the  as- 
sociated text  on  p.  220).  On  balance,  this  is  not  a book  for  ornithologists  and,  considering 
the  competition  available,  it  may  be  of  marginal  value  (at  $15.00)  to  anyone. 

Johnsgard's  prolific  pen  adds  yet  another  waterfowl  book  to  his  long  list  of  credits.  To  be 
sure,  this  is  a shortened  version  of  his  treastise  Waterfowl  of  North  America  (1978),  as  the 
author  freely  admits.  The  range  maps  and  “in  hand,”  “in  field”  identification  sections  are 
unchanged  for  the  most  part,  whereas  a “natural  history”  section  condenses  the  more  de- 
tailed material  appearing  in  the  earlier  volume.  The  condensed  text  remains  a useful  refer- 
ence to  habitat,  foods,  behavior,  breeding  and  conservation  for  each  species,  but  without 
extensive  literature  citations.  Most  species  accounts  are  accompanied,  however,  by  at  least 
one  suggested  reading  and,  while  some  of  these  may  be  overly  general  or  indeed  quite 
narrow,  it  is  not  easy  to  assign  a single  reference  or  two  with  equal  coverage  for  the  likes  of 
a Mallard  (Anas  platyrhynchos)  or,  conversely,  a Steller's  Eider  {Polysticta  stelleri). 

The  color  plates  are  largely  the  same  as  appeared  before  but  some  new  drawings  (the 
author's)  are  included.  Sketches  of  head  profiles  (where  plate  numbers  are  missing  but  the 
sketches  are  cross-referenced  with  a numbered  species  list)  are  new  and  supplement  the 
dichotomous  key  repeated  from  the  1978  volume. 

Johnsgard  intended  this  volume  to  reach  the  “middle  ground”  of  his  reading  audience, 
those  whose  needs  lie  between  heavier  works  and  a simple  field  guide.  Perhaps  one  might 
have  wished  for  a slightly  smaller-sized  book,  with  soft  cover,  to  make  it  more  field-worthy, 
but  I believe  Paul  Johnsgard  adequately  has  met  his  goal,  and  1 can  recommend  the  book, 
as  is,  for  its  stated  purpose. — Eric  C.  Bolen. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(1),  1981,  pp.  135-136 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  NEWS 


ANNUAL  MEETING— THE  WILSON  ORNITHOLOGICAL 
SOCIETY,  1981 

The  62nd  annual  meeting  of  The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  will  be  held  on  the  campus 
of  Mount  Allison  University,  Sackville,  New  Brunswick,  Canada,  4-7  June  1981.  This  will  be 
only  the  second  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  meeting  to  be  held  in  Canada,  and  the  only 
one  to  feature  excursions  in  3 provinces.  The  Maritimes  feature  small  towns  and  largely 
undeveloped  surroundings,  with  a leisurely  pace  of  life  that  should  provide  a relaxing  change. 

The  meeting  will  be  hosted  by  the  Canadian  Wildlife  Service — Atlantic  Region,  Mount 
Allison  University,  and  the  Chignecto  Naturalists  Club.  Information  concerning  accommo- 
dations, transportation,  excursions  and  related  matters,  and  a call  for  papers  will  be  mailed 
to  the  society  membership  in  early  March  1981.  The  deadline  for  submission  of  abstracts 
is  15  April  1981. 

Chairman  of  the  local  arrangements  committee  is  Anthony  J.  Erskine,  Canadian  Wildlife 
Service,  P.O.  Box  1590,  Sackville,  New  Brunswick,  Canada  EOA  3C0.  Chairman  of  the 
program  committee  is  Jerome  A.  Jackson,  Department  of  Biological  Sciences,  P.O.  Drawer 
GY,  Mississippi  State  University,  Mississippi  State,  Mississippi  39762  U.S.A. 


EDITOR’S  ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

The  processing  of  manuscripts  received  by  The  Wilson  Bulletin  during  1980  was  greatly 
facilitated  by  careful  evaluations  by  more  than  75  persons  who  served  as  referees.  I and 
i the  rest  of  the  editorial  staff  are  most  grateful  to  them  for  their  constructive  criticism  on 
behalf  of  our  contributing  authors.  I also  thank  all  authors  for  their  courteous  cooperation 
with  our  staff  as  we  work  together  to  produce  the  best  papers,  both  with  respect  to  scientific 
I content  and  quality  writing.  The  regular  editorial  staff  and  others  who  provided  assistance  in 
• 1980  were:  Dave  Ankney,  Melinda  Barlow,  Tim  Barlow,  Keith  Bildstein,  Gary  Bortolotti, 

David  Brooks,  Ann  Crabtree,  Brete  Griffin,  Rosemary  Johnson,  Dot  Richardson,  Jim  Rising, 
i|  Richard  Snell,  Pat  Urquahart  and  especially  Margaret  May  and  Nancy  Flood.  We  also 
appreciate  the  fine  efforts  in  our  behalf  from  the  staff  at  Allen  Press,  who  direct  their 
>1  best  energy  to  our  journal.  Thanks  also  for  support  from  Jerry  Jackson,  Ken  Parkes,  Bob 
i;  Storer  and  George  Miksch  Sutton. — ^Jon  C.  Barlow,  Editor 


MEMORIAL  NOTE 

» On  21  December  1980,  Helen  Van  Tyne,  widow  of  Josselyn  Van  Tyne,  died  in  Carmel, 
1 California.  During  her  life,  most  of  which  was  spent  in  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan,  she  was  very 
1 active  in  juvenile  court  matters,  Mrs.  Van  Tyne  donated  her  late  husband’s  extensive  library 
( ‘ to  The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  where  it  is  now  part  of  the  library  which  bears  his  name. 

135 


136 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  1,  March  1981 


AARON  M.  BAGG  STUDENT  MEMBERSHIP  AWARDS 

Student  Membership  Awards  in  The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  have  been  made  avail- 
able through  funds  generously  donated  in  memory  of  Aaron  M.  Bagg,  a former  president 
of  the  society.  The  Student  Membership  Committee  has  designated  the  award  recipients 
for  1981  as  follows:  Douglas  A.  Bell,  Westfalische  Wilhelms-University,  Munster;  Thomas 
Bicak,  University  of  Montana;  Scott  P.  CarroU,  University  of  Oklahoma;  Dominick  A. 
Della  Sala,  Wayne  State  University;  Sylvia  L.  Halkin,  University  of  Wisconsin;  Elizabeth  G. 
Henderson,  University  of  South  Carolina;  William  J,  Hilton,  Jr.,  University  of  Minnesota; 
Mark  L.  Hoffman,  University  of  Florida;  Charles  T.  LaRue,  Northern  Arizona  University; 
David  M.  Lyons,  Frostburg  State  College,  Maryland;  Kelvin  G.  Murray,  University  of  Florida; 
James  S.  Quinn,  University  of  Oklahoma;  Roland  L.  Redmond,  University  of  Montana;  David 
E.  Seibel,  University  of  Kansas;  David  A.  Spector,  Stockton  State  College,  New  Jersey;  and 
Kathy  A.  Winnett-Murray,  University  of  Florida. 


FIFTH  ANNUAL  MEETING  OF  THE  COLONIAL  WATERBIRD  GROUP 

The  Colonial  Waterbird  Group’s  fifth  annual  meeting  will  be  held  22-25  October  1981.  A 
symposium  on  the  factors  affecting  reproductive  success  in  colonial  birds  is  also  planned; 
papers  may  be  submitted  to  Colonial  Waterbirds,  the  Group’s  new  journal.  Symposium 
abstracts  should  be  sent  to  J.  Burger,  Dept.  Biology,  Livingston  College,  Rutgers  University, 
New  Brunswick,  New  Jersey  08903  by  1 August  1981;  regular  session  paper  abstracts 
should  be  received  by  J.  Burger  by  1 September  1981.  For  information  on  registration, 
contact  Brian  Chapman,  Dept.  Biology,  Corpus  Christi  State  University,  Corpus  Christi, 
Texas  78412. 


SOUTHEASTERN  COASTAL  AND  ESTUARINE  BIRDS 
CONFERENCE-WORKSHOP 

This  conference-workshop  will  be  held  11-13  September  1981  at  the  field  laboratory  of 
the  Belle  W.  Baruch  Institute  for  Marine  Biology  and  Coastal  Research,  University  of 
South  Carolina,  near  Georgetown.  This  meeting  will  focus  on  a total  ecosystem  approach 
and  will  assemble  past  and  present  federal,  state  and  academic  researchers  from  Delaware, 
Maryland,  Virginia,  North  and  South  Carolina,  Georgia  and  Florida  in  an  attempt  to  deter- 
mine levels  and  directions  of  current  research.  Results  of  research  on  coastal  and/or 
estuarine  species,  avian  prey  bases,  avian  competitors,  etc.  can  be  presented  in  conven- 
tional or  poster  format.  Graduate  students  are  especially  welcome.  A limited  number  of 
graduate  student  awards  (room  and  registration  fee  waivers)  are  available.  For  information 
on  presentations  and  student  awards  write  Keith  Bildstein,  Dept.  Biology,  Winthrop  Col- 
lege, Rock  Hill,  South  Carolina  29733  or  phone  (803)  323—2111.  Information  on  registra- 
tion and  housing  will  be  mailed  to  A.O.U.,  W.O.S.  and  C.O.S.  members  in  the  southeast. 
Others  wishing  to  be  placed  on  the  mailing  list  should  write  Bobbie  Christy,  Baruch  Field 
Lab.,  P.O.  Box  1630,  Georgetown,  South  Carolina  29440  or  phone  (803)  546-3623. 


This  issue  of  The  VI  i I son  Bulletin  was  published  on  14  May  1981. 


The  Wilson  Bulletin 


Editor  Jon  C.  Barlow 

Department  of  Ornithology 
Royal  Ontario  Museum 
100  Queen’s  Park 

Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6 
Assistant  Editor  MARGARET  L.  May 
Senior  Editorial  Assistants  Gary  Bortolotti 
Nancy  Flood 

Editorial  Assistants  Keith  L.  Bildstein  Richard  R.  Snell 

C.  Davison  Ankney  James  D.  Rising 

Review  Editor  Robert  Raikow  Color  Plate  Editor  William  A.  Lunk 

Department  of  Biological  Sci-  865  North  Wagner  Road 

ences  Ann  Arbor,  MI  48103 

University  of  Pittsburgh 
Pittsburgh,  PA  15260 

Suggestions  to  Authors 

See  Wilson  Bulletin,  87:144,  1975  for  more  detailed  “Suggestions  to  Authors.” 
Manuscripts  intended  for  publication  in  The  Wilson  Bulletin  should  be  submitted  in  tripli- 
cate, neatly  typewritten,  double-spaced,  with  at  least  3 cm  margins,  and  on  one  side  only  of 
good  quality  white  paper.  Do  not  submit  xerographic  copies  that  are  made  on  slick,  heavy 
paper.  Tables  should  be  typed  on  separate  sheets,  and  should  be  narrow  and  deep  rather 
than  wide  and  shallow.  Follow  the  AOU  Check-list  (Fifth  Edition,  1957)  and  the  32nd  Sup- 
plement (Auk,  90:411^19,  1973),  insofar  as  scientific  names  of  U.S.  and  Canadian  birds  are 
concerned.  Summaries  of  major  papers  should  be  brief  but  quotable.  Where  fewer  than  5 
papers  are  cited,  the  citations  may  be  included  in  the  text.  All  citations  in  “General  Notes” 
should  be  included  in  the  text.  Follow  carefully  the  style  used  in  this  issue  in  listing  the 
literature  cited;  otherwise,  follow  the  “CBE  Style  Manual”  (1972,  AIBS).  Photographs  for 
illustrations  should  have  good  contrast  and  be  on  gloss  paper.  Submit  prints  unmounted  and 
attach  to  each  a brief  but  adequate  legend.  Do  not  write  heavily  on  the  backs  of  photographs. 
Diagrams  and  line  drawings  should  be  in  black  ink  and  their  lettering  large  enough  to  permit 
reduction.  Original  figures  or  photographs  submitted  must  be  smaller  than  22  x 28  cm. 
Alterations  in  copy  after  the  type  has  been  set  must  be  charged  to  the  author. 

Notice  of  Change  of  Address 

If  your  address  changes,  notify  the  Society  immediately.  Send  your  complete  new  address 
to  Ornithological  Societies  of  North  America,  % Sandra  L.  L.  Gaunt,  Box  21160,  Columbus, 
Ohio  43221. 

The  permanent  mailing  address  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  is:  do  The  Museum 
of  Zoology,  The  University  of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  Persons  having  business 
with  any  of  the  officers  may  address  them  at  their  various  addresses  given  on  the  back  of 
the  front  cover,  and  all  matters  pertaining  to  the  Bulletin  should  be  sent  directly  to  the 
Editor. 


Membership  Inquiries 

Membership  inquiries  should  be  sent  to  Dr.  Robert  C.  Whitmore,  Division  of  Forestry, 
West  Virginia  University,  Morgantown,  West  Virginia  26506. 


CONTENTS 


OBSERVATIONS  OF  SEABIRDS  DURING  A CRUISE  FROM  ROSS  ISLAND  TO  ANVERS  ISLAND,  ANT- 
ARCTICA   Robert  M.  Zink  \ 

DISPLAY  BEHAVIOR  OF  OVENBIRDS  {Seiurus  aurocapillus)  II.  SONG  VARIATION  AND  SINGING 

BEHAVIOR  M.  Ross  Lein  21 

THE  MAYFIELD  METHOD  OF  ESTIMATIIS[G  NESTING  SUCCESS:  A MODEL,  ESTIMATORS  AND  SIMU- 
LATION RESULTS  - Gary  L.  Hensler  and  James  D.  Nichols  42 

NARROWLY  DISJUNCT  ALLOPATRY  BETWEEN  BLACK-CAPPED  AND  CAROLINA  CHICKADEES  IN 

NORTHERN  INDIANA  Peter  G.  Merritt  54 

TOE  FUSION  IN  OSCINES George  A.  Clark,  Jr.  67 

ENVIRONMENTAL  EFFECTS  ON  ROOSTING  BEHAVIOR  OF  CHIMNEY  SWIFTS 

Richard  M.  Zammuto  and  Edwin  C.  Franks  77 

GENERAL  NOTES 

OBSERVATION  OF  A BROOD  OF  SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS  IN  ONTARIO,  WITH  COMMENTS 
ON  THE  FUNCTIONS  OF  SEXUAL  DIMORPHISM 

Helmut  C.  Mueller,  Nancy  S.  Mueller  and  Patricia  G.  Parker  85 

FOOD  DEPRIVATION  AND  TEMPERATURE  REGULATION  IN  NESTLING  FERRUGINOUS  HAWKS 

Diana  F.  Tomback  and  Joseph  R.  Murphy  92 

AERIAL  “play”  OF  BLACK  VULTURES  Walter  A.  Thurber  97 

THE  SHOULDER-SPOT  DISPLAY  IN  RUFFED  GROUSE Allan  Garbutt  98 

THE  AGONISTIC  REPERTOIRE  OF  SANDHILL  CRANES 

Stephen  A.  Nesbitt  and  George  W.  Archibald  99 

NOTES  ON  THE  SLENDER  ANTBIRD  Edwin  O.  Willis  and  Yoshika  Oniki  103 

NOTES  ON  THE  UNIFORM  CRAKE  IN  COSTA  RICA F.  G.  Stiles  107 

TRICHOMONIASIS  IN  BALD  EAGLES  Ward  B.  Stone  and  Peter  E.  Nye  109 

Protocalliphora  INFESTATION  IN  BROAD-WINGED  HAWKS 

Scott  Crocoll  and  James  W.  Parker  110 

HERRING  GULL  ATTACKS  AND  EATS  ADULT  MALE  OLDSQUAW  Richard  R.  Snell  110 

RED-LEGGED  KITTIWAKES  FORAGE  IN  MIXED-SPECIES  FLOCKS  IN  SOUTHEASTERN  ALAS- 
KA   Douglas  Siegel-Causey  and  Thomas  E.  Meehan  111 

GROUND-FEEDING  METHODS  AND  NICHE  SEPARATION  IN  THRUSHES  Alan  Tye  112 

AMERICAN  COOT  DISTRIBUTION  AND  MIGRATION  IN  COLORADO 

Warner  P.  Gorenzel,  Ronald  A.  Ryder  and  Clait  E.  Braun  115 

REPRODUCTIVE  RATE  AND  RENESTING  OF  RED-WINGED  BLACKBIRDS  IN  MINNESOTA 

Daniel  W.  Moulton  119 

MIGRATION  SPEEDS  OF  THREE  WATERFOWL  SPECIES 

Frank  C.  Bellrose  and  Robert  C.  Crompton  121 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE  125 

ORNITHOLOGICAL  NEWS  135 


w/ 


COMP.  ZOOL 
ARY 


VOL.  93,  NO.  2 JUNE  1981  PAGES  137-300 


The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
Founded  December  3,  1888 

Named  after  ALEXANDER  WILSON,  the  first  American  Ornithologist. 

President — George  A.  Hall,  Department  of  Chemistry,  West  Virginia  University,  Morgan- 
town, West  Virginia  26506. 

First  Vice-President — Abbot  S.  Gaunt,  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  Co- 
lumbus, Ohio  43210. 

Second  Vice-President — Jerome  A.  Jackson,  Department  of  Biological  Sciences,  P.O.  Draw- 
er Z,  Mississippi  State  University,  Mississippi  State,  Mississippi  39762. 

Editor — Jon  C.  Barlow,  Department  of  Ornithology,  Royal  Ontario  Museum,  100  Queen’s 
Park,  Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6. 

Secretary — Curtis  S.  Adkisson,  Department  of  Biology,  Virginia  Polytechnic  Institute  and 
State  University,  Blacksburg,  Virginia  24061. 

Treasurer — Robert  D.  Burns,  Department  of  Biology,  Kenyon  College,  Gambler,  Ohio  43022. 

Elected  Council  Members — Sidney  A.  Gauthreaux,  Jr.  (term  expires  1981);  Richard  C.  Banks 
(term  expires  1982);  Mary  H.  Clench  (term  expires  1983). 

Membership  dues  per  calendar  year  are:  Active,  $16.00;  Student,  $14.00;  Sustaining,  $25.00; 
Life  memberships  $250  (payable  in  four  installments). 

The  Wilson  Bulletin  is  sent  to  all  members  not  in  arrears  for  dues. 

The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library 
The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  housed  in 
the  University  of  Michigan  Museum  of  Zoology,  was  established  in  concurrence  with  the 
University  of  Michigan  in  1930.  Until  1947  the  Library  was  maintained  entirely  by  gifts  and 
bequests  of  books,  reprints,  and  ornithological  magazines  from  members  and  friends  of  the 
Society.  Now  two  members  have  generously  established  a fund  for  the  purchase  of  new 
books;  members  and  friends  are  invited  to  maintain  the  fund  by  regular  contribution,  thus 
making  available  to  all  Society  members  the  more  important  new  books  on  ornithology  and 
related  subjects.  The  fund  will  be  administered  by  the  Library  Committee,  which  will  be 
happy  to  receive  suggestions  on  the  choice  of  new  books  to  be  added  to  the  Library.  William 
A.  Lunk,  University  Museums,  University  of  Michigan,  is  Chairman  of  the  Committee.  The 
Library  currently  receives  195  periodicals  as  gifts  and  in  exchange  for  The  Wilson  Bulletin. 
With  the  usual  exception  of  rare  books,  any  item  in  the  Library  may  be  borrowed  by  members 
of  the  Society  and  will  be  sent  prepaid  (by  the  University  of  Michigan)  to  any  address  in  the 
United  States,  its  possessions,  or  Canada.  Return  postage  is  paid  by  the  borrower.  Inquiries 
and  requests  by  borrowers,  as  well  as  gifts  of  books,  pamphlets,  reprints,  and  magazines, 
should  be  addressed  to:  The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library,  University  of  Michigan 
Museum  of  Zoology,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  Contributions  to  the  New  Book  Fund 
should  be  sent  to  the  Treasurer  (small  sums  in  stamps  are  acceptable).  A complete  index  of 
the  Library’s  holdings  was  printed  in  the  September  1952  issue  of  The  Wilson  Bulletin  and 
newly  acquired  books  are  listed  periodically.  A list  of  currently  received  periodicals  was 
published  in  the  December  1978  issue. 


The  Wilson  Bulletin 
(ISSN  0043-5643) 

The  official  organ  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  published  quarterly,  in  March,  June,  September,  and  December. 
The  subscription  price,  both  in  the  United  States  and  esewhere,  is  120.00  per  year.  Single  copies,  $4.00.  Subscriptions, 
changes  of  address  and  claims  for  undebvered  copies  should  be  sent  to  the  Treasurer.  Most  back  issues  of  the  Bulletin  are 
available  and  may  be  ordered  from  the  Treasurer.  Special  prices  will  be  quoted  for  quantity  orders. 

All  articles  and  communications  for  publications,  books  and  publications  for  reviews  should  be  addressed  to  the  Editor. 
Exchanges  should  be  addressed  to  The  Joselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library,  Museum  of  Zoology,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan 
48109.  Known  office  of  publication:  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  1827  Neil  Avenue,  Columbus,  Ohio 
43210. 

Second  class  postage  paid  at  Columbus,  Ohio  and  at  additional  mailing  office. 

© Copyright  1981  by  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
Printed  by  Allen  Press,  Inc,,  Lawrence,  Kansas  66044,  U.S.A. 


The  Rufous-faced  Crake  (Laterallus  xenopterus)  and  its  Paraguayan  cong®[iers 
the  Red-and-White  Crake  (L  leucopyrrhus)  top,  the  Gr^-breasted  Crake  (L 
and  the  Rufous-sided  Crake  (L  melanophaius)  middle  right.  The  Rufous-faced  Crake  is  show  two-third 
life  size  and  the  last  three,  one-third  life  size.  From  a painting  by  William  A.  Lunk. 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN 

A QUARTERLY  MAGAZINE  OF  ORNITHOLOGY 

Published  by  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 


VoL.  93,  No.  2 June  1981  Pages  137-300 

Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  137-144 


THE  RUFOUS-FACED  CRAKE  {LATERALLUS 
XENOPTERUS)  AND  ITS  PARAGUAYAN 
CONGENERS 

Robert  W.  Stoker 

The  Black  Rail  {Laterallus  jamaicensis)  and  other  crakes  of  the  genus 
Laterallus  are  among  the  least  known  American  birds,  and  Ripley 
(1977:192)  points  out  that  “of  all  the  rail  family,  this  group  of  species 
collectively  is  the  least  studied.”  This  is  not  surprising  because  they  are 
secretive  birds  living  in  dense  grassy  places.  But  one  relatively  tame 
species,  the  Galapagos  Rail  {L.  spilonotus)  has  been  well  studied  in  the 
field  (Franklin  et  al.  1979).  A second  species,  the  Red-and-White  Crake 
{L.  leucopyrrhus)  is  commonly  kept  in  aviaries  where  some  of  its  habits 
have  been  reported  (Meise  1934,  Everitt  1962,  Levi  1966).  Museum  spec- 
imens of  Laterallus  are  few,  hence  their  distribution  and  status  are  poorly 
known;  anatomical  material  is  even  scarcer. 

The  least  known  species  of  the  group,  the  Rufous-faced  Crake  (L.  xen- 
opterus),  was  first  taken  in  Paraguay  in  1933  and  described  the  following 
year  (Conover  1934).  It  was  not  found  again  until  Philip  Myers  rediscov- 
ered it  in  1976  and  Rick  Hansen  in  1978  (Myers  and  Hansen  1980).  The 
species  has  not  been  illustrated  previously,  probably  because  the  tail  was 
missing  from  the  type  specimen  and  information  on  the  color  of  the  soft 
parts  was  not  available. 

In  1979,  I spent  5 weeks  in  Paraguay  with  a field  party  from  the  Uni- 
versity of  Michigan  Museum  of  Zoology  led  by  Philip  Myers,  III.  One  of 
my  aims  was  to  collect  and  find  out  as  much  as  possible  about  the  secretive 
crakes  of  this  country.  Two  other  species  of  Laterallus,  the  Red-and- 
White  Crake  and  the  Rufous-sided  Crake  (L.  melanophaius),  were  already 
known  from  Paraguay.  On  my  last  day  in  the  field.  Dr.  Myers  presented 
me  with  a fresh  specimen  of  the  Gray-breasted  Crake  (L.  exilis).  The 
nearest  localities  from  which  this  bird  was  previously  known  are  in  the 


137 


138 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Amazon  VaUey,  nearly  1500  miles  to  the  north.  Although  we  never  heard 
or  saw  a crake  in  the  wild,  we  captured  specimens  of  all  four  in  mammal 
traps  and  were  able  to  obtain  information  on  habitat,  associated  mammals 
and  soft-part  colors  (the  last  by  photographing  fresh  specimens  in  color). 
Study  skins  and  these  photographs  were  used  by  William  A.  Lunk  in 
preparing  the  accompanying  plate.  Specimens  of  all  the  known  species  of 
Laterallus  were  examined  in  or  borrowed  from  the  following  museums: 
American  Museum  of  Natural  History  (AMNH),  Field  Museum  of  Natural 
History,  Museum  of  Comparative  Zoology,  Academy  of  Natural  Sciences 
of  Philadelphia  and  National  Museum  of  Natural  History. 

The  colors  and  patterns  of  the  4 Paraguayan  species  are  well  shown  in 
the  plate.  Of  particular  interest  are  those  of  the  under  tail  coverts,  which 
are  black  in  L.  xenopterus,  black  barred  with  white  in  L.  exilis,  rufous  in 
L.  melanophaius,  and  white  laterally  and  black  medially  in  L.  leucopyr- 
rhus.  These  feathers  are  displayed  conspicuously  during  tail  bobbing,  a 
common  behavior  pattern  in  rails,  which  is  known  for  at  least  L.  leuco- 
pyrrhus  (Levi  1966:25)  and  L.  spilonotus  (Franklin  et  al.  1979:213-214). 
Presumably  it  occurs  in  all  species,  and  it  may  be  important  in  species 
recognition. 

The  heavy,  white  barring  on  the  scapulars  and  upper  wing  coverts  is 
one  of  the  striking  characteristics  of  the  Rufous-faced  Crake  and  is  pre- 
sumably the  basis  for  the  specific  name  xenopterus  (xen-  = strange,  pter- 
on  = wing).  Although  I can  find  no  reference  in  the  literature  to  barring 
on  the  upper  surface  of  the  wings  of  the  other  Paraguayan  species  of 
Laterallus,  barring  is  found  in  varying  degrees  on  at  least  a few  examples 
of  each.  The  Red-and-White  Crake  is  the  most  variable  in  this  barring. 
The  wing  coverts  of  1 specimen  (AMNH  472,173)  are  extensively  and  ir- 
regularly marked  with  white  bars  up  to  4 mm  in  width  that  are  bordered 
with  narrower  black  bars.  Two  other  examples  have  white  bars  2-2.5  mm 
wide,  5 have  bars  less  than  2 mm  wide  and  6 lack  white  bars.  In  addition, 
13  of  20  specimens  of  L.  leucopyrrhus  have  diffuse  rufous  barring  on  the 
wing  coverts.  Rufous-sided  Crakes  also  may  have  narrow  barring  on  the 
upper  wing  coverts  and  scapulars:  9 specimens  I examined  have  well- 
marked  barring,  16  have  faint  barring  and  32  are  unbarred.  In  all  instances 
the  barring  was  narrow,  the  white  bars  being  less  than  2 mm  in  width. 
Gray-breasted  Crakes  show  a variable  amount  of  narrow  white  barring  on 
the  upper  wing  coverts.  Specimens  used  in  preparing  the  plate  included 
those  showing  the  maximum  amount  of  white  barring  found  in  L.  melan- 
ophaius and  L.  exilis.  This  was  done  to  emphasize  the  occurrence  of  this 
patterning. 

Measurements  of  the  4 Paraguayan  forms  of  Laterallus  are  given  in 
Table  1.  Although  males  average  slightly  larger  than  females,  data  for  the 


Storer  • PARAGUAYAN  CRAKES 


139 


Table  1 

Measurements  (in  mm)  and  Weights  (in  g)  of  Paraguayan  Species  of  Latekallus 


N 

Range 

X 

SD 

Wing 

exilis 

12 

69-78 

72.0 

±2.73 

melanophaius 

40 

74-90 

82.9 

±3.30 

leucopyrrhus 

18 

80-86 

82.4 

±1.82 

xenopterus 

4 

83-91 

86.5 

±3.42 

Bill  from  nostril 

exilis 

12 

7. 7-8.8 

8.29 

±0.33 

melanophaius 

40 

9.5-11.5 

10.47 

±0.62 

leucopyrrhus 

18 

8. 1-9.8 

9.03 

±0.56 

xenopterus 

4 

7.5-8.7 

8.00 

±0.50 

Bill  depth 

exilis 

9 

4.7-5.8 

5.17 

±0.39 

melanophaius 

26 

5.2-6.7 

5.91 

±0.42 

leucopyrrhus 

15 

5.1-6.6 

5.75 

±0.34 

xenopterus 

4 

5.9-^.2 

6.03 

±0.13 

Tarsus  length 

exilis 

11 

22.5-26.0 

24.07 

±0.95 

melanophaius 

40 

28.4-35.3 

31.44 

±1.59 

leucopyrrhus 

18 

30.1-35.0 

32.82 

±1.23 

xenopterus 

4 

28.3-30.6 

29.73 

±1.07 

Length  middle  toe 

exilis 

12 

24.8-28.4 

26.43 

±1.09 

melanophaius 

39 

29.0-38.0 

33.63 

±1.85 

leucopyrrhus 

15 

29.6-35.1 

31.41 

±1.22 

xenopterus 

4 

27.4-28.9 

28.35 

±0.69 

Weight 

exilis 

3 

27-28 

27.53 

— 

melanophaius 

2 

46-56.6 

51.30 

— 

leucopyrrhus 

10 

34-52 

45.35 

±5.61 

xenopterus 

3 

51-53 

52.00 

— 

sexes  are  combined  in  the  table  because  of  small  size  of  some  samples 
and  the  high  proportion  of  unsexed  specimens  of  L.  exilis.  Aside  from 
differences  in  size  indicated  by  wing  length  and  weight,  several  differences 
in  proportions  are  evident.  L.  melanophaius  is  notably  long-billed,  where- 
as L.  xenopterus  has  a very  short,  high  bill,  as  well  as  a relatively  short 
tarsus  and  middle  toe.  The  last  two  are  probably  associated  with  the 
species’  habitat,  which  is  densely  vegetated  and  has  a relatively  firm  sub- 
strate. 

HABITAT 

The  dense,  tussock-like  habitat  of  the  Rufous-faced  Crake  has  been 
well  described  by  Myers  and  Hanson  (1980).  The  Red-and-White  Crake 
was  taken  in  the  same  habitat  near  Curuguaty,  Dept.  Canendiyu,  and  was 
also  taken  in  an  adjacent  wetter  part  of  the  marsh  among  tall  (2  m +) 


140 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


coarse  grass  with  scattered  tree  ferns.  Others  were  taken  near  Tobati, 
Dept.  Cordillera,  in  a heavily  grazed  marsh  with  relatively  short  (50  cm) 
grasses  over  damp  ground  and  in  a coarser  marsh  with  dense,  tall  (2  m+) 
grasses  and  some  shrubs.  All  were  captured  in  traps  set  in  runways 
made  through  the  grass  by  small  mammals  or  water  runoff.  We  encoun- 
tered the  Rufous-sided  Crake  only  on  the  Arroyo  Hondo,  24  km  NNW  of 
Carayao,  Dept.  Caaguazu.  The  2 specimens  were  taken  in  traps  on  the 
edge  of  a marsh  between  open  water  with  scattered  vegetation  and  dense 
grasses  approximately  1 m tall.  Erickson  and  Mumford  (1976:8-9)  reported 
taking  both  Red-and-White  and  Rufous-sided  crakes  in  the  same  cattail 
marsh  at  Vicosa,  Minas  Gerais,  Brazil.  The  Gray-breasted  Crake  was  tak- 
en in  a runway  used  by  swamp  rats  {Holochilus  brasiliensis)  along  a fence- 
row  bordering  a road  and  a heavily  grazed  marsh  with  water  depths  ranging 
from  a few  centimeters  to  several  feet.  The  marsh  contained  a mixed 
growth  of  several  kinds  of  grasses  and/or  sedges.  Miller  (1960:235)  reported 
collecting  a Gray-breasted  Crake  “as  it  fluttered  from  marsh  vegetation  50 
cm  high  where  the  water  was  10  to  15  cm  deep.” 

From  the  few  accounts  available,  it  appears  that  the  habitat  of  the  Ru- 
fous-faced Crake  may  be  the  most  restricted  of  the  four,  whereas  the 
Rufous-sided  and  Red-and-White  crakes  are  found  in  more  varied  habitat 
which  often  includes  sparser  vegetation  and  deeper  water. 

DISTRIBUTION 

The  Rufous-sided  Crake  is  the  most  widely  distributed  of  the  Para- 
guayan species,  both  within  the  country  and  in  South  America.  It  is  also 
the  best  represented  in  collections.  It  is  found  in  suitable  habitats  through- 
out South  America,  south  at  least  to  central  Argentina  (near  Buenos  Aires) 
and  southern  Uruguay.  In  Paraguay,  it  has  been  recorded  both  in  the 
eastern  part  of  the  country  and  the  Chaco  to  the  west. 

The  Red-and-White  Crake  is  known  from  the  vicinity  of  Rio  de  Janeiro 
south  to  Buenos  Aires  and  west  to  eastern  Paraguay.  It  has  not  been 
recorded  from  the  Chaco.  The  record  from  Tucuman  is  in  error  (Olrog 
1963:125).  The  bird  is  locally  common  and  is  frequently  kept  in  zoos  and 
aviaries. 

The  Rufous-faced  Crake  has  the  most  limited  known  distribution  of  the 
four,  having  been  taken  at  3 localities  in  eastern  Paraguay  and  1 in  Brazil 
(Myers  and  Hansen  1980).  The  locality  shown  on  the  map  in  Ripley 
(1977:205)  is  in  error.  Presumably,  it  was  meant  to  show  the  type  locality, 
Horqueta,  Paraguay,  but  the  locality  is  actually  shown  in  Brazil. 

The  Gray-breasted  Crake  has  been  recorded  from  British  Honduras  to 
Paraguay.  The  single  Paraguayan  specimen  was  taken  6 August  1979,  in 
the  pantanal  (palm  swamp  region),  24  km  NW  of  Villa  Hayes  (Dept.  Pres- 


Storer  • PARAGUAYAN  CRAKES 


141 


idente  Hayes).  Whether  it  was  a migrant,  a stray,  or  part  of  a resident 
population  is  unknown. 


GENERIC  LIMITS 

Of  the  10  species  included  in  Laterallus  by  Peters  (1934:189-192)  one, 
'"hauxivelW  (=  fasciatus)  (Black-banded  Crake),  has  been  considered  a 
species  of  Anurolirnnas  by  Stresemann  and  Stresemann  (1966:149),  who 
followed  the  lead  of  Sharpe  (1894:88),  and  by  Olson  (1973:393),  who  felt 
that  viridis  (Russet-crowned  Crake)  also  was  closer  to  the  latter  genus 
than  to  Laterallus.  Ripley  (1977:157-158,  192-194),  without  stating  his 
reasons,  placed  castaneiceps  (Chestnut-headed  Crake),  the  type  species 
of  Anurolirnnas,  in  Rallina  and  left  fasciatus  and  viridis  in  Laterallus. 
After  examining  specimens  of  the  species  involved,  I find  Olson’s  argu- 
ments reasonable  and  prefer  to  retain /a5cia^W5  and  viridis  in  Anurolirnnas 
with  castaneiceps  at  least  for  the  present.  More  recently,  Blake  (1977:501) 
has  included  the  species  spilopterus  (Dot-winged  Crake)  in  Laterallus. 
Although  this  species  was  described  in  Laterallus,  it  has  long  been  kept 
in  Porzana,  on  the  basis  of  its  plumage  color  and  pattern.  I know  of  no 
anatomical  material  of  this  rarely  taken  species,  and  tentatively  consider 
it  properly  placed  in  Porzana.  This  leaves  9 species  in  Laterallus:  ja- 
niaicensis,  spilonotus,  exilis,  albigularis,  melanophaius,  levraudi  (Rusty- 
flanked  Crake),  ruber  (Ruddy  Crake),  leucopyrrhus  and  xenopterus  (the 
last  described  too  late  for  inclusion  in  Peters  1934). 

SPECIES  RELATIONSHIPS 

The  relationships  among  the  species  have  never  been  carefully  re- 
viewed. If  the  species  are  grouped  by  the  color  of  the  under  tail  coverts 
they  fall  into  groups  which  can  be  further  defined  on  the  basis  of  other 
characters.  Although  differing  in  proportions,  L.  spilonotus  is  clearly  an 
insular  derivative  of  L.  jamaicensis  and  need  not  be  discussed  further. 
Three  species,  L.  melanophaius,  L.  levraudi  and  L.  ruber,  have  unmarked 
rufous  under  tail  coverts.  All  three  are  large  (for  Laterallus)  and  have 
relatively  long,  slender  bills.  The  Venezuelan  species  L.  levraudi  is  almost 
entirely  allopatric  with  L.  melanophaius  and  differs  from  that  species  in 
having  rufous,  instead  of  black-and-white  barred  flanks.  This  difference 
parallels  that  between  Paint-billed  {Neocrex  erythrops)  and  Colombian  (A^. 
colombianus)  crakes,  which  are  considered  conspecific  by  some  authors 
(e.g.,  Meyer  de  Schauensee  1970,  Short  1975)  and  full  species  by  others 
(e.g.,  Blake  1977,  Ripley  1977).  If  not  considered  conspecific,  they  are 
best  thought  to  form  a superspecies.  I recommend  similar  status  for  L. 
levraudi  and  L.  melanophaius.  L.  ruber  is  more  distinct,  differing  from  L. 


142 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


levraudi  in  having  a gray  head  and  completely  rufous  underparts.  It  may 
be  a Central  American  representative  of  the  melanophaius  group. 

L.  albigularis,  L.  exilis  and  L.  jamaicensis  all  have  black  and  white 
barred  under  tail  coverts,  are  small  and  thin  billed  and  have  rufous  nape 
patches.  L.  albigularis  has  often  been  considered  nonspecific  with  L. 
melanophaius,  which  differs  from  it  in  color  of  the  under  tail  coverts, 
uniformly  colored  upper  parts  and  larger  size.  I believe  that  L.  albigularis 
is  closest  to  L.  exilis,  differing  primarily  in  the  rufous  vs  gray  breast.  L. 
jamaicensis  is  also  close  to  L.  exilis  and  L.  albigularis,  but  differs  con- 
siderably in  the  spotting  and  barring  of  the  upper  parts. 

In  addition  to  its  tail-covert  pattern,  L.  leucopyrrhus  differs  from  its 
congeners  in  having  bright  coral  red  legs  and  in  laying  unspotted  eggs. 
(The  eggs  of  L.  levraudi  and  L.  xenopterus  are  unknown.) 

L.  xenopterus  differs  from  the  other  species  in  its  blue-gray  legs  and 
bill,  white-barred  wing  coverts  and  scapulars,  huffy  chest,  short,  high  biU, 
and  short  tarsi  (as  pointed  out  by  Conover  1934).  The  high,  arched  culmen 
is  evident  in  the  skeleton,  as  is  the  short,  more  domed  cranium.  This 
species  is  the  most  divergent  member  of  the  genus,  but  at  this  stage  I see 
no  advantage  in  removing  it  from  Laterallus. 

Not  only  has  there  been  little  agreement  about  which  species  belong  in 
Laterallus,  but  sequences  used  by  various  authors  also  have  differed  con- 
siderably. In  Table  2 are  shown  3 earlier  sequences  plus  one  which  ex- 
presses my  beliefs  concerning  the  relationships  among  the  species  indi- 
cated earlier.  Of  the  3 other  arrangements  shown,  only  that  of  Peters 
(1934)  places  L.  exilis  next  to  L.  albigularis  and  none  place  L.  levraudi 
next  to  L.  melanophaius.  Furthermore,  Ripley  (1977)  separates  L.  jamai- 
censis from  its  derivative,  L.  spilonotus,  by  L.  melanophaius,  L.  albigu- 
laris and  L.  leucopyrrhus,  which  are  not  closely  related  either  to  each 
other  or  to  the  2 species  they  separate. 

SUMMARY 

Four  species  of  Laterallus  (melanophaius,  leucopyrrhus,  xenopterus  and  exilis)  are  known 
from  Paraguay.  L.  melanophaius  is  found  in  both  the  Chaco  and  eastern  Paraguay,  whereas 
L.  leucopyrrhus  and  L.  xenopterus  have  only  been  recorded  east  of  the  Rio  Paraguay.  L. 
exilis  is  here  reported  for  the  first  time  from  the  country.  L.  xenopterus  differs  from  the 
other  three  in  having  a relatively  short,  high  biU  and  short  tarsi  and  toes.  The  latter  may  be  an 
adaptation  for  moving  about  on  a firmer  substrate. 

The  species  of  the  genus  can  be  placed  into  4 groups  on  the  basis  of  the  color  and  pattern 
of  the  under  tail  coverts.  Other  color  characters  and  similarities  of  proportions  within  the 
groups  are  further  indications  of  the  naturalness  of  the  grouping. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

I am  particularly  grateful  to  Philip  Myers,  III,  for  inviting  me  to  accompany  him  in  the 
field,  for  collecting  many  of  the  specimens  here  reported  on  and  for  help  in  many  other  ways. 


Storer  • PARAGUAYAN  CRAKES 


143 


Table  2 

Sequence  in  Laterallus 

Peters  (1934)  Blake  (1977)  Ripley  (1977)  This  paper 


jamaicensis 

spilopterus^ 

spilonotus 

jamaicensis 

exilis 

exilis 

albigularis 

xenopterus 

rnelanophaius 

albigularis 

ruber 

rnelanophaius 

levraudi 

ruber 

viridis'^ 

levraudi 

hauxwelli^''^ 

leucopyrrhus 

leucopyrrhus 

fasciatus^ 

viridis^ 

faciatus^ 

rnelanophaius 

levraudi 

levraudi 

ruber 

ruber 

viridis^ 

albigularis 

exilis 

exilis 

spilonotus 

jamaicensis 

rnelanophaius 

spilonotus 

albigularis 

leucopyrrhus 

leucopyrrhus 

xenopterus 

jamaicensis 

xenopterus 

' Here  considered  species  of  Anurolimnas. 

^ Synonym  of  fasciatus. 

^ Considered  by  others  a species  of  Porzana. 

^ Bracketed  species  members  of  superspecies. 


Mr.  and  Mrs.  Philip  Myers,  Jr.  provided  a welcome  base  for  our  operations  in  Asuncion, 
and  Ed  Borjesson,  Carlos  Centurion  and  Antonio  Espinosa  kindly  permitted  us  to  carry  out 
fieldwork  on  their  estancias.  G.  K.  Creighton,  F.  S.  Dobson,  and  Lora,  Philip,  IV,  and  Roger 
Myers  assisted  with  fieldwork.  Steven  Goodman,  Janet  Hinshaw,  Philip  Myers,  III,  Robert 
B.  Payne  and  Robert  S.  Voss  read  the  manuscript  and  provided  valuable  comments.  An 
earlier  draft  of  the  systematics  sections  was  sent  to  the  American  Ornithologists’  Union’s 
Committee  on  the  Classification  and  Nomenclature  of  North  American  Birds  and  to  Storrs 
L.  Olson.  The  fieldwork  was  supported  in  part  by  Grant  DEB  77-04887  to  Philip  Myers,  III. 

The  accompanying  color  plate,  carefully  executed  by  William  A.  Lunk,  not  only  is  the  first 
illustration  of  L.  xenopterus,  but  also  provides  accurate  rendering  of  the  soft-part  colors  of 
all  4 species. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Blake,  E.  R.  1977.  Manual  of  Neotropical  birds.  Vol.  1.  Univ.  Chicago  Press,  Chicago, 
Illinois. 

Conover,  H.  B.  1934.  A new  species  of  rail  from  Paraguay.  Auk  51:365-366. 

Erickson,  H.  T.  and  R.  E.  Mumford.  1976.  Notes  on  birds  of  the  Vicosa,  Brazil  region. 
Dept.  Forestry  and  Nat.  Res.  Agric.  Exper.  Stat.,  Purdue  Univ.  Bull.  131. 

Everitt,  C.  1962.  Breeding  the  Red-legged  Water-Rail.  Avic.  Mag.  68:179-181. 

Franklin,  A.  B.,  D.  A.  Clark  and  D.  B.  Clark.  1979.  Ecology  and  behavior  of  the 
Galapagos  Rail.  Wilson  BuU.  91:202-221. 

Levi,  P.  J.  1966.  Observations  on  the  Southern  White-breasted  Crake  in  captivity  {Later- 
allus leucopyrrhus).  Avic.  Mag.  72:24-26. 

Meise,  W.  1934.  Zur  Brutbiologie  der  Ralle  Laterallus  leucopyrrhus  (Vieill.)  J.  Orn.  82:257- 
268. 

Meyer  DE  Schauensee,  R.  1970.  A guide  to  the  birds  of  South  America.  Livingston, 
Wynnewood,  Pennsylvania. 


144 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Miller,  A.  H.  1960.  Additional  data  on  the  distribution  of  some  Colombian  birds.  Nove- 
dades  Colombianas  1:235-237. 

Myers,  P.,  Ill  and  R.  L.  Hansen.  1980.  Rediscovery  of  the  Rufous-faced  Crake  {Laterallus 
xenopterus).  Auk  97:901-902. 

Olrog,  C.  C.  1963.  Lista  y Distribucion  de  las  Aves  Argentinas.  Opera  Lilloana  9:1-377. 
Olson,  S.  L.  1973.  A classification  of  the  Rallidae.  Wilson  Bull.  85:381-416. 

Peters,  J.  L.  1934.  Check-list  of  birds  of  the  world.  Vol.  II.  Harvard  Univ.  Press,  Cam- 
bridge, Massachusetts. 

Ripley,  S.  D.  1977.  Rails  of  the  world.  David  R.  Godine,  Boston,  Massachusetts. 

Sharpe,  R.  B.  1894.  Catalogue  of  the  Fulicariae  and  Alectorides  in  the  collection  of  the 
British  Museum.  Longmans,  London,  England. 

Short,  L.  L.  1975.  A zoogeographic  analysis  of  the  South  American  chaco  avifauna.  Bull. 
Am.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.  154:165-352. 

Stresemann,  E.  and  V.  Stresemann.  1966.  Die  Mauser  der  Vogel.  J.  Orn.  Sonderheft 
107:1-448. 

MUSEUM  OF  ZOOLOGY,  UNIV.  MICHIGAN,  ANN  ARBOR,  MICHIGAN  48109. 
ACCEPTED  20  MAR.  1981. 


COLOR  PLATE 


The  color  plate  Frontispiece  of  Laterallus  spp.  by  William  Lunk  has  been  made  possible 
by  an  endowment  established  by  George  Miksch  Sutton. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  145-163 


RESOURCE  USE  STRATEGIES  OF  WADING  BIRDS 

James  A.  Kushlan 

Ciconiiform  wading  birds  possess  several  characteristics  that  make 
them  useful  as  subjects  for  ecological  studies.  They  are  relatively  large 
birds,  ranging  from  the  Least  Bittern  {Ixobrychus  exilis),  28  cm  long,  to 
the  Goliath  Heron  {Ardea  goliatha),  140  cm  long,  (Hancock  and  Elliott 
1978).  They  are  widespread,  with  some  superspecies  ranging  from  the 
temperate  zone  through  the  tropics.  They  represent  a diversity  of  varia- 
tions on  a general  avian  theme,  that  of  a long-legged  aquatic  predator. 
Their  relatively  high  energy  demands  and  locally  large  populations  make 
them  important  components  of  aquatic  ecosystems.  Interspecific  differ- 
ences in  size,  habitat  use,  sociality,  distribution,  responses  to  seasonal 
environmental  pressures  and  food  habits  provide  the  fabric  for  natural 
experiments  in  the  elaboration  of  resource  use  strategies  in  aquatic  envi- 
ronments. 

In  this  paper,  1 present  a selective  review  of  the  current  state  of  knowl- 
edge of  resource  use  strategies  in  wading  birds.  This  report  and  its  com- 
panion paper,  a review  of  foraging  ecology  (Kushlan  1978a),  have  as  their 
common  goal  the  stimulation  of  further  use  of  this  group  of  birds  for  eco- 
logical study. 

PHYSICAL  FACTORS 

Short-term  weather  conditions,  such  as  extreme  temperature  or  rainfaU, 
often  constrain  resource  use  in  birds  (Anderson  1965,  Robins  1970).  Most 
adult  wading  birds  appear  to  be  relatively  immune  to  the  direct  stresses 
of  excessively  hot  or  cold  conditions,  compared  with  smaller  birds,  by 
virtue  of  behavioral  and  physiological  adaptations  (Kahl  1963,  Hafez  1964, 
Steen  and  Steen  1965),  not  the  least  of  which  is  their  relatively  large  body 
mass  (Calder  1975).  Rainfall  and  cool  temperatures  may  have  indirect 
effects  such  as  delay  or  interruption  of  feeding  schedules.  Consistently 
cold  weather  may  have  the  long-term  energetic  cost  of  reducing  availability 
of  the  poikilothermic  prey  that  are  commonly  consumed  by  wading  birds 
(Kushlan  1978a),  by  lessening  the  prey’s  activity  level,  or  by  driving  them 
into  deeper  water.  Rainfall  increases  turbidity  in  waters  used  by  visually 
foraging  birds,  which  can  influence  habitat  choice  and  feeding  rates  (Krebs 
1974,  Custer  and  Osborn  1978a,  Thompson  1978)  and  may  affect  nestling 
mortality  (Owen  1960). 

Seasonality,  particularly  seasonal  variation  in  resource  availability,  is  a 
dominating  feature  in  the  evolution  of  many  species  and  communities 
(Beals  1970,  Fogden  1972,  Leek  1972).  The  importance  of  seasonal  vari- 

145 


146 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


ation  in  weather  conditions  on  wading  birds  has  been  conclusively  dem- 
onstrated for  the  temperate  Grey  Heron  {Ardea  cinerea).  Mortality,  par- 
ticularly of  first-year  birds,  and  resulting  population  trends  of  this  heron 
are  determined  in  part  by  the  severity  of  winter  conditions  (Lack  1966, 
Stafford  1971,  Reynolds  1979,  North  1979). 

Seasonal  rainfall  patterns  affect  prey  availability  by  causing  water  levels 
to  fluctuate  in  shallow-water  habitats.  Relationships  between  water  level 
changes  and  wading  bird  foraging  have  been  demonstrated  for  a number 
of  species  (Kushlan  1978a).  However,  most  information  available  is  for  the 
Wood  Stork  {Mycteria  americana).  Kahl  (1964)  found  in  the  Big  Cypress 
Swamp  of  Florida  that  Wood  Storks  began  to  nest  at  a specific  water  level. 
Kushlan  et  al.  (1975)  found  a mathematical  relationship  between  the  timing 
of  Wood  Stork  nesting  and  the  rate  of  water  level  decline  in  the  Florida 
Everglades.  A similar  relationship  was  later  confirmed  in  a separate  sys- 
tem in  central  Florida  (Clark  1978).  The  existence  of  such  precise  rela- 
tionship between  seasonal  hydrologic  conditions  and  nesting  phenology  of 
the  Wood  Stork  suggests  that  seasonal  fluctuations  in  the  physical  envi- 
ronment may  also  play  a crucial  role  in  the  availability  of  resources  to 
other  species  of  wading  birds. 


GEOGRAPHY 

Geography  affects  resource  use,  especially  as  it  influences  population 
size  and  diversity  along  latitudinal  or  continent-to-ocean  gradients.  For 
example,  the  richness  of  the  wading  bird  guild  increases  with  decreasing 
latitude  in  eastern  North  America  (Fig.  1).  The  data  figured  are  for  coastal 
regions  but,  as  wading  birds  are  primarily  coastal  in  distribution  over  much 
of  the  area,  this  constraint  does  not  much  affect  regional  species  richness. 
Richness  declines  rapidly  north  of  40°  N,  probably  because  of  adverse 
climate  for  wading  birds  and  a decrease  in  the  amount  of  coastal  marshes 
along  the  east  coast.  It  has  been  clearly  demonstrated  that  the  amount  of 
habitat  available  is  correlated  with  both  population  size  (Custer  and  Os- 
born 1977,  Kushlan  1978a)  and  colony  site  location  (Lack  1954,  Kushlan 
1976a,  Fasola  and  Barhieri  1978).  Changes  in  wading  bird  species  richness 
affect  interactions  among  foraging  wading  birds,  particularly  among 
species  that  feed  aggregately,  because  the  diversity  of  foraging  aggrega- 
tions increases  from  temperate  to  tropical  latitudes  (Kushlan  1978a). 

The  drop  in  species  richness  between  20  and  25°  N in  eastern  North 
America  (Fig.l)  is  an  exception  to  the  general  temperate  to  tropical  pat- 
tern. This  particular  drop  occurs  on  the  Florida  Keys,  an  island  chain  off 
the  lower  Florida  coast.  Such  oceanic  environments  appear  to  have  a lower 
resource  base  actually  available  to  a bird  that  feeds  by  wading  than  do 
extensive,  shallow  inland  or  coastal  marshes  (Kushlan  and  Robertson 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


147 


Fig.  1.  The  relationship  between  latitude  and  wading  bird  species  richness  in  eastern 
North  America.  Data  analyzed  in  5°  blocks.  Data  on  nesting  species  from  Custer  and  Osborn 
(1977),  supplemented  for  high  latitudes  by  Palmer  (1962)  and  at  low  latitudes  by  Green  (1946) 
and  Kushlan  and  Robertson  (1977).  Isolated  point  corresponds  to  the  Florida  Keys,  an  oceanic 
environment. 


1977).  Recher  and  Recher  (1972)  have  shown  that  only  a single  wading 
bird,  the  Reef  Heron  {Egretta  sacra),  can  exist  on  the  Great  Barrier  Reef, 
an  area  of  low  resource  availability  to  herons,  despite  its  high  productivity 
overall. 

POPULATION  MOVEMENTS 

Movements  of  individuals  and  entire  segments  of  populations  are  tactics 
commonly  employed  by  wading  birds  in  response  to  fluctuations  in  re- 
source availability.  Wading  birds  use  up  to  3 types  of  population  shifts: 
migration,  dispersal  and  intraregional  movement  (Fig.  2). 

Seasonal  migrations,  which  may  be  intercontinental  in  extent,  are  well 
known  among  wading  birds,  including  such  diverse  species  as  the  White 
Stork  {Ciconia  ciconia).  White-faced  Ibis  [Plegadis  chihi).  Green  Heron 
{Butorides  virescens).  Cattle  Egret  [Bubulcus  ibis)  and  other  temperate 
herons  (Meyerriecks  1960;  Ryder  1967,  1978;  Kahl  1972;  McClure  1974; 
Siegfried  1978). 

Dispersal  of  juveniles  and  adults  occurs  at  the  end  of  nesting.  Such 
dispersal  probably  results  in  movement  of  birds  from  areas  where  re- 


148 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Eig.  2.  Movement  tactics  of  wading  birds  as  illustrated  by  the  situation  in  Florida. 

sources  are  scarce  or  unavailable  to  more  favorable  areas  (Byrd  1978).  For 
example,  after  completing  nesting  in  Florida,  birds  tend  to  disperse  north- 
ward into  temperate  marshes  (Kushlan  and  Robertson  1977),  where  food 
may  be  approaching  its  seasonal  maximum.  As  a result,  the  annual  re- 
source base  available  to  the  population  is  increased. 

Intraregional  movement  is  a less  appreciated  component  of  wading  bird 
foraging  tactics  (Kushlan  1978a).  Such  movements  have  been  well  docu- 
mented for  species  such  as  the  Cattle  Egret  in  Africa,  Sacred  Ibis  [Thres- 
kiornis  aethiopica)  in  Africa  and  Australia,  Strawnecked  Ibis  {T.  spini- 
collis)  in  Australia,  and  White  Ibis  {Eudocimus  albas)  and  Wood  Stork  in 
Florida  (Garrick  1962,  Craufurd  1966,  Urban  1974,  Ogden  et  al.  1978, 
Clark  and  Clark  1979,  Kushlan  1979a). 


i 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


149 


The  relation  of  the  3 types  of  population  movements  to  environmental 
conditions  can  be  illustrated  by  the  pattern  shown  by  the  Wood  Stork  in 
southern  Florida  (Kushlan  et  ah  1975,  Ogden  et  al.  1978).  After  nesting  is 
completed  in  spring  and  early  summer,  Wood  Storks  migrate  northward, 
spending  the  summer  and  early  autumn  in  north  Florida  and  other  coastal 
plain  areas.  Return  migration  takes  place  later  in  the  fall.  On  their  return 
to  south  Florida,  storks  forage  initially  in  coastal  marshes  and  then  begin 
intraregional  movements,  moving  through  a succession  of  coastal  and  in- 
land foraging  sites  throughout  the  nesting  period.  These  movements  cor- 
respond with  the  timing  of  seasonal  drying  of  various  marshes  and  result- 
ing concentration  of  fish  on  which  the  storks  depend  (Ogden  et  al.  1976). 
Should  water  levels  rise  instead  of  fall,  storks  abandon  their  feeding  pat- 
tern and  disperse  to  scattered  sites. 

PREDATION 

Avoidance  of  or  alertness  for  potential  predators  may  consume  time  and 
energy  and  may  affect  the  types  of  foraging  activity  in  which  a bird  can 
engage.  Healthy  adult  wading  birds  are  preyed  upon  rarely  and,  concom- 
itantly, specific  antipredator  adaptations  appear  to  be  poorly  developed 
(Milstein  et  al.  1970).  For  adult  wading  birds,  passive  awareness  of  possible 
predators  generally  suffices,  supplemented  by  simple  responses  such  as 
alert  posture,  freezing,  bill  thrusts,  bittern  stance,  ducking  and  flight. 
Characteristics  such  as  cryptic  coloration,  which  may  serve  antipredator 
functions  in  other  groups,  appear  most  readily  explainable  as  foraging 
adaptations  in  wading  birds  (Kushlan  1978a).  Social  foraging,  common 
among  wading  birds,  may  confer  an  extra  measure  of  group  vigilance 
through  feeding  vocalizations  that  can  communicate  that  “all  is  well” 
(Kushlan  1976b).  Available  reports  of  successful  predation  on  adult  wading 
birds  are  often  circumstantial  (Bent  1937,  Monson  1951,  Cottrille  and  Cot- 
trille  1958,  Blaker  1969,  Milstein  et  al.  1970,  Callahan  and  Carey  1979). 
Bayer  (1979)  presented  the  best  account  of  a predator,  a Bald  Eagle  {Hal- 
iaeetus  leucocephalus)^  attacking  wading  birds.  Great  Blue  Herons  {Ardea 
herodias).  Although  unsuccessful,  the  eagle’s  attack  and  its  presence  al- 
tered the  foraging  behavior  of  the  herons,  leading  to  the  formation  of 
temporary  groupings.  It  appears  that  the  threat  of  predation  is  usually  not 
a major  determinant  of  foraging  patterns  in  most  wading  birds,  but  this  is 
an  aspect  of  wading  bird  foraging  about  which  much  more  information  is 
needed. 

Predation  threat  to  nestlings  has  critical  effects  on  resource  use  tactics 
of  the  parents.  A number  of  predators  eat  wading  bird  eggs  and  nestlings 
(Baker  1940,  Owen  1960,  Teal  1965,  Dusi  and  Dusi  1968,  Dickerman  and 
Gavino  1969,  Blaker  1969,  Milstein  et  al.  1970,  Taylor  and  Michael  1971). 


150 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Black-crowned  Night  Herons  {Nycticorax  nycticorax)  may  prey  on  other 
species  of  wading  birds  nesting  in  the  same  colony  (Beckett  1964,  Teal 
1965,  Kale  1965,  Blaker  1969,  Wolford  and  Boag  1971).  Sibling  cannibalism 
also  occurs  (Dusi  1968).  Because  of  the  long  growth  period  and  exposed 
nesting  sites,  adult  wading  birds  guard  chicks  past  the  post-hatching 
brooding  period  (Milstein  et  al.  1970).  The  need  for  such  nest  attendance 
affects  resource  use  in  2 major  ways.  During  the  guarding  period,  growth 
rates  are  relatively  rapid  until  biomass  growth  and  organ  development  are 
such  as  to  permit  the  chicks  to  be  left  alone  (Kushlan  1977a,  b;  Werschkul 
1979).  Adult  nest-guarding  also  requires  that  the  burden  of  early  food 
gathering  fall  to  the  non-guarding  adult,  which  in  Wood  Storks  may  also 
feed  the  attending  parent  (Kahl  1966).  The  female  White  Ibis  tends  to 
forage  during  the  middle  of  the  day  while  the  larger  male  guards  the  nest 
(Kushlan  1976c,  1977c).  If  such  a schedule  imposes  time  and  energy  con- 
straints on  the  adults,  the  need  for  effective  foraging  may  become  increas- 
ingly critical  during  this  period  of  nesting.  When  young  achieve  the  size 
and  locomotor  maturity  that  permits  independence  of  parental  guardian- 
ship, both  parents  can  forage  at  the  same  time,  which  should  increase  the 
amount  of  food  available  to  young  (Owen  1955,  Siegfried  1972,  Kushlan 
1977b)  or  decrease  stress  on  adults  (Werschkul  1979). 

FORAGING  SOCIALITY 

Wading  birds  forage  alone  or  in  groups  of  varying  size.  Large  aggrega- 
tions of  wading  birds  form  at  sites  of  high  prey  availability  (Kushlan  1976a). 
By  foraging  in  an  aggregation,  wading  birds  probably  decrease  search  time 
between  food  patches,  increase  the  likelihood  of  foraging  in  a suitable 
location,  decrease  risk  of  obtaining  no  food  and  perhaps  have  more  po- 
tential food  available  (Kushlan  1978a).  Some  species  have  characteristics, 
such  as  white  plumage,  that  aid  in  establishing  aggregations  by  attracting 
other  birds  to  feeding  sites  (Kushlan  1977d). 

Aggregated  foraging  permits  efficient  use  of  the  ephemeral  resources 
that  characterize  many  wading  bird  habitats.  The  amount  and  distribution 
of  resources  and  their  availability  change  markedly  on  a daily  or  seasonal 
basis  such  that  wading  birds  may  be  able  to  feed  on  these  resources  for 
oidy  relatively  short  periods  at  a time.  By  aggregating,  birds  find  and  use 
these  locations  as  they  become  available.  In  south  Florida,  seasonal 
changes  in  prey  availability  cause  aggregations  to  form  daily  at  newly 
suitable  sites  and,  as  a result,  the  birds  sequentially  use  much  of  the 
habitat  available  over  the  annual  drying  cycle  (Kushlan  1976b,  1979a). 

Aggregative  foraging  also  brings  birds  into  close  proximity  and  increases 
social  interactions  (Grubb  1976,  Woolfenden  et  al.  1976,  Kushlan  1978b, 
Russell  1978).  Wading  birds  reserve  a feeding  space  within  an  aggregation 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


151 


by  continuing  to  defend  individual  distances.  The  amount  or  types  of  re- 
sources consumed  within  aggregations  could  increase,  decrease,  or  be 
unchanged  depending  on  the  circumstances.  1 have  proposed  (Kushlan 
1978a)  that  within  an  aggregation  some  wading  birds  gain  a net  energy 
benefit  with  increasing  aggregation  size  up  to  some  maximum.  Such  an 
increase  may  possibly  be  due  to  a subtle  commensal  benefit  derived  from 
nearby  birds,  similar  to  that  noted  below.  The  point  of  maximum  com- 
mensal benefit  may  be  determined  by  increasing  interference  among  the 
birds.  The  influence  of  the  2 effects  depends  on  the  number  of  birds 
present,  feeding  tactics  employed  by  each  species  and  prey  availability. 
An  increased  return  from  aggregative  foraging  has  been  demonstrated  for 
Little  Blue  Herons  {Florida  caerulea)  foraging  with  White  Ibis  (Kushlan 
1978b)  and  for  Snowy  Egrets  {Leucophoyx  thula)  and  Great  Egrets  {E. 
alba)  foraging  with  Roseate  Spoonbills  {Ajaia  ajaja)  (Russell  1978).  Many 
other  possibly  commensal  associations  have  been  reported  in  wading  birds 
(Kushlan  1978a).  Krebs  (1974)  studied  the  intake  of  Great  Blue  Herons 
feeding  within  and  outside  of  aggregations  in  an  attempt  to  determine 
whether  aggregating  was  beneficial.  Unfortunately,  his  results  were  equiv- 
ocal. DesGranges  (1978)  found  higher  feeding  rates  when  Great  Blue  Her- 
ons fed  in  larger  than  in  smaller  groups.  This  result  supports  the  possibility 
that  there  may  be  a commensal  advantage  to  feeding  in  aggregations. 
Interference  phenomena  have  been  documented  by  Goss-Custard  (1970) 
for  shorebirds,  but  their  occurrence  in  wading  birds  requires  study.  Rus- 
sell (1978)  suggested  that  the  subordinate  position  of  Snowy  Egrets  in 
mixed  aggregations  reduced  their  potential  increase  in  net  energy  gain. 
Similarly,  piracy,  such  as  Great  Egret  piracy  on  White  Ibis  (Kushlan 
1978c),  adversely  affects  foraging  efficiency  and  presumably  energy  gain 
of  the  victim. 

The  relationship  between  aggregation  size  and  energy  gain  should  differ 
among  wading  bird  species.  A species  with  a foraging  repertoire  composed 
of  behaviors  that  are  effective  within  aggregations  could  increase  its  en- 
ergy intake  with  increasing  aggregation  size.  Other  species,  such  as  stand- 
ing-feeders or  active-feeders  whose  behavior  brings  them  into  conflict  with 
larger  standing-feeding  birds,  would  have  the  effectiveness  of  their  for- 
aging decreased  as  aggregation  size  increases.  Such  species  may,  how- 
ever, “parasitize”  aggregations  of  other  birds  by  using  them  to  find  patches 
of  abundant  prey  and  then  feeding  near,  but  not  within  the  aggregation 
(Kushlan  1977d). 


COMPETITION 

The  role  of  indirect  competition  in  channelling  resource  use  patterns  in 
wading  birds  is  unclear.  Considering  an  entire  regional  wading  bird  com- 


152 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


inunity,  species  can  be  shown  to  partition  resources  through  a combination 
of  characteristics,  including  habitat,  time  of  feeding,  general  feeding  strat- 
egy (visual  or  tactile),  size  of  bird  and  feeding  behavior  (Kushlan  1978a). 
Studies  of  more  compact  species  groups,  such  as  sympatric  herons  or  ibis, 
have  shown  that  species  differ  by  size,  food,  feeding  location,  or  behavior 
(Garrick  1959,  Meyerriecks  1962,  Jenni  1969,  Kahl  1972,  Kushlan  1976b, 
Willard  1977,  Custer  and  Osborn  1978a,  Thompson  1978,  Hoffman  1978). 
However,  it  remains  useful  to  consider  the  degree  of  overlap  among  sym- 
patric species.  Resources  generally  appear  to  be  divided  with  a low  degree 
of  overlap  among  herons  of  different  sizes  (Willard  1977)  and  between  ibis 
and  storks  using  similar  feeding  behavior  (Kushlan  1979b).  However,  over- 
lap in  food  and  habitat  among  certain,  similarly-sized  herons  has  been 
found  to  be  high  in  specific  instances  (Jenni  1962,  Willard  1977,  Custer 
and  Osborn  1978a).  Such  overlap  need  not  imply  competition  in  itself,  as 
little  is  known  about  the  resource  state  in  these  cases,  but  it  does  suggest 
the  desirability  of  additional  study  to  determine  whether  any  competition 
may  be  involved. 

The  effect  of  direct  competition  on  resource  division  may  be  especially 
critical  within  a feeding  aggregation;  there  have  been  no  studies  that  clear- 
ly show  how  resources  are  actually  shared  or  whether  competition  occurs 
in  such  situations.  Kushlan  (1976b)  showed  that  7 species  (Great  Blue 
Heron,  Great  Egret,  Snowy  Egret,  Little  Blue  Heron,  Louisiana  Heron 
[Hydranassa  tricolor].  Wood  Stork  and  White  Ibis)  differed  by  location, 
water  depth  or  behavior  while  feeding  together.  These  differences  suggest, 
although  do  not  prove,  that  in  this  case  the  birds  were  taking  different 
segments  of  the  available  resource  spectrum.  Competition  certainly  does 
occur  at  times.  In  the  above  example.  Black-crowned  Night  Herons,  which 
overlapped  in  foraging  characteristics  with  Snowy  Egrets,  did  not  use  the 
site  while  the  aggregation  fed  there,  probably  because  of  dominance  by 
Great  Egrets  (Kushlan  1973a).  When  resources  are  limited,  competition 
among  Cattle  Egrets  clearly  resulted  in  differential  resource  allocation  to 
older,  more  competitively  dominant  birds  (Woolfenden  et  al.  1976).  These 
results  suggest  that  direct  interference  competition  may  be  widespread 
among  wading  birds. 

FORAGING  TACTICS 

^ here  to  search. — An  important  consideration  in  understanding  wading 
bird  searching  tactics  is  that  habitat  and  prey  for  most  species  are  dis- 
tributed in  spatial  and  temporal  patches  (Kushlan  1976a).  As  a result, 
wading  birds  need  to  sample  potential  patches  to  decide  where  to  forage. 
Eor  species  such  as  the  W hite  Ibis,  choice  of  foraging  patch  may  in  some 
cases  be  a primary  factor  in  foraging  optimization  (Kushlan  1979a).  The 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


153 


energ>'  penalty  for  wrong  choices  may  be  rather  severe,  because  of  the 
relatively  high  costs  incurred  by  large  birds  in  moving  from  place  to  place. 
Thus,  there  is  probably  considerable  pressure  for  wading  birds  to  sample 
foraging  patches  efficiently. 

Several  tactics  are  employed  by  wading  birds  in  patch  selection.  A num- 
ber of  species  typically  travel  and  forage  in  single  species  flocks.  Cattle 
Egrets  (Siegfried  1971,  1978)  and  White  Ibis  (Kushlan  1979a)  are  notable 
examples.  Custer  and  Osborn  (1978b)  found  that  these  2 species  often  flew 
in  interspecific  groups  to  feeding  sites  in  contrast  with  8 other  wading  bird 
species.  It  has  been  proposed  that  information  regarding  the  location  of 
food  patches  is  transferred  among  wading  birds  at  colony  and  roost  sites 
(Krebs  1974).  At  least  some  cirumstantial  evidence  supports  this  hypoth- 
esis (Krebs  1974,  Custer  and  Osborn  1978b,  DesGranges  1978).  Informa- 
tion is  no  doubt  transfered  in  flight  lines  and  at  foraging  sites  as  1 bird 
sees  another  feeding.  Species  that  feed  in  aggregations  typically  choose 
sites  where  other  birds  are  foraging  (Kushlan  1977d). 

Other  tactics  used  to  decrease  the  frequency  of  wrong  sampling  choices 
include  trial  and  error,  learning,  return  to  previously  used  patches,  terri- 
toriality and  sequential  sampling.  The  dynamics  of  wading  birds  foraging 
in  a temporally  changing  patch  demonstrated  the  role  of  trial  and  error 
(Kushlan  1976a).  A few  wading  birds  regularly  visited  the  feeding  site  for 
short  periods  when  prey  abundance  was  low.  As  prey  became  more  abun- 
dant, feeding  time  increased  and  number  of  wading  birds  increased  until 
a large  aggregation  formed.  This  succession  of  events  suggests  that  birds 
were  sampling  by  trial  and  error.  Length  of  stay  in  a patch  may  have  been 
determined  by  how  much  food  a bird  was  able  to  obtain.  Similarly,  habitat 
use  in  the  White  Ibis  appears  to  be  determined  in  part  by  the  energy  value 
of  the  food  located  there  (Kushlan  1979a). 

Learning  probably  plays  an  important  role  in  feeding  site  selection.  The 
importance  of  learning  to  forage  effectively  is  demonstrated  by  atypical 
foraging  sites  being  chosen  by  juveniles  (Kushlan  and  Kushlan  1975)  and 
also  by  the  lowered  foraging  effectiveness  of  young  birds  (Recher  and 
Recher  1969a,  Cook  1978a). 

Wading  birds  return  to  a previously  used  patch  provided  its  profitability 
continues  to  be  sufficient  (Owen  1955,  Bateman  1970,  Kushlan  1976b, 
Cook  1978b).  This  means  that  a given  patch  will  be  used  for  a period  of 
time  determined  by  the  rates  of  resource  depression  and  renewal.  Prey 
availability  in  some  patches,  such  as  reef  crests  used  by  the  Reef  Heron 
(Recher  and  Recher  1972),  may  be  renewed  daily.  Daily  renewal  in  some 
cases  permits  the  development  of  permanent  territory  holding  systems.  A 
' single  species  of  wading  bird  can  use  several  tactics  in  its  use  of  foraging 
I patches.  Great  Blue  Herons,  for  example,  in  different  situations  can  hold 


154 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


permanent  territories  (Bayer  1978),  switch  between  intertidal  and  terres- 
trial foraging  (Krebs  1974),  or  show  temporal  variability  in  feeding  sites 
used  around  a colony  (DesGranges  1978). 

When  resource  status  changes  gradually,  patches  may  be  sampled  se- 
quentially as  they  become  available.  As  south  Florida  swamps  dry  sea- 
sonally, birds  can  use  1 patch  until  it  is  depleted  and  then  move  to  a 
nearby  patch  where  prey  become  available  later.  Intraregional  movements 
exhibited  by  many  wading  birds,  discussed  previously,  result  from  re- 
peated shifts  in  foraging  sites. 

The  search. — The  effectiveness  of  a wading  bird’s  search  for  prey  is 
determined  in  part  by  the  foraging  behaviors  used.  These  behaviors,  which 

1 have  cataloged  elsewhere  (Kushlan  1978a),  can  be  expected  to  be  chosen 
to  increase  net  energy  gain.  Snowy  Egrets,  for  example,  apparently  select 
feeding  behavior  in  relation  to  prey  availability  (Kushlan  1972).  When  dis- 
solved oxygen  is  low,  such  as  in  the  early  morning,  fish  may  have  to  come 
to  the  surface  where  diffusion  provides  a higher  oxygen  concentration 
(Kushlan  1974).  Under  these  conditions  Snowy  Egrets  feed  by  standing, 
a behavior  of  relatively  low  cost.  However,  as  oxygen  levels  increase  dur- 
ing the  day  and  fish  become  less  available  at  the  surface,  the  egrets  may 
have  to  resort  to  more  energy  intensive  behavior,  such  as  foot-dragging. 
Thus,  when  resources  were  easily  obtained,  egrets  used  an  energetically 
efficient  behavior.  Use  of  passive,  low  cost  behaviors  for  abundant,  con- 
spicuous and  easily  captured  prey  or  use  of  active,  more  variable  behaviors 
when  prey  are  less  available  may  be  characteristic  of  wading  birds.  In  one 
such  case,  when  standing-feeding  was  the  least  successful  behavior  for 
Snowy  Egrets,  success  was  about  inversely  related  to  energy  expenditure 
(Kushlan  1973b).  If  energy  gain  is  proportional  to  success,  then  the  net 
energy  gain  may  be  about  equivalent  for  each  behavior.  Birds  using  various 
behaviors  may  all  forage  effectively,  perhaps  because  individuals  may  spe- 
cialize in  different  behaviors  (Kushlan  1973b). 

Prey  location,  selection  and  pursuit. — The  location,  pursuit  and  selec- 
tion of  prey  have  been  the  subject  of  considerable  discussion.  MacArthur 
(1972)  suggested  that  if  a bird  chooses  the  diet  that  minimizes  average 
pursuit  and  search  time  per  gram  of  prey,  a species  should  be  more  spe- 
cialized in  a productive  environment  than  in  an  unproductive  one.  In  sup- 
port of  this  hypothesis,  MacArthur  (1972)  used  Recher’s  (unpubl.)  data  on 
the  Great  Blue  Heron,  which  showed  that  birds  took  a narrower  food  size 
range  in  a more  productive  tropical  habitat  (south  Florida),  than  in  a less 
productive  temperate  habitat  (New  York).  However,  in  using  this  example 
MacArthur  overlooked  2 confounding  factors,  that  the  birds  of  the  2 pop- 
ulations are  not  the  same  size  and  that  the  ranges  of  prey  available  to  the 

2 populations  may  not  be  similar.  The  Great  Blue  Heron  population  resi- 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


155 


dent  in  Florida  consists  of  larger  birds  than  in  New  York.  Because  they 
can  handle  larger  prey  the  larger  birds  may  be  expected  to  take  a wider 
size  range  of  prey  in  Florida,  rather  than  the  smaller  range  predicted  by 
the  productivity  hypothesis.  Also,  the  types  and  sizes  of  prey  available 
undoubtedly  differ  between  a subtropical  marine  site  and  a temperate 
freshwater  site.  Should  a more  limited  range  of  prey  exist  in  1 site,  irre- 
spective of  total  productivity,  the  heron  should  not  be  expected  to  take  a 
wider  range  of  prey  there. 

There  are,  however,  other  examples  from  wading  bird  studies  that  may 
be  useful  in  assessing  the  relationship  of  productivity  to  food  selection. 
When  food  is  most  available  diet  breadth  should  be  restricted  (MacArthur 
1972).  Thus  prey  taken  in  a single  location  in  summer,  when  availability 
would  be  high,  should  be  less  diverse  than  in  winter  when  productivity 
presumably  declines.  However,  Willard’s  (1977)  data  on  Great  Egrets, 
Great  Blue  Herons  and  Snowy  Egrets  suggest  that,  during  the  spring-sum- 
mer period  of  higher  food  availability,  prey  sizes  were  more  diverse  than 
in  fall-winter.  Similarly,  a greater  diversity  of  prey  was  taken  by  White 
Ibis  in  presumably  more  productive  coastal  habitats  than  in  inland  habitats 
(Kushlan  1979b).  These  results  are  not  in  accord  with  predictions  and  are 
particularly  puzzling  for  searching  predators  such  as  Great  Egrets  and 
Great  Blue  Herons,  because  these  birds  would  minimize  search  time  by 
I taking  additional  prey  when  food  is  scarce.  Such  apparent  discrepancies 
from  predicted  results  merit  further  study. 

Smaller  species  should  have  a more  restricted  diet  because  of  shorter 
search  times  for  smaller  more  abundant  prey,  while  a larger  forager  should 
have  a longer  search  time  and  eat  a wider  range  of  prey  (MacArthur  1972). 
In  support  of  this,  it  appears  that  large  herons  do  tend  to  take  a wider  size 
range  of  prey  than  small  herons  (Willard  1977). 

A species  that  searches  for  its  prey  should  be  a generalist,  whereas  a 
. species  that  pursues  its  prey  should  be  a specialist  (MacArthur  1972). 

Jenni  (1969)  showed  that  for  2 herons  of  similar  size  in  north  Florida  the 
! Snowy  Egret,  the  pursuer,  was  relatively  more  specialized  than  the  Little 
Blue  Heron,  the  searcher.  Thus,  in  the  same  foraging  area,  the  searcher 
1 had  a more  diverse  diet,  probably  because  taking  each  prey  encountered 
, decreased  search  time  between  prey  items.  However,  the  distinction  be- 
I tween  searcher  and  pursuer  is  often  not  easy  to  make,  because  most 
■I  species  can  use  either  tactic  at  different  times  or  in  different  habitats. 

Pursuit  time  has  been  reduced  to  zero  for  those  wading  birds  that  forage 
by  tactile  detection  of  prey,  a strategy  that  apparently  has  evolved  at  least 
twice  in  ciconiiform  wading  birds  (Kushlan  1978a,  1979b).  The  American 
* White  Ibis  and  the  Wood  Stork  are  examples  of  such  species  (Kahl  and 
' Peacock  1963,  Kushlan  1977e).  Pursuit  is  nonexistent  because  the  first 


156 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


contact  with  prey  is  the  moment  of  capture.  This,  of  course,  makes  the 
decision  of  where  to  search  and  how  to  search  critical  to  such  foragers. 

Other  species  appear  to  specialize  in  pursuit  strategies.  These  active 
foragers,  including  the  Reddish  Egret  {Dichromanassa  rufescens),  Louisi- 
ana Heron  and  Snowy  Egret  (Kushlan  1978a),  are  species  that  typically 
run  down  prey,  especially  schools  of  fish.  The  Reddish  Egret  appears  to 
specialize  in  pursuing  schooling  fish  in  shallow  water.  There  has  been  no 
test  of  the  relative  efficiencies  of  various  types  of  tactics  used  by  wading 
birds  specializing  in  active  pursuit. 

MacArthur  and  Pianka  (1966)  predicted  that  a predator  should  rank  prey 
types  according  to  their  energy  value  per  handling  time.  For  the  White 
Ibis  in  south  Florida,  handling  time  for  most  prey  actually  consumed  is 
small  and,  in  general,  energy  content  appears  to  have  minimal  relevance 
to  prey  selection  (Kushlan  1979a).  This  is  because,  as  a tactile  forager,  the 
ibis  should  accept  any  prey  it  catches  and  can  consume  quickly.  Thus, 
ibis  should  encounter  their  environment  in  a fine-grained  way;  and,  since 
prey  density  should  control  search  time  in  this  completely  searching  pred- 
ator, ibis  should  take  prey  in  proportion  to  the  prey’s  density.  However, 
density  is  also,  in  many  cases,  irrelevant  to  the  ibis,  which  selectively  con- 
sume certain  species  of  prey  even  at  very  low  density  (Kushlan  1979a). 
Prey  selection  is  made  in  large  part  on  the  basis  of  a potential  prey’s  catch- 
ability,  that  is,  the  ability  of  ibis  to  effect  its  capture  (Kushlan  1979b). 
Visually  foraging  wading  birds,  on  the  other  hand,  at  least  have  the  po- 
tential to  rank  prey  before  capturing.  However,  there  is  currently  little 
evidence  that  they  do  so. 

Capture  and  handling. — The  motor  patterns  associated  with  capturing, 
handling  and  swallowing  prey  have  been  summarized  elsewhere  (Kushlan 
1978a).  The  maximum  possible  size  of  prey  for  a wading  bird  is  not  easily 
determined,  since,  given  sufficient  time  and  freedom  from  robbing,  a wad- 
ing bird  can  eat  even  very  large  prey,  piece-by-piece.  Recher  and  Reeher 
(1969b)  suggested  that  a heron  is  capable  of  seizing  and  holding  a fish  at 
least  25%  longer  than  its  hill.  Smaller  birds  take  more  time  to  handle  prey 
per  gram  of  prey  than  do  larger  birds  (Kushlan  1978a).  Thus,  ease  of 
handling  may  he  an  important  factor  in  the  wider  prey  size  taken  by  large 
wading  birds. 

Schoener  (1971)  predicted  that  handling  time  would  be  constant  below 
a certain  prey  size,  hut  for  larger  prey  it  becomes  an  exponential  function 
of  prey  size.  This  appears  to  be  so  for  the  White  Ibis  (Kushlan  1979a). 
Handling  time  inereases  drastically  with  increasing  prey  size  to  the  point 
where  the  predator  becomes  satiated  or  where  it  becomes  uneconomical 
to  eat  the  prey  because  of  other  considerations.  In  the  White  Ibis,  cap- 
turing of  large  prey  by  individual  birds  increases  the  probability  of  an 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


157 


attack  by  robbers,  particularly  Great  Egrets.  This  robbing  could  restrict 
the  diet  of  the  victim,  since  some  typical  prey  items  are  lost  through 
robbing  and  handling  time  is  increased  by  avoiding  the  pursuit  of  the 
potential  robbers.  The  White  Ibis,  when  feeding  in  an  aggregation,  will 
often  drop  newly  caught  large  prey  rather  than  attempt  handling  (Kushlan 
1979a).  Similarly,  Great  Blue  Herons  drop  struggling  fish  (Recher  and 
Recher  1969b). 

Recher  and  Recher  (1969b)  showed  that  defensive  structures  and  be- 
havior increase  handling  time,  increase  the  likelihood  of  prey  escape  and 
decrease  the  average  net  energy  value  of  a captured  prey  item.  In  the  prey 
of  wading  birds,  structures  appear  more  effective  in  defense  than  behavior 
alone.  A prey  that  has  a high  handling  time  should  not  be  added  to  the 
diet  if  its  value  per  time  exceeds  the  mean  of  previous  prey  taken  in  the 
diet.  Great  Blue  Herons,  however,  at  times  take  prey  requiring  high  han- 
dling time.  Recher  and  Recher  (1972)  showed  that  the  average  weight  of 
food  gained  by  Great  Blue  Herons  for  each  minute  spent  subduing  and 
swallowing  a puffer  (Tetradontidae)  was  considerably  less  than  the  average 
weight  of  food  obtained  for  each  minute  of  foraging  time.  It  would  be 
advantageous  for  herons  to  ignore  fish  such  as  puffers.  Some  individual 
Great  Blue  Herons,  however,  have  learned  to  subdue  puffers  by  piercing 
them  and  consume  them  even  though  it  is  apparently  relatively  inefficient 
for  them  to  do  so. 

Foraging  maximization. — Wading  birds  may  prove  useful  in  studies  of 
the  tactics  used  by  animals  to  increase  food  intake  or  energy  gain.  Present 
information  on  wading  bird  foraging  suggests  areas  that  merit  particular 
attention.  For  example,  components  of  prey  other  than  energy  may  restrict 
a wading  bird’s  ability  to  maximize  energy  at  any  given  time.  Marabou 
Stork  {Leptoptilos  crumeniferus)  chicks  require  calcium,  which  is  obtain- 
able from  natural  prey  but  not  from  the  normal  marabou  food,  carrion, 
(Kahl  1966)  which  probably  could  be  gathered  more  efficiently. 

Feeding  may  not  be  performed  in  a maximally  efficient  manner  in  some 
cases,  such  as  when  it  interferes  with  a wading  bird  optimizing  its  entire 
activity  pattern.  During  pair  formation  wading  birds  spend  long  hours  in 
territorial  defense  (Burger  1978)  and  thereby  may  need  to  shorten  their 
feeding  time,  perhaps  by  choosing  poor  feeding  sites  near  the  colony. 
Similarly,  social  dominance  interactions  in  flocking  or  aggregating  wading 
birds  (Woolfenden  et  al.  1976,  Grubb  1976)  may  decrease  the  possibility 
of  using  the  most  efficient  foraging  behavior  or  feeding  site. 

Wading  birds  usually  respond  dramatically  to  short-term  availability  of 
easily-obtained  prey,  such  as  at  fish  kills  where  they  can  feed  with  high 
efficiency  (Hoffman  1978,  Dombe  and  McFarlane  1978).  However,  a long- 
term strategy  of  optimizing  energy  return  may  require  ignoring  such  short- 


158 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


term  energy  bonanzas.  Great  Blue  Herons  that  hold  territories  during  nest- 
ing may  not  be  as  likely  as  nonterritorial  birds  to  take  advantage  of  foraging 
opportunities  at  such  ephemeral  food  sources  (Bayer  1978). 

On  a long-term  basis,  an  animal  must  maintain  a positive  or  neutral 
energy  balance.  Thus,  a wading  bird  must  obtain  sufficient  energy  to  offset 
its  energy  expenditure.  During  nesting  and  feeding  young,  high  foraging 
efficiency  may  be  required.  On  the  other  hand,  non-rigorous  foraging  may 
be  possible  when  constraints  on  time  or  energy  budgets  are  not  great 
(Kushlan  1978c). 

Thus,  wading  birds  appear  to  demonstrate  important  factors  impinging 
on  energy  maximization  such  as  non-energetic  foraging  requirements, 
complex  activity  patterns,  long-term  considerations  and  short-term  behav- 
ioral choices.  All  can  affect  approaches  to  increasing  net  energy  gain  and 
as  such  can  provide  considerable  insight  into  resource  use  strategies. 


SUMMARY 


Wading  birds,  because  of  their  large  size  and  diversity,  appear  to  offer  some  promise  for 
unraveling  of  certain  aspects  of  foraging  ecology  and  resource  use  strategies.  This  paper 
presents  a selective  review  of  the  current  status  of  knowledge  of  resource  use  strategies  in 
ciconiiform  wading  birds. 

Because  of  their  large  size,  adult  wading  birds  appear  to  be  relatively  immune  to  direct 
adverse  effects  of  inclement  weather,  although  indirect  effects  of  weather  on  prey  availability 
are  important.  Seasonal  variation  in  resource  availability  can  limit  population  levels  of  tem- 
perate species  and  is  also  a dominating  influence  in  tropical  populations,  where  seasonal  fluctu- 
ation of  surface  water  conditions  determine  in  large  part  the  nature  of  resource  use.  Wading 
bird  species  richness  increases  latitudinally  toward  the  tropics  and  decreases  away  from  conti- 
nental areas.  Population  movements,  including  seasonal  migration,  radiative  dispersal  and 
intraregional  movement,  are  adaptations  to  variable  resource  conditions.  Predation  pressure 
on  nestlings  may  influence  the  evolution  of  growth  patterns  and  time  budgets  of  nesting 
adults. 

Enraging  sociality,  especially  aggregative  feeding,  is  an  important  aspect  of  resource  use, 
and  may  involve  commensalism,  competitive  interactions  and  interference.  Differently-sized 
wading  birds  appear  to  forage  differently,  but  similarly-sized  species  may  show  considerable 
overlap  in  foraging  parameters.  Territoriality  and  aggregative  feeding  can  decrease  search 
time,  or  increase  or  decrease  foraging  effectiveness.  Foraging  behavior  is  diverse  and  variable, 
and  responds  to  prey  availability.  Prey  selection  of  wading  birds  require  study,  particularly 
with  respect  to  factors  associated  with  effectiveness  of  prey  choices. 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

This  report  is  based  on  a paper  presented  at  The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  Symposium 
on  Resource  Use  Strategies  in  Birds,  held  at  Morgantown,  West  Virginia.  I thank  J.  Barlow, 
P.  Frohring,  J.  Rising,  S.  Rohwer  and  E.  Tramer  for  discussions  or  comments  on  this 
manuscript. 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


159 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Anderson,  D.W.  1965.  Spring  mortality  in  insectivorous  birds.  Loon  37:134-135. 

Baker,  R.  H.  1940.  Crow  depredations  on  heron  nesting  colonies.  Wilson  Bull.  62:124-125. 

Bateman,  D.  L.  1970.  Movement-behavior  in  three  species  of  colonial  nesting  wading  birds: 
a radio  telemetric  study.  Ph.D.  diss.  Auburn  Univ.,  Auburn,  Alabama. 

Bayer,  R.  D.  1978.  Aspects  of  an  Oregon  estuarine  Great  Blue  Heron  population.  Pp.  213- 
217  in  Wading  birds  (A.  Sprunt,  IV,  J.  C.  Ogden  and  S.  Winckler,  eds.).  Natl.  Audubon 
Soc.,  New  York,  New  York. 

. 1979.  Bald  Eagle-Great  Blue  Heron  interactions.  Murrelet  60:32-33. 

Beals,  E.  W.  1970.  Birds  of  a Euphorbia  Acacia  woodland  in  Ethiopia:  habitat  and  seasonal 
changes.  J.  Anim.  Ecol.  39:277-291 . 

Beckett,  T.  A.  1964.  Black-crowned  Night  Heron  feeding  behavior.  Chat  28:93-94. 

Bent,  A.  C.  1937.  Life  histories  of  North  American  birds  of  prey.  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull. 
170. 

Blaker,  D.  1969.  Behaviour  of  the  Cattle  Egret  Ardeola  ibis.  Ostrich  40:75-129. 

Burger,  J.  1978.  Competition  between  Cattle  Egrets  and  native  North  American  herons, 
egrets  and  ibis.  Condor  80:15-23. 

Byrd,  M.  A.  1978.  Dispersal  and  movements  of  six  North  American  ciconiiforms.  Pp.  161- 
183  in  Wading  birds  (A.  Sprunt,  IV,  J.  C.  Ogden  and  S.  Winckler,  eds.).  Natl.  Audubon 
Soc.,  New  York,  New  York. 

Calder,  W.  a.,  hi.  1975.  Consequences  of  body  size  for  avian  energetics.  Pp.  86-144  in 
Avian  energetics  (R.  A.  Paynter,  Jr.,  ed.).  Publ.  Nutt.  Ornithol.  Club  15. 

Callahan,  R.  and  W.  Carey.  1979.  Great  Horned  Owl  suspected  of  preying  upon  Snowy 
Egret.  Florida  Field  Nat.  7:29. 

Garrick,  R.  1959.  The  food  and  feeding  habits  of  the  Straw-necked  Ibis  Threskiornis 
spinicollis  (Jameson)  and  the  White  Ibis  T.  molucca  (Cuvier)  in  Australia.  CSIRO  Wildl. 
Res.  4:69-92. 

. 1962.  Breeding  movements  and  conservation  of  ibises  (Threskiornithidae)  in  Aus- 
tralia. CSIRO  Wildl.  Res.  7:71-88. 

Clark,  E.  S.  1978.  Factors  affecting  the  initiation  and  success  of  nesting  in  an  east-central 
Florida  Wood  Stork  colony.  Proc.  Colonial  Waterbird  Group  2:178-188. 

Clark,  R.  A.  and  A.  Clark.  1979.  Daily  and  seasonal  movements  of  the  Sacred  Ibis  at 
Pretoria,  Transvaal.  Ostrich  50:94-103. 

Cook,  D.  C.  1978a.  Foraging  behavior  and  food  of  Grey  Herons  (Ardea  cinerea)  on  the 
Ythan  Estuary.  Bird  Study  25:17-22. 

. 1978b.  Grey  Herons  Ardea  cinerea  holding  feeding  territories  on  the  Ythan  Estuary. 

Bird  Study  25:11-16. 

COTTRILLE,  W.  P.  AND  B.  D.  CoTTRiLLE.  1958.  Great  Blue  Herons:  behavior  at  the  nest. 
Misc.  Publ.  Mus.  Zool.  Univ.  Michigan  102:1-15. 

Craufurd,  R.  Q.  1966.  Notes  on  the  ecology  of  the  Cattle  Egret  Ardeola  ibis  at  Rokupr, 
Sierra  Leone.  Ibis  108:411^18. 

Custer,  T.  W.  and  R.  G.  Osborn.  1977.  Wading  birds  as  biological  indicators:  1975  colony 
survey.  U.S.  Fish  Wildl.  Serv.  Spec.  Scientific  Rept.  Wildl.  No.  206. 

AND . 1978a.  Feeding  site  description  of  three  heron  species  near  Beaufort, 

North  Carolina.  Pp.  355-360  in  Wading  birds  (A.  Sprunt,  IV,  J.  C.  Ogden  and  S.  Winck- 
ler, eds.).  Natl.  Audubon  Soc.,  New  York,  New  York. 

AND . 1978b.  Feeding  habitat  use  by  colonially-breeding  herons,  egrets,  and 

ibises  in  North  Carolina.  Auk  95:733-743. 

DesGranges,  j.  Y.  1978.  Adaptive  value  of  social  behaviour  in  the  Great  Blue  Heron  (Ardea 
herodias).  Proc.  Colonial  Waterbird  Group  2:192-201. 


160 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Dickerman,  R.  W.  and  G.  Gavino.  1969.  Studies  of  a nesting  colony  of  Green  Herons 
at  San  Bias,  Nayarit,  Mexico.  Living  Bird  8:95-111. 

Dombe,  a.  H.  and  R.  W.  McFarlane.  1978.  Feeding  ecology  of  Little  Blue  Herons  at  a 
radionuclear-contaminated  reservoir.  Pp.  361-364  in  Wading  birds  (A.  Sprunt,  IV,  J. 
C.  Ogden  and  S.  Winckler,  eds.).  Natl.  Audubon  Soc.,  New  York,  New  York. 

Dust,  J.  L.  1968.  The  competition  between  Cattle  Egrets  and  Little  Blue  Herons.  Alabama 
Birdlife  16:4-6. 

AND  R.  T.  Dust.  1968.  Ecological  factors  contributing  to  nesting  failure  in  a heron 

colony  . Wilson  BuU.  80:458^66. 

Fasola,  M.  and  F.  Barbieri.  1978.  Factors  affecting  the  distribution  of  heronries  in  north- 
ern Italy.  Ibis  120:537-540. 

Fogden,  M.  P.  L.  1972.  The  seasonality  and  population  dynamics  of  equatorial  forest  birds 
in  Sarawak.  Ibis  114:307-343. 

Goss-Custard,  j.  D.  1970.  The  responses  of  Redshanks  Tringa  totanus  to  spatial  variations 
in  their  prey  density.  J.  Anim.  Ecol.  39:91-113. 

Green,  E.  R.  1946.  Birds  of  the  lower  Florida  Keys.  Quart.  J.  Florida  Acad.  Sci.  8:198- 
265. 

Grubb,  T.  C.,  Jr.  1976.  Adaptiveness  of  foraging  in  the  Cattle  Egret.  Wilson  BuU.  88:145- 
148. 

Hafez,  E.  S.  E.  1964.  Behavioral  thermoregulation  in  mammals  and  birds.  Int.  J.  Biome- 
teorology 7:231-240. 

Hancock,  J.  and  H.  Elliott.  1978.  The  herons  of  the  world.  London  Editions,  London, 
England. 

Hartwick,  E.  B.  1976.  Foraging  strategy'  of  the  Black  Oystercatcher  Haematopus  bach- 
mani.  Can  J.  Zool.  54:142-155. 

Hoffman,  R.  D.  1978.  The  diets  of  herons  and  egrets  in  southwestern  Lake  Erie.  Pp.  365- 
369  in  Wading  birds  (A.  Sprunt,  IV,  J.  C.  Ogden  and  S.  Winckler,  eds.).  Natl.  Audubon 
Soc.,  New  York,  New  York. 

Jenni,  D.  a.  1969.  A study  of  the  ecology  of  four  species  of  herons  during  the  breeding 
season  at  Lake  Alice,  Alachua  County,  Florida.  Ecol.  Monogr.  39:245-270. 

Kahl,  M.  P.  1963.  Thermoregulation  in  the  Wood  Stork,  with  special  reference  to  the  role 
of  the  legs.  Physiol.  Zool.  36:141-151. 

. 1964.  Food  ecology  of  the  W ood  Stork  (Mycteria  americana)  in  Florida.  Ecol.  Mono- 
gr. 34:97-117. 

. 1966.  Comparative  ethology  of  the  Ciconiidae.  Pt.  1.  The  Marabou  Stork,  Leptop- 

tilos  crumeniferus  (Lesson).  Behaviour  26:76-106. 

. 1972.  Comparative  ethology-  of  the  Ciconiidae.  Pt.  4.  “Typical  storks”  (genera 

Ciconia,  Sphenorhynchus,  Dissoura,  and  Euxenura).  Z.  Tierpsychol.  30:225-252. 

AND  L.  J.  Peacock.  1963.  The  bill-snap  reflex:  a feeding  mechanism  in  the  American 

Wood  Stork.  Nature  199:505-506. 

Kale,  H.  W.,  II.  1965.  Nesting  predations  by  herons  in  a Georgia  heronry.  Oriole  30:69- 
70. 

Krebs,  J.  R.  1974.  Colonial  nesting  and  social  feeding  as  strategies  for  exploiting  food 
resources  in  the  Great  Blue  Heron  (Ardea  herodias).  Behaviour  51:99-134. 

Kushlan,  j.  a.  1972.  Aerial  feeding  in  the  Snowy  Egret.  Wilson  Bull.  84:199-200. 

. 1973a.  Black-crowned  Night  Heron  diving  for  prey.  Florida  Field  Nat.  1:27-28. 

. 1973b.  Bill  vibrating — a prey  attracting  behavior  of  the  Snowy  Egret,  Leucophoyx 

thula.  Am.  Midi.  Nat.  89:509-512. 

. 1974.  Effects  of  a natural  fish  kiU  on  the  water  quality,  plankton,  and  fish  population 

of  a pond  in  the  Big  Cypress  Swamp,  Florida.  Trans.  Am.  Fish.  Soc.  103:235-243. 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


161 


. 1976a.  Site  selection  for  nesting  colonies  by  the  American  White  Ibis  Eudocirnus 

albus  in  Florida.  Ibis  118:590-593. 

. 1976b.  Wading  bird  predation  in  a seasonally-fluctuating  pond.  Auk  93:464^76. 

. 1976c.  Feeding  rhythm  in  nestling  White  Ibis.  Wilson  Bull.  88:656-658. 

. 1977a.  Differential  growth  of  body  parts  in  the  White  Ibis.  Auk  94:164-167. 

. 1977b.  Growth  energetics  of  the  White  Ibis.  Condor  79:31-36. 

. 1977c.  Sexual  dimorphism  in  the  White  Ibis.  Wilson  Bull.  89:92-98. 

. 1977d.  The  significance  of  plumage  colour  in  the  formation  of  feeding  aggregations 

of  ciconiiforms.  Ibis  119:361-364. 

. 1977e.  Foraging  behavior  of  the  White  Ibis.  Wilson  BuU.  89:342-345. 

. 1978a.  Feeding  ecology  of  wading  birds.  Pp.  249-296  in  Wading  birds  (A.  Sprunt, 

IV,  J.  C.  Ogden  and  S.  Winckler,  eds.).  Natl.  Audubon  Soc.,  New  York,  New  York. 

. 1978b.  Commensalism  in  the  Little  Blue  Heron.  Auk  95:677-681. 

. 1978c.  Nonrigorous  foraging  by  robbing  egrets.  Ecology  59:649-653. 

. 1979a.  Foraging  ecology  and  prey  selection  in  the  White  Ibis.  Condor  81:376-389. 

. 1979b.  Prey  choice  by  tactile-foraging  wading  birds.  Proc.  Colonial  Waterbird 

Group  3:133-142. 

AND  M.  S.  Kushlan.  1975.  Food  of  the  White  Ibis  in  southern  Florida.  Florida  Field 

Nat.  3:31-38. 

AND  W.  B.  Robertson,  Jr.  1977.  White  Ibis  nesting  in  the  lower  Florida  Keys. 

Florida  Field  Nat.  5:41-42. 

, J.  C.  Ogden  and  A.  L.  Higer.  1975.  Relation  of  water  level  and  fish  availability 

to  Wood  Stork  reproduction  in  the  southern  Everglades.  U.S.  Geol.  Survey,  Open-file 
Rept.,  Tallahassee,  Florida. 

Lack,  D.  1954.  The  stability  of  the  heron  population.  Br.  Birds  47:111-119. 

. 1966.  Population  studies  of  birds.  Oxford  Univ.  Press,  Oxford,  England. 

Leck,  C.  F.  1972.  Seasonal  changes  in  feeding  pressures  of  fruit  and  nector-feeding  birds 
in  the  neotropics.  Condor  74:54-60. 

MacArthur,  R.  H.  1972.  Geographical  ecology.  Harper  and  Row,  New  York,  New  York. 

AND  E.  PlANKA.  1966.  On  optimal  use  of  a patchy  environment.  Am.  Nat.  100:603- 

609. 

McClure,  H.  E.  1974.  Migration  and  survival  of  the  birds  of  Asia.  Bangkok,  Thailand,  U.S. 
Army  Medical  Component,  SEATO  Medical  project. 

Meyerriecks,  a.  j.  1960.  Comparative  breeding  behaviour  of  four  species  of  North  Amer- 
ican herons.  Publ.  Nutt.  Ornithol.  Club  2. 

. 1962.  Diversity  typifies  heron  feeding.  Nat.  Hist.  71(6):48-59. 

Milstein,  P.  le  S.,  I.  Prestt  and  A.  A.  Bell.  1970.  The  breeding  cycle  of  the  Grey 
Heron.  Ardea  58:171-257. 

Monson,  G.  1951.  Great  Blue  Heron  killed  by  bobcat.  Wilson  Bull.  63:334. 

North,  P.  M.  1979.  Relating  Grey  Heron  survival  rates  to  winter  weather  conditions.  Bird 
Study  26:23-28. 

Ogden  J.  C.,  J.  A.  Kushlan  and  J.  T.  Tilmant.  1976.  Prey  selectivity  by  the  Wood  Stork. 
Condor  78:324-330. 

, AND . 1978.  The  food  habits  and  nesting  success  of  Wood  Storks  in 

Everglades  National  Park  in  1974.  Nat.  Resour.  Rept.  16.  U.S.  National  Park  Service, 
Washington,  D.C. 

Owen,  D.  F.  1955.  The  food  of  the  heron  Ardea  cinerea  in  the  breeding  season.  Ibis  97:276- 
295. 

. 1960.  The  nesting  success  of  the  heron  Ardea  cinerea  in  relationship  to  the  avail- 
ability of  food.  Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  London  133:597-617. 


162 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Palmer,  R.  S.  1962.  Handbook  of  North  American  birds.  Vol.  1.  Yale  Univ.  Press,  New 
Haven,  Connecticut. 

Recher,  H.  F.  and  J.  a.  Recher.  1969a.  Comparative  foraging  efficiency  of  adult  and 
immature  Little  Blue  Herons  (Florida  caerulea).  Anim.  Behav.  17:300-322. 

AND . 1969b.  Comments  on  the  escape  of  prey  from  avian  predators.  Ecology' 

49:560-562. 

AND . 1972.  The  foraging  behavior  of  the  reef  heron.  Emu  72:85-90. 

Reynolds,  C.  M.  1979.  The  heronries  census:  1972-77  indices  and  a review.  Bird  Study 
26:7-12. 

Robins,  J.  D.  1970.  Feeding  behavior  of  several  aerial  foragers  after  an  extended  rainy 
period.  Trans.  Kansas  Acad.  Sci.  73:434-438. 

Russell,  J.  K.  1978.  Effects  of  interspecific  dominance  among  egrets  commensaUy  follow- 
ing Roseate  Spoonbills.  Auk  95:608-610. 

Ryder,  R.  A.  1967.  Distribution,  migration  and  mortality  of  the  White-faced  Ibis  (Plegadis 
chihi)  in  North  America.  Bird-Banding  38:257-275. 

. 1978.  Breeding  distribution,  movements  and  mortality  of  Snowy  Egrets  in  North 

America.  Pp.  197-205  in  Wading  birds  (A.  Sprunt,  IV,  J.  C.  Ogden  and  S.  Winckler, 
eds.).  Natl.  Audubon  Soc.,  New  York,  New  York. 

ScHOENER,  T.  W.  1971.  Theory  of  feeding  strategies.  Ann.  Rev.  Ecol.  Syst.  2:369^04. 

Siegfried,  W.  R.  1971.  Communal  roosting  of  the  Cattle  Egret.  Trans.  R.  Soc.  S.  Africa 
39:419^43. 

. 1972.  Food  requirements  and  growth  of  Cattle  Egrets  in  South  Africa.  Living  Bird 

11:193-206. 

. 1978.  Habitat  and  the  modern  range  expansion  of  the  Cattle  Egret.  Pp.  315-324  in 

Wading  birds  (A.  Sprunt,  IV,  J.  C.  Ogden  and  S.  Winckler,  eds.).  Natl.  Audubon  Soc., 
New  York,  New  York. 

Stafford,  J.  1971.  The  heron  population  of  England  and  Wales,  1928-1970.  Bird  Study 
12:218-221. 

Steen,  I.  and  J.  B.  Steen.  1965.  The  importance  of  the  legs  in  the  thermoregulation  of 
birds.  Act.  Physiol.  Scand.  63:285-291. 

Taylor,  R.  J.  and  E.  D.  Michael.  1971.  Predation  on  an  inland  heronry  in  eastern  Texas. 
Wilson  Bull.  83:172-177. 

Teal,  J.  M.  1965.  Nesting  success  of  egrets  and  herons  in  Georgia.  Wilson  BuU.  77:257- 
263. 

Thompson,  D.  H.  1978.  Feeding  areas  of  Great  Blue  Herons  and  Great  Egrets  nesting 
within  the  flood  plain  of  the  upper  Mississippi  River.  Proc.  Conf.  Colonial  Waterbird 
Group  2:202-213. 

Urban,  E.  K.  1974.  Breeding  of  Sacred  Ibis  Threskiornis  aethiopica  at  Lake  Shala,  Ethiopia. 
Ibis  116:263-277. 

Werschkul,  D.  F.  1979.  Nestling  mortality  and  the  adaptive  significance  of  early  loco- 
motion in  the  Little  Blue  Heron.  .\uk  96:116-130. 

W ILLARD,  D.  E.  1977.  The  feeding  ecology  and  behavior  of  five  species  of  herons  in  south- 
eastern New  Jersey.  Condor  79:462^70. 

Wolford,  J.  W.  and  D.  A.  Boag.  1971.  Food  habits  of  Black-crowned  Night  Herons  in 
Alberta.  Auk  88:435-437. 

WooLFENDEN,  G.  E.,  S.  C.  WHITE,  R.  L.  Mumme  AND  W.  B.  Robertson,  Jr.  1976. 
Aggression  among  starving  Cattle  Egrets.  Bird-Banding  47:48-53. 


Kushlan  • WADING  BIRD  RESOURCE  USE 


163 


NATIONAL  PARK  SERVICE,  SOUTH  FLORIDA  RESEARCH  CENTER,  P.O.  BOX 
279,  HOMESTEAD,  FLORIDA  33030.  ACCEPTED  30  JULY  1980. 


Erratum. — Vol.  92,  No.  4,  “Intersexual  niche  partitioning  in  Downy  Woodpeckers”  by 
Joseph  B.  Williams.  Fig.  3,  p.  446,  should  be  disregarded  and  the  figure  appearing  on  p.  444 
substituted.  Fig.  3 caption  remains  the  same.  The  figure  printed  below  should  be  substituted 
for  Fig.  2,  p.  444.  Fig.  2 caption  remains  the  same. 


HART  UPLAND 


HEIGHT  (M) 


Wilson  Bull..  93(2i.  1981.  pp.  164-188 


AGE  RATIOS  AND  THEIR  POSSIBLE  USE  IX 
DETERMINING  AUTUMN  ROUTES  OE 
PASSERINE  MIGRANTS 

C.  John  R\lph 

A principal  interest  of  early  students  of  passerine  migration  was  the 
determination  of  direction,  location  and  width  of  migratory  routes.  In  such 
studies,  it  was  presumed  that  an  area  where  the  species  was  most  abun- 
dant was  the  main  migration  route.  However,  during  fall,  passerine  mi- 
grants tend  to  be  silent  and  inconspicuous,  rendering  censusing  subjective 
at  best.  Species  also  differ  in  preferred  habitat,  affecting  the  results  of 
censuses  and  the  number  captured  by  mist  netting.  In  this  study.  I used 
the  abundance  of  migrants  with  another  possible  criterion,  their  age  ratios, 
in  order  to  hypothesize  possible  migratory  routes. 

Based  upon  information  about  species  abundance  in  different  areas,  a 
lively  debate  sprang  up  in  the  past  between  a school  favoring  narrow  routes 
and  one  advocating  broad  front  migration.  The  former  suggested  that  birds 
followed  topographical  features  (’‘leading  lines”)  such  as  river  valleys, 
coast  lines  and  mountain  ranges  (Baird  1866.  Palmen  1876.  inkenwer- 
der  1902.  Clark  1912.  Schenk  1922).  The  latter  group  proposed  that  a 
species  migrated  over  a broad  geographical  area  regardless  of  topograph- 
ical features  (Gatke  1895:  Cooke  1904.  1905;  Geyr  von  Schweppenburg 
1917.  1924:  Moreau  1927).  Thompson  (1926)  and  later  Lincoln  (1935).  sug- 
gested that  both  schools  were  probably  right,  depending  upon  the  species 
involved.  Early  ornithologists  were  possibly  misled  because  of  the  differ- 
ences between  easily  observable  (and  often  narrow  front),  diurnal  migra- 
tion and  less  obvious,  but  probably  more  common,  nocturnal  movements. 
A species  could  participate  in  both  and  yet  be  considered  only  a narrow 
front,  diurnal  migrant. 

In  the  last  50  years,  the  dichotomy  was  apparently  resolved  with  narrow 
routes  being  generally  ascribed  to  diurnal  migrants  following  topographical 
features,  and  broad  routes  ascribed  to  migrants  moving  at  night,  ignoring 
topographical  features  (Moreau  1961.  Dorst  1962).  This  view,  however,  is 
by  no  means  unanimous  (cf.  Van  Dobben  1935.  Deelder  1949).  Indeed. 
Phillips  (1951).  King  et  al.  (1965).  Clench  (1969)  and  Leberman  and  Clench 
(1975)  have  suggested  that  populations,  as  well  as  different  age  and  sex 
classes  of  some  nocturnal  migrants.  foUow  different  routes. 

On  the  basis  of  radar  observations.  Drury  and  Keith  (1962)  have  divided 
fall  nocturnal  land  bird  migrants  on  the  Atlantic  coast  into  2 groups.  They 
suggest  that  most  migrate  southwest  on  a broad  front  over  land,  while  a 
few  species  flv  over  water  from  the  Atlantic  coast  of  North  America  to 

164 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


165 


South  America.  Among  the  passerine  migrants,  only  the  Blackpoll  Warbler 
{Dendroica  striata)  is  known  to  use  this  second  route  (Nisbet  et  al.  1963, 
Nisbet  1970). 

The  ''coastal  effect d' — Interest  in  age  ratios  of  birds  captured  during 
fall  migration  has  focused  on  the  85-95%  incidence  of  young  of  most 
species  on  the  Atlantic  coast  (Robbins  et  al.  1959,  Drury  and  Keith  1962, 
Murray  1966),  as  compared  to  65-70%  inland  (Nisbet  et  al.  1963;  Barry 
1970;  Leberman  and  Clench  1972,  1973).  Similarly,  high  numbers  of  young 
prevail  on  the  Pacific  coast  (Ralph  1971,  Stewart  et  al.  1974).  I shall  refer 
to  this  high  percentage  of  young  as  the  “coastal  effect.”  In  Europe,  despite 
much  fieldwork,  the  coastal  effect  is  reported  only  in  passing  by  William- 
son (1959)  and  Evans  (1968). 

King  et  al.  (1965:497),  Barry  (1970)  and  Leberman  and  Clench  (1975:10) 
have  suggested  that  the  age  classes  follow  different  routes,  the  adults 
inland,  the  young  along  the  coast.  In  view  of  the  substantial  number  of 
young  inland,  even  in  species  with  a coastal  effect,  this  interpretation 
must  be  incorrect  for  most  species.  Most  young  migrate  inland  (Ralph 
1975).  An  alternate  hypothesis,  that  high  percentages  of  young  denote  the 
periphery  of  a species’  migration  route,  is  a major  thesis  of  this  paper. 

Use  of  age  ratios. — The  rationale  for  this  latter  hypothesis  is  as  follows. 
I assume  that  a species’  main  routes  are  adaptive  and  take  the  birds 
through  the  most  congenial  habitats  to  the  most  salubrious  wintering 
grounds.  Assume  then  that  individuals  straying  from  main  routes  (see 
Ralph  1978)  will  suffer  a higher  mortality  rate  than  those  following  them. 
The  next  year,  strays  either  will  have  perished  or  perhaps  have  learned 
a more  appropriate  orientation.  Therefore,  by  their  second  fall  migration, 
relatively  few  individuals  should  be  wandering  from  the  mainstream  of  the 
routes.  Those  individuals  at  the  periphery  of  routes  should  be  almost  en- 
tirely young  birds  on  their  first  trip. 

If  the  coast  (with  high  percentages  of  young)  is  the  edge  of  a route,  then 
areas  with  relatively  low  percentages  of  young  should  represent  the  actual 
routes  that  the  species  used.  In  this  study,  I document  age  ratios  and 
abundances  of  migrants  and  recognize  5 main  patterns. 

Age  ratios  from  more  than  1 site  have  been  compared  in  the  past,  giving 
a geographical  perspective  (Drury  and  Keith  1962;  Nisbet  et  al.  1963; 
Johnson  1965,  1970,  1973;  Stewart  et  al.  1974;  Robbins  1976).  These  au- 
thors had  few  data  from  sites  at  any  sufficient  distance  from  coasts,  and 
none  postulated  routes  based  on  age  ratio  data. 

MATERIALS  AND  METHODS 

Sites  and  species  involved. — Data  were  obtained  from  records  of  mist  netting  and  collec- 
tions of  nocturnal  migrants  killed  by  colliding  with  man-made  structures  (Table  1 and  Fig. 
1).  Certain  sites  in  Massachusetts  (Fig.  2)  are  located  on  or  near  the  Cape  Cod  Peninsula, 


Table  1 

Continued 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


167 


o 

o 

o 

"§  s 

c 

OJ  3 

=1 

oi 

m ^ 

OJ 

c« 

-Q  -Q 

3 

3 

£ 

^ 3 

1 1 

crl 

£ 

V 

-3 

<u 

;-l 

3 

3 

-3  O 

cn  ^ 

< 

'£ 

OS 

^ C/) 

O 

Dh 

z 

5 > 
H ^ 

o m 

o 

c 


£ 

2 

^ e 

a .S 


eg  ^ 
.2 
I 

o V 

D, 

On 


x:  >1 

f— I 4J 

r-  a. 

Ch 


> 

o 

Z 

Cl-  LO 

T s, 

CO  o; 
r-  c/i 
as 


a 
a ^ 


esg 

os 


"O 

Si 

'2 

CQ 


-o 

3 


-o 

3 


O 

SO 

I 


o 

so 

I 


-rj  o; 
3 
a} 

r-  ^ 

3 

^ I 

(U  Os 

-Q  ^ 

(U  ^ 


LO 

eg 

I 


,o 


S .2 

O 3 
*“  JS 


■O 

• S -2, 

I E 


3 

1) 


Oh 


O 

o 

<u 


cc: 


V ^ 

^ 2 
c •£ 


+ •< 


168 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


1"IG.  1.  Map  of  the  northeast  United  States  showing  principal  sources  of  data  outside  of 
Massachusetts.  Shaded  areas  indicate  mountainous  regions,  and  the  dotted  line  indicates 
the  western  boundary  of  the  coastal  plain. 


which  extends  some  130  km  out  into  the  Atlantic.  In  this  study,  I consider  the  Monomoy 
station  to  be  130  km  beyond  the  main  coast  line,  Manomet  approximately  60  km  out,  and 
Boston  on  the  coast  line.  Carlisle  lies  at  the  junction  of  the  coastal  plain  and  the  Appalachians 
and  perforce  should  usually  yield  age  ratios  and  abundances  intermediate  between  those 
characteristic  of  coastal  plain  stations  and  those  of  Rector,  the  station  on  the  western  side 
of  the  .Appalachians.  1 considered  only  those  species  with  samples  of  N ^ 20  in  at  least  2 
locations.  On  this  basis,  data  for  61  species  of  autumn  migrants  (out  of  a possible  150+)  were 
available,  representing  42,219  individuals.  The  61  species  comprised  90-95%  of  the  passerine 
migrants. 

rhe  period  of  fall  migration  was  considered  to  be  from  1 August  to  mid-November.  Use 
of  this  time  period  helped  to  minimize  bias  caused  by  including  local  residents.  Some  post- 
breeding wandering  occurring  in  early  .August  is  not  “true"  migration,  and  usually  involves 
relatively  few  birds.  Such  movements  are  overshadowed  by  true  fall  migration  (Ralph,  un- 
publ.).  Furthermore,  many  migratory  movements  begin  as  early  as  the  first  week  in  August 
and  should  be  included.  Migration  occurring  after  mid-November  involves  relatively  few 
individuals  (Leberman  and  Clench  1972,  1973,  1975). 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


169 


Fig.  2.  Map  of  the  stations  located  in  Massachusetts.  The  dotted  line  indicates  the  coast 
as  it  is  considered  in  this  paper.  Stations  to  the  east  were  considered  coastal  stations,  and 
those  to  the  west  were  considered  coastal  plain  stations. 


Age  ratios,  abundances  and  timing  of  migration. — Age  was  usually  determined  by  assess- 
ing degree  of  skuU  ossification  (see  Miller  1946,  Norris  1961).  This  technique  could  be  reliably 
employed  through  at  least  mid-November.  Age  was  also  determined  in  a few  species  on  the 
basis  of  diagnostic  plumage  characters  (e.g..  White-crowned  Sparrows  [Zonotrichia  leuco- 
phrys]}. 

Significance  levels  between  percentages  of  young  were  calculated  using  an  arcsine  trans- 
formation (Sokal  and  Rohlf  1969:607-8).  1 examined  the  year-to-year  variability  of  age  ratio 
data  at  Rector,  Pennsylvania,  and  found  that  two-thirds  of  the  species  vary  by  less  than  10% 
annually  with  regard  to  percentage  of  young  in  the  sample  (Ralph  1975).  After  2 years  of 
data  collection,  more  than  95%  of  the  age  ratios  of  all  species  are  within  5%  of  a long-term 
(e.g.,  10  years)  average.  I have  at  least  2 years  of  data  for  all  but  2 stations;  the  average  was 
3.3  years.  For  rarer  species  at  Rector  and  Manomet,  data  from  additional  years  (so  labeled 
in  Table  1)  were  used. 

To  compare  species  abundances  between  sites,  I determined  the  percentage  each  species 
comprised  of  the  total  number  of  all  passerine  birds  at  that  location.  At  most  stations  abun- 
dance values  were  calculated  on  the  basis  of  previously  uncaught  birds,  in  order  to  help 
eliminate  residents.  At  Rector  and  Manomet,  abundance  values  were  based  only  on  those 
years  in  which  data  from  all  species  were  tabulated. 


Table  2 

Assigned  Routes,  Percentage  of  Young,  Numbers  and  Percentage  of  Total  of  Passerine  Migrants 


170 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


c75  '=  a 


^ 2 


^ 2 


_ 'fat 

4.  = 

5 p 


CO 

CO 

CO 

X 

X 

CO 

NO 

CO 

CO 

ON 

X 

c^ 

X 

o 

X 

o 

o 

CO 

CM 

r- 

X 

r- 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

o 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

o 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

CM 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

d 

q 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

On 

Csl 

0^ 

CM 

X 

X 

X 

CM 

X 

CM 

ON 

NO 

r- 

CM 

00 

CO 

On 

00 

CM 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

ON 

X 

CO 

NO 

CM 

X 

X 

CM 

CM 

X 

t-- 

o 

O 

o 

O 

o 

lO 

o 

o 

1 

X 

NO 

o 

X 

CM 

X 

o 

CO 

lO 

o 

NO 

X 

r- 

X 

X 

X 

X 

o 

00 

ON 

iOn 

o 

o 

CO 

On 

ON 

ON 

ON 

c^ 

X 

ON 

X 

r- 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

CG 

C/) 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

1 

* 

+ 

* 

* 

* 

* 

+ 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

Z 

Z 

* 

o 

O' 

uO 

rt 

CM 

X 

X 

CM 

On 

lO 

CO 

CM 

LO 

CM 

X 

CM 

Tj- 

CM 

CM 

X 

r- 

o 

O 

o 

X 

o 

Tf 

o 

CM 

o 

c 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

O 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

lO 

r- 

NO 

X 

X 

ON 

NO 

X 

o 

o 

o 

c- 

ON 

CM 

X 

X 

X 

ON 

NO 

X 

Tj< 

o 

CM 

NO 

X 

CM 

Tt 

X 

CM 

00 

CO 

o 

h- 

X 

X 

X 

1 

X 

O 

X 

r- 

CM 

>o 

CO 

NO 

1 

nO 

o 

o 

o 

CM 

X 

o 

nO 

nO 

1 

nC 

3 

r- 

X 

X 

X 

X 

ON 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

d 

d 

* 

1 

1 

1 

C/i 

* 

c/) 

* 

1 

* 

1 

1 

* 

* 

1 

* 

1 

1 

+ 

Z 

* 

* 

* 

Z 

* 

* 

1 

* 

* 

1 

* 

1 

ON 

CO 

o 

00 

o 

On 

CM 

X 

CM 

o 

X 

00 

CO 

NO 

X 

CM 

CM 

X 

o 

o 

O 

o 

CO 

o 

X 

X 

q 

q 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

q 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

Tf< 

nC 

On 

rf> 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CM 

X 

X 

o 

CM 

CO 

CM 

CM 

X 

CM 

X 

X 

nO 

■rf> 

CM 

X 

CM 

o^ 

nO 

X 

X 

CM 

c^ 

X 

1 

00 

ON 

X 

X 

X 

nO 

1 

X 

1 

r- 

1 

1 

r- 

1 

lO 

nO 

nO 

X 

•rf> 

1 

1 

X 

1 

- * 

^ — * 

' — ' 

' ' 

' ' 

' ' 

* 

1 

* 

1 

’J'j 

IT) 

* 

* 

C/} 

* 

1 

* 

1 

CG 

1 

* 

* 

1 

* 

* 

1 

Z 

Z 

* 

* 

z 

* 

1 

* 

* 

1 

Z 

1 

lO 

Tf 

nO 

ON 

X 

2 

CM 

nO 

o 

2; 

CM 

CM 

CO 

s 

X 

ON 

o 

CM 

O 

o 

§ 

X 

X 

o 

X 

o 

Tf 

q 

8 

q 

— ■ 

— ' 

— 

sC 

LO 

CO 

CO 

X 

On 

X 

CM 

On 

X 

00 

Tt 

CM 

X 

CM 

X 

CM 

CM 

CM 

rf 

CM 

X 

X 

X 

CM 

CM 

CO 

0^ 

CO 

On 

CM 

CM 

X 

nO 

X 

ON 

o 

CO 

CO 

ON 

X 

ON 

nO 

00 

X 

1 

3 

nC 

nC 

lO 

r- 

q 

q 

X 

X 

q 

1 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

=== 

d 

o 

3 

3 

LO 

3 

X 

X 

X 

X 

ON 

X 

CM 

CM 

CM 

o 

2 

o 

X 

r- 

o 

nO 

X 

q 

X 

nO 

X 

d 

d 

d 

-J 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

'X  O 

d 

d 

d 

- a. 


i ^ S 3 
> B & I 

-r  .Si  fcjj  ^ 

- i -I 

is 


o 2 

■r  cH 


C 

CTj 


2 — 
c ^ 2 


f 7T.  = ^ 


i-  c5  ^ 


0;  i: 
3 


^ 53 

=<  t CO 


— — 
i _c 

II 


^ i :h  I - -r 


33  C 

m Di  u 


^ cj 


e*  I 

3 S S3 

a,  :0  C; 


a. 

CO 


S3 

a. 

V ^ 

>■  C tiL 


£,  ^ 


X "5  3 

D£  CO 


cfi  a 

2 Cd 
CJ 


Tentative  coastal  migrant 

Bl^^^k-capped  Chickadee  0.00  0.898  303  0.0390  — _ 13  0.0032  — 0.923  562  0.0362  * 0.946  4282  0.1443 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


171 


CM 

w 

hJ  -S 

m - 

< o 

H o 


0^  c 

5 E 


CO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CM 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CM 

CO 

CO 

CM 

fO 

CM 

r- 

ON 

CO 

CM 

ON 

LO 

o 

ON 

CM 

CO 

CM 

CM 

ON 

o 

LO 

VO 

CO 

LO 

CM 

o 

o 

VO 

o 

CO 

O 

r- 

CO 

o 

o 

CM 

o 

O 

O 

O 

o 

CM 

O 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

q 

O 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o' 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

Os 

so 

Os 

CM 

LO 

CO 

ON 

CM 

vO 

o 

On 

00 

CM 

vO 

Os 

r- 

ON 

O 

LO 

o 

o 

VO 

ON 

o 

CO 

CO 

o 

CM 

vO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CO 

lO 

00 

CM 

o 

O 

VO 

ON 

CM 

LO 

VO 

CO 

vO 

00 

r— 

ON 

CO 

00 

LO 

Tf 

CM 

so 

rf 

CM 

t}< 

CO 

Tf- 

CO 

vO 

o 

CO 

00 

CM 

•NTf 

00 

sO 

ON 

Os 

Os 

On 

On 

ON 

00 

r- 

00 

CO 

c^ 

O 

ON 

On 

00 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

C/) 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

■X- 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

o 

CO 

CM 

CO 

o 

VO 

ON 

LO 

o 

CM 

CM 

lO 

r- 

CO 

CO 

VO 

CO 

00 

CO 

LO 

ON 

r- 

CM 

On 

00 

CO 

ON 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

00 

CO 

CM 

LO 

CO 

CO 

r- 

LO 

o 

r- 

CM 

o 

LO 

CO 

o 

Csl 

CM 

O 

o 

o 

00 

LO 

CM 

On 

VO 

00 

LO 

LO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CO 

CM 

(M 

o 

LO 

CO 

VO 

o 

Tj- 

CM 

r- 

CO 

o 

CO 

CO 

CM 

Ov 

LO 

lO 

On 

CM 

o 

O 

00 

r- 

CO 

CO 

c- 

lO 

r- 

LO 

Tf 

LO 

LO 

vO 

vO 

VO 

00 

r- 

r- 

LO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

* 

1 

C/i 

* 

* 

c/o 

1 

1 

1 

* 

C/] 

1 

* 

C/i 

* 

* 

* 

1 

Z 

Z 

* 

* 

* 

Z 

1 

1 

1 

* 

* 

Z 

1 

* 

Z 

CO 

CM 

LO 

ON 

lO 

ON 

LO 

o 

rt 

o 

CM 

o 

■rf 

o 

CO 

CM 

CM 

e- 

VO 

CM 

LO 

r- 

CM 

vO 

Tf 

CM 

VO 

VO 

O 

o 

O 

Tf* 

o 

o 

O 

O 

o 

CO 

O 

O 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

O 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

so 

CO 

o 

CM 

LO 

CO 

CO 

CM 

o 

vO 

Tf 

o 

ON 

LO 

On 

VO 

lO 

lO 

i-H 

CO 

CM 

CM 

CO 

t-H 

CM 

■rf 

VO 

CM 

lO 

CM 

VO 

CO 

VO 

o 

CO 

LO 

r- 

CO 

CO 

1 

VO 

VO 

1 

1 

CM 

1 

CM 

CO 

Os 

VO 

1 

e- 

vO 

t-- 

VO 

VO 

1 

SO 

1 

q 

00 

1 

VO 

vO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

* 

* 

* 

* 

* 

C/] 

1 

* 

* 

1 

+ 

* 

* 

* 

* 

-\- 

* 

1 

* 

1 

* 

* 

1 

1 

* 

* 

Z 

* 

CO 

CO 

LO 

CO 

o 

LO 

CM 

r- 

ON 

CO 

vO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

r- 

CO 

CO 

CM 

O 

00 

CM 

r- 

00 

CSI 

lO 

o 

o 

o 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

CM 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

q 

o 

q 

q 

o 

q 

q 

o 

q 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

VO 

CM 

r- 

r- 

CM 

CO 

o 

o 

00 

e- 

00 

LO 

r- 

VO 

c- 

vO 

o 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CO 

r- 

CO 

1^ 

LO 

LO 

CO 

LO 

00 

o 

so 

CM 

I— H 

CM 

LO 

CM 

CO 

CM 

1— 1 

vO 

Os 

CM 

r- 

o 

LO 

CO 

LO 

00 

CM 

o 

LO 

CM 

CO 

VO 

CM 

CO 

CM 

CO 

CM 

e- 

1 

CM 

CM 

00 

On 

00 

ON 

00 

On 

00 

00 

c^ 

q 

00 

1 

00 

O 

On 

CO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

c- 

vO 

CM 

lO 

LO 

r- 

Tf* 

o 

o 

00 

lO 

LO 

CO 

CM 

VO 

LO 

00 

LO 

CO 

r- 

LO 

00 

00 

q 

o 

00 

00 

00 

ON 

LO 

q 

iOn 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

^ £ 


J2 

■2  c 

CO  03 

CJ  « 

l-i 

>>  1) 
2 S 
O < 


_c 

2 ^ 

-O  C 

o £ 
^ X 


jS  T3 

^ g 

s I J 

> CQ  CQ 


JJ  PQ 
V "2  "o 

3 « 2 

§ 

^ ^ 

1 

3 


bC  O 
cO  cO 

^ s 


3 

o3  “O 

^:s 
J S 

X 6 


2 £ 

-C  r£ 

i I 

C c 

s 2 

_C  3 
u £ 

o o 

Z U 


1 


American  Redstart  0.79  0.849  523  0.0123  **  0.685  47  0.0115  NS  0.653  406  0.0261  ***  0.968  721  0.0243 

Scarlet  Tanager  0.90  0.900  251  0.0082  ***  0.714  65  0.0160  NS  0.702  45  0.0029  ***  0.938  113  0.0038 

Rose-breasted  Grosbeak  0.77  0.774  124  0.0061  NS  0.732  80  0.0197  NS  0.589  22  0.0014  **  0.899  50  0.0017 

Rufous-sided  Towhee  0.21  0.791  492  0.0108  ***  0.418  62  0.0152  ***  0.709  170  0.0109  ***  0.910  304  0.0102 


Table  2 

Continued 


T 


172 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


■=  SO 


# 3 


lO  sC 
lC  fO 


I I 


I I I 

I I I 


S 2 

o 


CO  fO  fO  fO  CO 


O' 

Tf" 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

CM 

Tt 

X 

3 

r- 

X 

1 00 

X 

X 

X 

CO 

O) 

3 

3 

O) 

1 2 

3 

o^ 

O^ 

Os 

3 

3 

d 

3 

3 

3 

3 

3 

3 

3 

d: 

I I 


I 1 O'  lO  I 

I I ®®.  I 

I + 


I I 

X 


X CM  ^ LO 

■■‘XX 

X 


Tt  O'  CM  ^ X 
X lO  CM  O'  •rj' 

— 


X X ^ X O' 

o O'  r-  O'  X 

X r-  lo  X X 


Xj  ct. 

2"  2^ 


O't'^CMXX  — 
^ X O X ^ 


3 nC  O sC  Tf  O 

O'  X O'  X X X 

X 3 X Tt 


Tf-  — sC  X CM 


C/^ 


X 

X 

X 

sC 

X 

lO 

3 

X 

X 

CM 

X 

CM 

O; 

CM 

CM 

3 

3 

3 

3 

3 

o 

3 

o 

3 

3 

3 

3 

3 

X 

I 

I 


I I 


I I 


Os 

3 

X 

X 

X 

CM 

CM 

lO 

X 

f 

O' 

3 

Os 

X 

CM 

OS 

X 

CM 

X 

X 

X 

r- 

CM 

X 

Tj- 

XXX 

X 

X 


5 c 


y) 


c:  3^ 

2 ^ 
■“ 

E ^ 


a.  a 2-  ’E 

^ a y) 

3;  ~ k.  >-  U 

u ? "i  - “ 


“1^=7; 

1 = ^5  -2  y 

u 4/  a. 

X 
< 


3j 

a 

^ u "O 

^ 4;  X,  !-  c: 

j a.  25  -i 


: ^ 

rt  _c  _E 


^ E 


« 3.  a 


Z ^ 3 


(ft  _ 

3 (ft 

5 

2 s 

!alj  ^ 


O 


X 

y) 


-r  w 
E ^ 
-2  ai 
2 — 
X 
< 


4;  'X  ~ 

2 ^ I 

_4;  2 eft 

ii 

^ ^ IE 

m ^ 


- -ox  P = 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


173 


CSI 

U 

hJ 

CQ 

< 


.Is 

-o 
c 


oj 

a 

ft 

< s 

■■S 


^ 03  OJ 

I It 

o Q.  ft 

<!  _c 


CM 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

lO 

CO 

o 

CO 

CM 

CO 

CM 

CO 

iOn 

O 

§ 

o 

NO 

o 

o 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

O' 

CO 

ON 

NO 

LO 

NO 

CO 

LO 

LO 

o 

CM 

CM 

lO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

o 

O' 

O) 

ON 

ON 

00 

r- 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

C/i 

c/) 

CD 

* 

* 

Z 

Z 

Z 

* 

* 

o 

CO 

CM 

CO 

CO 

On 

o 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

Tf 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

NO 

CM 

NO 

ON 

CM 

LO 

NO 

LO 

NO 

O 

CO 

CM 

NO 

r- 

o 

CM 

CO 

CM 

o 

o 

O' 

CO 

CO 

NO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

c/:) 

1 

CD 

CD 

1 

Z 

1 

Z 

Z 

1 

NO 

CM 

CO 

r^ 

CO 

CM 

NO 

CO 

o 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

LO 

O' 

NO 

CO 

CO 

CM 

LO 

O 

CM 

CO 

1 

1 

C^ 

1 

r- 

00 

1 

d 

d 

d 

1 

1 

CD 

CD 

1 

1 

1 

Z 

Z 

1 

O' 

r- 

o 

CM 

CO 

Tt 

CO 

00 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

CO 

CM 

o 

ON 

CO 

o 

o 

CM 

CO 

'~' 

LO 

CM 

CM 

CM 

o 

CO 

O 

LO 

o 

ON 

lO 

CM 

CO 

O' 

CO 

CO 

O) 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

O' 

CO 

CM 

00 

CO 

NO 

ON 

ON 

CO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

a 

fcH  03  3 

, 

^ o ^ a; 

S ^ :h  "S 


CD  4- 
I CD 

c S 

-Q 

4^  " "o  -C  ^ 

C/:  (J  U CQ 


_Q 

O ^ i)  c« 

— EZ  ft3  a « 


S -S 

^ -2 

'z:  03 

J ^ 

<1  'o 

r 

V s 


O rH  Csj 
CM  CO 

o o o 
o o o 


CM  CM  CM 

CO  I— I 

0^  O'  O' 


^ U^I 

z z ^ 


r^  Tf*  ON 
-— I CM  r-H 

o o o 
o o o 


NO 

CO  O' 
CO  CO  CO 


r-  CM 
O CO  CO 
a>  o CD 
o o o 


I I I 
I I I 


NO  ^ CO 

o o o 
o o o 


CO  CM  CO 
CO  NO 

CO 


CM  CO 
>Ol  NO 
O CO 


■="  o 

C/5  ”0 

.«  o 

O _Q 

4- 

ft  o 
S c 

c«  -a 

■s 

3 O 
o o 


o3  ^ 
-C 

3 -S 

5 ^ 


- c s 
S C ^ 

^ -Q  -2 
O "2  ft 

•=  « 3 
JS  J= 
^ m cj 


II 

CT)  '' 


(/)  ^ 

<b 

— <v 


5 

u 2 


. bfi  c c/3 

o ® 013 

O V ^ 3 

bt)  ^ ^ 

^11  4.^  ^ > 

c * «3  £ ^ 

o * C 

5 

£ ° ^ i ® 

« V i'  -"o 
S = -^  m 
•-  ™ 
^ Qoa 


CQ 


1 


174 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Each  netting  station  differed  from  others  in  vegetation,  location  of  nets,  net  height  and 
operation  schedule.  These  variables  affect  the  capture  rate  of  certain  species;  they  probably 
do  not  significantly  affect  the  age  composition.  Certainly  the  pooling  of  data  from  several 
sites  and  years  tends  to  reduce  potential  biases  from  this  source.  At  2 stations,  Carlisle  and 
Ashby,  it  was  possible  to  use  techniques  previously  described  (Ralph  1976)  to  correct  the 
number  of  captures  for  varying  number  of  nets  and  time  of  operation.  This  method  provides 
a somewhat  more  accurate  data  set,  but  does  not  alter  the  comparisons  between  stations. 

To  define  nocturnal  and  diurnal  migrants,  an  index  to  the  diel  timing  of  migration  was 
determined  from  data  taken  at  Long  Point  Bird  Observatory,  Ontario  (Bradstreet  and  Wood- 
ford 1970,  Woodford  1970).  In  this  analysis  (Ralph  1975),  a ratio  was  calculated  between  the 
natural  logarithms  of  the  number  taken  during  nocturnal  flight  at  a lighthouse  and  the  number 
taken  during  the  day  at  netting  and  trapping  operations.  The  Ovenbird  {Seiurus  aurocapillus) 
had  the  highest  ratio  (1.02),  with  1142  at  the  lighthouse  and  1005  caught  in  nets  and  traps. 
Most  Ovenbirds  apparently  migrate  at  night.  A value  approaching  0.00  indicated  the  most 
diurnal  of  the  migrants  (species  with  no  individuals  at  the  lighthouse  were  assigned  the  value 
0.00). 

Assignment  of  routes. — I regarded  a station  to  be  in  the  mainstream  of  a species’  migratory 
route  if,  compared  to  other  stations:  (1)  adults  of  that  species  were  more  abundant  (i.e.,  the 
percentage  of  young  was  lower),  and  (2)  the  species  made  up  a higher  percentage  of  the  total 
catch  of  passerine  birds.  Conversely,  a station  with  a relatively  high  percentage  of  young 
and  low  abundance  of  that  species  was  considered  to  be  on  the  periphery  of  the  species’ 
route.  These  determinations  are,  of  necessity,  subjective.  Three  factors  were  weighed  in 
making  the  determinations:  (1)  the  relative  magnitude  of  differences  between  age  ratios  and 
abundances  at  different  stations,  (2)  the  level  of  significance  of  differences  between  stations, 
and  (3)  the  sample  sizes  involved.  I gave  greater  weight  to  age  ratios  than  to  abundances, 
since  I assumed  that  there  would  be  more  site  bias  in  the  abundance  of  a given  species. 

RESULTS 

The  coastal  effect. — Fifty-two  of  59  (88.1%)  species  (N  ^ 20)  at  coastal 
sites  (Table  2)  had  a higher  proportion  of  young  on  the  coast  than  at  the 
nearest  inland  (coastal  plain  or  Carlisle)  location  with  adequate  data.  Of 
these,  40  species  had  a significantly  {P  ^ 0.05)  higher  percentage  of  young 
than  the  age  ratio  at  the  inland  location. 

Patterns  of  age  ratios  and  abundances. — Data  in  Table  2 were  grouped 
according  to  distance  from  the  coast  into  4 general  regions.  The  route 
taken  by  a species  could  encompass  one  or  more  regions.  I outline 
below  the  5 patterns  of  age  ratios  and  abundances  that  emerged 
in  the  area  under  consideration.  Additional  data  from  other  areas,  when 
available,  will  undoubtedly  alter  these  assignments.  No  attempt  was  made 
to  integrate  the  information  on  “known’'  migratory  routes  from  other  pub- 
lications, as  these  are  usually  based  on  somewhat  subjective  information. 

(1)  Possible  broad  front  migrants. — A species  migrating  through  the 
northeastern  United  States  in  a general  southwesterly  direction,  regardless 
of  topography,  is  considered  to  be  a broad  front  migrant.  According  to  my 
hypothesis,  the  percentage  of  young  should  be  higher  and  abundance  lower 
on  the  coast  than  inland.  Twelve  species  appeared  to  ht  this  pattern, 
20.0%  of  the  total  (Table  2). 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


175 


Three  additional  species  were  tentatively  assigned  to  this  pattern,  the 
Ruby-crowned  Kinglet  {Regulus  calendula).  Yellow  Warbler  {Dendroica 
petechia)  and  Song  Sparrow  {Melospiza  rnelodia).  Skull  ossification  is  com- 
pleted in  this  kinglet  somewhat  earlier  than  in  most  species  (Leberrnan 
1970),  potentially  biasing  age  data.  Therefore,  kinglet  data  from  Carlisle, 
mostly  from  later  in  the  migratory  period,  were  excluded.  The  Yellow 
Warbler  (an  early  migrant)  was  uncommon  at  all  sites,  so  I disregarded  its 
absence  at  Carlisle  and  classified  it  in  the  broad  front  pattern.  A high 
percentage  of  young  in  Song  Sparrows  prevailed  until  Carlisle,  but  the 
species  was  abundant  on  the  coastal  plain. 

(2)  Possible  coastal  migrants. — A species  concentrating  its  migration 
along  the  coast  would  be  more  abundant  and  have  a lower  percentage  of 
young  there  than  on  the  coastal  plain  and  inland.  Only  the  Red-breasted 
Nuthatch  {Sitta  canadensis)  showed  this  pattern  clearly  (Table  2). 

The  Black-capped  Chickadee  [Parus  atricapillus)  also  probably  follows 
this  route  through  the  northeast.  Although  it  had  high  percentages  of  young 
at  all  sites,  its  exceptional  abundance  (14%  of  the  total)  on  the  coast, 
suggests  this  is  the  main  path.  Chickadee  populations  frequently  irrupt, 
and  first-year  birds  are  the  ones  that  move  (Ralph,  unpubl.),  so  that  they 
would  predominate  at  all  sites.  In  contrast,  the  nuthatch,  also  an  irruptive 
species,  had  substantial  numbers  of  adults  in  the  coastal  migration. 

The  Tennessee  Warbler  {Vermivora  peregrina)  appears  to  use  both  the 
coast  and  the  coastal  plain.  However,  the  high  percentage  of  young  (94.9%) 
and  great  abundance  at  Rector,  making  up  2.3%  of  the  total  catch  of  all 
species,  suggest  that  this  warbler  probably  has  a major  route  to  the  west. 

(3)  Possible  coastal  plain  migrants. — This  category  includes  those 
species  that  would  avoid  both  the  coast  and  the  Appalachian  Mountains. 
Such  species  would  be  most  common  and  have  the  lowest  percentage  of 
young  on  the  coastal  plain.  They  would  decline  in  abundance  and  increase 
in  percentage  of  young  on  the  coast  and  also  at  Carlisle  on  the  boundary 
of  the  coastal  plain  and  the  Appalachians  (and  certainly  at  Rector).  Twenty 
species  (32.8%)  were  assigned  to  this  group. 

An  additional  3 species  were  tentatively  regarded  as  coastal  plain  mi- 
grants. The  Brown  Creeper  {Certhia  farniliaris)  showed  a relatively  low 
percentage  of  young  at  Rector,  indicating  it  may  fit  the  broad  front  pattern, 
but  its  high  abundance  at  the  coast,  coupled  with  the  relatively  low  per- 
centage of  young  on  the  coastal  plain,  suggests  that  its  route  may  lie  in 
the  latter  area.  The  Yellow-rumped  Warbler  [Dendroica  coronata)  was 
common  at  Rector,  but  had  a significantly  [P  < 0.01)  higher  percentage 
of  young  there  than  on  the  coastal  plain.  This  higher  incidence  at  Rector, 
in  addition  to  the  relative  scarcity  of  this  species  at  Carlisle,  indicated  its 
pattern  fits  the  coastal  plain  one.  The  Savannah  Sparrow  [Passerculus 
sandwichensis)  is  more  abundant  on  the  coast  than  the  coastal  plain,  but 


176 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


the  low  percentage  of  young  at  the  latter  location  would  indicate  its  route 
is  on  the  coastal  plain. 

(4)  Possible  Appalachian  and  west  migrants. — If  a route  of  a species  lay 
in  and  to  the  west  of  the  Appalachians,  one  would  expect  the  species  to 
be  most  common  and  have  the  lowest  percentage  of  young  at  Rector,  on 
the  western  side  of  the  Appalachians.  Towards  the  coast,  the  percentage 
of  young  would  increase  either  at  Carlisle  or  the  coastal  plain,  and  the 
species  would  decline  in  abundance.  Fourteen  species  (23.0%)  appeared 
to  fit  this  pattern.  Six  species  (possibly  7)  apparently  had  routes  west  of 
Carlisle.  Three  (possibly  4),  had  a pattern  indicating  a route  west  of  Rec- 
tor. 

Five  additional  species  were  tentatively  considered  to  have  a route  west 
of  Rector.  These  were  species  having  equally  high  percentages  of  young 
at  all  stations.  The  House  Wren  {Troglodytes  aedon)  seemingly  belonged 
to  this  group,  with  a possible  route  through  the  region  including  Carlisle, 
where  the  lowest  percentage  of  young  (53.4%)  was  found.  At  Rector,  the 
percentage  of  young  was  significantly  higher  (87.3%),  indicating  that  this 
wren  might  concentrate  its  route  in  the  Appalachians,  although  this  cannot 
be  determined  from  available  data. 

The  Golden-crowned  Kinglet  [Regulus  satrapa)  had  its  lowest  percent- 
age of  young  at  Rector,  suggesting  that  its  main  migration  route  is  from 
here  to  the  west.  However,  nowhere  were  very  high  percentages  of  young 
found,  and  the  species  may  actually  be  a broad  front  migrant.  As  with  the 
Ruby-crowned  Kinglet,  many  young  birds  had  ossified  skulls  early  in  the 
season,  so  age  ratios  are  not  reliable,  further  compounding  the  problem. 

(5)  Possible  western  Atlantic  migrants  (the  Blackpoll  route). — Blackpoll 
Warblers,  after  apparently  pausing  on  the  eastern  coastal  plain,  leave 
North  America  and  head  south  over  water  to  tropical  x\merica  (Nisbet 
1970).  Their  main  route  thus  crosses  the  coast,  explaining  the  relatively 
low  percentages  of  young  at  the  coast  and  on  the  eastern  coastal  plain. 
The  lowest  percentage  of  young  was  on  the  coastal  plain  rather  than  the 
coast  (Table  2).  This  might  be  expected  because  most  individuals  appar- 
ently begin  their  long  flight  somewhat  inland  (Nisbet  et  al.  1963;  Richard- 
son, in  press).  Most  species  (70.4%)  of  migrants  wintering  south  of  the 
United  States  have  more  than  90%  young  at  the  coast  (Table  2).  However, 
a few  species,  like  the  blackpoll,  had  relatively  low  percentages  of  young 
(<85%)  at  the  coast.  I will  consider  these  species  with  those  that  Drury 
and  Keith  (1962)  suggested  as  potential  overwater  migrants  (Table  3).  They 
based  their  list  on  subjective  impressions  of  density  at  Bermuda. 

Young  make  up  79.8%  of  the  blackpolls  on  the  coast,  more  than  10% 
below  the  average  for  all  warbler  species  (90.4%).  Of  the  species  with  80% 
young  along  the  coast,  only  2 are  on  Drury  and  Keith’s  (1962)  list  (Table 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


177 


Table  3 

Possible  Western  Atlantic  Migrants,  their  Assigned  Routes,  the  Percent 
Young  Found  at  Coastal  Stations,  and  the  Number  Killed  at  TV  Towers  in 
Florida  and  Tennessee  in  the  Autumn 

Species 

Assigned  route 

Wc  young 
on  coast 

Number 

in 

Florida'^ 

Number 

in 

Tennessee^ 

Palm  WarbleP 

Coastal  Plain 

98.4 

1944 

60 

American  Redstart' 

Coastal  Plain 

96.8 

1099 

229 

Yellow  Warbler' 

Tent.  Broad  Front 

93.2 

63 

11 

Ovenbird' 

Coastal  Plain 

92.5 

1128 

2140 

Myrtle  Warbler' 

Tent.  Coastal  Plain 

92.4 

1006 

16 

Black-and-white  Warbler' 

Coastal  Plain 

86.8 

502 

362 

Northern  Waterthrush' 

Coastal  Plain 

84.4 

484 

110 

Veery 

Coastal  Plain 

84.3 

668 

3 

Bay-breasted  Warbler 

App.  and  W. 

84.3 

157 

409 

Parula  Warbler' 

Coastal  Plain 

80.7 

1041 

8 

BlackpoU  Warbler' 

W.  Atlantic 

79.8 

11 

20 

Eastern  Wood  Pewee 

App.  and  W. 

79.3 

34 

3 

Common  YeUowthroat' 

Coastal  Plain 

78.2 

3477 

97 

Tennessee  Warbler 

Coastal  and  Coastal  Plain 

66.0 

331 

1242 

‘ Species  in  Drur\  and  Keith  (1%2). 
^ Sources  given  in  text. 


3)  and  4 are  not.  Five  of  Drury  and  Keith’s  species — Palm  {Dendroica 
palmarum)^  Yellow-rumped  and  Yellow  warblers,  American  Redstart  {Se- 
tophaga  ruticilla)  and  Ovenbird — have  more  than  90%  young  along  the 
coast.  In  view  of  the  strong  coastal  effect  which  these  5 species  show,  it 
seems  unlikely  that  they  follow  a western  Atlantic  route. 

Species  using  primarily  an  overwater  route  would  be  expected  to  be 
rare  in  the  southeastern  U.S.  in  migration.  Indeed,  the  BlackpoU  Warbler 
largely  bypasses  the  southern  states  in  migration  (Nisbet  1970).  TV  tower 
kills  in  Florida  (Stoddard  and  Norris  1967,  Taylor  and  Anderson  1973)  and 
in  Tennessee  (Laskey  1969a,  b)  show  that  of  the  species  in  Table  3,  only 
Eastern  Wood  Pewee  [Contopus  virens),  BlackpoU  and  Yellow  warblers 
are  apparently  rare  in  both  areas.  The  pewee  may  actually  be  more  com- 
mon but  may  not  be  susceptible  to  nocturnal  accidents.  The  Veery  {Ca- 
tharus  fuscescens),  Yellow-rumped  and  Parula  {Parula  americana)  war- 
blers, relatively  uncommon  in  Tennessee,  are  common  in  Florida  kiUs. 
Their  principal  route  might  lie  to  the  east  of  Tennessee. 

Abundance  in  Bermuda  would  be  a good  indicator  of  birds  flying  be- 
tween the  northeast  and  South  America,  since  this  island  group  is  almost 
midway  in  this  flight.  Adults  should  comprise  a substantial  percentage  of 


178 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  4 

Percentage  of  Young,  and  the  Number  Caught,  of  Warblers  and  Vireos 
Captured  in  Bermuda  during  the  Autumn 

Species 

Percent 

young 

Number 

caught 

Species 

Percent 

young 

Number 

caught 

Black-and-white  Warbler 

96.4 

28 

Bay-breasted  Warbler 

66.7 

3 

Worm-eating  Warbler 

100.0 

6 

Blackpoll  Warbler 

44.4 

234 

Swainson’s  Warbler 

100.0 

1 

Prairie  Warbler 

100.0 

2 

Prothonotarv'  Warbler 

100.0 

9 

Palm  Warbler 

100.0 

9 

Blue-winged  Warbler 

100.0 

1 

Ovenbird 

89.5 

19 

Tennessee  Warbler 

92.3 

13 

Northern  Waterthrush 

100.0 

30 

NashviUe  Warbler 

83.3 

6 

Kentucky  Warbler 

100.0 

1 

Orange-crowned  Warbler 

100.0 

2 

Connecticut  Warbler 

0.0 

2 

Parula  Warbler 

85.7 

21 

Mourning  Warbler 

100.0 

1 

Yellow  Warbler 

100.0 

5 

Common  YeUowthroat 

100.0 

24 

Magnolia  Warbler 

100.0 

23 

Yellow-breasted  Chat 

100.0 

2 

Cape  May  Warbler 

100.0 

5 

Hooded  Warbler 

100.0 

8 

Black-th.  Blue  Warbler 

100.0 

8 

Wilson’s  Warbler 

100.0 

1 

Myrtle  Warbler 

100.0 

1 

Canada  Warbler 

100.0 

1 

Black-th.  Green  Warbler 

100.0 

5 

American  Redstart 

98.8 

87 

Blackburnian  Warbler 

100.0 

4 

Red-eyed  Vireo 

100.0 

43 

Chestnut-sided  Warbler 

100.0 

6 

Philadelphia  Vireo 

100.0 

4 

birds  passing  through  there.  Records  of  warblers  and  vireos  from  occa- 
sional mist  netting  in  Bermuda  by  J.  Baird  and  D.  Wingate  (pers.  comm.) 
show  that  only  the  blackpoll  is  both  common  and  represented  by  high 
numbers  of  adults  (Table  4).  Thus,  it  would  appear  that  only  the  blackpoll, 
among  passerines,  uses  an  overwater  route  as  its  major  pathway. 

Undetermined  routes. — It  was  not  possible  to  determine  the  route 
taken  by  the  White-breasted  Nuthatch  {Sitta  carolinensis),  Blackburnian 
Warbler  {Dendroica  fusca)  and  Chipping  Sparrow  {Spizella  passerina). 
Too  few  data  were  available  for  age  ratio  assessments,  and  no  coastal 
effect  was  found.  Perhaps  additional  capture  data  would  clarify  the  type 
of  routes  for  these  3 species. 

Association  of  routes  with  other  species  characteristics. — To  deter- 
mine if  particular  routes  were  associated  with  the  distance  migrated,  I 
divided  the  species  in  this  study  according  to  the  northernmost  area  in 
which  they  are  found  commonly  in  winter  (Table  2,  last  column):  (1)  the 
central  Atlantic  states,  (2)  the  southeastern  Unites  States,  or  (3)  south  of 
the  United  States.  Of  all  species  for  which  I determined  a route,  nearly 
half  (48.1%)  of  those  wintering  south  of  the  United  States  are  coastal  plain 
migrants  (Table  5).  One-half  of  the  species  wintering  in  the  central  Atlantic 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


179 


Table  5 

Northernmost  Wintering  Area  of  Species  Probably  and  Possibly  (in 

PARENTHESES)  ASSOCIATED  WITH  DIFFERENT  ROUTES 


Route 

Central 

Atlantic 

States 

Southeastern 
United  States 

South  of 
United  States 

Totals 

Broad  front 

8(2) 

0(0) 

4(1) 

12  (3) 

Coastal 

1 (1) 

0(0) 

1 (0)" 

2(1) 

Coastal  Plain 

5(3) 

3(0) 

13  (0)« 

21  (3) 

West  of  Appalachians 

2(1) 

3(1) 

9(0) 

14  (2) 

Western  Atlantic 

0(0) 

0(0) 

1 (0) 

1 (0) 

Undetermined 

1 

1 

1 

3 

Totals 

17  (7) 

7 (1) 

28(1) 

61 

^ One  species  had  its  route  on  both  the  coast  and  the  coastal  plain. 


states  showed  a broad  front  migration  pattern,  while  5 (31.3%)  were  coastal 
plain  migrants. 

Migrant  species  using  the  coastal  plain  tended  to  have  more  individuals 
migrating  at  night  than  those  using  other  routes  (Table  2),  but  not  signif- 
icantly so  {P  > 0.10,  Mann-Whitney  U-test).  The  average  coastal  plain 
migrant  had  a diel  timing  index  value  of  0.717,  while  broad  front  migrants 
averaged  0.575,  and  Appalachian  and  west  migrants  averaged  0.583. 

When  1 grouped  the  migrants  by  taxa  and  compared  their  routes,  more 
than  half  of  the  warblers  (52.4%)  used  the  coastal  plain,  as  did  the  6 thrush 
species  (Table  2).  These  relationships  were  weak,  but  significant  (P  < 
0.05).  No  other  correlations  of  routes  with  taxa  were  found. 

DISCUSSION 

Possible  causes  of  the  coastal  effect. — Evidence  marshalled  in  this  paper 
supports  the  existence  of  the  “coastal  effect”  and  indicates  that  a high 
percentage  of  young  in  a local  sample  indicates  that  a site  is  on  the  edge 
of  the  main  migration  route.  I previously  discussed  some  of  the  hypotheses 
that  have  been  advanced  to  explain  the  coastal  effect  (Ralph  1971).  How- 
ever, additional  published  data,  as  well  as  this  study’s  thorough  docu- 
mentation of  the  phenomenon,  have  shed  new  light  on  the  hypotheses. 
They  fall  into  5 main  categories. 

(1)  Differential  timing  of  migration. — This  hypothesis  explains  the 
coastal  effect  as  an  artifact  of  sampling.  Brewster  (1887),  noting  that  few 
adults  were  collected  in  autumn,  postulated  that  the  post-breeding  adults 
migrated  in  July  before  the  young  and  before  the  collectors  were  active. 
Differences  between  adults  and  young  of  about  30  days  between  peaks  of 


180 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


passage  were  shown  in  the  Least  Flycatcher  {Ernpidonax  minimus)  at  Long 
Point  (Hussell  et  al.  1967,  Ely  1970)  and  in  the  Western  Flycatcher  {E. 
difficilis)  (Johnson  1973,  Stewart  et  al.  1974).  However,  Clench  (1969) 
found  synchronous  migration  in  E.  minimus  at  Rector.  Also,  few  inland 
data  were  available  for  the  Western  Flycatcher,  making  conclusions  nec- 
essarily tentative.  Murray  (1966)  and  Leberman  and  Clench  (1973)  showed 
as  many  as  15  days  difference  at  Rector  in  average  migration  dates  be- 
tween adults  and  young  for  a few  species,  including  the  Red-eyed  Vireo 
(Vireo  olivaceus)  and  White-throated  Sparrow  [Zonotrichia  albicollis).  In 
most  species,  however,  the  age  classes  migrate  more  or  less  in  synchrony. 
Certainly,  in  no  known  case  (with  the  possible  exception  of  the  Western 
Flycatcher)  is  the  migration  asynchronous  enough  to  explain  the  coastal 
effect.  That  is,  it  does  not  appear  that  adults  of  any  species  migrate  so 
early  as  to  be  missed  by  netting  beginning  in  August.  Finally,  the  high 
percentage  of  adults  of  most  species  at  inland  locations  also  precludes 
Brewster’s  (1887)  explanation,  which  would  require  that  young  predomi- 
nate at  both  inland  and  coastal  sites  during  the  latter  part  of  the  migration. 

Alternatively,  perhaps  adults  fly  greater  distances  without  stopping  and 
so  would  not  be  captured  as  often.  This  hypothesis  may  obtain  for  those 
few  species  with  a high  percentage  of  young  at  all  localities,  both  coastal 
and  far  inland,  considered  above  to  be  migrating  west  of  the  Appalachians. 
1 know  of  no  published  discussions  for  or  against  this  hypothesis. 

(2)  Adults  overflying  the  coast. — In  discussing  Blackpoll  Warblers,  Mur- 
ray (1966)  postulated  that  both  adult  and  young  nocturnal  migrants  regu- 
larly stray  offshore.  Both  age  classes  return  to  the  mainland,  with  the 
young  stopping  at  landfall,  whereas  the  adults  fly  farther  inland,  perhaps 
seeking  more  suitable  habitat.  Thus,  the  adults  pass  over  the  coast  but  do 
not  land.  This  hypothesis  requires  that  any  bird  offshore  at  dawn  (1)  have 
enough  energy  reserves,  (2)  have  offshore  winds  slower  than  its  airspeed, 
and  (3)  be  able  to  relocate  land.  1 doubt  whether  a substantial  fraction  of 
birds  offshore  fulfill  these  conditions. 

Kills  of  several  species  of  migrants  at  tall  structures  contradict  Murray’s 
hypothesis.  The  kills  at  the  Prudential  Center  on  the  coast  in  Boston  would 
likely  include  adults  if  they  were  flying  over  the  coast.  However,  90.6% 
of  all  birds  here  were  young  (N  = 427).  At  the  Boylston  TV  tower,  60  km 
inland,  only  46.6%  of  the  kill  were  young  (N  = 682).  Apparently,  the  age 
composition  of  migrants  in  the  airspace  above  the  coast  is  similar  to  that 
on  the  ground.  Adults  are  overflying  the  coast  in  much  lower  proportions 
than  they  are  overflying  inland  locations. 

(3)  Different  routes. — Clench  (1969)  postulated  that  adult  Least  Fly- 
catchers migrate  along  the  Appalachians  (as  1 found),  and  the  young  to  the 
east  and  along  the  coast,  which  was  not  confirmed  by  this  study.  I found 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


181 


5 species  that  might  have  different  routes  used  by  different  age  classes. 
Yellow-bellied  Flycatcher  {Empidonax  flaviventris).  Solitary  Vireo  (Vireo 
solitarius),  and  Cape  May  {Dendroica  tigrina).  Chestnut-sided  {D.  pen- 
sylvanica)  and  Bay-breasted  {D.  castanea)  warblers.  These  species  had 
high  percentages  of  young  at  all  stations  in  the  area  of  study,  suggesting 
that  the  adults  might  have  a route  to  the  west.  However,  data  are  lacking 
that  show  a preponderance  of  adults  at  any  location. 

(4)  Learning. — Drury  and  Keith  (1962)  have  hypothesized  that  young  on 
their  first  autumn  flight  learn  to  avoid  the  hazards  (e.g.,  dehydration  and 
depletion  of  energy  reserves)  of  an  overwater  flight.  As  adults,  they  do  not 
repeat  a route  taking  them  to  the  coast. 

Supporting  this  hypothesis,  migrants  offshore  have  been  observed  by 
radar  to  reorient,  at  times  towards  land  (Myres  1964,  Richardson  1978). 
Indeed,  it  appears  that  there  is  a mechanism  allowing  migrants  with  energy 
reserves  to  regain  land.  Those  lacking  these  reserves  would  perish. 
Whether  the  returning  birds  could  learn  from  their  experience,  and  not 
repeat  their  offshore  flight,  is  problematical. 

Although  the  learning  hypothesis  has  considerable  merit,  it  does  require 
many  individuals  to  experience  an  offshore  flight.  The  hypothesis  may  not 
hold  for  those  beginning  their  flight  near  the  coast  and  going  southeast 
offshore  within  the  first  few  hours  of  night.  (Many  individuals  also  head 
SSW-SW  along  the  shore.)  Since  individuals  offshore  observed  by  radar 
do  not  normally  change  their  direction  during  the  night  (Richardson  1975), 
by  dawn  they  would  be  too  far  out  to  return,  given  normal  energy  reserves 
(e.g.,  Odum  et  al.  1961)  and  a definitely  offshore  direction.  With  the  north- 
east-southwest orientation  of  the  eastern  U.S.  coast,  even  a southward 
orientation  would  lead  a bird  too  far  offshore  to  return.  Emlen  (1969,  1970) 
has  shown  that  the  orientation  of  Indigo  Buntings  {Passerina  cyanea)  is 
defined  at  an  early  age,  and  thus  is  probably  not  easily  modified  by  ex- 
perience. Finally,  nocturnal  migrants  are  often  reported  far  offshore,  in- 
dicating that  some  individuals  persist  in  flying  offshore,  with  fatal  results. 

(5)  Maladaptive  orientation. — Drury  and  Keith  (1962)  suggested  that 
“the  birds  which  live  to  be  adults  are  those  inheriting  a tendency  to  move 
on  courses  which  keep  them  over  the  mainland.”  This  suggests  that  birds 
surviving  to  become  adults  generally  move  on  overland  routes  that  avoid 
the  coast  because  they  have  proper  orientation  (Ralph  1971,  1978),  ade- 
quate compensation  for  wind  drift  (Baird  and  Nisbet  1960)  and/or  begin 
migration  under  conditions  not  leading  to  wind  displacement  from  the 
normal  route  (Evans  1968).  In  contrast,  the  birds  along  the  coast  would  be 
largely  immature  birds  not  possessing  these  abilities,  as  shown  in  Ralph 
(1978).  They  have  been,  or  will  be,  exposed  to  the  hazards  of  overwater 
flight. 


182 


THE  WILSOX  BULLETIX  • Vo/.  93,  Ao.  2,  June  1981 


Some  data  support  this  explanation.  The  literature  abounds  with  ref- 
erences to  land  bird  migrants  at  sea,  apparently  persisting  in  flying  in 
directions  leading  potentially  to  their  death.  These  include  observations 
from  ships  at  sea  (e.g.,  Sprunt  1931,  Buckley  1946,  Kuroda  1955,  Scho- 
lander  1955,  Hubbs  and  Banks  1966,  Jenson  and  Livingstone  1969,  Wil- 
liams et  al.  1977),  on  offshore  islands  (e.g.,  Howell  1959;  Kuroda  1961, 
1964;  Ralph  1968)  and  with  radar  (Williams  et  al.  1977).  Most  critical  to 
this  hypothesis  is  that  these  birds  often  appear  exhausted  and  sometimes 
quite  emaciated.  This  has  been  quantified  on  an  offshore  island  by  Elias- 
sen  and  Hjelmtvedt  (1958)  and  on  the  coast  by  Murray  and  Jehl  (1964)  and 
Ralph  (unpubl.). 

Williams  et  al.  (1977)  provides  evidence  for  offshore  mortality,  and  they 
“.  . . suggest  significant  mortality  for  at  least  some  groups  of  birds.”  All 
of  these  points  argue  strongly  that  many  individuals  do  persist  in  flights 
involving  offshore  directions,  often  with  fatal  conclusions. 

The  first  3 hypotheses  discussed  above  may  well  account  for  some  of 
the  young  along  the  coast.  However,  the  last  two  are  the  most  tenable,  and 
in  my  opinion  the  last  has  special  merit  and  is  probably  the  cause  of  the 
majority  of  young  that  reach  the  coast.  The  learning  and  disorientation 
hypotheses  are  not  necessarily  mutually  exclusive.  Disoriented  young  that 
reach  the  coast  and  survive  might  learn  to  avoid  the  coast  during  subse- 
quent migrations. 

Species  showing  little  or  no  coastal  effect. — My  analysis  suggests  that 
the  Blackpoll  Warbler  is  possibly  the  only  passerine  species  using  a route 
over  the  western  Atlantic.  It  was  the  only  species  with  all  of  the  following 
characteristics:  (1)  relatively  low  percentage  of  young  along  the  coast  and 
in  Bermuda,  (2)  uncommon  in  both  Florida  and  Tennessee,  and  (3)  rela- 
tively abundant  in  Bermuda. 

The  relatively  low  percentages  of  young  along  the  coast  in  7 other 
species  (Table  3),  could  be  explained  in  one  of  two  ways.  One  would  be  that 
the  coast  is  not  actually  the  edge  of  the  route  of  these  species.  These 
apparent  exceptions  to  the  principal  generalization  of  this  study  could  have 
evolved  strategies  allowing  some  individuals  to  survive,  despite  a tendency 
to  migrate  near  or  even  beyond  the  coast.  Richardson's  (1978)  radar  ob- 
servations of  nocturnal  passerines  migrating  off  the  Atlantic  coast  of  Can- 
ada are  particularly  relevant  to  this  point.  In  addition  to  those  flying  over- 
land (southwest  or  west),  some  fly  over  water  and  parallel  the  coast 
(southwest  or  south  southwest).  At  dawn,  with  following  winds,  they  usu- 
ally kept  that  course,  probably  intersecting  shore  near  Virginia.  In  con- 
trast, those  individuals  flying  under  unfavorable  winds  with  a head  or  side 
component  often  changed  direction  and  flew  northwest  toward  the  coast 
during  the  morning  hours.  This  strategy  would  enable  some  of  those  flying 
in  a generally  southwesterly  direction  near  land  to  survive.  Bay-breasted 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


183 


and  Tennessee  warblers  (west  of  the  Appalachian  migrants)  with  relatively 
low  percentages  of  young  at  the  coast  and  no  coastal  effect,  may  use  this 
strategy.  A change  in  flight  direction  may  also  be  in  the  repertoire  of  the 
other  5 long-distance  migrants  with  relatively  low  percentages  of  young 
along  the  coast. 

Determining  routes  of  migration. — Using  age  ratios  and  abundances  of 
birds  at  a given  capture  station  to  hypothesize  routes  of  migration  is  sub- 
ject to  many  potential  sources  of  error.  It  is  not  the  purpose  of  this  paper 
to  explore  them  all,  but  it  is  my  judgment  that  the  effect  of  these  variables 
is  relatively  minor  and  does  not  affect  my  basic  interpretations  of  the 
overall  picture  to  any  significant  degree. 

This  study,  although  essentially  restricted  to  northeastern  North  Amer- 
ica, supports  Thompson’s  (1926)  suggestion  that  some  species  probably 
migrate  over  a broad  front,  while  others  may  follow  more  narrow  paths. 
In  the  area  studied,  43%  of  the  species  apparently  avoid  the  mountains, 
channeling  their  flight  down  the  coastal  plain  or  coast.  This  group  does 
not  appear  to  be  restricted  to  diurnal  migrants,  as  has  been  suggested.  It 
seems  unlikely  that  these  birds  would  fly  over  the  Appalachians  without 
being  detected  there.  Habitats  in  the  Appalachian  Mountains  are  rather 
similar  to  neighboring,  lower  areas,  and  these  species  would  be  expected 
to  land  if  they  were  present.  Few  of  the  coastal  plain  migrants  breed  only 
to  the  north  of  this  area.  Undoubtedly,  other  populations  of  these  species 
also  have  routes  to  the  west  of  the  area  of  this  study. 

Independent  evolution  of  routes. — I had  expected  to  find  strong  rela- 
tionships between  routes  and  other  factors,  such  as  diel  timing  of  migra- 
tion, taxa  and  distance  to  wintering  grounds,  among  others.  Only  weak 
relationships  were  found.  Most  long-distance  migrants  moving  along  the 
coastal  plain  are  insectivorous.  This  route  is  near  the  stabilizing  influence 
of  the  ocean,  perhaps  allowing  a more  dependable  food  source  than  a more 
inland  route.  Even  a slight  temperature  differential  could  be  a selective 
force  if,  for  instance,  early  frosts  occasionally  decimated  insect  popula- 
tions. Those  more  hardy  bird  species  wintering  in  the  central  Atlantic 
states,  by  contrast,  apparently  tended  to  be  broad  front  migrants  in  our 
area,  passing  through  the  slightly  colder  mountains,  as  well  as  the  coastal 
plain. 

The  lack  of  an  association  between  routes  and  other  characteristics  of 
migration  suggests  independent  evolution  of  routes  by  each  species.  It 
also  indicates  that  other  aspects,  such  as  timing  of  migration  (on  both  a 
seasonal  and  daily  basis),  have  also  evolved  independently  to  meet  the 
particular  selective  pressures  acting  on  each  species. 

CONCLUSIONS 

These  data  and  the  resulting  hypotheses  will,  I hope,  stimulate  further 
investigations.  Most  especially,  I would  urge  that  the  following  avenues  of 


184 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


research  and  predictions  be  explored  and  tested:  (1)  Accurate  age  ratio 
data  be  accumulated  from  stations  to  the  south  and  west  of  the  area  in 
this  paper.  Far  too  many  banders  fail  to  ensure  that  95%  of  captured  birds 
are  accurately  aged.  (2)  The  limited  recovery  data  available  on  passerines 
should  be  compiled  and  published,  with  due  regard  to  the  fact  that  recov- 
eries are  usually  of  birds  which  have  been  selected  against.  (3)  If  a banding 
station  is  on  the  edge  of  a migratory  route  of  a species,  individuals  there 
may  be  of  lower  weight,  and  with  less  fat  reserves  than  in  the  hypothesized 
central  part  of  a route.  (4)  If  the  hypothesis  is  correct,  quantitative  data 
from  offshore  ships  and  from  islands,  at  some  distance  from  a “main” 
route,  should  include  records  of  lighter  birds.  (5)  It  is  logical  that  mist  net 
capture  in  an  area  of  a given  species  should  be  an  accurate  assessment 
of  the  amount  of  migration  of  the  species  in  that  area.  In  this  paper  I held 
that,  due  to  site  bias,  they  perhaps  are  not.  Several  stations  or  nets  set 
over  a wider  area  than  is  usually  the  case  should  confirm  or  deny  this 
supposition. 


SUMMARY 

Age  ratios  and  abundance  of  61  migrant  passerine  species  comprising  more  than  42,000 
birds  were  analyzed  in  an  attempt  to  determine  patterns  of  migration.  The  data  were  collected 
at  10  stations  from  coastal  Massachusetts  to  inland  Pennsylvania.  Age  ratios  are  thought  to 
be  useful  in  determining  routes  of  migration.  The  principal  criteria  for  the  edge  of  a species’ 
route  are  suggested  to  be  a higher  proportion  of  young  and  a lower  density  of  the  species  than 
in  other  areas.  Almost  all  species  in  this  area  fell  readily  into  I of  5 patterns  that  suggest  5 
possible  routes:  (I)  immediately  along  the  coast  (3  species);  (2)  on  the  coastal  plain  (24 
species);  (3)  west  of  the  Appalachians  (17  species);  (4)  overwater,  direct  to  South  America 
(only  the  Blackpoll  Warbler);  and  (5)  an  unconfined,  broad  front,  encompassing  the  entire 
area  (14  species).  I’or  3 species,  no  route  could  be  determined. 

Almost  all  species  showed  the  “coastal  effect,”  a higher  percentage  of  young  along  the 
coast  than  elsewhere.  By  the  criteria  given,  this  indicates  that  the  coast  is  the  edge  of  the 
migratory  route  of  most  species.  Most  probably  the  young  found  near  the  coast  lack  some 
navigational  capabilities  and  are  off  course;  many  of  them  probably  perish. 

In  general,  a given  route  was  not  strongly  associated  with  either  diurnal  or  nocturnal 
migration,  distance  to  wintering  grounds,  or  with  any  genus  or  family  of  birds.  I speculate 
that  this  is  evidence  that  routes  have  evolved  independently  in  each  species. 

.ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

rhe  dedicated  help  of  many  persons  made  this  study  possible.  I would  especially  like  to 
acknowledge:  Kathleen  .Anderson,  Joseph  Hagar,  Helen  Passano  and  Ken  Youngstrom  at 
Manomet  Bird  Observatory;  Frederic  and  Evelyn  Davis,  John  Goguen,  Michael  Stopler, 
Timothy  Terrio  and  many  other  volunteers  at  .Ashby  Bird  Observatory;  Ed  and  Iris  Swab, 
Kevin  Hess,  W alt  Tyler,  Carter  .Atkinson,  Dick  and  Meridith  Rhindress,  Sherry  D.  .AUshouse, 
and  many  other  friends  and  students  at  Carlisle;  and  especially  Lois  Vaughn  and  Natalie 
Houghton  Harrington  who  helped  far  above  and  beyond  the  call  of  duty,  both  at  Ashby  and 
Manomet.  Stephen  T.  Emlen  and  Richard  B.  Root  provided  me  with  much  help  during  a 
year  spent  at  Cornell  University.  W . John  Richardson,  Jon  C.  Barlow,  Erica  H.  Dunn,  David 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


18S 


F.  DeSante,  Stephen  T.  Emlen,  L.  Richard  Mewaldt,  Bertram  G.  Murray,  Charles  and 
Sandra  G.  van  Riper,  and  Francis  S.  L.  Williamson  read  drafts  of  this  manuscript  and 
contributed  helpful  advice. 

I received  financial  support  from  the  Frank  M.  Chapman  Memorial  Fund  of  the  American 
Museum  of  Natural  History;  the  National  Institutes  of  Health;  the  Smithsonian  Institution; 
Dickinson  College;  and  from  F.  B.  Bang  and  F.  S.  L.  WiUiamson,  who  managed  to  find  ways 
to  support  me  during  lean  times.  Carol  Pearson  Ralph  helped  as  a volunteer  in  Carlisle,  a 
manuscript  reader  and  friend  throughout  this  study.  Thanks  are  inadequate  for  her  help. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Baird,  J.  1971.  Mortality  of  fall  migrants  at  the  Boylston  television  tower  in  1970.  Chickadee 
40:17-21. 

. 1972.  Mortality  of  birds  at  the  Boylston  television  tower  in  September  1971. 

Chickadee  41:20-23. 

AND  I.  C.  T.  Nisbet.  1960.  Northward  fall  migration  on  the  Atlantic  coast  and 

its  relation  to  offshore  drift.  Auk  77:119-149. 

Baird,  S.  F.  1866.  The  distributions  and  migrations  of  North  American  birds.  Am.  J.  Sci. 
41:78-90,  337-347. 

Barry,  J.  J.  1970.  Differential  fall  migration.  EBBA  News  34:55-56. 

Bradstreet,  M.  S.  W.  and  P.  S.  Woodford.  1970.  Nocturnal  migration.  Pp.  19-23  in 
Long  Point  Bird  Observatory  1960-1969,  ten-year  report  (M.  S.  W.  Bradstreet  and  G. 
L.  Holroyd,  eds.).  Long  Point  Bird  Observatory,  Ontario. 

Brewster,  W.  1887.  Scarcity  of  adult  birds  in  autumn.  Auk  4:268-269. 

Buckley,  J.  L.  1946.  Land  birds  at  sea.  Auk  63:440. 

Clark,  W.  E.  1912.  Studies  in  bird  migration.  Gurney  and  Jackson,  London,  England. 
Clench,  M.  H.  1969.  Additional  observations  in  the  fall  migration  of  adult  and  immature 
Least  Flycatchers.  Bird-Banding  40:238-243. 

Cooke,  W.  W.  1904.  Distribution  ar-d  migration  of  North  American  warblers.  USDA  Biol. 
Surv.  BuU.  18. 

. 1905.  Routes  of  bird  migration.  Auk  22:1-11. 

Deelder,  C.  L.  1949.  On  the  autumn  migration  of  the  Scandinavian  Chaffinch  {Fringilla 
c.  coelebs  L.)  Ardea  37:1-88. 

Dorst,  j.  1962.  The  migrations  of  birds.  Houghton  Mifflin  Co.,  Boston,  Massachusetts. 
Drury,  W.  H.  and  J.  A.  Keith.  1962.  Radar  studies  of  songbird  migration  in  coastal  New 
England.  Ibis  104:449-489. 

Eliassen,  E.  and  I.  Hjelmtvedt.  1958.  The  loss  of  water  in  wind  drifted  migratory  birds. 
Univ.  Bergen  Arbok  Naturv.  Rekke.  Nr.  11. 

Ely,  C.  a.  1970.  Migration  of  Least  and  Traill’s  Flycatchers  in  west-central  Kansas.  Bird- 
Banding  41:198-204. 

Emlen,  S.  T.  1969.  The  development  of  migratory  orientation  in  young  Indigo  Buntings. 
Living  Bird  8:113-126. 

. 1970.  Celestial  rotation:  its  importance  in  the  development  of  migratory  orientation. 

Science  170:1198-1201. 

Evans,  P.  R.  1968.  Reorientation  of  passerine  night  migrants  after  displacement  by  the 
wind.  Br.  Birds  61:281-303. 

Gatke,  H.  1895.  Heliogoland  as  an  ornithological  observatory.  David  Douglas,  Edinburgh, 
Scotland. 

Geyr  von  Schweppenburg,  H.  1917.  Vogelzug  in  der  westlichen  Sahara.  J.  Orn.  Lpz. 
65:43-65. 


186 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


. 1924.  SO-NW  Zugstrasse  durch  die  Sahara.  J.  Orn.  Lpz.  72:102-114. 

Howard,  D.  V.  1967.  Five  years  of  mist  netting  (1962-1966)  at  Round  HiU,  Sudbury,  Massa- 
chusetts. Rec.  New  Eng.  Birds  23:iii-vi. 

Howell,  T.  R.  1959.  Land  birds  from  Clipperton  Island.  Condor  61:155-156. 

Hubbs,  C.  W.  and  R.  C.  Banks.  1966.  Wandering  onto  the  eastern  Pacific  Ocean  of 
an  eastern  North  America  bird,  the  Bay-breasted  Warbler.  Auk  83:680-682. 

Hussell,  D.  J.  T.,  T.  Davis  and  R.  D.  Montgomerie.  1967.  Differential  fall  migration  of 
adult  and  immature  Least  Flycatchers.  Bird-Banding  38:61-66. 

Jenson,  A.  C.  and  R.  Livingstone,  Jr.  1969.  Offshore  records  of  land  birds.  Kingbird 
19:5-10. 

Johnson,  N.  K.  1965.  Differential  timing  and  routes  of  the  spring  migration  in  the  Hammond 
Flycatcher.  Condor  67:423^37. 

. 1970.  Fall  migration  and  winter  distribution  of  the  Hammond  Flycatcher.  Bird- 

Banding  41:169-190. 

. 1973.  Spring  migration  of  the  Western  Flycatcher,  with  notes  on  seasonal  changes 

in  sex  and  age  ratios.  Bird-Banding  44:205-220. 

King,  J.  R.,  D.  S.  Earner  and  L.  R.  Mewaldt.  1965.  Seasonal  sex  and  age  ratios  in 
populations  of  the  White-crowned  Sparrows  of  the  race  garnbelii.  Condor  67:489-504. 

Kuroda,  N.  1955.  Observations  on  pelagic  birds  of  the  northwest  Pacific.  Condor  57:290- 
300. 

. 1961.  The  oversea  crossings  of  land  birds  in  the  western  Pacific.  Misc.  Rept. 

Yamashina's  Inst.  Ornith.  and  Zool.  3:47-53. 

. 1964.  A survey  of  seabirds  of  Teuri  Islands,  Hokkaido,  with  notes  on  land  birds. 

Misc.  Rept.  Yamashina’s  Inst.  Ornith.  and  Zool.  3:363-383. 

Laskey,  A.  R.  1969a.  T.V.  tower  casualties  at  NashviUe  in  autumn  1968.  Migrant  40:25- 
26. 

. 1969b.  Autumn  1969.  T.V.  tower  casualties  at  Nashville.  Migrant  40:79-80. 

Leberman,  R.  C.  1970.  Pattern  and  timing  of  skull  pneumatization  in  the  Ruby-crowned 
Kinglet.  Bird-Banding  41:121-124. 

AND  M.  H.  Clench.  1972.  Bird-Banding  at  Powdermill,  1971.  Res.  Rept.  No.  30, 

Carnegie  Museum. 

AND . 1973.  Bird-Banding  at  Powdermill,  1972.  Res.  Rept.  No.  31,  Carnegie 

Museum. 

and . 1975.  Bird-Banding  at  Powdermill,  1974.  Res.  Rept.  No.  36,  Carnegie 

Museum. 

Lincoln,  F.  C.  1935.  Fhe  migration  of  North  American  birds.  USDA  Circ.  No.  363. 

Miller,  A.  H.  1946.  A method  of  determining  the  age  of  live  passerine  birds.  Bird-Banding 
17:33-35. 

Moreau,  R.  E.  1927.  Migration  as  seen  in  Egypt.  Ibis  (12)3:443^168. 

. 1961.  Problems  of  Mediterranean-Saharan  migration.  Ibis  103a:373-427,  580-623. 

Murray,  B.  G.,  Jr.  1966.  Migration  of  age  and  sex  classes  of  passerines  on  the  Atlantic 
coast  in  autumn.  Auk  83:352-360. 

AND  J.  R.  Jehl,  Jr.  1964.  Weights  of  autumn  migrants  from  coastal  New  Jersey. 

Bird-Banding  35:253-263. 

Myres,  M.  T.  1964.  Dawn  ascent  and  reorientation  of  Scandinavian  thrushes  (Turdus  spp.) 
migrating  at  night  over  the  northeastern  Atlantic  Ocean  in  autumn.  Ibis  106:7-51. 

Nlsbet,  1.  C.  T.  1970.  Autumn  migration  of  the  Blackpoll  warbler:  evidence  for  long  flight 
provided  by  regional  survey.  Bird-Banding  41:207—240. 

,W.  H.  Drury,  Jr.  and  J.  Baird.  1963.  Weight-loss  during  migration.  Pt.l.  Depo- 
sition and  consumption  of  fat  by  the  Blackpoll  Warbler  Dendroica  striata.  Bird-Banding 
34:107-138. 


Ralph  • AGE  RATIOS  AND  PASSERINE  MIGRATION 


187 


Norris,  R.  A.  1961.  A modification  of  the  Miller  method  of  aging  live  passerine  birds.  Bird- 
Banding  32:55-57. 

Odum,  E.  P.,  C.  E.  Connell  and  H.  L.  Stoddard.  1961.  Flight  energy  and  estimated 
flight  ranges  of  some  migratory  birds.  Auk  78:515-527. 

Palmen,  J.  a.  1876.  Uber  die  Zugstrassen  der  Vogel.  Leiden. 

Phillips,  A.  R.  1951.  Complexities  of  migration:  a review.  Wilson  Bull.  63:129-136. 

Ralph,  C.  J.  1968.  Migrants  on  the  Farallon  Islands,  1853-1967.  Pt.  Reyes  Bird  Obs.  Bull. 
9:2-7. 

. 1971.  An  age  differential  of  migrants  in  coastal  California.  Condor  73:243-246. 

. 1975.  Age  ratios,  orientation,  and  routes  of  land  bird  migrants  in  the  northeastern 

United  States.  Sc.D.  thesis,  Johns  Hopkins  Univ.,  Baltimore,  Maryland. 

. 1976.  Standardization  of  mist  net  captures  for  quantification  of  avian  migration. 

Bird-Banding  47:44-47. 

. 1978.  The  disorientation  and  possible  fate  of  young  passerine  coastal  migrants. 

Bird-Banding  49:237-247. 

Richardson,  W.  J.  1975.  Bird  migration  over  southeastern  Canada,  the  western  Atlantic, 
and  Puerto  Rico:  a radar  study.  Ph.D.  thesis.  Cornell  Univ.,  Ithaca,  New  York. 

. 1978.  Reorientation  of  nocturnal  landbird  migrants  over  the  Atlantic  Ocean  near 

Nova  Scotia  in  autumn.  Auk  95:717-732. 

. Autumn  landbird  migration  over  the  western  Atlantic  Ocean  as  evident  from  radar. 

In  Proc.  17th  Int.  Ornithol.  Congr.,  Berlin,  West  Germany.  In  Press. 

Robbins,  C.  S.,  D.  Bridge  and  R.  Feller.  1959.  Relative  abundance  of  adult  male  red- 
starts at  an  inland  and  a coastal  locality  during  fall  migration.  Maryland  Birdlife  15:23- 
25. 

. 1976.  Atlantic  flyway  review — region  V.  N.  A.  Bird  Bander  1:75-77. 

Schenk,  J.  1922.  Bericht  uber  die  ungarischen  Vogelberingungen  in  den  Jahren  1920- 
22.  Aquila  29:65-104. 

ScHOLANDER,  S.  I.  1955.  Land  birds  over  the  western  north  Atlantic.  Auk  72:225-239. 

SoKAL,  R.  R.  AND  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1969.  Biometry.  W.  H.  Freeman  & Co.,  San  Francisco, 
California. 

Sprunt,  a.,  Jr.  1931.  Certain  land  birds  observed  at  sea.  Auk  48:133-134. 

Stewart,  R.  M.,  L.  R.  Mewaldt  and  S.  Kaiser.  1974.  Age  ratios  of  coastal  and  inland 
fall  migrant  passerines  in  central  California.  Bird-Banding  45:46-57. 

Stoddard,  H.  L.,  Sr.  and  R.  A.  Norris.  1967.  Bird  casualties  at  a Leon  County,  Florida 
T.V.  Tower.  BuU.  Tall  Timbers  Res.  Sta.  No.  8. 

Taylor,  W.  K.  and  B.  H.  Anderson.  1973.  Nocturnal  migrants  killed  at  a central  Florida 
T.V.  tower:  autumns  1969-1971.  Wilson  Bull.  85:42-51. 

Thompson,  A.  L.  1926.  Problems  of  bird-migration.  Houghton  Mifflin  Co.,  Boston,  Mas- 
sachusetts. 

Van  Dobben,  W.  H.  1935.  Vogelstrek  over  Nederland  H.  Org.  Club  Ned  Vogelk.  7:143- 
173. 

Williams,  T.  C.,  J.  M.  Williams,  L.  C.  Ireland  and  J.  J.  Teal.  1977.  Autumn  bird 
migration  over  the  western  North  Atlantic  Ocean.  Am.  Birds  31:251-267. 

W illiamson,  K.  1959.  The  September  drift-movements  of  1956  and  1958.  Br.  Birds  52:334- 
377. 

WiNKENWERDER,  H.  A.  1902.  The  migration  of  birds  with  special  reference  to  nocturnal 
flight.  Bull.  Wisconsin  Nat.  Hist.  Soc.  2:177-263. 

Woodford,  P.  S.  1970.  Banding  summary.  Pp.  5-11  in  Long  Point  Bird  Observatory  1960- 
1969,  ten-year  report  (M.  S.  W.  Bradstreet  and  G.  L.  Holroyd,  eds.).  Long  Point  Bird 
Observatory,  Ontario. 


188 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Appendix  A 

Scientific  Names  of  Species  in  Tables  Not  Mentioned  in  Text 


Eastern  Phoebe  {Sayornis  phoebe) 

Blue  Jay  {Cyanocitta  cristata) 

Gray  Catbird  {Durnetella  carolinensis) 
Brown  Thrasher  (Toxostoma  rufurn) 
American  Robin  {Turd us  migratorius) 
Wood  Thrush  {Catharus  mustelina) 
Hermit  Thrush  (C.  guttata) 

Swainson’s  Thrush  (C.  ustulata) 
Gray-cheeked  Thrush  (C.  minima) 
Philadelphia  Vireo  {Vireo  olivaceus) 
Black-and-white  Warbler 
{Mniotilta  varia) 

Prothonotary  Warbler 
{P rot onot aria  citrea) 

Swainson’s  Warbler 

{Limnothylypis  swainsonii) 
Worm-eating  Warbler 
{Helmitheros  vermivorus) 

Nashville  Warbler 

{V ermivora  ruficapilla) 

Blue-winged  Warbler  {V.  pinus) 
Orange-crowned  Warbler  {V.  celata) 
Magnolia  Warbler  (Dendroica  magnolia) 
Black-throated  Blue  Warbler 
{!).  nigrescens) 

Black-throated  Green  Warbler 
{!).  virens) 


Prairie  Warbler  {D.  discolor) 

Northern  Waterthrush 
{Seiurus  noveboracensis) 

Kentucky  Warbler  (Oporornis  formosus) 
Mourning  Warbler  (O.  Philadelphia) 
Connecticut  Warbler  (O.  agilis)  \ 

Common  YeUowthroat  ' 

{Geothlypis  trie  has)  i 

Yellow-breasted  Chat  {Icteria  virens)  | 

Wilson’s  Warbler  (Wilsonia  pusilla)  i 

Hooded  Warbler  {W.  citrina) 

Canada  Warbler  {W.  canadensis) 

Scarlet  Tanager  {Piranga  olivacea) 
Rose-breasted  Grosbeak 
(Pheucticus  ludovicianus) 

Purple  Finch  {Carpodacus  purpureas) 
American  Goldfinch  {Spinus  tristis) 
Rufous-sided  Towhee 

{Pipilo  erythrophthalmus) 

Dark-eyed  Junco  (Junco  hyemalis) 

Field  Sparrow  (Spizella  pusilla) 

Fox  Sparrow  (Passerella  iliaca) 

Lincoln’s  Sparrow  {Melospiza  lincolnii) 
Swamp  Sparrow  (M.  georgiana) 


INSTITUTE  OF  PACIFIC  ISLANDS  FORESTRY,  U.S.  FOREST  SERVICE,  1151 
PUNCHBOWL  STREET,  HONOLULU,  HAWAII  96813.  ACCEPTED  8 JULY 
1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981.  pp.  189-195 


WEATHER,  MIGRATION  AND  AUTUMN  BIRD 
KILLS  AT  A NORTH  ELORIDA  TV  TOWER 

Robert  L.  Crawford 

Most  accounts  of  nocturnal  accidents  to  migrating  birds  emphasize  oc- 
casional large  kills  and  the  bewildered  behavior  of  birds  encountering 
towers  and  lights  in  fog  and  overcast  weather  (Weir  1976,  Avery  et  al. 
1978).  Tower  kills  are  rarely  compared  to  weather  and  migration  on  a daily 
basis  (Avery  et  al.  1977),  probably  because  few  towers  are  checked  for 
dead  birds  regularly.  Possibly  unique  in  this  respect  is  the  WCTV  tower 
in  northwest  Florida  where  daily  checks  have  been  made  since  1955  (Stod- 
dard 1962,  Stoddard  and  Norris  1967,  Crawford  1974).  This  paper  uses 
data  from  the  WCTV  tower  to  determine  the  influence  of  weather  on 
autumn  bird  kills  at  the  WCTV  tower  and  relates  these  to  other  migration 
studies  in  the  southeastern  United  States. 

METHODS 

Stoddard  and  Norris  (1967)  described  the  308  m WCTV  tower  and  its  14-ha  cleared  site 
located  on  Tall  Timbers  Research  Station,  Leon  Co.,  Florida.  Data  on  file  at  Tall  Timbers 
include  daily  logs  of  dead  birds  (primarily  passerines)  and,  for  the  years  1955-1967,  maps 
of  the  tower  grounds  locating  the  dead  birds  as  they  were  found  on  most  mornings  with  ^ 
5 birds.  1 classified  each  night  (24  August-15  November,  1962-1966)  for  the  presence  or 
absence  of  north  winds,  clouds  and  rain.  Local  climatological  data  sheets  (U.S.  Dept.  Com- 
merce) for  Tallahassee,  Florida,  33  km  SW  of  the  WCTV  tower  provide  readings  of  sky 
cover,  wind  and  rain  at  1-  or  3-h  intervals  for  each  night  (dates  herein  are  the  mornings  on 
which  the  birds  were  found;  weather  readings  used  w'ere  at  19:00  and  22:00  the  day  before 
and  at  01:00,  04:00  and  07:00  the  morning  of  the  kill).  Cloud  data,  expressed  in  tenths  of  sky 
covered  by  aU  types  of  clouds,  were  averaged;  nights  with  a value  of  ^4  were  classified 
cloudy  except  that  aU  nights  with  rain  were  considered  cloudy.  Nights  with  ^2  wind  readings 
>270°  and  <90°  1 classified  for  the  north  winds.  Groups  of  nights  with  different  weather  con- 
ditions were  compared  for  their  numbers  of  birds  with  a Kruskal-Wallis  test  followed  by 
I Dunn’s  procedure  for  nonparametric  multiple  comparisons  (HoUander  and  Wolfe  1973:115- 
120,  125)  with  significance  at  the  P < 0.05  level. 

RESULTS 

In  the  5 autumn  periods  (N  = 420  nights),  8123  birds  were  killed  (Table 
1).  Nights  with  north  winds  (N  = 313,  74.5%)  accounted  for  6744  birds 
(83%)  and  nights  with  south  winds  (N  = 107,  25.5%)  had  1379  dead  birds 
(17%).  Two  hundred  and  fourteen  nights  were  classified  cloudy  (50.9%) 
and  these  accounted  for  6686  birds  (82.3%);  the  206  clearer  nights  had 
only  1437  birds  killed  (17.7%).  Birds  were  killed  on  all  but  32  of  the  420 
nights  (7%);  one-half  of  these  were  south-wind  nights  (although  only  one- 
quarter  of  the  nights  were  classified  for  south  winds)  and  22  (68%)  were 


189 


190 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  1 

Bird  Kills  and  Weather  Conditions  at  the  WCTV  Tower:  Autumns  1962-1966 


Nights  with 

No  clouds 
A 

Clouds 

B 

Clouds  and  rain 
C 

No.  nights 

162 

110 

41 

No.  birds 

1326 

3427 

1991 

North  winds 

.V  ± SD 

8.19  ± 25.29 

32.06  ± 84.22 

48.56  ± 91.15 

Nights  with  0 

12 

4 

0 

Sig.  dif.^  from 

B,  C,  D,  F 

A,  D,  E 

D,  E,  A 

Not  from 

E 

C,  F 

F,  B 

D 

e 

F 

No.  nights 

44 

43 

20 

No.  birds 

111 

715 

553 

South  winds 

T ± SD 

2.52  ± 3.73 

16.62  ± 25.63 

27.65  ± 30.17 

Nights  with  0 

10 

5 

1 

Sig.  dif.  from 

A,  B,  C,  E,  F 

B,  C,  D,  F 

A,  D,  E 

Not  from 

none 

A 

B,  C 

“ F < 0.05,  Kruskal-Wallis  //-value  = 103. 141  (P  < 0.0005). 


classified  as  clear.  Nights  with  clear  skies  and  north  winds  resulted  in 
more  birds  killed  than  those  with  clear  skies  and  south  winds  but  neither 
equalled  the  number  killed  with  cloudy  skies  and  north  winds.  Nights  with 
cloudy  skies  and  south  winds  had  more  birds  killed  than  those  nights  with 
clear  skies  and  south  winds  but  neither  had  as  great  an  effect  as  cloudy 
skies  and  north  winds.  There  were  no  significant  differences  among  groups 
classified  for  north  winds  and  clouds,  north  winds,  clouds  and  rain,  and 
south  winds,  clouds  and  rain. 

On  36  nights,  more  than  50  birds  were  killed;  the  largest  kill  was  on  19 
September  1962  (N  = 828)  and  10  other  nights  exceeded  100  birds.  All 
hut  2 of  the  nights  with  more  than  50  birds  were  associated  with  cold-front 
passage  (14  October  1964,  with  57  birds  and  4 September  1966,  with  65 
birds  were  not).  Cold  fronts,  the  leading  edges  of  cold  air  masses,  are  often 
preceded  by  south  winds  and  clouds  in  the  vicinity  of  the  WCTV  tower. 
As  fronts  near  the  site,  storms  and  rain  may  occur  and  then  the  winds 
shift  to  the  north.  During  these  conditions  the  largest  kills  at  WCTV  occur, 
fhe  2 nights  with  large  kills  that  were  not  associated  with  the  passage  of 
cold  fronts  had  clouds  and  north  winds  nonetheless.  During  these  5 au- 
tumns about  20  fronts  passed  the  WCTV  tower  vicinity  and  did  not  result 
in  kills  of  over  50  birds,  although  they  regularly  resulted  in  kills  of  15-30 
birds.  Usually  these  fronts  came  through  2-4  days  after  another  front,  had 
relatively  clear  skies  along  their  leading  edges,  or  were  early  in  the  season. 


Crawford  • FLORIDA  AUTUMN  MIGRANT  KILLS 


191 


DISCUSSION 

The  WCTV  tower  data  show  4 consistant  patterns:  (1)  kills  occur  vir- 
tually every  night  in  autumn,  (2)  kills  result  during  south  winds  as  well  as 
north  winds,  but  north  winds  result  in  greater  kills  than  south  winds,  (3) 
large  kills  usually  result  during  the  passage  of  cold  fronts,  and  (4)  overcast 
skies  affect  the  number  of  birds  killed  whether  with  north  or  south  winds. 
Except  for  the  last,  the  effects  of  clouds,  the  characteristics  of  autumn 
kills  at  the  WCTV  tower  are  also  those  of  passerine  migration  in  the 
southeastern  U.S.,  according  to  radar  and  direct-visual  studies. 

Able  (1972,  1973)  monitored  night  migration  flights  in  autumn  at  Lake 
Charles,  Louisiana  and  at  Athens,  Georgia.  He  recorded  migration  flights 
every  night,  but-  flights  with  north  winds  were  greater  than  those  with 
south  winds:  70.5%  of  the  migration  volume  he  recorded  at  Athens  was 
with  north  winds.  Heaviest  flights  were  with  cold-front  passage,  but  often 
large  flights  were  with  north  winds  not  associated  with  a front,  especially 
if  several  days  had  passed  with  no  front.  Buskirk  (1968)  watched  incoming 
flights  of  autumn  migrants  in  Yucatan;  migrants  arrived  daily  but  large 
flights  only  occurred  when  cold  fronts  reached  the  northern  waters  of  the 
Gulf  of  Mexico.  Able  (1972)  noted  a similar  pattern  in  departing  autumnal 
migrants  from  Louisiana  and  Richardson  (1978:239)  found  this  to  be  a 
pattern  typical  of  migrants  departing  over  water  for  long  flights.  Inland, 
however,  large  migration  flights  in  the  eastern  U.S.  seem  dependent  on 
synoptic  conditions  (north  winds  and  a drop  in  temperature)  that  are  usu- 
ally present  with  the  passage  of  a cold  front  but  are  not  unique  thereto 
(Graber  and  Cochran  1960,  Hassler  et  al.  1963,  Able  1973). 

During  intervals  between  fronts,  the  southeastern  U.S.  experiences 
southerly  air  flows  but  frontal  passage  brings  colder,  northerly  winds  rath- 
er abruptly  (Able  1972).  Observations  that  flights  with  north  winds  are 
greater  than  those  with  south  winds  suggest  that  a pool  of  physiologically 
prepared  migrants  accumulates  in  the  presence  of  southerly  winds.  Frontal 
passage  brings  north  winds  and  these  apparently  stimulate  a large  flight 
of  migrants.  At  a given  site,  mass  movements  continue  for  2-3  nights  and 
then  decline  as  the  numbers  of  prepared  migrants  are  fewer  or  as  weather 
conditions  become  less  favorable.  These  events  are  repeated  every  few 
days  during  autumn  with  the  passage  of  fronts  (Able  1973,  Alerstam  et  al. 
1973,  Weir  1976). 

Fig.  1 illustrates  this  sequence.  Between  4 and  25  September  1965,  no 
front  passed  over  the  WCTV  site  and  during  most  of  the  interval  a warm 
high-pressure  center  dominated  the  eastern  U.S.  A warm  front  went  to 
Hudson  Bay  and  the  southerly  flow  of  warm  air  precluded  mass  flights;  a 
large  pool  of  migrants  was  assembled  for  the  next  front  which  reached  the 
WCTV  vicinity  on  24  September.  The  front  stalled  as  it  neared  the  tower; 


192 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


tower  maps  locating  dead  birds  (dots)  and  the  tower's  3 systems  of  guy-wires  (scale  on  24 
September  map  points  a-b:116  m).  Arrows  show  predominant  winds  and  speed  in  knots; 
partially  or  completely  filled  circles  indicate  amount  of  overcast;  N = number  of  birds.  Rain 
(hatched  areas)  is  indicated  on  the  weather  maps  only  for  the  WCTV  vicinity  (star). 


Crawford  • FLORIDA  AUTUMN  MIGRANT  KILLS 


193 


a warm  high-pressure  center  northeast  of  WCTV  and  a squall  line  pro- 
duced stormy,  rainy  weather  with  shifting  winds  which  are  conditions 
possibly  not  ideal  for  migration.  By  25  September  the  high-pressure  center 
behind  the  front  moved  eastward  bringing  clearing  skies  north  of  the 
WCTV  tower  and  northwest  winds  which  initiated  a mass  movement  of 
birds.  By  26  September,  the  center  of  the  high  had  moved  northeast  and  at 
WCTV  the  winds  came  from  the  north  and  northeast.  By  27  September, 
the  front  was  stationary  300  km  south  of  WCTV  and  northeast  winds 
continued  to  move  birds  in  large  numbers.  A low-pressure  storm  began  to 
develop  in  the  mid-Gulf.  No  WCTV  map  is  available  for  28  September 
when  32  birds  were  killed  under  overcast  skies  with  northeast  winds;  after 
3 days  of  massed  flights,  the  numbers  of  birds  aloft  began  to  decline.  By 
29  September,  the  low-pressure  center  in  the  Gulf  became  Tropical  Storm 
Debbie  and  tracked  towards  the  mouth  of  the  Mississippi  River.  The  storm 
created  southerly  winds  and  rain  along  the  northeast  Gulf  coast  but  at 
WCTV  winds  were  still  northeast  into  the  rain.  On  30  September,  Debbie 
went  ashore  and  dissipated;  a warm  frontogenesis  began  north  of  WCTV; 
rain,  overcast  and  southeast  winds  prevailed  at  the  tower  site.  Richardson 
(1978:261)  noted  that  “reverse  flights,”  with  winds  contrary  to  the  expected 
direction  of  autumn  migration,  are  often  associated  with  the  approach  of 
such  a low-pressure  area  as  Debbie.  Other  large  kills  at  WCTV  with  south- 
erly winds  were  usually  ahead  of  a front  oriented  SW-NE,  or  behind  a 
front  with  local  wind  shifts.  Lowery  and  Newman  (1966:281)  and  Able  {in 
Bagg  1971:22)  noted  large  autumnal  flights  with  southerly  winds  under 
similar  circumstances. 

The  regular  association  of  large  kills  and  cold  front  passage  at  WCTV 
can  be  attributed  to  mass  movement  of  birds  initiated  by  the  north  winds 
behind  the  fronts  and  the  presence  of  cloudy,  sometimes  stormy  weather 
often  along  the  fronts.  Birds  encountering  these  inclemencies  on  night 
migration  flights  may  experience  north  or  south  winds  at  the  edge  and  this 
may  account  for  the  lack  of  significant  difference  between  the  night  groups 
C and  F (Table  1);  they  are  essentially  the  same  situation. 

Apparently  clouds  are  a major  factor  in  tower  kills.  Weir  (1976)  and 
Avery  et  al.  (1978)  summarized  avian  mortality  in  migration;  papers  they 
listed  regularly  referred  to  overcast  during  the  kills.  Avery  et  al.  (1977), 
in  a thorough  study  of  tower  kills  in  North  Dakota,  found  70%  of  their 
autumn  casualties  after  cloudy  nights.  Clouds  may  affect  the  birds  in  2 
ways.  First,  in  the  southeast  U.S.  birds  generally  fly  at  lower  altitudes 
under  overcast  (Able  1970);  this  behavior  may  simply  bring  more  birds 
into  the  range  of  a tower.  Second,  the  bewildered  behavior  of  birds  around 
lights  apparently  occurs  even  without  fog  or  precipitation  because  light  is 
refracted  by  a greater  number  of  minute  moisture  droplets  in  the  air  during 
overcast.  Avery  et  al.  (1976)  considered  Graber’s  (1968)  explanation  of  the 


f 


194  THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 

phenomenon  most  appropriate:  birds  enter  the  illuminated  area  of  a tower 
and  are  reluctant  to  leave;  they  mill  around  and  many  are  killed  by  hitting 
the  tower,  guy  wires,  or  other  birds.  The  attraction  to  the  lighted  area  is 
limited  to  the  immediate  vicinity  of  the  tower;  the  birds  are  not  drawn 
from  a considerable  distance.  Herbert  (1970)  and  Gochfeld  (1973)  dis- 
cussed this  behavior  in  birds  and  the  effect  has  been  recorded  for  a variety 
of  lighted  situations  under  overcast  (Crawford  1974). 

The  high  percentage  of  nights  with  overcast  (50.9%)  at  WCTV  may  be 
because  of  the  close  proximity  of  the  Gulf  of  Mexico  (76  km  S);  warm, 
moist,  marine  air  forms  convection  clouds  and  storm  systems  along  cold 
fronts.  If  overcast  is  as  important  as  it  seems  to  the  production  of  tower 
kills  then  towers  in  areas  prone  to  cloudy  conditions  during  migration  may 
kill  more  birds  than  towers  in  areas  where  clear  skies  prevail.  If  the  ex- 
periences from  the  WCTV  tower  are  applicable  elsewhere,  however,  birds 
will  be  killed  nearly  every  night  during  autumn  migration,  even  under 
clear  skies.  Large  kills  will  be  predictable  and  consistent  with  migration 
volume  but  the  smaller  day-to-day  kills  may  not  be  noticed  because  of  the 
activities  of  scavenging  predators  or  the  condition  of  the  grounds  (Craw- 
ford 1971,  1974,  1976). 

SUMMARY 

Autumn  bird  kills  at  the  308  m WCTV  tower  in  northwest  Florida  during  1962-1966  were 
strongly  associated  with  north  winds  and  overcast.  Nights  with  north  winds  (N  = 313,  74.5% 
of  total)  accounted  for  6744  (83%)  dead  birds;  south-wind  nights  (N  = 107,  25.5%)  had  1379 
(17%)  birds.  Cloudy  nights  (N  = 214,  50.9%)  had  6686  (82.3%)  birds  killed;  non-cloudy  nights 
(N  = 206,  49.1%)  had  1437  (17.7%)  dead  birds.  Large  kills  were  usually  associated  with  the 
passage  of  a cold  front,  but  birds  were  killed  on  all  but  32  (7%)  of  the  nights.  The  data  seem 
consistent  with  radar  and  direct  visual  studies  of  migration  in  the  southeastern  U.S. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

W.  Wilson  Baker,  Harriett  Knowles,  Lucinda  Peace,  Henry  M.  Stevenson,  Noel  O.  Warner 
and  Ly!in  Wiese  helped  iri  various  stages  of  the  preparation  of  the  paper.  Steve  Rathbun 
advised  on  statistical  analysis  and  Alvan  A.  Karlin  helped  prepare  the  figure.  Referees  W. 
J.  Richardson  and  S.  A.  Gauthreaux,  Jr.  strengthened  and  clarified  an  earlier  version  of  the 
paper  with  valuable  suggestions.  Herbert  L.  Stoddard,  Sr.,  and  Robert  A.  Norris  gathered 
the  original  data. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Able,  K.  P.  1970.  A radar  study  of  the  altitude  of  nocturnal  passerine  migration.  Bird- 
Banding  41:282-290. 

. 1972.  Fall  migration  in  coastal  Louisiana  and  the  evolution  of  migration  patterns  in 

the  Gulf  region.  Wilson  Bull.  84:231-242. 

. 1973.  The  role  of  weather  variables  and  flight  direction  in  determining  the  magnitude 

of  nocturnal  bird  migration.  Ecology  54:1031-1041. 


Crawford  • FLORIDA  AUTUMN  MIGRANT  KILLS 


195 


Alerstam,  T.,  a.  Lindgren,  S.  G.  Nelsson  and  S.  Ulfstrand.  1973.  Nocturnal  passerine 
migration  and  cold  front  passages  in  autumn — a combined  radar  and  field  study.  Ornis 
Scand.,  Fenn.  4:103-111. 

Avery,  M.  [L.],  P.  F.  Springer  and  J.  F.  Cassel.  1976.  The  effects  of  a tall  tower  on 
nocturnal  bird  migration — a portable  ceilometer  study.  Auk  93:281-291. 

, AND . 1977.  Weather  influences  on  nocturnal  bird  mortality  at  a North 

Dakota  tower.  Wilson  Bull.  89:291-299. 

, AND  N.  S.  Dailey.  1978.  Avian  mortality  at  man-made  structures:  an  an- 
notated bibliography.  Biol.  Ser.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS-78/58,  U.S.  Dept.  Interior. 

Bagg,  a.  M.  1971.  The  changing  seasons.  Am.  Birds  25:15-23. 

Buskirk,  W.  H.  1968.  The  arrival  of  trans-Gulf  migrants  on  the  northern  coast  of  Yucatan 
in  fall.  M.S.  thesis,  Louisiana  State  Univ.,  Baton  Rouge,  Louisiana. 

Crawford,  R.  L.  1971.  Predation  on  birds  killed  at  TV  tower.  Oriole  36:33-35. 

. 1974.  Bird  casualties  at  a Leon  County,  Florida  TV  tower:  October  1966— September 

1973.  Bull.  Tall  Timbers  Res.  Sta.  18:1-27. 

. 1976.  Some  old  records  of  TV  tower  kills  from  southwest  Georgia.  Oriole  41:45-51. 

Graber,  R.  R.  1968.  Nocturnal  migration  in  Illinois — different  points  of  view.  Wilson  BuU. 
80:36-71. 

AND  W.  F.  Cochran.  1960.  Evaluation  of  an  aural  record  of  nocturnal  migration. 

Wilson  Bull.  72:253-273. 

Gochfeld,  M.  1973.  Confused  nocturnal  behavior  of  a flock  of  migrating  Yellow  Wagtails. 
Condor  75:252-253. 

Hassler,  S.  S.,  R.  R.  Graber  and  F.  C.  Bellrose.  1963.  Fall  migration  and  weather,  a 
radar  study.  Wilson  Bull.  75:56-77. 

Herbert,  A.  D.  1970.  Spatial  disorientation  in  birds.  Wilson  Bull.  82:400^19. 

Hollander,  M.  and  D.  A.  Wolfe.  1973.  Nonparametric  statistical  methods.  J.  Wiley  & 
Sons,  New  York,  New  York. 

Lowery,  G.  H.,  Jr.  and  R.  J.  Newman.  1966.  A continentwide  view  of  migration  on  four 
nights  in  October.  Auk  83:547-586. 

Richardson,  W.  J.  1978.  Timing  and  amount  of  bird  migration  in  relation  to  weather:  a 
review.  Oikos  30:224-272. 

Stoddard,  H.  L.,  Sr.  1962.  Bird  casualties  at  a Leon  County,  Florida  TV  tower,  1955- 
1961.  Bull.  Tall  Timbers  Res.  Sta.  1:1-94. 

AND  R.  A.  Norris.  1967.  Bird  casualties  at  a Leon  County,  Florida  TV  tower:  an 

eleven-year  study.  Bull.  Tall  Timbers  Res.  Sta.  8:1-104. 

Weir,  R.  D.  1976.  Annotated  bibliography  of  bird  kills  at  man-made  obstacles:  a review  of 
the  state  of  the  art  and  solutions.  Dept.  Fisheries  and  Environment,  Canadian  Wildl. 
Serv.,  Ontario  Region. 


TALL  TIMBERS  RESEARCH  STATION,  RT.  1,  BOX  160,  TALLAHASSEE,  FLOR- 
IDA 32312.  ACCEPTED  1 MAY  1980. 


Wilson  Bull..  93(2),  1981,  pp.  196-206 


CLIMATIC  INFLUENCES  ON  PRODUCTIVITY  IN  THE 
HOUSE  SPARROW 

W.  Bruce  McGillivray 

The  selection  of  a poor  nesting  site  could  drastically  lower  the  breeding 
success  of  a pair  of  birds.  Given  particular  habitat  requirements,  selection 
should  constrain  nest  structure  and  nest-site  to  characteristic  types.  This 
conclusion  has  been  tacitly  assumed  by  biologists  and  field  naturalists  who 
were  aware  that  nest-sites  were  not  randomly  scattered  but  associated 
with  species  specific  habitat  and  structural  configurations.  However,  sev- 
eral studies  have  demonstrated  intraspecific  variation  in  nest-site  choice 
and  subsequent  differences  in  productivity  among  sites  (Orians  1961,  Rob- 
ertson 1973,  Will  1973,  Caccamise  1977,  Murphy  1977,  Anderson  1978). 

Cody  (1971)  suggested  that  predation  is  the  single  greatest  cause  of 
reproductive  failure  in  most  species  of  birds.  Predation  was  also  consid- 
ered to  be  an  important  factor  in  the  distribution  of  nests  (Horn  1968, 
Lack  1968),  with  higher  productivity  in  the  best  concealed  or  least  acces- 
sible nests.  Climate  is  accorded  a role  in  nesting  success  but  usually  only 
indirectly  as  a determinant  of  food  abundance.  Severe  weather  can  have 
a major  effect  on  reproductive  output  at  a site  (Mitchell  et  al.  1973)  by 
damaging  or  destroying  nests.  Birds  thus  could  be  expected  to  construct 
nests  to  reduce  the  detrimental  influence  of  weather  (Collias  1964,  Austin 
1974,  Inouye  1976,  Schaeffer  1976,  Mertens  1977);  this  tendency  would  be 
strong  if  weather  was  the  major  cause  of  reproductive  failure.  This  situ- 
ation obtains  for  a population  of  House  Sparrows  {Passer  domesticus)  on 
a ranch  near  Calgary,  Alberta.  Human  activity,  plus  thick  blue  spruce 
{Picea  pungens)  foliage  combine  to  reduce  egg  and  nestling  loss  to  pred- 
ators (Black-billed  Magpies  [Pica  pica]  and  cats)  to  negligible  levels  (Mur- 
phy 1977,  McGillivray  1978).  The  large  population  of  House  Sparrows  pro- 
vided an  opportunity  to  assess  variation  in  reproductive  performance 
associated  with  nest-site  differences.  This  study  was  undertaken  to  de- 
termine if  weather-related  factors  could  influence  the  nest  placement  and 
reproductive  performance  of  House  Sparrows. 

MATERIALS  AND  METHODS 

Study  site. — The  study  area  was  on  a ranch  located  8 km  east  of  Calgary,  Alberta  (51°05'N, 
113°50'W).  The  146  nests  found  were  in  2 rows  of  blue  spruce  originally  grown  as  windbreaks. 
Generally,  several  thousand  bushels  of  grain  were  stored  on  the  ranch  and  high  protein  feed 
was  always  available.  Two  heated  barns  supplied  shelter  for  the  sparrows  in  inclement 
weather  and  throughout  the  winter.  Twenty-four  nest  boxes  were  installed  on  the  sides  of 
farm  buildings  at  this  site  in  late  1974  (Murphy  1977). 


196 


McGiUivray  • HOUSE  SPARROW  PRODUCTIVITY 


197 


Data  collection. — Nests  were  inspected  at  3-5-day  intervals  from  2 May-15  August  1977. 
Usually  4-day  intervals  were  maintained,  but  if  weather  conditions  were  severe,  the  inspec- 
tions were  curtailed  to  avoid  affecting  nestling  survival.  Eggs  were  numbered  and  weighed 
to  the  nearest  0.1  g on  a 5 g capacity  Pesola  scale  and  nestlings  were  weighed  to  the  nearest 
0.5  g on  a 50  g capacity  Pesola  scale.  Nestlings  5-6  days  old  were  banded  with  a U.S.  IVsh 
and  Wildlife  Service  aluminum  band  and  also  color  leg  bands  after  their  weight  reached  20 
g.  Nest  height,  tree  height,  tree  basal  diameter  and  distance  between  trees  were  measured 
directly  with  a tape  measure.  Tree  volume  was  calculated  assuming  a conical  shape  for  the 
trees  [U  = Vi27r  (basal  diameter)^(height)].  Nest  orientation  was  measured  as  the  direction 
of  a line  from  the  center  of  the  entrance  to  the  back  of  the  nest.  Standard  compass  orientation 
was  used;  N = 0°,  E = 90°,  S = 180°,  W = 270°. 

Breeding  success. — Estimates  were  sometimes  needed  to  determine  nestling  age  when  first 
found,  date  of  clutch  initiation  and  10-day  nestling  weight.  The  estimation  procedures  used 
were  those  of  Murphy  (1978a).  As  nests  were  not  checked  each  day,  it  was  not  always 
possible  to  know  the  fate  of  some  eggs  and  nestlings.  Therefore,  maximum  and  minimum 
estimates  of  success  were  used.  The  estimate  of  maximum  hatching  success  was  based  on 
the  assumption  that  eggs  disappearing  between  successive  nest  checks  hatched  and  the 
young  subsequently  died.  The  estimate  of  minimum  hatching  success  was  based  on  known 
hatch.  The  estimate  of  the  minimum  number  of  fledged  nestlings  was  the  number  reaching 
a weight  of  24.8  g or  more  (mean  10-day  weight)  before  leaving  the  nest.  The  number  of 
nestlings  with  a weight  of  20.0  g before  leaving  tbe  nest  was  the  estimate  of  the  maximum 
number  fledged. 

Data  analysis. — Counts  of  the  number  of  young  fledged  and  eggs  hatched  were  bimodally 
distributed  due  to  a high  frequency  of  nest  failures.  For  this  reason,  the  Mann-Whitney  U- 
test  was  used  for  paired  and  the  Kruskal- Wallis  test  was  used  for  grouped  comparisons  of 
reproductive  performance.  Substantial  variation  existed  in  the  reproductive  output  from  the 
nests;  both  seasonal  and  per  clutch  output  were  investigated.  Multiple  regression  analysis 
(BMDP2R,  Dixon  1975)  was  used  to  determine  the  extent  to  which  reproductive  performance 
could  be  accounted  for  by  variation  in  the  continuous  variables  describing  nest-site  position. 
Where  the  data  were  normally  distributed,  /-tests  were  used  for  paired  comparisons  and 
correlation  analysis  used  to  investigate  temporal  variation. 

RESULTS 

The  2 spruce  rows  studied  were  oriented  in  a north-south  direction  on 
either  side  of  the  main  ranch  house.  The  average  distance  between  the 
rows  was  82  m.  Both  rows  were  bordered  on  1 side  by  a honeysuckle 
hedge  {Lonicera  sp.).  A plowed  field,  house,  garage  and  driveway  lay 
between  the  2 rows  (Fig.  1).  Deciduous  hedges  and  trees  on  the  ranch 
were  not  used  as  nest-sites  by  the  sparrows.  The  west  tree  row  was  more 
densely  vegetated  and  more  varied  in  tree  size  and  number  of  nests  per 
tree  than  the  east  tree  row  (Table  1).  A significant  positive  regression  was 
found  when  the  frequency  of  nests  was  plotted  against  tree  volume  (Y  = 
0.2014X  + 0.0107;  r = 0.562,  P ^ 0.01,  N = 60).  This  was  not  a simple 
consequence  of  overpopulation  as  some  box  nests  and  former  nest-sites 
in  unoccupied  trees  remained  vacant  while  large  trees  contained  up  to  14 
nests.  Almost  all  (97%,  N = 146)  nests  were  built  adjoining  the  main  trunk 
of  a tree. 


198 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Fig.  1.  Map  of  the  study  area.  Major  House  Sparrow  nesting  occurred  in  rows  of  spruce 
windbreaks  as  well  as  in  nest  boxes  attached  to  buildings  on  the  ranch.  Buildings  are  outlined, 
fence  rows  shown  by  dashed  lines,  roads  by  solid  lines,  hedgerows  by  irregular  outlines  and 
spruce  rows  by  solid  outlines.  Only  nests  along  the  east  tree  row,  west  tree  row  and  in  the 
nest  boxes  provided  the  data  for  this  study. 


McGillivray  • HOUSE  SPARROW  PRODUCTIVITY 


199 


Table  1 

Some  Characteristics  of  Nest-sites  in  the  East  and  West  Rows 


Variable  West  row  East  row  T-value 


Total  length  (m) 

Total  number  of  trees 
Total  number  of  nests 
Mean  tree  height  (m) 

Mean  inter-tree  distance  (m) 

Mean  tree  volume  (m‘^) 

Mean  nest  height  (m) 

Mean  distance  to  nearest  neighbor  (m) 
Mean  number  of  nests  within  1 m 
Mean  nest  height/tree  height  ratio 


94 

81 

NA 

34 

24 

NA 

110 

36 

NA 

5.61** 

4.50** 

678 

2.77** 

3.58** 

249 

13.42** 

10.09** 

692 

3.87** 

3.27** 

1167 

0.66** 

1.52** 

439 

3.43** 

0.76** 

1345 

0.69* 

0.73* 

610 

* P < 0.05,  Mann-Whitney  f/-test. 
**  P < O.OI  Mann-Whitney  t/-test. 


Nest  position. — Univariate  inter-row  comparison  of  means  describing 
reproductive  success  show  few  differences  (Table  2).  No  clear  trends  are 
apparent,  but  this  is  noteworthy  since  the  average  density  of  nests  was 
much  higher  in  the  west  row.  Each  row  was  then  partitioned  into  4 equal 
sections  to  determine  whether  position  within  a row  affected  reproductive 
performance.  The  west  row  displayed  considerably  more  intra-row  varia- 
tion (Table  3).  The  nests  were  significantly  more  grouped  in  the  southern 
half  of  the  rows  (west  row:  t = 7.5,  df  = 97,  P < 0.001;  east  row:  t = 
2.5,  df  = 33,  P < 0.05).  In  addition,  the  ratio  of  nest  height  to  tree  height 
increased  from  north  to  south  along  the  west  row. 


Table  2 

Seasonal  Averages  of  Reproductive  Output  from  Box  and  Tree  Nests 


Variable 

Box 

West 

East 

H-stat. 

Number  of  clutches^ 

2.71 

2.12 

2.06 

8.86** 

Total  number  of  eggs 

12.10 

10.43 

9.39 

12.67** 

Min.  number  hatching 

7.50 

5.43 

4.89 

12.24** 

Max.  number  fledging 

5.29 

3.38 

3.21 

14.33** 

Clutch-size 

4.91 

4.92 

4.79 

2.74 

Min.  hatch/clutch 

2.86 

2.70 

2.57 

1.86 

Max.  fledge/clutch 

1.95 

1.67 

1.76 

4.17 

Egg  weight 

2.91 

2.90 

2.88 

1.21 

Nestling  10-day  wt. 

23.55 

25.35 

23.49 

4.34 

**  P < 0.01  Kruskal- Wallis  test. 

® Lines  connect  groups  not  significantly  different  by  comparison  of  rank  sums  (Dunn  1%4). 


200 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  3 

Average  Values  of  Seasonal  Reproductive  Output  for  Nests  along  both  Tree 
Rows;  Each  Row  Was  Partitioned  into  4 Equal  Sections:  NQ  = North  Quarter, 
NM  = North  Middle,  SM  = South  Middle,  SQ  = South  Quarter 


West  tree  row  East  tree  row 


Variable 

NQ 

NM 

SM 

SQ 

H 

NQ 

NM 

SM 

SQ 

H 1 

Sample  size 

27 

18 

14 

30 

6 

8 

8 

7 

1 

Nest  height  (m) 
Nest  height/tree 

3.83 

3.76 

3.83 

3.97 

1.1 

3.67 

3.23 

3.14 

3.09 

3.9 

height 

0.62 

0.68 

0.70 

0.74 

12.2** 

0.74 

0.74 

0.70 

0.73 

1.2 

Nests  within  1 m 
Total  clutches 

2.67 

2.44 

4.50 

4.20 

6.4 

0.28 

0.55 

1.11 

0.92 

3.84 

per  nest 
Total  eggs  per 

2.29 

2.50 

1.85 

1.86 

9.3* 

2.28 

2.00 

2.00 

2.00 

3.09 

nest 

11.04 

12.22 

9.07 

9.43 

8.3* 

9.71 

9.22 

9.33 

9.37 

1.14 

Total  fledging 

per  nest 

2.70 

5.40 

3.57 

2.67 

20.1** 

2.12 

3.66 

4.00 

2.12 

1.97  j 

Fledging  success 

0.56 

0.78 

0.67 

0.62 

3.2 

0.49 

0.74 

0.78 

0.66 

1.84  ; 

Clutch-size 
Fledging  per 

4.80 

4.90 

4.95 

5.01 

0.6 

4.31 

4.83 

4.80 

5.17 

4.60  : 

1 

clutch 

1.35 

2.35 

1.88 

1.54 

8.2* 

0.93 

1.94 

2.12 

1.89 

3.86 

' Maximum  value. 

^ Pledging  success  = number  of  fledglings/number  of  nestlings. 

* P < 0.05. 

**  F < 0.01  (Kruskal-Wallis  test). 

Table  4 gives  the  average  weather  conditions  prevailing  in  the  Calgary 
region  during  the  House  Sparrow  breeding  season.  The  relatively  strong 
northerly  winds  and  high  incidence  of  storms  suggests  that  southerly  po- 
sition in  a row  or  low  nest  height  could  enhance  nest-site  security.  The 
earliest  breeders  suffered  the  most  rigorous  weather  conditions  of  the 
breeding  season,  including  6 days  of  snow,  13  of  rain  and  an  average  daily 


Table  4 

Climatic  Table  for  Calg.ary,  Alberta"* 


Month 

Femperature 

(C) 

Wind 

(m/sec) 

Precipitation 

(mm) 

No.  of  days 
of  rain 

Thunderstorms 

April 

3.6 

SE  5.0 

35 

10 

0.2 

May 

9.8 

NW  5.0 

52 

11 

1.4 

June 

13.0 

N,  NW  4.6 

88 

14 

5.8 

July 

16.7 

NW  4. 1 

58 

11 

8.2 

August 

15.1 

N 4.0 

59 

12 

5.3 

^ Data  from  R. 

a.  Bryson  and  E.  K. 

Hare  (1974).  Figures  given  represent  multi- 

■year  means. 

\ 

I 


McGillivray  • HOUSE  SPARROW  PRODUCTIVITY 


201 


Table  5 

Seasonal  Variation  of  Nest  Orientation  in  the  West  Row" 


Nest  orientation 


Portion  of  row 

NE 

NW 

SW 

SE 

Northern-most  quarter 

N = 

5 

16  June 

7 

4 July 

8 

13  May 

8 

29  May 

Middle  half 

N = 

6 

9 June 

9 

14  June 

9 

16  May 

10 

11  May 

Southern-most  quarter 

N = 

5 

6 June 

8 

13  May 

10 

31  May 

8 

5 June 

Whole  row 

N = 

16 

4 June 

24 

10  June 

27 

17  May 

26 

21  May 

“ Dates  are  the  average  date  of  the  initiation  of  first  clutches  for  nests  oriented  in  I of  4 directions:  NE  = l°-90°,  SE  = 
9I°-I80°,  SW  = I8r-270°,  NW  = 27I°-360°. 


minimum  temperature  of  3.5°C  in  May.  Positive  correlations  between 
clutch  initiation  date  and  absolute  and  relative  (to  tree  height)  nest  height 
indicated  an  early  start  for  low  nests.  For  means  calculated  over  10-day 
intervals,  the  correlation  of  clutch  initiation  date  and  absolute  height  was: 
r = 0.809,  df  = 9,  P < 0.01,  west  row;  and  r = 0.629,  df  = 8,  P < 0.05, 
east  row;  the  correlation  of  clutch  initiation  date  and  relative  nest  height 
was  r = 0.630,  df  = 9,  P < 0.05,  west  row;  and  r = 0.550,  df  = 8,  P < 
0.1,  east  row. 

Orientation. — Nests  found  in  the  spruce  trees  were  bulky,  ball-like 
structures  composed  of  grasses,  straw,  small  twigs  and  occasionally,  pa- 
per, plastic  and  hair.  The  nest  entrance  was  usually  at  the  side  of  the 
j structure,  but  it  was  frequently  found  on  top  or  even  underneath  the  main 
body  of  the  nest.  The  direction  in  which  the  entrance  faced  was  the  mea- 
sured orientation.  When  nests  were  grouped  in  45°  arcs,  the  nest  entry 
Ij  directions  were  randomly  distributed  (x^  = 9.07,  df  = 7,  P > 0.1,  NS). 
Nest  entry  direction  depended  on  the  date  of  first  clutch  initiation.  North- 
facing nests  (arc  270°-90°)  were  built  by  late  nesters,  particularly  at  the 
northern  end  of  the  west  row  (Table  5).  The  east  tree  row,  perhaps  due  to 
) restricted  number  of  nest-sites  showed  little  variation  of  nest  orientation. 
Only  8 first  clutches  were  initiated  after  1 June  along  the  east  row;  how- 
ever, 5 of  these  were  oriented  either  northeast  or  northwest.  A trend  for 
i higher  annual  productivity  from  south-facing  nests  (arc  90° -270°)  was  not- 
ed. South-facing  nests  contained  more  successful  clutches  (2.26  vs  1.91) 
and  correspondingly  more  fledglings  (3.56  vs  3.11).  In  addition,  the  average 
number  of  fledglings  per  clutch  was  slightly  higher  for  south-facing  nests 
I (1.74  vs  1.66). 


202 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  6 

Regressions  of  Measures  of  Reproductive  Success  on  Nest  Height  for  Both 
Rows  AND  THE  WEST  RoW  AlONE"* 


Y 

Both  rows 

West  row  alone 

Total  number  of  young  fledging 

Y = -0.729X  + 6.4 

Y = 

-0.9575X  + 6.6 

F = 6.1,  P < 0.05 

F = 

12.76,  P < 0.01 

df  = 1,  106 

df  = 

1,  88 

Total  fledgling  weight 

Y = -20.89X  + 182.5 

Y = 

-24.90X  -f  179.15 

F = 6.52,  P < 0.05 

F = 

10.19,  P < 0.01 

df  = 1,  106 

df  = 

1,  88 

Total  nestling  weight 

Y = -20.I6X  + 200.02 

Y = 

-33.77X  + 242.7 

F = 6.4,  P < 0.05 

F = 

11.21,  P < 0.01 

df  - I,  106 

df  = 

1,  88 

® Residuals  were  checked  for  departures 

from  normality  (X  = nest  height  in  m). 

Variation  in  productivity. — Significant  relationships  were  found  be- 
tween measures  of  reproductive  performance  and  simple  parameters  of 
nest-site  position.  Seasonal  totals  of  number  of  young  fledged,  fledgling 
weight  and  nestling  weight  were  negatively  related  to  nest  height  (Table 
6).  There  was  a clear  inverse  relationship  between  the  number  and  quality 
of  fledged  young  and  the  height  of  the  nests.  This  result  is  surprising 
because  other  factors  known  to  influence  House  Sparrow  breeding  pro- 
ductivity, such  as  age  and  experience  of  the  parents  and  the  nutritional 
state  of  the  female  (Summers-Smith  1963,  Dawson  1972,  Pinowska  1979), 
did  not  mask  the  effect  of  nest  height. 

Many  of  the  preceding  analyses  have  assumed  a relationship  between 
nest-site  exposure  and  security.  Data  from  this  study  support  this  as- 
sumption. Eighteen  nests  were  either  destroyed  or  severely  damaged  after 
violent  windstorms.  All  but  2 of  the  nests  were  oriented  in  a northerly 
direction  and  61%  (11  of  18)  were  in  the  northernmost  quarter  of  each  row. 
The  ratio  of  nest  height  to  tree  height  was  slightly  higher  than  average  for 
the  destroyed  nests  (west  row:  0.72,  east  row:  0.77,  see  Table  1 for  av- 
erages). All  of  the  destroyed  nests  had  at  least  one  of  the  characteristics 
reducing  nest-site  security. 

Box  nests  vs  tree  nests. — Seasonal  totals  of  productivity  were  higher  for 
birds  nesting  in  boxes  (Table  2).  This  probably  occurred  because  more 
clutches  were  initiated  at  each  box  nest  over  the  breeding  season.  First 
clutches  were  initiated  earlier  at  box  nests  and  the  peak  initiation  of  sec- 
ond clutches  at  box  nests  coincided  with  the  peak  of  first  clutch  initiation 
at  tree  nests. 


McGillivray  • HOUSE  SPARROW  PRODUCTIVITY 


203 


DISCUSSION 

A unique  aspect  of  this  study  site  was  the  predominance  of  natural  vs 
artificial  nest-sites.  The  box  nests  were  clearly  the  best  nest-sites  since 
clutches  were  initiated  earlier  in  the  box  than  tree  nests.  The  observed 
increase  in  productivity  at  box  nests  is  probably  due  to  the  protection 
afforded  by  the  structure.  They  are  impervious  to  wind  damage,  almost 
waterproof  and  probably  provide  a warmer  micro-climate  for  the  nest 
(Mertens  1977).  The  probability  that  an  egg  laid  in  a box  nest  resulted  in 
a fledged  young  was  0.44;  in  contrast,  for  the  tree  nests,  it  was  only  0.32 
(west  row)  and  0.34  (east  row).  Differential  productivity  should  also  exist 
between  differently  positioned  tree  nests.  The  weather-related  nest  de- 
struction supports  the  qualitative  assessment  that  certain  nest-sites  and 
orientations  are  better  suited  to  ambient  conditions. 

The  attractiveness  of  large  trees  as  nest-sites,  the  close  proximity  of  the 
nests  to  the  main  trunks  and  the  negative  relationship  between  nest  height 
and  fledgling  number  all  suggest  the  importance  of  nest-site  security. 
Nests  near  the  tops  of  spruce  trees  are  exposed  to  rain  and  wind  and  wind- 
induced  movements  of  the  tree.  Such  movements  may  dislodge  the  nest 
more  readily  than  the  wind  alone.  The  choice  of  spruce  trees  over  decid- 
uous trees  for  nest  substrates  is  probably  due  to  the  thicker  foliage  and 
greater  protection  offered  by  the  conifers. 

Thunderstorms  and  strong  winds  are  common  in  Calgary  throughout  the 
summer,  but  wind  direction  is  predictable  and  a response  to  lower  its 
effect  would  be  anticipated  (Austin  1974,  Cink  1976).  The  decrease  in  the 
ratio  of  nest  height  to  tree  height  towards  the  north  end  of  both  rows  can 
be  interpreted  as  a response  to  the  usual  wind  direction. 

The  random  distribution  of  nest  entry  direction  is  surprising  considering 
the  importance  of  nest  orientation  for  other  birds  such  as  the  Cactus  Wren 
{Campylorhynchus  brunneicapillus)  and  Verdin  {Auriparus  flaviceps) 
(Ricklefs  and  Hainsworth  1969,  Austin  1974)  and  the  demonstrated  effect 
of  weather  on  House  Sparrow  nests  (Mitchell  et  al.  1973).  Cactus  Wrens 
orient  their  nest  entrances  into  the  wind  to  reduce  heat  stress  on  the 
nestlings.  Kendeigh  (1976)  found  that  heat  stress  begins  at  22°C  in  adult 
j House  Sparrows.  Throughout  July  and  August  in  Calgary,  the  maximum 
, daily  temperature  often  reaches  or  exceeds  22°C  (23  days  in  1977).  This, 
combined  with  a long  daylength  and  concomitant  high  level  of  incident 
' radiation  on  the  nests,  could  produce  heat  stress  in  nestlings.  In  Calgary, 
though.  House  Sparrows  begin  breeding  in  poor  conditions  relative  to  pop- 
ulations studied  at  lower  latitudes  (Murphy  1978a).  Cool  temperatures  and 
rain  early  in  the  season  combine  to  lower  productivity  compared  to  mid- 
summer values  (Murphy  1978b,  McGillivray  1978). 


204 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


The  preponderance  of  south-facing  nests  early  in  the  season  is  perhaps 
a response  to  protect  the  nests  from  the  north  wind.  The  increase  in  the 
number  of  north-facing  nests  later  in  the  season  for  both  rows  would  be 
expected  if  wind  ventilation  were  used  as  a cooling  agent  in  warm  weather. 
The  change  in  average  nest  entrance  orientation  (Table  5)  from  early  to 
late  nesters  may  serve  to  minimize  the  detrimental  influence  of  weather 
on  reproductive  output.  The  overall  randomness  of  nest  entry  direction 
can  be  accounted  for  by  the  long  House  Sparrow  breeding  season  and  the 
resultant  variation  in  optimal  orientation. 

The  apparent  relationship  between  average  weather  conditions  and  the 
pattern  of  nest  position  and  construction  may  explain  the  variation  in  nest 
entrance  orientation,  nest  height  and  the  date  of  first  clutch  initiation.  The 
better  protected  box  nests  allow  birds  to  breed  earlier,  thus  increasing  the 
number  of  clutches  possible  in  a season.  Birds  nesting  in  low  tree  nests 
begin  to  breed  earlier  and  fledge  more  young.  Birds  nesting  in  south-facing 
nests  begin  to  breed  earlier  and  show  marginally  higher  fledgling  produc- 
tion. Sometimes  the  adaptive  significance  of  the  pattern  is  not  entirely 
evident.  Despite  the  presence  of  unused  nest-sites,  there  is  a strong  re- 
lationship between  the  number  of  nests  in  a tree  and  tree  size,  suggesting 
that  large  trees  make  good  nest  substrates.  Yet,  there  is  no  relationship 
between  the  number  of  fledglings  from  a nest  and  the  size  of  the  tree 
containing  that  nest. 

These  data  imply  that  some  of  the  variation  in  reproductive  performance 
could  he  predicted  with  a knowledge  of  local  climate  and  the  position  of 
the  House  Sparrow  nests.  However,  as  noted  earlier,  other  variables  affect 
reproductive  output  and  may  prevent  the  determination  of  the  importance 
of  weather.  Recent  handing  recaptures  at  my  study  site  have  shown  a 
large  turn-over  of  birds.  It  is  likely  that  at  least  50%  of  the  birds  nesting 
on  the  ranch  are  either  first-year  birds  or  immigrants  from  neighboring 
ranches.  Summers-Smith  (1963)  has  shown  that  first-year  birds  start 
breeding  later  in  the  season  and  are  less  skilled  at  nest  building  than  older 
adults.  It  is  interesting  that  at  this  site  the  best  protected  nests  were  built 
by  pairs  who  began  to  breed  early.  This  suggests  that  the  older  adults  built 
the  early,  and  hence,  more  productive  nests;  unfortunately,  the  ages  of 
the  breeding  birds  were  not  known.  Weather  is  clearly  a major  force  de- 
termining the  productivity  of  House  Sparrows.  Many  nests  on  the  study 
area  were  positioned  to  minimize  the  severity  of  the  weather.  The  obser- 
vation that  many  poor  nest-sites  were  also  chosen  could  be  the  result  of 
experiments  by  first-year  birds  or  simply  an  inability  of  pairs  to  find  and 
maintain  better  sites. 

SUMMARY 

rhe  variation  in  reproductive  performance  observed  in  this  study  appears  to  be  partly 
attributable  to  the  influence  of  weather.  Box  nests  are  well  protected  against  the  elements 


McGillivray  • HOUSE  SPARROW  PRODUCTIVITY 


205 


and  usually  birds  nesting  in  boxes  were  more  successful  than  those  nesting  elsewhere.  Tree 
nests  are  exposed  and,  to  be  productive,  must  be  constructed  to  withstand  strong  winds, 
storms  and  cold  temperatures.  South-facing  nests,  central  position  along  a tree  row  and  low 
nest  height  to  tree  height  ratio  were  determined  to  be  beneficial  in  reducing  the  impact  of 
the  cool  north  winds.  South-facing  nests  fledged  more  young  early  in  the  season,  while  north- 
facing nests  were  more  productive  in  mid-summer.  Throughout  both  tree  rows,  the  number 
and  weight  of  fledglings  was  negatively  correlated  with  nest  height. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

I express  my  gratitude  to  E.  C.  Murphy,  whose  own  fieldwork  and  ideas  prepared  the 
groundwork  for  this  study.  Peter  E.  Lowther  is  given  special  thanks  for  preparing  the  figure 
and  typing  the  manuscript.  I am  grateful  to  the  Van  family,  the  Jones  family  and  Jerry  de 
Zeeuw  on  whose  lives  I intruded  for  4 months.  H.  Levenson,  R.  F.  Johnston,  J.  T.  Paul,  Jr., 
A.  M.  Finfrock,  J.  A.  Cox  and  P.  W.  Hedrick  all  read  and  commented  constructively  on  the 
manuscript.  Finally,  I would  like  to  thank  my  wife,  Sandy,  who  aided  immensely  with  field- 
work and  tolerated  protracted  absences  and  an  array  of  behaviors  associated  with  the  com- 
pletion of  this  study.  Support  for  this  research  was  given  by  NSF  grant  BMS-76-02225  to  R. 
F.  Jobnston. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Anderson,  T.  R.  1978.  Population  studies  of  European  sparrows  in  North  America.  Occ. 
Paps.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.,  Univ.  Kansas,  No.  70. 

Austin,  G.  T.  1974.  Nesting  success  of  the  Cactus  Wren  in  relation  to  nest  orientation. 
Condor  76:216-217. 

Bryson,  R.  A.  and  E.  K.  Hare.  1974.  Climates  of  North  America,  Vol.  11  in  World  survey 
of  climatology  (H.  E.  Landsberg,  ed.).  Elsevier  Scientific  Publ.  Co.,  Amsterdam,  Neth- 
erlands. 

Caccamise,  D.  F.  1977.  Breeding  success  and  nest-site  characteristics  of  the  Red-winged 
Blackbird.  Wilson  Bull.  89:396^03. 

CiNK,  C.  L.  1976.  The  influence  of  early  learning  on  nest  site  selection  in  the  House 
Sparrow.  Condor  78:103-104. 

Cody,  M.  L.  1971.  Ecological  aspects  of  reproduction.  Pp.  461-512  in  Avian  biology,  Vol. 
1 (D.  S.  Earner  and  J.  R.  King,  eds.).  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

CoLLIAS,  N.  E.  1964.  The  evolution  of  nests  and  nest-building  in  birds.  Am.  Zool.  4:175- 
190. 

Dawson,  D.  G.  1972.  The  breeding  ecology  of  House  Sparrows.  Ph.D.  thesis,  Edward  Grey 
Inst.,  Oxford,  England. 

Dixon,  W.  J.  (ed.)  1975.  BMPD  Biomedical  Computer  Programs.  Univ.  California  Press, 
Berkeley,  California. 

Dunn,  O.  J.  1964.  Multiple  comparisons  using  rank  sums.  Technometrics  6:241-252. 

Horn,  H.  S.  1968.  The  adaptive  significance  of  colonial  nesting  in  the  Brewer's  Blackbird 
(Euphagus  cyanocephalus).  Ecology  49:682—694. 

Inouye,  D.  W.  1976.  Non-random  orientation  of  entrance  holes  to  woodpecker  nests  in 
aspen  trees.  Condor  78:101-102. 

Kendeigh,  S.  C.  1976.  Latitudinal  trends  in  the  metabolic  adjustments  of  the  House  Spar- 
row. Ecology  57:509-519. 

Lack,  D.  1968.  Ecological  adaptations  for  breeding  in  birds.  Methuen  and  Co.,  Ltd.,  Lon- 
don, England. 

McGillivray,  W.  B.  1978.  The  effects  of  nest  position  on  reproductive  performance  of  the 
House  Sparrow.  M.A.  thesis,  Univ.  Kansas,  Lawrence,  Kansas. 


206 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Mertens,  J.  a.  L.  1977.  Thermal  conditions  for  successful  breeding  in  Great  Tits  (Pams 
major  L.).  II.  Thermal  properties  of  nests  and  nestboxes  and  their  implications  for  the 
range  of  temperature  tolerance  of  Great  Tit  broods.  Oecologia  28:31-56. 

Mitchell,  C.  J.,  R.  O.  Hayes,  P.  Holden  and  T.  B.  Hughes,  Jr.  1973.  Nesting  activity 
of  the  House  Sparrow  in  Hale  Co.,  Texas,  during  1968.  Ornithol.  Monogr.  14:49-59. 

Murphy,  E.  C.  1977.  Breeding  ecology  of  House  Sparrows.  Ph.D.  thesis,  Univ.  Kansas, 
Lawrence,  Kansas. 

. 1978a.  Breeding  ecology  of  House  Sparrows:  spatial  variation.  Condor  80:180-193. 

. 1978b.  Seasonal  variation  in  reproductive  output  of  House  Sparrows:  the  determi- 
nation of  clutch  size.  Ecology  59:1189-1199. 

Orians,  G.  H.  1961.  Social  stimulation  within  blackbird  colonies.  Condor  63:330-337. 

PlNOWSK.\,  B.  1979.  The  effect  of  energ\'  and  building  resources  of  females  on  the  produc- 
tion of  House  Sparrow  (Passer  dornesticus  (L.))  populations.  Ekologia  Polska  27:363- 
396. 

Ricklefs,  R.  E.  and  F.  R.  Hainsworth.  1969.  Temperature  regulation  in  nestling  Cactus 
Wrens:  the  nest  environment.  Condor  71:32-37. 

Robertson,  R.  J.  1973.  Optimal  niche  space  of  the  Red-winged  Blackbird.  Spatial  and 
temporal  patterns  of  nesting  activity  and  success.  Ecolog>  54:1084-1093. 

Schaeffer,  V.  H.  1976.  Geographic  variation  in  the  placement  and  structure  of  oriole 
nests.  Condor  78:443^48. 

Summers-Smith,  D.  1963.  The  House  Sparrow.  Collins,  London,  England. 

Will,  R.  L.  1973.  Breeding  success,  numbers  and  movements  of  House  Sparrows  at 
McLeansboro,  Illinois.  Ornithol.  Monogr.  14:60-78. 


MUSEUM  OF  NATURAL  HISTORY,  UNIV.  KANSAS,  LAWRENCE,  K.\NSAS  66045. 
ACCEPTED  30  SEPT.  1980. 


WORKING  GROUP  ON  GRANIVOROUS  BIRDS — INTECOL 
rhe  Third  International  Congress  of  Ecolog>  will  take  place  in  arsaw,  Poland,  5-11 
September  1982.  The  Working  Group  on  Granivorous  Birds — INTECOL — is  organizing  a spe- 
cial symposium  along  the  theme  of  “The  role  of  granivorous  birds,  especially  Corvidae  and 
Columhidae,  in  ecosystems.”  Such  problems  as  population  dynamics,  biomass  and  production 
rates,  energetics,  impact  of  granivorous  birds  on  ecosystems  and  management  of  pest  situ- 
ations will  be  covered. 

.\11  correspondence  and  requests  for  scientific  information  should  be  sent  to:  Prof.  Dr.  Jan 
Pinowski.  Institute  of  Ecolog>’  PAN.  Dziekanow  Lesny,  05-150  Lomianki,  Poland  or  by  telex 
817378  lEPANPL. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  207-217 


CANADA  GOOSE  BROOD  BEHAVIOR  AND  SURVIVAL 
ESTIMATES  AT  CREX  MEADOWS,  WISCONSIN 

Michael  C.  Zicus 

Many  studies  have  reported  on  the  biology  of  Canada  Goose  {Branta 
canadensis)  broods  in  a variety  of  geographical  locations  and  habitat  types. 
Different  methods  based  on  observations  of  both  marked  and  unmarked 
broods,  however,  have  yielded  a wide  range  of  results  concerning  brood 
loss,  brood  mixing  and  gosling  survival.  This  paper  reports  on  a study  of 
Canada  Goose  broods  in  a managed,  reestablished  flock.  The  objectives 
were  to  describe  certain  aspects  of  the  behavior  and  survival  in  individ- 
ually identified  broods,  to  examine  some  potential  biases  inherent  in  goose 
brood  studies  and  to  compare  the  results  with  data  collected  in  other 
studies. 

STUDY  AREA  AND  METHODS 

The  study  was  done  at  the  12,185-ha  Crex  Meadows  Wildlife  Management  Area  in  north- 
western Wisconsin  near  Grantsburg,  Burnett  Co.  The  Wisconsin  Department  of  Natural 
Resources  began  management  of  marsh-prairie  habitats  on  Crex  Meadows  in  1947  and  an 
effort  to  reestablish  nesting  Giant  Canada  Geese  {B.  c.  maxima)  began  in  1952  (Hunt  and 
Jahn  1966).  Production  of  goslings  increased  from  virtually  nothing  in  1957  to  approximately 
480  in  1973  (Zicus  1974:83).  These  Canada  Geese  are  migratory  and  usually  arrive  in  early 
March  and  begin  nesting  in  mid-March  or  early  April.  Nesting  now  occurs  throughout  the 
study  area,  but  most  brood  rearing  takes  place  in  5 marshes.  Accordingly,  other  marshes 
are  used  very  little  by  Canada  Geese  during  the  summer. 

Wetlands  vary  in  size  from  less  than  a hectare  to  several  hundred  hectares  in  size.  Many 
wetlands  are  shallow  sedge  (mostly  Carex  stricta)  and  grass  (mostly  Calamagrostis  cana- 
densis) meadows.  There  are  also  numerous  impounded  marshes  with  varying  amounts  of 
open  water,  emergent  vegetation  and  floating  mats  of  sedge  {Carex  spp.)  and  cattail  (Typha 
angustifolia).  Uplands  are  forests  of  jack  pine  {Pinus  banksiana)  and  northern  pin  oak  (Quer- 
cus  ellipsoidalis)  and  brush-prairie  savanna  (Vogl  1964).  The  area  has  had  a long  fire  histor>% 
and  habitats  are  managed  intensively  through  controlled  burning  of  wetlands  and  uplands 
and  the  manipulating  of  water  levels  in  many  once  drained  marshes.  Approximately  121  ha 
of  cropland  are  also  planted  annually  in  the  center  of  the  management  area  to  provide 
supplemental  food  for  wildlife. 

Sixty-three  marked  families  from  known  nest  locations  and  74  marked  pairs  with  goslings 
from  undiscovered  nests  were  observed.  Several  marking  techniques  were  used,  but  most 
of  the  data  involved  131  families  in  which  one  or  both  of  the  adults  had  vinylite  neckbands 
(Sherwood  1966a).  Limited  data  were  also  obtained  from  6 clutches  of  eggs  injected  with 
vegetable  dyes  (Evans  1951).  Geese  were  captured  by  cannon  netting  in  autumn  (Dfll  and 
Thornsberry  1950),  summer  drive  trapping  (Gooch  1955)  and  mist  netting  nesting  females 
(Zicus  1975).  Nests  were  located  in  1972-1974  by  intensively  searching  the  study  area  on 
foot  and  from  a canoe.  Clutch-size,  fate  of  the  clutch,  egg  fertility  and  number  of  goslings 
hatched  were  determined  for  each  nest. 

Observations  were  made  daily  from  dike  roads  and  accessible  points  in  the  marshes.  Time 
of  day,  number  of  broods  seen  together,  number  of  goslings  in  each  family,  gosling  age,  as 

207 


208 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


well  as  location  and  activity  were  recorded  for  all  marked  families  observed.  The  ages  of 
goslings  hatched  from  undiscovered  nests  were  estimated  by  comparing  gosling  size,  plumage 
and  behavior  with  goslings  in  known-age  broods.  Goslings,  usually  brooded  on  the  nest  until 
the  morning  after  hatching  (Cooper  1978:53),  were  considered  1 day  old  at  departure.  All 
references  to  broods  are  to  individually  identified  broods  unless  otherwise  stated. 

Gosling  survival  was  estimated  at  7-day  intervals  each  year  by  counting  goslings  with 
neckbanded  adults.  Survival  during  the  first  7 days  after  hatching  was  determined  using  the 
last  complete  gosling  count  during  the  first  week  after  hatching  for  marked  families  whose 
numbers  at  hatching  were  known.  Estimates  for  each  week  of  age  up  to  8 weeks  were  made  i 
using  the  last  gosling  counts  from  families  observed  in  consecutive  weeks.  Weekly  survival 
estimates  determined  in  this  way  should  not  have  been  biased  by  gosling  adoption  if  the  ! 
marked  pairs  under  observation  adopted  goslings  and  lost  goslings  to  adoption  at  the  same 
rate  as  those  pairs  not  being  observed  during  the  7-day  interval. 

The  survival  of  goslings  through  8 weeks  of  age  was  estimated  by  2 methods.  The  first 
method  combined  weekly  survival  estimates,  while  the  second  involved  a modeling  process 
using  a number  of  reproductive  parameters  estimated  during  the  study.  These  estimates 
included  the  proportion  of  the  pairs  raising  broods  through  8 weeks  (successful  pairs)  and 
the  proportion  of  the  pairs  hatching  goslings  but  not  raising  a brood  (unsuccessful  pairs),  and 
the  average  brood  size  at  hatching  for  both  successful  and  unsuccessful  pairs.  This  allowed 
the  number  of  goslings  hatched  by  any  given  number  of  pairs  to  be  determined.  Next  the 
number  of  goslings  hatched  by  both  successful  and  unsuccessful  pairs  was  multiplied  by  the 
apparent  gosling  survival  for  each  type  of  pair.  The  apparent  survival  of  goslings  in  broods 
of  unsuccessful  pairs  was  zero,  while  the  apparent  survival  in  broods  with  successful  pairs 
was  determined  by  comparing  the  numbers  of  8-week-old  goslings  with  different  marked 
pairs  to  the  number  of  goslings  hatched  by  these  pairs.  In  this  way,  the  total  number  of 
goslings  alive  after  8 weeks  could  be  compared  with  the  total  number  hatched  by  any  given 
number  of  pairs. 

RESULTS  I 

Canada  Geese  were  never  observed  rearing  broods  singly  on  marshes  j 

that  were  not  being  used  by  other  families.  Many  Canada  Goose  pairs  left  i 

their  nesting  marshes  soon  after  hatching  a brood  (Table  1).  Between  60  j 

and  67%  of  all  marked  families  moved,  although,  in  many  cases,  the  nest-  i 

ing  marshes  appeared  to  be  similar  to  those  used  for  brood  rearing.  Fur-  ( 

thermore,  25-53%  of  the  pairs  observed  nesting  successfully  on  a major  ^ 

brood  rearing  marsh  also  moved  their  young  to  a different  brood  rearing 
marsh. 

The  time  between  departure  from  the  nest  and  the  first  observation  of 
a brood  on  the  marshes  used  for  brood  rearing  was  short  and  suggested 
immediate  and  direct  movement  to  the  brood  rearing  marshes  (Table  2). 
One  family  was  observed  4.8  km  from  the  nest  within  24  h of  leaving  the 
nest;  in  2 consecutive  years,  another  pair  moved  their  broods  8.4  km  in 
a maximum  of  2 days.  Almost  53%  of  the  families  originating  from  nests 
in  major  brood  rearing  marshes  were  observed  on  a different  marsh  within  j 

4 days  after  departure  from  the  nest.  Similarly,  40%  of  the  broods  leaving  j 

nests  in  marshes  that  were  not  used  for  brood  rearing  were  observed  on  ■ 

another  marsh  within  4 days.  j 


i\ 


Zicus  • CANADA  GOOSE  BROOD  BEHAVIOR 


209 


Table  1 

Early  Movement  of  Marked  Canada  Goose  Families,  1972-1974 


Marked  famihes 

1972 

1973 

1974 

AU 

years 

All  successful  nests 

9 

20 

22 

51 

Broods  leaving  marsh 

6 

12 

14 

32 

Percent 

67 

60 

64 

63 

Successful  nests  in  major  brood  rearing  marshes 

4 

15 

17 

36 

Broods  leaving  marsh 

1 

7 

9 

17 

Percent 

25 

47 

53 

47 

Distances  between  the  nest  locations  and  the  centers  of  the  marshes 
that  were  first  used  for  brood  rearing  ranged  from  0. 7-8.4  km  (Table  3). 
The  major  brood  rearing  marshes  were  centrally  located  and  Canada 
Goose  families  leaving  nests  in  these  marshes  did  not  have  to  move  as  far 
to  reach  other  major  brood  rearing  marshes  as  families  from  nests  in  pe- 
ripheral marshes.  Nonetheless,  41%  of  the  families  from  nests  in  major 
brood  marshes  moved  more  than  3.0  km  to  reach  their  initial  brood  marsh- 
es. The  longest  distances  moved  were  those  from  nesting  marshes  that 
were  not  used  for  brood  rearing  with  27%  of  these  families  moving  more 
than  7.5  km. 

Canada  Geese  usually  remained  on  their  first  brood  rearing  marsh  for 
the  entire  brood  rearing  period.  For  the  3 years,  an  average  of  86%  (N  = 
70)  of  the  marked  pairs  were  observed  each  year  on  only  1 brood  rearing 
marsh.  In  contrast,  10  pairs  (14%)  were  seen  during  the  early  portion  of 
the  brood  rearing  period  on  1 marsh  and  later  on  a second  marsh.  The 


Table  2 

Number  of  Days  Between  Nest  Departure  and  First  Observation  of  Canada 
Goose  Broods  on  Marshes  Other  Than  the  Nesting  Marshes,  All  Years 

Combined 


Days  since 
leaving  nest 

Hatched  in 
major  brood 
rearing  marshes 

N 

% 

1-4 

9 

53 

5-8 

1 

6 

9-12 

2 

12 

13-16 

1 

6 

17-20 

1 

6 

21  + 

3 

18 

Not  hatched  in 
major  brood 
rearing  marshes 

N % 


6 40 

4 27 

1 7 

1 7 

0 0 

3 20 


All  broods 


N 


47 

16 

9 

6 

3 

19 


6 


210 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  3 

Distances  Between  Nest  Locations  and  Center  of  Initial  Brood  Rearing 
Marshes  Used  by  Canada  Goose  Broods  Leaving  Nesting  Marshes,  All  Years 

Combined 


Distance 

(km) 

Hatched  in 
major  brood 
rearing  marshes 

Not  hatched  in 
major  brood 
rearing  marshes 

All  broods 

N 

% 

N 

% 

N 

% 

0.0-1.5 

4 

24 

2 

13 

6 

19 

L6-3.0 

6 

35 

8 

53 

14 

44 

3.1-4.5 

5 

29 

1 

7 

6 

19 

4.6-6.0 

2 

12 

0 

0 

2 

6 

6.1-7.5 

0 

0 

0 

0 

0 

0 

7. 6-9.0 

0 

0 

4 

27 

4 

12 

movement  to  a second  marsh  usually  occurred  within  a few  days  of  the 
time  the  adults  molted  their  flight  feathers.  Movement  distances  between 
the  center  of  the  initial  and  second  brood  rearing  marsh  ranged  from  1.4- 
4.8  km  and  averaged  2.3  km. 

The  brood  rearing  period  was  divided  into  pre-molt  and  post-molt  seg- 
ments to  examine  yearly  fidelity  to  specific  brood  rearing  marshes.  Indi- 
vidual pairs  were  faithful  to  specific  marshes  from  year  to  year.  Before 
molting,  11  pairs  successfully  raising  broods  in  2 consecutive  years  and 
2 pairs  raising  broods  in  3 consecutive  years  used  the  same  marshes  each 
year.  Likewise,  after  molting  and  before  gaining  flight,  13  pairs  raising 
broods  in  2 consecutive  years  and  2 pairs  raising  broods  in  3 consecutive 
years  used  the  same  marshes.  In  addition,  2 pairs  moving  to  a second 
marsh  for  the  post-molt  segment  during  the  same  season  made  a similar 
move  when  rearing  young  in  a second  year.  Another  pair  used  the  same 
marsh  through  the  brood  period  in  1972  and  1973,  but  used  a different 
marsh  through  the  brood  period  in  1974. 

Of  3 marked  females  with  previous  brood  rearing  experience  that  paired 
with  different  ganders  and  5 marked  males  with  previous  experience  rear- 
ing broods  and  paired  with  different  females,  all  3 females  used  the  same 
marshes  they  last  used,  while  2 of  the  5 ganders  reared  broods  on  marshes 
other  than  the  one  they  last  used.  In  1 case,  both  members  of  the  new 
pair  had  previously  raised  broods  on  different  marshes.  The  goslings 
hatched  by  this  pair  were  raised  on  the  marsh  last  used  by  the  female  and 
not  the  male. 

As  broods  began  to  concentrate  in  the  brood  rearing  areas,  both  marked 
and  unmarked  broods  fed  together  along  dikes  and  on  floating  mats  of 
vegetation.  At  times,  goslings  became  separated  from  their  parents  and 


Zicus  • CANADA  GOOSE  BROOD  BEHAVIOR 


211 


Table  4 

Minimum  Estimates  of  Gosling  Adoption  for  Canada  Goose  Pairs  by 
Brood-age  Intervals,  All  Years  Combined 

Weekly 

Brood-age  interval 

Pairs  observed 

Pairs  showing 
increased  brood  size 

N 

Hatch-week  1 

46 

11 

24 

Week  1-week  2 

42 

11 

26 

W eek  2-week  3 

47 

15 

W eek  3-week  4 

49 

8 

16 

Week  4-week  5 

40 

4 

10 

Week  5— week  6 

27 

2 

7 

Week  6— week  7 

26 

2 

8 

W eek  7-week  8 

23 

1 

4 

broodmates,  and  many  were  adopted  into  other  families;  of  87  pairs  ob- 
' served  during  the  3 years,  a minimum  of  40  (46%)  adopted  goslings  at 
I some  time.  All  pairs  adopting  goslings  could  not  be  determined,  because 
1'  all  broods  were  not  observed  frequently  enough  to  detect  gosling  adoption 
i that  compensated  for  goslings  lost  to  other  pairs  or  through  mortality. 

I However,  based  on  observations  of  only  those  broods  increasing  in  size, 

I a minimum  of  36-50%  of  the  marked  pairs  adopted  young  into  their  broods 
[ between  hatch  and  8 weeks  of  age.  Adopted  goslings  were  usually  about 
f the  same  age  as  their  new  broodmates. 

Adoption  was  most  common  during  the  first  2 weeks  after  hatching 
(Table  4).  The  number  of  goslings  associated  with  some  pairs  would  change 
daily  as  the  broods  fed  on  mats  of  vegetation  or  moved  from  favored  feed- 
ing sites  along  the  dikes.  Sometimes,  goslings  joining  another  brood  would 
i rejoin  their  own  family  within  several  minutes.  Other  instances  of  adoption 

(appeared  to  be  more  permanent,  and  limited  gosling  adoption  continued 
through  8 weeks  of  age. 

1!  An  average  of  24.2%  of  the  successful  nesting  pairs  did  not  raise  broods 

to  flight  (range  17.9-33.3%).  Pairs  raising  broods  and  those  that  did  not 
I had  different  reproductive  characteristics  (Table  5).  Egg  fertility,  egg  suc- 
I cess  and  the  average  brood  size  at  hatching  were  significantly  lower  for 
: pairs  that  did  not  raise  a brood  (x“  = 8.21,  df  = 1,  P < 0.01;  x"  = 6.74, 
df  = 1,  P < 0.01;  and  t = 2.24,  df  = 60,  P < 0.05).  Pairs  that  did  not 
) raise  young  also  tended  to  have  lower  average  clutch-sizes  {t  = 1.19,  df  = 
I 60,  P = 0.24  [NSD,  but  not  different  hatching  success  (x“  = 0.02,  df  = 
i 1,  NS).  In  addition,  a greater  proportion  of  the  pairs  that  failed  to  raise 
I young  also  tended  to  have  at  least  one  2-year-old  pair  member  than  did 
. those  pairs  that  successfully  raised  young. 


212 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  5 

Brood  Rearing  Success  and  Reproductive  Characteristics  of  Successful 
Nesting  Canada  Geese,  1972-1974 


Characteristic 

Raised  brood 
(N  = 47) 

Did  not 
raise  brood 
(N  = 15) 

Average  clutch 

5.8 

5.3 

Fertility  (%) 

92.7 

81.1 

Hatching  success  {%'f 

97.4 

95.0 

Egg  success  (%)*’ 

86.2 

73.6 

Average  brood  size  at  hatching^ 

5.0 

3.9 

“ Equals  percent  of  fertile  eggs  that  hatch  (Cooper  1978:61). 
**  Equals  percent  of  all  eggs  that  hatch  (Cooper  1978:61). 
Equals  average  clutch  times  egg  success. 


Most  gosling  mortality  occurred  during  the  first  12  days  after  hatch 
(Table  6).  In  1 interval,  a survival  estimate  greater  than  100%  was  ob- 
tained when  the  observed  pairs  adopted  more  goslings  than  they  lost  to 
adoption  in  the  interval.  Based  on  the  combination  of  weekly  estimates, 
gosling  survival  through  8 weeks  averaged  61.2%  (range  47.7-71.5%)  dur- 
ing the  study.  By  comparison,  the  apparent  survival  to  8 weeks  in  broods 
with  successful  pairs  averaged  80.5%  (range  76.7-82.9%),  but  estimated 
gosling  survival  was  lowered  to  an  average  of  62.5%  (range  60.7-70.5%) 
when  the  reproductive  performance  of  the  flock  was  modeled  to  include 
goslings  produced  by  pairs  unsuccessful  in  rearing  a brood.  Both  estimates 
based  on  a combination  of  weekly  survival  rates  and  those  based  on  mod- 
eling reproductive  performances  gave  similar  results  in  each  year. 


Table  6 

Canada  Goose  Gosling  Survival  Estim.^tes  by  Weekly  Age  Intervals,  1972-1974 


Brood-age 

.\verage  age 
(days) 

No.  of 
broods 

Survival 

(%) 

Hatch-week  1 

5 

30 

86.5 

W eek  1-week  2 

12 

32 

76.9 

W eek  2-week  3 

20 

28 

98.5 

W eek  3— week  4 

26 

33 

100.0 

W eek  4-week  5 

33 

31 

100.8 

W eek  5-week  6 

40 

21 

96.7 

W eek  6— week  7 

46 

15 

97.3 

W eek  7-week  8 

56  + 

15 

98.5 

Zicus  • CANADA  GOOSE  BROOD  BEHAVIOR 


213 


DISCUSSION 

The  movement  of  Canada  Goose  broods  from  nest  locations  to  brood 
rearing  areas  immediately  after  hatching  at  Crex  Meadows  was  similar  to 
that  reported  for  Canada  Geese  in  other  areas.  Canada  Goose  brood  move- 
ments to  selected  rearing  marshes  depend,  in  part,  on  the  distribution  of 
rearing  habitat  in  relation  to  nesting  areas.  Geis  (1956:416)  reported  that 
geese  nesting  on  islands  where  no  food  was  available  in  Flathead  Lake, 
Montana,  moved  broods  to  rearing  areas  6-10  miles  (10-16  km)  away  im- 
mediately after  hatching;  those  geese  nesting  along  the  Flathead  River 
moved  broods  downstream  from  the  nest-sites  to  brood  rearing  areas. 
Macinnes  and  Lieff  (1968:99-101)  observed  that  broods  near  the  Mc- 
Connell River  in  the  Northwest  Territories  moved  10-15  km  from  nest 
locations  to  feeding  areas.  In  contrast,  Dimmick  (1968:53)  reported  goose 
broods  at  Jackson  Hole,  Wyoming,  left  the  immediate  vicinity  of  the  nest, 
but  remained  in  the  nesting  area  for  several  weeks.  Most  individual  pairs 
and  females  at  Crex  Meadows  made  the  same  movements  to  a brood 
rearing  area  each  year,  and  once  there,  they  rarely  changed  location  until 
the  young  were  grown  and  the  adults  had  regained  flight.  Similarly,  Geis 
(1956:416)  reported  that  broods  rarely  moved  to  other  rearing  grounds  once 
they  were  established  on  a rearing  area,  and  Martin  (1964:23)  observed 
many  pairs  at  Ogden  Bay,  Utah,  using  the  same  rearing  areas  in  consec- 
utive years. 

The  patterns  of  rearing  marsh  selection  observed  at  Crex  Meadows  have 
probably  developed  with  the  growth  of  the  flock  and  reflect  the  distribution 
of  marshes  with  food  and  molting  security  and  the  preference  of  individual 
nesting  females.  Cooper  (1978:23)  reported  individual  female  Canada 
Geese  nesting  in  approximately  the  same  locations  each  year,  and  Martin 
(1964:16)  and  Brakhage  (1965:768)  observed  older  geese  establishing  nests 
first.  These  factors  may  force  novice  nesters  to  establish  territories  in  the 
available  unoccupied  habitat  which  may  or  may  not  be  near  the  brood 
rearing  marshes.  Sherwood  (1966b:70)  reported  novice  nesting  2-year  olds 
nested  and/or  raised  their  broods  in  the  same  general  area  that  they  had 
been  hatched  or  reared  in,  but  did  not  discuss  any  specific  influence  on 
site  selection  by  the  male  or  the  female  of  the  pair.  Martin  (1964:23), 
however,  was  unable  to  observe  any  definite  pattern  in  rearing  area  selec- 
tion by  adults  with  their  young  in  Utah.  The  movements  of  broods  to 
rearing  areas  and  from  rearing  marshes  used  for  nesting  to  different  ones 
that  were  observed  at  Crex  Meadows  could  persist  if  geese  established 
nests  wherever  possible  in  the  marshes,  but  females  preferred  the  marshes 
for  brood  rearing  that  they  had  previsouly  used.  I speculate  that  females 
may  initially  use  the  marshes  that  they  themselves  were  raised  in,  thus 
explaining  how  these  movement  patterns  might  evolve.  Numerous  authors 


214 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


have  concluded  that  the  female  Canada  Goose  rather  than  the  male  choos- 
es the  nest-site  (Collias  and  Jahn  1959:485,  Brakhage  1965:757,  and  oth- 
ers), and  that  rearing  marsh  selection  may  be  similar  and  may  depend  on 
the  initial  and  subsequent  experiences  of  the  female. 

Crex  Meadows  goslings  were  commonly  adopted  from  one  brood  to 
another.  Gosling  adoption  was  most  prevalent  during  the  first  2 weeks  of 
age,  but  occurred  until  at  least  8 weeks  of  age.  In  contrast,  Martin 
(1964:25)  observed  no  change  in  brood  size  after  3 weeks  of  age,  and 
Sherwood  (1966b:  124-127)  found  that  brood  size  changes  were  most  prev- 
alent during  the  first  2 or  3 weeks,  but  that  they  still  occurred  into  the 
fourth  week  after  hatch.  Sherwood  (1966b:  129)  also  reported  that  goslings 
could  not  recognize  their  broodmates  or  parents  until  5 or  6 weeks  of  age. 
Unlike  observations  in  Missouri  (Brakhage  1965:767),  abnormally  large 
broods  or  broods  escorted  by  more  than  1 pair  formed  infrequently  and 
were  never  observed  with  marked  pairs  at  Crex  Meadows. 

The  loss  of  entire  broods  at  Crex  Meadows  was  similar  to  that  reported 
at  the  Seney  National  Wildlife  Refuge  (NWR),  but  different  from  that 
reported  near  the  McConnell  River.  At  least  6 of  20  marked  pairs  observed 
at  the  Seney  NWR  in  1965  lost  their  entire  broods  (Sherwood  1966b:  132); 
brood  loss  may  have  been  even  greater  if  some  pairs  lost  their  broods 
before  Sherwood  first  observed  them.  However,  only  3 of  96  marked  pairs 
lost  entire  broods  near  the  McConnell  River  (Maclnnes  et  al.  1974:696). 
Pairs  losing  broods  at  Crex  Meadows  had  smaller  clutches  with  lower 
fertility  and  hatched  fewer  young;  these  characteristics  have  been  asso- 
ciated with  younger  birds  (Brakhage  1965:760,  Cooper  1978:53,  74).  Sher- 
wood (1966b:  130-131)  also  observed  4 of  6 marked  pairs  with  2-year-old 
females  lose  their  broods.  While  not  defining  the  phrase,  he  concluded 
that  the  ability  to  hold  a brood  was  related  primarily  to  the  “age  of  the 
pair.“  Since  he  presented  only  data  on  the  ages  of  the  females  in  the 
marked  pairs,  I believe  he  was  referring  primarily  to  female  age  when 
discussing  “age  of  the  pair."  Other  aspects  of  pair  age,  such  as  the  age 
of  the  gander  or  the  length  of  time  individual  geese  had  been  mated,  could 
also  be  important  if  older  geese  and  those  mated  for  the  longest  time 
developed  the  strongest  brood  rearing  abilities.  Sherwood  (1966b)  further 
concluded  that  the  ability  to  hold  a brood  was  secondarily  related  to  the 
size  of  the  brood  at  hatching.  The  actual  size  of  the  brood  might  be  im- 
portant if  goslings,  unable  to  recognize  their  parents  or  broodmates,  were 
attracted  to  larger  broods  as  Sherwood  speculated.  Broods  were  concen- 
trated at  both  Crex  Meadows  and  the  Seney  NWR,  where  brood  loss  was 
higher,  whereas  they  were  more  dispersed  at  the  McConnell  River  where 
loss  was  lower.  The  greater  loss  of  broods  at  Crex  Meadows  and  the  Seney 
NWR  probably  resulted  from  prolonged  contacts  between  different  pairs 


Zicus  • CANADA  GOOSE  BROOD  BEHAVIOR 


215 


with  those  pairs  made  up  of  the  youngest  geese,  or  perhaps  those  paired 
for  the  shortest  time  the  most  likely  to  lose  golsings. 

Studies  relying  solely  on  marked  geese  to  estimate  gosling  survival  are 
few  in  number  and  make  comparisons  with  Crex  Meadows  difficult. 
Macinnes  et  al.  (1974:697-699)  reported  that  the  survival  of  goslings  with 
neckbanded  adults,  from  6 days  before  hatching  to  approximately  7 days 
of  age,  ranged  from  64.7-87.3%  near  the  McConnell  River.  Survival 
from  7-35  days  of  age  was  91.9-99.3%.  When  the  McConnell  River  esti- 
mates are  combined  with  the  approximate  3%  loss  of  entire  broods 
Macinnes  et  al.  (1974:697)  reported,  survival  to  35  days  old  ranged  from 
60.0-83.7%  with  a 5-year  mean  of  72.6%.  In  comparison,  survival  to  33 
days  old  at  Crex  Meadows  ranged  from  57.2-81.0%  with  a 3-year  mean  of 
64.7%. 

Gosling  survival  has  been  determined  in  other  studies  by  comparing 
total  goslings  hatched  with  the  goslings  alive  at  some  time  later  or  by 
observing  changes  in  average  brood  size  over  a period  of  time.  Estimates 
using  total  gosling  counts  are  as  reliable  as  the  investigator  is  accurate  in 
determining  the  number  of  goslings  hatched  that  use  a specific  rearing 
area  and  in  subsequently  counting  all  survivors  from  this  group  of  goslings. 
In  many  situations,  accurate  counts  of  all  goslings  hatched  in  an  area  are 
almost  impossible.  Estimates  obtained  using  total  gosling  counts  have 
ranged  from  80-84%  in  Montana  (Geis  1956:417),  64-80%  in  Missouri 
(Brakhage  1965:768)  and  16-78%  in  Michigan  (Sherwood  1966b:47).  In 
comparison,  survival  estimates  based  on  average  brood  size  comparisons 
are  biased  if  any  pairs  lose  their  entire  brood.  Estimates  using  this  method 
have  sometimes  revealed  average  brood  sizes  greater  than  the  average 
hatch  per  successful  nest  (Williams  and  Marshall  1938:17-18,  Steel  et  al. 
1957:4,  Martin  1964:50).  These  authors  estimated  gosling  survival  until 
late  in  the  brood  period  at  93-97%. 

The  behavior  of  Canada  Goose  broods  at  Crex  Meadows  created  a se- 
rious potential  bias  for  estimating  gosling  survival  and  flock  production. 
More  than  half  of  the  marked  pairs  observed  with  broods  were  from  nests 
that  were  not  found.  This  resulted  from  my  inability  to  find  all  the  nests 
on  Crex  Meadows  and  the  tendency  for  geese  to  move  broods  considerable 
distances  to  brood  rearing  marshes.  As  a result,  gosling  survival  could  not 
be  assessed  by  comparing  total  goslings  hatched  with  total  goslings  alive 
at  some  time  later.  Likewise  some  successful  nesting  pairs  lost  aU  of  their 
goslings  to  mortality  and/or  to  adoption  into  other  broods.  If  gosling  sur- 
vival was  calculated  by  a comparison  of  average  brood  size  at  hatching  to 
the  average  brood  size  at  fledging,  production  would  have  been  overesti- 
mated by  an  average  of  27%.  These  potential  biases  seem  likely  to  exist 
to  varying  degrees  in  any  goose  brood  study.  The  degree  to  which  esti- 


216 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


mates  will  be  biased  depends  on  the  behavior  of  pairs  and  their  young. 
Consequently,  gosling  survival  and  production  estimates  without  the  ben- 
efit of  marked  geese  should  be  viewed  cautiously. 

SUMMARY 

A study  of  marked  Canada  Geese  examined  the  use  of  brood  rearing  areas  and  brood  and 
gosling  survival  between  1972  and  1974  in  managed  marshes  in  northwestern  Wisconsin. 
Between  60  and  67%  of  the  pairs  hatching  goslings  moved  them  to  I of  5 major  rearing 
marshes  where  there  were  other  broods.  However,  25-53%  of  the  pairs  nesting  on  a major 
brood  rearing  marsh  also  moved  to  a different  brood  rearing  marsh  to  raise  their  young. 
Movements  of  aU  pairs  with  broods  ranged  from  0. 7-8.4  km,  and  were  made  immediately 
after  hatch  with  47%  of  the  families  reaching  their  first  rearing  marsh  in  less  than  4 days. 
Once  on  a rearing  marsh,  families  rarely  moved  to  another.  Almost  aU  pairs  raised  young  on 
the  same  marshes  in  subsequent  years.  Observations  of  males  and  females  with  previous 
brood  rearing  experience  that  had  formed  new  pairs  between  years  suggested  females  may 
influence  the  selection  of  a brood  marsh.  At  least  36-50%  of  the  pairs  adopted  goslings  into 
their  broods  at  sometime  between  hatch  and  8 weeks.  Adoption  was  most  prevalent  before 
goslings  were  2 weeks  old.  Lrom  18-33%  of  aU  pairs  failed  to  raise  their  young  to  flight. 
These  pairs  also  had  lower  egg  fertility  and  brood  sizes  at  hatching  than  pairs  raising  young. 
Overall  gosling  survival  determined  by  the  observation  of  young  in  marked  broods  ranged 
from  60.7-70.5%.  Serious  biases  due  primarily  to  the  behavior  of  the  broods  affected  survival 
estimates  determined  in  other  ways  with  production  being  overestimated  by  an  average  of 
27%  if  the  loss  of  entire  broods  was  not  considered. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Special  thanks  to  W.  H.  Marshall  for  his  suggestions  during  the  study  and  for  reviewing 
the  manuscript.  N.  R.  Stone  and  J.  O.  Evrard,  of  the  Wisconsin  Department  of  Natural 
Resources,  and  the  other  members  of  the  Crex  Meadows  staff  provided  valuable  assistance 
in  the  field,  while  R.  A.  Hunt  provided  helpful  comments  on  the  study  and  with  J.  Bartelt 
reviewed  the  manuscript.  University  of  Minnesota  graduate  students  and  A.  H.  Grewe  with 
students  from  St.  Cloud  State  University,  St.  Cloud,  Minnesota,  helped  capture  the  many 
geese  banded  in  this  study  while  J.  A.  Cooper  contributed  in  many  ways.  Financial  support 
was  provided  by  a National  Defense  Education  Assistantship,  the  Malvin  E.  and  Josephine 
I).  Herz  Foundation,  the  Dayton's  Natural  History  Fund  and  the  Department  of  Entomology, 
Fisheries  and  Wildlife  at  the  University  of  Minnesota.  This  is  Paper  No.  1 1660  of  the  Scientific 
Journal  Series  of  the  Minnesota  Agricultural  Experiment  Station. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Brakhage,  G.  K.  1965.  Biology  and  behavior  of  tub-nesting  Canada  Geese.  J.  Wildl.  Man- 
age. 29:751-771. 

CoLLlAS,  N.  E.  AM)  L.  R.  Jahn.  1959.  Social  behavior  and  breeding  success  in  Canada 
Geese  (Branata  caruulensis)  confined  under  semi-natural  conditions.  Auk  76:478-509. 
CooCH,  G.  1955.  Modifications  in  mass  goose  trapping  technique.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  19:315- 
316. 

Cooper,  J.  A.  1978.  The  history  and  breeding  biology  of  Canada  Geese  of  Marshey  Point, 
Manitoba.  Wildl.  Monogr.  61. 

Dill,  H.  H.  and  W.  H.  Thornsberry.  1950.  A cannon  projected  net  trap  for  capturing 
waterfowl.  J.  W ildl.  Manage.  14:132-137. 


Zicus  • CANADA  GOOSE  BROOD  BEHAVIOR 


217 


Dimmick,  R.  W.  1968.  Canada  Geese  of  Jackson  Hole,  their  ecology  and  management. 
Wyoming  Game  Fish  Comm.  Bull.  No.  11. 

Evans,  C.  D.  1951.  A method  of  color  marking  young  waterfowl.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  15:101- 
103. 

Geis,  M.  B.  1956.  Productivity  of  Canada  Geese  in  the  Flathead  Valley,  Montana.  J.  Wildl. 
Manage.  20:409-420. 

Hunt,  R.  A.  and  L.  R.  Jahn.  1966.  Canada  Goose  breeding  populations  in  Wisconsin. 
Wisconsin  Conserv.  Dept.  Tech.  Bull.  38. 

MacInnes,  C.  D.  and  B.  C.  Lieff.  1968.  Individual  behavior  and  composition  of  a local 
population  of  Canada  Geese.  Pp.  93-101  in  Canada  Goose  management  (R.  L.  Hine  and 
C.  Shoenfeld,  eds.).  Dembar  Educational  Res.  Serv.  Inc. 

, R.  A.  Davis,  R.  N.  Jones,  B.  C.  Lieff  and  A.  J.  Pakulak.  1974.  Reproductive 

efficiency  of  McConnell  River  small  Canada  Geese.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  38:686-707. 
Martin,  F.  W.  1964.  Behavior  and  survival  of  Canada  Geese  in  Utah.  Utah  State  Dept. 
Fish  Game  Infor.  Bull.  64-67. 

Sherwood,  G.  A.  1966a.  Flexible  plastic  collars  compared  to  nasal  discs  for  marking  geese. 
J.  Wildl.  Manage.  30:853-855. 

. 1966b.  Canada  Geese  of  the  Seney  National  Wildlife  Refuge.  Ph.D.  thesis,  Utah 

State  Univ.,  Logan,  Utah. 

Steel,  P.  E.,  P.  D.  Dalke  and  E.  G.  Bizeau.  1957.  Canada  Goose  production  at  Gray’s 
Lake,  Idaho,  1949-51.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  21:38-41. 

VoGL,  R.  J.  1964.  Vegetational  history  of  Crex  Meadows,  a prairie  savanna  in  northwestern 
Wisconsin.  Am.  Midi.  Nat.  72:157-175. 

Williams,  C.  S.  and  W.  H.  Marshall.  1938.  Survival  of  Canada  Goose  goslings.  Bear 
River  Refuge,  Utah,  1937.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  2:17-19. 

Zicus,  M.  C.  1974.  A study  of  the  giant  Canada  Geese  nesting  at  Crex  Meadows,  Wisconsin. 

M.S.  thesis,  Univ.  Minnesota,  St.  Paul,  Minnesota. 

. 1975.  Capturing  nesting  Canada  Geese  with  mist  nets.  Bird-Banding  46:168. 

DEPT.  ENTOMOLOGY,  FISHERIES,  AND  WILDLIFE,  UNIV.  MINNESOTA,  ST. 
PAUL,  MINNESOTA  55108.  (PRESENT  ADDRESS:  MINNESOTA  DEPT.  NAT- 
URAL RESOURCES,  WETLAND  WILDLIFE  RESEARCH  GROUP,  BEMIDJI, 
MINNESOTA  56601.)  ACCEPTED  2 JUNE  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2).  1981,  pp.  218-242 


BREEDING  BIRD  POPULATIONS  IN  THE  GREAT 
SMOKY  MOUNTAINS,  TENNESSEE  AND 
NORTH  CAROLINA 

S.  Charles  Kendeigh  and  Ben  J.  Fawver 

The  analysis  of  bird  populations  in  mountain  systems,  such  as  the  Great 
Smoky  Mountains,  is  of  special  interest  because  of  the  relations  of  these 
populations  to  the  mosaic  of  vegetation  types  and  to  variations  in  climate 
and  physical  conditions  that  occur.  The  Great  Smoky  Mountains  of  eastern 
Tennessee  and  western  North  Carolina  lie  at  the  southern  end  of  the 
Appalachian  Mountain  System,  escaped  glaciation  during  the  Pleistocene 
epoch,  have  sufficient  elevation  to  provide  a considerable  gradient  of  tem- 
perature and  contain  a variety  of  slope  exposures  with  striking  contrasts 
in  moisture  conditions  (Shanks  1954,  Whittaker  1956). 

The  many  different  types  of  vegetation  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains 
National  Park  are  in  nearly  virgin  condition.  The  deciduous  plant  com- 
munities have  remained  relatively  undisturbed  since  the  early  Tertiary, 
and  their  diversity  of  plant  species  is  the  richest  within  the  deciduous 
forest  biome.  Coniferous  forests  of  different  types  and  past  history  occur 
at  both  low  and  high  elevations. 

Breeding  bird  censuses  were  taken  in  26  areas,  representing  8 types  of 
climax  or  relatively  stable  vegetation  and  4 serai  stages.  The  fieldwork 
was  carried  out  during  June  and  July  1947  and  from  May  through  July 
1948. 


PL\NT  COMMUNITIES 

The  vegetation  shows  a continuum  of  change  with  elevation  and  slope  exposures  (Fig.  1). 
For  convenience,  however,  separate  plant  communities  are  recognized,  based  on  their  dom- 
inant species  (\^  hittaker  1956). 

Cove  hardwoods  occur  characteristically  at  lower  elevations  in  moist  shaded  coves  and  on 
north-facing  slopes.  Eastern  hemlock  (Tsuga  canadensis)  occurs  in  some  cove  forests  in 
mixed  or  nearly  pure  stands.  Chestnut  oak  (Quercus  prinus)  (chestnut)  and  oak-hickory 
forests  prevail  on  east  and  west  slopes,  while  on  exposed,  drier  south  and  southwest  slopes, 
subject  also  to  more  frequent  fires,  southern  pine  {Pinus  spp.)  forest  or  pines  with  an  un- 
dergrowth of  heath  predominate. 

.\t  intermediate  elevations,  cove  forests  extend  up  into  beech  forests  in  gaps  between 
mountains  or  on  sheltered  slopes,  northern  red  oak  (Q.  rubra)  (chestnut)  and  white  oak  {Q. 
alba)  (chestnut)  forests  replace  the  chestnut  oak  (chestnut)  forest,  while  on  exposed  ridges 
grassy  balds  replace  trees.  Chestnut  (Castanea)  is,  of  course,  no  longer  a dominant,  although 
at  the  time  of  this  study  many  dead  stubs  scattered  through  the  forest  indicated  their  former 
importance.  At  cooler  high  elevations,  spruce  (Pzcea )-fir  (Abies)  forests  predominate,  al- 
though they  give  way  to  heath  balds  on  xeric  exposed  ridges. 


218 


Elevation 


Kendeigh  and  Faivver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  219 


Meters  Feet 

6500 

6000 

5500 

5000 

4500 

4000 

3500 

3000 

2500 

2000 

1500 

coves,  sheltered  slopes  exposed  slopes,  ridges 

Fig.  1.  Mosaic  of  plant  communities  showing  variations  with  elevation  and  slope  expo- 
sure (modified  from  Whittaker  1956). 

METHODS  OF  ANALYSIS 


spruce -Fir  Forest 


Grassy 

Bald' 

Heath. 

Bald 


/ I 1 
/ / 


1500 


Beech  Forest 


/ 


/ / 
/ 


Cove 

Forest 


1000- 


500  - 


/ 


//  Red  Oak 
y y (Chestnut) 
/ ^ Forest 

/ ^ // 

/ // 7 

-// 


/ 


White  \ 
/ 0°' 
/(Chestnut)' 
t Forest  ^ 


/ 


V 


/ 


Table 

Mountain 

Pine  Forest 


/ 

/ ^ / A C*^®stnut  Oak 

I ^ ^ (Chestnut) 

/ //  \ Forest 

/ \ 

I Red  Oak  - 
' Hickory  Forest  , 

! 


/ 


/ I 

/Chestnut  i 

I Oak  I 

|(Chestnut)| 

, Heath  • 


\ 1 
\ I 


Pitch  Pine 
Heath 


Virginia  Pine 
Forest 


Mesic  ^ 


Xeric 


Bird  populations. — The  spot-map  method  for  censusing  breeding  birds  was  used  where 
the  terrain  and  available  time  permitted.  Areas  varied  in  size  and  shape  depending  on  terrain 
I and  slope.  They  ranged  from  6-12  ha  (Table  1),  which  are  rather  small  areas,  but  since  each 
major  vegetation  type  was  censused  at  different  localities,  the  total  area  covered  in  each 


I 


220 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


vegetation  type  was  at  least  23  ha  and  usually  over  40  ha.  Boundaries  were  measured  with 
a steel  tape  or  by  pacing  and  mapped  to  scale.  Use  was  made  of  natural  landmarks  and  of 
numbered  white  cards  nailed  to  trees  at  50-m  intervals  for  locating  birds,  especially  singing 
males,  on  maps.  In  several  instances,  areas  were  long  strips  along  trails  and  only  1 line  of 
markers  was  used.  Bird  counts  were  obtained  in  each  area  at  least  4 times  in  1947  and  5 
times  in  1948. 

Cruising  counts  only  were  taken  of  singing  males  in  several  areas.  These  areas  were  of 
known  size  and  thoroughly  covered  on  each  count.  Two  to  6 counts  were  made  on  each  area; 
the  largest  number  of  each  species  observed  on  any  count  was  taken  as  the  population  of 
that  species.  This  follows  Palmgren’s  (1930)  procedure  except  that  no  corrections  were  made 
for  smaller  values  often  obtained  with  fewer  than  4 or  5 counts.  Ten  comparable  censuses 
obtained  by  spot-map  and  cruising  procedures  in  5 different  types  of  vegetation,  although 
not  in  the  same  area  the  same  year,  showed  only  a slight  tendency  for  population  size 
estimates  to  be  lower  with  the  cruising  procedure. 

Species  occurring  in  different  plots  of  the  same  vegetation  type  have  been  combined  and 
their  population  sizes  averaged  (Table  2).  Persons  wishing  the  precise  location,  more  com- 
plete description  of  the  vegetation  and  data  on  the  bird  population  of  each  sample  plot  should 
consult  the  doctorate  thesis  of  the  junior  author  (Fawver  1950)  filed  in  the  University  of 
Illinois  Library,  Urbana. 

Data. — Coefficients  of  species  similarity  (S,)  were  calculated  with  the  Sprenson  equation 
(Able  and  Noon  1976): 

S,  = 2C/(A  + B)-100 

where  C represented  number  of  species  common  to  the  2 communities  and  A and  B total 
number  of  species  in  each  community.  Multiplying  by  100  transfers  coefficients  into  per- 
centages. In  these  comparisons,  species  were  included  with  less  than  0.5  pairs/40  ha  (shown 
by  -I-  marks  in  Table  2).  Larger-sized  sampling  areas  would  doubtless  have  permitted  quan- 
tification of  their  densities.  Percentages  higher  than  50  indicate  that  the  2 communities  have 
more  species  alike  than  different. 

The  above  equation  does  not  evaluate  the  difference  in  abundance  (number  of  pairs)  of  a 
species  when  it  occurs  in  both  communities.  Coefficients  of  population  similarity  (S„)  were 
obtained  by: 


5, 


1.0  - 


- P„) 


/^,  + 

where  p„  is  the  population  of  a species  in  community  a and  pt,  in  community  b and  Pa  and 
Pt,  are  total  populations  of  all  species  in  communities  a and  h,  respectively  (Odum  1950).  In 
this  calculation  the  plus  sign  in  Table  2 was  taken  as  zero  population. 

I’he  Shannon-Weiner  species  diversity  index  (//'  = Ip  log«.p)  and  equitability  index  W = 
D'lH'aiax)  were  calculated  for  the  birds  in  each  plant  community  where  p is  the  proportion 
each  species  population  is  of  the  whole  and  the  maximum  possible  diversity  for  the 

given  population  size  and  number  of  species,  is  the  logarithm  of  the  number  of  species 
(Fielou  1966,  Sheldon  1969). 

Mean  number  of  pairs  per  species  (p/s)  and  median  population  of  each  species  were  also 
determined.  Skewness  (^gi)  in  the  distribution  of  population  sizes  among  species  in  a com- 
munity was  calculated  with  the  equation  (Zar  1974): 


= 


(x  - .V)-’ 


(n  — l)(n  — 2)(sd)Vn 


where  Xi  = p/s  for  a species,  .v  is  the  mean  p/s,  n is  number  of  species,  and  sd  is  standard 
deviation.  Testing  for  statistical  significance  was  performed  using  Table  D25  of  Zar  (1974). 


Kendeigh  and  Fawver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  221 


Table  1 

Census  Areas  and  Bird  Censusing  Methods 

Plant  community  and  location 

Size 

(ha) 

Spot-mapping 

Cruising  counts 

1947 

1948 

1947 

1948 

Pine-oak  forest 

Mature,  6.4  km  NW  of  Gatlinburg 

12.0 

V 

V 

Serai,  opposite  Park  Headquarters 

10.2 

V 

V 

Serai,  1.6  km  farther  south 

6.0 

V 

Cove  forest 

Porters  Creek 

7.2 

V 

Porters  Creek 

9.2 

5 

Ramsey  Prong 

12.8 

5 

Ramsey  Prong 

6.8 

V 

3 

Ramsey  Prong 

6.0 

V 

Hemlock-deciduous  forest 

Roaring  Fork 

10.0 

V 

Roaring  Fork 

8.0 

2 

Brushy  Mountain 

11.6 

V 

4 

Chestnut  oak  (chestnut)  forest 

BuUhead  Trail 

7.6 

V 

6 

Greenbrier  Pinnacle  Trail 

9.0 

V 

Red  oak  (chestnut)  forest 

Greenbrier  Pinnacle  Trail 

7.5 

3 

Thomas  Divide,  N.C. 

10.0 

2 

Thomas  Divide,  N.C. 

7.2 

2 

Pine  heath 

BuUhead  Trail 

12.0 

V 

V 

Greenbrier  Pinnacle  Trail 

10.0 

3 

Gray  beech  forest 

Double  Springs  Gap 

7.6 

V 

West  of  Siler’s  Bald 

8.0 

V 

Spruce-fir  forest 

Climax,  between  Newfound  Gap  and 

Clingmans  Dome 

11.6 

V 

V 

Fraser  fir,  Clingmans  Dome 

6.8 

2 

Early  serai.  Mount  Buckley 

6.6 

V 

3 

Mid-seral,  Forney  Ridge 

5.6 

3 

Late  serai,  north  of  Andrews  Bald 

7.7 

3 

Heath  bald  „ 

BuUhead  Trail 

4.0 

2 

Brushy  Mountain 

10.0 

2 

I 


222 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


CENSUS  AREAS 

Pine -oak  forest  and  sere. — The  mature  forest  was  an  open  stand  of  trees  with  a shrub  layer 
of  Kalniia  and  other  Ericaceae.  The  ground  was  covered  with  a dry  litter  of  pine  and  broad 
leaves.  Two  samples  of  a serai  stage  leading  to  the  above  forest  contained  both  deciduous 
and  pine  trees,  1.8-6  m high,  scattered  through  shrubs  and  open  areas.  Shrubs  were  pre- 
dominantly greenbrier  (Smilax  spp.),  smooth  sumac  {Rhus  glabra)  and  briers  {Rubus  spp.). 
A luxurious  growth  of  herbs  covered  most  of  the  ground. 

Cove  forest. — AU  5 areas  censused  were  in  the  Greenbrier  section  of  the  Park.  In  2 areas 
along  Porters  Creek,  trees  were  widely  spaced  but  made  a deciduous  canopy  25^5  m, 
occasionally  60  m,  above  the  ground.  Great  rhododendron  {Rhododendron  maximum)  made 
dense  tangled  thickets  along  the  stream  and  herbs  were  luxuriant.  The  larger  area  had  been 
censused  by  Aldrich  and  Goodrum  (1946)  the  previous  year.  The  3 areas  along  Ramsey  Prong 
are  listed  in  ascending  elevations.  The  lowest  area  had  previously  been  cut  over  but  had 
regained  a closed  canopy.  The  shrub  layer  in  the  highest  area  included  sevenbark  {Hydran- 
gea arborescens)  prominently,  as  well  as  rhododendron. 

Mixed  hemlock-deciduous  forest. — Although  eastern  hemlock  occurred  prominently  in  the 
cove  forest,  it  was  the  principal  dominant  in  3 areas  censused.  Hemlock  attained  diameters 
over  1 m and  heights  of  30  m.  Beech  formed  an  understory  at  low  elevations  and  sweet 
{Betula  lenta)  and  yellow  {B.  allegheniensis)  birches  at  high  elevations.  Rhododendron  and 
sevenbark  were  the  principal  shrubs.  Herbs  were  much  reduced  compared  with  the  cove 
forest,  and  the  ground  was  covered  with  a thick  layer  of  dry  leaf  litter. 

Chestnut  oak  (chestnut)  forest. — Trees  in  this  forest  rarely  exceeded  0.5  m diameter  and 
were  usually  only  6-18  m tall.  Ericaceous  shrubs  were  dense,  especially  at  high  elevations. 

Red  oak  (chestnut)  forest. — No  tree  counts  were  taken  in  this  forest,  but  northern  red  oak 
was  most  prominent.  The  tree  canopy  was  more  closed  than  in  the  chestnut  oak  forest 
because  of  fewer  dead  chestnut  trees  and  the  shrub  stratum  was  greatly  reduced. 

Pine  heath. — Pines  were  widely  spaced  and  only  6-12  m high.  The  shrub  stratum  was 
dense.  The  herb  stratum  was  greatly  reduced  and  contained  bracken  fern  {Pteridium  sp.) 
and  some  creeping  vines. 

Gray  beech  forest. — Beech  {Fagus  grandifolia)  here  is  probably  a different  variety  than 
occurs  at  low  elevations  (Camp  1950).  Trees  were  generally  8-12  m tall,  widely  spaced  and 
with  intervening  space  covered  with  grasses,  sedges  and  shrubs. 

Spruce-fir  forest  and  sere. — The  climax  forest  contained  dense  tangles  of  mountain  rose 
bay  {Rhododendron  catawbiense)  and  the  ground  was  covered  everywhere  with  thick  spongy 
moss,  low  herbs  and  ferns.  Numerous  fallen  trees  were  covered  with  mosses  and  lichens. 
At  the  highest  elevation  on  Clingmans  Dome,  the  forest  consisted  principally  of  Eraser  fir 
{Abies  fraseri)  and  included  a few  mountain  ash  {Sorbus  americana)  with  the  ground  covered 
with  thick  mosses  and  Oxalis.  Trees  were  numerous  but  only  12-15  m high. 

\ recently  burned  over  area  was  well  covered  (90%)  with  shrubby  vegetation,  0.6-1. 5 m 
high,  composed  of  fire  cherry  {Primus  pennsylvanicus),  red  maple  {Acer  rubrum),  blackberry 
{Rubus  canadensis),  sevenbark.  red-berried  elder  {Sarnbucus  pubens)  and  several  species  of 
Ericaceae.  Dead  stubs  of  spruce  and  fir  were  scattered.  The  herb  layer  was  dense  and 
covered  70%  of  the  ground.  A second  area,  burned  over  earlier,  had  a dense  stand  of  fire 
cherry,  1.8-3  m high.  The  area  with  most  advanced  vegetation,  3-6  m high,  had  serviceberry 
{Arnelanchier).  fire  cherry,  yellow  birch  and  a few  small  spruce. 

\ heath  bald  along  Bullhead  Trail  was  covered  mostly  with  mountain  laurel  {Kalmia 
latifolia)  but  contained  some  fire  cherry  and  sourwood  {Oxydendrurn  arboreum).  The  vege- 
tation was  dense  and  about  3 m high.  Another  heath  bald  on  Brushy  Mountain  contained 
chiefly  mountain  rose  bay  1-2  m high. 


Kendeigh  and  Faivver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  223 


BIRD  POPULATIONS 

The  data  on  species  populations  (Table  2)  are  listed  in  taxonomic  order 
and  the  plant  communities  in  which  they  were  found.  The  plant  commu- 
nities are  arranged  as  to  whether  they  are  serai  or  mature  (climax),  ac- 
cording to  zones  of  altitude,  and  in  horizontal  gradients  from  north-facing 
slopes,  coves,  or  gaps  around  the  mountains  to  south-facing  slopes. 

Succession. — The  shrubby  serai  stage  at  low  elevations  had  a high  per- 
centage of  restricted  species  and  hence  low  Sg  and  S„  compared  with 
either  pine-oak  or  cove  forest  (Table  3).  It  likewise  had  little  similarity 
with  shrubby  serai  stages  at  high  elevations.  In  contrast,  the  shrubby  serai 
stages  at  high  elevations  had  an  avifauna  with  considerable  similarity  to 
that  of  the  spruce-fir  forest.  Heath  balds  consist  of  very  dense  shrubby 
vegetation  of  uncertain  successional  status  and  contained  only  3 species, 
none  of  them  restricted:  Gray  Catbird  (2.5  pairs/40  ha).  Black-throated 
Blue  Warbler  (19  pairs).  Rufous-sided  Towhee  (II  pairs). 

Forest  communities. — The  forest  contains  both  deciduous  and  conifer- 
ous species,  in  mixed  or  relatively  pure  stands,  and  with  physiognomies 
varying  from  taU,  luxuriant  cove  forest  to  short-statured  gray  beech  “or- 
chard” to  open  stands  of  pine  heath.  Relatively  high  Sg  occurred  among 
bird  populations  in  cove,  chestnut  oak,  red  oak  and  hemlock-deciduous 
forests  (Table  3).  Likewise  there  was  similarity  between  cove  forest  avi- 
fauna (after  those  species  commonly  associated  with  hemlock  were  elim- 
inated) and  pine-oak  avifauna.  Individual  bird  species  extended  widely 
among  these  deciduous  plant  communities  although  at  different  population 
levels. 

When  bird  species  commonly  associated  with  deciduous  trees  were 
eliminated  from  the  hemlock-deciduous  forest,  the  remaining  “hemlock” 
avifauna  was  similar  to  that  of  the  spruce-fir  forest.  There  was  consider- 
1 able  similarity  also  between  avifaunas  of  spruce-fir  forest  and  high  serai 
I stages,  even  though  these  plant  communities  are  of  different  vegetation 
I types. 

The  avifauna  of  the  pine  heath  was  not  closely  similar  to  any  other 
\ avifauna  nor  did  any  of  its  14  species  reach  maximum  population  here. 
I Many  of  its  species  occurred  commonly  in  deciduous  forests  or  shrub 
stages. 

I The  above  classification  of  avifaunas  into  separate  units  has  been  based 
largely  on  coefficients  of  Sg.  However,  no  Sg  above  50  has  a value  of  Sp 
lower  than  31,  and  no  Sg  below  50  has  a value  above  20. 

Slope  exposure  (moisture). — North  slopes  and  coves  in  the  mountains 
are  moist  and  shady,  east  and  west  slopes  intermediate  and  south  slopes 
dry  and  sunny.  Correlated  with  changes  in  habitat  is  a continuum  of  plant 


Table  2 

AVER.AGE  Bird  Populations  (Pairs/40  Ha)  in  Different  Types  of  Vegetation 


224 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


q 

fO 


o 

d 


q 


fO 


q 

CO 


CO  r- 

CO  —3 


CO  CO 
f-I  CO  d 


+ 

o r- 

CO  ^ d 


d 


q 

CO 


cm’ 


q 

lo  d d 


CO  lO  CO  lO  O 
CM  d d CO 


q q 

d CM 


CO 


q 

d 


o 

d 


CO 


Q 


W 

u. 


u 


u, 

Q 


O 


a 


o 


tin 


Uh 

Q 


CD 


E 3 


-M  ^ ^ 

T3 

D 3 .ii 


-i 

cc 


CJ 


3 o 
<u 

S 3 

3 2 

2 ^ 


iZ 


3 ^ 

3,  4; 

V3  G- 


^3^2; 


a; 

“C 
3 cd 

■3 


3 4^ 


k!  r ■)  a,  *3 


C .0 

c 


^ 2 

I -B  §:  3 d 

f 2 S s 2 

3 ^ 3 A 3 4:3 

3 >.  -1r  8 o 


w 


Kendeigh  and  Fawver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS 


225 


q 


q 

lO 


o 


q 

fO  + 


lO  O 

lO 


q 


q 

eg 


c6 


q q q 

eg  ^ ^ 


W 3 
hJ  c 
03  ’-S 
< ^ 
H o 


+ 

q 

ui 


q q 

fO  00 


o 

eg 


1 

i 

! 

i 

i 


o 

j5 

3 

z 

^ I 

s s 

-o 

§ 

:a 


0)  3 

P ^-fe  .2  - . ^ ^ ^ 

cd  o ^ ^ ^ 

^ .Si 

o ^ q ^ If®  S 
^ c~~i  3 o c 


e ^ ^ -R 
u :s  ^ ^ o 

CJ3^ 

o O .5 

m ^ cj  6 


t>t/  -C 
'3 

3 3 


•3;  "o 
K o 


Veery 

Catharus  fuscescens  BE  0.6  8.7  15.0  28.0  21.0  14.0  26.0  18.0 

Blue-gray  Gnatcatcher 

Polioptila  caerulea  SM  3.0 


226 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


o 

Csl 


LO 


o 

LO 


o 

lO 


o 


o 

eg 

o 

r-’ 


o 

fO 


csl 

u 

hJ 

DQ 

< 

H 


o 

O 


+ 


o o 

Tj<’  CO 


O CO 
CM 


CO 

CO 


+ 


q q 

LO  LO 


LO 

csi 


q 

vO 


o 

o' 

LO 


q 

LO 

CM 


CO 

00 


lO 


LO 

00 


q 

sC 


q 

LO 


. V. 
5 c 


C;  oa  Cl 


w 


w 


= a > 


I* 


C3 

^ -S 

? Ic 


a 

a o z;  a 
b i,  a •=  » 


u S ^ 

o"  o>.o^o5-2 
o 


^ ^ "i  ^ i ^ 

oi  CQ 


SF  8 
'Z  -a 
.a: 

1)  ~ 
r 

c =C 


ca  ^ 

^ C’ 

^ ~sr 

bc  a 
n 

• “ o 

^ -- 
c S 
Ji  t 

-a 


II 

a CO 

8 ^ 
a c 


-a 

U 

cd 

a ^ 

J 


CO 

8 ^ 


a i; 

t O 

a ^ 

= I 

^ cd 


CQ 


Kendeigh  and  Fawver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  227 


c ^ 

iZ 


P -c  P 


c: 

P,  ^ 
a <u 


5 S ^ 


_Q  3 

C 'S 
O 


-2-2 

^ 1 ^ 

«3  ^ 

.2  a « 

c«  •—  3 


O 

^ P, 
3 C/^  C O 

O OJ  ^ 

J ^ 


3 O </:  2 
^ u Q -3 
2 ~3  U 

k.  ^ ~o 
. o o i OJ 

P=S  .2  1 


i?  t 
I s I 
l'^=3 


=yj  jj 

II 

’C  "P 
-2  o 

^ o 


1;  C; 

3 p 

!-i  P 

^ I 
^ J 
= S 

u 


228 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


u 

hJ 

CQ 

< 


^ ^ I 2 


E-g, 

-r  “ ' 
J-  -e 


c ^ 
£ 


_ . ”5- 

c E ■“ 


•-i  Cfl  ;3 

C _Q  _C 


C ?! 


S o a 


■C  bC 


«5 


a a S 5 

£ I M 

P S ^ ® 

•b  “C  S Oi  ~C  *!‘D 


O “2  0^  ^ '-r  a- 


cd  -5 


^ a 

2 

Jd  >2 


Population  below  0.5  pair/40  ha. 


Kendeigh  and  Fawver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  229 


communities.  At  low  elevations,  there  was  no  observable  difference  be- 
tween total  bird  species  and  total  pairs  between  cove  and  hemlock-decid- 
uous forests  on  north  slopes  and  chestnut  oak  on  intermediate  slopes,  but 
fewer  species  and  smaller  populations  occurred  in  pine-oak  forests  on 
south  slopes  (Table  4).  At  higher  elevations,  there  was  an  observable  de- 
crease in  both  total  species  and  pairs  from  red  oak  forests  on  intermediate 
slopes  to  pine  heath  on  exposed  south  slopes. 

Bond  (1957),  working  in  southern  Wisconsin  with  a continuum  of  plant 
communities  in  the  ecotone  between  grassland  and  deciduous  forest, 
found  an  increase  in  number  of  bird  species  and  total  populations  from 
moist  to  intermediate  stages  and  then  a decline  to  the  dry  end.  Individual 
species  varied  in  their  point  of  greatest  abundance  along  the  gradient. 
Likewise,  Smith  (1977)  observed  3 of  the  8 species  studied  restricted  to 
the  moist  end  of  a deciduous  forest  continuum  in  the  Ozark  Mountains  of 
Arkansas  and  only  1 species  extending  to  the  extreme  dry  end. 


230 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  3 

Comparison  of  Species  Composition  and  Populations  Between  Communities 


Restricted  species 


1st  2nd  Similarity 

community  community  indices 


Communities  compared 

N 

% 

N 

% 

N 

s,. 

Low  serai  stage:  pine-oak  forest 

13 

57 

13 

57 

44 

5 

Low  serai  stage:  cove  forest 

8 

35 

23 

74 

30 

5 

Low  serai  stage:  high  and  early  mid-seral  stages 

19 

83 

10 

71 

22 

10 

High  serai  stages:  spruce-fir  forest 

5 

36 

6 

40 

62 

40 

Cove  forest:  chestnut  oak  forest 

8 

26 

9 

28 

73 

31 

Chestnut  oak  forest:  red  oak  forest 

11 

34 

4 

16 

74 

41 

Cove  forest:  gray  beech  forest 

23 

74 

5 

38 

36 

20 

Gray  beech  forest:  high  serai  stages 

2 

15 

8 

42 

69 

58 

Cove  forest:  hemlock-deciduous  forest 

10 

32 

5 

19 

74 

55 

Cove  forest:  spruce-fir  forest 

21 

68 

5 

33 

22 

18 

“Hemlock”  forest^:  spruce-fir  forest 

2 

13 

2 

13 

87 

34 

Pine  heath:  spruce-fir  forest 

8 

57 

9 

60 

21 

9 

Pine  heath:  pine-oak  forest 

8 

57 

17 

74 

32 

5 

Pine-oak  forest:  “deciduous  cove  forest”^ 

10 

44 

10 

44 

56 

35 

® “Deciduous  forest  bird  species"  eliminated  from  the  hemlock-deciduous  forest. 
“Hemlock  bird  species"  eliminated. 


Elevation. — Increase  in  altitude  or  elevation  brings  lower  temperatures,  | 
more  precipitation  including  snowfall,  shorter  growing  seasons,  greater  ^ 
wind  velocities,  and  more  cloudiness  and  fog  (Shanks  1954). 

Vertical  ranges  of  the  Black-capped  and  Carolina  chickadees  over-  . 
lapped  in  the  chestnut  oak  (chestnut)  forest  (Table  5),  but  the  Black-  * ] 
capped  was  there  only  in  1947  and  the  Carolina  Chickadee  only  in  1948.  ‘ ] 

The  2 species  have  nearly  identical  territorial  requirements,  and  compet-  1 
itive  interrelations  of  the  two  are  well  established  (Tanner  1952,  Brewer  ^ ^ 
1963).  ; ! I; 

The  Wood  Thrush  and  Veery  overlapped  broadly  in  vertical  distribution  • j j 
and  in  the  same  census  plots.  Cavanaugh  and  Magee  (1967)  observed  that  tl 
when  1 species  was  numerous  in  a mixed  coniferous-deciduous  forest,  the  | ,r 
other  was  less  so  with  the  situation  reversing  in  another  year,  which  in-  - 
dicated  the  possibility  of  conflict  between  them.  Bent  (1949)  cited  several  ' \ 
instances  of  the  Veery  and  American  Robin  being  driven  from  their  ter-  !|  tj, 
ritories  by  the  Wood  Thrush,  and  he  observed  a Veery  driving  a Wood  |; 
Thrush  from  its  territory.  Bertin  (1977)  suggested  that  in  mixed  forest  •;  jj 


Kendeigh  and  Faivver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  231 


Table  4 

Statistics  on  Bird  Populations  in  Plant  Communities 


Pairs  per  species 


Plant  community 

Elevation 

(m) 

Total 

species 

Total 

pairs 

Species 

diversity 

H' 

Equita- 

bility 

J' 

mean 

pis 

median 

pis 

skew- 

ness 

gi 

Pine-oak  forest 

Serai 

455-488 

23 

172  + 

2.707 

0.904 

7.5 

5.0 

0.99 

Mature 

411-488 

23 

140+ 

2.397 

0.814 

6.1 

1.6 

1.8 

Cove  forest 

640-1250 

31 

254  + 

2.206 

0.669 

8.2 

2.0 

4.5 

Hemlock-deciduous 

forest 

873-1356 

26 

376  + 

2.574 

0.810 

14.5 

5.0 

1.4 

Chestnut  oak 

(chestnut)  forest 

817-1074 

32 

247  + 

2.706 

0.851 

7.7 

3.2 

1.7 

Red  oak  (chestnut) 

forest 

1222-1506 

25 

204 

2.762 

0.858 

8.2 

3.7 

1.3 

Pine  heath 

1219-1349 

14 

108  + 

1.707 

0.741 

7.7 

1.6 

1.7 

Gray  beech  forest 

1646-1713 

13 

177  + 

1.821 

0.733 

13.6 

2.6 

1.5 

Spruce-fir  forest 

Early  serai 

1890-1950 

12 

369+ 

1.527 

0.734 

30.8 

4.5 

1.6 

Mid-seral 

1798-1920 

10 

288 

1.824 

0.792 

28.8 

18.0 

2.2 

Late  serai 

1740-1795 

14 

328+ 

1.974 

0.794 

17.0 

5.0 

2.2 

Climax 

1760-1790 

15 

314+ 

1.889 

0.760 

20.9 

6.5 

2.7 

the  Veery  may  segregate  into  sites  with  cooler  microclimates  than  those 
preferred  by  the  Wood  Thrush. 

Yellow-throated,  Red-eyed  and  Solitary  vireos  were  all  present  in  pine- 

i oak  plots  and  Red-eyed  and  Solitary  vireos  occurred  in  red  oak  (chestnut) 
at  higher  elevations.  The  Yellow-throated  Vireo  feeds  and  nests  in  both 

I deciduous  and  pine  trees  and  may  be  a competitor  of  the  Solitary  Vireo 

ii  in  the  pine  forests  of  the  Piedmont  region  (Odum  1948).  Both  Solitary  and 
I Red-eyed  vireos  were  abundant  in  the  cove  forest  but  only  the  Solitary 
! Vireo  occurred  in  the  hemlock  forest.  The  Solitary  Vireo  commonly  sang 
; and  fed  in  both  deciduous  and  coniferous  trees  from  2 m-ca.  12  m above 
1;  the  ground.  The  Red-eyed  Vireo  was  never  observed  in  coniferous  trees 
i|  and  in  deciduous  trees  carried  on  its  activities  from  2 m above  ground  to 

the  tree  tops.  This  agrees  in  general  with  observations  of  the  2 species  in 
i mixed  forests  in  New  York  state  (Kendeigh  1945). 

The  vertical  ranges  of  the  2 nuthatches  broadly  overlapped  but  the 
White-breasted  Nuthatch  was  largely  confined  to  deciduous  stands  and 
>11  the  Red-breasted  Nuthatch  to  coniferous  ones.  The  ranges  of  warbler 
species  from  the  upper  and  lower  elevations  did  not  overlap  except  for  the 
• Hooded  and  Canada  warblers  in  the  cove  forest. 


1 


232 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


LO 

u 

CQ 

< 


C C S 

a; 

CD  CD  D 

_Q  -Q  >!h 

CO  ca  o, 


c c 

a;  V 

CD  CD 
O;  (U 


C C C S c 

V V V CD  V 

CD  CD  CD  a;  CD 

ja  jQ  _Q  u ja 

cO  CO  CO  Oa  CO 


O O CM  LO  CO 
CM  CM  O r-  00 

— a^  ■ 


op  op  0^  c^ 

O O CM  ON  ^ 

tOK  Tt  fO  ON  ^ 

NO  NO  r-  00 


Cu  2 

? £ 

A -Q 


OJ 

CQ  CQ 


> > 


CD  C.C 
CD 


A 3 

■s  J 

^ c o 
3 ^ 

-c  -3  ^ 

^ £ 3 -Q  SF 

^ o:  o >H  co 

CO  3 .2  3 

2 £ £ ^ 

^ ^ 5 1 -S 

2 ^ S = 3 
2S  -2  -iS  3 O 
CQ  CQ  CQ  CJ  c/i 


CD  CD  CD  CD 


2 30  30 
O.  CO  cO 


lC 

nO 

;0 

00 

fO 

o 

00 

00 

00 

T#< 

00 

o 

LO 

u6 

00 

CO 

00 

00 

CO 

00 

T 

00 

00 

t 

T 

M 

00 

M 

t 

'y 

T 

r- 

LO 

1— 1 

CM 

T#- 

LO 

lO 

CM 

ON 

00 

Tt 

CM 

Tt 

CM 

-3 

CO  __ 

y O 

Ic 

O u 


_ ; - oj  O 1.1 

“cD  ^ ;J0  -3  _0 

2 i3  £ ^ 3 CO  E 


n iJ 

^ bC 
_£  cO 
u 3 
CO  CO 


_li3 


3 0“  0“  O O O 

u^^^>-cQ>-a,>Q- 


X 


Kendeigh  and  Fawver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  233 


Summer  and  Scarlet  tanagers  both  occurred  in  the  pine-oak  census  plot 
but  in  different  years.  Stupka  (1963)  states  that  the  2 species  commonly 
meet  at  457-610  m. 


DISCUSSION 

Local  factors. — The  predominant  factor  controlling  bird  distribution  in 
the  Great  Smoky  Mountains  is  the  relation  of  bird  species  to  plant  com- 
munities. The  change  in  moisture  gradients  from  north  to  south  slopes  and 
temperature  gradients  with  elevation  are  of  secondary  importance.  Plant 
communities  are  identified  by  life  forms  of  their  dominant  plant  species 
(deciduous  tree,  coniferous  tree,  shrub),  physiognomy  (dense  closed  for- 
est, open  forest,  heath),  location  (cove,  bald)  and  species  composition. 
Each  plant  community,  or  at  least  vegetation  type,  provides  a different 
environment  for  birds  with  respect  to  microclimate  (modification  of  the 
macroclimate),  plant  structure  (dimensions,  branching,  leaf  size  and  ar- 
rangement) and  food  supplies  (seeds,  nuts,  fruit,  foliage,  insects  and  other 
invertebrates).  Each  bird  species  has  evolved  adjustments  to  these  factors 
but  little  new  information  concerning  their  precise  niche  requirements  and 
role  in  the  community  can  be  provided  beyond  those  discussed  for  many 
of  these  species  by  Kendeigh  (1945,  1947),  MacArthur  (1958),  James  (1971), 
Anderson  and  Shugart  (1974)  and  Rabenold  (1978). 

The  difference  in  bird  species  composition  between  serai  shrubs  and 
mature  pine-oak  forest  at  low  elevation  appears  clearly  related  to  change 
in  life  form  of  the  conspicuous  plants  and  the  physiognomy  of  their  stands. 
Forest  species  with  the  largest  populations  are  segregated  into  either  de- 
ciduous or  needle-leaved  coniferous  vegetation  types. 

Food  resources  may  be  a factor  affecting  population  size.  Whittaker 
(1952)  has  shown  that  productivity  of  foliage  insects  in  the  Great  Smoky 
Mountains  decreases  with  slope  exposure  from  moist  cove  forests  to  dry 
oak  and  pine  types  and  with  elevation.  Bond  (1957)  found  that  foliage 
insect  gleaners  decreased  and  plant  feeders  increased  from  moist  to  dry 
I forests  in  Wisconsin. 

i Circumstantial  evidence  indicated  that  competition  as  well  as  changes 
: in  vegetation  affected  vertical  limits  of  some  species  in  the  Great  Smoky 
I Mountains.  Able  and  Noon  (1976)  found  no  convincing  cases  of  altitudinal 
competitive  exclusion  between  species  in  the  temperate  mountain  forests 
I of  New  York  and  Vermont.  Upper  and  lower  distributional  limits  of  species 
coincided  with  ecotones  in  vegetation.  In  the  tropical  forests  of  New  Guinea, 
Diamond  (1973)  believed  competition  to  be  more  important  than  changes 
in  vegetation  in  controlling  vertical  distribution.  In  the  tropical  mountains  of 
Peru,  Terborgh  (1971)  ascribed  changes  in  vegetation  to  account  for  less 
than  20%  of  the  altitudinal  limits  of  species,  competition  for  about  33% 


234 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


and  gradually  changing  physical  and  other  biological  conditions  for  about 
50%.  Included  in  this  latter  category  were  not  only  changes  in  temperature 
and  cloudiness,  but  also  changes  in  net  annual  plant  productivity,  density 
of  insects  and  importance  of  epiphytic  plants  in  the  vegetation. 

Geography. — Avifaunas  in  similar  plant  communities  at  different  local- 
ities tend  to  be  similar  in  species  composition  (S.s)  but  not  identical.  Of 
301  species  recorded  in  6 comparisons  of  localities  (Table  6),  62%  occurred 
in  both  communities,  21%  were  found  in  other  plant  communities  in  the 
vicinity,  11%  were  absent  because  the  locality  was  beyond  their  geograph- 
ic ranges,  while  6%  were  not  recorded  in  the  locality.  Failure  to  record  a 
species  in  a locality  within  its  distributional  range  may  indicate  that  the 
sampling  area  was  too  small,  and  this  may  also  be  partly  responsible  for 
finding  species  only  in  other  plant  communities. 

Other  factors,  however,  often  affect  local  occurrence  of  species.  For 
instance,  presence  of  Red-breasted  Nuthatch,  Brown  Creeper,  Winter 
Wren  and  Golden-crowned  Kinglet  in  hemlock  communities  in  the  Great 
Smoky  Mountains  and  not  on  the  Heldenberg  Plateau  of  New  York  may 
represent  an  overflow  from  large  populations  of  these  species  in  nearby 
spruce-fir  communities.  In  New  York  state,  spruce-fir  forests  are  sepa- 
rated by  25-60  km  from  the  hemlock  areas  censused.  Another  local  factor 
is  the  presence  of  competing  species.  In  hemlock-deciduous  forests  on  the 
Highlands  Plateau  of  North  Carolina,  the  Carolina  Chickadee  replaces  the 
Black-capped  Chickadee. 

rhe  bird  species  composition  varies,  of  course,  when  the  plant  com- 
munity is  beyond  the  limit  of  distribution  of  the  bird  species.  The  Scarlet 
Tanager  was  not  recorded  in  the  pine-oak  community  on  the  Piedmont 
Plateau  of  Georgia  because  it  is  rare  or  absent  there;  the  Brown-headed 
Nuthatch  {Sitta  pusilla)  recorded  in  the  Georgia  area  is  rare  or  absent  in 
the  Great  Smoky  Mountains.  Nineteen  species  found  in  spruce-fir  forests 
of  northern  Maine  do  not  extend  their  ranges  south  as  far  as  the  Great 
Smoky  Mountains.  The  decrease  in  species  richness  in  Appalachian 
spruce-fir  forests  from  north  to  south  is  a progressive  one  (Rabenold  1978). 

Coefficients  of  population  similarity,  Sp,  were  not  used  in  these  com- 
parisons of  avifaunas.  They  are  more  sensitive  indicators  of  similarity 
because  they  depend  not  only  on  the  presence  or  absence  of  a species  as 
does  Ss,  but  on  the  replication  of  identical  characteristics  of  the  environ- 
ment to  permit  equal  sized  populations  to  develop.  Populations  may  also 
vary  locally  in  sex  and  age  ratios  and  other  properties  that  would  affect 
realization  of  comparable  population  sizes.  In  our  comparisons  of  avifaunas 
in  different  localities,  there  was  an  additional  complication  in  that  cen- 
suses were  taken  in  different  years,  and  population  sizes  in  a locality 
fluctuate  from  year  to  year. 


Table  6 

Geographic  Comparison  of  Avifaunas 


Kendeigh  and  Faivver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  235 


>>  a; 
-o  c 

3 O 


IS  ^ 
^ O 


o 

3 

£ 

-£  o 


*-  ■£ 
CD  -Q 

c«  3 


CO  CM 
CO 


vq  o 
lo 


CM  O 
CO 


o o o o 

lO  lO  LO 


r-H  Os 

vd  o 


r--  Os 
CO 


lO  lO  lO 

o’ 


•s  1 
o i -a 

^ e 


O CO  CM 


^ ^ CO  Tj- 


LO  LO 


I CO  I CM 

I O I O 


I § 


bC 
c«  u 
3 O 

‘3  ^ 

S o 

1“' 

>>  s 

O ^ 
£ 3 

C/1  o 

p 

3 -5 
4J  i 

O £ 


.£  o 
5 Z 

3 

3 3 

0 3 

Dh 

1 -c 

^ J 

3 1c 
IJ  bC 

O E 


3 

3 3 

O 3 

^ I 

2 

3 -3 

J 

3 !S 


3 3 

3 3 

O 3 

Oh 

"o 

s 3 

c/1  -£ 

^ 3 

3 "O 


P OX  ox 


Oh 

c/1 


Great  Smoky  Mountains  — — 14  0 0 1 20.9  6.5  2.7  this  study 

Great  Smoky  Mountains  25  80  14  5 0 1 14.0  8.0  2.0  Alsop  1969 

Greak  Smoky  Mountains  — — 13  1 1 0 20.9  6.5  2.7  this  study 

Aroostock  County,  Maine  1800  44  13  12  19  0 7.9  10.0  3.2  Stewart  and  Aldrich  1952 


236 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Bird  communities,  their  origins  and  history. — Hubbard  (1971)  has  a gen- 
eral discussion  of  the  origins  of  avifaunas  in  the  southern  Appalachians 
but  we  wish  to  consider  these  origins  and  history  from  a somewhat  differ- 
ent viewpoint.  Our  analysis  above  of  succession,  forest  communities,  and 
geography  shows  considerable  similarity  in  the  species  composition  of  avi- 
faunas that  occur  in  plant  communities  of  the  same  vegetation  type,  par- 
ticularly deciduous  forest,  needle-leaved  coniferous  forest  and  shrubby 
vegetation  at  low  elevations.  Each  combination  of  avifaunas  in  the  same 
vegetation  type  or  bird  community  shows  little  similarity  to  either  of  the 
others  (Table  3).  “Preference”  of  bird  species  for  particular  types  of  vege- 
tation is  indicated  by  maximum  populations  being  attained  in  them.  Such 
maximum  populations  suggest  a high  degree  of  anatomical,  physiological 
and  behavioral  adaptation  through  association  with  the  vegetation  over  a 
long  period  of  geological  time.  In  Table  2,  each  species  is  assigned  to  the 
bird  community  to  which  it  most  characteristically  belongs,  although  most 
species  have  enough  flexibility  that  they  occur  in  small  numbers  in  other 
communities  as  well.  Much  concerning  the  origin  and  geological  history 
of  each  bird  community  can  be  learned  from  the  presence  of  associated 
plant  species  in  the  fossil  record  of  various  localities  and  times,  as  was 
traced  by  Kendeigh  (1974). 

The  uniqueness  of  the  low  elevation  shrub  avifauna  is  the  result  of 
intermingling  of  bird  species  belonging  to  the  deciduous  forest-edge  com- 
munity (FE)  and  what  we  have  called  the  southeastern  mixed  community 
(SM).  Belonging  to  this  latter  are  the  southern  pines  and  several  bird  species 
found  in  the  pine-oak  community.  This  vegetation  is  derived  from  the 
Madro-Tertiary  Geoflora  originating  on  the  Mexican  Plateau,  which  during 
the  Miocene  or  earlier  extended  continuously  around  the  Gulf  of  Mexico. 
The  southeastern  portion  and  its  avifauna  became  separated  when  prairie 
vegetation  penetrated  to  the  Gulf  of  Mexico. 

The  deciduous  forest  (DF)  and  forest-edge  (FE)  communities  are  derived 
from  the  temperate  unit  of  the  Arcto-Tertiary  Geoflora  that  during  the 
Eocene  epoch  extended  from  southeastern  United  States  to  Alaska  (Ken- 
deigh 1974).  This  forest  and  its  serai  stages  have  been  much  buffeted  by 
climatic  changes  during  the  last  65  million  years  and  are  now  largely  lim- 
ited to  the  eastern  United  States.  The  mixed  mesophytic  forest  of  the  south- 
ern Appalachian  Mountains,  best  represented  here  by  the  cove  forest,  has 
been  little  affected  by  these  changes  and  may  be  the  oldest  forest  stand 
at  any  locality  in  temperate  North  America  (Braun  1950). 

The  gray  beech  forest,  unique  to  the  Appalachian  Mountain  System,  is 
a segregate  from  the  mixed  mesophytic  forest  (Braun  1950).  It  is  a young 
forest  geologically,  and  since  it  is  subjected  to  more  climatic  stresses  than 
the  deciduous  forest  at  lower  elevations,  only  a few  bird  species  from  the 


Kendeigh  and  Fawver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  237 


deciduous  forest  community  have  been  able  to  occupy  it.  Instead,  it  has 
become  occupied  by  species  from  the  spruce-fir  coniferous  forest  com- 
munity. 

Serai  stages  at  high  elevations  became  extensive  only  in  recent  times 
as  the  result  of  fires  and  human  disturbances.  Although  a few  species  have 
invaded  from  shrubby  serai  stages  of  the  lowlands,  most  of  the  avifauna 
is  of  boreal  origin.  There  are  no  unique  species  here,  although  the  Veery 
and  Chestnut-sided  Warbler  reach  peak  populations  in  this  vegetation  and 
in  the  gray  beech  forest. 

The  spruce-fir  forest  (BF)  is  derived  mainly  from  the  boreal  unit  of  the 
Arcto-Tertiary  Geoflora  (Kendeigh  1974).  In  the  early  Tertiary  period,  this 
unit  was  widely  spread  over  northern  North  America.  During  the  Pleis- 
tocene epoch  it  became  fragmented,  with  the  eastern  portion  becoming 
restricted  and  isolated  in  the  northeastern  United  States  and  south  through 
the  Appalachian  Mountains.  During  the  height  of  glaciation,  the  spruce- 
fir  forest  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains  probably  extended  to  lower  ele- 
vations and  occupied  mountain  tops  farther  south  than  at  present.  During 
the  warm  dry  xerothermic  period,  beginning  some  8000-9000  years  ago 
following  the  retreat  of  the  last  major  or  Wisconsin  glacier,  this  forest 
retreated  to  higher  elevations  and  became  eliminated  from  lower  peaks 
south  of  Clingmans  Dome.  During  this  period  the  southern  distributional 
limits  of  many  bird  species  probably  retreated  northward.  The  cooler, 
moister  climate  of  the  last  2000-3000  years  may  have  again  permitted  the 
I spruce-fir  forest  to  expand  down  the  slopes  and  bird  species  to  extend 
their  ranges  southward.  The  gray  beech  forest  probably  originated  during 
these  up  and  down  movements  of  the  vegetation  (Whittaker  1956). 

The  eastern  hemlock  forest  lies  geographically  between  boreal  and  de- 
ciduous forests  but  occurs  more  often  in  mixed  stands  with  deciduous 
trees  than  with  spruce  and  fir.  It  was  subjected  to  several  contractions 
) and  extensions  of  range  during  the  Pleistocene  and  Holocene  epochs.  It 
1 differs  from  the  boreal  forest  in  the  luxurious  development  of  rhododendron 
I and  other  shrubs  in  the  Appalachian  Mountain  System,  which  together  with 
' a more  temperate  and  moist  climate  presents  a somewhat  different  envi- 
ronment for  birds.  This  has  led  to  a secondary  differentiation  of  its  avi- 
, fauna  from  that  of  the  spruce-fir  forest  which  is  more  evident  when  the  2 
forests  are  some  distance  apart,  as  on  the  Helderberg  Plateau  of  New 
I York,  than  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains.  Other  variations  in  avifaunas 
of  the  boreal  forest  have  been  described  by  Erskine  (1977). 

The  fact  that  2 species  in  hemlock  forests  of  New  York  and  19  species 
I in  spruce-fir  forests  of  Maine  were  not  found  as  breeding  birds  in  the  Great 
Smoky  Mountains  may  be  the  result,  in  part,  of  these  historical  shifts  in 
the  vegetation.  Since  the  last  contraction  of  their  ranges  northward,  time 


238 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


may  not  have  been  sufficient  for  them  to  reoccupy  southward  all  favorable 
breeding  areas  that  have  become  available.  These  areas  are  scattered  and 
require  even  migratory  bird  species  to  jump  from  one  mountain  to  another 
for  breeding  purposes.  For  example,  Golden-crowned  Kinglet,  Winter 
Wren,  Black-capped  Chickadee  and  Black-throated  Green  Warbler,  al- 
though common  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains,  were  not  found  in  the 
census  areas  on  the  Highlands  Plateau  of  the  Blue  Ridge  Mountains.  The 
Black-throated  Green  Warbler  is  of  special  interest  as  the  Highlands  Pla- 
teau lies  in  a hiatus  between  the  ranges  of  the  subspecies  in  the  mountains 
{Dendroica  v.  virens)  and  the  coastal  subspecies  {D.  v.  waynei).  Odum 
(1950)  has  suggested  that  in  dispersal  of  the  species  southward,  the  ranges 
of  the  mountain  population  became  split  from  the  coastal  population  and 
that  the  plateau  has  not  yet  become  occupied  by  the  mountain  race. 

In  addition  to  slow  dispersal  rates  southward,  restriction  of  species 
southward  may  also  be  in  response  to  unfavorable  climate,  food  resources, 
or  inter-species  competition.  Rabenold  (1978)  ignores  the  probable  influ- 
ence of  the  post-glacial  xerothermic  period  on  bird  distribution  in  the 
Appalachians  and  argues  that  the  lower  species  richness  is  related  to  lower 
food  levels  during  the  reproductive  season. 

Community  structure. — The  structure  of  bird  communities  is  commonly 
analyzed  in  terms  of  species  richness  (5),  equitability  or  eveness  of  distri- 
bution of  populations  {]')  and  segregation  of  species  into  different  niches. 

The  first  2 factors  are  usually  combined  into  a species  diversity  index  (//'). 

We  are  not  here  concerned  with  analysis  of  ecological  niches.  In  our  data 
(Table  4),  H'  varied  positively  with  s {r  = 0.87,  P < 0.001)  but  J'  varied 
independently  of  5.  We  agree  with  Hurlbert  (1971)  that  the  usefulness  of 
H'  is  limited.  There  is  little  advantage  in  combining  the  2 components. 

Levels  of  population  size  attained  by  different  species  in  communities 
or  areas  is  important.  Where  the  distribution  of  different  population  sizes 
follows  a normal  or  Gaussian  curve,  mean  number  of  pairs  per  species 
{pis)  is  a reliable  estimate.  In  each  of  our  plant  communities,  however,  the 
distribution  of  populations  was  skewed  in  that  the  median  population  was 
less  than  the  mean,  and  the  mode,  usually  poorly  defined,  was  less  than 
the  median.  The  difference  (±  SD)  between  the  mean  and  median  pis  !,  [ 
averaged  65  ± 17%  of  the  mean.  The  degree  to  which  distribution  of  ; I 
populations  was  skewed  {gi)  is  indicated  in  Table  4,  all  populations  being  j n 
significantly  different  from  symmetrical  {P  < 0.05),  although  that  of  the  ' a 
serai  stage  to  pine-oak  forest  (^1  = 0.99)  was  borderline.  >] 

There  is  a highly  significant  negative  correlation  between  gi  and  J'  {r  = : ' 

0.69,  P < 0.0025).  A high  value  of  gi  and  a low  value  of  J'  both  indicate  I 
wide  scattering  of  pis,  hence  there  is  no  need  to  use  both  indices.  The 
distribution  of  measurements  of  biological  variables  commonly  adheres  to 


Kendeigh  and  Fawver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  239 


a normal  curve,  and  since  indicates  the  departure  from  symmetry  it  is 
preferable  to  J'  which  indicates  departure  from  equality,  which  is  rare  or 
absent  in  biological  phenomena. 

Pis  varied  negatively  with  s {r  = 0.69,  P < 0.025)  and  this  was  related 
to  altitude.  At  elevations  below  1600  m,  average  5 per  community  was  25 
and  pis  8.6;  above  1600  m,  s was  13  and  pis  22.2.  Skewness,  g^ , was  not 
statistically  correlated  with  s and  varied  from  0.99  in  the  serai  shrub  stage 
to  4.5  in  the  cove  forest,  both  at  low  elevations  (Table  4).  If  the  cove  forest 
is  excluded  for  reasons  given  beyond,  gi  averaged  1.5  below  and  2.0  above 
1600  m.  At  1800  m elsewhere  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains,  5,  pis  and 
gi  values  were  intermediate  (Table  6,  Alsop  1969).  In  spruce-fir  forests 
occurring  at  low  elevations  in  Maine,  s was  higher  and  pis  lower  than  in 
the  spruce-fir  forest  at  high  elevation  in  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains,  but 
gi  was  higher  (Table  6).  At  both  low  and  high  elevations,  gi  was  lower  in 
serai  shrubby  stages  than  in  forests  (Table  4).  Able  and  Noon  (1976)  state 
that  decreasing  s and  increasing  pis  with  elevation  seem  to  be  a general 
rule  in  forested  regions.  Other  investigators  agree  that  fewer  species  and 
greater  variability  in  population  size  tend  to  occur  in  rigorous  environments 
with  variable  weather  and  other  conditions  than  in  moderate  ones  (Tramer 
1969,  Kricher  1972,  Rotenberry  1978,  Rotenberry  et  al.  1979). 

The  considerably  skewed  distribution  of  populations  in  the  cove  forest 
{gi  = 4.5)  is  caused  by  1 of  the  31  species  present.  Black-throated  Blue 
Warbler,  having  41%  of  the  total  number  of  pairs.  This  species  sings, 
nests  and  feeds  in  rhododendron  and  other  ericaceous  shrubs  and  in  the 
rich  herbaceous  stratum.  Rhodendron  maximum  is  a characteristic  dom- 
inant in  the  undergrowth  of  southern  Appalachian  Mountains  but  very 
local  or  absent  from  mixed  mesophytic  forests  elsewhere  (Braun  1950). 

‘ The  warbler  has  exploited  this  special  niche  and  without  effective  com- 
;i  petition  from  other  species,  its  population  has  exploded  in  size.  The  high 
1,  gx  of  hemlock-deciduous  forests  on  the  Highlands  Plateau  of  North  Car- 
1,  olina  (Table  6)  was  also  caused  by  the  predominance  of  this  species. 

I High  values  of  gi  found  in  gray  beech  and  high  elevation  mid-  and  late 
H serai  communities  are  correlated  with  exploitation  of  favorable  local  niches 
•I  by  the  Dark-eyed  Junco  and  Chestnut-sided  Warbler.  The  early  serai  stage 
j has  even  larger  populations  of  these  2 species,  but  gx  is  lower  because  3 
)i  other  species  also  have  large  populations  so  as  to  give  a better  balance 

II  among  the  12  species  that  occur.  The  climax  spruce-fir  forest  has  several 
\ species  with  large  populations,  but  gx  is  high  because  the  Dark-eyed  Junco 
' comprised  40%  of  the  total.  Inter-species  competition  is  reduced  in  these 

high  elevation  communities  because  other  potentially  competitive  boreal 
0 species  have  not  extended  their  ranges  this  far  south. 

The  high  gx  for  pine  forests  on  the  Piedmont  Plateau  of  Georgia  (Table 


240 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


6)  resulted  from  the  Pine  Warbler  constituting  25%  of  the  total  populations 
of  all  29  species.  Pine  Warblers  nest  and  feed  in  long-leaved  pines. 

In  the  spruce-fir  forest  of  Maine,  the  high  resulted  from  Bay-breasted 
[Dendroica  castanea)  and  Magnolia  {D,  magnolia)  warblers  making  up 
30%  of  total  populations  even  with  44  species  present.  This  may  be  a 
temporary  unbalance,  however,  as  these  species  were  favored  by  feeding 
on  the  spruce  budworm  insect  infestation  then  in  progress. 

Pairs  per  species  {pis)  in  hemlock-deciduous  and  spruce-fir  forests  of 
the  Great  Smoky  Mountains  are  higher  and  in  the  chestnut  oak  and  red 
oak  forests  lower  than  at  other  localities  (Table  6).  In  each  comparison, 
pis  varied  inversely  with  s.  It  appears  that  when  few  species  are  present 
the  fundamental  niches  for  at  least  some  species  may  be  almost  fuUy 
occupied  so  that  populations  are  large.  On  the  other  hand,  with  many 
species  present  to  provide  inter-species  competition,  realized  niches  are 
smaller,  hence  smaller  populations. 

SUMMARY 

Species  composition  and  population  sizes  of  birds  were  determined  in  12  plant  communities 
of  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains  during  the  summers  of  1947  and  1948.  Plant  communities 
form  a mosaic  of  serai  and  climax  stages  that  varies  with  altitude  and  slope  exposure. 

Coefficients  of  species  and  population  similarities  indicated  that  distinct  bird  communities 
may  be  identified,  associated  with  deciduous  forest,  forest-edge,  boreal  forest  and  a south- 
eastern mixed  complex.  Each  type  of  vegetation  with  its  bird  life  has  had  a different  geological 
history  that  affects  its  present  composition  and  characteristics.  Bird  species  are  classified 
to  the  vegetation  type  to  which  they  appear  best  adapted  as  indicated  by  their  attainment  of 
highest  populations. 

Composition  of  bird  species  within  particular  stands  of  a vegetation  type  is  influenced  by 
the  location  of  the  stand  in  respect  to  species*  ranges,  neighboring  avifaunas,  annual  fluc- 
tuations (especially  of  the  less  common  species),  inter-species  competition  and  responses  to 
temperature  and  possibly  moisture  as  determined  by  elevation  and  slope  exposure. 

riie  species  diversity  index  (//')  varied  positively  with  species  richness  (5)  and  was  of 
limited  value  in  comparing  bird  populations.  Distributions  of  bird  population  sizes  in  aU  plant 
communities  was  positively  skewed.  Skewness  (^g, ) varied  negatively  with  {]')  and  is  preferred 
as  an  index  as  it  indicates  degree  of  departure  from  a symmetrical  distribution  rather  than 
from  equal  population  sizes  of  species. 

Increasing  elevation  was  correlated  with  lower  species  richness  (5),  larger  number  of  pairs 
per  species  (p/5)  and  a tendency  toward  higher  g, . although  the  latter  also  varied  indepen- 
dently of  altitude,  .\vifaunas  with  g,  greater  than  2.0  contained  one  or  more  species  with 
high  abundance  resulting  from  local  prevalence  of  favored  vegetation  niches  and  lack  of 
inter-species  competition  or  with  temporary  super-abundance  of  a food  resource.  Values  of 
p/5  may  be  compared  when  the  g,  of  avifaunas  are  similar.  Pis  varied  negatively  with  5, 
indicating  that  with  larger  number  of  species  present,  inter-species  competition  caused  fun- 
damental niches  not  to  be  fully  realized,  with  the  consequence  that  growth  of  populations 
for  individual  species  was  limited. 

.ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

rite  junior  author  was  responsible  for  the  fieldwork  and  a preliminary  analysis  of  data  for 


Kendeigh  and  Fawver  • GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAIN  BREEDING  BIRDS  241 


his  doctorate  thesis  at  the  University  of  Illinois.  The  senior  author  revised  and  up-dated  the 
manuscript.  We  are  grateful  to  Robert  H.  Whittaker  for  providing  vegetation  analyses  for 
several  of  the  census  areas  and  for  cooperation  in  other  ways  and  to  Arthur  Stupka,  at  that 
time  Park  Naturalist,  for  pointing  out  and  providing  access  to  suitable  study  areas  in  the 
various  types  of  vegetation.  James  R.  Karr,  Frances  C.  James,  James  F.  Parnell  and  Jerrold 
H.  Zar  provided  helpful  comments  on  early  drafts  of  the  manuscript. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Able,  K.  P.  and  B.  R.  Noon.  1976.  Avian  community  structure  along  elevational  gradients 
in  the  northeastern  United  States.  Oecologia  (Berk)  26:275-295. 

Aldrich,  J.  W.  and  P.  Goodrum.  1946.  Tenth  breeding  bird  census:  virgin  hardwood 
forest.  Audubon  Mag.  48:144-145. 

Alsop,  F.  J.,  III.  1969.  Tbirty-third  breeding  bird  census:  virgin  spruce-fir  forest.  Audubon 
Field  Notes  23:716. 

Anderson,  S.  H.  and  H.  H.  Shugart,  Jr.  1974.  Habitat  selection  of  breeding  birds  in  an 
east  Tennessee  deciduous  forest.  Ecology  55:828-837. 

Bent,  A.  C.  1949.  Life  histories  of  North  American  thrushes,  kinglets,  and  their  allies. 
U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  BuU.  196. 

Bertin,  R.  I.  1977.  Breeding  habitats  of  the  Wood  Thrush  and  Veery.  Condor  79:303-311. 

Bond,  R.  R.  1957.  Ecological  distribution  of  breeding  birds  in  the  upland  forests  of  southern 
Wisconsin.  Ecol.  Monogr.  27:351-384. 

I Braun,  E.  L.  1950.  Deciduous  forests  of  eastern  North  America.  The  Blakiston  Co.,  Phila- 
delphia, Pennsylvania. 

Brewer,  R.  1963.  Ecological  and  reproductive  relationships  of  Black-capped  and  Carolina 
chickadees.  Auk  80:9^7. 

Camp,  W.  H.  1950.  A biogeographic  and  paragenetic  analysis  of  the  American  beech  {Fa- 
gus).  Am.  Philos.  Soc.  Yearbook  1950:166-169. 

Cavanaugh,  J.  and  A.  Magee.  1967.  Thirty-first  breeding  bird  census:  climax  hemlock- 
white  pine  forest,  with  transitional  hardwood.  Audubon  Field  Notes  21:626-627. 

Diamond,  J.  M.  1973.  Distributional  ecology  of  New  Guinea  birds.  Science  179(4075):759- 

.1  769. 

i Erskine,  a.  j.  1977.  Birds  in  boreal  Canada:  communities,  densities  and  adaptations. 

! Canadian  Wildl.  Serv.,  Rept.  Ser.  41:1-71. 

1:  Fawver,  B.  J.  1950.  An  analysis  of  the  ecological  distribution  of  breeding  bird  populations 
in  eastern  North  America.  Ph.D.  thesis,  Univ.  Illinois,  Champaign,  Illinois, 
i.!  Hubbard,  J.  P.  1971.  The  avifauna  of  the  southern  Appalachians:  past  and  present.  Pp. 

; 197-232  in  The  distributional  history  of  the  biota  of  the  southern  Appalachians,  Pt.  HI, 

Vertebrates  (P.  C.  Holt,  ed.).  Virginia  Polytechnic  Inst,  and  State  Univ.,  Res.  Div. 
j Monogr.  4,  Blacksburg,  Virginia. 

I Hurlbert,  S.  H.  1971.  The  nonconcept  of  species  diversity:  a critique  and  alternative 
I explanations.  Ecology  52:577-586. 

James,  F.  C.  1971.  Ordinations  of  habitat  relationships  among  breeding  birds.  Wilson  Bull. 
83:215-236. 

Johnstone,  D.  W.  and  E.  P.  Odum.  1956.  Breeding  bird  populations  in  relation  to  plant 
succession  on  the  Piedmont  of  Georgia.  Ecology  37:50-62. 

I Kendeigh,  S.  C.  1945.  Community  selection  by  birds  on  the  Helderberg  Plateau  of  New 
V York.  Auk  62:418^36. 

f'  . 1946.  Breeding  birds  of  the  beech-maple-hemlock  community.  Ecology  27:226-244. 


242 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  .\o.  2,  June  1981 


. 1947.  Bird  population  studies  in  the  coniferous  forest  biome  during  a spruce  bud- 

worm  outbreak.  Dept.  Lands  and  Forests  Ontario,  Canada,  Div.  Res.,  Biol.  Bull.  1:1- 

100. 

. 1974.  Ecolog>  with  special  reference  to  animals  and  man.  Prentice-HaU,  Inc., 

Englewood  Cliffs,  New  Jersey. 

Kricher,  J.  C.  1972.  Bird  species  diversity:  the  effect  of  species  richness  and  equitability 
on  the  diversity  index.  Ecology’  53:278-282. 

MacArthur,  R.  H.  1958.  Population  ecology’  of  some  warblers  of  northeastern  coniferous 
forests.  Ecolog>  39:599-619. 

Odum,  E.  P.  1948.  Nesting  of  the  Mountain  Vireo  at  Athens,  Georgia,  conclusive  evidence 
of  a southward  invasion.  Oriole  13:17-20. 

. 1950.  Bird  populations  of  the  Highlands  (North  Carolina)  Plateau  in  relation  to  plant 

succession  and  avian  invasion.  Ecology  31:587-605. 

Palmgren,  P.  1930.  Quantitative  Untersuchungen  iiber  die  Vogelfauna  in  den  Waldern 
Siidfinnlands  mit  besonderer  Beriicksuchtingung  Alands.  Acta.  Zool.  Fenn.  7:1-219. 

PlELOU,  E.  C.  1966.  The  measurement  of  diversity  in  different  types  of  biological  conditions. 
J.  Theoret.  Biol.  13:131-144. 

Rabenold,  K.  N.  1978.  Foraging  strategies,  diversity,  and  seasonality  in  bird  communities 
of  Appalachian  spruce-fir  forests.  Ecol.  Monogr.  48:397-424. 

Rotenberry,  j.  T.  1978.  Components  of  avian  diversity  along  a multifactorial  climatic 
gradient.  Ecology  59:693-699. 

, R.  E.  Fitzner  and  W.  H.  Rick.\RD.  1979.  Seasonal  variation  in  avian  community 

structure:  differences  in  mechanisms  regulating  diversity  . Auk  96:499-505. 

Shanks,  R.  E.  1954.  Climates  of  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains.  Ecology  35:354-361. 

Sheldon,  A.  L.  1969.  Equitability  indices:  dependence  on  the  species  count.  Ecology 
50:466-467. 

Smith,  K.  G.  1977.  Distribution  of  summer  birds  along  a forest  moisture  gradient  in  an  Ozark 
watershed.  Ecology  58:810-819. 

Stewart,  R.  E.  and  J.  W.  Aldrich.  1952.  Ecological  studies  of  breeding  bird  populations 
in  northern  Maine.  Ecology  33:226-238. 

Stupka,  a.  1963.  Notes  on  the  birds  of  Great  Smoky  Mountains  National  Park.  Univ. 
Tennessee  Press,  Kingsport,  Tennessee. 

Tanner,  J.  R.  1952.  Black-capped  and  Carolina  chickadees  in  the  southern  Appalachian 
Mountains.  Auk  69:407-M^24. 

Terborgh,  j.  1971.  Distribution  on  environmental  gradients:  theory  and  a preliminary 
interpretation  of  distributional  patterns  in  the  avifauna  of  the  Cordillera  Vilcabamba, 
Peru.  Ecology  52:23-40. 

Tramer,  E.  j.  1969.  Bird  species  diversity:  components  of  Shannon's  formula.  Ecology 
50:927-929. 

Whittaker,  R.  H.  1952.  \ study  of  summer  foliage  insect  communities  in  the  Great  Smoky- 
Mountains.  Ecol.  Monogr.  22:1—44. 

. 1956.  Vegetation  of  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains.  Ecol.  Monogr.  26:1-80. 

Zar,  j.  H.  1974.  Biostatistical  analysis.  Prentice-Hall,  Inc.,  Englewood  Cliffs,  New  Jersey. 


VIVARIUM  BLDG.,  UNIV.  ILLINOIS,  CHAMPAIGN,  ILLINOIS  61820  AND  BIOLOGY 
DEPT.,  SOUTHWESTERN  OREGON  COLL.,  COOS  B.AY,  OREGON  97420. 
ACCEPTED  1 M.\Y  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  243-248 


NEST-SITE  SELECTION  AMONG  ADELIE,  CHINSTRAP 
AND  GENTOO  PENGUINS  IN  MIXED 
SPECIES  ROOKERIES 

Nicholas  J.  Volkman  and  Wayne  Trivelpiece 

The  literature  on  pygoscelid  penguins,  the  Adelie  {Pygoscelis  adeliae)^ 
the  Chinstrap  (P.  antarctica)  and  the  Gentoo  {P.  papua)  penguins,  is 
replete  with  statements  about  their  nest-site  preferences  (Clarke  1906, 
Murphy  1936,  Conroy  et  al.  1975,  Muller-Schwarze  and  Muller-Schwarze 
1975,  White  and  Conroy  1975).  Similar  cohabiting  avian  species  might  be 
expected  to  develop  specific  habitat  preferences  (Klopfer  and  Mailman 
1965)  and  some  authors  (White  and  Conroy  1975,  Muller-Schwarze  and 
Muller-Schwarze  1975)  have  suggested  that  sympatrically  breeding  pygos- 
celid penguins  have  nest-site  preferences.  However,  these  preferences 
have  never  been  quantified.  The  purpose  of  this  study  was  to  quantitatively 
describe  the  nest-sites  selected  by  sympatrically  breeding  Adelie,  Chin- 
strap and  Gentoo  penguins. 


METHODS 

All  3 pygoscelids  breed  in  2 rookeries  (after  Penney  1968)  located  near  Point  Thomas 
(62°10'S,  58°30'W),  King  George  Island,  South  Shetland  Islands.  The  rookeries  are  separated 
by  a glacial  tongue,  3 km  wide.  The  Polish  Academy  of  Sciences  Antarctic  Station,  Henryk 
Arctowski,  is  located  1 km  from  the  west  rookery.  Data  were  collected  between  1 November 
1977  and  21  February  1978. 

A census  of  penguin  colonies  (after  Penney  1968)  was  conducted  in  both  rookeries  1-2 
weeks  following  peak  egg-laying  of  each  species.  Individual  counts  were  made  of  gentoo  and 
chinstrap  nest-sites.  Adelie  nests  were  counted  individually  in  colonies  of  fewer  than  150 
pairs,  and  were  estimated  in  larger  colonies  by  determining  the  colony’s  area  and  using  the 
figure  of  1.13  pairs/ m'^  obtained  from  small  colonies  (Trivelpiece  and  Volkman  1979). 

The  majority  of  measurements  were  taken  on  penguin  colonies  in  the  west  rookery,  al- 
though some  were  taken  in  the  east  rookery  to  avoid  interfering  with  on-going  studies. 
!j  Measurements  of  elevation,  slope  (degrees)  and  distances  to  the  nearest  landing  beach  (mea- 
! sured  from  the  center  of  colonies)  were  obtained  after  mapping  the  colonies  on  a detailed 
topographical  map  of  the  west  rookery.  The  number  of  obstacles  (rocks  and  whale  bones) 

, large  enough  to  act  as  windbreaks  (higher  than  25  cm)  were  counted  in  the  west  rookery. 
The  distances  between  the  outer  rim  of  a sample  of  40  penguin  nests  and  the  outer  rim  of 
the  3 nearest  nests  were  measured  in  the  east  rookery.  The  length,  width  and  volume  (dis- 
placement of  water)  of  5 stones  selected  at  random  from  a sample  of  nests  from  13,  6 and 
25  different  Adelie,  Chinstrap  and  Gentoo  penguin  colonies,  respectively  (one-third  or  more 
of  the  colonies  of  each  species)  were  measured  in  both  rookeries.  Whenever  possible,  nest- 
stone  samples  were  collected  from  areas  in  which  the  3 species  nested  in  close  proximity. 

For  comparisons,  measurement  of  elevation,  slope  and  the  number  of  obstacles/colony 
were  weighted  by  multiplying  each  colony’s  value  by  the  number  of  pairs  in  the  colony.  AH 
statistical  comparisons,  unless  otherwise  indicated,  were  performed  using  a 1-way  analysis 
of  variance  and  the  Duncan’s  new  multiple  range  test. 

243 


244 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

The  west  rookery  consisted  of  20  Adelie,  9 Chinstrap  and  28  Gentoo 
penguin  colonies  (Fig.  1),  and  a population  of  11,000  Adelie,  750  Chinstrap 
and  700  Gentoo  penguin  pairs.  The  east  rookery  consisted  of  22  Adelie, 
4 Chinstrap  and  55  Gentoo  penguin  colonies,  and  a population  of  7000 
Adelie,  290  Chinstrap  and  1900  Gentoo  penguin  pairs.  In  the  2 rookeries, 
the  number  of  pairs  per  Adelie  colony  was  statistically  greater  than  the 
number  of  pairs  in  Chinstrap  and  Gentoo  penguin  colonies,  which  were 
statistically  equal  (Table  1).  In  the  west  rookery  Adelies  nested  at  higher 
elevations  than  chinstraps  which  nested  at  higher  elevations  than  gentoos 
(Table  2).  The  greatest  differences  in  elevation  of  nest-sites  were  evident 
between  Adelies  and  gentoos.  The  majority  of  Adelies  nested  more  than 
20  m above  sea  level,  while  the  majority  of  gentoos  selected  nest-sites  at 
less  than  10  m elevation.  The  distance  that  Adelies  nested  from  the  nearest 
landing  beach  was  statistically  greater  than  the  distances  chinstraps  and 
gentoos  nested  from  the  nearest  landing  beaches  (Table  1).  Chinstraps 
nested  in  areas  of  greater  slope  than  gentoos  (Table  1).  The  mean  number 
of  obstacles/colony  was  greater  in  gentoo  colonies  than  in  chinstrap  col- 
onies, which  had  more  obstacles  than  Adelie  colonies  (Table  1).  Adelies 
nested  more  closely  together  than  chinstraps  which  nested  more  closely 
together  than  gentoos  (Table  1).  The  volume  of  stones  used  by  chinstraps 
to  build  nests  was  statistically  greater  than  those  used  by  Adelies  and 
gentoos  (Table  3). 

Several  differences  were  apparent  among  the  nest-site  characteristics 
of  penguins  at  Point  Thomas.  The  Adelie  Penguin  nested  in  larger,  denser 
colonies  which  contained  fewer  obstacles,  were  at  higher  elevations  and 
were  farther  from  landing  beaches  than  those  of  its  congeners.  The  Chin- 
strap Penguin  tended  to  nest  in  steeply  sloped  areas,  whereas  its  conge- 
ners nested  in  generally  flat  or  gently  sloped  areas,  and  chinstraps  built 
nests  with  larger  stones  than  Adelies  and  gentoos.  On  Signy  Island,  Ade- 
lie, Chinstrap  and  Gentoo  Penguins  are  “crest,”  “slope”  and  “ridge”  nest- 
ers,  respectively  (White  and  Conroy  1975).  In  the  area  of  the  Antarctic  Pen- 
insula, Adelie  Penguins  nested  on  knolls  and  ridges,  chinstraps  on  rocky 
slopes  at  higher  elevations,  and  gentoos  in  low  flat  areas  (Muller-Schwarze 
and  Muller-Schwarze  1975).  With  the  exception  of  the  fact  that  White  and 
Conroy  (1975)  reported  gentoos  at  Signy  Island  nesting  primarily  on  ridges, 
our  findings  concur  with  these. 

Chinstraps,  in  addition  to  using  larger  nest-stones  than  Adelies  and 
gentoos,  build  their  nests  with  fewer  stones  than  gentoos  (Bagshawe  1938). 
Stones  are  abundant  at  Point  Thomas,  and  competition  for  them  is  prob- 
ably nonexistent.  Nests  built  of  relatively  larger  and  fewer  stones  may  be 
an  adaptation  to  nesting  in  steeply  sloped  areas  where  larger  stones  would 
provide  a more  stable  anchorage  for  the  nest  cup. 


Volkman  and  Trivel piece  • PENGUIN  NEST-SITE  SELECTION 


245 


Fig.  1.  Map  of  the  Point  Thomas  west  rookery  showing  the  positions  of  pygoscelid  col- 
onies with  respect  to  landing  beaches  and  elevation. 


I As  ice-  and  snow-free  areas  suitable  for  nesting  are  limited,  the  nest-site 
I preferences  of  pygoscelid  penguins  described  in  this  and  other  studies 
j may  be  the  result  of  competition  and  resource  partitioning.  Alternatively, 
we  suggest  that  these  preferences  may  result  from  differences  in  their 
ecology,  especially  in  the  degree  of  coloniality  evident  in  each  species.  In 
terms  of  colony  size  and  nest  density,  Adelies  are  the  most  colonial  and 
gentoos  the  least  colonial.  The  formation  of  relatively  larger  colonies  by 
Adelies  requires  relatively  larger  areas,  free  of  obstacles.  At  Point  Thomas 
these  areas  are  either  flat  or  gentle  slopes.  In  contrast,  chinstraps  and 
gentoos,  which  are  less  colonial,  can  exploit  nesting  habitat  which  is  more 
broken  up,  i.e.,  has  more  obstacles,  is  steeper,  or  is  flat,  but  can  accom- 
modate only  a few  nests,  i.e.,  ridges.  The  Adelie  Penguin,  the  only  py- 
goscelid which  breeds  in  high  latitude  rookeries  (e.g..  Cape  Crozier  and 
Cape  Royds,  Ross  Island),  nests  there  on  open,  wind-swept  knoll  and 
ridge  tops  in  order  to  avoid  drifting  snow  (Yeates  1975).  The  majority  of 
Adelies  at  Point  Thomas  do  not  nest  on  knoll  and  ridge  tops,  and  drifting 
snow  (possibly  because  of  warmer  temperatures)  does  not  appear  to  affect 


246 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  1 

Characteristics  (r  ± SE)  of  Pygoscelid  Penguin  Colonies  at  Point  Thomas 


Adehe 

Penguins 

Chinstrap 

Penguins 

Gentoo 

Penguins 

Number  of  pairs 
Distance  to  nearest 

416.5a,b**  + 131  7 

81.5  ± 18.7 

35.5  ± 4.9 

landing  beach  (m) 

ISp.b*  + 4 3 

93  ± 1.1 

92  ± 3.3 

Slope  (degrees) 
Number  of  obstacles 

5.7  ± 0.4 

9 3a,c**  + 0.2 

4.0"**  ± 0.2 

per  nesting  pair 

0.09  ± 0.06 

0.22  ± 0.04 

0 39a."**  + 0.04 

Internest  distances  (cm) 

43.2  ± 1.3 

59.9"**  ± 2.2 

74  3a,"**  + 3 g 

“ Differs  statistically  from  chinstrap. 
*’  Differs  statistically  from  gentoo. 

' Differs  statistically  from  Adelie. 

* P < 0.05. 

**  P < 0.01. 


their  reproductive  success  (Trivelpiece  and  Volkman,  unpubl.),  and  thus, 
is  probably  not  as  important  a factor  in  their  choice  of  nest-sites  here  as 
it  is  at  higher  latitudes. 

Among  the  possible  adaptive  advantages  of  coloniality  to  penguins  are: 
(1)  protection  from  predation,  (2)  protection  from  adverse  weather  condi- 
tions, (3)  social  facilitation  (i.e.,  colonies  as  “information  centers,”  [Ward 
and  Zahavi  1973]),  (4)  “social  stimulation”  (after  Darling  1938),  and  (5) 
maximal  exploitation  of  limited  ice-  and  snow-free  areas.  At  present,  data 
to  refute  or  substantiate  any  of  these  possibilities  are  limited.  However, 
based  on  available  data,  a preliminary  analysis  suggests  that  none  of  the 
first  4 possibilities  explains  the  differing  degrees  of  coloniality  among  the 


Table  2 

Percent.^ges  of  Pygoscelid  Penguins  Nesting  at  Different  Elevations  in  the 
Point  Thomas  West  Rookery 


Elevation  (m) 

Species 

1-10 

11-20 

21-30 

31^ 

Adelie** 
N = 20 

13.8 

1.2 

40.0 

45.0 

Chinstrap** 
N = 9 

17.0 

15.2 

67.8 

0.0 

Gentoo** 
N = 28 

66.5 

22.6 

10.9 

0.0 

**  X~  significantly  different  from  either  congener  (P  < 0.01). 


Volkrnan  and  Trivelpiece  • PENGUIN  NEST-SITE  SELECTION 


247 


Table  3 

The  Mean  (±  SE)  Length,  Width  and  Volume  of  Nest-stones  Used  By  Pygoscelid 

Penguins 


Species 


Length  Width  Volume 

(mm)  (mm)  (ml) 


Adelie 

N = 45  nests 

Chinstrap 
N = 50  nests 

Gentoo 

N = 54  nests 


41.1  ± 1.4 

52.1  ± 1.5** 
35.0  ± 1.2 


11.1  ± 0.4 

11.5  ± 0.6 

10.6  ± 0.5 


6.2  ± 0.4 
11.4  ± 0.9** 

5.2  ± 0.5 


**  Significantly  different  from  Adelie  and  gentoo  {P  < 0.01). 


pygoscelid  penguins.  The  Adelie  Penguin  does  nest  farther  south  than  its 
congeners  (see  Watson  1975)  and  there  is  a correlation  between  coloniality 
and  latitudinal  distribution.  Assuming  that  the  amount  of  ice-  and  snow- 
free  habitat  decreases  with  increasing  latitude,  coloniality  in  pygoscelids 
may  be  related  to  exploitation  of  ice-free  habitat.  This  conclusion  is,  of 
course,  preliminary  and  further  data  on  the  ecology  of  pygoscelid  penguins 
are  necessary  to  substantiate  its  validity. 

SUMMARY 

The  nest-site  preferences  of  sympatrically  breeding  Adelie  (Pygoscelis  adeliae),  Chinstrap 
{P.  antarctica)  and  Gentoo  (P.  papua)  penguins  were  quantified  in  rookeries  at  Point  Thom- 
as, South  Shetland  Islands.  Adelies  nested  in  larger,  denser,  more  open  colonies,  at  higher 
elevations  and  farther  from  landing  beaches  than  those  of  its  congeners.  Chinstraps  nested 
in  steeply  sloped  areas;  Adelies  and  gentoos  nested  generally  in  flat  and  gently  sloped  areas. 
It  is  suggested  that  differences  in  pygoscelid  nest-site  preferences  may  be  partially  attrib- 
utable to  differences  in  the  degree  of  coloniality  evident  in  each  species. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

The  Polish  Academy  of  Sciences  provided  logistical  and  technical  support  during  the 
austral  summer  of  1977/1978  at  Arctowski  Station  where  the  authors  were  visiting  scientists. 
We  thank  the  personnel  of  Arctowski,  particularly  J.  Jersak  and  M.  Zalewski,  station  leaders, 
for  their  generous  assistance.  L.  Dutkiewicz,  Geographic  Institute,  Lodz  University,  kindly 
provided  the  topographical  map  of  the  west  rookery.  We  thank  him  for  his  time,  patience 
and  companionship. 

Further  logistical  support  was  provided  by  the  U.S.  Antarctic  Research  Program  and  crew 
of  the  R/V  Hero  under  the  command  of  Captain  P.  Lenie.  We  thank  H.  Church  for  photo- 
graphic asistance  and  A.  Frost  for  computer  facilities.  D.  Ainley,  R.  Butler  and  an  anonymous 
reviewer  provided  helpful  criticisms  of  this  manuscript.  This  research  was  supported  by 
grant  75-15506  from  the  Division  of  Polar  Programs,  National  Science  Foundation,  to  D. 
MuUer-Schwarze. 


248 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Bagshawe,  T.  W.  1938.  Notes  on  the  habits  of  the  Gentoo  and  Ringed  or  Antarctic  pen- 
guins. Trans.  Zool.  Soc.  Lond.  24:185-306. 

Clarke,  W.  E.  1906.  Ornithological  results  of  the  Scottish  National  Antarctic  Expedition. 
II.  On  the  birds  of  the  South  Orkney  Islands.  Ibis  ser.  8(6):  145-187. 

Conroy,  J.  W.  H.,  O.  H.  S.  Darling  and  H.  G.  Smith.  1975.  The  annual  cycle  of  the 
Chinstrap  Penguin  {Pygoscelis  antarctica)  on  Signy  Island,  South  Orkney  Islands.  Pp. 
353-363  in  The  biology  of  penguins  (B.  Stonehouse,  ed.).  University  Park  Press,  Lon- 
don, England. 

Darling,  F.  F.  1938.  Bird  flocks  and  the  breeding  cycle:  a contribution  to  the  study  of  avian 
sociality.  The  University  Press,  Cambridge,  England  . 

Klopfer,  P.  H.  and  J.  P.  Hailman.  1965.  Habitat  selection  in  birds.  Pp.  279-303  in 
Advances  in  the  study  of  behaviour,  Vol.  1 (D.  S.  Lehrman,  R.  A.  Hinde  and  E.  Shaw, 
eds.).  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

Muller-Schwarze,  C.  and  D.  Muller-Schwarze.  1975.  A survey  of  twenty-four  rook- 
eries of  pygoscelid  penguins  in  the  Antarctic  Peninsula  region.  Pp.  309-320  in  The 
biology  of  penguins  (B.  Stonehouse,  ed.).  University  Park  Press,  London,  England. 

Murphy,  R.  C.  1936.  Oceanic  birds  of  South  America,  Vol.  1.  Am.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.,  New 
York,  New  York. 

Penney,  R.  L.  1968.  Territorial  and  social  behavior  in  the  Adelie  Penguin.  Pp.  83-131  in 
Antarctic  bird  studies.  Antarctic  Res.  Ser.,  Vol.  12  (O.  L.  Austin,  Jr.,  ed.).  Am.  Geo- 
phys.  Union,  Washington,  D.C. 

Trivelpiece,  W.  and  N.  J.  Volkman.  1979.  Nest-site  competition  between  Adelie  and 
Chinstrap  penguins:  an  ecological  interpretation.  Auk  96:675-681. 

Ward,  R.  and  A.  Zahavi.  1973.  The  importance  of  certain  assemblages  of  birds  as  “infor- 
mation centres”  for  food  finding.  Ibis  115:517-534. 

Watson,  G.  E.  1975.  Birds  of  the  Antarctic  and  Sub-Antarctic.  Am.  Geophys.  Union, 
Washington,  D.C. 

White,  M.  G.  and  J.  W.  H.  Conroy.  1975.  Aspects  of  competition  between  pygoscelid 
penguins  at  Signy  Island,  South  Orkney  Islands.  Ibis  117:371-373. 

Yeates,  G.  W.  1975.  Microclimate,  climate  and  breeding  success  in  Antarctic  penguins. 
Pp.  397^09  in  The  biology  of  penguins  (B.  Stonehouse,  ed.).  University  Park  Press, 
London,  England. 

STATE  UNIV.  NEW  YORK,  COLL.  ENVIRONMENTAL  SCIENCES  AND  FORESTRY, 
SYRACUSE,  NEW  YORK  13210.  (PRESENT  ADDRESSES:  BOX  1507, 
SEDONA,  ARIZONA  86336  [NJV];  POINT  REYES  BIRD  OBSERVATORY, 
4990  SHORELINE  HIGHWAY,  STINSON  BEACH,  CALIFORNIA  94970  [WTJ.) 
ACCEPTED  23  JULY  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  249-258 


COWBIRD  PARASITISM  AND  EVOLUTION  OF 
ANTI-PARASITE  STRATEGIES  IN  THE 
YELLOW  WARBLER 

Karen  L.  Clark  and  Raleigh  J.  Robertson 

The  Yellow  Warbler  {Dendroica  petechia)  is  frequently  parasitized  by 
the  Brown-headed  Cowbird  {Molothrus  ater)  resulting  in  reduction  of  nest 
success  at  parasitized  nests  (Schrantz  1943,  McGeen  1972).  There  are 
several  avenues  open  to  a Yellow  Warbler  once  a cowbird  egg  has  been 
deposited  in  its  nest.  It  could  accept  the  egg,  thereby  running  the  risk  of 
the  egg  hatching  and  the  cowbird  nestlings  competing  with  the  Yellow 
Warbler  nestlings.  Alternatively,  it  could  reject  the  egg.  This  could  occur 
by  ejection,  where  the  cowbird  egg  is  removed  from  the  nest  (cf.  Rothstein 
1975),  by  nest  desertion,  or  by  burial,  in  which  the  cowbird  egg,  along 
with  any  Yellow  Warbler  eggs  present  at  the  time,  are  covered  by  the 
addition  of  nesting  material.  The  response  favored  by  natural  selection 
depends  upon  the  potential  for  a successful  nest  attempt.  The  possibility  of 
success  varies  with  the  amount  of  time  and  energy  already  invested  in  the 
nesting  attempt,  and  the  possibility  of  the  cowbird  egg  hatching.  The  ob- 
jectives of  this  study  were  to  determine  the  frequency  of  occurrence  of 
these  various  responses  by  Yellow  Warblers  to  naturally  deposited  cowbird 
eggs  and  to  investigate  the  factors  eliciting  each  response. 

METHODS 

Yellow  Warbler  nests  were  located  in  several  study  areas  near  the  Queen’s  University 
Biological  Station,  Chaffey's  Locks,  Ontario,  from  1975-1977.  Most  nests  were  found  during 
nest  building.  Nests  for  which  the  date  of  clutch  initiation  was  unknown  have  not  been 
included  in  this  analysis  unless  noted.  In  1975  and  1976,  nests  were  checked  every  second 
day.  In  1977,  nests  were  checked  daily  during  egg-laying  and  early  incubation  and  then  every 
third  day  until  the  young  fledged.  All  references  to  number  of  Yellow  Warbler  eggs  indicate 
the  number  present  when  the  nest  was  checked.  In  some  parasitized  nests,  1 or  more  Yellow 
Warbler  eggs  may  have  been  removed  by  cowbirds.  Our  measure  of  nest  success  was  the 
number  of  young  leaving  the  nest  as  a proportion  of  the  number  of  eggs  laid.  All  references 
to  nest  success  are  only  to  those  nests  not  preyed  upon.  For  nests  which  received  more  than 
1 cowbird  egg,  only  the  response  to  the  first  egg  is  included  in  tables  and  text  unless  specified 
otherwise. 


RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

Forty-one  percent  (45/109)  of  all  Yellow  Warbler  nests  were  parasitized, 
containing  1 or  more  cowbird  eggs  (Table  1).  The  mean  nest  success  of 
parasitized  nests  was  0.44  ± 0.33  compared  to  the  mean  nest  success  of 
unparasitized  nests  of  0.80  ± 0.16  (Mann  Whitney  L-test,  U for  large 


249 


250 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  1 

Mean  Nest  Success  of  Par\sitized  and  Unpait\sitized  Nests 


Nest  status 

No.  Yellow- 
Warbler  nests 

No.  cowbird  eggs 

.f  ± sd 

Nest  success® 
(no.  of  nests) 

Parasitized 

Buried 

20'’ 

28 

0.78  ± 0.21  (13) 

Deserted 

10 

12 

0.0  ± 0.0  (10) 

Accepted 

12 

16 

0.53  ± 0.34  (8) 

Preyed  upon*^ 

3 

3 

(0) 

Total 

45 

59 

0.44  ± 0.33  (31) 

Not  parasitized 

64 

0 

0.80  ± 0.16  (35) 

“ Nest  success  = Yellow  ^ arbler  young  to  leave  the  nest  per  egg  laid,  including  buried  eggs,  based  only  on  nests  (number 
as  indicated)  which  were  not  preyed  upon  prior  to  fledging. 

**  Two  nests  are  included  which  were  found  after  clutch  initiation  and  are  not  included  in  subsequent  tables. 

' Number  of  nests  preyed  upon  before  the  response  to  the  cowbird  egg  could  be  determined.  They  are  included  here  to 
indicate  the  incidence  of  parasitism.  These  nests  are  not  included  in  subsequent  tables  or  in  percent  frequency  of  responses 
cited  in  text. 


samples  = 3.01,  P ^ 0.01).  The  number  of  nestlings  lost  per  nest  varied 
greatly  from  0 to  some  (variable)  threshold  number  leading  to  termination 
of  the  nesting  attempt.  The  reduction  in  nest  success  depended  on  the 
number  of  Yellow  Warbler  eggs  removed  by  the  cowbird,  the  stage  of 
nesting  when  the  cowbird  egg  was  laid,  and  the  response  of  the  Yellow 
Warbler  to  the  cowbird  egg  (Table  2).  For  8 of  9 nests  which  received  more 
than  1 cowbird  egg  the  response  to  subsequent  eggs  was  the  same  as  for 


Table  2 

Acceptance  and  Rejection  of  Cowbird  Eggs  as  a Function  of  the  Number  of 
Yellow  arbler  Eggs  When  the  Cowbird  Egg  was  Deposited 


No.  YW 
eggs® 

No.  YW 
nests 

No.  CB 
eggs^ 

No.  nests  with 
cowbird  eggs 

.f  (±  SD) 
nest  success*^ 

buried 

de- 

serted 

ac- 

cepted 

YW 

CB 

0 

20 

30 

13 

7 

0 

0.52  ± 0.42 

0.0  ± 

0.0 

1 

4 

4 

3 

0 

1 

0.48  ± 0.21 

0.0  ± 

0.0 

2 

6 

6 

1 

1 

4 

0.31  ± 0.52 

0.17  ± 

0.41 

3 

4 

7 

0 

2 

2 

0.30  ± 0.48 

0.67  ± 

0.58 

4 

5 

5 

1 

0 

4 

0.50  ± 0.25 

0.20  ± 

0.45 

5 

1 

1 

0 

0 

1 

— 

— 

“ Number  of  Yellow  W arbler  eggs  present  when  the  cowbird  egg  was  laid.  In  some  cases,  a \ eUow  arbler  egg  may- 
have  been  removed  by  the  cowbird. 

Total  number  of  cowbird  eggs  laid,  not  per  nest.  Most  nests  contained  only  1 cowbird  egg.  although  some  contained 
more  than  one. 

' Nest  success  measured  as  the  number  of  young  to  leave  nestVeggs  laid/nest,  excluding  nests  that  were  preyed  upon. 


Clark  and  Robertson  • YELLOW  WARBLER  ANTI-PARASITE  STRATEGIES  251 


the  first.  At  1 nest  the  first  cowbird  egg  was  laid  before  any  Yellow  Warbler 
eggs,  and  it  was  buried.  A second  egg  laid  when  there  were  3 Yellow 
Warbler  eggs  resulted  in  desertion  of  the  nest. 

Acceptance  of  cowbird  eggs. — Cowbird  eggs  were  accepted  at  only  29% 
(12/42)  of  all  parasitized  Yellow  Warbler  nests  (Table  1).  The  mean  nest 
success  of  Yellow  Warblers  which  accepted  cowbird  eggs  was  0.53  ± 0.34 
(Table  1)  and  where  cowbird  young  fledged  was  0.46  ± 0.33  (N  = 6). 
Acceptance  occurred  most  frequently  at  nests  which  had  2 or  more  Yellow 
Warbler  eggs  at  the  time  the  cowbird  egg  was  laid  (Tables  2,  3).  These 
results  are  similar  to  those  of  Rothstein  (1975)  who  found  100%  acceptance 
at  16  Yellow  Warbler  nests  which  were  experimentally  parasitized  when 
they  contained  at  least  2 warbler  eggs.  Accepted  cowbird  eggs  that  were 
laid  when  there  were  3 Yellow  Warbler  eggs  in  the  nest  had  the  highest 
success,  although  the  small  sample  size  of  cowbird  eggs  accepted  when 
there  were  0,  1 or  5 Yellow  Warbler  eggs  was  insufficient  to  assess  cowbird 
success  in  these  nests. 

The  cowbird  incubation  period  is  10-11  days  (Friedmann  1963)  whereas 
the  Yellow  Warbler’s  is  11-12  days  (Schrantz  1943,  this  study).  Yellow 
Warblers  will  initiate  incubation  before  their  clutch  is  complete.  With  a 
mean  clutch-size  of  3.6  ± 0.82  eggs  for  parasitized  Yellow  Warblers,  cow- 
bird eggs  deposited  on  or  before  the  day  the  third  egg  was  laid  hatch  with 
or  before  the  Yellow  Warbler  eggs.  The  chance  of  hatching  for  a cowbird 
egg  laid  when  there  were  3 or  fewer  Yellow  Warbler  eggs  in  the  nest  was 
83%  (5/6,  including  only  nests  with  accepted  cowbird  eggs  which  were  not 
preyed  upon).  In  3 of  these  5 nests  where  the  cowbird  egg  did  hatch,  only 

1 Yellow  Warbler  fledged  along  with  the  cowbird.  In  each  of  the  three, 
the  cowbird  hatched  earlier  than  any  of  the  Yellow  Warblers.  In  the  other 

2 nests,  in  which  the  cowbirds  hatched  synchronously  with  or  later  in  the 
day  than  the  warblers,  3 and  4 Yellow  Warblers,  respectively,  fledged 

i along  with  the  parasite’s  young.  The  time  of  hatching  of  Yellow  Warbler 
j eggs  relative  to  cowbird  eggs  thus  appears  to  be  a key  determinant  of 
I Yellow  Warbler  hatching  success  and  nestling  survival.  Mayfield  (1960: 

! 173)  found  that  Kirtland’s  Warbler  {Dendroica  kirtlandii)  nestlings  never 
I survived  when  there  were  2 or  more  older  cowbird  nestlings  in  the  nest, 

I and  survival  was  greatly  reduced  when  there  was  1 older  cowbird  nestling. 

However,  Kirtland’s  Warblers  which  hatched  2 or  more  days  before  the 
I cowbird  egg  hatched  were  not  adversely  affected  by  the  presence  of  the 
I cowbird. 

j'  Another  factor  predicted  to  influence  the  response  of  the  Yellow  War- 
ij  bier  to  parasitism  is  the  timing  of  the  event  with  respect  to  the  breeding 
!|  season.  A delay  due  to  egg  burial  or  renesting  could  have  detrimental 
( effects  associated  with  the  timing  of  the  nest,  relative  to  the  rest  of  the 


252 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  \o.  2,  June  1981 


Table  3 

Frequency  of  Occurrence  of  Rejection  and  Acceptance  of  Cowbird  Eggs 
Relative  to  Yellow  arbler  Nest  Stage 


No.  \ W eggs  when 
CB  egg  was  laid 

No.  VW 
nests 

Frequency  of  response 
% (no.)  of  nests  where  cowbird  eggs  were® 

buried 

deserted 

accepted 

0,  1 

24 

67  (16)^’ 

29  {If 

4(1)" 

2-5 

16 

12  (2)*’ 

19  (3)" 

69  (11)" 

® X'  = 20.02.  df  = 2.  P < 0.001 — indicating  that  the  3 responses  occurred  with  different  frequency  within  host  egg 
number  groupings. 

X"  = 11.38,  df  = I.  P < 0.001 — indicating  that  the  frequency  of  burial  is  different  for  clutches  of  0-1  vs  2-5,  when 
other  responses  are  grouped. 

' X"  = 0.55.  df  = 1.  P > 0.05 — indicating  that  the  frequency  of  desertion  is  similar  regardless  of  number  of  host  eggs 
when  nest  is  parasitized. 

X’  ~ 19.07.  df  = 1,  P < 0.001 — indicating  that  the  frequency  of  acceptance  is  different  for  clutches  of  0—1  vs  2-5. 
when  other  responses  are  grouped. 


avian  community.  Asynchronous  Yellow  Warbler  nests  were  subjected  to 
higher  predator  pressure  than  nests  in  synchrony  with  the  community  as 
a whole  (Clark  and  Robertson  1979),  a difference  possibly  attributable  to 
either  the  “swamping  effect"  or  “selfish  herd  effect"  on  predators  (Rob- 
ertson 1973,  Hamilton  1971).  Furthermore,  since  a bird's  initial  nesting 
attempt  is  thought  to  be  timed  to  take  advantage  of  optimal  conditions,  | 
delay  could  put  the  Yellow  Warbler  nest  out  of  phase  with  the  food  supply  > 
(Immelmann  1971).  Late  in  the  nesting  season  the  risk  of  loss  associated  ^ 
with  a delay  could  outweigh  the  potential  benefits  of  cowbird  egg  rejection.  • 
Consequently,  acceptance,  which  minimizes  any  delay  in  nesting,  is  ex- 
pected to  occur  more  frequently  later  in  the  breeding  season.  f 

Since  response  was  shown  to  depend  on  the  stage  of  the  nest  at  the 
time  of  parasitism  (Table  3)  this  factor  should  be  considered  when  inves-  ' 
tigating  seasonal  changes  in  response.  Rearranging  the  data  into  the  many 
small  categories  necessary  for  such  an  analysis  produced  sample  sizes 
inadequate  for  statistical  analysis.  It  was  evident,  however,  that  there  was  . 1 

a relationship  between  date  and  stage  of  the  nest  when  parasitized.  De-  ’ r 
fining  the  peak  of  clutch  initiation  as  the  day  on  which  the  maximum  d 
number  of  Yellow  Warbler  clutches  w ere  initiated,  we  found  that  parasit-  ( 
ism  of  nests  containing  0 or  1 \ellow  arbler  egg(s)  occurred  most  fre-  u 
quently  before  this  peak  (18  of  24  nests  containing  0 or  1 Yellow’  Warbler 
egg(s)  were  parasitized  before  the  peak  of  clutch  initiation).  Nests  with  2 le 
or  more  Yellow  \\  arbler  eggs  were  less  frequently  parasitized  before  the  v 
peak  of  clutch  initiation  (where  4 of  18  nests  with  2 or  more  'bellow  | i 
Vi  arbler  eggs  were  parasitized  before  the  peak  in  clutch  initiation,  j 
X~  = 11.49,  df  = 1.  P < 0.01).  Because  of  this  association  between  the  ; i 


4l 


Clark  and  Robertson  • YELLOW  WARBLER  ANTI-PARASITE  STRATEGIES  253 


Table  4 

Frequency  of  Occurrence  of  Rejection  and  Acceptance  of  Cowbird  Eggs  in 
Yellow  Warbler  Nests  During  the  Breeding  Season 


Frequency  of  response 
% (no.)  of  nests  where  cowbird  eggs  were“ 

Breeding  season  when  No.  YW  

YW  nest  parasitized  nests  accepted  deserted  buried 


Before  peak  of  Yellow 


Warbler  clutch  initiation*’ 

22 

68  (15)^^ 

23  (5)‘* 

9 (2f 

After  peak  of  Yellow 

Warbler  clutch  initiation 

18 

17  (3)" 

28  (5)“ 

56  (5f 

^ = 13.06,  df  = 2,  P < 0.002 — indicating  that  the  3 responses  occurred  with  different  frequency  within  season  cat- 

egories. 

Peak  of  clutch  initiation  defined  as  the  day  when  the  maximum  number  of  Yellow  Warbler  clutches  were  initiated:  22 
May  1975,  29  May  1976  and  26  May  1977. 

' “ 10.18,  df  = 2,  P < 0.002 — indicating  that  acceptance  occurred  with  different  frequency  before  vs  after  peak, 

when  other  responses  are  grouped. 

**  X^  = 0.13,  df  = 1,  P > 0.05 — indicating  that  desertion  rate  was  similar  before  and  after  peak. 

* X^  = 10.61,  df  = 1,  P < 0.001 — indicating  that  burial  occurred  with  different  frequency  before  vs  after  peak,  when 
other  responses  are  grouped. 


number  of  host  eggs  present  when  the  nest  was  parasitized  and  date, 
it  is  apparent  that  the  different  responses  to  cowbird  eggs  may  have 
resulted  from  either  nest  stage  or  date,  or  a combination  of  both;  although 
acceptance  did  occur  more  frequently  after  the  peak  of  clutch  initiation 
(Table  4),  this  is  also  when  nests  with  2 or  more  Yellow  Warbler  eggs  were 
more  frequently  parasitized.  Thus,  it  is  not  possible  to  decide  which  factor 
was  more  influential  in  determining  the  response.  Interestingly,  the  only 
nest  in  which  acceptance  occurred  when  there  were  0 or  1 Yellow  Warbler 
egg(s)  present  was  parasitized  after  the  peak  of  clutch  initiation. 

Cowbird  egg  rejection  by  ejection. — No  instances  of  egg  ejection  by  the 
Yellow  Warbler  were  recorded.  Rothstein  (1975)  has  shown  that  the  Yellow 
Warbler  beak-length-to-parasite-egg-width  ratio  is  larger  than  the  same 
ratio  for  some  other  species,  suggesting  that  Yellow  Warblers  are  capable 
of  ejecting  cowbird  eggs.  However,  Rothstein  (1976)  also  found  that  the 
Cedar  Waxwing  {Bombycilla  cedrorum)  has  problems  ejecting  eggs,  often 
incurring  nest  damage  and/or  bruising  in  the  process.  He  attributed  this 
to  the  small  bill  size  of  the  Cedar  Waxwing.  Yet  this  species  has  a beak- 
length-to-ejected-egg-width  ratio  well  above  that  of  the  Yellow  Warbler, 
which  has  the  smaller  exposed  culmen  (9.1  mm  vs  10.1  mm)  of  the  two 
(Godfrey  1966).  The  Yellow  Warbler  is  also  smaller  in  body  size,  with  a 
range  of  weight  of  9.3-12.3  g (Raveling  and  Warner  1978)  compared  to  the 
Cedar  Waxwing  which  has  a weight  in  the  range  of  30^2.5  g (Roberts 


254 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


1955).  If  the  Cedar  Waxwing  has  problems  ejecting  cowbird  eggs  the  small- 
er Yellow  Warbler  would  likely  have  even  greater  difficulty  in  this  regard. 

Possibly  a Yellow  Warbler  incapable  of  ejecting  an  intact  cowbird  egg 
might  first  break  the  egg  and  then  remove  it.  However,  piercing  and/or 
breaking  up  an  egg  would  likely  be  disadvantageous,  as  spilling  the  con- 
tents on  the  other  eggs  in  the  nest  could  make  them  difficult  to  roU  during 
incubation  (Rothstein  1975).  In  addition,  the  nest  might  be  more  vulnerable 
to  ant  infestations. 

Egg  rejection  by  burial. — Egg  burial  was  the  Yellow  Warbler’s  most 
common  response  to  a cowbird  egg  and  occurred  at  20  of  42  (48%)  para- 
sitized nests  (Table  1).  Burial  occurred  most  frequently  when  0 or  1 Yellow 
Warbler  egg(s)  were  in  the  nest  (Table  3).  Egg  burial  requires  a small  energy 
expenditure  on  the  part  of  the  Yellow  Warbler  in  building  a new  floor  and 
increasing  the  sides  of  the  nest.  It  also  allows  the  bird  to  lay  a new  clutch, 
thus  eliminating  the  threat  of  the  cowbird  egg  hatching  and  a reduction  in 
clutch-size  due  to  the  cowbird’s  removing  a host  egg.  The  mean  clutch- 
size  (excluding  buried  eggs)  of  Yellow  Warbler  nests  with  buried  cowbird 
eggs  (4.1  ± 0.92)  was  the  same  as  at  nests  which  were  not  parasitized  (4.1 
± 0.55  eggs),  suggesting  that  females  were  physiologically  capable  of  pro- 
ducing replacement  eggs  to  compensate  for  those  buried.  The  0.5  egg 
difference  between  the  unparasitized  clutch-size  of  mean  4.1  and  the  par- 
asitized clutch-size  of  mean  3.6  suggests  that,  on  average,  the  cowbird 
removes  a host  egg  from  1 out  of  2 nests  it  parasitizes. 

The  delay  in  nesting  caused  by  egg  burial  depended  upon  the  number 
of  Yellow  Warbler  eggs  that  were  buried  along  with  the  cowbird  eggs, 
since  these  would  have  to  be  replaced  in  the  new  clutch.  The  mean  time 
delay  to  initiation  of  a new  clutch  was  3.1  ± 1.6  days.  When  the  cowbird 
egg  was  laid  in  a nest  which  was  not  complete  the  delay  was  shorter,  since 
it  could  be  almost  entirely  buried  by  a thick  layer  of  lining.  For  each 
Yellow  Warbler  egg  that  was  buried  the  delay  was  increased  by  1 day. 
The  energy  loss  from  the  investment  in  the  buried  eggs  would  also  increase 
with  each  buried  egg.  Perhaps  because  of  the  large  energy  losses  and 
extended  time  delays  cowbird  eggs  were  seldom  buried  along  with  more 
than  1 Yellow  Warbler  egg. 

Rothstein  (1975)  has  suggested  that  the  Yellow  Warbler’s  choice  of  nest 
material  may  be  an  anti-parasite  adaptation.  In  his  study,  the  lining  of  the 
nests  was  very  similar  to  the  material  used  in  the  nest  frame  so  cowbirds 
may  have  been  unable  to  determine  when  the  nest  was  complete.  The 
cowbird  might  then  lay  before  completion,  and  its  egg  could  be  buried 
while  the  Yellow  Warbler  was  finishing  the  nest.  Mayfield  (1960:156)  noted 
that  in  some  Kirtland’s  Warbler  nests  the  cowbird  eggs  laid  before  the 
nest  was  completed  were  occasionally  buried  in  the  lining.  The  Yellow 


Clark  and  Robertson  • YELLOW  WARBLER  ANTI-PARASITE  STRATEGIES  255 


Warbler  nests  in  our  study  tended  to  be  lined  with  a material  distinctive 
from  that  used  in  the  nest  frame.  The  lining  was  usually  a fluffy  plant 
down,  while  the  frame  was  usually  coarse  plant  fibers.  Although  cowbirds 
may  have  mistaken  some  nests  as  complete  when  laying,  other  times  cow- 
bird  eggs  were  laid  when  the  floor  of  the  frame  was  obviously  incomplete. 
McGeen  (1972)  noted  that  the  cowbird  has  difficulty  timing  its  egg-laying 
with  the  nesting  of  the  Yellow  Warbler,  especially  when  there  are  Song 
Sparrows  {Melospiza  melodia)  nesting  in  the  vicinity.  Song  Sparrows  are 
a better  host  for  the  cowbird  than  the  Yellow  Warbler,  and  cowbird  egg- 
laying  is  usually  synchronized  with  the  first  nesting  of  the  Song  Sparrow, 
which  is  earlier  than  that  of  the  Yellow  Warbler.  Synchronization  of  the 
egg-laying  period  by  cowbirds  in  our  study  areas  with  that  of  Song  Spar- 
rows (which  were  common  in  the  area)  might  account  for  the  laying  of 
cowbird  eggs  in  unfinished  Yellow  Warbler  nests. 

The  occurrence  of  egg  burial  at  5 nests  where  Yellow  Warbler  eggs  were 
buried  along  with  a cowbird  egg  indicates  that  egg  burial  was  not  always 
a result  of  overlap  between  cowbird  laying  and  Yellow  Warbler  nest  build- 
ing. In  these  5 nests,  egg  burial  must  have  been  a direct  response  to  the 
cowbird  egg. 

Egg  burial  occurred  most  commonly  before  the  peak  of  clutch  initiation, 
when  a delay  would  not  place  the  nest  greatly  out  of  synchrony  with  the 
rest  of  the  avian  community  (Table  4).  An  extremely  late  nesting  Yellow 
Warbler  would  be  susceptible  to  the  disadvantages  of  asynchronous  nest- 
ing described  earlier. 

Yellow  Warbler  nests  which  had  a cowbird  egg  buried  were  no  more 
I susceptible  to  being  parasitized  again.  Of  20  nests  which  had  a cowbird 
! egg  buried  only  two  were  parasitized  again  compared  to  the  incidence  of 
parasitism  at  other  nests  where  25  out  of  89  were  parasitized  (x^  = 2.87, 

1 df  = 1,  P > 0.10,  NS).  Egg  burial  resulted  in  a mean  nest  success  of  0.78 
i ± 0.21,  which  was  not  significantly  different  from  0.80  ± 0.16,  the  mean 
t success  of  unparasitized  nests  (Table  1;  Mann-Whitney  f/-test,  U for  large 
Ij  samples  = 0.85,  P > 0.05).  The  significantly  lower  nest  success  of  accep- 
tor  nests  (0.53  ± 0.34)  compared  to  nests  where  cowbird  eggs  were  buried 
(j  (0.78  ± 0.21)  (Mann-Whitney  f/-test,  U = 47,  P < 0.05)  suggests  that  egg 
fi  burial  may  be  an  adaptive  response  to  cowbird  parasitism. 

Egg  rejection  by  nest  desertion  . — Nest  desertion  occurred  at  24%  (10/42) 

. of  the  parasitized  nests,  most  commonly  when  0 or  1 Yellow  Warbler  egg(s) 
V were  in  the  nest  (assuming  that  in  at  least  some  cases,  the  cowbird  removed 
a Yellow  Warbler  egg)  (Table  2).  The  advantages  of  nest  desertion  were 
» impossible  to  assess  as  the  success  of  a second  nesting  attempt  could  not 
5 be  determined  without  individually  marked  birds.  Also,  this  estimate  of 
' desertion  rate  is  likely  conservative  since  deserted  nests  are  more  difficult 


256 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


to  find.  Nests  deserted  early  involved  minimal  time  and  energy  investment 
and  the  potential  for  successful  renesting  would  have  been  high.  In  con- 
trast, pairs  of  Yellow  Warblers  deserting  nearly  complete  clutches  would 
have  incurred  a delay  of  6-9  days  (2-4  days  to  build  a nest  and  4-5  days 
to  lay  a new  clutch).  The  nesting  season  of  the  Yellow  Warbler  is  suffi- 
ciently short  (they  are  normally  single  brooded  at  this  latitude)  that  the 
potential  for  renesting  after  a delay  of  this  length  is  much  reduced.  Se- 
lection may  thus  favor  burial  over  desertion  early  in  the  season  because 
of  the  shorter  time  delay  and  lower  energy  costs.  In  the  case  of  the  pair 
which  buried  I cowbird  egg  but  deserted  after  a second  was  laid,  it  may 
be  that  building  a second  floor  and  replacing  the  buried  3-egg  clutch  re- 
sulted in  a delay  that  made  desertion  the  best  strategy. 

Desertions  occurred  with  the  same  frequency  before  and  after  the  peak 
of  clutch  initiation.  One  explanation  for  desertion  regardless  of  the  time 
in  the  nesting  season  would  be  that  in  some  cases  the  nest  support  struc- 
ture was  inadequate  to  allow  a new  floor  to  be  built  for  egg  burial.  In  fact, 
we  observed  I nest  where  the  floor  had  been  initiated  over  a cowbird  egg, 
but  before  it  was  complete  the  nest  became  unstable.  This  nest  was  then 
deserted  and  a new  nest  was  initiated  less  than  I m away.  Nest  desertion 
may  have  also  occurred  late  in  the  season  as  an  alternative  means  of 
rejection  when  egg  burial  would  have  resulted  in  a deleterious  time  delay. 
Selection  may  act  to  favor  desertion  and  termination  of  the  nesting  if  the 
potential  for  Yellow  Warbler  success  is  low  and  if  desertion  would  increase 
fitness  in  the  following  breeding  season.  High  adult  mortality  during  mi- 
gration may  seriously  weaken  evidence  supporting  this  last  hypothesis. 

It  is  difficult  to  determine  whether  nest  desertion  occurred  in  response 
to  a cowbird  egg,  human  observer  disturbance  at  nests,  altered  clutch- 
size  or  the  discovery  of  the  cowbird  at  the  nest  (Rothstein  1976).  In  this 
study,  desertions  occurred  at  24%  (10/42)  of  parasitized  nests  and  only  3% 
(2/64)  of  unparasitized  nests  (x^  = 15.43,  df  = I,  P < 0.001).  Since  all 
nests  were  checked  in  a similar  fashion  the  majority  of  desertions  are 
probably  due  to  cowbird  parasitism.  Desertion  at  the  2 unparasitized  nests 
occurred  after  a single  egg  had  been  removed  each  day  until  in  I nest 
there  was  I egg  left  and  in  the  other  2 eggs  were  left.  The  eggs  at  these 
nests  may  have  been  removed  by  either  cowbirds  or  predators.  Since  there 
were,  however,  few  predators  which  take  eggs  in  this  fashion  in  our  study 
area,  desertion  in  these  cases  may  also  have  been  due  to  cowbirds. 

Cowbirds  would  frequently  remove  a host  egg  before  laying  their  own 
so  that  clutch-size  was  not  increased  in  parasitized  nests.  Yellow  Warblers 
occasionally  had  clutches  of  5 eggs,  which  were  successful;  the  total  num- 
ber of  eggs  in  a parasitized  nest  exceeded  5 in  only  I nest.  The  cowbird 
and  2 of  the  Yellow  Warbler  eggs  in  the  clutch  eventually  hatched.  Thus, 


Clark  and  Robertson  • \ELLO^  Vi  ARBLER  A\1 1-PARASITE  SIRATEGIES  257 


an  inhibition  of  incubation  behavior  by  the  alteration  of  clutch-size  does 
not  likely  account  for  the  desertion  of  parasitized  nests. 

Desertion  occurred  most  frequently  when  there  were  no  Yellow  Warbler 
eggs  in  the  nest.  The  appearance  of  a cowbird  egg  before  the  Yellow 
Warbler  had  initiated  her  own  clutch,  or  the  replacement  of  the  first  war- 
bler egg  with  a cowbird  egg  on  the  day  of  initiation,  may  have  been  the 
main  cause  of  nest  desertion.  Desertion  may  thus  be  an  anti-parasite  strat- 
egy- evoked  in  direct  response  to  the  appearance  of  cowbird  eggs.  Alter- 
natively, desertion  may  be  a response  to  foreign  objects  in  the  nest  (Roth- 
stein  1975).  Discovery  of  the  cowbird  at  the  nest  may  also  have  resulted 
in  a sufficient  disturbance  to  cause  desertion  in  some  instances.  Since 
birds  wiU  often  desert  if  disturbed  by  a predator,  the  presence  of  a cowTird 
might  provide  a similar  stimulus  to  desert.  However,  Robertson  and  Nor- 
man (1976,  1977)  showed  that  aggressive  responses  to  cowbirds  can  reduce 
the  incidence  of  parasitism,  so  one  might  expect  that  a fleeing  response 
of  hosts  should  be  selected  against.  Also,  cowbirds  which  harass  their 
hosts  to  the  extent  of  causing  nest  desertion  would  be  selected  against, 
since  they  would  be  lowering  the  number  of  available  host  nests  and  re- 
ducing the  success  of  their  own  eggs.  Thus,  it  seems  most  likely  that  some 
Yellow  Warblers  desert  nests  due  to  the  presence  of  the  cowbird  egg  per 
se. 

In  conclusion,  the  Yellow  Warbler  appears  to  have  evolved  a finely 
j tuned  anti-parasite  strategy  involving  the  rejection  of  cowbird  eggs  by 
! either  egg  burial  or  nest  desertion  dependent  upon  the  stage  of  the  nest 
I in  which  the  cowbird  egg  is  deposited  and  upon  the  timing  of  the  nest  with 
respect  to  its  neighbors.  This  strategy  reduced  both  the  success  of  cow  bird 
eggs  in  Yellow  Warbler  nests  and  Yellow  Warbler  losses  due  to  parasitism. 

SUMMARY 

t We  recorded  the  responses  of  nesting  Yellow  Warblers  to  naturally  deposited  Brown- 
1^1  headed  Cowbird  eggs.  The  response  varied,  depending  upon  the  stage  of  the  nest  when  the 
ill  cowbird  egg  was  deposited,  the  time  of  the  breeding  season  and  the  structure  of  nest  support. 
1 An  association  between  nest  stage  and  time  in  the  breeding  season  did  not  allow  any  con- 

i elusions  about  the  relationships  between  either  of  these  factors  and  response  to  the  cowbird 
egg  to  be  made,  although  both  were  thought  to  be  influencing  the  choice  of  response.  Ac- 
t ceptance  of  cowbird  eggs  resulted  in  significantly  lower  nest  success  for  Yellow  arblers. 
{ The  most  frequent  rejection  response  by  the  YeDow  Warbler  was  burial  of  cowbird  eggs. 
i Parasitized  nests  in  which  burial  occurred  had  success  rates  comparable  to  unparasitized 
9 nests.  Egg  burial  was  used  as  an  anti-parasite  strateg>'  primarily  when  the  cowbird  egg  was 
i deposited  early  in  the  Yeflow  Warbler's  laying  cycle.  \est  desertion  was  the  alternative 
I rejection  response.  Desertion,  which  released  the  pair  from  a nesting  attempt  in  which  the 
V potential  for  success  w'as  low,  occurred  throughout  the  breeding  season.  Desertion  was 
V thought  to  occur  when  egg  burial  was  not  possible,  either  because  of  the  resulting  delay,  or 
<1  when  the  nest  support  structure  would  not  aflow  burial. 


l 


258 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

This  study  benefited  greatly  from  the  use  of  the  Queen’s  University  Biological  Station. 
Dave  Cameron,  Richard  Norman  and  Patrick  Weatherhead  assisted  with  the  nest  records 
and  Patrick  Colgan  helped  with  the  statistics.  This  paper  was  greatly  improved  by  the  reviews 
of  S.  I.  Rothstein  and  A.  L.  A.  Middleton  and  the  editorial  assistance  of  N.  Flood.  These 
contributions  and  research  support  to  Raleigh  J.  Robertson  from  Queen’s  University  and  the 
National  Research  Council  of  Canada  are  gratefully  acknowledged. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Clark,  K.  L.  and  R.  J.  Robertson.  1979.  Spatial  and  temporal  multi-species  nesting 
aggregations  in  birds  as  anti-parasite  and  anti-predator  strategies.  Behav.  Ecol.  Socio- 
biol.  5:359-371. 

Friedmann,  H.  1963.  Host  relations  of  the  parasitic  cowbirds.  Smithson.  Inst.  Washington, 
D.C. 

Godfrey,  W.  E.  1966.  The  birds  of  Canada.  Natl.  Mus.  Canada  BuU.  No.  203. 

Hamilton,  W.  D.  1971.  Geometry  for  the  selfish  herd.  J.  Theoret.  Biol.  31:295-311. 
Immelmann,  K.  1971.  Ecological  aspects  of  periodic  reproduction.  Pp.  342-489  in  Avian 
biology,  Vol.  1.  D.  S.  Earner  and  J.  R.  Kings,  eds.  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New 
York. 

Mayfield,  H.  1960.  The  Kirtland’s  Warbler.  Cranbrook  Inst.  Sci.,  Bloomfield  Hills,  Mich- 
igan. 

McGeen,  D.  S.  1972.  Cowbird-host  relationships.  Auk  89:360-380. 

Raveling,  D.  G.  and  D.  W.  Warner.  1978.  Geographic  variation  of  Yellow  Warblers  killed 
at  a TV  tower.  Auk  95:73-79. 

Roberts,  T.  S.  1955.  Manual  for  the  identification  of  birds  of  Minnesota  and  neighboring 
states.  Revised  ed.  Univ.  Minnesota  Press,  Minneapolis,  Minnesota. 

Robertson,  R.  J.  1973.  Optimal  niche  space  of  the  Red-winged  Blackbird:  spatial  and 
temporal  patterns  of  nesting  activity  and  success.  Ecology  54:1085-1093. 

AND  R.  F.  Norman.  1976.  Behavioral  defenses  to  brood  parasitism  by  potential 

hosts  of  the  Brown-headed  Cowbird.  Condor  78:166-173. 

AND . 1977.  The  function  and  evolution  of  aggressive  host  behavior  towards 

the  Brown-headed  Cowbird  {Molothrus  ater).  Can.  J.  Zool.  55:508-518. 

Roi'HSTEIN,  S.  1.  1975.  An  experimental  and  teleonomic  investigation  of  avian  brood  par- 
asitism. Condor  77:250-271. 

. 1976.  Experiments  on  defences  Cedar  Waxwings  use  against  cowbird  parasitism. 

Auk  93:675-691. 

ScHRANTZ,  F.  C.  1943.  Nest  life  of  the  eastern  Yellow  Warbler.  Auk  60:367-387. 

DEPT.  BIOLOGY,  QUEEN’s  UNIV.,  KINGSTON,  ONTARIO  K7L  3n6  CANADA. 
ACCEPTED  16  JULY  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  259-264 


INTERACTIVE  BEHAVIOR  AMONG  BALD  EAGLES 
WINTERING  IN  NORTH-CENTRAL  MISSOURI 

Curtice  R.  Griffin 

Despite  the  increasing  interest  in  the  Bald  Eagle  {Haliaeetus  leucoceph- 
alus)  attested  to  by  recent  field  studies  (Shea  1973;  Lish  and  Lewis  1975; 
Servheen  1975;  Steenhof  1976;  Stalmaster  and  Newman  1978,  1979),  few 
published  reports  describe  intraspecific  behavior  of  Bald  Eagles  in  winter. 
The  present  paper  describes  the  frequency  and  extent  of  intraspecific 
conflict,  and  discusses  the  possible  consequences  of  patterns  of  interac- 
tions between  adults  and  immatures. 

Eagles  commonly  displace  one  another  from  food  items  and  perches 
(Southern  1963,  Shea  1973,  Servheen  1975).  Intraspecific  aggression  may 
be  most  common  during  feeding  periods  (Jonen  1973),  and  adults  usually 
dominate  immature  birds  in  aggressive  encounters  (Erskine  1968).  In 
Oklahoma,  Lish  (1973)  described  displacement,  tail  chasing  (aerial  pursuit) 
and  talon  presentation  behavior  of  wintering  Bald  Eagles.  From  these  ob- 
servations, Lish  suggested  that  a social  hierarchy  might  exist  on  the  win- 
tering grounds.  Stalmaster  and  Newman  (1979)  stated  that  the  oldest  bird 
i usually  occupied  the  highest  site  when  eagles  of  different  age  classes 
perched  in  the  same  tree. 


METHODS 

I watched  the  behavior  of  Bald  Eagles  almost  daily  from  October  1975-March  1976  at 
Swan  Lake  National  Wildlife  Refuge,  Chariton  Co.,  north-central  Missouri.  1 observed  in- 

I traspecific  behavioral  dominance  at  or  near  feeding  areas  and  perch  sites,  but  not  near  night 
ji  roosts.  I used  binoculars  (7  X 50  mm)  and  a spotting  scope  (15-60 x),  and  made  all  obser- 
j vations  from  a vehicle  or  blind.  Birds  with  entirely  white  heads  and  tails  were  classed  as 
ji  adults;  all  others  were  classed  as  immatures. 

ji  Types  of  aggressive  encounters  included  displacement,  aerial  pursuit  and  talon  presen- 
tation. Criteria  of  dominance  in  displacement  encounters  included  the  supplanting  of  1 eagle 
j|j  by  another  from  a food  item  or  perch,  or  the  fleeing  of  an  eagle  when  another  approached. 

II  Aerial  pursuits  involving  more  than  1 chasing  bird  were  tallied  according  to  age  classes  of 
i the  birds  involved.  Only  1 talon  presentation  was  tallied  per  encounter  regardless  of  the 
l|  number  occurring.  To  compare  participation  in  aggressive  encounters  by  birds  of  the  2 age 

! classes,  it  is  necessary  to  take  into  account  the  proportion  of  birds  in  each  age  class.  Ac- 
cordingly, these  analyses  followed  Hallman’s  (1975)  procedure,  incorporating  a Chi-square 
|l  test.  Twenty-two  ground  counts  were  made  during  the  study. 

RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

Numbers  of  Bald  Eagles  on  Swan  Lake  Refuge  fluctuated  throughout 
the  winter.  A peak  of  66  birds  occurred  on  2 December.  In  22  censuses, 
353  observations  of  immatures  and  248  of  adults  were  recorded.  The  adult 

259 


260 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  1 

Displacement  Attempts,  Aerial  Pursuits  and  Talon  Presentations  of  Immature 
AND  Adult  Bald  Eagles,  Swan  Lake  National  Wildlife  Refuge 


Types  of  encounter 

Immature — 
Immature^* 

Immature — 
Adult 

Adult — 
Immature 

Adult- 

Adult 

Total 

Displacement'^ 

102"  (10) 

40"  (3) 

32'*  (1) 

64'*  (1) 

238  (15) 

Percent  of  total 

43% 

17% 

13% 

27% 

100% 

Aerial  pursuits 

45^' 

9 

3‘' 

4'' 

61 

Percent  of  total 

74% 

15% 

5% 

6% 

100% 

Talon  presentation 

23'' 

3 

0" 

1 

27 

Percent  of  total 

85% 

11% 

— 

4% 

100% 

“ Initiator — recipient. 

*’  Numbers  of  unsuccessful  displacement  attempts  in  parentheses. 

P < 0.05,  numbers  of  encounters  according  to  age  class  differing  from  expected  (see  text). 
P < 0.01,  numbers  of  encounters  according  to  age  class  differing  from  expected  (see  text). 


component  ranged  from  0-^4  of  the  observed  winter  population.  Detailed 
information  on  eagle  populations  at  the  refuge  during  this  study  is  in  Griffin 
(1978). 

Feeding. — Feeding  was  highly  communal.  A few  (2-3)  to  more  than  30 
eagles  fed  close  together.  Eagles  rarely  shared  the  same  food  item.  Most 
of  the  heavier  food  items,  waterfowl  and  fish  carcasses,  were  consumed 
on  the  ground  or  ice.  Eagles  waded  to  carcasses  in  shallow  water  and 
dragged  them  to  shore  or  onto  a low  perch  to  feed.  Eagles  sometimes 
gathered  small  carcasses  from  the  water,  frozen  impoundments  or  shore 
by  swooping  upon  them  without  landing. 

Displacement. — Bald  Eagles  were  seen  attempting  to  displace  each  oth- 
er from  food  carcasses  and  perches  at  the  feeding  areas  238  times.  All  but 
15  (6.3%)  attempts  were  successful.  Of  238  attempts,  102  (43%)  were 
between  immature  birds  and  64  (27%)  between  adults.  Immature  eagles 
attempted  to  displace  adult  birds  in  40  (17%)  of  the  observations  and  adult 
eagles  tried  to  displace  immatures  in  32  (13%)  (Table  1). 

Aerial  pursuit  and  talon  presentation. — Aerial  pursuits  and  talon  pre- 
sentation (Eig.  1)  occurred  at  both  low  and  high  altitudes  throughout  the 
winter,  whether  or  not  food  was  being  carried.  In  a pursuit,  one  or  more 
eagles  chased  another,  sometimes  flying  within  0.5  m of  each  other.  Eagles 
used  a fast  descending  glide  when  chasing  and  a labored  flapping  flight 
when  pursuit  was  intense.  The  pursued  eagle  performed  evasive  maneu- 
vers, usually  steep  dives  or  dives  followed  by  a steep  climb.  Most  aerial 
pursuits  lasted  less  than  30  sec;  however,  some  pursuits  lasted  at  least  8 
min,  with  the  birds  flying  out  of  sight.  Of  61  observations  of  aerial  pursuits, 
27  (44%)  involved  at  least  1 talon  presentation.  In  some  instances,  after 
presenting  talons,  the  pursued  bird  became  the  pursuer. 


Griffin  • WINTER  BEHAVIOR  OE  BALD  EAGLES 


261 


Fig.  1.  Aerial  pursuit  and  talon  presentation  of  Bald  Eagles  (after  Lish  1973). 


Talon  presentation  occurred  when  one  of  the  pursuing  eagles  dived  at 
another  in  flight.  As  the  diving  eagle  neared  the  lower-flying  eagle,  the 
latter  flipped  over  and  presented  its  talons.  Contact  between  the  bodies 
of  the  2 eagles  occurred  occasionally.  This  behavior  is  similar  to  Bald 
Eagle  courtship  displays  (Brown  and  Amadon  1968)  except  that  whirling 
with  the  talons  locked  does  not  occur. 

The  interactions  of  immature  and  adult  eagles  during  aerial  pursuit  and 
talon  presenting  are  tallied  in  Table  1.  Immature  eagles  pursued  and  pre- 
sented talons  to  adults  or  other  immatures  in  most  observations.  Adult 
eagles  pursued  immatures  or  other  adults  in  only  11%  of  the  aerial  pursuit 
observations.  There  were  no  observations  of  adults  presenting  talons  to 
immatures,  and  only  1 observation  (4%)  of  an  adult  presenting  talons  to 
another  adult. 

Some  aerial  pursuits  and  talon  presentations  involved  stealing  of  food 
in  flight.  The  number  of  cases  was  not  determined  because  of  the  difficulty 
of  seeing  food  in  the  talons.  In  food-stealing  encounters,  1-5  eagles  ap- 
proached the  food-carrying  bird  from  the  rear,  pursuing  birds  flipped  over 
and  took  the  food  from  the  pursued  bird’s  talons  or  dived  repeatedly, 
forcing  the  pursued  eagle  to  drop  the  food.  Once  food  was  dropped,  a 
pursuing  eagle  attempted  to  recover  it. 

Patterns  of  interactions  related  to  age. — Considering  their  relative  abun- 
dances, adult  eagles  initiated  significantly  fewer  displacement  attempts 


262 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


(X^  = 11.427,  df  = 1,  P < 0.01,  N = 238),  aerial  pursuits  (x^  = 9.389, 
df  = 1,  P < 0.01,  N = 61)  and  talon  presentations  (x^  = 6.540,  df  = 1, 
P < 0.05,  N = 27)  with  immatures  than  expected.  However,  adult  birds 
attempted  to  displace  other  adults  more  frequently  than  expected  (x^  = 
13.611,  df  = 1,  P < 0.01,  N = 238).  Immatures  often  entered  into  dis- 
placement attempts  (x^  = 4.828,  df  = 1,  P < 0.05,  N = 238),  aerial  pur- 
suits (x^  = 27.285,  df  = 1,  P < 0.01,  N = 61)  and  talon  presentation  (x^ 
= 20.151,  df  = 1,  P < 0.01,  N = 27)  with  other  immatures;  however,  im- 
matures infrequently  attempted  displacement  of  adults  (x^  = 5.415,  df  = 
1,  P < 0.05,  N = 238)  (Table  1).  These  apparent  patterns  must  be  viewed 
with  some  caution,  because  the  behavior  was  possibly  influenced  by  se- 
verity of  weather,  abundance  of  food  and  numbers  of  eagles  present.  These 
conditions  changed  frequently  and  their  effects  could  not  be  analyzed. 

The  potential  is  great  for  severe  injury  from  aggressive  fighting  of  such 
a large  and  pugnacious  bird  as  an  adult  Bald  Eagle,  and  immature  eagles 
are  probably  seldom  competitive  with  adults  at  aggressive  fighting.  Con- 
ditioning from  previous  encounters  probably  has  encouraged  a dominance- 
subordinance  relationship  favoring  adults  as  has  been  indicated  for  many 
large  aggressive  mammals  such  as  lions  {Panthera  leo)  (Schaller  1972), 
wolves  {Canis  lupis)  (Mech  1970)  and  a number  of  primate  species  (Brown 
1975). 

Several  studies  of  avian  foraging  have  shown  that  the  ability  to  obtain 
food  improves  with  age  (reviewed  by  Buckley  and  Buckley  1974,  Verbeek 
1977).  To  compensate  for  lesser  prowess  at  finding,  capturing  and  defend- 
ing food,  immature  eagles  may  be  forced  to  (1)  spend  more  time  than 
adults  in  searching  for  food,  (2)  seek  alternative  food  sources,  (3)  use 
different  wintering  areas  than  adults,  or  (4)  resort  to  stealing.  The  fourth 
alternative  may  lead  to  the  strong  tendency  noted  in  this  study  for  immature 
eagles  to  enter  into  aggressive  encounters  at  feeding  areas.  The  third 
alternative  may  also  be  important.  The  age  ratio  of  immatures  to  adults 
at  the  refuge  is  lowest  during  the  periods  of  harshest  weather  and  lowest 
food  availability  (Griffin  1978).  This  fact  and  the  known  tendency  for  im- 
mature eagles  to  winter  farther  south  than  adults  (Sprunt  and  Cunningham 
1962,  Ingram  1965,  Sprunt  and  Ligas  1966)  indicate  than  many  immature 
eagles  may  seek  wintering  areas  not  used  by  adults. 

SUMMARY 

Wintering  Bald  Eagles  displayed  food  and  perch  displacement,  aerial  pursuit  and  talon 
presentation  behavior  on  the  wintering  grounds.  Immature  eagles  initiated  most  of  these 
behavioral  interactions,  and  most  displacement  attempts  were  successful.  In  displacement 
attempts,  immatures  tried  to  displace  adults  somewhat  more  frequently  than  adults  tried  to 
displace  immatures.  Nearly  half  of  all  aerial  pursuits  involved  at  least  1 talon  presentation. 
.\n  undetermined  number  of  aerial  pursuits  and  talon  presentations  involved  food  being 


Griffin  • WINTER  BEHAVIOR  OF  BALD  EAGLES 


263 


stolen  from  the  pursued  eagle.  Considering  their  relative  abundances,  adult  eagles  initiated 
significantly  fewer  displacement  attempts,  aerial  pursuits  and  talon  presentations  with  im- 
matures  than  expected.  Although  immatures  often  entered  into  all  3 behavioral  interactions 
with  other  immatures,  they  infrequently  attempted  displacement  of  adults. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

I thank  J.  R.  Acker,  B.  S.  Johnson  and  M.  Murphy  for  their  assistance  in  the  field.  I am 
grateful  to  F.  B.  Samson  and  T.  S.  Baskett  for  their  critical  review  of  this  manuscript.  This 
report  is  a contribution  from  the  Missouri  Cooperative  Wildlife  Research  Unit  (School  of 
Forestry,  Fisheries  and  Wildlife,  University  of  Missouri,  Missouri  Department  of  Conser- 
vation, U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife  Service,  and  Wildlife  Management  Institute,  cooperating). 
The  work  was  funded  in  part  by  the  National  Audubon  Society  and  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife 
Service  Contract  No.  USDI  14-16-0008-757,  awarded  to  the  University  of  Missouri. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Brown,  L.  and  D.  Amadon.  1968.  Eagles,  hawks,  and  falcons  of  the  world.  Vol.  1. 
McGraw-Hill,  New  York,  New  York. 

Brown,  J.  L.  1975.  The  evolution  of  behavior.  W.  W.  Norton  and  Co.,  New  York,  New 
York. 

Buckley,  F.  G.  and  P.  A.  Buckley.  1974.  Comparative  feeding  ecology  of  wintering  adult 
and  juvenile  Royal  Terns.  Ecology'  55:1053-1063. 

Erskine,  a.  j.  1968.  Encounters  between  Bald  Eagles  and  other  birds  in  winter.  Auk 
85:681-683. 

Griffin,  C.  R.  1978.  The  ecology  of  Bald  Eagles  wintering  at  Swan  Lake  National  Wildlife 
Refuge,  with  emphasis  on  eagle-waterfowl  relationships.  M.S.  thesis,  Univ.  Missouri, 
Columbia,  Missouri. 

Hailman,  j.  P.  1975.  Analysis  of  aggression  in  White-throated  Sparrow  types  of  different 
proportions.  Bird-Banding  46:236-240. 

Ingram,  T.  N.  1965.  Wintering  Bald  Eagles  at  Guttenberg,  Iowa — CassviUe,  Wisconsin, 
1964-1965.  Iowa  Bird  Life  35:66-78. 

JONEN,  J.  R.  1973.  The  winter  ecology'  of  the  Bald  Eagle  in  west-central  Illinois.  M.S.  thesis. 
Western  Illinois  Univ.,  Macomb,  Illinois. 

Lish,  j.  W.  1973.  Status  and  ecology  of  Bald  Eagles  and  nesting  of  Golden  Eagles  in 
Oklahoma.  M.S.  thesis,  Oklahoma  State  Univ.,  Stillwater,  Oklahoma. 

AND  J.  C.  Lewis.  1975.  Status  and  ecology  of  Bald  Eagles  wintering  in  Oklahoma. 

Proc.  Southeastern  Assoc.  Game  and  Fish  Comm.  29:415^23. 

Mech,  L.  D.  1970.  The  wolf:  the  ecology'  and  behavior  of  an  endangered  species.  Natural 
History  Press,  Garden  City,  New  York. 

SCHALLER,  G.  B.  1972.  The  Serengeti  lion:  a study  of  predator-prey  relations.  Univ.  Chicago 
Press,  Chicago,  Illinois. 

Servheen,  C.  W.  1975.  Ecology  of  the  wintering  Bald  Eagles  on  the  Skagit  River,  Wash- 
ington. M.S.  thesis,  Univ.  Washington,  Seattle,  Washington. 

Shea,  D.  S.  1973.  A management  oriented  study  of  Bald  Eagle  concentrations  in  Glacier 
National  Park.  M.S.  thesis,  Univ.  Montana,  Missoula,  Montana. 

Southern,  W.  E.  1963.  Winter  populations,  behavior  and  seasonal  dispersal  of  Bald  Eagles 
in  northwestern  Illinois.  Wilson  BuU.  75:42-55. 

Sprunt,  a.,  IV  AND  R.  L.  Cunningham.  1962.  Continental  Bald  Eagle  project.  Progress 
Rept.  No.  2. 


264 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


AND  F.  J.  Ligas.  1966.  Audubon  Bald  Eagle  studies  1960-1966.  Proc.  Natl.  Audubon 

Soc.  Conv.  62:25-30. 

Stalmaster,  M.  V.  AND  J.  R.  Newman.  1978.  Behavioral  responses  of  wintering  Bald 
Eagles  to  human  activity.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  42:506-513. 

AND  . 1979.  Perch-site  preferences  of  wintering  Bald  Eagles  in  northwest 

Washington.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  43:221-224. 

Steenhof,  K.  1976.  The  ecology  of  wintering  Bald  Eagles  in  southeastern  South  Dakota. 

M.S.  thesis,  Univ.  Missouri,  Columbia,  Missouri. 

Verbeek,  N.  a.  M.  1977.  Comparative  feeding  behavior  of  immature  and  adult  Herring 
Gulls.  Wilson  BuU.  39:415-121. 


MISSOURI  COOPERATIVE  WILDLIFE  RESEARCH  UNIT,  SCHOOL  OF  FORESTRY, 
FISHERIES  AND  WILDLIFE,  112  STEPHENS  HALL,  UNIV.  MISSOURI,  CO- 
LUMBIA, MISSOURI  65211.  ACCEPTED  5 JUNE  1980. 


PRESIDENTS  MESSAGE 

As  part  of  its  efforts  to  encourage  research  by  students  and  amateurs  The  Wilson 
Ornithological  Society  sponsors  a series  of  awards.  Of  these,  the  two  oldest,  named  in 
honor  of  Louis  Agassez  Fuertes  and  Margaret  Morse  Nice,  are  not  supported  by  en- 
dowment. Therefore,  their  distribution  is  not  guaranteed.  The  Council  and  I prefer 
that  support  for  their  endowment  he  gathered  from  the  membership  rather  than  a 
single  donor.  The  many  small  donations  would  provide  a special  kind  of  honor, 
both  for  the  awards  and  the  recipients,  and  the  awards  truly  would  come  from  the 
Society. 

Most  of  you  will  shortly  he  returning  your  1982  Dues  Notice  to  the  Ornithological 
Societies  of  North  America.  I have  instructed  Treasurer  Robert  D.  Burns  that  any 
donations  to  the  Wilson  Endowment  Fund  for  1982  be  credited  to  the  Fuertes  and 
Nice  Awards.  Treasurer  Burns  would  also  he  happy  to  accept  contributions  mailed 
directly  to  him.  The  required  amount  is  about  $6,000.  The  Society  has  just  over 
2,250  active  members.  Hence,  a contribution  from  each  of  us  of  only  $3.00  would 
he  sufficient. 

Two  years  ago  many  of  the  “older"  Life  Members  responded  to  a plea  from  then 
President  George  Hall  with  major  contributions  that  helped  the  Society  through  a 
difficult  period.  I trust  the  general  membership  will  now  respond  as  generously  to 
this  solicitation  for  a much  happier  cause. 


Abbot  S.  Gaunt,  President 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  265-267 


GENERAL  NOTES 


Interspecific  song  mimesis  by  a Lincoln  Sparrow. — Some  oscines  learn  the  songs 
of  alien  species  in  the  laboratory,  but  in  the  wild  generally  learn  only  conspecific  songs 
(Kroodsma,  in  Ontogeny  of  Behavior,  Burghardt  and  Bekoff,  eds..  Garland  Publ.  Co.,  New 
York,  New  York,  1978).  Of  those  groups  which  do  mimic  in  nature,  finches  (carduelids  and 
emberizids)  vary  in  their  propensity  to  imitate  heterospecific  songs.  In  Germany,  European 
Greenfinches  {Chloris  chloris)  imitate  a wide  variety  of  sympatric  species  and  use  these 
imitations  in  their  advertising  songs  (Guttinger,  J.  Ornith.  115:321-337,  1974;  Baptista,  un- 
publ.).  Indigo  {Passerina  cyanea)  and  Lazuli  {P.  amoena)  buntings  regularly  imitate  each 
other  in  a zone  of  sympatry  in  Nebraska  (Emlen  et  ah,  Wilson  Bull.  87:145-179,  1975).  Other 
species  mimic  rarely  (Baptista,  Z.  Tierpsychol.  30:266-270,  1972;  Kroodsma,  Wilson  Bull. 
84:173-178,  1972;  Williams  and  McRoberts,  Condor  79:113-118,  1977). 

Several  learning  strategies  appear  to  exist  in  Melospiza.  Swamp  Sparrows  {M.  georgiana) 
exposed  to  conspecific  songs  and  those  of  sympatric  Song  Sparrows  (M.  melodia)  learned 
conspecific  songs  but  failed  to  mimic  interspecificaUy.  Song  Sparrows  similarly  exposed 
learned  their  own  plus  heterospecific  songs  (Marler  and  Peters,  Science  198:519-521,  1977). 
Indeed,  Song  Sparrows  are  now  known  to  mimic  other  species  both  in  the  laboratory  and  in 
the  field  (Eberhardt  and  Baptista,  Bird-Banding  48:193-205,  1977;  Kroodsma,  Anim.  Behav. 
25:390-399,  1977).  Lincoln  Sparrows  (M.  lincolnii)  to  our  knowledge,  have  not,  hitherto, 
been  known  to  copy  songs  of  heterospecifics.  We  document  herein  a case  of  interspecific 
song  mimesis  in  a Lincoln  Sparrow  and  speculate  on  the  conditions  leading  to  this  behavior. 

In  1978,  we  began  studies  on  song  dialects  and  their  possible  function(s)  in  montane  White- 
crowned  Sparrows  {Zonotrichia  leucophrys  oriantha)  at  Tioga  Pass  Meadow,  Mono  Co., 
California,  119°E  38°N,  elev.  ca.  3000  m.  About  25  pairs  of  White-crowned  Sparrows  and  2 
pairs  of  Lincoln  Sparrows  breed  on  this  meadow.  On  25  June  1979,  we  recorded  a territorial 
Lincoln  Sparrow  whose  songs  possessed  components  virtually  identical  with  syllables  from 
a White-crowned  Sparrow.  Songs  of  White-crowned  Sparrows  at  Tioga  Pass  have  been 
studied  in  great  detail  (Orejuela  and  Morton,  Condor  77:145-153,  1975;  Baptista  and  King, 
Condor  82:267-284,  1980).  Typically,  each  song  begins  with  a whistle  (Fig.  lA,  syllable  type 
a),  followed  by  a buzz  (b),  a complex  syllable  (c),  a trill  (d  and  e),  ending  with  another  buzz 
(f).  The  complex  syllable  (c)  shows  regional  variation.  This  particular  form  of  syllable  (c) 
illustrated  is  found  at  Gardisky  Lake,  about  4 km  north  of  the  meadow.  However,  several 
White-crowned  Sparrows  used  this  syllable  at  tbe  meadow  and  on  mountain  slopes  to  the 
east. 

A detailed  study  of  song  variation  and  ontogeny  in  Lincoln  Sparrows  is  still  lacking.  Borror 
(Ohio  J.  Sci.  61:161—174,  1961)  analyzed  some  Lincoln  Sparrow  songs  from  Ontario  and 
Wyoming.  He  noted  that  each  bird  sang  3 or  more  themes  (unique  combinations  of  syllables), 
and  that  birds  at  a locality  shared  similar  or  identical  phrases.  We  identified  5 themes  in  23 
recorded  songs  from  our  Lincoln  Sparrow  (Fig.  IB-F).  Each  theme  consisted  of  5-7  syllable 
types,  each  of  which  occurred  singly  or  in  groups  of  2-8.  Three  themes  contained  4 (Fig. 
IB),  2 (Fig.  1C)  or  1 (Fig.  ID)  syllables  in  common  with  White-crowned  Sparrow  song. 

Syllable  e (theme  B)  is  similar  to  a modified  White-crowned  Sparrow  syllable  arranged  in 
a trill.  Another  White-crowned  Sparrow  (not  illustrated)  recorded  on  the  study  meadow  used 
syllables  similar  to  this  type  e.  The  buzz  in  theme  B (syllable  b)  is  more  rapidly  modulated 
than  any  local  White-crowned  Sparrow  buzz.  However,  similar  buzzes  are  known  from  songs 
of  other  populations  of  White-crowned  Sparrows  (Baptista,  Univ.  Calif.  Publ.  Zool.  105:1- 
52,  1975).  The  2 terminal  syllables  in  theme  B are  similar  to  those  in  other  typical  Lincoln 
Sparrow  themes  (see  theme  E). 


265 


266 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


A 


D 


B 


b c 


Tffffff 


II 


C F 


0 Secs. 


Fig.  1.  A.  Song  of  a White-crowned  Sparrow  recorded  at  Gardisky  Lake,  4 km  north  of 
Tioga  Pass  Meadow.  B-F.  Themes  sung  by  a Lincoln  Sparrow  recorded  at  the  Meadow,  25 
June  1979.  Note  that  the  Lincoln  Sparrow’s  themes  include  0 (themes  E and  F),  1 (e  in  theme 
D),  2 (b  and  c in  theme  C),  or  4 (b,  c,  d,  e in  theme  B)  syllables  borrowed  from  a White- 
crowned  Sparrow. 


It  is  possible  that  syllables  from  songs  of  different  species  may  be  alike  accidentally. 
However,  the  fact  that  as  many  as  4 syllable  types  in  theme  B match  those  of  the  model  (A) 
in  both  structure  and  sequence  is  strong  evidence  that  song  mimesis  has  occurred. 

I'his  Lincoln  Sparrow  appeared  to  show  considerable  improvisation  in  developing  its  song 
repertoire.  The  introductory  whistle  typical  of  all  White-crowned  Sparrow  songs  has  been 
replaced  by  a series  of  syllables  in  all  its  mimicked  songs.  White-crowned  Sparrow  syllables 
were  incorporated  in  3 themes  (B,  C and  D).  The  same  Lincoln  Sparrow  syllables  were  also 
used  in  several  themes.  Thus,  as  in  the  congeneric  Song  Sparrow  (Eberhardt  and  Baptista 
1977),  Lincoln  Sparrows  may  use  the  same  syllables  in  different  themes. 

Several  explanations  have  been  postulated  for  interspecific  song  mimesis  in  nature.  Large 
song  repertoires  may  advertise  individual  fitness.  For  example,  in  an  habitual  mimic,  the 
Northern  Mockingbird  {Mirnus  polyglottos),  repertoire  size  was  correlated  with  territorial 
quality  (Howard,  Evolution  28:428-438,  1974).  Since  Howard  did  not  age  the  birds,  possibly 
those  with  higher  quality  territories  were  dominant,  reflecting  their  age  and  experience. 
Older  birds  presumably  would  have  had  more  time  to  copy  greater  numbers  of  alien  species 
(Dobkin,  Z.  Tierpsychol.  50:348-363,  1979).  Birds  hatched  late  in  the  year  may  be  exposed 
to  fewer  conspecific  songs  in  their  acoustic  environment  and  thus  mimic  interspecifically 
(Baptista  1972,  Kroodsma  1972).  Since  Lincoln  Sparrows  are  rare  on  Tioga  Meadow,  a ju- 
venile developing  its  vocal  repertoire  would  encounter  few  conspecifics  to  imitate  and  may 
thus  be  stimulated  to  imitate  interspecifically.  Occasional  interspecific  mimicry  may  simply 
reflect  individual  variation  in  dispositions  to  improvise  (Baptista  1972).  Interspecific  com- 
petition may  result  in  convergence  of  distance  producing  signals,  e.g.,  territorial  song. 


GKNKRAL  NOTES 


267 


through  learning  interspecifically  (Cody,  Condor  71:222-239,  1969;  Emlen  et  al.  1975;  Brown, 
Can.  J.  Zool.  55:1523-1529,  1977). 

In  territorial  species,  song  learning  and  matching  of  themes  often  follows  intense  inter- 
male interaction  (Bitterbaum  and  Baptista,  Auk  96:462^74,  1979).  Nice  (Trans.  Linn.  Soc. 
6:1-238,  1943)  hand-raised  2 Song  Sparrows  which  vied  for  dominance,  each  soon  producing 
6 identical  themes.  The  closely  related  Lincoln  Sparrow  is  also  highly  territorial  and  responds 
strongly  to  playback  of  conspecific  song.  We  have  several  observations  of  the  Lincoln  Spar- 
row interacting  aggressively  with  sympatric  White-crowned  Sparrows  and  vice-versa.  Per- 
haps the  Lincoln  Sparrow  learned  the  White-crowned  Sparrow’s  song  during  such  interspe- 
cific interaction. 

Fieldwork  was  supported  in  part  by  National  Science  Foundation  Crant  DEB  77-12980  to 
Baptista  and  Morton. — Luis  F.  Baptista,  Martin  L.  Morton  and  Maria  E.  Pereyra, 
Dept.  Biology,  Occidental  College,  1600  Campus  Road,  Los  Angeles,  California  90041.  (Pres- 
ent address  LFB:  Dept.  Birds  and  Mammals,  California  Academy  of  Sciences,  San  Francisco, 
California  94118.)  Accepted  20  May  1980. 


Wilson  Bull..  93(2),  1981,  pp.  267-271 


Notes  on  Purple  Gallinules  in  Colombian  ricefields. — Little  has  been  reported  on 
Purple  Callinules  (Porphyrula  martinica)  in  ricefields,  despite  the  species’  affinity  for  this 
habitat  as  a nesting  site  (Ensminger,  La.  Conserv.  11:19,  1959;  Meanley,  Auk  80:545-547, 
1963).  Descriptions  of  nests  and  food  habits  are  few  and  limited  to  populations  in  naturally 
occurring  marshes  (e.g..  Bent,  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  135,  1926;  Cross  and  Van  Tyne,  Auk 
46:431-446,  1929;  Imhof,  Alabama  Birds,  Univ.  Alabama  Press,  University,  Alabama,  1962). 
In  certain  Neotropical  areas.  Purple  Callinules  are  considered  pests  due  to  loss  of  harvestable 
rice  incurred  by  bending  rice  (Oryza  sativa)  plants  into  nests  and  feeding  platforms  (Feakin, 
ed.,  Pest  control  in  rice,  PANS  Manual  No.  3,  Tropical  Pesticide  Research  and  Information 
Unit,  London,  England,  1970).  Gallinules  are  seasonally  abundant  in  much  of  the  extensive 
rice-producing  region  east  of  the  Andes  in  northern  South  America,  prompting  experimen- 
tation with  various  control  procedures.  Endrin  has  been  used  as  a control  agent  for  gallinules 
in  Surinam  (Haverschmidt,  Birds  of  Surinam,  Livingston  Publ.  Co.,  Wynnewood,  Pennsyl- 
vania, 1968)  and  is  presently  being  used  in  Colombia,  but  few  data  are  available  on  the 
effects  of  this  practice.  In  this  paper,  I describe  nest  abandonment  by  Purple  Gallinules  in 
response  to  endrin  applications,  and  report  on  nest  construction  and  placement,  observations 
of  an  unusual  escape  behavior,  and  food  habits  of  the  species  in  Colombian  ricefields. 

Study  area  and  methods. — The  Hacienda  La  Corocora  (3°57'N,  73°24'W;  elev.  310  m)  is 
located  in  a large  rice-growing  zone  in  the  tropical  savanna  of  the  Llanos  Orientales  in  Meta, 
Colombia.  Annual  rainfall  averages  2600  mm,  with  the  rainy  season  occurring  from  April- 
October.  Descriptions  of  climate  and  vegetation  of  the  region  were  given  by  Bates  (Geogr. 
Rev.  38:555-574,  1948)  and  Blydenstein  (Ecology  48:1-15,  1967).  Rice  is  grown  year-round 
in  10-90  ha  plots  bordered  by  marshes  and  shrubby  pastures.  Ricefields  in  various  stages 
of  growth  occupy  a contiguous  area  of  600  ha  with  a mean  water  level  of  13.5  cm  in  cultivated 
plots.  Purple  Gallinules  migrate  to  the  area  in  late  March  and  nest  from  May  at  least  through 
August.  Gallinules  are  occasionally  found  in  green  ricefields,  but  usually  do  not  enter  fields 
until  the  “yellowing”  or  maturing  stage,  when  rice  grains  are  forming  (about  10  weeks  after 
germination).  As  water  is  drained  from  each  plot  prior  to  harvesting,  gallinules  move  into 


268 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


nearby  ricefields  that  are  beginning  to  mature.  Most  gaUinules  leave  the  area  at  the  end  of 
the  w^et  season,  although  small  numbers  remain  all  year  in  local  marshes.  Little  is  known 
regarding  dispersal  of  the  migrant  population. 

I estimated  minimum  density  of  gaUinules  in  ricefields  by  averaging  flush  counts  from 
paraUel  300  X 40-m  transects  spaced  250  m apart.  I sampled  3-5  transects  per  plot  (de- 
pending on  plot  size)  on  different  days  to  reduce  bias  associated  with  birds  flushing  from  one 
transect  to  another  and  being  counted  twice.  Density  figures  were  obtained  prior  to  and 
following  aerial  applications  of  19.5%  endrin  (1  gal/ha  or  3.79  1/ha)  by  the  Hacienda  in  1977 
and  1978. 

Nests  were  located  by  systematicaUy  searching  ricefields  and  nearby  marshes  on  repeated 
occasions  during  May-July  1977  and  1978.  For  each  nest  I recorded  height  of  nest  rim  above 
water,  greatest  diameter  of  nest,  outside  depth,  bowl  depth,  estimated  percent  of  nest  surface 
covered  when  viewed  from  directly  above,  size  and  placement  of  runway  (Gross  and  Van 
Tyne  1929)  and  number  and  sizes  of  eggs.  I investigated  correlates  of  nest  placement  in 
ricefields  by  recording  water  depth,  height  of  rice  above  water  and  distance  to  nearest 
ricefield  border  at  nest-sites  and  comparing  these  values  with  similar  data  obtained  at  an 
equal  number  of  ricefield  locations  determined  from  a table  of  random  numbers.  1 recorded 
data  only  from  nests  known  to  be  active,  i.e.,  containing  eggs,  since  unfinished  nests  are 
frequently  found  near  active  nests  (Bent  1926). 

I collected  48  adult-plumaged  gaUinules  in  ricefields  during  May-June  1977  and  1978. 
Food  items  removed  from  the  esophagus,  proventriculus  and  gizzard  were  preserved  in  8% 
formaldehyde  (Martin,  Procedures  in  Wildlife  Food  Studies,  USFWS  Wildlife  Leaflet  325, 
1949)  and  subsequently  identified.  Aggregate  volume  of  each  item  was  determined  by  water 
displacement.  Six  birds  with  empty  digestive  tracts  or  extensively  digested  stomach  contents 
were  excluded.  The  remaining  sample  contained  14  males,  14  females  and  14  birds  of  un- 
known sex  whose  digestive  tracts  had  been  removed  by  farm  workers  before  the  gonads 
could  be  examined. 

Gallinule  densities  and  response  to  endrin  applications. — In  1977,  migrant  gaUinules  first 
appeared  in  marshes  on  the  study  area  during  the  last  week  of  March.  Numbers  increased 
through  April  and  scattered  individuals  were  observed  in  the  earliest  maturing  ricefields  (an 
area  of  50  ha)  during  the  second  week  of  May.  GaUinules  became  abundant  in  these  plots 
during  the  third  week  of  May  with  a minimum  density  of  21/ha.  This  density  indicated  a 
large  influx  of  migrants,  since  the  number  of  birds  in  the  marshes  did  not  appear  to  decrease. 
Egg-laying  began  on  17-18  May.  On  2 June,  endrin  was  applied  to  plots  with  nesting  gaUi- 
nules. By  3 June,  all  nests  (N  = 11)  had  been  abandoned  and  minimum  density  of  birds  had 
decreased  to  2/ha.  Many  gaUinules  had  apparently  moved  and  begun  to  nest  in  untreated 
plots  (area  22  ha)  which  were  beginning  to  mature.  Minimum  density  in  the  second  plots  was 
27/ha  on  15  June.  Endrin  was  then  applied  to  all  untreated  plots  on  1 July,  after  which  the 
new  nests  (N  = 18)  were  abandoned  and  virtually  all  gaUinules  left  the  Hacienda’s  ricefields. 
No  dead  birds  were  found  foUowing  either  application.  Where  the  gaUinules  went  is  unknown. 

In  1978,  gaUinules  appeared  in  the  earliest  maturing  ricefields  during  the  first  week  of 
May,  but  the  large  influx  of  migrants  did  not  occur  until  the  first  week  of  June.  Minimum 
density  was  20/ha  by  7 June.  Endrin  was  applied  to  aU  plots  during  9-20  June,  after  which 
most  gaUinules  left  the  area  without  initiating  nests.  1 found  5 dead  gaUinules  after  the  1978 
pesticide  applications.  The  birds  presumably  died  from  pesticide  poisoning,  but  facilities  for 
analysis  were  not  available.  Although  gaUinules  remained  in  ricefields  at  low  densities  (7/ha) 
until  I left  the  study  area  in  mid-August,  1 found  only  1 nest  in  1978,  which  was  abandoned 
by  1 July. 

Nests  and  eggs. — AU  ricefield  nests  (N  = 30)  were  constructed  entirely  of  leaves  and  pan- 
icles of  growing  rice  plants  wound  into  a roughly  circular  cup  supported  by  rice  stems. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


269 


Platforms  were  numerous  in  the  vicinity  of  nests,  but  nests  were  not  located  on  these  struc- 
tures. Nests  had  a mean  diameter  of  21.8  ± 0.4  (SE)  cm  (range  16-28  cm),  outside  depth  of 
10.0  ± 0.5  cm  (6-16  cm),  bowl  depth  of  5.2  ± 0.4  cm  (2-9  cm),  and  were  placed  29.8  ± 1.0 
cm  (21-42  cm)  above  the  water.  Most  nests  were  partially  covered  by  leaves  and  panicles 
bent  over  into  a high  arch  25  cm  or  more  above  the  nest  rim.  Surface  areas  of  3 nests  were 
covered  50%  or  more,  10  nests  were  25-50%  covered,  7 were  10-25%  covered,  6 were  1- 
10%  covered  and  4 were  not  covered  to  any  extent.  Most  nests  had  an  entrance  indicated 
by  a low  section  of  the  rim  adjacent  to  a runway  of  bent  leaves  which  often  led  to  feeding 
platforms.  Runways  were  approximately  15  cm  wide,  20  cm-1  -I-  m long  and  either  led  directly 
to  the  entrance  or  were  built  tangentially  to  it,  apparently  allowing  the  birds  to  enter  the  nest 
from  2 directions.  Several  runways  were  poorly-defined,  and  3 nests  lacked  evidence  of 
runways  or  entrances. 

Nest-site  means  for  water  depth  (14.7  ± 0.8  cm,  range  6-26  cm),  rice  height  (65  ± 1.3 
cm,  55-85  cm)  and  distance  to  nearest  border  (108  ± 16.7  m,  29-200  m)  did  not  differ  from 
those  for  30  random  points  {t  = <1.7),  indicating  that  gaUinules  nested  randomly  with  re- 
spect to  these  factors.  Data  regarding  nest  spacing  are  incomplete,  since  more  nests  would 
probably  have  been  initiated  if  endrin  had  not  been  applied.  Observed  inter-nest  distance 
was  usually  40  m or  more,  although  2 nests  were  located  only  11m  apart. 

Clutches  contained  up  to  7 eggs,  but  many  nests  were  obviously  abandoned  before  clutches 
were  complete  (13  nests  contained  1-2  eggs).  Fifty-three  eggs  averaged  41.0  ± 0.3  X 29.3 
± 0.2  mm.  Eggs  within  clutches  varied  by  as  much  as  5.5  mm  in  length  and  2.5  mm  in  width. 
Weights  of  2 eggs  of  unknown  age  were  15.2  g and  14.8  g. 

Seven  additional  nests  were  found  in  dense  growths  of  “platanillo”  {Thalia  geniculata)  in 
narrow  strips  of  marsh  habitat  (<0.5  ha)  along  streams  and  drainage  canals.  Nests  were 
constructed  of  T.  geniculata  leaves  and  differed  from  ricefield  nests  by  being  placed  at 
greater  heights  above  the  water  {.x  = 56.7  ± 5.0  cm,  t = 2.80,  P < 0.01),  and  having  greater 
outside  depths  (x  = 15.6  ± 1.9  cm,  t = 2.77,  P < 0.01).  No  more  than  1 nest  was  found  in 
each  area  of  marsh. 

Escape  behavior. — When  approached,  gaUinules  usuaUy  flushed,  although  they  frequently 
remained  on  the  ground  and  moved  away  through  the  vegetation.  On  several  occasions,  I 
closely  pursued  running  gaUinules  whose  locations  were  apparent  from  movements  of  the 
rice  plants.  After  running  several  meters,  the  birds  that  did  not  flush  lowered  their  bodies 
2-3  cm  under  the  water,  flattened  out  and  remained  completely  submerged  with  eyes  closed. 
I captured  by  hand  male  and  female  birds  in  this  submerged  posture.  Local  farm  workers 
often  use  this  technique  to  catch  gaUinules  for  food.  To  my  knowledge,  this  unusual  escape 
behavior  has  not  been  reported  for  the  species. 

Food  habits. — Rice  grains  constituted  68%  of  the  food  by  volume,  occurring  in  aU  but  1 
of  the  stomachs  examined  (Table  1).  The  remainder  comprised  ricefield  weed  seeds  (5%)  and 
animal  matter  (27%),  the  most  frequent  being  borer  moth  (Noctuidae)  pupae  and  larvae, 
dragonfly  (Odonata)  adults  and  nymphs,  and  various  beetles  (Coleoptera).  An  unusual  item 
was  a 2.6-cm  section  of  the  unfeathered  crus  and  knee  of  an  adult-sized  Purple  Gallinule, 
found  in  the  gizzard  of  a female  coUected  26  June  1977.  This  was  presumably  taken  from  a 
dead  bird,  implying  that  gaUinules  occasionaUy  will  feed  on  carrion.  On  2 occasions,  I found 
partiaUy-eaten  sections  of  10-cm  fish  (Cichlidae)  on  feeding  platforms.  Grit,  obtained  from 
adjacent  gravel  access  roads,  was  present  in  aU  stomachs  examined. 

Purple  GaUinules  are  known  to  feed  largely  on  seeds  and  fruit  of  aquatic  plants  supple- 
mented by  small  invertebrates  (Gross  and  Van  Tyne  1929;  Imhof  1962;  Krekorian,  Condor 
80:382-390,  1978),  although  feeding  on  flower  blossoms  (Crosby,  Florida  Nat.  42:171,  1969), 
tree  fruit  (Meanley  1963)  and  opportunistic  predation  on  eggs  and  young  of  other  birds  (Bailey, 
Auk  44:560,  1927;  Mcllhenny,  Auk  53:327-328,  1936;  Beadel,  Auk  63:87-88,  1946)  have  also 


270 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  1 

Esophageal  and  Stomach  Contents  of  42  Purple  Gallinules  Collected  in 
Colombian  Ricefields  May-June  1977  and  1978 


Food  item 

Percentage 

Volume 

Occurrence 

Plant  (seeds) 

Rice,  Oryza  saliva 

68 

98 

Cyperaceae  spp. 

2 

19 

Paspalurn  not  alum 

1 

5 

Thalia  geniculata 

trace 

5 

Panicum  sp. 

trace 

2 

Echinochloa  colonurn 

trace 

5 

Scleria  pterota 

trace 

2 

Polygonum  sp. 

trace 

7 

Gramineae  sp. 

trace 

2 

Scirpus  sp. 

trace 

7 

Croton  tinctoria 

trace 

2 

Paspalurn  virgatum 

trace 

7 

Total  plant 

73^ 

98 

Animal 

Noctuidae  pupae  and  larvae 

14 

12 

Odonata  adults  and  nymphs 

6 

12 

Coleoptera  (undetermined) 

1 

10 

Curculionidae 

1 

7 

Tenebrionidae 

trace 

5 

Hemiptera  (undetermined) 

trace 

2 

Pentatomidae 

trace 

2 

Corydalidae  larvae 

trace 

2 

Cyclorrhapba  pupae 

trace 

2 

Hymenoptera 

trace 

2 

Other  insect  (undetermined  fragments) 

4 

26 

Arachnida 

trace 

5 

Gastropoda 

trace 

2 

Crus-knee  segment  (gallinule) 

1 

2 

Total  animal 

27“ 

43 

® Totals  include  trace  (>0.5%)  items. 

been  reported.  This  study  demonstrates  that  gallinules  nesting  in  ricefields  feed  mostly  on 
rice,  but  take  a variety  of  other  items  when  available. 

Sexes  did  not  differ  in  amounts  of  rice  or  plant  material  eaten,  but  females  consumed 
greater  volumes  of  animal  matter  (.r  = 1.9  ml  for  females  vs  <0.1  ml  for  males,  t = 2.42, 
P < 0.05)  and  total  food  (.f  = 6.2  ml  vs  3.6  ml,  t — 2.07,  P < 0.05).  Mean  weights  of  males 
(213.4  ± 5.1  g)  and  females  (223.6  ± 15.6  g)  were  not  significantly  different  (f-test,  NS). 
Sexual  differences  in  food  habits  may  correspond  to  selection  by  females  for  more  animal 


GKNERAL  NOTES 


271 


matter  during  the  nesting  season.  For  birds  in  general,  egg  production  requires  more  protein 
than  does  sperm  production  (King,  pp. 79-107  in  Breeding  Biology  of  Birds,  Farner,  ed., 
Natl.  Acad.  Sci.,  Washington,  D.C.,  1973).  Krekorian  (1978)  assumed  that  the  heavier  bird 
in  each  pair  was  male;  the  lack  of  significant  sexual  dimorphism  in  the  weight  of  breeding 
Purple  Gallinules  in  this  study  suggests  that  this  may  not  be  a reliable  criterion  for  sexing 
the  birds. 

In  eastern  Colombia  the  amount  of  land  converted  to  rice  culture  is  steadily  increasing. 
Ricefields  present  an  advantageous  nesting  habitat  for  Purple  Gallinules  by  affording  an 
abundant  food  supply  and  stable  water  levels.  Some  of  the  insects  consumed  by  gallinules 
are  serious  pests  in  rice  (notably  the  noctuid  caterpillars),  indicating  that  food  habits  of  this 
species  are  to  some  extent  beneficial.  I was  informed  by  local  farmers  that  endrin  is  used 
against  gallinules  in  varying  quantities  and  apparently  with  no  established  guidelines.  Little 
is  known  regarding  the  effect  of  pesticides  of  gallinule  population  dynamics.  In  view  of  the 
potential  for  crop  and  environmental  contamination,  studies  integrating  damage  analysis  with 
feeding  habits  are  needed  to  assess  accurately  the  impact  of  Purple  Gallinules  in  tropical 
ricefields. 

This  study  was  sponsored  by  the  Institute  Colombiano  Agropecuario.  I am  grateful  for  the 
assistance  of  Danilo  Valencia,  Ernesto  Barriga,  Dario  Leal  and  La  Corocora  Ltda.;  I espe- 
cially thank  Patricia  Chacon  for  identifying  insects.  E.  E.  Good,  Thomas  Lemke,  Jeffrey 
Jorgenson  and  Clait  Braun  offered  valuable  advice  on  drafts  of  this  manuscript. — WALLACE 
D.  McKay  (deceased),  Smithsonian-Peace  Corps  Environmental  Program,  U.S.  Embassy, 
Bogota,  Colombia.  (Corresponding  address:  Douglas  McKay,  17  Hamilton  Ave.,  Wheeling, 
West  Virginia  26003).  Accepted  20  Mar.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  271-274 

Agonistic  behavior  of  the  White-breasted  Nuthatch. — My  studies  of  agonistic  be- 
havior of  White-breasted  Nuthatches  {Sitta  carolinensis)  were  begun  in  Bethesda,  Maryland, 
in  1953,  but  generally  undertaken  in  Lyme,  New  Hampshire,  between  1961  and  1973.  Pre- 
vious detailed  reports  of  agonistic  behavior  of  the  White-breasted  Nuthatch  are  lacking, 
although  Tyler  (Wilson  Bull.  28:18-25,  1916),  Butts  (Bird-Banding  2:1-26,  59-76,  1931),  Bent 
(U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  195,  1948)  and  Brackbill  (Maryland  Birdlife  25:87-91,  1969)  have  been 
helpful. 

Agonistic  displays. — Included  are  a spectrum  of  displays  which,  as  noted  for  the  European 
Nuthatch  (S.  europaea)  (Lohrl,  Z.  Tierpsychol.  15:191-252,  1958),  may  merge  confusingly. 
Displays  most  discernible  are: 

(1)  Tail-fanning.  Here  the  tail  is  raised  and  fanned,  displaying  the  black  and  white  mark- 
ings. It  is  given  frequently  by  the  female  when  her  mate  comes  close  to  the  nest  where  she 
is  dominant,  as  well  as  in  conflicts  with  rival  pairs. 

(2)  Wing-flicking.  This  action,  combined  with  raising  the  tail,  was  used  chiefly  against 
predators. 

(3)  Threat  display.  Usually  the  bill  is  raised,  wings  are  down  and  tail  is  cocked  up  as  shown 
in  Fig.  1 and  by  Lohrl  (1958)  for  the  European  Nuthatch.  The  pose  is  assumed  by  a sub- 
ordinate when  threatened  by  a dominant  bird  of  the  same  or  a different  species. 

(4)  Aggressive  threat  display.  It  resembles  (3),  except  for  a raising  of  the  back  feathers 
and  a pointing  downward  of  head  and  bill  (Fig.  2).  It  is  given  in  severe  conflicts. 

(5)  Raising  back  feathers  with  wings  and  tail  in  normal  position.  This  display  (Fig.  3)  is 


272 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Eig.  1.  Threat  display  of  a White-breasted  Nuthatch. 


usually  seen  just  prior  to  one  nuthatch  attacking  another,  as  when  a male  is  about  to  fly 
at  his  mate  prior  to  a pursuit  flight  (Kilham,  Auk,  89:115-129,  1972). 

(6)  Bill  pointed  forward.  A female  runs  with  biU  pointed  straight  forward  without  any 
display,  either  at  a male  intruder  by  the  nest  or  at  a juvenile  she  is  trying  to  drive  away. 

Displacement  pecking.  Males  excited  in  border  conflicts  (or,  as  one  seen  when  disturbed 
by  a Barred  Owl  [Strix  varia]),  will  occasionally  peck  at  places  unconnected  with  food,  the 
head  and  bill  held  straight  up  and  down.  Brackbill  (1969)  noted  this  activity  among  nuthatches 
coming  to  a tray  where  other  birds  were  feeding.  I interpreted  it  as  a displacement  activity 
in  which  a drive  to  attack  is  both  activated  and  thwarted,  the  pecking  being  a way  of  relieving 
surplus  excitation  (Bastock  et  al.  Behaviour  6:66-84,  1953). 

Vocalizations. — Several  kinds  are  involved  as  follows: 

Tchup  expresses  mild  excitement.  Kun,  ka-un  and  kaan  all  express  excitement  (Kilham 
1972),  the  degree  depending  on  whether  they  are  loud  or  soft,  or  given  singly  or  in  a rapid 
series.  As  with  tchup,  the  excitement  may  be  from  any  cause. 


Fig.  2.  Aggressive  threat  display. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


273 


Fig.  3.  About-to-attack  display. 

Grr,  grr-n.  Various  notes  take  on  a harsh  quality  with  an  rr  sound  when  a nuthatch  is 
aroused  by  a rival  or  predator  in  or  out  of  the  nesting  season.  A nuthatch  may  start  giving 
them  without  apparent  cause.  However,  White-breasted  Nuthatches  can  be  aroused  by  a 
distant  rival  that  is  not  always  perceptible  to  a human  observer. 

Brr-a.  I have  heard  this  note  given  by  nuthatches  coming  to  a feeding  tray,  apparently  as 
a warning  (territorial)  to  Black-capped  Chickadees  (Parus  atricapillus)  or  conspecifics. 

Medley-of-notes-in-conflict.  Mixtures  of  the  vocalizations  of  3 or  4 nuthatches  in  conflict 
may  become  staccato  or  even  musical,  and  include  a qua  or  quavering  qua-rr  heard  only 
then. 

Agonistic  song. — This  is  a rapid  series  of  hn-hn  notes  generally  confined  to  the  breeding 
season  (January-June),  regarded  by  Tyler  (1916)  as  the  rarely  heard  second  main  song  of 
this  species.  I heard  this  song  only  in  agonistic  situations.  On  17  January,  a male  made  a 
continuous  series  of  these  notes,  almost  like  a buzzer,  when  close  to  a Barred  Owl.  Another 
male  gave  these  notes  several  times  when  I was  6 m from  a nest,  as  if  he  regarded  me  as 
an  intruder. 

The  agonistic  song  appears  at  times  to  be  a combination  of  agonistic  and  courtship  be- 
havior, corresponding  to  a similar  song  of  the  Red-breasted  Nuthatch  (S.  canadensis)  (Kilham 
1973).  In  S.  carolinensis,  this  song  usually  requires  a setting  similar  to  that  in  which  courtship 
song  occurs  (i.e.,  usually  early  in  the  day  with  the  female  resting  close  by),  plus  the  presence 
of  a rival  at  a distance. 

Territory  and  territorial  encounters. — White-breasted  Nuthatches,  even  deep  in  their  own 
territory,  appear  aware  of  neighboring  pairs.  At  06:00  on  10  April,  the  members  of  a pair 
were  exchanging  low  hit -tucks  when  the  male  switched  to  hn-hn  notes.  Both  sexes  then 
made  loud  kun  and  ka-uns  as  they  flew  to  the  nearest  border.  I heard  a second  pair  there, 
but  the  2 pairs  quickly  separated.  Other  encounters,  arranged  below  by  order  of  severity, 
were  of  greater  duration. 

Grade  I.  Two  severe  encounters  were  in  March.  At  06:10  on  31  March,  the  members  of 
2 pairs  were  all  in  aggressive  threat  displays  in  a hornbeam  {Carpinus).  The  2 males  flew  at 
each  other,  fluttering  beak  to  beak  in  midair. 

Grade  II.  At  09:00  on  7 December,  a different  pair  of  nuthatches  each  moved  2-3  m above 
the  gound  in  saplings  on  either  side  of  a dirt  road.  All  4 birds  gave  aggressive  threat  displays 
and  uttered  a mixture  of  notes,  including  buzzer-like  grrs.  Each  male  remained  on  his  side, 
only  the  females  crossed  over.  Whenever  a female  returned  to  her  mate  both  birds  started 
displacement  pecking.  The  conflict,  occuring  before  the  onset  of  active  courtship,  primarily 
involved  the  females. 


274 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  /Vo.  2,  June  1981 


Relation  of  territorial  conflicts  to  pair  bond. — In  some  species  (e.g.,  Hairy  Woodpecker 
[Picoides  villosus])  (Kilham,  Wilson  BuU.  81:169-183,  1969),  the  pitch  of  emotion  aroused 
against  a rival  can  be  diverted  to  courtship  during  lulls  in  territorial  conflict.  This  antecedent 
situation  was  seldom  evident  for  S.  carolinenesis.  On  a number  of  occasions  there  was  an 
increase  in  intimate,  antiphonal  notes  between  members  of  pairs  following  conflicts.  I also 
once  saw  courtship  feeding  after  a conflict.  Generally,  however,  there  were  no  indications 
that  agonistic  behavior  stimulated  courtship.  Severe  conflict  appeared  to  have  the  opposite 
effect.  In  some  instances,  a male  attacked  his  mate  during  lulls  in  fighting  with  a rival, 
behavior  also  noted  by  Lohrl  (1959)  in  S.  europaea.  This  would  seem  to  be  a case  of  redirected 
attack  in  the  sense  used  by  Bastock  et  al.  (1953). 

Size  and  nature  of  territories. — By  following  limits  of  wanderings  as  well  as  noting  location 
of  conflicts,  I estimated  that  1 pair  of  nuthatches  had  a territory  of  15  ha,  approximating  that 
given  by  Brackbill  (1969)  for  a banded  pair  in  Maryland. 

Effects  of  a feeding  station. — A feeder  with  suet  and  sunflower  seeds  in  the  territory  of  1 
pair  in  the  winters  of  1968  and  1969  attracted  a second  pair  whose  territorial  boundary  was 
only  12  m away.  Efforts  of  the  second  pair  to  enter  the  territory  of  the  first  pair  led  to  daily 
conflicts.  The  second  pair  usually  gave  threat  displays  when  they  were  by  their  territorial 
border  and  trying  to  reach  the  feeder.  The  male  not  only  drove  away  the  intruders,  but  often 
his  own  mate  as  well.  The  feeder  disrupted  the  daily  movements  of  the  chickadees  and 
nuthatches  to  such  an  extent  that  studies  of  natural  behavior  became  impossible.  Interest- 
ingly, Bock  (Ecolog>'  50:903-905,  1969),  in  discussing  White-breasted  as  weU  as  Pygmy  (S. 
pygrnaea)  nuthatches,  stated  that:  “The  artifact  of  having  an  abundant  food  source  of  precise 
and  predictable  localities  caused  a breakdown  in  flock  organization  and  a rapid  sort  of 
‘competitive  exclusion’  at  the  feeders.”  Present  studies  were  made  in  woods  away  from 
feeders. 

Reactions  to  predators. — The  most  intense  reaction  witnessed  was  at  16:00  on  17  January, 
when  a male  nuthatch  stayed  within  5-7  m of  a Barred  Owl,  alternating  bouts  of  displacement 
pecking  with  rapid  hn-hns.  The  nuthatch’s  tail  was  raised  slightly  and  he  oecasionaUy  flicked 
his  wings.  A pair  of  Hairy  Woodpeckers,  present  part  of  the  time,  also  engaged  in  displaee- 
ment  pecking.  Perhaps  the  intensity  of  the  nuthateh’s  reaction  may  have  been  due  to  the 
lateness  of  the  afternoon  and  the  proximity  of  the  owl  to  the  nuthatch’s  (and  the  wood- 
pecker’s) roosting  plaee. 

A male  nuthatch  travelling  with  chickadees  and  a Brown  Creeper  {Certhia  familiaris)  on 
2 March  encountered  a Barred  Owl  dozing  in  the  open.  The  male  uttered  kun  and  harsh 
kaan  notes,  hut  did  not  come  close.  After  1-2  min  the  flock  departed.  Possibly  sleepy  owls 
at  mid-day  evoke  different  reactions  than  alert  ones  at  dusk. 

Acknowledgments. — I thank  my  wife,  Jane  Kilham,  for  drawing  the  illustrations. — LAW- 
RENCE Kilham,  Dept.  Microbiology,  Dartmouth  Medical  School,  Hanover,  New  Hampshire 
03755.  Accepted  20  Mar.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2).  1981,  pp.  274-275 


Evasive  behavior  of  American  Cools  to  kleptoparasitisin  by  waterfowl. — On  17 

.\pril  1976,  at  Dewey’s  Pasture  ildlife  Management  Area  in  northwestern  Iowa,  I saw 
.\merican  Wigeons  (.4na5  americana)  and  GadwaUs  (.4nas  strepera)  kleptoparasitizing  Amer- 
ican Coots  (Fulica  americana).  One  or  2 wigeon  or  GadwaUs,  but  not  both  species  at  once, 
closely  attended  and  followed  a coot.  .\t  times  all  the  coots  present  (15-25)  were  attended 
by  kleptoparasites.  Both  duck  species  dabbled  at  vegetation  brought  to  the  surface  by  coots 


GENERAL  NOTES 


275 


and  stole  vegetation  directly  from  the  bills  of  the  coots.  This  note  discusses  the  evasive 
behavior  exhibited  by  coots  when  being  kleptoparasitized. 

Food  piracy  by  Gadwalls  has  not  been  previously  noted.  Wigeon  have  been  reported  steal- 
ing food  from  coots  (Munro,  Can.  J.  Res.  27:289-307,  1949;  Hellyer,  Pac.  Search  11:26-27, 
1977;  Knapton  and  Knudsen,  Can.  Field-Nat.  92:403-404,  1978),  but  aggressive  or  evasive 
behavior  by  coots  when  being  kleptoparasitized  has  not  been  reported. 

The  coots  I watched  (at  distances  between  35-90  m with  a spotting  scope)  were  not 
aggressive  .toward  their  kleptoparasites,  but  some  of  them  did  make  evasive  maneuvers. 
When  followed  by  wigeon  or  Gadwalls,  coots  dived,  brought  plant  material  to  the  surface, 
dropped  it,  swam  several  meters  away  and  quickly  dived  again.  By  dropping  the  vegetation 
from  the  first  dive,  coots  seemingly  gained  time  to  dive  again  and  feed  unmolested.  The 
wigeon  or  Gadwalls  fed  on  the  plants  from  the  first  dive  until  it  was  consumed  or  sank  and 
then  pursued  the  coot  again.  If  sufficient  food  for  the  waterfowl  was  brought  to  the  surface 
and  they  were  distracted  by  it,  the  coot  successfully  evaded  them.  When  a coot  did  evade 
its  kleptoparasites,  the  waterfowl  sought  out  another  coot  host.  During  the  2 h that  I watched 
the  interactions,  food-dropping  was  rarely  successful  in  allowing  coots  to  completely  evade 
the  kleptoparasites.  Coots  that  did  not  drop  food  for  the  waterfowl  and  attempted  to  evade 
them  solely  by  swimming  away  were  not  successful. 

This  evasive  behavior  is  costly  in  time  and  energy’.  Coots  attended  by  kleptoparasites 
dived  more  often  (median  = 4/min,  N = 19,  range  1-6)  than  did  coots  foraging  alone  (me- 
dian = 3/min,  N = 19,  range  1^,  P < 0.002,  Mann-Whitney  L-test)  on  the  same  wetland. 
If,  by  leaving  the  food  from  the  first  dive,  coots  were  successful  in  evading  further  klepto- 
parasitism,  the  strategy’  is  advantageous.  Even  if  the  waterfowl  were  not  distracted,  if  more 
than  half  of  the  food  collected  during  each  dive  was  stolen,  it  would  be  advantageous  for  the 
coots  to  leave  the  food  from  1 dive  for  their  kleptoparasites  and  to  feed  unmolested  on  the 
next  dive. 

Because  many  studies  have  noted  the  high  intensity  of  interspecific  aggressiveness  in  coots 
(e.g.,  GuUion,  Condor  55:169-186,  1953;  Ryder,  Auk  76:424-438,  1959;  but  see  Ryan  and 
Dinsmore,  Auk  96:704-713,  1979),  the  lack  of  aggression  toward  kleptoparasites  is  surprising. 
Several  factors  suggest  that  outright  aggression  may  be  less  effective  than  the  food-dropping 
strategy.  Aggression  may  be  ineffective  because,  even  if  driven  off  before  a foraging  dive, 
nothing  prevents  the  kleptoparasite  from  returning  and  stealing  food  when  the  coot  resur- 
faces. Knapton  and  Knudsen  (1978)  noted  the  importance  of  food  piracy  to  wigeon  when  the 
only  vegetation  available  was  in  deep  water,  as  was  true  in  the  spring  when  I made  my 
observations.  Opportunistic  kleptoparasitism  may  make  the  waterfowl  tenacious  in  their 
piracy  attempts  and  might  result  in  the  coot  food  resources  being  economically  non-defend- 
able.  It  may  also  be  energetically  too  costly  for  coots  to  defend  their  food  from  several 
attending  pirates,  the  kleptoparasites  effectively  swamping  the  aggression. 

This  is  Journal  Paper  J-9495  of  the  Iowa  Agriculture  and  Home  Economics  Experiment 
Station,  Ames,  Iowa,  Project  No.  2170.  Observations  were  made  while  I was  supported  by 
the  Iowa  Agriculture  and  Home  Economics  Experiment  Station  and  grants  from  the  Frank 
M.  Chapman  Memorial  Fund,  American  Museum  of  Natural  History  and  Sigma  Xi.  I thank 
J.  J.  Dinsmore,  M.  G.  Henry,  P.  A.  Heagy , L.  H.  Fredrickson,  C.  Braun  and  J.  C.  Barlow 
for  their  criticisms  of  an  earlier  draft  of  this  note.  This  is  contribution  No.  4 of  the  Avian 
Research  Laboratory,  Department  of  Animal  Ecology  . — M.\RK  R.  Ryan,  Dept.  Animal  Ecol- 
ogy, Iowa  State  Univ.,  Ames,  Iowa  50011.  Accepted  29  Jan.  1980. 


276 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  276-277 


Additional  evidence  of  egg-moving  behavior  by  female  Gadwalls.  — Johnson  and 
Kirsch  (Wilson  Bull.  89:331-332,  1977)  noted  moving  of  eggs  between  nest  bowls  by  a female 
Gadwall  (Anas  strepera)  in  North  Dakota.  This  note  documents  additional  evidence  of  such 
behavior  and  suggests  that  egg  moving  may  be  more  frequent  in  this  species  than  previously 
indicated. 

During  a study  of  the  Gadwall  in  southern  Manitoba,  188  females  were  trapped  on  the 
nest,  using  a modification  of  the  automatic  nest  trap  originally  designed  by  Weller  (J.  Wildl. 
Manage.  21:456^58,  1957).  From  1973  through  1975,  12  instances  of  egg  moving  were  re- 
corded in  which  the  female  moved  aU  or  portions  of  the  original  clutch  from  within  the  trap 
to  another  nest  bowl  outside  (Table  1).  Two  instances  involved  moving  of  eggs  in  2 successive 
trapping  attempts. 

In  this  study,  moving  of  eggs  appeared  to  be  the  result  of  an  improper  trapping  technique 
at  the  nest-site.  The  capture  method  relied  on  the  correct  orientation  of  the  trap  entrance 
to  the  most  frequently  used  approach  path  of  the  hen  to  the  nest  bowl.  Once  inside,  the 
female  tripped  the  door  release  and  was  captured.  However,  the  placement  of  the  closed 
portion  of  the  trap  over  the  path  probably  resulted  in  the  accidental  release  of  the  door  while 
the  female  was  still  outside  the  trap.  I suspect  that  the  female  then  moved  the  eggs  by 
pulling  them  through  the  5.1  X 5.1  cm  (2x2  in)  mesh  of  the  trap  with  the  underside  of  its 
bill.  Most  of  the  eggs  were  removed  through  the  mesh  of  the  trap  cylinder.  However,  2 cases 
were  recorded  in  which  the  female  appeared  to  roll  the  eggs  out  of  the  open  door.  In  both 
instances,  the  female  was  captured  after  removing  part  of  the  clutch. 

Eight  Gadwall  hens  were  captured  from  these  nests  and  aged  as  yearlings  (1  year  old)  or 


Table  1 

Summary  of  Egg  Moving  and  Other  Nest  Data  eor  Female  Gadwalls  in  Southern 

Manitoba,  1973-1975 

't  ear 

Age  of  female 

Clutch-size 

Stage  of 
incubation 

No.  of 
eggs  moved 

1973 

Yearling^ 

8 

19 

8 

Yearling^* 

8 

20 

8 

Adult 

7 

18 

7 

Yearling 

10 

20 

2 

Unknown 

8 

4 

2 

.\dult 

10 

14 

10 

1974 

Adult 

7 

25" 

1 

Adult 

12 

8 

6 

Unknown'’ 

9 

21 

9 

Unknown'’ 

9 

22 

9 

Adult 

8 

24’' 

8 

1975 

Yearling 

9 

22 

9 

“ Same  female. 

Same  female. 

One  egg  in  the  original  nest  bowl  was  partially  hatched. 
.\11  eggs  were  pipped. 


k 


GENERAL  NOTES 


277 


adults  (2  years  old  or  older)  (Blohm,  M.S.  thesis,  Univ.  Wisconsin,  Madison,  Wisconsin, 
1977).  Eggs  were  aged  to  the  nearest  day  to  determine  the  stage  of  incubation  when  moved. 
No  relationship  appeared  to  exist  between  clutch-size,  stage  of  incubation,  or  age  of  the 
female  and  the  occurrence  of  egg  moving  in  this  study.  Lorenz  and  Tinbergen  (Z.  Tierpsychol. 
2:1-29,  1938),  Sowls  (Prairie  Ducks,  Stackpole  Co.,  Harrisburg,  Pennsylvania  and  Wildl. 
Manage.  Inst.,  Washington,  D.C.,  1955:101),  Oring  (Auk  81:88-89,  1964)  and  Prevett  and 
Prevett  (Auk  90:202-204,  1973)  have  observed  other  species  of  waterfowl  retrieving  displaced 
eggs  with  the  ventral  portion  of  the  bill.  I suspect  that  this  behavior  is  not  uncommon  in  the 
GadwaU,  especially  in  situations  in  which  it  is  necessary  to  move  all  or  portions  of  the  clutch 
short  distances  because  of  natural  or  man-made  disturbances. 

Fieldwork  was  supported  by  the  Delta  W aterfowl  Research  Station  and  the  University  of 
Wisconsin.  I thank  R.  A.  McCabe  for  his  comments  on  this  note.  I am  indebted  to  the 
owmers  of  East  Meadows  Ranch,  the  Peter  Curry  and  Arthur  Vincent  families  and  to  Law- 
rence King,  manager,  for  their  generosity  during  my  stay  at  Marshy  Point,  Manitoba.  I owe 
special  thanks  to  all  those  who  assisted  me  in  the  field  during  this  study. — Robert  J.  Blohm, 
Dept.  Wildlife  Ecology,  Univ.  Wisconsin,  Madison,  Wisconsin  53706.  (Present  address:  Of- 
fice of  Migratory  Bird  Management,  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife  Service,  Laurel,  Maryland  20811.) 
Accepted  27  May  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  277-278 


Mallard  using  moving  vehicles  for  predator  avoidance. — Distraction  displays  are 
often-cited  adaptations  for  predator  avoidance  in  a variety  of  vertebrate  organisms  (see  Eibl- 
Eibesfeldt,  Ethology — the  Biology  of  Behavior,  Holt,  Rinehart  and  W inston,  Inc.,  New  York, 
New  York,  1975).  Examples  of  such  behavior  commonly  relate  to  use  of  body  appurtenances 
(i.e.,  feather-ruffling,  break-away  tail)  and  less  often  to  use  of  extrinsic  environmental  fea- 
tures. W e report  here  an  apparent  attempt  by  a duck  to  use  moving  vehicles  as  a distraction 
during  predator  avoidance. 

In  mid-afternoon  on  21  January  1977,  we  w^ere  driving  south-west  on  Interstate  Highway 
90  about  12  km  NE  of  Vantage,  Grant  Co.,  Washington,  when  a female  Mallard  {Anas 
platyrhynchos)  appeared  suddenly  over  the  left  front  of  the  car,  flying  about  2 m above  the 
roadw^ay.  We  were  traveling  80-85  km/h  when  the  duck  passed  us  rocking  slightly  from  side- 
to-side  as  if  preparing  to  land  on  the  roadway.  W ithin  2 or  3 sec  a Prairie  Falcon  (Falco 
mexicanus,  age  and  sex  unknown)  stooped  at  the  duck  from  a position  above  and  to  the  left 
rear.  This  stoop  was  aborted  and  the  Mallard  continued  flying  along  the  highway  1^  m 
above  the  surface,  weaving  left  to  right  between  several  cars,  very  close  to  the  vehicles.  The 
falcon  appeared  to  hit  the  duck  during  the  second  stoop  because  the  duck  tumbled  to  the 
ground  on  the  right  side  of  the  highway.  This  attack  occurred  about  2 km  from  the  point 
where  we  initially  saw  the  duck. 

The  downed  duck  moved  to  unmowed  roadside  vegetation  dominated  by  big  sagebrush 
{.Artemisia  tridentata),  Russian  thistle  {Salsola  kali)  and  cheatgrass  {Bromus  tectorum)  dm'mg 
which  time  the  falcon  made  several  more  stoops  without  contacting  the  duck.  As  we  ap- 
proached and  stopped  near  where  the  duck  had  gone  down,  the  falcon  flew  across  the 
highway  from  the  downed  duck  and  perched  on  a high  voltage  pole  about  80  m away.  We 
left  the  car  and  walked  perhaps  15  or  20  m when  the  MaUard  flushed  from  under  a big 
sagebrush  and  flew  NE  with  no  visible  injuries  or  flight  impairment.  While  we  looked  for  the 
duck,  the  falcon  left  its  perch  in  an  unknown  direction  and  did  not  initiate  another  attack 
before  the  duck  flew  out  of  our  sight. 


278 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


It  is  possible  that  the  Mallard  was  never  hit  by  the  falcon  as  Dekker  (Can.  Field-Nat. 
94:371-382,  1980)  has  suggested  that  erratic  plunging  flight  routinely  exhibited  by  waterfowl 
when  pursued  by  falcons  may  appear  like  a “hit”  when  in  fact  no  contact  is  made.  The  lack 
of  apparent  injury  to  the  duck  seemingly  supports  Dekker’s  suggestion.  However,  the  Mallard 
was  definitely  harassed  during  this  episode  as  evidenced  by  its  speed  which  approached  the 
maxima  of  80-96  km/h  previously  reported  by  Cottam  (Wilson  BuU.  54:121-131,  1942)  and 
Cooke  (U.S.  Dept.  Agric.  Circ.  428,  1937).  The  weaving  among  cars  likely  reduced  the 
opportunities  for  stoops  by  the  falcon  for  an  extended  distance  along  the  roadway.  Thus,  the 
combination  of  rapid  flight  and  maneuvering  among  cars  at  least  prolonged  the  predator 
avoidance  for  this  duck  and  aided  its  survival  (albeit  we  were  the  final  distracting  factor). 
Whether  the  cars  were  used  by  the  duck  as  a surrogate  “flock”  is  a matter  for  speculation. 

U.S.  Army  Corps  of  Engineers  Contract  No.  DACW68-76-C-0184  supported  travel  during 
which  this  observation  was  made.  R.  K.  Stocker  and  C.  Taylor  also  witnessed  this  event.  A. 
J.  Erskine  and  G.  Barber  provided  helpful  comments  on  earlier  drafts. — Bruce  C.  THOMP- 
SON AND  James  E.  Tabor,  Washington  Dept.  Game,  Olympia,  Washington  98504.  (Present 
address  BCT:  Dept.  Wildlife  and  Fisheries  Sciences,  Texas  A&M  Univ.,  College  Station, 
Texas  77843.)  Accepted  24  Apr.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  278-279 

Oehraeeous  Wren  fails  to  respond  to  mobbing  calls  in  an  heterospecific  flock. — 

On  6 October,  1970,  I was  following  a mixed-species  foraging-flock  through  a tract  of  Lower 
Montane  Wet  Forest  at  Monteverde,  Puntarenas  Province,  Costa  Rica  (see  Buskirk  and 
Buskirk,  Am.  Midi.  Nat.  95:288-298,  1976;  Powell,  Auk  96:375-390,  1979  for  descriptions 
of  this  location).  The  flock  had  just  passed  me  when  2 Common  Bush-Tanagers  [Chloro- 
pingus  ophthalmicus),  trailing  behind  the  flock,  discovered  a tree  viper  (Bothrops  lateralis) 
and  began  giving  high-intensity,  rapid  twitters.  Within  30  sec  2 Golden-crowned  Warblers 
(Basileuterus  culicivorus)  and  2 Slate-throated  Redstarts  [Myioborus  miniatus),  all  of  which 
had  recently  passed  the  snake,  returned  and  joined  the  mobbing  bush-tanagers  0. 5-1.0  m 
from  the  snake.  Within  another  30  sec  a Black-and-White  Warbler  (Mniotilta  varia),  a 
Wilson’s  Warbler  (Wilsonia  pusilla)  and  2 Oehraeeous  Wrens  {Troglodytes  ochraceus)  ar- 
rived. The  warblers  actively  joined  the  mob.  But  the  wrens  remained  a few  meters  away  and 
foraged  normally,  searching  the  surfaces  of  major  branches.  The  behavior  of  a wren  in  my 
line  of  vision  gave  no  indication  that  it  recognized  the  presence  of  the  snake  or  the  meaning 
of  the  mobbing  activity.  When  the  wren  approached  within  1.5  m of  the  snake,  the  wren 
looked  up  from  its  foraging  and  at  the  snake.  The  wren  froze  for  an  instant  and  then  began 
uttering  high-intensity  calls  and  joined  the  mobbing.  Immediately  the  second  wren  joined 
the  group.  The  wrens  had  returned  with  the  flock  but  had  not  reacted  to  the  predator  until 
one  of  them  saw  it.  In  all,  the  mobbing  lasted  only  about  3 min  before  the  flock  moved  away 
from  the  snake. 

This  incident  demonstrates  different  responses  among  species  to  the  mobbing  calls  of 
other  species  with  which  they  flock.  The  wren  had  not  shown  unusual  excitation  or  orientation 
toward  the  viper  prior  to  its  own  discovery  of  the  snake.  The  immediate  response  of  the 
second  wren  once  the  first  gave  mobbing  calls  demonstrates  intraspecific  recognition  of  such 
a signal.  That  wrens  returned  with  the  flock  suggest  they  do  respond  positively  to  visual  and/ 
or  auditory  cues  of  the  other  species.  However,  the  behavior  of  their  associates  elicited 
gregariousness,  not  alarm. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


279 


Several  investigators  of  heterospecific  flocks  have  cautioned  that  the  potential  advantages 
and  disadvantages  derived  from  flocking  may  differ  among  participants  (e.g.,  Moynihan, 
Smithson.  Misc.  Coll.  134:1-140,  1962;  Morse,  Ecol.  Monogr.  40:119-168,  1970).  The  dif- 
ferent behaviors  of  the  wrens  and  their  associates  substantiate  this  caution. 

The  potential  advantages  of  flocking  include  the  greater  surveillance  capability  of  the 
group  and  the  corresponding  benefits  of  early  warning  and  foraging  efficiency  (Powell,  Anim. 
Behav.  22:501-505,  1974).  The  advantage  of  early  warning  depends  on  appropriate  response 
to  predator-alert  signals. 

If,  however,  “alarm”  and/or  mobbing  caUs  are  prey-to-predator  communication  of  recog- 
nition/alertness and  thereby  identify  reduced  vulnerability  (Buskirk,  unpubl.),  then  predators 
may  avoid  hunting  in  areas  where  an  alarm  or  mobbing  has  been  given  (e.g.,  Trivers,  Q. 
Rev.  Biol.  46:35-57,  1971).  If  so,  some  advantage  to  flock  participation  may  exist  for  gre- 
garious species  not  cued  to  the  fuU  information  content  of  these  signals.  Essentially,  a 
protective  “halo”  would  exist  around  an  alerted  flock.  A large  proportion  of  flock  attendants 
cannot  be  of  this  type  or  predators  will  find  successful  hunting  in  the  vicinity  of  grouped 
calls.  Eliciting  mobbing  to  attract  these  unaware  but  gregarious  species  or  individuals  may 
be  a successful  hunting  ploy  for  some  predators.  Smith  (Ibis  111:241-243,  1969)  found  forest 
falcons  {Micrastur)  provoking  mobbing  as  a hunting  technique.  Flock  attendants,  like  the 
wrens,  that  are  unresponsive  to  the  “predator-present”  context  of  these  calls  should  be  more 
vulnerable  than  the  others.  Their  frequency  of  attendance  in  flocks  should  be  optimized  at 
relatively  low  levels  if  anti-predation  advantages  are  a predominant  selective  force  for  het- 
erospecific gregariousness. 

J.  Iverson,  L.  Baptista  and  E.  Tramer  provided  helpful  comments  on  early  drafts.  This 
observation  was  made  during  studies  supported  by  a pilot  study  grant  (No.  70-17)  from  the 
Organization  for  Tropical  Studies,  a Frank  M.  Chapman  Grant  from  the  American  Museum 
of  Natural  History  and  a NSF  grant  (GB-17180)  to  the  University  of  California,  Davis. — 
William  H.  Buskirk,  Biology  Dept,  and  Joseph  Moore  Museum,  Earlham  College,  Rich- 
mond, Indiana  47374.  Accepted  13  Apr.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  279-280 


Fish  attack  on  Black  Guillemot  and  Common  Eider  in  Maine. — Data  on  bird  mor- 
tality at  sea  are  scarce  and,  although  predation  and  scavenging  by  marine  organisms  are 
assumed,  few  cases  have  been  documented.  The  subject  was  generally  reviewed  by  Glegg 
(Ibis  87:422-433,  1945;  Ibis  89:433^35,  1947).  Additional  reports  include  predation  or  scav- 
enging by  grey  seals  [Halichoerus  grypus)  (Grant  and  Bourne,  Seabird  Rep.  52-53,  1971; 
Kinnear,  Scot.  Birds  9:342,  347,  1977),  octopuses  {Octopus  sp.)  (Hindwood,  Emu  64:69-70, 
1964),  sharks  (Galeocerdo  cuvieri,  Carcharodon  carcharias,  Carcharinus  leucas,  C.  longi- 
manus)  (Brooke  and  Wallett,  Ostrich  47:126,  1976;  Dodrill  and  Gilmore,  Auk  95:585-586, 
1978;  Harrison,  Oceans  5:25-26,  1979),  monkfish  {Squatina  squatina)  (Davenport,  Br.  Birds 
72:77-78,  1979)  and  cod  (Gadus  macrocephalus)  (Scheffer,  Murrelet  23:17,  1942).  Foot  and 
leg  damage  is  fairly  common  in  some  seabirds  and  has  been  assumed  to  represent  attempted 
predation,  probably  by  fish.  The  foUowing  account  documents  1 source  of  foot  and  leg  damage 
on  the  coast  of  Maine. 

On  11  August  1975,  a newly  fledged  Black  Guillemot  {Cepphus  gryile)  and  2 eclipse- 
plumaged  Common  Eider  drakes  {Somateria  mollissima)  were  observed  being  attacked  by 
several  fish  off  Eastern  Egg  Rock,  Muscongus  Bay,  Maine.  The  sea  was  extremely  calm. 


280 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


making  disturbances  at  the  surface  very  conspicuous.  The  guillemot  was  first  observed  being 
tugged  repeatedly  under  water  and  immediately  bobbing  back  to  the  surface.  After  each  tug 
the  guillemot  flapped  its  wings  against  the  surface  of  the  water,  but  seemed  unable  to  take 
flight.  The  flapping  propelled  the  guillemot  forward  only  4—6  m at  a time.  The  surface  was 
continually  disturbed  by  what  appeared  to  be  about  3 fish  approximately  0.75  m long.  The 
appearance  of  the  dorsal  and  caudal  fins,  overall  size,  manner  of  surface-feeding,  locality 
and  season  suggested  bluefish  {Pornatornus  saltatrix),  but  this  could  not  be  verified.  The 
attack  was  occasionally  discontinued  for  periods  of  several  minutes,  during  some  of  which 
fish  attacked  the  2 eclipse-plumaged  Common  Eider  drakes  nearby.  The  eiders  flapped  their 
wings,  ran  across  the  water  and  eluded  the  fish  for  short  distances  only  to  be  attacked  again 
as  soon  as  they  settled  on  the  water.  The  eiders  were  eventually  able  to  escape  harassment, 
but  the  guillemot  appeared  disabled. 

The  attack  on  the  guillemot  was  observed  for  1 h.  When  last  seen  the  guillemot  was 
drifting  toward  Eastern  Egg  Rock,  where  presumably  the  same  bird  was  found  beached  the 
next  morning.  Both  legs  had  numerous  lacerations  and  the  webbing  was  pierced  in  several 
places.  These  injuries  had  severed  the  main  tendons  on  both  legs,  leaving  them  completely 
paralyzed,  but  the  bird  was  otherwise  unharmed  and  apparently  healthy. 

Eastern  Egg  Rock  has  been  occupied  by  seabird  researchers  each  summer  from  1974 
through  1979.  On  2 August  1974,  another  immature  Black  Guillemot  was  found  with  similar 
leg  injuries  and  on  20  August  1974  an  adult  drake  Common  Eider  in  eclipse  plumage  was 
found  similarly  disabled.  These  2 birds  also  appeared  healthy  but  their  legs  were  paralyzed 
due  to  severed  tendons. 

I gratefully  acknowledge  S.  W.  Kress  for  reviewing  the  manuscript  and  the  Fratercula 
Fund  of  the  National  Audubon  Society  for  providing  funding  and  logistical  support  that  made 
the  field  work  possible. — Thomas  W.  Fre.NCH,  Atlantic  Center  for  the  Environment,  951 
Highland  Street,  Ipswich,  Massachusetts  01938.  Accepted  25  Apr.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  p.  280 


Crows  steal  golf  balls  in  Banglatlesh. — The  omnivorous  Large-billed  Crow  {Corvus 
macrorhynchos)  is  widespread  in  Bangladesh  and  occurs  commonly  in  towns  and  villages.  In 
Dacca,  this  crow  and  the  House  Crow  (C.  splendens)  serve  as  important  means  of  helping 
keep  the  city  sanitary  by  scavenging  on  animal  and  vegetable  materials.  It  is  commonplace 
to  observe  a mixed  flock  of  25-100  crows  scrambling  through  a fresh  pile  of  trash  on  the 
street. 

On  2 December  1978,  while  at  the  Dacca  Golf  Course,  I struck  a golf  ball  about  50  m from 
the  green.  .\s  the  ball  descended  to  an  altitude  of  about  30  m,  a Large-billed  Crow  flew  from 
nearby,  seized  the  ball  in  mid-air  and  fled.  On  the  same  green  I chipped  a ball  to  within  1 
m of  the  hole,  only  to  have  a second  large-bill  flee  with  the  ball. 

Sucb  occurrences  are  common  in  Dacca  and  golf  enthusiasts  must  either  give  up  the  sport 
or  tolerate  crows.  Young  boys  are  hired  and  stationed  along  fairways  to  frighten  crows  during 
golf  matches. 

1 was  unable  to  determine  if  the  observed  crow  behavior  occurred  because  the  balls  were 
mistaken  for  food  or  if  the  behavior  was  a manifestation  of  the  tendency  of  this  species,  in 
the  words  of  Ali  and  Ripley  (Handbook  of  the  Birds  of  India  and  Pakistan,  Vol.  5,  Oxford 
Univ.  Press,  Bombay,  India,  1972:257),  to  indulge  in  “puckish  pranks,  apparently  with  no 
object  other  than  fun,  such  as  surreptitiously  tweaking  its  fellows'  wing-tips  or  toes,  or  a 
sleeping  dog's  tail  . . . ." — Richar  ■ M.  POCH^;,  Route  2,  Box  164,  St.  Martinville,  Louisiana 
70582.  Accepted  10  Feb.  1980. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


281 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  281-282 

Notes  on  the  status  of  the  Common  African  Waxbill  in  Amazonia. — The  Common 
African  Waxbill  {Estrilda  astrild),  a species  widespread  south  of  the  Sahara  in  its  native 
Africa  (Hall  and  Moreau,  An  Atlas  of  Speciation  in  African  Passerine  Birds,  Br.  Mus.  Nat. 
Hist.  Publ.  No.  780,  1970),  is  the  only  exotic  bird  other  than  the  Rock  Dove  (Colurnba  livia) 
and  the  House  Sparrow  (Passer  domesticus)  (Smith,  Condor  75:242-243,  1973;  Condor 
82:109-110,  1980),  to  have  adapted  successfully  to  Amazonia.  E.  astrild  has  also  been  in- 
troduced in  the  Cape  Verde  Islands,  Sao  Tome,  Principe,  St.  Helena,  Mauritius,  Reunion, 
Rodriguez,  the  Seychelles,  Amirantes,  New  Caledonia  and  Tahiti.  Attempted  introductions 
on  Madagascar  and  the  Comoros  failed  (Peters,  Check-list  of  the  Birds  of  the  World,  Vol. 
14,  Mus.  Comparative  Zoology,  Cambridge,  Massachusetts,  1968). 

The  species  has  been  feral  in  Manaus  at  least  since  1967  (Sick,  Bonn.  Zool.  Beitr.  19:298- 
306,  1968)  and  was  first  sighted  in  Belem  in  December  of  1977  (Novaes,  pers.  comm.).  The 
Manaus  population  comprises  between  500  and  1000  birds  (Oren  and  Smith,  Acta  Amazonica 
8:699-701,  1978),  whereas  the  Belem  population  probably  numbers  no  more  than  a few  dozen 
individuals.  This  waxbill  has  occurred  in  Rio  de  Janeiro  perhaps  since  the  late  eighteenth 
century  (Santos,  Passaros  do  Brasil,  2nd  ed.,  R.  Briguiet,  Rio  de  Janeiro,  Brazil,  1948).  From 
there  it  appeared  in  Sao  Paulo  (1930)  and  subsequently  in  Vitoria  (1940),  Salvador  (1953), 
Brasilia  (1964),  Maceio  (1967),  Curitiba  (date  unknown)  and  Porto  Alegre  (date  unknown) 
(Pinto,  Rev.  Mus.  Paulista  22:362,  1944;  Sick  1968).  The  species  is  known  to  readily  escape 
from  standard-sized  bird  cages,  and  most  subsequent  populations  probably  started  from 
escaped  cage  birds,  although  the  population  in  Brasilia  was  established  intentionally  when 
over  100  individuals  were  released  there  (Sick  1968). 

We  have  observed  the  waxbill  in  Manaus  for  more  than  a year,  noting  food  plants,  group 
size  and  relations  with  native  species.  This  species  disperses  throughout  the  city  and  im- 
mediate environs  during  the  day.  At  night  birds  concentrate  in  flocks  of  50-200  individuals 
and  roost  in  African  elephant  grass  (Penniseturn  purpureurn)  in  a few  sites  in  the  city.  During 
most  of  the  year  the  waxbills  form  flocks  of  2-20  to  forage,  feeding  mostly  between  06:00 
and  09:00,  and  16:00  and  18:00.  This  is  the  same  pattern  found  by  Skead  (Ostrich,  Suppl. 
11:1-55,  1975)  in  Africa  for  the  closely  related  Black-cheeked  Waxbill  (E.  erythronotos).  At 
the  onset  of  the  rainy  season  in  late  December  and  January,  the  flocks  break  up  and  the 
waxbiUs  travel  singly,  in  pairs,  or  sometimes  in  trios  to  forage.  These  months  apparently 
encompass  the  peak  of  breeding,  and  flocks  with  many  juveniles  reform  by  mid-March. 

The  nest,  constructed  of  panicles  of  Guinea  grass  (Panicum  maximum),  is  in  the  form  of 
a hollow  ball  with  a short  tubular  entrance,  as  is  the  case  in  Africa  (Chapin,  Bull.  Am.  Mus. 
Nat.  Hist.  75b:545-549,  1954).  One  nest  was  in  an  orange  jasmin  tree  (Murraya  paniculata, 
Rutaceae),  and  a second  in  an  oiti  tree  (Licania  tomentosa,  Chrysobalanaceae) — both  be- 
I tween  2.5  and  3.0  m from  the  ground. 

Guinea  grass,  with  seed  heads  available  all  year,  was  by  far  the  most  important  food  for 
! E.  astrild.  Seeds  of  this  grass  are  important  in  the  diet  in  Africa  also  (Skead  1975).  In  fact, 

j all  grasses  eaten  by  this  waxbill  in  Manaus  are  species  commonly  found  in  Africa,  and 

include  the  crabgrass  Digitaria  horizontalis  (cosmopolitan  in  distribution),  Sporobolus  in- 
I dicus  (pantropical),  Echinochloa  sp.  (pantropical)  and  P.  purpureurn  (pantropical).  The  wax- 
bills  also  feed  on  the  amaranth  Amaranthus  spinosus  (pantropical)  and  the  sedge  Cyperus 
surinarnensis  (Neotropical).  Abundant  native  South  American  grasses,  such  as  Paspalum 
repens,  are  ignored  by  the  Common  Waxbill. 

1 The  bird  feeds  on  the  seeds  of  Panicum  maximum  and  Penniseturn  purpureurn  by  perching 
on  the  panicle  and  plucking  seeds,  whereas  D.  horizontalis  panicles  are  jumped  on,  brought 
to  the  ground  and  stepped  on  so  the  waxbill  can  pluck  the  seeds.  C.  surinarnensis,  the  only 


»l 


282 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


food  plant  we  recorded  for  the  waxbiU  not  found  in  Africa,  is  fed  on  in  the  same  fashion  as 
D.  horizontalis. 

Establishment  of  the  Common  Waxbill  in  Amazonia  has  been  aided  by  the  availability  of 
introduced  grasses,  including  Panicum  maximum  and  Pennisetum  purpureurn.  Some  of  these 
introduced  grasses  have  long  been  established  in  South  America  (Parsons,  Tiibinger  geo- 
grafische  Studien  34:141-153,  1970;  J.  Range  Manage.  25:12-17,  1972)  and  are  spreading 
with  deforestation  and  other  human  disturbance.  In  addition,  Panicum  maximum  is  com- 
monly planted  as  a pasture  grass  in  Amazonia.  The  15,000  km  network  of  newly  constructed 
roads  in  Amazonia  could  provide  corridors  along  which  African  grasses  grow  and  the  waxbill 
might  move  to  found  new  colonies.  This,  in  conjunction  with  the  waxbilLs  flocking  behavior, 
could  facilitate  the  further  spread  of  this  species  in  Amazonia. 

Native  Manaus  finches,  such  as  the  Lesser  Seedfinch  {Oryzoborus  angolensis),  the  Blue- 
black  Grassquit  {Volatinia  jacarina),  the  Chestnut-bellied  Seedeater  {Sporophila  castaneiv- 
entris)  and  the  YeUow-browed  Sparrow  {Ammodramus  aurifrons),  frequently  fed  in  close 
proximity  to  waxbills.  These  native  birds  eat  native  grass  seeds  in  addition  to  the  seeds  of 
introduced  plants,  such  that  it  seems  unlikely  that  the  Common  Waxbill  will  displace  any 
native  species. — David  C.  Oren,  Dept.  Biology,  Harvard  Univ.,  Cambridge,  Massachusetts 
02138  and  Institute  Nacional  de  Pesquisas  da  Amazonia,  Caixa  Postal  478,  69.000  Vlanaus, 
Amazonias,  Brazil  AND  NiGEL  J.  H.  Smith,  Institute  Nacional  de  Pesquisas  da  Amazonia, 
Caixa  Postal  478,  69.000  Manaus,  Amazonias,  Brazil.  Accepted  30  May  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  282-284 


Distribution  ami  repro<luctive  success  of  Zone-tailed  Hawks  in  west  Texas. — The 

Zone-tailed  Hawk  {Buteo  albonatus)  occurs  throughout  the  pine-oak  belt  of  Mexico,  including 
Baja  California,  and  throughout  Central  America.  In  South  America  it  also  occurs  widely 
but  locally  from  Peru  to  Trinidad.  In  the  United  States  the  Zone-tailed  Hawk  breeds  only 
locally  in  southern  and  central  Arizona,  southwestern  New  Mexico  and  west  Texas  (Brown 
and  Amadou,  Eagles,  Hawks  and  Falcons  of  the  W orld,  Vol.  2,  McGraw-Hill  Co.,  New  York, 
New  York,  1968). 

There  are  no  historical  data  on  the  status  or  size  of  any  Zone-tailed  Hawk  population.  In 
1976,  Rich  Glinski  (pers.  comm.)  found  25  pairs  of  zone-tails  in  Arizona,  but  made  no  estimate 
of  population  size.  In  Texas,  the  species  has  nested  recently  in  Taylor  and  Comal  counties, 
in  Brewster  County  in  Big  Bend  National  Park  and  in  the  Edwards  Plateau  area  (Oberholser 
and  Kincaid,  The  Bird  Life  of  Texas,  Vol.  1.,  Univ.  Texas,  Austin,  Texas,  1974).  Oberholser 
and  Kincaid  (1974)  suggested  that  Texas  populations  have  declined  due  to  destruction  of 
nesting  habitat. 

From  1 June-28  July  1975  and  from  19  April-15  July  1976,  we  conducted  a behavioral 
studv  and  population  survey  of  the  Zone-tailed  Hawk  in  west  Texas.  We  surveyed  the  Chisos 
and  Glass  mountains  and  the  BoquiUas  and  Mariscal  canyons  of  the  Rio  Grande  River,  aU 
in  Brewster  County,  and  the  Davis  Mountains  in  Jeff  Davis  County.  We  did  not  survey  the 


GENERAL  NOTES 


283 


entire  Trans-Pecos,  but  checked  the  most  likely  breeding  habitat  for  Zone-tailed  Hawks: 
high  mountains  and  lowland  riparian  cliffs.  We  surveyed  230  km'^  of  the  Davis  Mountains, 
195  km^  of  the  Chisos  Mountains,  5 km^  of  the  Glass  Mountains  and  30  km  of  the  Mariscal 
and  Boquillas  canyons. 

The  Davis  Mountains  reach  an  elevation  of  2515  m,  and  zone-tail  pairs  nested  on  steep 
north-facing  slopes  among  open  stands  of  ponderosa  pine  (Pinus  ponderosa).  Nests  were 
found  between  1750  and  1900  m.  The  Chisos  Mountains  reach  elevations  of  2350  m;  1 zone- 
tail  pair  nested  at  2000  m on  a north-facing  slope  among  a dense  canopy  of  Emory  oak 
(Quercus  emoryi),  grey  oak  (Q.  grisea)  and  juniper  (Juniperus  spp.). 

Both  mountain  ranges  receive  abundant  rainfaU  during  July,  August  and  September.  An- 
nual rainfall  is  46  cm  (maximum)  in  the  Davis  Mountains  (Ohlendorf,  Wilson  Bull.  86:357- 
373,  1974)  and  64  cm  (maximum)  in  the  Chisos  Mountains  (Wauer,  Southwestern  Nat.  16:1- 
29,  1971). 

Vegetation  and  weather  conditions  in  Boquillas  and  Mariscal  canyons  contrast  sharply 
with  the  high  montane  habitats.  Desert  shrubs  predominated  where  zone-tail  pairs  nested  in 
Boquillas  Canyon  on  the  upper-third  of  north-facing  cliffs  (averaging  65  m in  height).  Here, 
vegetation  consisted  mainly  of  creosote  bush  (Larrea  tridentata),  honey  mesquite  (Prosopis 
glandulosa),  ocotiUo  (Fouqueria  splendens)  and  lechuguiUa  (Agave  lechuguilla).  Average 
annual  rainfall  in  the  river  canyons  is  ca.  25  cm.  Temperatures  for  5 months  each  year  reach 
or  exceed  mean  daily  maximums  of  38°C  (Ohlendorf  1974). 

All  nesting  territories  used  in  1975  were  reused  in  1976.  We  located  7 breeding  pairs  of 
Zone-tailed  Hawks  in  1975  and  9 in  1976  as  follows:  1975 — Davis  (5)  and  Chisos  (1)  mountains, 
Boquillas  Canyon  (1);  1976 — same  as  1975  except  1 new  nest  each  in  the  Davis  Mountains 
and  Boquillas  Canyon. 

In  1976,  5 Davis  Mountain  nests  were  within  1 m of  the  tops  of  20^0  m ponderosa  pines. 
The  sixth  nest  was  ca.  6 m from  the  top  of  a 40  m ponderosa  pine.  The  Chisos  Mountain 
nest  was  near  the  top  of  a 10  m Emory  oak  at  the  base  of  a 60  m cliff.  All  montane  nests 
occurred  near  igneous  rock  faces  9-90  m high. 

The  Boquillas  Canyon  nests  were  on  south-facing  cliffs.  We  found  1 nest  5 m below  the 
top  of  a 70  m cliff  on  a ledge  about  3 m long  and  1 m wide.  We  found  another  nest  about  15 
m below  the  top  of  a 60  m cliff  in  a hole  in  the  cliff  wall. 

Although  we  never  encountered  Zone-tailed  Hawks  elsewhere,  Deborah  Davis  (pers. 
comm.)  observed  a pair  frequenting  canyon  cliffs  south  of  the  Davis  Mountains  at  ca.  1500 
m.  We  doubt  that  more  than  15  pairs  nested  in  the  Trans-Pecos.  In  Arizona  and  New  Mexico, 
however,  these  hawks  apparently  range  widely  over  the  desert  slopes  and  up  into  the  conif- 
erous zone  (Brown  and  Amadon  1968).  Glinski  (pers.  comm.)  indicates  that  in  Arizona  the 
Zone-tailed  Hawk  breeds  in  a wide  range  of  habitats.  In  New  Mexico,  John  Hubbard  (in 
Porter  and  White,  pp.  39-57  in  Kept.  Proc.  World  Conf.  Birds  of  Prey,  Vienna,  Austria,  R. 
D.  Chancellor,  ed.,  Int.  Council  Bird  Preserv.,  1975)  found  zone-tails  nesting  in  pine  forests, 
pine-oak,  conifer  and  riparian  woodlands. 

In  1976,  4 of  9 nests  fledged  young  compared  to  6 of  7 nests  in  1975  (Table  1).  The  7 
mountain  pairs  raised  an  average  of  1.0  young  in  1976,  compared  to  1.2  for  the  preceding 
season.  Neither  of  the  pairs  at  the  Boquillas  Canyon  sites  fledged  any  young  in  1976,  but  1 
young  fledged  in  1975  from  the  single  nest  there. 

During  the  second  week  of  July  1977,  Riley  monitored  the  fledging  success  of  the  Chisos 
Mountain  pair  and  4 pairs  in  the  Davis  Mountains  occupying  the  same  sites  as  in  1976.  At 
a nest  approximately  100  m north  of  2 previous  nests,  2 young  were  near  fledging  on  8 July. 
Two  other  pairs,  one  at  a new  nest  200  m east  of  a nest-site  on  Sawtooth  Mountain  and  one 
at  the  nest  on  Emor>'  Peak,  failed  to  hatch  young.  Nests  at  2 sites  near  Timber  Mountain  in 
the  Davis  Mountains  had  apparently  been  abandoned. 


284 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


Table  1 

1975-1976  Reproductive  Success  of  Zone-tailed  Hawks  in  West  Texas 

Nests 

Eggs 

Eggs  hatched 

Percent 

hatching 

success"* 

Young  fledged 

Percent 
nest  success'’ 

1975  7 

14  (2)" 

12  (1.7)“ 

86 

8 {l.Uf 

67 

1976  9 

18  (2)^ 

7 (0.7)“ 

39 

7 (0.78)*“ 

100 

® Hatching  success  = no.  of  eggs  hatched/no.  of  eggs  laid. 
**  Nest  success  = no.  of  young  fledged/no.  of  eggs  hatched. 
Mean  = no.  of  eggs/no.  of  nests. 

Mean  = no.  of  eggs  hatched/no.  of  nests. 

'■  Mean  = no.  of  young  fledged/no.  of  nests. 


We  wish  to  acknowledge  the  support  of  the  Chihuahuan  Desert  Research  Institute  which 
provided  funds  and  equipment;  .this  is  Contribution  No.  8 from  that  Institute.  We  would 
also  like  to  thank  Roger  Krueger  and  James  T.  Harris  for  their  indispensable  field  assis- 
tance.— Sumner  W.  Matteson,  Dept.  Agricultural  Journalism  and  Environmental  Studies, 
Univ.  Wisconsin,  Madison,  Wisconsin  53706  AND  John  O.  Riley,  Dept.  Zoology,  Univ. 
Wisconsin,  Madison,  Wisconsin  53706.  Accepted  5 Apr.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  284-285 


Three  Creste<l  Eagle  records  for  Guatemala. — The  Crested  Eagle  [Morphnus  gui- 
anensis)  has  not  previously  been  reported  for  Guatemala  (Brown  and  Amadon,  Eagles,  Hawks 
and  Falcons  of  the  World,  McGraw-Hill  Book  Co.,  New  York,  New  York,  1968:631).  Russell 
(Ornithol.  Monogr.  1,  1964)  included  no  records  for  Belize,  and  Monroe  (Ornithol.  Monogr. 
7,  1963:83)  stated  that  the  2 Honduran  records  “are  the  northernmost  records  for  the 
species."  Peterson  and  Chalif  (A  Field  Guide  to  Mexican  Birds,  Houghton  Mifflin  Co., 
Boston,  Massachusetts,  1973)  omitted  this  species  from  their  field  guide  for  Mexico,  Gua- 
temala, Belize  and  El  Salvador.  Herein  we  report  a visual  record  (with  photographic  support) 
and  2 specimen  records  for  Guatemala. 

On  the  evening  of  7 February  1978,  while  camped  in  a recently  cleared  area  in  the  Peten 
area  of  east-central  Guatemala,  a medium-sized  eagle  passed  directly  over  (25-35  m over- 
head) our  camp  and  perched  ca.  100  rn  away  on  a taU  snag  in  the  burned-over  swamp.  The 
bird  remained  ca.  5 min  while  we  carefully  observed  it  through  20X  spotting  scopes  and 
photographed  it  with  300  and  350  mm  lenses  on  35  mm  cameras.  Thereafter,  the  eagle  flew 
to  another  snag  (ca.  150  m distant),  remained  ca.  2 min,  then  flew  into  the  dense  forest  on 
a nearby  mountain  slope.  After  several  minutes  an  eagle  of  the  same  species  was  again 
observed  flying  along  the  mountain  slope,  then  into  a tunnel-like  opening  in  the  dense  forest 
canopy. 

This  observation  took  place  near  the  southwest  corner  of  Belize,  less  than  2 km  west  of 
the  Peten  highway  (gravel)  at  a point  ca.  48  km  northwest  of  the  Rio  Dulce  crossing  of  Lago 
Izabal  and  38  km  south-southeast  of  the  village  of  Poptun. 

The  following  field  characters  were  clearly  noted,  separating  this  bird  from  the  somewhat 
similar  Harpy  Eagle  (Harpia  harpyja).  The  broadly  barred  primaries,  secondaries  and  rec- 
trices  distinguished  it  from  immature  birds  of  both  species.  The  bird  did  not  have  the  blackish 


GENERAL  NOTES 


285 


breast  band  of  an  adult  Harpy  Eagle;  rather,  it  had  a light  gray  breast  and  very  light  belly 
characteristic  of  a normal  phase  adult  Crested  Eagle  (Brown  and  Ainadon  1968:  plate  109). 
The  general  appearance  of  the  bird  was  of  a slender  medium  sized  eagle  rather  than  a stout 
large  eagle  as  would  be  expected  if  the  bird  were  a Harpy  Eagle.  Less  clearly  identified  were 
the  distinguishing  traits  of  the  crown.  The  bird,  although  carefuhy  observed  with  its  crown 
erect  for  several  minutes,  did  not  appear  to  have  the  double  crest  of  Harpia  but  rather  a 
single  broad  crest  as  in  Morphnus. 

Two  final  identifying  traits  were  not  clearly  seen  in  the  field  but  did  show  in  photographs 
taken  just  as  the  bird  left  its  nearer  perch  (photographs  on  file  at  the  Institute  for  Raptor 
Studies).  Brown  and  Amadon  (1968:  underwing  plate  11)  show  the  under  wing  coverts  on  the 
adult  Harpy  Eagle  as  heavily  marked  and,  in  general,  as  dark  as  the  primaries  and  second- 
aries. The  adult  light  phase  Crested  Eagle  has  nearly  immaculate  under  wing  coverts  which 
contrast  with  the  darker  primaries  and  secondaries.  The  Harpy  Eagle  also  has  a light  basal 
patch  in  its  proximal  primaries,  a trait  lacking  in  the  Crested  Eagle.  In  our  photographs,  the 
light  basal  patch  characteristic  of  the  Harpy  Eagle  is  lacking  and  the  under  wing  coverts 
appear  unmarked.  Both  of  these  features  are  diagnostic  of  an  adult  Crested  Eagle.  In  sum- 
mary, all  features  observed  on  the  bird  lead  to  the  conclusion  that  it  was  a light  phase  adult 
Crested  Eagle. 

On  8 February  1978,  while  visiting  a small  bakery  in  Flores,  Guatemala  (ca.  35  km  south- 
west of  the  Mayan  ruins  at  Tikal),  we  noticed  that  the  whisk  broom  used  to  dust  the  counter 
had  been  made  from  the  remiges  and  rectrices  of  a large,  heavily  barred  raptor.  The  matron 
reported  that  the  bird  had  been  taken  by  hunters  in  the  forest  around  Flores.  We  obtained 
a portion  of  the  whisk  broom,  and  subsequently  compared  these  feathers  with  specimens  at 
the  American  Museum  of  Natural  History  where  they  are  now  deposited  (AMNH  No.  812849). 
Our  conclusion,  later  confirmed  by  John  BuU,  was  that  the  feathers  had  come  from  an  adult 
or  subadult  Crested  Eagle. 

An  additional  record  came  to  our  attention  after  our  return  to  the  United  States  (K.  Kauf- 
man and  A.  R.  Phihips,  pers.  comm.).  A Crested  Eagle  was  recovered  dead  by  Robert  W. 
Dickerman  (pers.  comm.)  30-50  km  east  of  Flores,  Peten,  Guatemala,  on  the  road  to  Melchor 
de  Mencos  between  5 and  7 April  1966.  The  specimen  (Royal  Ontario  Museum  No.  115862), 
consists  of  a skeleton  together  with  some  remiges  and  rectrices,  and  was  identified  as  an 
adult  male  (Jon  C.  Barlow,  pers.  comm.). 

The  previous  northernmost  and  westernmost  locations  for  this  species  were  La  Ceiba, 
Honduras,  and  San  Pedro  Sula,  Honduras  (Monroe  1963:83).  All  3 records  for  Guatemala 
are  north  of  La  Ceiba,  Honduras,  and  between  140  and  200  km  west  of  San  Pedro  Sula, 
Honduras.  These  records  extend  the  known  range  of  the  Crested  Eagle  over  much  of  northern 
Guatemala.  The  close  proximity  of  both  the  visual  record  and  the  1966  specimen  to  the  Belize 
border  suggests  that  the  bird  may  also  occur  there. — David  H.  Ellis,  Institute  for  Raptor 
Studies,  Box  4420  OM  Star  Rt.,  Oracle  Arizona  85623  AND  Wayne  H.  Whaley,  224  North 
250  East,  Orem,  Utah  84057.  Accepted  10  Apr.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(2),  1981,  pp.  286-300 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


Population  Ecology  of  Raptors.  By  Ian  Newton.  Buteo  Books,  Vermillion,  South 
Dakota  and  T.  & A.  D.  Poyser  Ltd.,  Hertfordshire,  England,  1979:399  pp.,  32  black-and- 
white  photographs,  50  figs.,  68  tables  at  end  of  text,  bibliography,  index.  $35.00. — This  book 
was  especially  enjoyable  to  review  because  I associated  with  Dr.  Newton  as  he  gathered  data 
for  it.  With  great  enthusiasm  he  took  meticulous  and  voluminous  notes  during  conversations 
on  raptors.  Authors  of  papers  that  caught  his  attention  at  meetings  were  interviewed,  ques- 
tioned and  quizzed  about  their  findings.  1 vividly  remember  how  intense  he  became  as  he 
hurriedly  jotted  down  and  chronicled  information  in  his  notebook  during  a meeting  we  both 
attended  in  South  Africa  on  African  predatory  birds.  As  a result  of  this  zeal,  there  is  some 
heretofore  unpublished  and  considerable  contemporary  material  in  the  book. 

The  author  was  among  the  last  students  of  David  Lack,  by  whom  he  was  greatly  influenced. 
Consequently,  there  is  considerable  reliance  on  food  relationships  throughout  the  text  in 
explaining  the  data. 

Although  this  book  is  published  in  the  U.S.  and  Britain,  the  text  is  decidedly  British  in 
style,  flavor,  spelling  and  sentence  structure.  For  example,  “the  last  but  one”  for  “next  to 
last,”  “in  fine  weather,”  and  “Gyr  Falcon”  instead  of  “Gyrfalcon”  are  typically  used.  In 
organization  the  text  runs  in  a reasonable  sequence.  The  sexes  of  raptors  are  discussed  in 
chapter  1,  followed  by  a series  of  chapters  on  breeding  biology:  chapter  3 breeding  density, 
chapter  5 nest-sites,  chapter  7 breeding  strategies,  chapter  10  fidelity  to  breeding  areas,  etc. 
This  is  followed  by  chapters  on  movements  and  mortality.  Here,  it  seems,  would  have  been 
a better  place  to  put  his  chapter  2 on  dispersion  and  chapter  4 on  winter  density.  His  final 
5 chapters  cover  management  oriented  topics  and  contemporary  problems  such  as  chapters 
14  and  15  on  chemical  pollutants  and  chapter  17  on  breeding  in  captivity.  He  is  extremely 
current  on  his  treatment  of  these  latter  chapters,  especially  in  light  of  the  recent  and  often 
serious  declines  of  raptors  directly  caused  by  humans  and  synthetic  chemicals.  The  chapter 
on  captive  propagation  evidences  the  recency  of  his  data.  Seemingly  chapter  14  on  DDT  and 
other  organo-chlorines,  and  chapter  15  on  other  pollutants  and  pesticides  could  have  been 
combined  and  perhaps  shortened,  although  only  about  12%  of  the  text  was  spent  on  the 
pollutant  problem.  The  bibliography  is  excellent  with  over  800  citations  and  in  itself  is  a good 
compendium  and  summary  on  raptor  biology. 

To  develop  support  for  some  of  the  concepts,  he  uses  carefully  selected  case  history 
studies.  For  example,  in  a discussion  of  increasingly  rare  birds,  he  gives  a blow-by-blow 
account  of  the  studies  on  Lesser  Spotted  [Aquila  pomarina)  and  Black  (A.  verreauxi)  eagles 
wherein  mortality  of  the  young  has  been  experimentally  reduced.  Rather  than  rely  totally  on 
work  gathered  from  the  literature,  however,  there  are  ideas  seemingly  expressed  for  the  first 
time  by  Newton.  For  example,  in  his  treatment  of  the  reversed  sexual  dimorphism  phenom- 
enon in  raptors,  he  offers  a fresh  new  way  to  view  the  data  (p.  23)  and  thus  provides  yet 
another  explanation.  1 particularly  appreciated  Newton's  insight  in  posing  questions  for  fruit- 
ful future  research  with  raptors.  Many  such  questons,  e.g.,  “Are  populations  of  raptors  so 
limited  in  winter  as  are  some  passerines  that  they  cannot  fully  occupy  the  available  breeding 
territories  in  spring  and  summer?"  are.  however,  the  very  ones  that  are  in  need  of  work  on 
most  groups  of  birds.  Some  insight  is  suggested  to  that  question  by  Newton’s  own  research 
on  the  Sparrowhawk  (Accipiter  nisus)  and  he  persuasively  argues  that  raptor  densities  are 
directly  correlated  with  food  supply,  which  in  turn  is  related  to  productivity  of  the  land.  This 
relationship  appears  to  operate  during  all  seasons. 

The  book  is  relatively  error-free.  1 found  only  3 typographical  errors  but  they  are  minor, 
eg.,  Richlefs  in  place  of  Ricklefs  (p.  126).  In  the  text  he  gives  the  citation,  Fyfe  1978  (p.  264) 


286 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


287 


while  the  closest  to  that  in  the  literature  cited  is  Fyfe  1976.  The  printing  is  nicely  done,  but 
in  my  copy  p.  237  is  poor  because  of  heavy  print,  especially  noticeable  where  double  letters 
occur,  as  in  egg.  The  citation  of  Belon  (1555)  (p.  199)  on  migration  represents  a fantastic  hit 
of  detective  work  in  the  literature. 

There  are  some  inconsistencies  in  the  format  of  the  text.  Some  of  the  chapters  have 
discussions  as  special  topics,  and  some  do  not.  Rather  than  a summary  at  the  end  of  each 
chapter,  I would  like  to  have  seen  a discussion  of  the  material  wherein  Newton  could  have 
used  his  own  insight  and  his  keen  inductive  reasoning.  Throughout  the  text  he  seems  to 
resist  speculating  about  concepts  that  are  not  clear  and  on  some  occasions,  Newton  even 
seems  to  apologize  for  having  theorized  or  speculated. 

I have  mixed  feelings  about  placing  all  the  tables  together  in  the  back.  When  readers  want 
to  find  a particular  bit  of  data  dealing  with,  for  example,  mortality  from  chapter  9,  they  must 
thumb  through  the  graphs  until  they  happen  upon  the  data,  unless  they  know  the  precise 
table  number.  I prefer  the  placement  of  tables  in  the  chapter  adjacent  to  the  introduction 
and  discussion  of  the  data. 

In  his  discussions  of  migration,  there  is  a rather  lengthy  bit  of  data  on  North  American 
migration,  and  I was  surprised  to  see  the  omission  of  the  very  significant  paper  by  Haugh 
(Search  2:1-60,  1972)  that  discusses  the  effects  of  the  Great  Lakes  on  migration.  Newton 
even  uses  a figure  to  show  migration  along  the  Great  Lakes  (p.  197).  It  would  also  have  been 
helpful  to  have  the  citation  for  Haldane’s  “incomplete  data”  methods  as  footnoted  on  p.  203. 

Some  observations  that  were  generated  during  the  reading  of  this  book  seem  in  order. 
Perhaps  most  salient  is  the  virtual  lack  of  data  from  the  neotropics.  It  is  not  that  Newton 
has  neglected  literature  from  there,  but  rather  that  there  is  little  if  any  literature  on  neo- 
tropical raptor  biology.  It  is  one  of  the  most  diverse  and  interesting  raptor  faunas  (primitive 
falconidae,  the  sub-buteos,  etc.)  and  yet  it  remains  so  little  known.  There  is  a fruitful  geo- 
graphic area  of  research  for  the  upcoming  raptor  biologists.  It  is  clear  from  reading  Newton 
that  most  of  our  knowledge  about  raptor  biology  pertains  to  holarctic  areas,  although  con- 
siderable data  are  also  available  from  Africa.  Newton  has  been  careful  to  explain  particular 
methodologies  used  in  the  study  of  raptors.  Few  other  books  do  this.  He  briefly  explains  how 
radio  telemetry  is  being  used  and  the  mechanical  basis  of  the  techniques  as  pertains  to 
raptors.  He  also  cautions  the  reader  (p.  231)  to  be  careful  in  comparing  data  on  ppm  expres- 
sions of  synthetic  chemical  residues  in  birds  or  their  eggs  by  pointing  out  that  some  data  are 
given  as  dry  weight,  some  as  wet  weight  and  some  as  lipid  weight.  Each  method  gives  a 
considerably  different  numerical  value.  It  is  unfortunate  that  his  book  came  on  the  tails  of 
Walter’s  book  on  the  Eleonora’s  Falcon  {Falco  eleonorae)  (1979,  Univ.  Chicago  Press).  New- 
ton’s discussion  of  the  colonial  species  would  have  been  more  complete  had  he  had  access 
to  Walter’s  book. 

There  is  a wealth  of  data  on  breeding  biology  that  can  be  implied  by  a use  of  morphological 
expressions  of  birds.  These  data  are  largely  ignored.  For  example,  further  support  for  the 
concept  of  fidelity  to  breeding  areas  can  come  from  the  study  of  geographic  variation,  lo- 
cations of  recognizable  demes,  etc.,  as  well  as  from  actual  banding  data.  Many  raptors  are 
noted  for  the  occurrence  of  recognizable  demes  based  on  external  morphology  (presumably 
this  win  be  corroborated  by  blood  protein  morphology^  once  that  technique  has  been  suffi- 
ciently tried  on  birds).  Biologically  this  recognition  is  perceptible  when  members  of  a given 
deme  return  to  the  general  region  to  breed  and  selection  pressure  intensity  for  a given  trait 
far  exceeds  the  rate  of  dispersal  to  another  area  or  immigration  rate  of  new  genetic  expres- 
sions. Such  morphological  traits  are  potent  evidence  of  the  lack  of  panmixis  on  a level  larger 
than  the  local  population  or  deme. 

The  overall  wealth  of  material  and  Newton’s  manner  of  presenting  it  make  this  a valuable 
book  for  those  who  not  only  want  to  learn  about  raptors  but  also  for  those  who  are  interested 


288 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


in  the  bioIog>  of  bird  populations  in  general.  I hope  that  the  price  is  not  too  restrictive  for 
those  young  biologists  who  can  profit  most  from  such  a book. — Clayton  M.  White. 


The  Foraging  Behavior  of  Mountain  Bluebirds  With  Emphasis  on  Sexual  For- 
aging Differences.  By  Harry  W.  Power.  Ornithological  Monographs  No.  28,  American 
Ornithologists’  Union,  1980:ix  + 72  pp.  $8.50. — This  is  a very  stimulating  monograph,  which 
is  a tactful  way  of  saying  that  it  makes  many  debatable  points.  Power  first  reviews  the  theory 
of  how  sexual  differences  in  foraging  might  come  about.  He  then  demonstrates  that  female 
Mountain  Bluebirds  (Sialia  currucoides)  tend  to  use  more  energetically  expensive  methods 
of  foraging  than  males,  and  recalls  his  theoretical  review  in  order  to  understand  why  this 
difference  exists.  He  sets  up  a series  of  experiments  adjusting  brood  sizes,  removing  parents 
and  manipulating  habitats,  all  designed  to  discriminate  between  the  theoretical  alternatives. 
He  concludes  that  none  of  the  alternatives  are  well  supported,  but  leans  toward  a “division 
of  labor”  interpretation.  He  does  discover,  however,  that  both  males  and  females  have  a 
common  tendency  to  use  more  expensive  foraging  tactics  when  their  work  load  increases, 
and  that  females  are  more  inclined  to  make  this  increase  than  males,  at  least  when  the  work 
load  increase  is  in  terms  of  young  to  feed  per  adult. 

The  detailed  biology'  in  the  midst  of  theoretical  boldness  and  experimental  ingenuity  in  the 
field  makes  this  work  a major  contribution  to  modern  ornithology.  I make  this  claim  in  spite 
of  the  fact  that  I would  have  interpreted  the  data  rather  differently. 

Power  presents  himself  as  an  evolutionary  biologist,  and  writes  with  a measure  of  arrogant 
dogmatism.  But  the  writing  is  so  clear  and  Power  himself  so  thoughtful  of  his  readers,  that 
one  is  willing  to  overlook  his  confidence  in  his  own  reasoning  (and  in  the  naivete  of  those  he 
disagrees  with).  The  net  effect  is  exciting,  and  I suspect  that  Mountain  Bluebirds  are  destined 
to  become  a “classic”  or  textbook  species  for  discussions  of  avian  foraging  strategies. — 
Stephen  D.  Fretwell. 

Population  Dynamics:  The  20th  Symposium  of  the  British  Ecological  Society. 
By  R.  M.  Anderson,  B.  D.  Turner  and  L.  R.  Taylor  (eds.).  Blackwell  Scientific  Publications, 
Oxford,  England;  distributed  in  the  U.S.A.  by  Halstead  Press  of  John  Wiley  & Sons,  Inc., 
New  York,  New  York,  1979:434  pp.  $69.95. — An  exciting  component  of  ecology  as  a disci- 
pline is  the  constant  challenge  provided  by  theoretical  and  empirical  advances.  The  breadth 
and  vitality  of  recent  advances  in  population  ecology  are  well  represented  in  this  volume.  A 
total  of  18  chapters  with  27  authors  surveys  a variety  of  subjects:  the  influence  of  behavior 
and  genetics  as  determinants  of  population  dynamics;  the  importance  of  mosaics  in  deter- 
mining population  size  in  space  and  time;  the  role  of  life  history  strategies  in  determining 
population  characteristics;  the  existence  of  multiple  stable  states  in  ecological  communities; 
and  the  influence  of  trophic  structure  on  community  dynamics.  The  foundations  of  many  of 
the  dogmas  of  the  past  two  decades  are  weakened  by  these  presentations. 

In  the  only  strictly  avian  chapter.  Diamond  examines  the  now  familiar  question  of  ran- 
domness vs  competition  in  the  evolution  of  island  faunas — neither  extreme  represents  truth. 
Avian  examples  are  involved  in  two  other  chapters.  Cowie  and  Krebs  use  foraging  patterns 
in  insects  and  birds  to  explore  optimal  foraging  in  patchy  environments.  They,  like  other 
authors,  emphasize  the  importance  of  integrating  the  behavior  (Taylor  and  Taylor)  and  ge- 
netics (Berry,  Law)  of  individuals  in  understanding  population  dynamics.  Both  Berry  and 
Law  note  that  life  histories — rates  of  reproduction  and  risks  of  death — are  evolving  under 
forces  imposed  by  prevailing  environments.  Since  they  may  be  doing  so  at  a pace  that  is 
within  the  time  scale  of  ecological  studies,  they  cannot  be  ignored.  In  an  analysis  of  spruce 
budworm  populations,  Peterman,  Clark  and  Holling  conclude  that  birds  may  play  a primary 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


289 


role  in  determining  the  lower  stable  population  attractor  and  thus,  equilibrium  densities,  of 
spruce  budworm. 

Although  only  one  chapter  concentrates  on  birds,  there  is  much  to  be  learned  here  by 
ornithologists  willing  to  make  the  effort.  Many  of  the  chapters  are  dominated  by  mathematical 
models.  Typically,  these  are  coupled  with  analysis  of  the  details  of  an  intriguing  biological 
system.  The  merger  of  theory  with  observations  often  yields  insights  that  either  alone  is 
incapable  of  producing.  Overall,  cogent  arguments  are  presented  for  avoidance  of  simplistic 
models  dependent  on  average  populations  and  their  rates  of  change.  More  realistic  models 
will  deal  with  the  spatial  and  temporal  variability  in  populations. 

Many  of  the  chapters  in  this  volume  are  integrative  combinations  of  observation,  theory 
and  application.  There  are  few  typographical  errors,  and  extensive  cross  referencing  among 
chapters  attests  to  the  efforts  of  the  editors  to  increase  the  value  of  the  volume.  Speaking 
of  value,  its  $69.95  price  tag  will  encourage  use  of  library  copies.  It  is  worth  the  effort  to 
search  out  a copy  and  read. — JAMES  R.  Karr. 


The  Island  Waterfowl.  By  Milton  W.  Weller.  Iowa  State  University  Press,  Ames,  Iowa, 
1980:x  + 121  pp.,  27  numbered  figs.,  12  tables.  $10.95. — The  island  waterfowl  this  book 
treats  are  the  distinctive  species  or  races  of  ducks  and  geese  (Anatidae)  that  have  evolved 
in  isolation  following  earlier  colonization  of  oceanic  islands  or  archipelagos,  largely  in  the 
tropics  and  the  southern  oceans.  The  lure  of  remote  islands  and  scarce  animal  forms  is 
probably  sufficient  to  ensure  that  this  book  will  find  readers,  despite  the  rather  high  price 
of  such  a slim  volume.  Nevertheless,  my  initial  scepticism  as  to  the  need  for  such  a book 
remained  after  2 readings.  Weller  had  already  published  the  results  of  his  field  studies  on 
5 island  groups — Tierra  del  Fuego,  Falklands,  South  Georgia,  New  Zealand  and  Auckland 
Islands — and  his  wish  to  synthesize  the  results  of  those  and  other  work  on  island  waterfowl 
could  have  been  done  as  effectively  in  a review  article  for  a journal.  The  objectives  of  the 
book,  which  emerge  only  gradually,  seem  to  be  to  define  the  characteristics  of  successful 
island  waterfowl  and  to  predict  whether  vacant  niches  exist  for  future  colonizations  of  specific 
archipelagos,  while  encouraging  further  research  on  these  birds  to  fill  gaps  in  existing  knowl- 
edge. The  book  is  thus  addressed  to  fellow-scientists  rather  than  to  the  general  public. 

One  may  carp  at  the  questionable  need  for  this  book,  but  its  standards  of  scholarship  are 
as  high  as  one  would  expect  from  a scientist  of  Weller’s  calibre.  The  individual  forms  are 
described  and  located  geographically,  the  factors  influencing  colonization  of  islands  and  the 
i subsequent  responses  to  constraints  posed  by  island  situations  are  described,  and  the  de- 
I'  velopment  of  the  waterfowl  faunas  of  the  island  groups  Weller  had  studied  is  analysed.  A 
I brief  chapter  considers  conservation  measures  for  scarce  species.  There  are  8V2  pages  of 
1 references  (in  fine  print)  and  an  index.  The  text  is  easily  read  and  adequately  proofed  (I 
I noted  only  2 or  3 typos),  and  the  diagrams  and  illustrations  are  clear  and  informative.  My 
chief  difficulty  with  the  make-up  of  the  book  was  its  lack  of  an  introduction,  the  first  chapter 
j plunging  straight  into  lists  and  descriptions  of  island  forms.  The  Preface  covers  some  of  the 
! introductory  material  needed,  but  how  many  readers  will  think  to  read  the  Preface  first? 

Weller  repeatedly  emphasizes  the  need  for  adaptability  in  successful  colonizers,  most  of 
I which  stemmed  from  the  dabbling  ducks  {Anas}.  Adaptations  to  islands  include  the  ability 
to  use  marine  and  shoreline  environments  for  part  or  all  of  the  year,  resistance  to  cold  on 
the  part  of  the  young  and  even  more  so  for  the  adults,  and  so  on.  He  seems  not  to  have 
remarked  that  such  characteristics  exist  in  several  of  the  largely  north  temperate  and  sub- 
I arctic  species  of  diving  ducks  (tribes  Somateriini  and  Mergini),  which  thus  may  be  preadapted 
for  colonization  of  cold  south  temperate  or  subantarctic  islands.  The  extinct  Auckland  Mer- 
ganser (Mergus  australis),  and  the  steamer  ducks  {Tachyeres),  which  latter  resemble  and  fill 


290 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


the  niches  occupied  by  the  eiders  {Sornateria)  in  the  north,  presumably  have  evolved  from 
representatives  of  such  northern  groups.  The  same  adaptations  allow  those  northern  diving 
ducks  to  winter  farther  north,  so  trans-equatorial  migrations  that  could  lead  to  colonization 
of  southern  islands  are  not  regular  among  those  groups;  those  individuals  that  do  reach 
remote  islands  may  be  as  promising  colonists  as  are  Anas  ducks. 

The  discussion  of  habitat  use  and  resource  partitioning  might  be  read  to  advantage  by- 
waterfowl  biologists  concerned  with  carrying  capacity  and  introductions,  though  these,  as 
well  as  biology  of  island  waterfowl,  need  more  quantitative  data.  Study  of  the  island  micro- 
cosm also  reminds  one  of  its  vulnerability,  and  the  extent  to  which  man  has  affected  the 
numbers  and  distribution  of  continental  waterfowl  in  the  past  and  present.  Some  species  of 
waterfowl,  as  well  as  other  game  birds,  were  nearly  eliminated  from  eastern  North  America 
by  unregulated  hunting  in  the  1800’s,  and  when  populations  began  to  recover  they  found 
eastern  habitats  transformed  from  forest  to  largely  open  landscapes.  Thus,  we  have  “prairie” 
ducks  moving  in  to  breed  aU  over  northeast  America,  even  into  artificial  impoundments  that 
simulate  prairie  sloughs  in  largely  forested  regions.  If  Weller's  book  stimulates  more  thought 
and  research  on  such  topics,  its  spinoff  value  will  go  a long  way  to  justify  its  publication. — 
Anthony  J.  Erskine. 

Woodland  Grouse  Symposium.  By  T.  W.  I.  Lovel  (ed.).  World  Pheasant  Association, 
Daws  HaU,  Lamarsh,  Bures,  Suffolk,  United  Kingdom,  1979:180  pp.,  3 photographs,  18 
maps,  40  text  figs,  and  32  tables.  £8. — This  report  of  a symposium  held  in  Scotland  in 
December  1978  contains  papers  and  notes  on  2 species  of  capercaillie,  the  Black,  Hazel, 
Ruffed,  Blue  and  Spruce  grouse,  and  briefly  W iUow  and  Rock  ptarmigan.  Participants  came 
from  most  of  the  northern  countries  of  Europe,  USSR,  Iran,  China,  Japan,  Canada  and  U.S. 
Sessions  were  devoted  to  the  present  status  of  woodland  grouse  species  in  7 countries;  the 
ecology  of  woodland  grouse  (7  papers),  field  and  analytical  techniques  (3  papers),  behavior 
of  woodland  grouse  (3  papers)  and  management  of  woodland  grouse  (5  papers). 

Marcstrbm  briefly  reviews  the  literature  on  population  fluctuations  of  European  woodland 
grouse.  Synchrony  in  population  changes  of  different  species  within  an  area,  and  variability 
in  periodicities,  are  features  of  the  Scandinavian  stocks. 

Reports  from  Norway,  Sweden,  Finland,  Denmark  and  Poland  suggest  a general  decline 
in  numbers  of  most  grouse  species,  which  are  attributed  to  changes  in  forest  exploitation 
practices,  habitat  loss,  changes  in  weather  patterns  and  acid  rain  pollution. 

In  France,  Capercaillie  (Tetrao  urogallus)  survive  in  4 isolated  mountain  chains.  They  are 
widespread  and  hunted  only  in  the  Pyrenees.  A relic  population  of  Black  Grouse  (Lyrurus 
tetrix)  remains  on  lowland  moorland  in  the  .\rdennes.  In  alpine  habitat,  the  species  is  still 
found  in  9 departments  adjacent  to  Switzerland  and  Italy.  They  were  never  found  in  the 
Pyrenees.  Ellison  reviews  his  work  on  hunted  and  unhunted  Black  Grouse  populations  in  the 
French  Alps.  Fall  hunting  removed  about  57%  of  the  males.  An  unbalanced  sex  ratio  and 
few  old  males  does  not  seem  to  have  affected  productivity. 

Tso-Hsin  Cheng  outlines  the  status  of  grouse  in  China.  Hazel  Grouse  (Tetrastes  bonasia) 
are  found  in  the  Northern  Region  and  a closely  related  species  {Tetrastes  sewerzowi),  in  the 
Southwest  Region.  Black  Grouse  of  3 subspecies  are  found  in  extreme  northeastern  and 
northwestern  China,  and  the  Black-billed  Capercaillie  (Tetrao  parvirostris)  in  the  northern 
part  of  the  Great  Khingan  Mountains,  and  perhaps  the  northern  part  of  the  Altai  Range. 

The  Black-billed  Capercaillie  is  confined  to  upland  forest  dominated  by  larch  (Larix  grne- 
lini).  Food  is  largely  shoots  and  buds  of  white  birch  and  larch.  In  summer  they  eat  blue- 
berries, huckleberries  (Vacciniurn  sp.)  and  bird  cherries  (Craetagus  sp.).  Spring  display 
begins  in  late  March  and  mating  from  mid-April  to  early  May. 

Andreev  reports  on  the  reproductive  behavior  of  the  Black-biUed  Capercaillie  in  Siberia. 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


291 


His  description  suggests  some  differences  from  the  displays  of  the  Chinese  birds.  In  China, 
display  is  largely  arboreal  and  starts  at  2:00-3:00,  lek  behavior  has  not  been  recorded.  In 
Siberia,  males  display  in  groups  of  6-10  on  the  ground,  usually  on  snow.  They  perform  all 
night,  starting  at  sunset,  with  a lull  around  midnight.  Their  lek  activity  may  persist  for  an 
extraordinarily  long  period,  exceeding  12  h. 

In  China,  the  birds  migrate  to  river  valleys  for  the  winter.  Their  numbers  may  be  declining 
because  of  changes  in  habitat  due  to  intense  land  use.  Tso-Hsin  Cheng’s  account  of  the 
Black  Grouse  includes  the  old  folk-lore  story  of  males  spitting  saliva  on  the  ground  and 
females  following  to  pick  it  up. 

The  winter  ecology  of  woodland  grouse  is  discussed  in  papers  by  Andreev  on  bioenergetics, 
Pulliainen  on  composition  and  nutrient  content  of  fall  and  winter  food  of  Capercaillie,  and 
Glutz  von  Blotzheim  and  students  on  the  winter  behavior  and  food  of  Black  Grouse  and  food 
of  the  Hazel  Grouse. 

Angelstam  reports  on  reproductive  success  and  survival  of  Black  Grouse  in  relation  to 
population  fluctuations  of  small  mammals.  Data  from  1977  and  1978  suggest  that  survival  of 
eggs,  chicks  and  adults  were  higher  when  small  mammals  were  abundant.  Little  information 
is  given  on  methods  used  to  judge  density  of  small  mammals  and  no  data  on  the  species 
present,  only  the  bank  vole  {Clethrionornys  glareolus)  is  mentioned. 

The  only  paper  on  North  American  woodland  grouse  is  given  by  Bendell  and  Zwickel. 
North  American  biologists  will  be  familiar  with  the  variety  of  papers  by  these  authors,  in  the 
case  on  Bendell,  extending  for  over  25  years.  Their  paper  describes  the  characteristics  of 
Spruce  {Canachites  canadensis).  Blue  (Dendragapus  obscurus)  and  Ruffed  (Bonasa  umbellus) 
grouse.  They  briefly  review  what  is  known  about  population  regulation,  causes  of  levels  of 
density,  management  and  distribution  of  these  species. 

Porkert,  in  an  important  paper,  discusses  the  influence  of  human  factors  on  the  populations 
of  tetraonids  in  north-eastern  Bohemia  and  northern  Moravia.  In  addition  to  the  often  rec- 
ognized factors  of  human  disturbance  and  habitat  changes,  there  is  also  the  impact  of  air 
pollution  and  acid  rain. 

Isolated  and  now  endangered  populations  of  Capercaillie,  Black  Grouse  and  Hazel  Hens 
live  in  the  Orlicke  hory  Mountains,  which  rise  to  1115  m.  The  prevailing  winds  that  flow  over 
the  Bohemian  industrial  basin  impinge  at  right  angles  to  this  range.  The  foothills  are  narrow 
and  do  not  intercept  the  precipitation  carried  by  air  masses  from  the  west.  The  geological 
structure  is  poorly  buffered  schist.  At  870  m precipitation  averages  126.5  cm  per  annum  and 
is  2-3  times  higher  than  in  the  neighboring  foothills.  Fog  and  ice  storms  are  frequent.  The 
average  annual  value  of  acid  precipitation  is  estimated  at  about  4.2.  Spring  thaw  is  accom- 
panied by  a sharp  drop  in  pH  amounting  to  about  1 unit. 

The  most  heavily  polluted  sites  are  those  under  the  crowns  of  old  trees,  mainly  spruce, 
j under  which  there  was  almost  complete  destruction  of  Vaccinium  myrtillus  in  the  winter  of 
: 1975-76,  which  had  a particularly  acute  episode  of  pollution.  In  open  sites,  V.  myrtillus 

i were  less  heavily  damaged  and  regenerated  better.  This  species  is  an  important  grouse  food, 
j particularly  for  Capercaillie. 

Decline  of  Capercaillie  and  Black  Grouse  numbers  might  not  be  due  entirely  to  air  pollution 
because  other  factors  are  also  influential.  However,  these  species  have  also  declined  rapidly 
, in  the  east-Sudeten  area.  There,  and  in  the  Krkonose  and  Beskydy  Mountains,  changes  in 
forest  management  were  introduced  much  later  than  in  the  Orlicke  hory  Mountains,  but  the 
decline  occurred  at  the  same  time  as  the  development  of  heavy  chemical  industry  and 
construction  of  large  thermal  power  plants. 

Recommendations  for  harvest  management  of  Capercaillie  in  Scotland  are  made  by  Moss, 
Weir  and  Jones.  After  consideration  of  a variety  of  field  studies,  they  conclude  that  under 
the  shooting  methods  (driving)  employed  in  open  natural  forest,  16%  would  be  a safe  harvest. 


292 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  \o.  2,  June  1981 


In  the  areas  studied  about  159?-  of  the  birds  seen  were  killed  on  the  first  shoot  of  the  year. 
This  safe  harvest  rate  is  much  lower  than  for  other  forest  species  of  grouse  for  which  data 
are  available. 

In  planted  forest,  densities  of  Capercaillie  were  similar  to  those  recorded  in  Finland  and 
were  lighter  than  in  natural  forests.  The  proposals  made  by  Bancik  for  the  conservation  of 
Capercaillie  in  Slovakia,  in  contrast,  seem  naive  and  contain  many  of  the  outworn  panaceas 
of  an  earlier  age.  There  is  no  presentation  and  analysis  of  field  data,  only  proposals  to 
regulate  certain  logging  practices  to  specific  times  of  the  year  to  minimize  disturbance  to  the 
birds,  but  nothing  about  regulating  logging  so  as  to  create  or  maintain  suitable  habitat. 

Potts,  in  an  all  too  brief  paper,  stresses  the  value  of  modeling  as  an  aid  to  studies  of  game 
bird  populations,  using  his  experience  with  the  Grey  Partridge  {Perdix  perdix)  as  an  example. 

This  symposium  gives  a good  overview  of  the  work  on  tetraonids  in  Europe  and  Asia. 
Over  most  of  Europe.  Capercaillie  and  Black  Grouse  are  declining.  The  view  of  the  partici- 
pants is  that  in  the  future,  priority  should  be  given  to  intensive  studies  of  population  dynam- 
ics.— Harry  G.  Lumsden. 


Proceedings  of  the  orkshop  Management  of  Southern  Forests  for  Nongame 
Birds.  Richard  M.  DeGraff  (Tech.  Coord.).  L.S.D.A.  For.  Serv.  Gen.  Tech.  Kept.  SE-14.. 
Southeastern  Forest  Experiment  Station.  Asheville,  North  Carolina,  1978:176  pp..  53  figs., 
29  tables.  No  price  given. — This  is  another  in  the  Forest  Service  sponsored  series  on  the 
regional  management  of  nongame  birds.  The  term  “nongame”  is  well  defined,  easy  to  un- 
derstand and  has  management  meaning,  but  as  of  late  has  fallen  from  grace  with  certain 
editors  and  its  use  in  print  may  be  diminishing.  Nevertheless,  almost  every  paper  in  this 
workshop  uses  the  term  to  denote  non-“harvested”  wildlife  species.  Although  concern  for 
nongame  wildlife  is  not  new.  only  recently  has  there  been  any  action  taken.  The  main  reason 
for  this  has  been  lack  of  funding,  both  in  the  failure  of  conservation  groups  to  raise  enough 
money  and  the  reluctance  of  state  and  federal  agencies  to  use  “hunting  and  fishing”  funds 
to  look  at  songbirds,  as  well  as  the  absence  of  any  key  legislation  to  provide  monies  for 
nongame  research.  Michael  Zagata  of  the  National  Audubon  Society  outlines  the  history  of 
such  problems  in  the  keynote  address.  In  sum.  he  cites  the  rise  in  interest  in  nongame 
management  and  documents  recent  legislation  such  as  the  Missouri  nongame  act  designed 
to  benefit  nongame  wildlife,  which  raised  about  S26  million  in  1978.  He  concludes  by  calling 
for  increased  amounts  of  funding  at  the  federal  level,  and  justifies  this  call  by  documenting 
the  current  widespread  public  interest  in  nongame  wildlife. 

The  workshop  is  divided  into  4 sections:  Forest  Ecosystem  Structure  and  Function  and 
Effects  on  Birdlife.  with  the  keynote  address  and  2 additional  papers;  Effects  of  Management 
Practices  on  Nongame  Birds,  with  6 papers:  Specialized  Bird  Habitats  and  Management, 
with  4 papers;  and  a concluding  paper  on  future  research  plans.  Like  all  workshops  there 
are  many  “nonpapers”  in  this  one  and  many  of  the  papers  could  have  been  written  for  any 
of  the  other  regional  workshops  by  simply  changing  the  names  of  the  bird  species  and  leaving 
most  of  the  text  unchanged.  Moreover,  there  is  much  interpaper  redundancy  in  these  particular 
workshop  proceedings.  However,  some  of  the  papers  are  of  great  interest  and  practical  use. 
Chandler  Robbins  provides  a very  useful  paper  on  census  techniques  for  forest  birds,  a topic 
on  which  there  is  probably  none  better  qualified  to  speak  than  he.  He  compares  spot  mapping 
(or  plot  census),  transect  methods,  point  counts  (the  IPA  method)  and  the  Breeding  Bird 
Survey  (BBS),  as  well  as  covering  banding,  nest  finding,  tape  recordings  and  techniques  for 
winter  and  other  nonbreeding  season  surveys.  He  concludes  that,  although  all  methods  have 
some  level  of  imprecision,  the  spot-mapping  method  is  generally  best:  but  other  techniques 
such  as  transect  lines  are  most  effective  when  many  plots  are  being  compared.  Another 
interesting  paper  is  by  Noon  and  Able,  “A  Comparison  of  Avian  Community  Structure  in 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


293 


the  Northern  and  Southern  Appalachian  Mountains.”  Not  only  is  the  methodology  in  this 
paper  of  interest,  but  useful  data  and  interesting  results  are  provided.  They  compare  bird 
data  from  Mount  Mansfield  in  Vermont  with  the  Great  Smoky  Mountains,  plotting  birds  on 
elevational  and  other  environmental  gradients.  It  is  unfortunate  that  they  do  not  have  data 
for  the  central  Appalachians,  such  as  West  Virginia,  to  add  to  their  comparisions. 

This  workshop,  like  most  others,  is  loaded  with  material  that  apparently  could  not  make 
it  in  a refereed  journal.  Yet  if  one  is  successful  at  sifting  the  wheat  from  the  chaff,  new 
information  and  knowledge  can  be  gleaned  from  its  pages. — Robert  C.  Whitmore. 


Audubon  Birds  of  America.  Introduction  and  Commentaries  by  Roger  Tory  Peterson. 
Crown  Publishers,  Inc.,  New  York,  New  York,  1979  (?  no  date)  :160  pp.,  72  full-page  plates 
(color),  30  much-reduced  illustrations  (color)  on  4 pp.  $17.95.  AUDUBON.  A BIOGRAPHY.  By 
John  Chancellor.  The  Viking  Press,  New  York,  New  York,  1978:224  pp.,  116  illustrations 
(16  color).  $17.95.  The  Double  Elephant  Folio.  The  Story  of  Audubon’s  Birds  of 
America.  By  Waldemar  Fries.  American  Library  Association,  Chicago,  Illinois,  1973: 
xxii  + 501  pp.,  frontispiece  (color),  45  test-figs.  $45.00. — John  James  Audubon  (1785-1851) 
was  unusual  in  various  ways,  as  in  being  an  ornithologist  who  made  money.  Indeed,  he  has 
continued  to  do  so  for  those  with  the  enterprise  to  market  his  evidently  inexhaustible  appeal. 
The  rather  large  number  of  these  now  includes  Roger  Tory  Peterson,  another  ornithologist 
of  financial  acumen,  and  Crown  Publishers,  Inc. 

If  there  is  additional  reason  for  the  present  book,  it  would  appear  to  be  whatever  interest 
attaches  to  Peterson’s  choice  of  “102  favorite”  (a  phrase  from  the  dust  jacket)  Audubon 
pictures.  Some  years  ago  I ventured  (Scientific  American  216:156,  1967)  to  nominate  a min- 
imum of  17,  all  but  2 of  which  have  made  it  onto  Peterson’s  list.  So  much  for  taste.  This  is 
not  a work  of  criticism  or  interpretation.  The  brief  biographical  introduction  provides  nothing 
new,  but  perpetuates  the  myth  laid  to  rest,  one  would  have  thought,  by  Alice  Ford  (John 
James  Audubon,  Univ.  Oklahoma  Press,  Norman,  Oklahoma,  1964)  that  Audubon  studied 
under  Jaques  Louis  David.  The  often  amusing  commentary  which  accompanies  the  pictures 
is  about  birds,  not  art,  and  has  no  clear  raison  d’etre  except  to  fill  space.  On  p.  44,  discussing 
the  immature  Bald  Eagle  {Haliaeetus  leucocephalus)  (the  “Bird  of  Washington,”  as  Audubon 
called  it),  Peterson  perpetrates  a howler  in  saying  that  Florida  has  more  of  this  species  than 
any  other  state,  while  on  p.  54,  discussing  the  adult,  he  correctly  notes  that  Alaska  has  more 
than  all  other  states  combined.  The  plates  are  well  reproduced,  but  the  color  is  more  intense 
than  in  any  elephant  “folio”  that  I have  seen.  They  provide  an  instructive  sampling,  however, 
for  any  one  without  ready  access  to  Audubon’s  birds.  The  legends  of  the  Leach’s  Petrel  and 
Trumpeter  Swan  are  transposed  on  p.  159. 

Biographies  of  Audubon  are  numerous  and  continue  to  appear  along  with  the  pictures. 
Chancellor’s  is  refreshing  in  presenting  a rarely  unbiased  picture  of  Audubon’s  complex  and 
not  altogether  adorable  personality.  The  highly  readable  but  nonetheless  scholarly  text,  with 
numerous  figures  of  contempory  scenes  and  events,  does  a good  job  of  placing  Audubon  in 
context.  There  are  occasional  perplexities,  as  on  p.  172,  where  Chancellor  observes  that  the 
California  Condor  was  “the  subject  of  an  Audubon  controversy”  not  otherwise  mentioned, 
nor  known  to  me.  Could  he  mean  the  one  over  the  sense  of  smell  in  Turkey  Vultures?  Anent 
this,  on  p.  41  he  has  Audubon  contending  that  the  latter  have  a well  developed  sense  of 
smell  (as  K.  Stager  has  shown  that  they  do),  while  on  p.  187  he  has  Audubon  contending  (as 
he  did)  that  they  do  not.  These  small  matters  notwithstanding,  this  is  probably  the  best  brief 
biography  of  Audubon  to  date. 

The  chief  of  Audubon’s  several  works,  the  so-called  double  elephant  “folio”  (actually 
broadsheet)  stands  as  the  most  ambitious  effort  at  bookmaking  in  history,  and  is  exceeded 
in  physical  size  only  by  the  Napoleonic  atlases  of  Egypt.  It  is  itself  the  subject  of  an  ex- 


294 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


haustive  monograph  by  Waldemar  Tries,  the  third  of  the  items  in  hand.  This,  which  shows 
every  evidence  of  relentless  and  careful  scholarship,  is  an  indispensable  reference  for  the 
serious  student  of  Auduboniana,  containing  not  only  the  details  of  the  undertaking:  history, 
subscribers,  costs,  sets  and  censuses  thereof,  but  also  appendices  covering  related  editions, 
reproductions,  prospectuses,  fates  of  copper  plates,  chronology  of  engraving  variants,  and 
related  miscellany. 

This  notice  will  complete  the  record  in  drawing  the  last  work  to  the  attention  of  Wilson 
Bulletin  readers.  That  it  did  not  appear  as  a contemporary  review  owes  to  one  of  the  present 
writer’s  occasional  lapses  into  terminal  procrastination.  Having  now  partially  made  amends, 
I shall  cease  avoiding  review  editor  Raikow  at  professional  meetings. — ROBERT  M.  Mengel. 


The  Warblers  of  America.  Edited  by  Ludlow  Griscom  and  Alexander  Sprunt,  Jr.  Re- 
vised and  updated  by  Edgar  M.  ReiUy,  Jr.  lUus.  by  John  Henry  Dick.  Doubleday  and  Co.,  Inc., 
Garden  City,  New  York,  1979:xv  + 302  pp.,  35  color  plates.  $19.95. — The  first  edition  of 
this  book  (1957)  was  apparently  a hastily  gathered  and  loosely  organized  work  assembled  as 
a frame  of  reference  for  John  Henry  Dick’s  color  plates.  Several  chapters  treated  general 
matters  such  as  parulid  taxonomy,  methods  of  study  and  song.  Then  followed  brief  accounts 
of  the  plumage  and  aspects  of  the  life  history  of  all  species  covered  by  the  A.O.U.  Check- 
list. Next,  a series  of  chapters  dealt  with  the  warblers  of  geographic  regions  lying  outside 
the  contiguous  United  States.  Dick’s  plates  depicted  adults  and,  in  many  cases,  sub-adults 
of  all  parulid  species  recognized  in  1957.  Finally,  brief  appendices  gave  further  attention  to 
systematics  and  described  certain  migration  routes.  The  manuscript  of  that  edition  had  been 
read  by  various  reviewers  and  authorities;  their  comments  and  additions,  instead  of  being 
integrated  into  the  text,  were  incorporated  as  bracketed  annotations. 

Although  this  second  edition  is  said  to  be  both  revised  and  updated,  nothing  has  been 
done  to  integrate  the  original  hodgepodge.  The  revisions  are  exceedingly  limited  and  deal 
principally  with  taxonomic  changes,  and  the  updating  consequently  ignores  much  of  the  work 
of  the  last  20  years.  I see  no  justification  for  republishing  the  book.  Compared  to  other  recent 
volumes  that  summarize  knowledge  of  entire  familes,  e.g..  Nelson  on  the  sulids  or  Hancock 
and  Elliott  on  the  herons,  it  falls  woefully  short. 

Two  early  chapters  on  song  still  describe  vocalizations  in  the  old-fashioned  way,  reporting 
such  qualities  as  hoarseness,  wiriness,  etc.,  and  endeavoring  to  spell  songs  out  in  the  letters 
of  the  alphabet.  This  was  acceptable  in  1957,  but  it  is  not  acceptable  today,  when  even  field 
guides  present  sound  spectrograms.  (W.  W.  H.  Gunn,  co-author  of  one  of  the  song  chapters, 
informs  me  he  was  given  no  opportunity  to  revise  his  chapter  and  indeed  was  not  told  that 
it  was  to  be  republished.)  Ludlow  Griscorn’s  chapter  entitled  “The  techniques  of  warbler 
study”  is  particularly  unfortunate.  The  novice  consulting  it  will  almost  surely  conclude  that 
the  study  of  warblers  consists  largely  of  learning  what  they  look  like,  counting  them  and 
recording  locations  and  dates  of  observation.  Nor  will  he  or  she  be  encouraged  to  attempt 
even  these  limited  tasks  after  reading  Griscom's  statement  that  expertise  calls  for  talents 
“completely  heyond  the  natural  capacity  of  most  individuals.”  Those  with  sufficient  hardi- 
hood to  continue  in  the  face  of  such  odds  are  instructed  in  heavily  pontifical  language  which 
too  often  has  little  substance.  “I  list  below  the  various  stages  by  which  expertness  in  warbler 
study  may  be  attained:  I.  Clearly  the  first  step  is  to  acquire  an  awareness  of  warblers.” 

Most  of  the  species  accounts  are  by  Sprunt,  who  is  inclined  to  tell  the  reader  less  about 
a species  than  about  how  he  feels  toward  it.  Some  warblers  are  clowns,  other  sprites  or 
animated  jewels;  they  are  petite,  dainty,  breathtaking,  enchanting;  their  haunting  melodies 
are  as  played  on  the  pipes  of  Pan.  Sprunt  at  times  reports  as  fact  points  for  which  it  is  hard 
to  imagine  the  supporting  data.  Can  he  have  had  evidence  for  Orange-crowned  Warblers 


ORN ITHOLOGIC AL  LITERATURE 


295 


{Vermivora  celata)  that  “even  normal  migrational  hazards  [do]  not  seem  ...  to  affect  them 
as  much  as  many  other  species,”  and  what  other  species  had  he  in  mind?  On  the  other  hand, 
when  facts  are  available,  he  sometimes  appears  either  to  ignore  them  or  to  write  so  loosely 
as  to  mislead.  For  example,  he  states  that  Prairie  Warblers  {Dendroica  discolor)  in  the  north 
are  victimized  by  cowbirds  {Molothrus  sp.)  and  that  in  the  south,  where  cowbirds  are  absent, 
racoons  and  snakes  sometimes  take  a toll,  as  though  these  threats  to  nest  success  replace 
each  other.  Those  species  reports  contributed  by  other  authors,  e.g..  Van  Tyne  on  Kirtland’s 
Warbler  (/).  kirtlandii),  are  often  more  factual  and  informative. 

Considering  that  the  book  has  color  plates  of  all  species  and  that  even  the  most  casually 
interested  person  can  be  expected  to  have  a field  guide,  a great  deal  of  space  in  the  species 
narratives  could  have  been  saved  had  the  rather  superficial  plumage  descriptions  been  elim- 
inated. Range  maps  (breeding  ranges  only)  are  presented  with  each  account  and  are  said  in 
the  preface  to  have  been  revised  for  this  edition.  I feel  qualified  to  comment  on  only  1 map: 
that  for  the  Prairie  Warbler  was  not  accurate  in  1957,  and  the  revised  version  has  not  corrected 
the  errors. 

The  most  useful  chapters  in  the  book  are  those  summarizing  the  warblers  of  Mexico, 
Central  America,  the  West  Indies,  Panama  and  South  America.  I found  Bond’s  contribution 
on  Central  America  and  Eisenmann’s  on  Panama  particularly  well  organized  and  helpful.  In 
contrast,  the  chapters  on  Alaska,  British  Columbia,  the  prairie  provinces  of  Canada  and 
eastern  Canada  overlap  a good  deal.  And  since  they  deal  with  the  species  covered  by  the 
A.O.U.  Check-list,  they  contain  much  material  that  either  duplicates  the  species  accounts 
or  that  probably  would  have  been  more  effectively  presented  in  good,  factual  species  ac- 
counts. This  is  not  the  fault  of  the  contributors  of  these  chapters,  who  wrote  to  fulfill  their 
assignments  from  the  editors  of  the  first  edition. 

If  the  foregoing  is  largely  critical  of  what  the  book  originally  contained,  I find  even  more 
fault  with  the  editorial  policy  that  preserved  the  earlier  work  at  the  expense  of  most  of  the 
recent  (and  not  so  recent)  studies  of  warblers.  Omitted,  for  example,  are  findings  of  Mayfield 
on  Kirtland’s  Warbler,  of  M.  Ficken  on  the  American  Redstart  (Setophaga  ruticilla),  of 
Foster  on  the  Orange-crowned  Warbler,  of  various  students  on  the  Blue-  (F.  pinus)  and 
Golden-winged  [V.  chrysoptera)  warblers  and  their  hybrids,  and  of  work  on  the  Prairie  War- 
bler. Meanley’s  study  of  Swainson’s  Warbler  {Limnothlypis  swainsonii)  is  mentioned  only  in 
a bracketed  1-sentence  interpolation.  Other  warbler  research  of  importance  is  also  ignored, 
e.g.,  MacArthur  on  population  ecology,  Morse  on  foraging  and  on  song,  Mengel  on  speciation, 
the  Fickens  on  comparative  ethology.  Van  Tyne  refers  to  banding  and  some  of  the  results 
produced  by  that  method  of  studying  Kirtland’s  Warbler,  but  I recall  no  other  reference  to 
banding;  the  significant  population  data  that  it  has  generated  are  not  discussed  anywhere. 

I am  not  competent  to  evaluate  Dick’s  plates.  Those  interested  in  that  feature  of  the  book 
may  want  to  refer  to  George  Sutton’s  critical  analysis  in  his  review  of  the  first  edition  (Auk 
75:226-228,  1958).— Val  Nolan,  Jr. 


To  A Young  Bird  Artist:  Letters  from  Louis  Agassiz  Fuertes  to  George  Miksch 
Sutton.  Commentary  by  George  Miksch  Sutton.  Univ.  Oklahoma  Press,  Norman,  Okla- 
homa, 1979:147  pp.,  4 color  plates,  5 details  of  Fuertes’  letters.  $9.95. — A tragic  accident 
in  August  of  1927  cut  short  the  very  abundant  career  of  perhaps  the  greatest  bird  painter  of 
all  time — Louis  Agassiz  Fuertes.  There  is  a resultant  sense  of  loss  not  only  for  the  man,  but 
also  for  the  work  never  produced  due  to  his  untimely  death.  Consequently,  any  new  work 
dealing  with  Fuertes  merits  celebration  and  a book  such  as  this,  which  deals  so  intimately 
with  Fuertes  as  an  individual,  as  a painter  and  as  the  genius  he  was,  deserves  special 
attention. 


296 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


This  book  consists  of  the  letters  Louis  Agassiz  Fuertes  wrote  to  George  Miksch  Sutton 
between  the  years  of  1915  and  1927,  interspersed  with  Sutton’s  commentary  on  those  letters, 
on  Fuertes  and  on  their  collective  influence  on  him.  The  correspondence  took  place  while 
Fuertes  was  at  the  height  of  his  career  and  while  Sutton  was  struggling  through  the  early 
stages  of  his  development.  The  correspondence  ended  with  Fuertes’  death.  Through  the 
letters  and  narrative,  the  reader  gains  insight  into  the  lives  of  both  Fuertes  and  Sutton  and 
their  special  relationship.  But  this  book  offers  more  than  that.  If  the  reader  will  endeavor  to 
truly  understand  the  principles  presented,  he  will  have  gained  an  awareness  and  knowledge 
enabling  him  to  better  understand  not  only  Fuertes  and  Sutton,  but  all  of  bird  art.  In  this 
sense,  the  book  is  a primer  on  bird  art  which  is  valuable  not  just  to  those  interested  in 
painting  but  to  anyone  who  is  interested  in  birds — for  birds  and  bird  art  are  inseparable. 
Interest  in  birds  is  highly  dependent  upon  our  visual  sense  and  it  is  in  the  visual  sense  that 
this  book  has  its  one  fault.  For  a book  dealing  with  bird  art  and  bird  artists,  it  does  not  seem 
to  fully  realize  its  potential  in  terms  of  illustration.  But  this  fault  is  acceptable  in  that  the 
book  is  intended  to  give  the  reader  an  understanding  of  bird  art  and  this  it  does  success- 
fully.— Larry  Barth. 


Parrots,  Their  Care  and  Breeding.  By  Rosemary  Low.  Blandford  Press  Ltd.,  Poole, 
Dorset,  United  Kingdom.  Distributed  in  the  U.S.A.  by  Sterling  Publishing,  Inc.,  New  York, 
New  York,  1980:654  pp.,  91  color  photos.  $55.00. — Until  recently,  parrot  aviculture  has  been 
mainly  restricted  to  the  maintenance  of  non-breeding  exhibit  collections,  or  household  pets. 
The  parrot  was  regarded  as  a household  fixture,  like  the  lap  dog,  or  the  fern.  Since  it  was 
soon  discovered  to  be  noisy,  messy  and  sometimes  destructive,  the  bird  was  probably  kept 
in  some  out-of-the-way  corner  of  the  house,  permanently  confined  to  a small  cage,  and  fed 
an  unvarying  diet  of  hard  seed.  Similarly,  with  few  exceptions,  zoos  maintained  collections 
to  satisfy  the  curiosity  of  the  public,  and  made  no  effort  to  provide  conditions  that  might 
promote  breeding.  During  the  last  50  years  the  increasing  awareness  of  ecology,  and  the 
application  of  scientific  techniques  to  animal  care  has  caused  a change  in  approach.  Parrots 
are  now  more  often  provided  with  diets  suited  to  their  specific  needs,  and  circumstances 
that  promote  a better  psychological  outlook.  Increasing  import  restrictions  and  depletion  of 
wild  populations  have  stimulated  more  and  more  parrot  fanciers  to  become  parrot  breeders. 

Whether  one  is  interested  in  the  proper  maintenance  of  a single  pet,  or  in  a large-scale 
breeding  program,  this  book  wiU,  by  itself,  supply  the  necessary  information.  Low  has  pro- 
duced the  most  comprehensive  and  scientifically  based  work  on  parrot-keeping  to  date.  The 
book  is  largely  a compilation  of  her  own  and  other  aviculturists’  experiences,  tempered  with 
biological  explanations.  The  result  is  a large  amount  of  information  presented  in  a very 
readable  manner. 

riie  book  is  divided  into  2 parts.  In  part  one,  topics  pertinent  to  the  care  of  any  type  of 
parrot  are  considered.  The  problems  of  choosing,  housing  and  feeding  are  discussed  in  detail. 
A constant  theme  of  these  chapters  is  that  prospective  parrot-keepers  be  practical  in  their 
choice  of  birds.  The  means  and  life-style  of  the  keepers  must  be  matched  with  the  needs  of 
their  parrots.  For  example.  Low  would  abhor  the  thought  of  a tame  Blue  and  Yellow  Macaw 
(Ara  ararauna)  living  in  a small  apartment  whose  owner  was  away  at  work  10  h a day. 

Specific  directions  for  aviary  construction  are  given,  along  with  suggestions  for  accom- 
modations in  colder  climates.  In  a section  on  aviary  management,  both  daily  and  long-range 
tasks,  as  well  as  emergency  measures,  are  discussed.  One  of  the  strongest  points  of  the  book 
is  the  detailed  coverage  of  feeding  requirements.  Commonly  encountered  seeds,  vegetables 
and  fruits  are  analyzed  for  food  value  and  vitamin  content.  Low  dispenses  with  the  commonly- 
held  and  harmful  notions  that:  (1)  aU  parrots,  other  than  lories  and  lorikeets,  eat  about  the 
same  thing — hard  seeds  of  one  size  or  another,  and  (2)  parrot  diets  should  be  conservative 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURL 


297 


and  unvaried.  Most  species  of  the  genus  Amazona,  she  feels,  could  do  very  well  on  the  diet 
of  their  keepers. 

The  chapters  related  to  breeding  give  excellent,  up-to-date  summaries  on  all  aspects  of 
the  subject.  Low’s  discussion  of  pair-bonding  points  out  a much  neglected  area  in  parrot- 
keeping. Some  species  bond,  and  some  do  not,  and  an  awareness  of  this  is  essential  for 
proper  aviary  management,  as  well  as  understanding  the  behavior  of  a tame  pet.  Methods 
of  sexing,  appropriate  nesting  materials  and  special  dietary  needs  of  breeding  parrots  are 
discussed.  Low  advocates  hand-rearing  not  just  as  an  emergency  measure,  or  a means  of 
creating  pets,  but  because  she  has  found  that  it  increases  breeding  success.  Guidelines  for 
construction  of  incubators  and  preparation  of  formulae  are  provided. 

Although  there  is  no  doubt  that  Low’s  primary  interest  is  in  breeding  parrots,  the  chapter 
on  maintaining  pets  is  exceptional.  The  over-riding  point  here  is  that  pet  parrots  should  be 
regarded  as  amusing  and  time-consuming  companions,  not  merely  as  mimics  or  household 
adornments.  Accordingly,  as  many  suggestions  are  given  for  their  psychological  as  for  their 
physical  well-being. 

Part  one  is  concluded  with  a chapter  on  sick  birds.  Anyone  who  has  been  subjected  to  a 
sick  parrot  can  understand  why  this  is  a relatively  neglected  area  in  aviculture.  Sick  birds 
deteriorate  very  rapidly,  and  even  if  proper  diagnosis  can  be  made,  treatment  is  often  un- 
certain and  more  traumatic  than  the  ailment.  Low,  nevertheless,  competently  summarizes 
what  is  known  about  sick  parrots  in  general,  along  with  methods  of  prevention,  diagnosis 
and  treatment  of  specific  illnesses.  Much  of  the  information  is  certainly  based  on  her  own 
extensive  experience. 

The  remainder  of  the  book,  part  two,  is  devoted  to  the  care  and  breeding  of  particular 
species.  AH  genera  and  most  species  of  parrots  encountered  in  aviculture  are  discussed 
individually.  Each  chapter  covers  a general  category  of  birds  such  as  Australian  parakeets, 
macaws,  or  hanging  parrots,  based  on  traditional  taxonomic  opinions.  Following  general 
comments  on  each  group,  there  is  a subsection  devoted  to  each  genus  and  species  that 
includes  range,  habitat,  description,  avicultural  history,  feeding,  accommodation  and  breed- 
ing in  sufficient  detail  to  prepare  any  prospective  aviculturist  for  maintenance  of  the  bird. 
Comments  on  natural  history  are  brief,  the  reader  being  referred  elsewhere  for  this  matter. 
Although  the  descriptions  are  excellent,  the  book  would  be  enhanced  by  more  color  plates. 
Those  plates  that  are  included  are  of  good  quality,  and  seem  to  have  been  selected  to 
represent  both  typical  and  aberrant  examples  of  the  various  parrot  groups. 

In  addition  to  her  obvious  delight  in  aviculture,  Rosemary  Low  has  an  objective  that  is 
both  openly  expressed  and  implied  throughout  her  book — the  elimination  of  the  necessity  of 
importing  birds  from  their  natural  habitats.  Aviculture  can  still  be  a hobby,  but  it  should  be 
refined  so  that  it  is  possible  to  acquire  any  bird,  whether  for  breeding  or  as  a pet,  from 
domestic  stocks.  Low  has  certainly  done  her  part  toward  making  this  a reality. 

I recommend  this  book  highly  for  anyone  interested  in  parrots,  and  for  general,  as  well  as, 
scientific  libraries.  The  price  is  a bit  high,  but  the  thorough  and  up-to-date  coverage  makes 
acquisition  of  other  books  on  parrot  aviculture  unnecessary. — SuSAN  L.  Berman. 

Lovebirds  and  Their  Color  Mutations.  By  Jim  Hayward.  Blandford  Press,  Ltd., 
Poole,  Dorset,  United  Kingdom.  Distributed  in  the  U.S.A.  by  Sterling  Publishing,  Inc.,  New 
York,  New  York,  1980:108  pp.,  32  color  photos,  numerous  black-and-white  photos  and  draw- 
ings. $14.95. — This  pleasant  little  book  covers  everything  one  needs  to  know  about  breeding 
lovebirds  (Agapornis  spp.),  from  the  maintainance  of  a simple  aviary  to  the  production  of 
the  rare  and  fancy  color  mutations.  Because  of  their  beauty,  small  size  and  willingness  to 
breed  in  captivity,  lovebirds  have  long  been  popular  among  aviculturists.  The  increasing 
variety  of  color  mutations  in  recent  years  has  been  an  added  incentive  for  breeding  them. 


298 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


In  spite  of  these  features,  lovebirds  have  never  attained  the  avicultural  status  of  the  Bud- 
gerigar, probably  because  of  their  shortcomings  as  pets.  Even  tame  lovebirds  can  be  pug- 
nacious toward  their  keepers  at  times,  and  their  nesting  habits  cause  them  to  be  destructive 
when  left  free  and  unsupervised  in  a household.  Accordingly,  the  book  contains  no  charming 
anecdotes  about  favorite  pets.  Hayward  discusses  only  topics  related  to  their  breeding. 

The  style  is  chatty  and  agreeable  if  one  can  overlook  the  occasional  misuse  of  words. 
Hayward’s  approach  is  that  of  a hobbyist,  not  a scientist,  emphasizing  the  “how-to,”  but 
seldom  the  “why.” 

There  are  4 chapters  on  subjects  of  a general  nature.  Aviary  construction  and  management 
is  covered  briefly,  and  adds  nothing  not  found  in  most  standard  parrot  books.  The  chapter 
on  feeding,  although  brief,  is  of  value  in  being  based  on  Hayward’s  extensive  experience  as 
a lovebird  breeder.  The  best  source  of  information  on  feeding  any  bird  is  one  who  has  bred 
them,  as  most  birds  won’t  breed  unless  they  are  maintained  on  an  appropriate  diet. 

Likewise,  Hayward’s  experience  is  evident  in  the  2 chapters  on  problems  related  to  breed- 
ing and  health.  In  a concise  but  thorough  manner,  he  summarizes  the  common  difficulties, 
their  prevention  and  treatment.  Brand  names  of  medication  and  precise  dosages  are  given. 
Hayward  is  particularly  helpful  in  providing  directions  for  administering  the  appropriate 
treatment.  AH  too  often  in  avicultural  literature  one  is  not  told  how  to  get  that  antibiotic  or 
worming  medication  into  the  bird.  Hayward  accomplishes  this  verbally  and  with  the  aid  of 
photographs. 

The  major  portion  of  the  book  is  devoted  to  descriptions  of  the  various  lovebird  species 
and  their  color  mutations.  The  reader  is  prepared  for  the  emphasis  on  the  latter  area  by  a 
short  and  very  elementary  chapter  on  bird  genetics.  Following  this,  a description  of  each 
species,  along  with  its  distribution  and  avicultural  status,  is  given.  Hayward  comments  on 
specific  food  and  housing  problems,  and  advises  which  species  are  more  likely  to  breed  in 
cold,  damp  climates.  For  species  commonly  yielding  color  mutations,  Hayward  provides 
“recipes”  for  producing  them — a handy  guide  for  the  beginner.  The  color  plates  are  of  good 
quality  and  exhibit  the  variety  and  beauty  of  these  little  parrots. 

Although  the  hook  is  unscientific  in  its  approach,  and  would  not  be  helpful  for  one  inter- 
ested in  the  behavior  or  natural  history  of  lovebirds.  I think  anyone  inclined  to  breed  them 
would  find  this  a useful  manual. — SuSAN  L.  Berman. 


Arctic  Summer;  Birds  in  North  Norway.  By  Richard  Vaughan.  Anthony  Nelson  Ltd., 
Salop,  England,  1979:152  pp.,  7 color  plates,  96  black-and-white  plates  with  captions,  2 
maps,  1 table.  £6.25.  Available  in  the  U.S..\.  from  Buteo  Books,  P.O.  Box  481,  Vermillion, 
South  Dakota  57069. — This  interesting  little  book  describes  the  author’s  38-day  bird-watching 
and  photography  adventure  on  the  Varanger  Peninsula  of  Norway’s  most  northerly  county, 
Einnmark.  The  hook  is  written  as  a naturalist’s  account  and  travelog  of  ornithological  events 
in  Einnmark  during  the  exceptionally  dry  spring  and  early  summer  (6  June-19  July)  of  1972. 
J'he  author's  photographs  are  excellent  and  comprise  over  half  the  book.  The  narrative  is 
personal  and  describes  the  immediate  events  surrounding  his  trip;  however,  Vaughan,  an 
historian  at  England’s  Hull  University,  also  contributes  a number  of  interesting  historical 
anecdotes,  and  provides  insights  into  the  life  histories  of  many  birds  in  the  area.  The  an- 
notated “Systematic  list  of  the  birds  of  the  Varanger  Peninsula”  at  the  end  of  the  book  is 
current  up  to  the  end  of  1978. 

In  some  parts,  complicated  sentences  and  overpunctuation  make  for  slow  reading.  How- 
ever. the  assets  of  the  book  outweight  its  minor  liabilities.  The  annotated  list,  along  with  the 
names  of  local  ornithologists  and  descriptions  of  the  best  camping  locations,  make  Arctic 
Summer  a valuable  campanion  for  bird  enthusiasts  visiting  northern  Norway. — STEPHEN  R. 
Johnson. 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


299 


Iceland  Summer:  Adventures  of  a Bird  Painter.  By  George  Miksch  Sutton.  Univ. 
Oklahoma  Press,  Norman,  Oklahoma,  1980  (1961):253  pp.,  7 color  plates,  numerous  mono- 
chrome sketches  and  photgraphs.  $5.95. — This  is  a reprinting  in  paper  covers  of  a book  first 
published  in  1961.  An  account  of  the  author’s  travels  in  search  of  birdlife,  it  received  the 
John  Burroughs  Medal  in  1962. — R.J.R. 

The  Complete  Birds  of  the  World.  By  Michael  Walters.  David  & Charles,  Inc.,  North 
Pomfret,  Vermont,  1980:340  pp.  $35.50. — This  book  consists  of  an  annotated  list  of  all  extant 
and  recently  extinct  species  of  birds  of  the  world.  They  are  listed  by  family  (but  not  by 
order),  and  the  basic  classification  followed  is  that  of  the  Peters  Check-List,  modified  in  a 
few  cases  by  reference  to  more  recent  works.  Although  it  was  published  in  1980,  work  on 
the  manuscript  ceased  in  1977,  so  some  recent  references,  including  Vol.  8 and  the  revised 
Vol.  1 of  Peters,  were  not  consulted.  For  each  species  there  are  a few  sentences  summarizing 
data  on  distribution,  habitat,  food,  nest-site,  clutch-size,  sexes  that  incubate  and  period  of 
incubation,  and  fledging  period.  This  is  a reference  work  that  aims  for  breadth  of  coverage 
rather  than  depth.  MisspeUings  are  numerous. 

Of  what  use  is  such  a book?  The  intended  purpose  is  not  explained.  It  could  be  helpful 
if  one  wanted  to  known  the  taxonomic  composition  of  a particular  family,  or  for  a very- 
sketchy  outline  of  its  biological  features.  The  book  could  have  been  made  much  more  useful 
by  providing  an  index  to  genera,  species  and  English  common  names,  instead  of  only  to 
families.  One  could  then  have  used  it,  for  instance,  to  identify  unfamilar  forms  encountered 
in  reading.  As  it  stands,  however,  to  do  this  one  must  first  know  the  family  and  then  search 
for  the  form  in  question.  In  a large  family  this  can  be  very  tedious.  In  short,  this  volume 
represents  a good  idea  whose  potential  was  not  fully  developed. — ROBERT  J.  Raikow. 


A Manual  for  Bird  Watching  in  the  Americas.  By  Donald  S.  Heintzelman.  Universe 
Books,  New  York,  New  York,  1979:255  pp.,  8 color  plates,  numerous  black-and-white  photos. 
$17.95. — In  this  book  Heintzelman  combines  a potpourri  of  practical  information  on  bird- 
watching with  extensive  discussions  of  birding  throughout  the  Americas  based  on  his  own 
travels.  The  first  part  deals  with  equipment  and  literature,  especially  field  guides,  and  in- 
cludes names  and  addresses  of  birding  organizations.  There  are  also  chapters  on  group 
activities  such  as  Christmas  bird  counts  and  Big  Day  counts,  nest  record  programs,  feeders 
and  the  like.  Much  of  this  will  be  familiar  to  experienced  birders,  but  beginners  should  find 
some  useful  information.  The  rest  of  the  book  gives  accounts  of  birding  trips  in  various  areas 
of  North  and  South  America,  as  well  as  Arctic  and  Antarctic  regions.  There  are  also  chapters 
on  watching  particular  groups  of  birds,  such  as  waterfowl,  hawks,  owls,  shorebirds  and 
warblers.  The  color  photos  are  of  good  quality,  but  many  of  the  black-and-white  photos  lack 
sharpness  and  contrast. — R.J.R. 


Call  Collect,  Ask  for  Birdman.  By  James  M.  Vardaman.  St.  Martin’s  Press,  New 
York,  New  York,  1980:256  pp.,  8 pp.  of  black-and-white  photos,  2 appendices,  charts, 
sighting  ledger,  map.  $10.95. — In  1 year,  1979,  the  author  attempted  to  see  700  of  the  800- 
plus  North  American  bird  species.  This  book  recounts  that  effort,  for  which  he  spent  $44,000, 
traveled  161,000  miles  and  fell  1 bird  short  of  his  goal.  The  book  is  not  about  birds,  but 
rather  about  the  strategy  and  logistics  of  finding  them.  Vardanian’s  approach  to  bird-watching 
brings  out  one  aspect  of  “ABA-ism”  at  its  worst — the  appreciation  of  birds  only  as  an  offshoot 
in  the  quest  for  a longer  life-list  (year-list,  in  this  case).  He  includes  copious  details  about 
the  “super-birders”  who  found  most  of  the  birds  for  him,  but  practically  no  information  is 
given  about  key  field  marks,  plumages,  or  behavior  of  the  birds  themselves.  Other  organized 


300 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  2,  June  1981 


attempts  to  set  high-count  birding  records  have  generated  money  for  conservation  causes. 
Vardanian  apparently  made  no  effort  to  grace  his  weU-publicized  “Big  Year”  by  soliciting 
such  donations.  Buyers  of  this  book  are  reimbursing  the  author’s  forestry  consulting  firm  for 
the  cost  of  the  Big  Year  attempt.  Birders  may  find  interesting  the  locations  of  hard-to-find 
species;  biologists  will  find  the  book  of  no  value. — Mark  Holmgren. 


The  Birds  of  Brevard  County.  By  Allan  D.  Cruickshank,  edited  by  Helen  G.  Cruick- 
shank.  Florida  Press,  Inc.,  Orlando,  Florida,  1980?  (undated):xv  -f-  204  pp.,  1 map,  1 black- 
and-white  photo,  hard  cover.  $12.00. — This  is  a collection  of  records  of  birds  occurring  in 
Brevard  County,  Florida,  as  compiled  from  1950-1974  by  the  late  Allan  D.  Cruickshank, 
with  contributions  by  other  observers,  and  added  records  (1974-1978)  by  Robert  D.  Barber. 
The  book  includes  a map  of  Brevard  County,  an  introduction  by  Helen  G.  Cruickshank  and 
a brief  biography  of  the  author.  The  bulk  of  the  book  is  devoted  to  the  records,  which  include 
the  following  information,  where  appropriate,  for  each  species:  common  and  scientific  names, 
arrival  dates,  dates  of  maximum  abundance,  winter  visitants,  departure  dates,  breeding 
status,  egg  dates  and  status  as  a resident  or  migrant.  This  work  should  be  a useful  aid  to 
birders  on  the  east  coast  of  Florida. — R.J.R. 


Bird  Finding  in  Tennessee.  By  Michael  Lee  Bierly.  Published  by  and  available  from  the 
author  at  3825  Bedford  Ave.,  NashviUe,  Tennessee  37215,  1980:255  pp.,  numerous  maps, 
paper  cover.  $8.00  postpaid. — One  hundred-twelve  birding  areas  in  Tennessee  are  discussed 
with  remarks  on  habitat,  travel  information  and  expected  species.  Several  large-scale  and 
numerous  local  maps  pinpoint  birding  areas.  In  addition,  there  are  brief  comments  on  status 
and  abundance  of  the  342  species  recorded  from  the  state,  a list  of  birders  in  various  areas 
who  can  be  contacted  for  local  information  and  a compilation  of  local  chapters  of  the  Ten- 
nessee Ornithological  Society. — R.J.R. 


Welcome  the  Birds  to  Your  Home.  By  Jane  and  Will  Curtis.  The  Stephen  Greene 
Press,  Brattleboro,  Vermont,  1980:154  pp.,  paper  cover.  $5.95. — Chatty  advice  on  attracting 
and  studying  birds,  and  a bit  of  painless  natural  science  are  the  features  of  this  little  book 
aimed  at  the  beginning  amateur  ornithologist. — R.J.R. 


This  issue  of  The  Wilson  Bulletin  was  published  on  20  August  1981. 


The  Wilson  Bulletin 


Editor  Jon  C.  Barlow 

Department  of  Ornithology 

Royal  Ontario  Museum 

100  Queen’s  Park 

Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6 
Assistant  Editor  MARGARET  L,  May 
Senior  Editorial  Assistants  Gary  Bortolotti 
Nancy  Flood 

Editorial  Assistants  Keith  L.  Bildstein  Richard  R.  Snell 

C.  Davison  Ankney  James  D.  Rising 

Review  Editor  Robert  Raikow  Color  Plate  Editor  WiLLiAM  A.  Lunk 

Department  of  Biological  Sci-  865  North  Wagner  Road 

ences  Ann  Arbor,  MI  48103 

University  of  Pittsburgh 
Pittsburgh,  PA  15260 

Suggestions  to  Authors 

See  Wilson  Bulletin  91:366,  1979  for  more  detailed  “Suggestions  to  Authors.’’ 
Manuscripts  intended  for  publication  in  The  Wilson  Bulletin  should  be  submitted  in  tripli- 
cate, neatly  typewritten,  double-spaced,  with  at  least  3 cm  margins,  and  on  one  side  only  of 
good  quality  white  paper.  Do  not  submit  xerographic  copies  that  are  made  on  slick,  heavy 
paper.  Tables  should  be  typed  on  separate  sheets,  and  should  be  narrow  and  deep  rather 
th.in  wide  and  shallow.  Follow  the  AOU  Check-list  (Fifth  Edition,  1957)  and  the  32nd  Sup- 
plement (Auk,  90:411^19,  1973),  insofar  as  scientific  names  of  U.S.  and  Canadian  birds  are 
concerned.  Summaries  of  major  papers  should  be  brief  but  quotable.  Where  fewer  than  5 
papers  are  cited,  the  citations  may  be  included  in  the  text.  All  citations  in  “General  Notes’’ 
should  be  included  in  the  text.  Follow  carefully  the  style  used  in  this  issue  in  listing  the 
literature  cited;  otherwise,  follow  the  “CBE  Style  Manual’’  (1972,  AIBS).  Photographs  for 
illustrations  should  have  good  contrast  and  be  on  gloss  paper.  Submit  prints  unmounted  and 
attach  to  each  a brief  but  adequate  legend.  Do  not  write  heavily  on  the  backs  of  photographs. 
Diagrams  and  line  drawings  should  be  in  black  ink  and  their  lettering  large  enough  to  permit 
reduction.  Original  figures  or  photographs  submitted  must  be  smaller  than  22  X 28  cm. 
Alterations  in  copy  after  the  type  has  been  set  must  be  charged  to  the  author. 

Notice  of  Change  of  Address 

If  your  address  changes,  notify  the  Society  immediately.  Send  your  complete  new  address 
to  Ornithological  Societies  of  North  America,  % Sandra  L.  L.  Gaunt,  Box  21160,  Columbus, 
Ohio  43221. 

The  permanent  mailing  address  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  is:  do  The  Museum 
of  Zoology,  The  University  of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  Persons  having  business 
with  any  of  the  officers  may  address  them  at  their  various  addresses  given  on  the  back  of 
the  front  cover,  and  all  matters  pertaining  to  the  Bulletin  should  be  sent  directly  to  the 
Editor. 

Membership  Inquiries 

Membership  inquiries  should  be  sent  to  Dr.  Robert  C.  Whitmore,  Division  of  Forestry, 
West  Virginia  University,  Morgantown,  West  Virginia  26506. 


CONTENTS 


THE  RUFOUS-FACED  CRAKE  {LATERALLUS  XENOPTERUS)  AND  ITS  PARAGUAYAN  CONGENERS 

Robert  W.  Storer 

RESOURCE  USE  STRATEGIES  OF  WADING  BIRDS James  A.  Kushlan 

AGE  RATIOS  AND  THEIR  POSSIBLE  USE  IN  DETERMINING  AUTUMN  ROUTES  OF  PASSERINE 

MIGRANTS  C.  John  Ralph 

WEATHER,  MIGRATION  AND  AUTUMN  BIRD  KILLS  AT  A NORTH  FLORIDA  TV  TOWER 

Robert  L.  Crawford 

CLIMATIC  INFLUENCES  ON  PRODUCTIVITY  IN  THE  HOUSE  SPARROW W.  Bruce  McGUUvray 

CANADA  GOOSE  BROOD  BEHAVIOR  AND  SURVIVAL  ESTIMATES  AT  CREX  MEADOWS,  WISCONSIN 

Michael  C.  Zicus 

BREEDING  BIRD  POPULATIONS  IN  THE  GREAT  SMOKY  MOUNTAINS,  TENNESSEE  AND  NORTH 

CAROLINA  S.  Charles  Kendeigh  and  Ben  J.  Fawver 

NEST-SITE  SELECTION  AMONG  AD^LIE,  CHINSTRAP  AND  GENTOO  PENGUINS  IN  MIXED  SPECIES 
ROOKERIES  Nicholas  J.  Volkman  and  Wayne  Trivelpiece 

COWBIRD  PARASITISM  AND  EVOLUTION  OF  ANTI-PARASITE  STRATEGIES  IN  THE  YELLOW 

WARBLER Karen  L.  Clark  and  Raleigh  J.  Robertson 

INTERACTIVE  BEHAVIOR  AMONG  BALD  EAGLES  WINTERING  IN  NORTH-CENTRAL  MISSOURI 

Curtice  R.  Griffin 

president’s  message 

GENERAL  NOTES 

INTERSPECIFIC  SONG  MIMESIS  BY  A LINCOLN  SPARROW 

Luis  F.  Baptista,  Martin  L.  Morton  and  Maria  E.  Pereyra 


NOTES  ON  PURPLE  GALLINULES  IN  COLOMBIAN  RICEFIELDS  Wallace  D.  McKay 

AGONISTIC  BEHAVIOR  OF  THE  WHITE-BREASTED  NUTHATCH Lawrence  Kilham 


EVASIVE  BEHAVIOR  OF  AMERICAN  COOTS  TO  KLEPTOPARASITISM  BY  WATERFOWL 

Mark  R.  Ryan 

ADDITIONAL  EVIDENCE  OF  EGG-MOVING  BEHAVIOR  BY  FEMALE  GADWALLS 

Robert  J.  Blohm 

MALLARD  USING  MOVING  VEHICLES  FOR  PREDATOR  AVOIDANCE 

Bruce  C.  Thompson  and  James  E.  Tabor 

OCHRACEOUS  WREN  FAILS  TO  RESPOND  TO  MOBBING  CALLS  IN  AN  HETEROSPECIFIC  FLOCK 

William  H.  Buskirk 

FISH  ATTACK  ON  BLACK  GUILLEMOT  AND  COMMON  EIDER  IN  MAINE  Thomas  W.  French 

CROWS  STEAL  GOLF  BALLS  IN  BANGLADESH  Richard  M.  Poche 

NOTES  ON  THE  STATUS  OF  THE  COMMON  AFRICAN  WAXBILL  IN  AMAZONIA 

David  C.  Oren  and  Nigel  J.  H.  Smith 

DISTRIBUTION  AND  REPRODUCTIVE  SUCCESS  OF  ZONE-TAILED  HAWKS  IN  WEST  TEXAS 

Sumner  W.  Matteson  and  John  0.  Riley 

THREE  CRESTED  EAGLE  RECORDS  FOR  GUATEMALA 

David  H.  Ellis  and  Wayne  H.  Whaley 

ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE  


137 

145 

164 

189  *1 
1% 

207 

218 

243 

Jl 

24^ 

259  Ij 

264 

265 
267 
271 

274 

276 

277 

278 

279 

280 

281  0 


284 

286 


WIL- 

■QH'-L.'i 


' idrapy  ' 


NOV  1 61981 

TfieWlsonBulte’ 


VARO 
SITy 


PUBLISHED  BY  THE  WILSON  ORNITHOLOGICAL  SOCIETY 


VOL.  93,  NO.  3 SEPTEMBER  1981  PAGES  301-456 


The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
Founded  December  3,  1888 

Named  after  ALEXANDER  WILSON,  the  first  American  Ornithologist. 

President — Abbot  S.  Gaunt,  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  Columbus,  Ohio 
43210. 

First  Vice-President — Jerome  A.  Jackson,  Department  of  Biological  Sciences,  P.O.  Drawer 
Z,  Mississippi  State  University,  Mississippi  State,  Mississippi  39762. 

Second  Vice-President — Clait  E.  Braun,  Wildlife  Research  Center,  317  West  Prospect  St., 
Fort  CoUins,  Colorado  80526 

Editor — Jon  C.  Barlow,  Department  of  Ornithology,  Royal  Ontario  Museum,  100  Queen’s 
Park,  Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6. 

Secretary — Curtis  S.  Adkisson,  Department  of  Biology,  Virginia  Polytechnic  Institute  and 
State  University,  Blacksburg,  Virginia  24061. 

Treasurer — Robert  D.  Burns,  Department  of  Biology,  Kenyon  College,  Gambler,  Ohio  43022. 

Elected  Council  Members — Richard  C.  Banks  (term  expires  1982);  Mary  H.  Clench  (term 
expires  1983);  Helen  Lapham  (term  expires  1984). 

Membership  dues  per  calendar  year  are:  Active,  $16.00;  Student,  $14.00;  Sustaining,  $25.00; 
Life  memberships  $250  (payable  in  four  installments). 

The  Wilson  Bulletin  is  sent  to  all  members  not  in  arrears  for  dues. 

The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library 
The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  housed  in 
the  University  of  Michigan  Museum  of  Zoology,  was  established  in  concurrence  with  the 
University  of  Michigan  in  1930.  Until  1947  the  Library  was  maintained  entirely  by  gifts  and 
bequests  of  books,  reprints,  and  ornithological  magazines  from  members  and  friends  of  the 
Society.  Now  two  members  have  generously  established  a fund  for  the  purchase  of  new 
books;  members  and  friends  are  invited  to  maintain  the  fund  by  regular  contribution,  thus 
making  available  to  aU  Society  members  the  more  important  new  books  on  ornithology  and 
related  subjects.  The  fund  will  be  administered  by  the  Library  Committee,  which  will  be 
happy  to  receive  suggestions  on  the  choice  of  new  books  to  be  added  to  the  Library.  William 
A.  Lunk,  University  Museums,  University  of  Michigan,  is  Chairman  of  the  Committee.  The 
Library  currently  receives  195  periodicals  as  gifts  and  in  exchange  for  The  Wilson  Bulletin. 
With  the  usual  exception  of  rare  books,  any  item  in  the  Library  may  be  borrowed  by  members 
of  the  Society  and  will  be  sent  prepaid  (by  the  University  of  Michigan)  to  any  address  in  the 
United  States,  its  possessions,  or  Canada.  Return  postage  is  paid  by  the  borrower.  Inquiries 
and  requests  by  borrowers,  as  well  as  gifts  of  books,  pamphlets,  reprints,  and  magazines, 
should  be  addressed  to:  The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library,  University  of  Michigan 
Museum  of  Zoology,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  Contributions  to  the  New  Book  Fund 
should  be  sent  to  the  Treasurer  (small  sums  in  stamps  are  acceptable).  A complete  index  of 
the  Library’s  holdings  was  printed  in  the  September  1952  issue  of  The  Wilson  Bulletin  and 
newly  acquired  books  are  listed  periodically.  A list  of  currently  received  periodicals  was 
published  in  the  December  1978  issue. 


The  Wilson  Bulletin 
(ISSN  0043-5643) 

The  official  organ  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  published  quarterly,  in  March,  June,  September,  and  December. 
The  subscription  price,  both  in  the  United  States  and  elsewhere,  is  $20.00  per  year.  Single  copies,  14.00.  Subscriptions, 
changes  of  address  and  claims  for  undelivered  copies  should  be  sent  to  the  Treasurer.  Most  back  issues  of  the  Bulletin  are 
available  and  may  be  ordered  from  the  Treasurer.  Special  prices  will  be  quoted  for  quantity  orders. 

All  articles  and  communications  for  publications,  books  and  pubUcations  for  reviews  should  be  addressed  to  the  Editor. 
Exchanges  should  be  addressed  to  The  Joselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library,  Museum  of  Zoology,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan 
48109.  Known  office  of  publication:  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  1827  Neil  Avenue,  Columbus,  Ohio 
43210. 

Second  class  postage  paid  at  Columbus,  Ohio  and  at  additional  maibng  office. 


(c)  Copyright  1981  by  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
Printed  by  Allen  Press,  Inc.,  Lawrence,  Kansas  66044,  U.S.A. 


] 


I 

f 

i, 

A 


i 


a 


(i 

4 


if 


.V 


Mainland  Rufous-backed  Robin  (Turdus  rufo-palliatus, 
and  Grayson’s  Robin  (T.  graysoni,  below).  Painting  by  Anne  Pulich. 


I 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN 

A QUARTERLY  MAGAZINE  OF  ORNITHOLOGY 

Published  by  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 

VoL.  93,  No.  3 September  1981  Pages  301-456 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  301-309 


SUBSPECIES  VS  FORGOTTEN  SPECIES: 

THE  CASE  OF  GRAYSON’S  ROBIN 
(TURDUS  GRAYSON  I) 

Allan  R.  Phillips 

Anyone  reading  ornithological  books  and  papers  of  the  past  30-50  years 
must  be  confused  about  subspecies.  On  one  side  is  the  tendency  to  reduce 
all  possible  species  to  subspecies,  as  long  as  they  are  thought  to  be  aUo- 
patric.  Biological  similarities  or  differences  tend  to  be  ignored,  especially 
because  we  know  so  little  about  them  in  many  tropical  species.  Once 
pronounced  a subspecies,  a bird  is  promptly  forgotten  by  most  ornitholo- 
gists, field  guides,  bird  watchers,  etc. 

On  the  other  side,  paralleling  the  anti-evolutionists  of  100  years  ago,  we 
have  the  anti-“subspecies  concept”  drive,  denying  the  reality  of  subspe- 
cies. In  the  recent  summary  of  avian  biology,  edited  by  Earner  and  King 
(1971),  we  read  that  “many  [unnamed]  avian  systematists  are  now  con- 
vinced that  the  subspecies  category  is  unsatisfactory  if  not  worthless”; 
while  a long  chapter  on  “Geographic  Variation”  (Earner  and  King  1971:76- 
92)  mentions  none  of  the  classic  striking  cases,  e.g.,  Motacilla  spp.  wag- 
tails or,  in  North  America,  Otus  spp.  owls  (Marshall  1967),  Junco  spp., 
and  various  other  emberizine  sparrows,  including  Pipilo  spp.  towhees 
(Phillips  1959). 

If  there  is  actually  a subspecies  concept,  it  is  that  such  very  unlike 
birds  interbreed  more  or  less  freely  to  form  a single  unit,  the  biological 
species.  In  other  genera,  on  the  other  hand,  much  subtler  differences 
separate  full  biological  species,  as  in  Empidonax  spp.  flycatchers  and 
Cisticola  spp.  and  Phylloscopus  spp.  warblers.  In  the  tropics,  there  may 
well  be  a number  of  similar  cases  where  our  fragmentary  data  have  led  to 
hasty  reduction  of  allopatric,  or  supposedly  allopatric,  species  to  subspe- 
cies. Let  us  examine  one  such  case,  that  of  Grayson’s  Robin  {Tardus 
graysoni)  of  Nayarit,  western  Mexico. 

301 


302 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


This  case  is  somewhat  parallel  to  that  of  The  Frantzius’  ( = Ruddy-capped) 
Nightingale-Thrush  {Catharus  frantzii);  both  T.  graysoni  and  C.  frantzii 
were  swept  into  oblivion  by  Hellmayr  (1934).  Whereas  some  authors  never 
accepted  Hellmayr’s  dictum  that  the  differences  between  C.  frantzii  and 
the  Russet  Nightingale-Thrush  (C.  occidentalis)  were  mere  individual  varia- 
tions (see  referenees  in  Phillips  1969),  Turdus  graysoni  disappeared  com- 
pletely except  as  a subspecies  in  technical  papers  (omitted  from  field  guides, 
as  are  most  subspecies).  Consequently,  we  still  lack  biological  evidence,  so 
ably  presented  for  Catharus  spp.  by  Rowley  and  Orr  (1964)  and  Raitt  and 
Hardy  (1970).  One  present  objective  is  to  awaken  interest  in  the  biology  of 
the  2 Tardus. 

In  recent  years,  ornithologists  and  bird-watching  tours  have  flocked  to 
San  Bias,  Nayarit,  by  the  hundreds.  Any  robins  seen  were  perforce  listed 
as  Rufous-backed  {T.  rufo-palliatus)  or  White-throated  robins  {T.  phae- 
opygus  of  Phillips,  unpubl.  [=assimilis]).  If  a T.  graysoni  was  seen,  read- 
ers of  Blake  (1953:423)  or  perhaps  Edwards  (1972)  might  call  it  a female 
T.  rufo-palliatus \ others  would  have  to  force  it  into  one  of  the  above  two 
forms  or  into  the  Clay-colored  Robin  {T.  grayi),  which  does  not  really 
occur  within  hundreds  of  kilometers  of  Nayarit.  This  cost  little  strain;  for 
now,  as  Robert  O.  Paxton  (1979),  president  of  the  Linnaean  Society  of 
New  York,  wrote  in  reviewing  recent  bird  books  in  a popular  magazine: 
“No  one  ‘sees’  a bird  in  totality,  feather  by  feather.  One  sees  parts  of  it, 
and  the  mind  fills  in  the  rest  by  guesswork  or  (if  one  knows  birds  already) 
by  memory.”  Thus,  sightings  of  robins  in  coastal  Nayarit  are  as  worthless 
scientifically  as  are  those  of  the  much  more  similar  kingbirds  (Tyrannus) 
of  the  Caribbean  slope  of  Mexico  and  Guatemala  (Traylor  1979). 

Grayson’s  Robin  was  considered  an  insular  form,  limited  to  the  Tres 
Marias  Islands  off  Nayarit,  until  Nelson  (1899)  reported  one  taken  at  San- 
tiago Ixcuintla,  on  the  opposite  mainland.  Hellmayr  (1934:356,  footnote) 
questioned  this,  writing  that  graysoni  “is  merely  a pale,  large-billed  race 
of  the  mainland  bird  [rufo-palliatus].  Certain  individuals  of  the  latter  in 
worn  breeding  plumage  closely  approach  it  in  coloration,  and  it  is  no  doubt 
on  such  a specimen  that  Nelson’s  record  of  T.  r.  graysoni  from  Santiago, 
Nayarit,  was  based.”  He  gave  no  measurements. 

Clearly,  Hellmayr  (1934)  should  have  examined  Nelson’s  bird.  Not  only 
is  it  very  dull,  but  it  is  larger  than  rufo-palliatus;  my  minimum  measure- 
ments may  be  compared  in  Table  1.  While  most  of  these  are  possible  for 
either  form,  the  tarsus  agrees  only  with  graysoni,  supporting  Nelson’s 
(1899)  identification.  The  longer  tarsus  of  graysoni,  overlooked  by  Hell- 
mayr (1934),  had  been  carefully  explained  by  Ridgway  (1907:106-107),  who 
gave  it  as  33-35  mm  (vs  30.5—34  mm  in  mainland  males,  30.5—32  mm  in 
females).  He  gave  the  exposed  culmen  as  22-23  mm  (vs  19-22.5  mm). 


Phillips  • GRAYSON’S  ROBIN? 


303 


Fig.  1.  Records  of  Grayson’s  (crosses)  and  White-throated  rohins  (circles)  in  the  lowlands 
of  western  Nayarit.  Upland  records  are  not  mapped,  but  upland  cities  (Tepic,  Compostela) 
are  shown  for  orientation.  (Upper  limit  of  mapping  records  is  500  m elev.)  Rufous-backed 
Robins  probably  occur  throughout  the  lowlands,  and  are  not  mapped. 


Table  1 

Measurements  oe  Turdus  spp.,  from  Nayarit  (Except  as  Noted) 


304 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


=5  B 


B 

S? 
y,  £ 


H E 


^ ^ 

^ 

LiO 

CO 

o 

00 

00 

NO 

CN-. 

uO 

CO 

csi 

CO 

LO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

1 

CO 

lo 

CO 

cd 

CO 

CO 

as 

7 

O 

II 

CO 

ON 

II 

CO 

CM 

II 

r-" 

cd 

CSj 

cd 

1— 1 

CO 

CM* 

CO 

CM 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CM 

CO 

o 

CO 

CS|  CO 
vd 


CM 

t-: 

I— I lO 

N 


CM  CO 
td  ^ 

■— I 

V 

fO  " 


NO 

lO 

CO 

CM 

CM 

1 

1 7 

CM* 

CM 

NO 

ud 

II 

CM 

II 

CM 

CM 

'T  r- 

CM  ^ 
CO 

T 

ON 

r-:  II 


^ II 

C/5  — 

^ lO 

I - 

i.  2 

o 


ON  ~r  cn 


fd 

CM 

7 ^ 

ON 

oo’  II 
7 -is. 


u 'r:, 

H 


ON 

CO 

1 

r}> 

o 

CO 

'C 

CM- 

CO 

II 

of 

II 

z 

o 

§ 

3 

z 

o3 

ud 

uO 

nO 

o 

NO 

ON 

0+  c 

ON 

c/T 

JS  o 

(fl 

O 
-c  — 
U 


CO  5^ 

I ^ 
00  II 


On 

ON 

, , 

CO 

CO 

CO 

ud 

NO 

' — ' 

d 

lO 

On 

d 

o 

CM 

NO 

o 

On* 

+ 

o 

o 

ud 

1 

O 

On* 

lON 

89 

1 

On 

1 

ON 

ud 

ON 

II 

iis- 

On 

>^* 

CO 

;is- 

NO 

On 

ON 

t}<* 

ON 

II 

liS- 

uo" 

ON 

CO 

II 

3 ^ - 

H oq 

*:  S II 

0 r-^ 

H ^ 


o -- 


CC 

£ II 


T.  rufo-palliatus 

Mainland,  3 3 II7.3-I28.2  91.8-105.4  19-22.5  12.2-14.6  29.4-33.8 

(Grant  1965)  (N  = 30-32)  (ic  = 123.19)  (r  = 99.29)  (r  = 20.5)  (x  = 13.26)  (x  = 31.48) 

San  Bias,  3 3 (N  = 7)  — — 17.2±-19.2±,  12.2-14,14.8  29.7-32.7,33.5 


Phillips  • GRAYSON’S  ROBIN? 


E 

£ E 


W ^ 

3 s 

CQ  H 
P— I 

^ CJ 


^ E 
t-  E 


E 


LO" 


CO  2 
CO 


+1  ^ 
CM  ^ 

sS 

I 

LO  u 

r<  O 


O 

ON  ^ 


3 " 


OS 

lO 

LO 

1 

LO 

r^* 

CO 

o 

CM* 

<7 

O 

CM 

Os* 

o^ 

T 

CM 

On 

II 

CO 

II 

II 

a:  o 

z 

z 

cn 

o 

"c? 

B 

Tf 

E S 
£ c 

cs 

, ^ 

' ' 

C 

LO 

NO 

ON 

0+ 

CH 

"O 

c 

cd 

"o 

CJ 

II 

E ^ 
1 ■- 

c/T 

c 

cd*^ 

z 

-o  3 

c 

cd 

Lh 
r K 

-2 

S 

iH 

D 

cd 

X 

cd 

cel 

li 

QJ 

c 

cd 

li 

3 

o < 

u ul 

c/D 

O 

2 a 

o E 


E S 

r ^ 


305 


306 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


[although,  as  has  sometimes  been  noted,  linear  extreme  measurements 
may  give  but  a poor  idea  of  differences  in  bill  size  (easily  distorted  by 
slight  differences  in  calipers  or  in  techniques;  cf.  for  example,  the  careful 
analyses  of  Knox  1976).]  The  accompanying  plate  (frontispiece)  of  graysoni 
was  painted  directly  from  Nelson’s  specimen,  which  is  worn  but  not  ex- 
cessively so. 

Nevertheless,  Hellmayr’s  (1934)  classification  has  been  followed  ever 
since,  even  by  Miller  et  al.  (1957),  with  Nelson’s  specimen  right  before 
one  of  the  co-authors  (Friedmann).  Grant  (1965)  identified  4 other  mainland 
specimens  as  graysoni,  also  from  coastal  Nayarit  (Sauta  and  Chacala); 
but  he  considered  all  these  to  be  stragglers  of  the  island  subspecies.  He 
reported  no  intermediates,  nor  have  1 identified  any. 

Grant  (1965:33)  also  cast  doubt  on  Ridgway’s  (1907)  reported  dif- 
ferences in  dorsal  colors,  writing  that  mainland  females,  on  the  back,  “are 
indistinguishable  from  all  island  birds”  and  males  only  “slightly  more  ru- 
fous.” But  Ridgway  (1907:105)  seems  to  me  quite  correct  in  calling  main- 
land females  “often  not  distinguishable,  but  usually  [?]  very  slightly  duller 
in  color”  than  mainland  males,  which  are  usually  strikingly  reddish- 
backed,  quite  unlike  graysoni.  There  is  a certain  range  of  variation  in  both 
sexes;  1 suspect  that  adult  males  are  more  consistently  bright  (reddish) 
dorsally  than  other  age/sex  classes.  Nevertheless,  aU  these  overlap  greatly, 
and  all  rufo-palliatus  seen  by  me  show  at  least  some  cinnamon-rufous  or 
chestnut  dorsally.  In  duU  extremes,  this  may  show  only  on  the  scapulars. 

I have,  however,  found  4 more  T.  graysoni  from  the  mainland  coast 
(San  Bias  and  west  of  Acaponeta).  That  all  nine  are  stragglers  from  the 
islands  is  hardly  believable,  for  they  show  a definite  distribution.  The  only 
somewhat  parallel  case,  where  the  island  race  has  been  taken  repeatedly 
on  and  near  the  mainland,  is  in  the  Parula  warblers.  Here  it  seems  clear 
that  the  “Tres  Marias  form”  {P.  americana  [=pitiayumi]  insularis)  com- 
pletely replaces  the  migratory  mainland  Tropical  Parula  {P.  americana  [ = 
pitiayumi]  pulchra)  in  the  breeding  season  in  the  coastal  mangroves  and 
islands;  the  2 races  are  allopatric  in  summer.  Turdus,  on  the  other  hand, 
is  not  regularly  migratory.  T.  rufo-palliatus  occurs,  and  appears  to  be 
irregularly  common,  at  most  or  all  of  the  mainland  points  where  T.  gray- 
soni has  been  taken.  If  it  alone  were  of  normal  occurrence,  it  should  far 
outnumber  graysoni  in  random  collecting.  But  in  fact,  along  the  im- 
mediate coast,  the  difference  is  not  very  great  (9  rufo-palliatus  to  6 gray- 
soni-, Table  2).  Furthermore,  collecting  is  not  all  random;  collectors  may 
prefer  pretty  or  distinctive  specimens.  When  I first  visited  Nayarit,  in  the 
1950’s,  I wanted  chiefly  distinctive  representatives  of  the  species:  either 
clearly  white-throated  or  definitely  reddish  birds.  If  in  fact  I ever  saw  any 
T.  graysoni,  I did  not  want  them,  being  quite  unaware  of  the  problems. 


Phillips  • GRAYSON’S  ROBIN? 


307 


Table  2 

Specimens  of  Turdus  Examined  from  Lowland  Nayarit'* 


T.  phaeopygus  { = assimilis) 
15  km  E,  San  Bias,  10  Feb.  1956 
8 km  E,  6 km  S,  San  Bias,  15  Apr.  1965 

10  km  E,  Las  Varas,  26  Mar.  1941 

15  road  km  E to  Las  Varas  area,  12-17  Nov.  1852,  17 
June  1970 

Lo  de  Marcos  (S  side),  5 Apr.  1955 
Total,  12  Nov. -17  Jun. 

T.  graysoni 

Novilleros,  4 Feb.  1966 
Sauta,  12  May  1940,  25  Apr.  1946 
Santiago  Ixcuintla,  20  June  1897 
San  Bias,  20,  25  Mar.  1948 
Chacala,  15-21,  Mar.  1941 

Total,  4 Feb. -20  June 

T.  rufo-palliatus 

8 km  S of  Acaponeta 
Sauta,  1-17  May 
Santiago  Ixcuintla,  1889 

San  Bias  and  vicinity,  10-19  Oct.,  27  Dec.,  4 May 
(1889,  1925,  1955,  1963) 

15  km  E,  San  Bias,  8-23  Feb. 

15  km  E to  Las  Varas  area, 

11  km  W,  Mazatan,  E Las  Varas  (lowlands?),  26  Dec. 
Chacala,  9 Mar.  1941 

Total 


1 9,  LSU'’ 

1 6,  UNM 
1,  RTM 

1 <J,  2 9 9,  ARP,  MEXU 

2 33,  2 9 9,  ARP 
4 33,  5 9 9 & 1? 


1 9 im.,  ARIZ 
13,19,  RTM 

1 9,  US 

3 9 9,  MVZ  RTM 

2 9 9,  RTM 

13,899 


6,  RTM 

2 33,  2 9 9,  RTM 
1 3,  BM 

5 3 3,  3 9 9,  BM,  CAS,  CM, 
MVZ 

3 33,  1 9,  LSU 
5,  RTM,  ARP 

1 9,  DEL 

I 9 , RTM 

II  33,  8 9 9 & 11  ( + ?) 


West  of  Tepic  area,  excluding  extreme  south. 

See  acknowledgments  for  abbreviations;  ARP,  author’s  private  collection. 


If  SO  many  graysoni  were  conspecific  stragglers,  we  should  expect  an 
influence  of  their  characters  in  coastal  rufo-palliatus.  But  neither  Grant 
(1965)  nor  I found  clear  evidence  of  this,  though  a few  specimens  (Moore 
Lab.  of  Zoology)  approach  graysoni  slightly  in  color,  being  duUer  than 
average  rufo-palliatus.  A regular  migration  from  and  to  the  islands  is  most 
unlikely;  and  such  a theory  is  not  favored  by  the  date  (20  June)  of  Nelson’s 
Santiago  specimen. 

Thus,  available  data  point  to  a mainland  coast  population  of  T.  graysoni, 
apparently  not  crossing  freely  (if  at  all)  with  the  sympatric  T.  rufo-pallia- 
tus. The  logical  conclusion  is  that  Turdus  graysoni  is  a good  species. 
However,  additional  specimens  and  biological  data  should  be  sought;  stud- 


308 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


ies  of  breeding  pairs  would  be  most  valuable,  as  well  as  nests,  eggs,  ju- 
veniles, vocalizations,  etc. 

Once  we  open  our  eyes  to  the  problem  and  abandon  our  preconceived 
idea  that  mainland  Grayson’s  Robins  must,  perforce,  be  strays  from  the 
islands,  it  becomes  unimportant  whether  or  not  they  coincide  exactly  with 
island  birds  in  color  and  measurements.  Slight  divergences  between  dis- 
tant, well-isolated  populations  are  only  to  be  expected,  unless  one  or  the 
other  has  populated  its  present  habitat  within  the  past  few  centuries.  (See 
Table  1,  in  which  differences  in  technique  of  different  workers  may  also 
be  noted.) 

I propose,  therefore,  to  recognize  3 species  of  robins  in  the  coastal 
lowlands  of  Nayarit:  (1)  Grayson’s  Robin,  mostly  along  the  immediate  coast 
and  poorly  known,  occurs  inland  at  least  to  Sauta  and  Santiago  Ixcuintla, 
in  open  country  below  500  m elevation;  (2)  Rufous-backed  Robin,  wide- 
spread but  apparently  most  numerous  in  woods  back  from  the  coast,  the 
most  common  robin  in  the  lowlands;  and  (3)  White-throated  Robin,  abun- 
dant in  the  mountains  and  fairly  common  (at  least  formerly)  in  the  tall 
forests  from  Compostela  west  and  south  in  the  lowlands,  to  sea-level, 
possibly  only  an  irregular  visitor  (in  small  numbers?)  to  the  woods  near 
San  Bias;  recognized  by  the  sharp  line  of  demarcation  of  the  clear  white, 
unmarked  lower  edge  of  the  throat  against  the  olive-brown  to  grayish 
chest. 


SUMMARY 

Supposed  subspecies  should  not  be  overlooked;  some  wiU  later  prove  to  be  good  species, 
when  properly  studied.  This  appears  to  be  true  of  Turdus  '"rufo-palliatus’’'  graysoni,  which 
widely  overlaps  the  range  of  T.  rufo-palliatus  in  coastal  Nayarit  without  known  hybridization. 
Apparently  3 species  are  present,  at  least  seasonally,  along  the  coast.  Field  studies  are  badly 
needed. 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

During  this  study  I examined,  or  received  information  from  the  curators  of  the  collections 
of  the  American  Museum  of  Natural  History,  British  Museum  (Natural  History)  (BM), 
California  Academy  of  Sciences  (CAS),  Carnegie  Museum  of  Natural  History  (CM),  Cornell 
University,  Delaware  Museum  of  Natural  History  (DEL),  Institute  de  Biologia  de  la  Univer- 
sidad  Nacional  Autdnoma  de  Mexico  (MEXU).  Louisiana  State  University  Museum  of  Zoology 
(LSU),  Moore  Laboratory  of  Zoology  at  Occidental  College  (RTM),  Museum  of  Vertebrate 
Zoology  of  the  University  of  California  (MVZ),  United  States  National  Museum  of  Natural 
History  (US).  University  of  Arizona  (ARIZ)  and  University  of  New  Mexico  Museum  of  South- 
western Biology  (UNM).  Dr.  Amadeo  M.  Rea  and  Lewis  D.  Yaeger  provided  field  notes  and 
important  help  in  the  field.  Collecting  permits  were  issued  by  the  Departamento  de  Con- 
servacion  de  la  Fauna  Silvestre,  Mexico,  D.  F.  Mary  C.  McKitrick  kindly  measured  the 
northernmost  T.  graysoni.  I am  grateful  to  all  of  these,  and  especially  to  Anne  Pulich  for 
painting  the  frontispiece.  Last  but  not  least,  I thank  Elsie  Marshall  and  Gwynne  S.  Leonard 


Phillips  • GRAYSON’S  ROBIN? 


309 


for  help  in  typing  early  drafts  of  the  manuscript.  Helpful  comments  on  the  manuscript  were 
received  from  J.  W.  Aldrich,  J.  C.  Barlow  and  F.  G.  Stiles. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Blake,  E.  R.  1953.  Birds  of  Mexico.  A guide  for  field  identification.  Univ.  Chicago  Press, 
Chicago,  Illinois. 

Edwards,  E.  P.  1972.  A field  guide  to  the  birds  of  Mexico.  Sweet  Briar,  Virginia. 

Earner,  D.  S.  and  J.  R.  King,  (eds.)  1971.  Avian  biology,  Vol.  I.  Academic  Press,  New 
York,  New  York. 

Grant,  P.  R.  1965.  A systematic  study  of  the  terrestrial  birds  of  the  Tres  Marias  Islands, 
Mexico.  Postilla  90:1-106. 

Hellmayr,  C.  E.  1934.  Catalogue  of  birds  of  the  Americas,  Pt.  VII.  Field  Mus.  Nat.  Hist., 
Zool.  Ser.  XIII. 

Knox,  A.  G.  1976.  The  taxonomic  status  of  the  Scottish  Crossbill  Loxia  sp.  Bull.  Br. 
Ornithol.  Club  96:15-19. 

Marshall,  J.  T.,  Jr.  1967.  Parallel  variation  in  North  and  Middle  American  screecb-owls. 

Monogr.  West.  Found.  Vert.  Zool.  No.  1:1-72. 

Miller,  A.  H.,  H.  Friedmann,  L.  Griscom  and  R.  T.  Moore.  1957.  Distributional  check- 
list of  the  birds  of  Mexico,  Pt.  II.  Pac.  Coast  Avif.  33. 

Nelson,  E.  W.  1899.  Birds  of  the  Tres  Marias  Islands.  N.  Am.  Fauna  14:7-62. 

Paxton,  R.  O.  1979.  N.Y.  Rev.  Books,  20  Dec.,  p.  14. 

Phillips,  A.  R.  1959.  The  nature  of  avian  species.  J.  Ariz.  Acad.  Sci.  1:22-30. 

. 1969.  An  ornithological  comedy  of  errors:  Catharus  occidentalis  and  C.  frantzii. 

Auk  86:605-623. 

Raitt,  R.  j.  and  j.  W.  Hardy.  1970.  Relationships  between  two  partly  sympatric  species 
of  thrushes  {Catharus)  in  Mexico.  Auk  87:20-57. 

Ridgway,  R.  1907.  The  birds  of  North  and  Middle  America,  Pt.  IV.  U.S.  Natl.  Mus. 
Bull.  50. 

Rowley,  J.  S.  and  R.  T.  Orr.  1964.  The  status  of  Frantzius’  Nightingale  Thrush.  Auk 
81:308-314. 

Traylor,  M.  A.  1979.  Two  sibling  species  of  Tyrannus  (Tyrannidae).  Auk  96:221-233. 

APARTADO  POSTAL  370,  SAN  NICOLAS  DE  LOS  GARZA,  NUEVO  LE6n,  M^IXICO. 
ACCEPTED  14  JAN.  1981. 


COLOR  PLATE 

Publication  of  the  frontispiece  of  the  Mainland  Rufous-backed  Robin  (Turdus  rufo-palliatus) 
and  Grayson  s Robin  {T.  graysoni)  painted  by  Anne  Pulich  has  been  made  possible  by  an  en- 
dowment established  by  George  Miksch  Sutton. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  310-324 


HYPERPHAGIA  AND  SOCIAL  BEHAVIOR  OE 
CANADA  GEESE  PRIOR  TO  SPRING  MIGRATION 

M.  Robert  McLandress  and  Dennis  G.  Raveling 

Several  papers  have  emphasized  the  evolutionary  and  functional  signif- 
icance of  spring  accumulated  body  reserves  for  reproduction  by  geese 
(Barry  1962;  Hanson  1962;  Ryder  1970;  Ankney  1977;  Raveling  and  Lums- 
den  1977;  Ankney  and  Macinnes  1978;  Raveling  1978a,  1979a,  b;  Mc- 
Landress and  Raveling  1981).  Numerous  studies  have  contributed  infor- 
mation on  behavior  of  Canada  Geese  (Branta  canadensis)  (see  Johnsgard 
1975  and  Bellrose  1976  for  reviews).  However,  no  study  of  wild  geese  has 
described  or  quantified  the  hyperphagia  presumably  associated  with  their 
increase  in  body  weight  in  spring.  The  objective  of  our  study  was  to  doc- 
ument behavior  of  Giant  Canada  Geese  {B.  c.  maxima)  prior  to  their  arrival 
on  their  nesting  grounds.  Emphasis  was  placed  on  behavior  related  to 
accumulation  and  use  of  energy  reserves  to  enhance  our  knowledge  of  the 
evolution  and  proximate  control  of  avian  reproductive  and  social  organi- 
zation systems. 


METHODS 

We  studied  Giant  Canada  Geese  wintering  at  Silver  Lake,  in  the  city  of  Rochester,  Min- 
nesota (43°55'N,  92°30'W).  These  birds  nest  between  Lake  Winnipeg  and  Lake  Manitoba- 
Winnipegosis,  in  Canada,  885  km  northwest  of  Rochester  (see  Hanson  1965,  Gulden  and 
Johnson  1968,  Raveling  1978b).  Refuges  at  both  wintering  and  nesting  areas  of  these  birds 
and  the  urban  environment  of  Rochester  provided  a unique  situation  for  close  observation 
of  wild  geese  which  were  habituated  to  human  activity.  Data  were  collected  within  a 30  km 
radius  of  Rochester  from  3 February-6  April  1974.  The  last  major  migration  occurred  on  the 
latter  date.  Measurements  of  temperature,  wind  and  snow  cover  were  obtained  from  the 
U.S.  Dept,  of  Commerce,  Weather  Bureau,  Rochester,  Minnesota. 

On  a daily  basis,  time  that  geese  spent  in  feeding  areas  and  on  lake  roosting  areas  was 
determined  for  the  Hock  as  a whole.  Major  Hights  of  geese  to  and  from  these  areas  were 
visible  from  almost  anywhere  in  the  study  area  and  indicated  transition  between  feeding  and 
roosting  periods.  Daily  feeding  regimes,  defined  as  the  duration  of  time  spent  away  from  lake 
roosting  areas  by  at  least  half  of  the  population,  were  categorized  as  follows:  (A)  more  than 
half  the  flock  did  not  leave  lake  roosting  areas  or  left  for  less  than  1.5  h;  (B)  geese  were  in 
feeding  areas  from  1. 5-4.5  h without  returning  to  water  roost  areas;  (C)  geese  remained  away 
from  lake  roosting  areas  from  4.5-7. 5 h (usually,  there  were  distinct  morning  and  evening 
feeding  periods,  but  one  long  period  was  normal  on  overcast  days);  (D)  geese  were  in  feeding 
areas  from  7.5-10.5  h during  the  day,  typically  in  one  continuous  period;  and  (E)  geese  spent 
more  than  10.5  h in  feeding  areas  because  at  least  half  the  flock  did  not  return  to  lake 
roosting  areas  for  at  least  part  of  the  night. 

Over  200  individually  identifiable  neck-banded  adult  geese  were  available  for  observation 
(see  Raveling  1978b).  Numbers  of  geese  in  feeding  areas  were  estimated  when  possible  and 
all  marked  birds  observed  were  recorded.  The  proportion  of  time  spent  in  these  fields  actually 


310 


McLandress  and  Raveling  • CANADA  GEESE  IN  SPRING 


311 


devoted  to  feeding  by  marked  individuals  (and  their  mates,  when  present)  was  recorded  for 
sample  periods  of  approximately  10  min  (timed  by  stopwatch). 

Family  status  of  geese  was  determined  from  behavioral  associations  (see  Raveling  1969), 
especially  the  triumph  ceremony.  This  display  involves  extensive  head  and  neck  movements 
with  associated  vocalizations  and  is  usually  exhibited  only  by  mated  pairs  of  adults  and 
among  members  of  a family  (see  Fischer  1965,  Raveling  1970).  The  occurrence  of  triumph 
ceremonies  and  aggressive  interactions  involving  physical  contact  between  individuals  were 
recorded  for  timed  periods  averaging  30  min. 

The  number  of  sexual  displays  was  recorded  when  a majority  of  the  goose  population  was 
at  Silver  Lake.  Approximately  1000  birds  were  observed  when  sexual  displays  were  being 
recorded  so  as  to  have  reasonably  comparable  samples  from  which  to  compare  rates  of  sexual 
behaviors.  Sexual  behavior  noted  included  copulation  and  the  pre-copulatory  neck-dipping 
display  (Klopman  1962).  Sperm  transfer  could  not  be  determined  but  copulation  was  consid- 
ered unsuccessful  only  when  the  post-copulatory  display  (Klopman  1962)  was  not  exhibited 
by  either  sex  following  coition.  Size  and  behavior  of  unmarked  geese  was  used  to  identify 
sex  of  the  individual  initiating  neck-dipping  and  terminating  incomplete  sexual  displays  when 
possible. 

Proportions  of  timed  sample  periods  spent  feeding  by  individuals  and  frequencies  of 
triumph  ceremonies  and  aggressive  interactions,  measured  as  the  number  of  displays  or 
contacts  per  unit  time  per  bird,  were  ranked  and  analysed  with  Mann-Whitney  f/-tests  (Sokal 
and  Rohlf  1973:218).  Changes  in  intensity  of  triumph  ceremonies  and  aggressive  interactions, 
which  were  categorized  according  to  vigor  and  duration  of  displays  (see  description  in  Re- 
sults), were  compared  with  Chi-square  tests  (Sokal  and  Rohlf  1973:300).  Significance  of  the 
regression  of  changes  in  frequency  of  sexual  behavior  over  time  was  also  tested  (Sokal  and 
Rohlf  1973:248). 


RESULTS 

Changes  in  food  habits  and  body  composition  determined  from  geese 
collected  during  this  study  are  reported  elsewhere  (McLandress  and  Rav- 
eling 1981).  In  summary,  the  results  were:  (1)  body  weight  of  adult  female 
and  male  geese  increased  36%  and  26%,  respectively,  above  average  win- 
ter (February)  weights;  (2)  adult  geese  without  mates  weighed  less  before 
the  weight  gain  period  and  gained  less  weight  than  paired  geese;  (3)  geese 
shifted  from  a winter  diet  of  corn  {Zea  mays)  to  a diversity  of  food  items 
in  spring  dominated  by  bluegrass  {Poa  pratensis);  and  (4)  bluegrass  was 
rich  in  protein  (>26%),  which  is  a requirement  that  may  limit  clutch-size 
in  Canada  Geese  (Raveling  1979a). 

F ceding  behavior. — Ambient  temperatures  ranged  from  —28  to  16°C.  Un- 
der the  coldest  conditions  geese  remained  at  Silver  Lake  with  bills  tucked 
under  their  scapular  feathers.  As  temperature  increased,  geese  spent  more 
time  in  feeding  areas  (Table  1:  r = 0.87,  P < 0.001).  This  resulted  in 
geese  feeding  7.5  h or  longer  (category  D or  E),  for  25  of  34  days  (74%)  for 
which  feeding  regime  data  were  obtained  from  1 March  to  the  6 April 
mass  migration.  Only  5 extended  feeding  regimes  (22%)  were  recorded  for 
23  days  of  data  between  3 and  28  February.  Geese  fed  during  the  night 
(category  E)  only  when  temperatures  remained  above  or  near  0°C  between 


312 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  Vo.  3,  September  1981 


Table  1 

Daily  Feeding  Pattern  of  Giant  Canada  Geese  Under  Different  Temperatures 
Prior  to  Spring  Migration  (3  February-6  April  I974f 


Feeding 

regime** 

Daily  temperature  ranges  (°C) 

.Max.  <-10° 
.Min.  <-10° 

-10°— 5° 
<-10° 

>-5° 

<-10° 

>-5° 
-10°— 5° 

>-5° 

-5°-0° 

b b 
A A 

A (0-1.5  h) 

2^ 

1 

B (1. 5-4.5  h) 

7 

2 

C (4.5-7.5  h) 

2 

7 

5 

1 

D (7.5-10.5  h) 

1 

7 

15 

3 

E (>10.5  h) 

1 

3 

Total  days 

2 

10 

10 

12 

17 

6 

® No  data  for  6 days  within  this  period. 

*’  See  text. 

' -Actual  daylight  maximum  temperatures  for  these  days  were  — 16°C  and  — 13°C. 


2 and  6 March.  These  warm  nights,  although  partially  overcast,  coincided 
with  a full  moon.  Patterns  of  decreased  feeding  (category  B or  C)  returned 
17-24  March  when  minimum  temperatures  were  less  than  — 5°C  and  max- 
imums  were  seldom  above  0°C. 

Changes  in  feeding  location  corresponded  with  deereasing  snow  depth 
in  February  (Table  2).  Geese  fed  rarely  in  grass  fields  and  frequently  in 
creeks  and  ponds  when  recorded  snow  depth  was  greater  than  20  cm. 
Geese  were  more  dispersed  when  at  creeks  and  ponds  than  when  in  corn- 
fields. Thawing  occurred  first  around  the  bases  of  trees  and  where  clumps 
of  vegetation  extended  through  the  snow.  These  snow-free  patches  ex- 
posed grass  when  recorded  snow  depth  fell  below^  20  cm.  Geese  fed  at 
these  patches  as  soon  as  they  appeared.  Snow  was  18  cm  deep  on  1 March 
and  all  hut  gone  by  6 March.  Snow  fell  on  6 days  after  11  March  but 
accumulation  did  not  exceed  8 cm  for  the  remainder  of  the  study. 

Corn  remained  an  important  food  item  throughout  the  study.  Waste 
corn,  frozen  in  top  soil,  became  available  to  geese  as  the  soil  thawed  (9- 
16  March).  More  geese  in  feeding  areas  were  observed  in  cornfields  in  this 
week  than  during  any  other  time  period  when  snow  depth  was  less  than 
20  cm.  Proportions  of  feeding  geese  that  used  pastures  increased  after  25 
March  and  peaked  just  before  the  6 April  migration.  Generally,  geese  fed 
in  cornfields  and  pastures  bordering  ereeks  and  rivers  flowing  into  Silver 
Lake. 

The  average  size  of  the  Giant  Canada  Goose  population  during  our  study 
was  12.000-15,000  birds.  Thirty-one  flocks  of  geese  feeding  in  cornfields 
of  approximately  65  ha  (160  acres)  averaged  1996  ±311  birds  during  Feb- 
ruary. Flock  size  in  cornfields  in  early  March  (869  ± 111  birds,  N = 123 


McLandress  and  Raveling  • CANADA  GEESE  IN  SPRING 


313 


Table  2 

Feeding  Locations  of  Giant  Canada  Geese  Prior  to  Spring  Migration,  1974 


Date 

Total  geese 
observed® 

Corn 

% of  geese  in: 
Grass 

Creeks  and 
ponds 

Snow  depth  >20  cm: 

3-28  February 

30,290 

86 

1 

13 

Snow  depth  <20  cm: 

3-28  February 

36,197 

63 

36 

1 

1-8  March 

44,870 

62 

31 

6 

9-16  March 

40,580 

86 

11 

3 

17-24  March 

46,620 

71 

24 

5 

25  March-1  April 

72,063 

47 

51 

2 

2-6  April 

16,842 

38 

62 

0 

® Summation  of  all  geese  in 

feeding  flocks  estimated  to  size; 

most  flocks 

were  repeatedly  observed 

over  the  time  period — 

see  text  for  average  flock  size. 


flocks)  was  significantly  smaller  {t  = 4.16,  P < 0.001)  and  it  diminished 
still  further  {t  = 4.52,  P < 0.001)  to  339  ± 35  geese  (N  = 122  flocks)  in 
the  2 weeks  prior  to  the  6 April  migration.  Decreasing  flock  size  was 
related  to  an  increase  of  distance  between  individuals.  No  significant 
change  was  detected  in  flock  size  of  grass  feeding  geese  among  comparable 
time  periods.  Geese  feeding  in  65  ha  (160  acres)  pastures  averaged  360  ± 
42  geese  (N  = 183  flocks). 

Proportions  of  marked  geese  of  known  social  status  feeding  in  grass 
changed  markedly  (x^  = 8.89,  df  = 2,  P < 0.002)  during  spring  (Table  3). 
The  percentage  of  marked  birds  that  were  paired  declined  slightly  as 
spring  progressed,  although  the  actual  number  of  paired  geese  showed 
little  change.  In  contrast,  the  number  of  marked  geese  never  observed 
with  a mate  doubled  between  each  observation  period  from  early  to  late 


Table  3 

Proportions  of  Individually  Marked  Adult  Giant  Canada  Geese  of  Different 
Social  Status  Observed  in  Grass  Fields  During  Spring  1974 


Date 

N 

Status 

Paired 

Unpaired 

Undetermined 

18  February -9  March 

56 

44  (79%) 

5 (9%) 

7 (13%) 

9-24  March 

69 

53  (77%) 

10  (15%) 

6 (9%) 

24  March-6  April 

79 

49  (62%) 

22  (28%) 

8 (10%) 

314 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  \o.  3,  September  1981 


Table  4 

Proportion  of  Time  Actually  Devoted  to  Feeding  by  Giant  Canada  Geese  oe 
Different  Sex  and  Social  Status  During  Spring  (3  February-6  April  1974) 


Feeding  site 


status  of  geese 

N® 

Cornfield 

N" 

Grass  field 

Males 

In  pairs 

31 

38%*’ 

26-53" 

20 

51% 

45-77 

Others 

55 

38% 

32-52 

30 

64% 

37-81 

Females 

In  pairs 

32 

52% 

39-62 

22 

65% 

54-79 

Others 

51 

24% 

17-33 

37 

52% 

36-79 

® Number  of  approximate  10  min  sample  periods  in  which  a marked  individual's  proportion  of  time  spent  feeding  was 
recorded. 

" Median. 

' Confidence  limits  (cf.  Snedecor  and  Cochran  1967:124). 


spring.  Accurate  determinations  of  social  status  of  geese  in  cornfields  were 
difficult  in  February  and  early  March  due  to  large  flock  sizes. 

W hile  in  grass  fields,  paired  geese  spent  higher  proportions  of  their  time 
actually  feeding  than  when  they  were  in  cornfields  (Table  4:  G — 1-66 
males  and  1.83  females,  P < 0.05).  Similarly,  “other”  females  (referring 
to  individuals  without  identifiable  mates  in  the  vicinity)  spent  a higher 
proportion  of  time  feeding  while  in  pasture  than  in  cornfields  (G  = 3.60, 
P < 0.001).  “Other”  males  showed  a tendency  toward  the  same  feeding 
pattern  (G  = 1.59,  P < 0.1).  Paired  females  tended  to  feed  for  a greater 
proportion  of  time  than  paired  males  when  in  either  cornfields  (G  = 1.50, 
P < 0.1)  or  grass  fields  (G  1.49,  P < 0.1).  Also,  when  both  members 
of  a pair  were  observed  simultaneously,  females  spent  more  time  feeding 
than  males  on  73%  of  observations  (N  — 63).  Proportions  of  time  spent 
feeding  by  “other”  and  paired  males  were  similar  but  “other”  females 
spent  less  time  feeding  when  in  cornfields  than  either  paired  females  (G  = 
3.60,  P < 0.001)  or  “other”  males  (G  = 2.85.  P < 0.005). 

Social  behavior. — No  significant  differences  were  detected  among  fre- 
quencies of  triumph  ceremonies  determined  from  observations  at  Silver 
Lake,  either  among  periods  of  time  during  the  day  (i.e.,  morning — 07:00- 
10:00,  mid-day — 10:00-15:00  and  evening — 15:00-19:00)  or  between  Feb- 
ruary and  March.  A significantly  higher  frequency  (G  = 3.45,  P < 0.001) 


McLandress  and  Raveling  • CANADA  GEESE  IN  SPRING 


315 


Table  5 

Frequency  and  Intensity  Differences  of  Triumph  Ceremony  Displays  and 
Aggressive  Interactions  of  Giant  Canada  Geese  At  and  Away  from  Silver  Lake 

During  Spring  1974 


Silver  Lake 

Other  areas 

3-28  Feb.  1 Mar. -6  .\pr. 

3-28  Feb.  1 .Mar. -6  Apr. 

Triumph  ceremonies  (T.C.) 

Frequency^ 

Median  (N)^ 

2.6  (26) 

5.4  (6) 

12.0  (5) 

20.0  (2) 

95%  C.L. 

1.9^.8 

1. 3-7.5 

3.3-38.0 

13.3-26.7 

Intensity 

T.C.’s  categorized 

199.0 

60 

37 

24 

Reduced  (%) 

19.1 

42 

3^ 

17b 

Normal  (%) 

63.3 

42 

32 

46 

Exaggerated  (%) 

17.6 

17 

Aggressive 

65 

interactions  (A.I.) 

38 

Frequency^’*^ 

Median  (N) 

0.5  (6) 

5.3  (3) 

8.2  (4) 

24.4  (2) 

95%  C.L. 

0.0-2. 7 

1.3-h.7 

8.0-12.0 

10.7-38.1 

Intensity 

A.i.’s  categorized 

114.0 

35 

31 

28 

Pecking  (%) 

36.8 

69 

29 

36 

Chasing  (%) 

49.1 

23 

61 

54 

Fighting  (%) 

14.0 

9 

lO^’ 

IP 

® Displays/IOOO  birds/min. 

**  N = number  of  observation  periods  (ca.  30  min  each). 

' Combined  with  closest  category  for  test  due  to  infrequent  occurrence. 
**  Mid-day  (10:00-15:00)  observations  only. 


of  displays  was  recorded  for  observations  in  areas  other  than  Silver  Lake 
compared  to  Silver  Lake  in  March  (Table  5). 

Triumph  ceremony  intensity  was  classified  “normal”  when  the  compo- 
nents (see  Raveling  1970),  rolling  (neck  extended  upward  and  forward)  and 
cackling  (neck  extended  downward  and  forward),  were  displayed  with 
about  equal  frequency.  Intensity  was  considered  “reduced”  when:  wing- 
flicking  (shown  to  some  extent  during  “normal”  displays)  was  not  exhibited 
by  either  adult;  cackling  exceeded  rolling;  head  and  neck  waving  during 
the  rolling  component  were  more  restricted;  and  vocalizations  were  less 
raucous  and  prolonged  than  in  “normal”  triumph  displays.  “Exaggerated” 
displays  involved  vigorous  rolling  by  males  and  extended  raucous  vocal- 
izations. The  criterion  for  classification  in  this  category  was  that  contact 
was  made  between  adults.  Typically,  adult  males  used  their  bills  to  grasp 


316 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


the  female’s  neck  near  the  back  of  her  head  or  base  of  the  black  neck 
stocking.  Occasionally,  females  grasped  the  breast  feathers  of  males. 

There  was  a significant  change  in  proportions  of  triumph  ceremony  dis- 
plays of  different  intensity  exhibited  at  Silver  Lake  (Table  5:  = 13.38, 

df  = 2,  P < 0.005)  and  at  areas  other  than  Silver  Lake  = 4.39,  df  = 

1,  P < 0.05)  between  February  and  March.  “Reduced”  intensity  displays 
increased  at  all  areas.  Little  change  occurred  in  the  proportion  of  “exag- 
gerated” intensity  displays  at  Silver  Lake  but  these  were  greatly  reduced 
at  other  areas.  The  percentage  of  “normal”  displays  was  less  in  March 
than  in  February'  at  Silver  Lake. 

Proportions  of  triumph  ceremonies  of  different  intensity  at  areas  other 
than  Silver  Lake  differed  significantly  from  displays  at  Silver  Lake  in 
February  (x^  = 38.06,  df  = 2,  P < 0.001)  and  in  March  (x^  = 6.46,  df  = 

2,  P < 0.05).  At  areas  other  than  Silver  Lake,  highly  vigorous  displays 
were  much  more  common  (or  “reduced”  intensity  triumph  ceremonies 
were  much  less  common)  than  at  Silver  Lake. 

Triumph  ceremonies  were  most  commonly  associated  with  aggressive 
conflicts  among  geese  (76%  of  191  displays  for  which  circumstances  were 
recorded).  Pairs  often  displayed  prior  to  conflict  and  immediately  following 
retreat  of  an  opponent.  The  intensity  of  display  increased  with  the  duration 
of  aggressive  interaction  and  the  degree  of  physical  conflict.  Most  triumph 
ceremonies  associated  with  aggressive  conflicts  (N  = 145)  were  classified 
as  either  “normal"  (47%)  or  “exaggerated”  (33%)  in  intensity.  Aggressive 
interactions  were  involved  in  48  (94%)  of  the  51  most  intense  triumph 
ceremonies. 

If  mated  pairs  were  spatially  separated  for  any  reason,  triumph  cere- 
mony occurred  when  they  were  reunited.  In  March,  triumph  ceremony 
was  occasionally  (9  times)  observed  when  1 member  of  a pair  involved  had 
just  awakened  (separation  in  a temporal  sense).  Triumph  ceremonies  fol- 
lowing either  type  of  separation  comprised  24%  of  the  191  displays.  Pro- 
portionately fewer  high  intensity  displays  and  more  low  intensity  displays 
were  noted  for  triumph  ceremonies  following  separation  of  mates  (“exag- 
gerated" = 7%,  “normal"  = 50%,  “reduced"  = 43%,  N = 46)  than  for 
triumph  ceremonies  related  to  aggression  (x^  = 16.82,  df  = 2,  P < 0.001). 
The  proportion  of  “reduced"  intensity  displays  was  highest  for  triumph 
ceremonies  related  to  reunion  of  mates  after  awakening  (7  of  9,  78%). 

Only  freciuencies  of  aggressive  interactions  obtained  from  observations 
at  mid-day  (10:00-15:00)  are  presented  (Table  5)  because  significant  dif- 
ferences were  found  among  different  time  periods  of  the  day  and  too  few 
observations  were  taken  during  other  time  periods  for  further  compari- 
sons. A difference  in  frequency  of  interactions  was  detected  for  these  mid- 


McLandress  and  Raveling  • CANADA  GEESE  IN  SPRING 


317 


day  observations  between  February  and  March  at  Silver  Lake  {Ug  = 17, 
P < 0.025).  In  addition,  geese  at  locations  away  from  Silver  Lake  were 
involved  in  higher  frequencies  of  aggressive  encounters  than  geese  at  Sil- 
ver Lake  {Us  = 54,  P < 0.001). 

A total  of  208  aggressive  interactions  between  geese  were  classified 
according  to  degree  of  physical  exertion  (Table  5).  “Pecking”  involved  1 
goose  striking  another  with  its  bill,  followed  by  a short  retreat  or  immediate 
submission  by  the  loser.  “Chasing”  was  characterized  by  the  victorious 
goose  chasing  the  loser  with  no  fighting  involved.  “Fighting”  referred  to 
encounters  in  which  2 geese  grasped  each  other  at  the  base  of  the  neck 
with  their  bills  and  hit  each  other  with  their  wings.  Proportions  of  aggres- 
sive encounters  of  different  physical  involvement  observed  at  Silver  Lake 
in  March  changed  significantly  from  proportions  recorded  for  geese  in 
February  aggressive  encounters  (x^  = 11.01,  df  = 2,  P < 0.005).  The  pro- 
portion of  aggressive  encounters  which  involved  “pecking”  doubled  from 
February -March  and  because  the  frequency  of  conflicts  was  greater,  the 
absolute  amount  of  “chasing”  and  “fighting”  may  have  increased  as  well. 
These  differences  between  months  were  not  evident  at  areas  other  than 
Silver  Lake.  There  was  a significant  difference  in  the  proportion  of  inter- 
actions of  different  exertion  between  geese  at  and  away  from  Silver  Lake 
in  March  (x^  = 6.76,  df  = 1,  P < 0.01),  but  not  in  February. 

Sexual  behavior. — Copulation  attempts  by  geese  were  observed  69  times 
during  spring  although  6 (9%)  were  considered  incomplete  due  to  lack  of 
the  post-copulatory  display.  In  addition,  the  pre-copulatory  display,  neck- 
dipping, was  not  followed  by  coition  on  67  occasions.  The  frequency  of  all 
sexual  behaviors  (which  included  successful  copulation,  neck-dipping  not 
followed  by  coition  and  unsuccessful  copulation)  increased  exponentially 
from  3 February-6  April  (Fig.  1:  Y = 0.05e«^'^'',  G = 5.72,  P < 0.001). 
Although  the  highest  rate  of  sexual  displays  was  recorded  in  the  first  week 
of  April,  only  8%  (1  of  13)  of  sexual  interactions  resulted  in  successful 
coition  compared  to  53.6%  (60  of  112)  for  March  sexual  displays  (x^  = 
9.81,  df  = 1,  P < 0.005).  The  frequency  of  successful  copulations  was 
highest  in  the  last  week  of  March. 

Males  initiated  neck-dipping  on  19  of  27  (70%)  occasions  when  sexes  of 
both  members  of  the  pair  were  identified — a significantly  higher  proportion 
than  an  expected  equal  contribution  by  each  sex  (x^  = 4.48,  df  = 1,  P < 
0.05).  The  copulatory  behavior  sequence  was  terminated  by  the  female 
before  coition  on  14  of  20  (70%)  observations  when  sex  of  the  birds  could 
be  determined,  which  tended  to  be  a higher  rate  than  would  be  expected 
from  equal  contribution  by  either  sex  (x^  = 3.20,  df  = 1,  P < 0.10).  Sim- 
ilar findings  were  reported  by  Klopman  (1962). 


318 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


N = number  of  hours  of  observation 


Fig.  I.  Frequency  of  displays  of  the  copulatory  sequence  of  Giant  Canada  Geese  during 
spring  1974. 


DISCUSSION 

Environmental  factors  affecting  food  consumption. — Daily  ambient  tem- 
perature range  appeared  to  influence  the  amount  of  time  spent  in  feeding 
areas  by  Giant  Canada  Geese  during  spring.  Raveling  et  al.  (1972)  reported 
that  cold  had  little  influence  on  the  length  of  the  feeding  period  of  Todd’s 
Canada  Geese  {B.  c.  interior)  in  southern  Illinois  and  concluded  that 
cloudy  conditions  caused  them  to  spend  more  time  in  feeding  areas  in 
winter.  In  this  study,  temperature  seemed  to  be  the  most  important  influ- 
ence on  daily  duration  of  feeding  by  geese.  Cloud  cover  was,  however, 
related  to  patterns  of  feeding.  Geese  usually  had  distinct  morning  and 
evening  feeding  periods  on  clear  days,  whereas,  on  cloudy  days  of  similar 
temperature  ranges  they  typically  had  single  feeding  periods  of  prolonged 
duration,  hut  total  time  per  day  spent  in  feeding  areas  was  unchanged. 

Higher  temperatures  melted  snow  and  thawed  soil  enabling  geese  to  use 
previously  inaccessible  food  resources.  These  conditions  corresponded 
with  geese  spending  all  day  and  some  nights  in  feeding  areas.  In  contrast 
to  the  results  of  this  study,  B.  c.  interior  held  in  captivity  and  supplied 
food  ad  libitum,  increased  energy  intake  with  decreasing  ambient  tem- 
perature regardless  of  photoperiod  (\^  illiams  1965).  However,  Raveling  et 
al.  (1972)  reported  that  temperatures  at  or  below^  — 9°C  (15°F)  caused  wild 


McLandress  and  Raveling  • CANADA  GEESE  IN  SPRING 


319 


B.  c.  interior  to  cease  activity.  Similarly,  in  this  study,  on  2 days  when 
temperature  remained  below  — 13°C  (9°F),  geese  stayed  at  Silver  Lake. 
LeFebvre  and  Raveling  (1967)  predicted  that  immature  female  Giant  Can- 
ada Geese  could  not  sustain  the  energetic  demand  caused  by  prolonged 
heat  loss  at  temperatures  less  than  — 15°C  (5°F).  Raveling  et  al.  (1972) 
suggested  that  inactivity  which  reduces  energy  loss  is  the  most  adaptive 
response  of  geese  to  extreme  cold.  Thus,  the  behavioral  response  of  Can- 
ada Geese  to  changing  energetic  demands  is  flexible  and  can  be  altered 
by  increased  food  availability.  After  appropriate  photo-stimulation  (see 
reviews  by  Lofts  and  Murton  1968,  1973;  Farner  and  Lewis  1971;  Lofts 
1975),  the  expression  of  hyperphagia  by  geese  may  be  regulated  by  tem- 
perature and  food  availability  in  spring.  Such  control  would  maximize 
positive  energy  balance  during  the  fattening  period. 

The  insulating  effect  and  translucent  quality  of  snow  may  provide  suf- 
ficient warmth  and  light  for  survival  and  early  germination  of  grasses  in 
years  of  ample  snow  cover  (see  Tieszen  1972).  During  the  spring  of  1974, 
geese  were  able  to  obtain  protein-rich  green  grass  (McLandress  and  Rav- 
eling 1981)  from  beneath  melting  snow  11  days  prior  to  the  major  thaw  in 
early  March.  Winters  with  little  precipitation  or  frequent  thawing  of  snow 
cover  could  result  in  decreased  survival  of  subnivean  grass  and  delay  in 
the  emergence  of  new  grasses.  Conversely,  mild  spring  weather  might 
allow  geese  earlier  access  to  grasses. 

Social  factors  and  food  intake. — Appropriate  behavior  between  mem- 
bers of  pairs  is  necessary  for  follicle  maturation  in  many  birds  (see  review 
by  Lofts  and  Murton  1973).  Results  of  our  study  indicate  that  pair  relations 
may  also  be  important  to  spring  fat  accumulation.  All  geese  spent  a greater 
percentage  of  time  devoted  to  feeding  when  in  grass  fields  than  in  corn- 
fields. Paired  geese  preceded  non-paired  birds  to  grass  fields  in  spring  and 
paired  females  spent  more  time  actually  feeding  when  in  cornfields  than 
unpaired  females.  Finally,  unpaired  geese  initially  weighed  less  and  gained 
less  weight  in  the  period  of  fat  accumulation  (McLandress  and  Raveling 
1981). 

Decreases  in  density  of  flocks  of  geese  feeding  in  cornfields  could  have 
been  due  to  diminishing  amounts  of  waste  corn  as  spring  progressed. 
However,  density  of  flocks  in  pastures  remained  unchanged  and  essen- 
tially equaled  the  final  density  of  geese  recorded  for  cornfields  in  spring. 
A higher  percentage  of  geese  feeding  in  pastures  coincided  with  the  de- 
crease in  density  of  flocks  of  geese  feeding  in  cornfields.  Flocks  of  geese 
feeding  in  pastures  contained  higher  proportions  of  unpaired  geese  in  the 
2 weeks  prior  to  spring  migration  than  earlier  in  spring.  Possibly,  this 
reflects  later  initiation  of  hyperphagia  in  potentially  non-reproductive 
geese. 


320 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


The  preponderance  of  rolling  and  cackling  components  of  triumph  cer- 
emonies displayed  in  different  releasing  situations  in  this  study  were  not 
as  clearly  separable  as  Fischer  (1965)  reported  for  Greylag  Geese  {Anser 
anser).  Rather,  these  components  appeared  to  form  a continuum  from 
exclusively  cackling  in  the  mildest  triumph  ceremonies,  to  predominantly 
rolling  in  displays  of  the  highest  intensity.  Rolling  is  almost  exclusively  a 
male  behavior  and  has  been  accepted  as  a predominantly  aggressive  dis- 
play (Lorenz  1959,  Fischer  1965,  Raveling  1970,  Radesater  1974).  Rade- 
sater  (1974)  suggested  that  the  frequency  of  rolling  may  depend  on  testos- 
terone levels  as  frequency  increased  during  seasons  when  agonistic 
encounters  were  common  in  captive  geese.  The  decrease  in  proportion  of 
“exaggerated”  (rolling  predominated)  triumph  ceremonies  by  Giant  Can- 
ada Geese  as  well  as  the  lower  proportion  of  “chasing”  and  “fighting” 
noted  in  aggressive  interactions  as  spring  progressed,  would  appear  to 
contradict  a correlation  with  higher  testosterone  levels  indicated  by  in- 
creasing testis  size  (McLandress  and  Raveling,  unpubl.).  However,  geese 
became  progressively  more  dispersed  in  feeding  areas  through  spring,  thus 
reducing  intensity  and/or  frequency  of  agonistic  stimuli.  Less  conspicuous 
chasing  and  threatening  behavior  than  during  winter  has  also  been  re- 
ported for  Canada  Geese  upon  their  arrival  at  the  breeding  grounds  in 
spring  (Raveling  and  Lumsden  1977). 

As  spring  progressed,  the  frequency  of  triumph  ceremonies  did  not 
change  significantly  despite  a decrease  in  the  proportion  of  aggressive 
related  displays.  The  lower  proportions  of  “normal”  and  “exaggerated” 
displays  were  balanced  by  an  increasing  number  of  “reduced”  intensity 
triumph  ceremonies.  “Reduced”  intensity  triumph  ceremonies  (predomi- 
nated by  cackling)  observed  in  this  study,  as  in  other  studies  (Lorenz  1959, 
Fischer  1965,  Raveling  1970,  Radesater  1974),  often  occurred  in  the  ab- 
sence of  agonistic  stimuli.  Hanson  (1953)  and  Raveling  (1970)  concluded 
that  families  and  mates  remaining  together  have  an  adaptive  advantage  in 
the  social  hierarchy  of  Canada  Geese  in  winter.  The  dominance  order 
provides  benefits  in  terms  of  food  and  space  acquisition,  and  reduction  of 
aggressive  encounters.  The  higher  number  of  “reduced”  intensity  triumph 
ceremonies  may  be  a reflection  of  increased  stimulation  of  geese  to  be 
with  mates  in  spring.  This  would  be  adaptively  advantageous  to:  (1)  en- 
hance the  status  of  paired  geese  in  the  dominance  hierarchy  during  the 
period  of  hyperphagia,  nest-site  selection  and  territorial  defense;  (2)  insure 
that  pairs  arrive  together  on  the  breeding  grounds;  and  (3)  stimulate  pair 
formation  in  unmated  geese. 

Termination  of  spring  hyperphagia. — Raveling  (1978a)  concluded  that 
factors  that  stimulate  the  gonadotropin  release  responsible  for  follicle  mat- 
uration in  female  Canada  Geese  occur  at  the  same  time  as,  or  just  before. 


McLandress  and  Raveling  • CANADA  GEESE  IN  SPRING 


321 


final  migration  to  nesting  grounds.  The  yolk  of  eggs  of  Canada  Geese  takes 
12-13  days  to  develop  (Raveling  1978a).  Geese  in  this  study  migrated  2- 
6 April,  arrived  on  nesting  grounds  on  8 and  9 April  and  began  egg-laying 
19  and  20  April.  However,  nest  initiation  was  probably  delayed  by  a lack 
of  meltwater  at  nest-sites  (see  also  Gooch  1958,  1961;  Barry  1962,  1967; 
Maclnnes  1962;  Ryder  1967;  Mickelson  1975).  Minor  follicular  develop- 
ment had  occurred  in  female  geese  collected  during  March,  but  rapid  yolk 
deposition  was  evident  only  in  geese  collected  4-6  April.  Based  on  average 
size  of  the  largest  follicles  of  4 females  collected  4-6  April  (23  mm; 
McLandress  and  Raveling,  unpubl.),  it  is  likely  that  rapid  ovarian  follicular 
development  began  during  the  last  week  of  March. 

Digestive  organs  which  were  enlarged  in  geese  collected  14-16  March 
had  decreased  in  size  in  birds  collected  4-6  April  (McLandress  and  Rav- 
eling, unpubl.)  indicating  that  hyperphagia  had  already  ended.  The  ex- 
ponential increase  in  sexual  displays  (Fig.  1)  during  spring  corresponded 
with  enlargement  of  testes  and  ovarian  follicle  size  of  collected  geese 
(McLandress  and  Raveling,  unpubl.).  Despite  the  highest  frequency  of 
displays  of  the  sexual  sequence  occurring  in  the  first  week  of  April,  cop- 
ulatory  success  indicated  that  the  most  important  week  for  coition  was 
21-28  March.  Coincidentally,  9 of  15  (60%)  post-copulatory  displays,  for 
which  information  was  recorded  during  the  week  of  21-28  March,  included 
the  female.  This  frequency  was  significantly  greater  (x^  = 6.84,  df  = 1, 
P < 0.01)  than  4 female  postcopulatory  displays  of  22  (18%)  copulations 
observed  throughout  spring  by  Klopman  (1962).  Thus,  rapid  ovarian  follicle 
growth  began  before  migration,  coincided  with  the  decrease  in  size  of 
digestive  organs  enlarged  during  hyperphagia,  and  was  correlated  with 
and  perhaps,  as  suggested  by  Raveling  (1978a),  stimulated  by  copulatory 
behavior. 

Giant  Canada  Geese  of  this  study  population  have  consistently  arrived 
on  their  nesting  grounds  in  Manitoba  between  5 and  9 April,  1968-1975 
(Raveling,  unpubl.).  Day  length,  the  most  consistent  indicator  of  season 
(Follett  1973),  is  likely  the  primer  for  migration  and  associated  physiolog- 
ical changes.  Secondary  factors  may  modify  timing  of  migration,  final  de- 
velopment of  ovarian  follicles  and  termination  of  hyperphagia,  once  pho- 
toperiod is  permissive. 

Hyperphagia  and  the  resulting  weight  gain  of  geese  began  1 March  when 
warming  temperatures  and  melting  snow  allowed  geese  to  have  prolonged 
access  to  new  grass.  Pre-reproductive  body  weight  gain  was  attained  by 
the  end  of  March.  Young  grass  eaten  by  geese  contained  high  levels 
(>26%)  of  protein  (McLandress  and  Raveling  1981).  Young  grass  remains 
highly  digestible  for  approximately  1 month  during  spring,  at  which  time 
digestibility  abruptly  decreases  (McDonald  et  al.  1973).  Thus,  duration  of 


322 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


the  fattening  period  of  Canada  Geese  and  their  use  of  fertile  lowlands 
along  creeks  coincided  with  availability  of  maximum  quality  of  grass.  Cold 
weather  which  was  related  to  reduced  feeding  efforts  occurred  17-24 
March.  Perhaps  the  required  increase  of  weight  could  occur  more  rapidly 
in  years  of  even  more  clement  weather. 

SUMMARY 

Behavioral  changes  of  Giant  Canada  Geese  associated  with  accumulation  of  body  weight 
prior  to  spring  migration  were  studied  in  southeastern  Minnesota.  Daily  duration  of  time 
spent  in  feeding  areas  by  geese  increased  with  increasing  temperature.  The  percentage  of 
geese  feeding  in  grass  fields  increased,  but  cornfields  remained  important  feeding  sites 
throughout  spring.  Adult  geese  spent  more  time  actually  feeding  when  in  pastures  than  when 
in  cornfields.  Paired  females  devoted  more  time  to  feeding  than  males  in  either  cornfields 
or  pastures  and  more  time  than  females  without  mates  in  cornfields.  Increased  dispersal  of 
geese  in  spring  coincided  with  proportionately  fewer  vigorous  triumph  ceremonies  and  a 
lower  percentage  of  aggressive  encounters  which  involved  actual  fighting.  However,  the 
frequency  of  all  triumph  ceremonies  and  aggressive  encounters  may  have  increased.  Sexual 
behavior  increased  exponentially  through  spring,  but  the  highest  frequency  of  successful 
copulations  coincided  with  the  onset  of  rapid  yolk  formation  and  with  the  termination  of 
hyperphagia  1 week  prior  to  migration.  Hyperphagia  and  the  associated  accumulation  of 
body  reserves  by  Giant  Canada  Geese  occurred  in  a period  of  less  than  1 month  prior  to 
departure  for  the  breeding  grounds  which  coincided  with  availability  of  the  highest  level  of 
protein  in  new  growth  grass. 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

This  research  was  supported  by  National  Science  Eoundation  grant  GB-38788.  We  are 
grateful  for  permission  from  the  P.  Curry  and  A.  Vincent  families  to  study  at  East  Meadows 
Ranch,  Manitoba,  and  from  the  Rochester  Parks  Board  and  the  Minnesota  Department  of 
Natural  Resources  (MDNR)  to  study  at  Silver  Lake,  Rochester.  N.  Gulden,  R.  Holmes  and 
R.  Jessen  (MDNR)  supplied  equipment  and  handled  many  administrative  matters.  J.  Gil- 
bertson (MDNR)  and  G.  Yaeger  of  the  Rochester  Post  Bulletin  were  invaluable  in  dealing 
with  public  relations.  Many  thanks  are  due  to  A.  Stegen  (MDNR),  L.  Salata  and  M.  Wege 
of  the  University  of  California,  Davis  (UCD)  for  their  assistance  in  the  field.  Additional  help 
was  provided  by  R.  Christman  of  Macalester  College,  L.  King,  Manager  of  East  Meadows 
Ranch,  and  K.  Brace  of  the  Canadian  ildlife  Service.  R.  Schwab  and  W.  Hamilton  (UCD) 
provided  valuable  critiques  of  early  drafts.  We  are  especially  grateful  to  Use  McLandress 
who  helped  with  fieldwork  and  typed  earlier  drafts  of  the  manuscript. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Ankney,  C.  D.  1977.  The  use  of  nutrient  reserves  by  breeding  male  Lesser  Snow  Geese 
Chen  caerulescens  caerulescens.  Can.  J.  Zool.  55:1984-1987. 

AND  C.  D.  MacInnes.  1978.  Nutrient  reserves  and  reproductive  performance  of 

female  Lesser  Snow  Geese.  Auk  96:459-471. 

Barry,  T.  W . 1962.  Effect  of  late  seasons  on  Atlantic  Brant  reproduction.  J.  Wildl.  Manage. 
26:19-26. 

. 1967.  The  geese  of  Anderson  River  Delta,  Northwest  Territories.  Ph.D.  thesis, 

Univ.  Alberta.  Edmonton,  Alberta. 


McLandress  and  Raveling  • CANADA  GEESE  IN  SPRING 


323 


Bellrose,  F.  C.  1976.  Ducks,  geese  and  swans  of  North  America.  Stackpole  Books,  Har- 
risburg, Pennsylvania,  and  Wildl.  Manage.  Inst.,  Washington,  D.C. 

COOCH.  F.  G.  1958.  The  breeding  biology  and  management  of  the  Blue  Goose  (Chen  cae- 
rulescens).  Ph.D.  thesis,  Cornell  Univ.,  Ithaca,  New  York. 

. 1961.  Ecological  aspects  of  the  Blue-Snow  Goose  complex.  Auk  78:72-89. 

Earner,  D.  S.  and  R.  A.  Lewis.  1971.  Photoperiodism  and  reproductive  cycles  in  birds. 
Pp.  325-370  in  Photophysiology,  Vol.  5 (A.  C.  Giese,  ed.).  Academic  Press,  New  York, 
New  York. 

Fischer,  H.  1965.  Das  Triumphgeschrei  der  graugans  (Anser  anser).  Z.  Tierpsychol. 
22:247-304. 

Follett,  B.  K.  1973.  The  neuroendocrine  regulation  of  gonadotropin  secretion  in  avian 
reproduction.  Pp.  209-243  in  Breeding  biology  of  birds  (D.  S.  Earner,  ed.).  Natl.  Acad, 
of  Sci.,  Washington,  D.C. 

Gulden,  N.  A.  and  L.  L.  Johnson.  1968.  History,  behavior  and  management  of  a flock  of 
Giant  Canada  Geese  in  southeastern  Minnesota.  Pp.  59-71  in  Canada  Goose  manage- 
ment (R.  L.  Hine  and  C.  Schoenfeld,  eds.).  Dembar  Educ.  Res.  Serv.,  Madison,  Wis- 
consin. 

Hanson,  H.  C.  1953.  Inter-family  dominance  in  Canada  Geese.  Auk  70:11-16. 

. 1962.  The  dynamics  of  condition  factors  in  Canada  Geese  and  their  relation  to 

seasonal  stresses.  Arctic  Inst.  N.  Am.  Tech.  Pap.  No.  12. 

. 1965.  The  Giant  Canada  Goose.  S.  Illinois  Press,  Carbondale,  Illinois. 

JOHNSGARD,  P.  A.  1975.  W^aterfowl  of  North  America.  Indiana  Univ.  Press,  Bloomington, 
Indiana. 

Klopman,  R.  B.  1962.  Sexual  behavior  in  the  Canada  Goose.  Living  Bird  1:123-129. 

Lefebvre,  E.  a.  and  D.  G.  Raveling.  1967.  Distribution  of  Canada  Geese  in  winter  as 
related  to  heat  loss  at  varying  environmental  temperatures.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  31:538- 
546. 

Lofts,  B.  1975.  Environmental  control  of  reproduction.  Pp.  176-197  in  Avian  physiology 
(M.  Peaker,  ed.).  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

AND  R.  K.  Murton.  1968.  Photoperiodic  and  physiological  adaptations  regulating 

avian  breeding  cycles  and  their  ecological  significance.  J.  Zool.  155:327-394. 

AND . 1973.  Reproduction  in  birds.  Pp.  1-107  in  Avian  biology,  Vol.  3 (D.  S. 

Earner  and  J.  R.  King,  eds.).  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New'  York. 

Lorenz,  K.  1959.  The  role  of  aggression  in  group  formation.  Pp.  181-252  in  Group  pro- 
cesses: transactions  of  the  fourth  conference  (B.  Schaffner,  ed.).  Josiah  Macy,  Jr.  Foun- 
dation, New  York,  New  York. 

MacInnes,  C.  D.  1962.  Nesting  of  small  Canada  Geese  near  Eskimo  Point,  Northwest 
Territories.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  26:247-256. 

McDonald,  P.,  R.  A.  Edwards  and  J.  F.  D.  Greenhalgh.  1973.  Animal  nutrition.  Hafner 
Publ.  Co.,  New  York,  New  York. 

McLandress,  M.  R.  and  D.  G.  Raveling.  1981.  Food  habits  and  body  composition  changes 
of  Giant  Canada  Geese  prior  to  spring  migration.  Auk  98:65-79. 

Mickelson,  P.  G.  1975.  Breeding  biology  of  Cackling  Geese  and  associated  species  on  the 
Yukon-Kuskokwim  Delta,  Alaska.  Wildl.  Monogr.  45. 

Radesater,  T.  1974.  Form  and  sequential  associations  between  the  triumph  ceremony  and 
other  behaviour  patterns  in  the  Canada  Goose  Branta  canadensis  L.  Ornis  Scand.  5:87- 
101. 

Raveling,  D.  G.  1969.  Social  classes  of  Canada  Geese  in  w inter.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  33:304- 
318. 

. 1970.  Dominance  relationships  and  agonistic  behavior  of  Canada  Geese  in  winter. 

Behaviour  37:291-319. 


324 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


. 1978a.  The  timing  of  egg  laying  by  northern  geese.  Auk  95:294-303. 

. 1978b.  Dynamics  of  distribution  of  Canada  Geese  in  winter.  N.  Am.  Wildl.  and 

Natl.  Res.  Trans.  43:206-225. 

. 1979a.  The  annual  cycle  of  body  composition  of  Canada  Geese  with  special  refer- 
ence to  control  of  reproduction.  Auk  96:234-252. 

. 1979b.  The  annual  energy  cycle  of  the  Cackling  Canada  Goose.  Pp.  81-93  in 

Management  and  biology  of  Pacific  flyway  geese:  a symposium  (R.  L.  Jarvis  and  J.  C. 
Bartonek,  eds.).  OSU  Bookstores,  Inc.  Corvallis,  Oregon. 

, W.  E.  Crews  and  W.  D.  Klimstra.  1972.  Activity  patterns  of  Canada  Geese 

during  winter.  Wilson  Bull.  84:278-295. 

AND  H.  G.  Lumsden.  1977.  Nesting  ecology  of  Canada  Geese  in  the  Hudson  Bay 

lowlands  of  Ontario:  evolution  and  population  regulation.  Ont.  Min.  Nat.  Res.  Fish  and 
Wildl.  Res.  Rept.  No.  98. 

Ryder,  J.  P.  1967.  The  breeding  biology  of  Ross’  Goose  in  the  Perry  River  region.  Northwest 
Territories.  Can.  Wildl.  Serv.  Rept.  Ser.  No.  3.  Ottawa,  Ontario. 

. 1970.  A possible  factor  in  the  evolution  of  clutch  size  in  Ross’  Goose.  Wilson  Bull. 

82:5-13. 

Snedecor,  G.  W.  and  W.  G.  Cochran.  1967.  Statistical  methods,  6th  ed.  Iowa  State  Univ. 
Press,  Ames,  Iowa. 

SoKAL,  R.  R.  AND  F.  Rohlf.  1973.  Introduction  to  biostatistics.  W.  H.  Freeman  and  Com- 
pany, San  Francisco,  California. 

Tieszen,  L.  L.  1972.  The  seasonal  course  of  above  ground  production  and  chlorophyll 
distribution  in  a wet  Arctic  tundra  at  Barrow,  Alaska.  Arctic  and  Alpine  Res.  4:307- 
324. 

Williams,  J.  E.  1965.  Energy  requirements  of  the  Canada  Goose  in  relation  to  distribution. 
Ph.D.  thesis,  Univ.  Illinois,  Urbana,  Illinois. 


DEPT.  WILDLIFE  AND  FISHERIES  BIOLOGY,  UNIV.  CALIFORNIA,  DAVIS,  CAL- 
IFORNIA 95616.  ACCEPTED  20  SEPT.  1980. 


RESEARCH  GRANTS 

The  Eastern  Bird  Banding  Association  and  the  Western  Bird  Banding  Association  are  each 
offering  a research  grant  of  $250  in  aid  of  research  using  bird  banding  techniques  or  bird 
handing  data.  Applicants  should  submit  a resume  of  his  or  her  banding  or  ornithological 
background,  the  project  plan  and  a budget  to  the  joint  selection  committee  chairman: 
Robert  C.  Leberman,  Powdermill  Nature  Reserve,  Star  Route  South,  Rector,  Pennsylvania 
15677.  No  formal  application  forms  are  available,  and  the  amount  requested  should  not 
exceed  $250.  The  deadline  for  receipt  of  applications  is  15  March  1982. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  325-333 


A MULTIPLE  SENSOR  SYSTEM  FOR  MONITORING 
AVIAN  NESTING  BEHAVIOR 

James  A.  Cooper  and  Alan  D.  Afton 

Avian  nest  construction,  laying  and  incubation  time  budgets  often  pro- 
vide a temporal  framework  for  ethological,  physiological  and  ecological 
investigations.  Numerous  methods  have  been  devised  to  measure  nest 
attentive  and  inattentive  periods.  Early  approaches,  reviewed  by  Kendeigh 
(1952:5)  and  Skutch  (1962),  involved  observations  from  a blind  or  simple 
mechanical  switch  devices  connected  to  graphic  recorders.  Advances  in 
electronic  and  photographic  equipment  have  led  to  the  development  of 
many  useful  instruments  for  detecting  the  presence  of  birds  at  or  near 
nests.  Thermistors  and  thermocouples  have  been  positioned  in  nests  (Bald- 
win and  Kendeigh  1927,  Earner  1958,  Norton  1972),  in  eggs  (Huggins  1941, 
Snelling  1972,  Caldwell  and  Cornwell  1975  and  others),  or  in  artificial  eggs 
(Holstein  1942,  Kossack  1947,  Norton  1972).  Photoelectric  sensors  were 
placed  in  or  near  nests  by  Kessler  (1962)  and  Weeden  (1966),  while  time 
lapse  cameras  were  used  by  Weller  and  Derksen  (1972)  and  Caldwell  and 
Cornwell  (1975).  Gilmer  et  al.  (1971),  Varney  and  EUis  (1974)  and  Miller 
(1976)  have  described  telemetry  systems.  PuUiainen  (1978)  recently  em- 
ployed closed-circuit  television  to  record  nesting  behavior. 

Each  approach  reflected  the  limitations  imposed  by  the  species  studied, 
the  availability  and  cost  of  electronic  and  photographic  equipment  and  the 
inventiveness  of  the  investigator(s).  Skutch  (1962)  emphasized,  and  we 
believe  correctly,  that  automatic  monitors  cannot  substitute  for  observa- 
tion. Yet,  investigations  of  individual  variation  or  nocturnally  active  birds 
cannot  easily  be  done  by  observation  alone.  Nocturnal  observations  re- 
quire expensive  night-vision  equipment,  and  large  samples  are  necessary 
for  statistical  analysis  of  differences  among  individual  birds.  Varney  and 
Ellis  (1974)  criticized  thermistor  and  thermocouple  techniques  because  a 
wire  must  be  attached  to  an  egg;  they  recommended  a telemetry  system 
in  an  artificial  egg.  However,  artificial  egg  temperatures  differ  from  heat 
levels  in  developing  eggs  (Drent  1970).  Earlier  methods  used  a single  sen- 
sor, not  allowing  crosschecking  for  accuracy  or  simultaneous  monitoring 
of  egg  temperatures,  incubating  bird  behavior  or  identity. 

This  paper  describes  the  construction  and  field  application  of  a multiple 
sensor  (photoelectric-thermistor-photographic)  system  for  nesting  studies. 
While  employed  primarily  in  waterfowl  investigations  to  date,  the  appa- 
ratus has  potential  for  studies  of  a wide  range  of  species. 


325 


326 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Fig.  1.  Block  diagram  of  the  multiple  sensor  system. 


METHODS  AND  MATERIALS 

Instrument  design  and  construction. — Basic  instrument  components  are: 
an  infrared  photoelectric  relay  (Microswitch  MLS-3A),  a medical  therm- 
istor (Rustrak  1331),  a super-8  movie  camera  (Minolta  XL-401),  a strip- 
chart,  temperature-event  recorder  (Rustrak  2133),  an  infrared  relay  inter- 
face and  a 1.5  VDC  clock  (Fig.  1).  The  recorder,  interface  and  clock  are 
housed  in  a weather-proof  case.  The  infrared  relay  and  camera  are  posi- 
tioned near  or  at  the  nest  and  connected  to  the  recorder  and  interface, 
and  the  thermistor  probe  is  inserted  in  an  egg  or  in  the  nest.  Connections 
are  via  multi-conductor,  insulated  cable.  The  recorder,  thermistor  and 
relay  are  powered  by  one  or  two  12  VDC  batteries;  the  clock  and  camera  are 
driven  by  rechargeable  1.5  VDC  dry  cells. 

Detectors  may  he  operated  independently  or  in  concert.  The  presence 
of  a bird  at  the  nest  is  detected  when  the  infrared  beam  is  blocked  by  the 
bird's  body.  Interruption  of  the  5-mm  diameter  beam,  projected  from  the 
lens  of  the  relay  to  an  8-cm  plastic  reflector  and  back  to  the  relay,  closes 
the  circuit  between  the  relay  and  the  interface,  which,  in  turn,  closes  the 
circuit  to  the  event  channel  of  the  recorder  (Fig.  1).  The  interface  (Fig.  2) 
is  necessary  because  the  infrared  relay  switching  transistor  has  a maxi- 
mum current  limit  of  120  niA  and  the  minimum  current  needed  to  activate 
the  event  pen  is  500  niA.  When  inserted  in  the  egg  air  cell  or  nest,  the 
thermistor  monitors  the  bird’s  presence  by  recording  cooling  during  an 


Cooper  and  Afton  • NESTING  BEHAVIOR  MONITOR 


327 


MLS-3A 


BATTERY 


(+) 


MLS-3A 

LOAD 


CAMERA  RELAY 


Fig.  2.  Schematic  diagram  of  infrared  relay-recorder  interface. 


absence.  In  independent  mode,  the  camera  shutter  is  activated  by  an 
internal  timer  at  preselected  intervals  over  a range  of  1-60  sec,  recording 
on  film  the  nest  and  its  surroundings  during  daylight  and  by  using  a strobe- 
light  at  night.  In  dependent  mode,  single  frame  exposures  are  triggered 
by  interruption  of  the  infrared  beam  so  that  the  animal  blocking  the  beam 
is  photographed. 

An  independent  time  reference  is  needed  for  the  recorder  because  the 
chart  speed  varies  with  the  battery  voltage,  which  is  a function  of  power 
demand,  battery  condition  and  temperature.  This  is  attained  by  opening 
briefly  the  thermistor  circuit  once  an  hour.  A magnetic  reed  switch  at- 
tached to  the  noon  position  of  the  clock  with  a small  magnet  glued  to  the 
minute  hand  provides  an  inexpensive  but  accurate  reference. 

Humid  conditions  may  cause  chart  paper  jamming.  This  can  be  pre- 
vented by  encasing  the  recorder  in  an  airtight  case  containing  a noncor- 
rosive desiccant  (CaS04). 

Field  application. — The  12  VDC  power  supply  permits  monitoring  in 


328 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


remote  locations  and  the  cable  connections  allow  maintenance  of  the  re- 
corder without  disturbing  the  bird  at  the  nest.  Installation  timing  and 
configuration  depend  on  the  characteristics  of  the  species  being  studied. 
A typical  application  entails  locating  a nest,  preferably  during  the  prelay- 
ing or  early  laying  stages,  and  choosing  or  constructing  a suitable  site  for 
the  instrument  case;  placement  of  the  relay,  thermistor  probe,  camera  or 
combination  of  these  at  the  nest  completes  the  process. 

The  infrared  relay  installation  depends  on  the  physical  construction  of 
the  nest,  the  posture  of  the  sitting  bird,  the  size  of  target  it  provides  and 
the  substrate  on  which  the  nest  is  built.  Nests  constructed  on  stable  sub- 
strate, e.g.,  most  dabbling  duck  nests,  present  little  difficulty.  Two  sharp- 
ened, metal  angle-irons  are  driven  into  the  soil  on  a line  bisecting  the  nest 
cup  and  the  relay  is  bolted  to  1 stake,  the  reflector  to  the  other.  The 
infrared  beam  is  adjusted  by  moving  the  relay  and  reflector  vertically  until 
the  beam  is  broken  by  the  sitting  bird’s  body.  The  possibility  that  vege- 
tation or  nest  materials  will  block  the  beam  can  be  reduced  by  placing  the 
relay  and  reflector  as  close  as  possible  to  the  rim  of  the  nest  cup.  In 
addition,  small  boards  can  be  positioned  between  the  nest  cup  and  the 
relay  and  reflector  to  arrest  growth  of  vegetation.  Prior  to  installation  ex- 
posed surfaces  should  be  painted  to  match  the  colors  at  the  nest-site. 
Relay  installation  at  overwater  nests  with  an  unstable  substrate,  e.g.,  nests 
of  most  diving  ducks,  is  accomplished  by  driving  2 metal  rods  into  the 
marsh  bottom.  The  relay  and  reflector  are  then  attached  to  the  rods  and 
aligned  in  the  same  manner  as  in  the  case  of  a ground  nest.  When  mon- 
itoring cavity  nesting  birds,  the  relay  and  reflector  are  fastened  to  brackets 
and  aligned  so  that  the  bird  interrupts  the  beam  when  entering  or  depart- 
ing. 

The  thermistor  probe  is  inserted  into  an  egg  using  a technique  similar 
to  that  of  Caldwell  and  Cornwell  (1975:709).  The  egg  air  cell  is  located 
with  a flashlight  and  outlined  on  the  egg  shell  with  a pencil;  a small  (0.8 
mm)  hole  is  drilled  in  the  shell  at  the  apex  of  the  air-cell  end  with  a sterile 
bit,  taking  care  not  to  perforate  the  air-cell  membrane,  and  the  hole  is 
enlarged  with  a sterile  scalpel  to  accommodate  the  thermistor.  The  probe, 
dipped  in  alcohol  and  allowed  to  dry,  is  inserted  adjacent  to  the  air-cell 
membrane  and  the  hole  sealed  with  epoxy  glue  (Fig.  3).  The  termistor  wire 
is  then  taped  to  the  long  axis  of  the  egg  with  adhesive  tape.  After  inserting 
the  probe,  the  egg  is  placed  in  the  nest,  the  probe  wire  drawn  through  the 
bottom  of  the  cup  and  out  the  side  of  the  nest.  For  species  in  which  the 
egg  air  cells  are  too  small  to  accept  the  probe,  e.g..  Spotted  Sandpiper 
{Actilis  macularia),  the  probe  may  be  glued  to  the  side  of  an  egg,  placed 
in  an  artificial  egg,  or  fastened  to  the  bottom  of  the  nest. 

Unlike  the  silent,  infrared  relay,  the  camera  emits  a faint  click  when 


Cooper  and  Afton  • NESTING  BEHAVIOR  MONITOR 


329 


THERMISTOR  TAPE 


Fig.  3.  Diagram  of  thermistor  implantation  in  an  egg. 


the  shutter  releases.  Thus,  the  camera  and  tripod  must  be  positioned  a 
sufficient  distance  from  the  nest  to  avoid  frightening  the  bird.  Because 
photographic  monitoring  in  independent  mode  is  not  continuous,  i.e.,  must 
be  set  at  a 1-60  sec  interval,  selection  of  the  shutter  release  rate  is  im- 
portant. If  the  frequencies  of  brief  activities,  e.g.,  egg  turning  and  preening 
on  the  nest,  are  to  be  measured,  the  interval  must  be  less  than  the  mini- 
mum duration  of  these  behaviors.  A clock  placed  in  the  field  of  view 
simplifies  the  film  analysis. 

For  species  monitored  to  date,  a chart  speed  of  5 cm/h  allowed  mea- 
surement of  periods  on  or  off  the  nest  to  the  nearest  minute.  Chart  rolls 
are  19.2  m long,  hence,  a paper  change  is  necessary  every  15  days.  A 
more  rapid  speed,  attained  by  a simple  and  inexpensive  gear  change, 
would  be  necessary  for  accurate  measurement  of  activities  of  shorter  du- 
ration. 

The  frequency  of  battery  change  depends  on  the  condition  of  the  bat- 
teries, length  of  connecting  sensor  cables  and  temperature.  Using  the 
infrared  relay,  the  thermistor  and  the  camera  in  dependent  mode  with  33 
m of  cable,  a single  65  A-h  battery  wiU  provide  power  for  4 days;  2 batteries 
in  parallel  last  8 days.  We  found  that  changes  at  2-4-day  intervals  are 
best.  Replacement  of  the  camera  batteries  with  each  film  change  reduces 
the  possibility  of  power  failure  during  a monitoring  session. 

Individual  cables  or  a single  multiconductor  to  the  detectors  may  be 
used.  Cables  need  not  be  shielded  but  must  be  waterproof  and  sufficiently 
durable  to  withstand  months  in  the  field.  The  infrared  relay  requires  3 


330 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Fig.  4.  Three-h  recording  of  the  activity  of  an  incubating  Wood  Duck:  (A)  event  channel 
showing  departure  from  cavity  at  05:16  (OFF)  and  return  at  06:58  (ON)  and  (B)  egg  air  cell 
temperature  in  °C.  Breaks  in  B denote  1-h  intervals. 


conductors  while  the  thermistor  and  camera  2 each.  By  cutting  cables  into 
33  m lengths  and  using  waterproof  connectors,  one  may  extend  the  cable 
needed  to  reach  the  nest  from  the  recorder.  The  advantage  of  this  is  that 
the  added  power  demand  of  the  longer  cable  can  be  easily  calculated.  The 
battery  must  be  changed  1 day  sooner  per  33  m of  cable  used.  Thus,  a 
monitor  with  two  33  m cable  sections  would  require  a battery  change  every 
3 days  vs  4 days  for  1 with  a single  section.  Cables  at  and  near  the  nest 
should  be  secured  by  taping  or  tacking  them  down,  then  covered  with 
vegetation  or  placed  underwater. 

EXAMPLES  AND  DISCUSSION 

Species  monitored  employing  components  of  the  system  include:  the 
Trumpeter  Swan  [Cygrius  cygnus  buccinator).,  Canada  Goose  {Branta  can- 
adensis), Wood  Duck  {Aix  sponsa).  Pintail  {Anas  acuta),  American  Wigeon 
{Anas  arnericana),  Gadwall  {Anas  strepera).  Green-winged  Teal  {Anas 
crecca  carolinensis).  Blue-winged  Teal  {Anas  discors).  Northern  Shoveler 
{Anas  clypeata).  Lesser  Scaup  {Aythya  affinis),  Canvasback  {Aythya  val- 
lisneria).  Redhead  {Aythya  americana).  Ring-necked  Duck  {Aythya  col- 
laris),  Ruddy  Duck  {Oxyura  jamaicensis).  Western  Grebe  {Aechmophorus 
occidentalis).  Spotted  Sandpiper,  Short-eared  Owl  {Asia  flammeus)  and 
Sharp-tailed  Grouse  {Pedioecetes  phasianellus). 

Based  on  data  from  72  nests  where  the  infrared  relay  and  thermistor 
probe  were  employed,  32  of  1066  (3.0%)  monitored  days  were  lost  due  to 


Cooper  and  Afton  • NESTING  BEHAVIOR  MONITOR 


331 


DAY  OF  INCUBATION 


Fig.  5.  Example  of  the  nest  attentiveness  of  a Ruddy  Duck  female,  3-27  June  1975, 
recorded  using  thermistor  and  infrared  relay  sensors. 


sensor  or  recorder  failure.  Moreover,  no  records  of  inattentive  and  atten- 
tive periods  at  the  nest  were  lost  when  using  3 sensors  (N  = 10  nests,  216 
days).  Statistical  treatment  of  incubation  time  budget  data  is  difficult  with- 
out relatively  complete  records;  therefore,  keeping  instrument  failures  to 
a minimum  is  important. 

The  multiple  sensor  approach  not  only  permits  recording  of  prelaying, 
laying  and  incubation  time  budgets,  it  also  allows  synchronous  measures 
of  egg  air  cell  and/or  nest  air  temperature  and  parent  bird  postures,  dis- 
plays, preening,  sleep  and  other  activities  at  the  nest.  An  example  of  the 
recorder  output  for  a Wood  Duck  recess  is  given  in  Fig.  4,  and  for  a Ruddy 
Duck  incubation  attentiveness  pattern  in  Fig.  5.  Individuals  frequenting 
a nest  can  be  identified  on  film  if  markers  or  unique  characters  are  present. 

The  cost  of  a unit  with  the  3 sensors  is  about  $800  per  monitor,  and  the 
construction  is  relatively  simple.  Except  for  the  infrared  relay,  recorder, 
camera  and  thermistor,  all  components  used  in  the  system  may  be  pur- 
chased in  most  electronic  stores.  A knowledge  of  simple  DC  parallel  and 
series  circuits  is  needed  to  assemble  or  repair  the  interface  and  time- 
reference  circuits. 

The  system  may  have  minor  disadvantages  when  used  to  monitor  birds 
that  are  disturbed  by  the  thermistor  wire  attached  to  the  egg  or  nest,  or 
by  changing  of  the  camera  film  and  batteries.  We  have  not  encountered 


332 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


the  former  and  have  found  that  84.1%  (N  = 44)  of  the  eggs  with  implanted 
thermistors  have  hatched.  We  suspect  that  species  such  as  raptors  may 
be  difficult  to  monitor  using  the  thermistor  (see  Varney  and  EUis  1974). 
But  the  camera  and  infrared  sensor  would  provide  data  in  these  cases. 
When  the  camera  must  be  placed  so  close  to  the  nest  that  the  sitting  bird 
is  disturbed  during  maintenance,  the  infrared  and  thermistor  recordings 
must  be  carefully  studied  and  the  film  and  batteries  changed  when  the 
bird  is  off  the  nest.  This  may  be  done  for  non-continuous  incubating 
species  but  not  for  one  in  which  both  sexes  incubate  or  others  whose  nest 
is  constantly  attended. 

SUMMARY 

The  construction  and  field  application  of  a multiple  sensor  (photoelectric-thermistor-pho- 
tographic) system  for  avian  nesting  studies  is  described.  The  portable,  battery-powered  sys- 
tem has  several  advantages  over  previously  described  techniques.  Foremost  is  the  accurate 
and  continuous  recording  of  incubation  time  budgets.  The  system  permits  synchronous  re- 
cordings of  egg  air  cell  and/or  nest  air  temperature,  postures,  displays,  preening,  nest  con- 
struction, and  prelaying  and  incubation  time  budgets. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

We  thank  William  H.  Marshall  and  Milton  W.  Weller,  Dept.  Entomology,  Fisheries  and 
Wildlife,  University  of  Minnesota,  for  reviewing  the  manuscript.  John  D.  Afton  assisted  with 
the  electrical  design.  Financial  support  was  provided  by  the  Graduate  School  and  Minnesota 
Agricultural  Experiment  Station,  University  of  Minnesota  and  the  Delta  Waterfowl  Research 
Station,  Portage  La  Prairie,  Manitoba.  This  paper  is  No.  10,939  of  the  Scientific  Journal 
Series  of  the  Minnesota  Agricultural  Experiment  Station. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Baldwin,  S.  P.  and  S.  C.  Kendeigh.  1927.  Attentiveness  and  inattentiveness  in  the  nesting 
behavior  of  the  House  Wren.  Auk  44:206-216. 

Caldwell,  P.  J.  and  G.  W . Cornwell.  1975.  Incubation  behavior  and  temperatures  of 
the  Mallard  Duck.  Auk  92:706-731. 

Drent,  R.  1970.  Functional  aspects  of  incubation  in  the  Herring  Gull  (Larus  argentatus). 
Behaviour  Suppl.  17:1-132. 

Earner,  D.  S.  1958.  Incubation  and  body  temperatures  in  the  Yellow-eyed  Penguin.  Auk 
75:249-262. 

Gilmer,  D.  S.,  V.  B.  Kuechle  and  I.  J.  Ball.  1971.  A device  for  monitoring  radio-marked 
animals.  J.  \\  ildl.  Manage.  35:829—832. 

Holstein,  V.  1942.  Duehogen,  Astur  gentilis  dubivs  (Sparrman).  Biol.  Stud,  over  Danske 
Rovfugle.  1 (Copenhagen). 

Huggins,  R.  A.  1941.  Egg  temperatures  of  wild  birds  under  natural  conditions.  Ecology 
22:148-157. 

Kendeigh,  S.  C.  1952.  Parental  care  and  its  evolution  in  birds.  Illinois  Biol.  Monogr. 
22:148-157. 

Kessler,  F.  1962.  Measurement  of  nest  attentiveness  in  the  Ring-necked  Pheasant.  Auk 
79:702-705. 


Cooper  and  Afton  • NESTING  BEHAVIOR  MONITOR 


333 


Kossack,  C.  W,  1947.  Incubation  temperatures  of  Canada  Geese.  J.  Wildl.  Manage. 
11:119-126. 

Miller,  K.  J.  1976.  Activity  patterns,  vocalization,  and  site  selection  in  nesting  Blue-w'inged 
Teal.  Wildfowl  27:33-43. 

Norton,  D.  W.  1972.  Incubation  schedules  of  four  species  of  calidridine  sandpipers  at 
Barrow,  Alaska.  Condor  74:164-176. 

PULLIAINEN,  E.  1978.  Behavior  of  a Willow  Grouse  Lagopus  1.  lagopus  at  the  nest.  Ornis 
Scand.  Fenn.  55:141-148. 

Skutch,  a.  F.  1962.  The  constancy  of  incubation.  Wilson  Bull.  74:115-152. 

Swelling,  J.  C.  1972.  Artificial  incubation  of  Sparrow  Hawk  eggs.  J.  Wildl.  Manage. 
36:1299-1304. 

Varney,  J.  R.  and  D.  J.  Ellis.  1974.  Telemetering  egg  for  use  in  incubation  and  nesting 
studies.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  38:142-148. 

Weeden,  j.  S.  1966.  Diurnal  rhythm  of  attentiveness  of  incubating  female  Tree  Sparrows 
{Spizella  arborea)  at  a northern  latitude.  Auk  33:368-388. 

Weller,  M.  W.  and  D.  V.  Derksen.  1972.  Use  of  time-lapse  photography  to  study  nesting 
activities  of  birds.  Auk  89:196-200. 

DEPT.  ENTOMOLOGY,  FISHERIES  AND  WILDLIFE,  UNIV.  MINNESOTA,  ST. 
PAUL,  MINNESOTA  55108.  (PRESENT  ADDRESS  ADA:  DELTA  WATERFOWL 
RESEARCH  STATION,  PORTAGE  LA  PRAIRIE,  MANITOBA  RIN  3a1  CANA- 
DA). ACCEPTED  15  JAN.  1981. 


THE  INTERNATIONAL  OSPREY  FOUNDATION,  INC. 

The  International  Osprey  Foundation,  Inc.  (TIOF),  a non-profit  organization,  has  been 
formed  to  study  the  problems  of  restoring  Osprey  numbers  to  a stable  population,  make 
recommendations  to  enhance  the  continued  survival  of  the  Osprey  and  initiate  education 
programs.  Based  on  Sanible  Island,  Florida,  TIOF  monitors  Osprey  nests  on  the  island 
and  is  responsible  for  an  artificial  nesting  program  already  established  there.  A computer- 
ized list  of  Osprey  researchers  and  a working  bibliography  are  being  compiled.  For  further 
information  write:  Mark  A.  Westall,  President  TIOF,  289  Southwinds,  Sanibel,  Florida 
33957. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  334-339 


FORAGING  SPEEDS  OF  WARBLERS  IN  LARGE 
POPULATIONS  AND  IN  ISOLATION 

Douglass  H.  Morse 

Earlier,  I (Morse  1968)  demonstrated  that  during  the  middle  of  the 
breeding  season  female  spruce-woods  warblers  {Dendroica  spp.)  foraged 
significantly  faster  than  did  their  mates,  a pattern  subsequently  reported 
in  a number  of  other  warblers  (e.g..  Black  1975,  Sherry  1979).  I attributed 
this  difference  to  the  females  performing  all  of  the  incubation  and  a ma- 
jority of  the  feeding  of  newly-hatched  young,  thus  putting  their  foraging 
time  at  a premium.  Data  subsequently  gathered  on  nearby  island  isolates 
of  the  same  populations  of  2 species  (Morse  1971,  1977),  combined  with 
data  on  standing  crops  of  insects  in  both  mainland  and  island  forests 
(Morse  1976a,  1977),  make  it  possible  to  assess  foraging  speeds  of  Black- 
throated  Green  {Dendroica  virens)  and  Yellow-rumped  {D.  coronata)  war- 
blers further. 

Data  on  insect  standing  crops  provide  a common  yardstick  between  the 
mainland  and  island  populations.  Their  analysis  suggests  that  no  signifi- 
cant differences  occur  between  the  mainland  and  island  areas  except  at 
the  end  of  the  season  (Morse  1976a,  1977).  Although  standing  crops  may 
be  imperfect  indicators  of  productivity  (e.g.,  Southwood  1966),  insect  fau- 
nas are  similar  on  the  islands  and  adjacent  (shoreline)  mainland  plots 
(unpubl.  data),  and  conditions  influencing  productivity  (weather,  etc.)  are 
probably  similar  also.  Therefore,  the  assumption  of  similarity  in  food  avail- 
ability seems  reasonable.  Given  the  limited  foraging  time  available  to  all 
females  and  the  high  speeds  at  which  mainland  females  forage  at  certain 
times,  availability  of  insect  food  may  be  a critical  factor  at  these  times. 

The  islands  studied  differ  from  the  mainland  in  that  only  1 pair  of  any 
spruce-woods  warbler  species  is  present.  Of  2 common  nest  predators  on 
the  mainland,  the  red  squirrel  {Tamiasciurus  hudsonicus)  is  absent  and 
the  Blue  Jay  {Cyanocitta  cristata)  rarely  visits.  Both  of  these  factors  might 
lower  the  demands  of  nest  attendance  on  females.  Similarly,  the  absence 
of  conspecifics  might  lower  the  demands  for  territorial  proclamation  and 
defense  by  the  males.  Male  Black-throated  Green  Warblers  sing  consid- 
erably less  on  these  islands  than  on  the  mainland  (Morse  1970). 

Given  the  apparent  similarity  in  resource  availability  on  the  mainland 
and  islands,  the  tendency  for  foraging  speeds  to  fluctuate  (Morse  1968), 
and  the  lessened  demands  on  these  individuals,  one  would  predict  island 
females  involved  with  nesting  activities  to  forage  slower  than  mainland 
females.  It  is  equivocal  whether  island  males  will  forage  slower  than  main- 


334 


Morse  • WARBLER  FORAGING  SPEEDS 


335 


land  males  when  females  are  performing  most  of  the  nesting  activities, 
since  mainland  males  do  not  appear  stressed  in  their  foraging  during  this 
part  of  the  cycle  (Morse  1968).  However,  one  would  predict  island  males 
to  forage  substantially  slower  than  mainland  males  when  they  are  making 
major  contributions  to  feeding  fledged  or  nearly-fledged  young. 

STUDY  AREAS  AND  METHODS 

The  “mainland”  data  were  gathered  on  Hog  Island  (Todd  Wildlife  Sanctuary),  Bremen, 
Lincoln  Co.,  Maine.  This  is  a large  island  of  132  ha  covered  by  a mature  red  {Picea  rubens) 
and  white  {P.  glauca)  spruce  forest.  It  is  separated  from  the  mainland  proper  by  a narrow 
channel,  and  its  species  composition  is  similar  to  that  of  the  adjacent  mainland  (Morse 
1976a).  This  forest  is  described  in  detail  elsewhere  (Morse  1968,  1976a,  and  references 
therein).  The  island  data  were  gathered  on  6 small  nearby  islands  with  spruce  forests  ranging 
from  0.35-1.50  ha.  These  islands  are  Crow  and  Jim’s  islands,  Bremen,  Lincoln  Co.;  Indian 
and  Thief  islands,  Bristol,  Lincoln  Co.;  and  Ram  and  Crane  islands.  Friendship,  Knox  Co., 
Maine.  They  are  described  in  detail  elsewhere  (Morse  1971). 

Data  on  foraging  speeds  of  island  birds  were  gathered  as  described  by  Morse  (1968). 
Briefly,  in  addition  to  data  gathered  on  foraging  sites  (reported  in  Morse  1968,  1971),  I scored 
birds  for  their  rapidity  of  foraging  movement.  The  scoring  technique  was  adopted  because 
individuals  often  became  temporarily  obscured  in  foliage  so  thick  that  detailed  beak  move- 
ments could  not  be  observed,  although  rate  of  overall  progress  could  nevertheless  be  ascer- 
tained accurately.  This  method  was  checked  regularly  on  birds  at  sites  where  frequency  of 
foraging  could  be  observed  closely.  The  following  scores  were  used:  1 = 1-2  pecks/min,  2 = 
3^,  3 = 5-^,  4 = 7-9,  5 = 10-12,  6 = 13-15,  7 = 16-20,  8 = 21-25,  9 = 26-30,  10  = 
31  . 

The  data  on  mainland  foraging  speeds  were  reported  earlier  (Morse  1968),  and  those  from 
the  islands  were  obtained  during  1967-1969  while  foraging  data  (Morse  1971)  were  being 
gathered.  Analyses  of  insect  standing  crops  were  not  available  when  the  2 foraging  papers 
were  prepared  (Morse  1968,  1971),  but  were  published  later  (Morse  1976a,  1977). 

At  least  12  pairs  of  each  island  and  mainland  group  of  warblers  were  studied,  except  island 
Black-throated  Green  Warblers,  for  which  the  sample  consisted  of  8 pairs.  Each  point  in  Fig. 
1 represents  observations  at  10-200-t-  foraging  sites. 

Sampling  of  insects  was  carried  out  on  the  islands  during  the  summers  of  1968-1970, 
overlapping  the  period  that  foraging  data  were  gathered.  Insect  sampling  on  the  mainland 
was  carried  out  during  the  summers  of  1969-1972  and  the  mainland  foraging  data  were 
gathered  during  the  summers  of  1966-1967.  Since  no  noticeable  insect  outbreaks  took  place 
in  the  mainland  forests  during  1966-1967  and  1969-1972,  and  since  rather  similar  arthropod 
biomasses  occurred  there  during  1969-1972  (see  Morse  1976a:  Fig.  5a),  I am  proceeding 
under  the  assumption  that  the  foraging  of  mainland  birds  was  carried  out  under  arthropod 
density  regimes  similar  to  those  encountered  during  the  summers  of  1969-1972.  Although 
I consider  this  to  be  a reasonable  assumption,  the  reader  should  be  aware  of  it,  since  insect 
outbreaks  are  not  uncommon  in  forests  and  may  have  a marked  effect  on  both  the  foraging 
patterns  and  abundance  of  insectivorous  birds  (e.g..  Holmes  and  Sturges  1975,  Morse  1978). 

RESULTS 

Foraging  speeds  of  warblers  are  represented  in  Fig.  1,  and  significance 
levels  between  mainland  and  island  individuals  of  the  same  species  and 


336 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Fig.  1.  Mean  foraging  speeds  of  male  (solid  line)  and  female  (dashed  line)  warblers.  Filled 
circles  = mainland  and  open  circles  = islands.  Mainland  data  points  from  Morse  (1968). 
Standard  deviations  accompany  each  data  point  from  an  island.  Standard  deviations  from 
mainland  data  points  are  published  in  Morse  (1968.  Fig.  5). 


sex  in  Table  1.  During  the  middle  of  the  season,  island  females  of  both 
species  foraged  significantly  more  slowly  than  did  mainland  females.  This 
period  coincides  with  the  time  during  which  they  incubate  and  feed  young 
still  in  the  nest  (Palmer  1949;  Morse  1968,  unpubl.  data).  No  significant 
differences  in  foraging  speeds  of  females  occurred  immediately  before 
this,  the  nest-building  period,  or  at  the  end  of  the  season,  after  young  had 
left  their  nests.  Island  females  of  both  species  did,  however,  forage  sig- 
nificantly faster  at  the  very  beginning  of  the  season,  which  includes  the 
period  during  which  they  search  for  nest-sites  and  accumulate  resources 
for  egg  production.  Only  then  did  either  island  males  or  females  forage 
significantly  faster  than  their  mainland  counterparts  (Table  1). 

Island  males  of  both  species  foraged  significantly  more  slowly  than 
mainland  males  during  the  middle  of  the  season,  a time  coinciding  with 
the  first  appearance  of  fledged  young.  This  pattern  held  through  the  end 
of  the  season  in  Black-throated  Green  Warblers,  but  disappeared  at  the 
very  end  of  the  season  in  Yellow-rumped  Warblers. 

Island  females  always  foraged  faster  than  island  males  (Fig.  1),  a dif- 
ference that  was  statistically  significant  {P  < 0.05  or  less  in  1-tailed  Mann- 
\\  hitney  U -tests)  in  all  but  2 cases:  30  May-11  June  for  the  Yellow-rumped 


Morse  • WARBLER  FORAGING  SPEEDS 


337 


Table  1 

Levels  of  Significance  for  Differences  in  Foraging  Speeds  Between  Mainland 

AND  Island  Warblers'* 


Spec  ies  and  sex 

Black-throated  Green  W arbler  Yellow-rumped  Warbler 

Date  Male  Female  Male  Female 


30  May-11  June 
12-18  June 
19-25  June 
26  June-2  July 
3-9  July 
10-16  July 
17-23  July 
24-30  July 


>0.05  (47,  217) 
>0.05  (15,  132) 
>0.05  (34,  43) 
>0.05  (89,  61) 
<0.01  (90,  49) 
<0.001  (161,  12) 
<0.025  (134,  18) 
<0.05  (126,  10) 


<0.00P  (43,  38) 
>0.05  (18,  35) 

<0.001  (20,  12) 
<0.001  (10,  21) 
<0.05  (17,  75) 

<0.001  (43,  16) 
>0.05  (14,  14) 

>0.05  (37,  17) 


>0.05  (25,  133) 
>0.05  (12,  192) 
>0.05  (74,  92) 
>0.05  (54,  105) 
<0.05  (35,  115) 
<0.05  (67,  108) 
<0.025  (79,  77) 
>0.05  (26,  29) 


<0.00P  (12,  69) 
>0.05  (29,  72) 

<0.001  (18,  33) 
<0.001  (14,  52) 
<0.001  (13,  56) 
>0.05  (52,  34) 

>0.05  (27,  16) 

>0.05  (16,  13) 


® Number  of  observations  in  parentheses,  with  mainland  birds  preceding  comma  and  island  birds  following  comma. 
**  Island  foraging  speed  faster  than  mainland  foraging  speed.  In  all  other  significant  differences,  mainland  birds  foraged 
faster  than  island  birds;  1-tailed  Mann- W hitney  f/-tests;  t/'s  supplied  by  author  upon  request. 


Warbler  and  12-18  June  for  the  Black-throated  Green  Warbler  (Table  1). 
Both  of  these  periods  preceded  the  incubation  stage  of  most  individuals. 

Foraging  speeds  of  the  2 species  shifted  strikingly  in  concert  over  the 
season,  greatly  strengthening  confidence  in  this  analysis.  In  7 of  the  8 time 
periods  island  male  Black-throated  Green  Warblers  and  island  male  Yel- 
low-rumped Warblers  simultaneously  foraged  significantly  slower  than  did 
their  mainland  equivalents,  or  foraging  speeds  of  these  island  and  main- 
land birds  were  simultaneously  not  significantly  different  (Table  1)  {P  = 
0.035,  N = 8,  in  a 1-tailed  binomial  test).  Results  for  females  of  the  2 
species  also  corresponded  in  the  same  way  during  7 of  the  8 time  periods 
{P  = 0.035,  N = 8),  with  the  only  difference  being  that  in  the  first  time 
period  island  females  of  both  species  foraged  significantly  faster  than  their 
mainland  equivalents. 

DISCUSSION 

In  that  climatic  factors  and  food  availability  were  apparently  similar  on 
island  and  mainland  study  areas,  most  of  these  results  may  be  attributed 
to  differences  in  interference  by  other  individuals  and/or  differences  in 
danger  of  nest  predation.  Since  no  experiments  were  performed  it  is  not 
possible  to  distinguish  unequivocally  between  the  alternatives,  but  strong 
inferences  can  be  made. 

Differences  between  island  and  mainland  males  can  be  tentatively  at- 
tributed to  levels  of  interference  or  potential  interference,  since  the  males 
make  no  contribution  to  incubation  and  little  to  new-born  young  (Morse 


338 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


1968).  Amounts  of  stationary  singing  by  Black-throated  Green  Warblers 
were  strikingly  lower  on  islands  during  the  middle  of  the  season  (Morse 
1970)  and,  judging  subjectively,  a similar  pattern  seemed  to  hold  for  the 
Yellow-rumped  Warbler  as  well.  The  stationary  song  of  the  Black-throated 
Green  Warbler  is  given  from  prominent  locations  and  seems  associated 
with  territorial  display  (Morse  1967,  1970).  The  slower  foraging  speed  of 
the  island  males  was  thus  probably  a consequence  of  a modified  time 
budget  resulting  from  a decrease  in  frequency  of  stationary  singing. 

Differences  in  habitat  use  are  unlikely  to  account  for  the  differences  in 
foraging  speed  of  male  Black-throated  Green  Warblers,  since  they  used 
the  same  parts  of  both  mainland  and  island  vegetation  (Morse  1971).  Al- 
though island-dwelling  Yellow-rumped  Warblers  did  change  their  habitat 
use  from  that  on  the  mainland  (Morse  1971),  the  close  parallels  of  their 
foraging  speeds  with  those  of  the  Black-throated  Green  Warblers  mitigates 
against  this  shift  being  a major  factor  in  the  differences  of  their  foraging 
speeds. 

Differences  in  frequencies  of  interactions  are  unlikely  to  account  for  the 
differences  in  foraging  speeds  of  mainland  and  island  female  warblers 
during  the  middle  of  the  season,  however.  Even  on  the  mainland  only 
infrequent  interactions  were  observed  between  females  and  other  individ- 
uals (Morse  1976b)  and  mainland  females’  activities  off  the  nest  consisted 
almost  entirely  of  extremely  rapid  foraging  (Morse  1968). 

The  virtual  absence  of  nest  predators  on  these  islands  may  be  a more 
important  factor  affecting  the  foraging  speed  of  females.  Brood  destruction 
is  high  where  nest  predators  are  common  (Skutch  1976),  and  release  from 
it  could  affect  activity  patterns  strikingly.  Unfortunately,  1 do  not  have 
comparative  information  on  the  attentiveness  of  females  at  island  and 
mainland  nests.  However,  attentiveness  at  mainland  nests  was  extremely 
high,  with  these  birds  foraging  for  only  short  periods  (Morse  1968).  The 
greater  ease  that  I experienced  in  observing  island  females  (Morse  1971) 
suggests  that  they  spent  greater  amounts  of  time  off  their  nests  than  did 
mainland  females.  If  correct,  this  interpretation  means  that  attendance 
patterns  of  these  warblers  are  only  partially  governed  by  thermoregulatory 
considerations  and  that  the  birds  in  question  can  adjust  their  attendance 
regimes  in  response  to  the  presence  or  absence  of  nest  predators  about 
them.  Current  reviews  of  incubation  behavior,  oriented  toward  thermo- 
regulatory aspects,  make  slight  reference  to  pressures  from  nest  predators 
as  a possible  modifier  of  attendance  regimes  at  the  nest  (Ricklefs  1974, 
White  and  Kinney  1974). 

1 have  no  explanation  for  the  greater  foraging  speeds  of  island  females 
of  both  species  at  the  very  beginning  of  the  season.  Insect  crops  at  that 
time  were  virtually  identical,  though  variable,  in  the  2 areas  (Morse  1977). 


Morse  • WARBLER  FORAGING  SPEEDS 


339 


These  were  the  only  cases  in  which  island  birds  foraged  faster  than  their 
mainland  counterparts. 


SUMMARY 

Male  and  female  Black-throated  Green  and  Yellow-rumped  warblers  isolated  from  con- 
specifics  and  nest  predators  on  small  islands  foraged  more  slowly  during  the  height  of  the 
breeding  season  than  did  ones  in  large  mainland  populations.  Since  food  supplies  on  mainland 
and  island  were  similar,  differences  between  males  are  interpreted  to  result  from  a decrease 
in  stationary  singing  associated  with  territorial  maintenance,  those  between  females  a con- 
sequence of  decreased  nest  attentive  behavior  associated  with  nest  predators. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

I thank  R.  T.  Holmes  for  comments  on  the  manuscript.  This  work  was  partially  supported 
by  the  National  Science  Foundation  (GB-3226,  GB-6071,  GB-31005). 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Black,  C,  P.  1975.  The  ecology  and  bioenergetics  of  the  Northern  Black-throated  Blue 
Warbler  {Dendroica  caerulescens  caerulescens).  Ph.D.  diss.,  Dartmouth  Coll.,  Hanover, 
New  Hampshire. 

Holmes,  R.  T.  and  F.  W.  Sturges.  1975.  Bird  community  dynamics  and  energetics  in  a 
northern  hardwoods  ecosystem.  J.  Anim.  Ecol.  44:175-200. 

Morse,  D.  H.  1967.  The  contexts  of  songs  in  Black-throated  Green  and  Blackburnian 
warblers.  Wilson  Bull.  79:64-74. 

. 1968.  A quantitative  study  of  foraging  of  male  and  female  spruce  woods  warblers. 

Ecology  49:779-784. 

. 1970.  Territorial  and  courtship  songs  of  birds.  Nature  226:659-661. 

. 1971.  The  foraging  of  warblers  isolated  on  small  islands.  Ecology  52:216-228. 

. 1976a.  Variables  affecting  the  density  and  territorial  size  of  breeding  spruce-woods 

warblers.  Ecology  57:290-301. 

. 1976b.  Hostile  encounters  among  spruce-woods  warblers  {Dendroica,  Parulidae). 

Anim.  Behav.  24:764-771. 

. 1977.  The  occupation  of  small  islands  by  passerine  birds.  Condor  79:399^12. 

. 1978.  Populations  of  Bay-breasted  and  Cape  May  warblers  during  an  outbreak  of 

the  spruce  budworm.  Wilson  BuU.  90:404^13. 

Palmer,  R.  S.  1949.  Maine  birds.  BuU.  Mus.  Comp.  Zool.  102:1-656. 

Ricklefs,  R.  E.  1974.  Energetics  of  reproduction  in  birds.  Publ.  Nuttall  Ornithol.  Club 
15:152-297. 

Sherry,  T.  W.  1979.  Competitive  interactions  and  adaptive  strategies  of  American  Red- 
starts and  Least  Flycatchers  in  a northern  hardwoods  forest.  Auk  96:265-283. 

Skutch,  a.  S.  1976.  Parent  birds  and  their  young.  Univ.  Texas  Press,  Austin,  Texas. 
SoUTHWOOD,  T.  R.  E.  1966.  Ecological  methods.  Methuen,  London,  England. 

White,  F.  N.  and  J.  L.  Kinney.  1974.  Avian  incubation.  Science  186:107-115. 

DIV.  BIOLOGY  AND  MEDICINE,  BROWN  UNIV.,  PROVIDENCE,  RHODE  ISLAND 
02912.  ACCEPTED  16  OCT.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  340-349 


DIFFERENTIAL  PASSERINE  DENSITY  AND 
DIVERSITY  BETWEEN  NEWEOUNDLAND  AND 
OEESHORE  GULL  ISLAND 

Monique  L Vassallo  and  Jake  C.  Rice 

Characteristically,  islands  have  impoverished  biotas  (MacArthur  and 
Wilson  1963,  1967;  Diamond  1975;  many  others).  Several  reasons  have 
been  proposed  to  account  for  this  depauperate  condition.  Remoteness  of 
the  island  (MacArthur  and  Wilson  1963,  1967;  Simberloff  and  Wilson  1969) 
and  island  size  (Diamond  1975,  MacArthur  and  Wilson  1967,  Power  1976) 
can  both  affect  the  equilibrium  number  of  species,  either  directly  or  in- 
directly, with  small,  remote  islands  having  the  fewest  species.  The  poten- 
tial reduction  of  habitat  diversity  on  small  islands  may  also  indirectly  lead 
to  increased  interspecific  competition,  and  consistent  with  the  competitive 
exclusion  principle,  a reduction  in  number  of  species  may  result  through 
loss  of  close  competitors  (Grant  1966a,  MacArthur  and  Wilson  1967,  Morse 
1971).  Other  indirect  effects  of  island  size,  isolation  or  topography  have 
been  proposed  as  factors  determining  equilibrium  numbers  of  species  on 
islands  (MacArthur  et  al.  1973,  Morse  1971).  Few  of  the  above  notions 
have  escaped  some  criticism  (e.g..  Grant  1966a,  Lynch  and  Johnson  1974). 

Differences  in  numbers  of  species  often  occur  concomitantly  with 
changes  in  the  density  of  island  avifaunas.  Increases  in  density  commonly 
occur  and  are  usually  attributable  to  an  increase  in  the  density  of  a few 
species,  relative  to  their  mainland  densities  (Crowell  1961,  Grant  1966b, 
MacArthur  et  al.  1972).  However,  lower  net  densities  of  island  avifaunas 
have  also  been  reported.  These  have  been  attributed  to  species  expanding 
into  suhoptimal  habitat  where  they  occur  in  lower  densities  (Diamond 
1970),  to  deterioration  of  the  local  gene  pool  (Diamond  1970),  or  to  simply 
a decrease  in  species  richness  without  a concomitant  increase  in  densities 
of  remaining  species  (MacArthur  et  al.  1972,  Yeaton  and  Cody  1974). 

Species  also  differ  in  their  abilities  to  colonize  small  islands,  making  it 
possible  in  some  instances  to  predict  systematically  the  order  of  coloni- 
zation of  island  chains,  knowing  the  likely  source  population  (Morse  1971, 
Diamond  1975,  Terborgh  et  al.  1978).  A consequence  of  this  differential 
colonization  ability  of  bird  species  is  that  an  island  adjacent  to  another 
island  will  have  a different  reservoir  of  potential  colonizers  than  an  island 
directly  offshore  from  a continental  land  mass.  The  island  source  com- 
munity ought  to  be  one  already  selected  for  colonization  ability,  as  has 
been  found  by  Terborgh  and  Faaborg  (1973)  and  Terborgh  et  al.  (1978). 


340 


Vassallo  ami  Rice  • NEWFOUNDLAND  INSULAR  BIOLOGY 


341 


Another  well-known  biogeographical  phenomenon  is  the  decline  of 
species  diversity  and  richness  with  increasing  latitude  and/or  harshness 
of  climate  (Klopfer  and  Mac  Arthur  1960,  1961;  Rotenberry  1978).  The 
proportion  of  the  community  made  up  of  nonpasserines  also  declines  with 
increasing  latitude  (Klopfer  and  MacArthur  1960).  Most  studies  of  island 
biogeography  have  been  conducted  in  tropical  or  warm  temperate  lati- 
tudes. The  few  studies  farther  north  have  considered  only  a few  taxa 
(Morse  1971,  but  see  Morse  1977,  Cody  and  Cody  1972). 

Newfoundland  is  a large  island  off  the  coast  of  eastern  Canada.  It  shows 
a markedly  depauperate  avifauna,  relative  to  adjacent  Nova  Scotia  and 
Gaspe,  Quebec  (Peters  and  Burleigh  1951,  Godfrey  1966).  For  example, 
Godfrey  (1966)  shows  that  over  a quarter  of  the  species  breeding  in  Nova 
Scotia  do  not  breed  in  Newfoundland,  although  recent  records  may  alter 
that  figure  slightly.  Gull  Island  (47°15'N,  52°46'W)  is  a small  island  off  the 
east  coast  of  Newfoundland,  in  the  Witless  Bay  Seabird  Sanctuary.  This 
study  examined  the  species  diversity,  richness  and  density  of  the  land 
birds  on  Gull  Island,  to  see  if  the  low  species  richness  of  Newfoundland 
itself  influenced  the  degree  or  pattern  of  species  change  between  Gull 
Island  and  the  adjacent  coast  of  Newfoundland. 

METHODS 

Study  area. — Gull  Island,  in  the  Witless  Bay  Seabird  Sanctuary,  comprises  0.95  km^  and 
is  1.6  km  from  the  nearest  point  of  land  (Fig.  1).  Open  grassy  areas  occur  along  the  shore 
in  which  there  are  large  numbers  of  nesting  burrows  of  Common  Puffins  (Fratercula  arctica). 
Inland  mature  boreal  forest  with  balsam  fir  {Abies  balsamea),  white  spruce  (Picea  glauca) 
and  white  birch  {Betula  papyrifera)  predominate.  Dead  trees  are  common  and  there  are 
bogs. 

South  Head,  Witless  Bay  (47°17'N,  52°47'W)  was  the  adjacent  mainland  area  studied. 
Grassy  fields  and  bogs  were  more  abundant  here  than  on  Gull  Island.  Forested  areas  were 
comparable  in  species  composition,  but  younger  due  to  cutting.  The  2 areas  lie  within  the 
boreal  forest  region  of  Rowe  (1972). 

The  vegetation  of  the  2 areas  was  compared  quantitatively  and  reported  with  a detailed 
comparison  of  the  ecological  differences  between  sites  for  selected  avian  species  (VassaUo 
and  Rice,  in  press).  Briefly,  the  South  Head  forest  had  fewer  large  dead  trees  and  the  most 
densely  vegetated  areas  were  denser  than  any  on  GuU  Island,  The  trees  in  these  exceptionally 
dense  areas  were  mostly  black  spruce  (P.  mariana),  which  were  more  abundant  on  South 
Head  than  on  Gull  Island.  White  birch  was  more  common  on  GuU  Island.  However,  aU 
habitat  types  were  present  at  both  sites  and  differences  were  of  quantitatively  extreme 
densities,  not  qualitative  attributes. 

Census  methods. — Line  transects  were  established  at  both  localities  along  preexisting  path- 
ways (Fig.  2).  Transects  on  the  island  and  mainland  measured  1.44  and  1.62  km,  respectively. 
Both  transects  traversed  open  and  forested  areas,  and  although  open  areas  were  more  abun- 
dant at  South  Head  than  Gull  Island,  lines  at  South  Head  were  oriented  so  that  comparable 
amounts  of  each  habitat  w ere  censused.  Censuses  began  within  1 h of  sunrise  and  data  from 


342 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  Vo.  3,  September  1981 


Fig.  I.  Map  of  the  island  of  Newfoundland,  showing  the  location  of  the  study  area  on  the 
Avalon  Peninsula.  The  inset  shows  GuU  Island  and  South  Head,  itless  Bay. 


Vassallo  and  Rice  • NEWFOUNDLAND  INSULAR  BIOLOGY  343 


Fig.  2.  Map  of  the  2 areas,  showing  major  topographic  features.  The  broken  lines  indicate 
the  transect  lines,  vertical  barring  areas  of  exposed  rock,  slanted  barring  areas  of  grassy 
meadow,  black  areas  open  fresh  water  and  all  unshaded  land  areas  are  boreal  forest. 


censuses  interrupted  by  inclement  weather  were  discarded.  Thirty  censuses  were  done  at 
South  Head  between  18  May  and  19  July  1977,  and  11  on  Gull  Island  between  16  June  and 
26  July.  Additional  species  seen  at  other  times  of  day  are  considered  in  the  comparisons  of 
species  richness,  but  not  in  the  calculations  of  density  or  diversity  measures. 

Analyses. — Diversity  indices  were  calculated  for  all  censuses,  using  Shannon’s  Index  (Pie- 
I lou  1966a,  b).  Because  the  underlying  distribution  is  unknown,  a Kruskal  Wallis  test  (Sokal 
1 and  Rohlf  1969)  was  used  to  compare  diversity  indices  between  sites. 

I Bird  densities  were  calculated  for  each  census  day  as  birds  per  km.  of  line  transect.  No 
I attempt  was  made  to  weight  density  by  proximity  to  the  line  transects,  because  of  greatly 
! different  patterns  of  species  detectability  between  habitat  types.  To  compare  monthly  and 
1 seasonal  densities  between  areas  we  used  t-tests  of  logio  of  the  abundance  measures,  after 
testing  for  homogeneity  of  variances.  For  species  common  at  both  sites,  individual  species 
densities  were  compared  in  the  same  way.  However,  densities  were  first  compared  within 
each  site  between  June  and  July,  and  data  for  the  2 months  were  not  combined  if  abundances 
I changed  significantly  between  months. 


RESULTS 

Thirteen  species  of  passerines  were  observed  on  Gull  Island,  whereas 
25  species  were  seen  on  South  Head  (Table  1).  Eleven  species  were  com- 


344 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Table  1 

Passerine  Birds  Observed  on  South  Head  or  Gull  Island  During  Morning 
Censuses  (X)  or  Other  Times  (T) 

Species 

South  Head 

Gull  Island 

Eastern  Kingbird  {Tyrannus  tyrannus) 

X" 

Yellow-bellied  Flycatcher  {Empidonax  flaviventris) 

X 

X 

Common  Crow  (Corvus  brachyrhynchos) 

X 

Common  Raven  (Corvus  corax) 

Ta 

X 

Black-capped  Chickadee  (Parus  atricapillus) 

X 

Boreal  Chickadee  (P.  hudsonicus) 

X 

X 

Red-breasted  Nuthatch  (Sitta  canadensis) 

Xa 

Brown  Creeper  (Certhia  familiaris) 

xa 

Winter  Wren  (Troglodytes  troglodytes) 

X 

American  Robin  (Turdus  migratorius) 

X 

X 

Gray-cheeked  Thrush  (Catharus  minimus) 

X 

X 

Golden-crowned  Kinglet  (Regulus  satrapa) 

xa 

Starling  (Sturnus  vulgaris) 

xa 

Northern  Parula  (Parula  americana) 

ja 

Blackpoll  Warbler  (Dendroica  striata) 

X 

X 

Northern  Waterhrush  (Seiurus  noveboracensis) 

X 

X 

Wilson  Warbler  (Wilsonia  pusilla) 

X 

Rusty  Blackbird  (Euphagus  carolinus) 

■ya 

Pine  Grosbeak  (Pinicola  enucleator) 

X 

X 

Pine  Siskin  (Carduelis  pinus) 

X 

X 

Red  Crossbill  (Loxia  curvirostra) 

X 

White-winged  Crossbill  (Loxia  leucoptera) 

X 

X 

Savannah  Sparrow  (Passerculus  sandwichensis) 

X 

Dark-eyed  Junco  (Junco  hyemalis) 

X 

White-throated  Sparrow  (Zonotrichia  albicollis) 

X 

Fox  Sparrow  (Passerella  iliaca) 

X 

X 

Swamp  Sparrow  (Melospiza  georgiana) 

X 

“ Not  seen  more  than  twice  during  summer. 


mon  to  both  sites,  2 others  were  recorded  only  on  the  island  and  14  were 
recorded  only  on  South  Head.  With  the  exception  of  the  Brown  Creeper 
{Certhia  familiaris),  all  rarely  encountered  species  were  recorded  at  South 
Head  rather  than  Gull  Island.  Not  all  species  restricted  to  South  Head 
were  rare  ones,  however,  as  8 species  frequently  seen  there  were  never 
seen  on  Gull  Island. 

One  striking  difference  between  the  avifaunas  of  South  Head  and  Gull 
Island  was  the  number  of  congeners  present.  At  South  Head  2 species  of 
Parus,  2 of  Loxia  and  2 of  Corvus  were  recorded,  whereas  on  GuU  Island 
only  1 species  of  each  genus  was  present.  Furthermore,  at  South  Head  5 
species  of  sparrows  and  juncos  and  4 species  of  warblers  were  noted,  but 


Vassallo  and  Rice  • NEWFOUNDLAND  INSULAR  BIOLOGY 


345 


Table  2 

Monthly  Mean  Bird  Species  Diversities  and  Densities  for  South  Head  and  Gull 

Island  for  Summer  1977 


Diversity 

Density  (birds/km) 

i ± sd 

Range 

jf  ± SD 

Range 

N 

South  Head 

June 

3.323  ± 

0.836 

2.983-3.749 

37.2  ± 1.22 

28.5-51.8 

15 

July 

3.617  ± 

0.843 

3.345^.278 

41.1  ± 1.19 

35.2-47.5 

7 

Gull  Island 

June 

2.819  ± 

0.171 

2.677-2.895 

31.5  ± 1.36 

19.4^3.7 

5 

July 

2.446  ± 

0.236 

2.330-2.615 

27.6  ± 1.20 

22.2-36.8 

6 

on  Gull  Island  only  1 sparrow  and  2 warblers  were  recorded.  The  de- 
creased number  of  species  on  Gull  Island  results  from  a loss  of  taxonom- 
ically  related  species,  rather  than  the  complete  loss  of  certain  higher  taxa 
or  ecological  guilds.  As  with  richness,  South  Head  species  diversity  was 
significantly  higher  than  species  diversity  on  Gull  Island  {P  < 0.005,  Ta- 
ble 2). 

Total  densities  of  birds  did  not  differ  between  June  and  July  at  either 
Gull  Island  {t  = 0.889,  df  ==  9,  NS)  or  South  Head  {t  = 1.125,  df  = 20, 
NS;  Table  2).  There  was  a significantly  higher  density  of  birds  per  km  at 
South  Head  than  on  Gull  Island  {t  = 3.458,  df  = 31,  P < 0.01).  When 
densities  of  individual  species  are  compared  between  sites  a number  of 
differences  appear.  American  Robins  {Turdus  migratorius)  and  Blackpoll 
Warblers  {Dendroica  striata)  have  significantly  higher  densities  at  South 
Head,  whereas  Northern  Waterthrushes  {Seiurus  noveboracensis),  Boreal 
Chickadees  {Parus  hudsonicus)  and  Gray-cheeked  Thrushes  {Catharus 
minimus)  have  higher  densities  on  Gull  Island.  In  fact,  for  June,  the  den- 
sity of  Boreal  Chickadees  on  Gull  Island  was  significantly  greater  than  the 
combined  density  of  both  chickadee  species  at  South  Head  {t  = 2.220, 
df  = 18,  P < 0.05),  although  the  difference  was  not  present  in  July  (^  = 
0.366,  df  - 11,  NS). 


DISCUSSION 

Consistent  with  contemporary  theory.  Gull  Island  had  a lower  number 
of  species  and  a lower  species  diversity  than  did  the  adjacent  mainland 
area.  The  species  that  were  missing  were  a nonrandom  subset  of  the  South 
Head  avifauna.  On  South  Head  19  of  25  species  recorded  had  a congeneric 
or  close  confamilial  species  present.  On  Gull  Island  only  5 species  had 


346 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Table  3 

Mean  Densities  and  SD  of  Individual  Species  at  Each  Locality'' 


South  Head 


GuU  Island 


Species 


June 
X ±SD 


July 
X ±SD 


June 
^ ± SD 


July 
x ± SD 


't”  between 
sites  (DF) 


Fox  Sparrow 

5.79  ± 

1.25 

6.75  ± 

1.23 

5.81  ± 1.58 

7.78 

1.85 

1.38  (31) 

Pine  Siskin 

4.50  ± 

1.90 

3.80  ± 

2.16 

4.43  ± 1.73 

2.33 

± 

1.72 

1.58  (31) 

American  Robin 

2.99  ± 

1.29 

2.56  ± 

2.78 

0.92  ± 1.34 

0.99 

1.24 

7.12  (31)*** 

Gray-cheeked 

1.35  ± 

1.56 

1.86  ± 

1.36 

2.74  ± 1.20 

3.32 

1.38 

6.10  (31)*** 

Thrush 

Northern 

4.27  ± 

1.30 

3.29  ± 

1.24 

5.39  ± 1.45 

7.39 

-h 

1.68 

1.55  (18)  June 

Waterthrush 

2.30  (20)* 

3.76  (11)  July** 

BlackpoU  Warbler 

6.18  ± 

1.39 

5.61  ± 

1.41 

2.48  ± 1.39 

0.99 

1.71 

5.89  (18)  June** 

3.66  (9)** 

7.06  (11)  July** 

Boreal  Chickadee 

2.28  ± 

1.93 

1.74  ± 

1.80 

6.45  ± 1.69 

2.57 

± 

2.00 

3.19  (18)  June** 

2.42  (9)* 

1.09  (11)  July 

Black-capped 

1.46  ± 

1.51 

1.45  ± 

1.68 

Chickadee 

Boreal  and  Black- 

3.74 

3.19 

vs  Boreal  Chickadee 

2.22  (18)  June* 

capped 

0.37  (11)  July 

chickadees 

® If  monthly  mean  densities  differed  significantly  within  a site,  “t-values  and  degrees  of  freedom  are  entered  below  the 
densities.  Mean  densities  and  standard  deviations  (SD)  are  birds/km  of  transect. 

* P < 0.05;  **  P < 0.01;  ***  P < 0.001. 


close  relatives  present  among  the  11  shared  species.  The  probability  of 
randomly  selecting  11  species  from  the  South  Head  avifauna  and  having 
only  5 close  taxonomic  relatives  is  0.014  (binomial  test;  the  Emberizidae 
were  separated  by  subfamily  and  all  other  divisions  were  at  family  level). 
Furthermore,  the  2 species  added  to  the  Gull  Island  avifauna  are  both  of 
families  not  recorded  at  South  Head  nor  with  other  species  present  on 
Gull  Island.  Taxonomic  closeness  is  commonly  taken  to  imply  some  degree 
of  ecological  similarity  and  potential  for  competition  (e.g.,  Emlen 
1973:316).  Therefore,  these  data  support  the  notion  that  competitive  eco- 
logical interactions  play  an  important  role  in  the  dynamics  of  island  bio- 
geography, at  least  for  small,  nearshore  islands  (Morse  1971,  1977).  The 
proximity  of  Gull  Island  to  South  Head  and  its  relatively  large  size  render 
isolation  or  inadequate  island  size  unlikely  explanations  for  the  nonrandom 
change  in  species  richness. 

As  with  most  other  studies,  it  would  be  difficult  to  fully  assess  all  effects 


Vassallo  and  Rice  • NEWFOUNDLAND  INSULAR  BIOLOGY 


347 


of  habitat  differences  between  Gull  Island  and  South  Head  on  bird  species 
richness,  density  and  diversity.  The  absence  of  pasture  and  small  extent 
of  bogs  on  Gull  Island  could  account  for  the  absence  of  Starlings  {Sturnus 
vulgaris)  and  Swamp  Sparrows  {Melospiza  georgiana),  respectively,  and 
the  larger  extent  of  windfallen  trees  and  associated  litter  and  undergrowth 
on  Gull  Island  could  account  for  the  presence  of  the  Winter  Wren  {Trog- 
lodytes troglodytes).  Otherwise,  the  vegetational  differences  between  the 
island  and  South  Head  are  consistently  differences  in  extremes  of  density 
reached  by  various  plant  taxa,  rather  than  differences  in  overall  habitat 
diversity  or  species  composition  (Vassallo  and  Rice,  in  press).  Therefore, 
using  habitat  differences  to  account  for  the  bird  species  distributions  found 
here  is  only  possible  on  a piecemeal,  nonpredictive  basis. 

Although  the  total  density  of  passerines  on  GuU  Island  was  lower  than 
at  South  Head,  this  difference  does  not  appear  to  be  simply  a reflection 
of  a lower  habitat  diversity  on  Gull  Island.  This  density  reduction  did  not 
reflect  either  an  overall  decline  in  numbers  of  each  species,  nor  merely 
the  effects  of  an  uncompensated  loss  of  some  taxa.  Some  density  differ- 
ences, such  as  the  increased  density  of  Boreal  Chickadees  on  Gull  Island 
in  the  absence  of  the  Black-capped  Chickadee  (P.  atricapillus),  are  con- 
sistent with  the  notion  of  competitive  release  (MacArthur  et  al.  1972, 
MacArthur  et  al.  1973,  Yeaton  and  Cody  1974).  However,  other  species, 
e.g.,  the  Gray-cheeked  Thrush,  also  showed  a significant  increase  in  den- 
sity, although  2 thrushes  were  present  at  both  sites.  Furthermore,  the 
Blackpoll  Warbler  was  exposed  to  fewer  potentially  competing  canopy 
foragers  and  at  least  as  much  suitable  habitat  on  the  island  but  nonetheless 
had  a lower  density  there. 

Apparently,  as  with  the  effects  of  changing  habitat  diversity  on  bird 
species  diversity  and  richness,  the  density  effects  of  changes  in  the  avian 
community  do  not  follow  a few  rigorously  predictable  rules.  The  commu- 
nity dynamics  are  complex,  and  conditions  in  which  density  compensation 
I and  competitive  release  will  occur  are  not  universally  specifiable. 

In  this  study,  we  found  a lower  species  diversity,  a loss  of  close  ecolog- 
ical and  taxonomic  relatives  and  a decrease  in  overall  avian  density  on 
; Gull  Island.  These  effects  were  all  observed,  although  the  mainland  area 
is  itself  an  island  showing  substantial  decreases  in  bird  species  richness 
and  diversity  relative  to  continental  North  America.  The  factors  producing 
the  difference  in  island  fauna  apparently  do  not  merely  filter  once  to  pro- 
duce an  island  fauna  of  good  colonists  (“supertramps”  of  Diamond  1975), 
but  similar  effects  are  produced  through  the  community  dynamics  of  col- 
onization when  the  island  itself  becomes  a source  for  another  island  (Ter- 
borgh  and  Faaborg  1973,  Terborgh  et  al.  1978). 


348 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


SUMMARY 

Although  the  island  of  Newfoundland  shows  a characteristically  depauperate  avifauna 
relative  to  continental  Maritime  Canada,  Gull  Island,  a small  offshore  island,  has  lower 
passerine  species  richness  than  does  a nearby  area  of  comparable  habitat  on  the  Newfound- 
land coast.  The  decrease  from  25  to  13  species  was  due  neither  to  the  loss  of  aU  members 
of  some  higher  taxa  nor  to  completely  stochastic  species  losses.  Rather  it  reflected  the 
reduction  of  groups  of  congeneric  or  confamilial  species  in  the  coastal  area  to  single  species 
represented  on  the  island.  Overall  density  compensation  did  not  occur,  and  patterns  of 
density  difference  of  individual  species  showed  some  insular  increases,  some  decreases,  and 
some  cases  of  no  difference.  Habitat  differences  between  the  sites  account  for  much  but  not 
all  of  these  avian  community  differences. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Much  of  this  work  was  completed  while  the  first  author  was  a M.Sc.  student  at  Memorial 
University  of  Newfoundland,  and  was  supported  in  part  by  a fellowship  from  the  graduate 
school.  Field  and  transportation  expenses  were  provided  by  a National  Research  Council  of 
Canada  grant  to  the  second  author.  Susan  Noseworthy  gave  field  assistance  on  Gull  Island 
and  Leonard  Vassallo  provided  both  field  assistance  and  much  appreciated  support.  Drs.  D. 
Morse,  P.  Scott,  D.  Steele  and  W.  ThrelfaU  gave  valuable  comments  on  the  thesis  and/or 
this  manuscript.  Request  reprints  from  J.  C.  Rice. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Cody,  M.  L.  and  B.  B.  J.  Cody.  1972.  Territory  size,  food  density  and  clutch  size  in  island 
wren  populations.  Condor  75:473^77. 

Crowell,  K.  L.  1961.  The  effects  of  reduced  competition  in  birds.  Proc.  Natl.  Acad.  Sci. 
U.S.A.  47:240-243. 

Diamond,  J.  M.  1970.  Ecological  consequences  of  island  colonization  by  Southwest  Pacific 
birds.  H.  The  effect  of  species  diversity  on  total  population  density.  Proc.  Natl.  Acad. 
Sci.  U.S.A.  67:1715-1721. 

. 1975.  Assembly  of  species  communities.  Pp.  342^144  in  Ecology  and  evolution  of 

communities  (M.  L.  Cody  and  J.  M.  Diamond,  eds.).  Belknap,  Cambridge,  Massachu- 
setts. 

Emlen,  j.  M.  1973.  Ecology:  an  evolutionary  approach.  Addison-\^  esley  Publishing  Co., 
Reading,  Massachusetts. 

Godfrey,  W.  E.  1966.  The  birds  of  Canada.  Natl.  Mus.  Canada  BuU.  203.  Ottawa,  Ontario. 
Grant,  P.  R.  1966a.  Ecological  compatibility  of  bird  species  on  islands.  Am.  Nat.  100:451- 
462. 

. 1966b.  The  density  of  land  birds  on  the  Tres  Marias  Islands  in  Mexico.  I.  Numbers 

and  biomass.  Can.  J.  Zool.  44:391^00. 

Klopfer,  P.  H.  and  R.  H.  MacArthlr.  1960.  Niche  size  and  faunal  diversity.  Am.  Nat. 
94:293-300. 

AND . 1961.  On  the  causes  of  tropical  species  diversity:  niche  overlap.  Am. 

Nat.  95:223-226. 

Lynch,  J.  F.  and  N.  K.  Johnson.  1974.  Turnover  and  equilibria  in  insular  avifaunas,  with 
special  reference  to  the  California  Channel  Islands.  Condor  76:370-384. 

MacArthur,  R.  H.  and  E.  O.  \\  ILSON.  1963.  An  equilibrium  theory  of  insular  zoogeog- 
raphy. Evolution  17:373-387. 

AND . 1967.  The  theory  of  island  biogeography.  .Monogr.  Pop.  Biol.  1.  Prince- 
ton University  Press.  Princeton,  N.J. 


Vassallo  and  Rice  • NEWFOUNDLAND  INSULAR  BIOLOGY 


349 


, J.  H.  Diamond  and  J.  R.  K.\RR.  1972.  Density  compensation  in  island  faunas. 

Ecology  53:330-342. 

, J.  MacArthur,  D.  MacArthur  and  A.  MacArthur.  1973.  The  effect  of  island 

area  on  population  densities.  Ecology  54:657-658. 

Morse,  D.  H.  1971.  The  foraging  of  warblers  isolated  on  smaU  islands.  Ecology  52:216- 
228. 

. 1977.  The  occupation  of  small  islands  by  passerine  birds.  Condor  79:399^12. 

Peters,  H.  S.  and  T.  D.  Burleigh.  1951.  The  birds  of  Newfoundland.  Nfld.  Dept.  Nat. 
Resour.,  St.  John’s,  Newfoundland. 

PlELOU,  E.  C.  1966a.  Shannon’s  formula  as  a measure  of  species  diversity:  its  use  and 
misuse.  Am.  Nat.  100:463^465. 

. 1966b.  The  measure  of  diversity  in  different  types  of  biological  collections.  J.  The- 

oret.  Biol.  13:131-144. 

Power,  D.  M.  1976.  Avifauna  richness  on  the  California  Channel  Islands.  Condor  78:394- 
398. 

Rotenberry,  j.  T.  1978.  Components  of  avian  diversity  along  a multifactorial  climatic 
gradient.  Ecology  59:693-699. 

Rowe,  J.  S.  1972.  Forest  regions  of  Canada.  Dept.  Environ.  Can.  For.  Serv.  Publ.  No. 
1300.  Information  Canada,  Ottawa,  Ontario. 

SiMBERLOFF,  D.  S.  AND  E.  O.  WiLSON.  1969.  Experimental  zoogeography  of  islands:  the 
colonization  of  empty  islands.  Ecology  50:278-295. 

SOKAL,  R.  R.  AND  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1969.  Biometry.  W.  H.  Freeman,  San  Francisco,  California. 

Terborgh,  j.  and  j.  Faaborg.  1973.  Turnover  and  ecological  release  in  the  avifauna  of 
Mona  Island,  Puerto  Rico.  Auk  90:759-779. 

, AND  H.  J.  Brockman.  1978.  Island  colonization  by  Lesser  Antillean  birds. 

Auk  95:59-72. 

Vassallo,  M.  I.  and  J.  C.  Rice.  Ecological  release  and  ecological  flexibility  in  habitat  use 
and  foraging  behavior  of  an  insular  avifauna.  Wilson  Bull.  In  press. 

Yeaton,  R.  I.  AND  M.  L.  Cody.  1974.  Competitive  release  in  island  Song  Sparrow  popu- 
lations. Theoret.  Pop.  Biol.  5:42-58. 

DEPT.  BIOLOGY,  MEMORIAL  UNIV.  OE  NEWEOUNDLAND,  ST.  JOHN’S,  NEW- 
FOUNDLAND aIB  2x8  CANADA.  (PRESENT  ADDRESS  JCR:  CENTER  FOR 
ENVIRONMENTAL  STUDIES,  ARIZONA  STATE  UNIV.,  TEMPE,  ARIZONA 
85281.)  ACCEPTED  1 OCT.  1980. 


INTERNATIONAL  COMMISSION  ON 
ZOOLOGICAL  NOMENCLATURE 

The  following  opinion  has  been  published  by  the  ICZN  in  the  Bulletin  of  Zoological 
Nomenclature,  Vol.  38,  Pt.  2,  30  Apr.  1981:  Opinion  No.  1180  (p.  12)  ""Thamnophilus 
amazonicus  Sclater,  1858  (Aves):  conserved.”  The  ICZN  cannot  supply  separates  of 
Opinions. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  350-356 


ENVIRONMENTAL  INFLUENCE  ON  SOARING  IN 
WINTERING  RED-TAILED  HAWKS 

Charles  R.  Preston 


A variety  of  environmental  factors  are  known  to  influence  the  flight 
activity  of  diurnal  raptors.  Craighead  and  Craighead  (1956)  reported  that 
flight  by  wintering  Red-tailed  Hawks  {Buteo  jamaicensis)  decreased  mark- 
edly during  periods  of  “bad  weather.”  SchneU  (1967)  demonstrated  that 
Rough-legged  Hawks  {B.  lagopus)  flew  significantly  less  as  wind  velocity, 
barometric  pressure  and  ambient  temperature  decreased,  and  as  cloudi- 
ness and  relative  humidity  increased.  Similarly,  Bildstein  (1978)  reported 
that  variations  in  solar  radiation,  ambient  temperature,  wind  velocity,  rel- 
ative humidity  and  precipitation  were  accompanied  by  shifts  in  the  flight 
activity  of  4 species  of  open  habitat  raptors.  In  one  of  the  few  quantitative 
studies  dealing  exclusively  with  soaring  flight,  Henty  (1977)  showed  that 
soaring  activity  in  several  raptor  species  increased  as  ambient  tempera- 
tures increased. 

In  this  study,  I used  multivariate  statistical  techniques  to  investigate 
the  effects  of  several  environmental  factors  on  the  soaring  activity  of  win- 
tering Red-tailed  Hawks  in  northwestern  Arkansas.  I also  examined  the 
influence  of  environmental  factors  on  habitat  use  and  altitude  of  soaring 
hawks. 


STUDY  AREA  AND  METHODS 

The  study  was  conducted  in  a 244-km^  area  near  Centerton,  Benton  Co.,  Arkansas.  There 
the  flat  to  gently  rolling  terrain  comprised  scattered  patches  of  pastureland,  mixed  hard- 
woods. old  fields  and  cultivated  fields.  The  few  distinct  ridges  in  the  study  area  were  grown 
primarily  in  mixed  hardwoods.  V(  oodlots  and  pastures  together  comprised  about  85%  of  the 
study  area. 

Data  were  collected  on  12  days  (6  li/day)  between  14  December  1976  and  25  February'  1977 
and  between  1 December  1977  and  28  January  1978.  I located  hawks  by  driving  along  sec- 
ondary roads  throughout  the  study  area  and  did  not  knowingly  collect  data  on  any  individual 
hawk  more  than  once  in  a day.  I measured  ambient  temperature  and  relative  humidity  every 
hour  afield  with  a sling  psychrometer.  AU  other  weather  data  were  recorded  as  each  hawk 
was  observed.  A Dwyer  wind  meter  was  used  to  measure  wind  velocity  at  chest  height.  Solar 
illumination  was  measured  with  an  illuminometer.  Percent  cloud  cover  was  obtained  with  a 
circular  mirror,  15  cm  in  diameter,  marked  with  a 25-unit  grid.  This  technique  is  described 
in  detail  elsewhere  (Preston  1980). 

In  addition  to  weather  variables,  measures  of  habitat  use  were  obtained  for  50  soaring 
hawks  chosen  at  random.  Due  to  time  restrictions,  it  was  not  feasible  to  sample  the  habitat 
below  every  soaring  hawk  observed.  The  site  above  which  a hawk  was  soaring  when  first 
observed  was  considered  the  center  of  a circular  0.162-ha  sampling  area.  Four  orthogonal 


350 


Preston  • RED-TAILED  HAWK  SOARING 


351 


Table  1 

Mean,  Standard  Deviation  and  Range  oe  Each  Weather  Parameter 


i ± SD 

Range 

Ambient  temp.  (°C) 

2.8  ± 7.31 

-16.0-14.0 

% relative  humidity 

49.5  ± 12.34 

21.0-79.0 

Wind  velocity  (mph) 

10.1  ± 9.77 

0.0-31.0 

Solar  ihumination  (foot  candles) 

702.0  ± 378.25 

105.0-1800.0 

% cloud  cover 

42.8  ± 30.96 

0.0-100.0 

transects  were  established  from  the  center  of  each  area,  the  first  being  set  by  the  random 
position  of  the  crosshairs  of  a sighting  tube.  Each  transect  was  45  m long  and  constituted 
the  radius  of  the  0.162-ha  circle.  The  habitat  type  (pasture,  old  field,  cropland,  woodlot) 
encountered  at  each  of  25  random  stops  along  each  transect  was  recorded.  These  100  stops 
were  used  to  calculate  habitat  percentages  for  each  sampling  area.  The  technique  is  modified 
from  James  and  Shugart  (1970). 

The  soaring  altitude  of  each  of  these  50  hawks  was  estimated  using  a transparent  pane  of 
glass  marked  with  silhouette  representations  of  Red-tailed  Hawks  as  they  would  appear  at 
various  distances,  up  to  92  m from  the  observer.  A taxidermy  specimen  was  used  to  calibrate 
the  scale.  Only  3 of  the  50  hawks  were  observed  soaring  above  92  m.  Estimates  derived  from 
the  scale  are  subject  to  some  error  due  to  the  intraspecific  size  variation. 

The  data  were  analyzed  using  statistical  programs  in  the  computer  library  at  the  University 
of  Arkansas.  The  72  h of  data  were  separated  initiahy  into  24  three-h  observation  periods 
(2/observation  day)  and  the  mean  value  of  each  weather  variable  was  calculated  for  each 
observation  period.  The  percentage  of  hawks  soaring  when  first  observed  was  also  calculated 
for  each  observation  period.  Pearson’s  product-moment  correlation  analysis  (Sokal  and  Rohlf 
1969)  was  used  to  test  for  associations  between  environmental  factors  and  soaring  activity 
and  habitat  use.  After  transforming  the  data  to  minimize  non-normality  and  heteroscedac- 
ticity  (Box  and  Cox  1964,  Sokal  and  Rohlf  1969.  Andrews  et  al.  1971).  a multivariate  analysis 
of  variance  (MANOVA)  (Morrison  1967)  with  a step-down  procedure  (Bargmann  1962)  was 
used  to  test  for  a significant  difference  in  soaring  incidence  with  respect  to  environmental 
factors.  Then  discriminant  function  scores  were  generated  and  were  used  to  characterize 
environmental  conditions  associated  with  soaring  activity. 

RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

The  means  and  standard  deviations  of  each  weather  factor  are  given  in 
Table  1.  Table  2 shows  that  the  percentage  of  hawks  observed  soaring 
increased  significantly  as  relative  humidity  and  cloud  cover  decreased, 
and  as  wind  velocity  and  solar  illumination  increased.  Because  these  4 
variables  were  highly  intercorrelated,  partial  correlation  analysis  (Morrison 
1967)  was  used  to  clarify  the  association  between  each  of  these  factors 
and  the  incidence  of  soaring.  The  partial  correlation  coefficients  (wind 
velocity  0.907;  P < 0.001,  illumination  0.101;  P > 0.05,  cloud  cover 
— 0.103;  P > 0.05,  relative  humidity  —0.036;  P > 0.05)  show  that  only 


352 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Table  2 

Product-Moment  Correlation  Coefficients  for  Environmental  Factors  and  the 
Incidence  of  Soaring  During  Observation  Periods^ 


Ambient 

temp. 

Relative 

humidity 

Wind 

velocity 

Solar 

illumination 

% cloud 
cover 

% of  hawks  soaring 

0.223 

-0.729* 

0.864* 

0.799* 

-0.765* 

Ambient  temperature 

0.211 

0.209 

0.207 

0.158 

Relative  humidity 

-0.707* 

-0.764* 

0.758* 

Wind  velocity 

0.701 

-0.768* 

Solar  illumination 

-0.871* 

® Twenty-four  3-h  observation  periods. 

* Indicates  significant  correlation  at  P ^ 0.05. 


wind  velocity  was  associated  significantly  with  the  incidence  of  soaring 
when  other  variables  were  held  constant.  Fig.  1 illustrates  the  observed 
relationship  between  wind  velocity  and  incidence  of  soaring. 

Similarly,  MANOVA  showed  a highly  significant  difference  in  soaring 
activity  with  respect  to  environmental  variables  ( — m In  X = 76.40,  P < 
0.001).  The  associated  step-down  procedure  identified  wind  velocity  as  the 
only  variable  which,  taken  by  itself,  contributed  significantly  {P  < 0.001) 
to  the  difference.  The  discriminant  function  (a  linear  function  of  the  orig- 
inal environmental  variables)  stressed  those  factors  separating  soaring 
from  non-soaring  activity  (Sokal  and  Rohlf  1969).  Wind  velocity,  illumi- 
nation, cloud  cover  and  relative  humidity  were  highly  correlated  with  the 
discriminant  function  (Table  3)  and  thus  were  important  in  characterizing 
a weather  gradient  associated  with  soaring  activity  (Fig.  2). 

In  his  discussion  of  soaring.  Cone  (1962)  differentiated  static  soaring 
involving  the  use  of  rising  air  columns,  from  dynamic  soaring  involving 
the  use  of  wind  gradients.  He  further  classified  static  soaring  into  declivity 
(or  slope)  and  thermal  soaring.  Declivity  currents  arise  when  wind  is  de- 


Table  3 

(a)efficients  of  Correlation  Between  Each  Weather  P.\rameter  and 
Discriminant  Function  (After  Data  Stabilization) 


Discriminant  function 

Ambient  temperature 

0.148 

Relative  humidity 

-0.459 

Vi  ind  velocity 

0.901 

Solar  illumination 

0.595 

Cloud  cover 

-0.503 

Freston  • RED-TAILED  HAWK  SOARING 


353 


100 

90- 

80  H 

e> 

Z 70  H 
q: 

< 60- 

CO 

50 

h- 

g 40 

o 

g 30 

0. 

20 

10 


© 


© 

© 


© 


© 

0 


© 

© © © 
® © 


© 


© ©^ 


© € 


■©■ 


“T" 

30 


35 


T 1 ] 1 

5 10  15  20  25 

WIND  VELOCITY  (MPH) 

Fig.  1.  Observed  association  between  wind  velocity  and  the  incidence  of  soaring  for  each 
of  the  24  three-h  observation  periods.  Circled  numbers  indicate  how'  many  hawks  were 
observed  during  each  period. 


fleeted  upward  by  surface  obstacles  such  as  hills.  Thermals  are  formed  as 
surface  layers  of  air  become  warmed  and/or  moisture-laden  by  the  sun- 
heated  earth.  These  less  dense  bubbles  of  warm  air  rise  steadily.  Cone 
(1962)  concluded  that  thermal  soaring  was  the  most  important  method  of 
soaring  flight  used  by  land  birds.  However,  Pennycuick  (1972)  emphasized 
that  among  raptors  the  use  of  declivity  currents  for  soaring  above  hillsides 
is  very  common. 

The  association  that  I found  between  wind  velocity  and  soaring  activity 
could  indicate  use  of  either  declivity  or  dynamic  soaring.  Although  the 
relatively  low-aspect-ratio  wings  of  Red-tailed  Hawks  are  not  particularly 
well-adapted  for  any  method  of  soaring,  they  are  common  in  birds  spe- 
cializing in  static  soaring  (Cone  1962,  Welty  1962,  Pennycuick  1972).  Fur- 
thermore, dynamic  soaring  has  generally  not  been  considered  important 
to  land  soarers  mainly  due  to  the  lack  of  a wind  gradient  above  land  masses 
(Pennycuick  1972).  Correlation  analysis  showed  general  independence  be- 
tween environmental  factors  and  habitat  use.  However,  there  was  a sig- 


354 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


50- 
45 
40- 
35- 
30- 
25- 
20- 
15  - 
10- 
5 - 


-5.0 


SOARING  HAWKS 


“T 

4.0 


NON- SOARING  HAWKS 


-3.0 


— 1 — 

2.0 


DECREASING  WIND  VELOCITY  AND  SOLAR  ILLUMINATION 

' » 

INCREASING  CLOUDINESS  AND  RELATIVE  HUMIDITY 

Fig.  2.  Separation  between  soaring  (N  = 77)  and  non-soaring  (N  = 103)  hawks  along  the 
discriminant  function  axis. 


nificant  positive  correlation  between  wind  velocity  and  soaring  altitude 
(P  < 0.05).  Higher  wind  velocities  can  increase  the  declivity  “soaring 
zone”  surrounding  hills  and  ridges,  thus  allowing  the  hawks  to  soar  higher 
(Pennycuick  1972,  Grace  1977).  The  relationship  between  wind  velocity 
and  soaring  activity  needs  to  be  quantified  in  relatively  barren,  flat  areas 
devoid  of  significant  declivity  currents.  Reports  by  Hankin  (1913),  Cone 
(1962)  and  Henty  (1977)  indicate  that  increasing  ambient  temperature, 
rather  than  wind  velocity,  may  be  the  dominant  weather  factor  associated 
with  soaring  activity  in  the  tropics  and  during  temperate  seasons  when 
conditions  are  conducive  to  thermal  soaring.  Because  adverse  conditions 
such  as  fog  or  precipitation  depress  most  raptor  flight  activity  (Craighead 
and  Craighead  1956,  Schnell  1967,  Bildstein  1978),  it  would  be  an  over- 
simplification to  attribute  absolute  control  of  soaring  frequency  to  1 en- 
vironmental factor  in  any  season  or  climate. 

Although  early  reports  emphasized  hunting  as  the  primary  function  of 
Buteo  soaring,  Wakeley  (1978)  found  that  Ferruginous  Hawks  {B.  regalis) 
spent  far  more  time  soaring  than  predicted  from  capture/cost  ratios  for 
that  hunting  method.  Thermoregulation,  territorial  display  and  exploration 
have  been  mentioned  as  some  alternative  functions  of  soaring  (see  Wake- 


Preston  • RED-TAILED  HAWK  SOARING 


355 


ley  1978).  Studies  designed  to  determine  Buteo  species  distributions  with 
respect  to  both  thermal  and  declivity  soaring  opportunities  will  prove  use- 
ful in  evaluating  the  importance  of  soaring  (other  than  migratory)  as  an 
adaptive  activity. 


SUMMARY 

The  incidence  of  soaring  by  Red-tailed  Hawks  wintering  in  northwestern  Arkansas  fluc- 
tuated with  several  weather  parameters.  The  percentage  of  hawks  observed  soaring  increased 
as  wind  velocity  and  solar  illumination  increased,  and  as  cloud  cover  and  relative  humidity 
decreased.  W ind  velocity  was  the  most  important  factor  associated  with  soaring  incidence. 
Hawks  also  soared  at  greater  altitudes  as  wind  velocity  increased.  The  results  are  interpreted 
to  reflect  the  use  of  declivity  updrafts  to  soar  under  conditions  not  conducive  to  thermal 
soaring. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

I wish  to  thank  D.  A.  James  for  advice  and  encouragement  during  the  study.  P.  S.  Hatcher 
provided  valuable  assistance  with  some  of  the  fieldwork.  I am  also  indebted  to  J.  Ballam,  G. 
D.  SchneU  and  K.  L.  Bildstein  for  comments  on  various  versions  of  the  manuscript.  The 
study  was  supported  in  part  by  the  Arkansas  Audubon  Society  Trust  Fund  and  the  Depart- 
ment of  Zoology,  University  of  Arkansas,  Fayetteville,  Arkansas. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Andrews,  D.  F.  R.,  R.  Gnanidesikan  and  J.  F.  W arner.  1971.  Transformation  of  multi- 
variate data.  Biometrics  27:825—840. 

Bargmann,  R.  1962.  Representative  ordering  and  selection  of  variables.  Coop.  Res.  Proj. 

No.  1132,  Final  Rept.  U.S.  Off.  Health,  Educ.,  Welfare,  Washington,  D.C. 

Bildstein,  K.  L.  1978.  Behavioral  ecology  of  Red-tailed  Hawks  (Buteo  jamaicensis).  Rough- 
legged Hawks  (B.  lagopus).  Northern  Harriers  (Circus  cyaneus),  American  Kestrels  (Fal- 
co  sparvarius),  and  other  raptorial  birds  wintering  in  south-central  Ohio.  Ph.D.  diss., 
Ohio  State  Univ.,  Columbus,  Ohio. 

Box,  G.  E.  P.  AND  D.  R.  Cox.  1964.  An  analysis  of  transformations.  J.  Roy.  Stat.  Soc. 
26:211-252. 

Cone,  C.  D.  1962.  Thermal  soaring  of  birds.  Am.  Sci.  50:180-209. 

Craighead,  J.  J.  and  F.  C.  Craighead,  Jr.  1956.  Hawks,  owls,  and  wildlife.  Stackpole 
Co.,  Harrisburg,  Pennsylvania. 

Grace,  J.  1977.  Plant  response  to  wind.  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

Hankin,  E.  H.  1913.  Animal  flight.  Watts  and  Co.,  London,  England. 

Henty,  C.  j.  1977.  Thermal  soaring  of  raptors.  Br.  Birds  70:471^75. 

James,  F.  C.  and  H.  H.  Shugart,  Jr.  1970.  A quantitative  method  of  habitat  description. 
Audubon  Field  Notes  24:727-738. 

Morrison,  D.  F.  1967.  Multivariate  statistical  methods.  McGraw  Hill.  New  York,  New 
York. 

Pennycuick,  C.  j.  1972.  Animal  flight.  Edward  Arnold,  London,  England. 

Preston,  C.  R.  1980.  Differential  perch-site  selection  by  color  morphs  of  the  Red-tailed 
Hawk  (Buteo  jamaicensis).  Auk  97:782-789. 

ScHNELL,  G.  D.  1967.  Environmental  influence  on  the  incidence  of  flight  in  the  Rough- 
legged Hawk.  Auk  84:173-182. 


356 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


SoKAL,  R.  R.  AND  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1969.  Biometry.  W.  H.  Freeman,  San  Francisco,  California. 
Wakeley,  J.  S.  1978.  Hunting  methods  and  factors  affecting  their  use  by  Ferruginous 
Hawks.  Condor  80:327-333. 

Welty,  j.  C.  1962.  The  life  of  birds.  W.  B.  Saunders  Co.,  Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania. 

DEPT.  ZOOLOGY,  UNIV.  ARKANSAS,  FAYETTEVILLE,  ARKANSAS  72701.  AC- 
CEPTED 20  OCT.  1980. 


THE  WILSON  ORNITHOLOGICAL  SOCIETY  ANNOUNCES  AWARDS 

Louis  Agassiz  Fuertes,  Margaret  Morse  Nice  and 
Paul  A.  Stewart  Awards 

Fuertes  Awards  are  available  to  aU  ornithologists  although  graduate  students  and  young 
professionals  are  preferred.  Nice  Awards  are  intended  for  independent  researchers  without 
access  to  funds  and  facilities  available  at  colleges  and  universities  and  thus  are  restricted 
to  amateurs,  including  high  school  students.  Any  type  of  research  may  be  funded  by  both 
Fuertes  and  Nice  awards. 

Stewart  Awards  are  available  to  any  applicant  for  ornithological  research,  especially 
studies  of  bird  movements  based  on  banding  and  analysis  of  recoveries  and  returns  and 
investigations  in  economic  ornithology. 

One  Fuertes  Award  of  at  least  $100.00,  one  Nice  Award  of  $100.00  and  one  or  more  Stewart 
Awards  of  $200.00  each  will  be  made.  Interested  applicants  should  write  to  Carl  D.  Marti, 
Department  of  Zoology,  Weber  State  College,  Ogden,  Utah  84408.  Completed  applications 
must  be  received  by  1 March  1982.  Decisions  will  be  announced  at  the  1982  Annual  Meeting 
of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  to  be  held  6-9  May  1982. 


Aaron  M.  Bagg  Student  Membership  Awards 

Student  membership  awards  in  The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  providing  a 1-year 
membership  in  the  Society  are  available  for  persons  not  currently  members  of  the  Society. 
These  awards  are  funded  by  a donation  made  in  the  memory  of  Aaron  M.  Bagg,  a former 
president  of  the  Society.  Application  forms  for  1982  awards  may  be  obtained  from  John 
L.  Zimmerman,  Division  of  Biology,  Kansas  State  University,  Manhattan,  Kansas  66506. 
Deadline  for  applying  is  1 November  1981. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  357-362 


BREEDING  SUCCESS  IN  AN  ISOLATED  POPULATION 
OE  ROCK  DOVES 

David  E.  Preble  and  Frank  H.  Heppner 

There  has  been  considerable  discussion  about  actual  or  potential  means 
by  which  some  animal  populations  might  maintain  stability  in  the  face  of 
variable  environmental  pressures.  This  paper  reports  the  results  of  a 
2-year  study  of  breeding  success  in  an  isolated  population  of  Rock  Doves 
{Columba  livia,  hereafter  referred  to  as  the  pigeon)  which  suggests  that 
an  increase  in  egg  predation  is  followed  by  a lowering  of  the  adult  nest 
desertion  rate,  thus  maintaining  the  recruitment  of  new  individuals  into 
the  population  at  a constant  rate. 

STUDY  AREA  AND  METHODS 

The  study  involved  a breeding  colony  of  45-55  pigeons  (depending  on  season)  in  the 
abandoned  Plum  Beach  lighthouse  in  Narragansett  Bay,  Rhode  Island.  This  structure  is 
separated  from  land  0.8  km  to  the  west  and  1.2  km  to  the  east.  Food  is  readily  available  year 
around  near  habitation  to  the  west  and  east  from  bird  feeders  and  natural  sources,  but  there 
is  no  food  or  water  on  the  lighthouse  itself.  Severe  storms  and  fog  during  the  winter  could 
cut  off  access  to  food  and  affect  adult  mortality.  Great  Cormorants  (Phalacrocorax  carbo) 
roosted  on  the  upper  outside  portion  of  the  structure.  The  Black-crowned  Night  Heron 
{Nycticorax  nycticorax)  was  an  occasional  summertime  visitor.  No  other  vertebrates  were 
known  to  be  in  the  lighthouse.  To  minimize  disturbance,  we  did  not  band  individuals,  and 
so  do  not  have  an  accurate  measure  of  the  number  of  non-breeders  or  colony  size.  The 
number  of  active  nests  ranged  from  two  during  breeding  lows  to  25  at  breeding  peaks. 

To  determine  the  recruitment  rate  (total  breeding  success,  or  number  of  young  fledged  per 
eggs  laid),  we  visited  the  lighthouse  every  1-2  weeks  in  1971  and  1972.  On  each  visit,  the 
location  of  every  nest,  egg  and  young  was  recorded  on  maps  of  each  of  the  5 levels  of  the 
8-m-diameter  lighthouse.  The  nests  were  scattered  through  each  level,  and  were  no  closer 
than  1 m to  each  other.  During  the  first  full  year  of  observation  we  left  the  nests  untouched 
and  in  the  second  year  we  removed  a fraction  of  eggs  laid,  simulating  the  action  of  an  egg 
predator.  The  objective  was  to  see  if  egg  desertion  would  drop  by  an  amount  corresponding 
to  the  number  removed,  thus  maintaining  a constant  recruitment  rate.  This  artificial  pre- 
dation was  performed  by  counting  the  number  of  eggs  laid  since  the  last  visit.  Twenty  percent 
of  this  number  were  then  removed  from  the  total  number  present. 

The  removed  eggs  were  checked  for  fertility  and  stage  of  development.  Egg  replacement 
by  the  birds  would  have  biased  the  results,  but  the  pigeon  is  a determinate  layer  with  a 
2-egg  clutch-size  (Sturkie  1954).  At  the  end  of  another  full  year,  the  results  were  tabulated 
and  compared  both  with  the  data  from  the  first  year,  and  with  breeding  data  from  Murton 
and  Clarke  (1968). 

RESULTS 

Breeding  season. — The  Plum  Beach  light  pigeons  are  year-round  breed- 
ers, but  demonstrate  a different  annual  pattern  than  seen  in  the  British 


357 


358 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


B. 

Fig.  I.  Number  of  eggs  (expressed  as  a monthly  percent  of  the  year’s  total)  laid  in  each 
month  by  pigeons  (A)  in  Yorkshire,  England  (Murton  and  Clarke  1968);  composite  of  2 years, 
1965-66;  (B)  in  Narragansett  Bay,  Rhode  Island;  monthly  average  for  the  years  1971-72  and 
1972-73. 

Isles  (Fig.  1)  by  Murton  and  Clarke  (1968)  or  Lees  (1946).  Lees  found  a 
constant  low  level  of  breeding  in  northern  Scotland,  with  high  activity  at 
4 periods;  April,  August,  November  and  January.  Murton  and  Clarke 
(1968)  found  the  same  pattern  in  England,  with  the  spring  and  summer 
periods  accounting  for  60%  of  the  total  annual  breeding.  At  the  Plum 
Beach  light,  however,  there  was  a marked  low  in  August  and  September, 
followed  by  a rather  steady  rise  to  a high  point  in  February  and  March, 
which  was  then  followed  by  a steady  drop  through  the  spring  until  the 
August  low  point  was  reached  again. 

Breeding  success. — The  observations  recorded  at  each  visit  to  the  light- 
house are  shown  in  Table  1.  The  difference  between  the  years  in  total  egg 
and  total  juvenile  observations  is  the  result  of  more  visits  in  the  second 
year,  since  the  total  egg  and  juvenile  observation  figures  are  the  sum  of 
the  total  number  of  eggs  or  juveniles  present  at  each  visit.  The  total  num- 
ber of  new  eggs  and  total  number  of  new  juveniles  was  unaffected  by  the 
number  and  frequency  of  visits,  since  visits  were  close  enough  together 


Preble  and  Heppner  • ROCK  DOVE  BREEDING  SUCCESS 


359 


Table  1 

Comparison  of  Breeding  Success  of  Pigeons  in  Non-isolated  (Murton  and  Clarke 
1968)  AND  Isolated  (Preble  1973)  Lighthouses 


Murton  and  Clarke 

Preble 

1965  1966 

1971-72 

1972-73 

Total  egg  observations 

— 

— 

948 

1295 

Total  juvenile  observations 

— 

— 

547 

860 

Total  eggs  laid 

346 

409 

562 

672 

Total  no.  hatched 

211 

286 

312 

388 

% hatched 

61 

70 

56 

58 

% egg  predation 

25 

15 

1 (8)" 

22  (145) 

% infertile 

5 

5 

5^’ 

4(6) 

% deserted 

9 

10 

38  (218) 

16  (106) 

% fledged  of  eggs  hatched 

70 

71 

77c 

0 

CO 

00 

% fledged  of  eggs  laid 

43 

49 

42" 

48" 

® Numbers  in  parentheses  are  actual  numbers  of  eggs. 
’’  Approximation,  see  text. 

' Calculated  value,  see  text. 


that  no  eggs  could  be  laid  or  juveniles  hatched  between  visits  without 
being  counted.  The  lower  new  egg  and  new  juvenile  production  for  the 
first  year  is,  in  part,  due  to  the  termination  of  the  first  year’s  data  acqui- 
sition, due  to  adverse  weather,  1 full  week  before  the  end  of  a complete 
year,  in  February  1972.  Inflating  the  production  of  the  second  year  was 
the  high  number  of  eggs  laid  in  January  and  February  1973,  probably  due 
to  an  unusually  mild  and  snowless  winter  which  resulted  in  a large  number 
of  adults  in  breeding  condition. 

The  parameters  involved  in  breeding  success  for  the  2 years  at  Plum 
Beach  light  and,  for  comparison,  the  2 years  that  Murton  and  Clarke  (1968) 
worked  on  the  beacon  tower  at  Flamborough  Head,  are  also  shown  in 
Table  1.  On  the  Plum  Beach  light,  1971-72,  total  egg  predation  consisted 
of  8 eggs  (1%)  pecked  open  or  removed  between  10  June  and  3 August 
1971.  During  1972-73  natural  predation  consisted  of  9 eggs  pecked  open 
or  removed,  and  was  supplemented  by  the  experimental  removal  of  an 
additional  136  eggs  to  bring  the  total  “predation”  level  for  that  year  to 
22%. 

The  number  of  infertile  eggs  (Table  1)  was  determined  in  1972-73  by 
opening  and  examining  the  eggs  removed.  The  number  of  infertile  eggs 
was  six,  which  is  4.4%  of  the  total,  or  1.8-9. 4%  at  95%  confidence  limits. 
This  value  is  in  the  same  range  (x^2>  = 0.01,  P > 0.99,  NS)  as  the  5% 
(3. 6-6. 8%  at  95%  confidence  limits)  reported  by  Murton  and  Clarke  (1968). 
Since  there  was  no  nest  disturbance  in  1971-72,  it  was  impossible  to  check 


360 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


directly  for  infertility  by  opening  and  examining  the  eggs,  although  it  was 
indirectly  determined  that  there  was  little  or  no  change  in  egg  fertility 
between  the  years  of  the  study  (Preble  1973).  Since  there  was  a constant 
level  of  infertility  in  the  2 years,  and  there  was  no  significant  difference 
in  fertility  between  the  British  and  American  populations,  Murton  and 
Clarke’s  (1968)  5%  figure  is  used  in  all  calculations  (Table  1). 

Egg  desertion  (Table  1)  was  determined  on  the  assumption  that  a fertile 
egg  which  is  not  destroyed  by  predation  will  hatch  if  incubated.  Failure 
to  incubate  then  constitutes  desertion  of  the  egg.  Desertion  may  be  caused 
by  adult  mortality  or  abandonment.  We  determined  the  number  deserted 
by  subtracting  the  number  removed  by  predation  and  infertility  from  the 
number  that  failed  to  hatch.  In  1971-72,  desertion  accounted  for  38%  of 
the  total  number  of  eggs  laid,  while  in  1972-73  it  accounted  for  16%. 

The  young  fledged  per  young  hatched  (juvenile  success)  and  the  young 
fledged  per  eggs  laid  (total  success.  Table  1),  were  determined  indirectly. 
If  there  was  no  mortality  at  any  state  of  the  reproductive  cycle,  then  we 
expected  that  a ratio  of  the  total  number  of  eggs  present  and  juveniles 
present  on  each  visit  would  equal  the  ratio  of  the  amount  of  time  an 
individual  spent  in  the  egg  compared  to  the  amount  of  time  spent  as  a 
juvenile:  17.5  and  24  days,  respectively  (Whitman  1919,  Goodwin  1967, 
Murton  and  Clarke  1968).  Taking  the  total  number  of  egg  observations  for 
1971-72,  and  the  known  time  individuals  spent  in  the  egg  as  a juvenile, 
and  assuming  no  mortality  of  eggs  or  juveniles,  the  expected  number  of 
observations  of  juveniles  would  reflect  the  following  ratio:  17.5  days/24 
days  = 948  egg  observations/X.  The  expected  number  of  observations  of 
juveniles  assuming  neither  egg  nor  juvenile  mortality  (X)  then  equals  1300. 
The  juvenile  mortality  was  determined  as  follows.  Since  the  number 
hatched  (Table  1)  shows  that  the  actual  egg  survival  was  56%  (egg  mortality 
44%),  then  the  expected  number  of  observations  of  juveniles  if  there  had 
been  no  juvenile  mortality,  but  with  the  observed  egg  mortality  is: 
0.56  Xl300  = 728  = expected  number  of  observations  of  juveniles  with 
the  observed  egg  mortality.  Since  there  were  only  547  actual  observations 
of  juveniles  (Table  1)  then  the  level  of  juvenile  success  must  be:  547  actual 
observations/728  expected  observations  = 75%  fledged  of  eggs  hatched, 
or  a juvenile  mortality  of  25%  of  the  eggs  hatched.  Since  312  eggs  hatched 
(Table  1),  and  inferentially  75%  were  successfully  fledged,  then  the  num- 
ber that  fledged  of  all  the  eggs  laid  equals:  0.75  X 312  = 234  eggs  fledged 
of  562  eggs  laid.  The  total  success  (fraction  of  eggs  laid)  then  becomes: 
234  eggs  fledged/562  eggs  laid — 42%.  The  same  procedure  applied  to  the 
data  from  1972-73  yields  a fraction  fledged  of  eggs  hatched  of  83%  and  a 
fraction  fledged  of  eggs  laid  of  48%. 

It  thus  appears  that  net  recruitment,  measured  by  total  nest  success. 


Preble  and  Heppner  • ROCK  DOVE  BREEDING  SUCCESS 


361 


remained  at  approximately  the  same  level  (42%  in  1971-72,  and  48%  in 
1972-73;  G — 1.29,  P < 0.90)  although  egg  “predation”  significantly  dif- 
fered, from  1%  in  the  first  year  to  21%  in  the  second  year  (C  = 8.03, 
P >0.001).  The  fraction  fledged  of  eggs  hatched  (Table  1)  is  somewhat 
higher  at  Plum  Beach  light  than  in  England,  and  somewhat  higher  at  Plum 
Beach  light  in  1972-73  than  in  1971-72,  probably  reflecting  the  milder 
winter. 


DISCUSSION 

Population  homeostasis  in  the  pigeon  could  be  maintained  through  a 
mechanism  which  regulates  the  recruitment  rate  of  young  adults  by  means 
of  a variable  rate  of  egg  desertion.  Lack  (1966,  1968)  suggested  that  each 
species  produces  as  many  young  as  it  can  successfully  rear  in  a food- 
limited  environment  and  that  adult  mortality  through  starvation  is  the  vari- 
able factor  that  controls  population  size.  Skutch  (1967)  has  argued  that 
some  species  could  produce  more  young  than  they  do,  and  that  the  rate 
of  egg-laying  may  be  a factor.  Fretwell  (1969)  has  attempted  to  mediate 
this  dispute  by  suggesting  that  there  may  be  a dominance  hierarchy  which 
extends  to  nestlings,  the  lower  ones  being  most  subject  to  selective  mor- 
tality in  hard  times. 

In  prior  work  on  the  breeding  biology  of  the  pigeon,  egg  predation  was 
sufficiently  high  to  mask  the  effects  of  egg  desertion  (Murton  and  Clarke 
1968).  On  the  Plum  Beach  lighthouse,  where  the  rate  of  natural  egg  pre- 
dation is  very  low  and  suitable  nest-sites  are  readily  available,  the  results 
of  this  study  suggest  that  the  population  has  a breeding  reserve  which 
enables  it  to  remain  stable  through  selective  desertion  of  eggs.  Although 
there  is  insufficient  evidence  to  do  more  than  speculate,  this  desertion 
appears  to  be  a behavioral  mechanism,  rather  than  the  result  of  starvation 
of  adults.  If  the  latter  was  the  case,  then  one  would  expect  to  see  a high 
rate  of  desertion  of  juveniles  as  well  as  eggs.  The  desertion,  however,  is 
suffered  primarily  by  eggs,  and  only  when  predation  of  eggs  is  low. 

There  are  several  mechanisms  that  might  account  for  this  differential 
desertion.  Wynne-Edwards’  (1963)  model  of  group  selection  is  not  incom- 
patible with  the  observations  reported  here,  but  the  more  recent  concept 
of  kin  selection  (Eberhard  1975)  requires  fewer  assumptions  about  con- 
ditions existing  in  the  colony.  The  observations  here  do  support  the  idea 
of  population  regulation  based  on  behavior,  but  cannot  help  to  differentiate 
between  the  competing  models  of  behavioral  regulation. 

SUMMARY 

Breeding  success  (number  fledged/eggs  laid)  and  nest  desertion  were  determined  in  a flock 
of  Rock  Doves  {Columba  livia)  breeding  in  an  isolated,  abandoned  lighthouse  in  Narragansett 


362  THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Bay,  Rhode  Island,  over  a 1-year  period.  Breeding  success  was  42%  in  that  year  and  38%  of 
nests  with  eggs  were  deserted.  In  the  following  year,  20%  of  eggs  laid  were  removed.  Nest 
desertion  dropped  to  16%,  while  breeding  success  was  48%,  suggesting  that  recruitment 
might  be  related  to  nest  desertion. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Our  thanks  to  the  Rhode  Island  Department  of  Natural  Resources  for  permission  to  visit 
the  lighthouse,  and  C.  Robert  Shoop  for  critically  reading  the  manuscript. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Eberhard,  M.  J.  W.  1975.  The  evolution  of  social  behavior  by  kin  selection.  Quart.  Rev. 
Biol.  50:1-33. 

Fretwell,  S.  D.  1969.  The  adjustment  of  birth  rate  to  mortality  in  birds.  Ibis  111:624-627. 
Goodwin,  D.  1967.  Pigeons  and  doves  of  the  world.  British  Museum  (Nat.  Hist.),  London, 
England. 

Lack,  D.  1966.  Population  studies  of  birds.  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford,  England. 

. 1968.  Ecological  adaptations  for  breeding  in  birds.  Methuen  and  Co.,  London, 

England. 

Lees,  J.  R.  1946.  AU  the  year  breeding  of  the  Rock  Dove.  Br.  Birds  39:136-141. 

Murton,  R.  K.  and  S.  P.  Clarke.  1968.  Breeding  biology  of  Rock  Doves.  Br.  Birds  61:429- 
448. 

Preble,  D.  E.  1973.  Breeding  homeostasis  in  the  feral  Rock  Dove  {Columba  livia).  M.S. 

thesis,  Univ.  Rhode  Island,  Kingston,  Rhode  Island. 

Sketch,  A.  F.  1967.  Adaptive  limitation  of  the  reproductive  rate  of  birds.  Ibis  109:579- 
599. 

Stlrkie,  P.  D.  1954.  Avian  physiolog>'.  Comstock  Publishing  Assoc.,  Ithaca.  New  York. 
Whitman,  C.  O.  1919.  Posthumous  works,  Vol.  3.  The  behavior  of  pigeons  (H.  A.  Carr, 
ed.).  (iarnegie  Inst.,  Publ.  257.  Washington,  D.C. 

W YNNE-Edw  ARDS,  V.  (L  1963.  Intergroup  selection  in  the  evolution  of  social  systems. 
Nature  200:623-626. 


DEPT.  ZOOLOGY,  UNIV.  RHODE  ISLAND,  KINGSTON,  RHODE  ISLAND  02881. 
ACCEPTED  22  OCT.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  363-371 


RELATIVE  ABUNDANCE  OE  GEORGIA  CAPRIMULGIDS 
BASED  ON  CALL-COUNTS 

Robert  J.  Cooper 

In  recent  years  there  has  been  much  interest  in  the  southward  range 
expansion  of  the  Whip-poor-will  {Caprimulgis  vociferus).  Baker  and  Peake 
(1966)  made  several  listening  counts  for  Whip-poor-wills  and  Chuck-will’s- 
widows  (C.  carolinensis)  around  Athens,  Georgia,  and  determined  that  the 
Whip-poor-will  was  extending  its  summer  range  southward  to  include  the 
lower  Piedmont  of  Georgia.  Allen  (1979)  found  the  Whip-poor-will  to  be 
fairly  abundant  in  the  suburban  areas  of  Clarke  County  although  it  was 
greatly  outnumbered  by  the  Chuck-will’s-widow.  He  reported  substantial 
clustering  in  the  local  distribution  of  the  Whip-poor-will,  so  that  in  some 
places  it  had  actually  replaced  the  Chuck-will’s-widow.  Prior  to  this,  the 
Whip-poor-will  had  been  described  as  “an  uncommon  transient  south  of 
the  mountain  counties”  (Burleigh  1958).  Odum  (1943)  reported  the  Whip- 
poor-will  as  not  having  substantially  changed  its  distribution  in  the  pre- 
vious 35  years.  By  1968,  however,  the  Whip-poor-will  was  listed  as  a locally 
common  summer  resident  around  Athens,  Georgia  (Tramer  1968).  The 
Chuck-will’s-widow  has  always  been  a common  summer  resident  in  this 
area. 

Because  caprimulgids  are  often  heard  but  seldom  seen,  listening  counts 
made  at  periodic  intervals  along  secondary  and  dirt  roads  are  a logical 
way  to  determine  their  abundance.  Brauner  (1952)  related  dawn  and  dusk 
activity  of  Poor-wills  {Phalaenoptilus  nuttallii)  to  light  intensity,  and  re- 
lated duration  of  the  active  period  of  this  species  to  several  factors,  es- 
pecially moon  phase.  Mengel  and  Jenkinson  (1971)  also  mentioned  the 
importance  of  moonlight  relative  to  caprimulgid  calling  activity.  Harper 
(1938)  found  that  on  moonless  nights,  Chuck-will’s-widow’s  singing  ap- 
' peared  to  be  limited  to  brief  periods  at  dusk  and  daybreak.  On  moonlit 
evenings,  however,  the  birds  continued  to  sing  indefinitely.  Baker  and 
Peake  (1966)  mentioned  the  negative  effect  of  wind  on  calling.  These  and 
other  studies,  however,  have  varied  in  both  techniques  and  results  so  that 
the  information  is  of  little  comparative  value  (Dillenbeck  1967,  Nunley 
1960). 

The  purpose  of  this  study  was  to  determine  the  relative  abundance  of 
Chuck-wilFs-widows  and  Whip-poor-wills  in  Clarke  County,  Georgia,  from 
a series  of  call  counts,  and  to  correlate  different  environmental  factors 
with  calling  activity. 


363 


364 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


METHODS 

Athens,  a city  of  50,000  people,  is  located  in  the  geographic  center  of  Clarke  County, 
which  is  characterized  by  gently  rolling  hiUs  of  red  clay  subsoils,  with  an  average  elevation 
of  700  ft  (228.5  m).  During  the  last  50  years  the  county  has  experienced  much  urbanization 
with  numerous  suburban  developments. 

In  the  southeast  portion  of  the  county,  20  roadside  listening  counts  were  made  by  the 
author  from  13  April-23  July  1975,  along  main,  secondary  and  dirt  roads.  Twenty  permanent 
stations  were  established  at  approximately  0.5  mile  (0.8  km)  intervals.  Barnett  Shoals  Road, 
a main  road  in  a partially  suburban  area,  contained  the  first  7 stations.  The  next  7 stations 
were  on  Belmont  Road,  a paved  road  running  through  farm  land  largely  maintained  as 
improved  pasture.  The  final  6 stations  were  along  a dirt  road  extending  into  pine-hardwood 
forest.  Thus,  it  was  possible  to  categorize  the  counts  by  land  use  type:  either  suburban, 
pasture,  or  forest.  The  counts  were  started  from  alternate  ends  of  the  route  each  evening  at 
sundown,  unless  birds  started  to  call  prior  to  sundown.  No  counts  were  made  at  dawn.  The 
time  spent  at  each  station  was  standardized  at  3 min,  although  it  was  sometimes  necessary 
to  spend  slightly  more  time  at  a station  where  many  birds  were  calling.  At  each  station  the 
number  of  Whip-poor-wiUs  and  Chuck-will’s-widows  was  recorded,  in  addition  to  vigorous- 
ness of  song.  Every  effort  was  made  to  avoid  counting  the  same  bird  twice.  Data  for  2 counts 
that  took  place  during  moderate  to  heavy  rain  were  not  included  in  the  statistical  analysis. 
Weather  data  were  obtained  from  records  at  the  nearby  Clarke  County  Airport. 

The  effects  of  7 variables  (calendar  date,  sine  curve  date,  temperature,  relative  humidity, 
visibility,  wind  velocity  and  moon  phase)  upon  the  number  of  calling  Chuck-wiU’s-widows 
and  W hip-poor-wills  were  tested  using  the  SAS  statistical  package  programs  (Barr  and  Good- 
night 1972).  Correlation  procedures  were  used  for  Chuck-wiU's-widows  and  W hip-poor-wiUs 
both  individually  and  collectively.  Moon  phase  was  expressed  as  a value  on  a 180°  sine  curve 
using  the  formula: 

u • Tiono/  nights  since  last  new  moon  \1 

moon  phase  = sm  180  ( ^l_ 1 

L Vtotal  nights  between  new  moons/ J 

On  nights  when  the  count  took  place  before  moonrise,  moon  phase  was  treated  as  a new 
moon,  with  a value  of  zero.  Date  was  expressed  as  both  calendar  date  and  as  a value  on  a 
360°  sine  curve  using  the  formula: 

date  = sin[360”(-^!S^^^^)] 
where  day  1 is  the  vernal  equinox. 

Differences  between  numbers  of  Chuck-wiU's-widows  and  W hip-poor-wills  in  each  habitat 
type  was  tested  using  Student's  t-test.  Differences  in  numbers  of  the  same  species  between 
different  habitat  types  were  tested  using  analysis  of  variance  and  least  significant  difference 
procedures  (Steel  and  Torrie  1960). 

On  the  night  of  24-25  May.  a fuU  lunar  eclipse  occurred.  On  this  night  3 counts  were 
taken:  one  during  the  waning  period,  one  during  the  period  of  total  eclipse,  and  one  during 
the  waxing  period. 

RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

Of  all  variables  tested,  moonlight  had  the  most  striking  effect  on  singing 
activity.  Only  phase  of  the  moon  and  calendar  date  showed  significant 
correlations  with  numbers  of  singing  birds.  Moon  phase  showed  a partic- 


Cooper  • CALL  COUNTS  OF  GEORGIA  CAPKIMULGIDS 


365 


Table  1 

T.\ble  of  Correl.\tion  Coefficients  for  Tested  Variables  Possibly  Affecting 
Calling  of  Whip-poor-wills  and  Chuck-will’s-widows 

Variables 

Singing  birds 

hip-poor-w  ills 

Chuck-wiirs-widow's 

Both  species 

Calendar  date 

-0.3899 

-0.1747 

-0.2877 

Sine  curve  date 

0.1681 

0.3616 

0.2591 

Temperature 

0.0557 

0.3246 

0.1825 

Relative  humidity 

-0.0126 

0.2325 

0.1031 

Wind  velocity 

0.2229 

-0.0264 

0.1049 

Visibility 

-0.0241 

-0.2469 

-0.1292 

Moon  phase 

0.6372* 

0.3841 

0.5167** 

* Significant  aX  P = 0.01  level. 
**  Significant  at  P = 0.001  level. 


ularly  strong  relationship  (r  = 0.52,  P ^ 0.001)  with  numbers  of  singing 
birds.  values  were  aeceptable.  Table  1 shows  eorrelation  eoefficients 
of  tested  variables  for  Chuck-wilFs-widows,  Whip-poor-wills  and  total 
birds.  Moon  phase  showed  a signifieant  correlation  [P  ^ 0.01)  with  total 
numbers  of  singing  birds  and  with  Whip-poor-wills,  but  not  with  Chuck- 
will’s-widows. 

Except  for  moonlit  nights,  singing  usually  was  restricted  to  the  period 
between  sunset  and  darkness.  Singing  Chuck-will’s-widows  and  Whip- 
poor-wills  were  recorded  2.23  times  as  often  and  3.15  times  as  often, 
respectively,  when  the  moon  was  greater  than  half  full  as  opposed  to  less 
than  half  full.  The  greatest  single  total  for  an  evening  was  on  the  moonlit 
night  of  27  April,  when  80  birds  were  recorded.  Some  of  these,  and  some  on 
earlier  counts,  may  have  been  transients.  The  full  moon  in  May  yielded 
similar  results,  but  by  July  the  birds  seemed  to  have  ceased  most  of  their 
singing.  No  counts  were  taken  during  the  full  moon  in  June.  Total  numbers 
of  calling  birds  of  both  species  are  shown  in  Fig.  1.  Two  counts  that  were 
subsequently  eliminated  from  statistical  analysis  were  taken  during  mod- 
erate to  heavy  rain,  during  which  neither  species  was  heard.  Chuck-will’s- 
widows  were  heard,  however,  on  the  evening  of  31  May,  when  the  rain 
slowed  to  a drizzle.  Periods  of  high  humidity  did  not  lessen  singing  activity 
in  either  species,  and  periods  after  rains  were  highly  productive  (the  27 
April  count  of  80  birds  took  place  after  a heavy  rain  with  a relative  hu- 
midity of  90%  that  evening). 

Differences  in  numbers  between  species  in  different  habitat  types  were 
tested  using  Student’s  ?-test  {P  ^ 0.05).  The  results,  not  shown,  are  sum- 
marized as  follows:  (1)  No  significant  difference  in  numbers  of  Chuck- 


366  THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


will’s-widows  and  Whip-poor-wills  was  observed  in  the  Barnett  Shoals 
Road  area.  A total  of  86  Chuck-will’s-widows  and  71  Whip-poor-wiUs  was 
counted  in  this  primarily  suburban  area.  (2)  A significant  difference  was 
observed  between  numbers  of  the  2 species  in  the  Belmont  Road  area. 
The  open  habitat  associated  with  pasture  land  was  more  favorable  to 
Chuck-will's-widows.  A total  of  133  Chuck-will’s-widows  and  81  Whip- 
poor-wills  was  counted.  (3)  A significant  difference  was  observed  between 
numbers  of  the  2 species  in  the  forested  area  in  favor  of  Whip-poor-wiUs. 
A total  of  70  Chuck-will’s-widows  and  129  Whip-poor-wills  was  counted. 

Differences  in  numbers  of  the  same  species  between  habitat  types  were 
tested  using  analysis  of  variance  and  least  significant  difference  proce- 
dures {P  ^ 0.05).  The  results  are  summarized  as  follows: 

(1)  There  was  no  significant  difference  between  numbers  of  Chuck- 
will’s-widows  observed  in  the  3 habitat  types. 

(2)  Vt  hip-poor-wills  were  significantly  more  numerous  in  the  forested 
area  than  in  the  other  2 habitat  types.  There  was  no  significant  difference 
between  numbers  of  Whip-poor-wills  observed  in  pasture  and  suburban 


areas. 


Cooper  • CALL  COUNTS  OF  GEORGIA  CAPRIMULGIDS 


367 


Date 


Moon  Phase 


Fig.  2.  The  relative  abundance  of  Whip-poor-wills  and  Chuck-will’s-widows  in  suburban, 
pasture  and  forested  areas. 


Fig.  2 compares  numbers  of  Chuck-will’s-widows  and  Whip-poor-wills 
in  each  habitat  type.  Fig.  3 compares  numbers  of  1 species  in  the  3 habitat 
types.  Each  set  of  graphs  shows  2 peaks  occurring  at  the  full  moon  in 
April  and  May. 

The  literature  concerning  habitat  preferences  of  Whip-poor-wills  and 


368 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Date 


Moon  Phase 


Eig.  3.  The  relative  abundance  of  Vi  hip-poor-wills  and  Chuck-wiU's-widows  in  3 habitat 
types. 


C'liuck-wiirs-widows  is  inconsistent.  In  this  study  Chuck-wiU’s-widows 
showed  a relative  affinity  for  open  habitat  and  Whip-poor-wills  showed  a 
preference  for  wooded  habitat.  Bent  (1940)  reported  Chuck-wiU’s-widows 
active  on  the  edges  of  woodlands  bordering  open  field,  often  making  saUies 
over  the  latter.  Harper  (1938)  found  that  Chuck-wiU’s-widows  in  the  Oke- 
fenokee  region  preferred  hammocks  for  roosting  and  more  open  country 
for  feeding.  Imhof  (1976)  reported  both  species  occurring  in  woodlands  of 
oak  and  pine.  The  Whip-poor-will  was  considered  by  Bent  to  be  a wood- 
land species  that  used  small  open  areas  for  feeding.  Allen  (1979)  found 
Whip-poor-wills  in  the  Athens  area  to  be  restricted  to  a few  areas,  sub- 
urban in  nature,  charaeterized  by  a mixture  of  pasture  and  pine  woods, 
with  hardwoods  restricted  to  creek  bottoms.  Baker  and  Peake  (1966)  found 
that  the  Whip-poor-will  seemed  limited  to  higher  ground.  Allen  (1979) 


Cooper  • CALL  COUNTS  OF  GEORGIA  CAPKIMULGIDS 


369 


could  not  confirm  or  refute  this  observation.  In  this  study  Whip-poor-wills 
were  most  abundant  in  the  areas  with  the  lowest  elevation  (<600  ft  [182.8 
ml),  so  that  elevation  can  probably  he  eliminated  as  a limiting  factor  in 
range  expansion  of  this  species. 

The  southward  range  expansion  of  the  Whip-poor-will  appears  to  he 
related  to  a general  southward  invasion  of  northern  species  as  discussed 
by  Odum  and  Burleigh  (1946).  They  noticed  that  Georgia  lies  at  the  end 
of  the  Appalachian  mountain  chain  which  acts  as  a funnel,  permitting 
northern  species  to  extend  their  ranges  into  the  south.  Evidence  of  a south- 
ward range  expansion  of  the  Whip-poor-will  can  be  found  elsewhere  be- 
sides Georgia.  In  Alabama  in  1924,  the  Whip-poor-will  was  listed  as  a 
mountain  summer  resident,  restricted  to  the  northeast  section  of  the  state 
(Howell  1924).  Imhof  (1976)  mapped  the  Whip-poor-will’s  summer  range  to 
include  areas  of  the  Piedmont  south  of  Birmingham,  a substantial  increase. 
Allen  (1979)  discussed  factors  contributing  to  the  southward  expansion  of 
many  species,  and  considered  changes  in  land  use  to  be  significant  in 
causing  the  Whip-poor-will’s  range  expansion.  From  1920-1940,  cotton 
fields  were  abandoned  in  north  Georgia  and  have  subsequently  produced 
extensive  areas  of  forest.  By  1973,  51%  of  Clarke  County  was  wooded, 
compared  to  38%  in  1938.  Allen  (1979)  determined  that  such  an  increase 
in  forested  land  would  be  favorable  for  the  Whip-poor-will. 

The  results  of  this  study  tend  to  corroborate  Allen’s  (1979)  findings  in 
that  the  Whip-poor-will  was  significantly  more  abundant  in  forested  areas 
than  elsewhere  in  the  study  area  and  was  significantly  more  abundant  in 
forested  areas  than  the  Chuck-will’s-widow.  Since  the  Whip-poor-will  was 
not  recorded  in  Clarke  County  as  a summer  resident  until  1956  and  not 
as  a breeder  until  1971,  change  in  land  use  contributes  much  as  a logical 
explanation  for  this  recent  phenomenon. 

Calling  activity  during  an  eclipse. — An  unusual  opportunity  arose  to 
reinforce  my  observations  on  the  effect  of  moonlight  on  calling  when,  on 
the  night  of  24-25  May  1975,  a total  lunar  eclipse  occurred.  On  this  night 
3 counts  were  taken:  1 starting  at  full  moon  and  continuing  through  the 
waning  period,  1 during  the  period  of  total  eclipse,  1 starting  at  total 
eclipse  and  continuing  through  the  waning  period  to  full  moon  again.  The 
contrasts  between  counts  were  dramatic.  The  first  count  started  with  vig- 
orous calling  typical  of  a moonlit  night,  then  decreased  with  a total  of  24 
Chuck-will’s-widows  and  9 Whip-poor-wills.  The  second  count  yielded 
only  13  Chuck-will’s-widows  and  5 Whip-poor-wills.  The  singing  was  also 
noticeably  less  vigorous.  Half-way  through  the  final  count  the  moon  was 
three-quarters  full,  and  by  the  time  it  reached  the  full  phase  there  were 
almost  too  many  birds  to  count  accurately,  e.g.,  33  Chuck-will’s-widows 
and  37  Whip-poor-wills. 


370 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Table  2 

Number  of  Calling  Chuck-will’s-widows  (C  WW)  and  Whip-poor-wills  (WPW)  During 
THE  Full  Lunar  Eclipse  of  24-25  May  1975 

Waning  period 

Full  ec 

lipse 

W axing  period 

Stop 

CWW 

WPW 

Moon 

phase 

CWW 

WPW 

CWW 

WPW 

Moon 

phase 

1 

2 

— 

FuU 

1 

— 

2 



Eel. 

2 

2 

1 

FuU 

1 

— 

1 

— 

Eel. 

3 

2 

1 

FuU 

— 

1 

2 

— 

Eel. 

4 

2 

2 

3/4 

2 

1 

1 

1 

Eel. 

5 

3 

— 

3/4 

— 

— 

2 

1 

1/4 

6 

2 

2 

3/4 

1 

— 

2 

2 

1/4 

7 

1 

1 

1/2 

— 

1 

2 

3 

1/4 

8 

— 

— 

1/2 

— 

— 

1 

2 

1/2 

9 

1 

— 

1/2 

1 

— 

2 

— 

1/2 

10 

1 

— 

1/4 

1 

— 

2 

1 

1/2 

11 

1 

— 

1/4 

— 

— 

2 

1 

3/4 

12 

— 

— 

1/4 

— 

— 

2 

2 

3/4 

13 

2 

— 

Eel. 

2 

— 

3 

3 

3/4 

14 

1 

1 

Eel. 

1 

1 

2 

2 

FuU 

15 

1 

— 

Eel. 

— 

— 

— 

2 

FuU 

16 

— 

— 

Eel. 

— 

— 

1 

2 

FuU 

17 

1 

— 

Eel. 

1 

— 

2 

3 

FuU 

18 

— 

1 

Eel. 

— 

1 

1 

5 

FuU 

19 

1 

— 

Eel. 

1 

— 

2 

3 

FuU 

20 

1 

— 

Eel. 

1 

— 

1 

4 

FuU 

Total 

24 

9 

13 

5 

33 

37 

None  of  the  other  tested  variables  changed  appreciably  during  this  pe- 
riod; thus,  the  change  in  numbers  of  calling  birds  is  likely  related  to  the 
change  in  moon  phase.  For  a complete  record  of  the  lunar  eclipse  counts, 
see  Table  2. 


SUMMARY 

A series  of  call  counts  was  taken  from  13  April-23  July  1975,  to  determine  relative  abun- 
dance of  Chuck-wilLs-widows  and  Whip-poor-wills  in  Clarke  County,  Georgia,  and  to  deter- 
mine how  different  environmental  variables  affected  calling  behavior.  A 20-station  route  was 
separated  into  3 general  habitat  types:  suburban,  pasture  and  mixed  forest.  Student’s  ^-test 
was  used  to  test  differences  between  numbers  of  the  2 species  in  each  habitat  type.  Analysis 
of  variance  and  Isd  procedures  were  used  to  test  differences  in  numbers  of  1 species 
between  habitat  types.  Tests  were  made  for  correlations  between  numbers  of  calling  birds 
and  different  environmental  variables.  Overall,  Whip-poor-wills  were  approximately  as  abun- 
dant as  Chuck-will's-widows  in  the  study  area.  Vt  hip-poor-wiUs  were  significantly  more  nu- 
merous in  forested  habitat  than  Chuck-will's-widows  and  the  opposite  was  true  in  open 
habitat.  \\  hip-poor-wills  were  significantly  more  numerous  in  forested  habitat  than  in  open 


Cooper  • CALL  COUNTS  OF  GEORGIA  CAPRIMUIAilDS 


371 


or  suburban  areas.  Chuck-wiirs-widows  were  approximately  equally  abundant  in  all  3 habitat 
types.  Change  in  land  use  from  agriculture  to  forest  is  offered  as  a partial  explanation  for 
the  south-ward  range  expansion  of  the  Whip-poor-will.  Of  all  variables  tested,  moon  phase 
showed  the  strongest  correlation  with  numbers  of  singing  birds.  This  observation  was  sup- 
ported by  a series  of  counts  taken  during  a total  lunar  eclipse,  during  which  numbers  of 
singing  birds  varied  directly  with  moon  phase. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Carl  W.  Helms  and  Eugene  P.  Odum  made  constructive  comments.  Philip  E.  Hale  re- 
viewed the  original  manuscript.  A.  Sydney  Johnson  and  Graham  H.  Brister  supplied  critical 
reviews  of  the  manuscript  and  G.  H.  Brister  supplied  statistical  advice.  Edward  Backus 
assisted  in  preparing  the  figures.  Paula  Greene  typed  the  manuscript.  To  all  these  individuals 
I extend  my  sincere  gratitude. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Allen,  O.  S.  1979.  An  analysis  of  range  extensions  of  certain  species  of  birds  in  the  Athens 
(Clarke  Co.),  Georgia  area.  M.S.  thesis,  Univ.  Georgia,  Athens,  Georgia. 

Baker,  W.  W.  and  R.  H.  Peake.  1966.  Whip-poor-will  populations  in  the  lower  Piedmont 
of  Georgia.  Oriole  31:15-19. 

Barr,  A.  J.  and  J.  H.  Goodnight.  1972.  A user’s  guide  to  the  statistical  analysis  system. 
Dept,  of  Statistics,  North  Carolina  State  Univ.,  Raleigh,  North  Carolina. 

Bent,  A.  C.  1940.  Life  histories  of  North  American  cuckoos,  goatsuckers,  hummingbirds, 
and  their  allies.  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  176. 

Brauner,  j.  1952.  Reactions  of  poor-wills  to  light  and  temperature.  Condor  54:152-159. 

Burleigh,  T.  D.  1958.  Georgia  birds.  Univ.  Oklahoma  Press,  Norman,  Oklahoma. 

Dillenbeck,  H.  L.  1967.  Whip-poor-will  foray.  Migrant  38:41. 

Harper,  F.  1938.  The  chuck-wiU’s-widow  in  the  Okefenokee  region.  Oriole  3:9-14. 

Howell,  A.  H.  1924.  Birds  of  Alabama.  Brown  Printing  Co.,  Montgomery,  Alabama. 

Imhof,  T.  a.  1976.  Alabama  birds.  Univ.  Alabama  Press,  University,  Alabama. 

Mengel,  R.  M.  and  M.  a.  Jenkinson.  1971.  Vocalizations  of  the  chuck-will’s-widow  and 
some  related  behavior.  Living  Bird  10:171-184. 

Nunley,  H.  W.  1960.  Chuck-wiU’s-widow.  Migrant  31:57-58. 

Odum,  E.  P.  1943.  Some  possible  range  extensions  in  north  Georgia.  Oriole  8:6-8. 

AND  T.  D.  Burleigh.  1946.  Southern  invasion  in  Georgia.  Auk  63:388^01. 

Steel,  R.  G.  D.  and  J.  H.  Torrie.  1960.  Principles  and  procedures  of  statistics.  McGraw- 
HiU  Book  Co.,  Inc.,  New  York,  New  York. 

Tramer,  E.  j.  1968.  A revised  list  of  the  birds  of  Athens,  Georgia  and  vicinity.  Oriole  38: 
2-17. 

SCHOOL  OF  FOREST  RESOURCES,  UNIV.  GEORGIA,  ATHENS,  GEORGIA  30602. 
(PRESENT  ADDRESS:  ENVIROSPHERE  COMPANY,  145  TECHNOLOGY  PARK, 
NORCROSS,  GEORGIA  30092.)  ACCEPTED  30  JULY  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  372-382 


NON-DRUMMING  MALES  IN  A 
RUFFED  GROUSE  POPULATION 

Gordon  W.  Gullion 


The  drumming  display  of  male  Ruffed  Grouse  {Bonasa  umbellus)  and 
persistent  use  of  easily  identified  sites  for  this  purpose  (site  association) 
has  provided  a basic  population  parameter  for  many  studies  of  this  species 
in  recent  years.  This  display  has  been  a basis  for  extensive  population 
inventories  (Petraborg  et  al.  1953,  Hungerford  1953,  Dorney  et  al.  1958, 
Ammann  and  Ryel  1963,  Porath  and  Vohs  1972),  for  studies  of  survival 
(Frank  1947,  Hardy  1950,  Dorney  and  Kabat,  1960,  Gullion  and  Marshall 
1968,  Rusch  and  Keith  1971,  Stoll  et  al.  1979),  for  studies  of  response  to 
habitat  change  (Gullion  et  al.  1962;  Doerr  et  al.  1970;  Gullion  1970a,  1977; 
Boag  1976)  and  as  a basis  for  judging  the  influence  of  various  extrinsic 
factors  upon  populations  of  these  birds  (Gullion  1970b,  1970c;  Rusch  and 
Keith  1971;  Fischer  and  Keith  1974;  Rusch  et  al.  1978).  The  underlying 
assumption  in  these  studies  is  that  most,  if  not  all,  male  Ruffed  Grouse 
engage  in  this  display  during  the  peak  of  the  spring  drumming  season  each 
year,  or  at  least  that  a relatively  constant  percentage  of  birds  do  so  each 
season. 

Earlier  papers  (Dorney  et  al.  1958,  Gullion  1966)  have  documented  that 
the  percentage  of  site-associated  males  which  can  be  heard  in  the  drum- 
ming display  is  not  constant  from  year  to  year.  Furthermore,  my  1966 
paper  agreed  with  the  findings  of  Eng  (1959)  Dorney  and  Kabat  (1960)  and 
Rusch  and  Keith  (1971)  that  a number  of  males  not  associated  with  a 
definite  display  site  are  usually  present  in  the  population. 

The  size  of  this  non-drumming  segment  is  an  important  consideration 
in  any  work  dealing  with  Ruffed  Grouse  densities  or  population  fluctua- 
tions since  each  male  (both  drumming  and  non-drumming)  appears  to  re- 
present an  equal  number  of  females  in  the  breeding  population.  The  work 
of  Bump  et  al.  (1947:516),  Gullion  and  Marshall  (1968:141)  and  Rusch  and 
Keith  (1971:816)  indicates  that  the  sex  ratio  is  essentially  1:1  as  the 
breeding  season  commences. 

The  purpose  of  this  paper  is  to  document  the  variations  in  the  size  of 
the  “non-drummer”  and  presumably  non-territorial  component  of  a Ruffed 
Grouse  population  in  east-central  Minnesota  from  1959-1978.  In  the  con- 
text of  this  paper,  the  non-drummer  is  a male  Ruffed  Grouse  which  could 
not  he  identified  as  an  occupant  of  a known  drumming  activity  center  and 
who  is  believed  to  have  been  a non-territorial  bird  for  1 or  more  seasons 
(spring  and  fall  are  each  considered  1 drumming  season).  While  there  may 


372 


GuUion  • N()N-I)HUMMIN(;  MALK  GROUSE 


373 


be  some  question  as  to  whether  or  not  drumming  eonstitutes  territorial 
defense,  in  the  context  of  this  paper  I consider  male  grouse  associated 
with  particular  display  sites  (i.e.,  drumming  logs)  for  several  weeks  or 
months  to  be  occupying  a territory.  Since  active  defense  of  a well  defined 
territorial  perimeter  has  not  been  demonstrated  for  this  species  I prefer 
to  call  these  occupied  areas  “activity  centers.”  The  fairly  even,  predict- 
able spacing  (normally  about  200-250  m apart)  of  occupied  centers  usual 
in  good,  homogeneous  habitat  strongly  suggests  that  drumming  is  a mech- 
anism for  spacing  and  at  least  partially  territorial  in  function.  As  used  here 
the  term  non-drummer  does  not  include  males  which  were  identified  as 
occupying  specific  activity  centers  but  which  were  not  heard  drumming 
during  the  period  when  most  other  site-associated  males  were  actively 
drumming. 


METHODS 

Data  for  this  paper  were  collected  during  a long-term  study  of  the  impact  of  forestry 
practices  upon  a grouse  population  that  began  in  1956  and  is  continuing  on  the  Cloquet 
Forestry  Center  of  the  University  of  Minnesota.  The  area  and  the  procedures  used  have  been 
adequately  described  elsewhere  (GuUion  1965,  1966,  1967;  GuUion  and  MarshaU  1968).  The 
terminology  concerning  drumming  activity  used  here  has  also  been  defined  previously  (Gul- 
lion  et  al.  1962;  GuUion  1966,  1967)  and  has  been  used  by  subsequent  authors  (cf.  Boag  and 
Sumanik  1969,  Archibald  1975,  Boag  1976,  Stoll  et  al.  1979). 

In  this  study,  we  recorded  activity  on  about  2300  drumming  logs  in  several  hundred  activity 
centers  performed  by  over  1200  banded  male  Ruffed  Grouse  on  a study  area  which  has  varied 
over  the  years  from  13-37  km^.  Numbers  of  occupied  centers  varied  from  61  in  1964  to  at 
least  254  in  1970-71.  Each  spring  we  have  attempted  to  identify  every  male  grouse  associated 
with  a drumming  log  (by  trapping  or  reading  colored  leg  band  codes)  and  our  success  has 
varied  from  90%  in  1961  (among  144  established  males  in  a 17.8  km^  area)  to  a low  of  54% 
in  1968  (171  established  males  on  a 37  km^  area). 

I have  drawn  on  information  collected  from  the  entire  Cloquet  study  area.  However,  the 
population  specificaUy  considered  in  this  paper  is  that  on  the  13  km^  Cloquet  Forestry  Center. 

In  this  study,  we  classify  Ruffed  Grouse  as  immature  from  the  time  the  8th  primary  is 
completely  grown  in  the  post-juvenal  molt  until  they  are  1 year  old  (the  following  June);  as 
yearlings,  12-23  months  of  age;  and  as  adults,  24  months  and  older.  Birds  which  have  molted 
their  Juvenal  9th  and  10th  primaries  before  being  handled  the  first  time  are  segregated  in  a 
special  category  of  adults,  since  some  may  be  less  than  24  months  old.  These  age  classes 
differ  from  those  used  by  some  other  authors  (cf.  Dorney  and  Kabat  1960,  Rusch  and  Keith 
1971). 


RESULTS 

There  are  4 sources  of  evidence  for  a population  of  non-drumming  males 
I in  a Ruffed  Grouse  population.  One  is  a group  of  birds  I have  called 
I “alternate  drummers”  (GuUion  1967:92).  These  are  birds  which  appear  to 
I be  secondary  birds  associated  with  specific  activity  centers,  although  they 
I are  not  site-associated  and  are  not  heard  drumming.  They  would  be  missed 


374 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


in  population  inventory  procedures  based  upon  drumming  activity  or  log 
occupancy. 

A second  indication  of  the  presence  of  non-drumming  males  is  the  ex- 
istence of  young  male  grouse  which  were  banded  during  summer  and  fall 
lily-pad  trapping,  and  were  later  determined  to  have  been  alive  during 
subsequent  drumming  seasons,  but  were  not  identified  as  drumming 
birds.  For  an  individual  to  be  included  in  this  category  there  had  to  be 
evidence  that  the  bird  in  question  had  been  present  in  areas  where  aU 
known  site-associated  males  were  identified  during  the  period  this  bird 
was  alive.  Third,  information  was  obtained  by  using  the  lily-pad  trapping 
technique  during  the  April-May  drumming  season  to  capture  males  not 
associated  with  drumming  logs. 

The  fourth  segment  of  the  non-drumming  component  is  comprised  of 
unbanded  yearling  and  adult  males  either  appearing  on  logs  for  the  first 
time  in  centers  where  there  was  no  indication  of  activity  the  previous 
season,  or  as  replacements  for  males  which  were  identified  the  year  before 
but  subsequently  killed.  While  there  is  some  possibility  of  birds  moving 
into  the  area  from  outside,  our  22  years  of  data  concerning  Ruffed  Grouse 
mobility  indicate  that  this  factor  is  of  little  consequence.  From  120  records 
of  movements  by  males  from  fall  or  wintering  areas  to  drumming  logs  the 
mean  distance  moved  was  436  m;  only  11  records  indicated  movements 
of  over  1 km.  Also,  emigration  from  the  area  would  be  as  likely  as  immi- 
gration. From  1959-1976  we  monitored  and  trapped  at  all  of  the  drumming 
activity  centers  in  a 400  m wide  buffer  zone  around  the  Cloquet  Forest  as 
intensively  as  on  the  Forest  in  order  to  determine  the  extent  of  egress  and 
ingress.  Movement  of  adult  male  grouse  was  found  to  be  slight. 

Male  Ruffed  Grouse  sharing  an  activity  center  in  “satellite”  status  are 
considered  territorial,  drumming  males  in  this  analysis.  Birds  in  this  group 
are  usually  immatures  and  are  always  nearly  identical  in  size  to  the  pri- 
mary drummer  whose  activity  center  they  are  sharing.  This  represents  2 
male  grouse  in  1 activity  center,  and  complicates  population  determina- 
tions based  on  occupied  activity  centers.  These  birds  have  to  be  counted 
as  drumming,  territorial  birds  since  they  often  engage  in  drumming  duels 
with  the  primary  bird  and  are  certain  to  be  heard  during  inventories  of 
drumming  grouse.  Logs  used  by  satellite  males  are  usually  within  10-30 
m of  the  log  occupied  by  the  primary  male. 

Fig.  1 shows  the  fluctuations  in  numbers  of  both  drumming  and  non- 
drumming male  Ruffed  Grouse  on  the  Cloquet  Forestry  Center  from  1959- 
1978  (and  drumming  bird  numbers  to  1979).  Also  shown  is  the  variation 
in  year-to-year  survival  among  territorial  drumming  males  during  this  pe- 
riod. 


Gullion  • NON-DRUMMING  MALE  GROUSE 


375 


Fig.  1.  Numbers  of  identified  and  known  active  drumming  and  non-drumming  male 
Ruffed  Grouse  on  the  Cloquet  Forestry  Center,  1959-1979. 


The  figures  for  non-drumming  males  were  based  on  the  following  com- 
putations. The  numbers  of  known,  banded  non-drummers  and  alternate 
drummers  present  in  the  population  were  taken  at  face  value,  i.e.,  5 band- 
ed males  known  to  be  in  the  population  in  April  but  not  associated  with 
a drumming  log.  For  birds  banded  in  the  fall  but  not  appearing  on  drum- 
ming logs  the  following  season  I used  the  annual  survival  rates  for  im- 
matures  during  their  first  6 months  and  adult  survival  rates  for  the  re- 
mainder of  the  time  (see  GuUion  and  Marshall  1968  and  Fig.  1 this  paper). 
These  3 groups  are  the  “known  non-drummer”  portion  in  Fig.  1. 

The  “calculated  non-drummer”  fraction  was  determined  by  applying 
the  survival  rate  of  the  preceding  year  to  the  number  of  unhanded  adults 
that  appeared  on  logs.  That  is,  if  the  1962-63  survival  rate  among  estab- 


376 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


lished  drummers  was  50%,  and  6 unbanded  adults  appeared  on  logs  in 
1963,  the  number  of  “calculated  non-drummers”  present  in  1962  would 
have  been  12. 


DISCUSSION 

The  presence  of  non-displaying  and  presumed  non-territorial  males  is 
known  among  some  Tetraonidae.  The  Red  Grouse  {Lagopus  lagopus)  stud- 
ies in  Scotland  (Jenkins  et  al.  1967,  and  others)  have  consistently  shown 
an  excess  of  males  which  are  non-territorial  and  usually  relegated  to  a 
surplus  which  dies  or  emigrates  fairly  quickly.  Hoffman  and  Braun  (1975) 
reported  the  existence  of  non-territorial  sub-adult  males  in  a Colorado 
White-tailed  Ptarmigan  [Lagopus  leucurus)  population  and  Ellison  (1971) 
documented  the  existence  of  a non-territorial  segment  in  a Spruce  Grouse 
[Canachites  canadensis)  population  in  Alaska.  The  work  of  Bendell  and 
his  co-workers  (Bendell  and  Elliott  1967,  Zwickel  and  Bendell  1967,  and 
others)  has  shown  that  a non-territorial  immature  (yearling)  segment  is  an 
integral  part  of  the  Blue  Grouse  [Dendragapus  obscurus)  population  struc- 
ture. Among  Blue  Grouse,  immature  males  have  little  opportunity  to  hold 
territories  unless  substantial  new  habitat  becomes  available  through  hab- 
itat modification  (Redfield  1974)  or  a population  of  established  birds  is 
destroyed  (Zwickel  et  al.  1977). 

Among  the  lekking  grouse  the  identification  of  non-displaying  males  is 
somewhat  more  difficult  due  to  the  daily  fluctuations  in  lek  attendance. 
While  recognizing  the  problems  associated  with  this  fluctuating  lek  atten- 
dance in  Sage  [Centrocercus  urophasianus)  and  Sharp-tailed  [Pediocetes 
phasianellns)  grouse  and  the  Greater  [Tympanuchus  cupido)  and  Attwa- 
ter’s  [T.  r.  attivateri)  prairie  chickens  none  of  several  studies  of  these 
species  in  the  1930—1950  period  suggested  the  presence  of  a non-territorial 
segment  in  the  male  population  (cf.  Lehmann  1941,  Grange  1948,  Patter- 
son 1952,  Ammann  1957,  Baker  1953).  However,  Robel  (1969)  documented 
the  existence  of  a non-territorial  component  among  lekking  Black  Grouse 
[Lyrurus  tetrix)  populations  in  Scotland  and  Rippin  and  Boag  (1974)  have 
found  the  same  to  he  true  in  an  Alberta  population  of  Sharp-tailed  Grouse. 

Ruffed  Grouse  seem  to  he  intermediate  in  status.  Young  male  Ruffed 
Grouse  frequently  become  established  as  territory  holders  when  only  4 
months  old.  In  1970,  24  (27%)  of  87  territorial  male  grouse  on  the  Cloquet 
Eorest  were  on  new  logs  where  we  had  never  recorded  drumming  activity 
previously.  Ten  of  these  were  adults  drumming  for  the  first  time,  while  7 
of  14  immatures  on  new  logs  evidently  had  commenced  using  their  re- 
spective logs  as  4-  or  5-month-old  birds  in  the  fall  of  1969.  If  large  enough, 
young  males  may  even  displace  older  established  males,  forcing  the  latter 


Gullion  • NON-DRUMMING  MALE  GROUSE 


377 


to  move  to  other,  usually  inferior  sites  (Dorney  and  Kabat  1960:19;  Gullion 
1967:95,  1970d:76). 

It  cannot  be  said  that  the  large  number  of  non-drummers  at  Clo(|uet  in 
the  1971-72  period  represented  birds  which  could  not  find  suitable  sites 
for  drumming.  In  this  period,  there  were  at  least  17  activity  centers  in 
relatively  static  habitats  on  the  Cloquet  Forest  which  had  been  acceptable 
to  drummers  in  the  1960-1962  period,  but  which  were  not  occupied  by  the 
non-drummers  in  the  1971-72  period.  This  is  all  the  more  interesting  when 
one  considers  that  33%  (29)  of  the  males  on  the  Cloquet  Forest  in  1970 
were  crowded  onto  86.2  ha  of  13— 25-year-old  aspen  (Populus)  regeneration 
(6%  of  the  forest  area),  at  a density  of  a male  per  3.0  ha  (or  33.6/100  ha). 
One  can  speculate  that  in  this  prime  cover  the  non-drummers  preferred 
to  await  their  turn  to  occupy  an  activity  center  rather  than  use  the  poorer 
quality  habitat  that  had  been  acceptable  as  drumming  cover  a decade 
earlier  (see  below). 

Some  birds  never  become  drummers.  Bird  188  was  banded  as  a chick 
in  August  1969,  and  then  killed  by  a hunter  in  October  1972,  2.4  km  from 
where  he  was  trapped,  without  being  associated  with  a drumming  log. 
Two  other  birds,  1372  and  1374,  both  banded  as  young  males  in  November 
1970,  were  recaptured  in  both  1971  and  1972,  but  never  appeared  on  a 
drumming  log.  In  1971,  1372  was  620  m from  where  he  was  originally 
banded,  and  in  1972  only  220  m from  his  1971  location.  Bird  1374  was 
retaken  at  the  site  of  his  original  trapping  in  the  fall  of  both  1971  and  1972, 
and  was  found  as  a predator  kill  only  164  m distant  in  May  1973. 

A male  associated  with  a drumming  log  may  rarely  relinquish  his  ter- 
ritory and  become  a non-drummer.  Bird  319  was  an  active  drummer  only 
in  1957  and  then  deserted  his  log  and  activity  center,  and  was  last  seen 
alive  1530  m distant  on  22  December  1960.  Bird  2123  used  a log  in  both 
1966  and  1967,  then  was  replaced  and  not  subsequently  associated  with 
a known  log,  although  he  was  seen  alive  on  29  April  1971,  1520  m distant 
from  the  log  he  occupied  earlier.  Territory  abandonment  seems  to  be  un- 
usual however,  for  among  the  records  for  more  than  1200  site-associated 
males  only  these  two  are  known  to  have  acted  in  this  manner. 

The  Cloquet  Forest  has  undergone  considerable  change  in  the  past  25 
years  and  this  change  has  affected  the  abundance  and  distribution  of  these 
male  grouse.  Sizeable  tracts  of  mixed  conifer-hardwood  forest  little  used 
by  Ruffed  Grouse  were  changed  to  open,  unoccupied,  clear-cut  areas, 
which  within  10-12  years  developed  into  excellent  aspen  sapling  habitat 
(with  displaying  male  densities  of  24/100  ha).  During  this  period  other 
aspen  tracts  in  almost  continuous  use  for  as  long  as  15  years  matured  and 
were  no  longer  acceptable  habitats  for  drummers.  Other  habitats  in  less 


378 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


dynamic,  mixed  aspen-conifer  forest  types  have  remained  fairly  constantly 
occupied  at  lower  densities  throughout  the  past  quarter  century. 

These  habitat  changes  have  had  some  obvious  effects  upon  the  Ruffed 
Grouse  population,  as  seen  in  Fig.  1.  For  example,  the  lowest  population 
level  during  the  1974-1976  “cyclic”  decline  stood  at  95%  of  the  highest 
peak  population  in  1960-61,  and  2.7  fold  above  the  1964-1966  lows.  The 
number  of  drumming,  site-associated  males  in  1972  was  1.6  times  larger 
than  in  1960-61,  and  there  were  at  least  6.5  times  as  many  non-drumming 
males  in  the  population. 

It  appears  to  me  that  much  of  this  increase  was  the  result  of  more 
secure  habitat  being  available  in  1972  as  compared  to  1961.  In  the  earlier 
years  the  established  drummers  not  only  occupied  the  best  quality  habitats 
available  to  them,  but  also  occupied  many  sites  which  we  now  consider 
to  have  been  sub-marginal.  Some  individual  grouse  survived  for  as  long 
as  5 years  in  these  sub-marginal  coverts.  Only  a few  activity  centers  were 
adequate  to  allow  the  sharing  of  resources  between  a primary  and  a non- 
drumming occupant.  Young  birds  that  could  not  fit  into  that  system  were 
lost  from  the  population,  one  way  or  another. 

As  the  forested  lands  cut  over  in  the  1950’s  began  to  develop  into  ac- 
ceptable coverts  in  the  early  1970’s  (Gullion  1972:19)  the  increased  num- 
bers of  territorial  males  showed  marked  shifts  in  the  type  of  habitats 
used  (Gullion  1970c:  108).  Additionally,  a greatly  increased  number  of  non- 
drumming males  survived  in  these  better  habitats  awaiting  their  oppor- 
tunity to  occupy  an  activity  center. 

These  changes  may  also  have  altered  the  perceptions  of  young  Ruffed 
Grouse  concerning  habitat  quality.  This  is  reflected  partly  by  their  un- 
willingness to  occupy  vacant  activity  centers  in  areas  of  essentially  static 
habitat  which  had  been  acceptable  to  earlier  generations.  Instead,  many 
chose  to  await  their  turn  to  occupy  a territory  in  this  newly  developed 
superior  habitat. 

Male  Ruffed  Grouse  increase  significantly  in  weight  and  size  (unpubl. 
data)  from  their  first  to  second  year  which  means  that  if  they  are  in  cover 
which  allows  another  season’s  growth  their  larger  size  places  them  in  a 
better  competitive  position  for  occupying  a drumming  log  and  activity 
center.  Five  of  6 banded  non-territorial  males  on  the  Cloquet  area  in  the 
spring  of  1970  occupied  activity  centers  in  1971.  But  only  2 of  the  9 banded 
non-drumming  birds  in  1971  were  established  in  activity  centers  for  the 
1972  season.  Among  the  13  banded  non-drummers  in  1972,  5 occupied 
centers  in  1973. 

It  may  be  that  losses  among  the  non-drumming  component  are  greater 
than  among  the  activity  center  occupants.  This  is  probably  true  among 
young  grouse  moving  into  inadequate  habitats  during  their  first  fall  and 


Gullion  • NON-DRUMMING  MALE  GROUSE 


379 


winter.  Losses  among  young  males  which  are  cohabiting  with  site-asso- 
ciated males  in  satisfactory  habitats  do  not  appear  to  be  greater  than 
among  the  drummers.  Elsewhere  we  have  shown  that  drumming  males 
associated  with  perennially-used  drumming  logs  have  significantly  shorter 
survival  than  those  on  “new”  logs  (Gullion  and  Marshall  1968:132).  At 
least  19  non-drumming  males  who  lived  for  2 or  3 years  before  they  oc- 
cupied logs  lived  longer  than  many  drummers  from  the  same  cohort. 

Among  these  19  banded  non-drummers  which  were  finally  successful  in 
occupying  activity  centers  11  survived  less  than  12  months  longer,  3 sur- 
vived less  than  2 years,  3 survived  less  than  3 years  and  the  last  2 less 
than  4 years. 

The  data  presented  here  do  not  agree  with  the  findings  of  Rusch  and 
Keith  (1971:809)  that  the  number  of  non-drumming  males  is  inversely  pro- 
portional to  the  size  of  the  population.  Data  from  this  Minnesota  study 
indicate  the  opposite:  the  lower  the  population  the  smaller  the  proportion 
of  non-drumming  males.  But  as  noted  earlier  (GuUion  1966:726),  the  lower 
the  population  the  greater  the  proportion  of  site-associated  males  who  are 
not  likely  to  be  heard  drumming.  The  presence  of  these  birds  can  be 
determined,  however,  by  the  signs  they  leave  at  their  logs  and  by  trapping 
(Gullion  and  Marshall  1968:128). 

For  the  biologist  attempting  to  quantify  Ruffed  Grouse  populations  this 
non-territorial  component  presents  a problem,  since  it  can  only  be  de- 
tected through  intensive  and  expensive  trapping  and  banding  activities. 
But  consolation  lies  in  knowing  that  the  greatest  error  in  making  inven- 
tories occurs  when  Ruffed  Grouse  are  most  numerous,  and  the  least  error 
when  these  birds  are  most  scarce. 

Insofar  as  population  processes  are  concerned,  these  non-drumming 
birds  appear  to  provide  “momentum”  to  the  population  upswing  at  a time 
when  annual  survival  of  adult  males  is  declining  sharply  (Fig.  1).  During 
the  1970-1972  period  when  annual  survival  declined  from  61.3  to  43.3% 
among  site-associated  males,  the  overall  population  continued  to  rise. 
When  survival  of  drumming  males  dropped  in  the  1972-1974  period  it  was 
this  portion  of  the  population  that  filled  some  of  the  vacancies  left  by  the 
death  of  established  males  and  buffered  the  rate  of  adult  male  decline. 
This  non-drumming  population  from  the  1971-72  period  was  then  at  least 
partly  responsible  for  maintaining  the  population  at  a higher  level  during 
the  ensuing  years  than  would  have  been  sustained  had  it  not  been  present. 

SUMMARY 

A non-drumming  and  presumably  non-territorial  component  is  a persistent  characteristic 
of  male  Ruffed  Grouse  populations  on  the  Cloquet  Forestry  Center  in  east-central  Minnesota. 
Based  upon  a 23-year  study  involving  more  than  1200  banded  male  grouse  using  in  excess 


380 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


of  2300  drumming  logs,  it  was  found  that  the  non-drumming  component  was  least  when  the 
Ruffed  Grouse  population  was  lowest  and  greatest  when  the  population  reached  peak  abun- 
dance. During  the  1972  period  of  peak  abundance  there  was  at  least  1 non-drumming  male 
grouse  for  every  2.3  known  drumming  grouse  on  this  13  km^  study  area.  A change  in  the 
quality  of  available  habitats  resulting  from  earlier  logging  resulted  in  a marked  increase  in 
the  density  of  drumming  male  grouse  in  this  forest.  Also,  there  was  an  apparent  change  in 
what  male  grouse  perceived  to  be  acceptable  habitat  for  drumming.  Although  at  least  17 
activity  centers  (i.e.,  territories)  in  relatively  stable  forest  situations  which  had  been  used 
earlier  remained  available  and  vacant,  the  non-drummers  chose  to  await  their  turn  in  better 
quality  habitats.  Survival  among  this  non-drumming  component  of  the  male  Ruffed  Grouse 
population  equalled  or  exceeded  that  of  birds  successfully  established  in  activity  centers  as 
drumming  males. 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

This  is  paper  11,435  Scientific  Journal  Series,  University  of  Minnesota  Agricultural  Ex- 
periment Station,  St.  Paul,  55108,  reporting  progress  on  the  Forest  Wildlife  Relations  Project 
(83H).  This  project  is  funded  by  the  Experiment  Station,  with  supplemental  funding  by  the 
Ruffed  Grouse  Society  and  the  Minnesota  Department  of  Natural  Resources. 

More  than  240  research  assistants,  students  and  interns  have  participated  in  developing 
the  data  base  for  this  report.  I am  grateful  to  all  for  their  assistance.  I especially  want  to 
acknowledge  the  long  continuing  encouragement  and  support  of  Drs.  William  H.  Marshall, 
Milton  W.  Weller  and  A.  C.  Hodson. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Ammann,  G.  a.  1957.  The  prairie  grouse  of  Michigan.  Michigan  Dept.  Conserv.,  Game 
Div. 

AND  L.  A.  Ryel.  1963.  Extensive  methods  of  inventorying  Ruffed  Grouse  in  Mich- 
igan. J.  Wildl.  Manage.  27:617-633. 

Archibald,  H.  L.  1975.  Temporal  patterns  of  spring  space  use  by  Ruffed  Grouse.  J.  Wildl. 
Manage.  39:472-181. 

Baker,  M.  F.  1953.  Prairie  Chickens  of  Kansas.  State  Biol.  Surv.,  Univ.  Kansas  Misc. 
Puhl.  No.  5,  Lawrence,  Kansas. 

Bendell,  j.  F.  and  P.  W.  Elliott.  1967.  Behaviour  and  the  regulation  of  numbers  in  Blue 
Grouse.  Can.  Wildl.  Serv.  Rept.  Ser.  No.  4. 

Boag,  D.  a.  1976.  Influence  of  changing  grouse  density  and  forest  attributes  on  the  occu- 
pancy of  a series  of  potential  territories  by  male  Ruffed  Grouse.  Can.  J.  Zool.  54:1727- 
1736. 

AND  K.  M.  SUMANTK.  1969.  Characteristics  of  drumming  sites  selected  by  Ruffed 

Grouse  in  Alberta.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  33:621-628. 

Bume>,  G..  R.  W.  Darrow,  F.  C.  Edminster  and  W.  F.  Crissey.  1947.  The  Ruffed 
Grouse:  life  history,  propagation,  management.  New  York  State  Conserv.  Dept. 

Doerr,  P.  D..  L.  B.  Keith  and  D.  H.  Rusch.  1970.  Effects  of  fire  on  a Ruffed  Grouse 
population.  Proc.  Ann.  Tall  Timbers  Fire  Ecol.  Conf.  10:25—16. 

Dorney,  R.  S.,  D.  R.  Thompson,  J.  B.  Hale  and  R.  F.  Wendt.  1958.  An  evalution 
of  Ruffed  Grouse  drumming  counts.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  22:35—10. 

AND  C.  Kabat.  1960.  Relation  of  weather,  parasitic  disease  and  hunting  to 

Wisconsin  Ruffed  Grouse  populations.  Wisconsin  Conserv.  Dept.,  Tech.  Bull.  No.  20. 

Ellison,  L.  N.  1971.  Territoriality  in  Alaskan  Spruce  Grouse.  Auk  88:652-664. 


Gullion  • NON-DRUMMING  MALU  GKOUSU 


381 


Eng,  R.  L.  1959.  A study  of  the  ecology  of  male  Ruffed  Grouse  {Bonasa  urnhellus  L.)  on 
the  Clocjuet  Forest  Research  Genter,  Minnesota.  Ph.D.  thesis,  Univ.  Minnesota,  Min- 
neapolis, Minnesota. 

hdsCHER,  C.  A.  AND  L.  B.  Keith.  1974.  Population  responses  of  central  Alberta  Ruffed 
Grouse  to  hunting.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  38:585-600. 

Frank,  W.  J.  1947.  Ruffed  Grouse  drumming  site  counts.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  11:307-316. 

Grange,  W.  B.  1948.  Wisconsin  grouse  problems.  Wisconsin  Conserv.  Dept.,  Publ.  328. 

Gullion,  G.  W.  1965.  Improvements  in  methods  for  trapping  and  marking  Ruffed  Grouse. 
J.  Wildl.  Manage.  29:109-116. 

. 1966.  The  use  of  drumming  behavior  in  Ruffed  Grouse  population  studies.  J.  Wildl. 

Manage.  30:717-729. 

. 1967.  Selection  and  use  of  drumming  sites  by  male  Ruffed  Grouse.  Auk  84:87-112. 

. 1970a.  Ruffed  Grouse  investigations — influence  of  forest  management  practices  on 

grouse  populations.  Minnesota  Dept.  Conserv.,  Game  Res.  Quart.  Rept.  30:104—125. 

. 1970b.  Factors  affecting  Ruffed  Grouse  populations  in  the  boreal  forest  of  northern 

Minnesota,  USA.  Proc.  VIII  Int.  Congr.  Game  Biol.,  Finnish  Game  Res.  30:103-117. 

. 1970c.  Factors  influencing  Ruffed  Grouse  populations.  Trans.  N.  Am.  Wildl.  and 

Nat.  Resour.  Conf.  35:93-105. 

. 1970d.  Ruffed  Grouse  investigations — population  dynamics  and  mobility.  Minnesota 

Dept.  Conserv.,  Game  Res.  Quart.  Rept.  30:63—87. 

. 1972.  Improving  your  forested  lands  for  Ruffed  Grouse.  The  Ruffed  Grouse  Society, 

Coraopolis,  Pennsylvania. 

. 1977.  Forest  manipulation  for  Ruffed  Grouse.  Trans.  N.  Am.  Wildl.  and  Nat.  Re- 
sour. Conf.  42:449-458. 

R.  T.  King  and  W.  H.  Marshall.  1962.  Male  Ruffed  Grouse  and  thirty  years  of 

forest  management  on  the  Cloquet  Forest  Research  Center,  Minnesota.  J.  Forestry 
60:671-622. 

and  W.  H.  Marshall.  1968.  Survival  of  Ruffed  Grouse  in  a boreal  forest.  Living 

Bird  7:117-167. 

Hardy,  F.  C.  1950.  Ruffed  Grouse  studies  in  eastern  Kentucky.  Kentucky  Div.  Fish  and 
Game,  Fed.  Aid  Proj.  18-R,  Prel.  Rept. 

Hoffman,  R.  W.  and  C.  E.  Braun.  1975.  Migration  of  a wintering  population  of  White- 
tailed Ptarmigan  in  Colorado.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  39:485^90. 

Hungerford,  K.  E.  1953.  A Ruffed  Grouse  drumming  count  technique  for  northern  Idaho 
conditions.  Univ.  of  Idaho,  Forest,  Wildl.  and  Range  Exper.  Stat.  Res.  Note  10. 

Jenkins,  D.,  A.  Watson  and  G.  R.  Miller.  1967.  Population  fluctuations  in  the  Red 
Grouse  Lagopus  lagopus  scoticus.  J.  Anim.  Ecol.  36:97-122. 

Lehmann,  V.  W.  1941.  Attwater’s  Prairie  Chicken — its  life  history  and  management. 
U.S.D.I.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  N.  Am.  Fauna  57. 

Patterson,  R.  L.  1952.  The  Sage  Grouse  in  Wyoming.  Wyoming  Game  and  Fish  Comm., 
Cheyenne,  Wyoming. 

Petraborg,  W.  H.,  E.  G.  Wellein  and  V.  E.  Gunvalson.  1953.  Roadside  drumming 
counts,  a spring  census  method  for  Ruffed  Grouse.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  17:292-295. 

PORATH,  W.  R.  and  P.  a.  Vohs,  Jr.  1972.  Population  ecology  of  Ruffed  Grouse  in  north- 
eastern Iowa.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  36:793-802. 

Redfield,  j.  a.  1974.  Demography  and  genetics  in  colonizing  populations  of  Blue  Grouse 
(Dendragapus  obscurus).  Evolution  27:576-592. 

Rippin,  a.  B.  and  D.  a.  Boag.  1974.  Recruitment  to  populations  of  male  Sharp-tailed 
Grouse.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  38:616-621. 

Robel,  R.  j.  1969.  Movements  and  flock  stratification  within  a population  of  Blackcocks 
in  Scotland.  J.  Anim.  Ecol.  38:755-763. 


382 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Rusch,  D.  H.  and  L.  B.  Keith.  1971.  Seasonal  and  annual  trends  in  numbers  of  Alberta 
Ruffed  Grouse.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  35:803-822. 

, M.  M.  Gillespie  and  D.  I.  McKay.  1978.  Decline  of  a Ruffed  Grouse  population 

in  Manitoba.  Can.  Field-Nat.  92:123-127. 

Stoll,  R.  J.,  Jr.,  M.  W.  McClain,  R.  L.  Boston  and  G.  P.  Honchul.  1979.  Ruffed  Grouse 
drumming  site  characteristics  in  Ohio.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  43:324-333. 

ZwiCKEL,  F.  C.  AND  J.  F.  Bendell.  1967.  Early  mortality  and  the  regulation  of  numbers 
in  Blue  Grouse.  Can.  J.  Zool.  45:817—851. 

, J.  A.  Redfield  and  j.  Kristensen.  1977.  Demography,  behaviour,  and  genetics 

of  a colonizing  population  of  Blue  Grouse.  Can.  J.  Zool.  55:1948-1957. 

DEPT.  ENTOMOLOGY,  FISHERIES  AND  WILDLIFE,  UNIV.  MINNESOTA,  ST. 
PAUL,  MINNESOTA.  ACCEPTED  19  JUNE  1980. 


ASSOCIATION  OF  SYSTEMATICS  COLLECTIONS 
PROGRAM  EVALUATION 

The  Division  of  Environmental  Biology  of  the  National  Science  Foundation  (NSF)  is 
evaluating  how  the  Systematic  Biology  and  Biological  Research  Resources  programs  might 
better  serve  the  systematic  biology  community  in  the  U.S.  Two  surveys,  developed  by  the 
Association  of  Systematics  Collections  (ASC)  under  contract  with  NSF,  will  gather  data  con- 
cerning the  physical  resources  available  to  the  research  community  and  demographic 
information  on  the  individuals  who  comprise  the  community. 

The  first  survey,  to  be  mailed  to  collection  curators  and  managers  in  November  1981, 
will  request  information  regarding  management,  financial  resources  available  for  support, 
services  provided,  and  future  needs  of  their  collections.  The  second  survey,  to  be  mailed  in 
July  1982,  will  collect  data  on  individual  systematic  biologists. 

If  you  do  not  receive  a survey  form  by  15  November  1981  please  write:  Nancy  Wert, 
NSF  Project  Coordinator,  Association  of  Systematics  Collections,  Museum  of  Natural  His- 
tory, University  of  Kansas,  Lawrence,  Kansas  66045  or  phone  (913)  864-4867.  Please  indi- 
cate the  taxonomic  emphasis  of  your  collection.  A preliminary  report  of  the  results  of  the 
survey  will  be  presented  at  the  ASC  annual  meeting  in  May  1982. 


Wihon  Hull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  383-390 


GENERAL  NOTES 


Behavioral  implications  of  aberrant  song  of  a Red-eyed  Vireo. — In  studies  of  avian 
social  behavior,  song  (or  other  relevant  stimuli,  sensu  Tinbergen  [The  Study  of  Instinct,  The 
Clarendon  Press,  Oxford,  England,  1951])  is  often  taken  to  be  prerequisite  to  potential  pairing 
or  territorial  defense.  Playback  experiments  have  been  used  to  identify  the  aspects  of  a 
species’  song  conveying  specific  sorts  of  information,  e.g.,  the  identity  and  condition  of  the 
species  and/or  individual  singer,  crucial  to  the  performance  of  these  social  activities  (Thomp- 
son, Anim.  Behav.  17:658-663,  1969;  Emlen,  Z.  Tierpsychol.  28:241-246,  1971;  Goldman, 
Auk  90:106-113,  1974;  Fletcher  and  Smith,  Auk  95:338-347,  1978;  and  others).  Some  recent 
theories  of  evolution  of  aspects  of  mating  systems  and  use  of  space  hold  that  social  signals 
may  play  an  evolutionary  pace-setting  role  (Wilson,  Sociobiology:  The  New  Synthesis,  Har- 
vard Univ.  Press,  Cambridge,  Massachusetts,  1975).  This  implies  that  certain  social  signals 
are  not  only  sufficient,  but  necessary  for  an  individual  to  defend  a territory  or  obtain  and 
keep  a mate.  To  test  whether  the  performance  of  specific  species-typical  social  activities  is 
necessary  as  well  as  sufficient  would  require  experimental  subjects  occurring  in  natural  field 
conditions,  which  were  deficient  in  the  social  signal  of  interest. 

The  primary  song  of  the  Red-eyed  Vireo  (Vireo  olivaceus)  is  structurally  diverse  and 
complex  (Lemon,  Can.  J.  Zool.  49:847-854,  1971;  Rice,  Anim.  Behav.  26:527-549,  1978a) 
and  plays  an  important  role  in  natural  behavioral  interactions  among  conspecifics,  including 
territorial  defense  and  pair  formation  and  maintenance  (Lawrence,  Can.  Field-Nat.  69:47- 
87,  1953;  Rice  1978a).  Playback  of  its  species-typical  song  elicits  responses  from  territory 
holders  that  are  qualitatively  and  quantitatively  similar  to  behaviors  seen  in  natural  encoun- 
ters (Rice  1978a;  Rice,  Anim.  Behav.  26:550-561,  1978b). 

In  June  1973  1 discovered  a Red-eyed  Vireo  consistently  singing  a song  unrecognizable  to 
me  as  belonging  to  that  species.  Dr.  Jon  Barlow,  who  has  recorded  songs  of  Red-eyed  Vireos 
throughout  the  range  of  the  species,  also  heard  this  bird  sing,  and  thought  the  song  was 
grossly  aberrant.  Although  Red-eyed  Vireos  occasionally  mimic  other  species  (James,  Can. 
J.  Zool.  54:1223-1226,  1976),  the  song  did  not  sound  like  that  of  any  other  bird.  Analysis  of 
the  behavioral  interactions  of  this  aberrantly  singing  bird  provides  a natural  experiment  on 
the  necessity  of  species-typical  song  for  successful  pair  formation  and  maintenance  and 
territoriality  in  Red-eyed  Vireos. 

Methods. — My  study  area  was  in  Kap-Kig-Iwan  Park,  1.6  km  south  of  Englehart,  Ontario. 
The  habitat  comprises  a forest  of  predominantly  trembling  aspen  {Populus  tremuloides)  and 
white  birch  {Betula  papyrifera)  with  an  understory  of  speckled  alder  {Alnus  rugosa)  and 
) beaked  hazelnut  {Corylus  cornuta).  A detailed  description  of  the  study  area  is  given  in  Rice 
1 (Ecology  59:526-538,  1978c). 

Behavioral  interactions  of  Red-eyed  Vireos  and  the  procedure  used  in  playback  experi- 
ments are  described  in  Rice  (1978a).  Briefly,  experiments  consisted  of  a 2-min  pre-test  period, 
followed  by  2-min  playback  of  song  of  the  aberrant  vireo,  and  then  a 2-min  post-test  period. 
After  a 10-min  inter-trial  interval  the  3 periods  were  repeated,  this  time  using  normal  song 
to  provide  a measure  of  the  readiness  of  the  subject  to  respond  to  a normal  stimulus  (Tin- 
bergen 1951).  During  each  2-min  period  17  variables  were  measured,  covering  aspects  of 
rate,  form  and  latency  of  song,  as  well  as  closeness  and  latency  of  approach  to  the  speaker. 
These  data  were  combined  multivariately  into  a single  response  intensity  score  (Rice  1978b). 
For  each  experiment  I present  the  scores  for  each  period.  To  maximize  precision,  data  from 
different  subjects  were  not  pooled.  An  instance  of  a test  or  post-test  score  being  higher  than 
the  pre-test  score  is  taken  as  possible  evidence  of  a response  to  the  song  played  during  the 
test. 


383 


384 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


ABERRANT 


I ' fH  st  f ^ 


4 


3 


(M 


X 

^ 2 


1 


NORMAL 

SMf 


Y\ 


0.5  1.0 

sec 


Fig.  I.  Tracings  of  sonograms  of  the  7 phrases  in  the  song  of  the  aberrant  vireo  in  June 
1973  and  7 randomly  selected  normal  phrases. 


Results. — (1)  The  song.  Lemon  (1971)  found  that  individual  Red-eyed  Vireos  had  reper- 
toires of  40-60  elements,  occurring  either  regularly  in  combinations  or  usually  singly.  Se- 
quence rules  for  the  elements  and  the  combinations  were  not  detectable,  although  extensive 
numbers  of  songs  were  analyzed. 

Fig.  1 shows  sonogram  tracings  of  phrases  of  the  aberrant  vireo  as  well  as  some  randomly 
chosen  phrases  from  normal  songs.  The  aberrant  phrases  are  consistently  shorter  and  less 
structurally  complex  than  those  depicted  here  or  in  Lemon  (1971)  or  Rice  (1978a).  The  7 
phrases  represent  all  the  phrases  of  the  aberrant  vireo  during  a 10-day  recording  period  in 
June.  When  examined  for  sequencing  rules,  frequency  of  occurrence  of  the  possible  pairs 
of  phrases  deviated  significantly  from  random  (Table  1).  Certain  sequences,  such  as  C-E  and 
E-G,  occurred  more  frequently  than  expected,  and  fewer  pairs  occurred  than  were  predicted 
by  the  Poisson  distribution. 

(2)  Playback  experiments.  hen  the  playback  stimulus  is  normal  Red-eyed  Vireo  song 
scores  are  higher  during  experimental  periods  (test  and  post-test)  than  during  pre-test  control 
period  in  11  of  20  cases  (Fig.  2).  Taking  the  response  rate  to  normal  song  as  a baseline  rate 
for  late  June,  the  expected  probability  of  an  increase  in  response  intensity  during  experi- 
mental periods  is  0.55.  After  the  aberrant  song,  4 of  20  periods  have  higher  scores  than  do 
their  control  periods  (Fig.  2).  The  probability  of  obtaining  a response  rate  this  low  or  lower 
with  a response  rate  of  0.55  is  0.002  (binomial  expansion).  The  responses  to  this  aberrant 
song  by  territorial  vireos  are  significantly  rarer  than  responses  to  normal  Red-eyed  Vireo 
song. 

In  the  playback  experiments  with  Red-eyed  Vireos  reported  in  Rice  (1978a,  b),  2 pre-test 
periods  were  measured  during  79  trials.  In  these  79  trials,  the  response  score  during  the 
second  pre-test  period  had  a 0.23  probability  of  being  greater  than  the  response  score  of  the 


GENERAL  NOTES 


385 


Table  1 

Transition  Frequencies  and  Goodness  of  Fit  Comparison  to  a Random 
Distribution  for  the  Phrases  of  the  Aberrant  Vireo 

Following 
phrase  of  pair 

Preceding  phrase  of 

pair 

A 

B 

c d 

E 

F 

G 

A 



1 

0 2 

1 

2 

2 

B 

4 

— 

0 1 

1 

1 

3 

C 

4 

1 

— 1 

0 

1 

4 

D 

0 

3 

0 — 

1 

1 

0 

E 

0 

0 

11  0 

— 

0 

0 

F 

0 

4 

0 0 

2 

— 

0 

G 

1 

1 

0 0 

5 

2 

— 

Total  of  62  phrases  in 

42  possible  pairs  of  the  phrases  in  Fig.  1 yield 

No.  pairs 

.No.  times  pair  occurred 

Expected  from  Poisson 

Observed 

0 

9.60 

17 

1 

14.17 

12 

2 

10.46 

5 

3 

5.14 

2 

4 or 

more 

2.63 

6 

Chi  square  = 15.11,  df  = 4.  P < 0.01. 


first  pre-test  period.  Using  this  as  a control  value  for  spontaneous  increase  in  response 
behaviors,  the  probability  of  obtaining  an  increase  in  4 or  fewer  of  20  paired  2-min  trials  is 
0.499.  The  behavior  of  the  vireos  during  playback  of  the  aberrant  song  was  not  different  from 
the  behavior  of  vireos  when  no  song  was  present.  Clearly  the  vireos  did  not  react  to  this 
aberrant  song  as  if  it  were  the  song  of  a conspecific. 

Several  studies  have  demonstrated  individual  recognition  of  immediate  neighbors  (Weeden 
and  Falls,  Auk  76:343-351,  1959;  Krebs,  Ecology  52:2-22,  1971;  Emlen  1971;  Goldman  1974; 
Kroodsma,  Condor  78:87-99,  1976;  and  others).  All  of  the  experimental  vireos  had  territories 
distant  from  that  of  the  aberrant  vireo.  Therefore,  we  cannot  definitively  conclude  that  the 
aberrant  song  played  no  role  in  territorial  defense  because  of  the  lack  of  response  in  the 
experiments.  To  examine  this  question  further  the  experiments  were  repeated  with  the  4 
immediate  neighbors  of  the  aberrant  bird. 

Eight  responses  to  8 exposures  of  normal  song  did  not  differ  from  6 responses  to  aberrant 
song  (Fig.  3).  This  may  reflect  the  small  sample  size.  However,  in  the  79  experiments  cited 
earlier  there  was  no  response  to  normal  song  in  22  instances.  Using  that  as  a measure  of  the 
normal  rate  of  nonresponse  to  conspecific  song,  a result  of  6 or  more  responses  in  8 trials 
has  a probability  of  occurrence  of  0.527.  The  neighboring  Red-eyed  Vireos  appeared  to  react 
to  the  aberrant  song  no  differently  than  they  did  to  normal  song.  Also,  their  response  fre- 
quencies were  those  of  territorial  Red-eyed  Vireos  (in  general)  to  normal  song.  The  aberrant 
song  may  have  functioned  to  restrict  incursions  by  close  neighbors. 

(3)  Late  song.  By  mid-July  I felt  that  the  song  of  the  aberrant  vireo  more  closely  resembled 
normal  song.  Sonograms  of  tapes  of  the  mid-July  song  revealed  several  changes  (Fig.  4).  The 


RESPONSE  SC  ORE 


386 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


5 


A 


p t a p t a 


A ☆ 


p t a p t a 


p t a p t a 


A 


p t a P t a 


p t a p t a 


p t a p t a 


Fig.  2.  Response  scores  for  the  experiments  to  non-neighboring  territorial  vireos,  using 
aberrant  (A)  and  normal  (N)  songs  in  June;  “p”  is  the  pre-test  period,  “t”  the  test  period 
and  “a"  the  post-test  period;  stars  mark  the  cases  where  experimental  period  scores  were 
higher  than  control  period  scores  for  tests  with  aberrant  song. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


387 


Pta  pta  pta  Pta  Pta  pta  pta  Pta 

Fig.  3.  Response  scores  for  the  experiments  to  neighboring  territorial  vireos  using  aber- 
rant (first)  and  normal  (second)  songs.  Symbols  as  in  Fig.  2. 


number  of  elements  had  increased  slightly,  with  the  addition  of  apparently  new  phrases  (e.g., 
T)  and  possible  differentiation  of  old  phrases  (e.g.,  E becoming  U and  Y,  D becoming  Z). 
The  new  phrases  were  more  similar  to  those  of  normal  Red-eyed  Vireo  song.  The  order  of 
sequencing  of  phrases  was  also  slightly  less  rigid  than  during  June. 

A second  series  of  playback  experiments  with  9 territorial  vireos  was  started  on  14  July. 
By  this  date  the  rate  and  intensity  of  response  to  normal  song  is  waning,  but  still  responses 
can  be  fairly  consistently  elicited  (Rice  1978b).  The  response  scores  of  the  vireos  to  normal 
song  were  greater  in  experimental  than  pre-test  periods  in  11  of  18  cases,  giving  a probability 
of  response  of  0.611  (Fig.  5).  Increases  in  response  scores  to  the  late  aberrant  song  occurred 
in  6 of  18  trials.  With  a response  of  probability  of  0.611,  6 or  fewer  responses  are  expected 
with  a probability  of  0.02.  The  probability  of  observing  the  6 or  more  responses  under  the 
rate  of  response  to  the  earlier  aberrant  song  is  0.14.  Although  the  response  rate  has  increased 
as  predicted,  the  amount  is  not  statistically  significant. 

(4)  Natural  encounters.  In  39  h of  observation  between  14  June  and  26  July  1973,  I saw 
the  aberrant  vireo  interact  with  other  vireos  5 times.  For  other  vireos  with  normal  songs,  in 
125.5  h of  observation  over  the  same  period,  I observed  27  encounters.  Also,  there  was  no 
evidence  of  an  increase  in  duration  or  intensity  of  encounters  for  the  vireo  with  aberrant 
song,  so  the  aberrant  song  did  not  make  the  bird  appear  less  effective  during  natural  en- 
counters. 

(5)  Habitat  quality.  It  is  possible  that  this  aberrant  Red-eyed  Vireo  was  occupying  a ter- 
ritory effectively,  but  the  territory  was  of  inferior  quality.  I studied  the  habitat  use  of  vireos 


4 


T U V W 


yi  * 


Fig.  4. 
1973. 


0 5 1.0 

sec 

Tracings  of  sonograms  of  the  phrases  in  the  song  of  the  aberrant  vireo  in  July 


RESPONSE  SCORE 


388 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


☆ 


P t a P t a 


☆ 


p t a P t a 


5 


4 


■3 


-2 

-1 


L 


P t a p t a 


L 


p t a p t a 


P t a p t a 


N 


pta  pla  pta  pta 


pta  pta 


Fig.  5.  Response  scores  for  the  experiments  to  non-neighboring  territorial  vireos,  using 
late  season  (L)  and  normal  song  (N)  from  July.  Symbols  as  in  Fig.  2. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


389 


in  this  area  in  detail  (Rice  1978c)  and  the  vegetation  on  this  territory  seemed  typical  of  most 
of  the  study  area;  a mature  and  entire  aspen-white  birch  canopy  and  a fairly  open  alder- 
hazelnut  understory.  Quantitatively,  across  3 data  sets  representing  species  composition, 
foliage  and  height  distribution,  and  occurrence  of  foraging  sites,  18  principal  components 
accounted  for  noteworthy  amounts  of  habitat  variability  (see  Rice  1978c  for  details).  The 
Euclidean  distance  of  this  territory  score  from  the  centroid  of  all  72  Red-eyed  Vireo  territory 
scores  is  closer  to  the  centroids  than  the  scores  of  more  than  70%  of  all  territories  for  each 
data  set.  This  means  that  quantitatively  the  habitat  occupied  by  the  aberrant  vireo  is  similar 
to  the  habitats  occupied  by  the  other  Red-eyed  Vireos  in  this  area. 

(6)  Breeding  success.  I was  not  able  to  find  the  nest  of  this  aberrant  vireo,  but  I did  see 
the  bird  frequently  with  a nonsinging  bird  and  observed  a variety  of  social  behaviors,  in- 
cluding a swaying  bout  (Nolan,  Condor  64:273-276,  1962;  Barlow  and  Rice,  Can.  J.  Zool. 
55:528-542,  1977).  From  these  observations  I inferred  that  the  aberrant  bird  was  a male  and 
had  a mate  which  stayed  on  its  territory  until  at  least  25  July. 

I frequently  saw  the  bird  and  its  presumptive  mate  feeding  2 Brown-headed  Cowbird 
{Molothrus  ater)  chicks,  but  no  vireo  chicks.  However,  of  13  Red-eyed  Vireo  pairs  in  the 
areas  whose  fledging  success  I knew  for  1973,  10  fledged  only  1 (4  cases)  or  2 (6  cases) 
cowbirds,  and  no  vireos.  Thus,  although  there  is  no  support  for  the  assertion  that  the  aberrant 
bird  was  able  to  breed  successfully,  it  was  no  less  fit  than  three-fourths  of  the  Red-eyed 
Vireos  breeding  in  the  area. 

Discussion. — There  are  extensive  data  demonstrating  that  species-typical  song  plays  an 
important  role  in  vireo  social  behavior,  particularly  in  inter-male  aggressive  encounters  and 
courtship  (Barlow  and  Rice  1977,  Rice  1978a).  However,  it  is  clear  from  this  study  that  in 
Red-eyed  Vireos  the  singing  of  a species-typical  song  is  not  always  essential  for  the  estab- 
lishment and  maintenance  of  a territory,  or  for  obtaining  and  keeping  a mate.  In  other  studies 
as  well,  birds  with  aberrant  songs  have  paired  and  sometimes  bred  successfully  (Baptista, 
Z.  Tierpsychol.  34:147-171,  1974;  Emlen,  Thompson  and  Rising,  Wilson  Bull.  87:145-179, 
1975). 

It  is  certainly  possible  to  find  specific  types  of  information  encoded  in  specific  aspects  of 
a species-typical  song  or  other  social  signal.  However,  it  does  not  necessarily  follow  that  in 
the  absence  of  any  specific  aspect  of  a song  the  information  cannot  otherwise  be  readily 
conveyed.  The  cost  of  conveying  information  along  these  alternate  channels  (visual  displays, 
call  notes,  etc.)  may  be  higher,  but  data  on  this  point  are  conspicuously  lacking.  This  vireo 
had  both  a mate  and  a territory  typical  of  Red-eyed  Vireos  in  the  area,  implying  that  the 
costs  were  not  excessive  and/or  the  alternative  channels  not  substantially  poorer.  Investi- 
gators using  playback  experiments  must  bear  in  mind  that  such  studies  can  only  demonstrate 
that  a parameter  is  or  is  not  sufficient  for  species  (or  individual)  recognition,  not  that  it  is 
necessary.  Possibly  for  behavioral  functions  as  important  as  social  activities  (or  orientation 
and  navigation  where  multiple  redundant  cues  are  also  common  [Schmidt-Koenig,  ed..  An- 
imal Migration,  Navigation,  and  Homing,  Springer- Verlag,  Berlin,  West  Germany,  1978]) 
selection  favors  systems  of  multiple  cues  and  responses.  There  is  a need  for  studies  providing 
realistic  measures  of  the  costs  of  such  systems,  relative  to  their  possibly  substantial  benefits, 
including  opportunistic  studies  of  naturally  occurring  “experiments”  such  as  the  one  reported 
here. 

Acknowledgments. — I thank  Dr.  Jon  Barlow  for  coming  to  Kap-Kig-Iwan  Park  to  hear  this 
bird,  for  his  encouragement  during  my  research  on  vireos  and  for  the  loan  of  playback  and 
taping  equipment  which  had  been  purchased  by  Dr.  Barlow  with  grant  support  from  the 
National  Research  Council  of  Canada  (A3472).  The  presentation  benefitted  from  discussions 
during  the  Memorial  University  Animal  Behaviour  Seminars,  and  readings  of  the  manuscript 
by  Bill  Montivecchi,  Luis  Baptista  and  2 other  reviewers.  The  cooperation  of  the  Ministry 


390 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


of  Natural  Resources  of  Ontario  and  the  staff  of  Kap-Kig-Iwan  Park  is  appreciated. — Jake 
C.  Rice,  Dept.  Biology,  Memorial  Univ.  of  Newfoundland,  St.  John’s,  Newfoundland  AIB 
28  Canada  AND  Dept.  Zoology,  Arizona  State  Univ.,  Tempe,  Arizona  85281.  (Present  ad- 
dress: Center  for  Environmental  Studies,  Arizona  State  Univ.,  Tempe,  Arizona  85281.)  Ac- 
cepted 20  June  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  390-391 


Courtship  feeding  and  copulation  of  Royal  Terns. — Although  mentioned  by  Buckley 
and  Buckley  (Ibis  114:344-359,  1972)  in  their  paper  on  the  nesting  of  Royal  Terns  {Sterna 
maxima),  courtship  feeding  and  copulation  have  not  been  described  for  this  species.  These 
performances  may  occur  on  beaches  away  from  islands  used  for  nesting  (Kale  et  ah,  Bird- 
Banding  36:21-27,  1965).  Such  breeding  activities  were  apparently  performed  off  site  by  the 
terns  I studied  on  Cabretta  and  Sapelo,  tree-covered  islands  in  Georgia  where  they  have 
never  been  known  to  nest.  Royal  Terns  have,  however,  nested  on  Little  Egg  Island  3-10  km 
away  (Kale  et  al.  1965). 

My  studies  were  made  from  18-28  April  1979.  It  was  difficult  to  quantify  all  phases  of 
behavior  since  the  terns  were  crowded  in  groups  of  from  20-200  or  more,  resting  and  preening 
at  the  water’s  edge. 

Males,  carrying  a fish  crosswise  in  their  bills  and  making  kur-itt,  kur-itt  notes,  initiated 
courtship  feedings  by  flying  low  over  the  flock,  apparently  to  locate  and  alert  their  mates, 
possibly  by  individually  recognizable  “fish  calls”  described  by  Hutchinson  et  al.  (Behavior 
32:150-157,  1968)  for  the  Sandwich  Tern  (S.  sandvicensis).  Transfers  of  fish  took  place  in  3 
situations.  (1)  Transfers  occurring  within  the  crowd  of  other  terns  were  noted  8 times,  but 
were  interfered  with  twice  by  other  terns  and  twice  by  Laughing  GuUs  {Larus  atricilla)  that 
rested  among  them.  In  the  other  21  feedings  or  attempted  feedings,  the  birds  which  were 
considered  female  (because  they  received  food)  either  (2)  walked  out  from  the  crowd  or  (3) 
flew  to  an  empty  part  of  the  beach  5-10  m away. 

Both  birds  displayed,  with  necks  extended  upward  and  the  fore  part  of  the  closed  wings 
held  outward  (Fig.  1).  The  female  stood  lower  than  the  male,  snatching  the  fish  so  quickly 
that  display  was  often  only  momentary.  The  size  of  the  fish  offered  appeared  to  be  important 
to  female  selection.  On  23  occasions  in  which  the  female  accepted,  the  fish  was  about  7 cm 
in  length.  Of  7 refusals  observed,  4 times  in  succession  by  1 female,  the  fish  was  5 cm  in 
length  or  less  and  slender.  When  1 male  offered  his  mate  a small  fish  and  was  refused,  he 
walked  over  to  offer  the  fish  to  a neighboring  tern.  This  tern,  sex  undetermined,  also  refused. 
The  male  then  returned  and  after  several  tries  mounted  his  mate,  swallowing  the  fish  as  he 
did  so.  Full  copulation  followed.  This  was  the  only  time  I observed  any  relation  between 
courtship  feeding  and  copulation. 

Sometimes  males  and  females  flew  to  dip  their  bills  in  the  waves  2-^  times.  Afterwards, 
males  flew  to  sea  and  females  returned  to  the  flock.  One  pair  fed,  then  walked  together  in 
full  display  to  the  water's  edge  and  dipped  their  bills  in  a mixture  of  wet  sand  and  water  6- 
8 times. 

Copulatory  or  pre-copulatory  behavior,  with  one  or  more  pairs  performing,  was  so  common 
as  to  be  almost  continuously  occurring  in  larger  flocks.  Displays  preceded  copulations.  The 
male,  with  neck  extended  and  slightly  back,  and  the  bends  of  wings  out  like  a skirt  (Fig.  1), 
tried  to  walk  around  the  female  who  kept  turning.  She  often  started  in  a low  resting  pose, 
assuming  the  display  only  as  the  time  of  mounting  approached.  The  male  held  his  head 
higher  than  hers,  pointing  his  bill  downward.  Copulations  lasted  50  sec^  min.  During  nearly 
all  of  this  time,  males  merely  stood  on  the  shoulders  of  the  females,  flapping  their  wings  to 


GENERAL  NOTES 


391 


Fig.  1.  Male  Royal  Tern  in  pose  for  courtship  feeding. 


maintain  balance.  The  body  of  the  female  sank  slightly,  as  her  biU  pointed  upward.  Periodically 
(2-3  times  in  longer  copulations)  the  male  worked  himself  near  the  rear  of  the  female,  coming 
to  rest  on  his  tarsi.  He  fell  slightly  to  the  right,  beat  his  wings  and  maneuvered  his  tail  under 
the  elevated  tail  of  the  female.  Cloacal  contacts  lasted  only  a few  seconds.  Many  copulations 
ended  with  the  female  walking  ahead,  thus  dumping  the  male.  Three  males  and  1 female 
displayed  briefly  following  copulations. 

A third  form  of  behavior  was  the  courtship  flight,  similar  to  the  “Gleitflug”  described  by 
Dircksen  (J.  Ornith.  80:427-521,  1932)  for  the  Sandwich  Tern.  Seen  only  once,  a pair  of 
Royal  Terns  flew  close  together,  circled  high  over  beach  and  dunes  and  then  flew  out  to  sea 
where  one  of  them  floated  down  on  the  other  3 times  with  its  wings  held  in  a “V.”  Aerial 
displays  of  other  terns  are  described  by  Cullen  'Ardea  48:1-39,  1960). 

In  discussion  of  these  observations,  Buckley  and  Buckley  (1972)  distinguished  drinking- 
skimming  and  feeding-skimming.  Dipping  of  bills  in  the  waves  following  courtship  feedings 
was  observed;  however,  this  behavior  appeared  to  be  a washing  of  the  bill  to  get  rid  of 
mucous  left  from  the  fish.  There  were,  curiously,  2 points  of  resemblance  between  Royal 
Terns  and  the  Swallow-Tailed  Kites  {Elanoides  forficatus)  whose  pre-nesting  behavior  I 
watched  in  a preceding  month  (Kilham,  Raptor  Res.  14:29-31,  1980).  Both  species  have 
unusually  long  copulations,  the  kites  up  to  40  sec.  This  may  be  related  to  both  species  having 
long  wings  and  bodies  plus  relatively  short  legs.  In  both  species,  the  male  stands  on  the  back 
of  the  female  prior  to  holding  on  while  sliding  his  forked  tail  under  hers.  Another  similarity 
relates  to  courtship  feeding.  The  females  of  both  species  demanded  an  offering  of  a definite 
type.  With  the  terns  this  was  a fish  about  7 cm  long  and  with  the  kites  an  anole  (Anolis 
carolinensis)  of  slightly  smaller  size.  Females  of  both  species  refused,  sometimes  repeatedly, 
other  offerings.  The  refusals  of  female  terns  are  interesting  in  relation  to  the  speculations 
of  Nesbit  (Nature  241:141-142,  1973)  that  1 function  of  courtship  feeding  may  be  to  give 
females  a chance  to  assess  potential  mates  as  future  providers  for  chicks. — Lawrence 
Kilham,  Dept.  Microbiology,  Dartmouth  Medical  School,  Hanover,  New  Hampshire  03755. 
Accepted  10  June  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  391-392 

Two  cases  of  commensal  feeding  between  passerines. — Observations  of  birds  ex- 
ploiting the  feeding  behavior  of  other  organisms  are  not  uncommon.  Birds  may  use  other 
types  of  animals,  such  as  monkeys  (Macaca  sp.)  (Stott,  Auk  64:130,  1947)  and  army  ants 


392 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  IVo.  3,  September  1981 


{Eciton  sp.)  (Willis  and  Oniki,  Ann.  Rev.  Ecol.  Syst.  9:243-263,  1978).  Numerous  instances 
of  interspecific  feeding  commensalism  have  been  noted  among  many  non-passerine  birds. 
For  instance,  several  cases  have  been  documented  of  Snowy  Egrets  {Egretta  thula)  benefiting 
from  the  foraging  behavior  of  other  ardeids,  mergansers  {Mergus  sp.)  and  terns  {Sterna  spp.) 
(Christman,  Condor  59:343,  1957;  Emlen  and  Abrose,  Auk  87:164-165,  1970;  Lamm,  Condor 
77:207,  1975;  Bertin,  Auk  94:390-391,  1977).  Although  the  literature  concerning  mixed 
species  bird  flocks  is  extensive,  the  degree  to  which  species  use  one  another  to  expose  or 
flush  prey  in  such  flocks  is  poorly  known  and  controversial  (Rand,  Fieldiana  Zool.  (Chicago] 
36:1-71,  1954;  Partridge  and  Ashcroft,  Condor  78:449^53,  1976). 

An  extensive  literature  search  yielded  only  1 observation  of  single  or  paired  non-flocking 
passerine  species  exploiting  one  another.  Willis  (Ornithol.  Monogr.  10,  1972)  observed  a 
commensal  feeding  association  between  Spotted  Antbirds  {Hylophylax  naevioides)  and  Scaly- 
throated  Leafscrapers  [Sclerurus  guatemalensis).  I report  2 similar  cases  of  this  type. 

On  the  morning  of  16  March  1976,  at  Puerto  Los  Mazos  near  Autlan  de  Navarro,  Jalisco, 
Mexico,  Kenn  Kaufman  and  I observed  a pair  of  Rose-breasted  Thrush-Tanagers  {Rhodino- 
cichla  rosea)  foraging  1^  m apart  on  a sloping  dry  oak-forest  floor  at  about  1385  m.  They 
were  exposing  food  items  by  flipping  over  leaves  and  other  litter  with  sweeping  motions  of 
their  bills.  Accompanying  them  was  a Fan-tailed  Warbler  {Euthlypis  lachrymosa)  that  re- 
mained within  0.5  m of  one  or  the  other  of  the  pair  and  usually  perched  on  a branch  or  log 
above  the  tanagers.  The  warbler  appeared  to  be  using  these  elevated  perches  to  see  prey 
items  exposed  by  the  tanagers.  It  darted  in,  captured  and  swallowed  insects  exposed  by  1 
tanager,  and  then  returned  to  the  same  elevated  perches  or  others  nearby.  When  no  vantage 
post  existed  near  the  tanagers,  the  warbler  observed  from  the  ground  nearby.  It  appeared 
to  be  successful  in  capturing  prey  uncovered  by  the  tanagers.  We  were  unable  to  determine 
what  type  of  prey  the  tanagers  and  the  warbler  were  taking;  however,  in  over  25  min  of 
observation  we  observed  no  conflicts  between  the  birds.  This  appears  to  indicate  that  the 
warbler  was  taking  prey  not  used  by  the  tanagers.  This  warbler  has  also  been  noted  following 
army  ants  in  Mexico  (Sutton,  Condor  53:16-18,  1951;  E.  O.  Willis,  pers.  comm.).  As  these 
birds  moved  up  slope,  they  passed  within  8 m of  a Blue  Mockingbird  {Melanotis  caerulescens) 
foraging  in  the  same  leaf-flipping  manner  as  Rhodinocichla.  The  warbler,  however,  showed 
no  interest  in  the  mockingbird  and  remained  with  the  tanagers. 

While  at  Gedi  Historical  Monument  (Gedi  Ruins,  Gedi  Forest),  Malindi,  Kenya,  on  1 July 
1977,  Dale,  Allan  and  Marian  Zimmerman  and  I observed  a Red-capped  Robin-Chat  (Co5- 
sypha  natalensis)  closely  following  a Spotted  Ground-Thrush  (Turdus  fisheri)  foraging  in  dry 
leaf  litter  on  the  forest  floor  in  dense  understory.  The  thrush  was  uncovering  food  items  by 
scratching  with  its  feet  and  occasionally  using  the  bill  to  toss  dead  foliage  aside.  The  manner 
in  which  the  chat  exploited  food  revealed  by  the  thrush  was  similar  to  that  described  above. 
The  chat  perched  from  0.5-1  m above  the  thrush  in  the  understory.  When  a food  item  was 
detected  the  chat  flitted  to  the  ground  after  the  prey  and  then  returned  to  its  perch.  This 
was  repeated  several  times,  hut  due  to  the  dense  foliage  we  were  unable  to  determine  the 
success  of  Cossypha  in  capturing  prey.  This  species  has  also  been  observed  exploiting  the 
feeding  behavior  of  elephant-shrews  (Macroscelididae)  in  much  the  same  manner  (Rathburn, 
Ph.D.  thesis,  Univ.  Nairobi,  Nairobi,  Kenya,  1976). 

My  observations  are  similar  to  those  of  Willis  (1972)  for  Hylophylax  and  Sclerurus.  In  all 
3 cases,  the  species  that  were  exploited  foraged  on  the  ground,  exposing  prey  by  flipping 
leaves  and  other  litter  with  their  bills  and/or  feet.  The  extent  to  which  such  associations  are 
rare  or  transitory  vs  a regular  foraging  strategy  on  the  part  of  the  follower  remains  to  be 
documented. 

I thank  William  H.  Buskirk,  Edwin  Willis,  J.  V.  Remsen,  Jr.  and  J.  P.  O’Neill  for  com- 
ments on  the  manuscript. — MARK  B.  Bobbins,  Louisiana  State  Univ.,  Drawer  MU,  Baton 
Rouge,  Louisiana  70893.  Accepted  1 June  1980. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


393 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  393-394 

Food  finding  in  Black-capped  Chickadees:  altruistic  communication? — Intra- 
specific flocking  may  increase  an  individual’s  fitness  by  facilitating  food  finding  and  decreas- 
ing predation.  An  interesting  problem  is  whether  a social  bird  finding  a rich  food  source 
behaves  selfishly  or  altruistically.  An  altruist  decreases  its  own  fitness  by  aiding  another.  A 
non-altruist  would  presumably  eat  without  announcing  the  presence  of  food  to  the  flock. 
Here  I report  behavior  of  Black-capped  Chickadees  {Parus  atricapillus)  on  finding  an  es- 
pecially rich  food  source. 

From  July-April  in  Wisconsin,  Black-capped  Chickadees  live  in  flocks  of  4-8  individuals 
of  both  sexes.  The  flocks  are  evidently  not  composed  of  close  kin,  because  juveniles  disperse 
from  their  natal  area  in  July  and  join  flocks  with  adults  other  than  the  parents  (Weise  and 
Meyer,  Auk  96:40-55,  1979).  Chickadees  utter  frequently  the  chick-a-dee  call  which  may 
facilitate  cohesion  of  the  flock  during  movements  (Ficken  et  al..  Auk  95:34-48,  1978).  Play- 
back of  these  calls  is  known  to  attract  chickadees. 

This  study  was  conducted  at  The  University  of  Wisconsin-Milwaukee  Field  Station,  Ozau- 
kee Co.,  Wisconsin,  over  a 6-year  period  (1971-1977).  Six  feeders  stocked  with  sunflower 
seeds  and  suet  are  open  from  mid-November-May.  A total  of  25  experiments  was  conducted 
(not  all  feeders  being  used  each  year).  Most  of  the  feeders  were  sufficiently  spaced  so  that 
a different  flock  used  each  feeder.  The  birds  were  individually  color  banded;  ages  and  sex 
for  most  birds  were  known.  The  feeders  were  removed  each  spring  and  replaced  a day  or 
two  before  being  stocked  with  food.  The  chickadees  did  not  show  any  attraction  to  the  feeder 
site  before  the  feeders  were  stocked  with  food.  Chickadee  behavior  was  observed  as  birds 
approached  feeders  stocked  for  the  first  time  that  season  (13-17  November,  depending  on 
the  year).  After  determining  that  there  were  no  chickadees  in  the  area,  sunflower  seeds  were 
placed  in  the  feeder.  While  in  a blind  we  recorded  the  activities  of  the  first  chickadee  to  find 
the  newly  stocked  feeder.  If  no  chickadees  approached  the  feeder  within  2 h,  we  moved  to 
another  feeder.  Observations  and  vocalizations  were  recorded  with  a Nagra  IV  tape  recorder 
and  Sennheiser  MKH  104  omni-directional  microphone.  Vocalizations  were  analyzed  with  a 
Kay  Elemetrics  6061B  Sona-Graph. 

Table  1 summarizes  the  results.  Most  birds  called  on  finding  the  feeder.  In  4 experiments, 
we  were  able  to  identify  the  color  band  code  of  the  first  bird  recruited  (i.e.,  the  second  bird 
to  land  on  the  feeder);  in  aU  cases  it  was  the  mate  of  the  bird  that  called.  In  addition,  several 
other  flock  members  often  accompanied  the  mate  to  the  feeder.  In  most  cases,  recruitment 
was  probably  achieved  through  the  chick-a-dee  vocalizations.  The  calls  given  by  birds  finding 
the  feeder  were  those  typical  of  flocking  situations,  and  there  were  no  special  types  of  calls 
associated  with  food  finding.  The  average  latency  between  calling  and  recruitment  was  128 
sec  (±41  sec).  In  2 cases,  the  bird  finding  the  feeder  did  not  call,  but  others  were  with  the 
first  bird  when  it  arrived  at  the  feeder  and  all  soon  began  feeding.  In  only  1 case  did  a lone 
bird  finding  the  feeder  fail  to  call. 

Chickadees,  on  finding  an  abundant  food  source,  often  vocalize  and  frequently  others, 
particularly  the  mate,  come  to  the  site  very  quickly.  Why  should  a chickadee  attract  others 
to  a food  source  even  if  food  is  abundant?  There  would  probably  be  costs  to  calling,  such  as 
decreased  feeding  rate  if  several  individuals  were  present. 

The  normal  winter  food  of  chickadees  is  probably  distributed  in  small  packets.  Chickadees 
show  no  evidence  of  altruism  in  their  winter  feeding  away  from  feeders.  Since  chickadee 
flocks  are  not  composed  of  close  kin,  a kin  selection  explanation  of  food  advertisement  seems 
unlikely.  The  chickadees  did  not  cache  the  seeds,  so  a communal  cache  site  was  improbable. 
Reciprocal  altruism  (Trivers,  Quart.  Rev.  Biol.  46:35-57,  1971)  seems  unlikely  because  mem- 
bership of  flocks  is  not  very  stable  and  the  possibility  of  cheating  is  high. 

The  most  likely  hypotheses  for  this  behavior  appear  to  be  the  following:  (1)  It  is  advan- 


394 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Table  1 

Finding  a Recently  Stocked  Feeder  by  Black-capped  Chickadees 


Outcome  Frequency 


No  chickadees  came  within  2 h 15 

Chickadees  came  within  2 h 11 

Did  not  call  within  1 min  of  finding  feeder  3 

Did  caU  within  1 min  of  finding  feeder  8 

Another  individual  came  within  3 min  of  first  call-recruitment  6 

No  recruitment  2 


tageous  for  an  individual  chickadee  to  be  in  a flock  for  reasons  not  directly  related  to  feeding 
efficiency,  as  the  flock  is  an  effective  anti-predator  strategy  (Ficken  and  Witkin,  Auk  94:156- 
157,  1977).  Therefore,  it  may  be  beneficial  to  share  food  to  keep  other  flock  members  alive. 
(2)  Chickadees  are  monogamous  and  the  mate  is  usually  in  the  same  winter  flock.  It  may  be 
advantageous  to  be  altruistic  toward  tbe  mate  under  some  circumstances  (Witkin  and  Ficken, 
Anim.  Behav.  27:1275-1276,  1979).  (3)  Tbe  cost  of  vocalizing  on  finding  food  may  be  so  small 
compared  to  the  advantages  of  this  vocalization  in  more  common  contexts  that  natural  se- 
lection has  not  acted  to  silence  chickadees  that  discover  locally  abundant  food  (W.  J.  Smith, 
pers.  comm.).  Hypotheses  other  than  kin  selection  to  explain  apparent  altruism  need  to  be 
tested  further  for  the  chickadee  as  well  as  other  social  species. 

I thank  R.  Ficken,  J.  Hailman,  W.  J.  Smith,  C.  Weise  and  S.  Witkin  for  their  criticisms 
of  the  manuscript.  R.  Ficken,  J.  Ingold,  M.  Plonczynski,  C.  Weise  and  S.  Witkin  aided  in 
obtaining  the  data,  and  C.  Weise  made  available  data  on  the  color  banded  birds.  Supported 
by  NSF  grant  BMS  74-19474.  Publ.  No.  25  of  The  University  of  Wisconsin-Milwaukee  Field 
Station. — Millicent  S.  Ficken,  Dept.  ZooL,  JJniv.  Wisconsin-Milwaukee,  Milwaukee,  Wis- 
consin 53211.  Accepted  3 June  1980. 


WiLson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  394-395 


The  sentinel  erow  as  an  extension  of  parental  eare. — In  bands  of  feeding  Common 
Oows  (Corvus  brachyrhynchos),  some  crows  sit  as  sentinels  and  apparently  warn  feeding 
conspecifics  of  oncoming  danger  (Bent,  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  191,  1946).  Other  species  as 
well  make  use  of  sentinels  (Conner,  Condor  77:517,  1975).  Goodwin  (Crows  of  the  World, 
Cornell  Univ.  Press,  Ithaca,  New  York,  1976)  disagrees  with  the  guardian  function  of  the 
sentinel  crows,  citing  personal  contradictory  observations  of  sentinel  corvids  fleeing  an  area 
before  all  of  the  feeding  individuals  are  warned.  A pair  of  nesting  crows  that  we  studied  in 
the  spring  of  1978  may  provide  further  insight  into  the  actual  function  of  the  sentinel  crow. 

A pair  of  crows  nested  in  one  of  a group  of  14  spruce  [Picea  sp.)  trees  on  the  St.  Bona- 
venture  University  campus,  Cattaraugus  Co.,  St.  Bonaventure,  New  York.  This  pair  was 
observed  from  hatching  4 May  to  fledging  on  7 June.  The  family  unit,  recognizable  because 
of  aluminum  leg  bands  on  the  young,  was  also  observed  in  the  vicinity  from  7 June-7  July. 

During  the  nesting  stage,  the  crows  were  observed  for  30  observation  periods  of  30  min 
each.  Three  main  forms  of  antipredator  behavior  were  observed:  chasing,  mobbing  and  nest 
guarding.  During  chasing  1 parent  would  fly  at  an  intruder  giving  a low  pitched  caU  until  the 
animal  left  the  area.  When  the  crow  exhibited  more  intense  mobbing  behavior,  it  gave  a 


GENERAL  NOTES 


395 


“rally  call,”  and  was  joined  by  its  mate  and  4 other  crows.  The  6 crows  made  occasional 
passes  at  the  intruder. 

Guarding  the  nest  is  a less  obvious  form  of  antipredatory  behavior,  but  is  a precursor  to 
chasing  or  mobbing.  Guarding  was  not  done  from  the  nest  itself  but  from  the  top  of  adjacent 
spruce  trees  and  from  deciduous  trees  107  m away.  Guard  changes  usually  took  place  in  the 
deciduous  trees.  Occasionally,  the  incoming  crow  went  directly  to  the  nest,  but  82%  of  the 
time  it  stopped  at  the  deciduous  tree  first  (N  = 90  of  110).  The  nest  was  only  left  unguarded 
for  a mean  of  1.04  times  per  30  min  of  observation  (N  = 23  observations).  These  unguarded 
periods  were  usually  brief,  with  a mean  duration  of  3.4  min  (N  = 24  unguarded  periods 
observed). 

After  fledging,  the  family  was  seen  6 times  in  the  adjacent  woodlot  between  7 June  and 
16  June.  Since  the  young  were  not  seen  on  the  ground  during  this  time,  it  was  assumed  that 
the  young  were  still  being  fed  by  the  parents.  The  young  were  not  seen  foraging  in  open 
fields  until  the  first  week  in  July.  Good  (Ph.D.  thesis,  Ohio  State  Univ.,  Columbus,  Ohio, 
1952)  also  found  that  fledglings  do  not  alight  in  open  fields  until  at  least  2 weeks  after  fledging. 
On  each  occasion,  when  the  family  was  seen  in  the  woodlot,  the  authors  were  mobbed  by 
the  adults.  On  2 occasions,  1 banded  young  tried  to  join  in  the  mobbing.  On  both  occasions, 
the  adults  began  to  vocalize  at  the  young  and  half  chased,  half  led  the  young  into  a tree. 
Once  the  young  was  concealed,  the  adults  continued  to  mob  the  authors. 

In  the  first  week  of  July,  the  family  unit  under  study  was  seen  foraging  in  an  open  field 
adjacent  to  the  woodlot.  One  adult  was  stationed  in  a nearby  tree  or  on  a fence  post.  The 
sentinel  occasionally  gave  calls  that  were  barely  audible  to  an  observer  100  m away.  Louder 
calls  led  to  the  other  adult  joining  its  mate  at  the  guard  post  while  the  3 young  continued  to 
forage.  Once  when  the  family  was  approached,  all  5 crows  flew  to  the  adjacent  woodlot. 

If  these  crows  had  not  been  banded  and  observed  for  the  2 months  that  preceded  these 
last  observations,  they  would  have  appeared  as  a band  of  feeding  crows  with  1 or  2 posted 
sentinels.  Instead,  we  interpreted  this  group  as  a feeding  family  unit  with  1 or  2 parents 
sitting  on  guard  over  the  young.  We  feel  that  the  sentinel  is  an  extension  of  parental  care 
originating  from  the  guarding  which  occurs  during  nesting.  The  mobbing  by  nesting  parents 
does  not  switch  immediately  into  sentinel  warning  upon  fledging.  Instead,  the  parents  go 
through  a transition  period  in  which  intruders  are  stiU  mobbed  while  the  recent  fledglings 
are  being  taught  to  flee  from  potential  danger. 

We  think  that  sentinel  crows  are  not  altruistic,  self-appointed  guardians  of  the  feeding 
flock.  Instead,  they  are  parent  crows  exhibiting  antipredatory  behavior  as  they  guard  their 
offspring.  The  contradictory  observations  by  Goodwin  (1976)  mentioned  above  could  be  ex- 
plained if  the  young  of  the  fleeing  sentinel  were  already  out  of  danger,  even  if  other  crows 
were  stiU  feeding.  Guarding  by  adults  of  a family  would  not  preclude  use  of  such  sentinels 
by  other  crows,  or  even  other  species. — GLORIA  M.  D’AGOSTINO,  Lorraine  E.  Giovinazzo 
AND  Stephen  W.  Eaton,  Dept.  Biol.,  St.  Bonaventure  Univ.,  St.  Bonaventure,  New  York 
14778.  Accepted  2 June  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  395-397 


Behavior  of  a male  Least  Bittern  incubating  after  loss  of  mate. — On  30  June  1978, 
I found  a 4-egg  completed  clutch  of  a Least  Bittern  {Ixobrychus  exilis)  in  a solid  stand  of 
cattails  {Typha  sp.)  at  RamsayviUe  Marsh,  4 km  east  of  Ottawa,  Ontario.  The  nest  was 
located  about  65  m from  shore  and  approximately  70  cm  above  the  surface  of  water  ca.  40 
cm  deep.  The  nest,  typical  for  the  species  (see  Weller,  Wilson  Bull.  73:11-35,  1961),  was 


396 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vo/.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


July 

Fig.  1.  Loss  of  interest  in  incubation  by  male  Least  Bittern  as  indicated  by  decrease  in 
nest  attentiveness. 


supported  beneath  by  both  dead  and  living  cattails.  The  latter  also  formed  a loose  canopy 
above  the  nest. 

When  I checked  the  nest  on  8,  11,  13  and  14  July,  either  the  female  or  the  male  incubated. 
I last  saw  the  female  on  14  July.  From  17-20  July,  inclusive,  I observed  the  nest  from  a blind 
about  3 m from  the  nest.  On  17  July,  during  11  h and  35  min  of  continuous  observation  I saw 
the  male  spend  96.9%  of  this  time  on  the  nest,  a remarkably  long  time  since  the  female 
usually  incubates  more  than  the  male  (WeUer  1961). 

Given  that  1 egg  is  laid  per  day  and  incubation  starts  with  the  first  or  second  egg  (Weller 
1961),  incubation  probably  began  by  27  June.  Since  the  first  egg  hatches  19  days  after  it  is 
laid  (Weller  1961),  the  last  date  for  the  beginning  of  hatching  in  the  above  nest  was  probably 
17  July.  The  male  attended  the  nest  until  20  July. 

While  incubating,  the  male  performed  nest  “jabbing”  (Weller  1961)  especially  frequently 
on  18  and  19  July.  He  often  performed  the  movements  of  picking  something  up  and  throwing 
it  out  of  the  nest,  hut  nothing  was  seen  falling  except  on  5 occasions  when  he  flicked  out 
small  pieces  of  nesting  material.  Several  times  he  suddenly  got  up,  then  vigorously  pecked 
his  feet  or  jabbed.  After  such  a session,  I found  an  unidentified  arthropod  in  the  nest. 
Possibly,  the  bittern  was  attempting  to  get  rid  of  insects,  perhaps  parasites,  rather  than  poke 
holes  in  the  nest  for  future  nest  sanitation  (as  suggested  by  Weller  1961). 

Each  day  the  male's  tendency  to  incubate  was  high  in  the  early  morning  and  waned  as  the 
day  progressed,  and  with  each  day  he  shortened  his  incubation  time  (Fig.  1).  I did  not  see 
him  on  21  July  (nest  observed  continuously  from  06:45-11:20).  I assumed  that  he  abandoned 
the  nest.  On  28  July,  I found  the  eggs  cracked  and  carrion  beetles  {Silpha  sp.)  eating  their 
contents.  Since  the  male  incubated  at  least  3 days  beyond  the  latest  presumed  hatching  date 
for  the  first  egg,  failure  to  hatch  may  have  been  due  to  infertility,  being  chilled  at  night  or 
overheated  in  the  sun. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


397 


I would  like  to  thank  all  those  who  offered  constructive  criticism  of  this  manuscript.  The 
above  observations  were  incidental  while  I was  working  on  rails  (Rallidae)  in  Ramsayville 
Marsh,  a study  which  was  supported  by  a Visiting  Fellowship  held  in  the  ornithology'  section 
of  the  National  Museum  of  Natural  Sciences. — B.  T.  Ani^KOWICZ,  National  Museum  of 
Natural  Sciences,  National  Museums  of  Canada,  Ottawa,  Ontario  KIA  0M8  Canada.  (Present 
address:  R.R.  #4,  Shawville,  Quebec  J OX  2Y0  Canada.)  Accepted  21  July  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  397-400 


Notes  on  Brown  Pelicans  in  Puerto  Rico. — The  biology  of  2 races  of  the  Brown 
Pelican  (Pelecanus  occidentalis  carolinensis,  P.  o.  californicus)  of  coastal  United  States  and 
Baja  California  is  well  known.  Few  data  exist  for  the  nominate  race  (P.  o.  occidentalis) 
inhabiting  the  Caribbean  region,  especially  on  breeding  distribution,  population  size  and 
aspects  of  breeding  biology  (Wetmore,  N.Y.  Acad.  Sci.  Survey  of  Porto  Rico  and  the  Virgin 
Islands  9:245^06,  1927;  Palmer,  Handbook  of  North  American  Birds,  Vol.  1,  Yale  Univ. 
Press,  New  Haven,  Connecticut,  1962).  A nesting  colony  on  Conejo  Cay,  a 2 ha  rock  ap- 
proximately 30  m high,  near  Salina  del  Sur  Bay  at  65°17'W,  18°7'N,  off  the  southeastern 
shore  of  Vieques  Island  (23  km  east  of  Puerto  Rico)  is  easily  viewed  from  the  military 
operations  headquarters  on  190  m Cerro  Matias  hill,  about  1 km  from  the  colony.  We  spent 
about  15  h observing  this  colony  from  this  location  using  a 30x  telescope  and  8x  binocular 
in  April-September  and  made  other  observations  in  Puerto  Rico  between  March  and  No- 
vember 1978  on  25  days  in  the  field. 

History  of  nesting  on  Vieques  Island  and  the  reason  for  this  study. — Conejo  Cay  is  1 km 
from  the  impact  area  for  air-to-surface  target  operations  on  the  United  States  Marine  Base 
Camp  Garcia.  This  cay  is  thus  subject  to  overflights  by  military  aircraft  on  an  irregular,  but 
frequent  basis  and  tbe  resulting  bombing  and  shell-fire  explosions  from  these  aircraft  and 
from  ships  off-shore.  On  a normal  bombing  run  the  jet  aircraft  pass  over  the  cay  at  about 
400  m.  We  were  interested  in  the  reactions  of  the  pelicans  to  these  military  activities. 

Dr.  Cameron  B.  Kepler  first  discovered  the  pelican  colony  on  Conejo  Cay  from  a Navy 
helicopter  on  20  July  1971,  and  estimated  50  nests  present  there  (Sorrie,  Caribbean  J.  Sci. 
15:89-103,  1975).  These  were  the  only  data  for  this  colony  until  we  began  a series  of  visits 
in  March  1978.  Local  fishermen  reported  nesting  in  former  years  on  nearby  Alcatraz  Rock, 
which  is  usually  awash  even  in  moderate  seas.  However,  unless  Alcatraz  Rock  has  changed 
materially  in  recent  years,  which  seems  unlikely,  close  inspection  suggests  that  it  is  only 
suitable  as  a roosting-loafing  site. 

Nesting  on  Conejo  Cay  in  1978. — Pelicans  built  nests  on  top  of  the  island  in  sea  grape 
{Coccoloba  uvifera),  limber  caper  {Capparus  fiexuosa),  Ipomoea  sp.  and  Opuntia  rubescens 
from  0.5-2  m above  ground.  Pelicans  nested  on  the  cay  from  the  autumn  of  1977  through 
August  1978  with  several  “waves”  of  laying  (Table  1).  Most  nests  were  established  during 
the  winter.  An  extended  nesting  cycle,  with  most  nesting  in  winter,  is  probably  typical  of 
Brown  Pelicans  in  the  tropics  (Schreiber,  Auk  97:491-508,  1980).  The  colony  was  abandoned 
in  late  August-early  September.  Although  a food  shortage  may  have  occurred,  human  in- 
terference probably  caused  the  desertion.  A shift  in  nesting  location  occurred  during  the 
season,  with  early  nests  formed  in  the  middle-highest  portion  of  the  cay  and  later  nests  on 
the  northeast  edge.  We  were  unable  to  determine  the  exact  number  of  nests  existing  in  the 
colony  during  1977  and  1978.  Based  on  the  known  productivity  of  P.  o.  carolinensis  (Schrei- 
ber, Contrib.  Sci.  Nat.  Hist.  Mus.  Los  Angeles  County  317:1-43,  1979)  and  the  number  of 


398 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Table  1 

Notes  on  Brown  Pelicans  Nesting  on  Conejo  Cay,  Vieques  Island,  Puerto  Rico, 

1978 

Date 

Observer 

12  March 
DWB 

18  May 
BAS 

25  May 
DWB 

1 June 
RWS 

23  July 
RWS 

3 Sept. 
RWS 

22  Sept. 
RWS 

20  Oct. 
RWS 

Adults 

60 

20 

26 

20 

13 

5 

0 

0 

Nests 

empty 

a 

0 

0 

0 

2 

6 

0 

0 

with  eggs 

6 

3 

1 

8 

6 

0 

0 

0 

with  naked  young 

1 

1 

2 

1 

0 

0 

0 

0 

downy  young 

1 1 

1 

6 

10 

0 

0 

0 

0 

medium  size  young 

20 

54 

18 

10 

5 

0 

0 

0 

large  young 

12  J 

1 

6 

12 

6 

3 

0 

0 

dead  nestlings 

0 

1 

0 

3 

1 

2 

0 

0 

Flying  juveniles 

a 

25+ 

a 

11 

19 

2 

0 

0 

® Present  but  not  counted. 


immature  birds  seen  in  the  area,  it  seems  likely  that  at  least  60  nests  existed  from  March 
through  July;  perhaps  an  equal  number  existed  from  November  1977-March  1978. 

Clutch-size  and  egg-size. — Clutch-size  in  7 nests  on  1 June  was  2.6.  This  figure  is  somewhat 
low  since  several  clutches  probably  were  incomplete.  On  the  basis  of  available  evidence  it 
would  appear  that  the  clutch-size  in  this  colony  closely  resembles  that  found  elsewhere  in 
the  species  (Schreiber  1979). 

Eighteen  eggs  from  7 nests  measured  on  1 June  and  23  July  1978  by  RWS  had  a mean 
length  of  73.00  ± 3.58  mm  (range  67.08-79.97  mm)  and  a mean  width  of  45.40  ± 3.01  mm 
(range  35.15-47.90  mm).  Most  clutches  fell  within  the  72-80  mm  X 44^8  mm  range,  but  1 
clutch  of  2,  noted  as  small  at  the  time,  was  69.90  X 35.15  mm  and  67.08  X 44.00  mm.  Eggs 
from  Conejo  Cay  were  significantly  shorter  in  length  (t  = 2.26  and  2.30,  df  = 56,  P < 0.05, 
N = 18,  39)  and  width  (t  = 3.77  and  5.45,  df  = 56,  P < 0.001,  N = 18,  39)  than  eggs  from 
both  the  east  and  west  coasts  of  Florida  (Schreiber,  unpubl.).  They  were  essentially  the  same 
size  as  the  only  known  preserved  eggs  of  P.  o.  occidentalis  (6  eggs  in  4 clutches:  x = 
72.26  X 45.86  mm,  range  = 75.0-68.4  mm  X 44.8-47.4  mm),  collected  from  Cacachita  Cay, 
Cuba,  in  September  1930  by  P.  Bartsch.  The  need  for  egg  measurements  of  pelicans  from 
the  Caribbean  is  obvious. 

Observations  oj  the  pelican  colony. — On  22  July,  RWS  watched  the  colony  during  an  air 
operation  involving  14  jet  overflights  into  the  target  region  while  several  smoke  bombs  and 
two  500  Ih  bombs  were  exploded.  During  this  time,  10  adults  and  9 nestlings  were  clearly 
visible  through  a telescope  in  the  colony.  Five  adults  rested  beside  nestlings  and  the  others 
sat  on  newly  constructed  nests.  Pelicans  are  most  easily  disturbed  during  the  courtship- 
incubation  phase  of  nesting  and  if  air  operations  were  to  have  a noticeable  effect  on  the 
adults,  it  should  be  readily  apparent  in  these  individuals  (Schreiber,  Ornithol.  Monogr.  22, 
1977). 

Throughout  the  air  operation,  the  nestlings  continued  to  stand  in  a relaxed  position,  gular 
fluttering  (Schreiber  1977).  They  neither  moved  from  individual  nests  nor  reacted  obviously 
to  the  jets  or  bombs.  The  adults  did  not  respond  noticeably  either.  In  fact,  1 pair  continued 


GENERAL  NOTES 


399 


to  perform  low  intensity  courtship  activity  on  their  nest  throughout  the  air  operation.  No 
birds  took  flight  or  moved  from  their  nests.  Three  juveniles  continued  to  swim  on  the  water 
between  the  cay  and  the  target  range;  washing,  bathing  and  practicing  normal  bill  plunging 
activities. 

We  believe  that  the  Brown  Pelicans  nesting  in  this  colony  have  acclimated  to  the  intensive 
air  operations.  It  would  appear  that  the  successful  nesting  during  the  9 months  of  our  studies 
1977-78  indicates  that  these  military  activities  have  not  negatively  affected  breeding  behavior 
of  this  population. 

Two  incidents  during  our  observation  period  on  1 June,  when  no  jet  activity  occurred,  are 
instructive.  During  mid-afternoon  a Navy  helicopter  flew  over  the  nesting  island  at  approx- 
imately 50  m elevation.  As  it  passed,  18  of  24  adult  pelicans  took  flight  from  their  nests  or 
perches  and  flew  in  a tight  circle  over  the  colony.  They  returned  to  their  nests  or  perches 
within  1 min.  This  lesponse  to  overflying  helicopter  and  small  fixed-wing  aircraft  also  occurs 
at  pelican  colonies  in  Florida  (Schreiber  1977).  We  do  not  know  the  long  term  effect  of  such 
disturbance  but  low  overflights  of  colonies  by  aircraft  should  be  prevented.  Later  the  same 
day,  a 5. 0-5. 5 m outboard  motor  boat  with  2 local  fishermen  approached  the  cay.  Several 
pelicans  flew  from  their  nests  or  perches  and  circled  over  the  island  as  the  fishermen  ap- 
proached. The  fishermen  landed  on  the  cay  25  m east  of  the  nesting  area  at  which  time  the 
remaining  adults  flew  from  their  nests.  The  birds  began  to  return  to  the  area  of  their  nests 
only  after  the  fishermen  departed  and  were  200  m from  the  cay.  This  type  of  human  distur- 
bance which  drives  adults  from  their  nests  for  extended  periods,  in  turn  exposing  eggs  and 
small  naked  young  to  insolation,  is  precisely  what  causes  major  problems  in  pelican  colonies. 
Fortunately,  the  naval  operations  in  the  region  usually  prevent  such  landings  by  local  people 
on  Conejo  Cay,  minimizing  this  sort  of  disturbance.  Kepler  and  Kepler  (Living  Bird  16:21- 
50,  1977)  noted  similar  protection  by  military  operations  in  seabird  colonies  on  Culebra,  only 
about  20  km  from  Conejo  Cay.  Because  of  our  study,  the  U.S.  Navy  maintains  the  cay  as  a 
‘no  entry’  zone  for  all  military  operations  and  restricts  air  traffic  over  and  around  it,  thus 
reducing  disturbance  to  the  colony  except  by  local  fishermen. 

Other  observations  on  Vieques. — During  more  than  40  days  on  Vieques  in  March-Septem- 
ber  1978  we  frequently  visited  places  along  the  coast  where  pelicans  would  be  expected  to 
roost  and  loaf.  Only  2 locations  were  consistently  used  by  the  birds:  a set  of  pilings  on  the 
north  coast  near  Mosquito  Pier  and  bushes  on  “Green  Beach”  on  the  west  end.  Both  of  these 
sites  are  inside  the  military  restricted  area.  On  most  of  our  visists  to  the  civilian  villages  we 
saw  few  or  no  pelicans  and  those  birds  seen  were  unusually  wary  of  approach  by  people.  We 
suspect  this  absence  of  pelicans  and  their  wariness  is  caused  by  persecution  and  harassment 
of  the  birds  by  local  people,  either  intentional  or  naive. 

The  age-class  distribution  of  the  pelicans  in  the  loafing  areas  on  11  observations  had  a 
mean  of  43%  adults  (range  38-62%),  30%  subadults  (range  6-38%)  and  27%  birds  less  than 
1 year  old  (range  13-62%)  (based  on  plumages,  Schreiber,  unpubl.).  An  age-class  distribution 
of  this  composition,  with  a high  percentage  of  young  birds,  probably  indicates  a stable 
population.  We  estimate  that  although  the  pelican  population  of  Vieques  comprises  fewer 
than  200-250  birds,  it  is  healthy  and  stable. 

Other  colonies  in  Puerto  Rico. — Raffaele  (Puerto  Rico  Environ.  Quality  Board,  1972)  sum- 
marized historical  records  of  pelicans  nesting  in  Puerto  Rico  and  reported  that  the  colony 
on  Conejo  Cay  was  the  only  then  known  viable  colony  in  Puerto  Rico.  He  stated  that  other 
colonies,  on  islands  near  La  Parguera  and  near  Humacao,  had  recently  been  abandoned, 
apparently  because  of  an  increase  in  boat  traffic  near  the  islands.  He  noted  that  4 colonies 
were  known  in  the  past  but  does  not  name  the  fourth.  Perhaps  it  was  at  Caballo  Blanco  off 
Port  Mulas,  Vieques,  mentioned  by  Wetmore  (Auk  33:403^19,  1916).  No  pelicans  nest  on 
Culebra  or  Monito  (Kepler  and  Kepler,  1977;  Kepler,  Condor  80:72-78,  1978). 


400 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Roger  Zimmerman  reported  a colony  of  pelicans  in  Montalva  Bay,  near  La  Parguera  on 
the  southwest  coast  of  Puerto  Rico  that  he  found  on  11  February  1977  (Zimmerman,  in  litt.). 
On  that  date  he  found  25  adults  on  nests  and  20  young  ranging  from  6 weeks  old  to  fledging 
age,  on  a small  mangrove  island.  He  noted  this  as  the  only  colony  he  found  during  extensive 
work  along  the  southwest  coast.  The  Montalva  Bay  colony  apparently  constitutes  the  only 
known  nesting  of  pelicans  in  Puerto  Rico  between  Kepler’s  1971  aerial  sightings  and  our 
1978  observations  on  Conejo  Cay. 

Observations  of  the  Montalva  Bay  colony. — RWS  found  27  adults,  17  on  nests,  on  20 
September  1978.  AU  had  brown  necks,  21  had  white  heads  but  6 had  fuU  yellow  heads, 
indicating  that  courtship  activity  had  just  begun.  One  pair,  a subadult  male  and  fuU  adult 
female,  copulated  while  RWS  was  present  and  2 other  subadult  males  and  3 unsexed  sub- 
adults were  associated  with  nests.  Age  and  sex  were  determined  using  plumage  and  com- 
parative bin  size  (Schreiber,  unpubl.).  Sixteen  nestlings,  4-10  weeks  old  were  visible  in  10 
nests  and  18  juveniles  were  in  the  immediate  vicinity.  This  colony  continued  nesting  activities 
through  the  fall  after  the  Conejo  Cay  colony  was  deserted.  The  extended  nesting  season  is 
also  obvious  here  and  the  colony  appeared  to  be  the  same  size  as  was  reported  in  February 
1977  by  Zimmerman. 

Surveys  of  the  pelicans  of  Puerto  Rico. — During  aerial  surveys  for  manatees  {Trichechus 
manat  us)  along  the  entire  coast  of  Puerto  Rico  during  3 days  each  in  early  August,  Septem- 
ber, mid-October  and  November  1978,  DWB  counted  250,  534,  250  and  398  pelicans.  These 
incomplete  counts  give  a conservative  estimate  of  the  size  of  the  total  population. 

A count  of  the  pelicans  in  San  Juan  harbor  on  30  October  1978,  yielded  a total  of  350 
birds,  comprised  of  26%  adults,  7%  subadults  and  67%  birds  less  than  1 year  old.  One-third 
were  loafing  on  the  tourist  ship  docks,  one-half  were  in  the  Casuarina  trees  on  the  Coast 
Guard  base,  and  the  remainder  were  equally  divided  among  the  mangrove  area  on  the  south- 
east portion  of  the  harbor,  the  channel  markers  and  a large  feeding  flock.  It  thus  appears 
that  the  Coast  Guard  base  provides  an  important  roost-loafing  site.  We  suggest  that  the 
mangrove  areas  of  the  harbor  are  important  habitat  for  Brown  Pelicans  and  should  be  care- 
fully protected  from  development  and  other  human  intrusion. 

Both  the  Montalva  Bay  and  Conejo  Cay  colonies  are  readily  accessible  and  would  make 
fine  study  sites  for  future  work  on  Brown  Pelicans  in  Puerto  Rico.  Studies  on  their  breeding 
biology  and  on  non-breeding  aspects  of  population  parameters  would  contribute  importantly 
to  our  understanding  of  the  marine  avifauna  of  the  Caribbean  region. 

.Acknouledgments. — W'e  tbank  Mary  Margaret  Goodwin  and  the  United  States  Navy,  M. 
Ralph  Browning,  W.  Reagan,  R.  Zimmerman,  R.  Fosberg,  W.  Raney,  W.  Rushing,  E.  A. 
Schreiber,  j.  C.  Barlow,  C.  Kepler,  W.  Robertson,  M.  Patrick  and  P.  Reynolds. — Ralph 
W.  Schreiber,  Natural  History  Museum,  900  Exposition  Blvd.,  Los  Angeles,  California 
90007,  David  W.  Belitsky,  Bureau  of  Land  Management,  Box  1869,  Rock  Springs,  Wyoming 
82901  AND  Bruce  A.  Sorrie,  Manomet  Bird  Observatory,  Manomet,  Massachusetts  02345. 
Accepted  9 June  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  400-403 


Eggs  of  the  Marbletl  Murrelet. — The  estern  Foundation  of  Vertebrate  Zoology 

(WF\  Z)  recently  obtained  the  Edward  J.  Booth  bird  egg  collection  from  the  Whatcom  Mu- 
seum of  History  and  Art  in  Bellingham,  ashington.  Among  the  specimens  in  the  collection 
is  a single  egg  (WFVZ  113.186)  which  apparently  is  that  of  the  Marbled  Murrelet  (Brachy- 
rarnphus  rnarmoratus). 

The  original  account  of  Booth’s  acquisition  of  this  specimen  appeared  in  the  Murrelet 


GENERAL  NOTES 


401 


(Anon.,  Murrelet  8:16,  1927)  and  reads  in  part:  “Mr.  E.  J.  Booth  of  Bellingham,  Washington, 
discovered  in  the  office  of  a logging  camp  in  Whatcom  County,  Washington,  an  egg  which 
he  was  unable  to  identify.  This  egg  was  given  to  him,  and  upon  showing  it  to  several  orni- 
thologists, including  Mr.  J.  Hooper  Bowles  and  Mr.  D.  E.  Brown,  it  was  identified  as  being 
beyond  reasonable  doubt  that  of  the  Marbled  Murrelet.  This  egg  had  been  found  about  fifteen 
miles  inland,  near  Saxon,  on  the  south  fork  of  the  Nooksak  River,  on  June  19,  1925.  The  egg 
lay  on  a bed  of  moss,  no  nest  being  apparent,  and  incubation  was  about  one-third  advanced.” 

The  specimen  was  later  mentioned  by  Jewett,  Taylor,  Shaw  and  Aldrich  (Birds  of  Wash- 
ington State,  Univ.  Washington  Press,  Seattle,  Washington,  1953),  who  also  believed  it  to 
be  an  egg  of  the  Marbled  Murrelet.  However,  it  has  not  been  included  in  recent  summaries 
of  the  breeding  biology  of  the  species  (Binford  et  al.,  Wilson  Bull.  87:303-319,  1975;  Sealy, 
Bird-Banding  46:141-154,  1975),  or  has  its  authenticity  been  questioned  (Drent  and  Guiguet, 
B.C.  Prov.  Mus.  Occ.  Pap.  12,  1961),  perhaps  because  its  whereabouts  were  not  generally 
known. 

The  egg  measures  58.32  X 36.51  mm  with  an  empty  shell  weight  of  2.222  g and  a shell 
thickness  at  the  waist  of  0.214  mm.  It  is  long  subeUiptical  in  shape  (Preston,  p.  13  in 
Handbook  of  North  American  Birds,  Vol.  1,  R.  Palmer,  ed.,  Yale  Univ.  Press,  New  Haven, 
Connecticut,  1962)  and  moderately  glossy.  The  ground  color  of  the  egg  is  pale  glass  green, 
and  it  bears  large  blackish -brown  splotches  and  scrawls  and  small  spots  of  gull  gray,  all 
concentrated  mostly  at  the  larger  end  (italicized  colors  from  Ridgway,  Color  Standards  and 
Color  Nomenclature,  published  by  the  author,  Washington,  D.C.,  1912).  Most  of  the  spots 
are  less  than  2 mm  in  diameter. 

In  these  characteristics  the  specimen  agrees  closely  with  indisputable  eggs  of  the  Marbled 
Murrelet,  which  now  include  the  following:  (1)  an  egg  taken  from  the  oviduct  of  a bird 
collected  on  23  May  1897,  at  Howcan,  Prince  of  Wales  Archipelago,  Alaska  (Cantwell,  Auk 
15:49,  1898).  Bent  (U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  BuU.  107,  1919)  reproduced  this  egg  in  color  (plate  48) 
and  described  it  as  having  a pale  chalcedony  yellow  ground  color  and  being  uniformly,  but 
not  thickly  spotted  with  dark  blackish -brown  or  nearly  black  spots.  The  egg  was  too  badly 
broken  to  be  measured  accurately,  but  was  reported  to  be  63  X 35  mm  (Ralph  in  Cantwell 
1898),  which  agrees  closely  with  Bent’s  plate  48;  (2)  an  egg  taken  from  the  oviduct  of  a bird 
collected  on  23  May  1934,  near  Mittelnach,  an  islet  in  the  Strait  of  Georgia,  just  east  of 
Campbell  River,  Vancouver  Island,  British  Columbia,  which  was  described  as  being  pale 
glass  green  spotted  with  light  lavender  gray,  deep  madder  blue,  sepia,  bone  brown,  and  black 
(Sutton  and  Semple,  Auk  58:580-581  and  plate  19,  1941).  It  measured  58.5  X 39.5  mm;  (3) 
another  oviduct  egg  taken  on  13  July  1941,  from  a bird  collected  near  Pleasant  Island,  SE 
Alaska,  by  Stanley  G.  Jewett,  who  stated  (Jewett,  Murrelet  23:67-75,  1942)  that  the  egg 
agreed  perfectly  with  the  color  description  given  by  Sutton  and  Semple  (1941)  for  the  pre- 
ceding egg,  although  the  Alaska  specimen  was  said  to  be  more  heavily  marked.  It  measured 
60.5  X 39.0  mm;  (4)  an  egg  photographed  in  a nest  on  East  Amatuli  Island,  Barren  Islands, 
Alaska,  on  8 July  1978,  by  Theodore  Simons.  This  egg,  which  was  allowed  to  hatch,  was 
described  as  being  “pale  olive  green  and  covered  with  irregular  brownish  black,  tar-colored 
spots.  These  spots  were  more  prevalent  around  the  larger  end  of  the  egg  but  covered  it 
entirely.”  The  egg  weighed  41.0  g and  measured  61.2  X 36.3  mm  (Simons,  Condor  82:1-9, 
1980);  and  (5)  another  egg  photographed  in  a nest  on  East  Amatuli  Island  in  July  1979  by 
Lee  Astheimer,  Katherine  Hirsch  and  Douglas  Woodby.  This  egg  measured  58.9  X 36.3  mm 
and  weighed  38.5  g when  found;  it  was  also  allowed  to  hatch.  I have  examined  transparencies 
of  this  egg,  and  its  ground  color  is  identical  to  that  of  the  Booth  specimen.  However,  it  is 
more  heavily  spotted  over  its  entire  surface,  and  some  of  the  spots  are  light  brown,  a color 
not  found  on  the  Booth  egg. 

Eggshell  fragments  taken  from  the  California  Marbled  Murrelet  nest  described  by  Binford 


402 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


et  al.  (1975)  were  identical  in  color  to  those  of  the  Sutton  and  Semple  specimen,  except  that 
markings  of  saccardo  umber  were  also  found.  Eggshell  fragments  obtained  with  a live  Mar- 
bled Murrelet  from  a felled  hemlock  tree  about  0.5  miles  E of  Masset,  Queen  Charlotte 
Islands  on  about  4 June  1953  (Guiguet,  Audubon  Mag.  58:164-167,  174,  1956),  were  compared 
to  the  Jewett  and  Cantwell  oviduct  eggs  in  the  United  States  National  Museum  by  I.  McT. 
McCowan.  He  found  that  they  agreed  with  the  latter  specimens  in  ground  color,  markings 
and  texture,  and  that  they  did  not  resemble  eggs  of  the  Ancient  Murrelet  {Synthliboramphus 
antiquus)  in  these  details  (Drent  and  Guiguet  1961). 

An  egg  of  the  Asian  race,  Brachyramphus  marmoratus  perdix,  was  collected  from  a nest 
on  17  June  1961,  by  Kuzyakin  (Ornithologyia  6:315-320,  1963,  English  translation  in  Josselyn 
Van  Tyne  Library,  Univ.  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan)  about  12  km  NW  of  Okhotsk, 
Siberia.  It  is  of  interest  that  his  egg  differs  from  the  North  American  specimens  in  color 
(blue-greenish  with  fine  spots  of  brownish  and  hazel)  and  in  being  slightly  more  elongated 
(63.6  X 39.3  mm). 

The  principal  distinguishing  characterstic  of  North  American  Marbled  Murrelet  eggs  ap- 
pears to  be  their  yellowish  or  pale  green  ground  color.  The  ground  color  of  eggs  of  the 
congeneric  Kittlitz’s  Murrelet  (B.  brevirostris)  has  been  described  as  “olive  lake”  (Thayer, 
Condor  16:117-118,  1914)  or  “olive-green”  (Bailey,  Condor  75:457-486,  1973).  A series  of 
over  100  sets  of  eggs  of  the  Ancient  Murrelet,  the  only  other  murrelet  breeding  sympatricaUy 
with  the  Marbled  Murrelet,  in  the  WFVZ  collection  have  ground  colors  ranging  from  a pale 
cream  color  to  rich  huffy  brown;  none  are  yellowish  or  greenish. 

In  addition  to  the  aforementioned  specimens,  a possible  Marbled  Murrelet  egg  was  col- 
lected by  Stanton  Warburton,  Jr.,  “on  a mossy  setting  within  a cavity  of  rocks”  in  a rock 
slide  far  above  timberline  on  Mt.  Doolth,  Chichagof  Island,  Alexander  Archipelago,  Alaska 
on  13  June  1931  (Gabrielson  and  Lincoln,  The  Birds  of  Alaska,  Stackpole  Co.  and  Wildlife 
Management  Inst.,  Washington,  D.C.,  1959;  Drent  and  Guiguet  1961).  A formal  description 
of  this  egg  has  not  been  published,  but  a photograph  of  it  is  shown  by  Alcorn  (Northwest 
Birds  Distribution  and  Eggs,  Western  Media  Printing  and  Publications,  Inc.,  Tacoma,  Wash- 
ington, 1978).  Judging  from  that  illustration,  the  egg  does  not  have  a ground  color  charac- 
teristic of  the  known  North  American  Marbled  Murrelet  eggs.  Bent  (1919)  described  an  egg 
in  the  Charles  Doe  collection  (now  at  the  Florida  State  Museum)  taken  on  10  June  1904, 
about  70  miles  N of  Nome,  Alaska  by  A.  H.  Dunham,  which  was  attributed  to  the  Marbled 
Murrelet  and  which  resembles  the  authenticated  specimens  in  color  {massicot  yellow  with 
small  spots  of  bone  brown  and  deep  quaker  drab)  and  size  (60.5  X 37.5  mm).  The  identity 
of  this  egg  was  questioned  by  Gabrielson  and  Lincoln  (1959)  on  distributional  grounds.  Four 
eggs  taken  by  S.  J.  Darcus  on  Langara  Island,  Queen  Charlotte  Islands  on  14-15  May  1927, 
and  claimed  to  be  those  of  the  Marbled  Murrelet  (Darcus,  Can.  Field-Nat.  41:197-199,  1927) 
are  almost  certainly  those  of  the  Ancient  Murrelet,  judging  from  their  color  and  the  nest 
descriptions.  One  of  the  latter  eggs,  now  in  the  collection  of  the  Delaware  Museum  of  Natural 
History,  is  illustrated  by  Harrison  (A  Field  Guide  to  the  Nests,  Eggs  and  Nestlings  of  North 
American  Birds,  Collins,  Glasgow,  Scotland,  1978). 

It  is  therefore  likely  that  the  Booth  specimen  is  the  only  whole  egg  specimen  of  the 
nominate  race,  B.  m.  marmoratus,  known  to  have  been  taken  from  a nest.  Had  the  nest-site 
from  which  it  was  collected  been  adequately  described  it  would  doubtless  have  qualified  as 
the  first  North  American  nest  of  the  species  known  to  ornithology. 

I am  grateful  to  Lee  Astheimer  for  providing  me  with  data  and  photographs  of  the  1979 
Alaska  egg,  to  Janet  Hinshaw  of  the  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Library  for  translations  of  Russian 
manuscripts  on  murrelet  breeding  biology,  and  to  Jon  C.  Barlow,  Chandler  Robbins  and 
Kenneth  Parkes  for  their  helpful  comments  on  an  earlier  draft.  David  Niles,  Dana  Gardner, 
Julie  Kiff.  Sam  Sumida  and  Roger  Cobb  also  provided  help  in  various  ways.  This  paper  was 


GENERAL  NOTES 


403 


supported  by  the  Western  Foundation  of  Vertebrate  Zoology. — LloyI)  F.  Kiff,  Western 
Foundation  of  Vertebrate  Zoology,  1100  Glendon  Ave.,  Los  Angeles,  California  90024.  Ac- 
cepted 20  .May  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  403-405 


First  documented  Cinnamon  Teal  nesting  in  North  Dakota  produced  hybrids. — 

Although  Cinnamon  Teal  {Anas  cyanoptera)  are  seen  in  North  Dakota  almost  every  year, 
Stewart  (Breeding  Birds  of  North  Dakota,  Tri-College  Center  for  Environmental  Studies, 
Fargo,  North  Dakota,  1975)  lists  the  breeding  status  as  hypothetical.  There  is  1 unpublished 
record  of  a hen  with  brood  sighted  at  Napoleon,  Logan  Co.,  on  17  July  1915,  by  H.  H. 
Sheldon  of  the  U.S.  Biological  Survey.  However,  without  substantiating  evidence,  this  sight 
record  is  unacceptable  because  hens  and  ducklings  of  Cinnamon  Teal  are  indistinguishable 
from  Blue-winged  Teal  {A.  discors).  There  are  no  verified  records  of  Cinnamon  Teal  breeding 
in  South  Dakota  (Whitney,  Harrell,  Harris,  Holden,  Johnson,  Rose  and  Springer,  The  Birds 
of  South  Dakota,  The  S.D.  Ornith.  Union,  Vermillion,  South  Dakota,  1978),  and  the  nearest 
breeding  record  to  North  Dakota  is  for  central  Montana  (Skaar,  Montana  Bird  Distribution, 
Bozeman,  Montana,  1975)  about  240  km  west  of  the  North  Dakota  border. 

On  30  April  1978,  a male  Cinnamon  Teal  with  a hen  was  sighted  in  McLean  County,  and 
observed  repeatedly  in  the  same  vicinity  during  spring;  we  suspected  the  hen  was  nesting. 
Biologists  on  the  study  area  examined  all  teal  hens  captured  and  on  9 June  1978,  a “large- 
billed Blue-winged  Teal  hen”  was  trapped  on  a nest.  This  hen  had  characteristics  of  a 
Cinnamon  Teal  hen  as  noted  by  Wallace  and  Ogilvie  (Br.  Brids  70:290-294,  1977),  including 
a more  sloping  forehead  than  a Blue-winged  Teal,  a darker  head,  a darker  loral  spot  and  a 
spatulated  biU.  Tbe  exposed  culmen  length  was  41.9  mm  which,  according  to  Spencer  (The 
Cinnamon  Teal  [Anas  cyanoptera  VieiUot]:  its  life  history,  ecology  and  management,  M.S. 
thesis,  Utah  State  Univ.,  Logan,  Utah,  1953),  placed  the  bird  outside  the  range  of  exposed 
culmen  lengths  for  Blue-winged  Teal  (36.5^1  mm)  and  within  that  of  Cinnamon  Teal  (41-46 
mm). 

After  measurements  and  photographs  were  taken,  the  hen  was  released;  6 eggs  of  her 
clutch  were  collected  for  propagation  at  the  Northern  Prairie  Wildlife  Research  Center. 
Three  males  were  raised  to  maturity.  By  early  March  1979  the  birds  developed  red-brown 
irises  and  cinnamon  breast  coloring  similar  to  Cinnamon  Teal,  but  also  had  partial  white 
crescents  on  their  heads  and  other  plumage  characteristics  resembling  the  Blue-winged  Teal 
(Fig.  1).  The  3 males  were  apparently  Cinnamon  Teal  X Blue-winged  Teal  hybrids.  Mea- 
surements of  these  birds  as  adults  compared  closely  with  those  of  the  5 hybrids  measured 
by  Bolen  (Wilson  Bull.  91:367-370,  1979).  Upper  mandible  lengths  of  the  3 hybrids  were  50 
mm  or  greater,  which  would  fit  Spencer’s  (1953)  criterion  for  Cinnamon  Teal  (Table  1). 

Crown  of  the  hybrids  were  purplish  iridescent  resembling  Blue-winged  Teal,  but  the 
cheeks  were  a mixture  of  cinnamon  and  black  flecking  with  facial  crescents  wider  at  the 
base  than  those  of  the  Blue-winged  Teal.  Also,  the  crescents  were  not  totally  white  but 
contained  many  red-brown  feathers.  The  chest,  belly  and  sides  of  the  hybrids  were  cinnamon 
colored  but  contained  black  spots  like  those  found  on  Blue-winged  Teal.  The  hybrids  had  a 
remnant  of  the  Blue-winged  Teal  flank  patch  but  it  was  smaller,  cinnamon  colored  with  black 
flecking.  There  are  numerous  reports  on  Cinnamon  Teal  X Blue-winged  Teal  crosses,  and 
those  pictured  by  Lahrman  (Blue  Jay  29:28,  1971)  and  Bolen  (1979)  appear  to  be  similar  to 
the  ones  reported  here.  An  unreported  Cinnamon  Teal  X Blue-winged  Teal  hybrid  collected 
near  Wishek,  McIntosh  Co.,  on  23  May  1970,  is  preserved  at  the  North  Dakota  Game  and 


404 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Eig.  1.  Male  Cinnamon  Teal  x Blue-winged  Teal  hybrid  (bottom)  compared  with  a nor- 
mal Blue-winged  Teal  male  (top). 


GENERAL  NOTES 


405 


Table  1 

Measurements  of  Cinnamon  Teal  x Blue-winged  Teal  Hybrids  at  Age  289  Days 


Bird 

1D“ 

Tarsus 

length 

(mm) 

Weight 

(gm) 

Total 

wing 

length 

(mm) 

Exposed 

culmen 

length 

(mm) 

Upper 

mandihle 

length 

(mm) 

Upper 

mandihle 

width 

(mm) 

Wing 
chord 
length 
( m m ) 

1049 



360 

255 

45 

50 

19 

178 

F500 

31 

370 

282 

45 

54 

18 

181 

F499 

29 

358 

262 

45 

52 

18 

182 

® Skins  of  birds  F499  and  F500  are  available  at  the  Northern  Prairie  Wildlife  Research  Onter,  Jamestown.  North  Dakota. 


Fish  Department  Offiee  at  Bismarek.  This  bird  is  similar  in  plumage  to  ones  described  above, 
but  lacks  the  facial  crescent. 

The  hybrids  observed  here  resulted  either  because  the  Cinnamon  Teal  female  or  the  male 
were  hybrids  or  because  the  female  was  mated  to  a male  Blue-winged  Teal.  We  believe  that 
the  McLean  County  hen  was  a pure  Cinnamon  Teal  because  of  her  bill  measurements  and 
plumage,  and  that  she  had  bred  with  Blue-winged  Teal  male(s).  Blue-winged  Teal  males 
were  abundant  in  the  vicinity  and  according  to  Connelly  (A  Comparative  Study  of  Blue- 
winged Teal  and  Cinnamon  Teal  Breeding  in  Eastern  Washington.  M.S.  thesis,  Washington 
State  Univ.,  Pullman,  Washington,  1977),  the  more  aggressive  Blue-winged  Teal  could  dom- 
inate Cinnamon  Teal,  particularly  in  certain  habitats. 

Nine  male  Cinnamon  or  cinnamon-like  teal  were  sighted  by  North  Dakota  Game  and  Fish 
Department  biologists  during  spring  waterfowl  surveys  between  1958  and  1978  (Charles  H. 
Schroeder,  pers.  comm.).  Three  were  considered  Cinnamon  Teal  X Blue-winged  Teal  hy- 
brids. This  surprisingly  large  percentage  of  hybrids,  plus  the  production  of  hybrid  young  by 
an  apparent  Cinnamon  Teal  hen,  may  indicate  that  the  Cinnamon  Teal  is  having  difficulty 
establishing  itself  as  a pure  species  in  North  Dakota  because  of  sexual  aggressiveness  of  the 
ubiquitous  Blue-winged  Teal. 

We  thank  Douglas  L.  Johnson  for  commenting  on  the  manuscript,  and  we  appreciate  the 
field  help  of  Randy  Naze  and  Jon  Anderson. — John  T.  Lokemoen  AND  David  E.  Sharp, 
U.S.D.I.  Fish  and  Wildlife  Service,  Northern  Prairie  Wildlife  Research  Center,  Jamestown, 
North  Dakota  58401.  Accepted  1 June  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  405^06 


First  record  of  the  Black-ehinned  Hummingbird  in  Alberta. — The  Black-chinned 
Hummingbird  {Archilochus  alexandri)  occurs  from  northwestern  Mexico  north  to  southern 
British  Columbia.  It  is  not  usually  found  east  of  the  Rocky  Mountains,  except  in  the  Plateau 
region  of  southwest  Texas  (A.O.U.  Check-list  Com.,  Checklist  of  North  American  Birds,  5th 
ed..  Lord  Baltimore  Press,  Baltimore,  Maryland,  1957). 

The  black-chin  is  uncommon  in  the  northern  portions  of  its  range.  In  Idaho,  the  species 
is  distributed  primarily  in  the  northern  portion  of  the  state  (Burleigh,  Birds  of  Idaho,  Caxton 
Printers,  CaldweU,  Idaho,  1972).  In  Montana,  it  is  restricted  to  the  northwest  portion  of  the 
state;  it  breeds  in  the  Missoula  and  Philipsburg  regions  and  non-breeding  individuals  have 
been  recorded  farther  north  in  Libby,  Kalispell,  Poison  and  Seely  Lakes  regions  (Skaar, 


406 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Montana  Bird  Distribution,  Bozeman,  Montana,  1975).  The  species  is  relatively  rare  in  Wash- 
ington, occurring  primarily  in  the  eastern  portion  of  the  state  (Jewett,  Taylor,  Shaw  and 
Aldrich,  Birds  of  Washington  State,  Univ.  Washington  Press,  Pullman,  Washington,  1953). 
In  British  Columbia,  the  species  is  limited  to  the  southern  interior  of  the  province;  its  range 
extends  east  from  Chilliwack  to  Creston  and  Kimberley  and  north  to  Grindrod  (Godfrey, 
Natl.  Mus.  Canada  Bull.  203,  1966). 

On  25  June  1979,  a Black-chinned  Hummingbird  was  picked  up  in  Calgary,  Alberta;  the 
bird  had  struck  a window  on  or  just  prior  to  25  June.  This  specimen,  preserved  as  a study 
skin  (Provincial  Museum  of  Alberta,  specimen  no.  Z79. 143.1),  was  identified  as  a mature 
male  by  both  plumage  and  age  criteria  given  by  Ortiz-Crespo  (Auk  89:851-857,  1972).  Its 
testes  were  approximately  1x1  mm.  The  specimen  had  very  little  body  fat  and  weighed 
2.9  g. 

This  specimen  constitutes  the  first  authentic  record  of  the  Black-chinned  Hummingbird 
in  Alberta.  There  are,  however,  unsubstantiated  indications  that  this  species  was  recorded 
in  Alberta  in  the  1800’s.  Saunders  (Ottawa  Nat.  16:97-103,  1902)  stated,  without  supportive 
evidence,  that  the  range  of  this  species  extends  east  from  the  Pacific  Ocean  to  the  Alberta 
foothills,  and  as  far  north  as  Banff  in  the  Rocky  Mountains.  Bendire  (Smithson.  Contrib.  to 
Knowledge  985,  1895)  includes  southern  Alberta  within  the  range  of  this  species,  but  placed 
a question  mark  after  this  statement  in  his  text. 

Several  lines  of  evidence  suggest  that  the  occurrence  of  the  Black-chinned  Hummingbird 
in  Alberta  is  accidental.  Firstly,  the  northern  limits  of  the  Black-chinned  Hummingbird’s 
range  are  quite  stable.  The  historical  distribution  of  the  black-chin  in  Montana  (Bendire 
1895;  Saunders,  Pac.  Coast  Avif.  14,  1921)  is  very  similar  to  its  present  distribution  (Skaar 
1975),  with  the  exception  of  a dead  bird  recovered  in  Bozeman  during  spring  migration  in 
1976  (Rogers,  Am.  Birds  30:867,  1976).  The  present  distribution  of  the  species  in  British 
Columbia  (Guiguet,  B.C.  Prov.  Mus.  Handbook  37,  1978)  parallels  its  historical  distribution 
(Brooks  and  Swarth,  Pac.  Coast  Avif.  17,  1925),  with  the  exception  of  a male  observed  at 
Nicholson,  8 km  south  of  Golden  (Rogers  1976).  There  is  also  a sight  record  for  Regina, 
Saskatchewan  (Jowsey  and  Jowsey,  Blue  Jay  28:120,  1970). 

Secondly,  the  Black-chinned  Hummingbird  prefers  the  dry  foothills  and  canyons  of  the 
Upper  Austral  Zone  (Grinnell  and  Miller,  Pac.  Coast  Avif.  27,  1944),  a habitat  type  not  found 
in  the  Transition  Zone  in  southern  Alberta  (Hunt,  Natural  Regions  of  the  United  States  and 
Canada,  W.  H.  Freeman  and  Co.,  San  Francisco,  California,  1974). 

Acknowledgments. — The  authors  extend  their  thanks  to  the  Alberta  Fish  and  Wildlife  Di- 
vision, particularly  G.  L.  Erickson,  for  recovering  and  donating  the  specimen  to  the  Provin- 
cial Museum.  This  paper  is  Provincial  Museum  of  Alberta  contribution  number  56. — PHILIP 
H.  R.  Stepney  and  Peter  C.  Boxall,  Ornithology  Program,  Provincial  Museum  of  Alberta, 
12845  102  Avenue,  Edmonton,  Alberta  T5N  0M6  Canada.  Accepted  30  July  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  407-437 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


Special  Review 

Old  Birds  and  New  Ideas:  Progress  and 
Controversy  in  Paleornithology 


Papers  in  Avian  Paleontology  Honoring  Hildegarde  Howard.  By  Kenneth  E. 
Campbell,  Jr.  (ed.).  Contributions  in  Science,  Natural  History  Museum  of  Los  Angeles  Coun- 
ty, No.  330,  1980:xxxviii  -I-  260  pp.,  numerous  tables,  line  drawings  and  black-and-white 
photographs.  $20.00  -I-  $1.25  shipping  charge.  Order  from  the  Museum  Bookshop,  Los  An- 
geles County  Museum  of  Natural  History,  900  Exposition  Blvd.,  Los  Angeles,  California 
90007.  Relationships  and  Evolution  of  Flamingos  (Aves:  Phoenicopteridae).  By 
Storrs  L.  Olson  and  Alan  Feduccia.  Smithsonian  Contributions  to  Zoology,  No.  316.  Smith- 
sonian Institution  Press,  Washington,  D.C.,  1980:73  pp.,  40  figs.,  2 tables.  Price  not  given. 
Presbyornis  AND  THE  ORIGIN  OF  THE  Anseriformes  (Aves:  Charadriomorphae).  By 
Storrs  L.  Olson  and  Alan  Feduccia.  Smithsonian  Contributions  to  Zoology,  No.  323.  Smith- 
sonian Institution  Press,  Washington,  D.C.,  1980:24  pp.,  15  figs.  Price  not  given.  The  Age 
OF  Birds.  By  Alan  Feduccia.  Harvard  University  Press,  Cambridge,  Massachusetts, 
1980:196  pp.,  numerous  black-and-white  illustrations.  $20.00. — The  publication  in  one  year 
of  several  major  works  in  avian  paleontology  provides  an  opportunity  to  assess  the  current 
situation  in  this  important  subject.  Included  in  this  survey  is  a Festschrift  volume  honoring 
one  of  the  field’s  leaders,  2 monographs  that  analyze  both  extant  and  fossil  birds  to  develop 
provocative  new  phylogenetic  theories  and  a popular  volume  that  aims  to  explain  the  history 
of  birds  and  the  methods  of  its  study  to  a nonspecialist  audience. 

Dr.  Hildegarde  Howard  has  spent  over  half  a century  studying  fossil  birds  at  the  Natural 
History  Museum  of  Los  Angeles  County.  The  volume  edited  by  Kenneth  F.  Campbell,  Jr. 
is  a tribute  to  her  immense  contributions  to  the  discipline  and  to  the  inspiration  that  she  has 
provided  to  other  workers.  It  includes  “appreciations”  by  several  colleagues  and  a review 
of  her  work  by  Campbell,  who  notes  among  Howard’s  major  contributions  her  studies  on  the 
Emeryville  sheUmound,  the  asphalt  deposits  of  Rancho  La  Brea,  the  tertiary  marine  birds 
of  southern  California,  and  the  use  of  trinomials  to  designate  chronoclinal  variation.  Her 
work  has  included  not  only  the  description  of  many  new  forms,  but  also  extensive  paleoavi- 
faunal  analyses  and  important  reviews.  Campbell  emphasizes  the  “caution,  restraint,  and 
thoroughness  in  methodology”  that  characterizes  Howard’s  work.  A bibliography  of  her  140 
titles  from  1923-1979  is  included.  Also  reprinted  here  are  the  detailed  drawings  of  avian 
bones  that  first  appeared  in  Howard’s  1929  paper  on  the  Emeryville  sheUmound,  and  which 
have  long  served  as  a basis  for  avian  osteological  nomenclature. 

The  volume  contains  19  contributed  papers  on  various  aspects  of  paleornithology,  including 
faunal  studies,  reviews  of  specific  groups,  descriptions  of  new  forms,  archeological  studies, 
and  theoretical  aspects.  The  paper  likely  to  cause  the  most  comment  is  Joel  Cracraft’s 
critique  of  the  application  of  phylogenetic  theory  and  method  in  avian  paleontology.  Craeraft 
argues  that  the  use  of  cladistic  methods  wiU  improve  systematic  practices  in  the  field;  this 
is  not  a new  idea.  Hopson  and  Radinsky  (Paleobiol.  6:250-270,  1980)  discuss  the  impact  of 
this  approach  on  vertebrate  paleontology,  noting  its  gradual  spread  into  the  field  as  weU  as 
the  resistance  to  it  in  some  quarters.  This  characterizes  avian  paleontology  as  weU.  Some 
paleontologists  have  especiaUy  resisted  the  argument  that  fossils  cannot  be  designated  un- 


408 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


ANSERANATIDAE  ANHIMIDAE 


PHOENICOPTERIDAE  ANATIDAE 


RECURVIROSTRIDAE  PRESBYORNITHIDAE 


EARLY  CHARADRIIFORMES 

Fig.  1.  An  attempted  reconstruction  of  Olson  and  Feduccia’s  theory  of  phylogenetic 
relationships  in  several  groups  of  birds. 


equivocally  as  direct  ancestors  of  later  forms.  Storrs  L.  Olson  in  one  paper  rails  against 
simplistic  applications  of  cladistics,  while  other  contributors  are  uncomfortable  and  apolo- 
getic about  their  continued  use  of  phenetic  methods.  In  my  opinion,  many  paleontologists 
are  overly  sensitive  to  criticisms  of  their  methods,  but  we  must  recognize  that  their  data  are 
by  their  nature  often  fragmentary,  both  in  terms  of  individual  specimens  and  of  the  fossil 
record  as  a whole.  Part  of  the  problem  arises  from  incomplete  fossils  that  lack  sufficient 
information  to  allow  unambiguous  classification,  let  alone  the  construction  of  phylogenies. 
Much  of  the  older  literature  contains  taxonomic  decisions  that  are  simply  not  justified  by  the 
available  data,  which  is  why  so  much  of  the  literature  involves  the  reallocation  of  fossils  to 
new  taxa.  Olson  has  done  a major  job  in  this  area  lately. 

The  goal  of  higher-level  systematics  is  to  work  out  the  relationships  between  the  major 
groups.  The  highest  level  at  which  presumably  “natural”  (monophyletic)  avian  groups  are 
usually  classified  is  the  order.  The  problem  is  to  work  out  the  relationships  between  these 
orders,  and  this  essentially  must  begin  by  determining  pairs  of  orders  that  are  related  either 
as  sister  groups,  or  by  having  one  derived  from  some  group  within  another.  The  2 Smithsonian 
Contributions  by  Olson  and  Feduccia  represent  major  efforts  in  this  direction  involving 
comparative  studies  of  both  fossil  and  extant  birds.  In  the  first  paper,  the  authors  argue  that 
the  flamingos  (Phoenicopteridae)  are  not  related  to  either  the  Ciconiiformes  or  the  Anseri- 
formes  as  previous  authors  have  suggested,  but  that  they  evolved  from  the  family  Recurvi- 
rostridae  (stilts  and  avocets)  of  the  order  Charadriiformes.  In  the  second,  they  argue  that 
the  Anseriformes  are  not  closely  related  to  the  Galliformes  as  often  stated,  but  have  evolved 
from  the  Charadriiformes  via  an  extinct,  intermediate  family  Presbyornithidae.  In  both  pa- 
pers they  marshall  impressive  arrays  of  data  with  which  they  evaluate  alternative  hypotheses. 
Both  papers  are  so  poorly  organized,  however,  that  it  is  often  difficult  to  follow  an  argument 
through  various  digressions,  and  worst  of  all.  there  are  no  dendrograms  summarizing  the 
phylogenetic  hypotheses  being  proposed;  indeed  these  hypotheses  are  not  clearly  articulated 
in  either  paper.  In  the  accompanying  figure  I have  reconstructed  from  their  texts  what  I 
believe  to  be  Olson  and  Feduccia's  combined  hypotheses. 

One  problem  with  these  papers  is  their  confusing  mixture  of  methods.  The  studies  them- 
selves are  like  those  intermediate  fossils  that  the  authors  analyze,  that  is,  mosaics  of  primitive 
(phenetic)  and  advanced  (cladistic)  clustering  techniques.  The  application  of  cladistic  meth- 
ods here  is  uncertain,  however,  because  there  is  usually  no  explanation  offered  of  how  the 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


409 


characters  were  determined  to  be  derived.  The  connection  of  the  Preshyornithidae  to  the 
Anseriformes  is  proposed  on  the  basis  of  a complex  shared  derived  cranial  morphology,  and 
appears  to  be  well  founded.  However,  the  critical  connection  between  the  Preshyornithidae 
and  the  Charadriiformes  is  supported  only  weakly  at  best.  Some  skull  characters  are  listed 
(pp.  14-15)  as  similarities  between  Fresbyornis  and  Charadriiformes,  but  these  appear  to  be 
mainly  primitive  avian  characters  from  which  the  Anseriformes  alone  diverge.  Thus,  they  do 
not  link  the  Preshyornithidae  to  the  Charadriiformes  any  more  than  to  any  other  group  of 
birds.  The  supposed  charadriiform  connection  is  also  based  on  characters  of  the  postcranial 
skeleton.  These  are  listed  on  pp.  12-13,  but  are  not  discussed  in  detail,  reference  being 
made  to  2 earlier  papers  by  Feduccia.  One  (Am.  Scientist  66:298-304,  1978)  is  a semipopular 
account  with  no  real  data;  the  basis  for  the  connection  evidently  lies  in  the  other  (Auk 
93:587-601,  1976).  This  paper  simply  does  not  provide  the  needed  documentation  for  such 
an  important  hypothesis.  For  one  thing,  Feduccia  claims  to  accept  the  superiority  of  cladistic 
methods  over  phenetic  clustering,  but  confesses  that  he  cannot  analyze  his  data  cladisticaUy 
with  much  confidence  (p.  599).  There  are  few  data  to  analyze  anyway.  The  reader  cannot 
determine  what  data  were  actually  used;  the  paper  has  no  methods  section  nor  any  list  of 
species  or  numbers  of  specimens  examined.  The  text  refers  vaguely  to  “recurvirostrids,” 
“shorebirds”  ( = Charadrii?)  and  “charadriiformes,”  but  only  1 charadriiform  species  (Re- 
curvirostra  americana)  is  really  analyzed.  There  is  no  indication  that  any  comparative  analysis 
of  the  Charadriiformes  was  made.  (Indeed,  in  this  1976  paper  Feduccia  had  used  these  same 
few  data  fo  support  a different  hypothesis,  namely  that  Fresbyornis  is  ancestral  to  both 
Anseriformes  and  flamingos,  which  are  sister  groups  in  a lineage  separate  from  that  con- 
taining recurvirostrids  and  “shorebirds.”)  Ultimately,  the  proposed  relationship  between 
Fresbyornis  and  the  Charadriiformes  appears  to  be  based  on  some  intuitive  notion  of  general 
similarity,  and  is  hardly  documented  in  these  papers.  What  is  needed  are:  (1)  derived  char- 
acters shared  by  the  Charadriiformes,  Preshyornithidae  and  Anseriformes,  plus,  (2)  addi- 
tional derived  states  shared  by  the  latter  2 groups.  The  second  requirement  is  well  met,  but 
the  first  is  not. 

There  are  other  difficulties.  The  hypothesis  that  Anseriformes  are  derived  from  Charad- 
riiformes means  that  the  often-claimed  connection  between  the  Anhimidae  (screamers)  and 
the  GaUiformes  must  be  incorrect.  The  authors  note  a resemblance  between  screamers  and 
the  Magpie  Goose  (Anseranas  semipalmata),  which  ties  the  screamers  to  the  Anatidae.  They 
report  the  discovery  of  a series  of  minute  ridges  in  the  ramphotheca  of  screamers;  these 
resemble  poorly-developed  lamellae,  and  are  considered  homologous  with  those  of  the  Anat- 
idae. Olson  and  Feduccia  claim  that  these  are  vestigial  lamellae,  and  that  screamers  evolved 
from  fully  lamellate  ancestors,  with  Anseranas  being  close  to  an  intermediate  stage  in  this 
trend.  Here  they  have  introduced  a revolutionary  view  of  the  history  of  the  waterfowl.  Tra- 
ditionally, screamers  were  regarded  as  very  primitive,  Anseranas  as  more  advanced  and  the 
Anatidae  as  most  advanced.  Olson  and  Feduccia  suggest  instead  that  Anseranas  and  scream- 
ers are  highly  derived.  This  provocative  thesis  poses  some  problems.  It  suggests  that  major 
anatid  specializations,  including  a spatulate  bill,  lameUae  and  simultaneous  wing  molt,  were 
secondarily  lost  in  screamers.  As  an  argument  that  the  feeble  lamellae  of  screamers  are 
vestigial  (reduced)  rather  than  primitively  rudimentary,  they  argue  that  screamers  are  not 
filter  feeders.  Interestingly,  in  the  flamingo  paper  they  have  provided  an  alternative  hypoth- 
esis in  another  context.  In  discussing  the  evolution  of  bill  lamellae  in  Fachyptila  (Procel- 
lariidae),  where  different  species  show  different  degrees  of  development,  they  suggest  that 
“the  rudimentary  lamellae  in  the  less  specialized  filter-feeding  petrels  provide  gaps  for  the 
expulsion  of  water  while  the  prey  is  held  in  place  with  the  tongue.”  Perhaps  the  lamellae  in 
screamers  serve  similarly.  Even  though  they  are  not  filter  feeders,  they  do  feed  on  marsh 
plants  and  might  have  use  for  such  drainage.  Olson  and  Feduccia’s  hypothesis  requires  the 


410 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


re-evolution  of  a set  of  primitive  characters.  In  the  flamingo  paper  (p.  67),  they  consider  such 
a process  in  the  Recurvirostridae  to  be  unlikely.  Should  they  not  apply  the  same  caution 
here?  Another  possibility  is  that  the  screamers  are  the  sister  group  of  the  Anseranatidae  + 
Anatidae.  This  would  require  fewer  evolutionary  reversals.  I am  not  arguing  one  hypothesis 
over  another  here,  but  merely  pointing  out  that  alternative  hypotheses  of  this  sort  can  best 
be  evaluated  with  cladograms  showing  the  distribution  of  characters  among  taxa. 

The  papers  by  Olson  and  Feduccia  are  powerful  efforts  to  synthesize  paleontological  and 
neobiological  data  so  as  to  analyze  relationships  between  major  higher  taxa.  The  flamingo 
paper  is  especially  well  argued,  as  it  includes  new  fossil  forms  that  reasonably  bridge  the 
gap  between  hypothesized  ancestors  and  descendents,  as  weU  as  an  excellent  functional 
interpretation  of  the  flamingo  feeding  mechanism.  The  strengths  of  these  papers  lie  in  the 
detailed  descriptive  comparisons  between  numerous  fossil  and  extant  forms,  and  in  their 
uninhibited  speculations  in  a context  of  stratigraphic  and  functional  considerations.  Their 
weaknesses  lie  in  their  indecisive  mixtures  of  phenetic  and  often  poorly-argued  cladistic 
methods,  and  in  their  failure  to  present  their  phylogenetic  hypotheses  in  the  form  of  den- 
drograms showing  the  proposed  relationships  and  the  characters  on  which  they  are  based. 
In  this  way  they  could  also  show  whether  their  data  really  allow  a particular  fossil  form  to 
be  hypothesized  as  the  direct  ancestor  of  later  groups;  that  is,  whether  it  possesses  aU  and 
only  those  derived  characters  that  such  an  ancestor  must  exhibit.  As  noted,  the  designation 
of  fossils  as  ancestors  is  a point  of  contention  between  many  paleontologists  and  some  of 
their  critics;  ultimately,  it  is  a matter  of  character  distribution,  which  can  be  displayed 
unambiguously  by  a cladogram.  An  example  of  the  splendid  work  that  can  result  from  the 
application  of  rigorous  analytical  methods  to  a good  fossil  record  is  the  study  of  the  Dro- 
mornithidae  by  Pat  Vickers  Rich  in  the  Howard  Festscrift  volume.  Here  the  hypothesis  is 
set  forth  precisely  in  a cladogram  with  the  characters  defining  each  node  shown  in  the 
diagram  and  discussed  in  the  text.  This  paper  sets  a standard  to  be  followed  in  studies  of 
this  sort. 

In  The  Age  of  Birds  Alan  Feduccia  presents  an  account  of  avian  evolution  for  the  nonspe- 
cialist. He  wisely  chose  to  discuss  highlights  of  avian  history  rather  than  getting  bogged  down 
in  endless  details  or  argument.  The  result  is  a readable  introduction  to  the  subject  with  a lot 
of  familiar  stories  adequately  retold,  some  interesting  new  material,  and  several  irksome 
shortcomings.  Chapter  1,  “The  feathered  reptile,”  is  a retelling  of  the  Archeopteryx  story. 
The  point  of  the  title  is  that  Archeopteryx  is  a true  evolutionary  intermediate  showing  that 
birds  evolved  from  “reptiles.”  I must  insist  that  it  is  not  a “missing  link,”  however,  because 
it  is  not  missing.  It  is  just  a link.  It  is  not  a “reptile”  either,  it  is  a bird  because  it  has 
feathers,  a derived  condition  that  defines  Aves  as  monophyletic.  Of  special  value  is  the 
review  of  the  more  recently  discovered  fossils,  the  Maxberg,  Teyler  and  Eichstatt  specimens. 
The  photographs  are  also  worthwhile;  some  are  of  the  familiar  London  and  Berlin  specimens, 
but  the  newer  specimens  have  not  previously  been  shown  in  any  popular  account  that  I 
know  ol. 

In  chapter  2,  The  Ancestry  of  Birds,  Feduccia  reviews  the  various  theories  of  avian  origins. 
There  is  a brief  mention  of  recent  challenges  to  John  Ostrom’s  popular  view  of  dinosaurian 
ancestry,  but  nothing  new  is  added.  Chapter  3,  The  Evolution  of  Flight,  carries  us  through 
a familiar  tale  of  hypothetical  behavioral  stages  (jumping,  falling,  parachuting,  etc.)  and 
organisms  (Nopsca’s  and  Heilmann’s  proavian  dreams,  Ostrom’s  flyswatters,  baby  hoatzins). 
As  usual  much  of  the  discussion  centers  on  the  flying  abilities  of  Archeopteryx,  and  some 
recent  anatomical  interpretations  are  mentioned.  However,  certain  provocative  new  theories 
published  in  recent  issues  of  the  Auklet  are  inexplicably  ignored.  The  topics  of  chapters  2 
and  3 are  hardly  mentioned  in  the  contributions  to  the  Howard  Festschrift,  most  of  which 
have  a satisfyingly  high  ratio  of  data  to  speculation.  Chapter  4,  Toothed  Birds  and  Divers, 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


411 


is  mostly  standard  stuff  (Hesperornis,  Ichthyornis)  with  brief  mention  of  more  recent  work 
on  wing-propelled  divers  by  Hildegarde  Howard  (auks)  and  Storrs  Olson  (plotopterids). 
Campbell  gives  an  account  of  Howard’s  studies  of  both  groups  in  the  Festschrift,  where 
Olson  also  has  a paper  on  the  plotopterids. 

Chapter  5 is  mostly  devoted  to  an  account  of  Olson  and  Feduccia’s  theory  of  relationships 
between  flamingos,  Anseriformes,  Charadriiformes  and  Presbyornis,  which  1 discussed 
above.  Although  stronger  in  assertion  than  documentation,  it  does  provide  a sort  of  phylogeny 
(p.  95)  outlining  some  of  the  relationships  proposed  in  the  2 Olson  and  Feduccia  papers, 
where  no  such  representation  was  given.  A problem  becomes  apparent  here  that  did  not 
surface  in  the  other  papers.  In  earlier  works  Feduccia  made  a point  that  in  Presbyornis  the 
nasal-frontal  bones  are  arranged  in  a V-shaped  conformation  otherwise  found  only  in  fla- 
mingos. This  seemed  important  while  he  was  advocating  a Presbyornis-^dimmgo  connection. 
In  The  Age  of  Birds  (p.  92),  Feduccia  merely  notes  that  “the  flamingolike  nasal-frontal  region 
may  stiU  link  Presbyornis  and  the  flamingos,  although  their  divergence  was  surely  an  ancient 
one.”  What  does  this  mean?  Is  this  one  of  those  mysterious  primitive  charadriiform  char- 
acters often  mentioned  but  seldom  documented  in  these  works?  This  example  illustrates  the 
vagueness  that  results  from  the  lack  of  a proper  character  analysis. 

Chapter  6,  The  Evolution  of  Flightlessness,  is  the  best  part  of  The  Age  of  Birds,  and  is 
the  best  discussion  of  the  subject  in  the  recent  literature,  analyzing  both  familiar  examples 
like  the  ratites,  and  illustrating  some  remarkable  new  fossils  of  flightless  forms,  such  as  an 
extinct  flightless  goose  from  Hawaii.  There  is  a good  discussion  of  how  flightlessness  has 
evolved  repeatedly  in  birds  through  neoteny.  This  is  based  particularly  on  Storrs  Olson’s 
analysis  of  the  flightless  rails  (Smithson.  Contrib.  Zool.,  No.  152,  1973),  an  outstanding 
example  of  the  use  of  fossils  to  interpret  the  epigenetic  processes  by  which  evolutionary 
change  occurs.  There  is  also  a chapter  on  birds  of  prey. 

The  final  chapter  of  Feduccia’s  book  deals  with  the  rise  of  land  birds.  This  is  a long  and 
rambling  survey  of  the  major  bird  groups  not  previously  covered.  The  fossil  record  does  not 
provide  clearcut  evidence  for  relationships  between  major  groups,  and  most  groups  are 
described  more  than  analyzed.  In  developing  a history  of  major  groups  an  excessive  reliance 
is  placed  on  the  relatively  few  important  fossils.  The  chapter  is  characterized  by  unsupported 
statements  like  “Lyrebirds  and  scrub-birds  are  clearly  the  most  primitive  of  the  oscines.” 
This  statement  has  a precise  meaning,  namely  that  all  oscines  other  than  scrub-birds  and 
lyrebirds  are  clustered  by  the  possession  of  some  derived  character(s)  for  which  the  latter 
are  primitive.  Perhaps  they  are,  but  if  so  we  should  be  told  at  this  point  what  the  characters 
are. 

The  book  ends  with  an  illustration  that  purports  to  show  relationships  between  the  Pas- 
seriformes, Coraciiformes  and  Piciformes.  Methods  now  exist,  through  cladistic  analysis,  to 
produce  precise  and  information-rich  graphic  representations  of  genealogy.  Feduccia  claims 
to  accept  the  validity  of  this  approach  (e.g.,  pp.  151-152),  and  so  it  is  difficult  to  understand 
why  he  would  present  the  virtually  meaningless  diagram  on  p.  180.  Most  of  the  groups  shown 
represent  lineages  that  do  not  connect  with  other  lineages  in  the  diagram.  Some  end  blindly 
as  solid  lines,  others  fade  into  dashed  lines  before  ending  in  open  space.  The  entire  assem- 
blage is  clustered  by  2 plesiomorphic  conditions,  the  primitive  stapes  and  anisodactyly, 
which  will  not  even  define  the  class  Aves.  The  “Alcediniformes”  and  Trogonidae  are  shown 
as  sister  groups,  clustered  by  an  unusual  stapes,  a character  discovered  by  Feduccia  and 
considered  derived  within  birds.  This  proves  that  he  knows  how  to  do  it  right.  The  suboscines 
and  oscines  are  shown  as  sister  groups,  but  only  with  dotted  lines,  whose  meaning  is  not 
mentioned,  and  no  characters  clustering  them  are  given  (though  a couple  are  mentioned  in 
the  text).  Four  other  groups  are  shown  without  connections,  except  that  they  approach  each 
other  at  various  angles  and  distances,  which  may  be  intended  to  hint  at  some  suspected 


412 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


relationship.  Some  groups  have  no  characters,  others  have  various  kinds  of  different  value. 
For  instance,  the  oscines  have  “derived  syrinx  morphology,”  which  is  good,  “advanced 
‘passerine’  morphology,”  which  is  vague,  and  “double  fossa  of  humerus  in  advanced  lines,” 
which  is  irrelevant.  In  this  example.  The  Age  of  Birds  does  not  provide  the  reader  with  a 
coherent  account  of  contemporary  methods  of  phylogenetic  analysis. 

In  recent  years,  paleontologists  have  turned  strongly  toward  the  use  of  analytical  methods 
that  place  their  fossils  in  the  context  of  biology  generally,  hence  the  increasing  use  of  the 
term  paleobiology.  A major  debate  now  rages  dealing  with  the  methods  of  interpreting  fossils 
in  systematic  studies.  Partly  this  involves  the  uses  to  which  the  stratigraphic  information 
about  fossils  can  be  put.  At  one  extreme  are  those  who  argue  that  the  age  of  known  fossils 
cannot  give  reliable  information  about  the  direction  of  evolutionary  change  because  the 
fragmentary  nature  of  the  record  makes  it  unlikely  that  correct  temporal  sequences  will  be 
preserved  and  discovered,  and  therefore  that  fossils  should  not  be  designated  as  direct 
ancestors  of  later  forms.  Instead,  it  is  suggested  that  all  forms  should  be  treated  as  terminal 
groups  in  cladograms.  At  another  extreme  is  the  traditional  paleontological  habit  of  linking 
together  many  or  all  fossils,  however  imperfect,  into  ancestor-descendent  sequences.  The 
heart  of  the  matter  is  the  need  for  rigorous  character  analysis,  recognition  of  specimen 
limitations,  and  an  amiable  consideration  of  other  viewpoints.  The  continuing  discovery  of 
new  and  often  remarkable  fossil  birds  is  intriguing,  but  in  my  opinion  it  is  the  controversy 
over  methodology  that  makes  paleornithology  the  exasperating  field  that  it  is  today. — Robert 
J.  Raikow. 


Character  Variation  and  Evolution  of  Sibling  Species  in  the  Empidonax  difficilis- 
flavescens  COMPLEX  (AvES:  Tyrannidae).  By  Ned  K.  Johnson.  Univ.  Calif.  Publ.  Zool., 
Berkeley,  California,  Vol.  112,  1980:x  + 151  pp.,  3 black-and-white  plates,  39  figs.,  15  tables, 
2 appendices.  $9.50. — Someday  in  the  distant  future  of  avian  systematics,  every  complex  of 
geographically  variable  species  and  species-groups  should  be  treated  by  a monograph  such 
as  this  one,  precisely  describing  the  nature  and  extent  of  character  variation  throughout  the 
ranges  of  its  component  taxa.  Degrees  of  geographic  concordance  between  variable  char- 
acters should  be  analyzed,  dines  and  discontinuities  identified  statistically  and  trinomials 
defended  on  precisely  stated,  objective  grounds.  Areas  of  uncertainty,  and  the  role  of  human 
judgement,  should  be  clearly  defined  whenever  they  appear.  Behavior  should  be  analyzed 
along  with  morphology. 

This  is  not  just  an  idle  pipe-dream  wafting  out  of  forgotten  museum  corridors.  Phenotypic 
variation  is  the  grist  upon  which  natural  selection  operates  to  produce  genetic  change  and 
evolution.  Careful  analyses  of  patterns  of  variation  thus  represent  one  of  the  best  tools  we 
have  for  examining  the  ongoing  processes  of  adaptation  and  speciation.  Furthermore,  birds 
are  among  the  few  animal  groups  in  which  such  analyses  are  relatively  easy  to  perform,  and 
their  rapid  responses  to  differing  selection  pressures  make  them  especially  informative  in 
this  respect. 

Although  we  still  lack  even  a single  example  of  a truly  complete  statistical  evaluation  of 
character  variation.  Johnson's  newest  contribution  to  the  biosystematics  of  the  genus  Em- 
pidoruix  is  within  striking  distance  of  the  ideal  model.  Johnson  examines  patterns  of  variation 
in  external  morphology,  color,  and  voice  within  a two-species  complex  distributed  from 
British  Columbia  to  Panama.  He  uses  a wealth  of  data,  and  modern  statistical  procedures, 
to  focus  on  critical  questions  regarding  biogeographic,  ecological  and  evolutionary  implica- 
tions of  character  variation  within  the  superspecies.  He  concludes  with  a well-defended 
taxonomic  treatment  in  which  the  Yellowish  Flycatcher  (E.  flavescens)  is  recognized  at  the 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


413 


specific  level,  and  several  confusing  forms  with  uncertain  breeding  distributions  are  syn- 
onymized  into  5 well-defined  subspecies  of  Western  Flycatcher  (E.  difficilis).  His  treatment 
was  adopted  in  the  recent  volume  8 of  Peters'  Checklist  of  Birds  of  the  World. 

The  data  base  of  this  monograph  consists  of  external  measurements  and  quantitative  color 
indices  from  1284  specimens  known  to  have  been  taken  on  breeding  grounds  and  sound 
recordings  made  from  208  individual  flycatchers  in  the  field.  For  the  first  time,  vocal  char- 
acters are  analyzed  with  nearly  the  same  rigor  as  are  classical  morphological  characters. 
(However,  levels  of  individual  and  contextual  variation  in  voice  are  not  examined  carefully, 
thereby  weakening  the  conclusions  in  some  cases.)  The  data  presentation  and  conclusions 
are  based  on  3 statistical  procedures.  Two  of  these — principal  components  and  phenogram 
analyses — are  widely  used  multivariate  approaches  for  clustering  populations  hierarchically 
according  to  degrees  of  numerical  similarity  between  them.  Strictly  a phenetic  study,  this 
report  includes  no  discussion  of  primitive-derived  sequences  or  directions  of  evolution,  out- 
side of  a few  generalized  hypothetical  scenarios  near  the  conclusion.  The  advantages  and 
drawbacks  of  this  phenetic  approach  at  the  species,  subspecies  and  population  level  of 
analysis  are  abundantly  discussed  elsewhere,  and  will  not  be  addressed  here.  Johnson's 
stated  intent  was  to  statistically  assess  the  nature  and  geography  of  variation  within  the 
entire  complex,  and  to  draw  evolutionary  inferences  from  the  observed  patterns.  For  the 
most  part,  his  assumptions  are  stated  or  clearly  implied. 

The  third  statistical  approach  still  is  relatively  little-used  in  ornithology,  although  mam- 
malogists  have  begun  using  it  extensively.  Under  the  alliterative  but  uninformative  term 
“Sum  of  Squares  Simultaneous  Test  Procedure”  (SS-STP),  this  univariate,  multiple  range 
analysis  ranks  populations  into  statistically  homogeneous  subsets  within  the  total  variation 
represented  in  a single  character,  without  regard  to  locality.  For  graphical  purposes,  Johnson 
arbitrarily  splits  the  total  variation  in  each  character  into  5 equal  parts,  then  plots  each 
population  on  a map  using  a symbol  that  displays  the  population’s  position  in  the  5-part 
ranking  with  respect  to  the  character  being  analyzed.  The  procedure  has  several  advantages 
and  disadvantages.  On  the  one  hand,  it  condenses  onto  1 figure  a tremendous  amount  of 
information  regarding  gross  similarities  between  sites.  A separate  figure  is  shown  for  each 
character  analyzed,  and  these  can  be  compared  easily  by  eye.  Regions  bearing  relatively 
little  geographic  variation,  zones  of  abrupt  character  change  and  widely  separated  areas  of 
convergence  or  parallelism  aU  emerge  clearly.  Each  figure  displays  the  statistical  significance 
of  between-site  variations  by  its  identification  of  homogeneous  subsets.  On  the  other  hand, 
the  procedure  is  not  sensitive  to  differing  degrees  of  character  variation  between  localities. 
Much  quantitative  information  is  lost  in  the  mapping  procedure  whenever  a character  shows 
pronounced  divergence  in  any  of  the  populations  (e.g.,  tail  length.  Fig.  13).  Furthermore,  in 
all  cases  the  maps  and  accompanying  graphs  of  statistical  data  require  long  and  careful 
scrutiny  before  their  full  meaning,  and  the  overall  picture,  can  be  grasped.  Sometimes, 
scrutiny  reveals  no  statistical  difference  between  populations  that  bear  different  symbols  on 
the  map.  It  is  not  clear  how  this  procedure  improves  upon  the  highly  informative  approach 
used  by  Crowe  (Ann.  South  African  Mus.  76:43-136,  1978),  Christman  (BuU.  Florida  State 
Mus.,  Biol.  Sci.  25:157-256,  1980)  and  others  for  mapping  univariate  character  variation 
using  isoclines.  Johnson  does  not  address  in  any  detail  the  relative  advantages  and  drawbacks 
of  the  SS-STP  technique. 

A variety  of  novel  and  intriguing  results  are  presented.  Perhaps  the  most  intriguing  begins 
with  abundant  evidence  that  zones  of  abrupt  character  change  are  interspersed  with  broad 
“adaptive  plateaus”  in  which  relatively  little  change  occurs  over  large  areas.  The  importance 
of  this  is  magnified  by  the  fact  that  the  areas  of  sharp  change  are  concordant  between 
characters,  including  certain  aspects  of  color  and  voice,  as  well  as  various  physical  struc- 
tures. This  pattern  probably  typifies  species  with  localized,  highly  differentiated  races  at 


414 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


upper  elevation  in  mountainous  areas  (e.g.,  the  Andes).  However,  Johnson’s  results  apply 
to  widespread,  low  altitude  forms  showing  suites  of  character  changes  across  relatively  tiny 
geographic  distances.  The  broad  picture  suggests  to  Johnson  that  step-wise  colonization  of 
new  areas  gave  rise  to  the  modern  forms.  Each  area  represents  a relatively  unvarying  adap- 
tive zone,  but  differences  are  pronounced  between  areas.  He  uses  this  as  a paradigm  of 
evolution  within  the  genus  as  a whole,  and  cites  a zone  of  apparently  recent  contact  between 
2 well  differentiated  races  in  northern  California  as  a case  in  which  divergence,  especially 
in  voice,  is  just  short  of  species-level.  I would  have  appreciated  even  more  elaboration  on 
why  this  interpretation  was  favored  over  a vicariance  model,  which  is  the  major  alternative 
to  the  one  presented. 

Among  other  informative  conclusions,  Johnson  shows  a possible  reversal  of  the  “Kluge- 
Kerfoot”  phenomenon  within  Empidonax.  Intrapopulational  variation  appears  to  be  inversely 
correlated  to  that  between  populations,  when  polytypic  E.  difficilis  was  compared  to  the 
widespread  and  monotypic  E.  hammondii.  A detailed  examination  of  this  question  would  be 
enlightening  within  such  a remarkably  homogeneous  genus. 

Johnson  maintains  that  difficilis  shows  less  sexual  dimorphism  than  does  its  allospecies, 
a potentially  exciting  result.  However,  in  the  one  character  he  chose  to  document  with  tables, 
a glaring  error  is  revealed  in  the  summary  table  (Table  2,  eighth  entry  in  right  column)  and 
the  conclusion  is  not  supported.  Together  with  the  miniscule  scale  of  those  differences  he 
does  show,  this  defect  (the  only  major  one  I found)  left  me  skeptical  of  the  overall  conclusion. 

I was  disappointed  by  Johnson’s  cursory  treatment  of  the  cause  of  divergence  in  vocal 
characters  during  aUopatry,  although  he  neatly  shows  that  the  phenomenon  frequently  oc- 
curs. Clearly  this  is  an  important  issue  in  this  and  other  tyrannid  genera.  Nevertheless, 
Johnson  skirts  the  question  in  one  sentence  by  invoking  either  pleiotropy  between  features 
of  song  and  morphology,  or  random  differences  in  the  auditory  environments  between  pop- 
ulations. In  his  conclusionary,  “founder-by-dispersal”  model  for  sibling-species  evolution, 
Johnson  allows  vocal  characters  to  differentiate,  albeit  more  slowly,  along  exactly  the  same 
monotonic,  gradual  path  toward  full  reproductive  isolation  as  do  his  morphological  charac- 
ters. However,  his  vocal  data  do  not  support  this;  degrees  of  morphological  and  vocal  dif- 
ferentiation are  not  well  correlated  with  one  another,  at  least  when  examined  carefully  by 
eye.  This  complicated  but  critical  problem  remains  unsolved,  and,  in  my  opinion,  it  remains 
the  problem  in  most  need  of  detailed  study  with  respect  to  Empidonax  evolution. 

In  sum,  this  is  far  more  than  a taxonomic  work.  Indeed,  the  well-argued  taxonomic  sum- 
mary emerges  merely  as  a logical  and  convenient  byproduct  of  a long  term  study  whose 
principal  intent  was  to  make  biological  sense  out  of  a variegated  pattern  of  physical  and 
behavioral  variation  in  a complex  taxon.  As  such,  in  ornithology  at  least,  Johnson’s  work 
represents  a new  state  of  the  art. — ^JOHN  W.  FITZPATRICK. 


Ecology  and  Evolution  of  Lek  Mating  Behavior  in  the  Long-tailed  Hermit  Hum- 
mingbird. By  F.  Gary  Stiles  and  Larry  L.  Wolf.  Ornithological  Monographs  No.  27,  Amer- 
ican Ornithologists’  Union,  1979:viii  -I-  78  pp.,  15  tables,  26  figs.  $8.50  ($7.50  to  AOU 
members). — This  monograph  presents  the  results  of  a 4-year  study  of  lek  behavior  in  Phae- 
thornis  superciliosus  in  primary  wet  forest  and  second  growth  at  Finca  La  Selva,  Costa  Rica. 
The  social  systems  of  the  hermit  hummingbirds  (Phaethorninae)  are  poorly  known,  although 
the  work  on  3 species  in  Trinidad  by  D.  Snow  and  B.  Snow  is  an  outstanding  exception.  This 
deficiency  is  understandable,  for  most  species  are  small,  drab-colored,  fast  moving,  and 
therefore  difficult  to  observe  in  the  poor  light  of  the  dense  forest  understory.  Stiles  and  Wolf 
studied  the  social  organization  of  4 leks  on  which  they  captured  and  color-marked  most 
resident  individuals.  They  provide  a detailed  description  of  visual  and  vocal  display  behavior 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


415 


(but  unfortunately  no  sonograms)  and  compare  it  with  that  of  other  hermits.  As  in  other 
studies  of  hermit  leks  few  matings  were  seen,  probably  because  the  frecjuency  is  low,  but 
possibly  because  many  occur  away  from  leks.  Homosexual  copulation  and  “false"  matings 
with  leaves  were  common,  and  an  analogy  is  drawn  between  the  latter  and  mammalian 
masturbation.  Seasonal  and  daily  patterning  of  lek  activity  is  described  in  detail  and  corre- 
lated persuasively  with  the  temporal  availability  of  nectar.  The  characteristics  of  lek  sites 
and  of  male  territories  are  also  documented. 

The  most  important  sections  of  this  paper  deal  with  foraging  ecology  and  the  dynamics  of 
male  relationships.  It  is  here  that  the  authors  make  their  two  most  significant  contributions 
to  our  understanding  of  lek  evolution.  Previous  workers  have  argued  that  the  evolution  of 
avian  lek  behavior  is  linked  to  the  abundance  and  spatio-temporal  dispersion  of  food,  but 
few  lek  studies  have  incorporated  a systematic  examination  of  the  pattern  of  resource  dis- 
tribution and  exploitation.  The  energetically  optimal  male  strategy  in  many  hummingbird 
species  is  to  defend  a food-centered  territory,  which  females  must  enter  for  feeding  and 
copulation.  Male  Long-tailed  Hermits  are  prevented  from  using  this  strategy  because  other 
hummingbird  species  aggressively  exclude  them  from  most  economically  defensible  clumps 
of  flowers,  including  the  larger  clusters  of  Heliconia  pogonantha,  which  is  adapted  for  hermit 
pollination  and  is  one  of  the  main  nectar  sources  for  P.  superciliosus.  Instead,  they  commute 
from  a non-food-centered  lek  territory  to  a series  of  regularly-patrolled,  undefendable  nectar 
sources  scattered  along  a “trapline."  Thus,  interspecific  competition  for  food  appears  to 
have  been  a key  influence  on  the  evolution  of  the  mating  system  in  this  species. 

Why  do  males  congregate  in  leks  rather  than  display  more  solitarily?  The  authors  reject 
enhanced  predator  detection  as  a significant  factor,  arguing  that  such  an  advantage  should 
be  restricted  mainly  to  open  habitats  where  several  lek  residents  can  simultaneously  observe 
approaching  predators.  But  the  advantage  could  be  even  greater  in  dense  forest  where  pred- 
ator detection  is  more  difficult,  although  presumably  a comparable  reduction  in  individual 
surveillance  levels  to  that  observed  in  more  open  habitats  would  be  impossible. 

Stiles  and  Wolf  discovered  a “dominance  gradient”  in  P.  superciliosus  leks.  The  most 
dominant  males  occupied  stable  central  territories  while  subordinate  males  held  less  stable 
peripheral  ones.  By  analogy  with  other  lekking  species  it  is  argued  that  most  mating  probably 
occurs  centrally,  and  that  the  proximate  cues  used  by  females  in  mate  selection  are  differ- 
ential male  activity  levels.  Central  males  were  more  closely  spaced  and  sang  more  than 
peripheral  ones.  The  authors  postulate  that  hermit  lek  systems  offer  females  an  index  of 
male  dominance  (a  term  they  use  rather  loosely).  But  while  such  “information"  would  clearly 
be  important  in  an  established  lek  system  where  intermale  relationships  affected  male  mating 
success,  I am  less  persuaded  that  it  could  be  an  important  selective  force  favoring  communal 
over  solitary  display  unless  it  also  indexed  performance  in  some  vital  off-lek  behavior.  If  the 
authors  are  correct  in  viewing  this  as  a major  factor  in  lek  evolution  in  the  Long-tailed 
Hermit,  ancestral  prerequisites  must  have  included  both  a potent  influence  of  stimulus  pool- 
ing on  females  and  a shallow  dominance  gradient,  as  indeed  is  currently  the  case. 

' The  few  sexually  monomorphic  lekking  species  are  vital  links  in  developing  our  under- 
i standing  of  lek  evolution,  but  they  have  been  little  studied.  This  monograph  does  not  really 

i elucidate  the  significance  of  monomorphism  in  P.  superciliosus,  but  it  does  establish  a 
convincing  link  between  surprisingly  high  adult  male  annual  mortality  levels,  the  absence  of 
sexual  bimaturism,  and  the  relative  shallowness  and  less  strictly  age-graded  nature  of  the 
I dominance  gradient  compared  with  those  of  other  lekking  birds.  Food  shortage  in  the  non- 
lekking  season  is  suggested  as  the  principal  source  of  mortality,  but  one  cannot  help  won- 
I dering  about  the  role  of  predation,  even  though  males’  survivorship  during  the  lekking  season 
when  they  are  most  conspicuous  is  better  than  that  between  seasons. 

This  monograph  is  well-written  and  contains  few  typographical  errors.  A number  of  im- 


416 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


portant  statements  and  conclusions  are  not  backed  by  any  quantitative  evidence,  and  sta- 
tistical tests  of  significance  are  used  rather  sparingly.  Nonetheless,  this  is  a very  valuable 
contribution  to  the  avian  polygamy  literature.  The  forest-dwelling  lek  hummingbirds  are 
notoriously  difficult  to  study,  and  the  authors  are  to  be  congratulated  on  the  depth  and  range 
of  their  investigation. — Alan  Lill. 


Form  and  Function  in  Birds,  Vol.  1.  By  A.  S.  King  and  J.  McLelland  (eds.).  Academic 
Press,  New  York,  New  York,  1979:xi  + 459  pp.  $64.50. — The  first  part  of  a 3-volume,  multi- 
author treatise,  this  book  contains  7 chapters  on  a diversity  of  topics.  The  intent  of  the  series 
is  to  provide  fairly  lengthy  and  detailed  accounts  of  the  functional  anatomy  of  birds.  As 
nearly  as  I can  determine  the  meaning  of  “functional  anatomy”  in  this  context,  it  is  what 
used  to  be  caUed  physiology  before  the  latter  became  indistinguishable  from  biochemistry. 
The  format  of  this  work,  then,  is  a combination  of  thorough  descriptive  anatomy  with  basi- 
cally nonchemical  interpretations  of  function.  The  latter  often  includes  material  dealing  with 
evolutionary,  comparative  and  adaptational  aspects  of  form  and  function.  Although  a dis- 
proportionate amount  of  experimental  work  is  still  limited  to  a few  species,  especially  the 
domestic  chicken,  the  book  definitely  lies  in  the  realm  of  zoology  rather  than  poultry  science. 

The  first  chapter  is  a general  review  of  principles  of  avian  morphology  by  A.  S.  King  and 
D.  Z.  King.  This  provides  an  overview  of  the  subject  as  a context  for  the  specific  topics  of 
subsequent  chapters,  but  it  is  not  a superficial  summary  of  avian  anatomy.  Instead,  the 
authors  chose  to  examine  the  way  in  which  the  most  general  specializations  of  birds,  endo- 
thermy  and  flight,  have  influenced  the  various  organ  systems.  The  chapter  also  includes  an 
excellent  review  of  recent  controversies  in  avian  evolution.  Rather  than  taking  sides.  King 
and  King  review  the  pros  and  cons  of  various  theories  on  the  reptilian  ancestors  of  birds, 
and  the  evolution  of  endothermy,  feathers  and  flight. 

The  remainder  of  the  book  is  devoted  to  chapters  with  no  particular  logical  relationship. 
The  editors  note  that  the  chapter  sequence  was  unfortunately  determined  by  the  various 
authors’  writing  schedules. 

Hans-Rainer  Duncker  provides  a review  of  the  little-studied  avian  coelomic  cavities.  An 
understanding  of  the  subdivisions  of  the  body  cavity  is  achieved  by  a study  of  their  embryonic 
development,  their  arrangement  as  compared  to  various  reptilian  and  mammalian  groups 
and  their  functional  significance,  especially  in  relation  to  the  respiratory  system.  The  diges- 
tive system  is  discussed  by  John  McLelland.  His  extensively  comparative  account  includes 
the  oral  cavity  and  pharynx,  esophagus,  stomach,  intestines,  pancreas  and  liver.  The  diges- 
tive system  is  broadly  defined  here  to  include  such  feeding  structures  as  the  tongue  and  bill 
as  well  as  the  alimentary  canal  and  its  glands.  There  is  a strong  emphasis  throughout  on 
adaptive  variations  as  related  to  different  methods  of  feeding  and  types  of  food. 

The  urinary  organs  are  reviewed  by  Oscar  W.  Johnson.  Gross  and  microscopic  anatomy 
of  the  kidney  and  associated  organs  is  analyzed  in  relation  to  function.  The  renal  blood 
supply  is  examined  in  detail,  including  the  complex  renal  portal  system  with  its  capability 
for  varying  patterns  of  blood  flow  through  the  kidney.  A.  B.  Gilbert’s  long  chapter  on  the 
female  genital  organs  includes  a detailed  account  of  the  avian  egg  as  well  as  the  reproductive 
organs  themselves.  This  account  is  especially  dependent  on  the  domestic  fowl  owing  to  the 
relative  lack  of  comparative  information.  The  same  is  true  for  the  brief  chapter  on  the  blood 
cells  by  R.  D.  Hodges.  In  the  final  chapter  on  the  autonomic  system,  A.  R.  Akester  empha- 
sizes that  the  neural  systems  regulating  visceral  and  somatic  functions  are  not  so  independent 
as  tradition  and  the  term  “autonomic”  might  suggest. 

A high  level  of  scholarship  and  thoroughness  is  evident  throughout  this  book.  AU  of  the 
chapters  are  more  than  adequately  illustrated  with  line  drawings  and  photographs.  Although 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


417 


the  chapter  topics  are  not  always  closely  related,  some  integration  is  provided  by  a compre- 
hensive index.  This  valuable  work  is  too  expensive  for  most  individual  purchasers,  hut 
institutional  libraries  should  obtain  the  series. — Robert  J.  Raikow. 


Evolution  of  the  Vertebrates,  Third  Edition.  By  Edwin  H.  Colbert.  John  Wiley  & 
Sons,  New  York,  New  York,  1980:510  pp.,  160  line  drawings.  $25.00. — First  published  in 
1955  and  revised  in  1969,  this  standard  textbook  of  vertebrate  paleontology  has  now  been 
revised  again  to  incorporate  new  fossil  discoveries,  reinterpretations  and  the  implications  of 
the  theory  of  continental  drift.  As  a relatively  nontechnical  survey  of  vertebrate  history  it  is 
an  excellent  college-level  textbook.  For  the  purposes  of  this  journal  I will  limit  further  com- 
ments to  the  book’s  coverage  of  birds.  Here  it  is  disappointing  because  the  subject  is  treated 
so  briefly,  about  7 pages  as  compared  to  over  200  for  the  mammals.  Colbert  justifies  this  on 
the  basis  that  birds  have  relatively  little  diversity,  especially  in  the  parts  that  fossilize;  there 
is  much  truth  in  this.  Nevertheless,  there  are  many  fascinating  forms  that  should  merit 
additional  discussion,  such  as  the  Cretaceous  toothed  birds,  the  great  flightless  land  birds 
of  the  early  Cenozoic,  the  pseudo-toothed  Osteodontornis  and  the  extensive  Pleistocene 
avifauna  from  the  California  tar  pits.  Likewise,  matters  of  controversy  such  as  the  reptilian 
group  ancestral  to  birds,  the  locomotor  habits  of  Archeopteryx  and  the  various  theories  on 
the  origin  of  flight  deserve  more  than  the  passing  mention  that  they  receive. — Robert  J. 
Raikow. 


The  Hawaiian  Goose:  An  Experiment  in  Conservation.  By  Janet  Kear  and  Andrew 
J.  Berger.  Buteo  Books,  Vermillion,  South  Dakota,  1980:154  pp.,  1 color  plate,  24  black-and- 
white  photographs,  numerous  drawings,  37  figs.,  2 tables,  8 appendices.  $30.00. — The  Nene, 
or  Hawaiian  Goose  {Branta  sandvicensis),  has  become  one  of  man’s  few  success  stories  in 
attempting  to  bring  back  a species  from  the  brink  of  extinction,  and  it  is  fitting  that  a 
monograph  be  done  to  document  these  efforts.  This  book  is  a historical  account  of  the  joint 
effort  of  Hawaiian  and  British  workers  to  save  the  Hawaiian  Goose,  and  is  broken  down  into 

3 major  sections:  chapters  1 and  2 give  a historical  background  of  the  Nene;  chapters  3 and 

4 document  the  captive  breeding  efforts  in  Hawaii  and  England;  and  chapters  5 and  6 deal 
with  the  release  program  in  Hawaii  and  prospects  for  the  continued  existence  of  the  species. 

The  book  starts  with  a historical  background  on  Hawaii  and  the  avifauna  beginning  with 
the  discovery  of  the  islands  in  1778  by  Captain  Cook.  I imagine  that  no  one  who  purchases 
this  book  will  be  without  Berger’s  Hawaiian  Birdlife  (University  Press,  Hawaii,  1972),  and 
much  of  this  section,  including  figures,  seems  redundant  to  that  effort.  It  is  not  until  p.  21 
that  the  Nene  is  introduced,  and  thereafter  follows  a very  complete  historical  background 
on  the  bird.  The  work  of  Baldwin  (Condor  47:27-37,  1945)  is  heavily  relied  upon  as  a base 
from  which  to  compare  population  declines.  The  authors  conclude  that  shooting  by  man  and 
the  impact  of  introduced  ground  predators  have  probably  been  the  major  contributing  factors 
affecting  the  Nene  decline. 

Chapter  2 deals  with  the  morphological,  behavioral  and  ecological  background  on  the 
Nene,  covering  voice,  size,  structural  adaptations,  habitat,  food  selection  and  reproduction 
of  the  bird  in  the  wild.  Sonograms  of  the  Nene  are  scattered  throughout  this  chapter  as  are 
good  line  drawings  of  the  trachea,  skull  and  feet  by  Tim  Halliday.  All  habitats  in  which  Nene 
have  been  found  are  described,  as  are  food  items  which  the  birds  have  been  known  to  eat. 
The  reader  is  sometimes  confused  in  this  section  because  of  the  authors’  ambiguity  in  dealing 
with  the  question  of  food  limitation.  For  example,  on  p.  43  they  say:  “At  present,  food  re- 
sources at  the  higher  altitudes  where  Nene  are  found,  are  not  considered  to  be  the  factor 


418 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


limiting  their  numbers,”  yet  on  p.  52  they  write:  . . and  a shortage  of  high-protein  and 

high-calcium  food  might  make  a second  clutch  unusual  in  the  wild,”  and  on  p.  102  . . the 

introduced  wild  turkeys  {Meleagris  gallopavo)  might  be  significant  competitors  for  the  ber- 
ries.” They  also  state  that  the  “stones”  (seeds)  of  the  pukiawe  are  passed  through  the  bird 
undigested,  and  this  may  be,  but  I know  of  no  study  to  date  which  has  shown  this  to  be  true. 
The  small  amount  of  data  presented  by  the  authors  on  reproduction  in  the  wild  points  up  the 
fact  that  very  little  is  still  known  about  this  bird  outside  of  information  available  from  captive 
breeding.  The  behavioral  aspects  of  reproduction  are  well  covered  and  are  supplemented  by 
black-and-white  plates  depicting  various  threat  displays. 

The  next  2 chapters  are  the  meat  of  the  book  and  deal  with  the  captive  rearing  programs 
at  Slimbridge,  England  and  Pohakuloa,  Hawaii.  Chapter  5 is  devoted  to  the  Hawaii  program 
and  follows  it  from  the  inception  in  1927  through  the  present.  It  is  evident  throughout  this 
chapter  that  little  data  were  made  available  to  the  authors  other  than  summary  information, 
because  the  overwhelming  amount  of  new  information  contained  within  the  book  is  from  the 
Slimbridge  project.  The  comparison  of  the  2 breeding  programs  shows  some  striking  differ- 
ences (e.g.,  egg  weights)  but  is  hampered  by  the  small  sample  size  or  lack  of  data  from  the 
Hawaii  project.  Hopefully,  some  day  all  of  the  information  from  the  Pohakuloa  project  will 
be  published  so  that  a complete  comparison  of  the  2 programs  can  be  made. 

The  captive  rearing  program  at  Slimbridge  is  very  well  covered  in  chapter  6.  More  often 
than  not  avicultural  efforts  such  as  this  are  left  unpublished,  and  this  synthesis  of  the  Nene 
program  at  Slimbridge  is  the  major  ornithological  contribution  of  the  book.  The  topics  of 
husbandry,  pairing,  breeding  season,  fertility,  clutch  and  egg  size,  hatching  and  growth, 
mortality,  and  a comparison  with  the  Hawaii  program  are  made.  There  is  a wealth  of  infor- 
mation presented  in  this  chapter,  but  instead  of  being  synthesized  into  a format  useable  to 
scientists,  the  data  are  presented  in  raw  form  (often  percentages),  and  principally  in  appen- 
dices. Many  superficial  comparisons  of  the  data  are  made,  but  the  reader  is  often  left  won- 
dering what  statistical  analysis  was  employed.  This  is  compounded  by  the  fact  that  the 
variances  associated  with  much  of  the  data  are  usually  quite  large.  In  some  figures,  lines  of 
best  fit  are  “eye-balled”  through  the  data  (e.g..  Figs.  35  and  36),  and  major  “trends”  are 
discussed,  which  in  one  instance  is  the  result  of  an  aberrant  point  with  a sample  size  of  one 
(Fig.  35).  However,  the  raw  data  are  available  in  the  book,  and  interested  persons  could 
pursue  it  if  they  so  desired. 

The  sections  on  fertility  and  mortality  of  the  Slimbridge  flock  are  extremely  well  done, 
with  the  latter  presenting  detailed  information  on  maladies,  diseases  and  parasites  of  the 
Nene  heretofore  not  recorded.  Atherosclerosis  was  a very  common  cause  of  death  in  older 
birds,  and  other  maladies  included  aspergillosis,  avian  tuberculosis  and  parasitic  helminths. 
Although  a number  of  the  Nene  in  England  had  lesions  indicative  of  avian  pox,  the  virus  was 
never  isolated  or  positively  identified.  The  authors  initially  qualified  their  diagnosis  of  this 
disease,  but  later  in  the  book  lapsed  into  assuming  that  avian  pox  was  the  pathogen  (see 
.Appendix  7 and  Index). 

Chapter  5 synthesizes  much  of  what  has  been  covered  previously  into  the  context  of  the 
release  program  in  Hawaii.  Separate  sections  deal  with  releases  on  state,  private  and  National 
Park  lands  on  Hawaii,  and  at  Haleakala  National  Park  on  Maui  to  where  the  Slimbridge 
birds  were  returned.  Reasons  for  the  failures  and  successes  of  the  releases  are  well  docu- 
mented. However,  the  problem  of  Nene  band  loss  is  mentioned  only  in  the  caption  of  Plate 
5 and  should  have  been  dealt  with  at  greater  length.  For  a variety  of  reasons,  band  loss  has 
been  extensive  in  the  wild  Nene  populations  and  has  become  a major  problem  in  determining 
the  success  of  the  release  programs. 

The  concluding  chapter  addresses  the  question  of  whether  the  over  1500  released  birds 
have  increased  the  breeding  potential  of  the  wild  Hawaiian  Goose  and  if  the  native  population 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


419 


will  now  be  able  to  sustain  itself  indefinitely.  The  authors  are  well  aware  of  the  problems  in 
attempting  to  answer  these  questions  and  write:  “We  still  do  not  know,  other  than  in  general 
terms,  what  brought  the  species  so  low,  and  so  cannot  be  sure  that  the  hazards  have  been 
removed  or  are  being  effectively  controlled.”  They  continue:  “Because  of  this  inadequacy 
of  information,  it  is  impossible  to  be  definite  about  the  outcome  of  the  reintroduction  phase 
of  the  programme.”  However,  their  attempt  to  synthesize  this  problem  into  tentative,  yet 
meaningful  conclusions  is  admirable.  They  warn  about  the  long-term  effects  of  captivity  on 
the  genetic  make-up,  pathological  conditions  and  the  effects  on  behavior  of  the  propagated 
birds.  They  further  caution  about  the  suitability  of  the  habitat  which  will  receive  the  rein- 
troductions (e.g.,  predator  levels,  newly  developed  adverse  factors),  gradual  settlement  tech- 
niques and  adequate  foUow-up  surveys.  They  continually  stress  throughout  the  book  the 
need  for  an  intensive  field  study  of  the  bird,  and  it  can  only  be  hoped  that  Nene  managers 
in  Hawaii  will  heed  their  suggestions.  There  is  no  question  that  the  captive  breeding  program 
has  saved  the  Nene  from  extinction,  but  when  the  authors  expand  their  discussion  to  en- 
compass other  endangered  species  they  recommend  that  only  as  a last  resort  should  animals 
be  taken  into  captivity,  bred  and  released. 

Overall,  the  book  is  well  written  and  is  easy  to  read.  Outright  errors  are  few,  but  some 
Hawaiian  plant  names  are  misspelled:  “pukeawe”  should  be  pukiawe;  “mamani”  should  be 
mamane;  “Mauna  Silver  Sword”  should  be  Mauna  Kea  Silver  Sword;  the  correct  common 
name  for  Melamprosops  phaeosoma  is  Po’o  Uli  not  the  “Black-faced  Honeycreeper.”  There 
are  8 appendices  of  which  numbers  1 and  8 add  little  to  the  book.  The  143  references  in  the 
bibliography  are  as  complete  a list  on  the  Nene  as  can  be  found.  The  4-page  index  is 
adequate. 

In  summary,  the  authors  should  be  commended  for  their  job  of  tying  together  what  is 
presently  known  about  the  Hawaiian  Goose.  They  have  not  only  provided  a scholarly  account 
of  the  bird,  but  have  also  successfully  bridged  the  gap  between  aviculture  and  ornithology. 
Although  the  price  of  the  book  is  quite  high,  even  by  today’s  inflated  standards,  those 
aviculturists  who  raise  Nene,  conservationists  concerned  with  endangered  species  and  or- 
nithologists interested  in  the  Hawaiian  avifauna  will  find  the  book  a good  reference  for  their 
libraries. — CHARLES  VAN  Riper  III. 

Bird  Community  Dynamics  in  a Ponderosa  Pine  Forest.  By  Robert  C.  Szaro  and 
Russell  P.  Baida.  Cooper  Ornithological  Society,  Studies  in  Avian  Biology,  No.  3,  1979:66 
pp.,  39  figs.,  21  tables.  $6.50. — This  monograph  reports  on  the  breeding-bird  populations  of 
5 ponderosa  pine  plots  in  northern  Arizona  that  were  subjected  to  a variety  of  modifications: 
clear-cutting,  uniform  thinning,  strip-cutting,  silvicultural  cutting  and  an  undisturbed  con- 
trol. Some  of  these  manipulations  were  apparently  feasible  as  a result  of  studies  by  others 
on  the  role  of  timber  management  in  modifying  stream  flow  and  wood  production.  This 
study’s  purpose  was  “to  measure  and  evaluate  (1)  the  effects  on  the  diversity,  density,  and 
behavior  patterns  of  the  breeding  birds  of  the  ponderosa  pine  forest  of  such  results  of  habitat 
manipulation  as  differing  foliage  volumes,  foliage  patterns,  and  densities  of  trees,  and  (2)  the 
standing  crop  biomass,  consuming  biomass,  and  existence  energy  requirements  of  the  breed- 
ing birds  on  each  plot.”  I believe  that  the  study  accomplishes  the  task  of  measuring  these 
variables  well,  but  that  its  success  in  evaluating  these  results  is  less  notable.  To  some  degree 
the  copious  production  of  data  may  have  compounded  the  problems  of  interpretation;  at  any 
rate,  an  appropriate  synthesis  is  never  really  accomplished.  One  is  exposed  to  large  amounts 
of  only  moderately  digested  data,  and  is  presented  with  one  example  after  another  in  the 
text,  rather  than  a concise  synthesis  of  emerging  principles.  The  data  reduction  in  many  of 
the  39  figures  and  21  tables  is  often  minimal,  although  a wide  array  of  statistical  tools  exists 


420 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  Vo.  3,  September  1981 


for  attacking  such  problems.  As  a consequence,  in  its  present  form  this  paper  may  be  of 
interest  primarily  to  investigators  who  work  on  the  birds  of  ponderosa  pine  forests.  On  the 
other  hand,  the  raw  data  are  published  for  others  to  use. 

Much  of  the  analysis  attempted  deals  with  such  topics  as  bird  species  diversity,  evenness, 
foliage  height  diversity,  plant  species  diversity,  etc.  The  problems  with  many  of  the  standard 
techniques  of  analyzing  these  topics  are  now  well-known,  and  have  generally  been  respon- 
sible for  their  decrease  in  popularity.  In  fact,  the  study  found  no  correlation  between  bird 
species  diversity  and  measures  of  vegetational  complexity,  and  the  authors  themselves  note 
that  the  technique  was  not  appropriate  to  find  systematic  differences  in  a study  such  as 
theirs. 

Enough  of  what  this  report  is  not.  There  are  several  interesting  points  that  emerge  from 
the  study.  One  is  the  regional  differences,  from  ponderosa  pine  forests  in  other  areas — 
population  densities  are  particularly  low  here,  even  in  the  undisturbed  study  area.  Between- 
year  differences  were  also  quite  marked  in  some  plots,  but  not  others  (replicates  would  have 
been  welcomed  here,  even  though  they  probably  would  be  infeasible  in  a study  conducted 
on  this  scale).  Specific  attention  to  regional  and  yearly  differences  and  their  likely  bases 
would  have  focused  this  paper  profitably  and  would  have  spoken  to  subjects  now  being 
recognized  as  important  and  interesting  ones  in  their  own  right.  In  this  paper  one  has  to 
glean  through  several  sections  to  pick  up  what  commentary  is  presented  on  the  subject. 

The  differences  in  bird  populations  between  ponderosa  pine  forests  and  eastern  coniferous 
forests  are  stressed,  which  the  authors  relate  to  the  low  representation  of  parulid  warblers 
in  the  western  coniferous  forests  (7-20%  of  the  avifauna  vs  50%  or  more  in  some  eastern 
forests).  The  basis  for  this  disparity  is  unclear,  although  the  authors  emphasize  Mengel’s 
thesis  of  geographical  accident.  However,  it  should  be  noted  that  the  appropriate  compari- 
sons here  are  to  eastern  hard-pine  forests  (e.g.,  pitch,  jack,  loblolly,  or  longleaf  pine),  rather 
than  the  eastern  spruce-fir  forests  that  are  renowned  for  their  high  and  varied  warbler  den- 
sities. 

Some  interesting  apparent  replacements  (pairs  of  species)  were  noted,  with  examples  of 
apparent  ecological  release  from  plot  to  plot  (e.g.,  Yellow-rumped  [Derulroica  coronata]  vs 
Grace's  [D.  graciae]  warblers).  However,  although  these  examples  are  treated  as  consistent 
with  competition  theory,  no  further  attempt  is  made  to  establish  whether  or  not  a causal 
relationship  exists. 

Thus,  the  authors  have  collected  a sizeable  data  set  on  the  breeding  birds  of  ponderosa 
pine  forests,  but  they  have  missed  a number  of  opportunities  in  their  analysis  of  it.  One 
hopes  that  they  have  planned  further  papers  to  exploit  this  resource. — DoUGLASS  H.  MoRSE. 


Conservation  Biology:  ,\n  Evolutionary-Ecological  Perspective.  By  Michael  E. 
Soule  and  Bruce  A.  Wilcox  (eds.).  Sinauer  Associates,  Inc.,  Sunderland,  Massachusetts, 
1980:395  pp.  $14.95. — This  collection  of  reports  is  unique  as  a conservation  book  for  several 
reasons,  the  most  surprising  of  which  is  that  most  of  the  authors  are  “pure"  scientists,  and 
not  applied  biologists.  The  text  is  divided  into  4 major  sections:  Ecological  Principles  of 
(Conservation  (4  chapters);  the  Consequences  of  Insularization  (5  chapters);  Captive  Propa- 
gation and  Conservation  (5  chapters);  and  Exploitation  and  Preservation  (4  chapters).  In  Part 
1,  Larry  Gilbert  presents  a holistic  approach  to  conservation  biology,  noting  in  particular  the 
complex  construction  and  interdependence  of  tropical  food  webs.  This  chapter  will  orient 
the  student  or  non-ecologist  to  the  great  complexity  of  species  diversity  patterns  in  the  tropics 
and  their  dependence  on  ecological  succession.  Gilbert  notes  the  importance  of  constructing 
preserves  so  that  key  species  (those  having  an  important  effect  in  food  web  cohesion,  etc.) 
are  included  in  an  area  that  is  diverse  enough  to  contain  aU  plants  and  animals  important  to 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


421 


their  continued  existence.  John  Eisenberg  compares  mammal  biomass  in  New  and  Old  World 
tropical  areas.  He  suggests  that  large,  hard-to-observe  species  (e.g.,  leopards,  jaguars)  should 
be  studied  to  gauge  the  “health”  of  the  environment  to  be  preserved,  because  they  are  often 
sensitive  to  environmental  degradation.  Jared  Diamond  reviews  the  conservation  aspects  of 
island  biogeography  theory  as  applied  to  patchy  habitats.  He  notes  that  patchy  distributions 
pose  difficult  problems  for  preservationists  intent  on  saving  several  species  distributed  in 
this  manner,  because  one  is  often  confronted  with  the  choice  of  either  making  a single  large 
preserve,  which  may  contain  only  1 or  2 species,  or  many  small  preserves,  which  may  contain 
a large  number  of  patchily-distributed  species,  but  may  not  contain  large  populations  of 
associated  species.  Throughout  this  volume  it  is  suggested  that  large  reserves  are  preferable 
to  small  ones.  Robin  Foster  continues  the  discussion  of  heterogeneous  environments,  noting 
their  importance  to  the  maintenance  of  species  diversity. 

In  Part  2,  Bruce  Wilcox  reviews  the  well-known  ideas  of  species  equilibria  and  their 
importance  to  conservation  strategy.  John  Terborgh  and  Blair  Winter  present  a short  essay- 
on  species  extinction,  and  conclude  that  species  should  be  kept  from  becoming  rare  if  they 
are  to  persist,  which  is  fairly  self-evident.  Ian  Franklin  presents  an  interesting  review  of  the 
evolutionary  changes  that  may  take  place  in  a small  population.  He  suggests  that  populations 
must  be  kept  above  a minimal  number  of  breeding  individuals  (anywhere  from  50-500)  if 
deleterious  genetic  effects  are  to  be  avoided.  Michael  Soule  continues  the  discussion  of  the 
genetic  aspects  of  conservation  and  emphasizes  the  importance  of  maintaining  high  levels 
of  heterogeneity  if  fitness  is  to  remain  high.  Soule  uses  the  wolf  as  an  illustration  of  how 
genetic  considerations  might  require  a wolf  preserve  to  contain  anywhere  from  12,000- 
120,000  km^  of  habitat!  Daniel  Goodman  presents  a fine  discussion  on  the  application  of 
demographic  theory  to  conservation  problems,  illustrating  the  importance  of  understanding 
the  demography  of  a species  before  attempting  long-term  game  management  practices. 

Part  3 contains  papers  dealing  with  the  propagation  of  captive  animals.  The  various  chap- 
ters by  William  Conway,  John  Senner,  Kurt  Benirschke  et  al.,  and  Devra  Kleiman  point  out 
the  complex  problems  that  must  be  solved  in  a successful  propagation  program.  The  captive 
species  must  be  extremely  well-studied,  biologically  speaking,  if  one  wishes  to  propagate  it 
so  that  ecological  and  genetic  parameters  are  part  of  the  propagation  program.  It  is  also  an 
expensive  endeavor.  Sheldon  Campbell  provides  a short  chapter  on  problems  of  reintroducing 
animals  to  their  original  habitats.  I would  have  preferred  a much  more  detailed  discussion 
of  this  topic  because  earlier  chapters  make  it  clear  that  zoos  will  not  be  a salvation  for 
extinction-prone  species. 

In  the  final  section  (Part  4),  Malcolm  Coe  reviews  the  status  and  history  of  wildlife  con- 
servation in  Africa,  while  T.  C.  Whitmore  discusses  tropical  rainforest  conservation.  R.  M. 
Pyle  presents  a brief  essay  on  nature  preserve  management.  The  final  chapter  by  P.  R. 
Ehrlich  wiU  be  familiar  to  anyone  who  has  kept  up  with  the  popular  writings  of  this  author. 
I feel  that  Ehrlich’s  analysis,  which  may  appear  pessimistic  to  some,  is  actually  optimistic. 
He  concludes  that  conservation  is  not  a lost  cause  for  2 reasons:  we  can  delay  environmental 
destruction  and  then  enjoy  the  diversity  of  nature  that  remains  for  the  time  being;  and,  we 
can  possibly  reach  a point  where  it  is  understood,  on  a world-wide  basis,  that  continued 
economic  and  population  growth  are  antithetical  to  conservation.  Few  could  argue  with  the 
first  point,  but  realistically,  who  can  expect  the  second? 

This  volume,  like  any  multiauthored  work,  suffers  from  a lack  of  cohesion,  from  repetition 
and  from  a lack  of  continuity  of  style.  It  is  not  a textbook  of  conservation  biology,  but  is 
more  involved  with  the  ethos  of  conservation,  and  should  be  read  by  everyone  interested  in 
the  problem  of  environmental  degradation.  I was  impressed  by  a single,  overriding  thread 
connecting  each  chapter.  We  need  to  know  a lot  more  about  the  biology  of  species,  com- 
munities and  even  ecosystems  if  we  are  to  manage  them  effectively.  In  many  cases,  theory 


422 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  \o.  3,  September  1981 


has  outstripped  our  empirical  understanding  of  nature.  Yet  few  people  are  engaged  in  aut- 
ecological  studies,  and  funding  for  such  research  is  slight.  Applied  biolog>  is  still  looked 
down  upon  by  the  scientific  establishment,  yet  each  author  implied  a desperate  need  for  just 
the  kind  of  data  that  are  no  longer  considered  worthwhile  or  elegant. 

Ehrlich  likened  the  conservation  strategy  of  ecologists  to  passengers  on  a plane.  If  we 
were  going  to  fly  in  an  airplane  and  the  pilot  began  tossing  out  a few  bolts  here,  a flap  handle 
there,  we  would  probably  be  apprehensive  about  the  flight.  He  suggests  that  in  nature,  as 
in  an  airplane,  we  must  save  all  the  parts,  for  they  form  an  integral  whole.  1 wiU  close  this 
review  by  continuing  the  analogy.  If  you  find  yourself  in  a plane  with  an  unconscious  pilot, 
what  should  you  do?  You  had  better  learn  how  to  fly,  and  you  had  better  learn  fast.  At  the 
moment,  conservationists  are  not  sufficiently  united,  nor  are  most  people  sufficiently  con- 
cerned, to  devote  any  significant  effort  to  conservation  biology.  The  authors  of  this  volume 
have  tried,  at  least  to  the  extent  of  writing  a chapter,  but  how  many  of  us  are  really  willing 
to  dedicate  a significant  effort  to  conservation  biology,  particularly  when  we  are  rewarded 
for  more  esoteric  research?  If  we’re  going  to  learn  how  to  fly  that  pilotless  airplane,  we  had 
best  get  to  practicing  while  we  stiU  have  some  altitude.  Because  when  that’s  gone,  it  will  be 
too  late. — Michael  A.  Mares. 


Allan  Brooks:  Artist  Naturalist.  By  Hamilton  M.  Laing.  Special  Publication  No.  3, 
British  Columbia  Provincial  Museum,  Victoria,  British  Columbia,  1979:x  -I-  249  pp.,  frontis- 
piece (portrait),  8 color  plates,  117  unnumbered  text  figs.  $10.00  (paper),  $16.00  (cloth). — 
Allan  Brooks  (1869-1946)  was  for  more  than  50  years  a resident  of  southern  British  Columbia 
(Chilliwack,  Sumas,  Okanagan  Landing,  Comox).  He  early  sustained  himself  by  professional 
collecting  as  well  as  by  trapping,  market  bunting  and  fishing.  He  assembled  an  important 
collection  of  more  than  9000  well-made,  copiously  labelled  bird  skins  now  in  the  Museum  of 
Vertebrate  Zoology,  University  of  California,  Berkeley. 

The  bare  facts  of  his  life  have  been  recorded  by  his  wife  M.  Brooks  (Condor  40:12-17, 
1938)  and  H.  M.  Laing  (Auk  64:430^144,  1947).  See  also  appreciations  by  W.  L.  Dawson 
(Condor  15:69-76,  1913)  and  H.  Harris  (Condor  48:145-153,  194b). 

Born  in  India  to  Northumberland  parents  (his  father  was  a well-known  amateur  naturalist), 
and  self-educated.  Brooks  (Fellow,  AOU)  was  a modest  but  respectable  contributor  to  the 
faunal  and  systematic  literature.  He  is  best  remembered,  however,  as  the  illustrator  of  some 
20  ornithological  works.  With  Louis  Agassiz  Fuertes  (1874-1927),  he  was  one  of  the  2 prin- 
cipal American  workers  in  this  area  for  the  first  third  of  the  twentieth  century. 

Although  his  painting  had  devoted  admirers  among  ornithologists,  as  art  I have  long  found 
it  seriously  wanting  (even  allowing  for  his  self-training,  for  the  devastating  requirements  of 
this  form  of  illustration,  and  for  the  generally  weak  landscape  of  bird  painters  to  the  time). 
His  vegetation  is  formularized,  his  water  tritely  faked,  his  palette  limited  and  endlessly 
repetitive,  running  too  much  to  baby  blues  and  sentimental  pinks.  Worse,  his  birds  seem 
conventionalized,  sometimes  almost  as  though  traced  around  generic  templates.  There  are 
too  many  surprised-looking  hawks  and  sway-backed  passerines.  There  is  no  strength  of  line 
and  no  subtlety;  many  flying  birds  are  grotesquely  misshapen.  I am  afraid  that  these  re- 
sponses once  led  me  to  suppose  that  the  author  of  such  pictures  must  be  of  weak  character, 
a conclusion  made  impossible  by  other  evidence. 

Brooks  was  an  inveterate,  if  taciturn,  diarist  and  a tireless  sketcher  and  it  is  here  that  the 
present,  well-illustrated  little  book  makes  its  principal  contributions.  Extensive  selections 
from  the  diaries  are  effectively  interwoven  with  letters  and  the  reminiscences  of  veteran 
naturalist  Laing  and  other  friends.  The  picture  emerges  of  an  extraordinarily  hardy,  self- 
sufficient.  hard-headed  near-loner  compulsively  and  narrowly  addicted  to  wild  creatures,  the 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


423 


outdoors  and  the  practical  and  sporting  uses  of  firearms  for  game  both  large  and  small.  His 
marriage  at  the  age  of  57  understandably  startled  his  friends  but  seems  to  have  been  suc- 
cessful. 

Equally  revealing,  and  more  surprising,  are  the  many  previously  unpublished  sketches 
and  informal  studies  which,  if  not  master  drawing,  clearly  suffer  from  none  of  the  deficiencies 
noted  above.  Compare,  for  example,  the  graceful,  well-appreciated,  truly  falconine  Peregrine 
on  p.  137  with  the  pop-eyed,  squab-like  travesty  portrayed  on  p.  110  of  J.  B.  May’s  Hawks 
of  North  America  (Natl.  Assoc,  of  Audubon  Societies,  1935).  Many  of  these  sketches  are 
charming,  economical,  forceful  and  alive  with  an  authenticity  that  cannot  be  faked.  1 note 
especially  the  Spotted  Owls  (pp.  78,  95),  magpie  (p.  97),  Bobolink  (p.  195),  Canada  Goose 
(p.  225),  Ruffed  Grouse  (p.  232)  and  Oldsquaws  (p.  236). 

How  does  one  resolve  the  enigma  of  the  uncommonly  great  gulf  between  sketch  and 
finished  illustration  found  here?  As  a working  hypothesis,  I think  that  this  untrained  and 
late-blooming  artist  (he  did  not  achieve  wide  recognition  until  he  was  past  50),  a man  given 
to  quick  and  firm  convictions  yet  perhaps  traumatized  by  the  requirements  of  “finish,”  fell 
too  easily  into  any  stereotypy  that  worked.  Once  he  learned  “how”  to  do  what  he  needed  to, 
he  saw  no  need  for  change.  He  never  thought  seriously  of  himself  as  an  artist,  listing  his 
occupation  as  “illustrator.” 

As  to  his  strength  of  character,  if  more  evidence  is  required.  Major  Allan  Brooks  (DSO), 
at  the  age  of  49,  voluntarily  far  in  advance  of  the  British  lines  in  February,  1918,  in  2 days 
of  furious  action  personally  killed  at  least  20  enemy  soldiers  with  his  own  rifle.  His  personal 
toU  of  the  enemy,  1914-1918,  was  evidently  far  greater.  He  seems  almost  never  to  have 
spoken  of  this  period  later. 

This  book  is  slow  reading  at  times  but  it  is  a significant  document  in  the  history  of  orni- 
thology and  bird  painting,  as  well  as  of  British  Columbia.  It  will  appeal  also  to  those  mature 
citizens  who  remember  with  nostalgia  the  Reed  bird  guides  and  the  unrestricted  collecting 
permit. 

The  book  needs  a map.  Its  many  typographical  errors  may  be  forgiven  an  author  who  was 
96  at  press  time.  The  editors,  if  any,  are  less  immune  to  censure  but  overall,  the  harm  was 
slight. — Robert  M.  Mengel. 


Hawk  Lady.  By  Stellanie  Ure.  Doubleday  and  Co.,  Inc.,  Garden  City,  New  York,  1980:216 
pp.,  14  black-and-white  plates  with  captions.  $11.95. — Hawk  Lady  presents  anecdotal  ac- 
counts of  the  attempts  of  Mrs.  Ure  to  rehabilitate  raptors.  She  is  not  a professional  orni- 
thologist, but  her  diary-like  vignettes  are  “down-right”  honest  and  serve  best  by  warning  of 
what  not  to  do  when  caring  for  raptors.  Overcrowding  birds,  housing  small  with  large  species, 
using  chicken  wire  on  cage  walls,  and  risking  injury  from  mishandling  are  clearly  examples 
of  what  not  to  do. 

The  book  is  aimed  at  a young  audience,  11-13  year  olds,  with  much  biographical  material 
about  the  Ure’s  personal  lives,  particularly  the  children’s.  This  approach  induces  the  author 
to  personify  nature,  a common  tendency  in  writing  for  children.  Personification  may  allow 
insight,  provided  those  selected  attributes  of  nature  are  accurate  and  repeatable.  If  not, 
natural  things  and  events  are  distorted.  Mrs.  Ure’s  naming  of  2 Great  Horned  Owls  {Bubo 
virginianus)  after  a rather  base  comedian  team  of  Cheech  and  Chong,  and  symbolizing  our 
national  bird  after  Shakespeare’s  Romeo  is  misleading  in  educating  the  young  about  birds 
of  prey. 

Although  the  Common  Flicker  {Colaptes  cafer)  was  incorrectly  referred  to  as  the  Red- 
shafted  Flicker,  I was  pleased  to  find  the  correct  common  name,  American  Kestrel  {Falco 
sparverius),  being  used  in  most  instances.  However,  the  means  for  determining  aging  of 


424 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


kestrels  by  the  amount  of  spotting  on  the  breast  is  unconfirmed.  Another  minor  error  is  found 
in  Mrs.  Ure’s  descriptions  of  a Goshawk  {Accipiter  gentilis)  “stooping”  to  its  prey.  The  term 
stoop  best  refers  to  a specialized  method  of  attacking  prey  by  various  species  of  falcons. 
This  is  also  true  of  the  term  tercel,  which  designates  male  individuals  of  the  Falconidae,  not 
males  of  the  Accipitridae  as  the  text  indicates  in  its  reference  to  “tercel  Cooper’s  Hawks” 
(A.  cooperii).  On  the  last  page,  under  the  listing  of  raptors,  the  scientific  name  of  the  Pigeon 
Hawk  {F.  columbarius)  was  misspelled. 

While  I cannot  recommend  the  purchase  of  the  book,  I applaud  the  Ure  family,  especially 
their  children,  for  being  concerned  with  injured,  diseased,  or  stranded  birds  of  prey.  I think, 
however,  that  the  book  reveals  that  we  raptor  biologists  should  provide  better  guidelines 
than  presently  available  for  the  successful  rehabilitation  of  raptors. — Thomas  G.  Balgoo- 
YEN. 


Strictly  for  the  Chickens.  By  Frances  Hamerstrom.  Iowa  State  University  Press, 
Ames,  Iowa,  1980:136  pp.,  58  black-and-white  photographs,  28  illustrations.  $11.95  (hard 
cover). — Since  the  mid-1930’s,  Fred  and  Frances  Hamerstrom  have  spent  the  greater  part 
of  their  lives  in  pursuit  of  information  about  the  Greater  Prairie  Chickens  (Tympanuchus 
cupido)  of  Wisconsin.  The  efforts  of  the  Hamerstroms  to  understand  the  lives  and  needs  of 
the  prairie  chickens  have  been  largely  responsible  for  saving  this  species  from  almost  certain 
extinction  in  that  region.  By  abandoning  a life  of  comparative  luxury  and  ease,  and  as 
biologists  daring  to  live  in  more  remote  areas  and  in  unconventional  ways,  the  Hamerstroms 
have  perhaps  attracted  more  than  their  share  of  memorable  experiences. 

Strictly  for  the  Chickens  is  a collection  of  stories  about  the  lives  of  the  Hamerstroms.  Yet 
this  book  could  scarcely  be  called  a biography  in  the  traditional  sense.  For  we  are  not  given 
a detailed  series  of  interrelated  chapters  neatly  following  in  chronological  order.  But  rather, 
a series  of  “short  stories”  about  events  and  experiences,  perhaps  roughly  in  chronological 
order,  but  which  leave  the  reader  to  hll  in  by  imagination  much  of  what  might  have  happened 
throughout  their  lives.  Each  chapter  presents  a light-hearted  behind  the  scenes  look  at  many 
of  the  problems,  people  and  events  through  50  years  of  wildlife  studies  in  central  Wisconsin. 

The  topics  range  from  the  struggle  to  survive  on  graduate  stipends  while  living  in  formerly 
abandoned  farmhouses,  to  coping  with  irate  and  incredulous  neighbours  who  misinterpreted 
and  exaggerated  or  refused  to  accept  the  well  intentioned  activities  of  wildlife  biologists. 
Frozen  water  pumps  and  smoking  wood  stoves,  the  problems  of  trapping  and  marking  prairie 
chickens,  graduate  student  life,  visits  by  federal  inspectors,  having  and  raising  children,  a 
visit  to  Germany,  raptor  banding  ‘on  the  sly,'  visiting  scientists  and  the  varied  antics  of  some 
7000  observers  who  came  to  ‘help’  with  the  work  have  all  provided  moments  of  tension  and 
frustration.  Yet  through  the  understanding  of  Frances  Hamerstrom  these  and  other  events 
have  been  brought  to  life  with  warmth  and  humor.  In  the  often  too  serious  world  of  science, 
it  is  indeed  a pleasure  to  be  reminded  of  the  equally  important  lighter  moments,  which  help 
to  put  life  back  into  proper  perspective. 

While  perhaps  somewhat  frustrating  for  lack  of  more  information  about  the  author’s  life, 
the  stories  are  a charming  insight  into  the  activities  of  2 very^  interesting  people.  The  book 
is  easy  to  read  and  the  style  compels  one  to  continue  to  the  end  of  each  chapter  and  indeed 
to  start  into  another.  The  text  is  liberally  sprinkled  with  a great  deal  of  prairie  chicken  biolog>' 
and  the  appropriate  illustrations  found  throughout  further  enhance  the  appeal  of  the  book. 

I would  highly  recommend  this  entertaining  work  to  anyone  with  a desire  to  mix  humor  and 
biology. — Ross  D.  JAMES. 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


425 


The  Imperative  Call:  A Naturalist’s  Quest  in  Temperate  and  Tropical  America. 
By  Alexander  F.  Skutch.  University  Presses  of  Florida,  Gainesville,  Florida,  1979:331  pp., 
photographs,  endpaper  maps,  index.  $20.00. — When  an  individual  achieves  prominence  in 
an  area  of  scientific  research,  others  often  speculate  as  to  how  that  person  came  to  he  in  so 
enviable  a circumstance.  Alexander  Skutch  occupies  such  a place  in  the  field  of  tropical 
biology;  he  also  possesses  the  literary  skills  to  satisfy  our  curiosity  with  an  interesting  story 
of  his  early,  career-forming  experiences.  He  shares  his  recollections  of  travels  and  study  in 
a vivid,  personal  style  that  should  appeal  to  the  seasoned  traveler  and  the  tropical  neophite 
equaUy. 

Chronologically,  The  Imperative  CaU  predates  his  other  biographical  works,  A Naturalist 
in  Costa  Rica  and  A Bird  Watcher’s  Adventures  in  Tropical  America,  and  describes  the 
beginnings  of  Skutch’s  fascination  with  nature.  From  sketchy  recountings  of  his  boyhood 
spent  in  rural  and  suburban  Maryland,  Skutch  moves  on  to  describe  his  formal  university 
training  in  botany.  He  ably  recounts  his  first,  awesome  encounter  with  the  neotropical  forest, 
an  experience  that  many  temperate-zone  trained  biologists  will  empathize  with.  He  relates 
how  a chance  observation  of  a nesting  Rufous-tailed  Hummingbird  {Amazilia  tzacatl)  drew 
this  serious  student  of  botany  into  his  dedicated  study  of  the  behavior  of  birds.  Throughout 
the  book,  Skutch  recounts  his  observations  of  birds,  places  and  people.  Mingled  with  these 
incidents  are  insights  into  his  developing  philosophical  point  of  view. 

The  casual  narrative  style,  moving  around  one  incident,  forward  and  back  in  time,  some- 
times becomes  deeply  involved  in  details  of  botanical  or  ornithological  interest.  Yet,  for  the 
general  reader,  the  details  should  prove  no  deterrent  to  pleasure,  and  for  those  who,  like 
Skutch,  experience  the  “imperative  call”  of  nature,  these  are  gems. — George  V.  N.  Pow- 
ELL. 


A Field  Guide  to  The  Birds,  Fourth  Edition.  By  Roger  Tory  Peterson.  Houghton 
Mifflin  Co.,  Boston,  Massachusetts,  1980:384  pp.,  136  color  plates,  range  maps.  $15.00 
(cloth),  $9.95  (paper). — Reviewing  a Peterson  Field  Guide  for  eastern  North  America  comes 
almost  in  the  same  category  as  reviewing  The  Bible,  for  to  myriads  of  bird  watchers  this 
book,  in  its  various  editions,  has  become  a veritable  “holy  writ”  and  the  author  at  least  a 
“major  prophet.”  The  new  edition  arrived  in  the  fall  of  1980  preceded  by  one  of  the  most 
intensive  selling  campaigns  for  any  recent  book.  Indeed,  its  publication  was  the  event-of- 
the-year  in  bird  books.  By  now  there  can  scarcely  be  anyone  interested  in  birds  who  has  not 
at  least  examined  a copy,  and  the  sales  have  been  so  large  that  what  is  said  in  any  review 
cannot  influence  them. 

Now  that  the  furor  has  subsided,  perhaps  we  can  look  objectively  at  this  edition.  A new 
edition  was  long  overdue  since  the  previous  one  appeared  in  1947.  In  the  intervening  33 
years  there  have  been  a number  of  changes  in  the  world  of  popular  bird  study.  For  one  thing, 
bird  watching  has  become,  to  quote  K.  C.  Parkes,  “an  organized  cult,”  and  the  numbers  of 
people  engaged  in  the  pastime  is  far  greater  than  it  was  in  1947.  I would  hazard  a guess, 
however,  that  today  a smaller  fraction  of  this  total  are  interested  in  birds  beyond  the  listing 
stage  than  was  the  case  in  the  1940’s.  The  art  of  bird  identification  has  improved  markedly, 
so  that  today  in  the  hands  of  some  people  it  is  a much  more  precise  skill  than  it  was  years 
ago.  To  cite  only  3 examples:  the  increased  number  of  bird-banders,  the  recent  popularity 
of  pelagic  trips  and  the  intensive  studies  of  the  autumn  raptor  migration  have  raised  birders’ 
identification  skill  of  the  fall  warblers,  the  seabirds  and  the  raptors  to  levels  not  prevalent 
in  the  1940’s.  Unfortunately,  it  would  appear  that  Peterson  has  not  kept  up  with  these  trends, 
and  in  most  ways  his  identification  guides  are  more  or  less  the  same  as  they  were  in  the 


426 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


I930's  and  I940’s  when  he  first  revolutionized  this  art  form.  For  this  reason,  I feel  that  the 
book  wiU  be  most  successful  in  introducing  the  tyro  to  the  birds,  and  will  be  less  successful 
to  the  more  advanced  birder.  Perhaps  that  is  the  main  purpose  of  the  book,  and  if  so  it 
should  meet  this  objective  handsomely. 

The  dust  jacket  proclaims  that  this  edition  is  “Completely  New”  and  indeed  it  is,  except 
for  the  silhouettes  on  the  inside  covers  which  appear  to  be  the  same  as  those  on  the  last 
edition.  A major  change  is  that  all  species  are  now  figured  in  color,  and  the  illustrations,  as 
in  other  recent  Peterson  guides,  are  now  more  nearly  portraits  rather  than  the  diagrammatic 
drawings  of  past  editions.  A few  pictures  of  flight  patterns  of  waterfowl  and  shorebirds  are 
still  in  monocolor.  The  number  of  species  on  a given  plate  has  been  greatly  reduced,  making 
the  pictures  less  crowded  and  somewhat  larger  than  formerly.  The  textual  material  for  each 
species  is  on  the  page  opposite  the  portrait  of  that  species,  thus  eliminating  a minor  source 
of  irritation.  But  as  a result  of  this,  the  amount  of  space  devoted  to  each  species  in  the  text 
is  somewhat  reduced,  and  the  convenience  of  having  text  and  figure  together  is  bought  at 
the  expense  of  less  full  descriptions  and  less  helpful  information  on  identification.  Peterson 
has  obviously  catered  to  the  “birder”  rather  than  to  the  general  ornithological  community. 
The  species  are  no  longer  listed  in  A.O.L.  Checklist  order,  although  this  may  have  been 
deliberate  since  the  forthcoming  1983  Checklist  may  give  us  quite  a different  order.  The 
English  names  used  are  those  of  the  recent  A.B.A.  Checklist,  and  some  may  be  unfamiliar 
to  those  not  acquainted  with  that  list. 

A welcome  new  feature  is  the  inclusion  of  390  range  maps,  which  are  grouped  at  the  back 
of  the  book.  These  maps,  which  were  researched  by  Mrs.  Peterson,  are  large  enough  to 
show  detail,  and  are  far  superior  to  the  small  maps  in  the  principal  competitor  of  this  guide. 
Unfortunately,  bird  ranges  are  transitory  things,  subject  to  constant  change,  and  in  any 
compilation  of  range  maps  it  is  easy  to  find  errors.  The  Petersons  have  shown  considerable 
bravery  in  publishing  these  detailed  maps.  I counted  31  maps  that  had  major  errors  in  the 
range  boundaries  in  the  region  with  which  I am  most  familiar — the  southern  Appalachians — 
and  no  doubt  others  in  areas  at  range  boundaries  can  find  as  many.  This  is  partly  the  result 
of  the  shifting  nature  of  the  ranges,  and  partly  because  we  locals  have  failed  to  publish  some 
of  the  known  changes.  However,  if  one  recognizes  these  deficiencies  the  maps  should  be 
quite  useful. 

The  illustrations  deserve  the  most  comment.  In  the  review  copy  many  of  the  plates  are  far 
too  dark,  especially  the  sandpipers  and  hawks;  The  Red-tailed  Hawk  {Buteo  jamaicensis)  is 
as  dark  as  any  melanistic  western  race.  A few  plates  are  washed  out  and  far  too  light,  i.e., 
the  jays  on  p.  209  and  the  cardueline  finches  on  p.  271.  I have  examined  several  other  copies 
and  have  found  less  variation  in  most,  and  truer  colors  in  some.  Perhaps  this  variation  in 
reproduction  is  inevitable  in  a mass-produced  book,  but  at  the  price  charged  it  would  appear 
that  Houghton  Mifflin  Company  should  have  been  more  interested  in  quality  control. 

However,  the  faults  with  the  plates  are  not  solely  those  of  reproduction,  since  Peterson 
has  not  been  all  that  careful  in  some  of  his  depictions.  I noted  small  inaccuracies  in  such 
things  as  soft-part  colors,  bill  shapes,  bodily  proportions  and  other  fine  details.  Such  details 
were  not  evident  on  the  diagrammatic  sketches  of  earlier  editions  but  become  noticeable  in 
the  portraits  of  this  edition.  It  is  true  however,  that  most  of  these  inaccuracies  wiU  not 
interfere  with  the  identification  of  birds  at  a distance,  but  we  should  not  take  the  pictures 
to  be  definitive  portraits. 

A serious  fault  is  the  lack  of  a consistent  scale  on  the  individual  plates.  On  the  plate  of 
spotted  thrushes  the  large  \^ood  Thrush  {Hylocichla  mustelina)  is  shown  approximately  the 
same  size  as  the  diminutive  Hermit  Thrush  (C.  guttata),  and  on  another  plate  the  chickadees 
are  about  the  same  size  as  the  titmice.  Body  proportions  are  often  out-of-true  as  when  the 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


427 


streamlined  Scissor-tailed  Flycatcher  (Muscivora  forficata)  is  shown  to  he  about  as  chunky 
as  a kingbird. 

Perhaps  the  most  regretable  portion  of  the  book  is  the  continued  adherence  to  the  myth 
that  fall  warblers  are  confusing.  Many  people  now  know  how  to  identify  the  fall  warblers, 
not  only  as  to  species,  but  as  to  sex  and  age  class,  with  ease,  although  in  some  cases  this 
does  remain  difficult.  But  Peterson  has  not  availed  himself  of  this  expertise,  which  could 
have  been  forthcoming  from  many  people.  The  2 plates  of  faU  warblers  are  not  really  very 
helpful,  since  the  colors  are  rather  muddy  and  dull  in  my  copy.  However,  in  this  edition 
appears  for  the  first  time  the  admission  that  some  (I  would  say  many)  fall  BlackpoU  Warblers 
may  have  dark  tarsi,  contrary  to  what  earlier  editions  and  other  guides  may  say.  It  is  implied 
that  most  immature  males  resemble  females  in  the  fall,  and  while  this  is  true  for  some 
species  for  others  it  is  not.  Thus,  contrary  to  the  implication  of  Peterson,  the  male  Black- 
throated  Blue  {Dendroica  caerulescens)  and  Cape  May  {D.  tigrina)  warblers  look  alike  in 
both  age  classes.  This  error  is  not  going  to  cause  any  misidentifications  but  it  is  going  to 
mislead  people,  and,  in  particular,  the  neophyte  bander  may  put  a bird  in  the  wrong  age 
class  if  he  relies  solely  on  this  book. 

The  fact  that  many  species  show  considerable  variation  in  plumage  is  not  really  recognized 
in  this  book.  It  is  regretable  that  the  author,  who  apparently  had  carte  blanche  from  the 
publisher,  did  not  take  the  opportunity  to  illustrate  more  of  this  variation.  For  example,  the 
2 oversized  kingfishers  (p.  187)  could  have  been  reduced  to  make  room  for  a picture  of  the 
immature  bird  which  has  a single  breast  band  which  is  both  brown  and  blue.  But  in  this 
connection  one  can  only  wonder  what  the  extra  unlabelled  head  of  a male  Hairy  Woodpecker 
(Picoides  villosus)  is  supposed  to  represent. 

The  harm  in  these  admittedly  minor  inaccuracies  and  omissions  comes  precisely  because 
so  many  people  do  consider  a Peterson  Field  Guide  to  be  the  “Holy  Writ.”  Some  years  ago 
Peterson  wrote  about  the  bird  watcher  who  sees  the  bird  through  his  binoculars,  not  as  it 
really  is,  but  as  the  Fuertes  painting  looks.  Today,  that  tyro  sees  the  Peterson  painting,  and 
in  too  many  cases  he  is  going  to  be  misled  and  confused. 

In  summary,  the  book  will  meet  its  unstated  purpose  of  teaching  the  tyro  to  identify  the 
spring  birds,  particularly  the  males — always  assuming  that  he  doesn’t  have  one  of  the  copies 
with  distorted  colors.  At  other  seasons  and  with  some  species  the  novice  will  encounter 
problems.  Novice  banders  should  be  aware  of  some  of  the  deficiencies  and  should  not  foDow 
the  age  and  sex  criteria  given  therein  blindly.  I must  admit  to  a great  sense  of  disappointment 
in  this  book.  I feel  that  the  author  passed  up  an  opportunity  to  give  us  more  useful  information 
than  he  did.  In  many  ways,  the  1947  edition  was  superior  to  this  one. — George  A.  Hall. 


Birds  of  the  West  Coast,  Vol.  H.  By  J.  F.  Lansdowne.  Houghton  Mifflin  Co.,  Boston, 
Massachusetts,  1980:167  pp.,  48  color  portraits,  47  pages  of  pencil  sketches.  140.00. — Being 
a painter  of  birds,  I have  never  missed  an  opportunity  to  view,  or  scrutinize,  almost  any 
depiction  of  a bird.  About  15  years  ago,  on  one  of  my  first  visits  to  the  home  of  a friend,  I 
was  captivated  by  paintings  displayed  in  his  dining  room.  The  birds,  a Turkey  Vulture 
(Cathartes  aura),  a Brown  Pelican  (Pelecanus  occidentalis),  a Herring  Gull  (Larus  argentatus) 
and  a Sandhill  Crane  {Grus  canadensis),  were  exceedingly  well  drawn  and  were  painted  in 
the  manner  in  which  I saw  birds — with  only  a hint  of  feather  detail.  Upon  closer  inspection 
I found  the  works  of  art  not  to  be  by  L.  A.  Fuertes,  but  by  an  “unknown”  bird  painter  by 
the  name  of  J.  F.  Lansdowne.  I was  tremendously  impressed  by  these  4 works  and  spent  a 
great  deal  of  time  looking  at  them.  In  the  ensuing  years  I was,  of  course,  to  see  a great  deal 
of  work  by  Lansdowne,  but  I became  disappointed  in  his  tremendous  attention  to  detail — 


428 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


detail  that  could  be  seen  if  a bird  were  sitting  on  the  artist’s  table,  but  detail  that  was  not 
evident  in  wild,  living  birds.  I don’t  know  if  Lansdowne  received  criticism  from  his  friends 
and  colleagues  concerning  his  overly-done  attention  to  detail,  but  he  has  slowly  backed  away 
from  it  and  his  work  is  again  “living”  and  beautiful.  The  work  in  the  second  volume  of  his 
Birds  of  the  West  Coast  includes  some  of  his  finest.  The  first  2 paintings,  Red-necked 
{Podiceps  grisegena)  and  Eared  (P.  caspicus)  Grebes,  are  no  less  than  incredible;  they  are 
also  rather  loosely  done  and  have  a lovely  softness  to  them.  The  Great  Blue  Heron  {Ardea 
herodius)  on  p.  25  is  one  of  the  finest  examples  of  Lansdowne’s  amazing  ability  to  convey 
different  textures,  be  they  contour  feathers,  plumes,  large  secondaries,  scutes  on  a leg  or 
foot,  or  hard,  dead  wood,  but  the  picture  seems  cramped  as  a vertical  and  would  have  been 
better  as  a horizontal.  My  favorite  works  are  the  ones  featuring  three  Ruddy  Ducks  (Oxyura 
jamaicensis)  on  p.  45,  a soft,  beautifully  designed  picture  in  which  the  birds  are  superbly- 
executed,  and  a rather  “un-Lansdowne”  painting  of  a Parasitic  Jaeger  (Stercorarius  para- 
siticus) robbing  a Common  Tern  (Sterna  hirundo).  The  latter  picture  shows  action  of  a sort 
rarely  seen  in  Lansdowne’s  work — not  only  is  the  action  weU-conveyed,  but  the  execution 
of  the  picture  is  outstanding. 

Lansdowne  seems  to  me  to  occasionally  have  some  problems  with  proportions.  The  head 
on  the  Oldsquaw  (Clangula  hyemalis)  on  p.  40  is  too  small,  as  is  that  of  the  Golden  Eagle 
(Aquila  chrysaetos)  on  p.  51  (and  featured  on  the  dust  jacket).  In  general,  his  passerine  birds 
are  not  as  well  done  as  the  nonpasserines.  The  bills  and  heads  on  the  Golden-crowned 
Kinglets  (Regains  satrapa)  on  p.  41  are  too  large,  and  the  feet  on  the  Winter  Wren  (Trog- 
lodytes troglodytes)  on  p.  91  appear  to  belong  to  a dried  museum  specimen  rather  than  to  a 
living  bird.  On  the  other  hand,  the  Brown  Towhee  (Pipilo  fuscus)  on  p.  107  is  “alive”  and 
exquisite,  and  the  bushtits  with  nest  on  p.  87  are  equally  well-done  and  “alive.” 

The  book  is  obviously  made  to  be  looked  at;  it  is  a collection  of  paintings.  It  does,  however, 
have  a page  of  text  that  accompanies  each  picture.  The  text  is  a combination  of  anecdotes 
and  general  information  about  the  species  under  discussion,  and  is,  as  it  should  be,  merely 
an  interesting  and  informative  adjunct  to  the  paintings. 

The  inclusion  of  the  drawings  upon  which  the  paintings  were  based  is  good.  They  are,  to 
me,  sometimes  “better”  than  the  final  painting.  I think  the  looseness  with  which  they  are 
rendered  makes  them  very  pleasing.  I am  not  a rapid  sketcher  and  thus,  to  me,  it  is  wonderful 
how  Lansdowne  can  often  capture  the  very  being  of  a bird  with  only  a few  simple  lines. 

It  is  impossible  to  mention  each  of  the  paintings,  but  all  are  beautiful  and  pleasant  to  look 
at.  Anyone  interested  in  birds  would  want  to  have  this  latest  collection  of  Lansdowne’s  work. 
Despite  any  “nit-picking”  criticism  that  I have  levied,  the  works  are  outstanding  and  beau- 
tiful, and  reproduction  is  good.  At  $40.00  the  price  seems  right. — John  P.  O’Neill. 


The  Avifauna  of  the  South  Farallon  Islands,  California.  By  David  F.  DeSante  and 
David  G.  Ainley.  Studies  in  Avian  Biology  No.  4,  Cooper  Ornithological  Society,  1980:vi  -t- 
104  pp.,  13  tables,  2 figs.,  frontispiece.  $10.00. — Imagine  that  you  direct  an  ornithological 
field  station.  Imagine  that  you  have  access  to  an  island  with  abundant  seabirds  and  visited 
by  migratory  birds  surprising  both  in  numbers  and  variety.  How  would  you  exploit  these 
opp(»rtunities?  The  Directors  of  Point  Reyes  Bird  Observatory  (PRBO)  had  to  answer  these 
(}uestions  when  they  were  granted  access  to  South  Farallon  Island  by  the  U.S.  Coast  Guard 
and  the  U.S.  Fish  and  ildlife  Service  in  1967.  This  publication  summarizes  that  part  of 
PRBO's  first  8 years  of  fieldwork  there,  which  was  devoted  to  the  obvious  task  of  describing 
the  avifauna.  Results  of  prior  investigations,  some  previously  unpublished,  are  included 
along  with  a synopsis  of  PRBO's  findings  during  the  subsequent  42  months.  Half  of  this 
publication  is  a heavily  annotated  checklist.  A total  of  346  species  is  reported  from  a flo- 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITEKATUKL 


429 


ristically  impoverished  island  less  than  0.5  km^  in  area,  and  the  list  already  has  been  ex- 
tended! Only  15  of  these  species  bred  there  while  this  study  was  conducted;  5 others  have 
bred  in  earlier  years. 

The  South  Farallon  Islands  are  now  established  as  a site  where  the  unexpected  avian 
visitor  is  to  be  expected.  Any  treatment  of  such  an  outlandish  avifauna  demands  great  care 
in  screening  and  documenting  records.  DeSante  and  Ainley  clearly  recognized  this.  Many 
new  records  are  substantiated  by  specimens  or  photographs.  However,  details  for  a few 
species  “new”  to  California  are  scanty.  The  mere  mention  of  a specimen,  given  the  chance 
for  misidentification  in  even  the  best  curated  collections,  is  insufficient.  In  a very  few  in- 
stances, PRBO’s  staff  released  birds  whose  value  as  a specimen  perhaps  exceeded  any 
potential  value  as  a banding  return.  However,  I recognize  that  this  is  a touchy  subject  for 
a project  that  depends  on  the  contributions  of  amateurs.  The  only  real  alternative  to  collecting 
such  birds  is  the  “rarities  committee.”  Many,  but  not  all,  records  of  extreme  rarities  have 
been  reviewed  by  the  California  Bird  Records  Committee  (see  Western  Birds  10:169—186, 
1979  11980  for  the  most  recent  reportl).  Hopefully,  all  such  records  will  be  so  reviewed  in 
the  future.  I was  disturbed  to  see  a still  tentative  identification  included  (p.  104)  in  a pub- 
lication of  this  caliber,  because  such  reports  have  an  unfortunate  tendency  to  become  fact 
even  if  they  are  later  rejected. 

An  avifauna  with  about  17  migrant  species  for  every  (potential)  breeder  is  quite  dynamic 
in  composition.  The  list  of  species  gives  cumulative  totals  and  peak  numbers  for  each  species 
broken  down  by  season,  but  conveys  no  feeling  for  the  day  to  day  variations  in  the  numbers 
of  any,  excepting  those  so  rare  that  aU  reports  are  enumerated.  The  seabirds  are  treated  in 
more  detail  in  other  publications  by  the  staff  of  PRBO.  Other  than  by  its  amazing  variety, 
the  landbird  fauna  remains  obscure  to  me  even  after  reading  this  monograph.  How  does  the 
avifauna  as  a whole  change  seasonally  and  from  year  to  year?  What  weather  conditions 
ground  migrants  and  what  conditions  allow  them  to  leave?  How  do  these  birds  fare  in  such 
an  extreme  environment?  The  reader  will  find  no  answers  to  such  questions.  It  is  only  clear 
that  once  landbirds  leave  South  Farallon  Island,  they  effectively  vanish.  A variety  of  methods 
that  convey  these  data  concisely  exist.  I look  forward  to  seeing  them  in  future  PRBO  pub- 
lications. 

The  second  half  of  this  monograph  is  mostly  a traditional  biogeographic  analysis  of  the 
avifauna.  I think  that  what  this  section  addresses  is:  How  does  one  explain  the  commonness 
or  rarity  of  species  in  this  avifauna?  I also  think  it  mostly  misses  the  mark.  If  these  patterns 
are  just  the  result  of  random  phenomena,  then  the  question  is  essentially  trivial  and  PRBO’s 
efforts  are  better  directed  elsewhere.  I share  with  authors  the  belief  that  these  patterns  are 
more  significant.  Unfortunately,  the  hiogeographic  analysis  presented  here  only  rearranges 
the  basic  data  without  providing  additional  insight  as  to  what  factors  produce  them.  The 
near  independence  of  species  abundance  in  spring  versus  fall  is  striking,  yet  this  observation 
is  not  further  pursued.  It  is  precisely  this  kind  of  observation,  and  the  biogeographic  affinities 
of  the  avifauna,  that  our  hypothesis  should  explain.  More  than  ever  I think  that  explanations 
based  on  the  degree  of  navigational  error  required  to  reach  South  Farallon  Island  ultimately 
will  prove  fruitful,  and  that  other  ecological  data,  such  as  overall  species’  abundance,  only 
condition  this  basic  hypothesis. 

One  might  expect  an  avifauna  with  so  few  breeding  landbirds  to  be  poor  material  for  an 
“island  biogeographic”  analysis.  The  authors’  efforts  are  most  effective  and  strongly  sub- 
stantiate some  serious  criticisms  of  prior  attempts  at  this  endeavor  in  other  island  systems. 
They  especially  emphasize  the  need  for  regular  censusing  and  care  in  determining  the  re- 
productive status  of  birds  observed. 

As  typical  of  Cooper  Ornithological  Society  publications,  the  monograph  is  technically 
well  produced.  I would  have  appreciated  a map  of  the  island,  although  sources  for  that  map 


430 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


are  cited.  Misspellings  are  scarce.  If  I seem  overly  critical,  let  that  be  taken  as  a compliment 
to  PRBO.  The  high  quality  of  their  publications,  including  this,  gives  one  high  expectations. 
If  you  have  a deep  interest  in  California’s  avifauna  or  in  island  biogeography,  this  publication 
is  worth  its  price.  Others  should  insist  that  their  local  library  acquire  a copy. — Paul  A. 
DeBenedictis. 

Handbook  of  the  Birds  of  Europe,  the  Middle  East,  and  North  Africa.  The 
Birds  of  the  Western  Palearctic.  Vol.  I,  Ostrich  to  Ducks.  Vol.  2,  Hawks  to  Bustards. 
Stanley  Cramp,  Chief  Ed.,  K.  E.  L.  Simmons,  Assoc.  Ed.  Authors  for  Vol.  1:  I.  J.  Ferguson- 
Lees,  Robert  GiUmor,  P.  A.  D.  HoUom,  Robert  Hudson,  E.  M.  Nicholson,  M.  A.  Ogilvie,  P. 
J.  S.  Olney,  K.  H.  Voous  and  Jan  Wattel.  Authors  for  Vol.  2:  Robert  Gillmor,  P.  A.  D. 
HoUom,  Robert  Hudson,  E.  M.  Nicholson,  M.  A.  Ogilvie,  P.  J.  S.  Olney,  C.  S.  Roselaar,  K. 
H.  Voous,  D.  I.  M.  WaUace  and  Jan  Wattel.  Artists  for  Vol.  I:  Paul  Barruel,  C.  J.  F.  Coombs, 
N.  W.  Cusa,  Robert  GiUmor,  Peter  Hayman  and  Sir  Peter  Scott.  Artists  for  Vol.  2:  C.  J.  F. 
Coombs,  Peter  Hayman  and  Ian  WiUis.  Oxford  University  Press,  Oxford,  England.  Vol.  I, 
1977:722  pp.,  108  color  plates.  $85.00.  Vol.  2,  1980:695  pp.,  96  color  plates,  $85.00.  Nu- 
merous range  maps,  black-and-white  drawings,  sonagrams  and  diagrams  in  each  volume. — 
This  work  is  fittingly  dedicated  to  the  memory  of  H.  F.  Witherby,  editor  of  the  Handbook 
of  British  Birds  (1938-1941).  This  indicates  the  magnitude  of  the  debt  that  Birds  of  the 
Western  Palearctic  (BWP)  owes  to  its  iUustrious  predecessor.  It  also  indicates  that  BWP  is 
in  part  intended  as  an  updated  version  of  “Witherby.”  But  while  this  would  have  been  a 
worthwhile  project  in  itself,  the  authors  decided  on  the  more  ambitious  course  of  treating  aU 
the  birds  of  the  Western  Palearctic.  I applaud  this  decision,  because  while  the  birds  of 
Britain  and  Ireland  are  already  very  weU-known,  those  of  other  countries,  especiaUy  in  the 
eastern  part  of  the  region,  are  not.  To  have  drawn  together  so  much  information  from  so 
many  different  countries  and  produced  a synthesised  account  in  a single  work  is  one  of  the 
major  contributions  of  BWP.  The  language  problems  must  have  been  horrendous.  How  many 
of  us  can  read  papers  in  Polish  or  Bulgarian? 

The  Western  Palearctic,  as  defined  here  (and  shown  on  an  exceUent  map  on  the  inside 
covers)  includes  the  Atlantic  islands  down  to  the  Cape  Verdes,  Northern  Africa  down  to  the 
central  Sahara,  the  Middle  East,  including  northern  Saudi  Arabia  and  Iraq  but  not  Iran,  and 
all  of  European  Russia  (i.e.,  east  to  the  Urals).  This  expanded  geographical  scope  has  almost 
doubled  the  number  of  species  covered  (743+  vs  424  in  Witherby).  The  743  figure  includes 
601  breeding  species,  11  “regular  non-breeding  migrants”  and  131  accidentals  that  have 
occurred  in  the  area  since  1900.  In  addition,  an  unstated  number  of  accidentals  recorded 
before  1900,  plus  some  doubtful  records,  are  briefly  treated  in  the  text. 

For  sequence  and  scientific  nomenclature  the  authors  have  followed  Voous,  List  of  Recent 
Holarctic  Bird  Species  (Ibis  115:612-^38,  1973;  119:223-250,  376-406,  1977).  This  is  sensi- 
ble, since  Voous’  list  has  won  general  acceptance.  With  regard  to  English  vernacular  names, 
however,  they  have  evaded  their  responsibilities.  It  was  the  manifest  duty  of  this  new  “bible” 
on  European  birds  to  bring  needed  change  to  these  names,  but  as  in  all  previous  British 
checklists  and  field  guides  the  authors  have  buried  their  heads  in  the  sand,  hoping  that  The 
Swallow  would  fly  away.  There  is  not  a single  sentence  in  the  36-page  introduction  dealing 
with  the  subject,  and  as  we  thumb  through  the  text  we  meet  once  again  our  old,  unmodified 
friends  (The)  Cormorant,  (The)  Bittern,  (The)  Wigeon,  (The)  Teal,  (The)  Eider,  (The)  Buzzard, 
(The)  Partridge,  (The)  Quail  and  (The)  Coot.  We  even  find  my  old  favorite,  the  Andalusian 
Hemipode,  bless  its  little  pink  heart,  though  at  least  in  this  case  someone  has  shamed  the 
authors  into  providing  an  alternative  name  in  parentheses  (Little  Button-Quail). 

Before  the  species  accounts  there  are  summaries  of  each  order  and  family.  The  orders  are 
briefly  treated,  but  the  family  summaries  can  run  to  2 pages.  They  are  nicely  done  and  form 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


431 


a miniature  reference  work  by  themselves.  The  species  accounts  are  long  and  detailed.  In 
Vol.  1,  158  species  are  covered  in  662  pages,  an  average  of  about  4 pages  per  species. 
Accidentals  receive  cursory  treatment  while  well-known  birds  have  much  lengthier  ac- 
counts— the  Grey  Heron  rates  10  pages.  These  are  large  pages  (10"  X 8")  of  fairly  close-set 
type,  and  no  space  is  wasted  on  broad  margins. 

The  accounts  are  broken  down  into  sections.  Field  Characters  is  fairly  lengthy,  about  half 
a page,  rather  a misnomer  if  you  are  expecting  a few  Peterson-type  italicised  field  marks. 
All  plumages  are  described  briefly,  plus  size,  shape,  notes  on  similar  species,  and  habits 
and  behavior  if  helpful  for  identification.  For  the  ordinary  birder  this  section  is  of  inestimable 
value.  The  habitat  section  contains  a complete  list  of  habitats  in  different  parts  of  the  bird’s 
range,  also  taking  into  account  altitude,  season,  migration  and  other  activities.  As  an  indi- 
cation of  the  thoroughness  of  this  work,  5 pages  of  the  introduction  are  devoted  to  a glossary 
defining  the  terms  used  in  this  section.  Distribution  for  all  breeding  species  and  regular 
migrants  is  shown  by  2 maps,  a small  scale  one  of  the  world  range  and  a large  scale  one  of 
the  range  in  the  Western  Palearctic.  The  maps  are  large  (large  scale  ones  up  to  three-fourths 
of  a page),  and  in  contrasting  colors,  red  for  breeding  range  and  gray  for  winter  range.  They 
are  mostly  easy  to  read,  but  a narrow  strip  of  gray  along  a coastline  can  be  difficult  to  see. 
In  central  Europe  the  boundaries  of  many  small  countries  come  close  together,  and  it  can 
be  hard  to  teU  if  a bird  occurs  in,  say,  Switzerland  or  not.  Regrettably,  there  is  no  text  to 
turn  to  in  these  cases.  The  distribution  section  is  short  and  only  intended  to  supplement  the 
maps  with  information  on  isolated  occurrences  and  recent  range  extensions.  The  onus  of 
providing  distributional  information  thus  lies  entirely  on  the  maps.  While  a full  range  de- 
scription for  each  bird  would  have  added  to  the  bulk  of  the  book,  I have  often  been  frustrated 
by  “borderline”  cases.  Here  is  one  field  where  Witherby  still  reigns  supreme — its  tremendous 
coverage  of  localities. 

The  section  on  population  takes  the  place  of  what  is  usually  called  “status,”  though 
sometimes  local  status  is  indicated  under  “distribution.”  There  is  tremendous  country-by- 
country detail  of  birds  easily  censused,  like  the  Grey  Heron  {Ardea  cinerea),  but  this  section 
can  be  very  short  for  birds  not  well-known,  such  as  Velvet  Scoter  {Melanitta  fusca).  There 
seems  to  be  a studious  avoidance  of  such  ill-defined  terms  as  “common,”  “abundant,”  or 
“scarce”  with  reference  to  status  in  general,  which  is  never  clearly  defined.  Perhaps  these 
terms  are  thought  too  “unprofessional”  for  a book  like  this,  but  it  does  mean  one  must  do 
a lot  of  reading  and  draw  inferences  from  population  counts  and  distribution  maps  to  deter- 
mine if  a given  bird  is  commonly  met  with  or  not.  A couple  of  lines  giving  a generalised 
status  of  each  species  would  save  a lot  of  eye-work.  There  are  plenty  of  trees;  what  we  want 
is  a quick  look  at  the  forest.  The  section  on  movements  includes  not  only  regular  migration 
but  other  things  like  dispersal,  nomadism,  irruptions  and  altitudinal  migration.  Under  foods 
an  exhaustive  list  of  food  items  are  provided  and  methods  of  feeding  are  also  described. 

Social  pattern  and  behaviour  covers  such  subjects  as  flocking,  pair  bonds,  territoriality 
and  roosting  and  deals  with  courtship  displays,  antagonistic  behaviour,  and  other  interac- 
tions. Displays  are  frequently  illustrated  by  excellent  drawings.  Descriptions  of  displays  and 
other  activities  are  largely  factual.  The  more  difficult  subject  of  interpretation  has  been 
avoided.  A lengthy  catalog  of  sounds,  vocal  and  non-vocal,  including  calls  of  the  young, 
liberally  illustrated  by  sonagrams  make  up  the  voice  section.  An  attempt  has  also  been  made 
to  indicate  the  significance  of  vocal  signals,  in  part  by  categorising  them  as,  e.g.,  advertising 
calls,  threat  calls,  alarm  calls,  alighting  calls,  greeting  calls,  etc.  Not  content  with  a simple 
catalog,  the  authors  state  that  “every  utterance  has  to  be  evaluated  in  relation  to  its  role  in 
behaviour,  communication,  and  location.  . . .”  The  lengthy  (12-page)  section  on  voice  in  the 
introduction  is  largely  a dissertation  on  the  problems  of  describing  and  interpreting  avian 
vocalizations.  Under  breeding  subjects  treated  include  season,  nest,  eggs,  clutch-size. 


432  THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


number  of  broods,  incubation  period,  development  of  young,  fledging  period  and  breeding 
success.  Finally,  we  come  to  a complete  description  of  the  bird,  broken  down  into  plumages, 
bare  (not  soft)  parts,  moults,  measurements,  weights  and  ‘structure,’  which  includes  shape, 
proportions,  wing  formulae,  numbers  of  wing  and  tail  feathers,  etc.  At  the  very  end  is  a 
section  on  geographical  variation.  This  is  brief  and  to  the  point,  not  a lengthy  discussion. 

I have  indicated  in  some  detail  what  can  be  found  between  the  covers  of  these  volumes 
because  that  is  their  greatest  asset — massive  amounts  of  detailed  information.  One  might 
retitle  the  work  “Everything  you’ve  always  wanted  to  know  about  European  birds — but  were 
afraid  to  ask.”  The  same  expansive  treatment  is  given  to  the  illustrations,  which  are  present 
in  abundance.  The  743  principal  species  are  all  illustrated  by  color  paintings  said  to  show 
“every  plumage  which  is  identifiable  in  the  field.”  I am  sure  this  claim  is  for  all  practical 
purposes  correct.  The  tremendous  wealth  of  illustrations  is  one  of  the  great  features  of  this 
work.  There  are  between  4 and  10  illustrations  of  most  species,  portraying  breeding,  non- 
breeding, immature,  and  juvenile  plumages  of  both  sexes,  and  downy  young.  Flight  pictures 
are  provided  for  most  species;  birds  of  prey  are  shown  in  flight  from  above  as  well  as  from 
below  (a  feature  lacking  in  most  field  guides),  and  polymorphic  species  like  Buzzards  {Buteo 
buteo)  and  Honey  Buzzards  (Pernis  apivorus)  are  given  lavish  treatment.  Given  the  tremen- 
dous contribution  made  by  the  illustrations,  I was  surprised  that  more  recognition  was  not 
given  to  the  artists.  Their  names  do  not  appear  in  the  front  of  the  book  but  are  buried  in  the 
introduction.  All  of  them  deserve  a great  deal  of  credit;  the  plates  are  of  very  high  quality. 

One  can  find  errors  in  any  book  if  one  looks  hard  enough.  In  this  book  I got  no  further 
than  the  Procellariiformes  before  I ran  into  trouble.  Peter  Hayman  has  done  stalwart  work 
elsewhere,  but  some  of  his  seabirds  are  not  too  successful,  particularly  as  to  “jizz”  or 
“gestalt.”  The  flying  Southern  Giant  Petrel  {Macronectes  giganteus)  on  Plate  14,  with  its 
narrow,  bent  and  pointed  wings,  looks  like  a hybrid  between  that  species  and  a Sooty  Al- 
batross (Phoebetria  sp.).  The  Wandering  Albatross  {Diomedea  exulans)  should  have  a pink 
bill  (shown  here  as  off-white),  and  the  text  errs  in  referring  to  the  biU  as  “appearing  almost 
white,”  however  likely  that  might  seem.  The  bill  always  looks  pink,  even  at  some  distance. 
Why  are  all  the  wanderers  shown  with  the  feet  projecting  beyond  the  tail?  This  is  certainly 
not  normal.  The  bill  of  an  adult  Black-browed  Albatross  [D.  melanophris)  is  bright  orange- 
yellow  with  a pink  tip,  not  the  washed-out  brownish  color  shown  on  Plate  13.  The  juvenile 
hlack-hrow  (#8  on  this  plate)  should  have  a dark  bill  with  blackish  tip,  whereas  it  is  shown 
with  a bill  like  an  adult.  1 would  never  have  recognised  the  top  left  hand  bird  on  Plate  16  as 
a Soft-plumaged  Petrel  (Pterodroma  mollis),  even  though  I have  recently  seen  thousands  of 
them.  The  shape  is  all  wrong,  and  there  is  no  hint  of  the  dark  band  across  the  wings  and 
rump,  obvious  in  good  light,  which  forms  a typical  Pterodroma  ‘W’  pattern.  The  flight  is 
described  as  “towering  into  sky.”  1 have  yet  to  see  mollis  tower;  it  flies  close  to  the  surface. 
I'he  dark  morph  of  mollis  is  stated  to  be  “virtually  indistinguishable  in  field  from  Kerguelen 
Petrel  P.  brevirostris."  This  is  what  I call  a “museum  skin  remark.”  The  Kerguelen  Petrel 
is  easily  told  from  mollis  and  other  petrels  by  its  curious  flight,  long  glides  interspersed  with 
a few,  quick  alcid-like  wingbeats.  It  also  towers,  unlike  mollisl 

The  above  remarks  are  only  intended  to  show  that  even  a “hible”  may  make  mistakes.  By 
and  large  mistakes  are  few,  and  the  work  maintains  a high  level  of  excellence.  Birds  of  the 
Western  Palearctic  is  a must  for  every  library,  public  or  private.  Even  though  the  cost  of 
ac(|uiring  all  7 volumes  (5  more  are  planned)  will  be  considerable,  it  is  the  best  ornithological 
investment  I know. — Stuart  Keith. 


Migrant  Birds  in  the  Neotropics:  Ecology,  Behavior,  Distribution,  and  Conser- 
vation. By  A.  Keast  and  E.  S.  Morton  (eds.).  Smithsonian  Institution  Press,  Washington, 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


433 


D.C.,  1980:576  pp.,  30  range  maps,  169  numbered  text  figs.,  138  tables,  33  black-and-white 
photos.  $27.50  (cloth),  $15.00  (paper). — This  long  overdue  and  highly  anticipated  volume  is 
the  proceedings  of  a symposium  held  27-29  October  1977.  Forty  papers  are  unequally  divided 
into  5 sections:  Conservation  (2  papers).  Migration  of  Taxonomic  Groups  (5  papers).  Regional 
Studies  (23  papers).  Implications  of  Overwintering  in  the  Tropics  (8  papers).  Integrations  (2 
papers).  Although  most  of  the  papers  deal  with  passerine  birds,  shorebirds  and  raptors  are 
treated,  as  are  communities  as  a whole.  Additional  review  papers  deal  with  population  bi- 
ology, meterological  patterns,  migration  strategies,  food  supply,  mixed  foraging  flocks  and 
intercontinental  comparisons. 

This  symposium  is  timely  in  view  of  the  potential  impact  of  widespread  destruction  of 
tropical  habitats  on  north  temperate  birds  wintering  in  tropical  America.  Terborgh,  in  his 
introductory  paper,  estimates  that  habitat  suitable  for  migrants  wiU  be  gone  by  the  turn  of 
the  century.  However,  we  know  very  little  about  the  biology  of  the  migrants  once  they  leave 
their  breeding  grounds  in  the  north.  Where  do  they  go?  What  habitats  do  they  use?  What 
food  do  they  eat?  WiU  the  destruction  of  tropical  forests  lead  to  a decrease  in  temperate 
migrants?  An  earlier  argument  stated  that  moderate  to  heavy  tropical  forest  destruction 
might  be  beneficial  to  some  temperate  migrants  that  prefer  “second  growth”  habitats.  Such 
thoughts,  however,  were  laid  to  rest  when  Terborgh  listed  55  species  know  to  winter  in 
mature  tropical  forests,  although  only  a few  of  these  were  obligate  forest  interior  users.  He 
further  states  that  we  know  of  no  temperate  migrant  species  that  use  “fenced  cattle  pasture, 
canefield  or  rice  paddy,”  yet  this  is  what  much  of  the  forest  tracts  are  becoming. 

Regional  studies  suggest  that  the  number  of  migrants  present  is  inversely  related  to  dis- 
tance from  tbe  breeding  grounds,  i.e.,  Mexico  has  more  migrants  than  Costa  Rica,  which  in 
turn  has  more  than  Panama.  As  a result,  ecological  comparisons  among  sites  are  difficult; 
since  factors  relating  to  abundance,  competition,  food  habits  and  behavior  differ  geograph- 
icaUy  no  single  explanation  for  the  overaU  ecology  of  migrants  in  the  tropics  is  satisfactory. 

Migrants  occupy  a wide  variety  of  habitats  ranging  from  coastal  dunes  to  lowland  and 
highland  forest.  Two  schools  of  thought  exist  as  to  why  second  growth  sites  are  preferentiaUy 
chosen  by  many  migrants.  WiUis,  Karr,  Morse  and  others  seem  to  support  the  irregularity 
principle,  that  migrants  tend  to  use  irregularly  abundant  resources  while  on  the  wintering 
grounds  and  choose  marginal  or  “suboptimal”  habitats  avoided  by  residents  in  order  to 
reduce  intense  competition  for  resources,  presumably  food.  Since  these  habitats  are  sub- 
optimal,  residents  will  not  use  them  when  the  migrants  leave.  The  other  view,  presented  by 
Schwartz  and  others  states  that  these  second  growth  habitats  are  not  marginal,  that  they  are 
“won”  through  competition  over  evolutionary  time  with  “residents,”  that  those  habitats 
(niches?)  are  used  by  the  migrants  for  up  to  70%  of  the  year  and  when  they  leave  the  sites 
I are  not  used  by  residents,  i.e.,  the  residents  know  their  place  and  stay  there.  There  are 
I problems  with  both  views.  There  are  no  real  data  to  support  the  claim  that  the  second  growth 
habitats  are  suboptimal  and  tbe  fact  that  so  many  birds  use  them  for  at  least  half  the  year 
I argues  that  they  are  not.  However,  if  these  sites  are  won  by  competition  “at  the  expense  of 
I some  tropical  breeding  residents”  then  one  would  expect  that  opportunistic  residents  tem- 
1 porarily  would  exploit  the  sites  during  the  summer  months.  What  is  needed  are  more  com- 
plete studies  of  the  winter  habitats  used  by  migrants  after  they  have  left.  Is  it  possible  that 
researchers  do  not  want  to  go  into  second  growth  sites  because  many  of  the  “glamorous” 
tropical  residents  aren’t  there? 

Unfortunately,  the  problem  of  what  the  migrants  eat,  especially  with  regards  to  what  is 
available,  is  not  answered  by  this  symposium.  This  would  seem,  however,  to  be  the  funda- 
mental question  with  regards  to  migrant  ecology  in  the  tropics,  especially  if  adequate  state- 
ments about  their  impact  are  to  be  made. 

It  should  be  noted  that  Cox’s  long  overlooked  1968  paper  on  “The  role  of  competition  in 


434 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


the  evolution  of  migration”  (Evolution  22:180-192,  1968)  has  had  a rebirth  in  this  symposium. 
Although  his  original  reasoning  may  be  subject  to  question,  the  idea  is  interestingly  supported 
by  several  of  tbe  symposium  papers.  Also,  one  should  be  aware  that  the  “south  ancestral 
home”  theory  of  migration  evolution  is  apparently  alive  and  well,  in  that  it  is  supported  by 
at  least  5 of  the  symposium  papers. 

Care  should  be  taken  in  interpretation  of  bird  census  data  presented  in  this  symposium. 
By  my  count  at  least  17  different  “censusing”  techniques  were  used  in  the  different  studies. 
These  ranged  from  Johnson’s  casual  observance  of  “birds  seen  on  the  walk  down  the  road,” 
to  Russell’s  “regularly  surveyed”  plots,  to  Willis’  “scale  of  censused  abundance,”  to  Pear- 
son’s species  lists,  to  Tramer  and  Kemp’s  6 “visual/auditory  counts.”  Both  Hilty  and  Hes- 
penheide  used  “trail  side  censuses”  while  several  walked  modified  Emlen  transect  lines  or 
did  strip  censuses  of  varying  (10,  20,  40  m)  widths.  The  Waide,  Emlen  and  Tramer  study 
actually  used  3 different  methods  on  21  sites  (9  sites  were  censused  by  “counts  of  birds  seen 
on  repeated  visits,”  6 sites  were  censused  using  mist  nets,  6 sites  were  censused  using 
Emlen  lines).  The  data  for  this  paper  were  collected  in  different  time  periods  (December- 
May)  on  different  sites  and  yet  all  the  data  are  pooled  into  one  table  with  a special  column 
labeled  percent  migrant  individuals.  So  many  sources  of  variation  suggest  caution  in  com- 
parative interpretations  such  as  those  of  Terborgh  and  Keast.  Based  on  my  experience  in 
the  tropics,  it  is  even  more  remarkable  to  think  that  one  can  take  walks  through  the  forest 
or  do  trailside  counts  or  other  transect  techniques  and  expect  to  accurately  sample  the 
avifauna.  Given  the  nature  of  the  terrain,  structure  of  the  habitat,  the  natural  history  of  the 
birds  and  the  limitations  of  the  above  methods  it  would  seem  impossible  to  deteet  even  25% 
of  the  existing  individuals. 

Except  for  a few  minor  flaws  and  a lot  of  typos,  Keast  and  Morton  have  done  a masterful 
job  of  editing.  My  only  complaints  with  style  are  minor,  dealing  mostly  with  wasted  space; 
e.g.,  in  Smith’s  paper  5 pictures  of  hawk  and  vulture  migrations  over  Panama  when  2 or  3 
would  do,  in  Barlow’s  paper  12  full  page  vireo  range  maps  when  they  all  could  have  fit  on 
1 page  and  13  poorly  drawn  bird  illustrations  in  the  Rappole  and  Warner  paper  (which  the 
authors  cite  as  excellent  work  in  their  acknowledgments  section). 

As  far  as  the  nuts  and  bolts  of  each  individual  paper  are  concerned  most  are  excellent  in 
content  and  superbly  written.  (3ne,  however,  stands  out  like  a sore  thumb  and  I wish  to 
make  a few  comments  about  it  (which  1 guess  is  a reviewer’s  prerogative  as  well  as  duty). 
The  Rappole  and  Warner  paper  proves  the  axiom  that  biggest  (42  pages)  is  not  best.  The 
paper  purports  to  show  that  individuals  of  14  migrant  species  defend  well-defined  (0.2-0.5 
ha)  territories  throughout  the  winter,  that  some  territorial  floaters  persist  in  peripheral  areas 
and  that  these  data  refute  the  irregularity  principle.  These  arguments  are  less  than  con- 
vincing. Territoriality  is  inferred  in  12  of  the  14  species  (Table  7).  Yet  2 speeies  showed  no 
territorial  defense,  5 had  1 observation  and  3 had  but  3 observations.  Why  are  2 species 
missing  from  the  table?  Although  “territory”  size  data  were  only  collected  for  Hooded  War- 
blers (Wilsonia  citrina)  based  on  the  territory  flush  teehnique  (and  12  birds  were,  in  fact, 
“mapped”),  territory  size  data  for  9 additional  of  the  14  species  are  given  in  Table  13.  How 
were  these  data  obtained?  Since  we  have  no  data  on  the  proportion  of  floaters  to  territory 
holders  and  sinee  we  have  fitness  data  for  neither,  we  have  no  way  of  knowing  if  the  habit 
is  adaptive.  For  example,  if  there  were,  in  fact,  12  territorial  Hooded  Warblers  and  1200 
floaters  and  each  type  had  2 young  per  pair  during  the  breeding  season,  well  ....  Not  only 
is  there  an  absence  of  hard  data  in  the  paper  but  much  of  the  data  given  in  tables  refutes 
the  text.  For  example.  Worm-eating  Warblers  [Helmitheros  vermivorus)  are  said  to  respond 
“vigorously  to  playbacks  of  vocalizations  and  displayed  to,  and  attacked,  caged  eonspecif- 
ics,”  yet  only  3 each  caged  conspecific  and  playback  trials  are  listed;  one  is  positive  and  2 
are  negative  in  both  cases  (Table  7).  Ovenbirds  (Seiurus  aurocapillus)  are  listed  as  territorial 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


435 


(Table  10),  but  there  were  no  responses  to  playback  (Table  7).  Moreover,  in  the  text  “this 
species  does  not  respond  strongly  to  introduced  caged  intruders,”  yet  5 of  the  7 trials  (Table 
7)  are  positive  but  in  Table  10  the  species  is  given  a negative  in  the  response  to  caged 
conspecifics  column.  For  the  Yellow-bellied  Flycatcher  {Empidonax  flaviventris)  Table  10 
presents  a positive  in  both  the  response  to  playback  and  response  to  caged  individual  columns 
yet  Table  7 shows  only  1 trial  for  each.  Many  other  statements  are  made  throughout  this 
paper  with  little  or  no  supportive  data.  Sample  sizes  are  not  given  and  the  tables  often  refute 
the  text.  Often  the  tables  are  so  unclear  that  statements  made  in  the  text  cannot  be  supported 
or  refuted  (e.g..  Table  5 does  not  show  that  “some  birds  remained  throughout  the  winter  on 
the  same  territory  and  returned  there  the  next  winter,”  regardless  of  what  the  authors  think). 
In  sum,  the  Rappole  and  Warner  paper,  while  potentially  of  major  importance,  is  sadly 
lacking  in  credibility. 

My  overall  impression  of  this  symposium  is  that  it  is  timely,  well  thought  out,  generally 
well  written  and  much  needed.  There  is  still  much  to  be  learned  and  this  symposium  provides 
a foundation  of  information  upon  which  to  build  future  research.  The  volume  asks  more 
questions  than  it  answers,  making  it  a very  valuable  addition  to  the  literature.  It  is  obvious 
that  something  must  be  done  now  to  preserve  the  tropics  or,  in  the  words  of  John  Terborgh, 
the  woods  in  North  America  “just  won’t  sound  the  way  it  used  to.” — Robert  C.  Whitmore. 


The  Birds  of  Hacienda  Palo  Verde,  Guanacaste,  Costa  Rica.  By  Paul  Slud.  Smith- 
sonian Contributions  to  Zoology  No.  292,  Smithsonian  Institution  Press,  Washington,  D.C., 
1980:92  pp.,  frontispiece,  6 figs.,  33  plates,  1 table.  Price  not  given. — According  to  its  author, 
this  study  represents  “the  first  comprehensive  report  on  the  avifauna  of  any  locality  in  the 
Central  American  arid  Pacific  lowlands”  and  “is  intended  to  provide  a point  of  reference  for 
avifaunal  or  environmental  comparisons  among  comparably  known  localities  anywhere  in 
tropical  America.”  In  the  introduction.  Dr.  Slud  discusses  the  biogeographic  position  of  NW 
Costa  Rica,  comparing  viewpoints  of  students  of  various  groups,  and  finally  endorsing  the 
existing  consensus  that  this  area  represents  the  southern  terminus  of  the  Central  American 
Arid  Pacific  biota.  There  follows  a detailed  discussion  of  the  climate,  topography,  and  vege- 
tation of  the  Guanacaste  lowlands  and  Palo  Verde,  mostly  abstracted  from  the  work  of  Hold- 
ridge  and  his  collaborators.  The  emphasis  on  vegetational  heterogeneity  within  the  tropical 
dry  forest  formation  raises  expectations  that  the  avifauna  will  be  treated  in  comparable 
detail.  An  account  of  the  author’s  3 months’  fieldwork  at  Palo  Verde  follows,  including 
t notable  aspects  of  each  of  his  4 visits  between  1970  and  1975,  two  each  in  wet  and  dry 
seasons. 

The  next  section.  Remarks,  contains  the  only  quantitative  analysis  in  the  book;  a com- 
parison of  the  numbers  of  species  seen  on  different  visits,  and  species  in  common  between 
visits.  The  general  conclusion  is  that  one  might  expect  to  see  about  the  same  number  of 
species,  and  about  the  same  proportion  of  species  in  common,  in  any  two  2-week  periods. 
However,  landbirds  were  not  distinguished  from  waterbirds  in  the  analysis,  time  spent  per 
habitat  evidently  was  not  standardized  and  quantitative  censuses  were  not  attempted.  More- 
over, another  observer  without  Dr.  Slud’s  skill  at  field  identification  might  well  come  up  with 
quite  different  results.  The  ecological  relevance  of  these  results  is  thus  limited  at  best. 

, The  rest  of  the  text  contains  an  annotated  list  of  species  recorded  by  Dr.  Slud,  supple- 
I mented  by  the  observations  of  P.  A.  Opler  and  a few  published  citations.  The  abundance  of 
I each  species  is  indicated,  but  terms  like  “abundant,”  “common,”  etc.  are  not  explicitly 
I defined,  and  alternate  with  stiU  more  vague  or  subjective  designations  as  “seen  daily”  and 
I “not  so  scarce  as  expected.”  A general  indication  of  habitat  is  given  (e.g.,  “inside  woodland,” 
i “nonforest”),  but  no  consistent  attempt  is  made  to  relate  habitat  choice  of  the  birds  to  the 


436 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


information  on  vegetation  given  in  the  introduetion,  nor  is  the  vegetation  on  the  “study 
tracts”  described  in  detail.  Species  recorded  in  adjacent  areas  but  not  at  Palo  Verde  are 
included  in  this  general  list;  space  and  coherence  would  have  been  saved  by  relegating  them 
to  an  appendix,  since  for  many  of  them  the  appropriate  habitat  simply  does  not  exist  at  Palo 
Verde.  There  is  a separate  “hypothetical  list”  of  other  unrecorded  species  that  occur  within 
30  km  of  Palo  Verde,  but  again,  because  the  habitats  of  many  of  these  species  are  totally 
absent  from  Palo  Verde,  the  list  does  not  seem  particularly  useful.  When  comparing  modern- 
day  observations  with  those  of  turn-of-the-century  observers  like  Nutting  and  Carriker,  some 
attention  might  have  been  profitably  paid  to  the  possibility  that  the  deforestation  of  well  over 
75%  of  Guanacaste  since  their  observations  might  have  had  some  effect  on  bird  distributions. 
Finally,  data  on  food  habits,  breeding,  molt,  etc.  are  given  for  very  few  species. 

The  33  plates  contain  black-and-white  photographs  of  habitats  (mostly)  in  the  Palo  Verde 
area.  There  is  much  redundance  here,  and  of  the  3 plant  identifications  essayed,  2 are 
incorrect  (the  “guanacaste”  trees  of  plates  1 and  16  are  saman  and  ceiba,  respectively).  In 
general,  I lament  the  dearth  of  botanical  information  in  the  book:  what  fruits  were  eaten  by 
birds?  what  flowers  visited?  Many  plates  are  labeled  simply  “view  in  woodland:”  however 
attractive,  these  serve  little  purpose  without  more  details  pointed  out. 

One  might  also  question  the  study’s  comprehensiveness,  as  a considerable  amount  of 
information  available  on  Palo  Verde  birds  was  evidently  not  consulted  in  its  preparation. 
Numerous  competent  observers  associated  with  the  Organization  for  Tropical  Studies  have 
worked  at  Palo  Verde  since  1971;  a detailed  field  checklist  of  Palo  Verde  birds  based  upon 
their  observations  and  my  own  over  an  8-year  span  has  been  available  for  a doUar  from  OTS 
for  several  years.  More  importantly,  since  1976-1977  important  ecological  studies  on  Palo 
Verde  birds  have  been  carried  out  by  Bio.  Julio  Sanchez  and  his  team  from  the  Costa  Rican 
Fish  and  Wildlife  Department.  This  information  could  have  added  20-25  species  to  Dr.  Slud’s 
list,  and  affected  the  status  of  many  others.  Given  the  long  delay  between  the  author’s 
fieldwork  and  publication,  it  is  unfortunate  that  more  effort  was  not  devoted  to  keeping 
abreast  of  developments:  the  present  report  is  far  from  an  up-to-date  summary  of  what  is 
known  of  Palo  Verde's  avifauna. 

The  most  appropriate  audience  for  this  book  might  well  be  the  better  modern  birding 
tours,  for  it  is  they  who  should  be  most  interested  in  which  and  how  many  species  they  could 
expect  to  see  at  Palo  Verde,  and  about  where  to  look  for  them.  For  the  ecological  ornithol- 
ogist, the  book  should  provide  a useful  general  introduction  to  the  area  and  its  birds — but 
he  or  she  should  not  expect  quantitative  data  pertinent  to  current  problems  in  avian  ecolo- 
gy.— F.  Gary  Stiles. 


Birds  of  Africa.  By  John  Karmali.  The  Viking  Press,  New  York,  1980:191  pp.,  72  color 
plates,  numerous  black-and-white  photographs.  125.00. — This  is  definitely  a picture  book, 
and  by  no  means  as  comprebensive  as  the  title  suggests.  As  the  author  states  in  his  preface, 
these  are  72  selected  portraits  of  East  African  birds,  his  favorites  among  the  thousands  that 
he  has  taken.  They  are  truly  portraits  in  the  sense  that  the  pictures  were  taken  with  a wide 
open  lens  so  that  the  bird  stands  sharp  and  clear  against  the  blurred  background.  Somewhat 
less  than  half  the  87  African  families  of  birds  are  represented,  the  great  majority  being 
among  the  non-passerines. 

There  is  a foreward  by  Roger  Tory  Peterson,  a preface  by  the  author  giving  his  philosophy 
of  bird  photography,  an  introduction  with  an  outline  of  the  zoogeography  of  Africa,  followed 
by  the  main  text  and  plates.  Each  family  represented  is  given  a chapter  of  its  own,  with  a 
general  discussion  of  the  family  as  a whole,  and  shorter  paragraphs  on  the  biology  of  each 
species  on  the  plates.  There  are  numerous  black-and-white  photos  scattered  through  the 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


437 


book,  many  of  which  give  a livelier  feel  for  the  birds  than  the  more  formal  portraits.  The 
text  is  neither  reaUy  good  nor  bad,  but  seems  to  have  been  done  freehand,  like  the  rivers  in 
the  facing  relief  and  vegetation  maps  (pp.  14  and  15).  The  author  gives  a good  general  picture 
of  the  African  continent  and  of  the  diversity  of  its  birds,  but  details,  such  as  the  subdesert 
coast  of  Upper  Guinea  (vegetation  map),  should  not  be  accepted  uncritically.  Following  the 
plates  is  an  enjoyable  section.  Notes  on  Colour  Plates,  which  gives  the  circumstances  under 
which  each  plate  was  made,  and  a bibliography  and  index. 

As  bird  portraits,  these  plates  are  beautiful,  and  the  color  reproduction  is  superb.  Both 
the  birds  themselves  and  the  backgrounds  are  alive  and  natural,  a tribute  to  the  patience 
and  eye  of  the  photographer  and  the  skill  of  the  printer.  At  the  comparatively  modest  price 
of  $25.00,  these  portraits  are  a bargain. — Melvin  A.  Traylor. 


African  Handbook  of  Birds:  Series  One.  Birds  of  Eastern  and  North  Eastern 
Africa.  Second  Edition.  By  C.  W.  Mackworth-Praed  and  C.  H.  B.  Grant.  Longman  Group 
Ltd.,  London,  England.  In  U.S. A.,  Longman  Inc.,  New  York,  New  York.  Vol.  1,  1980:836  pp., 
53  color  plates,  6 black-and-white  photo  plates,  many  line  drawings  and  maps.  $60.00.  Vol. 
2,  1980:1177  pp.,  43  color  plates,  13  black-and-white  photo  plates,  many  line  drawings  and 
maps.  $75. (K). — This  is  an  unrevised  reprint  of  the  second  edition  (1957)  of  a work  first  pubhshed 
in  1952.  The  only  changes  are  the  addition  of  political  maps  of  Africa  in  1945  and  1979  so 
as  to  permit  the  recognition  of  old  place  names,  and  of  brief  biographies  of  the  authors. 
Volume  1 treats  the  non-passerines  and  the  suboscines,  volume  2 the  oscines.  For  each 
species  the  brief  text  covers  distinguishing  characters,  distribution  and  habits.  The  margins 
contain  range  maps  and  sometimes  small  sketches  of  the  birds.  The  color  plates  are  some- 
what faded  but  should  serve  their  intended  purpose  adequately.  These  books  are  intended 
as  field  guides,  but  with  coverage  of  nearly  1500  species  even  their  concise  organization 
limits  their  portability.  For  readers  elsewhere  these  volumes  should  continue  to  serve  as  a 
good  introduction  to  the  avifauna  of  the  area,  even  though  the  nomenclature  has  not  been 
revised. — Robert  J.  Raikow. 


A Complete  Checklist  of  the  Birds  of  the  World.  By  Richard  Howard  and  Alick 
Moore.  Oxford  University  Press,  New  York,  New  York,  1980:701  pp.  $49.50. — This  is  a 
1-volume  list  of  the  birds  of  the  world.  It  is  similar  in  format  and  purpose  to  The  Complete 
Birds  of  the  World  by  Michael  Walters,  also  published  in  1980,  but  appears  to  be  a more 
useful  work.  Unlike  the  latter  book,  this  one  contains  no  natural  history  information,  but  on 
the  other  hand  it  has  several  advantages.  The  layout  and  printing  are  superior.  Walters’ 
book  lists  only  species,  while  the  present  work  lists  subspecies  as  well,  along  with  their 
distributions.  Most  important,  this  book  contains  an  index  to  the  genera  and  species  so  that 
it  is  not  necessary  to  hunt  through  the  text  for  a particular  form.  Each  species  is  also  provided 
with  an  English  name.  As  in  other  such  undertakings  the  classification  is  eclectic,  being 
based  in  considerable  part  on  the  Peters  Check-List  plus  numerous  more  recent  research 
papers.  This  literature  is  cited  by  family  in  a 40  page  reference  list,  so  that  the  book  may 
also  serve  as  a good  introduction  to  the  taxonomic  literature  of  recent  years.  Altogether  this 
promises  to  be  a useful  reference  work  for  ornithologists. — Robert  J.  Raikow. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(3),  1981,  pp.  438-456 


REPORT  OF  THE  CONSERVATION  COMMITTEE- 

1980 

1980,  The  Year  of  the  Coast:  Birds 

Thousands  of  kilometers  of  undulating  coastlines  rim  the  U.S.  from  Washington  to  Maine, 
the  Hawaiian  Islands  and  the  Alaskan  Peninsula.  These  are  fragile  environments — whether 
arctic,  temperate,  or  tropical — yet  man  and  his  exploitative  energies  despoil  coastal  resources 
unrelentingly.  Historically,  man  has  concentrated  cities,  commerce  and  recreation  on  coast- 
lines that  today  are  immense  centers  of  human  activities.  Some  estimates  place  four  of  every 
10  industrial  facilities  on  the  nation’s  coast,  and  the  ugly  sores  of  waste  disposal  are  all  too 
commonplace.  Drainage  schemes  or  landfills  modify  our  coastal  endowment.  Estuaries  reflect 
the  chemical  and  physical  abuses  of  their  upstream  sources  and,  beyond  land’s  end,  flows 
the  garbage  of  human  carelessness.  Tidal  ecosystems  are  self-maintaining  only  so  long  as 
man  does  not  upset  the  delicate  balance  that  has  evolved  over  the  centuries.  Man  may  claim 
selected  areas  for  his  own  needs  as  long  as  other  tracts  remain  undisturbed  by  intrusion  and 
pollution;  but  this  can  be  accomplished  only  with  careful  long-range  planning.  Our  coasts 
are  choking  under  the  pressures  of  their  own  magnetism. 

In  their  pristine  condition,  our  coasts  harbored  ecological  “goods  and  services’’  beyond 
our  current  imagination.  Today,  the  richness  of  their  species  and  functions  is  regrettably 
diminished.  In  southern  Florida  alone,  waterbird  populations  have  declined  from  2.5  million 
in  1870,  to  1.5  million  in  1935,  to  150,000  in  1974.  Although  early  losses  can  be  attributed 
to  “plume  hunting,’’  declines  in  the  past  60  years  are  the  result  of  man’s  destruction  of 
feeding  and  nesting  habitat  (Soots  and  Landin  1978). 

For  many  birds,  coastlines  are  requisites  for  nesting  and  feeding;  stiU  other  species  seek 
freshwater  environments  for  breeding  but  tap  the  rich  resources  of  the  coastline  during 
migration  and  wintering  periods.  Freshwater  birds  wintering  on  seacoasts  employ  various 
strategies.  Many  use  any  stretch  of  sand,  gravel,  or  rocky  shore  that  harbors  suitable  foods. 
Others  use  restricted  topographical  and/or  ecological  situations  that  provide  protection  as 
well  as  special  foods  (e.g.,  Brant,  Branta  bernicla,  and  eelgrass,  Zostera  marina). 

Seabirds  are  not  without  adaptability.  Some  feed  at  sea,  ranging  over  wide  areas  so  that 
patchy  food  resources  are  used  where  they  occur.  Some  pioneer  nesting  sites  on  islands  or 
coastlines,  precluding  endemic  status.  Conversely,  some  features  of  seabirds  potentially  limit 
their  success  in  a rapidly  changing  world.  Many  have  low  reproductive  rates  with  delayed 
maturation,  or  experience  considerable  competition  for  nesting  sites. 

Nesting  sites  for  seabirds  often  are  uniquely  situated.  Some  birds  use  rocky  cliff  edges  or 
crevices  where  nesting  sites  are  limited  but  where  the  nests  also  are  well  protected  from 
predators  and  man.  Conversely,  beach  sites  are  more  available  but  remain  vulnerable  to 
human  disturbances.  Moreover,  many  species  have  proven  especially  sensitive  to  man-made 
disturbances  because  of  their  specialized  behavioral  responses  to  potential  predation. 

Offshore,  uses  of  seacoasts  are  less  observable,  but  there  birds  feed  on  schools  of  fish  and 
plankton  in  shoal  and  other  areas  that  may  be  especially  vulnerable  to  pollution  and  con- 
comitant loss  of  basic  productivity.  Coastal  regimes  represent  one  of  our  planet’s  more 
complex  energy-flow  systems,  and,  with  their  continued  abuse,  “The  coast  is  informing  us 
that  there  is  a saturation  point  beyond  which  its  natural  functions  no  longer  flourish,  often 
diminish,  or  simply  cease’’  (Simon  1978).  With  1980  designated  as  the  Year  of  the  Coast,  we 
focus  on  a major  component  of  coastal  systems  to  iUustrate  the  national  concern  for  natural 
resources — birds  of  the  American  coastline. 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


439 


Geographical  Review 

West  Coast. — The  West  Coast  of  the  United  States  extends  approximately  2750  km  from 
the  Mexican  border  to  the  Straits  of  Juan  de  Fuca  in  the  north.  This  extensive  mainland  and 
island  coast  includes  some  of  the  richest  seabird  habitats  found  anywhere  in  North  America. 
In  addition  to  the  resources  of  the  open  sea,  the  western  coastline  is  interspersed  with 
seacliffs,  beaches,  bays,  estuaries,  tidal  mud  flats  and  saltwater  marshes.  Four  geographic 
areas  characterize  this  diverse  coastal  environment:  southern  California,  San  Francisco  Bay, 
Columbia  River  Estuary  and  Puget  Sound  in  Washington. 

The  seacoast  of  southern  California  supports  about  158  species  of  birds  (Corwin  and 
Heffernan  1978).  Most  of  these  sea-  and  shorebirds  are  strongly  migratory,  and  the  largest 
number  of  individuals  and  species  are  found  during  the  autumn  and  spring  migrations.  The 
most  abundant  species  during  migration  is  the  Sooty  Shearwater  {Puffinus  griseus).  During 
autumn  migration,  tropical  seabirds  such  as  the  Magnificent  Frigatebird  {Fregata  magnifi- 
cens)  and  the  Red-biUed  Tropicbird  {Phaethon  aethereus)  which  are  of  infrequent  occurrence 
so  far  north  appear  along  the  southern  California  coast.  Partially  responsible  for  these  oc- 
currences is  the  Davidson  Current,  a counter-current  between  the  coast  and  the  cool  Cali- 
fornia Current  from  the  north-central  Pacific  Ocean  (Small  1974). 

Along  the  southern  California  coast,  the  continental  shelf  is  narrow;  instead,  a series  of 
ranges  and  basins  exists.  Close  to  these  submarine  banks  strong  northerly  winds  displace 
the  surface  waters,  especially  during  May  and  June.  The  result  is  an  upweUing  of  deep,  cold 
waters  rich  in  nutrients  and  dissolved  oxygen,  fostering  a proliferation  of  phytoplankton — 
the  base  of  virtually  aU  marine  food-chains.  The  abundance  of  pelagic  birds  such  as  jaegers 
{Stercorarius  spp.),  storm-petrels  (Hydrobatidae)  and  albatrosses  (Diomedea  spp.)  is  related 
to  the  surface  concentrations  of  zooplankton,  schools  of  surfacing  anchovies  and  spawning 
squids  (Small  1974). 

Extending  from  Los  Angeles  to  Point  Conception  are  the  eight  Channel  Islands.  During 
the  Pleistocene,  these  islands  were  part  of  an  archipelago  associated  with  the  continental 
landmass;  subsequent  deformation  of  the  earth’s  crust  and  changes  in  sea  level  separated 
them  from  the  mainland  (Casey  1969).  Two  of  the  islands,  Anacapa  and  Santa  Barbara 
Island,  are  parks  within  the  Channel  Islands  National  Monument,  but  most  of  the  offshore 
islands  are  relatively  inaccessible  to  the  public.  Thus,  nesting  and  roosting  seabirds  such  as 
the  Xantus’  Murrelet  (Endomychura  hypoleuca),  Cassin’s  Auklet  {Ptychoramphus  aleutica) 
and  the  Brown  Pelican  {Pelecanus  occidentalis),  an  endangered  species,  remain  undisturbed. 
In  1969,  nesting  colonies  of  Brown  Pelicans  produced  almost  no  young  because  of  eggshell 
breakage;  DDT  interfered  with  enzyme  production  involved  with  calcium  mobilization  from 
bones  to  egg  shells  (Small  1974).  A major  source  of  contamination  was  discharge  from  a Los 
Angeles  plant  that  manufactured  technical  DDT.  After  1970,  this  plant’s  liquid  wastes  were 
put  in  a sanitary  landfill  and  the  oceanic  input  of  DDT  declined.  When  DDE  contamination 
of  the  birds’  major  food  source,  anchovies,  declined,  hatching  of  pelican  eggs  increased 
significantly  but  productivity  has  remained  low  (Anderson  et  al.  1975). 

Besides  large  offshore  islands,  the  southern  California  coast  is  characterized  by  sandy 
beaches  (75%  of  the  coast).  Furthermore,  the  climate  south  from  San  Francisco  is  Mediter- 
ranean with  three  months  of  winter  rains  followed  by  a hot,  dry  summer.  These  favorable 
conditions  make  southern  California  a major  focus  of  the  state’s  human  population,  and  thus, 
of  recreation  and  economic  development.  Large  population  centers  in  southern  California 
have  altered  the  coastal  habitat  considerably.  For  example,  of  the  vast  wetlands  along  the 
Los  Angeles  County  coast,  only  110  ha  remain.  Dredging  and  filling  have  been  the  primary 
causes  of  this  habitat  destruction  (Corwin  and  Heffernan  1978). 

Along  these  sandy  beaches  two  species  breed,  the  California  Least  Tern  (Sterna  albifrons 
browni)  and  the  western  race  of  the  Snowy  Plover  (Charadrius  alexandrinus  nivosus).  At  the 


440 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


turn  of  this  century  the  Least  Tern  was  an  abundant  colonial  nester  along  the  seacoast  from 
the  Mexican  border  to  San  Francisco  Bay.  However,  human  activities  reduced  this  population 
to  such  a low  level  that,  by  1966,  the  California  Least  Tern  was  declared  an  endangered 
species.  Plovers  have  not  declined  similarly,  probably  because  of  a broader  breeding  range 
(southern  Washingon  to  southern  Baja  and  inland);  they  also  nest  further  back  from  the 
beachfronts  (Small  1974). 

Prospects  of  large  discoveries  of  oil  and  gas  on  the  California  coast  are  favorable,  whereas 
the  Oregon- Washington  coast  has  the  least  potential  for  petroleum  development  of  any  region 
considered  by  the  Department  of  Interior’s  accelerated  leasing  program  (Feldmann  and 
Hershman  1978).  It  was  under  this  program  that  portions  of  the  Santa  Barbara  Channel  were 
leased  in  1968.  On  28  January  1969,  a well  at  Union  Oil’s  Platform  A blew  out.  As  a result, 
12.3  million  liters  of  oil  coated  1710  km^  of  channel,  160  km  of  shore  were  contaminated  and 
an  estimated  8000  seabirds  died  (Dedera  1970).  The  risk  of  oil  spills  will  only  increase  as 
more  tracts  are  leased  for  development.  Unfortunately,  the  rookeries  of  South  Anacapa 
Island  are  especially  endangered  because  of  their  proximity  to  shipping  lanes  (Corwin  and 
Heffernan  1978). 

San  Francisco  Bay  originally  consisted  of  1814  km^,  but  now  only  1165  km^  remain  (Knight 
1972).  The  ability  of  the  Bay  to  flush  its  waste  loads  has  been  reduced  both  by  filling  and  by 
the  diversion  of  the  Sacramento  River  to  southern  California,  cutting  the  river’s  flow  into  the 
Bay  by  50%  and  seriously  affecting  the  estuarine  balance  between  salt  and  fresh  water. 
Further  alteration  has  been  caused  by  the  estimated  3.2  billion  liters  of  waste  water  entering 
the  Bay  each  day  (Knight  1972).  The  remaining  portions  of  the  San  Francisco  Bay  consist 
mostly  of  mud  flats  with  a few  pocket  beaches  and  marshes.  Tidal  salt  marshes  are  char- 
acterized by  cordgrass  {Spartina  foliosa),  pickleweed  (Salicornia  spp.),  saltgrass  {Disticlis 
spicata)  and  gumplant  {Grindelia  spp.).  These  salt  marshes  are  used  by  most  migrant  shore- 
birds  such  as  WiUets  {Catoptrophorus  semipalmatus)  and  Marbled  Godwits  {Limosa  fedoa) 
as  roosting  areas  at  high  tide;  during  ebbtide  the  exposed  mud  flats  become  important  feeding 
areas  (Page  et  al.  1979).  A resident  species  that  uses  salt  marshes  almost  exclusively  is  the 
California  Clapper  Rail  (Rallus  longirostris  obsoletus),  an  endangered  species.  This  raUid 
once  occupied  most  estuarine  marshes  of  central  and  northern  coastal  California.  After  1900, 
a significant  decrease  in  range  and  numbers  resulted  from  habitat  loss  (diking,  filling  or 
conversion  of  salt  evaporation  ponds)  (Gill  1979). 

Forty-three  km  west  of  San  Francisco  Bay  are  the  Farallon  Islands.  Because  of  commercial 
egging  in  the  19th  century,  these  small  islands  were  declared  a wildlife  refuge  in  1909.  The 
Farallon  Islands  today  contain  the  largest  seabird  colonies  in  the  contiguous  U.S.  (Small 
1974).  Just  northwest  of  San  Francisco  is  the  Point  Reyes  Peninsula,  the  W est  Coast’s  only 
National  Seashore. 

In  Oregon  and  Washington  the  proximity  of  the  Coast  Range  to  the  coast  limits  the  area 
available  for  development.  Furthermore,  the  maritime  climate  along  this  seacoast  produces 
rainy  winters  (as  much  as  254  cm  of  annual  rainfall)  and  cool,  dry  summers.  Despite  low 
population  densities  along  this  coast,  the  harbors  and  coastal  ocean  have  been  subject  to 
contamination.  Waste  chemicals  and  paper  mill  wastes  in  certain  estuaries  cause  severe 
pollution  locally. 

Of  the  14  estuary  systems  along  the  Oregon  coast,  the  Columbia  River  is  the  largest  with 
6075  ha.  Relatively  little  mixing  of  salt  and  fresh  water  occurs  in  this  estuary.  In  fact,  during 
summer  and  fall  it  is  the  largest  source  of  fresh  water  along  the  entire  West  Coast  of  the 
U.S.  The  Columbia  River  is  an  important  wintering  area  for  waterfowl.  The  largest  concen- 
tration of  Mallards  {Anas  platyrhynchos)  along  the  Pacific  Fly  way  occurs  in  the  Columbia 
Basin.  Irrigated  grain  farms  provide  an  important  food  resource  for  this  large  population 
(Bellrose  1976). 

Puget  Sound  encompasses  647,775  ha,  the  largest  estuary  in  Washington.  During  the 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


441 


spring,  Puget  Sound  is  an  important  breeding  area  for  pelagie  species.  Puffins  (Alcidae), 
Rhinocerous  Auklets  {Cerorhinca  monocerata)  and  Pigeon  Guillemots  {Cepphus  rolurnha)  dig 
burrows  in  cliffs  and  banks  near  tbe  saltwater  beaches  of  the  Sound.  In  the  fall  about  10 
million  ducks  and  1 million  geese  use  the  Pacific  Flyway.  Puget  Sound,  with  its  extensive 
shores  and  shore  water  estuaries,  mud  flats  and  marshes,  is  protected  from  direct  marine 
influence  by  the  Olympic  Peninsula  and  thus  provides  feeding  and  resting  areas  for  these 
waterfowl  (Feldmann  and  Hershman  1978). 

Ports  on  Puget  Sound  are  prizes  for  the  oil  industry  as  Alaskan  petroleum  resources 
develop.  Increased  tanker  traffic  will  further  increase  the  risk  of  oil  spills  damaging  the 
substantial  resources  of  the  Sound.  For  example,  Puget  Sound  has  five  National  Wildlife 
Refuges,  four  of  which  lie  along  an  important  oil-tanker  route  through  Rosario  Straits. 

East  Coast. — The  Atlantic  coast  of  the  United  States  and  Canada  is  used  heavily  by  a 
great  variety  of  sea-  and  shorebirds  at  aU  seasons  of  the  year.  The  sea  cliffs  of  eastern 
Canada  are  famous  for  their  colonies  of  gannets,  cormorants,  gulls  and  alcids,  the  marshes 
of  the  Middle  Atlantic  States  for  their  wintering  waterfowl,  and  the  islands  and  wetlands  of 
the  Southeast  for  their  colonies  of  herons,  pelicans  and  terns,  and  hordes  of  wintering  shore- 
birds  (Fig.  1). 

The  Atlantic  Ocean  has  a modifying  influence  on  the  coastal  climate.  In  the  coldest  weather 
the  immediate  coast  from  Georgia  to  Maine  is  about  5°C  warmer  than  a few  miles  inland  and 
the  frost-free  period  is  20-40  days  longer.  Mean  annual  temperatures  range  from  24°C  in 
South  Florida  to  6°C  in  eastern  Maine,  and  mean  annual  precipitation  from  110  cm  in  Maine 
to  150  cm  in  South  Florida  (Visher  1954).  Add  to  this  the  higher  average  relative  humidity, 
the  greater  annual  precipitation  and  higher  percentage  of  sunshine  (Visher  1954)  and  it 
becomes  clear  that  this  is  an  area  of  high  productivity. 

In  the  Mid-Atlantic  Bight  alone  (Nantucket  to  Cape  Hatteras),  the  commercial  fisheries 
yield  about  31,800  metric  tons  of  fish  and  shellfish  per  year  at  a value  of  |70  million;  and  2.5 
million  saltwater  anglers  in  1970  generated  more  than  $318  million  of  business  activity  (Saila 
1973). 

As  early  as  the  colonial  period  the  potential  value  of  tidal  marsh  for  agriculture  and  other 
uses  was  recognized,  and  extensive  areas  of  tidal  marsh  in  the  Carolinas  and  Georgia  were 
reclaimed  for  rice  production  (Knights  and  Phillips  1979).  Reclamation  for  living  space, 
recreation  and  industry  has  continued  at  an  increasing  pace  and  to  these  activities  have  been 
added  pressures  from  mining,  oil  production,  transportation,  fishing,  military  uses,  waste 
disposal  and  other  direct  and  indirect  sources  of  pollution  that  threaten  the  survival  of  not 
only  the  coastal  ecosystem,  but  the  future  of  our  planet’s  living  oceans.  Senator  Ernest  F. 
HoUings  {in  Edge  1972),  citing  testimony  from  Jacques  Cousteau,  stated  that  life  in  the  sea 
during  the  past  20  years  had  diminished  as  much  as  30—50%. 

Bird  life  along  the  Atlantic  coast  is  rich  and  varied  at  all  seasons.  Saila  (1973)  listed  380 
species  from  the  coastal  lands  and  offshore  waters  of  the  Mid-Atlantic  Bight.  Colonial  nesting 
species  have  been  well  documented  in  recent  years  by  Brown  et  al.  (1975),  Osborn  and 
Custer  (1978),  Erwin  (1979),  Erwin  and  Korschgen  (1979),  Parnell  and  Soots  (1979),  and 
others. 

From  Massachusetts  east  through  the  Maritime  Provinces,  the  petrels,  gannets,  cormo- 
rants, ducks,  guUs,  terns  and  four  species  of  alcids  nest  high  on  rocky  islands,  beyond  the 
reach  of  storm  tides.  Nevertheless,  many  species  lead  a perilous  existence  because  of  gulls 
and  other  predators  and  are  not  prepared  to  stand  the  stress  of  further  disruption  of  nesting 
activities  by  human  intervention. 

Stretches  of  undisturbed  beach  are  needed  by  two  species  of  breeding  plovers.  Islands 
free  of  mammalian  predators  and  human  disturbances  are  essential  for  the  nesting  of  10 
species  of  terns,  and  three  of  guUs  {Lams),  as  well  as  for  cormorants  {Phalacrocorax), 
oystercatchers  {Haematopus)  and  skimmers  {Rynchops).  Along  the  Middle  and  South  Atlantic 


442 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  • \ol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


A 

B 

C 

D 

E 

F 


American  Oystercatcher  G 


Piping  Plover  H 

Wilson's  Plover  I 

Black-bellied  Plover  J 

Willet  / 

Lesser  Yellowlegs  ^ 


Purple  Sandpiper 

Dunlin 

Sanderling 

Short-billed  Dowicher 
Major  Winter  Distribution 
Fall  Migration  Concentration 
Area 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


443 


coasts  many  of  these  birds  nest  barely  above  high  tide  where  their  eggs  are  subject  to  mass 
destruction  by  storm  tides;  and  a single  visit  by  human  intruders  can  also  wipe  out  an  entire 
year’s  production. 

The  great  tidal  marshes  that  stretch  intermittently  from  New  Jersey  and  Chesapeake  Bay 
south  to  Florida  are  vital  to  nesting  of  Willets,  Clapper  (Rallus  longirostris)  and  (locally) 
Black  (Laterallus  jamaicensis)  rails,  and  Sharp-tailed  {Ammospiza  caudacuta)  and  Seaside 
{A.  maritima)  sparrows  as  well  as  to  several  kinds  of  gulls  and  terns.  These  marshes  also 
serve  as  nurseries  for  much  of  the  aquatic  life  on  which  the  offshore  fisheries  depend  (for 
humans  as  well  as  birds). 

From  mid-summer  into  mid-autumn,  the  coastal  flats  and  marshes  support  vast  hordes  of 
migrating  shorebirds  en  route  from  their  Arctic  nesting  grounds  in  Canada  to  their  winter 
homes  in  Latin  America.  In  preparation  for  long  non-stop  flights  over  water,  these  millions 
of  migrants  require  extensive  unpolluted  feeding  grounds.  For  the  shorebirds,  waterfowl, 
cormorants  and  pelicans  wintering  along  the  Atlantic  coast  unpolluted  coastal  waters  are 
essential.  And  for  the  gannets  (Morus),  kittiwakes  {Rissa),  fulmars  {Fulmarus),  skuas  (Ca- 
tharacta),  jaegers  and  alcids  that  winter  in  our  offshore  waters,  a clean  environment  also 
must  be  maintained. 

The  changing  ecology  of  Chesapeake  Bay,  if  not  typical  of  trends  along  the  Atlantic  coast, 
may  at  least  be  symptomatic,  and  underscore  the  need  for  closer  monitoring  of  habitat 
conditions  in  all  coastal  bays.  Stevenson  et  al.  (1979)  have  shown  the  sharp  decline  in  sub- 
merged aquatic  vegetation  from  1971-1978.  Using  data  from  more  than  600  sites,  the  per- 
centage of  stations  with  aquatic  grasses  decreased  from  28-10%,  and  there  was  a comparable 
decrease  in  diversity.  No  single  cause  for  this  change  could  be  proven,  but  contributing 
factors  considered  of  minor  or  local  importance  were  overgrazing  by  carp,  cownose  rays  and 
Mute  Swans  {Cygnus  olor),  effects  of  Hurricane  Agnes  (in  June  1972),  warming  trends  of  Bay 
waters,  natural  diseases,  point-source  pollutants,  petrochemicals,  dredging,  and  boat  traffic. 
Possible  major  contributing  factors  were  chlorine  (about  13,200  metric  tons  entered  the  Bay 
in  1973),  increasing  levels  of  turbidity  in  shallow  waters,  excessive  nutrients  (phosphorus 
and  nitrogen)  from  waste  water,  a dramatic  increase  in  herbicides,  and  locally,  competition 
from  water  chestnut  (Trapa  natans)  and  Eurasian  watermilfoil  {Myriophyllum  spicatum). 

Effects  on  wintering  waterfowl  from  the  loss  of  submerged  aquatic  plants  have  been  sum- 
marized by  Perry  et  al.  (1981).  Wintering  waterfowl  numbers  have  declined  50%,  with  only 
the  Buffleheads  (Bucephala  albeola)  showing  increases.  Species  feeding  predominantly  on 
submerged  aquatics  have  been  affected  the  greatest.  There  has  been  a decline  in  the  diversity 
of  estuarine  food  organisms  available  and  in  the  percentage  of  vegetation  eaten  by  the  ducks. 
Submerged  aquatic  vegetation  is  no  longer  a major  food  for  Canvasbacks  {Aythya  valisineria) 
and  Ruddy  Ducks  {Oxyura  jamaicensis)  wintering  in  Chesapeake  Bay. 

An  example  of  the  inadvertent  loss  of  an  endangered  bird  from  habitat  alteration  in  a 
coastal  marsh  is  provided  by  the  Dusky  Seaside  Sparrow  {Ammospiza  maritima  nigrescens). 
Sharp  (1970)  contrasted  the  33-34  males  he  found  at  Merritt  Island,  Brevard  Co.,  Florida, 
in  1968,  with  the  70  pairs  he  found  there  in  1963,  and  with  an  indirect  estimate  of  about  600 
pairs  in  1957.  Annual  censuses  by  Sykes  (1980)  in  1969-79  revealed  30,  18,  8,  11,  2,  2,  2,  0, 
2,  0 and  0 singing  males  in  this  area.  Sharp  (1970)  estimated  a total  (world)  population  of  894 


Fig.  1.  Winter  distribution  of  shorebirds  on  the  eastern  U.S.  coastline.  Redrawn  from 
an  original  map  courtesy  of  National  Oceanic  and  Atmospheric  Administration.  The  Con- 
servation Committee  notes  that  certain  winter  distributions  (e.g.,  American  Oystercatcher) 
located  north  of  Virginia  may  not  be  typical. 


444 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


males  in  1968,  primarily  in  the  St.  John’s  River  marshes.  The  last  remnants  of  this  population 
were  13  males  in  1979  and  4 males  and  no  females  in  1980  (Sykes  1980).  AU  except  one  male 
have  been  taken  into  captivity  in  an  effort  to  produce  a nearly  pure  population  through 
backcrossing,  using  2 female  nigrescens  X peninsulae  hybrids  produced  in  1980  (Sykes, 
pers.  comm.). 

Causes  of  the  final  decline  at  Merritt  Island,  summarized  by  Sykes  (1980)  include  elimi- 
nation of  about  91%  of  the  former  habitat  by  impounding  and  flooding,  invasion  of  shrubs 
(and  probably  predators)  along  the  dike  system,  and  an  increase  in  Red-winged  Blackbirds 
{Agelaius  phoeniceus)  because  of  the  increase  in  shrubs.  Management  activities,  including 
dike  removal  and  periodic  burning,  were  undertaken  in  an  effort  to  restore  the  habitat  to  its 
original  condition,  but  not  in  time  to  be  effective. 

On  the  brighter  side,  the  successful  reintroduction  of  Common  Puffins  [Fratercula  arctica) 
on  Eastern  Egg  Rock  in  Maine  (Graham  1976)  demonstrates  that,  given  sufficient  knowledge, 
interest  and  support,  we  have  the  opportunity  to  retain  important  features  of  coastal  systems. 

The  Gulf  Coast. — Coastal  habitats  along  the  Gulf  of  Mexico  are  probably  best  known  as 
wintering  grounds  for  a large  percentage  of  North  America’s  waterfowl  population.  But  in 
addition  to  providing  seasonal  habitat  for  ducks  and  geese,  the  extensive  wetlands  along  the 
Gulf  Coast  support  large  migratory  and  resident  populations  of  shorebirds,  wading  birds  and 
other  wetland  species. 

Climatic  conditions  along  the  Gulf  Coast  are  somewhat  varied.  The  growing  season  is  long 
and  the  winter  is  usually  mild.  Summer  temperatures  and  humidities,  which  are  generally 
high,  favor  luxuriant  vegetative  growth.  Precipitation  is  variable,  but  averages  over  125  cm 
per  year  in  all  coastal  areas  except  extreme  south  Texas.  Rainfall  is  less  than  80  cm  per 
year  in  southern  Texas  and  the  area  is  semi-arid. 

The  southern  tip  of  Florida  is  classified  as  a humid  tropical  savannah  (Bailey  1978).  Man- 
grove swamps  are  common  in  the  Everglades  and  Florida  Keys.  Mangroves  produce  large 
amounts  of  detritus,  providing  the  basis  for  food  webs  in  the  swamps.  These  swamps  provide 
habitat  for  countless  terrestrial  and  aquatic  animals,  particularly  birds  (Hanlon  et  al.  1975). 

Salt  and  freshwater  marshes  are  common  along  the  upper  Gulf  Coast.  Marshes  and  marsh 
vegetation  produce  large  amounts  of  detritus  that  are  exported  from  the  marsh  into  adjacent 
bay  waters.  Kirby  and  Gosselink  (1976)  estimated  70%  export  of  the  net  production  from  the 
marsh  they  studied;  production  for  a Louisiana  Gulf  Coast  salt  marsh  was  estimated  as  1176 
g/m^/yr.  It  is  well  documented  that  marshes  serve  as  nursery  grounds  for  commercially 
important  fin-fishes  and  shellfishes.  Marshes  are  also  important  habitats  for  migratory  birds. 
For  example,  almost  the  entire  North  American  population  of  the  Lesser  Snow  Goose  {Anser 
c.  caerulescens)  is  dependent  upon  the  marshes  on  the  Louisiana  and  Texas  coasts  for  winter 
habitat  (Gosselink  et  al.  1974). 

Barrier  islands  are  found  along  many  parts  of  the  upper  Gulf  Coast  but  are  most  well- 
developed  in  Texas.  Barrier  islands  have  several  ecological  values:  the  dune  systems  protect 
landward  areas  from  storm  wave  and  tidal  damage;  the  islands  form  and  protect  productive 
estuaries  on  the  landward  side;  and  the  islands  support  a unique  flora  and  fauna.  In  past 
times  the  barrier  islands  were  significant  nesting  habitats  for  colonial  waterbirds,  especially 
terns  and  Black  Skimmers  {Rynchops  nigra)  (Portnoy  1977,  Chapman  1980).  Human  en- 
croachment has  caused  a drastic  decline  in  the  use  of  the  Texas  barrier  islands  as  nesting 
habitat  (Chapman  1980);  the  decline  is  not  as  drastic  in  Louisiana  (Portnoy  1978). 

In  shallow  bay  waters  at  depths  up  to  1.5  m extensive  beds  of  submerged  seagrasses  are 
common  on  the  Gulf  Coast.  The  sediments  of  seagrass  systems  perform  similar  functions  to 
those  of  tidal  marshes.  Seagrass  systems  provide  wildlife  habitat  and  serve  as  spawning 
grounds  for  many  marine  organisms.  The  Laguna  Madre,  a 202  km  long  lagoon  with  extensive 
seagrass  beds  in  southern  Texas,  provides  rich  feeding  habitat  for  birds.  In  addition  to  large 
concentrations  of  shorebirds  and  wading  birds,  78%  of  the  North  American  population  of 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


445 


Redheads  (Aythya  americana)  winter  in  the  Laguna  (Weller  1964).  Many  waterfowl  use  the 
Central  Fly  way,  including  80%  of  the  Pintails  {Anas  acuta),  90%  of  the  Northern  Shovelers 
{A.  clypeata),  98%  of  the  Red-breasted  Mergansers  {Mergus  serrator)  and  43%  of  the  Buf- 
fleheads  also  winter  there. 

Large  nesting  populations  of  seabirds  and  wading  birds  occur  along  the  Gulf  Coast.  Portnoy 
(1977)  found  168  colonies  totaling  more  than  847,000  breeding  birds  of  26  species  in  the 
coastal  areas  of  Louisiana,  Mississippi  and  Alabama;  Chaney  et  al.  (1978)  counted  more  than 
257,000  breeding  birds  of  28  species  nesting  along  the  Texas  coast.  Most  of  the  colonial 
waterbirds  in  Texas  nest  upon  dredged-material  islands. 

Populations  of  most  colonial  waterbird  species  have  been  declining  in  recent  years  in 
Texas  (Chapman  1980);  reliable  information  on  population  trends  is  not  available  for  most 
of  the  northern  Gulf  Coast  (Portnoy  1980).  Although  there  are  many  factors  that  have  con- 
tributed to  the  decline,  two  are  probably  of  major  importance — pesticide  contamination  and 
habitat  alteration. 

Before  1920,  populations  of  Brown  Pelicans  were  estimated  at  50,000-85,000  birds  in 
Louisiana  and  5000  in  Texas.  The  last  record  of  nesting  Brown  Pelicans  in  Louisiana  was  in 
1961  (Van  Tets  1965)  and  the  Texas  population  declined  to  about  100  by  1974  (King  et  al. 
1977).  Weather  and  disease  also  may  have  contributed  to  the  decline  of  Brown  Pelican 
populations,  but  there  is  significant  evidence  that  pesticides  contributed  to  the  pelican  de- 
cline. 

Effects  of  habitat  alteration  are  more  difficult  to  assess  because  the  changes  are  often 
subtle  and  may  involve  an  alteration  of  the  food  web  rather  than  direct  avian  habitat  loss. 
For  example,  there  is  abundant  nesting  habitat  in  Texas  on  dredged-material  islands  (Chaney 
et  al.  1978),  but  waterbird  populations  are  declining  as  are  fishery  stocks  in  the  bays.  Most 
development  of  wetland  habitat  contributes  to  declining  avian  populations. 

Hawaiian  Islands. — Isolated  in  the  Central  Pacific  at  19°-29°N  latitude,  the  Hawaiian 
Islands  span  about  2400  km  from  the  largest  and  most  easterly  island  of  Hawaii  to  the  smaller 
Midway  and  Kure  atoUs  to  the  west.  The  10  larger  Windward  Islands  are  well  known  to 
tourists,  but  among  the  30  or  more  islets,  reefs  and  volcanic  pinnacles  making  up  the  Leeward 
Islands  are  the  most  unique  tropical  seabird  habitats  of  the  U.S.  Many  of  the  Leewards  were 
among  the  earliest  additions  to  the  National  Refuge  System  in  1909. 

The  Hawaiian  Islands  are  ornithologically  distinctive  for  their  many  and  often  rare  forest 
endemics,  their  evolutionary  offshoots  from  surrounding  continents  and  for  their  several 
species  of  threatened  or  endangered  waterbirds.  But  the  extensive  and  varied  coastal  areas 
serve  many  birds,  including  nesting  seabirds  and  migratory  shorebirds. 

The  indigenous  seabirds  include  two  albatrosses,  two  shearwaters,  three  petrels,  two 
storm-petrels,  two  tropicbirds,  three  boobies  {Sula),  one  Frigatebird  (Fregata  minor  pal- 
merstoni),  six  terns  and  noddies,  and  13  other  proceUariiforms  or  pelecaniforms  that  are 
migratory  visitors  or  stragglers.  Not  only  do  these  resident  birds  nest  on  the  sand  and  cliff 
shorelines,  but  some  endemic  races  move  inland  where  they  nest  in  peaty  burrows  (the 
threatened  Newell’s  Shearwater  [Puffinus  pujfinus  newelli})  or  rocky  burrows  at  elevations 
up  to  2900  m (the  endangered  Dark-rumped  Petrel  [Pterodroma  phaeopygia  sandivichensis]). 
The  smaller  Leeward  Islands  are  the  major  nesting  areas  for  several  species  of  seabirds  that 
are  present  in  tens  of  thousands.  Best  known  of  these  are  the  Black-footed  {Diomedea 
nigripes)  and  Laysan  {D.  immutabilis)  albatrosses,  which  once  nested  on  numerous  other 
Pacific  Islands  and  presumably  were  disturbed  by  human  activities,  just  as  a pioneering 
colony  of  Laysan  albatrosses  now  is  being  affected  on  Kauai. 

Five  species  of  shorebirds  regularly  winter  in  Hawaii,  and  33  have  been  recorded  there 
enroute  to  more  southerly  islands  and  continents.  However,  the  commonest  (Golden  Plover 
[Pluvialis  dominica  fulva])  and  rarest  (Bristle-thighed  Curlew  [Numenius  tahitiensis])  tend 
to  use  upland  areas  or  freshwater  shorelines,  respectively.  Several  widespread  species  use 


446 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


the  seashore,  including  Ruddy  Turnstones  {Arenaria  interpres)  and  Sanderlings  {Crocethia 
alba).  No  species  of  gulls  are  regular  winter  visitors,  although  numerous  continental  gulls 
and  terns  have  been  sighted  or  collected  in  Hawaii. 

The  historical  use  of  coastal  areas  by  birds  is  not  well  known,  but  there  are  examples 
suggesting  that  it  once  was  much  greater  on  the  larger,  now-inhabited.  Windward  Islands. 
Human  disturbance  has  discouraged  nesting  in  several  areas  in  recent  times,  but  some  re- 
establishment has  occurred.  Land-use  planning,  pollution  control  and  protection  are  partic- 
ularly essential  to  the  ecological  maintenance  of  Hawaiian  coastlines  with  high  bird  popu- 
lations. 

Alaska. — Because  of  its  size  and  configuration,  Alaska  has  diversity  in  climate  and  topog- 
raphy that  influences  how  birds  use  coastal  habitats.  The  northern  Beaufort  Sea  coastline 
has  severest  climate  in  the  region,  with  the  sea  being  partially  open  only  6-10  weeks  a year, 
and  all  but  a few  terrestrial  species  move  south  after  breeding.  The  southeastern  coast  and 
the  Aleutians  have  climates  moderated  by  the  sea,  and  although  severe,  they  provide  win- 
tering areas  for  a variety  of  coastal  seabirds  and  ducks  supported  by  nutrient-rich,  cold 
waters. 

Alaska’s  geomorphology  offers  diverse  coastal  habitats  for  birds.  Coastal  wetlands  may 
constitute  25-50%  of  the  area  of  the  Coastal  Plain.  Extensive  lagoons  formed  by  the  dynamic 
shorelines  and  bars  of  the  Arctic  Ocean,  and  Izembak  Lagoon  of  the  Alaska  Peninsula, 
harbor  most  of  the  Brant  {Branta  bernicla  nigricans)  of  western  North  America  during  post- 
breeding periods  and  spring  migration. 

Huge  river  deltas  such  as  the  Yukon-Kuskokwim  rivers  of  west-central  Alaska  and  the 
Copper  River  delta  of  the  south  form  extensive  wetland  habitats  on  land  and  enrich  extensive 
marine  areas.  Both  are  extensively  used  by  Brant,  shorebirds  and  other  near-shore  species. 
Rugged  cliffs  of  the  southern  coasts  and  many  offshore  islands  (Pribilof,  Aleutian,  St.  Law- 
rence and  Kodiak  islands)  offer  diverse  nest-sites  for  alcids,  kittiwakes,  guUs  and  cormorants. 

The  southeast  coast  and  fjordland  is  one  of  the  most  complex  shores  in  North  America 
because  of  the  offshore  islands  that  form  a protected  coastal  waterway.  Not  only  are  these 
areas  excellent  for  a variety  of  seabirds  and  shorebirds,  but  coastal  fisheaters  like  eagles, 
ospreys  and  gulls  are  prominent. 

The  Alaskan  avifauna  is  dominated  by  birds  that  use  the  coast  during  migration  as  well 
as  breeding,  and  includes  numerous  seabirds.  There  are  two  phalaropes  (Phalaropodidae), 
two  grebes  (Podicipedidae),  four  loons,  {Gavia),  one  fulmar,  three  cormorants,  two  storm- 
petrels,  more  than  30  waterfowl,  six  shearwaters,  two  petrels,  two  albatrosses,  three  jaegers, 
15  or  more  gulls,  kittiwakes  and  terns,  16  alcids  (including  six  auklets),  and  more  than  40 
shorebirds,  including  some  Asian  migrants.  Large  marine  bays  favor  concentration  of  South- 
ern Hemisphere  shearwaters  that  spend  the  southern  winter  in  the  north.  Bristol  Bay  is 
regarded  as  one  of  the  greatest  known  concentrations  of  seabirds,  numbering  annually  in  the 
millions,  and  the  Gulf  of  Alaska  is  a gathering  area  for  migrants  of  many  kinds.  Offshore 
waters  are  especially  important  for  Southern  Hemisphere  seabirds  that  migrate  from  the 
New  Zealand-Australia  area  to  the  Gulf  of  Alaska:  Slender-billed  Shearwater  (Puffinus  ten- 
iiirostris)  and  Sooty  Shearwater. 

Coastal  disturbance  and  modification,  until  recently,  have  been  mostly  confined  to  the 
southeastern  coasts  where  timber,  fishing  and  mining  interests  have  increased  activity  and 
modified  habitats.  Beginning  in  the  1960's,  exploration  for,  and  development  of,  oil  fields  in 
the  Cook  Inlet  and  the  Beaufort  Sea  have  produced  some  pollution  and  habitat  modifications. 

Coastal  Wildlife  Resources. — Increasing  focus  on  barrier  islands,  estuaries  and  sounds 
followed  President  Carter’s  environmental  message  in  May  1977.  In  it  he  called  for  an 
effective  plan  to  improve  protection  for  Atlantic  and  Gulf  Coast  barrier  islands  and  their 
associated  aquatic  and  terrestrial  habitats.  Subsequent  efforts  produced  a draft  report  (U.S. 
Dept,  of  the  Interior  1979).  It  shows  there  are  about  295  barrier  islands,  ranging  in  size  from 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


447 


I less  than  20  ha  to  more  than  40,400  ha,  from  Maine  to  Florida  and  along  the  Gulf  of  Mexico 
to  Texas.  About  200  of  these  islands,  totaling  98,400  ha,  have  some  developments,  while  95 
. are  undeveloped.  The  295  islands  in  108  counties  of  18  coastal  states  total  about  648,000  ha, 

I!  of  which  approximately  261,200  ha  are  managed  by  federal  (177,600  ha),  state  (72,000  ha), 

or  local  (11,900  ha)  governments  and  private  conservation  organizations.  Of  the  251,000  ha, 
the  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife  Service  administers  31  National  Wildlife  Refuges  totaling  157,000 
ha  that  contain  nearly  300  km  of  beach  in  12  coastal  states  from  Maine  to  Texas.  About 
• 389,000  ha  are  in  prviate  ownership  and  are  unprotected  from  future  developments. 

I Approximately  one-half  of  the  648,000  ha  is  wetland,  one-quarter  is  barren,  and  one- 
quarter  is  urbanized.  Because  of  heavy  demands  for  development  of  primary  and  secondary 
home  sites  and  recreation  resorts,  the  barrier  islands  are  urbanizing  at  a rate  twice  that  of 
the  nation  as  a whole.  This  unprecedented  growth  in  wind-  and  water-hazardous  locations 
is  (1)  exposing  the  owners’  personal  properties  to  high  risk  of  damages;  (2)  increasing  the 
potential  for  enlarging  taxpayer-funded  disaster  relief  payments;  (3)  usurping  and  continuing 
to  threaten  essential  aquatic  and  terrestrial  habitats  of  plants  and  animals;  and  (4)  com- 
pounding pollution  problems. 

I The  estuaries  and  sounds  protected  by  barrier  islands  from  ocean  winds  and  waves  are 
among  the  most  productive  and  richest  ecosystems  known.  They  provide  nesting,  resting 
1 and  feeding  areas  for  a broad  spectrum  of  birds  and  mammals,  as  well  as  spawning,  nursery 
and  feeding  grounds  for  a wide  variety  of  fin-  and  shellfish.  Because  of  their  importance  to 
i the  nation  as  natural  resources  and  because  of  growing  demands  for  housing,  recreation  and 
^ petroleum  developments,  the  Office  of  Coastal  Zone  Management  and  the  Council  on  En- 
vironmental Quality,  jointly,  have  produced  a data  atlas  on  the  eastern  United  States  coastal 
i and  ocean  zones  (Ray  et  al.  1980).  Each  of  approximately  125  maps  provides  information 
, organized  in  five  categories:  (1)  physical  environments,  13  maps;  (2)  living  environments,  five 
maps;  (3)  distribution  and  status  of  animal  and  plant  species,  68  maps;  (4)  economic  activities, 

■ 29  maps;  and  (5)  jurisdictional  boundaries,  and  management  and  protected  areas,  10  maps. 

I The  objective  of  the  atlas  is  to  identify  East  Coast  areas  least  suitable  for  major  energy  and 

I other  developments,  as  well  as  areas  that  should  be  analyzed  on  a site-specific  basis  for 

1 1 possible  special  protection  status  because  of  their  biological  and  ecological  importance, 

f ' Shorebird  wintering  areas  are  one  example  of  important  habitats  to  receive  careful  evaluation 

; i and  consideration. 

: I Current  plans  of  the  Office  of  Coastal  Zone  Management  caU  for  preparing  similar  atlases 
for  the  entire  Gulf  of  Mexico  and  Beaufort,  Chukchi  and  Bering  seas  off  Alaska  by  1982. 
I Further  information  on  these  plans,  as  well  as  copies  of  the  East  Coast  atlas,  can  be  obtained 

: from  the  Office  of  Ocean  Resources  Coordination  and  Assessment,  Office  of  Coastal  Zone 

Management,  National  Oceanic  and  Atmospheric  Administration,  Washington,  D.C.  20235 
(telephone  202/634-4120). 

Environmental  Perturbations 

Dredging  Activities. — Dredged  material  (=  spoil)  by  the  millions  of  cubic  meters  is  removed 
I each  year  in  the  creation  and  maintenance  of  channels  in  wetland  habitats.  Additionally, 

I dredging  is  employed  in  the  construction  of  harbors  and  marinas,  in  pipeline  rights-of-way, 

' I and  for  obtaining  fill  or  building  materials  (La  Roe  1977). 

I Dredging  activities  and  the  subsequent  deposition  of  spoil  can  affect  adversely  the  coastal 
1 ecosystem  in  many  ways.  Dredging  may  alter  water  current  patterns,  rates  of  water  circu- 
lation, change  mixing  and  flushing  patterns  and  affect  salinity  levels.  Further,  the  removal, 
transportation  and  deposition  of  sediments  often  produce  large  quantities  of  silt  that  remain 
suspended  in  the  water  column;  the  larger  sediments  settle  rapidly,  but  the  finer  particles 
may  be  carried  great  distances  over  extended  periods  of  time. 


448 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  9.L  Vo.  -L  September  1981 


Suspended  silt  can  smother  bottom-dwelling  plants  and  animals;  it  can  clog  the  gill  struc- 
tures of  fish.  High  turbidities  reduce  vision  and  can  mask  odors,  thus  affecting  the  welfare 
of  aquatic  species.  Most  invertebrates,  especially  filter-feeders,  cease  feeding  under  a regime 
of  high  turbidity.  The  silt  suspended  during  dredging  operations  decreases  light  penetration 
into  the  water,  thus  reducing  photosynthesis  and  basic  productivity. 

Estuarine  sediments,  which  generally  have  a high  organic  content,  are  aerated  by  the  roots 
of  marine  seagrasses,  but  fine  silts  can  smother  plants  and  seal  the  bottom.  Once  this 
happens,  the  upper  sediments  become  anaerobic  and  may  produce  toxic  hydrogen  sulfide 
deposits.  The  anaerobic  sediments  and  increased  biochemical  oxygen  demand  (BOD)  in  the 
water  column  caused  by  suspended  organic  matter  also  can  accentuate  the  reduction  of 
dissolved  oxygen  in  the  vicinity  of  dredging  operations. 

Under  normal  conditions,  marine  seagrasses  and  tidal  marshes  are  not  only  productive 
habitats  for  birds  and  other  wildlife,  but  they  also  provide  other  benefits.  These  habitats 
prevent  erosion  by  stabilizing  emergent  and  submergent  sediments.  They  also  act  as  an 
efficient  filtration  system  that  maintains  water  quality.  Removal  or  destruction  of  vegetative 
associations  in  wetlands  induces  instability  throughout  the  aquatic  system. 

Dredging  also  produces  material  that  requires  disposal.  Habitat  is  affected  two  ways:  (1) 
bottom  habitat  is  removed;  and  (2)  the  material  is  deposited  over  bottom  or  terrestrial  habitats 
elsewhere.  By  1967,  the  nation  had  lost  7.7%  of  its  wetland  habitat  and,  more  recently,  it 
was  estimated  that  23%  of  United  States  estuaries  had  been  severely  modified,  and  50%  had 
been  moderately  modified  (La  Roe  1977).  These  impacts  have  not  been  uniform  as  some 
states  have  experienced  disproportional  losses  (e.g.,  67%  of  California’s  estuaries  have  been 
destroyed). 

.\lthough  dredging  activities  can  have  disastrous  effects  on  wetland  ecosystems,  not  all 
effects  have  been  totally  detrimental.  Today  more  than  2000  man-made  spoil  islands  dot  the 
U.S.  coastal  and  inland  waterways  (Soots  and  Landin  1978).  Many  of  these  islands  have 
become  significant  breeding  habitats  for  wildlife,  most  notably  for  colonially-nesting  water- 
birds.  An  estimated  2 million  waterbirds  nest  on  dredged-material  sites,  mostly  along  the 
Atlantic  and  Gulf  coasts.  As  the  dredged-material  islands  develop  vegetation,  they  sometimes 
offer  alternate  nesting  habitats  similar  to  those  found  where  industrial  and  other  develop- 
ments have  destroyed  natural  systems. 

In  sum.  however,  dredging  was  once  largely  indiscriminate  and  without  measured  impacts 
on  a w ide  array  of  wildlife  and  delicate  ecological  systems  potentially  affected.  Cost-benefit 
ratios  almost  routinely  omitted  the  ecological  dislocations  incurred  when  dredging  was  pro- 
posed. and  any  fortuitous  outcomes  (e.g.,  man-made  islands)  were  random  and  unplanned. 
Only  recently  have  systematic  studies  of  plant  succession  and  the  related  nesting  require- 
ments of  seabirds  been  described  for  management  purposes  (Parnell  et  al.  1978.  Soots  and 
Landin  1978),  and  a useful  summary  of  dredging  impacts  on  birds  and  other  wildlife  was 
compiled  by  .\llen  and  Hardy  (1980). 

Besides  dredging,  coastal  environments  are  also  vulnerable  to  other  development  or  de- 
velopment-related activities  (e.g..  oil,  thermal  pollution,  waste  disposal  and  urbanization). 
Erom  1960-1974.  U.S.  offshore  oil  production  increased  from  4%  of  the  total  to  16.3%  (Clark 
et  al.  1978).  \\  ithin  the  next  15-25  years,  offshore  petroleum  may  account  for  40-50%  of  all 
domestic  production.  Major  offshore  areas  include  the  central  Gulf  of  Mexico,  Gulf  of  Alaska, 
west  Gulf  of  Mexico,  southern  California,  Mid-Atlantic,  east  Gulf  of  Mexico,  North  Atlantic. 
Bristol  Bay  and  Beaufort  Sea.  Effects  on  the  onshore  environment,  piers,  bulkheads,  beach 
stabilization,  roadways  and  bridges,  housing,  schools,  recreation,  etc.,  wdll  be  substantial 
(Zinn  1978). 

Thermal  pollution  from  nuclear  facilities  (4000  coastal  nuclear  parks  are  proposed  by 
\\  einberg  and  Hammond  1970)  affects  reproductive  cycles  and  growth  in  plants  and  alters 
composition  of  fish  communities.  Effects  on  bird  communities  are  relatively  unknown,  al- 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


449 


though  bird-species  diversity  decreased  following  intense  thermal  loading  of  an  inland  wet- 
land in  South  Carolina  (Gibbon  and  Sbaritz  1974).  Tbe  installed  capacity  of  nuclear  plants 
is  expected  to  increase  from  53  gigawatts  in  1978  to  207  by  tbe  year  2000.  Nineteen  plants 
are  licensed  to  operate  on  or  near  the  coast,  15  are  under  construction,  and  11  are  planned. 

Combustion  of  fossil  fuels  with  high  sulfur  content  has  increased  the  acidity  of  precipitation 
in  the  northeast.  A two-  to  five-fold  increase  in  metric  tons  of  sulfur  oxides  is  projected  by 
the  year  2000  (Cavender  et  al.  1973).  Effects  include  regional  decreases  in  numbers  and 
species  in  fish  and  invertebrate  populations  and  a reduction  in  forest  growth.  Effects  on  birds 
again  are  largely  unknown,  but  decreases  in  invertebrate  food  resources  may  be  a factor  in 
the  survival  of  juvenile  Black  Ducks  {Anas  rubripes)  in  coastal  marshes  (J.  R.  Longcore, 
pers.  comm.). 

Waste  disposal  (industrial,  agricultural  and  urban)  is  projected  to  affect  86%  of  nearly  700 
coastal  U.S.  estuaries  in  1980;  most  seriously  affected  will  be  the  Chesapeake  Bay  and  those 
along  the  South  Atlantic  Coast,  Florida  and  the  Gulf  Coast  (U.S.  Dept,  of  the  Interior  1970). 
Problems  associated  with  components  of  agricultural  or  industrial  pollution — DDT,  DDE, 
dieldrin,  polychlorinated  biophenyls,  kepone,  lead,  cadmium,  mercury  and  others — clearly 
involved  birds  in  recent  decades.  In  sum,  between  70-80%  of  the  U.S.  population  will  live 
near  coastlines  in  the  1980’s  (L.  Shank,  pers.  comm.),  emphasizing  the  need  for  coastal  zone 
management. 

The  Migratory  Bird  Treaty  Act  (1918)  is  one  of  four  major  legislative  acts  affecting  birds 
and  coastal  management.  Two  cases,  the  U.S.  vs  FML,  S72  F 2d  902  (2d  Civ.  1978)  and  U.S. 
vs  Corbin  Farm  Service,  444  F.  Supp.  SlO  (E.D.  Cal.  1978)  affirm  the  protection  of  migratory 
birds  from  toxic  pollutants,  even  without  the  intent  or  knowledge  of  the  actor.  Further,  U.S. 
vs  Brown,  S22  F 2d  817  1977  permits  Congress  to  enact  legislation  protecting  federal  lands 
from  “spill  over”  effects  of  activities  occurring  on  nearby  non-federal  public  lands  or  waters. 

Congress  enacted  the  Coastal  Zone  Management  Act  of  1972  to  establish  a voluntary 
federal-state  partnership  for  management  of  coastal  resources.  The  Act  encourages  coastal 
states  to  participate  in  the  development  and  implementation  of  comprehensive  coastal  man- 
agement programs.  By  the  end  of  1979,  75%  of  the  U.S.  shoreline  came  under  federally- 
approved  state  coastal  zone  management  programs  (Speth  et  al.  1979).  These  operate  to 
minimize  the  destruction,  loss,  or  degradation  of  wetlands  and  flood  plains  including  those 
within  the  coastal  zone.  However,  unsettled  interpretation  of  the  statute  has  lessened  joint 
planning  for  coastal  zones  among  federal  and  state  agencies  (Dedman  1979),  and  to  date. 
Congress  has  not  reauthorized  federal  assistance  to  state  coastal  zone  management  programs. 

The  Fish  and  Wildlife  Coordination  Act  (1934)  required  that  federal  agencies  give  full 
consideration  to  wildlife  in  major  water  development  projects.  Although  historically  ineffec- 
tive, the  Act,  strengthened  by  a presidential  water  policy  memorandum  issued  in  July  1978, 
now  mandates  consideration  of:  (1)  measures  to  conserve  wildlife,  (2)  alternatives  to  the 
project  and  (3)  the  implementation  of  conservation  measures. 

The  fourth  act  is  the  Endangered  Species  Act  of  1973,  as  reauthorized  for  three  years  in 
1979.  At  least  27  endangered  species  of  birds  breed  or  have  migratory  areas  in  coastal  zones 
(Woodard  1980).  Determining  critical  habitat  for  these  species,  however,  requires  consid- 
eration of  myriad  economic  and  ecological  issues. 

Petroleum  Discharges  and  Oiled  Birds. — The  most  publicized  oil  spiUs  are  tanker  accidents 
or  offshore  platform  blowouts,  but  millions  of  tons  of  petroleum  also  enter  the  marine  en- 
vironment annually  from  a variety  of  other  sources  (National  Academy  of  Sciences  1975, 
American  Institute  of  Biological  Sciences  1976).  Approximately  one-third  of  the  total  petro- 
leum entering  the  sea  is  introduced  as  a result  of  transportation  activities,  of  which  tanker 
accidents  comprise  only  a small  part.  River  and  urban  run-offs  account  for  another  third  of 
the  petroleum  entering  marine  environments.  The  remainder  comes  from  coastal  oil  refin- 
eries, offshore  production,  natural  seeps,  atmospheric  fallout  and  other  minor  sources. 


450 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Crude  oil  and  oil  products  are  composed  of  thousands  of  aliphatic  and  aromatic  com- 
pounds, each  possessing  its  own  set  of  physio-chemical  characteristics  (Lee  1977).  Some 
compounds  are  more  toxic  to  aquatic  organisms  than  others;  some  are  more  soluble  than 
others  and  rapidly  enter  the  water  column  and  contact  organisms.  For  example,  No.  2 fuel 
oil,  which  contains  toxic  and  soluble  hydrocarbon  compounds,  is  likely  to  have  a greater 
impact  on  organisms  than  No.  6 bunker  fuel,  which  has  fewer  highly  toxic  or  soluble  com- 
pounds (Nadeau  1977).  Thus,  the  effect  that  an  oil  spill  has  on  a marine  environment  is 
influenced  by  a number  of  factors:  type  of  petroleum,  volume  spiUed  or  released,  hydrography 
of  the  affected  area,  climate,  treatment  methods,  previous  exposure  of  the  affected  area  to 
petroleum  releases  and  habitat  type. 

Avian  mortality  resulting  from  petroleum  spills  was  reported  as  early  as  1910  (Bourne 
1968).  It  was  not  until  1967,  however,  when  the  Torrey  Canyon  accident  occurred  and  an 
estimated  40,000-100,000  seabirds  died,  that  the  problem  of  oil-induced  mortality  became 
widely  recognized  (Bourne  1970).  Many  birds,  particularly  those  species  that  spend  a great 
deal  of  time  in  the  water,  are  coated  with  oil  and  die  from  exposure  when  the  insulative 
properties  of  their  plumage  is  impaired.  Oil  also  can  be  directly  harmful  when  birds  feed, 
drink,  or  preen;  indirectly  when  consumed  through  food  chains,  or  when  applied  to  eggs  by 
incubating  adults.  The  literature  dealing  with  toxicity  of  ingested  oils  and  oiled  eggs  has 
been  reviewed  by  Albers  (1977)  and  Eastin  (1979). 

In  other  areas  of  the  world,  particularly  in  Europe  and  South  Africa,  oil  pollution  is  believed 
responsible  for  a steady  decline  in  seabird  populations.  With  the  current  and  projected 
demands  for  energy  in  the  United  States  and  with  increased  tanker  traffic  and  accelerated 
development  of  offshore  petroleum  reserves,  oil  spills  and  oiled  birds  will  become  more 
common  in  our  waters.  In  more  than  100  oil  spills  studied  throughout  the  world  between 
1960  and  1971,  approximately  20%  involved  50  birds  or  more  (Ottway  1971).  However,  es- 
timates of  avian  mortality  in  any  oil  spill  may  be  far  lower  than  actual  mortality — deaths  at 
sea  may  be  6-25  times  more  than  the  number  washed  ashore  (Tanis  and  Mozer-Bruyns  1968). 

Although  the  effects  of  oil  on  birds  are  the  subject  of  much  current  research,  few  investi- 
gators have  examined  the  results  of  oil  spills  on  bird  populations  and  their  habitats.  In  most 
cases,  reliable  pre-impact  estimates  for  the  affected  populations  are  scarce  (Bourne  1968, 
Vermeer  1976),  and  population  data  on  food-chains  are  virtually  non-existent.  For  example, 
oil-contaminated  sediments  almost  certainly  have  residual  toxicity  to  invertebrate  popula- 
tions, and  oiled  hard-surfaces  (e.g.,  rocky  seashores)  are  not  colonized  easily  by  attaching 
organisms  (Nadegu  1977),  all  potentially  affecting  the  avian  carrying  capacity  of  an  oil-im- 
pacted area. 

Although  the  “state  of  the  art"  for  cleaning  oil-soaked  birds  has  improved  eonsiderably 
since  the  Torrey  Canyon  disaster,  birds  stiU  cannot  be  rehabilitated  in  biologically  significant 
numbers  (Williams  1977).  Only  a small  percentage  of  oil-soaked  birds  are  ever  captured  and 
brought  to  cleaning  centers;  many  more  that  are  oiled  at  sea  never  reach  shore  (Tanis  and 
Mozer-Bruyns  1968,  Hope-Jones  et  al.  1970)  and  still  others  come  ashore  in  remote  and 
inaccessible  sites  (Chapman  1981).  For  these  reasons  most  recent  attention  has  focused  on 
deterring  birds  from  visiting  areas  of  oil  contamination  (Ward  1977). 

Unfortunately,  most  oiled  birds  cannot  be  captured  easily  until  they  have  become  some- 
what debilitated  by  oil  toxicity,  exposure,  or  starvation.  Therefore,  by  the  time  that  a bird 
reaches  a treatment  center,  the  odds  for  survival  are  already  low.  The  success  of  rehabili- 
tating an  oiled  bird  depends  upon  many  factors:  the  toxicity  of  the  oil,  the  degree  of  plumage 
fouling,  weather  conditions  to  which  the  oiled  bird  is  exposed,  the  time  elapsed  between 
oiling  and  treatment,  the  condition  of  the  bird  prior  to  oiling  and  the  species  involved.  Of 
equal  importance  are  the  presence  of  trained  personnel  and  the  availability  of  appropriate 
equipment,  supplies  and  facilities. 

Procedures  for  cleaning  and  rehabilitation  of  oiled  birds  have  been  detailed  by  Williams 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


451 


(1977,  1978).  Upon  capture,  the  initial  treatment  includes:  (1)  the  removal  of  oil  from  the 
nostrils  and  mouth  to  prevent  further  oil  ingestion  and  to  permit  unhampered  breathing;  (2) 
tube-feeding  a warm  solution  of  2-5%  glucose  in  fresh  water  to  provide  hydration  and  energy; 
(3)  taping  the  beak  shut  to  prevent  preening  and  oil  ingestion;  (4)  wrapping  the  bird  in  cloth 
to  reduce  movements  and  to  provide  insulation;  and  (5)  putting  the  bird  in  an  individual  box 
(placed  in  a quiet  sheltered  area)  for  transport  to  a rehabilitation  center. 

After  arrival  at  the  center,  each  bird  is  tube-fed  additional  hydrating  solution,  weighed 
and  banded;  an  oral  temperature  measurement  is  recorded.  Birds  with  temperatures  below 
38°C  should  be  held  under  a heat  lamp  producing  ambient  temperatures  of  29-32°C.  Cleaning 
of  a bird  should  not  commence  until  its  body  temperature  approaches  the  normal  range  of 
39°C. 

A solvent  such  as  Shell  Solvent  70  is  generally  recommended  for  cleaning  heavy  viscous 
oils,  for  large  birds  and  for  cleaning  large  numbers  of  birds;  detergent  is  recommended  for 
small  birds  (Williams  1977).  Care  must  be  taken  to  avoid  damage  to  feather  structure.  After 
cleaning,  each  bird  should  be  dried  thoroughly  with  hot  air  and  kept  in  individual,  warm 
pens  with  abundant  food  and  water. 

Cleaned  birds  are  kept  in  pens  until  they  are  free  from  all  injury  and  damage,  capable  of 
swimming  without  loss  of  water-proofing  and  are  in  physical  condition  adequate  for  survival 
in  the  wild.  During  the  rehabilitation  process,  which  may  take  many  months,  the  bird  must 
be  kept  free  from  stress.  However,  force-feeding  is  sometimes  necessary  during  the  first  few 
days  of  captivity. 

In  most  cases,  mortality  rates  during  the  cleaning  and  rehabilitation  process  are  high. 
Probably  the  highest  success  rate  (41%)  has  been  maintained  by  the  International  Bird 
Rescue  Research  Center  (IBRRC,  2701  Eighth  Street,  Berkeley,  California  94710).  However, 
few  coastal  areas  presently  have  the  equipment,  expertise  or  facilities  necessary  to  properly 
clean  and  rehabilitate  oiled  birds  and,  as  a result,  most  oiled  birds  succumb  to  the  toxic 
effects  of  the  oil,  exposure  or  shock  (Smith  1975). 

Oiled-bird  rehabilitation  can  be  expensive.  In  the  1971  San  Francisco  oil  spill,  approxi- 
mately $900  was  spent  per  successfully  released  bird.  During  that  spiU  95%  of  the  birds  died 
in  captivity.  More  recently,  however,  the  IBRRC  reduced  costs  to  $15  per  successfully 
released  bird.  It  is  not  known  how  well  rehabilitated  birds  survive  after  release.  Of  218 
banded  birds  released  after  treatment  following  the  San  Francisco  spiU,  14  were  recovered 
dead  within  a few  months  but  several  were  recovered  up  to  two  years  later  and  1046  km 
away  (Hay  1975).  • 

National  Estuarine  Sanctuary  Program 

The  Coastal  Zone  Management  Act  of  1972,  as  amended  in  1976,  authorized  a series  of 
nationwide  estuarine  sanctuaries.  Section  312  charged  the  Department  of  Commerce  to 
award  50%  matching  grants  to  coastal  states  for  their  acquisition,  development  and  operation; 
the  sanctuaries  would  be  owned  and  managed  by  the  states. 

The  primary  purpose  of  the  National  Estuarine  Sanctuary  Program  is  to  provide  long-term 
protection  for  natural  areas  so  that  they  may  be  used  for  education  and  research.  Within 
each  sanctuary  certain  alterations  such  as  dredging,  filling,  bulkhead  construction,  expansion 
of  existing  channels,  creation  of  new  channels  and  alteration  of  water  circulation  patterns 
will  be  prohibited.  However,  public  use  for  recreation,  sport  and  commercial  fishing,  hunting 
and  wildlife  observation  would  be  permitted  as  long  as  the  activities  did  not  permanently 
alter  the  natural  system  or  detract  from  its  educational  and  research  uses. 

The  research  value  of  such  protected  areas  cannot  be  overstated.  Undisturbed  estuaries 
permit  studies  of  naturally-functioning  systems  for  comparisons  with  disturbed  areas.  Clear- 


452 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


ly,  the  protection  of  these  sites  will  be  invaluable  for  birds  and  other  estuarine-dependent 
biota. 

At  least  20  protected  estuaries  were  chosen  to  represent  all  geographic  regions  (for  the 
purposes  of  the  program,  estuaries  was  defined  intentionally  to  include  the  estuarine-like 
areas  of  the  Great  Lakes).  At  present,  seven  National  Estuarine  Sanctuaries  are  in  operation. 
The  benefits  of  this  program  will  accrue  to  not  only  the  coastal  region  where  the  sanctuary 
is  located,  but  also  to  the  entire  nation. 

Recommendations 

Based  on  the  barrier-island  protection  plan  (Dept,  of  Interior  1979)  and  a review  of  state- 
ments provided  by  a number  of  conservation  organizations,  the  following  recommendations 
focus  on  actions  required  to  maintain  and  improve  the  management  of  aquatic  and  terrestrial 
habitats  associated  with  barrier  islands.  Individuals  and  organizations  with  a deep  interest 
in  populations  and  habitats  of  birds  and  other  natural  resources  have  a unique  opportunity 
to  help  perpetuate  these  resources. 

(1)  Request  Congress,  and  indeed  all  levels  of  government,  to  recognize  fuUy  the  special 
ecological  and  biological  characteristics  of  barrier  islands  and  their  associated  habitats;  and 
to  provide  a coordinated,  consistent  policy  and  appropriate  planning  procedures  to  avoid 
degrading,  destructive  and  inappropriate  developments.  Procedures  should  identify  means 
by  which  proposed  developments  can  be  designed  to  remain  consistent  with  conservation 
objectives  and  natural  resource  functions  and  values. 

The  need  for  new  ways  to  handle  barrier  islands  is  emphasized  by  a cumulative  and  tangled 
record  of  activities  and  jurisdiction;  nearly  20  federal  agencies  have  programs  that  impact 
barrier  islands.  One-quarter  of  these  agencies  administers  programs  that  directly  or  indirectly 
protect  barrier  islands.  Nonetheless,  more  than  one-half  of  them  administer  grant,  loan, 
permit  and  construction  programs  that  have  fostered  adverse  impacts.  Another  one-quarter 
administers  property,  insurance  and  disaster  relief  programs  that  encourage  or  perpetuate 
unwise  use  of  these  fragile,  dynamic  landscapes.  In  the  past  three  fiscal  years  alone,  nearly 
a half-billion  taxpayer  dollars  have  been  committed  to  barrier-island  developments,  many  of 
which  were  incompatible  with  the  islands’  physical,  ecological  and  biological  characteristics. 

(2)  Support  the  conservation  concepts  embodied  in  bills  (H.  R.  857  and  S.  96)  pending  in 
Congress.  These  bills  seek  to  establish  a barrier-island  protection  system.  The  objective  is 
to  improve  maintenance  and  management  of  the  unique  natural,  ecological  and  biological 
values  of  barrier  islands.  Eederal  subsidies  would  be  cut  off  for  developing  these  unstable, 
storm-prone  areas,  thereby  minimizing  risks  and  threats  to  human  life  and  property,  while 
simultaneously  perpetuating  wild  living  resources — including  bird  populations  and  their  hab- 
itats. 

As  part  of  its  coastal  barrier-island  inventory,  the  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife  Service  has 
identified  in  “concept  plans”  nearly  50  barrier  islands  along  the  Atlantic  and  Gulf  coasts  that 
have  exceptional  value  for  fish  and  wildlife.  Key  congressional  committees  handling  the 
barrier-island  bills  should  be  urged  to  provide  that  the  islands  identified  on  the  Service’s  list, 
if  ultimately  acquired  by  the  federal  government,  be  administered  by  the  U.S.  Fish  and 
Wildlife  Service  as  units  of  the  National  Wildlife  Refuge  System. 

(3)  Focus  special  attention,  with  research  and  other  educational  programs,  on  barrier 
islands  and  other  coastal  habitats  where  birds  and  other  forms  of  plants  and  animals  serve 
as  indicators  of  environmental  health.  In  particular,  the  National  Estuarine  Sanctuary  sites 
offer  functional  control  areas  for  many  types  of  research  that  may  act  as  biological  corner- 
stones for  desirable  legislation  and  protection  in  the  future.  Pristine  or  near-pristine  coastal 
environments  are  rarities  that,  with  study,  may  stimulate  the  orderly  recovery  of  already- 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


453 


degraded  sites  elsewhere.  Among  the  many  opportunities  is  work  dealing  with  densities  of 
birds  and  their  dependent  ecological  requirements  in  undisturbed  coastal  habitats,  d'he  pleth- 
ora of  real  or  potential  abuses,  particularly  those  related  to  petroleum  extraction,  suggests 
that  ornithological  activities  on  coastlines  will  be  especially  desirable  in  the  coming  decade. 


Literature  Cited 

Albers,  P.  H.  1977.  Effects  of  oil  on  birds.  Pp.  61-68  in  Proc.  1977  Oil  Spill  Response 
Workshop  (P.  L.  Fore,  ed.).  U.S.  Fish  and  Wdldl.  Serv.,  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FW’S/OBS/ 
77-24. 

Allen,  K.  O.  and  J.  W.  Hardy.  1980.  Impacts  of  navigational  dredging  on  fish  and  wildlife: 
a literature  review.  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv. 

American  Institute  of  Biological  Sciences.  1976.  Sources,  effects  and  sinks  of  hy- 
drocarbons in  the  aquatic  environment.  Washington,  D.C. 

Anderson,  D.  W.,  J.  R.  Jehl,  Jr.,  R.  W.  Risebrough,  L.  A.  Woods,  Jr.,  L.  R.  DeWeese 
AND  W.  G.  Edgecomb.  1975.  Brown  Pelicans:  improved  reproduction  off  the  southern 
California  coast.  Science  190:806-808, 

Bailey,  R.  G.  1978.  Ecoregions  of  the  United  States.  U.S.  For.  Serv.  Intermountain  Region, 
Ogden,  Utah. 

Bellrose,  F.  C.  1976.  Ducks,  geese  and  swans  of  North  America.  Stackpole  Books,  Har- 
risburg, Pennsylvania. 

Bourne,  W.  R.  P.  1968.  Oil  pollution  and  bird  populations.  Pp.  99-121  in  The  biological 
effects  of  oil  pollution  on  littoral  communities  (J.  D.  Carthy  and  D.  R.  Arthur,  eds.). 
Suppl.  to  Field  Studies  Vol.  2,  British  Petroleum  Industry.  London,  England. 

. 1970.  Special  review  after  the  Torrey  Canyon  disaster.  Ibis  112:120-125. 

Brown,  R.  G.  B.,  D.  N.  Nettleship,  P.  Germain,  C.  E.  Tull  and  T.  D.avis.  1975.  Atlas 
of  eastern  Canadian  seabirds.  Can.  Wdldl.  Serv.,  Ottawa,  Ontario. 

Casey,  .H.  D.  1969.  The  Channel  Islands:  their  discovery.  Oceans  1:69-77. 

C.AVENDER,  J.  H.,  D.  S.  Kircher  AND  A.  J.  HoFFMAN.  1973.  Nationwide  air  pollutant 
emission  trends  1940-1970.  Environmental  Protection  Agency,  Research  Triangle  Park, 
North  Carolina. 

Chaney,  A.  H.,  B.  R.  Chapman,  J.  P.  ICarges,  D.  A.  Nelson,  R.  R.  Schmidt  and  L.  C. 
Thebeau.  1978.  Use  of  dredged  material  islands  by  colonial  seabirds  and  wading  birds 
in  Texas.  U.S.  Army  Eng.  Waterways  Exper.  Stat.  Dredged  Material  Res.  Prog.  Tech. 
Rept.  D-78-8. 

Chapman,  B.  R.  1980.  Current  status  of  colonial  waterbird  populations  on  the  Texas  coast. 
Pp.  14-18  in  Management  of  Colonial  W'aterbirds  (J.  F.  Parnell  and  R.  F.  Soots,  Jr., 
eds.).  Univ.  North  Carolina  at  Wilmington,  Sea  Grant  Publ.  UNC-SG-80-06. 

. 1981.  Effects  of  the  Ixtox  I oil  spiU  on  Texas  shorebird  populations.  Pp.  461-4b5 

in  Proc.  of  the  1981  Oil  SpiU  Conf.  Am.  Petrol.  Inst.,  Washington,  D.C. 

Cl\RK,  j.,  j.  Zinn  and  C.  Tenel.  1978.  Environmental  planning  for  offshore  oil  and  gas. 
U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.  FW'S/OBS-77/12. 

CORW  IN,  R.  AND  P.  H.  Heffernan.  1978.  Environmental  planning  for  offshore  oil  and  gas. 
Vol.  V.  Regional  Status  Repts.,  Pt.  4,  California.  The  Conservation  Foundation,  Wash- 
ington, D.C.,  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS-77/16.4. 

Dedera,  D.  1970.  Santa  Barbara  and  beyond.  Oceans  3:17-32. 

Dedman,  j.  1979.  State-federal  relations  in  the  coastal  zone.  Pp.  175-184  in  Proc.  of  Gulf 
of  Mexico  Coastal  Ecosystem  Workshop  (L.  Fore  and  R.  D.  Peterson,  eds.).  U.S.  Fish 
and  Wildl.  Serv. 

Eastin,  W.  C.,  Jr.  1979.  Methods  used  at  Patuxent  Wildlife  Research  Center  to  study  the 


454 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


effects  of  oil  on  birds.  Pp.  60-66  in  Proc.  1979  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.  Pollution 
Response  Workshop,  St.  Petersburg,  Florida.  Washington,  D.C. 

Edge,  B.  L.  1972.  Coastal  zone  pollution  management.  Clemson  Univ.,  Clemson,  South 
Carolina. 

Erwin,  R.  M.  1979.  Coastal  waterbird  colonies:  Cape  Elizabeth,  Maine  to  Virginia.  U.S. 
Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS-79/10. 

AND  C.  E.  Korschgen.  1979.  Coastal  waterbird  colonies:  Maine  to  Virginia,  1977. 

An  atlas  showing  colony  locations  and  species  composition.  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv., 
Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS-79/08. 

Feldmann,  J.  H.  and  J.  J.  Hershman.  1978.  Environmental  planning  for  offshore  oil  and 
gas.  Vol.  V,  Regional  Status  Repts.,  Pt.  5,  Alaska,  Washington  and  Oregon.  The  Con- 
servation Foundation,  Washington,  D.C.,  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  Biol.  Serv.  Prog., 
FWS/OBS-77/16.5. 

Gibbon,  J.  W.  and  R.  R.  Sharitz.  1974.  Thermal  alteration  of  aquatic  ecosystems.  Am. 
Sci.  62:660-670. 

Gill,  R.,  Jr.  1979.  Status  and  distribution  of  the  California  Clapper  Rail  (Rallus  longirostris 
obsoletus).  Calif.  Fish  and  Game  65:36^9. 

Gosselink,  j.  G.,  E.  P.  Odum  and  R.  M.  Pope.  1974.  The  value  of  the  tidal  marsh.  Center 
for  Wetlands  Resources,  Louisiana  State  Univ.,  Publ.  LSU-SG-74-03,  Baton  Rouge, 
Louisiana. 

Graham,  F.,  Jr.  1976.  Seabirds  under  seige.  Audubon  78(6):52-73. 

Hanlon,  R.,  F.  Bayer  and  G.  Voss.  1975.  Guide  to  the  mangroves,  buttonwood,  and 
poisonous  shoreline  trees  of  Florida,  the  Gulf  of  Mexico,  and  the  Caribbean  Region. 
Univ.  Miami,  Sea  Grant  Prog.  Field  Guide  Ser.  3:1-29. 

Hay,  K.  G.  1975.  The  status  of  oiled  wildlife:  research  and  planning.  Pp.  249-253  in  Proc. 
1975  Conf.  on  Prevention  and  Control  of  Oil  SpiUs.  Am.  Petrol.  Inst.,  Washington,  D.C. 

Hope-Jones,  P.,  G.  Howells,  E.  I.  S.  Rees  and  J.  Wilson.  1970.  Effect  of  the  “Hamilton 
trader”  oil  on  birds  in  the  Irish  Sea  in  May  1969.  Br.  Birds  63:97-110. 

King,  K.  A.,  E.  L.  Flickinger  and  H.  H.  Hildebrand.  1977.  The  decline  of  Brown 
Pelicans  on  the  Louisiana  and  Texas  Gulf  Coast.  Southwest  Nat.  21:417^31. 

Kirby,  C.  J.  and  J.  G.  Gosselink.  1976.  Primary  production  in  a Louisiana  Gulf  Coast 
Spartina  alterniflora  marsh.  Ecology  57:1052-1059. 

Knight,  G.  1972.  The  Bay:  San  Francisco  Bay  and  the  emerging  regional  reality.  Oceans 
5:25-32. 

Knights,  B.  and  A.  J.  Phillips,  (eds.).  1979.  Estuarine  and  coastal  land  reclamation  and 
water  storage.  Saxon  House,  Farnborough,  Hants,  England. 

LaRoe,  E.  T.  1977.  Dredging — ecological  impacts  in  Coastal  ecosystem  management  (J. 
R.  Clark,  ed.).  John  Wiley  and  Sons,  New  York,  New  York. 

Lee,  R.  F.  1977.  Fate  of  oil  in  the  sea.  Pp.  43-54  in  Proc.  1977  Oil  Spill  Response  Workshop 
(P.  L.  Fore,  ed.).  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS/77-24. 

Nadeau,  R.  J.  1977.  Assessing  the  biological  impact  of  oil  spills:  a new  role  for  FPA 
biologists.  Pp.  55-60  in  Proc.  1977  Oil  Spill  Response  Workshop  (P.  L.  Fore,  ed.).  U.S. 
Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv..  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS/77-24. 

National  Academy  of  Sciences.  1975.  Petroleum  in  the  marine  environment.  Washing- 
ton, D.C. 

Osborn,  R.  G.  and  T.  \^  . Custer.  1978.  Herons  and  their  allies:  atlas  of  Atlantic  coast 
colonies.  1975  and  1976.  U.S.  Fish  and  Wild.  Serv.,  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS/77- 
08. 

Ottway,  S.  M.  1971.  A review  of  world  oil  spillages,  1960-1971.  Oil  Pollution  Res.  Unit. 
Orielton  Field  Centre,  Pembroke,  Wales. 


CONSERVATION  REPORT  1980 


455 


Page,  G.  W.,  L.  E.  Stenzel  and  C.  M.  Wolfe.  1979.  Aspects  of  the  occurrence  of 
shorebirds  on  a central  California  estuary.  Pp.  15-32  in  Shorebirds  in  marine  environ- 
ments (F.  A.  Pitelka,  ed.).  Studies  in  Avian  Biology  No.  2.  Cooper  Ornithol.  Soc. 

Parnell,  J.  F.,  D.  M.  DuMond  and  R.  N.  Needham.  1978.  A comparison  of  plant  succes- 
sion and  bird  utilization  of  diked  and  undiked  dredged  material  islands  in  North  Carolina 
estuaries.  Univ.  North  Carolina  at  Wilmington,  U.S.  Army  Eng.  Waterways  Exper.  Stat., 
Tech.  Rept.  D-78-9. 

AND  R.  F.  Soots,  Jr.  1979.  Atlas  of  colonial  waterbirds  of  North  Carolina  estuaries. 

UNC  Sea  Grant  78-10.  Raleigh,  North  Carolina. 

Perry,  M.  C.,  R.  E.  Munro  and  G.  M.  Haramis.  1981.  Twenty-five  year  trends  in  Ches- 
apeake Bay  diving  duck  populations.  Proc.  46th  N.  Am.  Wildl.  and  Nat.  Res.  Conf.  In 
press. 

Portnoy,  J.  W.  1977.  Nesting  colonies  of  seabirds  and  wading  birds,  coastal  Louisiana, 
Mississippi,  and  Alabama.  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS-77/ 
07. 

. 1978.  Black  Skimmer  abundance  on  the  Louisiana-Mississippi-Alabama  coast. 

Wilson  BuU.  90:438^42. 

. 1980.  Status  of  colonial  waterbird  populations  of  the  Louisiana,  Mississippi  and 

Alabama  coasts.  Pp.  10-13  in  Management  of  colonial  waterbirds  (J.  F.  Parnell  and  R. 
F.  Soots,  Jr.,  eds.).  Univ.  North  Carolina  at  Wilmington,  Sea  Grant  Publ.  UNC-SG- 
80-06. 

Ray,  G.  C.,  J.  A.  Dobbin,  C.  H.  Ehler  and  B.  J.  Basta.  1980.  Eastern  United  States 
coastal  and  ocean  zones:  data  atlas.  Off.  Coastal  Zone  Manage.,  U.S.  Dept.  Commerce, 
Washington,  D.C. 

Saila,  S.  B.  1973.  Coastal  and  offshore  environmental  inventory.  Cape  Hatteras  to  Nan- 
tucket Shoals.  Marine  Publ.  Ser.  No.  2,  Univ.  Rhode  Island,  Kingston. 

Sharp,  B.  1970.  A population  estimate  of  the  Dusky  Seaside  Sparrow.  Wilson  BuU.  82:158- 
166. 

Simon,  A.  W.  1978.  The  thin  edge:  coast  and  man  in  crisis.  Harper  & Row,  New  York, 
New  York. 

Small,  A.  1974.  The  birds  of  California.  Winchester  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

Smith,  D.  C.  1975.  Rehabilitating  oiled  aquatic  birds.  Pp.  241-247  in  Proc.  1975  Conf.  on 
Prevention  and  Control  of  Oil  SpiUs.  Am.  Petrol.  Inst.,  Washington,  D.C. 

Soots,  R.  F.,  Jr.  and  M.  C.  Landin.  1978.  Development  and  management  of  avian  habitat 
on  dredged  material  islands.  U.S.  Army  Eng.  Waterways  Exper.  Stat.,  Tech.  Rept.  DS- 
78-18. 

Speth,  G.,  j.  Yarn  and  R.  Harris.  1979.  Environmental  quality.  Tenth  Ann.  Rept.  Council 
on  Environmental  Quality,  GPO,  Washington,  D.C. 

Stevenson,  J.  C.,  N.  Confer  and  C.  B.  Pieper.  1979.  Decline  of  submerged  aquatic 
plants  in  Chesapeake  Bay,  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS- 
79/24. 

Sykes,  P.  W.,  Jr.  1980.  Decline  and  disappearance  of  the  Dusky  Seaside  Sparrow  from 
Merritt  Island,  Florida.  Am.  Birds  34:728-737. 

Tanis,  j.  j.  C.  and  M.F.  Mozer-Bruyns.  1968.  The  impact  of  oil  poUution  on  seabirds  in 
Europe.  Pp.  69-74  in  Proc.  Inter.  Conf.  on  Oil  PoUution  of  the  Sea,  Rome,  Italy. 

U.S.  Department  of  the  Interior.  1970.  National  estuary  study.  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl. 
Serv. 

. 1979.  Alternative  policies  for  protecting  barrier  islands  along  the  Atlantic  and  Gulf 

coasts  of  the  United  States  and  draft  environmental  statement.  Heritage  Conserv.  and 
Rec.  Serv.,  Washington,  D.C. 


456 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  3,  September  1981 


Van  Tets,  G.  F.  1965.  A comparative  study  of  some  social  communication  patterns  in  the 
Pelecaniformes.  Ornithol.  Monogr.  2:1-88. 

Vermeer,  K.  1976.  Colonial  auks  and  eiders  as  potential  indicators  of  oil  pollution.  Mar. 
PoU.  BuU.  7:165-167. 

ViSHER,  S.  S.  1954.  Climatic  atlas  of  the  United  States.  Harvard  Univ.  Press,  Cambridge, 
Massachusetts. 

Ward,  J.  G.  1977.  Techniques  for  dispersing  birds  from  oil  spiU  areas.  Pp.  113-124  in 
Proc.  1977  Oil  Spill  Response  Workshop  (P.  L.  Fore,  ed.).  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv., 
Biol.  Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS/77-24. 

Weller,  M.  W.  1964.  Distribution  and  migration  of  the  Redhead.  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  28:64- 
103. 

Weinberg,  A.  M.  and  R.  P.  Hammond.  1970.  Limit  to  the  use  of  energy.  Am.  Sci.  58:412- 
418. 

Williams,  A.  S.  1977.  Current  methods  of  oiled  bird  rehabilitation,  Pp.  125-134  in  Proc. 
1977  Oil  SpiU  Response  Workshop  (P.  L.  Fore,  ed.).  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  Biol. 
Serv.  Prog.,  FWS/OBS/77-24. 

. 1978.  Saving  oiled  seabirds:  a manual  for  cleaning  and  rehabilitating  oiled  water- 

fowl.  Am.  Petrol.  Inst.,  Washington,  D.C. 

Woodard,  D.  W.  1980.  Selected  vertebrate  endangered  species  of  the  seacoast  of  the 
United  States.  U.S.D.I.,  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  Washington,  D.C. 

ZiNN,  J.  1978.  Environmental  planning  for  offshore  oil  and  gas,  Vol.  2.  Effects  on  coastal 
communities.  U.S.D.I.,  Fish  and  Wildl.  Serv.,  FWS/OBS-77-13. 

Eric  G.  Bolen,  Chairman 
Brian  R.  Chapman 
Marcia  M.  Wilson 
Milton  W.  WeUer 
Laurence  R.  Jahn 
Chandler  S.  Robbins 
Fred  B.  Samson 


This  issue  of  The  Wilson  Bulletin  was  published  on  29  October  1981. 


The  Wilson  Bulletin 


Editor  Jon  C.  Barlow 

Department  of  Ornithology 

Royal  Ontario  Museum 

100  Queen’s  Park 

Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6 
Assistant  Editor  MARGARET  L.  May 
Senior  Editorial  Assistants  Gary  Bortolotti 
Nancy  Flood 

Editorial  Assistants  Keith  L.  Bildstein  Richard  R.  Snell 

C.  Davison  Ankney  James  D.  Rising 

Review  Editor  ROBERT  Raikow  Color  Plate  Editor  William  A.  Lunk 

Department  of  Biological  Sci-  865  North  Wagner  Road 

ences  Ann  Arbor,  MI  48103 

University  of  Pittsburgh 
Pittsburgh,  PA  15260 

Suggestions  to  Authors 

See  Wilson  Bulletin,  91:366,  1979  for  more  detailed  “Suggestions  to  Authors.” 
Manuscripts  intended  for  publication  in  The  Wilson  Bulletin  should  be  submitted  in  tripli- 
cate, neatly  typewritten,  double-spaced,  with  at  least  3 cm  margins,  and  on  one  side  only  of 
good  quality  white  paper.  Do  not  submit  xerographic  copies  that  are  made  on  slick,  heavy 
paper.  Tables  should  be  typed  on  separate  sheets,  and  should  be  narrow  and  deep  rather 
than  wide  and  shallow.  Follow  the  AOU  Check-list  (Fifth  Edition,  1957)  and  the  32nd  Sup- 
plement (Auk,  90:411^19,  1973),  insofar  as  scientific  names  of  U.S.  and  Canadian  birds  are 
concerned.  Summaries  of  major  papers  should  be  brief  but  quotable.  Where  fewer  than  5 
papers  are  cited,  the  citations  may  be  included  in  the  text.  AU  citations  in  “General  Notes” 
should  be  included  in  the  text.  Follow  carefully  the  style  used  in  this  issue  in  listing  the 
hterature  cited;  otherwise,  follow  the  “CBE  Style  Manual”  (1972,  AIBS).  Photographs  for 
illustrations  should  have  good  contrast  and  be  on  gloss  paper.  Submit  prints  unmounted  and 
attach  to  each  a brief  but  adequate  legend.  Do  not  write  heavily  on  the  backs  of  photographs. 
^Diagrams  and  line  drawings  should  be  in  black  ink  and  their  lettering  large  enough  to  permit 
reduction.  Original  figures  or  photographs  submitted  must  be  smaller  than  22  X 28  cm. 
Alterations  in  copy  after  the  type  has  been  set  must  be  charged  to  the  author. 

Notice  of  Change  of  Address 

If  your  address  changes,  notify  the  Society  immediately.  Send  your  complete  new  address 
to  Ornithological  Societies  of  North  America,  % Sandra  L.  L.  Gaunt,  Box  21160,  Columbus, 
Ohio  43221. 

The  permanent  mailing  address  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  is:  do  The  Museum 
of  Zoology,  The  University  of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  Persons  having  business 
with  any  of  the  officers  may  address  them  at  their  various  addresses  given  on  the  back  of 
the  front  cover,  and  all  matters  pertaining  to  the  Bulletin  should  be  sent  directly  to  the 
Editor. 

Membership  Inquiries 

Membership  inquiries  should  be  sent  to  Dr.  Keith  Bildstein,  Department  of  Biology,  Winth- 
rop  College,  Rock  HiU,  South  Carolina  29733. 


CONTENTS 


SUBSPECIES  VS  FORGOTTEN  SPECIES:  THE  CASE  OF  GRAYSON’S  ROBIN  {Turdus  graysoni) 

Allan  R.  Phillips  301 

HYPERPHAGIA  AND  SOCIAL  BEHAVIOR  OF  CANADA  GEESE  PRIOR  TO  SPRING  MIGRATION 

M.  Robert  McLandress  and  Dennis  G.  Raveling  310 

A MULTIPLE  SENSOR  SYSTEM  FOR  MONITORING  AVIAN  NESTING  BEHAVIOR 

James  A.  Cooper  and  Alan  D.  Afton  325 

FORAGING  SPEEDS  OF  WARBLERS  IN  LARGE  POPULATIONS  AND  IN  ISOLATION 

Douglass  H.  Morse  334 

DIFFERENTIAL  PASSERINE  DENSITY  AND  DIVERSITY  BETWEEN  NEWFOUNDLAND  AND  OFF- 
SHORE GULL  ISLAND Monique  /.  Vassallo  and  Jake  C.  Rice  340 

ENVIRONMENTAL  INFLUENCE  ON  SOARING  IN  WINTERING  RED-TAILED  HAWKS 

Charles  R.  Preston  350 

BREEDING  SUCCESS  IN  AN  ISOLATED  POPULATION  OF  ROCK  DOVES 

David  E.  Preble  and  Frank  H.  Heppner  357 

RELATIVE  ABUNDANCE  OF  GEORGIA  CAPRIMULGIDS  BASED  ON  CALL-COUNTS 

Robert  J.  Cooper  363 

NON-DRUMMING  MALES  IN  A RUFFED  GROUSE  POPULATION  Gordon  W.  Gullion  372 

GENERAL  NOTES 

BEHAVIORAL  IMPLICATIONS  OF  ABERRANT  SONG  OF  A RED-EYED  VIREO 


Jake  C.  Rice  383 

COURTSHIP  FEEDING  AND  COPULATION  OF  ROYAL  TERNS  Lawrence  Kilham  390 

TWO  CASES  OF  COMMENSAL  FEEDING  BETWEEN  PASSERINES Mark  B.  Robbins  391 


FOOD  FINDING  IN  BLACK-CAPPED  CHICKADEES:  ALTRUISTIC  COMMUNICATION? 

Millicent  S.  Ficken  393 

THE  SENTINEL  CROW  AS  AN  EXTENSION  OF  PARENTAL  CARE 

Gloria  M.  D'Agostino,  Lorraine  E.  Giovinazzo  and  Stephen  W.  Eaton  394 

BEHAVIOR  OF  A MALE  LEAST  BITTERN  INCUBATING  AFTER  LOSS  OF  MATE 

B.  T.  Aniskowicz  395 

NOTES  ON  BROWN  PELICANS  IN  PUERTO  RICO 

Ralph  W.  Schreiber,  David  W.  Belitsky  and  Bruce  A.  Sorrie  397 

EGGS  OF  THE  MARBLED  MURRELET  Lloyd  F.  Kiff  400 

FIRST  DOCUMENTED  CINNAMON  TEAL  NESTING  IN  NORTH  DAKOTA  PRODUCED  HYBRIDS 

John  T.  Lokemoen  and  David  E.  Sharp  403 

FIRST  RECORD  OF  THE  BLACK-CHINNED  HUMMINGBIRD  IN  ALBERTA 

Philip  H.  R.  Stepney  and  Peter  C.  Boxall  405 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE  407 

REPORT  OF  THE  CONSERVATION  COMMITTEE — 1980  438 


8 


TfieWIsonBulleftn 


PUBLISHED  BY  THE  WILSON  ORNITHOLOGICAL  SOCIETY 


VOL.  93,  NO.  4 DECEMBER  1981  PAGES  457-609 


MUS.  COMP.  ZOOU. 


The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
Founded  December  3,  1888 

Named  after  ALEXANDER  WILSON,  the  first  American  Ornithologist. 

President — Abbot  S.  Gaunt,  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  Columbus,  Ohio 
43210. 

First  Vice-President — Jerome  A.  Jackson,  Department  of  Biological  Sciences,  P.O.  Drawer 
Z,  Mississippi  State  University,  Mississippi  State,  Mississippi  39762. 

Second  Vice-President — Clait  E.  Braun,  Wildlife  Research  Center,  317  West  Prospect  St., 
Fort  Collins,  Colorado  80526 

Editor — Jon  C.  Barlow,  Department  of  Ornithology,  Royal  Ontario  Museum,  100  Queen’s 
Park,  Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6. 

Secretary — Curtis  S.  Adkisson,  Department  of  Biology,  Virginia  Polytechnic  Institute  and 
State  University,  Blacksburg,  Virginia  24061. 

Treasurer — Robert  D.  Burns,  Department  of  Biology,  Kenyon  College,  Gambler,  Ohio  43022. 

Elected  Council  Members — Richard  C.  Banks  (term  expires  1982);  Mary  H.  Clench  (term 
expires  1983);  Helen  Lapham  (term  expires  1984). 

Membership  dues  per  calendar  year  are:  Active,  $16.00;  Student,  $14.00;  Sustaining,  $25.00; 
Life  memberships  $250  (payable  in  four  installments). 

The  Wilson  Bulletin  is  sent  to  aU  members  not  in  arrears  for  dues. 

The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library 
Tbe  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library  of  tbe  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  housed  in 
the  University  of  Michigan  Museum  of  Zoology,  was  established  in  concurrence  with  the 
University  of  Michigan  in  1930.  Until  1947  the  Library  was  maintained  entirely  by  gifts  and 
bequests  of  books,  reprints,  and  ornithological  magazines  from  members  and  friends  of  the 
Society.  Now  two  members  have  generously  established  a fund  for  the  purchase  of  new 
books;  members  and  friends  are  invited  to  maintain  the  fund  by  regular  contribution,  thus 
making  available  to  aU  Society  members  the  more  important  new  books  on  ornithology  and 
related  subjects.  The  fund  wiU  be  administered  by  the  Library  Committee,  which  wiU  be 
happy  to  receive  suggestions  on  the  choice  of  new  books  to  be  added  to  the  Library.  WiUiam 
A.  Lunk,  University  Museums,  University  of  Michigan,  is  Chairman  of  the  Committee.  The 
Library  currently  receives  195  periodicals  as  gifts  and  in  exchange  for  The  Wilson  Bulletin. 
With  the  usual  exception  of  rare  books,  any  item  in  the  Library  may  be  borrowed  by  members 
of  the  Society  and  wiU  be  sent  prepaid  (by  the  University  of  Michigan)  to  any  address  in  the 
United  States,  its  possessions,  or  Canada.  Return  postage  is  paid  by  the  borrower.  Inquiries 
and  requests  by  borrowers,  as  weU  as  gifts  of  books,  pamphlets,  reprints,  and  magazines, 
should  be  addressed  to:  The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library,  University  of  Michigan 
Museum  of  Zoology,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  Contributions  to  the  New  Book  Fund 
should  be  sent  to  the  Treasurer  (smaU  sums  in  stamps  are  acceptable).  A complete  index  of 
the  Library’s  holdings  was  printed  in  the  September  1952  issue  of  The  Wilson  Bulletin  and 
newly  acquired  books  are  listed  periodicaUy.  A list  of  currently  received  periodicals  was 
published  in  the  December  1978  issue. 


The  Wilson  Bulletin 
(ISSN  0043-5643) 

The  official  organ  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  published  quarterly,  in  March,  June,  September,  and  December. 
The  subscription  price,  both  in  the  United  States  and  elsewhere,  is  S20.00  per  year.  Single  copies,  $4.00.  Subscriptions, 
changes  of  address  and  claims  for  undelivered  copies  should  be  sent  to  the  Treasurer.  Most  back  issues  of  the  Bulletin  are 
available  and  may  be  ordered  from  the  Treasurer.  Special  prices  will  be  quoted  for  quantity  orders. 

All  articles  and  communications  for  publications,  books  and  publications  for  reviews  should  be  addressed  to  the  Editor. 
Exchanges  should  be  addressed  to  The  Joselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library,  Museum  of  Zoology,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan 
4fll09.  Known  office  of  publication:  Department  of  Zoology,  Ohio  State  University,  1827  Neil  Avenue,  Columbus,  Ohio 
43210. 

Second  class  postage  paid  at  Columbus,  Ohio  and  at  additional  mailing  office. 

(c)  Copyright  1982  by  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
Printed  by  Allen  Press,  Inc.,  Lawrence,  Kansas  66044,  U.S.A. 


Capella  gaUinago,  the  Common  Snipe,  in  spectacular  ground  display. 
Photographed  at  a nest  near  Jackson,  Michigan, 
by  Betty  Darling  Cottrille  on  21  May  1967. 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN 

A QUARTERLY  MAGAZINE  OF  ORNITHOLOGY 

Published  by  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 


VoL.  93,  No.  4 December  1981  Pages  457-609 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  457-177 


ON  AERIAL  AND  GROUND  DISPLAYS  OF  THE 
WORLD’S  SNIPES 

George  Miksch  Sutton 

! While  reading  Leslie  M.  Tuck’s  monographic  The  Snipes:  a study  of 
the  genus  Capella  some  years  ago,  I found  the  chapter  titled  “Pair-for- 
mation and  mating  behaviour”  especially  interesting  (Tuck  1972:167-179). 
Every  word  brought  to  mind  the  bewilderment  I had  felt  while  watching 
! the  courtship  flights  of  Common  Snipes  {Capella  gallinago).  When,  more 
! recently,  Betty  Cottrille  showed  me  her  remarkable  photograph  of  a Com- 
mon Snipe  displaying  on  the  ground  near  its  nest  (see  colorplate),  my 
interest  sharpened,  for  I had  never  witnessed  any  such  performance  my- 
1 self.  So  explicit  was  the  photograph  that  most  of  the  tail’s  16  feathers 
could  easily  be  counted.  The  more  1 looked  at  that  tail  the  more  I wondered 
.1  about  the  part  it  might  take  in  aerial  displays. 

I first  observed  breeding  Common  Snipes  in  1922.  That  spring,  while 
studying  the  bird-life  of  Pymatuning  Swamp,  an  extensive  boggy  woodland 
in  northwestern  (Crawford  County)  Pennsylvania  (Sutton  1928),  1 was  sur- 
i prised  to  find  snipes  nesting  in  a cattail  marsh  near  Hartstown,  the  village 
.1  in  which  I was  staying.  The  Common  Snipe  of  North  America  was  believed 
il  in  those  days  to  be  of  a different  species  from  the  Common  Snipe  of 
I,  Eurasia  and  was  widely  known  as  Wilson’s  Snipe  (A.O.U.  1931:110).  On 
i the  very  first  evening  of  my  sojourn  (27  April),  I heard  many  snipes  “hoot- 
i ing.”  1 had  no  idea  that  they  were  nesting  in  the  area.  I assumed  that  they 
I were  courting,  that  pairs  were  forming,  that  presently  the  whole  noisy 
population  would  move  on  to  breeding  grounds  in  Canada.  The  hooting 
sounded  like  the  rapid  beating  of  wings.  At  times  it  was  so  sudden  and 
loud  that  it  was  almost  frightening.  Since  I had  heard  it  many  times  before 

457 


.1 


458 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


entering  the  marsh,  I knew  that  my  being  there  had  not  started  it,  but 
when  performing  birds  flew  low  over  me,  hooting  loudly  and  shot  off  sky- 
ward at  spectacular  speed,  I could  not  help  feeling  that  I had  been  threat- 
ened with  bodily  attack. 

Several  days  before  finding  a nest,  I watched  and  listened  eagerly,  for 
though  I had  seen  much  of  the  Common  Snipe  before,  its  courtship  be- 
havior was  new  to  me.  On  3 May  I observed  a “new  antic,”  a springing 
from  the  ground  of  a bird  that  “after  a few  energetic,  direct  wingbeats, 
put  his  wings  high  above  his  body,  and  describing  a graceful  arc,  dropped 
toward  the  ground,  his  legs  trailing,  only  to  rise  again  to  repeat  the  per- 
formance” (Sutton  1923).  It  did  not  occur  to  me  that  this  was  a “display.” 
A bird  collected  just  after  performing  the  “new  antic”  proved  to  be  a male. 
What  I had  witnessed  has  been  called  the  “arched-wing  display”  of  C. 
gallinago  (Tuck  1972).  According  to  this  author,  my  description  of  it  was 
the  first  to  have  been  published. 

Perusal  of  the  literature  convinces  me  that  this  “arched-wing  display” 
has  been  witnessed  many  times  in  both  North  America  and  Eurasia,  but 
it  has  been  variously  described  and  I may  never  have  seen  the  whole  of 
it.  In  Witherby  et  al.  (1941),  and  also  in  Bannerman  (1961),  F.  M.  Ogilvie 
is  credited  with  having  seen  a flying  bird  “sinking  gently  through  air  with 
raised  wings  and  legs  extended,  as  though  about  to  alight,  alternating  with 
turning  over  first  on  one  side  then  the  other  and  ending  with  turning  on 
back.”  Stubbs  (1912)  saw  a performing  bird  in  England  “on  some  six 
different  occasions  twist  completely  over  and  proceed  for  some  yards  with 
outstretched  wings  belly  uppermost.”  During  my  stay  at  Pymatuning 
Swamp  in  1922  and  in  Iceland  in  1958  (Sutton  1961)  I never  saw  a Common 
Snipe  turning  “completely  over”  in  this  way. 

What  I did  see,  and  in  both  places,  was  circling  aerial  display  accom- 
panied by  fervent  “hooting”  or  “bleating.”  In  Pennsylvania  all  of  the  per- 
forming was  done  over  cattail  (Typha)  marsh;  in  Iceland  I observed  it 
performed  over  flat,  low-lying  (but  not  marshy)  meadow  near  Reykjavik 
(Sutton  1961).  The  display  is  an  important  part  of  courtship  and  pair  for- 
mation and  possibly  of  territory  defense.  “It  occurs  sporadically  at  any 
time  of  the  year,  but  is  most  intense  and  continuous  on  the  breeding 
grounds.  It  is  mostly  a male  display,  and  males  can  be  distinguished  at 
this  season  by  their  frayed  middle  tail-feathers.  The  females  bleat  occa- 
sionally during  early  pair  formation  and  usually  after  the  laying  of  the  first 
and  second  eggs”  (Tuck  1972:167). 

For  years  I have  pondered  this  remarkable  hooting  or  bleating,  won- 
dering whether  it  has  ever  been  explained  fuUy  and  correctly.  Bahr  (1907) 
wrote  at  length  about  it,  naming  several  early  writers  who  had  expressed 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


459 


their  views  about  it,  and  paying  special  attention  to  a paper  by  Meves 
(1858)  (translated  from  the  Swedish  by  John  WoUey)  in  which  he  discussed 
the  “neighing  sound  which  accompanies  the  single  Snipe’s  . . . flight  dur- 
ing pairing  time  . . . C.  gallinago  was  sometimes  called  the  Single 
Snipe  in  those  days,  presumably  to  distinguish  it  from  the  slightly  larger 
Double  Snipe  (C.  media),  a species  that  breeds  in  Europe  and  western 
Asia. 

What  Meves  (1858)  had  to  say  was  thought  provoking.  According  to  him, 
opinions  concerning  the  Single  Snipe’s  “neighing”  were  varied:  “Bech- 
stein  thought  that  it  was  produced  by  means  of  the  beak;  Naumann  . . . 
that  it  originated  in  powerful  strokes  of  the  wings;  but  since  Pralle  in 
Hanover  observed  that  the  bird  makes  heard  its  well-known  song  or  cry 
...  at  the  same  time  with  the  neighing  sound,  it  seemed  to  be  settled  that 
the  latter  is  not  produced  through  the  throat.  In  the  mean  time,  1 have 
remarked  with  surprise,  that  the  humming  sound  could  never  be  observed 
whilst  the  bird  was  flying  upwards,  at  which  time  the  tail  is  closed;  but 
only  when  it  was  casting  itself  downwards  in  a slanting  direction,  with  the 
tail  strongly  spread  out.” 

This  paper’s  illustrations,  pen-and-ink  pictures  that  Wolley  (1858)  had 
“caused  to  be  drawn”  of  what  he  called  the  “musical  feathers  of  the  tail” 
in  six  snipe  species,  are  excellent.  And  entertaining  indeed  is  WoUey’s 
account  of  the  way  in  which  Meves,  in  “a  little  room  in  the  middle  of 
Stockholm,”  blew  on  these  feathers  and  fixed  them  “on  levers  that  he 
might  wave  them  with  greater  force  through  the  air,”  thus  demonstrating 
how  they  produced  the  “deep  bleat”  of  the  male  snipe  and  the  “fainter 
bleat”  of  the  female.  As  for  the  extra  wide  spreading  of  the  outermost 
feather  on  each  side  of  the  tail,  a spreading  that  has  been  illustrated  by 
drawings  from  time  to  time  (but  never  by  photographs),  neither  Meves  nor 
Wolley  had  anything  to  say.  This  outermost  rectrix  is  slightly  narrowed  in 
most  Common  Snipes  of  North  America  and  Eurasia,  though  not  in  all  of 
them  (see  Tuck  1972:83,  Fig.  24),  and  what  has  been  written  about  aerial 
displays  of  snipes  in  general  expresses  almost  universal  belief  that  the 
narrowing  of  the  one  to  several  outer  pairs  of  rectrices  is  responsible  for 
the  neighing. 

Bahr’s  (1907)  lengthy  paper  stated:  (1)  that  in  displaying  Common  Snipes 
observed  by  himself  in  England,  the  outermost  rectrix  on  each  side  was 
spread  so  wide  that  it  stood  apart  from  the  rest  of  the  rectrices  (1907:16, 
Fig.  3);  (2)  that  the  tail  muscles  of  C.  gallinago  make  possible  this  extra 
wide  spreading  of  the  outermost  rectrix  (1907:20,  Fig.  20);  and  (3)  that  the 
two  outermost  rectrices,  one  on  either  side,  though  believed  to  be  respon- 
sible for  the  neighing,  are  not  by  any  means  as  conspicuously  narrowed 


460 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


(1907:18,  22;  Figs.  4,  6)  as  they  are  in  most  other  snipes,  notably  the 
Pintail  or  Asiatic  Snipe  (C.  stenura),  a species  that  breeds  in  eastern 
Siberia. 

In  my  opinion,  the  importance  of  the  narrowness  of  the  outermost  rectrix 
in  C.  gallinago  has  been  overemphasized.  In  his  scholarly  paper  on  the 
aerodynamics  of  the  Common  Snipe’s  hooting,  Carr-Lewty  (1943)  so 
stresses  the  strength,  flexibility  and  narrowness  of  this  outermost  rectrix — 
in  contrast  to  the  wideness,  weakness  and  inflexibility  of  the  middle  feath- 
ers— that  I am  puzzled  by  the  photograph  in  Tuck  (1972:83)  of  a 14-feath- 
ered  tail  in  which  the  outermost  rectrix  on  either  side  is  almost,  if  not 
fuUy,  as  wide  as  the  other  12.  This  tail  is  that  of  an  “adult  male”  bird  from 
Ireland.  Might  that  particular  bird  have  been  incapable  of  hooting  because 
its  outermost  rectrices  were  not  narrowed?  Not  so,  in  my  opinion.  In  my 
opinion,  that  bird  hooted  by  fanning  wide  and  depressing  its  whole  tail, 
perhaps  switching  aU  14  feathers  from  side-to-side  as  it  went  into  a “pow- 
er-dive.” The  tail  of  C.  gallinago  must  indeed  be  equipped  with  powerful 
muscles,  for  in  ground  displays  it  is  spread  wide,  lifted  high  and  moved 
from  side-to-side  in  a truly  remarkable  manner  (see  Williamson  1950  and 
colored  frontispiece  of  this  paper). 

An  important  fact  about  the  tails  of  the  “true”  snipes  of  the  genus 
Capella  may  well  be  stated  at  this  point:  the  number  of  rectrices  in  more 
than  one  species  is  remarkably  inconstant.  In  the  Forest,  Marsh,  or  Swin- 
hoe’s  Snipe  (C.  megala)  of  Asia  the  rectrices  usually  number  20,  but  “oc- 
casionally 18,  22,  or  even  26”  (Tuck  1972:89).  Of  nine  specimens  examined 
for  me  by  David  M.  Niles  at  the  Delaware  Museum  of  Natural  History, 
four  (2  males,  2 females)  have  18  rectrices  each,  three  (1  male,  2 females) 
16  each,  and  only  two  (males)  20  each.  In  the  Pintail  Snipe  the  number 
is  usually  26,  “but  individuals  with  24  or  even  28  have  been  recorded” 
(Tuck  1972:91).  The  two  specimens  at  the  Delaware  Museum  of  Natural 
History  represent  the  extremes:  a male  has  24  rectrices,  a female,  28.  In 
all  “true”  snipes,  whatever  the  species,  there  is  a tendency  for  the  outer 
rectrices  to  be  narrowed,  but  the  tendency  is  less  noticeable  in  the  three 
geographical  races  long  believed  to  constitute  the  species  C.  gallinago 
than  it  is  in  most  other  species.  The  breeding  of  these  three  races — deli- 
cata  of  North  America,  faeroensis  of  Iceland  and  the  Faeroes,  and  nom- 
inate gallinago  of  continental  Eurasia — is  restricted  to  the  northern  part 
of  the  Northern  Hemisphere. 

Authors  seem  to  agree  that  in  C.  gallinago  vibration  of  the  outer  tail 
feathers — whether  these  are  narrowed  or  not — is  responsible  for  the  hoot- 
ing. Ludlow  (Ludlow  and  Kinnear  1934),  who  observed  courting  Common 
Snipes  in  Chinese  Turkestan,  was  so  close  to  performing  birds  that  he 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


461 


“could  see  the  vibration  of  the  outer  tail  feathers.”  Yet  Tuck’s  (1972:171) 
own  words  concerning  the  “somewhat  frayed”  condition  of  the  “two  cen- 
tral feathers”  of  the  tail  of  a male  that  he  netted  in  Newfoundland  on  17 
April  1960,  finding  that  he  had  banded  that  very  bird  “three  years  previ- 
ously at  the  same  location,”  read  as  if  he  considered  those  two  feathers 
themselves  to  some  extent  responsible  for  the  bleating. 

Be  that  as  it  may,  there  can  be  little  doubt  that  the  narrowed  outer 
rectrices  in  most  species  of  Capella  play  an  important  part  in  producing 
the  sounds  that  accompany  aerial  displays.  Morphologically,  the  most  bi- 
zarre of  the  world’s  snipes  assuredly  is  the  Pintail  Snipe,  referred  to  above, 
in  whose  tail  the  10  middle  feathers  are  broad  while  the  remaining  eight 
pairs  on  each  side  become  gradually  narrower  and  stiffer,  the  outermost 
being  mere  spikes  about  1 mm  wide  from  base  to  tip. 

The  earliest  account  of  this  species’  courtship  may  well  be  that  of  Pop- 
ham  (1898),  who  found  the  bird  nesting  along  the  Yenesei  River  in  Siberia. 
Concerning  the  aerial  part  of  its  display  he  wrote:  “1  never  heard  the 
Pintailed  Snipe  utter  any  call  when  rising  from  its  nest,  but  its  ‘drumming’ 
sounds  like  bubbling  water,  while  it  is  continued  much  longer  and  is  far 
louder  than  the  drumming  of  the  Common  Snipe.  The  bird  works  its  way 
to  a considerable  height  and  then  descends  rapidly,  ‘drumming’  as  it  goes; 
if  close  overhead  the  noise  is  terrific.”  Later,  Popham  (1901)  summarized 
his  observations  thus:  “The  drumming  of  the  Pintailed  Snipe  may  best  be 
described  as  resembling  the  sound  made  by  unwinding  the  line  from  a 
salmon-reel  with  rapidly  increasing  speed.” 

More  recently,  Berman  and  Kuz’min  (1965),  as  quoted  by  Tuck 
(1972:57),  reported  that  male  Pintail  Snipes  perform  communally  in  toks, 
a tok  being  the  aerial  equivalent  of  a lek.  Their  words  were:  “Males  in 
flocks  of  10  to  15  birds  flew  impetuously.  From  time  to  time,  the  whole 
flock  suddenly  plunged  sideways  or  each  bird  glided  downwards.  Maneu- 
vering in  a beautiful  manner  in  the  direction  of  the  wind,  turning  from 
side  to  side,  like  large  butterflies,  the  birds  plunged  more  and  more  ver- 
tically, uttering  short  metallic  calls,  tcheka-tcheka-tcheka.  As  the  speed 
of  the  birds  increased  the  cries  became  increasingly  more  frequent,  until 
they  merged  with  the  fizzing-and-whistling  sounds  which  originated  from 
the  cutting  of  the  air  by  the  narrow  tail-feathers.  This  sound  became 
stronger,  increasingly  higher  and  longer,  and  each  bird,  descending  almost 
to  the  ground,  stopped  dropping,  soared  upwards  and  caught  up  with  the 
flock.” 

In  their  monumental  Birds  of  the  Soviet  Union,  Dement’ev,  Gladkov 
and  Spangenberg  (1969)  do  not,  surprisingly  enough,  have  anything  to  say 
about  the  Pintail  Snipe’s  communal  displays.  Basing  their  words  on  a 


462 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


description  by  Dorogostaiskii,  they  state  that  the  circling  bird  produces  a 
sound  like  chvin,  slowly  repeated  at  first  but  gradually  becoming  “an 
uninterrupted  trill  of  beautiful  metallic  sounds”  and  ending  with  a “siz- 
zling” chiz-zh. 

Most  snipes  currently  placed  in  Capella  have  aerial  displays,  a notable 
exception  being  the  Double  or  Great  Snipe,  a comparatively  slow  flying 
species  whose  courtship  displays  are  largely,  if  not  wholly,  terrestrial  and 
whose  white  tail  corners  are  conspicuous  as  the  male  birds  shuffle  through 
the  grass  of  the  lek  in  the  half-light  (Blair,  in  Bannerman  1961).  The 
species  of  Capella  that  do  not  have  boldly  white-cornered  tails  all  take  to 
the  air  when  displaying.  The  aerial  performances  are  accompanied  by 
sounds,  but  how  those  who  hear  can  be  sure  that  certain  sounds  are  vocal 
while  others  are  not  is  beyond  me.  1 am  prepared  to  believe  that  some, 
perhaps  all,  of  the  windy,  feathery,  buzzing,  fizzing,  whistling,  whirring, 
bleating,  winnowing,  neighing,  or  drumming  sounds  are  produced  by  the 
whole  tail  or  part  of  it,  or  perhaps  by  the  wings  and  tail,  although  some 
of  these  sounds  may  be  vocal  to  some  extent. 

Note  that  performing  Solitary  Snipes  (C.  solitaria)  observed  by  Hume 
in  India  uttered  a “loud,  sharp,  jerky  call,”  then  descended  rapidly  “with 
quivering  wings  and  outspread  tail,  producing  a harsh  buzzing  sound  some- 
thing like,  but  shriller  and  louder,  than  that  produced  by  the  Common 
Snipe”  (Hume  and  Marshall  1881).  Presumably  that  “loud,  sharp,  jerky 
call”  was  vocal,  while  the  “harsh  buzzing  sound”  was  made  by  sudden 
fanning  and  depressing  of  the  tail  feathers.  A much  more  recent  observer. 
Baker  (1929),  also  seemed  to  believe  that  solitaria  made  two  different 
sorts  of  sound  while  performing.  His  words  were:  “In  the  breeding  season 
they  drum  and  bleat  over  their  breeding-haunts  like  the  Fantail  [a  common 
name,  widely  used  in  Asia,  for  the  Common  Snipe],  being  found  at  this 
season  between  9,000  and  15,000  feet.” 

According  to  Dement’ev  et  al.  (1969),  who  call  C.  solitaria  the  Hermit 
Snipe,  performing  males  ascend,  “flying  smoothly  like  a bat  and  describing 
small  circles;  then,  with  wings  half-folded  and  tail  spread  like  a fan,  the 
bird  plummets  downward.  This  is  accompanied  by  sharp  jarring  sound, 
and  as  the  drop  is  interrupted  by  several  pauses,  so  too  the  sound  is  not 
continuous  but  intermittent.  When  stiU  high  above  ground,  the  bird  halts 
for  an  instant  . . . and  emits  a loud  cry,  which  may  be  taken  for  call  of 
willow  ptarmigan  [Lagopus  lagopus].  The  sounds  may  be  rendered  as 
‘zhzhzh’  . . . (brief  pause)  ‘zhzhzh’  . . . (brief  pause)  . . . ‘zhzhzh’  (longer 
pause)  . . . ‘chok  . . . chok  . . . chaaa,’  the  syllables  ‘chok  . . . chok 
. . .’  jerky,  repeated  in  quick  succession  and  ‘chaaa’  uttered  after  a brief 
pause,  drawn  out  and  nasal.  After  this  the  male  again  soars  upward,  again 
plummets  downward,  and  so  on,  several  times  in  succession.  This  mating 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


463 


activity  has  much  in  common  with  that  of  Forest  Snipe,  but  Hermit  Snipe 
male  [gives]  louder  calls  and  flies  higher,  not  descending  to  treetops.”  The 
tail  of  C.  solitaria  usually  has  20  rectrices  (sometimes  22  or  24),  the  3-6 
outer  pairs  of  which  are  narrowed. 

Concerning  the  aerial  displays  of  the  Japanese,  Latham’s  or  Australian 
Snipe  (C.  hardwickii),  a species  with  18  tail  feathers,  the  outermost  three 
pairs  of  which  are  narrowed,  Bahr  (1907)  quotes  thus  from  the  notes  of  a 
“Mr.  Alan  Owston,  of  Yokohama,”  who  had  sent  him  a skin  of  the  species: 
“They  breed  on  the  grassy  moorland  at  the  foot  of  Mt.  Fugiyama,  at  an 
elevation  of  2000-3000  ft.  above  the  sea  ....  When  alarmed  they  fly 
. . . overhead,  circling  round  generally  against  the  sun,  and  every  now  and 
again  they  begin  to  cry  ‘chip,  chip,  chip,  sheep,  cheo,  che-cheo,’  and  then 
rush  downwards  at  the  intruder,  beating  the  air  in  the  descent  and  making 
a terrific  rushing  noise.”  Owston  also  sent  Baker  this  extract  from  T.  W. 
Blakiston’s  “Birds  observed  on  the  southeast  coast  of  Yezo  [Hokkaido]  in 
May,”  an  article  published  in  the  Japanese  journal  Chrysanthemum  for 
November  1882:  “The  Australian  species  act  very  like  the  Snipe  of  North 
America,  by  flying  round  pretty  high  and  making  sudden  descents  almost 
to  the  ground,  which  latter  movement  is  accompanied  by  a whisping 
noise.” 

More  recently,  Fennell  (1953)  calls  the  courtship  performance  of  C. 
hardwickii  a “circular  flight,  some  25  to  30  feet  above  the  ground,  accom- 
panied by  a rather  harsh  zrack,  zrack,  zrack  note  uttered  quite  regularly 
and  interrupted  only  by  the  rapid  ga,  ga,  ga,  ga,  ga,  ga,  ga,  ga,  ga,  ga, 
accompanying  the  frequent  power  dives.  The  latter  call  has  a rather  weird, 
feathery  quality  and  increases  in  both  tempo  and  volume  as  the  bird  nears 
the  ground.  A halting  sort  of  choke  interrupts  the  series  of  notes  some 
three  or  four  syllables  before  the  end,  adding  to  the  feeling  of  rush  and 
stumbling  haste.  None  of  the  performers  appeared  actually  to  alight  on 
the  ground  at  the  end  of  this  dive  but  seemed  to  veer  off  and  rise  into  the 
air  to  continue  the  circling  flight.”  Here  the  author  calls  the  zrack  a “note” 
and  the  ga  both  a “call”  and  a “note,”  making  us  suspect  that  both  sounds 
might  be  vocal.  For  me  the  word  “feathery”  describes  a non-vocal  hooting, 
drumming,  or  winnowing  not  unlike  that  of  the  Common  Snipe,  a sound 
produced  by  the  spread  tail.  As  for  the  zrack,  I can  only  guess  that  it  is 
wholly  vocal.  Since  C.  hardwickii  winters  widely  in  Australia  (Peters 
1934),  it  has  from  time  to  time  been  called  the  Australian  Snipe. 

The  Forest  or  Swinhoe’s  Snipe,  found  by  Gardner  (1930)  to  be  the  “most 
abundant”  of  the  snipes  “from  September  to  February”  in  paddy  fields  in 
the  Philippines,  was  said  by  him  to  make  a “whistling  or,  better,  winnow- 
ing sound.”  As  observed  by  Kozlova  (1932)  on  its  breeding  grounds  in 
northern  Mongolia,  the  species  “soars  up  into  the  air  to  an  immense 


464 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


height,  uttering  sounds  something  like  ‘tchiki-tchiki-tchiki’;  then  it  de- 
scends again  with  great  rapidity,  producing  a clear  whistling  or  howling 
noise.  At  about  7 A.M.  it  seems  to  become  tired  of  its  play,  and,  sitting 
on  some  dry  trunk  of  a tree,  continues  only  its  ‘tchiki-tchiki,’  without 
soaring  up  into  the  air.”  The  ‘tchiki-tchiki’  obviously  is  vocal  and  I strongly 
suspect  that  the  “whistling  or  howling  noise”  is  non-vocal.  A translation 
in  Tuck  (1972:55)  from  Koslov’s  “Fauna  of  the  USSR”  elaborates  a bit, 
explaining  that  the  performing  bird  “ascends  in  spirals,  closes  its  wings, 
spreads  its  tail  and  plunges  downward,  making  first  a low,  then  a con- 
stantly louder  noise  as  from  a rapidly  twirling  metallic  object.” 

Dement’ev  et  al.  (1969)  state  that  the  courting  Forest  Snipe  produces  a 
sound  like  chvi  or  chchvi  as  it  describes  “part  of  a circle”  then  “half  folds 
its  wings  behind  back  and,  with  a slight  loss  of  height  and  speed  beats  its 
wings  and  begins  flying  horizontally.  It  then  begins  a sudden  drop  accom- 
panied by  a sharp  sound  resembling  rustling  of  paper  kite.”  In  the  Forest 
Snipe’s  tail  the  outermost  four  or  five  pairs  of  rectrices  are  narrowed  (see 
figure  in  Tuck  1972:59). 

The  Wood  or  Himalayan  Snipe  (C.  nemoricola),  a somewhat  chunky, 
slow  flying  species  that  breeds  “in  the  Himalayas,  between  2,000  and 
12,000  feet,  from  northeastern  Punjab  to  the  southern  Shan  States”  (Pe- 
ters 1934);  that  has  been  recorded  in  winter  southward  to  “southern  India, 
southern  Assam  and  Burma”  (Peters  1934);  that  Adams  (1858),  who  called 
it  the  Solitary  Snipe,  considered  a bird  of  “lonely  glens  . . . where  the 
pine  grows  tall  and  dense,  and  the  sun’s  rays  seldom  penetrate”;  and  that 
Irby  (1861)  found  “in  little  rushy  patches  of  bog  on  the  sides  of  the  hiUs, 
never  on  streams”  in  May  1859  at  6000  to  7000  feet  in  the  Province  of 
Kumaon  [in  the  State  of  Uttar  Pradesh  in  northern  India,  just  west  of 
northern  Nepal]  is  surely  among  the  least  migratory  of  the  Northern  Hemi- 
sphere’s snipes.  It  is  “probably  a resident  bird  throughout  the  lower  Hi- 
malayas . . . between  6,000  and  2,000  feet”  (Baker  1929).  According  to 
Ludlow  and  Kinnear  (1937),  the  many  Himalayan  Snipes  observed  “in  the 
hiUs  west  of  Mago  [on  accompanying  map  shown  as  a district,  not  a town, 
in  eastern  Tibet]  in  early  August”  were  “flighting  like  Woodcock  of  an 
evening,  uttering  a croaking  ‘chur,  chur’  call.”  1 hazard  the  guess  that  this 
‘chur,  chur’  was  a non-vocal  sound  produced  by  spreading  and  depressing 
the  tail.  The  species  has  18  rectrices,  the  outermost  three  or  more  pairs 
of  which  are  narrowed. 

Of  the  seven  species  of  Capella  thus  far  discussed,  only  C.  gallinago 
breeds  in  both  the  Old  and  the  New  World.  The  three  above-mentioned 
races  of  C.  gallinago  are  all  strongly  migratory,  moving  southward  in 
winter  to  areas  largely  south  of  the  breeding  grounds  (see  map  in  Tuck 
1972:107).  The  species’  spread  across  two  large  continents  throughout  an 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


465 


area  cold  enough  in  winter  to  require  extensive  migration  bespeaks  har- 
dihood, aggressiveness  and  reproductive  potential  unique  within  the  ge- 
nus. Tuck  (1972:9-10  et  seq.)  obviously  believes  that  these  attributes  have 
led  the  species  to  establish  breeding  populations  also  in  southern  parts  of 
the  world,  a concept  that  1 find  acceptable  not  only  because  the  southern 
forms  are  much  like  the  three  northern  ones  morphologically — though  in 
all  of  them,  without  exception,  the  outermost  rectrices  are  more  conspic- 
uously narrowed  than  they  are  in  delicata,  faeroensis  and  nominate  gal- 
linago — but  also  because  the  aerial  part  of  their  courtship  behavior  is 
much  the  same. 

Tuck  was  not,  of  course,  the  first  ornithologist  to  believe  that  some  of 
these  southern  snipes  might  be  subspecies  of  gallinago.  Seebohm  (1886), 
whose  paper  on  “the  species  of  the  genus  Scolopax''  dealt  chiefly  with 
morphology  rather  than  behavior,  long  ago  had  this  to  say:  “The  last  half- 
dozen  [southern]  species  or  subspecies  . . . can  scarcely  be  regarded  as 
more  than  tropical  forms  of  the  Common  Snipe.  They  vary  very  slightly 
in  colour  or  pattern  of  colour,  the  variations  between  the  species  being 
scarcely  greater  than  those  within  each  species.” 

The  five  southern  snipes  that  are,  in  my  opinion,  races  of  C.  gallinago 
are  paraguaiae,  magellanica  and  andina  of  South  America  and  nigripen- 
nis  and  angolensis  of  Africa.  Whether  all  five  of  these  are  worthy  of  re- 
cognition is  beyond  the  scope  of  this  paper,  for  1 have  made  no  attempt  to 
borrow  series  of  specimens  for  comparison,  measurement,  etc.  The  five 
southern  races  resemble  the  three  northern  ones  closely  in  proportions, 
color  and  size.  Considered  together,  they  and  the  three  northern  races 
form  a composite  aggregate  quite  different  from  any  of  the  six  other  north- 
ern species  discussed  above,  and  they  are  sufficiently  different  from  the 
Madagascar  Snipe  (C.  macrodactyla)  of  Madagascar  and  Mauritius,  and 
the  Paramo  Snipe  (C.  nobilis)  of  the  northern  Andes  to  form  a discrete 
conspecies.  I confess  to  being  puzzled  because  breeding  of  the  five  south- 
ern races  is  not  restricted  to  high  southern  latitudes  as  that  of  the  three 
northern  races  is  to  high  northern  latitudes.  Not  one  of  the  southern  races 
is,  so  far  as  known,  strongly  migratory. 

Let  us  see  what  observers  have  reported  about  these  southern  races  of 
C.  gallinago.  The  earliest  comment  on  the  courtship  of  C.  g.  paraguaiae 
may  well  be  that  of  Dumford  (1877),  who,  having  watched  the  snipes  in 
northern  Argentina,  had  this  to  say:  “During  the  spring  they  go  through 
the  same  aerial  movements  as  the  Common  Snipe  at  home,  rising  to  a 
great  height  by  a circling  motion,  and  ‘drumming’  whilst  descending  in  a 
diagonal  line.”  Following  this  statement,  Dumford  asks  a pertinent  and 
thought-provoking  question:  “How  is  this  curious  habit  to  be  accounted 
for  in  the  South  American  and  European  forms  except  by  the  theory  of 


466 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


inheritance  from  a common  progenitor?”  Another  early  report  on  para- 
guaiae  is  that  of  Aplin  (1894),  who  became  acquainted  with  the  bird  in 
Uruguay.  According  to  him,  the  form’s  drumming  differed  from  that  “pro- 
duced by  the  English  Snipe.”  He  called  the  sound  “a  long  shaking  kurrrrrr 
(the  sound  can  be  produced  to  some  extent  in  the  back  of  the  human 
throat);  sometimes  it  varies  to  a deep  low  throated  gurrr  . . . .”  Hudson, 
in  his  “Birds  of  La  Plata”  (1921),  tells  us  that  performing  birds  “produced 
singular  grinding  and  scythe-whetting  sounds  ...  in  their  violent  descent 
from  a great  height.”  In  my  opinion,  the  words  “grinding”  and  “scythe- 
whetting”  both  aim  at  describing  non-vocal  sounds. 

Wetmore  (1926),  discussing  a “mating  display”  observed  by  him  in  Entre 
Rios,  northern  Argentina  on  9 October  1920,  wrote  that  the  birds  “flew 
swiftly  12  or  15  meters  above  the  ground  and  suddenly  extended  the  wings 
stiffly  in  a V-shaped  angle  above  the  back  and  fell  laterally  through  the 
air  for  a considerable  distance.”  How  this  graphic  description  of  the 
“arched-wing  display”  takes  me  back  to  the  hours  1 had  with  the  snipes 
in  northwestern  Pennsylvania  in  the  spring  of  1922! 

Pinto  (1935),  describing  the  behavior  of  paraguaiae  observed  by  him  in 
Bahia,  eastern  Brazil,  says:  “On  moonlit  nights  it  is  wont  to  entertain  itself 
making  swift  parabolas  in  space,  when  one  hears  a characteristic  guttural 
noise  that  is  responsible  for  the  dismal  name  Rasga-mortalha  [Death- 
rattle]  by  which  it  is  known  in  some  areas.”  A much  more  recent  observer, 
Barlow  (1967),  who  witnessed  the  “typical  aerial  courtship  flights  . . . each 
night  and  on  overcast  days”  between  29  April  and  13  May  [1963]  in  Uru- 
guay, called  the  sound  that  accompanied  flights  “winnowing.” 

Helmut  Sick  (in  litt.),  writing  of  paraguaiae  observed  on  the  snipe’s 
breeding  ground  in  Brazil,  says  that  the  displaying  bird  “makes  a strong 
noise  that  reminds  one  of  the  bleating  of  a she-goat.”  The  performance 
consists  of  phrases  that  ascend  in  pitch,  each  lasting  1 or  2 sec.  The  sound 
is  produced  by  a “channeled  current  of  air  . . . conducted  by  the  wings 
to  the  tail,  which  functions  as  a ‘musical  instrument’”  (see  Welty 
1975:211).  At  the  height  of  the  breeding  season.  Sick  tells  us,  male  birds 
call  ke-ke-ke  or  pi-kjer,  pi-kjer,  not  from  the  air  but  from  the  ground. 

On  the  courtship  of  C.  g.  magellanica,  a subspecies  that  is  “partiaUy 
resident”  in  continental  South  America  “from  Chile  . . . and  Argentina 
. . . south  to  Tierra  del  Fuego”  (Peters  1934);  that  Reynolds  (1935)  found 
“common  enough”  on  Guffen,  an  islet  just  north  of  False  Cape  Horn;  and 
that  Woods  (1975)  found  “fairly  common”  on  the  Falkland  Islands,  little 
has  been  published.  Cawkell  and  Hamilton  (1961),  writing  of  birds  heard 
on  the  Falklands,  report:  “The  drumming  note,  made  in  flight,  is  decidedly 
musical  and  is  produced  only  at  dusk  or  in  the  night.”  According  to  Tuck 
(1972:53),  a Reynolds  manuscript  comments  “that  sportsmen  who  are  fa- 
miliar with  both  gallinago  of  England  and  magellanica  cannot  differen- 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


467 


tiate  between  the  bleating  of  the  two.”  Woods  (1975)  states  that  Falkland 
Islands  birds  in  “nocturnal  display-flight”  circle  high  in  the  air  “producing 
a musical  bleating  sound  with  the  spread  rigid  outer  tail  feathers.” 

The  small  subspecies  C.  g.  andina,  which  presumably  is  largely  resident 
in  bogs  of  the  high  Andes  of  southern  Peru,  western  Bolivia,  northern 
Chile  and  northwestern  Argentina  (Meyer  de  Schauensee  1970),  is  consid- 
ered a full  species  by  some  taxonomists.  Nothing  seems  to  have  been 
published  about  its  courtship  behavior.  Judging  from  what  has  appeared 
in  print  about  its  ecology  and  distribution,  I suspect  that  it  is  locally  sym- 
patric  with  the  Paramo  Snipe  along  the  southernmost  edge  of  the  range 
of  that  much  larger  and  perhaps  more  slender  species.  About  the  Paramo 
Snipe  itself,  more  later. 

Concerning  the  subspecies  C.  g.  nigripennis,  a bird  long  known  as  the 
African  or  Ethiopian  Snipe,  whose  “drumming  or  bleating  noise”  is  “much 
the  same”  as  that  of  the  Common  Snipe  in  Europe  (Mackworth-Praed  and 
Grant  1952),  and  whose  Mallophaga  are  “identical”  with  those  of  the  Eu- 
ropean form  (Meinertzhagen  1952),  Thomas  Ayers  {in  Gurney  1868)  had 
this  to  say  of  courting  birds  observed  in  Natal,  South  Africa:  “At  this 
season  the  cock  birds  are  a great  deal  on  the  wing — evidently  wooing. 
They  fly  about  like  so  many  Swallows — rising  in  the  air,  and  descending 
with  a rapid  sweep  and  beat  of  the  wings  to  within  a few  feet  of  the  ground, 
then  rising  again  and  repeating  the  movement,  at  the  same  time  making 
a curious,  loud,  vibratory,  rushing  noise,  which  I once  heard  as  late  as 
midnight  on  a still  moonlight  night.  The  cock  birds  on  the  ground  almost 
incessantly  utter  a loud  ‘chuck,  chuck.’  ” Gurney  (1864)  himself  said  that 
the  flight  of  nigripennis  was  “precisely  like  that  of  the  common  English 
Snipe.” 

Cheesman  and  Sclater  (1935),  having  observed  the  courtship  of  nigri- 
pennis in  northwestern  Ethiopia,  report:  “The  drumming  cruise  takes 
place  not  more  than  30  feet  in  the  air  in  circles  of  300  yards  in  diameter. 
As  they  fly  they  fall  and  make  a whirring  noise,  repeated  six  times.  The 
fall  takes  them  almost  to  the  ground;  then  they  rise  again  and  repeat  the 
performance.  The  note  produced  does  not  seem  as  high  pitched  as  that  of 
the  English  Snipe,  and  does  not  resemble  a bleating  goat,  but  rather  the 
wing-beat  of  a swan  flying  in  the  distance,  but  more  rapid.”  According  to 
Breslford  (1947),  who  found  the  snipes  on  “sand-bank”  islands  in  Lake 
Bangweulu  in  northern  Rhodesia,  their  “drumming”  was  heard  ...  in 
July. 

I suspect  that  the  “chuck,  chuck”  reported  by  Ayers  {in  Gurney  1868) 
was  vocal  and  that  the  “curious,  loud,  vibratory  noise”  was  that  of  the 
flying  bird’s  tail  spread  to  its  fullest  and  pushed  downward.  In  view  of 
what  has  been  reported  about  the  Double  Snipe’s  use  of  its  white  tail- 
corners  in  terrestrial  display  (Bannerman  1961),  I was  prepared  to  find 


468 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


that  nigripennis,  with  its  largely  white  outermost  rectrices,  would  also  be 
content  with  displaying  on  the  ground — not  so,  apparently. 

Concerning  the  race  C.  g.  angolensis,  a very  long-billed  form  said  to 
breed  from  “Angola  to  Ngamiland  and  Northern  Rhodesia,  east  to  Ndola” 
(White  1945),  I have  no  comment,  since  I do  not  know  what  its  range  is 
now  known  to  be.  Some  of  what  is  quoted  above  may  have  to  do  with 
angolensis  rather  than  nigripennis. 

So  much,  then,  for  the  species  C.  gallinago,  the  one  snipe  of  the  world 
that  breeds  in  both  the  Northern  and  Southern  hemispheres,  and  for  six 
of  its  congeners  that  breed  only  in  the  Northern  Hemisphere.  The  con- 
geners that  breed  wholly  or  largely  in  the  Southern  Hemisphere  include 
the  Madagascar  Snipe,  already  mentioned,  a large,  slow-flying  form  en- 
demic to  Madagascar  and  Mauritius,  and  a remarkable  congeries  of  South 
American  forms  ranging  in  size  from  that  of  the  fairly  large  Paramo  Snipe, 
above  mentioned,  through  that  of  the  slightly  larger  Imperial,  Banded,  or 
Bogota  Snipe  (C.  imperialis),  which  is  known  from  only  two  or  three  lo- 
calities in  the  mountains  of  Colombia  and  Peru,  and  through  that  of  the 
still  larger  Andean  Snipe  (C.  jamesoni)  and  Cordilleran  Snipe  (C.  strick- 
landii),  respectively  of  the  northern  and  southern  Andes,  to  that  of  the 
strikingly  big  Giant  Snipe  (C.  undulata)  of  the  northern  part  of  the  con- 
tinent. C.  jamesoni  and  C.  stricklandii  may  be  conspecific:  they  resemble 
each  other  in  many  ways  and  are  nowhere  sympatric  (see  Meyer  de 
Schauensee  1970).  My  calling  C.  nobilis  the  Paramo  Snipe,  rather  than 
the  Noble  Snipe,  follows  Phelps  and  Phelps  (1958),  who  gave  it  the  Spanish 
common  name  Becasina  Paramera.  Paramo  Snipe  is  a meaningful  name, 
whereas  Noble  Snipe  is  not. 

To  be  noted  is  the  fact  that  while  the  above-named  Southern  Hemi- 
sphere forms  vary  greatly  in  size,  no  species  in  that  part  of  the  world  has 
rectrices  by  any  means  as  highly  specialized  as  those  of  the  Pintail  Snipe. 
Taxonomically,  the  most  puzzling  of  the  Southern  Hemisphere  forms  are 
C.  macrodactyla  and  C.  nobilis,  species  which  are  so  much  alike  that  one 
early  systematist  considered  them  conspecific  despite  their  being  a con- 
tinent removed  from  each  other  (Seebohm  1886).  Admittedly  it  is  difficult 
to  see  why,  if  the  process  of  evolution  eventuates  in  two  races  of  C. 
gallinago  in  continental  Africa,  it  should  not  also  eventuate  in  a third  one 
in  Madagascar;  but  macrodactyla  is  not  only  proportionately  longer-billed 
and  longer-legged  than  gallinago,  it  is  different  in  behavior.  From  Novem- 
ber 1942-April  1944,  van  Someren  (1947)  saw  much  of  macrodactyla  in 
the  mountains  near  Fianarantsoa,  Madagascar.  He  considered  its  flight 
“quite  unlike  the  sharp  zigzagging  of  the  European  Snipe.”  On  23  Novem- 
ber he  flushed  a parent  bird  from  a small  chick  whose  “clambering  and 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


469 


running  through  the  long  grass”  was  “unlike  the  cryptic  behaviour  of  the 
chicks  of  the  Common  Snipe.” 

I find  no  comparable  statements  about  the  behavior  of  the  Paramo 
Snipe,  a bird  that  may,  for  all  that  is  now  known,  be  sympatric  with  C. 
g.  andina  in  the  northwesternmost  part  of  that  bird’s  range.  Assuredly  no 
specimens  indicating  even  the  slightest  intergradation  between  the  two 
forms  have  been  reported. 

Certain  basic  attributes  common  to  the  habitats  of  macrodactyla  and 
nobilis — chilly  nights  at  the  high  bogs,  food  hard  to  reach  in  the  deep 
mud — evidently  have  continued  for  so  long  to  favor  the  survival  of  heavier, 
longer-billed,  longer-legged  individuals  that  both  forms  have  come  to  be 
much  tougher  than  Common  Snipes,  this  despite  obvious  similarities  in 
colors  and  patterns  of  plumage.  The  fact  that  the  two  resemble  each  other 
superficially  suggests  that  evolutionary  forces  have  operated  in  much  the 
same  way  in  two  far-apart  yet  ecologically  congruous  areas.  One  can  but 
wonder  what  the  precise  habitat-differences  may  be  between  C.  nobilis 
and  C.  g.  andina  in  the  montane  area  throughout  which  their  ranges  abut 
or  overlap;  the  latter  is  such  a little  bird  in  comparison! 

The  “nuptial  flight  song”  of  the  Madagascar  Snipe,  as  heard  on  9 Sep- 
tember 1930,  at  Doany,  Madagascar,  by  Rand  (1934),  was  “similar  to  that 
of  Capella  delicata  [Capella  gallinago  delicata  of  this  paper]” — a com- 
ment that  seems  to  argue  for  calling  macrodactyla  a race  of  gallinago. 
But  if,  as  Rand  states,  the  native  names  of  the  bird,  Harakaraka  and  Rava 
rara,  are  indeed  “imitations  of  the  flight  song,”  1 cannot  help  feeling  that 
the  sounds  most  often  accompanying  aerial  courtship  must  differ  radically 
from  the  Common  Snipe’s  hu-hu-hu-hu-hu  that  1 have  heard  so  many 
times.  Rand  (1934)  may  have  erred  in  supposing  that  the  native  names 
imitated  the  flight  song.  According  to  Harting  (1882),  the  name  used  by 
natives  at  Fianarantsoa  was  kekekeka  (presumably  in  imitation  of  a call 
given  from  the  ground),  the  name  rava-rava  being  used  not  for  any  “true” 
snipe,  but  for  the  Painted  Snipe  {Rostratula  benghalensis) . As  for  the 
spelling  rava-rava,  rather  than  rava  rara,  see  Newton  (1865). 

Almost  nothing  has  been  published  on  the  behavior  of  the  Paramo  Snipe, 
a species  found  by  Moore  (1934)  to  be  “the  most  conspicuous  bird”  in  a 
valley  at  11,000  feet  in  the  vicinity  of  Mt.  Sangay  in  the  Ecuadorean  Andes. 
Moore’s  statement  that  “at  sundown”  the  snipes’  “ecstatic  forms  whirled 
overhead  to  the  accompaniment  of  strange  sounds  that  reminded  one  of 
a deep-pitched  policeman’s  rattle”  may  not  pertain  at  all  to  the  Paramo 
Snipe,  for  an  elderly  Indian  of  Moore’s  party  insisted  that  the  sound  was 
made  by  a much  larger  bird,  the  sympatric  Andean  Snipe.  Whether  what 
Moore  heard  was  Paramo  Snipes  or  Andean  Snipes  (or  both!),  the  sound 


470 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


could  not  have  been  much  like  that  of  courting  C.  gallinago.  According 
to  Harry  Lumsden,  who  heard  the  Paramo  Snipe’s  “bleating”  in  Colombia, 
the  sound  was  “very  low  and  deep  in  tone”  (Tuck  1972:57). 

The  Strickland’s  or  Cordilleran  Snipe  (C.  stricklandii),  as  observed  by 
Reynolds  (1935)  on  six  of  the  Wallaston  Islands  at  South  America’s  south- 
ernmost tip  between  11  and  22  December  1922,  was  “heard  continuously 
throughout  the  night  . . . when  the  wind  was  not  roaring.”  On  Herschel 
Island,  while  one  bird  was  “drumming”  overhead,  another  bird,  thought 
to  be  the  female,  “kept  up  a continuous  ‘chip-chip-chip’  etc.  from  the 
ground.”  This  same  “chip-chip-chip”  was  “uttered  frequently  in  flight,” 
followed  by  drumming  of  such  exceedingly  low  pitch  as  almost  to  reach 
“the  lowest  limit  of  human  audibility.”  A loud  cha-wheu  or  cha-whoo, 
cha-whoo,  cha-whoo  repeated  a number  of  times  was  distinctly  audible 
when  the  drumming  could  “no  longer  be  picked  up.”  How  Reynolds  (1935) 
knew  that  the  drumming  was  continuing  when  he  could  no  longer  hear  it 
is  not  clear  to  me.  In  my  opinion,  the  cha-whoo  was  produced  by  the 
outspread  tail,  which  has  14  feathers,  none  of  them  noticeably  narrowed 
or  stiffened  (see  figure  in  Tuck  1972:71). 

The  Andean  or  Jameson’s  Snipe  (C.  jamesoni)  of  the  northern  Andes, 
as  observed  by  Vuilleumier  (1969)  in  “wooded  thickets  and  grassy  openings 
at  altitudes  from  3,300  to  3,400  m.”  in  the  Bolivian  Andes,  gave  a double 
note,  a whee-tschwu,  “repeated  at  a frequency  of  about  two  per  second, 
while  the  calling  bird  flies  in  wide  circles  on  a level  course.”  After  calling 
constantly  for  30  sec  to  a full  minute,  the  circling  bird  began  to  descend, 
slowly  at  first,  but  gaining  speed.  As  it  neared  the  ground  a “muffled,  low- 
pitched  sound  which  vaguely  reminds  one  of  a cow’s  bellow”  became 
audible.  Vuilleumier  (1969)  presumed  that  this  low-pitched  sound  was 
“produced  by  the  vibration  of  feathers,  and  not  vocally,  although  neither 
tail  nor  wing  feathers  show  obvious  modifications.”  To  be  noted  is  the 
significant  fact  that  the  second  syllable  of  this  whee-tschu  of  jamesoni 
rhymes  with  the  cha-whoo  of  stricklandii  (see  paragraph  above).  The  tail 
of  jamesoni  also  has  14  feathers,  the  outermost  three  pairs  of  which  are 
somewhat  narrowed  in  a specimen  from  Colombia  (USNM  386788)  at  hand. 
If  the  outermost  rectrices  are  narrowed  in  most  jamesoni  but  not  in  most 
stricklandii  the  difference  would,  in  my  opinion,  argue  for  calling  the  two 
forms  separate  species. 

The  aerial  displays  of  the  little  known  Imperial  Snipe  (C.  imperialis), 
as  witnessed  in  July  1968  “just  below  the  timberline  at  3,300  m (10,000 
feet)”  in  the  “vast  and  largely  unexplored  northern  massif  of  the  Cordillera 
Vilcabamba”  of  central  Peru,  were  “of  equal  intensity  at  dawn  and  dusk,” 
reaching  “peak  intensity”  in  clear  weather,  “heavy  cover  almost  entirely 
squelching  the  usual  performance.”  The  display  flight  was  accompanied 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


471 


by  a “song”  that  began  “with  a series  of  rough  staccato  notes  that  rapidly 
increase  in  volume.  A climactic  middle  section  is  marked  by  a complex 
rhythmic  pattern  of  double  and  triple  notes.  After  a final  triple  burst,  the 
song  enters  a terminal  phase  in  which  the  sound  intensity  diminishes  in 
a sequence  of  evenly  spaced  notes”  (Terborgh  and  Weske  1972).  These 
authors  obviously  believed  this  “song”  to  be  wholly  vocal.  According  to 
them,  the  “first  two-thirds”  of  it,  “comprising  the  crescendo  and  climactic 
phases,  are  given  in  level  flight  powered  by  rapid  shallow  beats  of  the 
stiffly  held  wings.  A gently  sloping  dive  commences  with  the  terminal 
sequence  of  single  notes.  An  instant  after  the  last  note  of  the  vocalization 
the  bird  puUs  sharply  out  of  the  dive,  producing  a rush  of  air  through  the 
remiges  (?)  that  is  clearly  audible  at  close  range.” 

The  “rush  of  air”  might,  in  my  opinion,  have  been  through  the  wide- 
spread and  depressed  tail  feathers,  an  opinion  based  on  my  belief  that  in 
most  species  of  Capella  the  rectrices  are  used  in  this  way.  Terborgh  and 
Weske  (1972)  consider  the  aerial  display  of  imperialis  similar  “in  several 
respects”  to  that  of  jamesoni  (see  quoted  material  above);  they  say  nothing 
about  the  courtship  of  the  big  snipe  of  the  southern  Andes  as  such,  for 
they  consider  jamesoni  a geographical  race  of  stricklandii.  Concerning 
imperialis  and  “C.  stricklandii  jamesoni'’  they  have  this  to  say:  “Both 
species  display  after  sundown  well  into  darkness  and  call  repeatedly  while 
flying  in  wide  nearly  level  circles”;  vocalizations  of  jamesoni  “are  appar- 
ently given  continuously  for  several  circuits,”  while  those  of  imperialis 
“are  more  complex  and  divided  into  discrete  episodes.  Both  species  pro- 
duce a low  whirring  sound  while  descending,  presumably  by  allowing  air 
to  pass  through  the  remiges  in  a certain  way” — imperialis  “at  the  end  of 
each  song  bout,”  jamesoni  “at  the  termination  of  a 30-  to  60-second  display 
period  as  it  spirals  back  to  earth.” 

1 cannot  dismiss  from  discussion  these  three  large,  somewhat  stocky 
South  American  snipes — imperialis,  jamesoni,  and  stricklandii — without 
mentioning  the  fact  that  Peters  (1934)  placed  them  in  the  genus  Chubbia, 
a taxon  erected  by  Mathews  (1913).  Insofar  as  their  courtship  behavior  is 
concerned,  there  seems  to  be  some  justification  for  considering  Chubbia 
a valid  genus.  The  three  species  resemble  woodcocks  of  the  genera  Scol- 
opax  and  Philohela  in  being  proportionately  shorter-tailed  and  shorter- 
winged  than  the  several  other  snipes  discussed  in  this  paper. 

Now  for  the  dramatically  big  Giant  Snipe  (C.  undulata),  a bird  whose 
habits  have  been  virtually  unreported.  The  species’  disproportionately 
short  tail  has  14  feathers,  the  outermost  two  or  three  pairs  of  which  are 
somewhat,  though  not  noticeably,  narrowed  (see  figure  in  Tuck  1972). 
Helmut  Sick  (in  litt.),  writing  of  the  bird’s  behavior  as  observed  by  him  in 
Brazil,  tells  us  that  it  is  “by  nature  lazy”;  that,  rather  than  flushing,  it 


472 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


“squats  or  escapes  by  walking  slowly,  taking  long  steps”;  that  it  is  “even 
more  nocturnal”  than  its  sympatric  congener,  C.  g.  paraguaiae;  and  that 
it  does  most  of  its  performing  on  “hot  rainy  nights.”  In  courtship  displays 
“high  above  its  territory”  it  produces  a sound  that  resembles  the  phrase 
ho-go,  go  or  ga-ga,  ga,  loud  at  the  beginning,  but  trailing  off  at  the  end, 
and  with  a timbre  so  much  like  that  of  the  human  voice  that  one  cannot 
help  feeling  that  it  is  vocal.  The  sound,  whether  vocal  or  not,  is  responsible 
for  the  vernacular  names  Agua-s6,  0-rapaz  and  Rola-pau.  In  addition  to 
this  trisyllabic  phrase,  the  bird  produces  a “strong  droning  sch  that  lasts 
four  seconds,  a sound  that  might  be  compared  to  . . . the  buzzing  of  a 
large  swarm  of  bees.”  The  general  appearance  of  this  very  large  snipe 
certainly  calls  woodcocks  to  mind.  In  the  one  specimen  of  the  species  at 
hand,  the  rectrices  are  hard  to  count  for  they  are  hidden  by  the  long  and 
abundant  coverts. 

The  little  Jack  Snipe  {Lymnocryptes  minimus)  of  Eurasia  is  sometimes 
placed  in  Capella  (Edwards  1974),  but  it  is  so  unlike  the  several  other 
snipes  already  discussed  that  it  may  well  belong  in  a genus  by  itself.  It  is 
famous  for  the  “cantering”  sounds  that  it  makes  while  courting.  It  has 
only  12  tail  feathers,  aU  soft  and  somewhat  pointed,  none  noticeably  nar- 
rowed. The  “cantering,”  which  has  been  transliterated  as  “lock-toggi, 
lock-toggi”  and  “clockety-clockey,  clockety-clock”  by  Blair  {in  Banner- 
man  1961),  and  which  must  be  vocal  since  it  is  given  from  the  ground  as 
well  as  from  the  air,  is  not,  apparently,  analogous  to  the  bleating,  hooting, 
or  drumming  of  C.  gallinago  and  most  of  that  bird’s  congeners. 

According  to  Blair  {in  Bannerman  1961),  the  Jack  Snipe  was  once  known 
in  parts  of  its  extensive  range  as  the  Silent  Snipe,  for  it  was  believed  to 
be  voiceless  during  fall  and  winter,  but  “in  the  breeding  season,  though 
difficult  as  ever  to  flush,”  it  made  its  presence  known  “by  what  must  rank 
as  one  of  the  most  peculiar  notes  uttered  by  a bird” — notes  that  take  on 
“a  liquid  quality,  bearing  some  resemblance  to  the  bubbling  of  a spring, 
or  even  to  the  boiling  of  a kettle.”  Such  Jack  Snipe  courtship  noises  as 
are  comparable  to  tbe  bleating  of  the  Common  Snipe,  are,  according  to 
Blair,  made  as  the  flying  bird  “glides  down  on  outstretched  wings,  its 
quills  meanwhile  producing  a whirring  sound  reminiscent  of  the  drumming 
of  its  ally.”  I feel  sure  that  the  word  “quills,”  as  used  here,  refers  to  wing 
quills,  not  tail  quills.  Unlike  most  of  the  snipes  discussed  in  this  paper, 
the  Jack  Snipe  does  not  use  its  tail  at  all  in  producing  a sound  while  in 
courtship  display. 

Finally,  a word  about  the  Common  Snipe  picture  that  set  me  to  writing 
this  paper  (see  colorplate).  The  photograph  was  taken  on  21  May  1967, 
near  Jackson,  Jackson  Co.,  southern  Michigan,  by  Betty  Darling  Cottrille. 
It  shows  a bird  with  lifted,  widespread  tail  displaying  at  the  nest.  The 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


473 


fluffy  coverts  look  like  under  tail  coverts,  but  they  are  upper  coverts.  After 
holding  the  tail  in  this  position  for  a second  or  so,  the  snipe  turned  it  so 
that  the  under  side,  with  the  coverts,  faced  the  camera. 

Betty  Cottrille  and  her  husband.  Dr.  W.  Powell  Cottrille,  have  been 
enthusiastic  observers  of  birds  for  a long  time.  The  Common  Snipe  has 
nested  regularly  in  a marshy  area  not  far  from  their  home  in  Jackson.  For 
14  successive  seasons,  beginning  in  1952,  the  Cottrilles  paid  special  at- 
tention to  that  species.  Nest  after  nest  that  they  found  held  a full  clutch 
of  four  eggs.  What  they  wanted  was  a nest  ready  for  eggs  or  with  an 
incomplete  clutch  so  that  they  could  observe  the  birds’  behavior  during 
the  incubation  period.  By  1967  their  search  had  become  almost  an  ob- 
session. 

Let  me  now  quote  from  Betty  Cottrille  (in  litt.)  herself:  “That  year 
[1967]  a pair  with  early  nest  weathered  the  vicissitudes  of  cold,  rain,  and 
finally,  on  23  April,  a three-inch  snowfall.  Hatching  began  late  in  the  day 
on  13  May.  Next  morning,  which  was  overcast  and  chilly,  we  found  one 
egg  in  the  nest  and  three  chicks  dispersed  in  the  grass  with  their  parents. 
Meanwhile,  we  had  discovered  another  nest,  this  with  one  egg  on  4 May 
and  four  eggs  on  7 May.  Having  learned  from  Bent’s  [1942:  86]  classic 
work  that  incubation  would  last  18-20  days,  we  made  plans.” 

“On  21  May,  the  15th  day  of  incubation,  the  weather  was  perfect  for 
photography.  My  husband  and  I,  he  with  a movie  camera,  1 with  a ‘still,’ 
spent  1 % hours  in  the  blind  that  morning,  hoping  that  the  bird  on  the  nest 
would  exhibit  some  variation  in  behavior  now  that  the  end  of  the  incubation 
period  was  at  hand.  The  blind  was  about  ten  feet  from  the  nest.  Our 
cameras  were  poised.  The  incubating  bird,  obviously  at  ease  while  we 
waited,  took  several  short  naps,  with  bill-tip  resting  on  the  ground.” 

“What  a surprise  was  in  store  for  us!  When  the  bird  decided  to  leave 
the  nest  it  stood  up,  took  a few  steps  away  from  the  eggs,  leaned  forward, 
and  displayed.  The  display  involved,  first,  spreading  and  lifting  the  tail 
until  it  stood  straight  up,  then  slowly,  not  jerkily,  turning  the  perfect  fan 
until,  with  upper  side  and  coverts  facing  the  camera,  its  plane  paralleled 
that  of  the  body’s  main  longitudinal  axis.  Nor  was  this  all.  Having  held 
the  bizarre  position  for  a second  or  two,  the  tail  swung  back  to  ‘normal,’ 
then  turned  once  more — through  an  arc  of  90  degrees — this  time  present- 
ing its  under  side  and  coverts  to  the  cameras.  We  were  indeed  fortunate: 
my  husband’s  movies,  as  well  as  my  stills,  recorded  what  we  had  wit- 
nessed.” 

“On  the  19th  day  of  incubation  (25  May)  luck  was  with  us  again,  but  of 
a different  sort.  The  weather  was  just  right.  When  we  visited  the  nest 
early  that  morning  it  contained  four  chicks,  two  of  them  dry,  the  other  two 
stiU  wet,  with  their  shells  nearby.  The  parents  were  beside  themselves 


474 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


with  excitement.  Within  minutes  we  had  set  up  the  blind  and  focussed 
cameras,  anticipating  a repeat  of  that  spectacular  display.  No  such  luck 
this  time,  for  the  emphasis  had  shifted  to  caring  for  the  chicks.  One  parent 
darted  in  and  out  of  the  grass,  which  was  now  tall,  clucking  and  dropping 
its  wings,  occasionally  running  to  the  brood  and  covering  them  hurriedly. 
As  the  two  youngest  dried  and  fluffed  out,  the  chicks  became  more  active. 
Now  the  parents,  hidden  in  the  grass,  seemed  to  increase  their  coaxing. 
One  by  one  the  chicks  tottered  and  stumbled  to  them.  Out  of  sight — the 
excitement  over!  Nothing  remained,  except  the  flattened  grass  where  the 
blind  had  been,  to  bear  witness  to  that  unforgettable  drama.  How  the 
pictures  would  remind  us  of  the  wonderful  antics  we  had  witnessed  so 
many  times  spring  after  spring!” 


SUMMARY 

AU  but  one  of  the  13  currently  recognized  species  of  the  scolopacid  genus  Capella 
display  in  the  air  during  courtship,  though  aerial  display  is  not  restricted  to  the  breed- 
ing season.  Display  flights  are  accompanied  by  hooting,  bleating,  neighing,  or  whin- 
nying sounds  that  are  widely  believed  to  be  nonvocal  and  that  almost  certainly  are  produced 
by  vibration  of  some  or  all  of  the  tail  feathers.  Drawings  showing  extra-wide  spreading  of  the 
narrowed  outermost  feather  on  each  side  of  the  tail  in  C.  gallinago  gallinago  have  led  to  the 
belief  that  that  feather  is  responsible  for  the  sound;  but  investigation  reveeds  the  fact  that 
this  feather  is  not  by  any  means  always  much  narrowed  in  the  Northern  Hemisphere’s  three 
races  of  C.  gallinago-,  that  in  the  several  Southern  Hemisphere  races  of  C.  gallinago  2 or 
3 pairs  of  outer  rectrices  are  narrowed;  that  in  several  other  species  of  Capella,  notably  C. 
stenura,  one  to  several  outermost  pairs  of  rectrices  are  narrowed;  and  that  in  ground  displays 
of  C.  gallinago  in  various  parts  of  that  species’  very  wide  range  the  movements  of  the  tail 
reveal  such  great  maneuverability  as  to  suggest  that  the  hooting  or  neighing  is  produced  by 
the  pressing  downward  or  from  side  to  side  of  the  whole  tail.  Courtship  flights  of  the  Giant 
Snipe  (C.  undulata),  hitherto  unreported,  are  like  those  of  smaller  snipes  in  some  ways  but 
are  accompanied  by  trisyllabic  sounds  that  are  probably  vocal.  The  courtship  of  C.  gallinago 
andina,  a form  that  inhabits  the  Andes,  apparently  has  not  been  described.  The  Double 
Snipe  (C.  media),  whose  outer  rectrices  are  largely  white,  displays  on  the  ground  rather  than 
in  the  air.  The  Jack  Snipe  {Lymnocryptes  minimus),  a small  species  placed  hy  some  taxon- 
omists in  Capella,  makes  strange  “cantering”  sounds  during  courtship,  but  since  these  are 
given  from  the  ground  as  well  as  from  the  air  they  are  presumably  vocal. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

The  following  are  to  be  thanked  for  their  assistance  in  preparing  this  paper:  Betty  Darhng 
CottriUe,  for  her  fine  photograph  of  the  displaying  snipe  and  for  her  account  of  taking  the 
picture;  personnel  of  the  U.S.  National  Museum  and  the  Carnegie  Museum  of  Natural  History 
for  lending  specimens;  David  M.  Niles,  for  counting  the  rectrices  of  snipe  specimens  at  the 
Delaware  Museum  of  Natural  History;  Helmut  Sick,  for  material  on  Capella  undulata  that 
will  appear  in  his  forthcoming  book  on  the  birds  of  Brazil;  Norman  Boke  and  Loretta  Nickel 
for  translating  textual  material  from  the  Portuguese;  Robert  M.  Mengel  for  looking  up  certain 
references;  John  Farrand  and  John  S.  Weske  for  checking  aU  of  my  statements  about  Capella 
imperialis;  Ohn  Sewall  PettingiU,  Jr.,  and  David  F.  Parmelee,  for  information  of  Capella 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


475 


gallinago  magellanica;  and  D.  Scott  Wood  for  borrowing  needed  specimens  and  for  counting 
the  rectrices  of  some  of  these. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Adams,  A.  L.  1858.  Notes  on  the  habits,  etc.  of  some  of  the  birds  of  India.  Proc.  Zool. 
Soc.  London  466-512. 

Aplin,  O.  V.  1894.  On  the  birds  of  Uruguay.  Ibis  6:149-215. 

American  Ornithologists’  Union.  1931.  Check-list  of  North  American  birds,  4th  ed. 
Lord  Baltimore  Press,  Baltimore,  Maryland. 

Bahr,  P.  H.  1907.  On  the  “bleating”  or  “drumming”  of  the  Snipe  {Gallinago  coelestis). 
Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  London  12-35. 

Baker,  E.  C.  S.  1929.  The  fauna  of  British  India,  including  Ceylon  and  Burma.  Birds,  Vol. 
6.  London,  England. 

Bannerman,  D.  a.  1961.  The  birds  of  the  British  Isles.  Vol.  9.  Oliver  and  Boyd,  London, 
England. 

Barlow,  J.  C.  1967.  Autumnal  breeding  of  the  Paraguay  Snipe  in  Uruguay.  Auk  84:421- 
422. 

Bent,  A.  C.  1942.  Life  histories  of  North  American  shore  birds.  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  BuU.  142. 
Berman,  D.  I.  and  I.  F.  Kuz’min.  1965.  Pintailed  Snipe  in  Tuva.  Ornithologica  7:209-216. 

Transl.  from  Russian  by  Canadian  Wildlife  Service. 

Breslford,  V.  1947.  Notes  on  the  birds  of  the  Lake  Bangweulu  area  in  northern  Rhodesia. 
Ibis  89:57-77. 

Carr-Lewty,  R.  a.  1943.  The  aerodynamics  of  the  drumming  of  the  Common  Snipe.  Br. 
Birds  36:230-234. 

Cawkell,  R.  a.  and  J.  E.  Hamilton.  1961.  The  birds  of  the  Falkland  Islands.  Ibis  103: 
1-27. 

Cheesman,  R.  E.  and  W.  L.  Sclater.  1935.  On  a collection  of  birds  from  north-western 
Abyssinia,  Pt.  2.  Ibis  77:297-329. 

Dement’ev,  G.  P.,  N.  a.  Gladkov  and  E.  P.  Spangenberg  (eds.).  1969.  Birds  of  the 
Soviet  Union.  Vol.  3.  Publ.  for  Smithsonian  Inst,  and  N.S.F.,  Washington,  D.C.  by 
Israel  Prog,  for  Sci.  Transl.  (Jerusalem). 

Durnford,  H.  1877.  Notes  on  the  birds  of  the  Province  of  Buenos  Ayres.  Ibis  19:166-203. 
Edwards,  E.  P.  1974.  A coded  list  of  birds  of  the  world.  Private  publ..  Sweet  Briar, 
Virginia. 

Fennell,  C.  M.  1953.  Notes  on  the  birds  of  Daikokujima,  Hokkaido,  Japan.  Condor  55: 
38-52. 

Gardner,  L.  L.  1930.  The  whistling  of  snipe.  Condor  32:164-165. 

Gurney,  J.  H.  1864.  A sixth  additional  list  of  birds  from  Nepal.  Ibis  6:346-361. 

. 1868.  An  eighth  additional  list  of  birds  from  Nepal.  Ibis  10:40-52. 

Harting,  j.  E.  1882.  On  the  eggs  of  some  rare  wading  birds  from  Madagascar.  Proc.  Zool. 
Soc.  London  353-357. 

Hudson,  W.  H.  1921.  Birds  of  La  Plata,  Vol.  2.  J.  M.  Dent  & Sons,  Ltd.,  London,  England. 
Hume,  A.  O.  and  C.  H.  T.  Marshall.  1881.  The  game  birds  of  India,  Burmah,  and  Ceylon, 
Vol.  3.  A.  O.  Hume  and  C.  H.  T.  Marshall,  Calcutta,  India. 

Irby,  L.  H.  1861.  Notes  on  birds  observed  in  Oudh  and  Kumaon.  Ibis  3:217-251. 
Kozlova,  E.  V.  1932.  The  birds  of  south-west  Transbaikalia,  northern  Mongolia,  and  central 
Gobi,  Pt.  3.  Ibis  74:567-5%. 

Ludlow,  F.  and  N.  B.  Kinnear.  1934.  A contribution  to  the  ornithology  of  Chinese  Tur- 
kestan, Pt.  4.  Ibis  76:95-125. 


476 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


AND . 1937.  The  birds  of  Bhutan  and  adjacent  territories  of  Sikkim  and  Tibet, 

Pt.  3.  Ibis  79:467-504. 

Mackworth-Praed,  C.  W.  and  C.  H.  B.  Grant.  1952.  The  birds  of  eastern  and  north- 
eastern Africa,  1st  ed.,  Vol.  1.  Longmans,  London,  England. 

Mathews,  G.  M.  1913.  The  birds  of  Australia,  Vol.  3.  H.  E.  and  G.  Witherby  Ltd.,  London, 
England. 

Meinertzhagen,  R.  1952.  Systematic  relationships  between  Old-world  birds.  Ibis  94:545- 
546. 

Meves,  W.  1858.  On  the  Snipe’s  “neighing”  or  humming  noise,  and  on  its  tail-feathers’ 
systematic  value.  (Transl.  from  Swedish  by  J.  WoUey.)  Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  London  199- 
203. 

Meyer  DE  Schauensee,  R.  1970.  A guide  to  the  birds  of  South  America.  Livingston  Publ. 
Co.,  Wynnewood,  Pennsylvania. 

Moore,  R.  T.  1934.  The  Mt.  Sangay  labyrinth  and  its  fauna.  Auk  51:141-156. 

Newton,  A.  1865.  List  of  animals  collected  at  Mohambo,  Madagascar,  by  Mr.  W.  T. 
Gerrard.  Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  London  832-837. 

Peters,  J.  L.  1934.  Check-list  of  birds  of  the  world.  Vol.  2.  Harvard  Univ.  Press,  Cam- 
bridge, Massachusetts. 

Phelps,  W.  H.  and  W.  H.  Phelps,  Jr.  1958.  Lista  de  las  aves  de  Venezuela  con  su 
distribucion.  Bol.  Soc.  Venezolena  Cienc.  Nat.  19,  Tomo  2,  Parte  1 (Num.  90). 

Pinto,  O.  M.  D.  O.  1935.  Aves  de  Bahia.  Rev.  Mus.  Pauhsta  19:1-326. 

POPHAM,  H.  L.  1898.  Eurther  notes  on  birds  observed  on  the  Yenesei  River,  Siberia.  Ibis 
10:489-520. 

. 1901.  Supplementary  notes  on  the  birds  of  the  Yenesei  River.  Ibis  13:449^58. 

Rand,  A.  L.  1934.  The  distribution  and  habits  of  Madagascar  birds.  Bull.  Am.  Mus.  Nat. 
Hist.  72:143-t90. 

Reynolds,  P.  W.  1935.  Notes  on  the  birds  of  Cape  Horn.  Ibis  77:65-101. 

Seebohm,  H.  1886.  A review  of  the  species  of  the  genus  Scolopax.  Ibis  28:122-144. 

Stubbs,  E.  J.  1912.  Flight  of  the  Common  Snipe.  Br.  Birds  6:102. 

Sutton,  G.  M.  1923.  Notes  on  the  nesting  of  the  Wilson’s  Snipe  in  Crawford  County, 
Pennsylvania.  Wilson  Bull.  35:191-202. 

. 1928.  The  birds  of  Pymatuning  Swamp  and  Conneaut  Lake,  northwestern  Penn- 
sylvania. Ann.  Carnegie  Mus.  18:19-239. 

. 1961.  Iceland  summer.  Univ.  Oklahoma  Press,  Norman,  Oklahoma. 

Terborgh,  j.  and  j.  S.  Weske.  1972.  Rediscovery  of  the  Imperial  Snipe  in  Peru.  Auk 
89:497-505. 

Tuck,  L.  M.  1972.  The  Snipes:  a study  of  the  genus  Capella.  Can.  Wildl.  Serv. 
Monogr.  No.  5.  Ottawa,  Canada. 

VAN  SOMEREN,  V.  D.  1947.  Field  notes  on  some  Madagascar  birds.  Ibis  89:235-267. 

VuiLLEUMiER,  F.  1969.  Field  notes  on  some  birds  from  the  Bolivian  Andes.  Ibis  111:599- 
608. 

Welty,  C.  1975.  The  life  of  birds,  2nd  ed.  Saunders,  Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania. 

Wetmore,  a.  1926.  Observations  on  the  birds  of  Argentina,  Paraguay,  Uruguay  and  Chile. 
U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  BuU.  133. 

White,  C.  M.  N.  1945.  On  the  validity  of  Capella  angolensis  Bocage.  Ibis  87:465-466. 

Williamson,  K.  1950.  The  distraction  behaviour  of  the  Faroe  Snipe.  Ibis  92:66-74. 

Witherby,  H.  F.,  F.  C.  R.  Jourdain,  N.  F.  Ticehurst  and  B.  W.  Tucker.  1941.  The 
handbook  of  British  birds,  Vol.  4.  H.  F.  & G.  Witherby  Ltd.,  London,  England. 

WOLLEY,  J.  1858.  (Comments  on  W.  Meves’s  paper  [originally  publ.  in  Swedish]  on  the 
Common  Snipe’s  drumming.)  Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  London  199-202. 


Sutton  • SNIPE  DISPLAYS 


477 


Woods,  R.  1975.  Birds  of  the  Falkland  Islands.  Compton  Press,  Salisbury,  Wiltshire, 
England. 

STOVALL  MUSEUM  OF  SCIENCE  AND  HISTORY,  UNIV.  OKLAHOMA,  NORMAN, 
OKLAHOMA  73019.  ACCEPTED  22  APR.  1981. 


COLORPLATE 


The  colorplate  Frontispiece  of  the  Common  Snipe  {Capella  gallinago)  has  been  made 
possible  by  an  endowment  established  by  George  Miksch  Sutton. 


ANNUAL  MEETING— THE  WILSON  ORNITHOLOGICAL  SOCIETY, 

1982 


The  63rd  annual  meeting  of  The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  will  be  held  at  Virginia 
Polytechnic  Institute  and  State  University,  Blacksburg,  Virginia,  6-9  May,  1982.  In  addition 
to  the  scientific  program,  there  will  be  an  art  exhibition  and  a program  for  spouses.  Daily 
fieldtrips  are  planned  for  the  Blacksburg  area.  On  the  morning  of  9 May,  there  will  be  a 
fieldtrip  to  Mountain  Lake,  Virginia,  elev.  4000+  feet,  one  hour  distant  from  Blacksburg 
to  see  northern  (boreal)  breeding  birds. 

Chairman  of  the  Local  Committee  is  Dr.  Curtis  Adkisson,  Dept.  Biology,  Virginia  Poly- 
technic Institute  and  State  University,  Blacksburg,  Virginia  24061.  Information  concerning 
accommodation,  transportation  and  related  matters  will  be  mailed  to  the  Society  member- 
ship. Chairman  of  the  Program  Committee  is  Dr.  Clait  Braun,  Wildlife  Research  Center, 
317  W.  Prospect  St.,  Fort  Collins,  Colorado  80526.  Abstracts  of  papers  to  be  given  in  the 
scientific  sessions  must  be  received  by  him  before  1 April  1982. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  478^90 


FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES  IN  TWO  FORESTS 
WITH  DIFFERENT  VEGETATION  STRUCTURE 

Brian  A.  Maurer  and  Rorert  C.  Whitmore 

Specific  foraging  patterns  of  many  bird  species  have  been  well  studied; 
however,  few  studies  have  dealt  with  the  influence  of  changes  in  habitat 
structure  on  foraging.  This  study  examines  the  influence  of  vegetation 
structure  on  the  foraging  behavior  of  Acadian  Flycatchers  {Empidonax 
virescens).  Red-eyed  Vireos  {Vireo  olivaceus).  Black-throated  Green  War- 
blers {Dendroica  virens),  American  Redstarts  (Setophaga  ruticilla)  and 
Scarlet  Tanagers  {Piranga  olivacea).  The  foraging  behavior  of  each  of 
these  species  was  observed  in  two  habitats  that  had  different  vegetation 
structures.  This  made  it  possible  to  relate  differences  in  foraging  by  the 
bird  species  to  changes  in  habitat  structure. 

STUDY  AREAS  AND  METHODS 

Study  areas. — Foraging  data  were  gathered  in  two  stands  in  the  Fernow  Experimental 
Forest  (avg.  elev.  760  m)  located  4.8  km  southeast  of  Parsons,  Tucker  Co.,  West  Virginia. 
Major  tree  species  in  the  Fernow  Forest  were  red  oak  (Quercus  rubra),  chestnut  oak  {Q. 
prinus),  red  maple  {Acer  rubrum),  sugar  maple  {A.  saccharum),  black  cherry  {Prunus  sero- 
tina),  American  beech  {Fagus  grandifolia),  yellow  poplar  {Liriodendron  tulipifera)  and  sweet 
birch  {Betula  lenta).  The  relative  abundances  of  these  trees,  however,  varied  between  stands. 

The  Fernow  Forest  is  in  a region  that  is  warm  in  summer.  Maximum  daily  temperature 
was  30°C  in  1977  and  29.4°C  in  1978.  Temperatures  at  night  ranged  from  near  0°C  in  May 
to  16. 1°C  during  July  for  both  years.  Weekly  precipitation  reached  a maximum  both  years 
of  >9  cm  in  the  month  of  July.  Precipitation  dropped  below  0.25  cm/week  only  one  week 
during  the  1977  season,  and  never  dropped  below  1 cm/week  in  1978. 

The  two  stands  were  located  on  watersheds  that  have  been  used  for  hydrological  research 
by  the  Forest  Service.  One  stand,  the  “young  forest,”  (30  ha)  was  clearcut  in  1958  and  has 
since  been  left  to  regrow.  In  1971  this  area  was  fertilized  using  257  kg/ha  urea.  The  stand 
grows  on  very  steep  terrain,  with  about  75%  of  the  slopes  21.8°  or  greater  (Reinhart  et  al. 
1963).  The  second  stand,  the  “mature  forest,”  (38.9  ha)  was  logged  early  in  the  1900’s,  and 
has  since  been  left  to  regrow.  None  of  the  slopes  in  this  stand  were  steeper  than  21.8°. 

Avifauna. — The  bird  communities  inhabiting  the  two  stands  represent  an  important  con- 
text within  which  the  species  studied  should  be  viewed.  The  species  found  in  these  com- 
munities included  several  species  typical  of  northern  coniferous  forests,  e.g.,  Blackburnian 
Warblers  {Dendroica  fusca),  as  well  as  species  typical  of  deciduous  forest  habitats,  e.g.. 
Hooded  Warblers  {Wdlsonia  citrina).  The  young  forest  had  fewer  species  of  flycatchers,  and 
had  several  species  typical  of  early  stages  of  forest  succession,  e.g..  Chestnut-sided  Warblers 
{D.  pensylvanica)  and  Canada  Warblers  {D.  canadensis).  Cerulean  {D.  cerulea)  and  Black- 
burnian warblers  occurred  only  in  mature  forest.  Further  information  on  the  avian  commu- 
nities in  these  stands  is  given  by  McArthur  (1980)  and  Maurer  (1980). 

Methods. — Both  study  areas  were  sampled  in  July  1978  for  vegetation  structure  and  com- 
position. Fifty  0.04-ha  circular  plots  were  located  randomly  in  each  area.  Vertical  structural 
diversity  was  measured  using  frequency  counts  of  vegetation  in  each  of  eight  canopy  layers 


478 


Maurer  and  Whitmore  • FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES 


479 


Frequency 

Fig.  1.  Foliage  height  profile  (A)  and  foraging  height  distribution  (B-F)  for  five  canopy- 
feeding birds  in  a mature  forest  (broken  lines)  and  a young  forest  (solid  lines).  ACF  = Aca- 
dian Flycatcher,  REV  = Red-eyed  Vireo,  BTG  = Black-throated  Green  Warbler,  AMR  = 
American  Redstart,  SCT  = Scarlet  Tanager. 


(low  vegetation;  shrubs:  0-1  m,  1.5-3.05;  trees:  3.05-6.1,  6.1-12.2,  12.2-18.3,  18.3-24.4, 
>24.4)  at  20  points  within  each  plot  and  calculating  a diversity  index,  -Spjlnpi  (Pielou  1976), 
for  each  plot.  Differences  in  plot  diversities  between  areas  were  tested  using  a 2-sided  Mann- 
Whitney  test  (Conover  1971).  A profile  of  the  canopy  was  obtained  for  each  stand  by  using 
the  frequency  counts  of  vegetation  in  each  of  seven  (excluding  low  vegetation)  canopy  layers 
(Fig.  lA).  These  profiles  represent  the  probability  of  encountering  a given  canopy  layer  at 
any  point  in  a study  area.  Probabilities  were  estimated  from  the  samples  taken  in  each  area. 
Maximum  canopy  height  was  determined  for  each  plot,  and  differences  in  canopy  height 
between  stands  were  tested  using  a 2-sided  Mann- Whitney  test.  Relative  densities  (RD)  were 
calculated  for  several  tree  species  groups  (Table  1)  as  follows:  RD  = SAj/SMi,  where  Aj  = 
number  of  individuals  of  species  group  A in  plot  i and  Mj  = total  number  of  trees  in  plot  i. 

Foraging  behavior  of  five  canopy  feeding  bird  species  were  recorded  from  8 May-27  July 
1978  and  7 May-22  June  1979.  Observations  were  classified  into  categories  for  each  of  four 
foraging  variables.  The  variables  (categories  are  in  parentheses)  were  (1)  foraging  maneuver 
(hover,  glean,  flycatch),  (2)  foraging  substrate  (leaf,  branch,  trunk,  air),  (3)  location  in  tree 
(proximal  to  the  trunk,  distal  to  the  trunk,  air),  and  (4)  tree  species  used  {Quercus  spp.,  Acer 
spp.,  Betula  spp.,  Fagus  spp.,  other  species,  shrubs  and  saphngs).  Niche  metrics  (niche 


480 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  1 

Vertical  Diversity,  Mean  Canopy  Height,  Relative  Densities  and  Tree  Species 
Diversity  of  Two  Stands  in  the  Fernow  Experimental  Forest  Based  on  50 
Random  Plots  Located  in  Each  Stand 


Young  forest 

Mature  forest 

Mean  vertical  diversity 

1.70 

1.89 

Mean  canopy  height  (m) 

20.31 

24.53 

Relative  density  (RD) 
Oaks 

0.03 

0.15 

Maples 

0.22 

0.39 

Beeches 

0.06 

0.12 

Birches 

0.07 

0.07 

Other  (total) 

0.62 

0.26 

Hickories 

0.02 

0.01 

Cherries 

0.02 

0.06 

Magnolias 

0.16 

0.16 

Miscellaneous* 

0.42 

0.03 

Tree  species  diversity 

1.08 

1.44 

* White  ash  [Fraxinus  americana),  basswood  {Tilia  spp.).  eastern  hemlock  (Tsuga  canadensis),  big-tooth  aspen  (Populus 
grandidentata),  elms  (Ulmus),  service  berry  (Amelanchier),  witch  hazel  (Hamamelis  Virginia),  black  locust  (Robinia  pseudo- 
acacia). 


breadth,  niche  overlap)  were  calculated  following  Colwell  and  Futuyma  (1971)  for  two  niche 
dimensions:  (1)  foraging  behavior  and  (2)  tree  use.  The  following  nine  independent  foraging 
states  were  used  to  categorize  the  foraging  data  and  calculate  niche  metrics  related  to  for- 
aging behavior:  (1)  hovering  at  foliage  proximal  to  the  trunk  of  a tree  or  shrub,  (2)  hovering 
at  foliage  distal  to  the  trunk,  (3)  hovering  at  woody  structures  (branches,  bark,  etc.)  proximal 
to  the  trunk,  (4)  hovering  at  woody  structures  distal  to  the  trunk,  (5)  flycatching,  (6)  gleaning 
foliage  proximal  to  the  trunk,  (7)  gleaning  foliage  distal  to  the  trunk,  (8)  gleaning  woody 
structures  proximal  to  the  trunk,  and  (9)  gleaning  woody  structures  distal  to  the  trunk.  Niche 
metrics  of  the  tree-use  dimension  were  calculated  using  13  independent  resource  states. 
These  states  were  created  using  six  tree  species  groups  {Quercus,  Fagus,  Acer,  Betula,  other 
trees,  shrubs)  and  dividing  each  of  these  groups  into  three  groups  based  on  the  height  of  the 
foraging  maneuver  (0-3  m,  3-9  m,  >9  m).  Since  the  five  species  did  not  use  some  trees  at 
certain  heights,  13  instead  of  18  resource  states  were  used.  Changes  in  niche  widths  between 
areas  were  tested  using  Hutcheson’s  (1970)  method  of  testing  for  differences  in  diversity. 

Heights  of  foraging  acts  were  recorded  for  each  species.  From  these  data  foraging  heights 
were  tested  for  significant  differences  between  stands  using  a 1-sided  Mann-Whitney  test 
(Conover  1971).  Foraging  height  profiles  for  each  species  were  constructed  for  both  forests. 
These  profiles  are  analogous  to  the  foliage  height  profiles  described  above  and  represent  the 
estimated  probabilities  that  any  one  foraging  act  by  a given  species  in  a given  forest  will  be 
in  a specific  canopy  layer. 

Contingency  tables  were  constructed  (forest  stand  X foraging  variable:  maneuvers,  sub- 
strates, locations,  tree  use)  to  test  for  differences  in  probabilities  of  each  category  between 
the  two  stands.  Since  multiple  observations  were  obtained  from  some  of  the  individuals  of 
each  species,  the  data  might  not  represent  a true  random  sample.  This  can  cause  the  Chi- 


Maurer  and  Whitmore  • FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES 


481 


square  tests  to  produce  liberal  significance  levels  (E.  J.  Harner,  pers.  comm.)-  Thus,  for  the 
Chi-square  tests,  the  significance  level  P < 0.01  was  used.  Tests  for  preferential  use  of  tree 
species  were  performed  using  a Chi-square  goodness  of  fit  test.  A hypothetical  probability 
distribution  was  estimated  from  the  relative  densities  for  each  tree  species  group  (Table  1) 
in  each  stand.  Using  a Chi-square  goodness  of  fit  test  (Conover  1971),  the  hypothesis  that 
the  probability  distribution  generated  from  relative  densities  of  the  tree  species  groups  was 
the  same  as  the  probability  distribution  of  foraging  acts  among  the  tree  groups  was  tested 
for  each  bird  species  in  each  stand.  For  a given  stand,  a significant  Chi-square  value  indicated 
that  a bird  species  was  deviating  from  a pattern  of  using  tree  species  based  on  their  abun- 
dance by  selecting  certain  tree  types  over  others.  If  a cell  in  any  contingency  table  had  an 
expected  value  <1,  categories  were  lumped  together  to  raise  the  value,  thus  reducing  the 
degrees  of  freedom.  When  a cell  in  a contingency  table  has  an  expected  value  of  <1,  the 
significance  level  may  be  poorly  approximated  (Conover  1971). 

HABITAT  STRUCTURE 

Canopy  structure  was  more  diverse  (Mann- Whitney  U = 195,  P < 
0.001)  in  the  mature  forest  than  the  young  forest  (Table  1).  This  means 
that,  on  the  average,  a point  in  the  mature  forest  had  more  canopy  layers 
than  a point  in  the  young  forest.  The  trend  of  greater  complexity  in  the 
mature  forest  was  reflected  in  the  vertical  profile  of  that  forest  (Fig.  lA). 
The  mature  forest  also  had  a higher  (Mann-Whitney  U = 358,  P < 0.001) 
mean  canopy  height  (Table  1)  than  the  young  forest.  The  frequencies  of  each 
canopy  layer  were  significantly  different  in  each  stand  (x^  = 867.3,  df  = 
6,  P < 0.001).  In  the  mature  forest,  the  most  abundant  canopy  layer  was 
at  18.3-24.4  m (Fig.  lA).  Layers  below  the  18.3-24.4  m layer  were  well 
represented  in  that  forest.  In  the  young  forest,  there  was  a high  incidence 
of  three  layers  (3. 1-6.1  m,  6.1-12.2  m,  12.2-18.3  m),  but  the  rest  of  the 
layers  were  poorly  represented. 

FORAGING  BEHAVIOR 

Acadian  Flycatcher. — Acadian  Flycatchers  used  essentially  the  same 
foraging  maneuvers  in  both  forests  (x^  = 0.09,  df  = 1,  NS).  Hovering  at 
various  substrates  was  used  most  often,  while  gleaning  was  rarely  used 
(Table  2).  The  use  of  locations  (proximity  to  trunk)  (x^  = 7.28,  df  = 2, 
P = 0.027),  and  the  use  of  substrates  (x^  = 13.20,  df  = 3,  P = 0.005) 
were  different.  Woody  structures  (e.g.,  branches)  were  used  as  foraging 
sites  in  the  young  forest  but  not  in  the  mature  forest. 

Acadian  Flycatchers  also  showed  significantly  different  patterns  of  tree 
use  in  both  forests  (x^  = 20.90,  df  = 5,  P = 0.001).  Shrubs  and  trees  other 
than  maples,  beeches,  birches  and  oaks  were  used  more  often  in  the  young 
forest  than  in  the  mature  forest  (Table  3).  Acadian  Flycatchers  did  not  use 
tree  types  based  on  their  abundance  in  the  young  forest  (x^  = 36.77,  df  = 
4,  P < 0.001)  or  the  mature  forest  (x^  = 23.51,  df  = 4,  P < 0.001).  In 
both  habitats,  beeches  were  used  more  often  than  expected  by  chance 


482 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  2 

Relative  Frequencies  of  Foraging  Variables  for  Five  Species  of  Insectivorous 

Birds 


No.  of 

Maneuver 

Location® 

Substrate® 

Species 

vations 

Hover 

Glean 

Hawk 

Prox*> 

Dist 

Leaf 

Branch 

Trunk 

E.  virescens 

Young  forest 

72 

59.7 

1.4 

38.9 

19.4 

40.3 

40.3 

5.6 

15.3 

Mature  forest 

53 

58.5 

0.0 

41.5 

3.8 

54.7 

58.5 

0.0 

0.0 

V.  olivaceus 

Young  forest 

59 

52.5 

47.5 

0.0 

8.5 

91.5 

81.4 

15.4 

3.4 

Mature  forest 

48 

52.1 

47.9 

0.0 

6.3 

93.8 

91.7 

4.2 

4.2 

D.  virens 

Young  forest 

31 

45.2 

38.7 

16.1 

0.0 

83.9 

61.3 

22.5 

0.0 

Mature  forest 

30 

43.3 

30.0 

26.7 

23.3 

50.0 

43.3 

23.3 

6.7 

S.  ruticilla 

Young  forest 

71 

54.9 

7.0 

38.0 

11.3 

50.7 

52.1 

7.0 

4.2 

Mature  forest 

47 

34.0 

10.6 

55.3 

6.4 

38.3 

23.4 

19.1 

2.1 

P.  olivacea 

Young  forest 

35 

74.3 

25.7 

0.0 

17.1 

82.9 

74.3 

11.4 

14.3 

Mature  forest 

31 

67.7 

32.3 

0.0 

12.9 

87.1 

83.9 

16.1 

0.0 

® Air  same  frequency  as  hawk  (=flycatch). 

**  Prox  = proximal  to  trunk,  Dist  = distal  to  trunk. 


(cell  for  beeches  made  up  84%  of  the  x^  value  for  young  forest,  64%  for 
mature  forest).  Smith  (1977)  found  that  Acadian  Flycatchers  tend  to  use 
mesic  sites,  and  since  beeches  are  often  associated  with  mesic  sites 
(Fowells  1965,  Brockman  1968)  the  association  between  beeches  and  this 
flycatcher  is  not  unexpected. 

Probabilities  of  using  each  canopy  layer  were  different  (x^  ==  21.26,  df  = 
4,  P < 0.001)  in  each  study  area  (Fig.  IB).  In  both  forests,  Acadian  Fly- 
catchers concentrated  their  foraging  efforts  below  the  densest  part  of  the 
canopy  (Fig.  lA),  as  Williamson  (1971)  also  noted.  Foraging  heights  were 
lowest  in  the  young  forest  (Table  3). 

Acadian  Flycatchers  had  a significantly  wider  foraging  behavior  niche 
in  the  young  forest  {t  = 4.77,  df  = 124,  P < 0.005)  than  in  the  mature 
forest.  However,  tree  use  was  significantly  more  diverse  in  the  mature 
forest  {t  = 2.35,  df  = 125,  P < 0.025).  The  wider  foraging  niche  was  ap- 
parently due  to  increased  use  of  different  substrates  (Table  2).  In  the 
young  forest,  Acadian  Flycatchers  had  higher  average  overlaps  with  other 
species  in  foraging  behaviors  (Table  4),  a pattern  observed  for  other 


Maurer  and  Whitmore  • FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES 


483 


Table  3 

Relative  Frequencies  of  Tree  Species  Use  and  Foraging  Heights  of  Five  Species 

OF  Forest  Birds 


Tree 

use 

Mean 

foraging 

height 

(m) 

Species 

Maple 

Oak 

Birch 

Beech 

Other 

Shrub 

p* 

E.  virescens 

Young  forest 

18.1 

0.0 

2.8 

16.7 

26.4 

36.1 

5.18 

<0.01 

Mature  forest 

26.4 

9.4 

11.3 

26.4 

7.5 

18.9 

8.09 

V.  olivaceus 

Young  forest 

39.0 

11.9 

8.5 

15.3 

16.9 

8.5 

9.96 

0.09 

Mature  forest 

22.9 

16.7 

12.5 

12.5 

20.8 

14.6 

13.31 

D.  virens 

Young  forest 

12.9 

12.9 

32.3 

16.1 

22.5 

3.2 

13.83 

<0.01 

Mature  forest 

13.3 

20.0 

23.3 

23.3 

20.0 

0.0 

18.72 

S.  ruticilla 

Young  forest 

40.8 

0.0 

11.3 

22.5 

21.1 

4.2 

9.53 

0.10 

Mature  forest 

51.1 

19.1 

8.5 

12.8 

4.3 

4.3 

11.43 

P.  olivacea 

Young  forest 

31.4 

34.3 

2.9 

0.0 

17.1 

14.3 

9.23 

>0.10 

Mature  forest 

38.7 

6.5 

0.0 

32.3 

12.9 

9.7 

11.87 

* Significance  level  from  1-sided  Mann- Whitney  test  (Conover  1971)  for  differences  in  foraging  heights  between  the  two 
stands. 


species.  However,  patterns  of  overlap  with  individual  species  varied 
(Maurer  1980).  There  was  less  average  overlap  between  Acadian  Flycatch- 
ers and  other  species  in  tree  use  in  the  young  forest  (Table  4). 

Red-eyed  Vireo. — Red-eyed  Vireos  did  not  show  any  significant  differ- 
ences in  foraging  or  tree  use  between  the  two  forests  (critical  levels  for  all 
tests  were  >0.05).  Frequencies  of  foraging  maneuvers,  foraging  locations 
and  substrates  used  were  very  similar,  although  there  was  a slightly  great- 
er use  of  branches  in  the  young  forest  (Table  2).  The  data  indicate  that 
these  birds  foraged  mainly  on  foliage  at  the  outer  perimeter  of  a tree, 
hovering  and  gleaning  about  equally,  as  reported  previously  by  James 
(1976)  for  southern  Ontario  and  Williamson  (1971)  for  Maryland. 

Red-eyed  Vireos  did  not  change  tree  use  between  areas,  but  foraged 
slightly  higher  in  the  mature  forest  (Table  3).  Probabilities  of  foraging  in 
each  canopy  layer  were  different  between  stands  (x^  = 29.33,  df  = 6, 
P < 0.001).  In  the  mature  forest,  the  12.2-18.3  m canopy  layer  was  used 
most  often,  whereas  the  6.1-12.2  m layer  was  used  most  often  in  the  young 


484 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  4 

Niche  Dimensions  of  Five  Canopy-feeding  Birds  in  Two  Structurally  Different 

Forests 


Species 

Niche  width  (bits) 

Average  overlap  (%) 

Foraging 

Tree  use 

Foraging 

Tree 

use 

Ya 

M 

Y 

M 

Y 

M 

Y 

M 

E.  virescens 

1.33 

0.82 

1.85 

2.13 

0.60 

0.55 

0.38 

0.55 

V.  olivaceus 

1.39 

1.09 

2.07 

1.91 

0.61 

0.47 

0.57 

0.64 

D.  virens 

1.47 

2.01 

1.63 

1.59 

0.64 

0.51 

0.44 

0.53 

S.  ruticilla 

1.30 

1.30 

1.85 

1.66 

0.64 

0.47 

0.50 

0.60 

P.  olivacea 

1.26 

1.21 

1.85 

1.74 

0.61 

0.52 

0.43 

0.54 

X = 

1.35 

1.28 

1.85 

1.81 

0.62 

0.50 

0.46 

0.57 

Significance*’ 

NS 

NS 

P 

< 0.025 

P < 

0.01 

® Y = young  forest,  M = mature  forest. 

**  Niche  widths  tested  using  d-test  of  Crow  et  al.  (1978);  overlaps  tested  using  paired  f-tests,  df  = 9. 

forest  (Fig.  1C).  Lower  foraging  heights  in  the  young  forest  were  related 
to  lower  canopy  height  of  that  forest  (Table  1,  Fig.  lA). 

Red-eyed  Vireos  in  the  young  forest  had  wider  niche  widths  for  foraging 
behaviors  (Table  4),  although  this  difference  was  not  statistically  signifi- 
cant {t  = 1.89,  df  = 3634,  NS).  The  slightly  wider  niche  in  the  young 
forest  is  due  to  the  slight  increase  in  use  of  branches  in  that  forest  (Table 
2).  Red-eyed  Vireos  also  had  a slightly  wider  tree-use  niche  in  the  young 
forest  (Table  4),  although  again  the  difference  was  not  statistically  signif- 
icant {t  = 1.50,  df  = 102,  NS).  This  suggests  relative  similarity  in  tree  use 
between  the  two  forests.  In  the  young  forest,  tree  use  was  not  based  on 
abundance  (x^  = 53.38,  df  = 4,  P < 0.001),  but  was  in  the  mature  forest 
(X^  = 5.81,  df  = 4,  NS).  In  the  young  forest,  a preference  for  mature  forest 
types  was  shown.  Niche  overlaps  of  Red-eyed  Vireos  with  other  species 
for  foraging  were  generally  higher  in  the  young  forest,  with  the  exception 
of  Acadian  Flycatchers.  For  tree  use,  niche  overlaps  were  generally  higher 
in  the  mature  forest,  except  for  overlap  with  American  Redstarts,  where 
tree-use  overlap  was  higher  in  the  young  forest  (Maurer  1980). 

In  summary.  Red-eyed  Vireos  showed  little  reaction  to  foliage  changes 
in  their  foraging  behavior,  except  for  foraging  higher  in  the  stand  with  a 
higher  canopy.  A slight  broadening  of  their  niche  may  occur  in  young 
forest,  and  relationships  (overlaps)  with  other  species  change.  Changes  in 
relationships  with  other  species  were  due  to  foraging  shifts  by  other 
species  rather  than  foraging  changes  by  Red-eyed  Vireos. 

Black-throated  Green  Warbler. — Black-throated  Green  Warblers  showed 


Maurer  and  Whitmore  • FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES 


485 


few  changes  in  foraging  between  areas,  except  that  they  foraged  in  dif- 
ferent locations  (x^  = 7.94,  df  = 1,  P = 0.005).  In  the  mature  forest,  war- 
blers used  the  inner  parts  of  branches  and  trunks,  while  neither  of  these 
locations  were  used  in  the  young  forest  (Table  2).  Such  limited  changes 
in  foraging  behavior  were  noticed  in  other  studies  conducted  in  spruce 
forests  (Mac Arthur  1958;  Morse  1968,  1971;  Rabenold  1978).  Foraging 
heights  were  higher  in  the  mature  forest  than  in  the  young  forest  (Table 
3),  although  probabilities  of  foraging  in  each  canopy  layer  were  not  differ- 
ent (x^  = 9.77,  df  = 4,  P = 0.046).  In  the  young  forest.  Black-throated 
Green  Warblers  used  a greater  variety  of  foraging  heights. 

Black-throated  Green  Warblers  had  a wider  foraging  niche  in  the  mature 
forest  than  in  the  young  forest  (Table  4;  ^ = 3.78,  df  = 58,  P ^ 0.001). 
Tree-use  niches  were  of  similar  width  in  both  forests  (Table  4,  ^ = 0.53, 
df  = 39,  P > 0.1).  As  opposed  to  foraging  behaviors,  tree  use  by  Black- 
throated  Green  Warblers  was  narrower  than  other  species. 

As  with  other  species,  foraging  overlaps  were  highest  for  Black-throated 
Green  Warblers  in  the  young  forest,  while  tree-use  dimension  overlaps 
were  lowest  for  aU  species  in  that  forest  (Table  4).  Contrary  to  the  pattern, 
foraging  overlaps  were  higher  with  American  Redstarts  in  the  mature  for- 
est (Maurer  1980)  because  both  species  did  more  flycatching  in  the  mature 
forest  (Table  2). 

American  Redstart. — American  Redstarts  changed  maneuvers  (x^  = 
9.47,  df  = 2,  P = 0.009)  in  the  two  forests;  however,  no  differences  in 
foraging  locations  were  detected  (x^  = 3.56,  df  = 2,  P = 0.24).  The  red- 
start was  the  only  species  that  changed  its  foraging  maneuvers  between 
forests.  Ficken  (1962)  noted  that  this  species  was  very  versatile  in  its  use 
of  foraging  behaviors.  The  change  in  maneuvers  was  probably  due  to  the 
greater  amount  of  flycatching  done  in  the  mature  forest,  coupled  with  less 
hovering.  In  the  young  forest,  there  was  a concentration  of  foraging  (mainly 
hovering)  on  leaves  (Table  2). 

Redstarts  used  different  tree  species  in  each  forest  (x^  = 16.67,  df  = 
4,  P = 0.003).  Foraging  was  concentrated  on  maples  and  oaks  in  the  ma- 
ture forest,  while  oaks  were  not  used  at  aU  in  the  young  forest  (Table  3). 
Use  of  tree  types  was  independent  of  their  abundance  in  the  young  forest 
(X^  = 69.74,  df  = 4,  P < 0.001)  and  the  mature  forest  (x^  = 11.46,  df  = 
4,  P = 0.023).  Generally,  maples  were  used  more  often  than  expected  by 
chance. 

Redstarts  foraged  significantly  higher  in  the  mature  forest  (Table  3). 
The  distribution  of  foraging  acts  (Fig.  IE)  among  canopy  layers  was  dif- 
ferent in  each  forest  (x^  = 19.11,  df  = 5,  P < 0.005).  In  the  young  forest, 
redstarts  foraged  heavily  in  the  6.1-12.2  m canopy  layer,  the  densest  layer 
in  that  forest.  In  contrast,  redstarts  in  the  mature  forest  spent  more  time 


486 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


foraging  in  higher  canopy  layers,  less  time  in  lower  layers  and  foraging  in 
the  6.1-12.2  m canopy  layer  was  less  pronounced. 

Widths  of  foraging  niches  were  not  statistically  different  (t  = 0.15,  df  = 
104,  NS)  between  stands  (Table  4).  Tree-use  niches  were  not  significantly 
different  {t  = 1.64,  df  = 103,  P > 0.1),  indicating  that  the  same  pattern 
observed  for  foraging  was  present  in  tree  use.  That  is,  redstarts  were 
highly  opportunistic  in  their  use  of  maneuvers  and  tree  species,  though 
this  was  not  reflected  in  niche  breadths.  Foraging  overlaps  were  greater 
in  the  young  forest  for  redstarts  and  tree-use  overlaps  were  lower  in  that 
forest  (Table  4). 

Scarlet  Tanager. — This  species  did  not  show  any  differences  in  foraging 
behavior  between  stands  (critical  levels  for  maneuvers,  substrates  and 
locations  all  >0.01).  Hovering  at  leaves  was  the  foraging  technique  used 
most  often  (Table  2),  more  so  than  for  any  other  species.  Although  no 
foraging  changes  occurred.  Scarlet  Tanagers  did  show  differences  in  tree 
use  between  forests  (x^  = 26.48,  df  = 3,  P < 0.001).  Oaks  were  used 
more  often  in  the  young  forest,  while  beeches  were  used  more  often  in  the 
mature  forest  (Table  3). 

Scarlet  Tanagers  foraged  at  the  same  heights  in  both  forests  (Table  3). 
No  difference  in  the  distribution  of  foraging  acts  among  canopy  layers 
(Fig.  IF)  was  detected  (x^  = 9.47,  df  = 45,  NS). 

Niche  widths  were  not  different  between  forests  for  foraging  {t  = 0.29, 
df  = 66,  NS)  and  tree  use  {t  = 1.07,  df  = 45,  NS).  Scarlet  Tanagers 
showed  the  same  patterns  for  overlaps  that  other  species  did  (Table  4). 
Contrary  to  the  pattern,  there  was  higher  tree-use  overlap  with  Black- 
throated  Green  Warblers  in  the  young  forest  (Maurer  1980). 

The  changes  these  five  bird  species  demonstrated  are  summarized  in  Ta- 
ble 5. 


DISCUSSION 

Differences  in  habitat  structure  between  the  two  stands  affected  some 
of  the  species  more  than  others  (Table  5).  The  amount  of  foraging  changes 
demonstrated  by  a species  was  related  to  the  amount  of  flycatching  that 
a species  did.  The  two  species  which  did  not  flycatch.  Red-eyed  Vireos 
and  Scarlet  Tanagers,  showed  few  changes  in  their  foraging  patterns  be- 
tween areas.  Acadian  Flycatchers  and  American  Redstarts,  which  had  the 
highest  frequencies  of  flycatching,  showed  the  most  changes  in  foraging 
(Table  5),  while  Black-throated  Green  Warblers  were  intermediate  in  both 
amount  of  flycatching  and  number  of  foraging  changes.  These  data  suggest 
that  flycatching  species  tend  to  be  more  opportunistic  when  presented 
with  different  foraging  opportunities  than  non-flycatching  species. 


Maurer  and  Whitmore  • FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES 


487 


Table  5 

Changes  in  Foraging  Strategies  of  Five  Species  of  Insectivorous  Birds'* 

Foraging  behavior 

Species 

Manuevers 

Locations  and 
substrates 

Tree  use 

Forciging 

heights 

.Niche  width 

E.  virescens 

no  changes 

decreased  use 
of  woody 
substrates, 
increase  in 
use  of 
peripheral 
foliage 

increased  use 
of  oaks, 
maples, 
beeches, 
decreased 
use  of 
shrubs 

higher 

narrower 
foraging, 
wider  tree 
use 

V.  olivaceus 

no  changes 

no  changes 

no  changes 

slightly  higher 

no  changes 

D.  virens 

no  changes 

decreased  use 
of 

peripheral 

foliage 

no  changes 

higher 

wider  foraging, 
no  change  in 
tree  use 

S.  ruticilla 

more 

flycatching 

less  use  of 
foliage 

increased  use 
of  maples 

slightly  higher 

no  changes 

P.  olivacea 

no  changes 

no  changes 

decreased  use 
of  oaks, 
increased 
use  of 
beeches 

no  change 

no  changes 

® Changes  are  those  that  occur  in  the  mature  forest  relative  to  the  young  forest. 


The  major  structural  difference  between  the  two  forests  was  an  in- 
creased amount  of  open  area  beneath  the  main  concentration  of  leaves  in 
the  mature  forest.  Acadian  Flycatchers  had  narrower  foraging  niches  in 
that  forest.  Since  this  species  foraged  mainly  in  these  open  areas  beneath 
the  canopy,  the  increase  of  open  areas  in  the  mature  forest  suggests  more 
resources  were  available  to  the  flycatchers.  The  narrower  foraging  niche 
of  this  species  is  consistent  with  the  predictions  of  several  optimal  re- 
source use  models  (MacArthur  1972,  Pyke  et  al.  1977).  Two  other  species 
also  appeared  to  react  to  the  increased  open  spaces  in  the  mature  forest. 
Black-throated  Green  Warblers  broadened  their  foraging  niche  in  the  ma- 
ture forest  by  flycatching  more  often.  Redstarts  switched  from  hovering 
most  often  in  the  young  forest  to  flycatching  most  often  in  the  mature 
forest.  These  last  two  species  reacted  opportunistically  to  an  increase  in 
a potential  resource  by  altering  their  use  of  foraging  techniques  to  reflect 
the  increase  in  the  resource. 


A 


488 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Parallel  changes  in  foraging  between  forests,  such  as  those  discussed 
in  the  preceding  paragraph,  suggest,  at  least  for  the  species  considered, 
that  competition  was  not  a proximate  cause  of  foraging  shifts.  Similar 
parallel  changes  in  foraging  heights  were  demonstrated  by  Red-eyed  Vir- 
eos,  American  Redstarts  and  Scarlet  Tanagers.  Each  of  these  species 
concentrated  foraging  behaviors  in  the  dense  6.1-12.2  m canopy  layer  in 
the  young  forest  which  again  seemed  to  suggest  changes  were  occurring 
in  response  to  greater  abundance  of  a potential  resource.  The  results  of 
this  study  clearly  demonstrate  that  bird  species  do  modify  their  foraging 
behavior  when  presented  with  altered  resource  distributions  due  in  part 
to  changes  in  the  structure  of  the  habitat  (see  Grubb  1979).  If  species 
respond  opportunistically  to  changes  in  resource  levels,  what  role  would 
such  opportunism  play  in  determining  which  species  were  present  in  a 
given  habitat?  Further,  which  is  more  important  in  determining  community 
structure,  the  species’  own  abilities  to  use  resources  in  a given  habitat  or 
the  number  of  competitors  using  that  resource?  This  study  implies  that 
the  answer  to  these  questions  may  be  different  than  the  “classical”  an- 
swers (e.g.,  Cody  1974).  Further  research  is  needed  to  clarify  the  rela- 
tionship between  individual  species’  foraging  patterns  and  patterns  of  com- 
munity structure.  Holmes  et  al.  (1979)  did  a community  wide  study  of 
foraging  behavior  and  concluded  that  the  structure  and  composition  of  the 
plant  community  played  a major  role  in  determining  bird  community  struc- 
ture within  and  between  habitats.  The  results  of  our  study  are  very  much 
in  agreement  with  this  idea,  and  suggest  to  us  that  changes  in  bird  com- 
munities between  habitats  are  related  to  altered  resource  distributions 
resulting  from  changes  in  the  structure  and  species  makeup  of  the  plant 
community. 


SUMMARY 

The  influence  of  habitat  structure  on  the  foraging  behavior  of  five  bird  species  was  studied 
in  two  stands  in  the  Fernow  Experimental  Forest.  Vegetation  structure  of  the  stands  was 
found  to  be  different.  One  stand  had  a high,  stratified  canopy  with  a weU-developed  under- 
story and  the  other  had  a lower,  less  stratified,  but  denser  canopy  without  a weU-developed 
understory.  Each  bird  species  showed  differences  in  foraging  behaviors  that  could  be  related 
to  differences  in  vegetation  structure.  One  species,  American  Redstart,  changed  foraging 
maneuvers  between  forests.  Changes  by  other  species  involved  use  of  different  substrates 
or  locations  in  trees,  use  of  different  tree  species,  or  changes  in  foraging  heights.  Species 
that  foraged  by  gleaning  and  hovering  at  foliage  demonstrated  different  foraging  behaviors 
less  often  than  species  which  foraged  by  flycatching.  Some  species  in  this  study  showed 
changes  in  their  foraging  that  coincided  with  increases  in  potential  resources,  e.g.,  Acadian 
Flycatchers  did  more  flycatching  in  the  forest  with  more  open  subcanopy  area.  Parallel 
changes  in  several  instances  suggested  that  competition  was  not  a proximate  cause  of  these 
changes. 


Maurer  and  Whitmore  • FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES 


489 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

We  thank  G.  E.  Lang,  E.  J.  Harner,  R.  F.  Johnston  and  J.  Rice  for  reading  a previous 
draft  of  this  manuscript  and  offering  many  valuable  suggestions.  E.  J.  Harner  provided  a 
great  deal  of  assistance  with  statistical  methods.  We  thank  the  Timber  and  Watershed 
Laboratory,  U.S.D.A.  Forest  Service,  at  Parsons,  West  Virginia  for  providing  housing.  Fund- 
ing for  this  research  was  provided  by  Mclntire-Stennis  Project  #17.  This  research  was  done 
in  partial  fulfillment  of  the  requirements  for  an  M.Sc.  degree  by  the  senior  author  with  the 
Division  of  Forestry,  West  Virginia  University.  Published  with  the  approval  of  the  Director, 
West  Virginia  University  Agricultural  and  Forestry  Experiment  Station  as  scientific  article 
1728. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Brockman,  C.  F.  1968.  Trees  of  North  America.  Golden  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

Cody,  M.  L.  1974.  Competition  and  the  structure  of  bird  communities.  Princeton  Univ. 
Press,  Princeton,  New  Jersey. 

Colwell,  R.  K.  and  D.  J.  Futuyma.  1971.  On  the  measurement  of  niche  breadth  and 
overlap.  Ecology  52:567-576. 

Conover,  W.  J.  1971.  Practical  nonparametric  statistics.  John  Wiley  and  Sons,  New  York, 
New  York. 

Crow,  E.  L.,  F.  A.  Davis  and  M.  W.  Maxfield.  1978.  Statistics.  Coles  Publishing,  To- 
ronto, Ontario. 

Ficken,  M.  S.  1962.  Maintenance  activities  of  the  American  Redstart.  Wilson  Bull.  74:153- 
165. 

Fowells,  H.  a.  1965.  Silvics  of  forest  trees  of  the  United  States.  USDA  For.  Serv.  Agric. 
Handbook  No.  271. 

Grubb,  T.  C.  1979.  Factors  controlling  foraging  strategies  of  insectivorous  birds.  Pp.  119- 
135  in  The  role  of  insectivorous  birds  in  forest  ecosystems  (J.  G.  Dickson,  R.  N.  Connor, 
R.  R.  Fleet,  J.  A.  Jackson  and  J.  C.  KroU,  eds.).  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New 
York. 

Holmes,  R.  T.,  R.  E.  Bonney,  Jr.  and  S.  W.  Pacala.  1979.  Guild  structure  of  the  Hubbard 
Brook  bird  community:  a multivariate  approach.  Ecology  60:512-520. 

Hutcheson,  K.  1970.  A test  for  comparing  diversities  based  on  the  Shannon  formula.  J. 
Theor.  Biol.  29:151-154. 

James,  R.  D.  1976.  Foraging  behavior  and  habitat  selection  of  three  species  of  vireos  in 
southern  Ontario.  Wilson  Bull.  88:62-75. 

MacArthur,  R.  H.  1958.  Population  ecology  of  some  warblers  of  northeastern  coniferous 
forests.  Ecology  39:599-619. 

. 1972.  Geographical  ecology.  Harper  and  Row,  New  York,  New  York. 

Maurer,  B.  A.  1980.  Avian  foraging  and  habitat  structure  in  an  eastern  deciduous  forest 
in  West  Virginia.  M.Sc.  thesis.  West  Virginia  Univ.,  Morgantown,  West  Virginia. 

McArthur,  L.  B.  1980.  The  impact  of  various  forest  management  practices  on  passerine 
community  structure.  Ph.D.  diss..  West  Virginia  Univ.,  Morgantown,  West  Virginia. 

Morse,  D.  H.  1968.  A quantitative  study  of  foraging  of  male  and  female  spruce-woods 
warblers.  Ecology  49:779-784. 

. 1971.  The  foraging  of  warblers  isolated  on  small  islands.  Ecology  52:216-228. 

PlELOU,  E.  C.  1976.  Mathematical  ecology.  John  Wiley  and  Sons,  New  York,  New  York. 

Pyke,  G.  H.,  H.  R.  Pulliam  and  E.  L.  Charnov.  1977.  Optimal  foraging:  a selective 
review  of  theory  and  tests.  Quart.  Rev.  Biol.  52:137-154. 


490 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Rabenold,  K.  N.  1978.  Foraging  strategies,  diversity  and  seasonality  in  bird  communities 
of  Appalachian  spruce-fir  forests.  Ecol.  Monogr.  48:397-424. 

Reinhart,  R.  G.,  A.  R.  Eschner  and  G.  R.  Trimble.  1963.  Effect  on  streamflow  of  four 
forest  practices  in  the  mountains  of  West  Virginia.  Upper  Darby,  Pennsylvania,  North- 
east For.  Exper.  Stat.,  USDA  For.  Serv.  Res.  Pap.  NE-1. 

Smith,  K.  G.  1977.  Distribution  of  summer  birds  along  a forest  moisture  gradient  in  an 
Ozark  watershed.  Ecology  58:810-819. 

Williamson,  P.  1971.  Feeding  ecology  of  the  Red-eyed  Vireo  (Vireo  olivaceus)  and  asso- 
ciated foliage-gleaning  birds.  Ecol.  Monogr.  41:129-152. 

DIV.  FORESTRY,  WEST  VIRGINIA  UNIV.,  MORGANTOWN,  WEST  VIRGINIA 

26506.  (present  address  bam:  center  for  quantitative  studies, 

COLL.  AGRICULTURE,  UNIV.  ARIZONA,  TUCSON,  ARIZONA  85721.)  AC- 
CEPTED 12  MAR.  1981. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  491^99 


AGE  AND  SEX  DIFFERENCES  IN  WING  LOADING  AND 
OTHER  AERODYNAMIC  CHARACTERISTICS  OF 
SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS 

Helmut  C.  Mueller,  Daniel  D.  Berger  and  George  Allez 

Wing  area,  wing  loading  and  other  aerodynamic  characteristics  are  par- 
ticularly important  for  the  diurnal  raptors,  birds  that  spend  considerable 
time  on  the  wing  or  rely  on  agility  in  flight  for  the  capture  of  prey.  Brown 
and  Amadon  (1968)  summarize  data  available  on  wing  loading  for  various 
Falconiformes  and  list  measurements  for  only  56  species.  Of  these  56, 
exactly  half  of  the  wing  loadings  are  based  on  a sample  of  only  one,  both 
sexes  were  measured  for  only  seven  species  and  age  was  not  noted  for 
any.  In  this  paper  we  examine  age  and  sex  differences  in  wing  area,  wing 
loading  and  other  aerodynamic  characteristics  of  Sharp-shinned  Hawks 
{Accipiter  striatus)  based  on  a sample  of  255  wings  and  108  tails.  The 
hawks  were  captured  in  a variety  of  traps  (see  Bub  1974)  at  the  Cedar 
Grove  Ornithological  Station,  on  the  western  shore  of  Lake  Michigan  near 
Cedar  Grove,  Sheboygan  Co.,  Wisconsin.  A description  of  the  Cedar 
Grove  region  can  be  found  in  Mueller  and  Berger  (1966)  and  an  account 
of  tbe  migrations  of  sharp-shins  is  given  in  Mueller  and  Berger  (1967). 

TECHNIQUES 

Birds  were  measured  on  the  day  they  were  captured.  Wing  chord  was  measured  hy  placing 
the  wrist  (bend)  of  the  wing  at  the  zero  point  of  a rule  and  pivoting  the  folded  wing  downward 
until  the  tip  of  the  longest  primary  just  touched  the  rule.  Tail  length  was  measured  by 
inserting  a thin  metal  rule  between  the  central  rectrices  and  sighting  across  the  tops  of  the 
longest  rectrices  of  the  folded  tail.  Both  linear  measurements  were  taken  to  the  nearest 
mm.  Birds  were  weighed  to  the  nearest  gram  on  a balance  graduated  in  0.1  g increments. 
Esophageal  (“crop”)  contents,  if  any,  were  estimated  and  subtracted  from  the  gross  weight. 

The  right  wing  of  a hawk  was  photographed  while  the  ventral  side  was  held  against  a 
vertical,  rigid  sheet  of  clear  plastic  ruled  into  5 cm  squares  (Fig.  1).  In  addition  to  the  squares 
a 10  X 30  cm  rectangle  was  outlined  in  fine  black  tape.  A white  window  shade  about  1 m 
behind  the  plastic  provided  contrast.  The  body  of  the  hawk  was  held  with  one  hand  against 
the  edge  of  the  plastic  and  the  wing  was  held  by  the  manus  with  the  other  hand  so  that  the 
wing  was  barely  in  contact  with  the  plastic.  Another  person  photographed  the  wing,  stationing 
himself  normal  to  the  wing.  The  date,  age,  sex  and  band  number  of  the  hawk  were  affixed 
to  the  plastic  and  included  in  the  photograph. 

The  negatives  of  the  wing  photographs  were  mounted  in  slide  holders  and  projected  to  life 
size  using  a projector  with  a zoom  lens  and  matching  the  10  x 30  cm  rectangle  with  one 
drawn  on  the  screen.  The  outline  of  the  wing  was  traced  on  a piece  of  paper,  along  with  an 
estimated  half  width  of  the  body.  In  some  photographs  the  wing  was  pulled  away  from  the 
body,  exposing  the  axiUars.  The  axillars  were  not  included  in  the  measurement  of  wing  area. 
The  tracing  was  measured  to  the  nearest  0.1  cm^  with  a compensating  polar  planimeter. 

491 


A 


492 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Fig.  1.  Photograph  of  spread  of  wing  of  a Sharp-shinned  Hawk. 


Each  tracing  was  measured  twice,  or  until  2 measurements  were  obtained  that  differed  by 
less  than  1%.  In  a test  of  the  accuracy  of  wing  measurement  the  wing  of  1 hawk  was 
photographed  15  times.  Holders  and  photographers  were  alternated  quasi-randomly  among 
the  three  authors.  Each  of  us  held  the  bird  5 times.  Between  photographs,  the  bird  was 
returned  to  its  holding  tube  for  at  least  a few  minutes.  An  analysis  of  variance  revealed  no 
significant  difference  among  holders.  The  area  for  the  right  wing  was  272.43  ± 10.18  (SD), 
yielding  a standard  error  of  measurement  of  2.07%  at  the  95%  confidence  interval. 

On  the  tracing,  wing  length  was  measured  from  the  tip  of  the  longest  primary  to  the  body. 
Wing  span  was  obtained  by  adding  wing  length  to  the  measurement  of  half  body  width  and 
multiplying  by  2.  Average  wing  width  was  calculated  by  dividing  the  area  of  one  wing  by  wing 
length.  Aspect  ratio  was  obtained  by  dividing  wing  length  by  wing  width.  The  wing  area 
given  in  Table  1 was  obtained  by  multiplying  the  area  of  the  measured  wing  by  2.  Wing  load 
was  calculated  by  dividing  the  weight  of  the  bird  by  its  total  wing  area. 

Holding  the  tail  of  a live  bird  for  photographing  the  area  is  difficult.  Tails  were  held  up 
against  the  vertical  plastic  used  for  wing  measurements  (Fig.  2).  Angles  of  spread  varied 
from  41°-111°.  Photographic  negatives  were  projected  and  traced  using  the  same  methods 
as  for  wings.  The  base  of  the  tail  was  obscured  by  the  holder,  and  we  resorted  to  the  following 
adjustments.  A straight  edge  was  oriented  along  the  outer  edge  of  the  outermost  rectrix  on 
the  tracing  and  a line  drawn  extending  the  base  of  the  tail  (dashed  lines  in  Fig.  3).  This 
procedure  was  repeated  for  the  other  side  of  the  tail  so  that  the  two  extended  lines  met.  The 
angle  thus  formed  (A)  was  measured  as  the  angle  of  spread.  The  distance  from  the  apex  of 


Mueller  et  al.  • AERODYNAMICS  OF  SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS 


493 


Table  1 

Wing  Area  and  Related  Measurements  for  Sharp-shinned  Hawks 


Adult  6 6 Juvenile  S cJ  Adult  9 9 Juvenile  9 9 

N = 54  N = 90  N = 54  N = 57 

Measurements  i ± SD  i ± SD  i ± SD  jt  ± SD 


Wing  chord  (cm) 
(corrected) 
Wing  length  (cm) 
Wing  width  (cm) 
Wing  span  (cm) 
Aspect  ratio 
Wing  area  (cm^) 
(corrected) 
Weight  (g) 
(corrected) 
Wing  load  (g/cm^) 
(corrected) 


17.24  ± 0.33**^ 
(17.15)** 
24.35  ± 0.72** 
8.91  ± 0.31* 
53.06  ± 1.45** 
2.74  ± 0.12** 
434.06  ± 20.78** 
(429.94)* 
101.24  ± 6.42** 
(102.89)** 
0.233  ± 0.02 
(0.239)** 


16.88  ± 0.32 
(16.87) 
23.77  ± 1.02 
8.83  ± 0.24 
51.98  ± 2.06 
2.69  ± 0.11 
419.93  ± 25.78 
(420.63) 
96.43  ± 5.97 
(97.50) 
0.230  ± 0.02 
(0.232) 


20.22  ± 0.78** 

28.52  ± 1.10** 
10.33  ± 0.44* 
64.02  ± 2.18* 
2.76  ± 0.13* 
589.77  ± 38.85** 

176.26  ± 11.57** 

0.300  ± 0.03** 


19.92  ± 0.35 

27.75  ± 0.73 

10.20  ± 0.30 

62.21  ± 1.47 
2.72  ± 0.09 

565.29  ± 24.84 

163.38  ± 9.03 
0.289  ± 0.02 


* Differs  significantly  from  juveniles,  P < 0.05,  t-test,  1-tailed;  **  differs  significantly  from  the  juveniles,  P < 0.01. 

® Significantly  larger  than  the  mean  given  in  Mueller  et  al.  (1979),  P < 0.05,  t-test,  2-tailed.  Corrected  means  for  wing 
chord  and  weight  are  from  the  larger  sample  of  Mueller  et  al.,  (1979).  Corrected  wing  areas  are  based  on  regressions  of 
area  on  wing  chord. 


this  angle  to  the  longest  rectrices  was  measured  (B),  and  from  this  measurement  we  sub- 
tracted tail  length  (D,  as  measured  from  the  live  bird).  This  difference  (C)  was  then  measured 
from  the  apex  of  the  angle  and  an  arc  drawn  across  the  tail  with  a compass  (dashed  line). 
The  tail  area  measured  with  the  planimeter  included  everything  within  solid  and  dashed  lines 
except  the  basal  segment  of  the  circle  with  radius  C (Fig.  3).  Since  tail  area  varies  with  angle 
of  spread,  we  developed  a formula  which  estimates  tail  area.  We  first  calculated  for  each 
tail:  (sin  A)(length^),  where  A is  the  angle  of  spread  and  length  is  the  tail  length  measured 
from  the  live  bird.  We  then  calculated  a hnear  regression  for  all  108  tails,  giving  us  the 
formula: 


Area  = 4.1189  + 0.9624<sin  Aflength^]). 

The  equation  is  an  excellent  fit  to  the  data  (r  = 0.98). 

RESULTS 

Wing  photographs  were  taken  when  sufficient  personnel  were  available, 
and  the  sample  of  255  wing  area  photographs  was  not  randomly  distributed 
throughout  the  autumn.  We  have  found  that  the  population  of  Sharp- 
shinned  Hawks  caught  at  Cedar  Grove  shows  complex  changes  in  wing 
chord  and  weight  through  the  autumn  (Allez  et  ah,  unpubl.),  and  the  birds 
sampled  for  wing  areas  thus  might  not  be  representative.  We  compared 
wing  chord  and  weight  from  our  sample  for  wing  areas  with  wing 
chord  and  weight  from  a sample  of  almost  2000  birds  taken  from  the  entire 
season  in  the  years  1953-1964  (Mueller  et  al.  1979). 


494 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Fig.  2.  Photograph  of  spread  of  tail  of  a Sharp-shinned  Hawk. 


Fig.  3.  Method  of  measuring  tail  area.  The  sohd  lines  are  traced  from  Fig.  2.  See  text  for 
explanation  of  dashed  hnes  and  symbols. 


Mueller  et  al.  • AERODYNAMICS  OF  SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS 


495 


Only  one  measurement  from  the  sample  wing  areas  differed  significantly 
(f-test,  P < 0.05,  2-tailed)  from  those  of  the  larger  sample — adult  males 
had  significantly  longer  wing  chords  in  the  sample  taken  for  wing  area.  In 
addition,  these  adult  males  were  almost  significantly  lighter  in  weight 
(P  < 0.08).  Since  wing  area  is  in  part  a function  of  wing  length,  our  sample 
of  wing  areas  for  adult  males  thus  overestimates  wing  area  and  underes- 
timates wing  loading.  To  obtain  better  estimates,  we  performed  linear 
regressions  of  wing  chord  on  wing  area  for  both  adult  and  juvenile  males 
and  then  calculated  “corrected”  wing  areas  and  wing  loadings  based  on 
the  mean  wing  chords  and  weights  from  the  larger  sample  of  Mueller  et 
al.  (1979). 

Adults  of  both  sexes  are  significantly  larger  than  juveniles  in  all  mea- 
surements: wing  chord,  wing  length,  wing  width,  wing  span,  aspect  ratio, 
wing  area,  weight  and  wing  loading;  with  one  exception — the  wing  loading 
of  males  (Table  1).  However,  the  corrected  estimates  of  wing  loading  of 
males,  based  on  the  larger,  more  representative  sample,  show  a signifi- 
cantly greater  wing  loading  for  adults  than  juveniles,  as  well  as  signifi- 
cantly greater  wing  chord,  wing  area  and  weight.  Two  differences  between 
adults  and  juveniles  are  disproportionate:  (1)  the  increase  in  width  of  wings 
in  adults  is  less  than  half  the  increase  in  wing  length,  resulting  in  a higher 
aspect  ratio  in  adults,  and  (2)  the  increase  in  wing  area  in  adults  is  pro- 
portionately only  about  half  as  great  as  the  increase  in  weight  resulting  in 
higher  wing  loading  in  adults.  Females  of  both  age  groups  are  significantly 
larger  than  males  {P  < 0.0001)  in  wing  chord,  wing  length,  wing  width, 
wing  span,  wing  area,  weight  and  wing  loading  but  do  not  differ  signifi- 
cantly in  aspect  ratio  {P  > 0.05,  Table  1). 

Estimated  tail  areas  for  three  angles  of  spread  are  presented  in  Table  2. 
The  tail  lengths  used  to  calculate  areas  using  the  regression  equation  are 
the  means  from  Mueller  et  al.  (1979)  because  three  of  the  four  means  from 
the  samples  taken  from  tail  photographs  differed  significantly  from  the 
means  from  the  larger  sample.  We  have  examined  spread  tails  in  many 
live  Sharp-shinned  Hawks,  a few  dead  individuals  and  the  102  photographs 
for  tracings;  in  all  of  these,  of  course,  the  tail  was  spread  manually  by  us. 
We  estimate  that  a Sharp-shinned  Hawk  in  normal  flapping  flight  has  its 
tail  spread  about  15°.  We  estimate  that  the  maximum  possible  spread, 
without  separation  of  rectrices,  is  about  100°.  The  tail  shown  in  Figs.  2 
and  3 is  spread  107°.  At  120°  of  spread  there  is  some  separation  of  rec- 
trices, and  the  areas  presented  for  this  angle  of  spread  probably  are  over- 
estimates. In  Table  2 we  also  present  total  flight  surface  area  (wings  plus 
tail)  and  the  loading  of  total  flight  surface.  The  tail  areas  of  juveniles 
average  about  3%  greater  than  that  of  adults.  The  total  flight  surface  area 
of  adults  is  greater  because  adults  have  greater  wing  areas  than  juveniles 
(Table  1).  Tail  area  constitutes  only  about  10%  of  the  total  flight  surface 


496 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  2 

Estimated  Tail  Areas  in  Sharp-shinned  Hawks 

Age 

Sex 

Area 

(cm^) 

Total  surface® 

Load'* 

(g/cm^) 

Adult 

3 

Tail  spread  15° 
47.75 

477.69 

0.215 

Juvenile 

3 

49.17 

469.80 

0.208 

Adult 

9 

64.50 

654.27 

0.269 

Juvenile 

9 

66.03 

631.32 

0.259 

Adult 

3 

Tail  spread  100° 
201.97 

631.91 

0.163 

Juvenile 

3 

208.39 

629.02 

0.155 

Adult 

9 

277.94 

867.71 

0.203 

Juvenile 

9 

284.84 

850.13 

0.192 

Adult 

d 

Tail  spread  120° 
256.99 

686.93 

0.150 

Juvenile 

d 

265.19 

685.82 

0.142 

Adult 

9 

354.08 

943.85 

0.187 

Juvenile 

9 

362.90 

928.15 

0.176 

® Wing  and  tail  area. 
Total  surface  load. 


area  when  the  tail  is  spread  15°,  but  when  the  tail  is  spread  100°  the  tail 
constitutes  about  a third  of  the  total  flight  surface  area.  The  difference 
between  adults  and  juveniles  in  total  flight  surface  area  is  about  1.7% 
(males)  to  3.6%  (females)  when  the  tail  is  spread  15°.  This  difference 
decreases  to  0.5%  and  2.1%,  respectively,  when  the  tail  is  spread  100°. 

The  total  surface  loading  is  about  3.5%  greater  in  adults  when  the  tail 
is  spread  15°;  this  difference  increases  to  about  5.5%  when  the  tail  is 
spread  100°.  Juveniles  thus  have  considerably  lower  flight-surface  loadings 
when  the  tail  is  spread. 

DISCUSSION 

Adults  are  larger  than  juveniles  in  every  measurement  taken  except  tail 
length  and  tail  area;  adults  are  larger  than  juveniles  in  many  animals  but 
in  birds  the  restrictions  imposed  by  flight  and  the  persistence  of  non- 
growing remiges  and  rectrices  for  a year  or  longer  impose  severe  limits  on 
the  magnitude  of  age  differences.  Birds  cannot  gradually  increase  in  size; 
the  change  is  rapid  as  old  flight  feathers  are  replaced  by  new.  A large 
change  in  the  length  of  remiges  would  require  compensatory  large  and 
rapid  changes  in  musculature  and  perhaps  even  in  the  skeleton  or  in  large 


Mueller  et  al.  • AERODYNAMICS  OF  SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS 


497 


changes  in  flight  and  foraging  behavior.  Mueller  and  Berger  (1970)  have 
shown  that  sharp-shins  require  considerable  experience  to  perfect  pred- 
atory techniques;  rapid  transitions  in  these  behaviors  are  unlikely.  The 
differences  in  size  between  adult  and  juvenile  Sharp-shinned  Hawks  are 
small,  but  are  statistically  significant,  and  we  believe  that  there  is  an 
adaptive  rationale  for  these  differences. 

A bird  is  able  to  fly  because  an  airfoil  moving  through  the  air  generates 
lift.  A bird  moving  through  the  air  also  induces  drag,  turbulent  eddies 
which  reduce  its  speed.  Much  of  the  induced  drag  is  generated  by  vortices 
at  the  tip  of  the  wing.  In  gliding  flight,  aerodynamic  efficiency  is  maximized 
by  increasing  the  aspect  ratio  of  the  wing  (length/ width),  in  effect  decreas- 
ing the  portion  of  the  total  wing  area  which  is  at  the  tip  where  lift  is 
diminished  by  induced  drag.  In  flapping  flight  a long  wing  is  less  “effi- 
cient” because  it  requires  more  power  to  move  a long  wing  than  a short 
one.  A gliding  bird  has  a minimum  (stalling)  speed.  Flight  is  impossible  at 
slower  speeds  because  the  airflow  over  the  wings  breaks  up  into  turbulent 
eddies  and  lift  is  lost.  Stalling  speeds  are  a function  of  wing  loading  (the 
weight  carried  by  the  wing  area).  High  wing  loadings  produce  high  stalling 
speeds  and  low  wing  loadings  permit  birds  to  fly  more  slowly.  A bird  can 
maintain  flight  speed  by  decreasing  its  angle  of  attack  (the  angle  the  wing 
makes  with  the  horizontal),  and  thus  losing  altitude,  or  by  powered  flight 
(flapping  its  wings).  A soaring  bird  is  actually  gliding  downward  but  the 
air  is  rising  more  rapidly  than  the  bird  is  sinking.  A bird  with  a light  wing 
loading  can  glide  more  slowly  than  a bird  with  a heavy  wing  loading  and 
thus  will  rise  more  rapidly  in  a given  updraft  and  and  be  able  to  use  less 
powerful  updrafts.  Low  wing  loadings  and  lower  stalling  speeds  also  in- 
crease maneuverability. 

Tails  also  contribute  to  lift,  particularly  in  slow,  soaring  flight;  this  is 
why  hawks  spread  their  tails  when  soaring  in  an  updraft.  Although  tails 
do  not  produce  as  much  lift  per  unit  area  as  wings,  a bird  with  a longer 
tail  and  larger  tail  area  when  spread  may  have  some  advantage  over  a bird 
with  a shorter  tail  and  less  tail  area.  This  is  because  a folded  tail  induces 
little  drag  in  flapping  flight,  need  not  be  flapped  as  a wing,  but  is  available 
for  added  lift  when  needed.  The  main  advantage  of  increased  tail  area, 
however,  is  that  it  enhances  maneuverability.  Large  “reserve”  control 
area,  as  produced  by  a spread  tail  is  of  particular  advantage  at  slow  flight 
speeds  since  the  force  produced  by  a deflecting  surface  is  proportional  to 
the  area  of  the  surface  and  the  square  of  air  speed.  Birds  which  fly  rapidly 
thus  need  smaller  control  surfaces  than  those  which  fly  slowly.  (For  a more 
detailed,  yet  comprehensible,  explanation  of  bird  flight  see  Barlee  1964.) 

The  above  aerodynamic  considerations  lead  to  the  conclusions  pre- 
sented below.  The  low  wing  loadings  of  juvenile  sharp-shins  give  them  the 


498 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


potential  of  using  lighter  updrafts  than  adults,  a capability  which  is  further 
enhanced  by  the  large  tail  area.  Shorter  wings  require  less  power  to  flap 
and  hence  less  energy  consumption  than  longer-winged  adults.  All  of  these 
considerations  suggest  that  juveniles  expend  less  energy  in  flying  than  do 
adults.  The  light  total  surface  loadings  of  juveniles  provide  them  with 
inherently  greater  maneuverability  than  adults,  with  the  large  tail  area 
playing  an  important  role. 

Adults  weigh  more  than  juveniles  and  this  difference  is  almost  certainly 
not  the  result  of  fat  deposition  and  is  most  likely  due  to  larger  flight  mus- 
cles in  adults  (Mueller  et  al.  1979).  The  greater  weight,  larger  wing  mus- 
cles, greater  wing  loading  and  greater  aspect  ratio  of  adults  should  produce 
a more  rapid  and  powerful  flight  and  a greater  force  in  striking  prey  than 
that  of  juveniles,  but  at  the  expense  of  greater  energy  expenditure.  The 
apparent  loss  in  aerodynamic  maneuverability  in  adults  may  weU  be  com- 
pensated for  by  the  greater  power  and  experience  of  adults  as  compared 
to  juveniles.  Adults  are  thus  faster,  higher  performance  “flying  machines” 
than  juveniles,  but  require  higher  energy  expenditures  and  thus  higher 
food  consumption  than  juveniles. 

Adults  thus  appear  to  be  an  “optimal  design”  for  rapid  and  powerful 
pursuit  of  prey  and  the  juveniles  a lower  performance  configuration,  but 
one  which  is  inherently  more  maneuverable  and  requires  less  energy  con- 
sumption and  expenditure.  As  juveniles  gain  experience  they  can  afford 
the  adult  configuration;  both  aerodynamic  forms  are  thus  adaptive.  Read- 
ers should  be  reminded  that  the  above  conclusions  are  based  on  morpho- 
metric measurements  and  aerodynamic  considerations;  direct  measure- 
ments of  performance  and  metabolic  needs  are  lacking  and  would  be 
extremely  difficult,  if  not  impossible,  to  obtain. 

SUMMARY 

Adult  sharp-shins  have  significantly  longer  and  wider  wings,  greater  wing  areas,  higher 
wing  loadings  and  higher  aspect  ratios  than  juveniles.  Juveniles  have  longer  tails  and  greater 
tail  areas  than  adults.  Within  an  age  class,  females  are  larger  than  males  in  all  of  the  above 
measures  except  aspect  ratio.  The  differences  in  aerodynamic  characteristics  between  the 
age  classes  permit  and  obligate  adults  to  fly  faster,  result  in  greater  striking  force  at  prey, 
and  probably  require  more  energy  consumption  than  in  juveniles.  Juveniles  are  more  ma- 
neuverable and  probably  require  less  energy  in  flight.  The  higher  weight  of  adults  is  probably 
due  to  increased  flight  musculature  and  the  added  power,  plus  experience,  probably  com- 
pensate for  the  aerodynamic  disadvantages  of  adults  as  compared  to  juveniles. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Sandra  Dziedzic  and  Patricia  Christy  traced  and  measured  the  photographs  of  the  wings 
and  tails  and  performed  statistical  calculations.  N.  S.  Mueller  and  R.  H.  Wiley  offered 
helpful  comments  on  the  manuscript.  The  fieldwork  was  aided  by  a grant  from  the  Frank  M. 
Chapman  Memorial  Fund. 


Mueller  et  al.  • AERODYNAMICS  OF  SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS 


499 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Barlee,  J.  1964.  Flight.  Pp.  299-307  in  A new  dictionary  of  birds.  (A.  L.  Thomson,  ed.). 
Nelson,  London,  England. 

Brown,  L.  and  D.  Amadon.  1968.  Eagles,  hawks,  and  falcons  of  the  world.  McGraw-Hill, 
New  York,  New  York. 

Bub,  H.  1974.  Vogelfang  und  Vogelberingung.  Teil  III.  A.  Ziemsen,  Wittenberg-Lutherstadt, 
E.  Germany. 

Mueller,  H.  C.  and  D.  D.  Berger.  1966.  Analyses  of  weight  and  fat  variations  in  transient 
Swainson’s  thrushes.  Bird-Banding  37:83-112. 

AND  . 1967.  Fall  migration  of  Sharp-shinned  Hawks.  Wilson  BuU.  79:397- 

415. 

AND  . 1970.  Prey  preferences  in  the  Sharp-shinned  Hawk:  the  role  of  sex, 

experience  and  motivation.  Auk  87:452-457. 

, AND  G.  Allez.  1979.  Age  and  sex  differences  in  size  of  Sharp-shinned 

Hawks.  Bird-Banding  50:34-44. 

DEPT.  ZOOLOGY  AND  CURRICULUM  IN  ECOLOGY,  UNIV.  NORTH  CAROLINA, 
CHAPEL  HILL,  NORTH  CAROLINA  27514  (HCM)  AND  CEDAR  GROVE  OR- 
NITHOLOGICAL STATION,  ROUTE  1,  CEDAR  GROVE,  WISCONSIN  53013 
(DDE,  GA).  ACCEPTED  19  MAR.  1981. 


A 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  500-505 


EVIDENCE  EOR  AERODYNAMIC  ADVANTAGES  OE 
TAIL  KEELING  IN  THE  COMMON  CRACKLE 

Scott  Hickman 

The  behavioral  function  of  tail  keeling  in  the  Common  Crackle  {Quis- 
calus  quiscula)  has  been  thoroughly  documented  as  a male  flight  display 
(Bent  1958;  Ficken  1963;  Wiens  1965;  Maxwell  1970;  Wiley  1976a,  1976b). 
The  purpose  of  this  study  is  to  determine  if  keeling  functions  aerody- 
namically  as  well. 

STUDY  AREA  AND  METHODS 

The  data  used  in  this  study  were  collected  through  field  observations  of  flying  Common 
Crackles.  A total  of  3507  nonbreeding  season  observations  was  recorded  from  August  1974 
through  February  1975.  Breeding  season  observations  totaled  1415  and  were  recorded  from 
March  1975  through  May  1975.  Observations  were  recorded  in  the  vicinity  of  Bloomington- 
Normal,  McLean  Co.,  Belleville,  St.  Clair  Co.,  and  Carbondale,  Perry  Co.,  all  in  Illinois. 

Each  observation  was  a record  of  the  configuration  of  the  rectrices  of  a flying  bird  and  its 
concomitant  flight  status.  The  tail  configuration  was  recorded  as  one  of  four  possible  degrees 
of  keeling:  (1)  flat  tail — tail  forms  one  plane  (180°),  (2)  shallow  keel — slight  depression  of 
midline  rectrices  folds  the  tail  into  a shallow  V of  approximately  160°,  (3)  medium  keel — 
increased  depression  of  central  rectrices  folds  the  tail  into  a deeper  V of  approximately  120°, 
(4)  deep  keel — complete  depression  of  central  rectrices  in  which  the  tail  is  folded  into  a deep 
V of  less  than  approximately  110°.  These  4 categories  were  selected  because  they  represent 
the  maximum  number  of  keeling  positions  I could  accurately  distinguish.  Observations  which 
I could  not  clearly  assign  to  any  of  these  categories  were  disregarded. 

In  addition  to  a flying  grackle’s  degree  of  keeling,  I also  recorded  the  following  14  char- 
acteristics: (1)  sex — male  or  female;  (2)  season — breeding  or  nonbreeding;  (3)  relative  wind 
direction — wind  direction  relative  to  bird  flight  direction;  (4)  wing  speed — m/sec;  (5)  wind 
character — steady  or  gusty;  (6)  bird  braking — yes  or  no;  (7)  bird  banking — yes  or  no;  (8)  bird 
angle — ascending,  descending  or  level  flight;  (9)  company — bird  accompanied  or  alone;  (10) 
bird  flight — flapping  or  gliding;  (11)  tail  spread — tail  fanned  or  not;  (12)  tail  molting — yes  or 
no;  (13)  entering  roost — whether  or  not  the  bird  was  entering  a roost;  (14)  leaving  roost — 
whether  or  not  the  bird  was  leaving  a roost. 

I treated  the  14  characteristics  of  bird  status  as  independent  variables  and  degree  of  tail 
keeling  as  the  dependent  variable.  For  mathematical  analysis  the  4 degrees  of  keeling  (flat, 
shallow,  medium  and  deep)  were  assigned  the  values  0,  1,  2 and  3,  respectively.  Stepwise 
multiple  regression  analysis  was  used  to  determine  which  independent  variables,  and/or 
combinations  of  independent  variables,  could  account  for  a significant  proportion  of  the 
variability  in  keel  depth.  Pearson  product-moment  correlations  were  also  calculated.  One- 
way analysis  of  variance  was  used  to  test  for  significant  differences  between  mean  degrees 
of  tail  keeling  associated  with  each  value  of  an  independent  variable.  The  breeding  season 
and  nonbreeding  season  data  were  treated  separately,  then  pooled,  for  aU  statistical  tests. 

RESULTS 

The  results  of  the  stepwise  multiple  regression  analysis  are  shown  in 
Tables  1,  2 and  3 for  the  nonbreeding  season,  breeding  season  and  corn- 


500 


Hickman  • COMMON  CRACKLE  TAIL  KEELING 


501 


Table  1 

Parameters  Associated  with  Keeling  During  the  Nonbreeding  Season 


Step 
and  df 

Independent  variable 

R^ 

F 

Fa 

1 

Tail  spread 

0.368 

1965 

12.10 

2 

Company 

0.426 

1305 

7.60 

3 

Banking 

0.479 

1077 

5.91 

4 

Molting 

0.487 

845 

5.00 

5 

Braking 

0.494 

648 

4.52 

® F value  that  must  be  exceeded  to  attain  P < 0.001. 


bined  seasons  data,  respectively.  These  tables  indicate  the  maximum  cu- 
mulative percent  variance  accounted  for  (/?^)  by  independent  variables. 
Table  4 indicates  what  mean  keel  depths  are  associated  with  various  in- 
dependent variable  values. 

Deep  keel  was  displayed  only  during  the  breeding  season  and  only  by 
males.  During  the  breeding  season  50%  of  all  observed  males  had  their 
tails  in  deep  keel.  Deep  keel  was  the  only  tail  shape  used  more  by  one  sex 
than  the  other. 

Medium  keel  was  rarely  used  by  either  sex.  Only  4.5%  of  observed 
males  and  females  showed  medium  keel. 

Only  nonbreeding  season  data  are  used  to  analyze  shallow  keel.  This  is 
because  frequent  sexual  display  of  deep  keel  by  males  during  the  breeding 
season  under  virtually  all  conditions  consistently  increased  the  average 
keel  depth  correlated  with  each  independent  variable.  This  masked  the 
actual  relationship  between  aerodynamic  factors  and  shallow  keel. 

Crackles  that  were  braking,  tail  spreading,  banking,  ascending  or  with 


Table  2 

Parameters  Associated  with  Keeling  During  the  Breeding  Season 


Step 
and  df 

Independent  variable 

F 

Fa 

1 

Sex 

0.228 

416 

12.10 

2 

Entering  roost 

0.302 

305 

7.60 

3 

Bird  angle 

0.320 

221 

5.91 

4 

Banking 

0.331 

174 

5.00 

5 

Wind  direction 

0.341 

145 

4.52 

“ F value  that  must  be  exceeded  to  attain  P < 0.001. 


502 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  3 

Parameters  Associated  with  Keeling  for  Combined  Data 

Step 
and  df 

Independent  variable 

R2 

F 

Fa 

1 

Season 

0.218 

1373 

12.10 

2 

Braking 

0.273 

926 

7.60 

3 

Sex 

0.315 

757 

5.91 

4 

Company 

0.349 

659 

5.00 

5 

Banking 

0.375 

592 

4.52 

6 

Entering  roost 

0.387 

524 

4.02 

7 

Leaving  roost 

0.394 

479 

3.72 

8 

Bird  angle 

0.409 

427 

3.48 

9 

Wind  direction 

0.413 

385 

3.30 

® F value  that  must  be  exceeded  to  attain  P < 0.001. 


tails  in  molt  often  used  shallow  keel.  Sixty-three  percent  of  all  braking, 
64.9%  of  all  tail  spreading,  85%  of  all  banking,  44.7%  of  all  ascending  and 
42.2%  of  all  tail  molting  birds  used  shallow  keel.  This  shallow  keel  differed 
significantly  (P  ^ 0.01)  from  the  nearly  flat  tail  seen  in  the  remaining 
contexts  (Table  4).  Flat  tail  was  noted  in  only  24%  of  all  braking,  23%  of 
all  tail  spreading,  17%  of  all  banking,  37%  of  all  ascending  and  22%  of  all 
tail  molting  grackles. 

Eighty-six  percent  of  all  grackles  flying  into  a head  wind  used  flat  tail. 
This  represents  at  least  11%  more  use  than  flat  tail  generated  with  any 
other  wind  direction.  Flat  tail  was  also  the  configuration  most  used  for 
level,  non-maneuvering  flight  such  as  when  flying  to  or  from  a roost.  Ap- 
proximately 90%  of  all  grackles  entering  or  leaving  a roost  possessed  flat 
tail  and  88%  of  all  grackles  recorded  as  flying  level  and  not  banking  had 
their  tails  in  a flat  position. 


DISCUSSION 

Aerodynamic  factors  account  for  variability  in  keel  depth.  Table  1 shows 
that  tail  spread  (usually  associated  with  landing),  banking,  tail  molting  and 
braking  are  significant  predictors  of  keel  depth  during  the  nonbreeding 
season.  Similarly,  Table  2 indicates  that  bird  angle,  banking  and  wind 
direction  account  for  variance  in  keel  depth  during  the  breeding  season 
and  braking,  banking,  bird  angle  and  wind  direction  are  predictors  of  keel 
depth  when  the  seasonal  data  are  pooled.  This  is  evidence  that  tail  keeling 
has  aerodynamic  functions. 

The  most  likely  aerodynamic  uses  of  tail  keeling  are  stall  prevention 
and  improvement  of  stability.  Grackles  observed  to  be  landing,  banking, 
taking  off,  or  with  tails  in  molt  typically  possessed  shallow  keel.  These  are 


Hickman  • COMMON  CRACKLE  TAIL  KEELING 


503 


Table  4 

Keel  Depths  Associated  with  Various  Independent  Variables  (Nonbreeding 

Season) 


Independent  variables 

X 

SE 

N 

Braking 

No 

0.12* 

0.0070 

2953 

Yes 

0.90* 

0.0250 

555 

Tail  spread 

No 

0.11* 

0.0065 

2905 

Yes 

0.90* 

0.0239 

601 

Bird  angle 

Level 

0.40* 

0.0337 

66 

Descending 

0.30* 

0.0091 

3125 

Ascending 

0.80* 

0.0235 

317 

Banking 

No 

0.20* 

0.0079 

3202 

Yes 

1.0* 

0.2150 

305 

Molting 

No 

0.41* 

0.0075 

3072 

Yes 

0.80* 

0.0344 

436 

* Significant  at  P < 0.001. 


all  conditions  during  which  birds  are  susceptible  to  stall  and/or  are  unsta- 
ble. 

Landing  birds  are  flying  slowly  and  in  danger  of  stalling  since  lift  is 
directly  proportional  to  air  speed.  Birds  typically  prevent  stalls  during 
landing  by  spreading  and  depressing  the  flat  tail  which  draws  the  airflow 
down  and  caudally  from  the  dorsal  surfaces  of  the  wings.  This  keeps  the 
airflow  from  breaking  away  from  the  wing  surfaces  and  prevents  stalling 
(Pennycuick  1972).  Landing  grackles,  however,  did  not  usually  possess  a 
flat  tail.  Grackles  that  were  landing  were  most  often  recorded  as  tail  spread 
and  braking.  The  correlations  in  the  results  section  and  Table  4 indicate 
that  these  grackles  used  shallow  keel  rather  than  the  flat  tail  described 
above.  Shallow  keel  may  be  more  effective  in  stall  prevention  than  flat  tail 
since  depression  of  the  central  rectrices  may  funnel  air  downward  from 
the  wings  more  effectively. 

Shallow  keel  may  also  reduce  the  instability  encountered  during  landing. 
When  used  as  an  air  brake  the  flat  tail  would  create  some  directional 
instability  to  be  controlled  by  the  wings.  However,  shallow  keel  positions 
the  ventral  surface  of  the  braking  tail  into  a wedge,  thereby  giving  the 
spread  tail  a guiding  quality  which  increases  landing  precision.  The  dorsal 
surface  of  a tail  in  shallow  keel  could  also  aid  in  the  development  of 


504 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


increased  landing  precision  as  it  may  funnel  the  caudal  airflow  into  a 
longitudinal  axis  and  thereby  minimize  yaw. 

Crackles  normally  used  shallow  keel  while  banking  (Table  4).  This  may 
also  be  functionally  interpreted  in  terms  of  stall  prevention  and  increased 
flight  stability.  Birds  are  susceptible  to  stall  during  banking  (Dalton  1977). 
Positioning  the  tail  in  shallow  keel  while  banking  may  function  to  prevent 
stalls  as  described  above.  Stability  is  also  reduced  during  banking  because 
the  bird’s  body  passes  through  a vertical  rather  than  horizontal  plane. 
Airfoils  are  not  parallel  to  the  ground  and  are  less  efficient  in  resisting 
gravity  and  regulating  yaw.  Shallow  keel,  however,  would  increase  the 
stability  of  a turn  by  making  the  tail  a 2-plane  surface  that  would  funnel 
the  airflow  into  a longitudinal  axis  and  minimize  yaw. 

The  mean  keel  depth  used  in  ascending  flight  also  approximated  shallow 
keel  (Table  4).  Most  ascending  grackles  were  observed  while  taking  flight. 
During  take-off,  air  speed  is  slow  and  thus  conducive  to  stall  (Salt  1966). 
Shallow  keel  may  prevent  stalling  during  take-off  in  the  same  manner  as 
proposed  above  for  landing  and  banking. 

Most  grackles  used  shallow  keel  while  their  tails  were  in  molt,  this 
shallow  keel  being  significantly  deeper  than  that  of  grackles  with  tails  not 
in  molt  (Table  4).  Many  birds  with  tails  in  molt  had  no  fuU  length  rectrices. 
Such  an  abnormally  short  tail  cannot  provide  the  stability  of  a full  length 
tail.  Shallow  keel  would  presumably  help  to  regain  the  stability  lost  during 
tail  molt. 

Shallow  keel  seems  to  be  the  only  recorded  keel  shape  that  is  primarily 
aerodynamic  in  function.  Deep  keel  was  correlated  with  behavioral  rather 
than  aerodynamic  conditions.  This  caused  sex  to  account  for  more  of  the 
variability  in  the  breeding  season  data  than  did  any  other  independent 
variable  (Table  2).  This  is  in  agreement  with  the  conclusions  of  earlier 
researchers  that  keeling  functions  behaviorally.  Medium  keel  was  seldom 
used  by  Common  Grackles  of  either  sex.  It  seems  to  exist  only  as  an 
intermediate  position  through  which  the  tail  passes  when  changing  from 
shallow  keel  to  deep  keel  or  vice  versa.  Flat  tail  functions  in  several  dis- 
plays performed  by  this  species  (Ficken  1963;  Wiley  1976a,  1976b).  My 
results  indicate  that  the  aerodynamic  situations  during  which  grackles 
most  often  use  flat  tail  are  (1)  when  flying  into  a head  wind,  apparently 
because  keels  increase  drag  inordinately  during  head  winds;  and  (2)  in 
level  and  non-maneuvering  flight,  as  when  grackles  fly  to  or  from  a roost. 
During  these  conditions  the  tail’s  aerodynamic  importance  is  relatively 
minimal.  The  wings  can  supply  all  the  lift,  thrust  and  control  required  to 
maintain  trim.  The  tail  is  then  most  efficiently  positioned  in  a flat,  narrow 
shape  to  minimize  drag. 

The  aerodynamic  uses  proposed  above  for  shallow  keel  are  hypothetical 


Hickman  • COMMON  CRACKLE  TAIL  KEELING 


505 


functions  based  upon  the  correlations  between  shallow  keel  and  conditions 
during  which  birds  are  unstable  and/or  susceptible  to  stall.  These  hypoth- 
eses are  strengthened  by  unquantified  observations  which  indicate  that 
other  birds,  such  as  the  Rock  Dove  {Columba  livia)  and  Herring  Gull 
{Lams  argentatus)  also  use  shallow  (but  U vs  V shaped)  keel  under  unsta- 
ble conditions  during  which  stall  is  likely. 

I am  publishing  my  conclusions  even  though  I am  uncomfortable  with 
the  degree  to  which  they  rely  on  pure  correlations  in  the  hope  that  re- 
searchers with  wind  tunnel  access  will  properly  test  the  aerodynamic  func- 
tions proposed  for  shallow  keel. 


SUMMARY 

This  study  indicates  that  tail  keeling  by  Common  Crackles  functions  aerodynamicaUy  as 
well  as  behavioraUy.  Deep  keel  functions  behavioraUy  and  is  restricted  to  males.  Medium 
keel  was  rarely  observed.  The  primary  function  of  shallow  keel  is  probably  aerodynamic, 
increasing  stability  during  tail  molt.  Shallow  keel  probably  also  functions  to  prevent  stalls 
during  landing,  banking  and  take-off.  The  Common  Crackle  showed  flat  tail  in  direct  non- 
maneuvering flight  and  when  flying  into  head  winds. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

I would  hke  to  thank  the  members  of  my  thesis  committee.  Dale  Birkenholz,  Larry  Cadwell 
and  Jared  Verner  for  their  aid  throughout  this  study.  I also  thank  Dale  Birkenholz,  Thomas 
Poulson  and  John  Mathwig  for  reviewing  an  earlier  draft  of  this  paper. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Bent,  A.  C.  1958.  Life  histories  of  North  American  blackbirds,  orioles,  tanagers,  and  allies. 
U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  211. 

Dalton,  S.  1977.  The  miracle  of  flight.  McGraw-HiU  Book  Co.,  New  York,  New  York. 
Ficken,  R.  W.  1963.  Courtship  and  agonistic  behavior  of  the  Common  Crackle,  Quiscalus 
quiscula.  Auk  80:52-72. 

Maxwell,  G.  R.  1970.  Pair  formation,  nest  building,  and  egg  laying  of  the  Common  Crackle 
in  northern  Ohio.  Ohio  J.  Sci.  70:284-291. 

Pennycuick,  C.  J.  1972.  Animal  flight.  Edward  Arnold  Publ.,  London,  England. 

Salt,  W.  R.  1966.  An  adaptation  to  produce  large  lift  forces  in  small  short-winged  birds. 
Can.  J.  Zool.  44:1037-1040. 

Wiens,  J.  A.  1965.  Behavioral  interactions  of  Red-winged  Blackbirds  and  Common  Crackles 
on  a common  breeding  ground.  Auk  82:356-374. 

Wiley,  R.  H.  1976a.  Affiliation  between  the  sexes  in  Common  Crackles  I:  specificity  and 
seasonal  progression.  Z.  Tierpsychol.  40:59-79. 

. 1976b.  Communication  and  spatial  relationships  in  a colony  of  Common  Crackles. 

Anim.  Behav.  24:570-584. 

DEPT.  BIOLOGY,  ILLINOIS  STATE  UNIV.,  NORMAL,  ILLINOIS  61761.  (PRESENT 
ADDRESS:  DEPT.  BIOLOGY,  COLL.  LAKE  COUNTY,  GRAYSLAKE,  ILLINOIS 
60030  AND  DEPT.  BIOLOGY,  UNIV.  ILLINOIS  AT  CHICAGO  CIRCLE, 
CHICAGO,  Illinois  60080.)  accepted  5 mar.  1981. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  506-518 


REPRODUCTIVE  CORRELATES  OF  ENVIRONMENTAL 
VARIATION  AND  NICHE  EXPANSION  IN  THE 
CAVE  SWALLOW  IN  TEXAS 

Robert  F.  Martin 

Natural  and  anthropogenic  environmental  factors  interact  to  form  the 
selective  background  that  shapes  the  reproductive  variability  of  many 
avian  species.  An  excellent  subject  for  investigations  of  such  interactions 
is  the  Cave  Swallow  {Petrochelidon  fulva),  a relatively  widespread  cavern- 
nesting species  presently  undergoing  human-mediated  breakdown  of  eco- 
logical segregation  from  other  swallows  at  the  northeastern  periphery  of 
its  range  in  North  America.  Martin  (1974,  1980),  Martin  and  Selander 
(1975),  and  Martin  and  Martin  (1978)  have  discussed  range  expansion  and 
intergeneric  hybridization  as  consequences  of  highway  culvert  nesting  in 
this  species,  while  Martin  et  al.  (1977)  outlined  the  basic  pattern  of  repro- 
duction of  a cavern-nesting  colony.  At  the  cavern  site,  minimal  or  non- 
existent nest  predation  and  interspecific  competition,  little  human  distur- 
bance, constant  availability  of  nesting  material  and  minimal  temperature 
fluctuation  operate  together  to  increase  environmental  stability,  and 
enhance  the  value  of  correlations  between  patterns  of  reproduction 
and  the  few  environmental  factors  that  do  fluctuate  here.  Such  di- 
minished ecological  variability  resembles  that  characteristic  of  islands 
(MacArthur  and  Wilson  1967;  Cody  1966,  1971)  and  suggests  com- 
parisons of  the  reproductive  parameters  of  cavern  populations  of  P. 
fulva  with  those  of  culvert  colonies  exposed  to  more  variable  selective 
influences.  In  this  report,  I (1)  document  variation  in  reproduction  in  an 
isolated  cave  colony  of  P.  fulva  in  successive  years;  (2)  use  these  data  to 
test  the  general  hypothesis  that  directly  relates  reproductive  output  and 
success  to  amount  of  precipitation  in  xeric  areas;  and  (3)  contrast  repro- 
ductive patterns  of  cave  and  culvert  populations  of  this  species  and  discuss 
possible  reasons  for  their  disparity. 

STUDY  AREA  AND  METHODS 

During  1974  and  1975,  data  were  taken  at  an  isolated  colony  of  Cave  Swallows  nesting  at 
Dunbar  Cave,  37  km  WSW  Rocksprings,  Edwards  Co.,  Texas.  Additionally  in  1974,  Cave 
Swallow  colonies  that  nested  syntopicaUy  with  Barn  Swallows  {Hirundo  rustica)  in  four 
highway  culverts  were  visited;  these  were  located  in  Kinney  and  Uvalde  counties,  approxi- 
mately 73  km  SE  of  the  cave  site,  along  23  km  of  U.S.  Highway  90  beginning  at  and  extending 
eastward  from  13  km  E of  BrackettviUe.  All  sites  lie  just  east  of  the  Chihuahuan  Desert  in 
a region  of  gently  rolling,  sparsely  vegetated  hiUs  dissected  by  semi-permanent  and  tempo- 
rary streams. 


506 


Martin  • CAVE  SWALLOW  REPRODUCTION 


507 


Table  1 

Study  Area  Precipitation  (1974  and  1975)  in  cm  Preceding  and  During  First  and 
Second  Reproductive  Cycles  of  Cave  Swallows’ 

Precipitation 

1974 

(Dry) 

1975 

(Wet) 

Location 

Jan.-May^ 

Jan.-JuV 

Jan. -May 

Jan. -July 

Carta  Valley 
(13  km  W cave) 

13.20 

16.70 

22.89 

44.39 

Rocksprings 
(40  km  NW  cave) 

17.45 

20.40 

36.58 

55.65 

Brackettville 
(59  km  SSE  cave) 

15.11 

16.64 

29.85 

51.79 

* Data  from  NOAA  1974,  1975,  1976. 

^ Period  precedes  and  includes  first  reproductive  cycle  of  season. 

^ Period  precedes  and  includes  first  and  second  reproductive  cycles. 


Precipitation  and  temperature  data  (NOAA  1973,  1974,  1975)  from  Carta  Valley  (29°48'N, 
100°48'W),  13  km  W of  Dunbar  Cave,  and  from  Brackettville  (29°19'N,  100°24'W)  were 
considered  representative  of  those  at  cave  and  culvert  sites,  respectively;  Table  1 presents 
rainfall  data  for  these  and  for  other  reference  sites  in  the  cave  area.  Ambient  temperatures 
at  representative  nest-sites  were  monitored  with  recording  thermographs. 

Dunbar  Cave  opens  by  a vertical  shaft  approximately  2.3  m in  diameter  and  3.4  m in  length 
into  the  roof  of  a chamber  nearly  25  m in  greatest  diameter  and  5 m in  height.  Here,  a colony 
of  from  250-300  pairs  of  P.  fulva  nested  in  isolation  from  other  birds.  Nests  were  flared-rim 
cups,  fashioned  of  mud  or  guano  pellets,  and  usually  attached  high  on  walls  or  in  pockets 
eroded  in  the  cave  ceiling. 

Sample  culverts  were  of  multiple-passageway  concrete  construction  and  were  selected 
from  a series  of  16  culverts  that  averaged  1.6  km  apart.  The  series  averaged  ca.  17  breeding 
pairs  of  swallows  (P.  fulva  and  H.  rustica)  per  culvert;  culverts  with  greater  concentrations 
of  swallows  usually  were  separated  by  culverts  possessing  considerably  fewer  birds;  no 
culvert  held  over  80  breeding  pairs.  The  four  sample  culverts  held  from  6-32  nesting  pairs  of 
P.  fulva-,  H.  rustica  formed  49%  of  the  overall  total  of  breeding  swallows  in  these  culverts. 
Culverts  54,  51,  48  and  40  had  6,  5,  11  and  10  passages,  respectively,  that  averaged  15.1 
m X 1.6  m X 1.7  m (length  X width  x height).  Nests  of  both  P.  fulva  and  H.  rustica  usually 
were  built  on  culvert  walls  within  0.3  m of  their  ceilings. 

Nests  were  identified  individually  by  inserting  numbered  nails  into  nest  bases  or  by  mark- 
ing adjacent  surfaces  with  pencil.  Active  nests  accessible  by  ladder  formed  the  annual 
samples  at  Dunbar  Cave;  approximately  40%  of  the  nests  of  the  colony  were  marked.  During 
1974,  nest  contents  usually  were  examined  at  2-  to  3-day  intervals;  a total  of  35  visits  was 
made  to  the  cave  (Fig.  1).  Fifty-two  visits  were  made  to  the  site  in  1975.  Early  in  the  1975 
season,  during  three  periods  of  6,  10  and  9 days  (Fig.  1),  nest  contents  were  examined  daily 
to  determine  incubation  and  nestling  periods;  before  and  between  these  periods  of  daily 
visitation  and  after  8 June  1975,  the  1975  visitation  schedule  approximated  that  of  1974. 
Culverts  were  visited  in  1974  on  the  same  days  as  cave  visits. 

Since  P.  fulva  lay  on  successive  days  until  clutch  completion,  clutch  initiation  dates  not 


508 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


APRIL  MAY  JUNE 

1974  

1975  

Eig.  1.  Investigator  nest-visitation  schedules  at  Dunbar  Cave  during  the  first  reproductive 
cycle  of  1974  and  of  1975.  Small  black  rectangles  represent  dates  on  which  nests  were  visited 
by  the  investigator;  small  vacant  rectangles,  dates  on  which  nests  were  not  visited. 


known  precisely  were  determined  by  backdating  from  the  samphng  day  on  which  eggs  first 
were  found  in  the  nest;  similarly,  clutch  completion  dates  were  determined  by  forward  dating 
to  account  for  aU  eggs  observed.  When  hatch  time  was  not  known  exactly,  the  average 
incubation  period  of  15  days  (Martin  et  al.  1977)  was  substituted.  Due  to  our  samphng 
schedule,  “hatch”  data  may  include  very  early  nestling  mortality  (Martin  et  al.  1977).  Egg 
loss  and  nestling  mortality  to  19  days  post-hatch  (1  day  prior  to  flight  potential)  arbitrarily 
were  assigned  to  the  midpoints  of  the  samphng  gaps  during  which  they  occurred. 

Due  to  the  more  intensive  (daily)  visitation  program  in  early  1975,  more  precise  hatch  and 
survival  data  were  available  for  nests  examined  during  this  period  than  for  those  visited  at 
other  times.  These  daily  data  were  not  used  in  scoring  of  reproductive  events  reported  here; 
for  this  purpose,  only  data  gathered  on  visits  of  a predetermined  schedule  that  approximated 
the  regular  (2-3-day  interval)  schedule  were  used  (Fig.  1).  Except  for  comparisons  of  cor- 
relation coefficients,  or  where  noted  otherwise,  statistical  comparisons  employed  the  Mann- 
Whitney  U-test  (Sokal  and  Rohlf  1969).  Results  of  statistical  tests  were  considered  significant 
at  P < 0.05. 


RESULTS 

Environmental  variables. — Precipitation  data  for  periods  preceding  and 
during  the  1974  and  1975  reproductive  seasons  of  the  cave  population  and 
the  1974  reproductive  season  of  the  culvert  populations  are  presented  in 
Table  1.  Precipitation  in  the  area  of  Dunbar  Cave  was  considerably  greater 
in  1975  than  in  1974.  Precipitation  at  the  culvert  area  was  only  slightly 
higher  than  at  Dunbar  Cave  in  1974.  Breeding  season  (April-August)  mean 
temperatures  at  the  area  surrounding  Dunbar  Cave  were  similar  in  1974 
(25.6°C)  and  1975  (24.3°C).  Temperature  records  for  the  culvert  area  are 
incomplete  for  June  1974,  but  the  cumulative  mean  for  the  remaining 
months  of  the  breeding  season  approximates  that  for  the  cave  area  (cave- 
25. 1°C;  culverts-24.4°C)  in  1974.  The  thermal  regime  at  cave  nests  during 
the  breeding  season  (17-22°C,  season  minima  and  maxima;  3°C  maximum 
daily  fluctuation)  was  less  variable  than  that  at  a sample  culvert  (14— 32°C, 
season  minima  and  maxima;  8°C  maximum  daily  fluctuation).  A moist 
substrate  of  mud  and  guano  provided  a constantly  available  source  of 
nesting  material  to  the  cave  colony,  while  culvert-nesting  birds  frequently 
travelled  longer  distances  to  gather  mud.  The  potential  of  catastrophic 
loss  due  to  flooding  existed  for  culvert,  but  not  cave  populations. 


Martin  • CAVE  SWALLOW  REPRODUCTION 


509 


The  cave  population  also  was  exposed  to  fewer  biotic  variables.  No  other 
swallows  nested  near  Dunbar  Cave,  while  culvert  P.  fulva  nested  syntop- 
ically  with  nearly  equal  numbers  (49%,  1974)  of  Barn  Swallows.  Vertebrate 
nest  predators  were  not  seen  at  Dunbar  Cave,  nor  is  it  likely  that  they 
could  pose  a serious  threat  at  such  a site.  Eggs  and  nestlings  in  culverts 
were  exposed  to  attack  or  predation  by  at  least  six  species  of  mammals, 
two  species  of  birds  and  six  species  of  snakes,  in  addition  to  ants  and 
several  other  invertebrates  (Martin,  unpubl.).  Casual  human  disturbance 
is  minimal  at  Dunbar  Cave  because  the  cavern  is  non-commercial,  lies  on 
private  property  and  entry  is  difficult  without  specialized  equipment.  Cul- 
vert colonies,  however,  are  disturbed  regularly  by  itinerants  and  highway 
maintenance  crews.  Although  catastrophic  losses  due  to  deliberate  human 
perturbation  occur  occasionally  in  culverts,  none  occurred  in  sample  cul- 
verts in  1974. 

Reproduction  at  Dunbar  Cave. — Clutch-size  ranged  from  1-6  eggs. 
Clutches  of  3-5  eggs  comprised  96.3%  of  all  clutches  (Tables  2,  3)  and 
clutches  of  one,  two  and  six  eggs  comrpised,  respectively,  0.9%,  2.6% 
and  0.2%  of  all  clutches.  Clutches  were  divided  arbitrarily  into 
early  and  late  categories  according  to  waves  of  synchrony  in  laying. 
Comparative  distributive  statistics  for  clutch-size,  hatch  (no.  of  young 
hatched)  and  survival  (no.  of  young  surviving  to  19  days)  of  these  and  total 
first  and  later  clutches  of  1974  and  1975  are  presented  in  Table  2.  Overall 
percentage  values  of  hatched  young/eggs  laid,  surviving  nestlings/hatched 
young,  and  surviving  nestlings/eggs  laid,  also  are  presented  in  Table  2. 
Table  4 depicts  a matrix  of  results  of  statistical  testing  relating  fecundity 
and  reproductive  success  to  precipitation  in  the  study  area. 

Clutch-size,  hatch  and  survival  decreased  with  time  within  and  between 
clutch  categories  in  1974  (Tables  2,  4);  these  decreases  were  statistically 
significant  (Table  4).  In  1975,  clutch-size  decreased  significantly  within 
and  between  clutch  categories  (Table  4).  Although  hatch  and  survival  in 
1975  decreased  between  early  and  late  first  clutches,  these  decreases  were 
not  statistically  significant  (Table  4).  Hatch  and  survival  decreased  signif- 
icantly between  early  and  late  second  clutches  in  1975  (Table  4).  No  sig- 
nificant differences  in  hatch  and  survival  occurred  between  first  and  sec- 
ond clutches  in  1975. 

To  assess  the  effects  of  differential  perturbation,  comparisons  of  data 
for  56  nests  in  which  contents  (eggs  and  nestlings)  were  marked  and  70  in 
which  contents  were  unmarked  were  made  for  first  clutches  of  1975.  Hatch 
was  not  significantly  lower  in  nests  with  marked  eggs  (2.71  ± 0.20,  marked 
vs  3.00  ± 0.16,  unmarked;  0.1  > P > 0.05,  NS).  Survival  was  significantly 
lower  in  nests  with  marked  contents  (2.43  ± 0.18,  marked  vs  2.84  ± 0.15, 
unmarked;  P < 0.05).  Comparisons  of  hatch  percentage  and  survival/ 
hatch  percentage  indicated  that  the  former  was  17%  lower  in  nests  with 


510 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  2 

Reproductive  Statistics  and  Hatch-survival  Percentages  for  Nests  of  P.  fulva 
AT  Dunbar  Cave  (1974  and  1975)  and  Culvert  Sites  (1974) 

Clutch  sequence' 

N 

Clutch-size 

Young  hatched 

Survival 
(19  days) 

Hatch/ 

laid 

Survive/  Survive/ 
hatch  laid 

X ±SE 

X ±SE 

jf  ± se 

Cave  1974 

Clutch  1 early 

87 

3.95  ± 0.069 

3.26  ± 0.133 

2.78  ± 0.145 

0.82 

0.85 

0.70 

(before 

11  May) 

Clutch  1 late 

28 

3.61  ± 0.129 

2.86  ± 0.250 

1.75  ± 0.227 

0.79 

0.61 

0.49 

(11  May- 

1 July) 

Clutch  2 early 

65 

3.45  ± 0.102 

2.55  ± 0.140 

1.31  ± 0.141 

0.74 

0.51 

0.38 

(before 

30  June) 

Clutch  2 late 

8 

2.75  ± 0.163 

1.00  ± 0.499 

0.63  ± 0.419 

0.36 

0.63 

0.23 

(July) 

Total  clutch  1 

115 

3.87  ± 0.062 

3.17  ± 0.118 

2.53  ± 0.129 

0.81 

0.80 

0.65 

Total  clutch  2 

73 

3.37  ± 0.096 

2.40  ± 0.146 

1.22  ± 0.136 

0.71 

0.52 

0.37 

Total  (1  + 2) 

188 

0.78 

0.71 

0.54 

Cave  1975 

Clutch  1 early 

111 

4.03  ± 0.057 

2.93  ± 0.128 

2.67  ± 0.125 

0.73 

0.91 

0.66 

(before 

11  May) 

Clutch  1 late 

15 

3.47  ± 0.133 

2.47  ± 0.412 

2.33  ± 0.398 

0.71 

0.95 

0.67 

(11  May- 

1 July) 

Clutch  2 early 

99 

3.71  ± 0.052 

3.18  ± 0.105 

2.81  ± 0.125 

0.86 

0.88 

0.76 

(before 

30  June) 

Clutch  2 late 

8 

2.88  ± 0.294 

2.38  ± 0.323 

2.13  ± 0.295 

0.83 

0.89 

0.74 

(July) 

Total  clutch  1 

126 

3.96  ± 0.055 

2.87  ± 0.123 

2.63  ± 0.120 

0.73 

0.91 

0.66 

Total  clutch  2 

107 

3.64  ± 0.056 

3.12  ± 0.102 

2.76  ± 0.119 

0.86 

0.88 

0.76 

Total  clutch  3 

8 

3.13  ± 0.124 

2.00  ± 0.597 

2.00  ± 0.597 

0.64 

1.00 

0.64 

Total  (1+2 

+ 3) 

241 

0.78 

0.90 

0.70 

Culverts  1974 

Total  clutch  1 

81 

4.31  ± 0.065 

3.44  ±0.119 

3.25  ± 0.127 

0.79 

0.94 

0.75 

Total  clutch  2 

49 

3.96  ± 0.087 

3.22  ± 0.166 

2.92  ± 0.184 

0.81 

0.90 

0.74 

Total  clutch  3 

8 

3.50  ± 0.188 

2.38  ± 0.564 

1.63  ± 0.497 

0.68 

0.16 

0.11 

Total  (1+2 

+ 3) 

138 

0.79 

0.90 

0.72 

' Chronology  by  nest,  not  adult  pair;  adults  unmarked. 


Martin  • CAVE  SWALLOW  REPRODUCTION 


511 


Table  3 

Nestling  Survival  (to  19  days)  in  Relation  to  Most  Common  Clutch-sizes  in 

P.  FULVA 


Clutch- 

size 

Frequency 

% nestlings 
surviving 

Nestling  survival 
per  brood  (r) 

Cave  (1974) 

Clutch  1 

3 

22 

70 

2.10 

4 

76 

65 

2.60 

5 

14 

63 

3.15 

Clutch  2 

3 

34 

42 

1.26 

4 

31 

35 

1.39 

Cave  (1975) 

Clutch  1 

3 

24 

74 

2.22 

4 

80 

65 

2.60 

5 

21 

66 

3.30 

Clutch  2 

3 

29 

76 

2.28 

4 

72 

76 

3.05 

Culvert  (1974) 

Clutch  1 

3 

5 

87 

2.61 

4 

46 

84 

3.36 

5 

30 

64 

3.20 

Clutch  2 

3 

10 

67 

2.00 

4 

31 

75 

3.00 

marked  contents  (63%  vs  80%),  while  the  latter  was  only  1%  lower  (91% 
vs  92%). 

No  significant  differences  in  clutch-size,  hatch  and  survival  existed  be- 
tween 1974  and  1975  first  clutches  (early,  late  and  total).  However,  second- 
clutch  size,  hatch  and  survival  increased  from  1974-1975;  with  one  ex- 
ception (size  of  late  second  clutch),  these  differences  were  statistically 
significant  (Table  4). 

When  cumulative  reproductive  data  for  both  clutches  of  1974  and  1975 
were  compared  (Table  2;  total  1 + 2 vs  total  1 + 2 + 3),  hatch  percent- 
ages were  equal,  but  nestling  survival  percentages  were  considerably 
higher  in  1975.  These  differences  were  due  primarily  to  very  high  nestling 
survival  within  the  second  brood  of  1975  (Table  2). 

Mean  nestling  survival  per  brood  increased  with  clutch-size  in  both 
clutches  of  both  study  years  at  Dunbar  Cave  (Table  3);  three  of  four  cor- 
relation coefficients  calculated  for  Dunbar  Cave  clutches  indicated  a mod- 
erate, but  significant  positive  correlation  between  them  (Table  5). 


512 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  4 

Statistical  Comparisons  among  Samples  Represented  by  Clutch-size,  Hatch  and 
Survival  Means  for  1974  and  1975  P.  fulva  Clutches  at  Dunbar  Cave* 


Clutch- 

size 

Hatch 

Survive 

1974 

Dry 

1975 

Wet 

1974 

Dry 

1975 

Wet 

1974 

Dry 

1975 

Wet 

Clutch  1 

Early 

3.95 

NS 

4.03  + 

3.26 

NS 

2.93  + 

2.78 

NS 

2.67  + 

* 

*** 

* 

NS 

*** 

NS 

Late 

3.61 

NS 

3.47+ 

2.86 

NS 

2.47+ 

1.75 

NS 

2.33+ 

Clutch  2 

Early 

3.45 

** 

3.71 

2.55 

* ** 

3.18 

1.31 

*** 

2.81 

** 

** 

** 

** 

* 

* 

Late 

2.75 

NS 

2.88 

1.00 

* 

2.38 

0.63 

* 

2.13 

Total  clutches 

1 

3.87 

NS 

3.96  + 

3.17 

NS 

2.87+ 

2.53 

NS 

2.63  + 

*** 

*** 

*** 

NS 

*** 

NS 

2 

3.37 

** 

3.64 

2.40 

*** 

3.12 

1.22 

2.76 

** 

* 

NS 

3 

3.13 

2.00 

2.00 

‘ Asterisks  appear  (vertically  and  horizontally)  between  means  representing  samples  between  which  statisticaUy  signifi- 
cant 1-tailed  Mann- Whitney  [/-tests  were  performed  {*  P < 0.05;  **  P < 0.01;  ***  P < 0.001.  NS  = results  not  significant. 
Hypothesis  tested:  fecundity  and  reproductive  success  are  directly  related  to  amount  of  precipitation  in  xeric  areas. 

+ follows  data  for  nests  periodically  under  daily  observation. 


The  number  of  nests  in  the  sample  area  increased  approximately  10% 
from  1974-1975  (Tables  2,  6).  Sixty-three  percent  of  nests  that  held  an 
early  clutch  in  1974  received  a second  clutch,  while  84%  of  the  1975  nests 
were  laid  in  a second  time  (Tables  2,  6).  Largely  due  to  increased  second- 
clutch  size  and  nestling  survival,  261  more  young  survived  to  19  days  in 


Table  5 

Relationships  (regression  equations  and  correlation  coefficients)  between 
Nestling  Survival  (Y)  and  Clutch-size  (X) 


Clutch  1 

Clutch  2 

Location 

Year 

Regression  equation 

r 

Regression  equation 

r 

Cave 

Cave 

1974 

1975 

Y = -0.043  + 0.663X 

Y = 0.263  + 0.597X 

0.318*** 

0.276** 

Y = 0.329  + 0.272X 

Y = -0.514  + 0.898X 

0.192 

0.426*** 

Culvert 

1974 

Y = 2.950  + 0.067X 

0.034 

Y = -0.359  + 0.828X 

0.392** 

' Asterisks  follow  correlation  coefficients  that  are  statistically  significant  (**  P < 0.01;  ***  P < 0.001). 


Martin  • CAVE  SWALLOW  REPRODUCTION 


513 


Table  6 

Numbers  of  Nestlings  Surviving  to  19  Days  at  Cave  and  Culvert  Study  Sites 


Dunbar  Cave 

Culvert  sites 

1974 

1975 

1974 

# 

Young 

# 

Nests 

# 

Young 

# 

Nests 

# 

Young 

# 

Nests 

Clutch  1 

290 

115 

331 

126 

263 

81 

Clutch  2 

91 

73 

295 

107 

143 

49 

Clutch  3 

— 

— 

16 

8 

13 

8 

Total 

381 

642 

419 

the  sampled  area  in  1975  than  in  1974,  an  increase  in  survival  of  40% 
(Table  6). 

Reproduction  at  culvert  sites. — Clutch-size  for  1974  culvert-nesting  P. 
fulva  ranged  from  3-5  (Table  3).  Distributive  statistics  for  grouped  culvert- 
site  reproductive  data  are  shown  in  Table  2.  Since  separate  culverts  were 
involved,  overall  nesting  synchrony  was  reduced  (see,  also,  Myres  1957) 
and  chronological  (early-late)  dichotomies  could  not  be  assigned  within 
clutches.  As  in  cave-nesting  P.  fulva,  clutch-size,  hatch  and  survival  de- 
creased in  culvert  birds  from  first  to  second  clutches  (Table  2);  of  these 
decreases,  only  that  between  clutch-size  of  clutches  one  and  two  was  statisti- 
cally significant  {P  < 0.005).  Reproductive  parameters  declined  further  in 
the  few  third  clutches  deposited.  Hatch  and  survival  percentages  of  first 
and  second  clutches  were  similar  (Table  2). 

Mean  nesthng  survival  per  brood  increased  from  clutches  of  three  to  clutch- 
es of  four  for  the  first  clutch,  but  decreased  slightly  for  clutches  of  five  (Table 
3);  the  correlation  coefficient  for  these  variates  was  not  significant  (Table 
5).  Mean  nestling  survival  per  brood  increased  with  clutch-size  in  the 
second  clutch  (Table  3);  the  correlation  coefficient  for  these  variates  was 
statistically  significant  (Table  5). 

Comparisons  between  cave  and  culvert  populations. — Clutch-size,  hatch 
and  nestling  survival  of  first  and  second  clutches  were  higher  for  the  cul- 
vert populations  than  for  the  cave  colony  in  1974  (Table  2);  with  one  ex- 
ception (first  clutch  hatch  0.1  > F > 0.05,  NS),  these  differences  were 
highly  significant  {P  < 0.001).  In  1974,  a third  clutch  was  deprosited  in 
some  culvert  nests,  but  in  no  cave  nests  (Table  2).  Overall  (total)  hatch 
percentages  were  similar  at  culvert  and  cave  in  1974,  but  total  survival 


514 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  i\o.  4,  December  1981 


percentages  were  considerably  higher  at  culvert  colonies  (Table  2).  At  the 
culvert  sites,  approximately  30%  fewer  nests  produced  a total  of  about 
10%  more  surviving  nestlings  (Tables  2,  6);  much  of  this  difference  was 
due  to  high  second  clutch  reproductive  success. 

DISCUSSION 

Pattern  of  food  availability  generally  is  considered  a major  determinant 
of  variation  in  avian  reproduction  (Lack  1954;  Klomp  1970;  Cody  1971; 
Ricklefs  1969,  1973;  von  Haartman  1971;  Dingle  and  Khamala  1972;  Im- 
melmann  1973;  Bryant  1975).  Other  environmental  factors,  in  turn,  influ- 
ence food  availability  and  also  may  affect  reproduction  in  other  ways. 
Much  information  relating  reproductive  parameters  of  birds  to  precipita- 
tion has  developed  in  the  past  few  decades;  in  arid  regions,  reproductive 
timing  and  success  appear  particularly  closely  tied  to  the  pattern  of  rainfall 
(for  example,  Moreau  1944;  Lack  1954;  Keast  and  Marshall  1954;  Immel- 
mann  1963,  1973;  Dingle  and  Khamala  1972;  Sinclair  1978).  Amount  of 
rainfall  frequently  appears  to  influence  reproduction  through  food  chain 
effects  but  also  may  affect  reproduction  indirectly  if  the  availability  of 
nesting  material  and  nest  concealment  are  altered.  Although  Cave  Swal- 
lows are  constrained  in  nest  building  by  scarcity  of  mud  in  some  areas 
(Baker  1962),  neither  this  moisture-dependent  limitation,  nor  that  of  nest 
concealment  by  foliage  are  operative  at  Dunbar  Cave  (although  the  former 
may  be  a factor  at  culvert  sites),  and  it  is  assumed  that  moisture-related 
constraints,  if  operative  here,  influence  reproduction  primarily  through 
limitation  of  food  resources. 

Seasonal  variation  in  reproductive  parameters. — The  seasonal  decrease 
in  clutch-size  exhibited  by  P.  fulva  is  typical  of  some  populations  of  Cliff 
Swallow  {P.  phrrhonota)  and  H.  rustica,  swallows  with  which  it  nests  syn- 
topically  (Samuel  1971,  Graber  et  al.  1972,  Anthony  and  Ely  1976,  Grant 
and  Quay  1977),  as  well  as  other  passerines  (Lack  1954,  Klomp  1970,  von 
Haartman  1971,  other  reviews).  In  P.  fulva,  hatch  and  nestling  survival 
usually  decline  seasonally  also  (Tables  2,  4;  Martin,  unpubl.);  these  declines 
are  particularly  evident  when  the  perturbed  first  clutch  of  1975  is  excluded 
from  comparisons.  The  effect  of  day  length  on  foraging  time  and  that  of 
prevailing  regional  precipitation  pattern  on  food  resources  may  determine 
seasonal  (between-clutch)  differences  in  avian  fecundity  (Lack  1954, 
Klomp  1970).  Day  length  does  not  appear  to  be  an  important  factor  at  the 
present  study  areas,  however,  for  maximum  day  length  occurs  here  just 
past  the  midpoint  of  the  period  including  the  first  and  second  reproductive 
cycles.  A stronger  argument  may  be  made  for  precipitation  as  an  indirect 
determinant  of  seasonal  productivity  of  P.  fulva.  In  this  region,  a bimodal 
annual  pattern  of  rainfall  exists,  with  maxima  occurring  during  April,  May 


Martin  • CAVE  SWALLOW  REPRODUCTION 


515 


Table  7 

Seasonal  Precipitation  and  Temperature  Patterns  for  Region  Including  Study 

Areas  (35  year  averages) 

Mean  monthly 

Mean  monthly  maximum 

Month 

precipitation  (cm) 

temperature  (°C) 

Jan. 

2.36 

16.4 

Feb. 

2.92 

18.9 

Mar. 

2.29 

22.9 

Apr. 

5.88 

28.3 

May 

7.37 

30.8 

June 

7.54 

33.2 

July 

3.84 

36.2 

Aug. 

4.72 

35.9 

Sept. 

7.59 

32.6 

Oct. 

5.33 

27.3 

Nov. 

2.06 

21.9 

Dec. 

2.29 

17.7 

and  June,  and  again  in  September  and  October  (Table  7;  NOAA  1975, 
1976).  During  the  breeding  season,  precipitation  usually  is  lowest  during 
July  and  early  August,  during  the  second  reproductive  cycle.  In  addition, 
temperature  is  maximal  during  this  period  (NOAA  1974,  1976),  and  con- 
tributes to  moisture  deficiency  through  evaporation. 

Annual  variation  in  reproductive  parameters. — At  Dunbar  Cave,  a great- 
er number  of  second  clutches,  in  concert  with  increased  clutch-size,  hatch 
and  survival  within  second  clutches,  contributed  to  make  the  wet  repro- 
ductive season  of  1975  considerably  more  productive  than  the  relatively 
dry  season  of  1974  (Tables  1,  2,  4,  6).  Similar  patterns  of  high  fecundity 
during  years  of  high  rainfall  and  abundant  food  resources  (Hoesch  1936, 
Guirtchitch  1937,  Moreau  1944,  Lack  1954)  or  of  dependence  of  repro- 
duction on  precipitation  (Keast  and  Marshall  1954,  Immelmann  1963,  Col- 
lias  and  Collias  1978,  Sinclair  1978)  have  been  well  documented  in  arid 
regions. 

First  clutch  reproductive  parameters  of  the  Dunbar  Cave  colony  did  not 
increase  markedly  from  1974-1975.  This  was  not  unexpected,  considering 
the  increased  perturbation  to  which  individual  nests  and  the  entire  colony 
were  subjected  during  the  first  reproductive  cycle  of  1975  (Methods,  also 
Fig.  1).  Data  taken  under  perturbed  conditions  may  be  of  somewhat  limited 
value  for  comparative  purposes,  but  the  environment  of  much  of  the  North 
Temperate  Zone  is  heavily  disturbed  (Lack  1965),  and  data  reflecting  quan- 


516 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


titatively  the  potential  effects  of  methodology  are  relevant.  The  lower  sur- 
vival in  nests  whose  contents  were  marked  appeared  to  be  due  largely  to 
low  hatchability.  Losses  ascribed  to  desertion  occurred  at  approximately 
equal  frequency  in  both  marked  (10%)  and  unmarked  (11%)  samples. 
These  intra-clutch  data  represent  only  the  proximal  individual  effects  of 
marking  and  must  be  considered  together  with  the  more  widespread  and 
concomitant  effects  expected  from  disturbance  of  the  entire  colony:  po- 
tential impairment  of  synchrony  and  attentiveness,  and  disruption  of  nest- 
ling and  adult  feeding  patterns. 

Comparisons  between  cave  and  culvert  populations  (1974). — Disturbance 
by  humans  is  not  a normal  phenomenon  at  Dunbar  Cave,  but  colonies  of 
P.  fulva  that  nest  in  highway  culverts  have  undergone  regular  anthropo- 
genic disturbance  and  risk  and  are  subjected  to  other  catastrophic  and 
non-catastrophic  biological  and  physical  influences  to  which  the  cave  col- 
ony is  not  exposed.  Populations  evolving  in  variable  environments  often 
display  greater  reproductive  effort  than  those  adapted  to  more  stable  con- 
ditions; theoretically,  this  increased  effort  balances  increased  mortality 
(Cody  1966,  1971).  First  and  second  clutches  were  larger  in  culvert  pop- 
ulations than  at  Dunbar  Cave  and  third  clutches  occurred  only  at  culvert 
sites  in  1974.  Hatch  and  nestling  survival  of  first  and  second  clutches  were 
higher  in  culverts  as  were  overall  hatching  and  survival  percentages,  the 
greatest  difference  appearing  in  comparisons  between  second  clutches. 
Following  Cody’s  postulations  (1966,  1971),  an  ultimate  interpretation  of 
the  data  would  be  that  differential  mortality  between  culvert  and  cave 
populations  (culvert  mortality  > cave)  must  occur  at  or  after  fledging.  I 
believe  that  this  is  probable;  recently  fledged  P.  fulva  usually  remain  in 
the  vicinity  of  the  culvert  and  return  to  the  nest  for  several  days,  and, 
lacking  in-flight  strength  and  maneuverability,  are  exposed  heavily  to  the 
hazard  of  vehicle  strike.  Relevant  to  the  evaluation  of  this  interpretation 
are  the  estimated  maximal  chronological  separation  of  culvert  from  cave 
populations  (20  generations)  and  the  distance  between  them  (65-80  km). 

Density-dependent  effects  may  be  involved  in  the  higher  fecundity, 
hatch  and  nestling  survival  of  culvert-nesting  P.  fulva;  in  other  species 
such  relationships  are  well  documented  for  clutch-size,  less  adequately  so 
for  nestling  survival  (Klomp  1970,  von  Haartman  1971).  Culvert  nesting 
has  been  established  only  recently  in  central  Texas,  and  has  resulted  in 
marked  range  expansion  (Martin  and  Martin  1978).  Although  the  culverts 
sampled  in  this  study  are  not  at  the  current  edge  of  the  range  of  P.  fulva, 
their  swallow  densities  (both  conspecific  and  cumulative)  are  lower  than 
at  Dunbar  Cave. 


Martin  • CAVE  SWALLOW  REPRODUCTION 


517 


SUMMARY 

Data  on  reproduction  from  Texas  cave  and  culvert  sites  are  presented  for  the  Cave  Swallow 
{Petrochelidon  fulva),  a peripheral  U.S.  species  presently  undergoing  breakdown  in  ecolog- 
ical segregation  at  the  northern  margin  of  its  range  through  modification  of  its  nesting  habits. 
Correlations  exist  between  environmental  influences,  both  natural  and  anthropogenic,  and 
variation  in  pattern  of  reproduction  of  cave-  and  culvert-nesting  colonies  of  this  species. 
Seasonal  declines  in  reproductive  parameters  occurred  at  both  sites  and  are  considered  to 
reflect  adaptations  to  within-season  increasing  aridity  and  diminished  food  supply.  During 
a relatively  wet  year  at  a cavern-nesting  colony,  overall  seasonal  reproductive  output  and 
nestling  survival  were  higher  than  during  a drier  year;  much  of  this  difference  was  due  to 
significantly  increased  second  clutch  reproductive  parameters.  Increased  investigator  visit- 
ation of  the  cave  colony  during  the  first  reproductive  cycle  of  the  wet  year  may  have  nega- 
tively affected  hatch  and  survival  of  that  year’s  first  clutch.  Culvert-nesting  populations 
displayed  significantly  higher  clutch-size  and  nestling  survival  than  did  the  population  nesting 
at  the  more  typical  cave  site. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

SaUie  Martin  initiated  the  culvert  research,  provided  much  encouragement  and  support 
and  improved  the  manuscript.  W.  F.  Blair  and  Robert  K.  Selander  offered  useful  suggestions. 
M.  R.  Lewis  gathered  most  of  the  data.  The  Texas  Highway  Department  permitted  marking 
within  culverts.  Walter  R.  Davis,  III,  assisted  in  selection  of  the  cave  site  and  Mr.  and  Mrs. 
Robert  Erekson  graciously  provided  site  access.  Mark  and  SaUie  Martin  assisted  during  an 
exploratory  visit  to  Dunbar  Cave.  R.  F.  Johnston,  C.  R.  Brown  and  Jon  C.  Barlow  reviewed 
and  improved  the  manuscript.  Portions  of  the  research  were  supported  by  the  National 
Geographic  Society  (Grant  Nos.  1301,  1418  and  1721)  and  The  Texas  Memorial  Museum. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Anthony,  L.  W.  and  C.  A.  Ely.  1976.  Breeding  biology  of  Barn  SwaUows  in  west-central 
Kansas.  BuU.  Kansas  Ornithol.  Soc.  27:37^3. 

Baker,  J.  K.  1962.  Associations  of  Cave  SwaUows  with  Cliff  and  Barn  swaUows.  Condor 
64:326. 

Bryant,  D.  M.  1975.  Breeding  biology  of  House  Martins  Delichon  urbica  in  relation  to 
aerial  insect  abundance.  Ibis  117:180-216. 

Cody,  M.  L.  1966.  A general  theory  of  clutch-size.  Evolution  20:174-184. 

. 1971.  Ecological  aspects  of  reproduction.  Pp.  461-517  in  Avian  biology,  Vol.  1 (D. 

S.  Earner  and  J.  R.  King,  eds.).  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

COLLIAS,  N.  E.  AND  E.  C.  COLLIAS.  1978.  Cooperative  breeding  behavior  in  the  White- 
browed  Sparrow  Weaver.  Auk  95:472-484. 

Dingle,  H.  and  C.  P.  M.  Khamala.  1972.  Seasonal  changes  in  insect  abundance  and 
biomass  in  an  East  African  grassland  with  reference  to  breeding  and  migration  in  birds. 
Ardea  59:216-221. 

Graber,  R.  R.,  j.  W.  Graber  and  E.  L.  Kirk.  1972.  lUinois  birds:  Hirundinidae.  lUinois 
Nat.  Hist.  Surv.  Biol.  Notes  80:1-36. 

Grant,  G.  S.  and  T.  L.  Quay.  1977.  Breeding  biology  of  Cliff  SwaUows  in  Virginia.  Wilson 
BuU.  89:286-290. 

Guirtchitch,  G.  1937.  Chronique  ornithologie  tunisienne  pour  I’annee  1936.  Oiseau  7:450- 
472. 


518 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Hoesch,  W.  1936.  Nester  und  Gelege  aus  dem  Damaraland.  II.  J.  Orn.  84:3-20. 

Immelmann,  K.  1963.  Drought  adaptations  in  desert  birds.  Pp.  649-657  in  Proc.  13th  Int. 
Ornithol.  Congr. 

. 1973.  Role  of  the  environment  in  reproduction  as  source  of  “predictive”  information. 

Pp.  121-147  in  Breeding  biology  of  birds  (D.  S.  Earner,  ed.).  Natl.  Acad.  Sci.,  Wash- 
ington, D.C. 

Keast,  J.  a.  and  a.  J.  Marshall.  1954.  The  influence  of  drought  and  rainfall  on  repro- 
duction in  Australian  desert  birds.  Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  London  124:493^99. 

Klomp,  H.  1970.  The  determination  of  clutch-size  in  birds,  a review.  Ardea  58:1-124. 

Lack,  D.  1954.  The  natural  regulation  of  animal  numbers.  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford,  En- 
gland. 

. 1965.  Evolutionary  ecology.  J.  Anim.  Ecol.  34:224-231. 

MacArthur,  R.  H.  and  E.  O.  Wilson.  1967.  The  theory  of  island  biogeography.  Princeton 
Univ.  Press,  Princeton,  New  Jersey. 

Martin,  R.  F.  1974.  Syntopic  culvert  nesting  of  Cave  and  Barn  swallows  in  Texas.  Auk 
91:776-782. 

. 1980.  Analysis  of  hybridization  between  the  hirundinid  genera  Hirundo  and  Petro- 

chelidon  in  Texas.  Auk  97:148-159. 

AND  S.  R.  Martin.  1978.  Niche  and  range  expansion  of  Cave  Swallows  in  Texas. 

Am.  Birds  32:941-946. 

AND  R.  K.  Selander.  1975.  Morphological  and  biochemical  evidence  of  hybridiza- 
tion between  Cave  and  Barn  swallows.  Condor  77:362-364. 

, G.  O.  Miller,  M.  R.  Lewis,  S.  R.  Martin  and  W.  B.  Davis,  II.  1977.  Repro- 
duction of  the  Cave  Swallow:  a Texas  cave  population.  Southwestern  Nat.  22:177-186. 

Moreau,  R.  E.  1944.  Clutch-size:  a comparative  study,  with  special  reference  to  African 
birds.  Ibis  86:286-347. 

Myres,  M.  T.  1957.  Clutch-size  and  laying  dates  in  Cliff  Swallow  colonies.  Condor  59:311- 
316. 

National  Oceanic  and  Atmospheric  Administration.  1973.  1974.  1975.  Climatological 
Data,  Annual  Summary,  Texas.  U.S.  Dept.  Commerce. 

. 1976.  Climatological  Summary,  BrackettviUe,  Texas.  U.S.  Dept.  Commerce. 

Ricklefs,  R.  E.  1969.  An  analysis  of  nestling  mortality  in  birds.  Smithson.  Contrib.  Zool. 
9:1-18. 

. 1973.  Fecundity,  mortality,  and  avian  demography.  Pp.  366-447  in  Breeding  biology 

of  birds  (D.  S.  Earner,  ed.).  Natl.  Acad.  Sci.,  Washington,  D.C. 

Samuel,  D.  E.  1971.  The  breeding  biology  of  Barn  and  Cliff  swallows  in  West  Virginia. 
Wilson  BuU.  83:284-301. 

Sinclair,  A.  R.  E.  1978.  Factors  affecting  the  food  supply  and  breeding  season  of  resident 
birds  and  movements  of  Palearctic  migrants  in  a tropical  African  savannah.  Ibis 
120:480-497. 

SoKAL,  R.  R.  AND  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1969.  Biometry.  W.  H.  Freeman  and  Co.,  San  Francisco, 
California. 

VON  Haartman,  L.  1971.  Population  dynamics.  Pp.  391—459  in  Avian  biology,  Vol.  1 (D. 
S.  Earner  and  J.  R.  King,  eds.).  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

TEXAS  MEMORIAL  MUSEUM  AND  DEPT.  ZOOLOGY,  UNIV.  TEXAS  AT  AUSTIN, 
AUSTIN,  TEXAS  78705.  ACCEPTED  22  SEPT.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  519-530 


HORNED  LARK  BREEDING  BIOLOGY  AT 
CAPE  ST.  MARY’S,  NEWFOUNDLAND 

Richard  J.  Cannings  and  William  Threlfall 

The  Horned  Lark  {Eremophila  alpestris)  occupies  a variety  of  barren 
habitats  in  Europe,  North  America  (Vaurie  1959,  Bent  1963)  and  Colombia. 
A number  of  studies  have  been  made  of  this  species  in  North  America, 
mostly  in  the  central  part  of  the  continent  (Pickwell  1931;  Garrett  1948; 
Reason  1970;  Reason  and  Franks  1973,  1974;  Boyd  1976).  Little  work  has 
been  done  in  the  northern  and  eastern  regions  of  the  continent,  however, 
other  than  that  of  Sutton  and  Parmelee  (1955),  who  worked  on  Baffin 
Island,  and  Drury  (1961)  whose  study  area  was  Bylot  Island. 

STUDY  AREA 

We  studied  Horned  Larks  continuously  during  the  1976  breeding  season  (May-August)  at 
Cape  St,  Mary’s  (46°47'N,  54°12'W),  a headland  at  the  southwestern  tip  of  the  Avalon  Pen- 
insula, Newfoundland  and  also  made  observations  weekly  in  April  and  biweekly  in  February, 
March  and  September  through  November.  In  April  1977  a last  visit  was  made. 

Cape  St.  Mary’s  has  a cool,  temperate,  maritime  chmate  with  long,  relatively  mild  winters 
and  short,  cool  summers  (Meades  1973).  Annual  precipitation  averages  150  cm,  spread  fairly 
evenly  throughout  the  year.  Fog  is  common,  and  occurred  on  about  60%  of  the  days  from 
May  to  August  1976,  and  on  24  days  in  July  alone.  Approximately  40%  of  the  summer  daylight 
hours  in  1976  were  foggy  (visibility  25-100  m). 

Temperatures  from  May  to  August  varied  from  0°C  (13  June)  to  23°C  (12  July);  daily 
maximums  were  usually  between  10  and  15°C.  Winds  stronger  than  20  km/h  were  common 
until  late  June  and  picked  up  again  in  late  August.  The  severest  storm  of  the  summer  (12- 
13  June)  brought  70  mm  of  precipitation  (rain  and  sleet)  and  winds  up  to  120  km/h. 

The  study  area  covered  approximately  90  ha  of  headland  heath  extending  back  from  the 
sea  cliffs  for  1-2  km.  Using  Meades’  (1973)  classification,  the  habitat  consists  of  rocky 
barrens  {Diapensia  heath.  Polygonum  viviparum  variant),  hard  ground  heath  (Rhacomitrium 
barrens  and  Potentilla  heath),  and  local  patches  of  soft  ground  heath.  The  tract  is  flat  to 
somewhat  undulating  and  treeless,  except  for  a few  very  stunted  balsam  firs  {Abies  balsamea) 
growing  in  places  protected  from  the  wind.  Lark  territories  were  m.apped  by  repeatedly 
flushing  individuals  or  pairs  and  recording  their  positions  with  regard  to  established  grid- 
points  (see  Cannings  1977).  Territorial  interactions,  such  as  threat  displays  with  flight  chases, 
were  also  considered  in  the  mapping  of  territories. 

METHODS 

Observations,  including  those  of  larks  at  their  nests,  were  made  with  8 X 30  binoculars 
and  a 15— 60x  spotting  scope.  No  blind  was  used.  One  male  and  15  female  larks  were 
captured,  individually  color-banded,  aged,  sexed,  weighed  to  the  nearest  2 g (300  g Pesola 
scale)  and  measured  (wing,  culmen,  tarsus,  tail,  hallux  plus  claw,  first  and  ninth  primaries). 
Each  adult  is  referred  to  by  a letter  prefix  (F — female,  M — male)  followed  by  a numeral  (e.g., 
F3  was  the  third  female  captured). 


519 


520 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


As  most  nests  found  already  contained  eggs,  dates  of  nesting  starts  were  determined  (and 
hence  defined)  by  back-dating  from  hatching.  Nests  with  eggs  were  usually  checked  several 
times  daily  to  discover  precise  hatching  times;  those  with  young  were  visited  at  least  once 
daily  to  measure  the  nestlings,  in  the  same  manner  as  adults.  For  individual  identification, 
birds  were  marked  on  the  tarsi  with  indehble  ink  at  hatching  and  banded  when  7 days  old. 
Young  larks  were  named  by  their  nest  number  followed  by  another  numeral,  indicating  the 
order  of  hatching  when  known  (e.g.,  34-1  was  the  first  young  to  hatch  in  nest  34).  Owing  to 
the  extreme  wariness  of  adult  larks  when  feeding  young  in  the  nest,  direct  observation  of 
nest-feeding  behavior  was  difficult.  After  the  young  had  left,  the  nests  were  measured, 
collected  and  frozen.  They  were  examined  later  to  determine  nest  composition,  dried  (at 
room  temperature)  and  weighed. 


RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

The  first  lark  seen  near  the  study  area  in  1976  was  a male  on  4 April. 
In  succeeding  weeks  larks  appeared  in  increasing  numbers  on  the  study 
area  from  8 April  (5  birds)  to  30  April.  Tuck  {in  Peters  and  Burleigh  1951) 
noted  that  “large  flocks”  arrived  in  Argentia,  Newfoundland  (50  km  north 
of  present  study  area)  in  spring,  a phenomenon  not  seen  during  the  present 
study.  Other  workers  (PickweU  1931,  Bannerman  1953,  Boyd  1976)  indi- 
cated that  male  larks  normally  return  shortly  before  the  females.  We  also 
noted  this  in  Newfoundland  in  1977,  but  not  1976,  when  many  presumed 
pairs  arrived  at  the  same  time.  The  larks  seen  between  14-18  April  1976 
showed  no  territorial  behavior  and  were  not  singing,  perhaps  due  to  wind 
conditions  (30-70  km/h),  which  inhibit  singing  (Beason  1970).  Larks  seen 
on  3 April  1977  were  singing  and  chasing  each  other.  On  arrival,  larks 
stayed  almost  exclusively  on  the  dry  headland  heath.  Larks  later  nested 
along  the  gravel  road  that  ran  through  the  bogs. 

General  behavior:  territories  and  courtship. — Twelve  lark  territories 
mapped  in  the  study  area  (Fig.  1)  ranged  from  2.3  ha  (E)  -5.1  ha  (A),  with 
a mean  of  3.5  ha.  The  territories  were  comparable  in  size  to  those  found 
by  Ryder  (3.1  ha,  at  Argentia,  Newfoundland,  pers.  comm.)  and  PickweU 
(1931),  but  were  larger  than  those  of  Beason  and  Franks  (1974,  0.6— 3.1 
ha),  Boyd  (1976,  0.29-1.35  ha)  and  Lobachev  and  Kapitonov  (1968,  0.15- 
0.25  ha),  who  worked  with  other  subspecies.  Territories  were  distributed 
in  a linear  fashion  along  the  road,  as  male  larks  favored  this  area  for  dust- 
bathing, roosting  and  singing.  Such  an  arrangement  was  also  reported  by 
Boyd  (1976). 

Territorial  behavior  (singing  and  the  three  types  of  hostile  behavior  de- 
scribed by  Beason  [19701)  was  evident  shortly  after  the  larks  arrived  in 
April  and  was  maintained  until  the  last  young  left  the  nest  in  early  August. 

Territorial  fights  and  chases  were  common  from  late  April  into  July, 
becoming  rare  by  the  end  of  July.  Aggressive  behavior  seemed  to  be  more 
evenly  distributed  over  the  nesting  season  at  Cape  St.  Mary’s  than  pre- 


Cannings  and  Threlfall  • HORNED  LARK  BREEDING  BIOLOGY 


521 


Fig.  L Map  of  study  area  showing  the  boundaries  of  the  mapped  territories.  The  large 
single  letter  in  each  territory  designates  the  territory,  the  letter-numeral  groups  (e.g.,  F12) 
indicate  the  resident  adult  larks.  Nest  locations  are  shown  by  the  small  open  squares  with 
the  nest  number.  (Lighthouse  complex  located  lower  left.) 


522 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


viously  reported  by  Pickwell  (1931)  and  other  workers.  With  the  large 
territories  and  very  foggy  weather  at  Cape  St.  Mary’s,  it  may  have  been 
more  difficult  for  male  larks  to  patrol  their  boundaries,  leading  to  many 
unchallenged  trespasses  and  thus  prolonging  territorial  disputes. 

Trespassing  was  fairly  common,  as  illustrated  by  the  following:  F13, 
observed  gathering  food  for  her  nestlings  in  the  immediate  area  of  nest  18, 
150  m from  her  territory,  was  completely  ignored  by  F5  and  Ml,  who  were 
feeding  young  in  nest  18  at  the  time.  Larks  from  territories  that  did  not 
take  in  the  road  sometimes  dust-bathed  on  the  road  without  being  at- 
tacked. 

Territoriality  broke  down  as  soon  as  the  last  brood  left  the  nest  and 
independent  juveniles  were  not  challenged  on  foreign  territories.  Young 
larks  that  had  left  their  nest,  but  were  still  being  fed,  seemed  to  be  kept 
within  territorial  limits  by  their  parents.  After  27  July  small  flocks  of  un- 
banded larks  were  common  on  the  study  area,  and  little  or  no  agonistic 
behavior  was  noted  between  them  and  the  resident  larks. 

Most  agonistic  behavior  observed  in  the  larks  was  restricted  to  male  vs 
male  confrontations.  On  only  five  occasions  were  females  involved  in  this 
type  of  behavior,  with  hostilities  being  directed  at  other  females,  in  a 
manner  similar  to — though  not  so  frequent — as  that  described  by  Tinber- 
gen (1939)  for  Snow  Buntings  {Plectrophenax  nivalis). 

Courtship  displays  were  seen  nine  times,  but  copulation  never  followed 
any  of  those  seen.  The  male  approached  the  female  with  body  held  low  to  the 
ground,  wings  drooping  and  quivering,  then  bowed  and  fanned  the  tail. 
When  the  male  stretched  and  held  his  head  high,  the  black  breast  feathers 
were  usually  raised,  enlarging  the  breast  patch.  Males  attempted  to  mount 
females  several  times,  but  each  time  she  side-stepped  or  fended  him  off 
with  a kick.  Often  the  female  ran  at  the  male  as  he  approached.  The 
similarity  of  the  invitatory  display  (see  Beason  and  Franks  1974)  to  dust- 
bathing was  demonstrated  in  instances  where  males  attempted  to  mount 
dust-bathing  females.  No  male-female  chases  were  seen  that  were  defi- 
nitely referable  to  the  “sexual  chase”  as  described  by  Beason  and  Franks 
(1974). 

Nesting,  eggs,  incubation  and  hatching. — In  Newfoundland,  Horned 
Larks  usually  begin  nesting  between  mid-May  and  early  June;  second 
broods,  if  any,  occur  in  July  and  early  August  (Fig.  2).  Nesting  behavior 
was  noted  from  13  May  (nest  building)  until  8 August,  when  the  young  left 
the  last  nest.  The  earliest  known  nesting  date  for  the  island  can  be  ex- 
trapolated from  a 3-week-old  juvenile  collected  in  St.  John’s  26  May  1968; 
this  bird  must  have  come  from  a nest  begun  around  24  April.  The  latest 
nesting  date  is  Tuck’s  report  of  essentially  flightless  young  at  Argentia  on 
11  September  1947  (Peters  and  Burleigh  1951). 


Cannings  and  Threlfall  • HORNED  LARK  BREEDING  BIOLOGY 


523 


3 
2 
6 

4 

5 
8 


10 

9 

21 

16 

25 

18 

20 


CD  17 
S 19 


3 

Z 


(n 

UJ 

z 


27 

14 
12 

28 
33 
35 

15 
24 
22 
37 

*34 

29 
*32 

30 
*39 
*38 


■A 


- Eggs 
••  Nestlings 
<’■’  Building 

* Nest  deserted 
or  destroyed 

* Definite  second  brood 


oo* 


I* 


15  20  25  30  1 5 10  15  20  25  1 5 10  15  20  25  30  1 5 10 

MAY  JUNE  JULY  AUGUST 


Fig.  2.  Nesting  phenology  of  Horned  Larks  in  1976  at  Cape  St.  Mary’s. 


Nesting  starts  occurred  from  about  25  May  until  11  June.  A severe  storm 
on  12-13  June,  however,  destroyed  all  three  nests  with  young,  and  caused 
desertion  of  4 of  5 nests  with  eggs.  Three  nests  found  after  the  storm 
(nests  16,  21,  25)  were  probably  built  before  12  June,  and  may  have  sur- 
vived the  storm  because  incubation  had  not  begun.  The  earliest  known 
second  brood  was  started  on  4 July  (nest  34),  in  territory  H,  where  one  of 
the  few  nests  to  survive  the  storm  was  located.  Three  of  the  later  nests 
(32,  38,  39)  were  second  broods  in  territories  where  nests  had  not  survived 
the  storm. 

Only  the  female  built  the  nest,  usually  in  the  morning.  The  site  was 
located  in  a depression  dug  in  the  ground,  or  in  a tight  mat  of  vegetation — 
crowberry  {Empetrum  sp.),  lichen  {Cladonia  spp.)  and/or  moss  {Rhaco- 
mitrium  lanuginosum).  Nests  consisted  of  a grassy  cup  which  was 
usually  lined  with  a softer  material  (feathers  or  wool).  Most  had  a small 
area  of  pebbles,  or  dried  mud  (paving)  next  to  them.  Vegetation  cover 
associated  with  nest-sites  is  given  in  Cannings  (1977).  Nine  of  31  nests 
examined  were  located  within  50  cm  of  bare  ground,  and  in  the  territories 
which  contained  the  road,  15  of  18  nests  were  found  within  10  m of  the 
road,  recalling  PickweU’s  (1931)  earlier  observations.  Three  nests  were 


524 


THE  ILSOX  BLLLETIX  • Vol.  93,  .\o.  4,  December  1981 


located  in  cinnamon  fern  [Osmonda  cinnamommea) — American  burnet 
{Sanguisorba  canadensis)  habitat,  but  none  were  found  in  moist  habitat, 
as  they  were  by  Verbeek  (1967).  While  the  female  built  the  nest,  the  male 
was  usually  nearby,  feeding,  singing  and  chasing  trespassers. 

The  mean  dry  weight  of  31  nests  w as  16  g (range  = 7-33  g),  wdth  the  nests 
having  a mean  outer  diameter  of  99  mm  (N  = 26,  range  = 65-135  mm, 
some  nests  being  asymetrical)  and  a mean  inner  diameter  of  71  mm  (N  = 
28,  range  = 55-85  mm).  There  was  no  significant  variation  in  nest  size  or 
composition  throughout  the  summer,  contra  PickweU  (1931). 

Most  nests  were  protected  by  vegetation  on  the  windward  side  (N  = 15, 
mean  height  of  protection  = 92  mm,  range  = 5-130  mm),  with  the  open 
side  facing  w^est  to  northeast.  The  distribution  of  the  directions  w^as  non- 
random,  based  on  the  Rayleigh  test  for  randomness  around  a circle  (r  = 
0.4142,  P < 0.05).  This  bias  in  nest  placement  was  probably  due  to  the 
fact  that  the  prevailing  winds  were  from  the  southwest,  and  strong  winds 
were  most  often  from  the  quadrant  south  to  east. 

Nests  were  constructed  primarily  of  grass,  mostly  Deschampsia  flexu- 
osa,  with  some  lichens,  rootlets,  moss,  leaves,  feathers,  small  twigs  and 
paper.  Most  of  the  lichens  (predominantly  Cladonia  spp.)  and  mosses 
were  on  the  outside  of  the  grass  cup,  and  were  perhaps  used  to  level  the 
nest  cup  dug  in  the  ground  before  the  main  grass  structure  was  built. 
Seabird  feathers  w^ere  present  in  all  nests  examined,  and  wool  in  more 
than  50%  of  them.  The  only  soft  plant  materials  used  in  nest  linings  were 
leaves  of  the  northern  honeysuckle  {Lonicera  villosus)  and  down  from 
wiUow^  {Salix  Uva-ursi)  catkins. 

Dubois  (1935),  Reason  and  Franks  (1974)  and  Boyd  (1976)  all  noted  that 
during  Horned  Lark  nest  construction  an  area  of  “pavings”  (small  pebbles 
and  mud)  was  usually  placed  on  one  side  of  the  nest.  Dubois  (1935)  be- 
lieved that  the  pavings  served  to  cover  and  camouflage  the  dirt  excavated 
from  the  nest  cup,  a view  with  which  Reason  and  Franks  (1974)  and  Boyd 
(1976)  concurred  and  which  is  supported  by  our  observations.  In  nest  cups 
made  in  the  mat  of  Empetrum  no  dirt  was  thrown  out  beside  the  nest. 
Seven  of  15  nests  built  in  this  habitat  had  no  trace  of  paving,  while  only 
1 of  15  nests  built  in  short  grass  on  the  roadside,  or  in  gravelly  habitat, 
had  no  trace  of  paving.  This  difference  (7/15  vs  1/15)  is  significant  (x“  = 
6.136,  P < 0.05).  This  may,  however,  simply  reflect  a closer  supply  of 
paving  material  at  the  roadside  nests.  The  nest  building  time  noted  in  this 
study  approximates  that  noted  by  Reason  (1970).  The  most  rapidly  built 
and  loosely  constructed  nest  of  those  observed  in  the  present  study  was 
a simple  cup  dug  in  the  Empetrum  nigrum  mat,  with  a few^  grass  stems, 
feathers  and  bits  of  wiUow^  down.  It  contained  an  egg  the  day  after  dis- 
covery. Nests  1,  9,  12,  15  and  40  found  in  the  study  area  were  incomplete 


Cannings  and  Threlfall  • HORNED  LARK  BREEDING  BIOLOGY 


525 


and  apparently  deserted  early.  Reason  (1970)  reported  a female  building 
two  nests  simultaneously,  a phenomenon  that  we  also  found  once;  the 
female  used  one  nest,  later  finishing  the  second  nest  in  which  she  raised 
a second  brood. 

Renesting  after  nest  destruction  or  desertion  was  recorded  nine  times.  The 
time  interval  between  nest  abandonment  and  renesting  (measured  to  the 
day  the  first  egg  was  laid)  averaged  5.8  days  (N  = 5,  range  = 4-8).  The 
interval  between  the  time  the  young  left  a successful  nest  and  the  initiation 
of  a second  brood  averaged  4.0  days  (N  = 4,  range  = 0-7).  The  zero  value 
came  from  territory  L,  where  F6  laid  the  first  egg  in  nest  32  on  the  same 
day  the  young  left  her  nest  13.  The  second  nest  must  have  been  built  while 
she  was  feeding  the  young  in  the  first.  Four  of  16  pairs  of  larks  raised  two 
successful  broods. 

Eggs  were  usually  laid  before  05:00,  at  a rate  of  one  per  day  until  com- 
pletion of  the  clutch.  In  contrast,  Boyd  (1976)  found  eggs  were  often  laid 
every  other  day  in  very  early  nests.  Clutch-sizes  of  26  nests  varied  from 
2—^  (1  [2],  16  [3],  9 [4]),  with  a mean  of  3.3  ± 0.6.  A significant  increase 
in  clutch-size  over  the  breeding  season  was  noted,  with  a mean  clutch- 
size  of  3.0  in  10  nests  begun  before  15  June.  That  of  16  nests  begun  after 
15  June  was  3.5  {t  = 2.48,  P < 0.05).  Of  eight  females  for  which  two  or 
more  nesting  attempts  were  recorded,  four  had  two  clutches  of  three,  and 
four  contained  a first  clutch  of  three  and  then  a clutch  of  four.  Only  one  first 
nest  contained  a clutch-size  of  four,  and  it  was  one  of  the  latest  first  nests. 
This  trend  to  increased  clutch-size  in  passerines,  over  the  breeding  season, 
is  well  documented  (e.g.,  Delius  1965). 

Incubation,  by  the  female  only,  usually  began  after  the  last  egg  was 
laid,  although  in  eight  nests  it  began  with  the  penultimate  egg.  The  latter 
led  to  asynchronous  hatching,  with  chicks  emerging  over  a 3-day  period 
in  one  instance.  In  only  four  nests  was  the  incubation  period  determined, 
averaging  12.3  days  (11,  11,  13,  14  days,  respectively).  PickweU  (1931) 
reported  an  11-day  incubation  in  this  species,  although  it  may  be  longer 
in  inclement  weather  (Boyd  1976). 

Posthatching  behavior. — The  chicks,  on  hatching,  were  covered  with  a 
long  buff  down,  as  noted  by  Dubois  (1935),  Wetherbee  (1957)  and  Reason 
(1970),  which  enabled  them  to  blend  into  the  background  when  they 
crouched  on  being  disturbed.  The  young  were  brooded  by  the  female  for 
the  first  few  days  after  hatching,  and  every  night  until  they  left  the  nest. 

Both  parents  normally  fed  the  chicks,  although  in  one  case  a female 
raised  two  broods  alone.  The  lack  of  help  in  the  latter  case  (whereabouts 
of  the  male  was  unknown)  did  not  appear  to  affect  nestling  growth,  al- 
though one  late-hatcher  probably  was  not  fed  and  died  5 days  after  hatch- 
ing. The  mid-afternoon  mean  feeding  rate  of  nestlings,  in  one  nest,  was 


526 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


once  every  3.0  min  vs  a mean  of  5.5  min  noted  by  Pickwell  (1931),  and 
the  extraordinary  case  noted  by  Levy  (1920)  where  two  adult  larks  fed 
eight  young  in  one  nest  once  every  minute. 

Forty-seven  nestlings  of  known  age  were  measured,  usually  daily,  for 
a total  of  302  nestling-days  of  measurement.  The  results  are  shown  in  Fig. 
3.  Growth  rate  (weight  increase)  was  analyzed  using  the  graphical  method 
of  Ricklefs  (1967).  Assuming  an  asymptote  weight  of  34  g,  nestling  overall 
growth  rate  was  0.543,  which  is  slightly  higher  than  the  0.464  calculated 
from  PickwelFs  (1931)  data  reported  by  Ricklefs  (1968),  but  very  near  the 
mean  values  given  for  ground-nesting  passerine  species  by  Ricklefs  (1967). 
As  noted  in  other  ground-nesting  passerines  the  legs  and  feet  developed 
rapidly,  this  being  highly  advantageous  in  aUowing  early  nest-leaving  and 
thus  avoidance  of  nest  predation  (Bums  1921). 

The  nestlings  in  the  early  nests  (those  which  perished  in  the  storm  of 
12-13  June)  grew  much  more  slowly  than  those  from  later  nests,  probably 
due  to  a scarcity  of  food  and  the  poor  weather  during  this  period.  Nestlings 
in  the  majority  of  nests  were  fed  equally,  despite  competition  for  food  from 
conspecifics.  However,  in  at  least  four  nests  the  last  hatched  chick  grew 
more  slowly  than  its  nest-mates.  In  one  case  a female  was  seen  to  feed  two 
chicks  in  a nest  while  at  the  same  time  the  male  fed  a third  chick  and  a 
juvenile  from  the  previous  brood. 

The  nestling  period  (time  between  hatching  and  fledging)  averaged  9 
days  (5  nests,  8 days;  26  nests,  9 days;  5 nests,  10  days).  The  chicks  left 
during  the  daylight  hours,  often  over  a period  of  2-3  days.  One  or  2 days 
after  leaving  the  nest  the  young  could  not  fly,  but  hopped  clumsily,  using 
their  wings  for  balance.  Their  main  predator-avoidance  behavior  was 
crouch-concealment  (Pickwell  1931).  As  their  locomotory  ability  in- 
creased, the  young  larks  began  to  run  along  behind  their  parents,  giving 
a distinctive  breet  call,  which  was  used  for  several  weeks  after  nest-leaving 
and  which  was  easily  distinguishable  from  the  adult  weet  call.  After  3 or 
4 days  out  of  the  nest,  the  young  could  fly  a few  meters,  and  by  14  days 
of  age  they  could  fly  distances  of  50  m repeatedly  without  any  noticeable 
tiring.  If  a juvenile  of  this  age  was  forced  to  fly  several  times  consecutively, 
its  parents  accompanied  it,  giving  loud  alarm  calls,  and  near  territorial 
boundaries  the  adults  seemed  to  try  to  herd  the  young  bird  back  into  the 
home  territory.  Young  larks  began  to  be  independent  of  their  parents  when 
they  were  about  3 weeks  old,  at  which  time  they  were  able  to  fly  strongly. 
Although  juveniles  as  old  as  26  days  were  seen  with  their  parents,  an  equal 
number  of  birds  this  age  were  observed  feeding  by  themselves  in  the 
territory  of  another  pair  of  adults.  Young  larks  fed  primarily  on  pink  crow- 
berries  {E.  eamesii)  after  reaching  independence. 

The  first  small  flocks,  indicative  of  post-breeding  activity,  were  seen  in 


Length  (mm)  Weight 


Cannings  and  Threlfall  • HORNED  LARK  BREEDING  BIOLOGY 


527 


Fig.  3.  Growth  rates  of  nestling  Horned  Lark  chicks.  Day  1 represents  the  day  the  chicks 
hatched.  Each  bar  shows  the  mean,  standard  deviation  and  range  of  measurements. 


528 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


late  July.  On  27  July,  a flock  of  13  Horned  Larks  (five  males,  two  females  and 
six  juveniles)  was  seen  on  the  study  area.  These  were  probably  not  local 
larks,  since  none  of  the  birds  were  banded.  By  early  August,  when  most 
local  juveniles  were  about  1 month  old,  several  small  mixed  flocks  of  local 
young  and  female  larks  were  seen  feeding  together. 

Horned  Larks  leave  their  breeding  grounds  in  Newfoundland  in  late 
September  and  October,  with  a few  remaining  until  early  November  (Pe- 
ters and  Burleigh  1951).  We  saw  small  flocks  of  larks  (3-35  individuals) 
on  the  study  area  through  September  in  1976,  and  had  seen  larks  until 
mid-November  in  1975  (during  a seabird  census). 

The  overall  breeding  success  (number  of  young  leaving  nest  per  eggs 
laid)  of  Horned  Larks  at  Cape  St.  Mary’s  in  1976  was  58.8%  (N  = 24 
nests,  80  eggs),  this  figure  being  high  when  compared  to  others  in  the 
literature.  The  mean  number  of  fledglings  produced  by  each  breeding  pair 
during  this  study  was  4.0  (N  = 12,  range  = 0-7).  The  birth  rate  (fledgling/ 
adult/season)  was  2.0,  with  a replacement  rate  of  approximately  67%. 

Predation  probably  accounted  for  43%  of  all  egg  and  nestling  loss,  with 
ermines  {Mustela  erminea),  mink  (M.  vison),  red  fox  {Vulpes  vulpes), 
meadow  voles  {Microtus  pennsylvanicus),  shrews  {Sorex  cinereus)  and  a 
domestic  cat  {Felis  domestica)  being  regularly  present  in  the  study  area. 
Ravens  {Corvus  corax)  were  also  present,  but  seemed  to  confine  their  nest- 
robbing activities  to  adjacent  seabird  colonies. 

Inclement  weather  (the  storm  of  12-13  June)  accounted  for  28%  of  egg 
and  nestling  losses.  All  nestlings  known  to  be  present  died  at  this  time 
and  nest  7,  which  contained  three  eggs,  was  deserted.  Three  other  nests 
(8,  10  and  11)  suffered  predation  during  or  shortly  after  the  storm.  Nest 
desertion  occurred  at  two  nests  after  the  incubating  female  had  been  cap- 
tured on  the  nest  for  banding.  Only  1 of  86  eggs  observed  through  the 
normal  hatching  period  proved  to  be  infertile,  and  one  egg  (containing  a 
well-developed  embryo)  in  nest  16  failed  to  hatch  after  it  was  pushed  up 
onto  the  nest  lip,  apparently  by  the  incubating  female.  An  egg  was  also 
found  just  outside  the  cup  of  nest  8.  It  was  marked  and  returned  to  the 
nest,  but  a predator  destroyed  the  nest  before  the  eggs  hatched.  Beason 
and  Franks  (1974)  also  reported  that  females  took  no  notice  of  eggs  outside 
the  nest  cup.  Starvation  caused  the  death  of  at  least  three  late-hatching 
nestlings  at  Cape  St.  Mary’s. 

Food  and  feeding. — Horned  Larks  usually  fed  by  probing  the  ground 
vegetation  with  their  bills,  searching  for  arthropods,  berries  and  seeds. 
They  sometimes  interrupted  this  ground-probing  to  run  quickly  after  a 
low-flying  wasp  or  moth.  On  5 August,  F3  was  seen  repeatedly  flying  after 
moths  in  the  manner  of  a flycatcher.  Larks  also  ate  bog  cranberries  {Vac- 
cinium  oxycoccos).,  pink  cro wherries,  large  black  ants,  moth  caterpillars 


Cannings  and  Threlfall  • HORNED  LARK  BREEDING  BIOLOGY 


529 


and  adults,  beetles,  craneflies  (Tipulidae)  and  spiders.  Nestlings  were  fed 
arthropods  almost  entirely,  whereas  adults,  especially  early  in  the  season, 
ate  more  seeds,  berries  and  small  leaves.  This  is  consistent  with  the  find- 
ings of  McAtee  (1905  fide  Reason  1970)  and  Boyd  (1976).  All  birds  exam- 
ined, except  the  nestlings  from  nest  2 (which  were  only  1 day  old  at  death), 
had  some  grit  in  their  gizzards.  Almost  half  (8/18)  of  the  gizzards  from 
birds  collected  at  Cape  St.  Mary’s  contained  small  bits  of  mollusc  shell. 

SUMMARY 

The  breeding  biology  of  the  Horned  Lark  {Eremophila  alpestris)  was  studied  during  the 
1976  breeding  season  at  Cape  St.  Mary’s,  Newfoundland.  Territorial  behavior  was  investi- 
gated, and  territories  were  subsequently  mapped  to  determine  their  size  (2.3-5. 1 ha,  x = 3.5 
ha). 

Nesting  phenology  was  studied  in  detail.  Nests  were  weighed,  measured  and  their  com- 
position determined;  they  were  so  placed  as  to  be  protected  on  the  windward  side.  Clutch- 
sizes  of  early  and  late  nests  were  compared  (early  3.0  eggs,  late  3.5  eggs,  overall  mean  3.31 
eggs).  Forty-seven  nestlings  were  measured  to  calculate  growth  curves  for  weight  and  other 
body  variables.  Breeding  success  (58.8%),  incidence  of  renesting,  and  the  mean  number  of 
fledglings  (4.0)  produced  by  each  pair  were  calculated.  Causes  of  egg  and  nestling  loss  were 
analyzed,  and  predation  determined  to  be  the  most  important  factor.  About  25%  of  the 
breeding  pairs  raised  two  successful  broods. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

We  wish  to  thank  O.  L.  Austin,  Jr.,  G.  Brassard,  S.  Cannings,  J.  Rice,  R.  A.  Ryder,  P. 
Scott  and  the  late  L.  M.  Tuck  for  their  help  and  advice  during  the  course  of  this  study.  This 
manuscript  was  written  while  the  junior  author  was  a Visiting  Professor  in  the  College  of 
Veterinary  Medicine,  University  of  Florida,  Gainesville.  This  work  would  not  have  been 
possible  without  generous  support  from  the  National  Research  Council  of  Canada. 

LITERATURE  CITED 

Bannerman,  D.  a.  1953.  The  birds  of  the  British  Isles,  Vol.  2.  Ohver  and  Boyd,  London, 
England. 

Beason,  R.  C.  1970.  The  annual  cycle  of  the  Prairie  Horned  Lark  in  west  central  Illinois. 
M.Sc.  thesis.  Western  Illinois  Univ.,  Macomb,  Illinois. 

AND  E.  C.  Franks.  1973.  Development  of  young  Horned  Larks.  Auk  90:359-363. 

AND . 1974.  Breeding  behavior  of  the  Horned  Lark.  Auk  91:65-74. 

Bent,  A.  C.  1963.  Life  histories  of  North  American  flycatchers,  larks,  swallows,  and  their 
allies.  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  BuU.  179. 

Boyd,  R.  L.  1976.  Behavioral  biology  and  energy  expenditure  in  a Horned  Lark  population. 

Ph.D.  thesis,  Colorado  State  Univ.,  Fort  CoUins,  Colorado. 

Burns,  F.  L.  1921.  Comparative  periods  of  nestling  life  of  some  North  American  nidicolae. 
Wilson  Bull.  33:4-15. 

Cannings,  R.  J.  1977.  The  breeding  biology  of  the  Horned  Lark  {Eremophila  alpestris  L.) 
at  Cape  St.  Mary’s,  Newfoundland.  M.Sc.  thesis.  Memorial  Univ.,  St.  John’s,  New- 
foundland. 

Delius,  J.  D.  1965.  A population  study  of  skylarks,  Alauda  arvensis.  Ibis  107:466-492. 


530 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Drury,  W.  H.  1961.  Studies  on  the  breeding  biology  of  Horned  Lark,  Water  Pipit,  Lapland 
Longspur,  and  Snow  Bunting  on  Bylot  Island,  Northwest  Territories,  Canada.  Bird- 
Banding  32:1^6. 

Dubois,  A.  D.  1935.  Nests  of  Horned  Larks  and  longspurs  on  a Montana  prairie.  Condor 
27:56-72. 

Garrett,  M.  L.  1948.  The  life  history  of  the  Prairie  Horned  Lark.  M.Sc.  thesis,  Ohio  State 
Univ.,  Columbus,  Ohio. 

Levy,  E.  1920.  An  unusual  Horned  Lark  family.  Bird-Lore  22:85-86. 

Lobachev,  Y.  S.  and  V.  I.  Kapitonov.  1968.  Ecology  of  the  Horned  Lark  {Eremophila 
alpestris  albigula  Bp.)  in  the  Karzhantau  Mountains  (Western  Tien-Shan).  ByuU.  Mosk. 
0-va.  Ispyt.  Prir.  Otd.  73:17-25. 

Meades,  W.  1973.  A phytosociological  classification  of  the  Avalon  Peninsula  heath,  New- 
foundland. M.Sc.  thesis.  Memorial  Univ.,  St.  John’s,  Newfoundland. 

Peters,  H.  S.  and  T.  D.  Burleigh.  1951.  The  birds  of  Newfoundland.  Dept.  Nat.  Resour., 
St.  John’s,  Newfoundland. 

PiCKWELL,  G.  B.  1931.  The  Prairie  Horned  Lark.  St.  Louis  Acad.  Sci.  Trans.  27:1-153. 
Ricklefs,  R.  E.  1967.  A graphical  method  of  fitting  equations  to  growth  curves.  Ecology 
48:978-988. 

. 1968.  Patterns  of  growth  in  birds.  Ibis  110:419^51. 

Sutton,  G.  M.  and  D.  F.  Parmelee.  1955.  Nesting  of  the  Horned  Lark  on  Baffin  Island. 
Bird-Banding  26:1-18. 

Tinbergen,  N.  1939.  The  behavior  of  the  Snow  Bunting  in  spring.  Trans.  Linn.  Soc.  N.Y. 
5:1-91. 

Vaurie,  C.  1959.  The  birds  of  the  Palearctic  fauna.  H.F.  and  G.  Witherby,  London,  En- 
gland. 

Verbeek,  N.  a.  M.  1967.  Breeding  biology  and  ecology  of  the  Horned  Lark  in  alpine  tundra. 
Wilson  BuU.  79:208-218. 

Wetherbee,  D.  K.  1957.  Natal  plumages  and  downy  pteryloses  of  passerine  birds  of  North 
America.  Am.  Mus.  Nat.  Hist.  Bull.  113:343-436. 

DEPT.  BIOLOGY,  MEMORIAL  UNIV.  OF  NEWFOUNDLAND,  ST.  JOHN’S,  NEW- 
FOUNDLAND AlB  3X9  CANADA.  ACCEPTED  17  FEB.  1981. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  531-537 


ASPECTS  OF  THE  BREEDING  BIOLOGY  OF  A 
SUBTROPICAL  ORIOLE,  ICTERUS  GULARIS 

Barbara  Yohai  Pleasants 

In  this  paper  I describe  several  aspects  of  the  breeding  biology  of  Lich- 
tenstein’s Oriole  {Icterus  gularis).  Similar  to  other  subtropical  and  tropical 
orioles  I.  gularis  is  sexually  monomorphic,  is  resident  throughout  the  year 
and  is  found  in  wooded  habitats.  Although  breeding  populations  of  I.  gu- 
laris are  established  in  the  U.S.,  little  information  is  available  for  this 
species  in  any  portion  of  its  range.  There  are  brief  discussions  of  its  life 
history  (Bent  1958,  Oberholser  1974)  and  a description  of  nest-building 
(Sutton  and  Pettingill  1943). 


METHODS 

I.  gularis  occurs  from  the  Rio  Grande  Valley  southward  through  eastern  Mexico  to  Gua- 
temala. This  study  was  conducted  at  the  northern  hmit  of  the  range  of  the  species,  on  the 
Santa  Ana  National  Wildhfe  Refuge  near  Alamo,  Hidalgo  Co.,  Texas.  The  area  is  warm  and 
relatively  dry,  with  a mean  annual  rainfall  of  45  cm  (Fleetwood  1973).  The  refuge  consists 
of  800  ha  covered,  in  most  sections,  by  primarily  evergreen,  dry,  subtropical  forest.  The  land 
surrounding  the  refuge  has  been  cleared  for  agriculture,  leaving  Santa  Ana  as  an  island  of 
natural  habitat.  The  commonest  trees  are  Texas  ebony  {Pithecellobium  flexicaule),  tepeguaje 
{Leucaena  pulverulenta),  elm  {Ulmus  spp.)  and  ash  {Fraxinus  spp.).  Below  the  canopy  is  a 
dense  understory  of  shrubs  and  thorny  vines. 

Almost  aU  observations  were  made  from  16  May-6  June  1974.  Two  brief  visits  during 
March  1977  and  March  1978  provided  additional  information.  I found  19  nests  (two  were 
outside  the  refuge  boundaries)  during  the  1974  study  and  at  eight  of  these  I collected  data 
on  various  aspects  of  parental  behavior  (Pleasants  1977).  Most  nests  were  found  while  clutch- 
es were  being  incubated.  Once  located,  a nest  was  checked  daily  and  extensive  observation 
began  when  the  eggs  hatched.  I then  observed  the  activity  at  each  nest  as  often  as  possible 
for  the  remainder  of  the  study.  Most  nests  were  observed  on  three  or  more  different  days 
(Table  1).  I obtained  information  on  type  of  food  brought  to  young,  frequency  of  feeding  of 
nestlings  and  adult  behavior  in  the  vicinity  of  the  nest.  A total  of  1744  min  of  these  obser- 
vations were  made  at  the  8 suitable  nests  (Table  1).  A single  observation  session  lasted  30- 
60  min;  generally  these  sessions  occurred  between  07:30  and  12:00  or  between  15:00  and 
20:00  when  the  adults  were  most  active.  Because  of  the  inaccessible  nature  of  the  nests,  no 
data  on  contents  could  be  obtained.  Locations  of  all  nests  were  plotted  on  a map  of  the 
refuge. 


RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

Characteristics  of  breeding  adults. — All  adults  seen  at  nests  were  in  fuU 
adult  plumage  (orange  and  black).  Temperate  oriole  populations,  on  the 
other  hand,  often  have  significant  proportions  of  breeding  first-year  males 
(pers.  obs..  Rising  1970,  Sealy  1980).  These  first-year  males  have  not  at- 
tained adult  male  appearance  and  resemble  females  in  coloration.  Adult 


I 

II 


531 


532 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  1 

Observation  Periods  for  Icterus  gularis  During  Nestling  Stage,  1974 


Pair/nest 

Dates  of  observation 

No.  of 
observation 
periods 

Total  time 
observed 
(min) 

1 

18-29  May 

10 

400 

2 

21  May-^  June 

10 

585 

3 

20  May-4  June 

4 

180 

4 

31  May-5  June 

3 

121 

5 

29  May-6  June 

3 

134 

6 

31  May-3  June 

2 

124 

7 

1 June^  June 

3 

110 

8 

24-28  May^ 

4 

90 

® Brood  apparently  destroyed  between  24  and  25  May. 


mortality  in  tropical  passerines  is  low  (Cody  1971);  breeding  territories  and 
pair  bonds  may  be  permanent  (Orians  1969).  Young  birds  will  find  it  dif- 
ficult to  enter  the  breeding  population  under  such  circumstances.  In  the 
tropical  Rufous-collared  Sparrow  {Zonotrichia  capensis),  young  birds  re- 
main on  the  permanent  territories  of  adults  waiting  for  vacancies  to  occur 
in  the  breeding  population  (Smith  1978).  Young  /.  gularis  may  employ  the 
same  strategy.  Activities  near  the  nest  of  a widowed  female  /.  gularis 
(nest  7)  may  aid  in  understanding  the  process  of  entry  into  the  breeding 
population  by  young  birds.  (The  presumed  mate  of  this  bird  was  found 
dead  on  the  road  quite  close  to  the  nest.)  She  continued  to  feed  the  nest- 
lings on  her  own  for  the  remainder  of  my  stay  on  the  refuge  (6  days).  A 
male  in  immature  plumage  (yellow  and  black)  was  repeatedly  seen  in  the 
vicinity  of  the  nest.  He  never  brought  food  to  the  nestlings  and  spent  a 
good  part  of  his  time  singing.  1 observed  no  interactions  between  him  and 
the  female. 

Nests. — I.  gularis  builds  a pendulous  nest  approximately  60  cm  in 
length.  The  nest  is  situated  on  a branch  tip  10  m or  more  above  the  ground, 
and  is  conspicuous  (also  see  Sutton  and  Pettingill  1943).  The  trees  selected 
as  nest-sites  are  typically  emergent  above  the  canopy,  so  the  nest  is  visible 
for  some  distance.  Leaves  of  the  preferred  nest  trees  (legumes  like  Pithe- 
cellobium)  are  woven  into  the  nest  and  the  structure  swings  freely.  Nine 
of  10  nests  checked  for  placement  were  within  the  NW  quadrant  of  the 
nest  tree.  The  length  of  the  nest  makes  it  necessary  for  adults  to  land  first 
on  the  upper  part  of  the  exterior  and  then  maneuver  to  crawl  head  down- 
wards into  the  bag  itself.  This  delay  facilitated  my  observation  of  individ- 
uals and  of  food  items  they  carried  for  nestlings. 


Pleasants  • ICTERUS  GULARIS  BREEDING  BIOLOGY 


533 


Nest-building  takes  up  to  26  days  in  I.  gularis  (Oberholser  1974).  This 
is  in  marked  contrast  to  the  5—6  days  temperate  species  (Northern  Oriole 
[/.  galbula],  Hooded  Oriole  [/.  cucullatus])  spend  building  their  smaller 
nests  (pers.  obs.).  This  difference  in  nest-size  and  building  period  may  be 
attributable  to  the  permanent  resident  status  of  I.  gularis,  which  allows 
the  species  more  time  to  build  a secure  structure.  Ricklefs  (1969)  sug- 
gested that  tropical  birds’  more  complex  forms  of  nest  construction  are 
due  to  predation  pressure.  The  length  of  /.  gularis  nests  makes  quick 
entry  and  exit  by  brood  parasites  or  predators  quite  difficult.  The  structure 
of  these  nests  is  presumably  the  result  of  selection  pressure  to  reduce  egg 
and  nestling  mortality  (see  below,  Behavior  at  nest). 

Spacing  system. — Mean  nearest-neighbor  distance  for  the  17  nests  lo- 
cated within  the  refuge  was  250  m (range  63-443  m,  distances  measured 
on  map).  Two  other  nests  were  located  in  small  stands  of  trees  outside  the 
refuge  boundary.  During  all  the  time  1 spent  watching  I.  gularis  from  nest- 
building to  fledging,  1 saw  only  one  instance  of  aggression  between  pairs. 
These  birds  are  solitary  nesters  with  exclusive  access  to  the  food  resources 
in  the  territories  which  surround  their  nests.  Year-round  residence  may 
greatly  lower  the  frequency  of  overt  territorial  defense.  All-purpose  ter- 
ritoriality (Type  A)  is  predicted  for  a species  nesting  in  a large  patch  of 
relatively  uniform  habitat,  such  as  a forest  (Brown  1964,  Horn  1968).  This 
contrasts  with  the  more  colonial  spacing  system  characteristic  of  Northern 
Orioles  breeding  in  small  patches  of  riparian  woodland  surrounded  by 
habitat  unsuitable  for  nesting  (Pleasants  1979). 

Behavior  at  nest. — Compared  to  /.  galbula,  I.  gularis  is  a quiet  species. 
Males  sing  and  whistle  softly,  usually  near  the  nest,  while  females  rarely 
sing.  The  chattering  sound  so  common  in  Northern  Orioles  is  lacking. 
Instead,  both  adults  utter  a soft  “nasal”  call  as  a contact  note  and  when 
arriving  at  the  nest  with  food. 

Nestlings  and  fledglings  of  I.  gularis  also  tend  to  be  quiet.  None  of  the 
fledglings  of  two  broods  made  any  sounds  that  I heard.  In  contrast,  I could 
easily  locate  fledgling  I.  galbula  by  their  constant  begging  calls.  This 
interspecific  difference  in  behavior  of  young  may  reflect  differences  in  the 
causes  of  fledgling  mortality;  predation  is  potentially  a more  significant 
factor  for  I.  gularis. 

I found  that  I.  gularis  adults  produced  a characteristic  call  as  their 
young  approached  fledging.  This  two-note  call,  similar  to  the  beginning  of 
full  song,  was  not  heard  earlier  in  the  nesting  cycle.  Parents  continued  to 
use  this  call  when  approaching  young  even  after  the  young  had  fledged. 

Feeding. — Like  most  icterids,  orioles  are  generalists  with  regard  to  diet. 
Although  primarily  insectivorous,  they  will  also  take  nectar  and  fruit.  I 
observed  no  I.  gularis  young  being  fed  fruit  and  saw  an  adult  eating  fruit 


534 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  2 

Food  Items  Brought  to  Nestlings 

Food  item 

No.  items 

% total 

Grasshoppers  (Orthoptera) 

42 

33 

Caterpillars  and  other  larvae 

36 

29 

Walkingsticks  (Phasmidae) 

26 

21 

Other  insects 

4 

3 

Unidentified 

18 

14 

only  once.  Oberholser  (1974)  states  that  adults  eat  hackberries  (Celtis  sp.) 
and  figs  {Ficus  sp.).  Table  2 lists  food  items  brought  to  nestlings  by  7. 
gularis  parents. 

Data  on  the  rate  at  which  nestlings  were  fed  were  gathered  at  all  nests 
which  had  young  during  the  study.  Fig.  1 shows  the  negative  relationship 
between  age  of  nestlings  and  the  interval  between  successive  feedings  by 
adults  (Spearman  rank  correlation,  r = —0.719,  P < 0.01).  During  the 
first  two  days  post-hatching,  feeding  intervals  are  substantially  longer  than 
on  succeeding  days  and  longer  than  intervals  for  /.  galbula  of  similar  age 
(Pleasants,  unpubl.).  From  the  third  day  on,  feeding  intervals  for  /.  gularis 


c 

E 


03 

> 


0) 

c 


U) 

c 

b 

0 

0 


± 


1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10  11  12  13  14 


Nestling  Age  (days) 

Fig.  1.  Feeding  interval  as  a function  of  nestling  age.  Each  point  is  based  on  data  for  1- 
4 pairs. 


Pleasants  • ICTERUS  GULARIS  BREEDING  BIOLOGY 


535 


fall  within  the  range  of  intervals  I found  for  seven  populations  of  I.  galbula. 
Interestingly,  feeding  intervals  for  I.  galbula  do  not  decrease  as  nestlings 
get  older.  (These  differences  between  species  will  be  discussed  further  in 
a separate  paper.)  The  widowed  female,  whose  nestlings  were  of  unknown 
age,  brought  food  at  intervals  from  4.5-5  min  on  three  successive  days. 
Surprisingly,  these  intervals  correspond  to  those  for  nestlings  with  two  par- 
ents. 

Skutch  (1943)  suggested  that  tropical  birds  feed  their  young  less  often 
than  their  temperate  relatives  in  order  to  reduce  chances  of  detection  by 
predators.  For  /.  gularis  this  may  be  true  when  nestlings  first  hatch,  but 
for  the  bulk  of  the  nestling  period  food  availability  and  demands  of  growing 
young  preclude  such  a strategy. 

Interactions  with  other  species. — The  Bronzed  Cowbird  {Molothrus 
aeneus)  is  abundant  on  the  refuge,  its  numbers  having  increased  in  recent 
years.  Parasitism  by  this  cowbird  is  believed  responsible  for  the  disap- 
pearance of  Hooded  Orioles  from  the  refuge  (Oberholser  1974).  I often 
saw  groups  of  3-5  cowbirds  near  /.  gularis  nests,  particularly  those  in 
which  eggs  had  not  yet  hatched.  During  a 30-min  period,  one  nest  was 
approached  by  three  cowbirds  shortly  after  the  orioles  had  left  the  tree.  The 
cowbirds  remained  for  15  min  despite  an  attempt  by  the  returning  male 
oriole  to  drive  them  away.  During  a later  60-min  period  of  observation  at 
the  same  nest  1-3  cowbirds  approached  five  separate  times,  often  landing 
directly  on  the  nest.  A female  cowbird  entered  the  nest  and  remained 
inside  for  4.5  min.  This  oriole  pair  was  probably  at  the  egg-laying  stage 
when  this  occurred;  nest-building  was  complete  and  incubation  began  a 
few  days  later. 

Orioles  often  nest  near  kingbirds  {Tyrannus  spp.)  or  other  large  tyran- 
nids,  according  to  anecdotal  accounts  (Bent  1958).  Many  flycatcher  species 
are  aggressive,  vociferous  birds  that  readily  attack  predators  larger  than 
themselves.  On  the  Santa  Ana  Refuge  the  Kiskadee  Flycatcher  {Pitangus 
sulphuratus)  is  the  most  conspicuous  large  flycatcher.  Four  of  10  /.  gularis 
nests  were  placed  within  3-4  m of  kiskadee  nests  and  one  of  the  four  was 
also  close  to  the  nest  of  a pair  of  Tropical  Kingbirds  {T.  melancholicus). 
This  suggests  the  existence  of  a nesting  association  from  which  the  orioles 
gain  protection  as  a result  of  the  presence  of  large  tyrannids. 

Several  observations  support  the  protection  hypothesis.  One  pair  of 
orioles  was  reluctant  to  approach  the  nest  with  food  when  I first  began  my 
observations.  Instead,  a bird  would  perch  in  a nearby  tree  with  food  items 
visible  in  its  bill.  After  the  return  of  a kiskadee  to  its  own  nest  about  3 m 
away,  the  oriole  would  fly  to  its  nest  and  feed  the  young.  When  feeding 
young,  each  kiskadee  adult  usually  waited  near  the  nest  for  its  mate  to 
return  before  leaving  on  its  next  foraging  trip,  so  that  at  least  one  adult 


536 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


was  in  the  vicinity  of  the  nest.  In  contrast,  orioles  would  often  leave  to- 
gether on  foraging  trips.  More  importantly,  kiskadees  do  chase  other  birds 
from  oriole  nests.  Bronzed  Cowbirds  were  chased  on  four  occasions; 
chachalacas  {Ortalis  vetula)  and  an  unidentified  dove  were  also  chased. 
One  kiskadee,  nesting  2 m from  the  widowed  female  oriole,  chased  the 
young  male  oriole  that  had  taken  up  residence  on  her  territory.  The  Trop- 
ical Kingbirds  nesting  near  pair  #2  chased  Great-tailed  Crackles  {Quis- 
calus  mexicanus) — known  predators  on  eggs  and  nestlings  (Bent  1958).  If 
this  is  an  actual  association  between  orioles  and  large  tyrannids,  rather 
than  chance  close  proximity,  there  should  be  selection  by  orioles  of  nest- 
sites  near  Kiskadee  Flycatchers.  Kiskadees  appear  to  begin  nest-building 
before  the  orioles  do  (Pleasants,  pers.  obs.  March  1977). 

SUMMARY 

Lichtenstein’s  Orioles  were  studied  for  three  weeks  during  the  breeding  season  on  the 
Santa  Ana  National  Wildlife  Refuge  in  southern  Texas  in  1974.  I located  19  nests  and  made 
extensive  observations  at  eight  of  them.  AH  breeding  birds  were  in  fuU  adult  plumage.  A 
first-year  male  was  repeatedly  seen  in  the  vicinity  of  a widowed  female  with  young  and  the 
possible  significance  of  this  is  discussed.  Nests  are  large,  pendulous  structures,  situated  in 
emergent  leguminous  trees  and  generally  located  in  the  NW  portion  of  the  tree.  Food  items 
brought  to  nestlings  are  listed.  The  mean  interval  between  successive  feedings  decreases  as 
nestlings  get  older.  There  may  be  a nesting  association  between  these  orioles  and  large 
tyrannid  flycatchers  which  would  benefit  the  orioles  by  protection  from  brood  parasites  and 
predators. 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

The  work  reported  on  here  was  part  of  a doctoral  dissertation  submitted  to  the  University 
of  California,  Los  Angeles.  I would  hke  to  thank  personnel  of  the  Santa  Ana  National  Wildlife 
Refuge,  particularly  Wayne  Shifflett  and  Cruz  Martinez,  for  sharing  their  knowledge  of  refuge 
birds.  John  M.  Pleasants  provided  invaluable  field  assistance.  M.  L.  Cody,  H.  A.  Hespen- 
heide,  J.  M.  Pleasants  and  reviewers  for  this  journal  offered  useful  criticism  of  various  drafts 
of  this  manuscript. 


LITERATURE  CITED 

Bent,  A.  C.  1958.  Life  histories  of  North  American  blackbirds,  orioles,  tanagers  and  allies. 
U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  BuU.  211. 

Brown,  J.  L.  1964.  The  evolution  of  diversity  in  avian  territorial  systems.  Wilson  Bull. 
76:160-169. 

Cody,  M.  L.  1971.  Ecological  aspects  of  reproduction.  Pp.  461-512  in  Avian  biology,  Vol. 

1 (D.  Earner  and  J.  King,  eds.).  Academic  Press,  New  York,  New  York. 

Fleetwood,  R.  J.  1973.  Plants  of  Santa  Ana  National  Wildlife  Refuge.  U.S.D.I.,  Fish  and 
Wildl.  Serv. 

Horn,  H.  S.  1968.  The  adaptive  significance  of  colonial  nesting  in  the  Brewer’s  Blackbird. 
Ecology  49:682-694. 

Oberholser,  H.  C.  1974.  The  bird  life  of  Texas.  Univ.  Texas  Press,  Austin,  Texas. 


Pleasants  • ICTERUS  GULARIS  BREEDING  BIOLOGY 


537 


Orians,  G.  H.  1969.  The  number  of  bird  species  in  some  tropical  forests.  Ecology  50:783- 
801. 

Pleasants,  B.  Y.  1977.  Ecological  factors  affecting  the  breeding  dispersion  patterns  of 
some  North  American  orioles  (Icterus).  Ph.D.  thesis.  Univ.  California,  Los  Angeles, 
California. 

. 1979.  Adaptive  significance  of  the  variable  dispersion  pattern  of  breeding  Northern 

Orioles.  Condor  81:28-34. 

Ricklefs,  R.  E.  1969.  The  nesting  cycle  of  songbirds  in  tropical  and  temperate  regions. 
Living  Bird  8:165-175. 

Rising,  J.  D.  1970.  Morphological  variation  and  evolution  in  some  North  American  orioles. 
Syst.  Zool.  19:315-351. 

Sealy,  S.  G.  1980.  Breeding  biology  of  Orchard  Orioles  in  a new  population  in  Manitoba. 
Can.  Field-Nat.  94:154-158. 

Skutch,  a.  F.  1949.  Do  tropical  birds  rear  as  many  young  as  they  can  nourish?  Ibis  91:430- 
455. 

Smith,  S.  M.  1978.  The  “underworld”  in  a territorial  sparrow:  adaptive  strategy  for  floaters. 
Am.  Nat.  112:571-582. 

Sutton,  G.  M.  and  O.  S.  Pettingill,  Jr.  1943.  The  Altamira  Oriole  and  its  nest.  Condor 
45:125-132. 

DEPT.  ZOOLOGY,  IOWA  STATE  UNIV.,  AMES,  IOWA  50011.  ACCEPTED  22 
JUNE  1981. 


l 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  538-540 


GENERAL  NOTES 


An  example  of  a hybrid  Green  Jay  X Blue  Jay. — While  attempting  to  secure  fledg- 
ling Blue  Jays  {Cyanocitta  cristata)  for  use  in  a behavioral  study,  Dellinger  learned  that  there 
was  a hybrid  between  a Green  Jay  {Cyanocorax  yncas)  and  a Blue  Jay  on  exhibit  at  the 
Zoological  Park  in  Fort  Worth,  Texas.  She  visited  the  park  on  18  September  1977,  and 
learned  that  the  jay  was  over  12  years  old  at  the  time,  having  hatched  on  17  June  1965. 

The  hybrid  was  the  result  of  a mating  between  a male  Green  Jay  and  a female  Blue  Jay 
during  the  spring  of  1965.  The  female,  a locally  hand-reared  bird  hatched  in  1964,  was 
obtained  from  a bird  rehabilitator  as  a possible  mate  for  the  Green  Jay.  There  was  no  record 
of  where  the  male  was  obtained.  The  mating  produced  two  clutches,  the  first  of  which  failed. 
The  second  clutch  comprised  three  eggs  laid  on  30,  31  May  and  1 June;  only  one  young 
hatched  successfully.  Soon  after  the  young  was  taken  from  its  parents  and  hand-reared.  The 
hybrid  was  placed  as  a juvenile  in  an  outdoor  cage  holding  other  jays. 

The  hybrid  died  on  28  January  1978,  and  was  donated  to  the  vertebrate  collection  at  the 
University  of  Texas  at  Arlington  (number  UTA  730).  Both  skin  and  carcass  were  preserved. 
The  specimen  proved  to  be  a male  weighing  104  g,  with  little  fat,  having  an  enlarged  left 
testis,  yellowish-orange  irides,  and  with  bill  and  fleshy  parts  of  the  mouth  black.  The  lower 
mandible  showed  a slight  deformity.  Measurements  were  as  follows:  total  length  320  mm, 
wing  129  mm,  tail  length  141  mm,  tarsus  35.5  mm,  culmen  21  mm. 

Unfortunately,  no  behavioral  notes  were  kept  on  the  hybrid  from  initial  fledging  in  1965 
to  18  September  1977.  Zoo  personnel  informed  us  that  at  times  the  hybrid  was  housed  with 
Blue  and  Green  jays.  When  Dellinger  observed  the  bird  in  September  1977,  it  was  with  a 
Scrub  Jay  {Aphelocoma  coerulescens)  and  eight  other  species.  From  late  October  1977  until 
its  death,  the  hybrid  was  housed  with  two  hand-reared,  first  year  Blue  Jays,  and  may  have 
been  forming  a pair  bond  with  one,  as  food  exchange  was  noted  several  times. 

On  18  September  and  again  on  20  October  1977,  Dellinger  heard  the  hybrid  give  a call 
much  like  that  of  a typical  Blue  Jay.  It  responded  to  wild  Blue  Jays  outside  by  flying  to  the 
side  of  the  cage,  clinging  to  the  wire  and  calling  jay.  The  hybrid  also  sang  quietly  on  20 
October,  a behavior  of  frequent  occurrence  according  to  zoo  personnel. 

Fig.  1 shows  the  morphological  features  of  the  hybrid.  Blue,  typical  of  Blue  Jays,  was 
dominant;  Green  Jay  colors  were  generally  absent.  The  forehead  and  crown  were  primarily 
white  with  blue  tips  and  edges;  feathers  obscuring  the  nares  were  blue  with  black  tips;  the 
lores  and  auriculars  were  black;  and  the  malar  region  was  blue  with  black  in  the  adjoining 
areas.  Small  patches  of  blue  were  present  above  and  below  the  eyes,  but  feathers  immedi- 
ately in  front  of  and  behind  the  eyes  were  black.  The  chin  and  throat  were  black  mottled 
with  blue.  The  upper  breast  was  also  black  and  separated  from  the  white  belly  by  a transition 
area  of  blackish-blue.  The  sides  of  the  breast  were  bluish-gray.  The  undertail  coverts,  fem- 
orals  and  crurals  were  white,  the  back  and  uppertail  coverts  blue.  The  nape  was  blue  mixed 
with  white  and  closely  resembled  that  of  a Blue  Jay.  The  alulae  and  primaries  were  blue. 
The  blue  secondaries  and  tertiaries  were  broadly  tipped  with  white;  the  upper  primary 
coverts  were  all  blue  and  the  upper  secondary  coverts  were  blue,  tipped  with  white.  The 
upper,  middle  and  lesser  wing  coverts  were  also  blue  with  the  marginals  showing  some  white 
on  the  inner  webs.  Faint  barring  was  apparent  on  the  largest  alular  feathers,  some  second- 
aries, tertiaries  and  the  upper  greater  secondary  coverts.  The  underwing  coverts  were  mostly 
white.  The  two  central  rectrices  were  blue;  those  adjoining  were  blue,  tipped  with  white; 
and  those  outermost  were  mainly  white.  The  barring  of  the  tail,  so  characteristic  of  a true 
Blue  Jay,  showed  only  faintly. 

Previous  records  of  hybridization  among  jays  in  the  wild  include  that  of  a White-tipped 


538 


GENERAL  NOTES 


539 


Fig.  1.  Captive  hybrid  Green  Jay  x Blue  Jay. 


Brown  Jay  {Psilorhinus  morio)  X Magpie  Jay  {Calocitta  formosa)  from  western  Chiapas, 
Mexico  (Pitelka  et  al.,  Condor  58:98-106,  1956),  and  a Blue  Jay  x SteUer’s  Jay  {Cyanocitta 
stelleri)  from  Boulder,  Boulder  Co.,  Colorado  (Williams  and  Wheat,  Wilson  BuU.  83:343- 
346,  1971). 

Hardy  and  Raitt  (abstract,  Proc.  16th  Int.  Ornithol.  Congr.  105,  1974)  reported  successful 
captive  hybridization  between  a Yucatan  Jay  {Cissilopha  yucatanica)  and  a San  Bias  Jay  (C. 
sanblasiana),  but,  unfortunately,  the  young  died  at  24  days.  In  1973,  a Beechey’s  Jay  (Pur- 
phsh-backed  Jay  [C.  beecheii])  was  crossed  with  a Magpie  Jay  at  the  Arizona-Sonora  Desert 
Museum  in  Tucson,  Arizona,  where,  according  to  Gale  Monson  (pers.  comm.),  the  hybrid  is 
stiU  ahve  and  on  display.  He  also  stated  that  as  an  adult  it  shows  more  characteristics  of  the 
Beechey’s  Jay  than  of  the  Magpie  Jay.  It  has  the  dark  eyes  of  the  latter,  rather  than  the 
yellow  eyes  of  the  adult  Beechey’s  Jay. 


540 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


The  possibility  of  the  Green  Jay  and  the  Blue  Jay  occurring  naturally  together  during  the 
breeding  season  is  remote,  so  that  hybrids  are  not  to  be  expected  in  the  wild.  In  Texas,  the 
Green  Jay  occurs  locally  throughout  the  Rio  Grande  Valley  to  Laredo,  Webb  Co.,  and  to 
Ealfurrias,  Brooks  Co.,  and  occasionally  north  to  Alice,  Jim  Wells  Co.,  and  sporadically 
north  to  San  Antonio,  Bexar  Co.  In  winter,  the  Blue  Jay  is  found  as  a straggler  on  Edwards 
Plateau  just  north  of  San  Antonio  and  rarely  in  the  Rio  Grande  Valley. 

W'e  should  like  to  thank  Jon  C.  Barlow,  John  Darling,  John  William  Hardy,  Ronald  KimbeU, 
Terry  C.  Maxwell  and  Gale  Monson  for  their  comments  and  assistance  in  the  preparation  of 
this  note.— Warren  M.  Puuch,  Dept.  Biology,  Univ.  Dallas,  Irving,  Texas  75061  AND 
Rebecca  M.  Dellinger,  P.O.  Box  163,  Duncanville,  Texas  75116.  Accepted  20  Oct.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  p.  340 


Dusky  Seaside  Sparrow  feeds  Red-wdnged  Blackbird  fledglings. — The  endangered 
Dusky  Seaside  Sparrow  {Ammospiza  maritima  nigrescens),  restricted  to  small  tracts  of  salt 
and  brackish  marsh  near  Titusville,  Brevard  Co.,  Florida,  is  rapidly  nearing  extinction.  While 
censusing  the  population  on  St.  Johns  National  Wildlife  Refuge  on  9 August  1976,  we  noted 
an  unmated,  color-banded  male  sparrow  had  abandoned  a territory  defended  since  10  June 
to  feed  two  recently  fledged  Red-winged  Blackbirds  (Agelaius  phoeniceus)  about  50  m away. 
The  mother  of  the  brood  was  completely  tolerant  of  the  sparrow’s  activity,  despite  the  spar- 
row’s strenuous  efforts  to  keep  her  away.  Both  birds  fed  the  young  until  12  August.  Among 
13  food  items  brought  by  the  sparrow  were  five  grasshoppers  and  three  spiders  (cf.  Howell, 
Florida  Bird  Life,  Coward-McCann,  New  York,  New  York,  1932).  Based  on  736  min  of 
observation  during  these  4 days,  foraging  accounted  for  36%  of  the  sparrow’s  activity,  food 
delivery  for  17%,  singing  for  18%  and  aggression  toward  the  female  red-wing  for  8%;  the 
remaining  time  was  spent  on  perch  or  in  unknown  activity.  Mean  duration  of  the  red-wing’s 
foraging  absences  was  longer  than  the  sparrow’s  (8  min  vs  14  min,  t — 2.40,  df  = 36,  P < 
0.05,  N = 22  and  16,  respectively),  and  she  spent  a greater  proportion  of  her  time  foraging 
(75%). 

Most  instances  of  interspecific  helping  have  involved  adults  actively  or  recently  engaged 
in  reproduction  (Skutch,  Condor  63:198-226,  1961).  While  the  color-banded  sparrow  was 
seen  with  a female  and  young  in  1973  and  presumably  had  ample  additional  breeding  ex- 
perience, we  are  convinced,  based  on  5 months  of  observation,  that  he  was  unmated  and 
had  neither  nest  nor  young  in  1976.  Factors  which  may  have  contributed  to  his  abnormal 
behavior  are  unclear.  Between  1970  and  1976,  wildfires  reduced  the  population  of  Dusky 
Seaside  Sparrows  on  St.  Johns  NWR  from  110  to  12  (11  dcJ,  1 9).  During  spring  1976,  local 
variation  in  the  rate  of  vegetative  recovery,  coupled  with  rapid  flooding  due  to  heavy  rains, 
resulted  in  considerable  shifting  of  sparrow  territories.  Prior  to  feeding  the  red-wings  the 
sparrow  was  occupying  his  second  territory  of  the  year,  the  first  (occupied  4 May-7  June) 
having  been  flooded.  We  beheve  that  the  low  level  of  the  population,  the  shortage  of  females 
and  perhaps  the  instability  of  sparrow  territories  may  have  acted  individually  or  in  concert 
to  prompt  this  male’s  unusual  behavior. 

These  data  were  collected  while  JLR  was  conducting  research  for  an  M.S.  degree  at  the 
University  of  Georgia.  Support  for  this  study  was  provided  by  the  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife 
Service. — James  L.  Rakestil\W,  School  of  F orest  Resources,  Univ.  Georgia,  Athens,  Georgia 
30602  AND  James  L.  Baker,  Merritt  Island  National  Wildlife  Refuge,  Titusville,  Florida 
32  780.  (Present  addresses:  JLR:  Museum  of  Natural  History,  Univ.  Kansas,  Lawrence,  Kan- 
sas 66045  AND  JLB:  Jacksonville  Area  Office,  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife  Service,  15  N.  Laura 
St.,  Jacksonville,  Florida  32202.)  Accepted  24  June  1980. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


541 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  541-542 


Statistical  significance  and  density-dependent  nest  predation. — Bradley  Gottfried 
(Wilson  BuU.  90:643-646,  1978)  recently  published  a note  on  an  experimental  study  of  the 
effect  of  nest  density  on  nest  predation.  He  tested  the  null  hypothesis  that  there  was  no 
difference  between  the  experimental  and  control,  the  high  and  low  density  plots.  The  study 
was  well  thought  out  and  presented,  and  is  in  fact  a model  for  the  kind  of  information  that 
alone  wiU  convince  us  that  our  hypotheses  are  or  are  not  valid.  Dr.  Gottfried’s  excellent 
study  produced  results,  however,  that  did  not  show  a statistically  significant  difference  be- 
tween the  rates  of  nest  predation  on  artificially  placed  nests  at  high  densities,  and  similar 
nests  placed  at  low  densities.  This  result  led  him  to  accept  the  hypothesis  that  there  was  no 
density-dependent  predation  in  the  field  he  was  studying,  in  contrast  to  the  results  reported 
in  other  studies.  In  his  discussion  he  then  addressed  the  question  of  potential  difference 
between  the  old  field  plots  he  studied  and  the  plots  that  others  have  studied.  To  show  that 
this  difference  exists,  we  need  to  show  that  his  data  were  significantly  different  from  the 
data  presented  by  scientists  who  worked  in  either  marshes  or  in  forests,  both  of  whom  have 
found  statistically  significant  differences  in  nest  predation  between  habitats  with  high  and 
low  densities  of  nests. 

For  example,  one  of  the  studies  which  he  supposed  produced  different  results  from  those 
in  his  study  is  Fretwell’s  (Populations  in  a Seasonal  Environment,  Princeton  Univ.  Press, 
Princeton,  New  Jersey,  1972).  FetweU  found  that  Field  Sparrows  {Spizella  pusilla)  nesting 
in  two  early  succession  pine  forests  at  two  different  densities  had  nesting  success  rates  of 
0.21  (high  nest  density)  and  0.33  (low  nest  density).  Fretwell  presented  this  difference  as 
being  statistically  significant,  but  a critique  by  Dow  (Wilson  BuU.  90:291-295,  1978)  on  tbe 
technique  used  by  FretweU  (1972)  is  valid  and  suggests  that  the  difference  should  be  re- 
evaluated by  a more  appropriate  method.  FretweU,  however,  did  note  the  same  trend  in  aU 
3 years  that  he  coUected  data,  and  also  found  a statisticaUy  significant  trend  in  an  intensive 
within-habitat  study.  The  difference  discovered  by  Gottfried  (1978)  for  1 week  of  nest  ex- 
posure was  0.69  success  in  the  high  density  plot  and  0.76  success  in  the  low  density  plot. 
Since  the  normal  successful  nest  is  exposed  for  at  least  3 weeks,  we  can  estimate  the 
magnitude  of  this  difference  for  nests  that  would  be  comparable  to  those  in  FretweU’s  study 
by  taking  these  survival  rates  to  the  third  power.  This  assumes  that  these  nests  would  be 
replaced  as  lost,  which  in  fact  is  what  would  occur  in  a natural  situation.  This  yields  0.33 
success  for  the  high  density  plot  and  0.45  for  the  low  density  plot.  Thus,  at  high  densities, 
sucess  was  about  27%  lower  than  the  value  in  the  low  density  plots.  This  compares  to  the 
difference  of  37%  in  FretweU’s  study. 

We  attempted  to  see  if  this  difference  in  studies  was  statisticaUy  significant,  by  doing  a 
z-test  on  the  difference  between  tbe  differences.  We  first  corrected  each  survival  rate  for 
the  average  in  the  study  in  which  it  was  measured,  since  we  are  interested  in  comparing 
relative  and  not  absolute  differences.  For  example,  a difference  in  survival  rates  of  10%  and 
5%  is  more  significant  than  a difference  of  50%  and  40%,  and  FretweU’s  nests  survived  less 
weU  than  Gottfried’s  nests.  We  tested  the  nuU  hypothesis  that  the  37%  density-dependent 
effect  in  FretweU’s  study  is  the  same  as  the  27%  density-dependent  effect  in  Gottfried’s 
study  using  the  foUowing  formula: 


Z = 


’#T( 


P, 


1 


Pl2 

Pi 

1 


P22 

P2 


1 


Pi^n„  Pi^ni2  p2^n2i  P2^H22 


where 


542 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


proportion  of  successful  nests  in  Eretwell’s  low  density  plot, 
proportion  of  successful  nests  in  Fretwell’s  high  density  plot, 
proportion  of  successful  nests  in  Gottfried’s  low  density  plot, 
proportion  of  successful  nests  in  Gottfried’s  high  density  plot, 
F*i2  p _ F*2i  + P22  p_Pi  + P2  „ — 1 p 


nil  = sample  size  in  Eretwell’s  low  density  plot, 
ni2  = sample  size  in  FretweU’s  high  density  plot, 
n2i  = sample  size  in  Gottfried’s  low  density  plot, 
n22  = sample  size  in  Gottfried’s  high  density  plot. 


The  value  obtained  is  0.33.  If  all  the  ratios  in  the  numerator  are  normally  distributed  with 
variances  as  calculated  beneath  the  square  root  bracket  in  the  denominator,  then  the  prob- 
ability of  the  value  calculated  follows  a z-distribution,  since  the  sum  of  normally  distributed 
variates  is  also  normally  distributed.  These  ratios  are  aU  calculated  from  means  and  should 
therefore  be  normally  distributed.  A z value  of  0.33  is  not  large  enough  to  reject  the  nuU 
hypothesis  (P  < 0.37). 

Statistical  significance  is  a statement  about  sample  size,  not  about  a biological  phenom- 
enon. The  presence  of  statistical  significance  in  some  data  simply  means  that  one’s  sample 
is  sufficiently  large  to  detect  the  biological  differences  that  are  present.  Absence  of  statistical 
significance  means  that  one’s  data  are  insufficient  to  detect  any  measurable  biological  dif- 
ferences that  are  present.  The  inability  to  detect  a difference  does  not  justify  the  conclusion 
that  no  differences  are  present,  however.  Gottfried’s  data,  which  are  invaluable  and  beyond 
any  doubt  deserve  our  attention,  are  yet  too  few  (one  breeding  season,  136  nests)  for  us  to 
know  whether  or  not  there  is  any  biologically  significant  density  dependence  of  nest  predation 
in  old  fields.  They  are  also  too  few  for  us  to  know  whether  or  not  there  are  any  differences 
between  old  fields  and  marshes  and  successional  woodlands. 

If  Gottfried  repficated  his  study  4.5  times,  he  would  have  an  80%  chance  of  detecting  the 
presence  of  a density  effect  (Sokal  and  Rohlf,  Biometry,  W.  H.  Freeman  and  Co.,  San 
Francisco,  California,  1968:609).  To  show  differences  between  habitats,  we  would  need  many 
more  replications  in  both  woods  and  old  field.  Replicating  both  Gottfried’s  experiments  and 
Fretwell’s  studies  the  same  number  of  times,  assuming  aU  samples  are  the  size  of  Gottfried’s 
and  correcting  to  equalize  mean  predation  rates  between  old  field  and  woods,  it  would  take 
over  30  sets  of  results  identical  to  Fretwell’s  and  Gottfried’s  (or  results  showing  a greater 
difference)  to  demonstrate  significance. — STEPHEN  D.  Fretwell  AND  Frank  S.  Shipley, 
Div.  Biology,  Kansas  State  Univ.,  Manhattan,  Kansas  66506.  Accepted  18  Nov.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  542-547 

A comparison  of  nest-site  and  perch-site  vegetation  structure  for  seven  species 
of  warblers. — One  aspect  of  the  study  of  avian  niche  structure  has  involved  habitat  rela- 
tionships of  breeding  birds.  In  general,  birds  seek  a characteristic  vegetation-structure  type, 
their  niche-gestalt  (James,  Wilson  Bull.  83:215-236,  1971),  in  which  to  establish  a territory 
(Hilden,  Ann.  Zool.  Fenn.  2:53-75,  1965).  This  territory  provides  many  breeding  passerines 
with  suitable  areas  for  singing,  feeding  and  nesting.  Some  previous  descriptions  of  avian 
habitat  relationships  (James  1971;  Whitmore,  Wilson  BuU.  87:65-74,  1975;  Smith,  Ecology 
58:810-819,  1977)  have  been  based  on  information  collected  from  within  a 0.04-ha  circular 


GENERAL  NOTES 


543 


Table  1 

Bird  Species  and  Number  of  Circular  Plots  Used  in  the  Analyses 


Species 

Habitat® 

Circles 

Number 
of  nests 

Perches 

NashviUe  Warbler  (Vermivora  ruficapilla) 

edge 

13 

3 

3*' 

Northern  Parula  (Parula  americana) 

forest 

16 

4 

3 

Yellow  Warbler  (Dendroica  petechia) 

open 

14 

4 

4 

Chestnut-sided  Warbler  (D.  pensylvanica) 

open 

16 

2 

2 

Palm  Warbler  (D.  palmarum) 

edge 

4 

2 

2 

Ovenbird  (Seiurus  auricapillus) 

forest 

18 

4 

3 

Common  YeUowthroat  (Geothlypis  trichas) 

open 

17 

4 

0 

Total 

98 

23 

17 

® CoUins  et  al.  (Oikos,  In  press),  based  upon  analysis  of  2II  plots  for  16  species  of  warblers. 
**  Number  of  perch  plots  with  corresponding  nest-site  samples. 


plot  centered  on  a song  perch  within  the  territory  of  a singing  male.  Various  structural 
attributes  of  the  vegetation  are  recorded  in  these  plots  (James  and  Shugart,  Audubon  Field 
Notes  24:727-736,  1970),  and  several  circles  are  sampled  to  determine  the  general  habitat 
structure  of  each  species.  In  the  past,  these  data  have  been  presented  as  averages  and  thus 
do  not  permit  analysis  of  subtle  within-habitat  structural  differences.  The  purpose  of  my 
research  was  to  determine  if  differences  in  vegetation  structure  occur  within  the  territories 
of  several  species  of  Paruhdae  (Table  1).  This  study  is  part  of  a larger  project  analyzing  the 
habitat  relationships  and  geographic  habitat  variation  of  the  warblers  in  Maine  and  Minne- 
sota. 

Study  area  and  methods. — This  study  was  conducted  in  Itasca  State  Park  located  in  Clear- 
water, Hubbard  and  Becker  counties,  in  north-central  Minnesota.  The  park  contains  12,500 
ha,  of  which  941  ha  (7%)  are  lakes  and  ponds  (Hansen  et  al.,  Univ.  Minnesota  Agric.  Exper. 
Stat.  BuU.  298,  1974).  The  area  is  located  in  the  hemlock  (T5u^a)-white  pine  {Pinus  strobus)- 
northern  hardwoods  forest  region  (Braun,  Deciduous  Forests  of  Eastern  North  America, 
Blakeston  Press,  Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania,  1950).  Both  logging  and  fires  have  created  a 
diversity  of  vegetation  types  in  the  region,  ranging  from  aspen  coppice  to  mature  upland 
spruce  (Picea)-fir  (Abies)  forests,  hardwood  stands  and  pine  stands.  Parmelee  (Loon  49:81- 
95,  1977)  reported  27  species  of  warblers  in  the  park,  of  which  13  are  considered  common 
nesting  species. 

Additional  habitat  data  were  obtained  for  Nashville  and  Palm  warblers  from  the  Red  Lake 
Peatlands  Natural  Area,  northern  Beltrami  Co.,  Minnesota.  The  vegetation  in  this  region 
consists  of  forested  “islands”  of  small  black  spruce  (Picea  mariana)  and  tamarack  (Larix 
laricina),  dense,  low  ericacious  shrubs  and  a continuous  ground  cover  of  sedges  and  Sphag- 
num spp. 

To  determine  if  within-habitat  variability  occurs,  two  0.04-ha  circular  plots,  one  at  the 
nest-site  and  one  at  a perch-site,  were  sampled  within  the  territory  of  a breeding  male  bird. 
Thirteen  structural  characteristics  of  the  vegetation  were  measured  in  each  circle  (Table  2). 
Supplemental  perch-site  data  were  obtained  from  another  data  set  in  which  nest-sites  were 
not  located.  A total  of  23  nest-sites  and  75  perch-sites  were  sampled  (Table  1). 

Statistical  differences  between  the  vegetation  structure  of  the  nest-sites  and  perch-sites 
were  measured  by  the  Wilcoxon  matched-pairs  signed-ranks  test.  This  test  determines  the 


544 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Table  2 

Vegetation  Structure  Variables  Considered  in  the  Analysis  of  0.04^ha  Nest- 
AND  Perch-site  Samples® 


GC  Percent  ground  cover — no.  of  sightings  of  ground  cover  vegetation  at  20 

evenly  spaced  points  across  a transect  dividing  the  circle 
CC  Percent  canopy  cover — no.  of  sightings  of  canopy  vegetation  at  20  evenly 

spaced  points  across  a transect  dividing  the  circle 
SC  No.  of  contacts  of  shrub  vegetation  by  the  outstretched  arms  at  20  evenly 

spaced  points  across  a transect  dividing  the  circle 
CO  Percent  coniferous  vegetation  in  the  canopy  at  20  evenly  spaced  points 

across  a transect  dividing  the  circle 
CH  Canopy  height 

SPT  No.  of  species  of  trees 

T1  No.  of  trees  7.5-15  cm  dbh 

T2  No.  of  trees  15.1-23  cm  dbh 

T3  No.  of  trees  23.1-30  cm  dbh 

T4  No.  of  trees  30.1-38  cm  dbh 

T5  No.  of  trees  38.1-53  cm  dbh 

T6  No.  of  trees  53.1-68.5  cm  dbh 

T7  No.  of  trees  greater  than  68.5  cm  dbh 


® After  James  and  Shugart  (1970). 


direction  and  magnitude  of  differences  (Siegel,  Nonparametric  Statistics  for  the  Behavioral 
Sciences,  McGraw-Hill,  New  York,  New  York,  1956)  between  the  structural  characteristics 
of  nest-  and  perch-sites.  Comparisons  for  each  species  were  made  at  two  levels:  (1)  individual 
nests  with  corresponding  perch-sites,  and  (2)  average  nest-site  structure  vs  average  perch- 
site  structure. 

Typically,  0.04-ha-plot  data  are  analyzed  by  multivariate  ordination  techniques.  These 
methods  elicit  habitat  patterns  and  indicate  the  most  important  vegetation  variables  which 
produce  these  patterns.  In  this  case,  discriminant  function  analysis  (DFA)  was  applied  to 
each  species  habitat  structure  matrix  to  determine  if  vegetation  structure  variables  can 
discriminate  between  nest-  and  perch-sites.  DFA  combines  the  habitat  variables  in  a stepwise 
fashion  into  the  linear  discriminant  function  which  can  best  segregate  nest-sites  from  perch- 
sites.  The  advantage  of  the  multivariate  DFA  over  the  univariate  Wilcoxon  tests  is  that  the 
former  method  incorporates  the  variability  inherent  in  any  habitat  structure  data  set.  For  a 
description  of  DFA  see  Morrison  (Multivariate  Statistical  Methods,  2nd  ed.,  McGraw-HiU, 
New  York,  New  York,  1979).  The  DFA  was  performed  with  BMDP  (Dixon,  Biomedical 
Computer  Programs,  Univ.  California  Press,  Los  Angeles,  California,  1977)  on  the  University 
of  Oklahoma  IBM  360/50  computer. 

Results  and  discussion. — The  general  habitat  structure  of  the  seven  species  of  warblers 
ranged  from  open-country  to  forest  and  forest-edge  nesting  species  (Table  1).  In  spite  of  the 
small  sample  size,  some  patterns  and  differences  in  nest-site  and  perch-site  structure  can 
be  extracted. 

Only  29%  (5/17)  of  the  nest-sites  had  vegetation  structures  that  were  significantly  different 
from  the  corresponding  perch-sites  within  a territory  (Table  3).  Four  of  the  five  differences 
were  in  open-country  nesting  species.  The  differences  in  the  structurally  simple  open  habitats 


GENERAL  NOTES 


545 


Table  3 

Comparison  of  Nest-site  vs  Perch-site  Vegetation  Structure® 


Species 

Nest-perch 

comparison 

r> 

P 

N 

Northern  Parula 

N2-P2 

4.0 

0.01 

11 

YeUow  Warbler 

Nl-Pl 

3.5 

0.05 

9 

YeUow  Warbler 

N3-P3 

0.0 

0.05 

7 

Chestnut-sided  Warbler 

Nl-Pl 

4.0 

0.05 

8 

Chestnut-sided  Warbler 

N2-P2 

0.0 

0.01 

9 

* Based  on  the  Wilcoxon  matched-pairs  signed-ranks  test  (Siegel  1956);  5 of  17  comparisons  were  significantly  different. 
” T = sum  of  ranks,  P = probability  level,  N = number  of  variables  in  comparison. 


of  the  Yellow  and  Chestnut-sided  warblers  were  due  to  the  greater  number  of  trees  at  perch- 
sites  which  increased  canopy  cover,  tree  height  and  percent  conifer  in  the  canopy.  The 
perch-site  of  the  Northern  Parula  had  higher  ground  and  shrub  cover,  and  percent  conifer 
in  the  canopy  than  at  the  nest  location. 

If  the  nest-site  and  perch-site  data  for  each  species  are  averaged  and  again  compared  by 
the  Wilcoxon  test,  the  within-territory  structure  of  the  Northern  Parula  is  no  longer  statis- 
tically different  (N  = 12,  T = 21).  However,  both  the  Yellow  and  Chestnut-sided  warblers 
stiU  showed  significant  differences  (N  = 12;  T = 1 and  T = 9,  respectively).  Average  perch- 
site  variables  of  these  species  agciin  contained  greater  tree  component  structure  than  average 
nest-sites  corroborating  the  results  of  the  within-territory  comparisons. 

The  F-values  for  the  six  discriminant  functions  were  significant  for  only  two  species — 
Common  YeUowthroat  and  Northern  Parula  (Table  4).  Percent  conifer  and  canopy  height 
significantly  separate  Common  YeUowthroat  nest-  and  perch-sites.  However,  the  DFA  re- 
classified one  perch-site  as  a nest-site,  and  vice  versa.  Thus,  within  this  data  set,  some 
structural  overlap  occurs  between  the  two  types  of  sites. 

Eight  variables  entered  into  the  Northern  Parula  discriminant  function,  most  of  which  were 
tree  size-class  variables.  The  nests  of  this  species  were  located  in  forest  to  forest-edge  habitat 


Table  4 

Discriminant  Function  Analysis  of  Species  Nest-site  vs  Perch-site  Structure 


Species 

Variables  entered® 

F-value  (df) 

P 

Number 

reclassified 

NashviUe  Warbler 

T2,  CO 

3.49  (2,  10) 

0.10 

1 

Northern  Parula 

T3,  T2,  T6,  CH, 

T5,  SPT,  GC,  T4 

5.69  (8,  7) 

0.02 

0 

YeUow  Warbler 

CH,  SPT,  SC,  T1 

1.79  (4,  9) 

NS 

2 

Chestnut-sided  Warbler 

CC 

3.83  (1,  14) 

0.10 

2 

Ovenbird 

T3,  T5 

2.36  (2,  15) 

NS 

5 

Common  YeUowthroat 

CO,  CH 

4.50  (2,  14) 

0.05 

2 

® Variables  are  listed  in  order  of  entry  into  the  discriminant  function;  see  Table  2 for  definition  of  variables. 


546 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


with  variable  numbers  of  large  deciduous  and  coniferous  trees,  and  a relatively  open  canopy. 
Perch-sites  were  also  variable  yet  they  were  most  often  located  in  the  forest  rather  than  at 
the  forest  edge.  No  nest-  or  perch-sites  were  reclassified,  so  complete  discrimination  between 
these  sites  is  possible. 

In  summary,  it  appears  that  for  the  ground  nesting  Palm  Warblers,  Nashville  Warblers 
and  Ovenbirds,  minimal  differences  exist  between  nest-site  and  perch-site  structure.  The 
Yellow  and  Chestnut-sided  warblers  showed  significant  differences  in  individual  and  average 
nest-site/perch-site  comparisons,  yet  these  nonconformities  did  not  appear  in  the  DFA.  The 
Common  Yellowthroat  showed  a difference  only  in  the  multivariate  analysis.  Lastly,  both 
uni-  and  multivariate  comparisons  of  the  Northern  Parula  nest-  and  perch-site  variables 
imply  locally  different  within-habitat  vegetation  structure. 

Several  factors  may  cause  the  differences  observed  in  these  tests,  one  of  which  is  the 
inherent  variability  of  the  vegetation.  Curtis  (The  Vegetation  of  Wisconsin,  Univ.  Wisconsin 
Press,  Madison,  Wisconsin,  1959)  stressed  the  compositional  variation  of  vegetation  and 
concluded  that  the  same  plant  communities  in  a region  resemble  each  other  only  to  the 
extent  of  50-70%.  Many  territories  of  forest  nesting  species  are  greater  than  0.5  ha  (Bent, 
Life  Histories  of  North  American  Wood  Warblers,  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  203,  1953)  thus 
incorporating  the  natural  variability  of  the  vegetation.  Secondly,  the  male  selects  and  defends 
the  territory,  whereas  the  female  chooses  the  nest-site.  Different  criteria  are  selected  at  each 
site:  conspicuousness  for  male  displays  vs  sheltered  nest  location.  Thirdly,  previous  ecolog- 
ical studies  of  warblers  have  shown  that  males  and  females  use  different  parts  of  the  territory 
(Morse,  Ecology  49:779-784,  1%8;  Ecology  54:346-355,  1973;  Busby  and  Sealy,  Can.  J.  Zool. 
57:1670-1681,  1979).  In  particular,  males  foraged  farther  from  the  nest  and  higher  in  the 
canopy  than  did  females.  Finally,  the  selection  of  a perch-site  as  the  center  of  a circular  plot 
implies  some  degree  of  vegetation  structure.  Therefore,  the  wide  ranging  foraging  behavior 
of  males,  large  territories  and  differential  territory  use  combine  to  introduce  within-habitat 
variability. 

Many  stimuli,  such  as  specific  aspects  of  habitat  structure,  presence  of  other  birds,  food 
and  previous  breeding  success,  are  proximate  factors  which  can  combine  to  elicit  a territorial 
settling  response  in  breeding  birds  (Hilden  1965).  The  measurement  of  vegetation  structure 
is  a reliable  means  of  summarizing  these  stimuli  since  the  physical  habitat  provides  the 
background  for  the  variables  in  the  life  cycle  of  a breeding  bird.  The  suitability  of  the  0.04- 
ha-circle  technique  for  summarizing  and  describing  the  3-dimensional  habitat  structure  of 
a species  remains  valid.  Certain  caveats,  however,  should  be  considered.  James  (1971)  stated 
that  centering  a circular  plot  on  a song  perch  “may  give  a biased  view  of  habitat  for  species 
which  occur  in  open  areas  and  choose  singing  perches  in  places  different  from  their  foraging 
areas,  hut  this  objection  is  minimized  in  the  forest.”  She  later  reiterated  this  statement 
noting  that  the  0.04-ha-circle  method  was  only  suitable  for  areas  with  trees  (James,  Am. 
Birds  32:18-21,  1978).  However,  my  evidence  for  the  Northern  Parula  suggests  that  within- 
hahitat  variability  exists  in  forest  nesting  species.  The  technique  is  stiU  very  useful  for 
describing  the  habitat  of  breeding  birds,  but  locating  circular  samples  around  nest-sites  or 
around  female  foraging  areas  is  recommended  whenever  possible  to  incorporate  within-hab- 
itat variation.  Otherwise,  caution  should  be  used  when  interpreting  habitat  structure  since 
perch-sites  of  forest  and  open-country  nesting  species  may  overestimate  the  tree  component 
of  the  habitat. 

Acknowledgments. — I would  like  to  thank  the  Behavioral  Ecology  class  and  the  Field  Or- 
nithology class  for  providing  some  nest  locations.  I especially  thank  my  wife,  Pat,  for  field 
assistance.  Dwight  Adams,  Karen  Dooley,  David  Gibson,  Frances  James,  Paul  Risser  and 
Gary  SchneU  provided  comments  on  earlier  drafts  of  the  manuscript.  This  research  was 
supported  by  a grant  from  the  Chapman  Memorial  Fund  and  by  a Malvin  and  Josephine  Herz 


GENERAL  NOTES 


547 


Foundation  Summer  Fellowship  to  the  University  of  Minnesota  Biological  Station. — ScoTT 
L.  Collins,  Dept.  Botany  and  Microbiology,  Univ.  Oklahoma,  Norman,  Oklahoma  73019. 
Accepted  8 Nov.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  547-548 


Use  of  artificial  perches  on  burned  and  unburned  tallgrass  prairie. — Kendeigh 
(Condor  43:165-175,  1941)  stated  that  territorial  male  birds  may  lack  sufficient  perches  in 
grasslands  from  which  to  conduct  display  activities.  I investigated  the  importance  of  artificial 
perch  availabihty  to  tallgrass  prairie  birds,  from  7 June-31  July  1979,  at  the  Konza  Prairie 
Research  Natural  Area.  This  area  of  native  bluestem  (Andropogon)  prairie  is  located  in  the 
extreme  south-central  portion  of  Riley  and  northern  portion  of  Geary  counties,  Kansas. 

Two  areas  on  annually  burned  prairie  and  two  on  unburned  prairie  were  selected  for  study. 
Artificial  perches  were  added  to  one  annually  burned  prairie  (35  ha)  and  one  unburned  prairie 
(25  ha)  with  the  other  annually  burned  prairie  (12  ha)  and  unburned  prairie  (39  ha)  used  as 
controls.  The  experimental  sites  were  located  adjacent  to  each  other.  Control  sites  were 
separated  from  experimental  sites  and  from  each  other. 

Perches  were  2x2  cm  wooden  stakes,  1.5  and  2.0  m above  ground  level.  Twenty-three 
perches  were  placed  on  the  35-ha  annually  burned  prairie  and  17  perches  on  the  25-ha 
unburned  prairie,  giving  approximately  equal  perch  density  (0.67  perch/ha)  in  each  area.  The 
perches  were  placed  in  15  m^  subplots  in  each  experimental  area  using  randomly  generated 
numbers.  Use  of  perches  in  burned  and  unburned  prairie  and  perch  height  preference  were 
recorded  during  36  spot  check  censuses.  Spot  check  censuses  were  performed  by  approach- 
ing each  perch  within  100  m and  noting  the  species  and  activity  of  each  bird. 

A vegetation  density  analysis  on  each  plot  was  made  using  randomly  selected  5 m^  areas, 
for  which  standing  height  and  percent  cover  by  life  form  were  recorded.  For  each  area,  50% 
of  the  total  area  was  analyzed. 

Vegetation  analyses  indicated  that  the  following  plants  were  dominant.  Grasses  included: 
big  bluestem  {Andropogon  gerardi),  little  bluestem  (A.  scoparius),  windmiUgrass  (Chloris 
verticillata),  switchgrass  {Panicum  virgatum)  and  side-oats  grama  {Bouteloua  curtipendula). 
Dominant  forbes  were  lead  plant  {Amorpha  canescens),  prairie  wild  indigo  (Baptisia  leuco- 
phaea),  Baldwin  ironweed  ( Vernonia  baldwini),  wild  alfalfa  {Medicago  lupulina),  fingeleaf 
rueUia  {Ruellia  humilis),  tick-trefoil  (Desmodium  illinoense),  butterfly  milkweed  {Asclepias 
tubersoa)  and  narrow-leaved  milkweed  (A.  stenophylla).  Woody  vegetation  consisted  of  the 
prairie  rose  {Rosa  arkansana)  and  buckbrush  {Symphoricarpos  abiculatus).  The  mean  stand- 
ing height  of  vegetation  for  burned  and  unburned  prairie  was  27.66  cm  and  45.50  cm,  re- 
spectively. 

Eleven  of  23  perches  (48%)  were  used  on  the  burned  area  and  5 of  17  perches  (29%)  on 
the  unburned  area.  This  difference  was  not  significant  using  the  Chi-square  test  for  equal 
proportions  (x^  = 1.18,  df  = 1,  P = 0.17).  Lack  of  significance  may  have  been  caused  by 
small  sample  size  and  similarity  in  proportions  of  bird  density/perch  use  in  each  area. 

Species  observed  using  perches  in  the  burned  area,  in  order  of  decreasing  perch  use  were 
Dickcissel  {Spiza  americana).  Eastern  Meadowlark  {Sturnella  magna).  Red-winged  Black- 
bird {Agelaius  phoeniceus).  Brown-headed  Cowbird  {Molothrus  ater).  Common  Nighthawk 
{Chordeiles  minor).  Grasshopper  Sparrow  {Ammondramus  savannarum).  Eastern  Kingbird 
{Tyrannus  tyrannus)  and  Upland  Sandpiper  {Bartramia  longicauda).  The  following  birds 
were  found  to  use  perches  in  the  unburned  area  in  order  of  decreasing  perch  use:  Eastern 
Meadowlark,  Grasshopper  Sparrow,  Dickcissel  and  Brown-headed  Cowbird.  Birds  using 


S48 


THE  U ILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  \o.  4,  December  1981 


perches  engaged  in  territorial  song,  establishment  of  territorial  boundaries,  call  notes,  preen- 
ing and  resting.  Birds  seemed  to  prefer  natural  perches  to  artificial  perches. 

No  difference  in  average  male  density  between  areas  with  and  without  perches  in  unburned 
prairie  (31  dd/ha  in  both)  was  observed.  In  the  burned  prairie,  however,  area  without 
perches  had  a density  twice  as  great  as  the  area  with  perches  (56  d d/ha  vs  27  d d/ha).  This 
difference  was  believed  to  be  due  to  IcU'ge  numbers  of  Dickcissels  and  Red-winged  Blackbirds 
attracted  to  a stream  in  the  area.  When  the  data  with  all  birds  were  tested,  differences  in 
density  between  the  two  areas  were  significant  (Wilcoxon  signed  ranks  test,  P = 0.008); 
however,  when  Dickcissels  and  Red-winged  Blackbirds  were  deleted  from  the  analyses,  the 
difference  was  not  significant  (P  = 0.11)  indicating  that  these  two  species  had  measurable 
impacts  on  the  observed  densities. 

This  research  was  funded  by  the  National  Science  Foundation’s  Undergraduate  Research 
Participation  Program  at  Kansas  State  University.  Thanks  are  due  to  John  L.  Zimmerman 
(Biol.  Dept.,  KSU),  Elmer  Finck  (Biol.  Dept.,  KSUO  and  VPI  & SU  Statistical  Consulting 
Laborator\-. — Janet  Jean  Knodel-Montz,  Fisheries  and  Wildlife  Dept.,  Virginia  Poly'tech- 
nic  Inst,  and  State  Unit.,  Blacksburg,  Virginia  24061.  Accepted  15  Aug.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  p.  548 

Juvenile  Peregrine  Falcon  swoops  on  Roseate  Spoonbills. — On  26  September  1979, 
I obser\ed  an  immature  Peregrine  Falcon  (Falco  peregrinus)  swoop  down  on  two  Roseate 
Spoonbills  {Ajaia  ajaja)  which  were  foraging  about  a meter  apart  in  an  impoundment  on  the 
Merritt  Island  National  Wildlife  Refuge,  Brevard  Co.,  Florida.  My  watch  began  at  the  im- 
poundment at  08:00,  and  the  falcon  was  noted  in  a dead  white  mangrove  {Laguncularia 
racemosa)  at  09:20.  At  10:04  the  falcon  left  the  tree  and  headed  directly  toward  the  spoonbills, 
which  were  20  m away,  in  a gliding-flapping  flight.  As  the  falcon  approached  the  spoonbills, 
they  stopped  feeding,  stood  erect,  faced  the  fcdcon  and  flashed  their  wings.  The  wing  flash 
consisted  of  opening  the  wings  to  the  wrist  and  allowing  the  remainder  of  the  wing  to  droop 
with  the  primaries  near  the  body.  .After  the  wing  flashes,  the  falcon  turned  abruptly  and 
landed  in  a nearby  mangrove  tree.  .Approximately  10  min  later,  the  foraging  spoonbills  were 
10  m apart  when  the  falcon  swooped  down  on  one  bird.  The  reaction  of  the  spoonbill  was 
the  same.  .Although  both  spoonbills  continued  to  feed  for  an  additional  10  min  before  de- 
parting, they  continuously  watched  the  falcon  which  remained  in  the  area  for  about  an  hour. 

The  Roseate  Spoonbill  is  a relatively  large  bird  to  be  taken  by  a Peregrine  Falcon,  and 
this  episode  may  have  been  a “mock  attack”  or  play.  However,  it  is  possible  the  swoops 
were  an  attempt  by  the  falcon  to  flush  the  spoonbills  so  it  could  take  one.  George  (Raptor 
Res.  13:88-90,  1979)  obser\ed  an  immature  Peregrine  Falcon  strike  a Snow  (Joose  {Chen 
caerulescens)  and  Cade  (Univ.  Calif.  Publ.  Zool.  63:151-267,  1%1)  found  a Peregrine  could 
take  a 1400  g Black  Brant  (Branta  bernicla)  and  a 1300  g Canvasback  {Ay'thya  valisineria). 
Palmer  (Handbook  of  North  .American  Birds,  Vol.  1,  Yale  Univ.  Press,  New  Haven.  Con- 
necticut, 1%2)  states  the  Roseate  Spoonbill  weighs  up  to  1600  g,  and  one  immature  bird 
weighed  1169  g.  Because  no  other  flight  intention  movement  was  observed,  and  the  posture 
of  the  spoonbills  was  different  from  a high  intensity  threat  display  (body  axis  parallel  to  the 
ground,  wings  held  above  the  body  and  neck  outstretched  [pers.  obs.]),  it  is  possible  the 
wing  flashes  gave  the  falcon  information  on  the  size  of  the  birds  or  may  have  served  to 
increase  their  effective  size.  Cade  (1%1)  observed  a similar  behavior  in  a molting  Canada 
(Joose  to  ward  off  a Peregrine  Falcon. 

I thank  Peter  Wrege  for  comments  on  this  note. — E.  ScOTT  Cl.ark,  Merritt  Island  Na- 
tional Wildlife  Refuge,  Titusville,  Florida  32780.  (Present  address:  LMNPD-RE,  US  Army 
Corps  of  Engeneers,  P.O.  Box  60267,  New  Orleans,  Louisiana  70160.)  .Accepted  29  .Aug.  1980. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


549 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  p.  549 


Symbiotic  interaction  between  Starlings  and  deer. — The  symbiotic  relationship  be- 
tween oxpeckers  (Buphagus  spp.)  and  large  African  mammals  is  well  documented  (Rice, 
Auk  80:196-197,  1963).  A few  North  American  birds  have  been  observed  eating  ectoparasites 
on  large  mammals.  Most  of  these  associations  involve  ungulates  and  corvids  (Dixon,  Condor 
46:204,  1944;  Rice  and  Mockford,  Wilson  Bull.  66:272-273,  1954).  A recent  note  describes 
interactions  between  Scrub  Jays  {Aphelocoma  coerulescens)  and  feral  hogs  {Sus  scrofa)  (Baber 
and  Morris,  Auk  97:202,  1980).  I observed  two  similar  interactions  between  Starlings  (Sturnus 
vulgaris)  and  white-tailed  deer  {Odocoileus  virginianus)  in  central  Wisconsin  where  Starlings 
commonly  feed  on  insects  flushed  by  grazing  cattle.  Observations  were  made  with  a 15  X 
60  spotting  scope. 

On  8 July  1979,  at  20:50  CST,  I saw  an  adult  female  deer  walking  through  a grass-shrub 
area;  an  adult  Starling  was  perched  on  her  nose.  The  bird  moved  up  to  the  crown  of  the 
deer’s  head,  down  the  neck  and  back  and  returned  to  the  head,  ostensibly  probing  for 
ectoparasites;  the  deer  showed  no  reaction.  The  observation  lasted  10  min  while  the  deer 
moved  over  200  m and  then  out  of  view. 

On  16  July  1979,  at  09:15  CST,  I saw  an  adult  Starhng  on  the  head  of  an  adult  deer  of 
unknown  sex.  The  deer  was  on  a little-used  road  which  bisected  a pastured  area  interspersed 
with  oak  {Quercus  spp.)  woodlots.  The  deer  was  visible  for  only  15  sec  before  it  disappeared 
into  cover  and  was  apparently  oblivious  to  the  presence  of  the  Starling.  Riney  (Condor 
53:178-185,  1957)  noted  similar  complacency  in  Scrub  Jay-mule  deer  (O.  hemionus)  inter- 
actions. That  advanced  feeding  behavior  is  extensive  in  another  sturnid,  the  oxpecker,  sug- 
gests that  family-related  learning  traits  may  be  developing  within  local  Starling  social  groups 
as  Baber  and  Morris  (1980)  speculated  for  Florida  Scrub  Jays. 

I wish  to  thank  Raymond  K.  Anderson  for  helpful  comments  on  this  manuscript. — Robert 
K.  Murphy,  College  of  Natural  Resources,  Univ.  Wisconsin  at  Stevens  Point,  Stevens  Point, 
Wisconsin  54481.  Accepted  10  Oct.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  549-550 


Cattle  Egrets  feeding  in  association  with  human  workers. — The  foraging  strategy 
of  Cattle  Egrets  {Bubulcus  ibis)  in  attendance  of  grazing  cattle  is  well-known.  Their  associ- 
ation of  a commensalistic  nature,  with  domestic  and  wild  ungulates  is  well-documented 
(Heatwole,  Anim.  Behav.  13:79-83,  1965;  Ali  and  Ripley,  Handbook  of  Birds  of  India  and 
Pakistan,  Vol.  1,  1968;  Jenni,  Ecol.  Monogr.  39:245-270,  1969).  Cattle  Egrets  also  use  human 
activity  to  their  advantage  in  so  far  as  following  plows,  tractors,  vehicles,  etc.  for  the  purpose 
of  feeding.  However,  to  the  best  of  my  knowledge,  there  are  no  reports  of  Cattle  Egrets 
associating  with  human  beings  on  foot,  for  food  procurement  in  the  field.  The  present  note 
is  a report  on  such  findings. 

During  the  past  few  years  of  bird  watching,  I often  visited  a large  farmland  area,  part  of 
which  is  swampy,  spanning  about  300  ha  along  the  Dabhoi  Road,  approximately  10  km  from 
Baroda  (73°13'E,  22°18'N),  Gujrat  State,  India.  This  farmland,  irrigated  with  sewage  water 
from  the  sewage  treatment  plant  of  Baroda  City  Corporation,  is  the  favorite  haunt  of  a large 
number  of  migratory  and  resident  birds,  including  a large  population  of  Cattle  Egrets.  Much 
of  the  area  is  covered  with  native  grasses  harvested  for  use  as  cattle  fodder.  Many  laborers 
make  a living  nearly  year-round  manually  cutting  grass  with  sickles.  Small  groups  of  egrets 
associate  with  the  laborers,  capturing  insects  flushed  during  harvesting  operations. 


550 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


On  commencement  of  harvesting,  the  egrets  walk  right  up  to  the  laborers  with  apparent 
confidence.  As  the  workers  advance  in  the  field,  the  egrets  also  keep  pace,  remaining  within 
0.5-1  m of  the  humans.  However,  the  egrets  have  not  been  observed  picking  insects  off  the 
laborers’  legs,  as  they  commonly  do  with  cattle.  At  times.  Pond  Herons  {Ardeola  greyii)  also 
feed  with  the  Cattle  Egrets  in  a similar  manner.  It  has  yet  to  be  ascertained  whether  the 
egrets  are  following  humans  preferentially.  Many  egrets  are  seen  feeding  by  themselves  in 
the  field  and  along  the  flowing  water  channels.  Small  groups  of  egrets  also  foUow  the  few 
water  buffalo  {Bubalus  bubalis)  grazing  at  this  locality. 

The  commensal  feeding  of  A.  greyii  with  the  Cattle  Egrets  using  human  workers  as  ‘beat- 
ers’ is  noteworthy.  Instances  of  Squacco  Herons  {A.  ralloides)  feeding  gregariously  among 
cattle,  as  do  Cattle  Egrets,  are  known  (Cramp  and  Simmons,  The  Birds  of  the  Western 
Palearctic,  Vol.  1,  1977).  Furthermore,  the  behavior  of  A.  greyii  and  A.  ralloides  is  consid- 
ered to  be  quite  similar  (Cramp  and  Simmons  1977).  There  is  considerable  discussion  as  to 
the  taxonomic  relationship  of  the  Cattle  Egret,  and  Payne  and  Risley  (Misc.  BuU.  Mus.  Zool., 
Univ.  Michigan,  No.  150,  1976)  concluded  that  B.  ibis  is  not  closely  related  to  A.  greyii. 
Thus,  additional  information  on  commensalistic  feeding  behavior  of  species  of  Ardeola  could 
facilitate  further  understanding  of  the  taxonomic  relations  of  these  herons. 

The  critical  comments  on  the  note  and  highly  useful  suggestions  from  J.  Kushlan  and  D. 
Mock  are  gratefully  acknowledged. — G.  K.  Menon,  Dept.  Zool.,  M.  S.  Univ.,  Baroda,  India. 
Accepted  10  Sept.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  550-551 


Scrub  Jay  captures  Hermit  Thrush  in  flight. — The  opportunism  demonstrated  by 
many  corvids  in  obtaining  food  has  been  well  documented  (Bent,  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull.  191, 
1946;  Goodwin,  Crows  of  the  World,  Comstock7CorneU  Univ.  Press,  Ithaca,  New  York,  1976; 
Coombs,  The  Crows:  a study  of  the  corvids  of  Europe,  B.  T.  Batsford  Ltd.,  London,  England, 
1978).  Predatory  behavior  by  corvids  is  not  unusual,  but  prey  rarely  includes  birds  in  flight. 
Use  of  the  feet  to  seize  flying  birds  has  been  reported  for  several  species  of  Corvus  (see 
Coombs  1978  for  review;  Heathcote,  Br.  Birds  71:134-135,  1978)  and  at  least  once  for  jays 
(Carothers  et  al.,  Wilson  Bull.  84:204,  1972).  A Magpie  {Pica  pica)  presumably  used  its  wings 
to  “beat”  a Swift  {Apus  apus)  to  the  ground  (Pulman,  Br.  Birds  71:363,  1978).  Our  report 
concerns  a Scrub  Jay  {Aphelocoma  coerulescens)  which  used  its  biU  to  capture  a Hermit 
Thrush  (Hylocichla  guttata)  in  flight. 

The  incident  occurred  at  12:30  on  28  September  1979  on  the  campus  of  the  University  of 
California  at  Davis.  A Hermit  Thrush  flew  from  beneath  a hedge  to  a sunlit  area  where  flying 
insects  were  visible.  The  bird  hovered,  apparently  attempting  to  capture  the  insects.  Seconds 
later,  a Scrub  Jay  flew  from  the  same  hedge  and  attacked  the  hovering  thrush.  After  a few 
seconds  of  struggling  and  a short  pursuit,  the  jay  managed  to  grasp  the  thrush  by  the  neck. 
Still  flying,  the  jay  carried  the  thrush  in  its  biU  to  a branch  in  a nearby  tree.  The  jay  placed 
its  foot  on  the  thrush,  released  the  bird’s  neck  and  struck  the  thrush’s  head  with  two  rapid 
strokes  of  its  biU.  The  thrush,  which  had  been  screaming  distress  caUs  since  its  capture, 
fluttered  briefly  and  became  silent.  The  jay  then  began  plucking  feathers  from  the  thrush’s 
back. 

In  order  to  examine  the  dead  thrush,  we  frightened  the  jay  from  its  prey.  The  Hermit 
Thrush  had  a single  hole  in  the  right  side  of  its  head,  just  behind  the  eye.  Hemorrhaging 
was  evident  on  the  right  side  of  the  neck.  Remiges  of  the  left  wing  and  contour  feathers  of 
the  back  and  neck  had  been  removed,  but  no  flesh  had  been  torn.  Internal  examination 


GENERAL  NOTES 


551 


revealed  that  the  thrush  was  an  immature  female.  Insect  remains,  almost  exclusively  elytra 
of  unidentified  beetles,  were  present  in  the  gizzard. 

Past  sightings  of  jays  killing  birds  capable  of  flight  did  not  involve  prey  as  large  as  the 
Hermit  Thrush.  Blue  Jays  {Cyanocitta  cristata)  killed  a Purple  Finch  {Carpodacus  purpureas) 
(Downs,  Bird-Banding  29:244,  1958),  a YeUow-rumped  Warbler  {Dendroica  coronata)  (John- 
son and  Johnson,  Wilson  BuU.  88:509,  1976)  and  a House  Sparrow  {Passer  domesticus)  (Mas- 
ter, Wilson  BuU.  91:470,  1979);  a Mexican  Jay  {A.  ultramarina)  caught  an  unidentified  spar- 
row (Roth,  Condor  73:113,  1971);  and  SteUer’s  Jays  (C.  stelleri)  kiUed  Gray-headed  Juncos 
iJunco  caniceps)  and  a Pygmy  Nuthatch  {Sitta  pygmaea)  (Carothers  et  al.  1972).  Only  the 
Pygmy  Nuthatch  was  flying  when  captured.  The  Scrub  Jay’s  method  of  holding  food  items 
with  its  feet  when  perched  was  typical  of  corvids  (Bent  1946,  Goodwin  1976),  as  was  kiUing 
a vertebrate  by  striking  repeated  blows  to  the  prey’s  head  near  the  eye  (Bent  1946;  Mac- 
Cracken,  Auk  66:210,  1949;  Bateman  and  Baida,  Auk  90:39-61,  1973;  Maser,  Wilson  BuU. 
87:552,  1975;  Mulder  et  al..  Condor  80:449-451,  1978). 

We  thank  D.  W.  Anderson,  C.  Ely,  B.  J.  Gray  and  D.  G.  Raveling  for  comments  on  the 
manuscript. — M.  Robert  McLandress,  Div.  Wildlife  and  Fisheries  Biology,  Univ.  Cali- 
fornia at  Davis,  Davis,  California  95616  AND  iLSE  McLandress,  RIM  Ecology  Ltd.,  203- 
225  Vaughan  St.,  Winnipeg,  Manitoba  R3C  1T7  Canada.  Accepted  10  Sept.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  551-554 


Food  habits  of  Black-bellied  Whistling  Ducks  occupying  rice  culture  habitats. — 

Apart  from  a few  anecdotal  reports  (Bent,  Life  Histories  of  North  American  Wildlife,  U.S. 
Natl.  Mus.  BuU.  130,  1925;  Cleare,  Birds,  The  Argosy  Co.,  Georgetown,  Guyana,  1938; 
Haverschmidt,  Field  Notes  on  the  Black-beUied  Tree  Duck  in  Dutch  Guyana,  Wilson  BuU. 
59:209,  1947;  Giglioli,  Crop  Histories  and  Field  Investigations,  1951-1957,  Br.  Guiana  Rice 
Development  Co.,  Georgetown,  Guyana,  1959;  Haverschmidt,  Birds  of  Surinam,  Livingston 
PubUshing  Co.,  Wynne  wood,  Pennsylvania,  1968)  and  two  studies  in  southern  Texas  (Bolen  and 
Forsyth,  Foods  of  the  Black-beUied  Tree  Duck  in  south  Texas,  Wilson  BuU.  79:43^9,  1967; 
Bolen  and  Beecham,  Notes  on  the  foods  of  juvenile  Black-beUied  Tree  Ducks,  Wilson  BuU. 
82:325-326,  1970),  the  food  habits  of  Black-beUied  Whistling  Ducks  {Dendrocygna  autum- 
nalis)  have  been  little  studied.  In  1973,  I initiated  an  ecological  study  of  Black-beUied  Whis- 
tling Ducks  to  evaluate  the  magnitude  of  their  foraging  activity  in  ricefields.  Preliminary 
results  (Bourne  and  Osborne,  Black-beUied  Whistling  Duck  utilization  of  a rice  culture  hab- 
itat, Intercencia  3:152-159,  1978)  indicate  that  overaU  depredation  levels  are  low  even  though 
the  ducks  ingest  newly  sown,  pregerminated  paddy  or  seed  rice  {Oryza  sativa).  The  purpose 
of  this  paper  is  to  present  data  on  the  food  habits  of  Black-beUied  WhistUng  Ducks  in  Guyana, 
South  America,  when  they  occupied  rice  culture  habitats  during  crop  sowing  in  June  1973 
and  July-August  1974. 

Materials  and  methods. — I conducted  fieldwork  in  Burma  (6°28'N,  57°45'W)  at  the  Mahai- 
cony  and  Abary  Rice  Development  Scheme  (MARDS).  Detailed  descriptions  of  the  study 
area  and  its  flora  and  fauna  are  available  in  GigUoli  (1959)  and  Osborne  and  Bourne  (Breeding 
behavior  and  food  habits  of  the  Wattled  Jacana,  Condor  79:98-105,  1977).  In  1973,  two 
methods  were  used  for  obtaining  specimens:  15  ducks  were  shot  between  05:00  and  07:16 
with  the  aid  of  playback  vocalizations  and  15  were  mist-netted  between  20:00  and  20:55. 
Adults  and  juveniles  coUected  in  1974  were  shot  between  05:00  and  07:16  with  the 
aid  of  playback  vocalizations.  Two  ducklings  were  hand  caught  in  a faUow  field  on 
8 August  1974,  at  08:10  and  08:25.  Specimens  were  dissected  within  30  min  and  the  entire 


552 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


alimentary  tract  of  each  duck  was  placed  in  a separate  large-mouth  bottle  containing  10% 
formalin.  The  contents  of  the  esophagus,  proventriculus  and  ventriculus  were  identified  to 
the  lowest  taxon  possible,  and  their  volumes  were  determined  by  water  displacement. 

Results  1973. — Food  items  identified  from  30  adult  Black-bellied  Whistling  Ducks  (15 
males,  15  females)  indicate  that  plant  material  accounted  for  97%  of  the  food  consumed 
(Table  1).  Most  (86%)  of  this  plant  food  consisted  of  pregerminated  paddy.  Other  plant  foods 
accounted  for  11%  of  the  ducks’  diet  while  animal  foods  made  up  only  3%  of  the  birds’  fare 
(Table  1).  Most  of  the  animals  consumed  were  invertebrates;  the  majority  were  aquatic 
insects  and  snails  (Table  1).  Two  ducks  (7%)  consumed  young  tadpoles  of  the  marine  toad 
{Bufo  marinus).  Of  the  30  ducks  examined  for  their  food  preferences,  63%  ingested  animal 
foods;  37%  of  these  were  males  and  63%  were  females. 

1974. — Plant  foods  accounted  for  90%  of  the  adults’  and  juveniles’  diets  (Table  1).  Paddy 
dominated  the  adults’  diet,  constituting  74%,  but  was  less  important  in  the  juveniles’  diet, 
accounting  for  15%  of  their  food  (Table  1).  In  1974,  adults  consumed  the  same  genera  and 
species  as  the  adults  did  in  1973  (Table  1),  but  juveniles  had  eaten  the  seeds  of  a grass, 
Paspalum  sp.,  which  accounted  for  71%  of  their  diet  and  occurred  in  95%  of  the  juveniles 
sampled  (Table  1).  Young  apple  snails  {Pomacea  sp.)  were  the  most  important  animal  foods 
consumed  by  juveniles  as  they  accounted  for  8.5%  of  their  diet,  and  occurred  in  70%  of  the 
sample  (Table  1). 

Two  ducklings  analyzed  for  their  food  preferences  consumed  54%  animal  foods  and  46% 
plant  foods.  The  plant  foods  consisted  of  the  seeds  of  a millet,  Echinochloa  sp.  and  Paspalum 
sp.;  they  accounted  for  31%  and  15%  of  the  diet,  respectively.  Shorefly  (Scatella  stagnalis) 
larvae  and  pupae  were  found  in  trace  amounts,  but  the  bulk  of  the  animal  food  consisted  of 
unidentified  terrestrial  spiders. 

Discussion. — Black-bellied  Whistling  Ducks,  like  other  dendrocygnids,  are  basically  her- 
bivorous (Johnsgard,  Waterfowl  of  North  America,  Indiana  Univ.  Press,  Bloomington,  In- 
diana, 1975).  But  at  sowing  time,  cultivated  cereals  dominate  the  plant  food  preferences  in 
Black-bellied  Whistling  Ducks’  diets.  For  example,  corn  {Zea  mays)  was  the  most  important 
constituent  in  the  species’  diet  in  Mexico  (Bent  1925),  and  Sorgum  vulgare  constituted  48% 
of  the  species’  diet  in  south  Texas  (Bolen  and  Forsyth  1967).  Paddy  accounted  for  86%  and 
74%  of  the  adult  Black-bellied  Whisthng  Duck’s  diet  in  Guyana  during  the  early  rice  growing 
season.  Observations  suggest  that  paddy  would  become  less  important  in  the  duck’s  diet  as 
the  growing  season  progressed  because  fewer  suitable  water-planted  ricefields  would  be 
available  as  foraging  sites.  This  may  explain  why  paddy  accounted  for  only  15%  of  the 
juveniles’  diet,  since  they  were  collected  after  the  adults  were  from  22  July-9  August  1974, 
when  planting  was  almost  completed. 

Animal  foods  do  not  appear  to  be  important  in  adult  and  juvenile  Black-beUied  Whisthng 
Ducks’  diets.  Even  though  adults  were  in  breeding  condition  (as  evidenced  by  gonadal  mea- 
surements, males,  N = 18;  left  testes  mean  24  X 12  mm  [10  X 5-32  X 16  mml;  females, 
N = 17,  largest  follicle  mean  40  mm  [3-56  mm]),  they  only  consumed  3%  animal  food  in 
1973  and  10%  in  1974,  while  juveniles  also  ingested  10%  animal  food  in  1974.  However, 
these  data  could  be  biased  downwards  due  to  the  faster  digestion  of  soft-bodied  invertebrates 
in  the  proventriculus  and  ventriculus.  Ducklings  consumed  54%  animal  foods  in  this  study, 
suggesting  that  younger  whistling  ducks  need  the  higher  protein  content  found  in  animal 
foods  for  growth  and  development. 

Acknowledgments. — Financial  support  of  this  investigation  was  provided  by  the  Frank  M. 
Chapman  Memorial  Fund,  the  Rob  and  Bessie  Welder  Wildlife  Foundation,  and  Mr.  Fred- 
erick N.  Stevens.  I thank  the  following  for  contributing  ideas  and  criticisms:  David  R. 
Osborne,  W.  Hardy  Eshbaugh,  Gary  W.  Barrett,  Michael  P.  Farrell  and  Fay  M.  Edwards. 
I am  particularly  indebted  to  Charles  P.  Kennard,  George  Hughes  and  their  staffs  of  the 
Guyana  Rice  Board  for  providing  facilities  at  MARDS.  Finally,  I am  thankful  to  Dick  L. 


Volume,  Percent  and  Frequency  of  Food  Items  in  the  Diet  of  30  Adult  (8-19  June  1973),  5 Adult  (16-17  July  1974)  and  20 
Juvenile  (22  July-9  August  1974)  Black-bellied  Whistling  Ducks  at  Burma,  Guyana 


GENERAL  NOTES 


553 


I I 
I I 


2^  I 


- I 


I I 


CO  (N  O 

o o’  csi 


CM  Tf  \0 


I s 


lO  LO  O 

CM  O O 


LO  O LO 

vd  00  fo 
Tj<  ^ r- 
CM  CM 


I I 


O CM  O CO 


CO  O O CO 


O O 

CM  CM 


o o 

CM  o’ 


O lO  lO  lO 
cd  vd 
LO 


LO  LO 

l-H 


CO  CO  CD 

co’  O g I 


O lO  lO 


og  I 


^ r-H  CM  LO 


CO 

O CM 


lO  o LO  o 

o CM  o’ 

--H  CM  ■— I 
CO 


LO  LO 

d ^ 


o o 
d 


dj  -o 

Tfi  « 

3 -C  'y  ^ tfi 

O 3 t«  "C 

<D  -C  d> 

- V <D 

" ~ d 


^ 
c 

5 S § 

O <D  o 


cv  -c: 
V5  ^ o 

3 C o 

_ _ o 


a 

S ^ 1 

o S - 

e 


• 2 

•d  a 
a 3 
c Q. 

^ -T- 

cO 


E S 

a CO 


O CO  O a,  H 


^0  5; 


e 5 ^ 

- S -g  s I 

§ I :s  f :! 

I =2  ^ I 

:§  < H 


554 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Deonier  for  identifying  insects,  to  my  wife  Carol,  Patrick  Dougal  and  the  Green  brothers  for 
assistance  in  the  field.  Data  are  taken  from  a M.Sc.  thesis  submitted  to  Miami  University. 
This  is  Welder  Contribution  195. — Godfrey  R.  Bourne,  Institute  of  Environmental  Sci- 
ences, Miami  Unit.,  Oxford,  Ohio  45056.  (Present  address:  School  of  Natural  Resources, 
Univ.  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.)  Accepted  3 Nov.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  p.  554 


House  Sparrows  flushing  prey  from  trees  and  shrubs. — House  Sparrows  {Passer 
domesticus)  exhibit  much  foraging  adaptabihty  (Potter,  Condor  33:30,  1931;  Bent,  Life 
Histories  of  North  American  blackbirds,  orioles,  tanagers,  and  aUies,  U.S.  Natl.  Mus.  Bull. 
211,  1958;  Summers-Smith,  The  House  Sparrow,  Collins,  London,  England,  1%3;  Marti, 
Wilson  Bull.  85:483,  1973).  Guillory  at  Eunice,  St.  Landry  Parish,  Louisiana,  on  22  August 
1976,  and  Deshotels  at  Kaplan,  Vermihon  Parish,  on  13  September  1977,  each  observed  a 
House  Sparrow  displaying  previously  unreported  foraging  behaviors. 

In  Eunice,  a female  was  seen  searching  in  a loose  30  X 46  cm  cluster  of  dry  southern  red 
oak  {Quercus  falcata)  twigs  and  leaves  located  on  peripheral  branches  approximately  6 m 
above  ground.  The  bird  shook  the  leaf  cluster  by  momentarily  grasping  a twig  with  her  feet 
and  vigorously  flapping  her  wings.  The  bird  repeated  this  while  hopping  from  twig  to  twig 
in  the  cluster.  The  bird  flushed  an  unidentified  white  moth  (Lepidoptera)  (2.5  cm),  captured 
it  in  flight  and  fed  it  to  one  of  her  nestlings.  She  returned  to  the  same  cluster  and  twice 
repeated  the  above  actions,  catching  two  more  white  moths  of  similar  size  and  fed  them  to 
her  nestlings.  The  bird  returned  to  the  cluster,  probed  among  the  leaves  and  caught  a brown 
moth  (2.5  cm). 

In  Kaplan,  a male  House  Sparrow  was  seen  flushing  beetles  (Coleoptera)  and  white  moths 
from  a densely  vegetated,  flat-topped  hedgerow  ca.  30  cm  high.  Prey  were  flushed  from  the 
top  of  the  hedgerow  by  hopping  and  wing  flapping  similar  to  that  of  the  aforementioned 
female.  The  bird  stopped  occasionally  and  probed  among  the  leaves  and  branches,  presum- 
ably for  insects.  The  bird  hovered  near  moving  insects,  apparently  attempting  to  flush  them. 
Prey  leaving  the  shrubbery  was  captured  in  flight  or  on  a nearby  sidewalk,  crushed  on 
the  concrete,  and  then  consumed. 

These  behaviors  are  further  examples  of  opportunistic  foraging  by  House  Sparrows. 

We  wish  to  thank  Dwight  J.  LeBlanc  for  his  helpful  criticisms  of  the  manuscript. — Har- 
LA\D  D.  Guillory,  Div.  Sciences,  Louisiana  State  Univ.  at  Eunice,  Eunice,  Louisiana  70535 
AND  Jack  H.  Deshotels,  Lot  4,  Azalia  Drive,  Youngsville,  Louisiana  70592.  Accepted  6 
Oct.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  554-556 


Differential  predation  by  two  species  of  piscivorous  birds. — The  piscivorous  Dou- 
ble-crested Cormorant  {Phalacrocorax  auritus)  and  White  Pelican  {Pelecanus  erythrorhyn- 
chos)  use  distinctly  different  foraging  techniques  (Palmer,  Handbook  of  North  American 
Birds,  Vol.  1,  Yale  Univ.  Press,  New  Haven,  Connecticut,  1962).  Cormorants  dive  to  depths 
of  20  m and  pursue  fish.  Pelicans  scoop  fish  “dip-net  fashion”  in  water  to  depths  of  1 m. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


555 


LENGTH  OF  TUI  CHUB  (mm) 

Fig.  1.  Percentages  of  tui  chubs  of  different  standard  lengths  observed  in  the  diets  of 
White  Pelicans  (solid  line)  and  Double-crested  Cormorants  (dashed  line)  at  Pyramid  Lake, 
Nevada. 


Despite  these  differences,  the  species  often  have  similar  diets  where  sympatric  (Behle, 
The  Bird  Life  of  Great  Salt  Lake,  Univ.  Utah  Press,  Salt  Lake  City,  Utah,  1958). 
Such  dietary  overlap  is  pronounced  at  Pyramid  Lake,  Nevada,  where  the  birds  feed 
heavily  upon  an  indigenous  population  of  tui  chub  {Gila  bicolor)  which  spawns  in  shallow 
(<1  m)  water  from  about  1 June-15  August  (Kucera,  Great  Basin  Nat.  38:203-207,  1978), 
during  the  nestling  and  pre-fledging  phases  of  cormorant  and  pelican  chick  development 
(Hall,  Condor  27:127-160,  1925;  Hall,  Condor  28:87-91,  1926).  Cormorants  feed  at  the  lake 
throughout  this  period,  whereas  pelicans  forage  there  only  during  June  (Knopf  and  Kennedy, 
Western  Birds  11:175-180,  1980).  In  this  study  we  assessed  characteristics  of  G.  bicolor 
preyed  upon  by  the  two  bird  species  at  Pyramid  Lake. 

Chicks  of  cormorants  and  pelicans  readily  regurgitate  fish  if  disturbed  shortly  after  they 
are  fed.  We  collected  fish  regurgitated  in  this  manner  at  nesting  sites  on  Anaho  Island 
National  Wildlife  Refuge,  Washoe  Co.,  Nevada  on  30  June  1976,  within  30  min  after  chicks 
were  fed.  We  weighed  and  measured  the  fish  within  1 h of  collection. 

We  collected  94  G.  bicolor,  totalling  10,625  g,  regurgitated  by  cormorant  chicks  and  236 
fish,  totalling  8681  g,  regurgitated  by  pelican  chicks.  Sixty-four  and  37  of  these  fish,  respec- 
tively, were  intact  and  could  be  measured  (Fig.  1).  The  distance  from  the  snout  to  end  of  the 
vertebral  column  (standard  length)  of  G.  bicolor  regurgitated  by  cormorant  chicks  averaged 
171.6  mm  (SD  ± 14.3  mm)  while  that  of  fish  from  pelican  chicks  averaged  110.1  mm  (SD  ± 
29.7  mm).  This  difference  is  statistically  significant  {t  adjusted  = 9.22,  P < 0.001).  The 
variance  in  length  of  G.  bicolor  collected  from  cormorants  and  pelicans  was  tested  (Lewontin, 
Syst.  Zool.  15:141-143,  1966)  and  also  found  to  be  significantly  different  (F  = 10.47,  P < 


556 


THE  ILSON  BULLETIN  * Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


0.001).  Since  the  mean  standard  length  of  fish  regurgitated  by  cormorants  is  greater  than 
that  of  pehcans,  this  test  is  conservative. 

All  fish  collected  from  cormorant  chicks  were  G.  bicolor.  In  contrast,  of  the  344  fish 
collected  from  pelican  chicks,  G.  bicolor  comprised  only  39.5%  (by  weight).  Carp  {Cyprinus 
carpio)  was  the  predominant  fish  in  the  pelican  diet  (58.6%  by  weight).  White  crappie  {Po- 
moxis  annularis),  Tahoe  sucker  {Catostomus  tahoensis),  Sacramento  perch  {Archoplites  in- 
terruptus)  and  brown  bullhead  (Ictalurus  nebulosus)  comprised  only  1.9%  of  the  diet  by 
weight.  Carp,  plus  the  other  fishes  in  the  pelican  diet,  are  rare  or  do  not  occur  in  Pyramid 
Lake,  and  these  species  were  captured  by  pelicans  foraging  in  outlying  wetlands. 

Prey  size  (Storer,  Auk  83:423-436,  1966;  Ashmole,  Syst.  Zool.  17:292-304,  1968)  and 
variation  in  prey  size  (MacArthur,  Geographical  Ecology,  Harper  and  Row,  New  York,  New 
York,  1972;  Reynolds,  Foods  and  habitat  partitioning  in  two  groups  of  coexisting  Accipiter, 
Ph.D.  diss.,  Oregon  State  Univ.,  Corvallis,  Oregon,  1979)  tend  to  increase  with  predator 
size.  Pelicans  often  take  carp  up  to  68  cm  (Hall  1925),  thus  supporting  those  studies.  Our 
data  on  G.  bicolor  are  not  comparable,  however,  since  they  represent  only  a portion  of  the 
pelican  diet. 

Cormorants  and  pelicans  exploited  the  G.  bicolor  population  differently,  apparently  rela- 
tive to  their  respective  foraging  techniques.  Prior  to  spawning,  G.  bicolor  forms  large  swarm- 
ing schools  at  the  lake’s  surface.  Both  bird  species  are  attracted  to  the  schools  where  they 
forage  simultaneously.  Cormorants  dive  and  select  only  the  larger  chubs  from  schools,  pre- 
sumably offsetting  the  greater  energetic  costs  of  underwater  pursuit.  Pelicans  remain  at  the 
surface  and  take  available  fish,  capturing  many  smaller  fish  but  with  less  effort. 

The  diet  of  cormorants  at  Pyramid  Lake  is  hkely  opportunistic  in  that  they  cannot  fly 
efficiently  to  outlying  wetlands  to  forage,  and  must  forage  from  the  predominantly  chub  fish 
community.  However,  the  cormorants’  species-  and  size-specific  diet  is  atypical  relative  to 
its  food  habits  in  other  regions  of  North  America  (Robertson,  Condor  76:346-348,  1974). 
Pelicans,  also  opportunistic,  often  fly  great  distances  from  nests  to  feed  (Low  et  al..  Auk 
67:345-356,  1950;  Lingle  and  Sloan,  Wilson  BuU.  92:123-125,  1980)  and  probably  nest  on 
Anaho  Island  since  no  other  suitable  islands  for  nesting  occur  in  the  area.  The  pelican 
exploits  the  large  G.  bicolor  population  when  available,  but  does  not  demonstrate  the  reliance 
of  the  cormorant  upon  that  fish  species. 

Piscivorous  bird  species  reduce  competition  for  food  where  they  coexist  by  foraging  on 
different  sizes  of  fish,  at  different  distances  from  nests,  or  by  having  non-overlapping  breed- 
ing seasons  (Cody,  Ecology  54:31— 44,  1973).  We  are  uncertain  whether  cormorants  and 
pehcans  compete  for  G.  bicolor.  The  potential  for  competition  is  high,  since  of  the  five  fish 
species  in  Pyramid  Lake,  G.  bicolor  comprises  86%  of  all  fish  (by  numbers)  available  to 
cormorants  in  water  0-15  m deep  (Vigg,  Calif.  Fish  and  Game  66:49-58,  1980)  and  virtually 
all  fish  available  to  pelicans  in  water  0-1  m deep.  The  cormorant  population  appeared  below 
the  area’s  carrying  capacity  since  cormorants  historically  nested  also  on  rocky  pinnacles 
jutting  from  the  north  end  of  the  lake  (Marshall  and  Giles,  Condor  55:105-116,  1953)  where 
272  nest  structures  were  present,  but  unused,  1976-1977. 

We  thank  W.  F.  Sigler  and  Associates,  Inc.  for  the  opportunity  to  conduct  this  study.  The 
Paiute  Indian  Tribe,  PLITE,  and  the  U.  S.  Fish  and  Wildlife  Service  extended  permission 
to  work  on  Pyramid  Lake  and  Anaho  Island  NW'R.  L.  Howard  and  M.  Barber  of  the  Stillwater 
Wildlife  Management  Area  provided  field  assistance  on  Anaho  Island.  B.  A.  Knopf  prepared 
the  figure.  S.  F.  Fox,  E.  A.  Gluesing  and  P.  A.  Vohs  commented  on  the  manuscript. — 
Fritz  L.  Knopf,  School  of  Biological  Sciences,  Oklahoma  State  Univ.,  Stillwater,  Oklahoma 
740  74  AND  Joseph  L.  Kennedy,  Dept.  Biology,  Western  Montana  College,  Dillon,  Montana 
59725.  (Present  address  FLK:  U.S.  Fish  & Wildlife  Service,  Denver  Wildlife  Research  Center, 
1300  Blue  Spruce  Dr.,  Fort  Collins,  Colorado  80524-2098.)  Accepted  8 Oct.  1980. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


557 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  p.  557 


Red  Phalarope  eating  carrion. — The  Red  Phalarope  {Phalaropus  fulicarius)  eats  nu- 
merous invertebrate  species  and  tiny  fish  outside  the  breeding  season,  and  at  sea  (Bent,  U.S. 
Natl,  Mus.  Bull.  142,  1927;  Niethammer,  1942,  in  Birds  of  the  Soviet  Union,  Vol.  3,  G.  P. 
Dement’ev,  N.  A.  Gladkov  and  E.  P.  Spangenberg,  eds.,  Israel  Prog,  for  Sci.  Transl., 
Jerusalem,  1969).  In  this  note  I describe  the  behavior  of  a Red  Phalarope  eating  carrion, 
which  is  the  first  such  record  for  this  species. 

On  11,  13  and  14  September  1978,  I observed  a Red  Phalarope  in  first  basic  plumage,  at 
the  freshwater  East  Pond  of  Jamaica  Bay  Wildhfe  Refuge,  New  York  City.  Observations 
were  made  from  a distance  of  about  15  m using  7 x 35  binoculars  and  a 15x  spotting  scope. 
The  phalarope  vigorously  picked  and  pulled  at  the  flesh  surrounding  the  ribs  and  sternum 
of  three  different  floating  bird  carcasses,  while  swimming  actively  around  them.  It  spent 
most  of  its  time  at  the  carcasses  of  a Red  Knot  {Calidris  canutus)  and  a Herring  GuU  {Laras 
argentatus).  Both  carcasses  had  the  flesh  exposed  in  the  area  of  the  sternum  and  body  cavity; 
examination  in  the  hand  revealed  no  invertebrates  on  either  carcass  upon  which  the  phal- 
arope might  have  been  feeding.  Several  other  observers  also  saw  the  phalarope  feeding  on 
the  same  carcasses  at  other  times  during  the  same  period. 

Many  other  carcasses  of  shorebirds,  guUs  and  waterfowl  in  various  stages  of  decomposition 
were  present  but  all  were  on  land  and  aU  were  unopened;  the  phalarope  was  never  seen 
visiting  them. 

It  seems  unlikely  that  the  Red  Phalarope  with  its  rather  blunt  biU  could  have  opened  the 
carcasses  by  itself  and  although  the  bird  was  often  observed  on  land  it  may  not  have  fed  on 
carcasses  on  land  because  they  were  unopened.  At  the  end  of  my  observations  very  little 
meat  remained  on  the  carcasses,  and  the  phalarope  was  observed  feeding  in  typical  phalarope 
fashion,  presumably  on  aquatic  invertebrates.  This  observation  suggests  that  Red  Phalarope 
could  feed  facultatively  on  carrion  while  at  sea,  an  aspect  of  their  feeding  ecology  previously 
unreported. 

I thank  Joanna  Burger,  Charles  F.  Leek  and  Sharon  Ann  Brady  for  comments  on  this  note. 
This  research  was  funded  by  a grant  to  J.  Burger  from  the  National  Park  Service. — Wade 
Wander,  Center  for  Coastal  Environmental  Studies,  Rutgers  Univ.,  Piscataway,  New  Jersey 
08854.  (Present  address:  RD3,  Box  270AA,  Somerset,  New  Jersey  08873.)  Accepted  20  Aug. 
1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  557-559 

Re-mating  of  a Lesser  Snow  Goose. — Mate  selection  and  pair  formation  in  most  geese 
occurs  during  winter  and  spring  (Delacour,  Waterfowl  of  the  World,  Vol.  IV,  Country  Life, 
London,  England,  1964).  Direct  observation  of  the  process  in  nature  is  difficult,  but  good 
evidence  of  winter  pairing  is  provided  indirectly  by  banding  studies  (e.g.,  Cooke  et  al..  Auk 
92:493-510,  1975).  Exact  timing  of  pair  formation  presumably  depends  on  reproductive  con- 
dition, availability  of  potential  mates,  previous  pairing  experience  and  perhaps  social  status. 
We  describe  events  in  the  formation  of  a new  bond  by  a female  Lesser  Snow  Goose  (Anser 
caerulescens  caerulescens)  immediately  following  mate  loss  and  discuss  their  timing  and 
significance. 

Observations  were  made  during  May  and  June  1977,  at  La  Perouse  Bay,  Manitoba 
(58°45'N,  94°30' W),  during  the  peak  of  nesting  in  a colony  of  Lesser  Snow  Geese.  The  female 


558 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


involved  nested  35  m from  our  live-in  observation  tower,  and  was  identifiable  by  colored  leg 
bands.  Her  mate  was  shot  on  23  May  1977,  on  the  third  day  of  incubation.  Subsequent 
behavior  and  movements  of  the  female  and  her  new  “mate”  were  monitored  opportunisti- 
cally. In  addition,  during  39  one-h  periods  of  instantaneous  scan  samphng  (Altmann,  Be- 
haviour 49:227-267,  1974)  over  a 19-day  period,  time  spent  in  alert,  feeding,  resting  and 
maintenance  activity  was  recorded. 

General  observations. — The  female  was  sexually  assaulted  by  neighboring  territorial  males 
(Mineau  and  Cooke,  Behaviour  70:280-291,  1979)  and  harrassed  by  non-nesting  pairs  after  her 
mate  was  shot.  In  the  early  morning  of  25  May,  between  32  and  41  h after  the  male’s  removal, 
she  abandoned  the  nest  and  the  eggs  were  preyed  upon.  Pairs  searching  for  nest-sites,  non- 
nesting pairs  and  yearhngs  aU  frequented  the  immediate  vicinity  of  the  nest  between  the 
time  of  the  male’s  death  and  nest  loss.  Thus,  we  do  not  know  when  the  yearling  male  which 
subsequently  formed  a bond  with  the  female  first  visited  the  area.  The  behavior  of  yearlings 
while  on  the  nesting  colony  suggests  that  he  was  present  before  and  during  the  female’s 
harassment.  Although  not  banded,  the  yearling  was  recognizable  by  his  distinctive  plumage. 
We  first  noted  his  presence  near  the  female  at  07:55  on  25  May,  within  hours  of  nest  loss. 

Bond  formation  and  sexual  behavior. — Throughout  25  and  26  May  the  yearling  chased  the 
female.  Both  birds  were  on  the  ground  and  vocalizing  during  the  chases.  The  female  held 
her  wings  partially  open  at  such  times.  The  ground  pursuits  changed  abruptly  into  aerial 
pursuits  lasting  less  than  1 min,  usually  culminating  with  the  birds  landing  together  near  the 
take-off  point.  In  four  of  five  fuUy  observed  sequences,  both  birds  fed  quietly  after  landing; 
in  the  first  sequence  seen,  the  male  returned  after  the  female,  remained  alert  and  fed  later. 
These  “courtship  flights”  were  observed  in  decreasing  frequency  during  the  week  after  the 
first  chase  was  seen.  Prevett  and  MacInnes  (Wildl.  Monogr.  No.  71,  1980)  observed  similar 
behavior  by  snow  geese  and  reported  its  occurrence  during  winter  and  spring. 

By  18:00  on  25  May,  nesting  pairs  and  other  geese  treated  the  female  and  yearling  as  a 
“pair,”  threatening  them  if  they  approached  too  closely  to  nests  or  other  individuals.  The 
new  “pair”  was  not  aggressive  in  these  encounters  at  first  and  usually  fled.  However,  late 
on  26  May,  they  began  responding  jointly  to  threats  by  assuming  threat  postures  and  rushing 
at  other  geese.  We  did  not  observe  the  two  performing  a triumph  ceremony. 

Copulation  is  an  important  pair  bond  reinforcement  behavior  in  waterfowl  (Delacour  1964). 
No  complete  copulations  were  observed  but  the  male  engaged  in  intensive  bouts  of  pre- 
copulatory  display  on  27  and  28  May,  and  8 June.  The  female  ignored  these  displays  except 
on  28  May  when  she  briefly  joined  in  typical  pre-copulatory  dipping.  The  male  first  displayed 
3 days  after  nest  loss,  that  is  7 days  into  the  female’s  “incubation”  stage.  Mineau  (M.Sc. 
thesis.  Queen’s  Univ.,  Kingston,  Ontario,  1978)  noted  that  females  of  established  pairs  ignore 
their  mates’  sexual  advances  soon  after  incubation  begins.  Our  failure  to  observe  copulation 
by  this  pair  probably  reflects  both  the  female’s  physiological  condition  and  the  infrequency 
of  the  event. 

Throughout  the  first  10  days  of  the  association,  the  male  was  more  alert  than  the  female 
(40.0%  vs  23.4%  of  instantaneous  scans)  and  fed  less  (53.8%  vs  70.3%)  (x^  = 9.49,  df  = 1, 
P < 0.01).  The  frequency  of  rest  and  comfort  behaviors  did  not  differ  significantly.  After 
the  first  10  days  the  frequency  of  each  behavior  did  not  differ  between  the  two  birds.  Mapping 
of  the  pair’s  location  at  each  scan  sample  (N  = 269)  showed  that  they  were  within  100  m of 
the  female’s  destroyed  nest  on  75%  of  the  scans.  Distance  from  the  nest  increased  gradually 
over  the  19  days  of  observation.  Neither  goose  was  seen  after  12  June;  this  last  sighting 
coincided  with  a molt  migration  when  most  non-nesting  snow  geese  left  La  Perouse  Bay. 

The  triumph  ceremony  is  usually  accepted  as  an  absolute  criterion  for  existence  of  a pair 
bond  in  geese.  Although  we  did  not  observe  this  display,  the  consistent  close  proximity  over 
3 weeks,  the  coordination  of  behaviors  and  pair-pair  interactions  all  indicated  that  a bond 


GENERAL  NOTES 


559 


of  some  sort  existed.  Because  the  mate  selection  process  in  geese  may  involve  a series  of 
temporary  associations,  the  bond  formed  here  need  not  have  been  permanent.  However, 
Wood  (J.  Wildl.  Manage.  29:237-244,  1965)  reported  that  Canada  Goose  {Branta  canadensis) 
pairs  formed  while  at  least  one  of  the  members  is  immature  (as  in  this  case)  are  as  likely  to 
persist  as  are  those  formed  when  both  members  are  adult.  In  the  spring  of  1978,  this  handed 
female  was  observed  nesting  with  a white  male  80  m from  her  1977  site,  within  the  home 
range  occupied  hy  the  newly  formed  “pair”  in  1977. 

Gooch  (Ph.D.  thesis,  Cornell  Univ.,  Ithaca,  New  York,  1958)  assumed  that  pair  formation 
of  some  snow  geese  began  in  their  yearling  summer;  our  observations  confirm  that  possibility. 
Although  bond  formation  such  as  we  describe  is  likely  to  occur  relatively  infrequently,  it 
runs  counter  to  the  assumption  that  all  pairing  occurs  during  winter  and  spring  (Cooke  and 
Sulzbach,  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  42:271-280,  1978). 

The  reported  length  of  the  period  between  mate  loss  and  re-mating  in  geese  is  variable. 
In  our  case,  a bond  of  some  sort  was  formed  within  48  h.  Re-mating  before  the  next  breeding 
season  is  common  in  snow  geese  (Cooke,  unpubl.).  Prevett  and  Macinnes  (1980)  reported 
that  some  snow  geese  remained  unpaired  in  the  breeding  season  after  mate  loss.  Similar 
variability  in  interval  from  mate  loss  to  re-mating  has  been  reported  for  Canada  Geese  and 
is  likely  true  for  other  species  (Sherwood,  Trans.  N.  Am.  Wildl.  Nat.  Res.  Conf.  32:340— 
355,  1967;  Weigand  et  al.,  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  32:894-905,  1968;  Jones  and  Obbard,  Auk 
87:370-371,  1970).  Much  of  the  variation  can  be  explained  by  difference  in  time  of  mate  loss 
relative  to  the  next  breeding  opportunity  and  the  availability  of  mates.  Although  a succession 
of  temporary  associations  may  usually  be  a part  of  the  pair  formation  process  in  geese 
because  it  allows  optimal  discretion  in  choice  of  mate,  circumstances  may  not  permit.  In 
particular,  loss  of  mate  during  the  nesting  period  characterized  both  cases  where  a new  bond 
was  formed  within  a very  short  time  (Jones  and  Obbard  1970,  this  study).  The  male’s  im- 
portant role  in  protection  of  the  female  and/or  nest  clearly  favored  the  short  interval  (Ewas- 
chuk  and  Boag,  J.  Wildl.  Manage.  36:1097-1106,  1972;  Mineau  and  Cooke,  Wildfowl  30: 
16-19,  1979).  Mate  loss  at  other  times  of  the  year  could  result  in  longer  re-mating  times 
because  both  need  for  a mate  and  availability  of  potential  mates  will  differ. 

Acknowledgments. — Funding  for  the  La  Perouse  Bay  snow  goose  study  was  provided  by 
the  Canadian  Wildlife  Service,  National  Research  Council  of  Canada  and  the  Wildlife  Man- 
agement Institute.  We  thank  M.  A.  Bousfield  for  assistance  in  the  field  and  C.  D.  Ankney 
for  comments  on  an  earlier  draft  of  the  paper. — Kenneth  F.  Abraham,  Pierre  Mineau 
AND  Fred  Cooke,  Dept.  Biology,  Queen  s Univ.,  Kingston,  Ontario  K7L  3N6  Canada.  (Pres- 
ent address:  KFA,  Dept.  Zoology,  Univ.  Western  Ontario,  London,  Ontario  N6A  5B7  Canada; 
PM,  Canadian  Wildlife  Service,  P.O.  Box  5050,  Burlington,  Ontario  L7R  4A6  Canada.) 
Accepted  17  Nov.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  559-560 


Common  Eider  plays  “possum.” — Death  feigning  is  widespread  but  not  extensively 
described  in  animals.  Reports  of  death  feigning  in  birds  are  given  by  Armstrong  (Bird  Display 
and  Behavior,  Dover  Publ.  Co.,  New  York,  New  York,  1965),  Vogel  (Auk  67:210-216,  1950), 
Francq  (Am.  Midi.  Nat.  81:556-568,  1969),  and  others.  Observations  of  invertebrates  and 
vertebrates  indicate  that  it  is  used  only  when  escape  is  otherwise  impossible  and  that  it 
appears  to  be  a stereotyped  response. 

At  13:30  on  17  January  1979,  at  Great  Island,  WeUfleet,  Barnstable  Co.,  Massachusetts, 
I observed  an  ill  adult  female  Common  Eider  {Somateria  mollissima)  feigning  death.  My  dog 


560 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


found  it  on  slush  tidal  ice  8 m from  a salt  marsh  channel  and  28  m from  open  water.  The 
eider  hobbled  weakly  away  and  beat  the  ground  with  its  wings  then  slumped  into  immobihty 
when  the  dog  came  alongside  it.  The  eider’s  head  and  biU  were  depressed  until  the  tip  was 
3 cm  above  the  ground.  The  neck  was  twisted,  body  slumped  forward,  wings  folded  and  tarsi 
tucked  under  the  body.  The  bird  remained  motionless  as  the  dog  wandered  no  more  than  5 
m away.  Closer  inspection  revealed  breast,  belly,  flanks  and  undertail  coverts  were  saturated 
with  water.  Ventral  feathers  around  the  cloaca  were  stained  green,  suggesting  either  lead 
poisoning  or  botulism;  grit  was  also  concentrated  around  the  cloaca.  The  eyes  were  open 
but  “glassy”;  they  were  rolled  and  exposed  much  white.  The  nictitating  membrane  was  not 
observed  to  function,  and  an  eye  did  not  respond  when  I touched  it.  I twisted  the  neck, 
pulled  feathers  on  the  back  and  wings,  tapped  head  and  bill  with  my  fingers,  let  the  head 
and  biU  drop  4 cm  on  the  ice,  and  stiU  observed  no  response.  I did  not  feel  the  bird  breathing. 
The  only  sign  of  life  was  involuntary  pedaUing  of  the  tarsus  and  feet  when  I inverted  the 
eider.  I released  it  2 m from  open  water  and  walked  away  20  m.  It  remained  on  the  ice  in 
a death-feigned  posture  for  barely  1 min.  The  head  and  neck  came  up  suddenly  and  it 
appeared  to  look  in  our  direction.  My  dog  pursued  it  but  the  eider  escaped  to  the  water, 
where  it  preened  vigorously  and  then  slowly  swam  toward  shore. 

Perry  (1938,  in  Armstrong  1965)  described  death  feigning  in  a male  eider  which  was 
apparently  in  good  health.  Armstrong  (1965)  records  death  feigning  and  injury  distraction  in 
a nesting  adult  female  Greater  Golden  Plover  {Pluvialus  apricaria)  suggesting  association 
of  these  responses.  Operational  definitions  of  death  feigning,  immobility  and  “freezing”  are 
lacking.  Relationships  of  immobility  responses  to  “freezing”  postures  have  not  been  inves- 
tigated (Hinde,  Animal  Behavior,  2nd  ed.,  1970:420).  Laboratory  studies  conducted  by  Rather 
and  Thompson  (Anim.  Behav.  8:186-191,  1960)  and  Francq  (1969)  suggest  the  response 
develops  at  the  period  of  emerging  physical  independence,  not  sexual  maturation.  Death 
feigning  may  be  a response  to  extreme  stress.  However,  Norton  et  al.  (Nature  204:162-163, 
1964),  studying  brain  wave  recordings  in  immobile  opossums  {Didelphis  virginianus)  during 
“feigned  sleep,”  found  no  changes  between  “feigned”  and  normal  states;  the  animals  main- 
tained normal  brain  wave  patterns  and  heart  rates,  suggesting  the  state  of  shock  in  the 
animal  is  erroneously  assumed. 

Controlled  experiments  are  needed  to  define  the  relationship  of  death  feigning  to  other 
similar  respones.  Natural  observations  can  do  little  more  to  provide  evidence  of  the  mech- 
anisms of  these  relationships.  Death  feigning  has  survival  value  to  the  individual  and  has 
almost  certainly  evolved  through  natural  selection. 

Thanks  to  R.  M.  Alison  and  J.  Jackson  who  were  very  helpful  referees. — DOUGLAS  B. 
McNair,  Dept.  Biological  Sciences,  P.O.  Drawer  GY,  Mississippi  State  Univ.,  Mississippi 
State,  Mississippi  39762.  (Present  address:  Dept.  Zoology,  Clemson  Univ.,  Clemson,  South 
Carolina  29631.)  Accepted  22  Sept.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  560-561 


Territorial  attachment  and  mate  fidelity  by  Homed  Grebes. — Although  territorial 
attachment  has  been  documented  for  many  migratory  birds  (see  Austin,  Bird-Banding  20:1- 
39,  1949;  Hilden,  Ann.  Zool.  Fenn.  2:53-75,  1%5),  evidence  that  grebes  (Podicipedidae) 
return  to  the  same  nesting  territory  in  consecutive  years  is  largely  circumstantial.  This  note 
documents  territorial  attachment  and  mate  fidelity  by  individually  marked  Horned  Grebes 
{Podiceps  auritus)  at  Minnedosa,  Manitoba  (50°15'N,  99°50'W).  Seven  of  50  grebes  (43  adults 
and  seven  juveniles)  banded  in  1974  and  1975  were  recaptured  in  either  1975  and  1976  by  using 


GENERAL  NOTES 


561 


a submerged  gill  net  (Ferguson,  J.  Field  Ornithol.,  51:179-180,  1980).  Whether  any  of  the 
other  banded  birds  returned  was  not  determined.  Each  pair  of  grebes  mentioned  in  this  note 
occupied  an  entire  pothole  marsh  as  its  territory.  Territorial  attachment  means  an  individual’s 
return  to  the  same  nesting  territory.  Mate  fidelity  is  defined  as  breeding  with  the  same  mate 
for  more  than  one  breeding  season. 

Five  of  the  seven  recaptured  grebes  (three  males  and  two  females)  nested  on  territories  on 
which  they  had  nested  and  raised  young  to  fledging  in  the  previous  year.  On  25  May  1976,  I 
recaptured  both  members  of  a nesting  pair  (pair  20)  which  had  fledged  young  from  the  same 
pond  in  1975.  Both  birds  had  been  banded  as  adults  on  this  pond  on  10  June  1975.  Their 
recapture  illustrates  both  territorial  attachment  and  mate  fidelity.  Fjeldsa  (Sterna  12:161- 
217,  1973)  suggested  that  mate  fidelity  by  Horned  Grebes  in  Sweden  may  be  due  to  a sus- 
tained monogamy.  At  Minnedosa,  this  is  unlikely  in  view  of  the  pattern  of  summer  departure 
of  adults.  In  1975,  the  male  of  pair  20  left  the  territory  between  2 and  7 July,  leaving  its 
mate  and  two  unfledged  young.  The  female  deserted  the  territory  between  19  and  23  July, 
at  least  12  days  after  the  male  had  departed.  This  pattern  of  dispersal  from  territories  was 
typical  of  adult  grebes  unless  a pair  attempted  to  raise  a second  brood  (Ferguson,  M.Sc. 
thesis,  Univ.  Manitoba,  Winnipeg,  Manitoba,  1977).  Renewal  of  a pair-bond  probably  occurs 
in  the  following  spring  (as  with  pair  20  in  1976),  facilitated  by  territorial  attachment.  Both 
sexes  demonstrated  an  attachment  to  former  nesting  territories  as  shown  by  the  following 
examples. 

Two  males  banded  as  adults  on  1 and  6 June  1975,  were  recaptured  on  the  same  territories 
on  31  and  25  May  1976,  respectively.  At  least  one  of  these  males  was  paired  with  a different 
mate  in  1976.  This  male,  whose  original  mate  had  been  banded  in  1975,  was  paired  with  an 
unmarked  female  in  1976.  On  3 June  1975,  a nesting  female  was  recaptured  on  a pond  on 
which  she  had  raised  young  in  1974.  She  had  been  banded  as  an  adult  on  19  June  1974.  I 
was  unable  to  capture  and  band  her  mate  in  either  year. 

Migrational  homing,  defined  as  the  return  of  individuals  to  the  same  nesting  area  (Dwyer, 
Derrickson  and  Gilmer,  Auk  90:687,  1973),  was  documented  for  two  males.  On  10  June  1975, 
a nesting  male  banded  as  an  adult  on  2 July  1974,  was  recaptured  on  a pond  3.7  km  from 
his  former  nesting  territory.  A second  male,  banded  on  1 July  1974,  was  recaptured  on  24 
May  1976,  on  a pond  0.8  km  from  his  1974  nesting  territory.  This  male  was  not  observed 
during  1975.  I found  no  evidence  of  Horned  Grebes  returning  in  their  first  breeding  season 
to  their  natal  ponds.  However,  this  was  inconclusive  considering  that  only  seven  juveniles 
were  banded. 

In  summary,  it  is  doubtful  that  mate  fidelity  by  Horned  Grebes  is  due  to  year-round  pairing. 
Alternatively,  territorial  attachment  by  both  sexes  may  result  in  contact  with  former  mates 
and  in  an  opportunity  for  renewing  pair-bonds  in  spring.  Behavioral  evidence  suggests  that 
some  pairing  occurs  on  the  breeding  grounds  (Ferguson  1977).  By  returning  to  nest  at  familiar 
marshes  where  nesting  was  previously  successful,  grebes  may  increase  both  their  chances 
of  survival  and  of  raising  young.  At  Minnedosa,  ponds  which  supported  a pair  that  fledged 
young  were  occupied  more  frequently  in  the  following  year  (8  of  18  ponds)  than  ponds  which 
initially  supported  a pair  that  failed  to  raise  young  (2  of  21  ponds). 

Financial  support  for  fieldwork  was  provided  by  the  National  Research  Council  of  Canada 
and  the  Northern  Studies  Committee  of  the  Department  of  Indian  and  Northern  Affairs 
(grants  awarded  to  S.  G.  Sealy,  University  of  Manitoba),  Gulf  Oil  Canada  Limited,  Manitoba 
Department  of  Renewable  Resources  and  Transportation  Services  and  National  Audubon 
Society.  Banding  permits  were  kindly  provided  by  the  Canadian  Wildlife  Service.  I am 
grateful  to  S.  D.  MacDonald  and  K.  S.  Yonge  for  commenting  on  an  earlier  draft  of  the 
manuscript. — Robert  S.  Ferguson,  #402  112-111  Street,  Saskatoon,  Saskatchewan  S7N 
1S7  Canada.  Accepted  25  Aug.  1980. 


562 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  562-563 


Effects  of  Redhead  nest  parasitism  on  Mallards. — Female  Redheads  {Aythya  amer- 
icana)  are  known  to  deposit  eggs  in  nests  of  Mallards  {Anas  platyrhynchos).  Joyner  (J.  Wildl. 
Manage.  40:33—38,  1976)  attributed  a high  rate  of  Redhead  parasitism  on  Mallard  nests  at 
Farmington  Bay,  Davis  Co.,  Utah,  to  crowding  of  host  and  parasite  into  the  same  habitat. 
Weller  (Ecol.  Monogr.  29:333-365,  1959)  stated  that  at  Knudson  Marsh,  Utah,  only  a few 
deep  channels  and  patches  of  water  were  suitable  for  feeding  and  courtship  by  Redheads, 
and  nests  of  other  ducks  located  near  those  areas  were  heavily  parasitized;  nests  farther 
from  the  shore  were  parasitized  less  often.  Redhead  nest  parasitism  resulted  in  reduced  host 
clutch-size  (Weller  1959),  displaced  and  broken  host  eggs  (Joyner  1976)  and  increased  nest 
abandonment  (Ryder,  Trans.  N.  Am.  Wildl.  Nat.  Resour.  Conf.  26:134-146,  1961). 

Although  the  Prairie  Pothole  Region  of  North  America  is  the  principal  breeding  ground  of 
both  species,  little  is  known  concerning  the  extent  and  effects  of  Redhead  parasitism  on 
Mallards.  Because  the  Mallard  commonly  nests  in  marshes  in  the  Prairie  Pothole  Region 
(Krapu  et  al.,  Wildl.  Soc.  Bull.  7:104-110,  1979),  potential  exists  for  high  levels  of  nest 
parasitism.  This  paper  describes  the  effects  of  nest  parasitism  by  Redheads  on  Mallards  that 
nested  in  marshes  in  south-central  North  Dakota. 

Data  were  collected  on  the  Medina  Study  Area  (93.2  km^)  in  western  Stutsman  County, 
North  Dakota.  The  study  area,  located  within  the  Missouri  Coteau,  is  moderately  rolling 
glacial  moraine  containing  2-14  wetlands  per  km^.  A detailed  description  of  the  study  area 
was  presented  by  Krapu  et  al.  (1979).  In  1976  and  1977,  wetlands  were  searched  for  Mallard 
nests  by  systematically  wading  through  emergent  vegetation.  A colored  flag  was  placed  6 m 
from  each  nest;  nests  were  periodically  revisited  until  the  eggs  hatched  or  until  the  nests 
were  abandoned  or  destroyed. 

Of  24  active  Mallard  nests  located  in  emergent  vegetation  of  semipermanent  marshes 
during  1976,  10  were  parasitized  by  Redheads  and  two  by  Ruddy  Ducks  {Oxyura  jamaicensis); 
one  nest  was  parasitized  by  both  species.  In  1977,  when  water  levels  in  semipermanent 
marshes  were  low  because  of  a drought,  only  three  nests  were  located,  and  we  observed  no 
interspecific  nest  parasitism.  Therefore,  all  calculations  were  based  on  our  1976  data.  Be- 
cause nest  parasitism  by  Ruddy  Ducks  was  infrequent,  we  ignored  the  nest  parasitized  solely 
by  a Ruddy  Duck  and  based  our  calculations  on  the  remaining  23  Mallard  nests. 

Success  rates  of  parasitized  and  unparasitized  nests  (4  of  10  [40%1  and  3 of  13  [23%], 
respectively)  were  not  significantly  different  (x^  = 0.77,  df  = 1,  NS).  Unparasitized  nests 
and  those  parasitized  by  Redheads  were  abandoned  at  similar  rates  (4  of  13  [30.8%]  and  3 
of  10  [30%],  respectively).  Additionally,  the  percentages  of  parasitized  and  unparasitized 
Mallard  nests  destroyed  (3  of  10  [30%]  and  6 of  13  [46.2%1,  respectively)  by  predators  were 
not  significantly  different  (x^  = 0.62,  df  = 1,  NS). 

Redhead  nest  parasitism  resulted  in  significantly  fewer  Mallard  eggs  per  nest  {t  — 9.71, 
df  = 21,  P < 0.05).  The  mean  number  of  Mallard  eggs  in  nests  parasitized  by  Redheads  was 
5.6  ± 2.2  compared  to  7.2  ±3.1  eggs  in  unparasitized  marsh  nests.  Also,  in  nests  that 
hatched  at  least  one  egg.  Mallard  egg  success  in  parasitized  nests  was  significantly  lower 
than  success  of  eggs  in  either  unparasitized  marsh  nests  (x^  = 6.83,  df  = 1,  P < 0.05)  or 
unparasitized  upland  nests  (x^  = 6.40,  df  = 1,  P < 0.05)  monitored  on  the  study  area  (Table 
1).  Mallard  egg  success  in  parasitized  marsh  nests  was  only  43%,  whereas  success  in  unpar- 
asitized marsh  nests  was  80%.  Thus,  the  primary  effects  of  parasitism  were  a reduced 
number  of  Mallard  eggs  in  nests  and  lowered  egg  success. 

Egg  success  at  parasitized  nests  was  decreased  by  a combination  of  factors;  egg  displace- 
ment was  the  most  important.  Thirty-five  percent  of  the  Mallard  eggs  in  parasitized  suc- 
cessful nests  were  displaced  from  nests.  Most  displaced  eggs  were  under  water  near  the 


GENERAL  NOTES 


563 


Table  1 

Comparative  Egg  Success  in  Parasitized  and  Unparasitized  Successful  Mallard 
Nests  on  the  Medina  Study  Area  in  1976 


Total  eggs 

Eggs  hatched 

Habitat 

Host 

Parasite 

Host 

Parasite 

Unparasitized 

Upland 

50 

— 

37 

— 

Marsh 

25 

— 

20 

— 

Parasitized 

Marsh 

23 

20 

10 

8 

nests.  Infertility  and  death  of  embryos,  primarily  because  of  cracked  eggs,  caused  most 
other  egg  losses. 

Egg  deposition  by  Redheads  often  preceded  incubation  by  Mallards  and  may  have  sup- 
pressed ovulation  in  Mallard  hens.  The  comparable  hatching  success  of  host  and  parasite 
eggs  (Table  1)  indicated  that  many  parasitic  eggs  were  deposited  in  Mallard  nests  before 
incubation  began.  On  average,  3.8  Redhead  eggs  were  deposited  in  each  parasitized  Mallard 
nest,  and  1.5  Redhead  ducklings  hatched  from  each  successful  parasitized  nest.  This  oc- 
curred when  densities  on  the  study  area  were  about  four  pairs  of  Redheads  per  km^  and 
three  pairs  of  Mallards  per  km^  (A.  D.  Kruse,  unpubl.). 

Our  data  suggest  that  Redhead  nest  parasitism  reduces  the  number  of  Mallard  ducklings 
hatched  at  marsh  sites  in  the  Prairie  Pothole  Region.  Presumably,  the  extent  of  Redhead 
nest  parasitism  varies  with  water  conditions,  densities  of  parasite  and  host,  and  the  relative 
number  of  Mallards  nesting  in  marsh  habitat.  Because  Mallards  commonly  nest  in  marshes, 
potential  exists  for  substantial  Redhead  nest  parasitism  and  attendant  reduction  in  number 
of  Mallard  eggs  per  nest  and  egg  success.  However,  additional  research  is  needed  to  evaluate 
this  potential. 

Acknowledgments. — The  study  was  supported  by  the  Northern  Prairie  Wildlife  Research 
Center  (Contract  No.  14-16-0003-2038)  and  conducted  under  the  auspices  of  the  Oregon 
Cooperative  Wildlife  Research  Unit;  Oregon  Department  of  Fish  and  Wildlife,  Oregon  State 
University,  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife  Service  and  Wildlife  Management  Institute  cooperating. 
Oregon  State  University  Agricultural  Experiment  Station  Technical  Paper  No.  5406. 

We  thank  J.  A.  Crawford  and  J.  R.  Serie  for  critically  reviewing  the  manuscript;  D.  G. 
Jorde,  L.  Kludt,  and  R.  Green  for  assisting  with  collection  of  data;  and  S.  D.  Becker  for 
helping  locate  Mallard  nests. — Larry  G.  Talent,  Oregon  Cooperative  Wildlife  Research 
Unit,  Oregon  State  Univ.,  Corvallis,  Oregon  97331,  Gary  L.  Krapu,  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife 
Service,  Northern  Prairie  Wildlife  Research  Center,  Jamestown,  North  Dakota  58401  AND 
Robert  L.  Jarvis,  Dept,  of  Fisheries  and  Wildlife,  Oregon  State  Univ.,  Corvallis,  Oregon 
97331.  (Present  address  LGT:  Dept.  Zoology,  Oklahoma  State  Univ.,  Stillwater,  Oklahoma 
74078.)  Accepted  14  Oct.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  563-565 

Survival  of  a demaxillate  Red-winged  Blackbird. — The  literature  contains  numerous 
reports  of  birds  with  abnormal  bills.  Surprisingly,  in  view  of  the  supposed  adaptiveness  of 


564 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Fig.  1.  Red-winged  Blackbird  with  abnormal  biU;  drawn  from  photograph  taken  16  April 
1977. 


biU  structure,  such  birds  often  appear  to  have  adjusted  successfuUy  to  their  deformity, 
judging  from  their  apparent  good  health  at  the  time  of  coUection  or  observation  or  from  the 
apparent  long  standing  of  the  deformity  by  the  time  it  is  noted.  However,  confirmations  of 
long-term  survival  are  few;  I have  found  only  five  reports  of  survival  for  a year  or  more 
(Stamm,  Kentucky  Warbler  49:75,  1973;  Donark,  Dansk  Ornithol.  Foren.  Tidsskr.  44:16-19, 
1950;  Nowak,  Der  Falke  12:122-130,  1%5;  Pomeroy,  Br.  Birds  55:49-72,  1%2;  Wystrach, 
Auk  94:781-782,  1977).  I report  the  survival  for  at  least  3 years  of  a male  Red-winged 
Blackbird  (Agelaius  phoeniceus)  that  lacked  most  of  its  maxiUa. 

I first  trapped  the  bird,  then  in  subadult  plumage,  on  25  May  1976,  at  the  University  of 
Michigan’s  E.  S.  George  Reserve,  Livingston  Co.,  Michigan.  The  stump  of  its  maxiUa  ex- 
tended to  the  middle  of  the  nostrils,  which  had  become  closed,  forcing  the  bird  to  breathe 
through  its  mouth.  The  normal  structure  of  the  base  of  the  maxiUa  suggested  that  the  loss 
was  the  result  of  an  accident  (perhaps  with  a spring-type  rodent  trap;  I have  seen  an  Amer- 
ican Robin  [Turdus  migratorius]  so  caught)  rather  than  a congenital  defect.  The  tongue  was 
normal  and  the  mandible  complete,  but  the  tomia  were  slightly  hypertrophic.  The  bird’s 
weight  (68.0  g)  was  normal,  and  it  was  in  vigorous  condition.  .Although  I happened  to  observe 
the  bird  arrive  at  the  corn-baited  trap  and  feed  briefly  before  becoming  caught,  its  behavior 
was  not  unusual  and  I noticed  the  abnormaUty  only  after  retrieving  the  bird.  I banded  and 
released  it,  but  did  not  observe  it  again  that  year. 

On  11  March  1977,  and  on  the  next  few  days,  I saw  the  same  bird  singing  in  woods  on  the 
George  Reserve.  The  area  in  which  it  sang  adjoins  areas  annuaUy  occupied  by  territorial 
males,  but  does  not  itself  contain  suitable  nest-sites,  and  the  bird  did  not  remain  there.  At 
this  time  the  bird  had  a recurved  horny  outgrowth  from  the  ventral  side  of  the  maxiUa  (Fig. 
1).  I trapped  it  again  on  16  April  1977,  and  found  it  stiU  in  good  condition,  weighing  71.8  g. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


565 


but  about  12  ectoparasites  crawled  onto  my  hand  as  I held  it.  This  has  not  occurred  on  any 
of  a few  hundred  captures  of  adult  males  with  normal  bills.  At  this  capture  the  bird  received 
a unique  color-band  combination.  I observed  it  once  more  that  year,  on  13  June,  in  an  area 
where  a flock  of  males  was  beginning  to  congregate.  By  then  it  had  lost  the  horny  outgrowth. 

I again  observed  the  bird  on  7 May  1978,  when  it  intruded  briefly  into  the  territory  of 
another  male.  On  several  occasions  from  28  July-13  August  1978,  it  appeared  with  other 
males  feeding  on  cracked  corn  on  the  lawn  under  my  feeding  tray.  It  picked  up  the  corn 
from  among  blades  of  grass  with  as  much  facility  as  the  other  birds,  scooping  up  a grain 
with  the  mandible  then  manipulating  it  at  the  base  of  the  bill  as  do  normal  birds.  Its  behavior 
was  sufficiently  normal  that,  although  I was  only  5 m distant,  I recognized  the  bird  by  its 
color  bands  sooner  than  by  its  bill. 

My  final  observation  of  the  bird  was  on  24  March  1979,  when  it  briefly  visited  the  trapping 
station.  I did  not  specifically  note  its  bill  on  this  occasion  and  identified  the  bird  only  after 
a later  check  of  the  color  bands. 

Bill  structure  is  usually  associated  most  closely  with  survival  aspects  of  fitness,  but  it 
probably  has  indirect  effects  on  reproductive  success  as  well.  Unfortunately,  I have  no  in- 
formation on  this  bird’s  reproductive  success.  During  the  four  breeding  seasons  in  which  I 
observed  the  bird  I was  studying  the  redwings  breeding  in  the  marshes  near  the  trapping 
site  and  would  have  found  its  territory  had  it  had  one  there.  However,  there  are  numerous 
other  marshes  slightly  more  distant  where  it  could  have  had  a territory. 

I thank  A.  Martin  for  editorial  suggestions. — Kent  L.  Fiala,  Museum  of  Zoology,  Univ. 
Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  (Present  address:  Dept.  Ecology  and  Evolution, 
SUNY,  Stony  Brook,  Long  Island,  New  York  11794.)  Accepted  24  Nov.  1980. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  565-569 

Minimizing  investigator  disturbance  in  observational  studies  of  colonial  birds: 
access  to  blinds  through  tunnels. — Colonial  nesting  birds  present  unique  advantages 
and  disadvantages  to  the  investigator  of  behavior  and  ecology.  A major  advantage  is  that 
there  are  many  birds  concentrated  in  a relatively  small  area,  which  allows  accumulation  of 
large  data  sets.  A disadvantage  is  that  investigator  disturbance  can  bias  or  affect  efficiency 
of  data  collection,  particularly  if  birds  in  a colony  are  not  accustomed  to  humans.  Investigator 
effects  can  range  from  simple  disruption  of  ongoing  breeding  activities  and  colony  dynamics 
(Vermeer,  Can.  Wildl.  Serv.  Kept.  Series  12,  1970;  Smith,  Br.  Birds  68:142-156,  1975;  Sears, 
Bird-Banding  49: 1-16,  1978)  to  chick  mortality  as  young  run  from  their  territories  and  become 
lost  or  are  killed  (Emlen,  Wilson  Bull.  68:232-238,  1956;  Ashmole,  Ibis  103b:297-364,  1961; 
Kadlec  and  Drury,  Ecology  49:644-676,  1968;  Kadlec  et  al.,  Bird-Banding  40:222-232,  1969; 
Roberts  and  Ralph,  Condor  77:495^99,  1975;  GiUet  et  al..  Condor  77:492^95,  1975;  Davies 
and  Dunn,  Ibis  118:65-77,  1976).  Predacious  gulls  (Larus  spp.)  also  may  take  advantage  of 
the  disturbance  and  eat  eggs  and  chicks  of  their  own  and  other  species  nesting  in  or  near 
the  same  colony  (Kury  and  Gochfeld,  Biol.  Conserv.  8:23-34,  1975;  Ellison  and  Cleary,  Auk 
95:510-517,  1978).  These  disturbance  related  effects  are  inherent  in  studies  conducted  from 
observation  blinds  placed  within  nesting  colonies  simply  because  the  investigator  creates  a 
disturbance  while  entering  a blind.  To  minimize  unwanted  disturbance  and  related  effects 
in  sparsely  vegetated  Lake  Michigan  bird  colonies,  we  have  designed  and  used  an  easily 
constructed  tunnel  system  which  permits  access  to  blinds. 

Methods  and  materials. — The  design  described  here  was  used  in  1978  and  modified  in 


566 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Fig.  I.  Cross  section  of  tunnel  showing  details  of  construction  where  two  tunnel  sections 
join;  drawn  to  scale. 


1979  to  withstand  high  winds.  The  tunnel  described  in  this  paper  is  the  one  used  in  1979 
unless  otherwise  noted.  Modifications  can  be  made  depending  on  conditions  unique  to  the 
investigator’s  study  area  and  species. 

The  tunnel  consisted  of  3.1  m (10  ft)  x 2.4  m (8  ft)  black  plastic  sheets  that  were  attached 
to  9.5  mm  (0.375  in)  X 38.0  mm  (1.5  in)  X 3.1  m (10  ft)  wooden  strips  (Figs.  1,  2).  English 
equivalents  are  provided  to  facilitate  purchasing  of  material.)  Sections  of  tunnel  were  pre- 
fabricated by  nailing  two  wood  strips  together  with  the  plastic  sandwiched  between  at  the 
center  of  each  3.1  m length  of  plastic  (Fig.  2).  At  0.8  m (2.5  ft)  on  either  side  of  the  center 
of  each  3.1  m length  of  plastic,  two  more  strips  were  nailed  together.  This  resulted  in  three 
wooden  frame  members  19  mm  0.75  in)  X 38.0  mm  (1.5  in)  per  tunnel  section. 

We  formed  the  tunnel  by  joining  the  three  frame  members  from  each  section  end-to-end 
with  frame  members  from  another  section  (with  0.2  m overlap)  using  eyescrews  or  eye-bolts 
(Figs.  1,  2).  Frame  members  were  pre-driUed  to  save  time  in  the  field  if  eye-bolts  were  used. 
Metal  supports  for  the  plastic  sections  were  2.4  m (8  ft)  lengths  of  6 mm  (0.25  in)  steel  rod 
bent  in  a U-shape.  Tunnel  erection  initially  consisted  of  threading  a metal  rod  through  the 
eyes,  standing  the  metal  support  rods  with  ends  down,  forcing  the  ends  of  the  metal  rod  into 
the  substrate,  and  spreading  the  plastic  over  the  metal  frame. 

The  end-to-end  attachment  of  tunnel  sections  created  uneven  overlaps  of  the  frame  mem- 
bers when  sharp  changes  of  direction  or  elevation  were  attempted.  When  such  angular 


GENERAL  NOTES 


567 


Fig.  2.  Plastic  removed  from  a portion  of  the  tunnel  (dotted  line)  to  show  construction 
details;  drawn  to  approximate  scale. 


changes  were  necessary  we  simply  lashed  the  wooden  frame  members  to  the  metal  frame 
after  compensating  for  the  uneven  overlap. 

After  initially  setting  the  tunnel  up  and  adjusting  the  plastic,  we  entered  the  tunnel  and 
attached  it  to  the  ground  with  augers  that  we  had  pre-formed  by  twisting  0.6  m (2  ft)  sections 
of  6 mm  (0.25  in)  steel  rod  around  24  mm  (1  in)  diameter  soil-pipe.  Two  augers  were  twisted 
into  the  ground  at  each  hoop,  and  the  eye-bolt  was  tied  to  the  auger  (Fig.  1).  Additional 
augers  or  conventional  stakes  were  added  midway  between  hoops  and  the  frame  members 
were  tied  to  these.  Augers  were  used  because  of  superior  holding  power  in  sand,  cobble, 
and  gravel  as  compared  with  conventional  stakes.  They  also  were  easier  to  manipulate  inside 
the  tunnel.  Staking  the  tunnel  down  from  inside  reduced  disturbance  to  incubating  birds 
during  tunnel  erection. 

Results  and  discussion. — We  bolted  five  sections  (15  m)  of  tunnel  together  outside  the 
colony,  threaded  the  rods  through  the  eyes  and  carried  six  15-m  lengths  of  tunnel  into  position. 
Bolting  the  sections  together  took  about  3 h and  moving  them  into  position  and  initial  erection 
took  approximately  1 h.  Six  hours  were  required  to  securely  stake  the  tunnel  from  the  inside. 
When  completed  the  tunnel  was  approximately  1 m wide  at  the  base  and  1-1.2  m high.  A 
2 m tall  human  could  easily  crawl  the  90  m length  with  a 10  kg  pack  around  the  neck  in  a 
few  minutes. 

The  tunnel  entrance  was  located  in  the  shrub  and  tree  covered  island  interior  where  we 
entered  without  being  seen  by  birds.  The  tunnel  passed  90  m through  a Herring  Gull  {Larus 
argentatus)  colony  and  terminated  in  a blind  adjacent  to  a Caspian  Tern  (Sterna  caspia) 
colony.  A Herring  GuU  observation  blind  was  also  placed  at  the  midpoint. 

Moving  through  the  tunnel  created  a peristaltic-like  movement  of  the  plastic  sheet.  Since 
the  plastic  flapped  and  fluttered  in  the  wind,  birds  did  not  notice  our  passing  on  windy  days. 
On  calm  days,  guUs  standing  on  top  of  the  tunnel  or  adjacent  to  it  gave  low  intensity  alarm 
calls  when  we  passed.  In  contrast,  aU  birds  within  100  m took  to  the  air  when  a human 
emerged  from  the  vegetation  and  walked  to  a blind. 

In  both  seasons,  the  tunnel  was  unprotected  from  winds.  Since  the  plastic  covering  pre- 
sents a large  surface  area  to  the  wind,  the  wood  and  metal  frame  was  subjected  to  consid- 


568 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


erable  stress.  In  1978,  this  caused  problems  when  smaller  diameter  rods  (3  mm  or  0.125  in) 
were  bent  out  of  shape  and  narrower  wood  strips  (19  mm  or  0.75  in)  were  broken  by  gales  in 
excess  of  60  kph.  With  6 mm  (0.25  in)  rods  and  38  mm  (1.5  in)  frame  members  in  1979 
neither  problem  recurred.  The  tunnel  withstood  five  gales,  each  of  which  blew  for  24  h or 
longer. 

Three  frame  members  per  section  were  used  to  minimize  weight  and  cost.  The  plastic 
tended  to  sag  in  between  frame  members,  but  this  did  not  necessitate  the  addition  of  another 
frame  member  on  each  side.  Placement  of  the  side  frame  members  at  0.8  m (2.5  ft)  from  the 
center  left  a 0.5  m (1.5  ft)  skirt  that  allowed  ventilation,  reduced  wind  stress  and  permitted 
young  guUs  to  enter  and  exit. 

We  used  4 and  6 mil  plastic  in  1978  and  1979,  respectively.  In  both  years.  Herring  Gulls 
pecked  and  tore  the  plastic  to  such  an  extent  that  it  had  to  be  replaced  or  repaired  on  50% 
of  the  sections.  The  6 mil  plastic  used  in  1979  apparently  fatigued  in  the  wind  or  sun  and 
tears  began  appearing  after  3 months  exposure.  The  4 mil  plastic  used  in  1978  retained  its 
resiliency  through  1979.  Reasons  for  the  different  wear  characteristics  apparently  were  re- 
lated to  different  composition. 

Preliminary  evaluation. — Is  the  tunnel  worth  the  effort?  We  can  provide  an  initial  as- 
sessment based  on  observations  of  Caspian  Terns  by  Shugart  and  Herring  GuUs  by  Fitch. 

During  1977  through  1979  breeding  seasons,  Shugart  observed  individually  marked  Cas- 
pian Terns  during  the  last  part  of  incubation  until  after  young  could  fly.  An  observation  blind 
was  approximately  10  m from  the  study  area  in  each  year.  During  this  period  in  1977,  Shugart 
entered  the  bhnd  one  day  and  exited  the  next,  creating  one  brief  disturbance  per  day.  Eight  (of  8) 
marked  pairs  and  their  young  moved  at  least  50  m away  from  the  blind  by  the  time  the  oldest 
study  chick  was  10  days  of  age.  Observation  of  these  individuals  was  terminated  at  this  time 
because  the  birds  were  too  far  away  from  the  blind  to  be  seen  clearly.  It  appeared  that  the 
movement  away  from  the  observation  blind  in  1977  was  due  to  disturbance  (see  also  Smith 
1975,  Sears  1978).  In  1978,  to  minimize  this  possible  effect  on  colony  dynamics  and  to 
increase  the  period  that  marked  individuals  could  be  observed,  Shugart  alternated  5-day 
periods  in  the  blind  and  2 days  out  after  the  first  study  chick  was  4 days  old.  In  1978, 
between  the  times  Shugart  left  the  blind  and  returned  2 days  later,  2 of  22  (10%)  study  chicks 
were  lost,  and  3 of  12  (25%)  family  groups  moved  too  far  away  to  been  seen  clearly.  We 
assume  that  the  chick  loss  and  movement  were  affected  by  exiting  and  entering  disturbances 
as  no  movement  or  chick  loss  occurred  after  the  birds  had  calmed  down  during  the  5-day 
periods  in  the  blind. 

In  1979,  tunnels  were  erected  during  late  incubation  and  used  to  enter  and  exit  the  blind 
after  the  oldest  study  chicks  were  4 days  old.  This  permitted  daily  entering  and  exiting  with 
minimal  disturbance  and  observation  of  a near  normal  colony.  Of  15  family  groups  marked, 
14  centered  activities  at  their  natal  site  until  at  least  1 week  after  young  could  fly.  The 
remaining  family  group  moved  from  10  m to  within  3 m of  the  blind  where  they  remained 
until  after  the  young  could  fly. 

Fitch’s  Herring  Gull  studies  during  1977-1979  required  placement  of  blinds  throughout  a 
colony  to  allow  observation  of  widely  spread  polygynous  groups.  At  blinds  where  tunnels 
were  not  used,  the  disturbance  Fitch  caused  by  walking  through  the  colony  to  a blind  indi- 
rectly resulted  in  chick  mortality  as  the  young  ran  from  their  territories  and  were  killed  by 
neighbors.  Four  of  10  (40%)  chicks  from  two  of  four  polygynous  groups  were  killed  in  this 
way  (Fitch,  pers.  obs.).  Fitch  observed  14  groups  (monogamous  and  polygynous)  from  blinds 
that  were  entered  by  tunnels,  and  no  chick  mortality  occurred  due  to  entering  blinds. 

The  disturbance-related  effects  on  Caspian  Terns  and  Herring  Gulls  that  we  have  observed 
may  not  be  important  in  all  studies,  but  they  were  in  ours.  Movement  of  Caspian  Tern 
families  away  from  blinds  reduced  or  eliminated  the  small  sample  of  marked  individuals. 


GENERAL  NOTES 


569 


and  altered  relationships  between  families  as  the  remaining  birds  shifted  to  fill  in  vacated 
areas.  In  Fitch’s  study,  the  dispersion  of  polygynous  groups  made  possible  the  observation 
of  only  a few  polygynous  groups  per  year.  When  chicks  were  lost  from  these  groups  differ- 
ences in  reproductive  success  and  behavior  between  and  within  polygynous  and  monogamous 
mating  types  were  masked  or  biased. 

Although  investigator  disturbance  and  associated  effects  probably  cannot  be  entirely  ehm- 
inated,  we  feel  it  is  best  to  conduct  observations  under  conditions  which  are  as  natural  as 
possible.  This  is  particularly  important  when  attempting  to  generalize  reproductive  and  be- 
havioral data  taken  from  the  necessarily  small  sample  sizes  that  detailed  observations  require. 
It  is  also  of  importance  when  investigators  attempt  to  assess  inter-individual  differences  in 
reproduction  and  behavior. 

We  thank  the  Frank  M.  Chapman  Memorial  Fund  for  financial  assistance  to  MAE  during 
the  study,  T.  O.  Lempke  for  assistance  in  prefabrication  of  the  tunnel  sections,  and  J.  R. 
Crook  and  B.  G.  Murray,  Jr.  for  evaluating  the  manuscript. — Gary  W.  Shugart,  Mary  A. 
Fitch,  Ecology  Program  Bldg.  4087 -Kilmer,  Rutgers  Univ.,  New  Brunswick,  New  Jersey 
08903  AND  Vern  M.  Shugart,  620  West  Front  St.,  Traverse  City,  Michigan  49684.  Accepted 
29  Sept.  1980. 


REQUEST  FOR  ASSISTANCE 

Study  collection. — Bird  study  skins,  skeletons  and  alcoholics  (world)  are  wanted  for 
undergraduate  and  graduate  ornithology  collection.  Labelled  and  unlabelled  specimens 
sent  collect  shall  be  greatly  appreciated  and  acknowledged  in  the  collection.  Contact 
John  P.  Ryder,  Dept.  Biology,  Lakehead  University,  Thunder  Bay,  Ontario  P7B  5E1 
Canada.  (807)  345-2121. 


Wilson  Bull.,  93(4),  1981,  pp.  570-574 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


Breeding  Biology  of  the  Egyptian  Plover  Pluvianus  aegyptius.  By  Thomas  R.  Howell. 
Univ.  Calif.  Publ.  Zool.  Vol.  113,  Univ.  California  Press,  Berkeley,  California,  1979:76  pp., 
color  frontispiece,  15  black-and-white  plates,  6 numbered  text  figs.,  5 tables.  $10.50. — The 
Egyptian  Plover,  which  is  not  a plover  and  no  longer  occurs  in  Egypt,  has  been  an  ornitho- 
logical enigma  fraught  with  the  uncertainties  attendant  to  a largely  anecdotal  and  embellished 
Literature.  This  courser  (Cursoriinae,  Glareolidae)  figures  in  the  textbooks  as  the  reputed 
symbiont  that  gleans  food  particles  from  between  the  teeth  of  crocodiles  (Crocodilus  nilot- 
icus)  and  as  a species  whose  buried  eggs  are  incubated  by  solar  heat.  To  our  good  fortune, 
a keen  student  of  “avian  adaptations  that  contribute  to  reproductive  success  in  difficult  and 
unusual  environments”  has  conducted  the  first  detailed  field  investigation  of  the  species. 
The  work  was  carried  out  on  the  Baro  River  at  Gambela,  Ethiopia  from  24  January-6  April 
1977,  and  this  publication  is  the  fascinating  product  of  these  efforts. 

The  principal  subject  of  the  research  is  evident  from  the  title;  the  plover-crocodile  asso- 
ciation was  only  of  minor  concern.  Nonetheless,  Howell  provides  a brief  but  informative 
historical  review  of  the  latter,  commencing  with  Herodotus  who  visited  Egypt  in  459  BC  and 
“wrote  of  a bird  called  the  Trochilos”  which  foraged  inside  “the  gaping  mouths  of  basking 
crocodiles.”  Although  Howell  never  observed  this  behavior,  he  concludes  from  the  published 
evidence  that  it  probably  occurs,  and  that  it  “may  have  been  more  frequent  and  widespread 
in  earlier  times,  when  both  crocodiles  and  Egyptian  Plovers  were  common  aU  along  the 
Nile.”  The  interesting  point  is  made  that  Herodotus’  Trochilos  cannot  be  identified  with 
certainty  as  the  Egyptian  Plover;  it  may  have  been  one  of  the  spur-winged  plovers  {Hoplop- 
terus  sp.)  or  some  other  species. 

Any  sense  of  disappointment  that  the  question  of  plover-crocodile  symbiosis  was  not  re- 
solved once  and  for  all  is  overwhelmed  by  the  wealth  of  information  on  breeding  biology 
contained  in  the  remainder  of  the  publication.  Moreover,  Howell’s  presentation  is  so  logically 
structured  and  devoid  of  prolixity  that  a thoroughly  lucid  exposition  of  a large  amount  of  data 
is  packed  within  a relatively  short  monograph.  Much  of  the  data  concern  the  remarkable 
egg-burying  behavior  of  the  Egyptian  Plover  which  is  central  to  a number  of  ecophysiological 
and  behavioral  adaptations  of  the  species.  The  salient  points  are  described  below. 

The  eggs  (usual  clutch  is  2 or  3)  are  laid  in  a scrape  and  kept  covered  with  sand  (to  a 
depth  of  2-3  mm  above  their  upper  surfaces)  during  the  day.  Adults  toss  the  sand  over  the 
eggs  with  their  beaks.  Howell  quantified  the  thermoregulatory  parameters  of  nesting  behavior 
by  simultaneously  recording  nest  temperatures  (using  thermocouple-implanted  eggs)  and 
relevant  ambient  termperatures.  The  pattern  which  emerged  demonstrated  that  incubation 
is  a “balanced  combination  of  body  heat,  solar  heat,  and  heat  retained  by  the  sand.”  During 
the  periods  from  sunrise  to  about  10:00  and  from  about  16:00  to  sunset,  the  latter  two  heat 
sources  incubate  the  eggs,  allowing  the  adults  time  to  rest  and  feed.  During  the  hottest  part 
of  the  day  (approximately  10:00  to  16:00)  the  eggs  are  in  imminent  danger  of  being  overheated 
and  solar  input  must  be  moderated.  Simple  shading  of  the  nest  is  inadequate;  instead  the 
adults  make  frequent  trips  to  the  nearby  river  where  they  soak  their  ventral  feathers,  then 
quickly  return  and  settle  on  the  nest  thereby  moistening  the  eggs  and  surrounding  sand.  The 
resultant  evaporative  cooling  keeps  the  eggs  below  lethal  temperatures.  Somehow  the  adults 
monitor  nest  temperatures  (possibly  with  the  beak),  and  their  behavior  varies  accordingly. 
On  cloudy  days,  for  example,  they  stop  wetting  the  eggs  and  regulation  of  nest  temperatures 
is  through  other  behaviors,  such  as  varying  the  depth  of  the  sand  cover  and/or  settling  on 
the  nest.  With  the  chill  of  night,  an  adult  scrapes  the  sand  (using  its  feet)  away  from  the 
eggs  and  incubates  them  with  the  direct  application  of  body  heat.  After  hatching,  the  egg- 


570 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


571 


burying  and  nest-wetting  behaviors  carry  over  to  the  chicks.  The  latter  are  highly  precocial 
and  respond  to  potential  danger  hy  flattening  themselves  on  the  ground  whereupon  the  adults 
cover  them  with  sand.  The  chick-burying  habit  appears  to  be  unique  among  birds,  and  is 
often  so  thorough  that  an  observer  cannot  find  them.  While  buried,  the  young  are  frequently 
wetted  by  the  adults.  This  has  significance  in  meeting  the  thermal  stresses  of  early  life, 
especially  during  the  hottest  hours  of  the  day.  The  foregoing  behaviors  are  nicely  illustrated 
by  accompanying  plates. 

Since  Egyptian  Plover  eggs  are  small  (9.5  g)  relative  to  body  weight  (78  g)  and  incubation 
is  protracted  (30  days),  Howell  devotes  a considerable  portion  of  the  monograph  to  the 
physiology  of  incubation.  He  hypothesizes  that  a lengthy  period  of  incubation  is  “adaptively 
advantageous”  since  it  produces  a highly  precocial  chick  able  to  survive  in  a difficult  envi- 
ronment, and  that  the  nest-wetting  habit  allows  the  small  egg  to  withstand  extended  incu- 
bation without  excessive  dehydration. 

The  text  is  replete  with  a weU-reasoned  evolutionary  theme  concerning  the  adaptive  values 
of  the  Egyptian  Plover’s  breeding  habits.  Howell  also  uses  his  findings  in  an  interesting 
“attempt  to  reconstruct  the  phylogeny”  of  the  species.  This  outstanding  monograph  merits 
the  attention  of  most  avian  biologists. — OsCAR  W.  JOHNSON. 


The  Peregrine  Falcon.  By  Derek  Ratcliffe.  Ulus,  by  Donald  Watson.  Buteo  Books,  P.O. 
Box  481,  Vermillion,  South  Dakota  57069,  416  pp.,  4 color  plates,  60  black-and-white  pho- 
tographs, numerous  drawings,  16  numbered  text  figs.,  23  tables.  $42.50. — This  is  a detailed 
account  of  the  biology  of  the  British  Peregrine  Falcon  (Falco  peregrinus),  surely  the  most 
thoroughly  studied  population  of  this  species  in  the  world.  Major  chapters  are  devoted  to 
distribution,  abundance,  food,  breeding  behavior,  population  regulation  and  dynamics,  and 
to  man-caused  impacts  endured  by  the  bird  in  historical  times.  The  author  claims  the  book 
has  “no  pretentions  of  scientific  sophistication.”  Nevertheless,  the  vast  data  are  exhaustively 
presented  in  table  form  and  are  tightly  reviewed. 

Ratcliffe  has  gone  far  beyond  his  own  experience  with  the  peregrine  and  provides  a per- 
spective based  on  both  the  formal  and  casual  observations  of  hundreds  of  people.  The  book 
is  probably  the  best  single  way  yet  to  find  out  what  peregrines  are  all  about. 

This  book  leaves  the  reader  with  two  major  impressions.  One  is  the  thoroughness  with 
which  the  author  and  his  many  co-workers  have  recorded  field  data  on  over  950  eyries  in 
England,  Scotland  and  Wales.  The  other  is  the  remarkable  resiliency  shown  by  peregrines 
in  habitat  so  populated  by  people  with  varied  interests  in  the  bird.  Falconry,  first  practiced 
by  the  Saxons  in  the  9th  Century,  flourished  into  the  1800’s.  Peregrine  territories  apparently 
remained  at  an  upper  numerical  limit  despite  protection  by  nobility  in  that  period.  Between 
1770  and  the  Second  World  War  gamekeepers  destroyed  large  numbers  of  peregrines,  mainly 
on  behalf  of  the  Red  Grouse  {Lagopus  lagopus).  In  one  small  locality  alone  in  Scotland,  98 
peregrines  were  killed  between  1837  and  1840.  Egg  collectors,  first  active  about  1840,  took 
several  hundred  clutches  in  England  such  that  few  young  fledged  in  some  districts  between 
1900  and  1960.  Between  1925  and  1959  racing  pigeon  fanciers  waged  war  on  peregrines.  As 
before,  immigration  from  more  remote  areas  prevailed  and  territory  occupancy  was  little 
affected.  From  1939-1945  the  Air  Ministry  organized  destruction  of  nests  to  protect  carrier 
pigeons,  resulting  in  a brief  13%  decline  of  the  national  peregrine  population.  Pesticides 
created  a more  severe  reduction.  Compounds  such  as  dieldrin  apparently  increased  adult 
mortality,  and  coupled  with  DDE-related  egg  breakage,  evoked  a population  crash  to  44% 
of  pre-war  numbers  in  the  period  1955-1964.  Following  a ban  on  dieldrin  and  greatly  reduced 
use  of  DDT,  the  population  had  increased  to  75%  of  the  pre-war  level  by  1979.  The  author 


572 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


meticulously  unravels  these  events,  accounts  which  give  cause  for  some  optimism  concerning 
reduced  peregrine  populations  elsewhere. 

Ratcliffe  focuses  on  the  British  bird  but  provides  a listing  of  the  recent  status  of  peregrines 
in  European  countries.  Spain  appears  to  have  the  only  remaining  intact  population.  He 
applauds  the  recovery  effort  through  captive  breeding  in  North  America,  suggests  the  tech- 
nique might  restore  European  peregrines,  but  points  to  the  loophole  it  provides  in  Britain  by 
sheltering  wild  eggs  taken  illegally. 

There  are  exceedingly  few  apparent  errors  or  misinterpretations  in  the  book.  It  is  Town- 
send’s ground  squirrel  (Spermophilus  townsendii),  not  Richardson’s  ground  squirrel  (S.  rich- 
ardsonii),  that  figures  so  importantly  into  the  diet  of  the  raptors  of  the  Snake  River  Birds  of 
Prey  Area  in  Idaho.  The  author  attributes  the  habit  of  feeding  exclusively  on  small  alcids  in 
the  Queen  Charlotte  Islands  as  a unique  subspecies  specialization  of  the  Peale’s  Falcon 
{F.  p.  pealei).  He  overlooked  the  fact  that  about  one-third  of  prey  taken  by  that  race  in  the 
Aleutians  are  not  alcids.  Even  in  coastal  Scotland  peregrines  catch  what  is  handy;  puffins 
(Fratercula  arctica)  are  much  the  favored  prey  there.  A chap  by  the  name  of  Tom  Speedy 
reported  to  Ratchffe  that  one  coastal  eyrie  was  “a  perfect  holocaust  of  Puffins.” 

The  author  admits  that  very  few  artists  catch  the  real  essence  of  the  peregrine.  Some  of 
the  paintings  in  this  book  are  stylized  and  reminiscent  of  the  work  of  Allan  Brooks.  Some 
are  especially  nice,  among  these  the  black-and-white  washes  that  are  scattered  in  the  text. 
The  photographs  are  superb. 

The  last  chapter,  entitled  “Conservation  and  the  future”  falls  short  of  what  I feel  the 
author  might  have  said  at  a time  when  peregrines  in  aU  but  a few  places  in  the  world  are 
threatened.  Is  it  not  appropriate  that  people  such  as  Ratchffe,  who  understand  the  bird  best, 
give  clear  recommendations  on  the  course  conservation  efforts  must  take?  He  might  have 
used  his  careful  objectivity  to  explore  the  usefulness  of  recovery  strategies  and  to  balance 
the  uses  of  peregrines  by  people  against  the  necessity  of  assuring  stable  wild  populations. 
Even  after  the  recovery  in  Britain  it  is  difficult  for  people  to  enjoy  the  bird  because  of 
regulations  and  surveillance. 

In  the  preface,  ten  or  so  of  the  major  European  and  North  American  works  on  peregrines 
are  briefly  reviewed.  This  book  surely  ranks  among  the  better  of  these  in  terms  of  scope, 
information  content  and  readability. — James  H.  Enderson. 


A Naturalist  on  a Tropical  Farm.  By  Alexander  F.  Skutch.  Ulus,  by  Dana  Gardner. 
Univ.  California  Press,  Berkeley,  California,  1980:405  pp.,  many  line  drawings.  $16.95. — Of 
23  chapters  in  this  book,  six  are  concerned  primarily  with  particular  species  of  wild  birds, 
two  with  wild  mammals  and  two  with  insects.  Much  of  the  book  concerns  plants,  and  all  of 
it  concerns  the  interesting  experiences  of  an  observer  unique  in  the  depth  and  breadth  of  his 
interests  and  biological  knowledge.  In  addition  to  the  chapters  on  particular  species  (Bico- 
lored antbird  [Gymnopithys  bicolor].  Hermit  Hummingbird,  Speckled  Tanager  [Tangara  gut- 
tata], Golden-naped  Woodpecker  [Melanerpes  chrysauchen],  Gray-necked  Wood  Rail  [Ar- 
amides  cajanea]  and  White-whiskered  Softwing  [Malacoptila  panamensis]),  virtually  every 
chapter  contains  interesting  observations  on  birds,  espeically  on  bird-plant  relationships,  as 
well  as  data  on  migrants  and  winter  birds  from  temperate  breeding  grounds,  as  well  as  tropical 
species.  The  data  on  the  Bicolored  Antbird  is  largely  the  same  as  in  Skutch  (1969  Pacific  Coast 
Avifauna  35).  As  the  author  acknowledges,  six  of  the  chapters  were  published  earlier 
elsewhere,  but  are  not  all  readily  available  now. 

It  seems  ironic  that  Skutch  has  better  data  on  certain  aspects  of  the  life  history  (e.g., 
duration  of  parental  care  after  nesting,  specific  food  of  young  and  adult  birds)  for  some 
tropical  species  than  American  ornithologists  have  been  able  to  acquire  over  the  years  for 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE 


573 


many  temperate  zone  breeders.  Conservation  being  what  it  is  in  much  of  tropical  America, 
we  should  be  glad  that  Skutch  was  there  to  make  the  observations.  This  book,  however,  is 
not  intended  as  a technical  reference.  It  is  to  be  read  for  enjoyment,  and  the  author’s  writing 
style  seems  generally  cheerful.  The  line  drawings  by  Dana  Gardner  are  pleasing  and  add 
greatly  to  the  reader’s  perception  of  the  places  and  animals  discussed. 

Anyone  planning  to  visit  Central  America  could  profit  from  the  book,  and  should  pay 
special  attention  to  Chapter  2 on  the  patterns  of  weather,  the  phenology  of  a tropical  year. 

First  and  last,  the  book  is  a statement  of  philosophy  from  a sensitive  man  in  a world  that 
seems  bent  on  destruction.  AU  of  us  wish  for  a world  without  suffering,  but  recognize  the 
futility  of  the  wish.  Skutch  perhaps  has  thought  about  it  more  than  most.  The  summary  of 
his  views  in  the  final  chapter  of  the  book  is  worth  reading  and  thinking  about,  but  it  is  like 
wishing  that  the  earth  was  another  planet. — Richard  R.  Graber. 


Handbook  of  the  Birds  of  India  and  Pakistan.  Vol.  2,  2nd  ed.  By  Salim  Ah  and  S. 
Dillon  Ripley.  Oxford  University  Press,  London  (England)  and  New  York  (New  York), 
1980:347  pp.,  13  color  plates,  numerous  drawings  and  maps.  $34.00. — This  is  the  second  of 
the  ten-volume  series  on  birds  of  the  Indian  subcontinent  to  be  reprinted.  A few  small 
changes  have  been  made  in  the  text  and  three  color  plates  have  been  replaced,  but  otherwise 
this  is  the  same  book  first  published  in  1969.  This  volume  covers  the  orders  GaUiformes, 
Gruiformes  and  Charadriiformes.  Each  species  is  described  concisely  in  a standard  format 
including  English,  scientific  and  local  names,  size,  field  characters,  status,  distribution  and 
habitat,  general  habits,  food,  voice  and  calls,  breeding,  and  museum  diagnosis.  In  addition, 
keys  and  distribution  maps  are  provided  for  some  groups.  As  in  Volume  1,  recently  reprinted, 
this  book  is  printed  on  cheap  paper.  This  no  doubt  keeps  the  price  down,  but  how  long  wiU 
it  last? — Robert  J.  Raikow. 


Finding  Birds  Around  the  World.  By  Peter  Alden  and  John  Gooders.  Houghton  Mifflin 
Co.,  Boston,  Massachusetts,  1981:683  pp.,  numerous  maps.  $17.95. — Two  widely-traveled 
observers  provide  a guide  to  111  of  the  best  and  most  accessible  places  to  see  birds  around 
the  world.  For  each  area  there  is  a description,  information  useful  to  travellers,  a map  and 
a checkhst.  The  index  lists  the  birds  so  that  you  can  find  out  where  to  go  to  see  the  species 
you  are  after.  Now  aU  we  need  is  a guide  to  financing  worldwide  birding  trips.  The  royalties 
from  books  like  this? — R.J.R. 


Chimney  Swifts  and  Their  Relatives.  By  Margaret  Whittemore.  Nature  Books  Pub- 
lishers, Jackson,  Mississippi,  1981:169  pp.,  numerous  drawings  and  black-and-white  pho- 
tographs, paper  cover.  $5.95. — A long-time  observer’s  anecdotes  and  musings  on  the  life  of 
swifts.  The  photographs  are  mostly  blurred  or  muddy. — R.J.R. 


The  “Mid-South  Bird  Notes”  of  Ben  B.  Coffey,  Jr.  By  Jerome  A.  Jackson  (ed.). 
Special  Publication  No.  1,  Mississippi  Ornithological  Society,  1981:127  pp.,  paper  cover. 
Order  from  Mississippi  Ornithological  Society,  % Dept,  of  Biological  Sciences,  Box  Z,  Mis- 
sissippi State,  Mississippi  39762.  $10.00. — This  is  a compilation  of  bird  records  collected 
and  privately  published  by  Coffey  from  1952-1956,  when  there  were  no  state  ornithological 
societies  in  Arkansas  or  Mississippi. — R.J.R. 


574 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


W ater  and  Shore  Birds.  By  W'alther  Thiede.  Chatto  & Windus,  London,  United  King- 
dom. Distributed  in  the  U.S.  by  Merrimack  Book  Service,  99  Main  St.,  Salem,  New  Hamp- 
shire 03078,  1981:143  pp.,  approx.  128  color  plates,  paper  cover.  $5.95. — Intended  as  a field 
guide  to  the  commoner  shore  and  water  birds  of  Europe,  this  book  includes  accounts  of 
about  117  species  of  loons,  grebes.  Fulmar,  Gannet,  cormorants,  herons,  storks,  ducks, 
geese,  swans,  rails,  waders,  gulls,  terns  and  auks.  Its  value  to  American  readers  will  lie  in 
the  excellent  set  of  color  photographs  of  the  species  taken  in  their  natural  surroundings. — 
R.J.R. 


Pheasants  in  Asia  1979.  By  Christopher  Savage  (ed.).  The  World  Pheasant  Association, 
1 Harraton  Square,  Church  Lane,  Exning,  Suffolk  CB8  7HA,  United  Kingdom,  1980:116  pp., 
numerous  black-and-white  illustrations.  £8.50. — This  paperback  book  contains  the  proceed- 
ings of  the  First  International  Symposium  on  Pheasants  in  Asia,  held  in  Nepal  in  November 
of  1979.  It  includes  some  29  papers  and  other  reports  on  the  status  of  pheasants  in  Asia, 
field  study  techniques,  captive  breeding  of  pheasants  and  conservation  management. — 
R.J.R. 

Birds  of  Regina.  Revised  Edition.  By  Margaret  Belcher,  iUus.  by  Fred  W . Lahrman. 
Special  Publication  No.  12,  Saskatchewan  Natural  History  Society,  Regina,  Saskatchewan, 
Canada,  1980:151  pp.,  numerous  black-and-white  photos  and  drawings,  paper  cover.  $5.00  + 
$0.50  postage. — Records  of  the  occurrence,  abundance  and  breeding  density  of  birds  in 
the  City  of  Regina  and  the  surrounding  farmlands  in  a 48  km  (30  mile)  radius. 

Birds  of  the  Qu’Appelle,  1857-1979.  By  E.  Manley  Callin.  Special  Publication  No.  13, 
Saskatchewan  Natural  History  Society,  Regina,  Saskatchewan,  Canada,  1980:168  pp.,  4 
maps,  10  black-and-white  habitat  pbotos,  paper  cover.  $7.(X)  + $0.50  postage. — An  anno- 
tated list  of  bird  records  from  the  Qu’Appelle  River  Valley  east  of  the  City  of  Regina, 
Saskatchewan. 

Nature  Saskatoon:  An  Account  of  the  Saskatoon  Natural  History  Society, 
1955-1980.  By  C.  Stuart  Houston.  Saskatoon  Natural  History  Society,  Saskatoon,  Saskatch- 
ewan, Canada,  1980:46  pp.,  7 black-and-white  photos,  paper  cover.  $2.50  + $0.50  postage. 

These  three  books  may  be  ordered  by  mail  from  the  Blue  Jay  Bookshop,  Box  1121,  Regina, 
Saskatchewan  S4P  3B4  Canada. — Robert  J.  Raikow. 


Avian  Mortality  at  Man-Made  Structures:  An  Annotated  Bibliography  (Re- 
vised). By  Michael  L.  Avery,  Paul  F.  Springer  and  Nancy  S.  Dailey.  U.S.  Fish  and  Wildlife 
Service,  Biological  Services  Program,  National  Power  Plant  Team,  FWS/OBS-80/54, 
1980:152  pp.  $5.50. — Revision  of  FWS/OBS-78/55  (1978).  Includes  1042  references,  of  which 
189  are  new.  Available  from  the  Superintendent  of  Documents,  Washington,  D.C.,  as  Stock 
No.  024-010-00560-2.— R.J.R. 


Erratum. — Vol.  93,  No.  2,  “Cowbird  parasitism  and  evolution  of  anti-parasite  strate- 
gies in  the  Yellow  Warbler”  by  Karen  L.  Clark  and  Raleigh  J.  Robertson.  Table  4, 
p.  253,  sub-headings  of  “Frequency  of  response”  should  read  “buried,”  “deserted”  and 
“accepted.” — KC 


PROCEEDINGS  OF  THE  SIXTY-SECOND 


575 


ANNUAL  MEETING 

Curtis  S.  Adkisson,  Secretary 

The  Sixty-second  Annual  Meeting  of  the  The  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
was  held  Thursday,  4 June  to  Sunday,  7 June,  1981,  at  Mount  Allison  Univer- 
sity, Sackville,  New  Brunswick.  The  hosts  for  the  meeting  were:  Canadian 

Wildlife  Service,  Atlantic  Region;  Mount  Allison  University;  and  the  Chig- 
necto  Naturalists'  Club.  Dr.  Anthony  J.  Erskine  chaired  the  local  arrange- 
ments committee.  One  hundred  nine  persons  attended  the  meeting. 

The  meeting  opened  with  registration  Thursday  evening  followed  by  a wine 
and  cheese  reception  in  Hessler  Hall  of  the  university.  On  Friday  morning 
the  society  was  welcomed  by  Dr.  C.  W.  J.  Elliot,  Acting  Academic  Vice  Presi- 
dent of  Mount  Allison  University.  President  Geroge  A.  Hall  responded  for 
the  society.  After  the  first  business  meeting,  the  paper  sessions  began. 

The  annual  banquet  was  held  in  Jennings  Hall  on  Saturday  night.  Dr.  R. 

G.  B.  Brown  of  the  Canadian  Wildlife  Service  was  the  banquet  speaker.  His 
talk,  summarizing  the  state  of  the  art  in  seabird  research  in  eastern  Canada, 
was  illustrated  by  many  excellent  slides. 

At  the  banquet.  President-elect  Abbot  S.  Gaunt  announced  the  following 
awards : 

EDWARDS  PRIZES  (for  best  papers  appearing  in  The  W^ilson  Bulletin  in  1980) 

1)  First  Edwards  Prize  to  M.  Ross  Lein,  "Display  behavior  of  Ovenbirds 

(Seiurus  aurocapillus) : 1 . Non-song  vocalizations"  (Wilson  Bulle- 

tin 92:312-329) . 

2)  Second  Edwards  Prize  to  James  F.  Wittenberger , "Feeding  of  secondary 

nestlings  by  polygynous  male  Bobolinks  in  Oregon"  (Wilson  Bulletin 
92:330-340) . 

MARGARET  MORSE  NICE  AWARD 

Linda  Heald,  "Behavioral  plasticity  in  a tyrannid  flycatcher:  effects  of 

environmental  variability" 

LOUIS  AGASSIZ  FUERTES  GRANT 

Mark  K.  Wourms,  "Avian  predator  discrimination  of  prey  flight  patterns" 
PAUL  A.  STEWART  AWARDS 

Kenneth  F.  Abraham,  "Winter  distribution  of  eastern  Arctic  Brant  geese 
and  the  role  of  the  family  in  locality-use  traditions" 

David  E.  Blockstein,  "Reproductive  behavior  and  parental  investment  in 
the  Mourning  Dove  (Zenaida  macroura) " 


576 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


Roger  L.  Boyd,  "Population  movements  of  Snowy  Plovers" 

Shari  Hahn,  "Parent-offspring  recognition  in  Cliff  Swallows" 

Katherine  J.  Kuletz,  "Breeding  success  in  relation  to  foraging  patterns  of 
Pigeon  Guillemots  (Cepphus  columba) , on  Naked  Island,  Prince  William 
Sound,  Alaska" 

Thomas  E.  Martin,  "Migrants  and  the  dynamics  of  the  bird  community  of  a 
neotropical  second-growth  woodland" 

Virginia  M.  Vitt,  "The  functional  basis  of  roosting  behavior  in  the  Common 
Crackle  (Quiscalus  quiscula)" 

Joseph  M.  Wunderle,  "Uncertainty  and  the  foraging  behavior  of  bananaquits 
on  artificial  flowers" 

ALEXANDER  WILSON  PRIZE  (for  the  best  student  paper  at  this  meeting) 

H.  Carolyn  Peach,  "The  foraging  ecology  of  adult  and  juvenile  Semipalmated 
Plover  (Charadrius  semipalmatus  Bonaparte)  on  the  Starrs  Point  Mudflat, 
Minas  Basin,  Bay  of  Fundy" 

There  were  field  trips  to  Amherst  Point  Bird  Sanctuary  and  Ram  Pasture  Salt 
Marsh.  On  Sunday  there  was  an  excursion  to  the  Upper  Bay  of  Fundy  shore.  On 
Sunday  there  were  all-day  trips  to  Cape  Tryon,  Prince  Edward  Island,  and  Fundy 
National  Park,  New  Brunswick. 

FIRST  BUSINESS  MEETING 

The  first  business  meeting,  held  on  5 June  1981,  was  presided  over  by  President 
George  A.  Hall.  He  announced  the  posting  of  the  list  of  new  members,  and  ap- 
pointed the  Alexander  Wilson  Prize  Committee,  consisting  of  Keith  Bildstein, 
George  Kulesza,  and  James  Rising,  and  the  Auditing  Committee,  consisting  of 
William  Klamm  and  Hubert  Zernichow.  He  also  announced  that  anyone  interested 
in  submitting  resolutions  to  be  published  as  part  of  this  meeting  should  con- 
tact Helen  Lapham  or  Kathleen  Anderson  of  the  Resolutions  Committee.  Secretary 
Curtis  S.  Adkisson  summarized  actions  taken  at  the  1981  meeting  of  the  Council, 
held  on  Thursday,  4 June.  Jon  C.  Barlow  was  re-elected  Editor  of  The  Wilson 
Bulletin.  It  is  the  Council's  intent  to  have  a list  of  the  holdings  of  the  Van 
Tyne  Memorial  Library  as  well  as  the  recommendations  of  the  Conservation  Commit- 
h?e  published  in  The  Wilson  Bulletin  in  the  near  future.  Council  recognizes  that 
planning  should  begin  soon  for  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  centennial  in 
1988.  President  Hall  then  presided  over  the  passage  by  voice  vote  of  an  amend- 
ment to  the  Bylaws  (see  Wilson  Bulletin  92:465)  such  that  membership  applica- 
tions be  sent  to  the  Treasurer  instead  of  to  the  Secretary. 

The  final  item  of  business  at  this  meeting  was  the  Treasurer's  Report,  by 


Robert  D.  Burns. 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


577 


REPORT  OF  THE  TREASURER 
1 JANUARY  1980  TO  31  DECEMBER  1980 
GENERAL  FUNDS 


RECEIPTS 


Student  and  Regular  Membership  Dues  Collected  in  1980 


For  1980  $10,779.58 

For  1981  15,284.50 

Family  Memberships  for  1981  292.00 

Sustaining  Membership  for  1981  . . . 1 ,462.00 


TOTAL  DUES $27,818.08 

Subscriptions  to  THE  WILSON  BULLETIN 

For  1980  6,181.00 

For  1981  5,477.50 


TOTAL  SUBSCRIPTIONS  11,658.50 

Back  Issues  of  THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  338.50 

Interest  and  Dividends  12,719.78 

Royalties  808.63 

Contributtions  from  Authors  2,339.17 

Contributions  to  Special  President's  Fund  100.00 

Contributions  to  the  General  Fund  445.00 

Contributions  and  Life  Memberships  to  Endowment  2,864 . 50 

TOTAL  RECEIPTS  - Jan.  1,  1980  to  Dec.  31  , 1980  $59,092.16 

DISBURSEMENTS 

WILSON  BULLETIN 


December  $ 8,983.59 

March 8,874.10 

June 9,005.82 

September  8,969.40 

Colorplates  3,366.20 

Insurance 23.00 

Reprint  Back  Issues  1 , 543 . 52 


TOTAL  WILSON  BULLETIN  PRODUCTION  COSTS  $40,765.63 

Additions  to  Endowment  Trust  at  Central  Counties  Bank  ....  1,700.00 

Endowment  Funds  Transferred  to  Stewart  Fund  800.00 

Endowment  Funds  Transferred  to  Wilson  Prize  100.00 


Dues  to  International  Council  for  Bird  Preservation  1979  & 1980  200.00 


578 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


DISBURSEMENTS  (continued) 

Editor's  Expenses  

Treasurer's  Expenses  (200  stamps)  

Secretary's  Expenses  (stationery)  

Ornithological  Societies  of  North  America  $9,678.72 
Grant  from  ARGO  plus  AOU  Refund  (1,854.63) 

TOTAL  DISBURSEMENTS  - Jan.  1,  1980  to  Dec.  31,  1980  . 

CASH  ACCOUNTS 

Checking  Account,  31  December  1980  . . $ 9,210.55 

Savings  Account,  31  December  1980  . . . 16,644.65 

Total  Cash  on  Hand  

DESIGNATED  ACCOUNTS 
VAN  TYNE  MEMORIAL  LIBRARY  FL^ND 


RECEIPTS 

Balance  1979  $ 1,283.39 

Sales  and  Gifts  ....  679.59 

DISBURSEMENTS 

Purchase  of  Books  788.40 

Balance  


LOUIS  AGASSIZ  FUERTES  RESEARCH  FUND 

RECEIPTS 

Gifts  200.00 

DISBURSEMENTS 

Gregory  S.  Butcher  200.00 

MARGARET  MORSE  NICE  R’ND 

RECEIPTS 

Gift  100.00 

DISBURSEMENTS 

Nancy  Newfield  100.00 

ALEXANDER  WILSON  PRIZE 

RECEIPTS 

From  Endowment  100.00 

DISBURSEMENTS 

M.  W.  Collopy  and  K.  Collopy  100.00 


$ 479.00 

30.00 
36.05 

. 7,824.09 
$51,934.77 


$25,855.20 


$ 1,174.58 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


579 


ERNEST  P.  EDWARDS  PRIZE 


RECEIPTS 

E.  P.  Edwards $ 350.00 

DISBURSEMENTS 

Ellen  D.  Ketterson 225.00 

Charles  R.  Brown  125.00 


PAUL  A.  STEWART  AWARDS 


RECEIPTS 

From  Endowment  800.00 

DISBURSEMENTS 

Gregory  A.  Perkins  200.00 

Scott.  R.  Winterstein  200.00 

Bruce  B.  Edinger 200.00 

Diane  E.  Riska 200.00 


AARON  BAGG  STUDENT  MEMBERSHIP  AWARD  FUND 


RECEIPTS 

Balance  from  1979  30.00 

Gift 200.00 

DISBURSEMENTS 

20  Student  Awards  200.00 

Balance  


RECEIPTS 


ENDOWMENT 


Value  of  General  Endowment 

Fund  Dec.  31  , 1979  132,812.78 

Life  Membership  Payments  and  Gifts  . . 2,864.54 

Appreciation  on  Principle  2,174.72 


Value  of  General  Endowment  Fund  Dec.  31,  1980  . . . 

GEORGE  M.  SUTTON  GOLORPLATE  FUND 

Value  

Total  combined  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 

Endowment  Funds  December  31,  1980  

Earnings  from  Endowment  for  1980  

Rate  of  return  for  1980  7.71% 


$ 30.00 


$137,852.04 

27,121.25 

164,973.29 

12,719.78 


580 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


SECOND  BUSINESS  MEETING 

The  second  business  meeting  was  convened  by  President  Hall  on  Saturday 
afternoon,  6 June  1981.  The  proposed  new  members  were  elected  unanimously. 

Hubert  Zernichow  presented  the  Auditors'  Report,  which  was  accepted  by  the 
membership.  Four  resolutions  presented  by  the  Resolutions  Committee,  after 
some  debate  and  amendment,  were  approved.  These,  and  summaries  of  committee 
reports  presented  at  the  meeting  of  Council,  are  included  below. 

AUDITING  COMMITTEE  REPORT  - 1980 

We,  the  undersigned,  have  examined  the  Treasurer's  records,  bank  statements, 
certificate  of  deposit,  cancelled  checks,  and  other  financial  records  of  the 
Society  covering  the  period  1 January  1980  to  31  December  1980. 

Our  examination  confirms  that  receipts  and  disbursements  have  been  correctly 
accounted  for,  and  bank  balances  are  in  agreement  with  the  Treasurer's  state- 
ment . 

Hubert  P.  Zernichow,  member 
William  A.  Klamm,  member 

EDITOR'S  REPORT  - 1980 

The  Wilson  Bulletin  editorial  staff  processed  330  manuscripts  in  1980,  in- 
cluding 150  received  as  new  in  that  year.  The  higher  quality  of  papers  submit- 
ted is  reflected  in  a 10.4%  decrease  in  rejection  rate.  Volume  92  contained 
33  major  articles,  and  49  notes,  for  a total  of  564  pages. 

There  is  good  material  on  hand  for  color  plates  for  the  next  several  issues. 

In  addition,  access  has  been  granted  to  several  previously  unpublished  paintings 
of  the  late  L.  A.  Fuertes. 

I have  geen  greatly  helped  by  people  in  the  editorial  office  and  elsewhere, 
including  K.  C.  Parkes,  Keith  Bildstein,  and  David  Ankney  (editorial  assistants), 
John  O'Neill,  George  Miksch  Sutton,  William  Lunk,  Sandra  Gaunt,  A.  S.  Gaunt, 
George  Hall,  Peter  Stettenheim,  and  Guy  Dresser  and  Ken  Blair  (both  of  Allen 
Press)  . I continue  to  enjoy  working  with  The  Wilson  Bulletin  and  will  be 
pleased  to  continue  as  Editor. 

Jon  C.  Barlow,  Editor 
LIBRARY  COMMITTEE  REPORT  - 1980 

The  Josselyn  Van  Tyne  Memorial  Library  continued  to  run  smoothly  during  the 
1980  calendar  year.  As  in  the  preceding  year,  149  journals  and  newsletters 
were  received  through  115  exchanges;  there  were  35  gifts  and  complimentary 
subscriptions,  plus  2 regular  subscriptions.  This  adds  up  to  186  titles. 

Col.  L.  R.  Wolfe  deserves  our  special  thanks  for  his  regular  donation  of 
Raptor  Research  and  of  other  journal  issues. 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


581 


Work  on  the  translation  file  goes  on;  this  adjunct  to  our  library  should 
more  and  more  help  workers  to  at  least  some  insight  into  foreign  publications 
in  the  field.  Sales  of  duplicates  during  the  year  brought  in  $263.00  to  our 
New  Book  Fund;  $460  was  spent  from  the  fund,  to  purchase  49  new  books  and  jour- 
nals deemed  to  be  critically  needed.  Contributions  of  cash,  as  well  as  of 
duplicates  for  sale,  would  always  be  most  welcome. 

Loans  to  61  members  involved  380  items  and  97  separate  shipments.  This,  it 
must  be  understood,  is  in  addition  to  the  constant  on-the-spot  usage  the  coll- 
ection receives.  Member  participation  is  still  the  essential  basis  of  the 
library's  growth:  2175  items  by  26  members;  46  books,  1380  reprints,  347  re- 

ports and  pamphlets,  272  journal  issues,  16  translations,  76  maps,  and  4 
theses . 

Donors  recorded  were  W.  and  D.  Behling,  A.  J.  Berger  (again,  1035  items, 
or  nearly  half  the  total!),  C.  R.  Brown,  F.  Hamerstrom,  H.  Harrison,  C.  J. 
Henny,  J.  Hinshaw,  T.  H.  Hubbell,  A.  S.  Hyde,  P.  Johnsgard , L.  Kelso,  F.  E. 
Lohrer,  H.  Mayfield,  R.  B.  Payne,  B.  Pinkowski,  S.  Postupalsky,  K.  Prescott, 

W.  J.  Richardson,  D.  Siegel-Causey , A.  Simon,  R.  W.  Storer,  J.  Strauch,  Jr., 

J.  Tate,  Jr.,  S.  Wilbur,  J.  E.  Willoughby,  and  Col.  L.  R.  Wolfe. 

As  always,  the  committee  thanks  the  entire  membership,  urging  your  contin- 
ued and  increased  support  in  the  years  to  come. 

William  R.  Lunk,  Chairman 
MEMBERSHIP  COMMITTEE  REPORT  - 1980 

Total  paid  membership  for  the  Society  was  2,162  in  calendar  year  1980. 

The  figure  for  1981  is  2,257  ( a net  increase  of  95  or  4.4%).  My  office  has 
handled  a total  of  53  requests  for  membership  applications  since  the  last 
meeting,  as  well  as  about  20  actual  payments.  Requests  for  applications 
were  usually  handled  on  the  same  day,  while  payments  were  forwarded  to  OSNA 
in  Columbus,  Ohio.  To  increase  membership  the  Society  should  have  new  fold- 
ers printed,  and  these  can  be  sent,  along  with  a letter  of  invitation,  to 
members  of  other  ornithological  societies  who  are  not  Wilson  members. 

Robert  C.  Whitmore,  Chairman 

STUDENT  MEMBERSHIP  COMMITTEE  REPORT  - 1980 

Announcement  of  the  Aaron  M.  Bagg  Student  Membership  Awards  was  made  in 
The  Wilson  Bulletin  and  Ornithological  Newsletter.  An  announcement  was  also 
sent  to  205  members  of  the  Society  along  with  application  materials  for  the 
Bagg  Award.  Application  materials  were  also  sent  in  response  to  21  letters 
of  inquiry.  There  were  46  eligible  applicants  for  the  Bagg  Award,  an  increase 
of  92%  over  last  year.  Funds  for  16  awards  were  available.  The  student 


582 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


awardees  and  their  institutions  are  as  follows:  Douglas  A.  Bell,  Westfalische 

Wilhelms-Univ . , Munster,  West  Germany;  Thomas  Bicak,  University  of  Montana; 
Scott  P.  Carroll,  University  of  Oklahoma;  Dominick  A.  Della  Sala,  Wayne  State 
University;  Sylvia  L.  Halkin,  University  of  Oklahoma;  Elizabeth  G.  Henderson, 
University  of  South  Carolina;  William  J.  Hilton,  Jr.,  University  of  Minnesota; 
Mark  L.  Hoffman,  University  of  Florida;  Charles  T.  LaRue,  Northern  Arizona 
University;  David  M.  Lyons,  Frostburg  State  College;  Kelvin  G.  Murray,  Univer- 
sity of  Florida;  James  S.  Quinn,  University  of  Oklahoma;  Roland  L.  Redmond, 
University  of  Montana;  David  E.  Seibel,  University  of  Kansas,  David  A.  Spec- 
tor,  Stockton  State  College;  Kathy  A.  Winnett-Murray , University  of  Florida, 
Nominations  for  student  membership  were  received  for  18  students.  These 
nominees,  as  well  as  the  30  unsuccessful  Bagg  Award  applicants  were  sent  a 
letter  of  invitation  to  join  the  Society.  These  students  also  received  a 
letter  from  Kathy  Groschupf  (VPl&SU,  Blacksburg)  and  her  a^  hoc  committee 
for  student  membership  composed  of  Bette  Schardien  (Mississippi  State)  and 
Susan  Dohlert  (West  Virginia  University) . This  committee  also  sent  a letter 
outlining  the  benefits  of  student  membership  to  100  graduate  departments. 

This  committee's  work  would  be  greatly  aided  by  an  increase  in  WOS  member- 
ship participation  in  the  process  of  nominating  both  potential  Bagg  Awardees 
and  regular  student  members. 

John  L.  Zimmerman,  Chairman 
Thomas  C . Grubb 
Roland  R.  Roth 
Charles  F.  Leek 

REPORT  OF  THE  RESOLUTIONS  COMMITTEE 
The  following  resolutions  were  read  and  passed  at  the  second  business 
meeting : 

WHEREAS  the  continental  shelf  of  Newfoundland,  Nova  Scotia,  and  New  England 
supports  seabirds  from  broad  areas  as  far  apart  as  the  Arctic  Ocean  of  Europe 
and  North  America  and  the  South  Atlantic  Ocean,  as  well  as  providing  renewable 
fisheries  resources  of  critical  importance  to  human  populations,  and, 

WHEREAS  the  exploration,  development,  and  transportation  of  hydrocarbon  resour- 
ces in  the  offshore  areas  are  imminent,  and, 

WHEREAS  the  environmental  impact  of  such  activities,  especially  when  resulting 
in  oil  spills,  poses  a serious  threat  to  marine  life,  especially  birds: 
THEREFORE  BE  IT  RESOLVED  that  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  asks  the  Canad- 
ian and  United  States  governments  to  proceed  with  utmost  caution  in  licensing 
all  aspects  of  such  operations  and  to  ensure  that  the  protection  of  the  marine 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


583 


environment  be  a fundamental  consideration  at  every  stage  of  the  decision- 
making process. 

WHEREAS  the  barrier  islands  and  their  associated  habitats  of  the  United  States 
have  unique  natural,  ecological  and  biological  values  important  to  wildlife 
and  a human  population,  and, 

IfflEREAS  these  values  have  been  recognized  in  the  Department  of  the  Interior's 
1979  "Barrier  Island  Protection  Plan"  under  which  the  Fish  and  Wildlife  Ser- 
vice has  identified  some  fifty  barrier  islands  along  the  Atlantic  and  Gulf 
coasts  with  exceptional  fish  and  wildlife  values,  and, 

WHEREAS  nearly  twenty  federal  agencies  presently  have  programs  affecting  these 
areas  of  which  three-quarters  foster  adverse  effects  through  grants,  loans, 
insurance  and  disaster-relief  programs  with  subsidized  development,  and, 
WHEREAS  the  present  administration  emphasizes  the  need  to  reduce  federal  spend 
ing: 

THEREFORE  BE  IT  RESOLVED  that  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  supports  pend- 
ing bills  H.R.  3252  and  S.  1018  which  seek  to  establish  a barrier  island 
protection  system  to  perpetuate  wildlife  resources  and  which  cut  off  the  sub- 
sidies encouraging  development,  and, 

FURTHERMORE  BE  IT  RESOLVED  that  the  Society  requests  the  administration  to  pro 
vide  a coordinated,  consistent  policy  among  its  agencies  to  preserve  this 
fragile,  valuable  habitat. 

WHEREAS  Ronald  Reagan  while  governor  of  California  showed  commendable  concern 
for  environmental  problems,  and  built  a fine  record  of  achievement  in  conser- 
vation legislation  and  executive  action,  and, 

WHEREAS  since  he  became  President  of  the  United  States  he  has  devoted  most  of 
his  energies  toward  solving  the  current  economic  and  defense  problems,  and  has 
been  further  hampered  in  his  attention  to  other  vital  concerns  by  the  sense- 
less and  lamentable  attempt  on  his  life,  and  has  allowed  many  of  his  appoint- 
ees to  operate  on  their  personal  biases,  which  so  not  reflect  the  wishes  of 
the  great  majority  of  the  American  people,  and, 

WHEREAS  national  surveys  have  shown  that  the  majority  of  American  citizens 
realize  the  environment  is  a complex  and  fragile  natural  system  and  must  be 
protected  from  unwarranted  destruction  and  degradation  - a concern  clearly 
reflected  by  Mr.  Reagan's  past  actions,  and, 

WHEREAS  the  members  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society,  part  of  the  con- 
cerned majority,  believe  the  future  of  humankind  on  this  planet  is  in  the 
hands  of  the  present  generation: 


584 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


THEREFORE  BE  IT  RESOLVED  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  requests  that 
President  Reagan  exert  closer  supervision  of  his  appointees  in  environmental 
agencies : 

FURTHERMORE  BE  IT  RESOLVED  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  encourages  its 
members  to  inform  their  congressional  representatives  and  the  executive  branch 
that  astute  environmental  legislation  provides  long-term  solutions  to  con- 
tinuing problems  and  should  not  be  sacrificed  to  short-term  palliatives  for 
momentary  crises. 

WHEREAS  The  Canadian  Wildlife  Service,  Mount  Allison  University,  and  the 
Chignecto  Naturalists'  Club  have  hosted  the  1981  Wilson  Ornithological  Society 
meeting  in  Sackville,  New  Brunswick,  June  fourth  through  eighth,  and, 

WHEREAS  the  meetings  have  been  conducted  smoothly  and  with  hospitality  on  the 
beautiful  Mount  Allison  campus,  and, 

WHEREAS  the  binding  trips,  the  three  offered  Friday  and  Saturday  mornings  and 
the  two  full-day  trips,  as  well  as  the  especially  enjoyable  spouses'  trips  to 
historical  sites,  have  covered  different  habitats  and  birds,  introducing  this 
beautiful  countryside  and  its  varied  birdlife  to  Wilson  members,  and, 

WHEREAS  the  lobster  boil  sponsored  by  the  government  of  the  province  of  New 
Brunswick  was  a delicious  treat  for  gourmet  and  non-gourmet  alike: 

THEREFORE  BE  IT  RESOLVED  that  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  gives  its 
grateful  thanks  to  these  organizations  and  especially  to  Anthony  Erskine  and 
the  rest  of  the  local  committee  for  the  success  and  enjoyment  of  the  meeting. 

NOMINATING  COMMITTEE  REPORT  - 1980 

As  the  final  item  of  business  the  Nominating  Committee  of  Kathleen  S. 
Anderson,  Jerome  A.  Jackson,  and  Kenneth  C.  Parkes , chairman,  submitted  the 
following  slate  of  officers  for  1981-82:  President,  Abbot  S.  Gaunt;  First 

Vice-President,  Jerome  A.  Jackson;  Second  Vice-President,  Clait  E.  Braun; 
Secretary,  Curtis  S.  Adkisson;  Treasurer,  Robert  D.  Burns;  Council  Member 
for  three-year  term  beginning  at  the  end  of  the  1981  meeting,  Helen  S.  Lapham. 
There  being  no  further  nominations,  it  was  moved,  seconded  and  passed  that 
the  Secretary  be  instructed  to  cast  a unanimous  ballot  for  the  slate. 

PAPERS  SESSION 

The  papers  session  was  organized  by  Jerome  A.  Jackson.  Individual  parts 
of  the  session  were  chaired  by:  Kathleen  Anderson,  Jon  Barlow,  Clait  Braun, 

A.  S.  Gaunt,  Jerome  Jackson,  George  Kulesza,  Helmut  Mueller,  and  Richard 
Stiehl.  A list  of  papers  presented  follows: 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


585 


R.  E.  Simmons,  Acadia  University,  "Polygyny  and  breeding  success  in  the 
Northern  Harrier,  - violation  of  a model?" 

Keith  L.  Bildstein,  Winthrop  College,  "Small  males  and  large  females:  causes 

and  consequences  in  Northern  Harriers" 

Norman  R.  Seymour,  St.  Francis  Xavier  University,  and  Robert  Bancroft, 

Nova  Scotia  Department  of  Lands  and  Forests,  "Use  of  two  habitats  by 
Ospreys  (Pandion  haliaetus)  related  to  changes  in  prey  availability" 

Paul  Kerlinger,  SUNY , Albany,  "Water  crossing  behavior  by  raptors  during 
migration" 

Maurice  N.  Lefranc,  Jr,  and  William  S.  Clark,  Raptor  Information  Center, 

National  Wildlife  Federation,  "Variability  of  wing  loading  in  North  Ameri- 
can birds  of  prey" 

Elizabeth  Henderson,  University  of  South  Carolina  and  Belle  Baruch  Inst., 
"Feeding  behavior  of  adult  White  Ibis  on  a South  Carolina  salt  marsh. 

1.  Habitat  effects" 

Richard  B.  Stiehl,  University  of  Wisconsin-Green  Bay,  "Food  habits  of  nestling 
Common  Ravens" 

P.  0.  Dunn,  M.  A.  McCollough,  and  T.  A.  May,  University  of  Maine-Orono, 

"Length  of  stay  and  fat  reserves  of  Semipalmated  Sandpipers  in  eastern 
Maine" 

Jonas  Hedberg  and  T.  A.  May,  University  of  Maine-Orono,  'Habitat  selection  by 
Spruce  Grouse  in  eastern  Maine" 

Clait  E.  Braun  and  Kenneth  M.  Giesen,  Golorado  Division  of  Wildlife,  "Survival 
of  female  White-tailed  Ptarmigan  in  Colorado" 

James  A.  Mosher  and  Kimberly  Titus,  University  of  Maryland-Frostburg , 

"Principal  components  analysis  of  nest  site  selection  by  the  Broad-winged 
Hawk  (Buteo  platypterus) " 

Bette  J.  Schardien,  Mississippi  State  University,  "Resident  status  and  nesting 
phenology  of  Killdeer  in  Mississippi" 

Rhett  Talbert,  media  specialist,  and  presented  by  Keith  Bildstein,  Belle  Baruch 
Institute,  University  of  South  Carolina,  "Opportunities  for  behavioral  and 
ornithological  research  at  Hobcaw  Barony" 

James  A.  Mosher  and  Kimberly  Titus,  University  of  Maryland-Frostburg,  "A  chance 
corrective  classification  procedure  for  use  in  discriminant  analysis" 

Maurice  N.  Lefranc,  Jr.,  National  Wildlife  Federation,  " The  National  Wildlife 
Federation's  Raptor  Information  Center" 

Robert  J.  O'Hara,  University  of  Massachusetts,  "The  evolution  of  longspurs 
(Emberlzldae) " 

J.  D.  Rising,  University  of  Toronto,  "Tests  of  hypotheses  about  the  evolution 


586 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


of  sexual  dimorphism  in  birds" 

S.  L.  Berman,  College  of  the  Holy  Cross,  "Hind  limb  myology  of  the  Mousebirds" 
Jay  Pitocchelli,  Memorial  University  of  Newfoundland,  "Song  dialects,  and  vocal 
development  in  Savannah  Sparrows  (Passerculus  sandwichensis  labradorius) 
in  Newfoundland  and  the  St.  Pierre  et  Miguelon  Islands" 

Charles  P.  Nicholson,  Tennessee  Valley  Authority,  "Song  variation  in  the 
Swainson's  Warbler" 

Daniel  S.  McGeen,  Pontiac,  Michigan,  "Kirtland's  Warbler,  endangered  or 
doomed" 

George  Kulesza,  Mississippi  State  University , "Life  history  correlations 
among  passerine  birds" 

Gharles  G.  Sibley  and  Jon  Ahlquist,  Yale  University,  "The  relationships  of  the 
vireos  (Vireonidae)  as  indicated  by  DNA-DNA  hybridization" 

Ann  Greene  and  David  N.  Nettleship,  Canadian  Wildlife  Service,  "Attendance 
patterns  of  Northern  Fulmars  at  Prince  Leopold  Island" 

Howard  R.  Postovit,  North  Dakota  State  University,  James  Tate,  Jr.,  ARCO  Coal 
Co.,  and  James  W.  Grier,  North  Dakota  State  University,  "A  new  nest  plat- 
form for  tree-nesting  eagles" 

C.  S.  Adkisson,  Virginia  Polytechnic  Inst,  and  State  University,  "An  etholo- 
gical  comparison  of  European  Bullfinches  and  Pine  Grosbeaks" 

Jerome  A.  Jackson,  Mississippi  State  University,  "Home  range  and  habitat  use 
of  Red-cockaded  Woodpeckers  in  'poor'  habitat" 

Norman  C.  Famous,  Machias,  Maine,  and  Stewart  I.  Fefer,  U.  S.  Fish  and  Wildlife 
Service,  "Migratory  shorebird  assessment  in  eastern  Maine" 

H.  Carolyn  Peach,  Acadia  University,  "The  foraging  ecology  of  adult  and  juve- 
nile Semipalmated  Plover  (Charadr ius  semipalmatus  Bonaparte)  on  the  Starrs 
Point  Mudflat,  Minas  Basin,  Bay  of  Fundy" 

Kevin  J.  Cash,  Acadia  University,  "Food  remains  of  the  Northern  Bald  Eagle 
(Haliaeetus  leucocephalus  alascanus  Townsend)  in  summer  at  nest  sites  on 
Cape  Breton  Island,  Nova  Scotia" 

Peter  R.  N.  MacDonald,  Acadia  University,  "Age-related  foraging  behaviour  of 
the  Northern  Bald  Eagle  (Haliaeetus  leucocephalus  alascanus  Townsend)  at  a 
wintering  site  in  Nova  Scotia. 


ATTENDANCE 

BRITISH  COLUMBIA:  Burnaby,  Wayne  Weber. 

NEW  BRUNSWICK:  Albert,  David  Christ! , Mary  Majka;  Memramcook,  Reid  McManus, 

Mary  O'Rourke;  Sackville,  Paul  Bogaard , Kevin  Cash,  Tony  Erskine,  Gay 
Hansen,  Peter  Hicklin,  Bob  Lamberton,  Lance  Laviolette,  Mag  Macinnis, 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


587 


H.  Carolyn  Peach,  A1  Smith,  Stuart  Tingley. 

NEWFOUNDLAND:  Glover  town,  Roger  T.  Burrows;  Portugal  Cove,  Margaret  Purdy; 

St . John ' s , Christine  Paton,  Jay  Pitochelli. 

NOVA  SCOTIA:  Antigonish , Norman  Seymour;  Dartmouth , Dick  Brown;  Elmsdale , 

Andrew  Macinnis;  Gaspman,  Cyril  Coldwell;  Halifax , Earle  Hickey,  Tony  Lock; 
Kentville , Peter  Austin-Smith , Mark  Forbes;  Wolf ville , Phoebe  Barnard, 
Carolyn  Crawford,  Andree  Dubois,  Peter  MacDonald,  Patricia  Reid,  Bob 
Simmons,  P.  C.  Smith. 

ONTARIO:  Gore's  Landing,  Norman  D.  Martin;  Toronto , Jon  C.  Barlow,  Margaret 

May,  Thomas  Parsons,  John  Reynolds,  James  D.  Rising, 

PRINCE  EDWARD  ISLAND:  Bloomfield , Alanagh  MacDougall,  Gerald  MacDougall. 

QUEBEC:  Senneville , Margaret  Elliot. 

COLORADO:  Fort  Collins , Clait  E.  Braun;  Golden , James  Tate,  Jr. 

GONNECTICUT:  New  Canaan , Elise  Lapham;  New  Haven , Charles  G.  Sibley. 

DISTRICT  OF  COLUMBIA:  Washington,  Earl  Baysinger . 

FLORIDA:  Gainesville,  Mary  H.  Glench;  Winter  Park,  Fred  H.  Glenny. 

IOWA:  Sioux  City , Morgan  Webb. 

KENTUCKY:  Richmond , Gary  Ritchison. 

MAINE:  Brunswick,  Charles  Huntington;  Orono , Thomas  A.  Allen,  Norm  Famous, 

Mark  McCollough,  Terry  May;  Tenant ' s Harbor , Ralph  S.  Palmer. 

MARYLAND:  Frostburg , James  Mosher. 

MASSACHUSETTS:  Amherst , Robert  O'Hara;  Lincoln,  Peter  Alden;  Manomet , 

Kathleen  Anderson,  Michael  Chick,  Trevor  Lloyd-Evans;  Petersham , John  and 
Rosalie  Fiske;  Worcester , Susan  L.  Berman. 

MICHIGAN:  Alma,  Alma  and  Lester  Eyer ; Grass  Lake , Harold  Ratcliff;  Jackson, 

Robert  A.  Whiting;  Pleasant  Lake , Hubert  P.  Zernichow;  Pontiac , Daniel 
McGeen . 

MINNESOTA:  Duluth,  Pershing  B.  Hofslund. 

MISSISSIPPI:  Mississippi  State,  Opal  Dakin,  Jerome  Jackson,  George  Kulesza, 

Bette  J.  Schardien. 

NEW  JERSEY:  Trenton , Mary  E.  Doscher . 

NEW  YORK:  Albany,  Paul  Ker linger;  Lansing , Helen  Lapham;  Phoenix , Vincent. J. 

Lucid . 

NORTH  CAROLINA:  Chapel  Hill,  Helmut  Meuller,  Nancy  Mueller. 

OHIO:  Columbus , Abbot  Gaunt,  Sandra  Gaunt;  Gambler , Robert  D.  Burns;  Lakewood , 

Willaim  Klamm,  Mrs.  William  Klamm;  Painesville , Mrs.  Robert  Booth. 

OKLAHOMA:  Edmond , Ghristine  Albas i,  W.  J.  Radke. 

PENNSYLVANIA:  Chester  Springs , Phillips  B.  Street;  Pittsburgh,  Jay  Loughlin. 


588 


ANNUAL  REPORT 


SOUTH  CAROLINA:  Chester , Mrs.  W.  S.  Robinson,  Mrs.  W.  S.  Stone,  Sr.; 

Columbia,  Elizabeth  Henderson,  Rock  Hill , Keith  Bildstein;  Spartanburg , 
Miller  C.  Foster,  Jr. 

TENT^ESSEE:  Knoxville , Marcia  Davis,  Beth  Lacy;  Maryville,  Ralph.  J.  Zaenglein; 

Norris , Charles  P.  Nicholson,  Linda  J.  Turner. 

TEXAS:  Austin , Charles  Hartshorne. 

VIRGINIA:  Blacksburg , C.  S.  Adkisson;  Bluemont , Maurice  Lefranc. 

WEST  VIRGINIA:  Morgantown,  George  A.  Hall. 

WISCONSIN:  New  Franklin,  Richard  Stiehl. 


The  next  Annual  Meeting  of  the  society  will  be  held  at  Virginia  Polytechnic 
Institute  and  State  University  in  Blacksburg,  Virginia,  from  6 to  9 May  1982. 

In  addition  to  a scientific  program,  an  art  exhibit,  and  a program  for  spouses, 
there  will  be  field  trips  into  the  mountains  around  Blacksburg  where  many  bird 
species  characteristic  of  northern  hardwood  and  coniferous  forests  will  already 
be  in  residence. 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93,  1981 


By  Nancy  J.  Flood,  Janet  T.  Mannone  and  Gary  R.  Bortolotti 

This  index  includes  references  to  genera,  species,  authors  and  key  words  or  terms.  In 
addition  to  avian  species,  references  are  made  to  the  scientific  names  of  all  vertebrates 
mentioned  within  the  volume.  Common  names  are  as  they  appeared  in  the  volume.  Reference 
is  made  to  books  reviewed,  reviewers  and  announcements  as  they  appeared  in  the  volume. 


abnormality 

of  bill  of  Agelaius  phoeniceus,  563-565 
Abraham,  Kenneth  F.,  Pierre  Mineau  and 
Fred  Cooke,  Re-mating  of  a Lesser 
Snow  Goose,  557-559 
Accipiter  cooperii,  110 
nisus,  89,  286 
striatus,  85-92,  491-499 
Acrocephalus  palustris,  59 
scirpaceus,  59 
Actitis  macularia,  328 
Aechmophorus  occidentalis,  330 
aerodynamics 

advantages  of  tail  keeling  in  Quiscalus 
quiscula,  500-505 

age  and  sex  differences  in  Accipiter  stria- 
tus, 491^99 

Afton,  Alan  D.,  see  Cooper,  James  A.  and 


Agapornis  spp.,  297-298 
Agelaius  phoeniceus,  119-121,  444,  540, 
547-548,  563-565 

Ainley,  David  G.,  see  DeSante,  David  F.  and 


Aix  sponsa,  330-331 
Ajaia  ajaja,  151,  548 

Albatross,  Black-browed,  see  Diomedea  me- 
lanophris 

Black-footed,  see  Diomedea  nigripes 
Gray-headed,  see  Diomedea  chrysostoma 
Laysan,  see  Diomedea  immutabihs 
Light-mantled  Sooty,  see  Phoebetria  pal- 
pebrata 

Wandering,  see  Diomedea  exulans 
albatross,  see  Diomedea  spp. 

Alca  torda,  110 
Alces  alces,  102 

Alden,  Peter  and  John  Gooders,  Finding 
birds  around  the  world,  reviewed,  573 
Ali,  Salim  and  S.  Dillon  Ripley,  Handbook 


of  the  birds  of  India  and  Pakistan. 
Vol.  2,  2nd  ed.,  reviewed,  573 
Allez,  George,  see  Mueller,  Helmut  C.,  Dan- 
iel D.  Berger  and 

allopatry 

in  chickadees,  54-66 
Amaurolimnas  concolor,  107-108 
Amazona,  297 

Ammodramus  aurifrons,  282 
savannarum,  547-548 
Ammospiza  caudacuta,  443 
maritima,  443 

maritima  nigrescens,  443,  540 
Anas  acuta,  330,  445 
americana,  274,  330 
clypeata,  330,  445 
crecca  carolinensis,  330 
cyanoptera,  403-405 
discors,  330,  403—405 
platyrhynchos,  121-124,  277-278,  440, 
562-563 
rubripes,  449 

strepera,  274,  276-277,  330 
Anas  spp.,  289 

Anderson,  R.  M.,  B.  D.  Turner  and  L.  R. 
Taylor  (eds.).  Population  dynamics: 
the  20th  symposium  of  the  British 
Ecological  Society,  reviewed,  288- 
289 

Aniskowicz,  B.  T.,  Behavior  of  a male  Least 
Bittern  incubating  after  loss  of  mate, 
395-397 
announcements 

Association  of  Systematics  Collections 
Program  evaluation,  382 
formation  of  the  International  Osprey 
Foundation,  Inc.,  333 
formation  of  the  Meandarra  Ornithological 
Field  Study  Unit,  41 


589 


590 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


International  Commission  on  Zoological 
Nomenclature,  41,  349 
memorial  note,  Helen  Van  Tyne,  135 
Anolis  sp.,  107 
Anser  anser,  320 

caerulescens  caerulescens,  121-124,  444, 
557-559 

Anseriformes,  407-412 
Antarctica,  1-20 

Antbird,  Bicolored,  see  Gymnopithys  bicolor 
Black-beaded,  see  Percnostola  rufifrons 
Narrow-billed,  see  Formicivora  iheringi 
Slender,  see  Rhopornis  ardesiaca 
Spotted,  see  Hylophylax  naevioides 
White-bellied,  see  Myrmeciza  longipes 
White-bibbed,  see  Myrmeciza  loricata 
ants,  528 

army,  see  Eciton  sp. 

Anurolimnas  castaneiceps,  141 
fasciatus,  141 
viridis,  141 

Aphelocoma  coerulescens,  538,  549,  550 
ultramarina,  551 
Aptenodytes  forsteri,  5-20 
Apus  apus,  79,  82,  550 
Aquila  chrysaetos,  92,  109 
pomarina,  286 
verreauxi,  286 
Aramides  cajanea,  572 
Aramides  sp.,  107,  108 
Archibald,  G.  W.,  see  Nesbitt,  Stephen  A. 
and 

Archilochus  alexandri,  405-406 
colubris,  218-242 
Archoplites  interruptus,  556 
Ardea  cinerea,  145-163 
herodias,  145-163 
goliatha,  145-163 
Ardeola  greyii,  550 
ralloides,  550 
Ardeola  spp.,  550 
Arenaria  interpres,  446 
Asio  flammeus,  330 
otus,  110 

Auklet,  Cassin’s,  see  Ptychoramphus  aleu- 
tica 

Rhinoceros,  see  Cerorhinca  monocerata 
Auriparus  flaviceps,  203 
Avery,  Michael  L.,  Paul  F.  Springer  and 
Nancy  S.  Dailey,  Avian  mortality  at 


man-made  structures:  an  annotated 
bibliography.  Rev.,  reviewed,  574 
awards  and  grants 

Aaron  M.  Bagg  student  membership 
awards,  136,  356 

Louis  Agassiz  Fuertes,  Margaret  Morse 
Nice  and  Paul  A.  Stewart  awards, 
356 

research  grants,  324 
Aythya  affinis,  330 

americana,  330,  445,  562-563 
coUaris,  330 

valisineria,  330,  443,  548 
Baida,  Russell  P.,  see  Szaro,  Robert  C.  and 


Balgooyen,  Thomas  G.,  review  by,  423^24 
Baker,  James  L.,  see  Rakestraw,  James  L. 
and 

Baptista,  Luis  F.,  Martin  L.  Morton  and 
Maria  E.  Pereyra,  Interspecific  song 
mimesis  by  a Lincoln  Sparrow,  265- 
267 

Barth,  Larry,  review  by,  295-296 
Bartramia  longicauda,  547-548 
Basileuterus  culicivorus,  278 
Baumel,  J.  J.,  A.  S.  King,  A.  M.  Lucas,  J. 
E.  Breazile  and  H.  E.  Evans  (eds.), 
Nomina  anatomica  avium:  an  anno- 
tated anatomical  dictionary  of  birds, 
reviewed,  125-126,  126-128 
beetles,  see  Coleoptera 
behavior 
agonistic 

Grus  canadensis,  99-103 

White-breasted  Nuthatches,  271-274 
breeding 

Anser  caerulescens  caerulescens,  557- 
559 

copulatory 

Sterna  maxima,  390-391 
courtship 

Sterna  maxima,  390-391 
display 

Bonasa  umbellus,  98-99 

Capella  gallinago,  457-461,  472-473 
(Frontispiece) 

Capella  gallinago  mageUanica,  466^67 

Capella  gallinago  nigripennis,  467^68 

Capella  gallinago  paraguaiae,  465-466 

Capella  hardwickii,  463 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


591 


Capella  imperialis,  470-471 
Capella  jamesoni,  470 
Capella  megala,  463^64 
Capella  nemoricola,  464 
Capella  solitaria,  462^63 
Capella  stenura,  461^62 
Capella  stricklandii,  470 
Capella  stricklandii  jamesoni,  471 
Capella  undulata,  471^72 
Eremophila  alpestris,  522 
Grus  canadensis,  99-103 
Lymnocryptes  minimus,  472 
Seiurus  aurocapiUus,  21-41 
diversionary 

Somateria  molissima,  559-560 
flight 

Buteo  jamaicensis,  350-356 
food-related 

Accipiter  striatus,  85-92 
Ammospiza  maritima  nigrescens  feeds 
Agelaius  phoeniceus  fledglings, 
540 

Branta  canadensis,  310-324 
Bubulcus  ibis,  549-550 
Dendrocygna  autumnalis,  551-554 
Dendroica  coronata,  334-339 
Dendroica  virens,  334-339,  478^90 
Empidonax  viriscens,  478^90 
Eremophila  alpestris,  528-529 
Parus  atricapillus,  393-394 
Passer  domesticus,  554 
passerines,  391-392 
Pelecanus  erythrorhynchos,  554-556 
Phalacrocorax  auritus,  554-556 
Phalaropus  fubcarius,  557 
Piranga  olivacea,  478-490 
Rhopornis  ardesiaca,  103-107 
Setophaga  ruticilla,  478^90 
wading  birds,  145-163 
incubation 

Ixobrychus  exilis,  395-397 
nesting 

system  for  monitoring,  325-333 
parental 

Corvus  brachyrhynchos,  394-395 
play 

Coragyps  atratus,  97 
roosting 

Chaetura  pelagica,  77-84 


sentinel 

Corvus  brachyrhynchos,  394-395 
social 

Branta  canadensis,  310-324 
territorial 

Eremophila  alpestris,  520-522 

Podiceps  auritus,  560-561 
winter 

Haliaeetus  leucocephalus,  259-264 
Belcher,  Margaret,  Birds  of  Regina,  Rev, 
Ed.,  reviewed,  574 

Belitsky,  David  W.,  see  Schreiber,  Ralph 

W., and  Bruce  A.  Sorrie 

BeUrose,  Frank  C,  and  R,  C.  Crompton, 
Migration  speeds  of  waterfowl  species, 
121-124 

Berger,  Andrew  J.,  see  Kear,  Janet  and 


Berger,  Daniel  D.,  see  Mueller,  Helmut  C., 

and  George  AUez 

Berman,  Susan  L.,  reviews  by,  296-297, 
297-298 

Bierly,  Michael  Lee,  Bird  finding  in  Tennes- 
see, reviewed,  300 
Bittern,  Least,  see  Ixobrychus  exilis 
Blackbird,  European,  see  Turdus  merula 
Red-winged,  see  Agelaius  phoeniceus 
Rusty,  see  Euphagus  carolinus 
Blackcap,  see  Sylvia  atricapiUa 
Blohm,  Robert  J.,  Additional  evidence  of 
egg-moving  behavior  by  female  Gad- 
waUs,  276-277 

Bluebird,  Mountain,  see  Sialia  currucoides 
bluefish,  see  Pomatomus  saltatrix 
Bolen,  Eric  G.,  review  by,  133-134 
BombyciUa  cedrorum,  253 
Bonasa  umbeUus,  98-99,  218-242,  291,  372- 
382 

boobies,  see  Sula  spp. 

Bothrops  laterabs,  278 
Bourne,  Godfrey  R.,  Food  habits  of  Black- 
bellied  Whistling  Ducks  occupying 
rice  culture  habitats,  551-554 
BoxaU,  Peter  C.,  see  Stepney,  Philip  H.  R. 
and 

Brachyramphus  brevirostris,  402 
marmoratus,  400^03 
marmoratus  marmoratus,  402 
marmoratus  perdix,  402 
Brant,  Black,  see  Branta  bernicla 


592 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Branta  bernicla,  438,  548 
bernicla  nigricans,  446 
canadensis  interior,  121-124,  207-217, 
318,  319,  330,  559 
canadensis  maxima,  310-324 
sandvicensis,  417^19 

Braun,  Clait  E.,  see  Gorenzel,  Warner  P., 

Ronald  A.  Ryder  and 

Breazile,  J.  E.,  see  Baumel,  J.  J.  et  al. 
breeding  biology,  general 

Eremophila  alpestris,  519-530 
Icterus  gularis,  531-537 
Pelecanus  occidentalis,  397-400 
Petrochelidon  fulva,  507-518 
breeding  success 

Columba  livia,  357-362 
Bubalus  bubalis,  550 
Bubo  virginianus,  110 
Bubulcus  ibis,  147,  549-550 
Bucephala  albeola,  443 
buffalo,  water,  see  Bubalus  bubalis 
Bufflehead,  see  Bucephala  albeola 
Bufo  marinus,  552,  553 
bug,  Mexican  chicken,  see  Haematosiphon 
indorus 

bullhead,  brown,  see  Ictalurus  nebulosus 
Bunting,  Indigo,  see  Passerina  cyanea 
Lazuli,  see  Passerina  amoena 
Snow,  see  Plectrophenax  nivalis 
Buphagus  spp.,  549 
Burger,  Joanna,  review  by,  131-132 
Bush-tanager,  Common,  see  Chlorospingus 
ophthalmicus 

Buskirk,  William  H.,  Ochraceous  Wren  fails 
to  respond  to  mobbing  calls  in  a het- 
erospecific flock,  278-279 
Buteo  albonotatus,  282-284 
jamaicensis,  110,  350-356 
lagopus,  350 
lineatus,  110 
platypterus,  110 
regalis,  92-97,  354 
Butorides  virescens,  147 
Calandra  sp.,  553 
Calidris  canutus,  557 

Callin,  E.  Manley,  Birds  of  the  Qu’appelle, 
1857-1979,  reviewed,  574 
Calocitta  formosa,  539 

Campbell,  Kenneth  E.,  Jr.,  Papers  in  avian 
paleontology  honoring  Hildegarde 
Howard,  reviewed,  407-412 


Campylorhynchus  brunneicapiUus,  203 
Canachites  canadensis,  291,  376 
Cannings,  Richard  J.  and  William  Threlfall, 
Horned  Lark  breeding  biology  at 
Cape  St.  Mary’s,  Newfoundland, 
519-530 

Canvasback,  see  Aythya  valisineria 
CapeUa  gaUinago,  457-474  (Frontispiece) 
gaUinago  andina,  465,  467,  469,  474 
gaUinago  angolensis,  465,  468 
gaUinago  delicata,  460,  465,  469 
gaUinago  faeroensis,  460,  465 
gaUinago  gaUinago,  460,  465,  474 
gaUinago  mageUanica,  465,  466^467 
gaUinago  nigripennis,  465,  467-468 
gaUinago  paraguaiae,  465-466,  472 
hardwickii,  463 
imperialis,  468,  470-471 
jamesoni,  468,  469,  470,  471 
macrodactyla,  465,  468-469 
media,  459,  462,  467,  474 
megala,  460,  463^64 
nemoricola,  464 
nobilis,  465,  467,  468^70 
solitaria,  462^463 
stenura,  460,  461^62,  468,  474 
stricklandii,  468,  470,  471 
stricklandii  jamesoni,  471 
undulata,  468,  471,  474 
CapercaiUie,  see  Tetrao  urogaUus 
Black-biUed,  see  Tetrao  parvirostris 
Caprimulgis  carolinensis,  363-371 
vociferus,  363-371 
Carcharinus  leucas,  279 
longimanus,  279 
Carcharodon  carcharias,  279 
Cardinal,  see  Cardinalis  cardinalis 
Cardinalis  cardinalis,  218-242 
Carduelis  pinus,  344,  346 
carp,  see  Cyprinus  carpio 
Carpodacus  purpureus,  551 
cat,  domestic,  see  FeUs  domestica 
Catbird,  Gray,  see  DumeteUa  carolinensis 
Catharacta  maccormicki,  12 
Catharacta  spp.,  443 
Catharus  frantzii,  302 
fuscescens,  218-242 
guttata,  164-188,  550-551 
minima,  164-188,  344,  345,  346,  347 
mustelina,  164-188 
occidentalis,  302 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


593 


ustulata,  164-188 

Catoptrophorus  semipalmatus,  440,  442,  443 
Catostomus  tahoensis,  556 
Centrocercus  urophasianus,  376 
Cepphus  columba,  441 
grylle,  279-280 
Cerorhinca  monocerata,  441 
Certhia  brachydactyla,  59 
familiaris,  59,  218-242,  273,  344 
Certhidae,  67-76 
chachalaca,  see  Ortalis  vetuia 
Chaetura  pelagica,  77-84 
Chaffinch,  see  FringiUa  coelebs 
Chancellor,  John,  Audubon.  A biography, 
reviewed,  293-294 

Charadrius  alexandrinus  nivosus,  439 
Chat,  YeUow-breasted,  see  Icteria  virens 
Chen  caerulescens,  548 
Chickadee,  Black-capped,  see  Pams  atri- 
capillus 

Boreal,  see  Parus  hudsonicus 
Carolina,  see  Parus  carolinensis 
Chiffchaff,  see  PhyUoscopus  coUybita 
Chloris  chloris,  265 
Chlorospingus  ophthalmicus,  278 
Chordeiles  minor,  547-548 
Chub,  tui,  see  Gila  bicolor 
Chubbia  sp.,  471 

Chuck-wdl’s-widow,  see  Caprimulgis  caro- 
linensis 

Ciconia  ciconia,  147 
Cissilopha  beecheii,  539 
sanblasiana,  539 
yucatanica,  539 
Cisticola  spp.,  301 
Clangula  hyemalis,  110-111 
Clark,  E.  Scott,  Juvenile  Peregrine  Falcon 
swoops  on  Roseate  Spoonbills,  548 
Clark,  George  A.,  Toe  fusion  in  oscines, 
67-76 

includes  table  of  55  oscine  families,  69-72 
Clark,  Karen  L.  and  Raleigh  J.  Robertson, 
Cowbird  parasitism  and  evolution  of 
anti-parasite  strategies  in  the  Yellow 
Warbler,  249-258 
Clethrionomys  glareolus,  291 
Cnemidophorus  sp.,  94 
cod,  see  Gadus  macrocephalus 
Colaptes  auratus,  88,  218-242 
Colbert,  Edwin  H.,  Evolution  of  the  verte- 
brates, third  edition,  reviewed,  417 


Coleoptera,  529,  554 

Collins,  Scott  L.,  A comparison  of  nest-site 
and  perch-site  vegetation  structure 
for  seven  species  of  warblers,  542- 
547 

Columba  livia,  109,  281,  357-362,  505 
Conservation  Committee  (1980),  Report  on 
1980,  the  year  of  the  coast:  birds, 
438^56 

Contopus  virens,  218-242 

Cooke,  Fred,  see  Abraham,  Kenneth  F., 

Pierre  Mineau  and 

Cooper,  James  A.  and  Alan  D.  Afton,  A mul- 
tiple sensor  system  for  monitoring 
avian  nesting  behavior,  325-333 
Cooper,  Robert  J.,  Relative  abundance  of 
Georgia  caprimulgids  based  on  call- 
counts,  363-371 

Coot,  American,  see  Fuhca  americana  amer- 
icana 

Cormorant,  Double-breasted,  see  Phalacro- 
corax  auritus 

Great,  see  Phalacrocorax  carbo 
cormorants,  see  Phalacrocorax  spp. 

Corvus  brachyrhynchos,  344,  394,  395 
corax,  344,  528 
macrorhynchos,  280 
splendens,  280 
Corvus  spp.,  550 
Coryagyps  auratus,  97 
Cossypha  natalensis,  392 
Cowbird,  Bronzed,  see  Molothrus  aeneus 
Brown-headed,  see  Molothrus  ater 
Crake,  Black-banded,  see  Laterallus 
[Anurohmnas]  “hauxweUi”  (=fascia- 
tus) 

Chestnut-headed,  see  Anurolimnas 
[RaUina]  castaneiceps 
Columbian,  see  Neocrex  columbianus 
Dot-winged,  see  Laterallus  [Porzana]  spi- 
lopterus 

Gray-breasted,  see  Laterallus  exilis 
Paint-biUed,  see  Neocrex  erythrops 
Red-and-White,  see  Laterallus  leucopyr- 
rhus 

Ruddy,  see  Laterallus  ruber 
Rufous-faced,  see  Laterallus  xenopterus 
Rufous-sided,  see  Laterallus  melano- 
phaius 

Russet-crowned,  see  Laterallus 
[Anurolimnas]  viridis 


594 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Rusty-flanked,  see  LateraUus  levraudi 
Uniform,  see  Amaurolimnas  concolor 
Cramp,  Stanley  (ed.).  Handbook  of  the  birds 
of  Europe,  the  Middle  East,  and 
North  Africa.  The  birds  of  the  West- 
ern Palearctic.  Vol.  1,  Ostrich  to 
ducks.  Vol.  2,  Hawks  to  bustards,  re- 
viewed, 430-432 

Crane,  Florida  Sandhill,  see  Grus  canaden- 
sis pratensis 

Greater  Sandhill,  see  Grus  canadensis 
tabida 

Hooded,  see  Grus  monacha 
Japanese,  see  Grus  japonensis 
Sandhill,  see  Grus  canadensis 
White-naped,  see  Grus  vipio 
Whooping,  see  Grus  americana 
craneflies,  see  Tipulidae 
crappie,  white,  see  Pomoxis  annularis 
Crawford,  Robert  L.,  Weather,  migration 
and  autumn  bird  kills  at  a north  Flor- 
ida TV  tower,  189-195 
Creeper,  Brown,  see  Certhia  familiaris 
Crocethia  alba,  446 

Crocoll,  Scott  and  James  W.  Parker,  Pro- 
tocalliphora  infestation  in  Broad- 
winged Hawk,  110 

Crompton,  Robert  C.,  see  BeUrose,  Frank  C. 
and 

Crossbill,  Red,  see  Loxia  curvirostra 
White-winged,  see  Loxia  leucoptera 
Crow,  Common,  see  Corvus  brachyrhynchos 
House,  see  Corvus  splendens 
Large-billed,  see  Corvus  macrorhynchos 
Cruickshank,  Allan  D.,  The  birds  of  Brevard 
County  (edited  by  Helen  G.  Cruick- 
shank), reviewed,  300 

Curlew,  Bristle-thighed,  see  Numenius  ta- 
hitiensis 

Curtis,  Jane  and  Will,  Welcome  the  birds  to 
your  home,  reviewed,  300 
Cyanocitta  cristata,  164-188,  218-242,  334, 
$38-540,  551 
stelleri,  539,  551 
Cyanocorax  yncas,  538-540 
Cygnus  cygnus  buccinator,  330 
olor,  443 

Cyprinus  carpio,  556 
Cypseloides  niger,  83 

D’Agostino,  Gloria  M.,  Lorraine  E.  Giovin- 


azzo  and  Stephen  W.  Eaton,  The  sen- 
tinel crow  as  an  extension  of  parental 
care,  394-395 

Dailey,  Nancy  S.,  see  Avery,  Michael  L., 

Paul  F.  Springer  and 

Daption  capense,  5-20 
DeBenedictis,  Paul  A.,  review  by,  428^30 
deer,  white-tailed,  see  Odocoileus  virgini- 
anus 

mule,  see  Odocoileus  hemionus 
DeGraff,  Richard  M.  (Tech.  Coord.),  Pro- 
ceedings of  the  workshop  manage- 
ment of  southern  forests  for  nongame 
birds,  reviewed,  292-293 
Dellinger,  Rebecca  M.,  see  Puhch,  Warren 
M.  and  

Dendragapus  obscurus,  291,  376 
Dendrocopos  pubescens,  218-242 
villosus,  218-242 

Dendrocygna  autumnalis,  551-554 
Dendroica  caerulescens,  218-242 
canadensis,  164-188,  218-242,  478 
castanea,  240 
cerulea,  478 

coronata,  88,  334-339,  551 
discolor,  164-188,  218-242,  295 
dominica,  218-242 
fusca,  218-242,  478 
kirtlandii,  251,  295 
magnolia,  164-188,  240 
nigrescens,  164-188 
palmarum,  542-547 

pensylvanica,  36,  218-242,  478,  542-547 
petechia,  218-242,  249-258,  542-547 
pinus,  218-242 
striata,  344-347 

Virens,  164-188,  218-242,  334-339,  478- 
490 

DeSante,  David  F.  and  David  G.  Ainley,  The 
avifauna  of  the  South  Farallon  Is- 
lands, California,  reviewed,  428^30 
Deshotels,  Jack  H.,  see  Guillory,  D.  and 


Dichromanassa  rufescens,  145-163 
Dickcissel,  see  Spiza  americana 
Didelphis  virginianus,  560 
Diomedea  chrysostoma,  5-20 
exulans,  5-20 
immutabilis,  445 
melanophris,  5-20 
nigripes,  445 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


595 


Diomedea  spp.,  439,  445,  446 
Dipodomys  sp.,  94 
distribution 

Cyanocitta  cristata,  540 
Cyanocorax  yncas,  540 
Fulica  americana  americana,  115—118 
LateraUus  sp.,  140-141 
of  Zone-tailed  Hawks  in  west  Texas,  282- 
284 

Dove,  Mourning,  see  Zenaida  macroura 
Rock,  see  Columba  livia 
Dryocopus  pileatus,  218-242 
Duck,  Black,  see  Anas  rubripes 

Black-bellied  Whistling,  see  Dendrocygna 
autumnalis 

Mallard,  see  Anas  platyrhynchos 
Oldsquaw,  see  Clangula  hyemalis 
Redhead,  see  Aythya  americana 
Ring-necked,  see  Aythya  coUaris 
Ruddy,  see  Oxyura  jamaicensis 
steamer,  see  Tachyeres  sp. 

Wood,  see  Aix  sponsa 
Dumetella  carolinensis,  104,  164-188,  218- 
242 

Eagle,  Bald,  see  Haliaeetus  leucocephalus 
Black,  see  Aquila  verreauxi 
Crested,  see  Morphnus  guianensis 
Golden,  see  Aquila  chrysaetos 
Harpy,  see  Harpia  harpyja 
Lesser-spotted,  see  Aquila  pomarina 
Eaton,  Stephen  W.,  see  D’Agostino,  Gloria 
M.,  Lorraine  E.  Giovinazzo  and 


Eciton  sp.,  392 

Editor’s  acknowledgments,  135 
eggs 

Amaurolimnas  concolor,  108 
Brachyramphus  marmoratus,  400-403 
moving  by  female  GadwaUs,  276-277 
Egret,  Cattle,  see  Bubulcus  ibis 

Reddish,  see  Dichromanassa  rufescens 
Snowy,  see  Egretta  thula 
Egretta  sacra,  147,  153 
thula,  392 

Eider,  Common,  see  Somateria  moUissima 
Elanoides  forficatus,  391 
Eleutherodactylus  sp.,  107 
Ellis,  David  H.  and  Wayne  H.  Whaley, 
Three  Crested  Eagle  reports  for  Gua- 
temala, 284-285 


Emberizidae,  67-76 
Empidonax  difficilis,  412^14 
flavescens,  412-414 
flaviventris,  344 
virescens,  218-242,  478^90 
Empidonax  spp.,  301 
Enderson,  James  H.,  review  by,  571-572 
Endomychura  hypoleuca,  439 
Eremophila  alpestris,  519-530 
ermine,  see  Mustela  erminea 
errata 

for  Williams,  J.  B.,  Vol.  92,  No.  4,  163 
for  Clark,  K.  L.  and  R.  J.  Robertson,  Vol. 

93,  No.  2,  574 
Estrilda  astrild,  281-282 
erythronotos,  281 
Eudocimus  albus,  148-163 
Euphagus  carolinus,  344 
Euthlypis  lachrymosa,  392 
Evans,  H.  E.,  see  Baumel,  J.  J.,  et  al. 

Falco  eleonorae,  287 
peregrinus,  548,  571-572 
peregrinus  pealei,  572 
Falcon,  Eleonora’s,  see  Falco  eleonorae 
Peale’s,  see  Falco  peregrinus  pealei 
Peregrine,  see  Falco  peregrinus 
falcons,  see  Micrastur  sp. 

Fawver,  Ben  J.,  see  Kendeigh,  S.  Charles 
and 

Feduccia,  Alan,  see  Olson,  Storrs  L.  and 


, The  age  of  birds,  reviewed,  407^12 

Felis  domestica,  528 

Ferguson,  Robert  S.,  Territorial  attachment 
and  mate  fidebty  by  Horned  Grebes, 
560-561 

Fiala,  Kent  L.,  Survival  of  demaxiUate  Red- 
winged Blackbird,  563-565 
Ficken,  MiUicent  S.,  Food  finding  in  Black- 
capped  Chickadees:  altruistic  com- 
munication?, 393-394 
Fieldfare,  see  Turdus  pilaris 
Finch,  Purple,  see  Carpodacus  purpureus 
Fire-eyes,  White-winged,  see  Pyriglena  leu- 
coptera 

Fitch,  Mary  A.,  see  Shugart,  Gary  W., 

and  Vern  M.  Shugart 

Fitzpatrick,  John  W.,  review  by,  412-414 
flamingo,  see  Phoenicopteridae 
Flicker,  Common,  see  Colaptes  auratus 
Florida  caerulea,  145-163 


596 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Flycatcher,  Acadian,  see  Empidonax  vires- 
cens 

Great-crested,  see  Myiarchus  crinitus 
Kiskadee,  see  Pitangus  sulphuratus 
Western,  see  Empidonax  difficilis 
Yellow-bellied,  see  Empidonax  flaviven- 
tris 

Yellowish,  see  Empidonax  flavescens 
foraging,  see  behavior,  food-related 
Formicivora  iheringi,  106 
fox,  red,  see  Vulpes  vulpes 
Franks,  Edwin  C.,  see  Zammuto,  Richard 
M.  and  

Fratercula  arctica,  110,  341,  444,  572 
Fregata  magnificens,  439 
minor  palmerstonia,  445 
French,  Thomas  W.,  Fish  attack  on  Black 
Guillemot  and  Common  Eider  in 
Maine,  279-280 

FretweU,  Stephen  D.  and  Frank  S.  Shipley, 
Statistical  significance  and  density- 
dependent  nest  predation,  541-542 
Fries,  W'aldemar,  The  double  elephant  folio. 
The  story  of  Audubon’s  birds  of 
America,  reviewed,  293-294 
frigatebird,  see  Fregata  minor  palmerstoni 
Magnificent,  see  Fregata  magnificens 
Fringilla  coelebs,  123 
frog,  see  Eleutherodactylus  sp. 

Fulica  americana  americana,  115-118,  274- 
275 

Fulmar,  Southern,  see  Fulmarus  glacialoides 
Southern  Giant,  see  Macronectes  gigan- 
teus 

fulmars,  see  Fulmarus  spp. 

Fulmarus  glacialoides,  12 

Fulmarus  spp.,  443,  446 

Gadus  macrocephalus,  279 

Gadwall,  see  Anas  strepera 

Galeocerdo  cuvieri,  279 

Gallinule,  Purple,  see  Porphyrula  martinica 

gannets,  see  Morus  spp. 

Garbutt,  Alan,  The  shoulder-spot  display  in 
Ruffed  Grouse,  98-99 
Gavia  spp.,  446 

Geothlypis  trichas,  218-242,  542-547 
Gila  bicolor,  555-556 

Giovinazzo,  Lorraine  E.,  see  D’Agostino, 

Gloria  M.,  and  Stephen  W. 

Eaton 


Gnatcatcher,  Blue-gray,  see  Pohoptila  cae- 
rulea 

Godwit,  Marbled,  see  Limosa  fedoa 
Goldfinch,  American,  see  Spinus  tristis 
Gooders,  John,  see  Alden,  Peter  and 


Goose,  Canada,  see  Branta  canadensis 
interior  and  B.  c.  maxima 
Greylag,  see  Anser  anser 
Lesser  Snow,  see  Anser  caerulescens  cae- 
rulescens 

Snow,  see  Chen  caerulescens 
Gorenzel,  Warner  P.,  Ronald  A.  Ryder  and 
Clait  E.  Braun,  American  Coot  distri- 
bution and  migration  in  Colorado, 
115-118 

Graber,  Richard  R.,  review  by,  572-573 
Crackle,  Common,  see  Quiscalus  quiscula 
Great-tailed,  see  Quiscalus  mexicanus 
Grant,  C.  H.  B.,  see  Mackworth-Praed,  C. 

W.  and  

grasshoppers,  see  Orthoptera 

Grassquit,  Blue-black,  see  Volatinia  jacarina 

Grebe,  Homed,  see  Podiceps  auritus 

W estern,  see  Aechmophorus  occidentalis 
grebes,  see  Podicipedidae 
Greenfinch,  European,  see  Chloris  chloris 
Griffin,  Curtice  R.,  Interactive  behavior 
among  Bald  Eagles  wintering  in  north- 
central  Missouri,  259-264 
Griscom,  Ludlow  and  Alexander  Sprunt,  Jr. 
(eds.).  The  warblers  of  America.  Up- 
dated by  Edgar  M.  Reilly,  Jr.,  re- 
viewed, 294-295 

Grosbeak,  Pine,  see  Pinicola  enucleator 
Rose-breasted,  see  Pheucticus  ludovici- 
anus 

Ground-Thrush,  Spotted,  see  Turdus  fisheri 
Grouse,  Black,  see  Lyrurus  tetrix 
Blue,  see  Dendragapus  obscurus 
Hazel,  see  Tetrastes  bonasia 
Red,  see  Lagopus  lagopus 
Ruffed,  see  Bonasa  umbellus 
Sage,  see  Centrocercus  urophasianus 
Severtzor’s  Hazel,  see  Tetrastes  sewer- 
zowi 

Sharp-tailed,  see  Pedioecetes  phasianel- 
lus 

Spruce,  see  Canachites  canadensis 
Grus  americana,  100 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


597 


canadensis,  99-103 
canadensis  pratensis,  100 
canadensis  tabida,  100 
japonensis,  99,  101 
monacha,  100 
vipio,  100 

Guillemot,  Black,  see  Cepphus  gryUe 
Pigeon,  see  Cepphus  columba 
GuiUory,  D.  and  Jack  H.  Deshotels,  House 
Sparrows  flushing  prey  from  trees  and 
shrubs,  554 

GuU,  Bonaparte’s,  see  Larus  Philadelphia 
Glaucous-winged,  see  Larus  glaucescens 
Herring,  see  Larus  argentatus 
Laughing,  see  Larus  atriciUa 
Mew,  see  Larus  canus 
Ring-billed,  see  Larus  delawarensis 
GuUion,  Gordon  W.,  Non-drumming  males 
in  a Ruffed  Grouse  population,  372- 
382 

guUs,  see  Larus  spp. 

Gymnopithys  bicolor,  572 
habitat  structure 

of  warblers,  542-547 
Haematopus  spp.,  441 
Haematosiphon  indorus,  110 
Haliaeetus  leucocephalus,  102,  109,  149, 
259-264 

Halichoerus  grypus,  279 
HaU,  George  A.,  review  by,  425^27 
Halobaena  caerulea,  5-20 
Hamerstrom,  Frances,  Strictly  for  the  chick- 
ens, reviewed,  424 
Harpia  harpyja,  284-285 
Harris,  James  T.,  The  Peregrine  Falcon  in 
Greenland:  observing  an  endangered 
species,  reviewed,  129-131 
Hawk,  Broad-winged,  see  Buteo  platypterus 
Cooper’s,  see  Accipiter  cooperii 
Ferruginous,  see  Buteo  regalis 
Red-shouldered,  see  Buteo  lineatus 
Red-tailed,  see  Buteo  jamaicensis 
Rough-legged,  see  Buteo  lagopus 
Sharp-shinned,  see  Accipiter  striatus 
Zone-tailed,  see  Buteo  albonotatus 
Hayward,  Jim,  Lovebirds  and  their  color 
mutations,  reviewed,  297-298 
Heintzelman,  Donald  S.,  A manual  for  bird 
watching  in  the  Americas,  reviewed, 
299 


, Hawks  and  owls  of  North  America, 

reviewed,  129 

, North  American  ducks,  geese  and 

swans,  reviewed,  133-134 
Helmitheros  vermivorus,  164-188,  218-242 
Hensler,  G.  L.  and  J.  D.  Nichols,  The  May- 
field  method  of  estimating  nesting 
success:  a model,  estimators  and  sim- 
ulation results,  42-53 

Heppner,  Frank  H.,  see  Preble,  David  E. 
and 

Heron,  Black-crowned  Night,  see  Nycticorax 
nycticorax 

Goliath,  see  Ardea  goliatha 
Green,  see  Butorides  virescens 
Grey,  see  Ardea  cinerea 
Little  Blue,  see  Florida  caerulea 
Louisiana,  see  Hydranassa  tricolor 
Pond,  see  Ardeola  greyii 
Reef,  see  Egretta  sacra 
Squacco,  see  Ardeola  raUoides 
Hickman,  Scott,  Evidence  for  aerodynamic 
advantages  of  tail  keehng  in  the  Com- 
mon Grackle,  500-505 
Hirundinidae,  67-76 
Hirundo  rustica,  506,  507,  514 
hog,  feral,  see  Sus  scrofa 
Holmgren,  Mark,  review  by,  299-300 
Hoplopterus  sp.,  570 

Houston,  C.  Stuart,  Nature  Saskatoon:  an 
account  of  the  Saskatoon  Natural  His- 
tory Society,  1955-1980,  reviewed, 
574 

Howard,  Richard  and  Alick  Moore,  A com- 
plete checklist  of  the  birds  of  the 
world,  reviewed,  437 

Howell,  Thomas  R.,  Breeding  biology  of  the 
Egyptian  Plover,  Pluvianus  aegyp- 
tius,  reviewed,  570-571 
Hummingbird,  Black-chinned,  see  Archilo- 
chus alexandri 
Hermit,  572 

Long-tailed  Hermit,  see  Phaethornis  su- 
perciliosus 

Ruby-throated,  see  Archilochus  colubris 
hybridization 
in  jays,  538-540 
in  teals,  403-405 
Hydranassa  tricolor,  145-163 
Hydrobatidae,  439,  445,  446 


598 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • VoL  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Hydrophilus  triangularis,  553 
Hydrurga  leptonyx,  9 
Hylocichla  guttata,  164-188,  550-551 
musteUna,  218-242 

Ibis,  Sacred,  see  Threskiornis  aethiopica 
Straw-necked,  see  Threskiornis  spinicoUis 
White,  see  Eudocimus  albus 
White-faced,  see  Plegadis  chihi 
Ictalurus  nebulosus,  556 
Icteria  virens,  164-188,  218-242 
Icteridae,  67-76,  94 
Icterus  cucuUatus,  533,  535 
galbula,  533-535 
gularis,  531-537 
Ixobrychus  exihs,  145,  395-397 
jackrabbit,  black-tailed,  see  Lepus  califor- 
nicus 

Jackson,  Jerome  A.  (ed.),  The  “mid-south 
bird  notes”  of  Ben  B.  Coffey,  Jr.,  re- 
viewed, 573 

jaeger,  see  Stercorarius  spp. 

James,  Ross  D.,  review  by,  424 

Jarvis,  Robert  L.,  see  Krapu,  Gary  L.  and 

Jay,  Beechey’s,  see  Cissilopha  beecheii 
Blue,  see  Cyanocitta  cristata 
Gray,  see  Perisoreus  canadensis 
Green,  see  Cyanocorax  yncas 
Magpie,  see  Calocitta  formosa 
Mexican,  see  Aphelocoma  ultramarina 
Purplish-backed,  see  Cissilopha  beecheii 
San  Bias,  see  Cissilopha  sanblasiana 
Scrub,  see  Aphelocoma  coerulescens 
SteUer’s,  see  Cyanocitta  stelleri 
White-tipped  Brown,  see  Psilorhinus  mo- 
rio 

Yucatan,  see  Cissilopha  yucatanica 
Johnson,  Ned  K.,  Character  variation  and 
evolution  of  sibling  species  in  the  Em- 
pidonax  difficilis-flavescens  complex 
(Aves:  Tyrannidae),  reviewed,  412- 
414 

Johnson,  Oscar  W.,  review  by,  570-571 
Johnson,  R.,  review  by,  126-128 
Johnson,  Stephen  R.,  review  by,  298 
Junco,  Dark-eyed,  see  Junco  hyemalis 
Gray-headed,  see  Junco  caniceps 
Junco  caniceps,  551 

hyemalis,  164-188,  218-242,  344 
Junco  spp.,  301 


juncos,  see  Junco  spp. 

Karmali,  John,  Birds  of  Africa,  reviewed, 
436^37 

Karr,  James  R.,  review  by,  289 
Kear,  Janet  and  Andrew  J.  Berger,  The 
Hawaiian  Goose:  an  experiment  in 
conservation,  reviewed,  417-419 
Keast,  A.  and  E.  S.  Morton  (eds.).  Migrant 
birds  in  the  neotropics:  ecology,  be- 
havior, distribution,  and  conserva- 
tion, reviewed,  432^35 
Keith,  Stuart,  reviews  by,  128-129,  430^132 
Kendeigh,  S.  Charles  and  Ben  J.  Fawver, 
Breeding  bird  populations  in  the 
Great  Smoky  Mountains,  Tennessee 
and  North  Carolina,  218-242 
Kennedy,  Joseph  L.,  see  Knopf,  Fritz  L.  and 


Kiff,  Lloyd  F.,  Eggs  of  the  Marbled  Murrelet, 
400-403 

Kilham,  Lawrence,  Agonistic  behavior  of  the 
White-breasted  Nuthatch,  271-274 

, Courtship  feeding  and  copulation  of 

Royal  Terns,  390-391 

King,  A.  S.  and  J.  McLeUand  (eds.).  Form 
and  function  in  birds.  Vol.  1,  re- 
viewed, 416-4'17 

, see  Baumel,  J.  J.,  et  al. 

Kingbird,  Eastern,  see  Tyrannus  tyrannus 
Tropical,  see  Tyrannus  melancholicus 
kingbirds,  see  Tyrannus  spp. 

Kinglet,  Golden-crowned,  see  Regulus  sa- 
trapa 

Kite,  Swallow-tailed,  see  Elanoides  forfica- 
tus 

Kittiwake,  Red-legged,  see  Rissa  breviros- 
tris 

kittiwakes,  see  Rissa  spp. 
kleptoparasitism 

by  waterfowl  on  American  Coots,  274-275 
Knodel-Montz,  Janet  Jean,  Use  of  artificial 
perches  on  burned  and  unburned  tail- 
grass  prairie,  547-548 

Knopf,  Fritz  L.  and  Joseph  L.  Kennedy,  Dif- 
ferential predation  by  two  species  of 
piscivorous  birds,  554-556 
Knot,  Red,  see  Calidris  canutus 
Krapu,  Gary  L.  and  Robert  L.  Jarvis,  Effects 
of  Redhead  nest  parasitism  on  Mal- 
lards, 562-563 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


599 


Kushlan,  James  A.,  Resource  use  strategies 
of  wading  birds,  145-163 
Lagopus  lagopus,  376,  462,  571 
leucurus,  376 

Laing,  Hamilton  M.,  Allan  Brooks:  artist 
naturalist,  reviewed,  422-423 
Laniidae,  67-76 

Lansdowne,  J.  F.,  Birds  of  the  west  coast. 

Vol.  II,  reviewed,  427-428 
Lark,  Horned,  see  Eremophila  alpestris 
Larus  argentatus,  110-111,  505,  557,  567, 
568 

atriciUa,  390 
canus.  111,  112 
delawarensis,  110 
glaucescens.  111,  112 
Philadelphia,  111,  112 
Larus  spp.,  123,  441,  446,  565 
Laterallus  albigularis,  141-144 
exilis,  141-144  (F rontispiece) 
fasciatus,  141-144 
jamaicensis,  137-144,  443 
leucopyrrhus,  137-144  (Frontispiece) 
levraudi,  141-144 

melanophaius,  137-144  (Frontispiece) 
ruber,  141-144 
spilonotus,  141-144 
spilopterus,  141-144 
viridis,  141-144 

xenopterus,  137-144  (Frontispiece) 
Laterallus  spp.,  108 

Leafscraper,  Scaly-throated,  see  Sclerurus 
guatemalensis 

Lein,  M.  Ross,  Display  behavior  of  Oven- 
birds  (Seiurus  aurocapiUus)  II.  Song 
variation  and  singing  behavior,  21-41 
Lepidoptera,  554 
Leptoptilos  crumeniferus,  157 
Lepus  californicus,  93 
Lill,  Alan,  review  by,  414^16 
Limnocryptes  minimus,  472 
Limnothlypis  swainsonii,  164-188,  295 
Limosa  fedoa,  440 
lizards,  see  Anolis  sp. 
see  Cnemidophorus  sp. 
see  Phrynosoma  sp. 

Lokemoen,  John  T.  and  David  E.  Sharp, 
First  documented  Cinnamon  Teal 
nesting  in  North  Dakota  produced  hy- 
brids, 403^05 


loons,  see  Gavia  spp. 

Lovell,  T.  W.  I.  (ed.).  Woodland  grouse  sym- 
posium, reviewed,  290-292 
Low,  Rosemary,  Parrots,  their  care  and 
breeding,  reviewed,  296-297 
Loxia  curvirostra,  344 
leucoptera,  344 

Lucas,  A.  M.,  see  Baumel,  J.  J.,  et  al. 
Lumsden,  Harry  G.,  review  by,  290-292 
Lycosidae,  107,  529,  540 
Lyrurus  tetrix,  290,  376 
Macaca  sp.,  391 

Mackworth-Praed,  C.  W.  and  C.  H.  B. 
Grant,  African  handbook  of  birds:  se- 
ries one.  Birds  of  eastern  and  north 
eastern  Africa.  Second  ed.,  reviewed, 
437 

Macronectes  giganteus,  5-20 
Magpie,  Black-biUed,  see  Pica  pica 
manatee,  see  Trichechus  manatus 
Mares,  Michael  A.,  review  by,  420-422 
Martin,  Robert  F.,  Reproductive  correlates 
of  environmental  variation  and  niche 
expansion  in  the  Cave  Swallow  in 
Texas,  506-518 
mate  fidelity 

Podiceps  auritus,  560-561 
Matteson,  Sumner  W.  and  John  O.  Riley, 
Distribution  and  reproductive  success 
of  Zone-tailed  Hawks  in  west  Texas, 
282-284 

Maurer,  Brian  A.  and  Robert  C.  Whitmore, 
Foraging  of  five  bird  species  in  two 
forests  with  different  vegetation 
structure,  478-490 

McGillivray,  W.  Bruce,  Climatic  influences 
on  productivity  in  the  House  Sparrow, 
196-206 

McKay,  Wallace  D.,  Notes  on  Purple  Galli- 
nules  in  Colombian  ricefields,  267- 
271 

McLandress,  M.  Robert,  Scrub  Jay  captures 
Hermit  Thrush  in  flight,  550-551 

and  Dennis  G.  Raveling,  Hyperpha- 

gia  and  social  behavior  of  Canada 
Geese  prior  to  spring  migration,  310- 
324 

McLelland,  J.,  see  King,  A.  S.  and 

McNair,  Douglas  B.,  Common  Eider  plays 
“possum,”  559-560 


600 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Meadowlark,  Eastern,  see  Sturnella  magna 
Meehan,  Thomas  E.,  see  Siegel-Causey, 

Douglas  and 

meetings  and  conferences 

annual  meeting.  The  Wilson  Ornithologi- 
cal Society,  1981,  135 
proceedings  WOS  annual  meeting  1981, 
575-588 

annual  meeting,  1982,  477 
fifth  annual  meeting  of  the  Colonial  Wa- 
terhird  Group,  136 

seventh  international  conference  on  bird 
census  work  and  fifth  meeting  of 
the  European  Atlas  Committee,  76 
southeastern  coastal  and  estuarine  birds 
conference-workshop,  136 
third  international  congress  of  ecology, 
206 

Melacoptila  panamensis,  572 
Melanerpes  chrysauchen,  572 
Melanotis  caerulescens,  392 
Melospiza  georgiana,  164-188,  265,  344,  347 
lincolnii,  164-188,  265-267 
melodia,  255,  265 

Menon,  G.  K.,  Cattle  Egrets  feeding  in  as- 
sociation with  human  workers,  549- 
550 

Mengel,  Robert  M.,  reviews  by,  293-294, 
422-423 

Merganser,  Auckland,  see  Mergus  australis 
Red-breasted,  see  Mergus  serrator 
mergansers,  see  Mergus  sp. 

Mergus  australis,  289 
serrator,  445 
Mergus  sp.,  392 

Merritt,  P.  G.,  Narrowly  disjunct  aUopatry 
between  Black-capped  and  Carolina 
chickadees,  54-66 
Micrastur  sp.,  279 
Microtus  pennsylvanicus,  528 
migration 

and  autumn  tower  kills,  in  relation  to 
weather,  189-195 
Branta  canadensis,  310-324 
Fulica  americana  americana,  115-118 
of  passerines  in  relation  to  age,  164-188 
Mimidae,  67-76 
Mimus  polyglottos,  266 
Mineau,  Pierre,  see  Abraham,  Kenneth  F., 
and  Fred  Cooke 


minimizing  observational  disturbance 
of  colonial  birds,  565-569 
mink,  see  Mustela  vison 
Mniotilta  varia,  164-188,  218-242,  278 
Mockingbird,  Blue,  see  Melanotis  caerules- 
cens 

Northern,  see  Mimus  polyglottos 
Molothrus  aeneus,  535,  536 
ater,  249-258,  389,  547-548 
Molothrus  sp.,  295 
monkey,  see  Macaca  sp. 
monkfish,  see  Squatina  squatina 
Moore,  Alick,  see  Howard,  Richard  and 


moose,  see  Alces  alces 
Morphnus  guianensis,  284-285 
Morse,  Douglas  H.,  Foraging  speeds  of  war- 
blers in  large  populations  and  in  iso- 
lation, 334-339 

, review  by,  419-420 

Morton,  E.  S.,  see  Keast,  A.  and 

Morton,  Martin  L.,  see  Baptista,  Luis  F., 

and  Marie  E.  Pereyra 

Morns  spp.,  443 
Motacilla  spp.,  301 
moth,  see  Lepidoptera 

Moulton,  Daniel  W.,  Reproductive  rate  and 
renesting  of  Red-winged  Blackbirds 
in  Minnesota,  119-121 
Mueller,  Helmut  C.,  Daniel  D.  Berger  and 
George  Allez,  Age  and  sex  differences 
in  wing  loading  and  other  aerody- 
namic characteristics  of  Sharp- 
shinned  Hawks,  491-499 

and  Nancy  S.  Mueller,  Observations 

of  a brood  of  Sharp-shinned  Hawks  in 
Ontario,  with  comments  on  the  func- 
tions of  sexual  dimorphism,  85-92 
Mueller,  Nancy  S.,  see  Mueller,  Helmut  C. 
and 

Murphy,  Joseph  R.,  see  Tomback,  Diana  F. 
and 

Murray,  Robert  K.,  Symbiotic  interaction 
between  Starhngs  and  deer,  549 
Murrelet,  Ancient,  see  Synthliboramphus 
antiquus 

Kitthtz’s,  see  Brachyramphus  brevirostris 
Marbled,  see  Brachyramphus  marmoratus 
Xantus,  see  Endomychura  hypoleuca 
Mustela  erminea,  528 
vison,  528 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


601 


Mycteria  americana,  146-163 
Myiarchus  crinitus,  218-242 
Myioborus  miniatus,  278 
Myrmeciza  longipes,  105 
loricata,  105 

Neocrex  columbianus,  141 
erythrops,  141 

Nesbitt,  Stephen  A.  and  G.  W.  Archibald, 
The  agonistic  repertoire  of  Sandhill 
Cranes,  99-103 

nest 

of  Amaurolimnas  concolor,  108 
of  Eremophila  alpestris,  523-524 
of  Icterus  gularis,  532-533 
site  selection  by  penguins,  243-248 
nesting 

association 

between  Icterus  gularis  and  large  ty- 
rannid  flycatchers,  535-536 

density 

statistical  significance  of  the  effect  on 
nest  predation,  541-542 

success 

estimation  of,  42-53 
nestling 
feeding  of 

Accipiter  striatus,  85-94 
temperature  regulation  of 
Buteo  regalis,  92-97 

Newton,  Ian,  Population  ecology  of  raptors, 
reviewed,  286-288 

niche 

expansion 

Petrochelidon  fulva,  506-518 
separation 

Turdus  sp.,  112-114 

Nichols,  J.  D.,  see  Hensler,  Gary  L.  and 


Nighthawk,  Common,  see  Chordeiles  minor 
Nightingale-Thrush,  Ruddy-capped,  see  Ca- 
tharus  frantzii 

Russet,  see  Catharus  occidentalis 
Nolan,  Val,  Jr.,  review  by,  294-295 
Numenius  tahitiensis,  445 
Nuthatch,  Pygmy,  see  Sitta  pygmaea 
Red-breasted,  see  Sitta  canadensis 
White-breasted,  see  Sitta  carolinensis 
Nycticorax  nycticorax,  150 
Nye,  Peter  E.,  see  Stone,  Ward  B.  and 


octopus,  see  Octopus  sp. 

Octopus  sp.,  279 
Odocoileus  hemionus,  549 
virginianus,  549 

Olson,  Storrs  L.  and  Alan  Feduccia,  Pres- 
byornis  and  the  origin  of  the  Anseri- 
formes  (Aves:  Charadriomorphae),  re- 
viewed, 407^12 

and  , Relationships  and  evo- 
lution of  flamingos  (Aves:  Phoenicop- 
teridae),  reviewed,  407^12 
Oncorhynchus  gorbusche.  111 
O’Neill,  John  P.,  review  by,  427^28 
Oniki,  Yoshika,  see  Willis,  Edwin  O.  and 


Oporornis  agilis,  164-188 
formosus,  164-188,  218-242 
Philadelphia,  164-188 
opossum,  see  Didelphis  virginianus 
Orcinus  orca,  10 

Oren,  David  C.  and  Nigel  J.  H.  Smith,  Notes 
on  the  status  of  the  Common  African 
Waxbill  in  Amazonia,  281-282 
Oriole,  Hooded,  see  Icterus  cucuUatus 
Lichtenstein’s,  see  Icterus  gularis 
Northern,  see  Icterus  galbula 
Ortalis  vetula,  536 
Orthoptera,  534,  540 
Oryzoborus  angolensis,  282 
Otus  spp.,  301 

Ovenbird,  see  Seiurus  auricapillus 
Owl,  Barred,  see  Strix  varia 

Great-horned,  see  Bubo  virginianus 
Long-eared,  see  Asio  otus 
Short-eared,  see  Asio  flammeus 
owls,  see  Otus  spp. 
oxpeckers,  see  Buphagus  spp. 

Oxyura  jamaicensis,  330,  331,  443 
oystercatchers,  see  Haematopus  spp. 
Pachyptila  desolata,  5-20 
Pagodroma  nivea,  5-20 
parasites 

Haematosiphon  indorus,  110 
Protocalliphora  in  Buteo  platypterus,  etc., 
110 

Trichomoniasis  in  Haliaeetus  leucoceph- 
alus,  etc.,  109 
parasitism 

of  Aythya  americana  on  Anas  platyrhyn- 
cos,  562-563 


Oceanites  oceanicus,  5-20 


602 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


of  Molothrus  aeneus  on  Icterus  gularis, 
535 

of  Molothrus  ater  on  Dendroica  petechia, 
249-258 
Paridae,  67-76 

Parker,  James  W.,  see  CrocoU,  Scott  and 


Partridge,  Grey,  see  Perdix  perdix 
Parula,  Northern,  see  Parula  americana 
Tropical,  see  Parula  americana  pulchra 
Parula  americana,  218-242,  344,  542-547 
americana  insularis,  306 
americana  pulchra,  306 
pitiayumi,  306 
Parula  spp.,  306 
Paruhdae,  67-76 

Parus  atricapillus,  54-66,  218-242,  273,  344, 
346,  347,  393,  394 
bicolor,  63,  218-242 
carolinensis,  54-66,  218-242 
hudsonicus,  344,  345,  346,  347 
Passer  domesticus,  196-206,  281,  551,  554 
Passerculus  sandwichensis,  344 
PassereUa  iliaca,  164-188,  344,  346 
Passerina  amoena,  59,  265 
cyanea,  59,  218-242,  265 
Pedioecetes  phasianellus,  330,  376 
Pelecanus  erythrorhynchos,  554-556 
occidentalis,  439,  445 
occidentalis  californicus,  397 
occidentalis  carolinensis,  397 
occidentalis  occidentalis,  397-400 
Pelican,  Brown,  see  Pelecanus  occidentalis 
White,  see  Pelecanus  erythrorhynchos 
Penguin,  Adelie,  see  Pygoscelis  adeliae 
Chinstrap,  see  Pygoscelis  antarctica 
Emperor,  see  Aptenodytes  forsteri 
Gentoo,  see  Pygoscelis  papua 
perch,  Sacramento,  see  Archoplites  inter- 
ruptus 
perches 

use  of  artificial,  547-548 
Percnostola  rufifrons,  106 
Perdix  perdix,  292 

Pereyra,  Marie  E.,  see  Baptista,  Luis  F., 

Martin  L.  Morton  and 

Perisoreus  canadensis,  88 
Peterson,  Roger  Tory,  Introduction  and 
commentaries  on:  Audubon,  birds  of 
America,  reviewed,  293-294 


, A field  guide  to  the  birds,  fourth 

edition,  reviewed,  425^27 
Petrel,  Antarctic,  see  Thalassoica  antarctica 
Blue,  see  Halobaena  caerulea 
Dark-rumped,  see  Pterodroma  phaeopygia 
sandwichensis 

Mottled,  see  Pterodroma  inexpecta 
Snow,  see  Pagodroma  nivea 
Petrochelidon  fulva,  506-518 
pyrrhonota,  514 
Phaethon  aetherus,  439 
Phaethornis  supercihosus,  414^16 
Phalacrocorax  auritus,  554-556 
carbo,  357 

Phalacrocorax  spp.,  441,  443,  446 
Phalaenoptilus  nuttaUii,  363 
Phalarope,  Red,  see  Phalaropus  fulicarius 
phalaropes,  see  Phalaropodidae 
Phalaropodidae,  446 
Phalaropus  fulicarius,  557 
Pheucticus  ludovicianus,  164-188,  218-242 
Phillips,  Allan  R.,  Subspecies  vs  forgotten 
species:  the  case  of  Grayson’s  Robin 
(Turdus  graysoni),  301-309  (Frontis- 
piece) 

Philohela  spp.,  471 

Phoebe,  Eastern,  see  Sayornis  phoebe 
Phoebetria  palpebrata,  5-20 
Phoenicopteridae,  407-412 
Phrynosoma  sp.,  94 
PhyUoscopus  coUybita,  59 
trochilus,  59 
PhyUoscopus  spp.,  301 
Pica  pica,  196,  550 
Picoides  pubescens,  63 
viUosus,  274 

Pigeon,  Cape,  see  Daption  capense 
Pinicola  enucleator,  344 
Pintail,  see  Anas  acuta 
Pipilo  albicoUis,  59 

erythrophthalmus,  218-242 
fuscus,  59 
Pipilo  spp.,  301 

Piranga  olivacea,  164-188,  218-242,  478-490 
Pitangus  sulphuratus,  535-536 
Pitelka,  Frank  A.  (ed.),  Shorebirds  in  marine 
environments,  reviewed,  131-132 
Pleasants,  Barbara  Yohai,  Aspects  of  the 
breeding  biology  of  a subtropiccd  ori- 
ole, Icterus  gularis,  531-537 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


603 


Plectrophenax  nivalis,  522 
Plegadis  chihi,  147 

Plover,  Egyptian,  see  Pluvialis  aegyptius 
Golden,  see  Pluvialis  dominica  fulva 
Greater  Golden,  see  Pluvialis  apricaria 
Snowy,  see  Charadrius  alexandrinus  ni- 
vosus 

plovers,  see  Hoplopterus  sp. 

Pluvialis  aegyptius,  570-571 
apricaria,  560 
dominica  fulva,  445 

Poche,  Richard  M.,  Crows  steal  golf  balls  in 
Bangladesh,  280 
Podiceps  auritus,  560-561 
Podicipedidae,  446,  560 
Polioptila  caerulea,  218-242 
Pomacea  sp.,  552,  553 
Pomatomus  saltatrix,  280 
Pomoxis  annularis,  556 
Poor-will,  see  Phalaenoptilus  nuttallii 
population 
density 

caprimulgids,  363-371 
passerine,  340-349 
Porphyrula  martinica,  267 
Porzana  spilopterus,  141 
Porzana  spp.,  108 

Powell,  George  V.  N.,  review  by,  425 
Prairie  Chicken,  Attwater’s,  see  Tympanu- 
chus  cupido  attwateri 
Greater,  see  Tympanuchus  cupido 
Preble,  David  E.  and  Frank  H.  Heppner, 
Breeding  success  in  an  isolated  pop- 
ulation of  Rock  Doves,  357-362 
predation 

by  Aphelocoma  coerulescens  on  Hylocich- 
la  guttata,  550-551 

by  Falco  peregrinus  on  Ajaia  ajaja,  548 
on  eggs  and  nestbngs  of  Eremophila  al- 
pestris,  528 
Presbyomis,  407^12 
President’s  message,  264 
Preston,  Charles  R.,  Environmental  influ- 
ence on  soaring  in  wintering  Red- 
tailed Hawks,  350-356 
Prion,  Antarctic,  see  Pachyptila  desolata 
productivity 

of  House  Sparrows  in  relation  to  climate, 
196-206 

ProtocaUiphora  avium,  110 


Protonotaria  citrea,  164-188 
Psilorhinus  morio,  539 

Ptarmigan,  White-tailed,  see  Lagopus  leu- 
curus 

Willow,  see  Lagopus  lagopus 
Pterodroma  inexpecta,  14 

phaeopygia  sandwichensis,  445 
Ptychoramphus  aleutica,  439 
Puffin,  Common,  see  Fratercula  arctica 
Puffinus  griseus,  14,  439,  446 
puffinus,  110 
puffinus  neweUi,  445 
tenuirostris,  446 

Pubch,  Warren  M.  and  Rebecca  M.  Dellin- 
ger, An  example  of  a hybrid  Green 
Jay  X Blue  Jay,  538-540 
Pygoscelis  adeliae,  2-20,  243-248 
antarctica,  243-248 
papua,  243-248 
Pyriglena  leucoptera,  105,  106 
Quiscalus  mexicanus,  536 
quiscula,  500-505 

Raikow,  Robert  J.,  reviews  by,  125-126,  299, 
300,  407-412,  416,  417,  437,  573,  574 
Rail,  Black,  see  Laterallus  jamaicensis 
California  Clapper,  see  RaUus  longirostris 
obsoletus 

Clapper,  see  RaUus  longirostris 
Galapagos,  see  Laterallus  spilonotus 
Gray-necked  Wood,  see  Aramides  cajanea 
Rakestraw,  James  L.  and  James  L.  Baker, 
Dusky  Seaside  Sparrow  feeds  Red- 
winged Blackbird  fledglings,  540 
RaUina  castaneiceps,  141 
RaUus  longirostris,  443 
longirostris  obsoletus,  440 
RaUus  spp.,  108 

Ralph,  C.  John,  Age  ratios  and  their  possible 
use  in  determining  autumn  routes  of 
passerine  migrants,  164-188 
rat,  kangaroo,  see  Dipodomys  sp. 

RatcUffe,  Derek,  The  Peregrine  Falcon,  re- 
viewed, 571-572 

Raveling,  Dennis  G.,  see  McLandress,  M. 

Robert  and 

Raven,  Common,  see  Corvus  corax 
RazorbiU,  see  Alca  torda 
Redstart,  American,  see  Setophaga  ruticiUa 
Slate-throated,  see  Myioborus  miniatus 
Redwing,  European,  see  Turdus  iliacus 


604 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Regulus  satrapa,  218-242,  344 
renesting 

of  Agelaius  phoeniceus,  1 19-121 
reproductive  rate 

of  Agelaius  phoeniceus,  119-121 
request  for  assistance 

for  study  skins,  skeletons  and  alcoholics, 
569 

resource  use 

by  wading  birds,  145-163 
Rhodinocichla  rosea,  392 
Rhopornis  ardesiaca,  103-107 
Rice,  Jake  C.,  Behavioral  implications  of 
aberrant  song  of  a Red-eyed  Vireo, 
383-390 

, see  Vassallo,  Monique  I.  and 


Richards,  Alan  J.,  British  birds — a field 
guide,  reviewed,  128-129 
Riley,  John  O.,  see  Matteson,  Sumner  W. 
and 

Ripley,  S.  Dillon,  see  Ali,  S^m  and  

Rissa  brevirostris,  111-112 
Rissa  spp.,  443 

Robbins,  Mark  B.,  Two  cases  of  commensal 
feeding  between  passerines,  391-392 
Robertson,  Raleigh  J.,  see  Clark,  Karen  L. 
and 

Robin,  American,  see  Turdus  migratorius 
Clay-colored,  see  Turdus  grayi 
Grayson’s,  see  Turdus  graysoni 
Rufous-backed,  see  Turdus  rufo-paUiatus 
White-throated,  see  Turdus  phaeopygus 
Robin-Chat,  Red-capped,  see  Cossypha  na- 
talensis 
roosting 

of  Chaetura  pelagica,  77-84 
Rostratula  benghalensis,  469 
Ryan,  Mark  R.,  Evasive  behavior  of  Ameri- 
can Coots  to  kleptoparasitism  by  wa- 
terfowl, 274-275 

Ryder,  Ronald  A.,  see  Gorenzel,  Warner  P., 

and  Clait  E.  Braun 

Rynchops  nigra,  444 
Rynchops  sp.,  123,  441 

salmon,  humpbacked,  see  Oncorhynchus 
gorbusche 

Sanderling,  see  Crocethia  alba 
Sandpiper,  Spotted,  see  Actitis  macularia 
Upland,  see  Bartramia  longicauda 


Savage,  Christopher  (ed.).  Pheasants  in 
Asia,  reviewed,  574 
Sayornis  phoebe,  164-188 
ScateUa  stagnalis,  552,  553 
Scaup,  Lesser,  see  Aythya  affinis 
Schreiber,  Ralph  W,,  David  W.  Belitsky  and 
Bruce  A.  Sorrie,  Notes  on  Brown  Pel- 
icans in  Puerto  Rico,  397-400 
Sclerurus  guatemalensis,  392 
Scolopax  spp.,  465,  471,  472 
seal,  grey,  see  Halichoerus  grypus 
leopard,  see  Hydrurga  leptonyx 
Seedeater,  Chestnut-beUied,  see  Sporophila 
castaneiventris 

Seedfinch,  Lesser,  see  Oryzoborus  angolen- 
sis 

Seiurus  auricapiUus,  21^1,  218-242,  542- 
547 

motacilla,  218-242 

noveboracensis,  164-188,  344,  345,  346 
Setophaga  ruticiUa,  188,  218-242,  295,  478- 
490 

sexual  dimorphism 

Accipter  striatus,  85-92 
sharks,  see  Carcharinus  leucas 
see  Carcharinus  longimanus 
see  Carcharodon  carcharias 
see  Galeocerdo  cuvieri 
Sharp,  David  E.,  see  Lokemoen,  John  T.  and 


Shearwater,  Manx,  see  Puffinus  puffinus 
NeweD’s,  see  Puffinus  puffinus  neweUi 
Slender-biUed,  see  Puffinus  tenuirostris 
Sooty,  see  Puffinus  griseus 
Shipley,  Frank  S.,  see  FretweU,  Stephen  D. 
and 

shorefly,  see  ScateUa  stagnalis 
Shoveler,  Northern,  see  Anas  clypeata 
shrew,  see  Sorex  cinereus 
Shugart,  Gary  W.,  Mary  A.  Fitch  and  Vern 
M.  Shugart,  Minimizing  investigator 
disturbance  in  observational  studies 
of  colonial  birds:  access  to  blinds 
through  tunnels,  565-569 
Shugart,  Vern  M.,  see  Shugart,  Gary  W., 

Mary  A.  Fitch  and 

Siala  currucoides,  288 

Siegel-Causey,  Douglas  and  Thomas  E. 
Meehan,  Red-legged  Kittiwakes  for- 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


605 


age  in  mixed  species  flocks  in  south- 
eastern Alaska,  111-112 
Siskin,  Pine,  see  Carduelis  pinus 
Sitta  canadensis,  218-242,  273,  344 
carolinensis,  63,  218-242,  see  271—274 
europaea,  274 
pygmaea,  551 
Sittidae,  67-76 

Skimmer,  Black,  see  Rynchops  nigra 
skimmers,  see  Rynchops  sp. 

Skua,  South  Polar,  see  Catharacta  maccor- 
micki 

skuas,  see  Catharacta  spp. 

Skutch,  Alexander  F.,  The  imperative  call: 
a naturalist’s  quest  in  temperate  and 
tropical  America,  reviewed,  425 
, A naturalist  on  a tropical  farm,  re- 
viewed, 572-573 

Slud,  Paul,  The  birds  of  Hacienda  Palo 
Verde,  Guanacaste,  Costa  Rica,  re- 
viewed, 435^36 

Smith,  Nigel  J.  H.,  see  Oren,  David  C.  and 


snail,  apple,  see  Pomacea  sp. 

Snell,  Richard  R.,  Herring  Gull  attacks  and 
eats  adult  male  Oldsquaw,  110-111 
Snipe,  African,  see  Capella  gaUinago  nigri- 
pennis 

Andean,  see  Capella  jamesoni 
Asiatic,  see  CapeUa  stenura 
Australian,  see  Capella  hardwickii 
Banded,  see  Capella  imperialis 
Bogota,  see  Capella  imperialis 
Common,  see  Capella  gaUinago 
CordiUeran,  see  CapeUa  stricklandii 
Double,  see  CapeUa  media 
English,  466,  467 

Ethiopian,  see  CapeUa  gaUinago  nigripen- 
nis 

Fantail,  see  CapeUa  gaUinago 
Forest,  see  CapeUa  megala 
Giant,  see  CapeUa  undulata 
Great,  see  CapeUa  media 
Hermit,  see  CapeUa  solitaria 
Himalayan,  see  CapeUa  nemoricola 
Imperial,  see  CapeUa  imperialis 
Jack,  see  Lymnocryptes  minimus 
Japanese,  see  CapeUa  hardwickii 
Lathmans,  see  CapeUa  hardwickii 


Madagascar,  see  CapeUa  macrodactyla 
Marsh,  see  CapeUa  megala 
Noble,  see  CapeUa  nobilis 
Painted,  see  Rostratula  benghalensis 
Paramo,  see  CapeUa  nobilis 
Pintail,  see  CapeUa  stenura 
Silent,  see  Lymnocryptes  minimus 
Single,  see  CapeUa  gaUinago 
Solitary,  see  CapeUa  solitaria 
Swinhoe’s,  see  CapeUa  megala 
Wilson’s,  see  CapeUa  gaUinago 
Wood,  see  CapeUa  nemoricola 
Softwing,  White-whiskered,  see  Melacoptila 
panamensis 

Somateria  moUissima,  279,  559—560 

Somateria  sp.,  290 

song 

Amaurolimnas  concolor,  107 
mimesis  by  Melospiza  lincolnii,  265-267 
Rhopornis  ardesiaca,  105-106 
Seiurus  aurocapiUus,  21-41 
Songthrush,  see  Turdus  philomelos 
Sorex  cinereus,  528 

Sorrie,  Bruce  A.,  see  Schreiber,  Ralph  W., 

David  W.  BeUtsky  and 

Soule,  Michael  E.  and  Bruce  A.  Wilcox 
(eds.).  Conservation  biology:  an  evo- 
lutionary-ecological perspective,  re- 
viewed, 420-422 

Sparrow,  Chipping,  see  SpizeUa  passerina 
Dusky  Seaside,  see  Ammospiza  maritima 
nigrescens 

Field,  see  SpizeUa  pusiUa 
Fox,  see  PassereUa  Uiaca 
Grasshopper,  see  Ammodramus  savanna- 
rum 

House,  see  Passer  domesticus 
Lincoln’s,  see  Melospiza  lincolnii 
Rufous-coUared,  see  Zonotrichia  capensis 
Savannah,  see  Passerculus  sandwichensis 
Seaside,  see  Ammospiza  maritima 
Sharp-taUed,  see  Ammospiza  caudacuta 
Song,  see  Melospiza  melodia 
Swamp,  see  Melospiza  georgiana 
White-crowned,  see  Zonotrichia  leuco- 
phrys 

White-throated,  see  Zonotrichia  albicoUis 
YeUow-browed,  see  Ammodramus  aurif- 
rons 


606 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Sparrowhawk,  see  Accipiter  nisus 
species  diversity 

Newfoundland  passerines,  340-349 
Spermophilus  richardsonii,  572 
townsendii,  572 
spider,  see  Lycosidae 
Spinus  tristis,  344,  346 
Spiza  americana,  547-548 
Spizella  passerina,  88 

pusiUa,  164-188,  218-242,  541 
Spofford,  Walter  R.,  review  by,  129 
Spoonbill,  Roseate,  see  Ajaia  ajaja 
Sporophila  castaneiventris,  282 
Springer,  Paul  F.,  see  Avery,  Michael  L., 

and  Nancy  S.  Dailey 

Sprunt,  Alexander,  Jr.,  see  Griscom,  Ludlow 
and 

Squatina  squatina,  279 
squirrel,  red,  see  Tamiasciurus  hudsonicus 
Richardson’s  ground,  see  Spermophilus 
richardsonii 

Townsend’s  ground,  see  Spermophilus 
townsendii 

Starling,  see  Sturnus  vulgaris 
status 

of  Estrilda  astrid  in  Amazonia,  281-282 
of  Morphnus  guianensis  in  Guatemala, 
284-285 

Stepney,  Philip  H.  R.  and  Peter  C.  Boxall, 
First  record  of  the  Black-chinned 
Hummingbird  in  Alberta,  405^06 
Stercorarius  spp.,  439,  443,  446 
Sterna  albifrons  browni,  439 
caspia,  567,  568 
maxima,  390-391 
paradisaea,  13 
sandvicensis,  390 

Sterna  spp.,  123,  392,  441,  445,  446 
Stiles,  F.  Gary,  Notes  on  the  Uniform  Crake 
in  Costa  Rica,  107-108 

, review  by,  435^36 

and  Larry  L.  Wolf,  Ecology  and  evo- 
lution of  lek  mating  behavior  in  the 
Long-tailed  Hermit  Hummingbird,  re- 
viewed, 414^16 

Stone,  Ward  B.  and  Peter  E.  Nye,  Tricho- 
moniasis in  Bald  Eagles,  109 
Storer,  Robert  W.,  The  Rufous-faced  Crake 
(Laterallus  xenopterus)  and  its  Para- 
guayan congeners,  137-144  (Frontis- 
piece) 


Stork,  Marabou,  see  Leptoptilos  crumenif- 
erus 

White,  see  Ciconia  ciconia 
Wood,  see  Mycteria  americana 
Storm-petrel,  Wilson’s  see  Oceanites  ocean- 
icus 

storm-petrels,  see  Hydrobatidae 

Strix  varia,  272 

SturneUa  magna,  547-548 

Sturnus  vulgaris,  344,  347,  549 

sucker,  Tahoe,  see  Catostomus  tahoensis 

Sula  spp.,  445 

Sus  scrofa,  549 

Sutton,  George  Miksch,  Commentary  on.  To 
a young  bird  artist:  letters  from  Louis 
Agassiz  Fuertes  to  Geroge  Miksch 
Sutton,  reviewed,  295-296 

, Iceland  summer:  adventures  of  a 

bird  painter,  reviewed,  299 

, On  aerial  and  ground  displays  of  the 

world’s  snipes,  457^77  (Frontispiece) 
Swallow,  Bam,  see  Hirundo  rustica 
Cave,  see  Petrochelidon  fulva 
Chff,  see  Petrochelidon  pyrrhonota 
Swan,  Mute,  see  Cygnus  olor 

Trumpeter,  see  Cygnus  cygnus  buccinator 
Swift,  Black,  see  Cypseloides  niger 
Chimney,  see  Chaetura  pelagica 
Common,  see  Apus  apus 
Sylvia  atricapiUa,  59 
communis,  59 
symbiosis 

between  Starlings  and  deer,  549 
Synthliboramphus  antiquus,  402 
Szaro,  Robert  C.  and  Russell  P.  Baida,  Bird 
community  dynamics  in  a ponderosa 
pine  forest,  reviewed,  419^20 
Tabor,  James  E.,  see  Thompson,  Bruce  C. 

and 

Tachyeres  sp.,  289 
Tamiasciurus  hudsonicus,  334 
Tanager,  Scarlet,  see  Piranga  olivacea 
Speckled,  see  Tangara  guttata 
Tangara  guttata,  572 
taxonomy 

Laterallus  spp.,  137-144 
Turdus  graysoni,  301-309 
Taylor,  L.  R.,  see  Anderson,  R.  M.,  B.  D. 

Turner  and 

Teal,  Blue-winged,  see  Anas  discors 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


607 


Cinnamon,  see  Anas  cyanoptera 
Green-winged,  see  Anas  crecca  carolinen- 
sis 

temperature  regulation 

in  nesting  Buteo  regalis,  92-97 
Tern,  Arctic,  see  Sterna  paradisaea 

California  Least,  see  Sterna  albifrons 
browni 

Caspian,  see  Sterna  caspia 
Royal,  see  Sterna  maxima 
Sandwich,  see  Sterna  sandvicensis 
terns,  see  Sterna  spp. 

Tetrao  parvirostris,  290 
urogaUus,  290 
Tetrastes  bonasia,  290 
sewerzowi,  290 

Thalassoica  antarctica,  5-20  (Frontispiece) 
Theide,  Walther,  Water  and  shore  birds,  re- 
viewed, 574 

Thompson,  Bruce  C.  and  James  E.  Tabor, 
Mallard  using  moving  vehicles  for 
predator  avoidance,  277-278 
Thrasher,  Brown,  see  Toxostoma  rufum 
Threlfall,  William,  see  Cannings,  Richard  J. 
and 

Threskiomis  aethiopica,  148-163 
spinicollis,  148 

Thrush,  Gray-cheeked,  see  Catharus  min- 
ima 

Hermit,  see  Catharus  [Hylocichla]  guttata 
Swainson’s,  see  Catharus  ustulata 
White-necked,  see  Turdus  albicoUis 
Wood,  see  Catharus  [Hylocichla]  muste- 
lina 

Thrush-tanager,  Rose-breasted,  see  Rho- 
dinocichla  rosea 

Thryothorus  ludovicianus,  218-242 
Thurber,  Walter  A.,  Aerial  “play”  of  Black 
Vultures,  97 

Thyromanes  bewickii,  59 
Tipulidae,  529 

Titmouse,  Tufted,  see  Parus  bicolor 
toad,  marine,  see  Bufo  marinus 
Tomback,  Diana  F.  and  Joseph  R.  Murphy, 
Food  deprivation  and  temperature 
regulation  in  nestling  Ferruginous 
Hawks,  92-97 
tower  kills,  189-195 
Towhee,  Brown,  see  Pipilo  fuscus 


Rufous-sided,  see  Pipilo  erythrophthal- 
mus 

White-throated,  see  Pipilo  albicoUis 
towhees,  see  Pipilo  spp. 

Toxostoma  rufum,  164-188,  218-242 
Traylor,  Melvin  A.,  review  by,  436^137 
tree  creepers,  see  Certhia  brachydactyla 
see  Certhia  familiaris 
Trichechus  manatus,  400 
Trichomonas  gaUinae,  109 
Trivelpiece,  Wayne,  see  Volkman,  Nicholas 

J.  and 

Troglodytes  aedon,  59 
ochraceus,  278-279 
troglodytes,  218-242,  344,  347 
Troglodytidae,  67-76 
Turdus  albicoUis,  112-114 
assimiUs,  302,  307 
fisheri,  392 
grayi,  302 

graysoni,  301-309  (Frontispiece) 
iUacus,  112-114 
merula,  112-114 

migratorius,  112-114,  164-188,  218-242, 
344,  345,  346,  564 
phaeopygus,  302,  303,  307 
philomelos,  112-114 
pilaris,  112-114 
rufo-paUiatus,  302-309 
Turner,  B.  D.,  see  Anderson,  R.  M., 

and  L.  R.  Taylor 

Turnstone,  Ruddy,  see  Arenaria  interpres 
Tye,  Alan,  Ground-feeding  methods  and 
niche  separation  in  thrushes,  112-114 
Tympanuchus  cupido,  376,  424 
cupido  attwateri,  376 
Tyrannus  melancholicus,  535-536 
tyrannus,  344,  547-548 
Tyrannus  spp.,  302,  535 
Ure,  SteUanie,  Hawk  lady,  reviewed,  423- 
424 

van  Riper,  Charles,  HI,  review  by,  417-419 
Vardaman,  James  M.,  CaU  coUect,  ask  for 
birdman,  review,  299-300 
VassaUo,  Monique  I.  and  Jake  C.  Rice,  Dif- 
ferential passerine  density  and  diver- 
sity between  Newfoundland  and  off- 
shore Gull  Island,  340-349 
Vaughan,  Richard,  Arctic  summer:  birds  in 
north  Norway,  reviewed,  298 


608 


THE  WILSON  BULLETIN  • Vol.  93,  No.  4,  December  1981 


Veery,  see  Catharus  fuscescens 
Verdin,  see  Auriparus  flaviceps 
Vermivora  celata,  164-188,  295 
chrysoptera,  218-242,  295 
pinus,  164-188,  295 
ruficapiUa,  164-188,  542-547 
viper,  tree,  see  Bothrops  lateralis 
Vireo,  Red-eyed,  see  Vireo  olivaceus 
Philadelphia,  see  Vireo  philadelphicus 
Solitary,  see  Vireo  solitarius 
White-eyed,  see  Vireo  griseus 
Yellow-throated,  see  Vireo  flavifrons 
Vireo  flavifrons,  218-242 
griseus,  218-242 

olivaceus,  164-188,  218-242,  383-390, 
478-490 

philadelphicus,  188 
solitarius,  218-242 
Vireonidae,  67-76 
vocahzation 

caprimulgids,  363-371 
Vireo  olivaceus,  383-390 
Volatinia  jacarina,  282 
vole,  bank,  see  Clethrionomys  glareolus 
meadow,  see  Microtus  pennsylvanicus 
Volkman,  Nicholas  J.  and  Wayne  Trivel- 
piece.  Nest-site  selection  among  Ade- 
lie,  Chinstrap  and  Gentoo  penguins  in 
mixed  species  rookeries,  243-248 
Vulpes  vulpes,  528 
wagtails,  see  MotaciUa  spp. 

Walter,  Hartmut,  review  by,  129-131 
Walters,  Michael,  The  complete  book  of 
birds  of  the  world,  reviewed,  299 
Wander,  Wade,  Red  Phalarope  eating  car- 
rion, 557 

Warbler,  Bay-breasted,  see  Dendroica  cas- 
tanea 

Black-and-white,  see  Mniotilta  varia 
Blackburnian,  see  Dendroica  fusca 
BlackpoU,  see  Dendroica  striata 
Black-throated  Blue,  see  Dendroica  ni- 
grescens 

see  Dendroica  caerulescens 
Black-throated  Green,  see  Dendroica  vi- 
rens 

Canada,  see  Wilsonia  [Dendroica]  cana- 
densis 

Cerulean,  see  Dendroica  cerulea 


Chestnut-sided,  see  Dendroica  pensylvan- 
ica 

Connecticut,  see  Oporornis  agilis 
Fan-tailed,  see  Euthlypis  lachrymosa 
Golden-crowned,  see  Basileuterus  culici- 
vorus 

Golden-winged,  see  Vermivora  chryso- 
ptera 

Hooded,  see  Wilsonia  citrina 
Kentucky,  see  Oporornis  formosus 
Kirtland’s,  see  Dendroica  kirtlandii 
Magnolia,  see  Dendroica  magnolia 
Marsh,  see  Acrocephalus  palustris 
Mourning,  see  Oporornis  Philadelphia 
Nashville,  see  Vermivora  ruficapiUa 
Orange-crowned,  see  Vermivora  celata 
Palm,  see  Dendroica  palmarum 
Parula,  see  Parula  americana 
Pine,  see  Dendroica  pinus 
Prairie,  see  Dendroica  discolor 
Prothonotary,  see  Protonotaria  citrea 
Reed,  see  Acrocephalus  scirpaceus 
Swainson’s,  see  Limnothlypis  swainsonii 
WiUow,  see  PhyUoscopus  trochilus 
Wilson’s,  see  Wilsonia  pusiUa 
Worm-eating,  see  Helmitheros  vermivorus 
YeUow,  see  Dendroica  petechia 
YeUow-rumped,  see  Dendroica  coronata 
YeUow-throated,  see  Dendroica  dominica 
warblers,  see  Cisticola  spp. 
see  Parula  spp. 
see  PhyUoscopus  spp. 
wasp,  528 

Waterthrush,  Louisiana,  see  Seiurus  mota- 
ciUa 

Northern,  see  Seiurus  noveboracensis 
WaxbiU,  Black-cheeked,  see  Estrilda  ery- 
thronotos 

Common  African,  see  Estrilda  astrild 
Waxwing,  Cedar,  see  BombyciUa  cedrorum 
WeUer,  Milton,  The  island  waterfowl,  re- 
viewed, 289 

whale,  kiUer,  see  Orcinus  orca 

Whaley,  Wayne  H.,  see  EUis,  David  H.  and 


Whip-poor-wiU,  see  Caprimulgis  vociferus 
White,  Clayton,  review  by,  286-288 
Whitethroat,  see  Sylvia  communis 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME  93 


609 


Whitmore,  Robert  C.,  reviews  by,  292-293, 
432-435 

, see  Maurer,  Brian  A.  and 

Whittemore,  Margaret,  Chimney  Swifts  and 
their  relatives,  reviewed,  573 
Wigeon,  American,  see  Anas  americana 
Wilcox,  Bruce  A.,  see  Soule,  Michael  E.  and 


WiUet,  see  Catoptrophorus  semipalmatus 
WiUis,  Edwin  O.  and  Yoshika  Oniki,  Notes 
on  the  Slender  Antbird,  103-107 
Wilsonia  canadensis,  164-188,  218-242,  478 
citrina,  164-188,  218-242 
pusiUa,  164-188,  278,  344 
Wolf,  Larry  L.,  see  Stiles,  F.  Gary  and 


woodcocks,  see  Philohela  spp. 
see  Scolopax  spp. 

Woodpecker,  Downy,  see  Picoides 
[Dendrocopos]  pubescens 
Golden-naped,  see  Melanerpes  chry- 
sauchen 

Hairy,  see  Picoides  [Dendrocopos]  villosus 
Pileated,  see  Dryocopus  pileatus 
Wood-rail,  see  Aramides  sp. 


Wren,  Bewick’s,  see  Thyromanes  bewickii 
Cactus,  see  Campylorhynchus  brunnei- 
capiUus 

Carobna,  see  Thryothorus  ludovicianus 
House,  see  Troglodytes  aedon 
Ochraceous,  see  Troglodytes  ochraceus 
Winter,  see  Troglodytes  troglodytes 
Yellowthroat,  Common,  see  Geothlypis  tri- 
chas 

Zammuto,  Richard  M.  and  Edwin  C.  Franks, 
Environmental  effects  on  roosting  be- 
havior by  Chimney  Swifts,  77-84 
Zenaida  macroura,  109 
Zicus,  Michael  C.,  Canada  Goose  brood  be- 
havior and  survival  estimates  at  Crex 
Meadows,  Wisconsin,  207-217 
Zink,  Robert  M.,  Observations  of  seabirds 
during  a cruise  from  Ross  Island  to 
Anvers  Island,  Antarctica,  1-20 
(Frontispiece) 

Zonotrichia  albicoUis,  344 
capensis,  532 
leucophrys,  169,  172 
leucophrys  oriantha,  265 


This  issue  of  The  Wilson  Bulletin  was  published  on  22  February  1982. 


• K ■ 


* . j ’ ■ V .i"'  ■ ■ i'^ 

i^;v  Xifv  '^4  Hr  * s*  If r/.  «;ll  P.; 


t,  ',«>■«■  ■•v,nr.i  /^f'^  • 


Ih 


',**'.V‘(**V^ 


4^rr,  |i»r*« 

* *v!jf  4i%  j 


nA  i.^i  - ' 

"41^1 ' ;^m3fc’HT. » ■ .-•»  - iisT»1^’t  «*  ' '^li 


. '*4,  V » t ■\HtMf  \^  4it^^  >iiI4'4r 


.M.  lisikTit-.  " ' ‘/.'i:. iW  -Af,  nnkm.^  -v^  *| 

n»r:Vro,... 


42rf«*  .>''v.,  ^ 'k^'  KTNfrJ  J 

V|^W.fi»iwo4yt?  ’■  ■ ‘ ■ 


.'.T'Vwuf*?  ^9im9  ^ *iirti»**- 

♦ I «^f«N||riJt»m‘i) 

: ui.pjfcijfi^.  * AtifAV''‘  r, 

■ '•f.  ‘tNu-  ‘U,  < ■'  f^'. 

* i?!'  .vti\  ,«tvJ*:€£‘u>4 

r-T:  -Of'Tirr, 

-.  , • -r  . ^ . 

V«»U  V.'-  I ,'■  : 

.■  ^<.  ^ ‘ . . =■ 

;>  . d,  • ■ . 4. 


,t  •'-'i  iiiTk  tn»  • ' i:*«M»«''rr*rrijh;T.v 


• i4ii»^oflif|  ^ hm'W 

'**n  ,♦  ^ '*  - t 

> 

■if*'  |jll^*i  ''lfiw:tM.«.'-« 


‘V.4 


f *(  *\r. 
, ■k'P-. 


>i|^,r«  % i )titei4|^ 


i»'>% 


l^n  3'<f*»f 


k,‘'»v  w 


" S»^u^i^^  '^  3 . ijf, 


-■d 
■■  r- 


4%V  -11*^4 


••ii  > 54UiMi  ' 4' 

••Hi,  cut  I .<r 


■'  • •.  .« .!<  Cjti»Apit 

> , ■ ..  ••  • -.«  bf ^ ' 

•4*h,|l  1.  f|-»  . %f*A  w/r 


7070  064 


t 

..%  ^ ' >1  i/W  ,uM 


■ r:k»Jii]i. 

%» 


The  Wilson  Bulletin 


Editor  Jon  C.  Barlow 

Department  of  Ornithology 

Royal  Ontario  Museum 

100  Queen’s  Park 

Toronto,  Ontario,  Canada  MSS  2C6 
Assistant  Editor  MARGARET  L.  May 
Senior  Editorial  Assistants  Gary  Bortolotti 
I Nancy  Flood 

' Editorial  Assistants  Keith  L.  Bildstein  Richard  R.  Snell 

C.  Davison  Ankney  James  D.  Rising 

Review  Editor  Robert  Raikow  Color  Plate  Editor  William  A.  Lunk 

Department  of  Biological  Sci-  865  North  Wagner  Road 

ences  Ann  Arbor,  MI  48103 

University  of  Pittsburgh 
Pittsburgh,  PA  15260 

Suggestions  to  Authors 

See  Wilson  Bulletin,  91:366,  1979  for  more  detailed  “Suggestions  to  Authors.” 
Manuscripts  intended  for  publication  in  The  Wilson  Bulletin  should  be  submitted  in  tripli- 
cate, neatly  typewritten,  double-spaced,  with  at  least  3 cm  margins,  and  on  one  side  only  of 
good  quality  white  paper.  Do  not  submit  xerographic  copies  that  are  made  on  slick,  heavy 
paper.  Tables  should  be  typed  on  separate  sheets,  and  should  be  narrow  and  deep  rather 
I than  wide  and  shallow.  Follow  the  AOU  Check-list  (Fifth  Edition,  1957)  and  the  32nd  Sup- 

I plement  (Auk,  90:411-419,  1973),  insofar  as  scientific  names  of  U.S.  and  Canadian  birds  are 

) concerned.  Summaries  of  major  papers  should  be  brief  but  quotable.  Where  fewer  than  5 
^ papers  are  cited,  the  citations  may  be  included  in  the  text.  All  citations  in  “General  Notes” 

^ should  be  included  in  the  text.  Follow  carefully  the  style  used  in  this  issue  in  listing  the 

literature  cited;  otherwise,  follow  the  “CBE  Style  Manual”  (1972,  AIBS).  Photographs  for 
j illustrations  should  have  good  contrast  and  be  on  gloss  paper.  Submit  prints  unmounted  and 
attach  to  each  a brief  but  adequate  legend.  Do  not  write  heavily  on  the  backs  of  photographs. 

' Diagrams  and  line  drawings  should  be  in  black  ink  and  their  lettering  large  enough  to  permit 

' reduction.  Original  figures  or  photographs  submitted  must  be  smaller  than  22  x 28  cm. 

Alterations  in  copy  after  the  type  has  been  set  must  be  charged  to  the  author. 

\ 

Notice  of  Change  of  Address 

; If  your  address  changes,  notify  the  Society  immediately.  Send  your  complete  new  address 

to  Ornithological  Societies  of  North  America,  % Sandra  L.  L.  Gaunt,  Box  21160,  Columbus, 
Ohio  43221. 

The  permanent  mailing  address  of  the  Wilson  Ornithological  Society  is:  do  The  Museum 
< of  Zoology,  The  University  of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  Michigan  48109.  Persons  having  business 

' with  any  of  the  officers  may  address  them  at  their  various  addresses  given  on  the  back  of 

, the  front  cover,  and  all  matters  pertaining  to  the  Bulletin  should  be  sent  directly  to  the 

j Editor. 

( 

Membership  Inquiries 

^ Membership  inquiries  should  be  sent  to  Dr.  Keith  Bildstein,  Department  of  Biology,  Win- 

\ throp  College,  Rock  Hill,  South  Carolina  29733. 


CONTENTS 


ON  AERIAL  AND  GROUND  DISPLAYS  OF  THE  WORLD’S  SNIPES George  Miksch  Sutton  457 

FORAGING  OF  FIVE  BIRD  SPECIES  IN  TWO  FORESTS  WITH  DIFFERENT  VEGETATION  STRUCTURE 

Brian  A.  Maurer  and  Robert  C.  Whitmore  478 

AGE  AND  SEX  DIFFERENCES  IN  WING  LOADING  AND  OTHER  AERODYNAMIC  CHARACTERISTICS 

OF  SHARP-SHINNED  HAWKS Helmut  C.  Mueller,  Daniel  D.  Berger  and  George  Allez  491 

EVIDENCE  FOR  AERODYNAMIC  ADVANTAGES  OF  TAIL  KEELING  IN  THE  COMMON  CRACKLE 

Scott  Hickman  500 

REPRODUCTIVE  CORRELATES  OF  ENVIRONMENTAL  VARIATION  AND  NICHE  EXPANSION  IN  THE 

CAVE  SWALLOW  IN  TEXAS Robert  F.  Martin  506 

HORNED  LARK  BREEDING  BIOLOGY  AT  CAPE  ST.  MARY’S,  NEWFOUNDLAND 

Richard  J.  Cannings  and  William  Threlfall  519 

ASPECTS  OF  THE  BREEDING  BIOLOGY  OF  A SUBTROPICAL  ORIOLE,  Icterus  gularis 

Barbara  Yohai  Pleasants  531 

GENERAL  NOTES 

AN  EXAMPLE  OF  A HYBRID  GREEN  JAY  X BLUE  JAY 

Warren  M.  Pulich  and  Rebecca  M.  Dellinger  538 

DUSKY  SEASIDE  SPARROW  FEEDS  RED-WINGED  BLACKBIRD  FLEDGLINGS 

James  L.  Rakestraw  and  James  L.  Baker  540 

STATISTICAL  SIGNIFICANCE  AND  DENSITY-DEPENDENT  NEST  PREDATION 

Stephen  D.  Fretwell  and  Frank  S.  Shipley  541 

A COMPARISON  OF  NEST-SITE  AND  PERCH-SITE  VEGETATION  STRUCTURE  FOR  SEVEN 

SPECIES  OF  WARBLERS Scott  L.  ColUns  542 

USE  OF  ARTIFICIAL  PERCHES  ON  BURNED  AND  UNBURNED  TALLGRASS  PRAIRIE 

Janet  Jean  Knodel-Montz  547 

JUVENILE  PEREGRINE  FALCON  SWOOPS  ON  ROSEATE  SPOONBILLS E.  Scott  Clark  548 

SYMBIOTIC  INTERACTION  BETWEEN  STARLINGS  AND  DEER  Robert  K.  Murphy  549 

CATTLE  EGRETS  FEEDING  IN  ASSOCIATION  WITH  HUMAN  WORKERS  G.  K.  Menon  549 

SCRUB  JAY  CAPTURES  HERMIT  THRUSH  IN  FLIGHT 

M.  Robert  McLandress  and  Use  McLandress  550 
FOOD  HABITS  OF  BLACK-BELLIED  WHISTLING  DUCKS  OCCUPYING  RICE  CULTURE  HABI- 
TATS   Godfrey  R.  Bourne  551 

HOUSE  SPARROWS  FLUSHING  PREY  FROM  TREES  AND  SHRUBS 

Harland  D.  Guillory  and  Jack  H.  Deshotels  554 

DIFFERENTIAL  PREDATION  BY  TWO  SPECIES  OF  PISCIVOROUS  BIRDS 

Fritz  L.  Knopf  and  Joseph  L.  Kennedy  554 

RED  PHALAROPE  EATING  CARRION  Wade  Wander  557 

RE-MATING  OF  A LESSER  SNOW  GOOSE 

Kenneth  F.  Abraham,  Pierre  Mineau  and  Fred  Cooke  557 

COMMON  EIDER  PLAYS  “POSSUM” Douglas  B.  McNair  559 

TERRITORIAL  ATTACHMENT  AND  MATE  FIDELITY  BY  HORNED  GREBES 

Robert  S.  Ferguson  560 

EFFECTS  OF  REDHEAD  NEST  PARASITISM  ON  MALLARDS 

Larry  G.  Talent,  Gary  L.  Krapu  and  Robert  L.  Jarvis  562 

SURVIVAL  OF  A DEMAXILLATE  RED-WINGED  BLACKBIRD Kent  L.  Fiala  563 

MINIMIZING  INVESTIGATOR  DISTURBANCE  IN  OBSERVATIONAL  STUDIES  OF  COLONIAL 
BIRDS:  ACCESS  TO  BLINDS  THROUGH  TUNNELS 

Gary  W.  Shugart,  Mary  A.  Fitch  and  Vern  M.  Shugart  565 


ORNITHOLOGICAL  LITERATURE  570 

PROCEEDINGS  OF  THE  SIXTY-SECOND  ANNUAL  MEETING 575 

INDEX  589 


i 


ACME 

BOCKSlNOiDG  CO..  INC. 

SEP  9 1983 

100  CAMBRIDGE  ST^IEET 
CHARLESTOWN,  M.vSS.